SDVASDFV
SDVASDFV
SDVASDFV
Author
Title
This byok should be returned on or before the date Last marked bdtov
EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
THE MACMILLAN COMPANY
MEW YORK BOSTON CHICAGO DALLAS
ATLANTA SAN FRANCISCO
BY
DANIEL STARCH, PH. Do
UNIVERSITY OK WISCONSIN
fork
THE MACMILLAN COMP^
1924
jt right'
CoPYttlQHT, 1919,
'
VTING COMPANY
TO MY WIFE
PREFACE
The preparation of this book has been carried out according to
two fundamental purposes: First, to present that material which
seems to be most useful and relevant to the problems of educa-
tional psychology; and second, to maintain a strictly experimental,
psychological process.
JnThelBfoadest sense, educationis
thejDroduction of useful
changes in human beings.
1
These~cnanges may be classified into
three" drvlslonsT cMhges in knowledge, in skill^ and in ideals.
Through education the child is to acquire useful knowledge; he is
1
Thorndike has defined the purpose of education thus: "The aim of education is, as
we have seen, to change uman
1 beings for the better, so that they will have more humane
and useful wants and be inore able to satisfy them." ('12, p. 52.)
2 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
Which changes are useful and which are not is a question that
cannot be answered as easily as it would seem at first glance.
Learning to read or to figure are obviously useful modifications;
but it is not so easy to say whether the study of a given drama, or
the knowledge of certain facts of history, or the understanding of
a certain theory of matter, or ability to read a given foreign lan-
guage, are useful, or sufficiently useful to be included in the com-
mon school, in the high school, or in the university. The term
useful should not be limited narrowly to the things which are
immediately applicable in making a living, but should include
all ^changes which will broaden and enrich, the life of the in-
. . .
dividual.
TEe Problems of Educational Psychology. In accordance with
our definition, the fundamental problems that we must raise
concerning education are as follows:
1. What changes are to be made in human beings?
2. What are the agencies by which the changes may be brought
about?
3. What are the capacities which human beings possess for
acquiring the changes?
4. What are the most economical methods by which these
Europeans.
The second problem deals primarily with the value of school
subjects and exercises in bringing about the changes that are to be
made. To what extent will the study of arithmetic, the study of
grammar, or the study of physics or Latin be able to produce the
training that philosophy and sociology dictate? This problem is
partly sociological and partly psychological. It is sociological in
so far as the determination of educational ager.cies depends upon
the physical and social environment of mankind; it is psychological
in so far as it necessitates a study of mental processes affected or
PROBLEMS AND SCOPE 3
each subject.
3. The most economical methods of learning the material
of each subject.
PARTI
THE NATIVE EQUIPMENT OF HUMAN BEINGS
CHAPTER H
THE INSTINCTIVE ELEMENTS OF NATIVE EQUIPMENT
getting food when hungry, to seek shelter when cold, to offer re-
sistance when hemmed in, to spit out what tastes bad, and the
like, are instinctive reactions. Capacities are distinguished from
reflexes and instincts in bemj^enera^naeiital abilities rather than
specific motor responses and in referring primarily to the native
mental equipment, such as the powers of sensation, perception,
retention, attention, imagination, and all the varied complex
psychic processes. f
Classification of Instinctive Responses. The olderjd^ssifica-
tions of instincts usually divided them intq^three or four large
groups of responses, such as individual, racial, and social, and re-
garded them rather as generartendencies than as specific responses.
The presentconccption of Instincts is to regard them as specific
responses with inherited neuraj. mechanisms which will be set
into action by specific stimuli or situations. On
this basis the
classification consists of an enumeration of as many definite, identi-
unlearned reactions to specific situations as can be observed
fiable,
and as can be recognized in human beings prior to training and
habituation in each particular type of activity. Accordingly,
Thorndike ('14, 1) enumerates forty or more different types of in-
stinctive reactions as follows:
13. Anger.
INSTINCTIVE ELEMENTS OF NATIVE EQUIPMENT II
27. Cooperation.
28. Suggestibility and opposition.
29. Envious and jealous behavior.
30. Greed.
31. Ownership.
32. Kindliness.
33. Teasing, tormenting, and bullying.
34. Imitation.
III. Minor bodily movements and cerebral connections.
35. Vocalizations.
36. Visual exploration.
37. Manipulation.
38. Cleanliness.
39. Curiosity.
40. Multiform mental activities.
41. Multiform physical activities.
42. Play.
much if not more to do with the latter than with the former. The
direct appeal to, and use of, instinctive reactions in actual concrete
instances in school work are not as frequent and specific as is com-
monly implied. The number of instincts enumerated in the pre-
ceding list which may be directly and concretely appealed to in the
learning of a school subject is relatively small. The best way to be
convinced on this point is to take the various instincts one by one,
and to determine to what extent each one may be appealed to or
used in teaching the various subjects. The number of specific
applications is much smaller than one is likely to anticipate. Two-
thirds or three-fourths of them are probably never immediately
but only indirectly concerned in school exercises, and most of the
remaining ones, such as rivalry, cooperation, collecting and hoard-
ing, are serviceable chiefly as general motives. As such they are,
to be sure, highly important.
Wemust, of course, not minimize .the place and importance of
instinctive reactions in behavior as a whole. They furnish the
general motives and mechanisms for doing and learning, but the
mental capacities are more directly and concretely involved in the
acquisition of knowledge and skill in school subjects.
The instinctive elements in learning any school subject are for
the most part simple reflex actions or undeveloped connections.
Take learning to read as an illustration. The chief instinctive
elements probably are the reflexes in the control of the eyes, the
neural mechanism for receiving and transmitting visual impulses
to the brain, the capacities for attentiveness and retentiveness,
and partial motor control of the speech organs. The process of
learning to read assumes these and uses them; but, what is
more important from the practical side of getting the meaning
of the printed word is the establishment of countless new cbn-
nections.
^be
no education except through the activity of the child himself;
and no activity can take place which does not ultimately depend
upon native tendencies. They are the origin of effort, the springs of
action. The skillful teacher plays upon them and appeals to them
in countless ways. The ability to do this is an art which is not
from books; it is acquired rather by patient practice
easily learned
and by sympathetic contact with children.
The energizing po^er of instinct makes itself felt largely through
INSTINCTIVE ELEMENTS OF NATIVE EQUIPMENT 13
the child his activities in the order in which the interests for
them appear." This is known as the recapitulation theory of
instincts.
ojLitJhejjarticular ways
inwhich the school may cooperate with the
inborn forces of 'child nature. It is easy enough to say "work with
nature," but just how is that to be done in teaching a pupil how to
make the letter "a," or to learn the reading of a printed word, or to
acquire correct speech, or to learn the grammatical rules of a foreign
language? Ultimately, the concrete use of the principle must be
determined experimentally. Our definite knowledge of the tech-
nique of learning in the case of school subjects is appallingly limited.
Only by careful and painstaking experimentation can this principle
be made useful in the concrete work of the school in anything more
than an offhand impressionistic manner.
The questionable assumption is that the instincts are infallible
guides of human life. It may be argued that since instincts are*
such powerful springs to action as to have maintained the individual
and the race for numberless generations, they must necessarily be
dependable in producing action and interest of the right sort. But
the question may fairly be raised: Are the native tendencies always
right so that we should always cooperate with them and never coun-
teract or curb them? The theory of the infallibility of instincts is
based on the belief that for countless ages nature has found by ex-
perimentation and natural selection what is best for the individual.
Whatever the child is inclined to do by virtue of his natural pro-
clivities is right and good for him; or, if apparently not useful, it is a
16 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
which original nature adapts man is probably far more like the life of
the wolf or ape, than like the life that now is, as a result of human art,
habit and reasoning, perpetuating themselves in language, tools, build-
ings, books and customs." ('14, 1, p. 280.)
P- 235.)
