This document summarizes two articles about the relationship between age and second language acquisition for school. The first article reviews studies that show children in middle childhood have an advantage over younger children and adolescents in formally acquiring a second language. The second article analyzes cognitive, sociocultural, affective, and linguistic factors that may account for age differences. Together the articles provide an overview to help practitioners make decisions about teaching limited-English-proficient students.
This document summarizes two articles about the relationship between age and second language acquisition for school. The first article reviews studies that show children in middle childhood have an advantage over younger children and adolescents in formally acquiring a second language. The second article analyzes cognitive, sociocultural, affective, and linguistic factors that may account for age differences. Together the articles provide an overview to help practitioners make decisions about teaching limited-English-proficient students.
This document summarizes two articles about the relationship between age and second language acquisition for school. The first article reviews studies that show children in middle childhood have an advantage over younger children and adolescents in formally acquiring a second language. The second article analyzes cognitive, sociocultural, affective, and linguistic factors that may account for age differences. Together the articles provide an overview to help practitioners make decisions about teaching limited-English-proficient students.
This document summarizes two articles about the relationship between age and second language acquisition for school. The first article reviews studies that show children in middle childhood have an advantage over younger children and adolescents in formally acquiring a second language. The second article analyzes cognitive, sociocultural, affective, and linguistic factors that may account for age differences. Together the articles provide an overview to help practitioners make decisions about teaching limited-English-proficient students.
In this issue of New Focus, which examines the relationship of age to
second language acquisition for school, two articles are presented. Virginia P. Collier reviews a number of studies, including her own recently completed one, that point to an advantage which children in middle childhood appear to have over younger children and adolescents in formally acquiring a second language. In the second article, Charles William Twyford analyzes a number of factors --cognitive, sociocultural, affective, and linguistic-- that may account for age differences in second language acquisition. Together the two articles provide an overview for practitioners that can form the basis for reasoned decisions in setting objectives, designing curricula, and selecting instructional strategies for limited-English-proficient students.
The Effect of Age on Acquisition of a Second Language
for School Virginia P. Collier George Mason University Introduction As the number of non-English- and limited-English-proficient students in our schools increases steadily, educators are looking for more efficient and cost-effective ways to improve these students' English skills so that they can participate fully in the curriculum of their schools. It is natural, then, for teachers, curriculum developers, administrators, and government officials to ask how long it should take for limited-English-proficient students to break through the language limitations that hinder their learning and advancement. The only simple answer to that question is "It depends." It depends on the learner's cognitive style, socioeconomic background, formal schooling in first language, and many other factors. A substantial amount of research, testing the old axiom that young learners learn best, also tells us that successful language acquisition depends on the learner's age. This article, recognizing the interaction of many variables in second language acquisition, will examine what has been said about the effect of age on the amount of time students need to acquire a second language. The conclusions presented here, when considered with other research findings and specific student information, can guide planners and practitioners toward the implementation of more effective programs for limited-English-proficient students.
Is There a Critical Period for Second Language Acquisition?
