Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

Bilingualism and Intelligence

Download as doc, pdf, or txt
Download as doc, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 3

Bilingualism and Intelligence

Parents who are thinking about bilingual education for their children quite
reasonably ask the question: How will the emphasis on learning a second
language impact on my childs intellectual development? In other words, parents
want to know if bilingualism assists or hinders cognitive development the ability
to reason, learn and solve problems.
For many years, as recently as the late 1960s, there was a widespread belief in
the general community and among researchers that bilingualism and bilingual
learning hindered both innate intelligence and success in learning. Researchers
argued that there was a balance effect that bilingual learners gained in
linguistic competence, but lost ground in cognitive development compared with
monolingual students. However, by the late 1940s, these beliefs were being
seriously challenged, and research in South Africa, Ireland and Canada in the
1960s and 70s effectively put an end to the simple theory that bilingualism
hindered cognitive development. In fact the pendulum has swung the other way,
though not from one extreme to the other.
The earlier beliefs were based on simplistic understandings of the value of IQ test
scores. Researchers had more faith than was justified in the validity and
universality of these scores. Often, bilingual learners were tested in their weaker
language and then compared with monolinguals who had been tested in their
stronger - in fact, their only language. This would not happen today, as
educational psychologists are all aware of the socio-economic and linguistic
factors that need to be taken into account in interpreting test scores of this kind.
In fact, recent research suggests that bilinguals whose two languages are both
well developed tend to perform better on IQ tests than monolinguals. (At this
point we might just note that IQ is a very limited means of testing intelligence.
At Sarasas Ektra School, students are engaged in activities that challenge and
develop their multiple intelligences, but we can discuss this further in a future
issue of Metro Life.)
Further studies, where differences in language, gender and socio-economic
background have been taken into account, have yielded the following findings:
1. Students in bilingual immersion programs have scored as well as their
non-bilingual peers in tests of their common language, but much higher in
the second (minority or foreign) language.
2. Students in bilingual programs have greater metalinguistic awareness than
monolingual students. For example, young children are more aware that
the name of something is simply a convention of language it is not an
inherent property of the thing itself. They understand that a language is
an arbitrary system; that there is no right language against which other
languages can be judged. This is an essential awareness in a multilingual
and multicultural world.
3. This greater metalinguistic awareness appears also to give bilingual
students greater flexibility in the use of language. They have been found
to have greater insight into the potential diversity of language and are
more creative in their own use of language.
4. There is some evidence that linguistic flexibility extends in bilingual
children to general cognitive flexibility, an asset in a world of constant

change and variation and a trait sought after by employers of people in


roles requiring sensitivity and problem-solving abilities.
5. Bilingual children have been found superior to monolinguals in higher
concept formation. One reason suggested for this is the wider range of
experiences bilingual children have due to their participation in two
cultures and linguistic systems.
6. The bilingual speakers habit of switching from one language to another
(code switching) though it may limited where bilingual speaking occurs
only in school settings assists in tasks requiring conceptual
reorganization (thinking about things in a new way).
The above are some findings that indicate that bilingualism is an asset, not a
liability, in cognitive development. There is little dispute with these findings.
Where dispute occurs in bilingual education research it is usually over the
question of methods and models of instruction and the nature and limits of the
research itself.
However, it should be noted that the cognitive benefits of bilingualism and
bilingual education are usually more evident as the childs ability in their weaker
language improves. One can think of the cognitive benefits in terms of a 3-level
progression.
Level One (weak in both languages)
Children have low levels of competence in both languages.
Children may have difficulty processing information in either language
Level Two (weak in one language)
Children have an age-appropriate level of competence in one language
(probably their mother tongue), but are weaker in the second language.
Their cognitive development will probably be similar to that of a
monolingual of similar age and circumstances.
Level Three (strong in both languages)
Children have age-appropriate level of competence in both languages.
They may have thinking advantages over monolinguals.
As we know with the linguistic advantages of bilingual education, so with
cognitive development we must allow some time for the advantages to take
effect. Where school-age students, as in Thailand, have limited opportunities to
speak their second language in authentic contexts outside school, it takes several
years of bilingual education before a high level of productive (speaking and
writing) as well as receptive (listening and reading) competence in the second
language is attained. However, the evidence is clear from the experience of
Sarasas Ektra School and other bilingual schools that students do arrive at high
levels of linguistic competence over time. One would hope that these students
have attained a superior level of cognitive development as well. The results of
Sarasas Ektra students on national tests and Entrance exams over the past six
years indicate that they have suffered no cognitive disadvantage. Their
performances at university level indicate that bilingual education has, indeed,
been of great benefit to them.
Amj170106

You might also like