"But with the teens all this begins to be changed and many of these
precepts must be gradually reversed. There is an outburst of growth
i8 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
that needs a large part of the total kinetic energy of the body. There is
;i new interest in adults, a passion to be treated like one's elders, to
170
160
150
'Z 140
^
1 180
120
55 6.5 7.5 8.5 9.5 10.5 11.5 12.5 13.5 14.5 15.5 16.5 17.5 18.5
Age in Years
make plans for the future, a new sensitiveness to adult praise or blame.
The large muscles have their innings and there is a new clumsiness of
body and mind. The blood-vessels expand and blushing is increased,
new sensations and feelings arise, the imagination blossoms, love of
140
16 17 18
nature is born, music is felt in a new, more inward way, fatigue come.,
easier and sooner; and if heredity and environment enable the individual
INSTINCTIVE ELEMENTS OF NATIVE EQUIPMENT 19
"The social instincts undergo sudden unfoldment and the new life of
love awakens. and of religion, of rapid fluctua-
It is the age of sentiment
tion of mood, and the world seems strange and new. Interest in adult
life and in vocations develops. Youth awakes to a new world and under-
stands neither it nor himself." ('04, Preface p. XV.)
FIG. 3. Memory for digits based upon tests of 937 pupils. After Smedle>
languages earlier than is customary, but not for any reason of more
rapid memorizing at an earlier age. If rapidity of tapping, Figure 2,
is any indication at all of endurance or of quickness of becoming
do not drop but tend to rise gradually even during the years from
eleven to fifteen.There is practically a level at the age of eleven
but no drop.
The unrevivability of instincts through disuse has been advocated
chiefly by James as follows:
no such objects are met with, then no habit will be formed; and, later
on in life, when the animal meets the objects, he will altogether fail to
react, as at the earlier epoch he would instinctively have done." ('90,
ii, P 39 8.)
.
678 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17
Ages
FIG. 6. The number of collections made by children at various ages. After
;
C. F. Burk ( oo).
games and sports, and learns neither to play ball, nor row, nor sail, nor
ride, nor skate, nor fish, nor shoot, probably he will be sedentary to the
end of his days; and, though the best of opportunities be afforded him
for learning these things later, it is a hundred to one but he will pass
them by and shrink back from the effort of taking those necessary first
steps the prospect of which, at an earlier age, would have filled him with
eager delight. The sexual passion expires after a protracted reign; but
it is well known that its peculiar manifestations in a given individual
depend almost entirely on the habits he may form during the early
period of its activity. Exposure to bad company then makes him a
loose liver all his days; chastity kept at first makes the same easy later
INSTINCTIVE ELEMENTS OF NATIVE EQUIPMENT 23
on. In all pedagogy the great thingis to strike the iron while hot, and
"Holding that the child and the race are each keys to the other, I have
constantly suggested phyletic explanations. ..." ('04, I, p. VIII.)
"The best index and guide to the stated activities of adults in past
ages is found in the instinctive, untaught, and non-imitative plays of
children. ... In play every mood and movement is instinct with
heredity. Thus we rehearse the activities of our ancestors, back we know
not how far, and repeat their life work in summative and adumbrated
ways. It is reminiscent, albeit unconsciously of our line of descent, and
each is the key to the other. . . . Thus stage by stage we enact their
(our ancestors') lives. Once in the phylon many of these activities were
elaborated in the life and death struggle for existence. Now the elements
and combinations oldest in the muscle history of the race are re-presented
earliest in the individual, and those later follow in order." ('04, I,
pp. 202-203.)
And Puffer says: "We are by turns vertebrates, gill-breathing verte-
brates, lung-breathing vertebrates (we make the great change at birth),
little monkeys, little savages, and finally civilized men and women."
('12, p. 77,)
20-
210
26
60 70 80
Letters per Minute
85i
30
25
15
10
1284 66 7 8 9 10 11 12 18 14 15
10 20 80 40
Associations per 15 Seconds
class or grade, we find that on the average the best pupil is able to
do from two to twenty-five times as much as the poorest pupil,
or is able to do the same task from two to twenty-five times as well
as the poorest pupil. The accompanying table shows the range of
differences between the highest and the lowest in a series of tests
made upon fifty university students.
TABLE i
and dropping at first rather gradually, then very rapidly and finally
"
very slowly. The statement attributed to Lincoln that God must
"
have loved the common people because He made so many of them
is psychologically true. If the middle third of the entire range
of abilities represents the common people, then two- thirds of all
tall or less short is larger and larger as the median is being ap-
proached. This uniformity throughout organic nature is an in-
teresting and significant fact. Apparently nowhere are there traits
VARIATION IN HUMAN CAPACITIES
which are discontinuous so that gaps would exist within the ranges
do we find that, on the whole, traits are distributed
of the traits, nor
68 69 70
200
i 160
100
61 62 63 54 66 56 67 58 59 60 61
Head Girths in Centimeters
FIG. 13. Distribution of the head girth of 1,071 boys, 16-19 years of age,
240
210
180
160
120
90
60
80
10
FIG. 14. Distribution of the number of heads up in tossing ten pennies 1,000
times. The horizontal axis gives the number of possible heads up in each tossing;
the vertical axis gives the number of times each number of heads was up.
rarely, likewise, thenumber of times all ten heads are up will occur
very rarely, the number of times one head is up or nine heads are
up will occur less rarely, and as you approach from either side
toward four, five, and six, the occurrences will be more and more
frequent. The actual records of a thousand such tossings are
represented in Figure 14. It would seem as though nature, in the
production of her creatures, aimed at a target. The largest number
of trials strikes somewhere near the bull's-eye, a smaller number
strikes within the next circle, and a still smaller number within the
next circle, and so on. The correspondence between the actual
distribution of abilities and the values of the probability integral is
exceedingly useful in permitting statistical treatment of series of
VARIATION IN HUMAN CAPACITIES 33
. IS
Grade 7
Grade
Grade 5
Grade 4
Grade 8
Grade 2
20 30 40 60 60 70 80 00 100 HO 120
FIG. 17.Distribution and overlapping of pupils in writing. Speed and
quality combined into one score as explained in the author's Educational
Measurements. The numbers along the horizontal axis represent speed and
quality in terms of speed, i. e., letters per minute. Quality was measured by the
Thorndike scale.
GradeS
!..yj::
Grade?
Grade6
Grade 5
Grade 4
GradeS
I
Grade 2
102080406060708090100
FIG. 18. Distribution and overlapping of pupils in spelling as measured by
the author's test. The numbers along the horizontal axis are the numbers of
words spelled out of a list of 100.
VARIATION IN HUMAN CAPACITIES 35
TABLE 2
Ranges of difference between the best and the poorest in a class of 36 eighth-
grade pupils.
5 10 15
Grade 8
Grade 7
Grade6
Grade5
5 10 15
the pupils in the second year of high school and could do the work
to be
equally well if he had been allowed to go on rapidly enough
in the second year of high school. Two pupils in every nine are
equal in ability to the average pupil in the first year of high school,
three of the nine pupils are correctly placed in the eighth grade,
VARIATION IN HUMAN CAPACITIES 39
two are equal only to the average seventh grader, and one is equal
only to the average sixth grader. Thus by proper promotion or
classification, one pupil in every nine could save two years in eight,
and two pupils in every nine could save one year in eight.
10 20 80 40 60 60 80 00 100
any given age are spread out over about nine years of maturity.
For example, children ten years old range in ability all the way
from fourteen-year-olds to six-year-olds or less, and the numbers
of pupils at each age of mentality are approximately those given
above. These facts are further borne out by recent tests of in-
telligence. (See Chapter VII.)
This enormous range of ability and the resulting overlapping of
successive grades, is probably the most important single fact discovered
with reference to education in the last decade. The import of it is so
significant of the situation as it exists in our schools to-day and of
the possibilities in the direction of the proper reclassification or
EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
10 40 50 60 70 80
10 15
Year 4
YcarS
Year 2
Yearl
20 40- 60 100
Scale Values
The plans which have been proposed for meeting the varying
two general classes :Jii5l, those which
abilities of pupils fall into
attempt to keep the pupils of a given class together but vary the
manner of instruction for the pupils of different capacities j^sgpond,
those which keep the manner of instruction uniform but promote
or retard pupils according to their achievements.