Some of the earliest studies of the effect of age on the acquisition of a second language focused on proving or disproving Lenneberg's (1967) critical period hypothesis. Lenneberg theorized that the acquisition of language is an innate process determined by biological factors which limit the critical period for acquisition of a language from roughly two years of age to puberty. Lenneberg believed that after lateralization (a process by which the two sides of the brain develop specialized functions), the brain loses plasticity. Lenneberg claimed that lateralization of the language function is normally completed at puberty, making post-adolescent language acquisition difficult. Many studies have tested this hypothesis by comparing children to adults in the acquisition of pronunciation. Studies examining subjects' pronunciation after over five years of exposure to the second language consistently find that the large majority of adults retain their accent when the second language is acquired after puberty, whereas children initiating second language acquisition before puberty have little or no foreign accent (e.g., Asher and Garcia, 1969; Oyama, 1976; Seliger, Krashen and Ladefoged, 1975; Tahta, Wood and Loewenthal, 1981). Two studies assessing students' acquisition of pronunciation after three years of exposure to the second language found that younger students had retained more accent-free pronunciation when compared to adolescents just past puberty (Fathman, 1975; Williams, 1979). Researchers have debated the age at which lateralization actually occurs. Kinsbourne (1975) proposes completion by birth; Krashen (1973) suggests it may be complete by age 5; Lenneberg (1967) proposes lateralization by puberty. Long (1988) suggests that the brain's loss of placticity is also due to other aspects of cerebral maturation unrelated to lateralization. Regardless of the exact timing of lateralization or other related factors, evidence is very strong that most people who acquire a second language after puberty retain an accent in the second language. It may be that the effort to test the critical period hypothesis is called too much attention to one aspect of language proficiency - pronunciation - and to the child/adult dichotomy. Educators may be more concerned about differences in language acquisition of young children (4-7), older children (8-12), and adolescence (13-16), and they are interested in more aspects of language to be mastered than just pronunciation. The sections which follow examine the effect of age on school children's acquisition of progressively complex language domains: first, basic oral skills, then language skills for school including oral and written skills, and finally language skills in content area development. Does Age Affect Basic Oral Second Language Development? The critical period studies usually focused on child-adult differences and suggested that younger learners, still operating within the critical period, should be superior learners. However, studies of oral language skill acquisition by children of different ages has led to the conclusion that, initially, older children acquire faster than younger children. For example, Ervin-Tripp (1974) found that after nine months of instruction in French, 7- to-9-year-olds performed better than 4- to 6-year-olds did in comprehension, imitation, and conversation. Similarly, Fathman (1975) found that in the first year of study, 11- to 15-year-olds were significantly better at acquiring English as a second language than 6- to 10-year-olds in pronunciation, morphology, and syntax. In Fathman's study, however, by year three the younger students outperformed the older ones on the same types of measures. This flip-flop pattern continues in study after study on short-term versus long-term exposure to the language; i.e., an early advantage is seen in the acquisition of oral skills for older acquirers (even those after puberty), but younger acquirers eventually catch up and outperform the older ones after several years' time (Grinder, Otomo and Toyota, 1962; Hamayan, Saegert and Larudee, 1977; Krashen, Scarcella and Long, 1982; Oyama, 1978; Patkowski, 1980; Ramirez and Politzer, 1978; Snow and Hoefnagel-Hohle, 1978; Stern, 1967; and Winitz, 1981). It is important to note that these studies report a pattern of age differences as seen in studies of basic oral skills in a second language, not the more complex skills required for formal schooling. The next section examines the effect of age on acquiring those skills. Does Age Affect the Development of Language Proficiency for School?? If English is being acquired for academic purposes, the level of proficiency expected is much more complex than English for day-to-day survival. Cummins (1979; 1980; 1981a; 1981b) has proposed a model for second language acquisition that distinguishes between these two types of language proficiency: language for general social interaction and language for school. Cummins' distinction between face-to-face conversational proficiency and proficiency which requires the speaker to rely solely on the language itself can help clarify the effect that age has on the language acquisition of limited- English-proficient students.
Cummins uses the term 'context-embedded' to describe face-to-face communication
