Bilingualism and Intelligence
Bilingualism and Intelligence
Bilingualism and Intelligence
Parents who are thinking about bilingual education for their children quite
reasonably ask the question: How will the emphasis on learning a second
language impact on my childs intellectual development? In other words, parents
want to know if bilingualism assists or hinders cognitive development the ability
to reason, learn and solve problems.
For many years, as recently as the late 1960s, there was a widespread belief in
the general community and among researchers that bilingualism and bilingual
learning hindered both innate intelligence and success in learning. Researchers
argued that there was a balance effect that bilingual learners gained in
linguistic competence, but lost ground in cognitive development compared with
monolingual students. However, by the late 1940s, these beliefs were being
seriously challenged, and research in South Africa, Ireland and Canada in the
1960s and 70s effectively put an end to the simple theory that bilingualism
hindered cognitive development. In fact the pendulum has swung the other way,
though not from one extreme to the other.
The earlier beliefs were based on simplistic understandings of the value of IQ test
scores. Researchers had more faith than was justified in the validity and
universality of these scores. Often, bilingual learners were tested in their weaker
language and then compared with monolinguals who had been tested in their
stronger - in fact, their only language. This would not happen today, as
educational psychologists are all aware of the socio-economic and linguistic
factors that need to be taken into account in interpreting test scores of this kind.
In fact, recent research suggests that bilinguals whose two languages are both
well developed tend to perform better on IQ tests than monolinguals. (At this
point we might just note that IQ is a very limited means of testing intelligence.
At Sarasas Ektra School, students are engaged in activities that challenge and
develop their multiple intelligences, but we can discuss this further in a future
issue of Metro Life.)
Further studies, where differences in language, gender and socio-economic
background have been taken into account, have yielded the following findings:
1. Students in bilingual immersion programs have scored as well as their
non-bilingual peers in tests of their common language, but much higher in
the second (minority or foreign) language.
2. Students in bilingual programs have greater metalinguistic awareness than
monolingual students. For example, young children are more aware that
the name of something is simply a convention of language it is not an
inherent property of the thing itself. They understand that a language is
an arbitrary system; that there is no right language against which other
languages can be judged. This is an essential awareness in a multilingual
and multicultural world.
3. This greater metalinguistic awareness appears also to give bilingual
students greater flexibility in the use of language. They have been found
to have greater insight into the potential diversity of language and are
more creative in their own use of language.
4. There is some evidence that linguistic flexibility extends in bilingual
children to general cognitive flexibility, an asset in a world of constant