High School
10
Seniors
High School
Uo Freshmen
,123456789101112181415
Steps -Grammatical Scale A
FIG. 26. Distribution and overlapping of pupils in a high school in ability
in discriminating between correct and incorrect English. The numbers along
the horizontal axis are the steps on the author's Grammatical Scale A.
votes a part of the class period to the usual recitation and instruc-
tional work, and the remainder to study done under the supervision
of the teacher. Sometimes the class period is considerably length-
ened and no home study is done; at other times, the class period
is kept at the usual length with some assignments for home
study.
These plans have been in operation in various schools during
44 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
properly belongs.
The schemes coming under the second general proposi-
different
namely,
tion, that of keeping the manner of instruction constant
and varying the rate of promotion, have been applied widely,
and many plans designed to produce greater flexi-
different
TABLE 3
The average attendance per grade of 1,439 pupils, graduates, required to com-
plete each of the eight grades. Forty weeks is assigned in the course of
btudy for each grade.
NUMBER OF AVERAGE NUMBER OF NUMBER OF AVERAGE NUMBER OF
PUPILS WEEKS TO COMPLETE PUPILS WEEKS TO COMPLETE
EACH GRADE EACH GRADE
i 33
2 18 49 42
I 20 29 43
I 21 27 44
8 22 19 45
8 23 20 46
13 24 15 47
25 9 48
19 26 5 49
25 27 4 50
46 28 4
43 29 2 52
52 30 2 53
83 31 2 54
32 I 55
99 33 2 56
109 34 2 57
92 35 2 58
no 36 I 59
87 37 I 60
104 38 2 62
39 I 63
95
87 40 2 70
Median ..35 weeks
Total average time of attendance. . . 288 weeks
.
with the pupils of a lower grade if he falls behind, he will put forth
his best efforts to hold his own. Dawdling could hardly be en-
couraged more than it is in many of our schools. Rewards in
adult life are more nearly according to ability and performance.
The same conditions would work to the advantage of school
life.
Latin and ability in German were i.oo, it would mean that the
best pupil in Latin would be also the best pupil in German, the
second best pupil in Latin would be the second best pupil in Ger-
man, etc., down to the poorest pupil in Latin who would also be
the poorest in German. As the correlation drops farther and
farther below i.oo toward o, the closeness of this agreement be-
comes correspondingly less until o is reached. If the coefficient
of correlation between ability in Latin and ability in German
were i.oo, it would mean that the best pupil in Latin would be
the poorest pupil hi German, the second best pupil in Latin would
be the second poorest in German, etc As the correlation rises
above i.oo toward o the reversal becomes less and less until o is
reached. A coefficient of o means that no relationship exists.
A pupil might have any amount of ability in one subject and any
other amount of ability in the other subject. 1
The Correlation Among Specific Mental Abilities. The early
investigations in this field found surprisingly small correlations
even among apparently very similar or closely related capacities.
Thus it was thought that a person might have a good
memory for
words but not for numbers or faces; he might have a keen percep-
tion of words but not of letters or geometrical figures and the like.
As typical of the earlier results on correlations we may cite a few
from Wissler (*oi) as follows:
TABLE 5
The average correlation of each test with all others at various points in the
curve of practice. After Hollingworth ('12).
CORRELATION AMONG HUMAN CAPACITIES 53
TABLE 6
TABLE 7
TABLE 8
TABLE 9
possess four modes with depressions between them, but would very
likelybe single continuous distribution surfaces of the usual normal
form with one mode. The human beings who even remotely ap-
proach any one type are very rare. The rule is that each person
possesses more or less of all different traits, and within certain limits,
roughly similar amounts of the various traits. Persons in whom
the divergences are large are the exceptions rather than the rule.
Correlation between Special Mental Capacities and General
Intelligence. So far as definite data are available on this pointy
the inference be drawn that many special mental functions
may
are correlated anywhere from moderately to very closely with
general intelligence. Men of intelligence have, on the whole, keen'
powers of perception, observation, and attention, remarkable re-
strength of grip, are either very low or zero. In the case of children^
the situation somewhat different. B. T. Baldwin made an elabo-
is
We may say then, on the basis of the results secured in this group
(472 pupils) which is sufficiently large to be representative, that if a
pupil has stood in the first quarter of a large class through high school,
the chances are four out of five that he will not fall below the first half
of his class in the university. . . . The chances are but about one in
five that the student who has done poorly in high school who has been
in the lowest quarter of his class will rise above the median or average
of the freshman class at the university, and the chances that he will
prove a superior student at the university are very slim indeed. . . . The
CORRELATION AMONG HUMAN CAPACITIES 61
over 80% of those who were found in the lowest or the highest quarter of
the group in high school are found in their respective halves of the
group throughout the university. Three-fourths of the students
. . .
who enter the university from these high schools will maintain through-
out the university approximately the same rank which they held in
high school.
F. 0. Smith made a similar study of 120 students entering the
College of Liberal Arts at the University of Iowa. He traced their
records from high school through the entire university course and
found almost the same situation. Expressed in terms of coefficients
of correlations, the results were as follows:
TABLE 10
Correlation between marks in the grades and marks in the first high school
year.
He then states:
the first year (H. S.) class from the marks he has received in the last four
62 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
fire,and the coldness of snow, and the chattering of jays, and the cooing
of the kokila, and the hypocrisy of the crane, and the fidelity of the
chakrawaka, and then compounding all these together, he made woman
and gave her to man. But after one week, man came to him and said:
Lord, this creature that you have given me makes my life miserable.
She chatters incessantly and teases me beyond endurance, never leaving
me alone; and she requires incessant attention, and takes all my time up,
and cries about nothing, and is always idle; and so I have come to give
her back again, as I cannot live with her. So Twashtrai said: Very well;
and he took her back. Then after another week, man came again to him
and said: Lord, I find that my life is very lonely since I gave you back
that creature. I remember how she used to dance and sing to me, and
look at me out of the corner of her eye, and play with me, and cling to
me; and her laughter was music, and she was beautiful to look at, and
soft to touch; so give her back to me again. So Twashtrai said: Very
well, and gave her back again. Then after only three days, man came
back to him again and said: Lord, I know not how it is; but after all I
have come to the conclusion that she is more of a trouble than a pleasure
to me; so please take her back again. But Twashtrai said: Out with you,
Be off. I will have no more of this. You must manage how you can.
Then man said: But I cannot live with her. And Twashtrai replied:
Neither could you live without her, and he turned his back on man, and
went on with his work. Then man said: What is to be done? For I
cannot live either with or without her. (Thomas, Source Book of Social
Origins, p. 512.)
Figure 28. The difference is so small that the groups could hardly
be distinguished.
By method of amounts of overlapping in the distribution
the
of one group over the other, the following results have been ob-
tained from students in the University of Wisconsin in a series of
tests on memory as just stated, on perception consisting in the
cancellation within one minute of as many of a certain geometrical
figure as possible, on motor ability consisting in tapping with a
pencil upon a card as rapidly as possible for thirty seconds, and on
mental addition as described elsewhere. 1
TABLE 12
TABLE 13
Percentages of men
reaching or exceeding the median of the women. After
Woolley as computed by Thorndike ('14, III, p. 178).