where meaning can be negotiated. This type of communication is enhanced by a wide range of paralinguistic (e.g., gesture, facial expression) and situational cues. On the other hand, context-reduced language relies primarily on linguistic or language-based cues to meaning and is more difficult to produce and comprehend. Cummins' model (see Figure 1) consists of two intersecting continuums: the first from context-embedded language to context-reduced language, and the second from cognitively undemanding language (which requires little conscious attention to language forms or choices) to cognitively demanding language (which requires the active cognitive efforts of the speaker/writer to produce). Language proficiency required for school tasks can incorporate the whole range of skills in all four quadrants, but it is especially for school that students need to develop context- reduced and cognitively demanding aspects of language in order to function successfully in the classroom. Swain (1981) describes some of these aspects of language for school: Language which is used to explain, to classify, to generalize, to abstract, to manipulate ideas, to gain knowledge, and to apply that knowledge (doing so eventually with only language providing the contextualizing cues) constitutes essential aspects of the cognitive demands made on students as they progress in school. One of the goals of the educational system is that students be able to make use of decontextualized language, that is, to be able to use language alone as a tool for learning in reading and listening; and to use language alone as a tool for conceptualizing, drawing abstract generalizations, expressing complex relationships in speaking and writing (p.5). For academic purposes, students need to acquire as complete a range of skills in the second language as possible. Language in school becomes increasingly abstract as students move from one grade level to the next. Language becomes the focus of every content area task, with all meaning and all demonstration of knowledge expressed through oral and written forms of language. It would be good to know, then, at what ages and after what length of time students do best in acquiring a second language for school. Several researchers have conducted studies comparing the performance of students of different ages on language tasks associated with school skills, including reading and writing. The short-term studies once again show an initial advantage for the older students, but in contrast to the previous studies cited on basic oral second language development, the long-term studies show a continuing advantage for the older students (ages 8 to 12). When examining age on arrival, most studies of both short-term and long- term acquisition find that students arriving between the ages of 8 and 12 are faster in early acquisition of second language skills, and over several years' time they maintain this advantage over younger arrivals of 4 to 7 years. Table 1, on the next page, summarizes the findings of several studies that support this conclusion. Based on this review, we can assert that older students (ages 8 to 12) are faster, more efficient acquirers of school language than younger students (ages 4 to 7). In many of the studies reviewed, young children beginning the study of a second language between the ages of 4 and 7 take much longer to master skills needed for academic purposes than older children do. Why is this so? Several explanations have been proposed, though none yet has conclusive research support. First, we know that children who enter school at age 5 or 6 have not completed acquisition of their first language, which continues through at least age 12. From ages 6 to 12, children still are in the process of developing in first language the complex skills of reading and writing, in addition to continuing acquisition of more complex rules of morphology and syntax, elaboration of speech acts, expansion of vocabulary, semantic development, and even some subtleties in phonological development (McLaughlin, 1984, pp. 41-43). It may be, then, that when young children are asked to learn a second language for use at school before their first language has sufficiently matured to serve as a source of transferable skills, the learning task is very burdensome and requires more time than older children need--children whose first language skills are available for transfer. (The older children in the studies cited had received schooling in their first language.) It has also been argued (Ausubel, 1964; Burtsall, et al., 1974; Cummins, 1981a; Taylor, 1974) that older learners have an advantage in cognitive maturity, which gives them more strategies for acquiring a new language. For example, Scarcella and Higa (1982) showed in an experimental study that older learners take a more active role than younger ones in negotiating understanding and sustaining conversations. As a result, they succeed in obtaining input that is better suited to their learning needs. TABLE 1
STUDIES OF SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISTION FOR SCHOOL
RESEARCHER STUDENTS SKILLS/TASKS RESULTS
Grinder, Otomo, and Second- and Vocabulary, listening Fourth-Graders
Toyota (1962) fourth- graders comprehension superior to second- after one year of graders study
Ervin-Tripp (1974) English speakers Listening, imitation, 7- to 9-year-olds
acquiring French taped natural superior to 4- to 6-year- in Switzerland, conversation, diary olds in syntax, tested in first 9 writing, translation morphology and months pronunciation
Burstall (1975) 16-year-old Listening, speaking, Students who started
English speakers reading, writing French at age 8 only introduced to slightly superior to French at age 8 or those starting at age 11 11
Ekstrand (1976) Immigrants to Listening, Older students better
Sweden LOR*2; pronunciation, free oral than younger ones on AOA**6-14 production, reading all measures comprehension, free writing
Snow and English speakers Pronunciation, auditory At LOR 6 months,
Hoefnagel-Hohle acquiring Dutch in discrimination, adults and 12- to 15- (1978) Holland. Age morphology, sentence year-olds superior on groups: 3-5, 6-7, repetition, sentence all measures. By LOR 8-10, 12-25, and translation, sentence 15-15 months, adult adults. Tested at judgement, story progress slower; 8- to LOR 6 months, 10 comprehension, 10-year-olds and 12- to months and 14-15 Peabody Picture 15-year-olds surpassed months. Vocabulary Test all others. 3- to 5-year- olds consistently worst performers on all measures
Skutnabb-Kangas Finnish Listening, speaking, Students AOA 9-11
(1979a) immigrants to reading, writing significantly better than Sweden students AOA 6-8
Kamin, and Kanna students who reading, writing at age 12 (1400 hours (1980) began French of instruction) roughly immersion equivalent to group program at age 5 starting at age 5 (4000 or age 12 hours of instruction)
Ferris and Politzer Eighth-grade Writing Students schooled in
(1981) Mexican Spanish in Mexico for immigrants to the grades K-3 equal to U.S. those schooled completely in English in the U.S. for 9 years. Students of AOA 9 years, LOR 5 years had more positive attitudes toward school and higher grade point averages than those of LOR 9 years, AOA 5 years
Cummins, Swain, Japanese students English vocabulary, On second language
Nakajima, in Canada, Grades reading, preposition school skills, older Hanscombe, Green 2-3 and 5-6; usage, sentence students significantly and Fran (1984) Vietnamese ages repetition, oral better; on context- 9-17 interviews embedded measures (basic skills), younger students better. First language school skills accounted for significant proportion of variance in second language skill
LOR = Length of residence
** AOA = Age on arrival Finally, Snow and Hoefnagel-Hohle (1978) concluded after a study of second language acquirers in Holland that their finding of superior initial performance by older learners was perhaps due to the greater academic demands placed on these learners by the schools, creating higher levels of motivation in them than in younger learners to learn the language necessary for success in school. Whatever the reasons might be, practitioners should be alert to the differences between younger and older school children in the amount of time required for them to develop second language skills adequate for schooling. Older learners (ages 8 to 12) have an advantage, at least initially. Does Age Affect Content-Area Achievement When Schooling is in a Second Language? Collier (1987) analyzed the length of time required for 1,548 immigrants to the U.S. to become proficient in second language skills for all content areas when schooled only in English. Students who had been mainstreamed after instruction in English as a second language were tested in the fourth, sixth, eighth, and eleventh grades on reading, language arts, social studies, science, and mathematics using standardized tests produced by Science Research Associates (SRA). The study included a range of students beginning with those who began exposure to English, their second language, at age 5 and continuing through those beginning at age 15. Length of residence ranged from two to five years. Over 75 first languages were represented in the sample. Only students who were at grade level when they entered the U.S. and who had no previous exposure to English were included in the study. Social class background of the sample was middle to upper class in the home country with relatively lower income in the U.S. but with strong middle-class aspirations. Collier found that students who were 8 to 12 years old on arrival were the first to reach norms for native speakers (50th percentile or normal curve equivalent [NCE]) on all content-area tests, doing so within four to five years. Students who were 5 to 7 years old on arrival fell significantly behind the older children in academic achievement, requiring five to eight years to reach the 50th NCE, assuming a continued rate of gain similar to the one at the time of the study. Arrivals at ages 12 to 15 experienced the greatest difficulty reaching age and grade norms, requiring 6 to 8 years at their same rate of gain. That finding appears at first glance to contradict the generalization that older students whose first language proficiency is better developed acquire a second language for school more rapidly than younger students. However, it may actually be pointing to the increasing complexity of language development at each succeeding grade level and the results of taking time away from content-area instruction while acquiring a second language. Students who are 12 years old on arrival, for example, and who enter seventh grade and begin their schooling completely in English with no previous exposure to English, must take time to acquire beginning levels of basic oral English. As they master sufficient basic English and develop a wide enough range of vocabulary in English to move into deeper development of English for school, they