Reaction time 68%
Tapping 81%
Sorting cards, speed 14%
Sorting cards, accuracy 44%
Thrusting at target 60%
Drawing lines 72%
Threshold of pain 46%
Threshold of taste 34% (22)
Threshold of smell 43%
Lifting weights 66%
Two-point discrimination 18% (43)
Memory (syllables and learning) 32% (46)
Ingenuity 63%
TABLE 14
to 14 years 15 to 17 years
Discrimination of weights 48% 58%
"colors 39% 58%
Reaction time 57% y6%
Resistance to size-weight illusion 55% 68%
Rate of tapping 64% 73%
TABLE 15
TABLE 16
Speed of handwriting, about noo boys and noo girls ................ 47%
Quality of handwriting,
" noo " " noo " ............... 39%
" " " "
Arithmetical reasoning, 1250 1250 ............... 60%
" " " "
History, 429 526 ............... 72%
" " "
Geography, 447 472 ............... 48%
TABLE 17
High school pupils
English .................................................. 41%
Mathematics ............................................. 57%
Latin .................................................... 57%
History ................................................. 60%
College students
English .................................................. 35%
Mathematics ............................................. 45%
History and economics ..................................... 56%
Natural sciences .......................................... 50%
Modern languages ..................................... 40%
^nrniTCgjr
m
the
part-whole test,
in the opposite test, in memory
1.20
Girls_
i.oo
Boys
.80
.eo
.40
.20
.00
8 9 10 11 12 18 14
Years
FIG. 20. Comparison of general intelligence of boys and girls as measured
by the Stanford revision of the Binet-Simon tests. After Terman ('16, p. 72).
The numbers along the vertical axis are intelligence quotients as explained in
Chapter VII.
by means of his revision of the Binet-Simon tests that for the ages
of five to fourteen girls tend to be very slightly superior to boys
and that after fourteen they are practically equal. His results are
set forth in Figure 28.
It seems a likely interpretation that motor superiority has been
carried over to include intellectual superiority as well. For centu-
ries women have been considered intellectually inferior to men.
They were thought to be incapable of acquiring anything more
than an elementary education. It has been only since the middle
of thei gth century that co-education and women's colleges have
id co m 10 o t- oo o o^je^ju
o i c!t
' ' ' ' ' ' ^ 2 i '
FIG. 30. Range of ability of men and women in color discrimination. After
Henmon (*io).
been inferred chiefly from motor and muscular inferiority and from
the conditions of a narrower environment and dependency due
to the bearing and rearing of children. The inference and belief
of intellectual inferiority is apparently unfounded. This conclu-
sion may be fairly drawn both from the specific psychological
tests that have been cited and also from the recent successes of
women in the acquisition of higher education.
Difference in the Range of Variations in Abilities. Besides
comparing the average amounts of any given ability in the two
sexes, we may compare also the range of abilities from the lowest
to the highest in the two sexes. Such comparisons have been made
in a few traits and the general inference has been that the range of
abilities is wider among men than among women. The distribution
of the abilities in the geometrical perception test made upon 193
SEX DIFFERENCES 71
and also the statistical fact that male and criminals at the
idiots
other extreme of the distribution curve considerably outnum-
ber the female. The fact that the great geniuses of the world
have been men rather than women would accordingly be explained,
not on the basis of luck of opportunity, but mainly on the basis
of greater exceptional ability. The theory seems plausible but
has been proposed rather in advance of a convincingly wide range
of experimental data. If it is true, it would mean that according
to the perception test the one or two per cent most gifted individu-
als are men and the i or 2% least gifted individuals arc also men,
that of the next 10 or 12% of most gifted individuals approximately
two-thirds would be men and one-third women, and likewise of
the next 10 or 12% least gifted individuals at the other extreme,
about two-thirds would be men and one-third women. For the
remainder of the distribution the number would be practically
identical. The facts should not be interpreted as implying that
men as a rule are superior to women, but would mean simply that
only the one or two exceptional persons in a hundred would be
superior to the most gifted women. The remaining 96 or 98%
would be largely identical.
CHAPTER VI
THE INHERITANCE OF MENTAL TRAITS
"i. That men who are gifted with high abilities even men of class
E easily rise through all the obstacles caused by inferiority of social
rank.
"2. Countries where there are fewer hindrances than in England, to a
poor man rising in life, produce a much larger proportion of persons of
culture, but not of what I call eminent men. (England and America are
taken as illustration.)
"3. Men who are largely aided by social advantages are unable to
achieve eminence, unless they are endowed with high natural gifts.'*
I. In intellect:
great-grandfather 15
1J. In morals:
Offspring and father 30
" "
grandfather 175
76 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
"There is one peculiar way in which a little more than half of all
males have had a considerable advantage over the others in gaining
distinction as important historical characters. The eldest sons, or if not
the eldest, those sons to whom the succession has devolved, have un-
doubtedly had greater opportunities to become illustrious than those to
whom the succession did not fall by right to primogeniture. I think
every one must feel that perhaps much of the greatness of Frederick II
of Prussia, Gustavus Adolphus, and William the Silent, was due to their
official positions; but an actual mathematical count is entirely opposed to
this view. The inheritors of the succession arc no more plentiful in the
higher grades than in the lower. The figures show the number in each
grade who came into power by inheriting the throne."
Grades
Total No. in each grade
i
7
23456789
21 41 49 71 70 68 43 18
10
7
Succession inheritors 5 14 26 31 49 38 45 23 12 4
Per cent 71 67 63 64 69 54 67 54 67 57
"It isthus seen that from 54 to 71% inherited the succession in the
different grades. The upper grades are in no way composed of men
whose opportunities were enhanced by virtue of this high position. Thus
we see that a certain very decided difference in outward circumstances
namely, the right of succession can be proved to have no effect on
intellectual distinction, or at least so small as to be unmeasurable without
/much greater data. The younger sons have made neither a poorer nor a
better showing. ('06, pp. 285-286.)"
"The upshot of it all is, that as regards intellectual life, environment is
a totally inadequate explanation. If it explains certain characters in
certain instances, it always fails to explain as many more; while heredity
not only explains all (or at least 90%) of the intellectual side of character
in practically every instance, but does so best when questions of en-
vironment are left out of the discussion. Therefore, it would seem that
we are forced to the conclusion that all these rough differences in in-
which are susceptible of grading on a scale of ten are
tellectual activity
due to predetermined differences in the primary germ-cells." ('06, p. 286.)
While heredity no doubt plays an important part in the produc-
tion of intellect and character the part attributed to it by Woods
that explains "at least go% of the intellectual side of character
it
lineage B there had come during the same period of time, 406
direct descendants, of whom all were normal individuals with the
THE
LAWFUL WIFE FEEBLE-MINDED GIRL
L
MMRTIN JR. RHOOA ZABETH
TAKE
GOOD 9 YR8. 4 VI
HOME
D. 86 YR8.
AUUHOUSE
FIG. 31. Descendants of the Kallikak Family. Squares males, circles
females, black squares or circles = feebleminded, open squares or circles =s
normal persons. The lineage was traced back from Deborah. After Goddard.
"But we now know that 65% of these children have inherited the
condition, and that if they grow up and marry they will transmit the
same condition to their offspring. Indeed, we know that this class of
peopleis increasing at an enormous rate in every community and unless
TABLE 18
Reading speed 51
Reading comprehension 64
Writing speed 72
Writing quality 46
Size of reading vocabulary 07
Spelling 05
Arithmetical reasoning 38
Addition attempts 71
Addition rights 44
Subtraction attempts 43
Subtraction rights 29
Multiplication attempts 37
Multiplication rights 2$
Division attempts 46
Division rights 56
Average 42
Memory 31
A-test 50
Geometrical form test 07
Tapping 6$
Average 3$
which arc directly affected by school work than in those which are not so
affected. The average correlation in the former group of tests is .42 and
in the latter .38. This seems to indicate that the mental similarities of
children of thesame parents are due primarily to heredity rather than to
similarity of environment since the resemblance is no greater in those
traits which arc more directly affected by environment.
"
(2) The resemblance of siblings is approximately as great in mental
traits as in physical traits. Pearson found the correlation between
brother and brother in height to be .50 and in cephalic index (ratio of
length to width of head) .49. These correlations for physical traits are a
little larger than the ones found here for mental traits taken separately.
Average 52
Arithmetic, 54 pairs 32
Spelling, 54 pairs 21
Reading, 54 pairs 31
Language, 54 pairs 24
TABLE 19
"There is no evidence, at least from these figures, for the notion that
special abilities in certain studies run in families. Mental traits running
in families are very likely more specialized than abilities in school studies
which involve large groups of mental functions. The children of any
given family are on the average equally good or equally poor in all studies.
Ability in school work is apparently inherited to the same extent as
physical features since the coefficients of correlation for children of the
same parents are approximately the same for both physical and mental
traits." (Starch, '15, pp. 609-610.)
TABLE 20
"i. One parent says: 'They have had exactly the same nurture from
their birth up to the present time; they arc both perfectly healthy and
strong, yet they arc otherwise as dissimilar as two boys could be, phys-
ically,mentally, and in their emotional nature.'
"2. 'I can answer most decidedly that the twins have been perfectly
dissimilar in character, habits, and likeness from the moment of their
birth to the present time, though they were nursed by the same woman,
went to school together, and were never separated till the age of fifteen.'
"3. 'They have never been separated, never the least differently
treated in food, clothing, or education; both teethed at the same time,
both had measles, whooping-cough, and scarlatina at the same time, and
neither had any other serious illness. Both are and have been exceed-
ingly healthy and have good abilities, yet they differ as much from each
other in mental cast as any of my family differ from another.'
"5. 'They were never alike cither in body or mind and their dissim-
ilarity increases daily. The external influences have been identical; they
have never been separated.'
"9. 'The home-training and influence were precisely the same, and
therefore I consider the dissimilarity to be accounted for almost entirely
by innate disposition and by causes over which we have no control.'"
('83, p. 170, Everyman's Library Edition.)
now these resemblances are due to the fact that the two members
"If
of any twin pair are treated alike at home, have the same parental
models, attend the same school and are subject in general to closely
similar environments, then (i) twins should, to the age of leaving home,
grow more and more alike, and in our measurements the twins 13 and
14 years old should be much more alike than those 9 and 10 years old.
Again (2), if similarity in training is the cause of similarity in mental
traits, ordinary fraternal pairs not over four or five years in age should
show a resemblance somewhat nearly as great as twin pairs, for the
home and school conditions of the former will not be much less similar
than those of a pair of the latter. Again (3) if training is the cause,
twins should show a greater resemblance in the case of traits much sub-
ject to training, such as ability in addition or in multiplication, than in
traits less subject to training, such as quickness in marking off the A's on a
sheet of printed capitals, or in writing the opposites of words.
"On the other hand (i) the nearer the resemblance of young twins
comes to equalling that of old, (2) the greater the superiority of twin
resemblance to ordinary fraternal resemblance is, and (3) the nearer twin
resemblance in relatively untrained capacities comes to equalling that in
capacities at which the home and school direct their attention, the more
must the resemblances found be attributed to inborn traits.
"The older twins show no closer resemblance than the younger twins,
and the chances are surely four to one that with an infinite number of
twins tested, the 12-14 year-olds would not show a resemblance .15
greater than the 9-11 year-olds. The facts are: (Thorndike '14, III,
pp. 248-249).
TABLE 21
The Influence
of Uniform Environment Upon Different Original
Abilities.All studies cited thus far have attempted to measure
the amount of similarity in related persons as compared with un-
related individuals on the assumption that the environment was
roughly constant for all, that whatever resemblances existed be-
opportunity.
A number of such experiments have been carried out. An in-
TABLE 22
Hence, both the greatest absolute and the greatest relative gain
was made by the group with the highest initial records.
Similar results have been found in the practice experiments of
substituting numbers for letters as described in the author's Ex-
periments in Educational Psychology, Chapter X. The follow-
ing table gives the highest five and the lowest five records from
among twenty persons. Each person practiced 120 minutes.
THE INHERITANCE OF MENTAL TRAITS 80
TABLE 23
Again the largest gain was made by the group having the greatest
initial ability.
Results pointing in the same direction have been obtained by
Thorndike, Whitley, and others. For example, Thorndike ('10)
found in the case of practice of nineteen persons in adding, the fol-
lowing results:
TABLE 24
The effect of equal amounts of practice upon individual differences in column
addition of one-place numbers. After Thorndike ('10).
40
80
20
10
Sept.
N
Oct. Nov. Dec. Jan. Feb. March April May]
FIG. 32. Progress in the four fundamental operations in arithmetic as
measured by the Courtis tests, Series B, given at monthly intervals. The
heavy continuous line represents all the pupils of the 4th grade. The four broken
lines represent these pupils divided into quartiles.
into four groups while those in the other grades were divided into
three groups. The results reveal the significant fact that the best
groups in each grade made the greatest progress, the poorest groups
made the least progress and the intermediate groups made average
progress. The graphs for the various groups in any grade gradu-
ally spread apart during the course of the year, indicating that the
differences increase rather than decrease or remain constant. The
more gifted pupils profit more by their school work than the less
gifted.
All experimental results point in the direction that practice does
not equalize abilities; in fact, equal practice tends to increase differ-
To i
R.I 25.6
N.Y 470
Wis 45-0
111 24.0
Ala 2.0
Miss i.o
Similar figures are given for other states, and the inference made
by Catteli is that the environment of Massachusetts and similar
states has been much more conducive to the development of scien-
tific men and that the number of such men could be determined
practically by the control of the proper educational stimuli.
Odin, in his study of 5,233 noted French men of letters living
during the period 1400 to 1830, found the following distribution
according to places of birth:
THE INHERITANCE OF MENTAL TRAITS 93
munity would have greater and different stimuli than one born
in an ancient or uncivilized community. His ultimate eminence
would be determined by his environment.
The pessimistic air may further be dispelled by noting the fact
that hardly one person in a thousand makes allthe absolute gain
possible forhim even in a single capacity. It has been proved over
and over again in numerous abilities which have been used daily
in one's occupation that by a little special practice each day their
efficiency may be enormously improved. Consequently, while the
possibilities of each individual are limited by his original inherited
equipment, each one may develop his capacities far beyond the
usual degree of attainment. While experimental evidence indicates
emphatically that under equal opportunities the more gifted surpass
the less gifted, yet rarely does anyone do his best or attain his limit
even in a single capacity. Life is a matter of competition; let every-
one compete to the fullest extent of his inherited ability.
CHAPTER VH
THE MEASUREMENT OF MENTAL CAPACITIES
97
98 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
should there not be special classes for the gifted pupils so that
they might be led to reach their fullest intellectual growth and
thus return to society the most that they are capable of?
Methods of Measuring Original Capacities. In general two
types of methods have been developed, at least in part, and used
for determining the native ability of human beings. The term
"native" of course, must be understood to signify not pure, native
ability unmodified by experience, but native or original only in the
sense of not being directly affected by specific training. The one
method consists of a considerable variety of reactions to questions
and situations which a child would be able to make as a result of
normal growth in a normal environment. The tests developed on
this principle are the Binet-Simon tests and the various modifica-
tions of them.
The second general method has proceeded on the basis of meas-
uring,by fairly precise methods, certain special mental functions
from year to year, and of determining thereby the mental status
and growth of the individual. Thus, for example, many capacities
might be measured by a definite psychological technique from year
to year, and certain norms might be established for each year so
that we could say that a given individual's memory has been de-
veloped to the norm or average of a child of ten. Similar tests and
norms could be developed in as many different mental capacities as
would seem to be necessary in order to obtain a fairly complete
evaluation of an individual's natural abilities. This second general
method has not as yet been developed to the same degree of com-
pleteness as the Binet-Simon type ('05) with respect to either the
selection of the particular capacities that should be tested, or the
types of tests that ought to be used, or the technique by which
they should be given. Brief consideration will be given to both
plans of measurement.
The Binet-Simon Scale. This series of tests is arranged in
groups according to years. Thus there is a series of tests for every
year from age three up to twelve or fifteen, and in some of the
revisions even to adult life. These tests were first prepared by the
French psychologist, Binet, and the French physician, Simon,
who collaborated for a period of twelve or fifteen years in the
selection of tests and in assigning them to the proper years accord-
ing to the growth and development of the child. These tests were
first published in 1905 and since then were revised by the original
(b) "When you are on your way to school and notice that you
are in danger of being tardy?"
(c) "If a playmate hits you without meaning to do it?"