may, in the meantime, lose two to three years of learning in mathematics, science, and social studies at their age and grade level. Secondary students can ill afford a loss of two to three years of complex content-area development if they are to achieve at the performance level of native speakers after a given period of time. After puberty, then, despite advantageous learning rates, there are two problems with beginning acquisition of a second language: (1) students are more likely to retain an accent, and (2) if academic work is not continued while students are acquiring a second language, there is not enough time left in high school to make up the lost years of academic instruction. Conclusions It is clear that age (or age-related factors) is a major variable in the acquisition of a second language for school. In the early stages of acquisition, older students are faster and more efficient than younger students. Older students have the advantage of cognitive development in their first language to assist them with acquiring school skills in the second language. This early advantage diminishes after the first year for adults, but remains for older children and adolescents for continuing development of their second language skills. Adolescents past puberty are likely to retain an accent in the second language. Otherwise, they are capable of developing complete second language proficiency. When schooled only in the second language, students in the 8-12 age range on arrival may be the most advantaged acquirers of school skills in the second language, since they have some first language skills to transfer and they still have time to make up the years of academic instruction lost while acquiring basic second language skills and beginning to acquire school skills in the second language. Even though adolescents can acquire second language school skills at a fast pace, they have less time to make up lost years of academic instruction easily. It is important to note that the effect of age diminishes over time as the acquirer becomes more proficient in the second language. Differences are generally found through the first five years after arrival. It takes language minority students in any type of program a minimum of four years to reach native speakers' level of school language proficiency and may take as many as eight or more years, depending on age on arrival and type of school program, as well as sociocultural factors and the individual characteristics of each second language acquirer. References Asher, J., and Garcia, G. (1969). The optimal age to learn a foreign language. Modern Language Journal, 38, 334-341. Ausubel, D.D. (1964). Adult versus children in second-language learning: Psychological consideration. Modern Language Journal, 48, 420-24. Burstall, C., Jamieson, M., Cohen, S., and Hargreaves, M. (1974). Primary French in the balance. Slough: NFELR Publishing Co. Burstall, C. (1975). Primary French in the balance. Educational Research, 17, 193-198. Collier, V.P. (1987). Age and rate of acquisition of second language for academic purposes. TESOL Quarterly, 21, 617-641. (ERIC Abstract) or (NCBE Abstract) Cummins, J. (1979). Cognitive/academic language proficiency, linguistic interdependence, the optimal age question, and some other matters. Working Papers on Bilingualism, 19, 197-205. Cummins, J. (1980). The entry and exit fallacy in bilingual education. NABE Journal, 4(3), 25-59. (ERIC Abstract) or (NCBE Abstract) Cummins, J. (1981a). Age on arrival and immigrant second language learning in Canada: A reassessment. Applied Linguistics, 2, 132-149. (NCBE Abstract) Cummins, J. (1981b). The role of primary language development in promoting educational success for language minority students. In California State Department of Education, Schooling and language minority students: A theoretical framework (pp. 3- 49). Los Angeles: Evaluation, Dissemination, and Assessment Center, California State University. (NCBE Abstract) Cummins, J. (1984). Bilingualism and special education: Issues in assessment and pedagogy. Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters. Cummins, J., Swain, M., Nakajima, K, Handscombe, J., Green, D., and Tran, C. (1984). Linguistic interdependence among Japanese and Vietnamese immigrant students. In C. Rivera (Ed.), Communicative competence approaches to language proficiency assessment: Research and application (pp. 60-81). Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters. Ekstrand, L. (1976). Age and length of residence as variables related to the adjustment of migrant children, with special reference to second language learning. In G. Nickel (Ed.), Proceedings of the Fourth International Congress of Applied Linguistics (Vol. 3, pp. 179-197). Stuttgart: Hochschulverlag. Ervin-Tripp, S.M. (1974). Is second language learning like the first? TESOL Quarterly, 8,111-127. (NCBE Abstract) Fathman, A. (1975). The relationship between age and second language productive ability. Language Learning, 25, 245-253. Ferris, M.R., and Politzer, R.L. (1981). Effects of early and delayed second language acquisition: English composition skills of Spanish-speaking junior high school students. TESOL Quarterly, 15, 