4. Gives similarities, two things. (2 of 4.) (Stanford addition.)
Wood and coal; apple and peach; iron and silver; ship and
automobile.
5. Definitions superior to use. (2 of 4.)
minute.)
"IQ Classification
"MydearAdele,
"
I am 4 years old and I can read any English book. I can say all the
Latin Substantives and Adjectives and active verbs besides 52 lines of
Latin poetry. I can cast up any sum in addition and can multiply by
2,3,4, 5,6, 7, 8, (9), 10, (n).
"
I can also say the pence table. I read French a little and I know the
clock.
"
Francis Galton,
"
February 15, 1827."
At the age of 10, young Galton wrote the following letter which
represents maturity of judgment and intellectual interest worthy
of a high school or college student:
"
December 30, 1832.
"
My
"
Dearest Papa:
It is now my pleasure to disclose themost ardent wishes of my heart,
which are to extract out of my boundless wealth in compound, money
sufficient to make this addition to my unequaled library.
The Hebrew Commonwealth by John 9
A Pastor's Advice 2
Hornne's Commentaries on the Psalms 4
Paley's Evidence on Christianity 2
"
27
years and n months old is cited. The test numbers refer to the
preceding list.
3-
4-
5-
6.
3. Failed.
"
4.
5. Passed.
"
6.
This boy passed alf the tests of the third year and twelve addi-
tional tests scattered through the years IV, V, and VI, for each
of which he receives two additional months of credit. Hence his
mental age is three years plus 24 months or five years, and his
intelligent quotient is .34. He falls into the class of imbeciles.
A different plan of evaluation has been prepared by Yerkes and
THE MEASUREMENT OF MENTAL CAPACITIES 107
TABLE 25
TABLE 26
CHRONOLOGICAL NUMBER OF AVERAGE CHRON- AVERAGE
AOE OLOGICAL ACES MNTAL AGE
I- S 7 4-6 2.6
6-10 85 8.7 3-8
11-15 194 12.9 5-i
16-25 353 20.0 5-5
26-60 367 36.5 5-5
TABLE 29
Estimated Intelligence and Memory of Words 52
" " "
Memory of Passages 56
" " "
Opposites 08
" " "
Mental Addition 50
" " "
Arithmetical Reasoning 83
" " "
All Tests 69
" " " " "
except Opposites 89
Thus it will appear that the tests individually, with the ex-
ception of the Opposites, as well as collectively, agree very closely
with the combined estimates of intelligence given by the 15 pupils
of one another. The estimates of intelligence of pupils agreed very
closely among themselves. The pupil who stood highest in the
estimates of his fellows was estimated first by all pupils but one.
The ranking of the others, of course, did not agree as closely, but the
pulses to the visual centers in the brain and simultaneously the audi-
tory stimulus from the pronunciation of the word by the teacher
transmits impulses to the auditory center in the brain; second, con-
nections between these visual and auditory stimuli in the brain and
arousal by the auditory stimuli of images and meanings of the object
"hat" which have been established through previous experiences
before coming to school; and third, a redirection of impulses to the
motor centers to attempt to speak the word "hat." A little later
the pupil is given a pencil and is asked to make these same black
marks which have the name "hat." The psychological series con-
sists of, first, visual stimuli from the form of the letters to the visual
brain between the visual centers and motor centers for the hand,
and third, redirection of impulses from the motor centers to attempt
to write the word "hat." Then from the muscular movements of
the hand and arm, made more or less by trial and error, kinesthetic
sensations are derived and associated in the mind with the visual
stimuli of the word "hat." These two sets of sensations become
associated and direct the motor responses in carrying out the writing
act. Still later the pupil is given (i) the visual or auditory stimulus,
"If you buy a pencil for three cents and give the clerk five cents,
how many pennies should you receive back?" which (2) arouses a
variety of association processes between various numbers such as
multiplication, division, addition, or subtraction, and out of the
mass of associations one is selected, namely, five minus three equals
two, and this in turn (3) directs the impulse to the motor centers
to say "two." All learning, even including reasoning, is probably
of the same fundamental type. The only difference is that there
are more elements involved in each of the parts of the three-series
connections and that, owing to the larger number of elements
aroused, a selecting or picking out of certain elements rather than
of others takes place. Learning facts of history, economics, or
science may be described in the same general schematic manner.
The facts are either read in a book, heard spoken by the teacher, or
observed directly. These sensory impressions are associated, dis-
sociated, and combined in various ways, which in the course of
time usually lead to some form of reaction either of speech or of
larger muscular activity.
The preceding examples of school learning depend for the most
part upon simple associations, that is, upon the law that things ex-
1
marks will set off promptly the reaction of speaking the word "hat."
But in reality the process of learning is almost never as simple as
this. While it is true that association bonds must be set up be-
tween situations and responses, a single situation is almost certain
to present to the mind of a child of school age a multiplicity of
aspects. As a consequence we find, instead of a single bond joining
the response to the situation, a number of bonds each joining the
response to a different part of the situation. Thus the word "tri-
angle" may be associated with an equilateral triangle of red card-
z
board, /s inch thick, 8 inches on each side, showing a dull gray
edge and weighing one ounce. Innumerably different combina-
ANALYSIS OF PROBLEMS 117
tions of bonds may accomplish this. There may be, for example,
a major bond leading to a reaction to the redness, a secondary bond
connecting the reaction with the size and symmetry of the sides,
and minor bonds emanating from the thickness and color of the
edge, while the weight and texture of the paper, the shade of red
and, most important of all, the number of sides and the angles may
not emeirge from the complex at all. Clearly such a set of bonds
would be worse than useless in the presence of a right-angled triangle
indicated in a book by black lines on a white page with the angles
labeled by letters and with a base }4. inch long. By dint of numer-
ous experiences with a variety of triangles and with the help of the
teacher who points out the essential three-straight-sidedness, the
characteristics peculiar to a triangle finally emerge more or less
clearly from the complex and become associated with the various
reactions appropriate to "triangle." The false bonds are either
destroyed or greatly weakened. Association is still the basis of the
process, but there is in addition the dissociation from one another
of the various characteristics which make up the complexes called
objects. This is conveniently spoken of as learning by analysis arid
abstraction. When complete, the process of analysis and abstrac-
tion, which makes possible the reaction to parts of situations rather
than wholes, clearly is an enormous advance over simple associative
learning. One association thus properly attached to the significant
part of a situation may function without any further effort in a great
variety of similar but otherwise entirely novel situations. This
is probably the essence of reasoning. But again we must note that
the process is rarely so simple as has been outlined. Rarely are the
preliminary analysis and abstraction so complete that a reaction
is transferred without delay to a very novel situation. Besides,
the attention may be distracted from the often subtle and incon-
spicuous but significant element in the new situation by the novel
and striking but irrelevant features. Sometimes some of these ir-
relevant features touch off a reaction which is entirely inappropriate.
For example, all but the very brightest pupils in a class, which has
learned to compute with facility the area of triangles from printed
problems and diagrams in a book and which knows how to measure
accurately a straight line, would be completely at a loss to know
what to do if given a 66-foot tape measure and confronted by an
area of ground in the shape of a scalene triangle measuring 4 by 7
by 10 rods, covered with flower beds in a setting of greensward and
surrounded by an ornamental iron fence three feet high. The
Ii8 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
every other day. All that the experiment indicates is the general
learning must take place. All that the general principle can do is to
point the way to a more or less probable solution, but the particular
direction and the course of the path must be determined from fur-'
ther observations. The factors and laws of the mind as set forth
in general psychology can therefore not be carried over bodily
into the psychology of a particular type of learning. General
psychology can furnish its experimental technique and its funda-
mental laws which will serve as guides in the development of the
psychology of special types of learning. From this point of view,
the psychology of school subjects and the pedagogy of these sub-
jects resulting therefrom, which is likely to be the only sound
pedagogy worthy of the name, are as amenable to experimental
attack according to rigorous, scientific procedure, as the problems
in other fields of psychology have been amenable to the technique
of experimental methods.
Program of Problems. According to our analysis, then, the
following problems result:
training.)
3. How are they redirected into responses?
B. The psychology of school subjects in particular.
1. What are the specific psychological processes involved
in the learning of each particular subject?
2. How may the capacities in each subject be measured?
3. What factors and conditions promote or retard the
learning in each particular subject?
CHAPTER IX
THE RECEPTION OF STIMULI: A. SENSORY DEFECTS
things which the normal child can see clearly or hear distinctly.
The sad aspect of the matter is that a great many children have
sensory defects of which neither they themselves nor their parents
or teachers are aware. They assume that every one else sees or
hears just as they do and consequently their attention is not called
to it. These defects, however, often become serious and remain as
"I now know I have always carried about 1.50 diopters of hyper-
metropia; in my very early days, possibly more. Books and school were
to me a nightmare, a source of unutterable disgust. I drove myself to
my tasks with the scourge of duty; I never took one moment's joy or
pleasure in the acquisition of knowledge, unless it was the satisfaction
of a task accomplished or conquest gained. I have no memory of a sense
of pleasure connected with my studies at school or college. The only
pleasant memories I have are those connected with outdoor sports, or
facts gained through observation, or in the lecture-room through my
122 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
'bogy of the teachers who for seven years had had him in their classes:
he was a chronic bad speller. This does not mean that he misspelled
some words sometimes. He misspelled every word always, and did it in
the same careful and serious manner with which he recited the history
lesson he loved. His reading was as bad as his spelling; he was absolutely
incapable of getting through a single sentence correctly, a, an, and, the,
and a few three-letter words being the net result of his seven years'
schooling. He read saw for was, water for weather; wrote hlat for thai,
soas, for soap, and other picturesque combinations of the sort in endless
variety. His case seemed hopeless. Dr. Witmer made a long
. . .
examination, the result of which was the discovery that Charles Oilman
had an ocular defect never, in all these years, so much as suspected by
either his parents or his teachers: at the distance of about three feet the boy
THE RECEPTION OF STIMULI 123
saw everything double: 'he lacked the power to direct the two eyes co-
ordinately upon the same point in space, the left eye looking a Little
higher than the right/ A page of ordinary print was thus a blur; when-
ever he attempted to write, the words doubled tinder his pen. Curiously
enough, he had never mentioned this peculiarity he seemed to think it
the natural process of vision. And he had repeated three whole years of
school on this account alone. ... lie was fitted with glasses and later
operated upon; then for the first time in his life the printed page and the
words he was tracing with his pen were clear. But his reading and writing
and spelling were just as bad as ever! The oculist had removed the
icfcct he had not removed the effect of the defect: that was in the boy's
mind. And it was here that the psychologist came to the rescue by show-
ing justwhat the effect was and how to remedy it.
"Now, it is an obvious truism of daily life that in order to recognize a
thing when we see it again, we must have seen it, at least once, clearly and
distinctly: a mental image of it must have been left in the mind. Read-
ing issimply a rapid-fire recognition process by means of the stored
mental images of words. Charles Oilman had no stores of images of
words, for he had never seen any he had seen only blurs of words. He
was even worse off than the child just groping its way through the primer,
for he had to unlearn the blurs he had patiently acquired through those
seven years when nobody knew what his trouble was; then word by word,
he had to restock his mind with the images of words shown him through
his glasses. ... In spite of this handicap, the boy learned to read, write,
and spell, and was finally graduated from the grammar school only three
years later than he should have been; which was better than not being
graduated at all." (Carter '09.)
1
This percentage is probably too high since it was discovered that the colored chil-
dren took peculiar pride in reporting headaches because they seemed to consider it a
sign of intellectual keenness.
126 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOCiV
TABLE 32
Bayonnc, N. J . . .
77%
Camdcn, N. J. (1906) ... .27 7
Milwaukee (1907). . . . .... 14.7
Minneapolis (1908) .... . . . . .... 23 9
Pawtucket, R. I. (1901) . . . . n.i
Utica,N. Y. (1897) 10.9
Worcester, Mass 19.1
TABLE 33
Grade Per Cent of Visual Defect
2 20.2
3 21.9
4 25.8
5 24.8
6 24.5
7 26 9
8 32 3
TABLE 34
Edinburgh, Scotland (1904) 12.2%
Dunfermline, Scotland (i97) 4-o
Cleveland, well-to-do district (1907) 5.2
"
congested district (1907) r 8
Massachusetts, except Boston (1907) 58
Boston and environment (1907) 77
Boston (1908) 7 '
Ninth Street Primary School 84 children, both sexes, 6-10 yrs. old.
Geography
and
Language Arithmetic History Average
179 cases normal children 74.4 72 76.6 74.3
252 cases average children 72.7 70 76 5 .
73 . i
say thirty seconds or a minute, after which they are asked to write
a report of their observations. This is usually supplemented by
an interrogatory report consisting of answers to questions regarding
the picture. Experimental inquiry into these matters has been
stimulated primarily from the practical importance of determining
the reliability of witnesses in court. Incidentally, the results have
an exceedingly significant bearing upon the accuracy of observation
involved in learning. The main results of these experiments have
been summarized by Whipple ('10, pp. 304 ff) as follows:
"The Aussage psychology is that an errorless
chief single result of the
report not the rule, but the exception, even when the report is made by
is
Report A:
"There was a high upright geranium plant having no buds but broad
leaves. This was surrounded by. low plants with drooping stems and
bearing many pointed, small leaves. The leaves had a pinkish center,
surrounded by a pale green band, the contour very irregular and the
general effect bushy. There were no buds on these plants either."
Report B:
"From a brown jardiniere arose one stalk of a geranium bearing nine
big green leaves. The leaves spread out in all directions and are round
in shape with large scallops. Lower was another plant with much smaller
leaves and more bushy. It had three large branches, one leaning over
the pot on each side and one across the front. The leaves of this plant
were more oblong in shape, rounding at the base and reaching a point.
The color was a pale pink in the center and back to the base, shading to
a deep red or wine (pink) color towards the tip end and the whole edge of
the leaf was green. The stem was much more delicate than the stem
of the geranium and the leaves were much more numerous."
136 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
Report C.
"
The plant was in a brown bowl. There were varieties of plants. The
one had one large stalk with five (nine) branches growing out of it. The
leaves were large and heart-shaped (rounding). The other plant was lower
and drooped, and had more leaves. The leaves were oblong (heart-
shaped) and smaller than the leaves of the first plant. They had a center
of very light green (pink) and were outlined by darker green. The leaves
were smooth and glossy (velvety). The leaves of the tall plant were
1
notched.'
Range
Number
Accuracy
of Report (Test) 12345
(BORST)
39.0
86.6
390
87.7
42.3
92.9
40.3
88.2
42.0
90.0
Assurance 96.6 96.4 97.8 97.9 98.6
Warranted assurance 84.0 87.0 91.0 88.0 89.0
Reliability of assurance 87.5 89.4 92.6 89.8 90.3
Accuracy of assurance 97.0 98.0 98.4 98,6 99.2
Tendency to oath 43.0 59.8 62.8 61.9 72.1
Warranted tendency to oath 402 53.2 58.5 57.5 66.5
Unwarranted tendency to oath .. . 2.8 6.6 4.3 4.4 5.6
Reliability of oath 93.0 88.8 92.5 93.0 91.7
Norr: The effect of practice in these tests is somewhat obscured by the fact that thj
firbt and third tcbts were made after a 3-day, the others after a y-day interval.
Tests such as these, but made with the material of school studies,
would probably be very useful in bringing about more concen-
trated attention upon, and greater reliability in, observation.
Thus a plant or a flower in a course in biology, might be exposed
for a definite period of time to a class of pupils who would then be
asked to write as accurate a description of the object as possible.