263-274. Grinder, R., Otomo, A., and Toyota, W. (1962). Comparisons between second, third, and fourth grade children in the audiolingual learning of Japanese as a second language. The Journal of Educational Research, 56(4), 463469. Hamayan, E., Saegert, J., and Larudee, P. (1977). Elicited imitation in second language learners. Language and Speech, 20(1), 86-97. Kinsbourne, M. (1975). The ontogeny of cerebral dominance. In D. Aaronson and R. Rieber (Eds.), Developmental psycholinguistics and communication disorders. New York: New York Academy of Sciences. Krashen, S.D. (1973). Lateralization, language learning, and the critical period: Some new evidence. Language Learning, 23, 63-74. Krashen, S.D., Scarcella R.C., and Long, M.A. (Eds.). (1982). Child-adult differences in second language acquisition. Rowley, MA: Newbury House. (ERIC Abstract) or (NCBE Abstract) Lapkin, S., Swain, M., Kamin, J., and Hanna, G. (1980). "Report on the 1979 evaluation of the Peel County late French immersion program, grades 8, 9, 10, 11 and 12." Mimeographed. Toronto: Ontario Institute for Studies in Education. Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Long, M. (1988). Maturational constraints on language development. University of Hawaii Working Papers in ESL, (7)1,1-53. McLaughlin, B. (1984). Second language acquisition in childhood: Vol. 1. Preschool children (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Oyama, S. (1976). A sensitive period for the acquisition of a nonnative phonological system. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 5,261-285. Oyama, S. (1978). The sensitive period and comprehension of speech. Working Papers on Bilingualism, 16, 1-17. Patkowski, M.S. (1980). The sensitive period for the acquisition of syntax in a second language. Language Learning, 30, 449- 472. (NCBE Abstract) Ramirez, A.G., and Politzer, R.L (1978). Comprehension and production in English as a second language by elementary school children and adolescents. In E.M. Hatch (Ed.), Second language acquisition: A book of readings (pp. 31-332). Rowley, MA: Newbury House. (NCBE Abstract) Scarcella, R.C. and Higa, C.A. (1982). Input and age differences in second language acquisition. In S.D. Krashen, R.C. Scarcella and M.H. Long (eds.), Child-adult differences in second language acquisition. Rowley, MA: Newbury House. (NCBE Abstract) Seliger, H., Krashen, S., and Ladefoged, P. (1975). Maturational constraints in the acquisition of second languages. Language Sciences, 38, 20-22. Skutnabb-Kangas, T. (1979). Language in the process of cultural assimilation and structural incorporation of linguistic minorities. Wheaton, MD: National Clearinghouse for Bilingual Education. (NCBE Abstract) Snow, C., and Hoefnagel-Hohle, M. (1978). The critical period for language acquisition: Evidence from second language learning. Child Development, 49, 1114-1128. Stern, H.H. (Ed.) (1967). Foreign languages in primary education: The teaching of foreign or second languages to younger children. London: Oxford University Press. Swain, M. (1981). Time and timing in bilingual education. Language Learning, 31,1-15. (NCBE Abstract) Tahta, S., Wood, M., and Loewenthal, K. (1981). Foreign accents: Factors relating to transfer of accent from the first language to a second language. Language and Speech, 24(3), 265-272. Taylor, B.P. (1974). Toward a theory of language acquisition. Language Learning, 24, 25-35. Williams, L. (1979). The modification of speech perception and production in second language learning. Perception and Psychophysics, 26(2), 95-104. Winitz, H. (1981). Input considerations in the comprehension of first and second language. Annals of the New York Academy Sciences, 379, 296-308. (NCBE Abstract) Other References for Further Reading Asher, J., and Price, B. (1967). The learning strategy of total physical response: Some age differences. Child Development, 38, 1219-1227. Brown, H.D. (1980). The optimal distance model of second language acquisition. TESOL Quarterly, 14, 157-164. (NCBE Abstract) Collier, V.P. (1987). "Choosing language tests: A taxonomy of language proficiency measures." Paper presented at the annual convention of Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages, Miami, FL Cummins, J. (1978). Educational implications of mother tongue maintenance in minority-language groups. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 34, 395-416. (NCBE Abstract) Cummins, J. (1986). Empowering minority students: A framework for intervention. Harvard Educational Review, 56, 18-36. (NCBE Abstract) Ekstrand, L (1978). English without a book revisited: The effect of age on second language acquisition in a formal setting. Didakometry, No.60. Malmo, Sweden: Department of Educational and Psychological Research, School of Education. Gale, K., McClay, D., Christie, M., and Harris, S. (1981). Academic achievement in the Milinigimbi bilingual education program. TESOL Quarterly, 15,297-314. Genesee, F. (1978). Is there an optimal age for starting second language instruction? McGill Journal of Education, 13, 145- 154. Gersten, R., and Woodward, J. (1985). A case for structured immersion. Educational Leadership, 43(1), 75-79. Harley, B. (1986). Age in second language acquisition. San Diego: College-Hill Press. (NCBE Abstract) Krashen, S.D., Long, MA, and Scarcella, R.C. (1979). Age, rate, and eventual attainment in second language acquisition. TESOL Quarterly, 13, 573-582. Long, M.H. (1983). Does second language instruction make a difference? A review of research. TESOL Quarterly, 13, 573-582. Malherbe, E.G. (1978). Bilingual education in the Republic of South Africa. In B. Spolsky and R. Cooper (Eds.), Case studies in bilingual education. Rowley, MA: Newbury House. (NCBE Abstract) McConnell, B.B., and Kendall, J.R. (1987, April). "Application of the cohort model to evaluate bilingual programs: The 'BELEPS' program." Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Washington, DC Medina, M., Saldate, S., and Mishra, S. (1985). The sustaining effects of bilingual instruction: A follow-up study. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 12(3), 132-139. Ogbu, J.U., and Matute-Bianchi, M.E. (1986). Understanding sociocultural factors: Knowledge, identity, and school adjustment. In California State Department of Education, Beyond language: Social and cultural factors in schooling language minority students (pp. 73-142). Los Angeles: Evaluation, Dissemination and Assessment Center, California State University. Olson, L, and Samuels, S.J. (1973). The relationship between age and accuracy of foreign language pronunciation. Journal of Educational Research, 66, 26-267. Plante, A.J. (1977). The Connecticut "pairing" model proves effective in bilingual/ bicultural education. Phi Delta Kappan, 58, 427. Ramsey, C.A, and Wright, E.N. (1974). Age and second language learning. The Journal of Social Psychology, 94,115-121. Rivera, C. (Ed.). (1984). Language proficiency and academic achievement. Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters. (NCBE Abstract) Schumann, J.H. (1978). The acculturation model for second language acquisition. In R.C. Gingras (Ed.), Second language acquisition and foreign language teaching (pp. 27- 50). Arlington, VA: Center for Applied Linguistics. (NCBE Abstract) Scovel, T. (1981). The effects of neurological age on nonprimary language acquisition. In R. Andersen (Ed.), New dimensions in second language acquisition research (pp. 33- 42). Rowley, MA: Newbury House. Seliger, H.W. (1981). Exceptions to critical period predictions: A sinister plot. In R. Andersen (Ed.), New dimensions in second language acquisition research (pp. 47-57). Rowley, MA: Newbury House. Seright, L (1985). Age and aural comprehension achievement in francophone adults learning English. TESOL Quarterly, 19, 455-473. (NCBE Abstract) Snow, C., and Hoefnagel-Hohle, M. (1977). Age differences in the pronunciation of foreign sounds. Language and Speech, 20, 357-365. Swain, M., and Lapkin, S. (1981). Bilingual education in Ontario: A decade of research. Toronto: Ministry of Education. (ERIC Abstract) or (NCBE Abstract) Tempes, F., Burnham, L., Pina, M., Campos, J., Matthews, S., Lear, E., and Herbert, C. (1984, January). "Implementing theoretically sound programs: Do they really work?" Paper presented at the annual conference of the California Association for Bilingual Education, San Francisco. (NCBE Abstract) Thogmartin, C. (1982). Age, individual differences in musical and verbal aptitude, and pronunciation achievement by elementary school children learning a foreign language. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 20(1), 66-72. Troike, R. (1978). Research evidence for the effectiveness of bilingual education. NABE Journal, 3(1), 13-24. (NCBE Abstract) Vorih, L, and Rosier, P. (1978). Rock Point community school: An example of a Navajo- English bilingual elementary school program. TESOL Quarterly, 12, 263-269. (ERIC Abstract) or (NCBE Abstract)
About the Author
Virginia P. Collier is currently Assistant Professor of Education and Associate Director of the Center for Bilingual/Multicultural/ESL Teacher Preparation at George Mason University in Fairfax, Virginia. Her research focuses on teaching methods, language assessment, curriculum development, and second language acquisition as applied to bilingual/ESL settings. Dr. Collier is co-author of the teacher training textbook, Bilingual and ESL Classrooms: Teaching in Multicultural Contexts (McGraw-Hill, 1985). These publications were prepared under Contract No. 300860069 for the Office of Bilingual Education and Minority Languages Affairs (OBEMLA), U.S. Department of Education. The contents of these publications do not necessarily reflect the views or policies of the Department of Education, nor does mention of trade names, commercial products, or organizations imply endorsement by the U.S. Government.
This digital version was prepared by ERIC Clearinghouse on Urban Education,
Teachers College, Columbia University as part of its subcontract activities with NCBE. NCBE Home Page http://www.ncbe.gwu.edu
1 Promoting Early Language and Literacy Development of English Language Learners.docx Filename UTF-8!1!20Promoting 20Early 20Language 20and 20Literacy 20Development 20of 20English 20Language 20Learners
Domain General Auditory Processing As A Conceptual and Measurement Framework For Second Language Speech Learning Aptitude A Test Retest Reliability Study