This description could then be definitely compared, point by point,
with the original object a.nd in this manner the errors and inaccu-
racies would be discovered and noted. Difficulty in acquiring the
138 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
ing different things: map, bear, trees, lake, cloud, chfld, bat, man
running. The same mental processes occur in a less variable man-
ner in kinds of observation. Incoming stimuli are interpreted
all
vium which assails our noses, no sooner enters our consciousness than it is
drafted off in some determinate direction or other, making connections
with the other materials already there, and finally producing what we
call our reaction. The particular connections it strikes into are deter-
*
mined by our past experiences and the associations' of the present sort
of impression with them. If, for instance, you hear me call out A, B, C,
it is ten to one that you will react on the impression by inwardly or out-
the way of 'ideas.' This way of taking in the objects is the process of
apperception. The conceptions which meet and assimilate it are called
by Herbart the 'apperceiving mass.' The apperceived impression is
engulfed in this, and the result is a new field of consciousness, of which
one part (and often a very small part) comes from the outer world, and
another part (sometimes by far the largest) comes from the previous con-
tents of the mind." ('99, p. 157.)
really based upon the greater practical value of the known and
nearer at home, and upon the urgent need of knowing something
140 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
balls, mirror tracing, substitutions, and the like. Little has been
done on the progress of analytical types of learning, on the advance
in the acquisition of facts of a science or of the history of a country,
260
240
^220
200
1,100
g 140
m
! 120
| 100
J 80
5 10 15 20 24
Five Minute Periods
FIG. 36. A curve of learning showing the progress of one person in learning
to substitute numbers for letters in the experiments outlined in Chapter X,
Experiments in Educational Psychology.
gioo
>
4 8 12 18 20 24 28 32 80
Weeks of Practice
FIG. 37. Improvement in telegraphy. Individual E. L. B. After Bryan
and Harter ('97, p. 49).
10 20 90
Weeks of Practice
"After what has been said our explanation of the general features of
our curves can be brief. The first rapid and continuous rise is due to
the fact that the learner making progress along many different lines at
is
once. Rapid strides of improvement arc possible and made simulta-
6 10 X5 20 25 80 85 40 45 60 65 60 65 70 75
Days
FIG. 39. Progress in learning Russian. After Swift ('08, p. 198).
neously in every department of the work. The learner is not only forming
and perfecting letter associations but syllable, word and phrase associa-
tions as well. He simultaneously improving his method of dealing
is
with every problem that the writing presents; locating the keys, directing
and controlling his fingers, * spelling' or initiating the movements, get-
ting his copy, learning to deal with special difficulties, learning to keep
attention more closely and economically applied to the work, etc. The
curves will rise rapidly and continuously so long as many of these possibil-
THE RATE AND PROGRESS OF LEARNING
ities of improvement exist. As they grow less numerous the rate of gain
will likewise decline until, as still more skill is acquired, a state is reached
where most adaptations or short cuts in method have been made; fewer
1234 5 7 8 9 10
Successive Trials
11 12 13 H
FIG, 40. Improvement in tracing a star outline when seen in a mirror.
Continuous lines represent reduction in seconds in successive trials. Dotted
line represents reduction in errors.
1100
1000
ooo
800
700
600
500
400
800
200
100
101520258085404560
FIG. 42. Progress in ball-tossing. The horizontal axis represents days.
The vertical axis represents the number of balls caught. After Swift ('08, p. 1 74)
.
Swift and Batson have each published curves based on the in-
crease in skill in ball-tossing which purport to be of the concave
type. A
careful examination of the original data shows that the
apparently concave form is due in each case to peculiari-
in reality
ties in the method of plotting. Fortunately some of the data pub*-
THE RATE AND PROGRESS OF LEARNING 147
BOOr
460
400
5300
120
100
2200
60
1 40
20
100 1
11 21 81 41 51 61 81
Successive Trials
4 8 12 16 20 24
Days Practice
FIG. 43. Lower graph shows Batson's original curve. Upper graph shows
the reconstruction of his curve as stated in the text.
0123456789
Time
10 11
Spent in Practice
12
FIG. 44. Curve to show the progress in solving puzzles. After Ruger (*io).
as reconstructed by Thorndike ('14), III, p. 342.
Grade
Geography
5678
25 46 72
American History 7 20 38
"A
hierarchy of habits may be described in this way: (i) There is a
certain number of habits which are elementary constituents of all other
habits within the hierarchy. (2) There are habits of a higher order which,
embracing the lower as elements, are themselves in turn elements of
language. They must copy close. They cannot attend much to the sense
of the message as it comes, but must get its form, and re-read for the
sense. Only when all the necessary habits, high and low, have become
automatic, docs one rise into the freedom and speed of the expert.
"
3. The Plateaus.
"
We are now prepared to offer an explanation for the salient peculi-
arity of the receiving curve its plateaus.
"
A
plateau in the curve means that the lower-order habits are ap-
proaching their maximum development, but arc not yet sufficiently
automatic to leave the attention free to attack the higher-order habits.
The length of the plateau is a measure of the difficulty of making the
lower-order habits sufficiently automatic."
"The real advance in the early stages of learning is made during the
periods of seeming arrest of progress. The manifest advance, that
which is revealed by the curve or by examination marks, which is the
same thing, is discouragingly brief. By far the greater part of the learning
period is spent on plateaus when both teacher and pupil, failing to under-
stand the situation, feel that they are marking time. Yet it is during
these days of retardation that the valuable and solid acquisitions arc
being made. Americans who spend several years in Germany pass
through a long period of discouragement. Though they study the lan-
guage faithfully, and avail themselves of every opportunity to practice
conversation, they seem to make absolutely no progress. The length of
this plateau-period varies with different persons, but all experience its
oppressiveness. Now the most curious feature of this plateau, aside from
itsoverpowering monotony, is the suddenness with which it finally dis-
appears. Several have told the writer that they went to sleep one night
unable to understand anything, as it seemed to them, and utterly dis-
couraged, and awoke the following morning to find that they had mas-
tered the language, that they could understand practically everything
that was said to them. The word associations and national peculiarities
of thought sequence had been automatized during the long period when
no visible progress was being made." ('06, pp. 310 f.)
THE RATE AND PROGRESS OF LEARNING 153
TABLE 38
Practice in substituting letters for other letters according to a key. After
Munn ('09)
"
Generally speaking, daily practice seems to give better returns than
the same number of periods distributed on alternate days or in twicc-a-
day periods. However, there is some evidence that in the early stages of
habituation, the second practice on the same day gives good returns and
that, later on. alternate ^ays may be the best distribution."
THE RATE AND PROGRESS OF LEARNING 155
FIG. 46. rractice in writing numbers for letters according to a key. After
Starch ('12).
TO min. curve = group working 10 min. twice a day.
" " = " " " " tl
20 20 once .
TABLE 39
Addition Division
Per Cent Gain over the Per Cent Gain over the
Period 22.5-Minule Period Period 2o-Minute Period
22.5 20
IS- 21 % IO 10-5%
6. I % 2 77 %
2. 46.5%
before an interval so long has elapsed that the original associations have
lost their color and cannot be recalled in the same shape, time, and order.
In general it was found that the most economical method for keeping
material once memorized from disappearing was to review the material
whenever it started to fade. Here also the intervals were found to be,
roughly speaking, in arithmetical proportion. For similar reasons the
student is advised to review his lecture notes shortly after taking them,
and, if possible, to review them again the evening of the same day. Then
the lapse of a week or two does not make nearly so much difference.
When once he has forgotten so much that the various associations orig-
inally made have vanished, a considerable portion of the material is
5 10 15 20 25 80 85 40
Days
FIG. 47. Curves of forgetting.
length of the curve. Every step in advance seems to cost as much as the
former. Indeed, each new step seems to cost more than the former. In-
quiry at the telegraph school and among operators indicates that between
sixty and seventy-five per cent of those who begin the study of telegraphy
become discouraged upon the plateau of the curve just below the main-
line rate. As a rule, ordinary operators will not make the painful effort
TABLE 40
TABLE 41
TABLE 42
Improvement in ability to memorize. After Dearborn (*io)
TOTAL PRAC- AMOUNT NUMBER OF TOTAL TIME RE- TIME RE-
JECT TiCETiMB LEARNED I *YS OF AMOUNT QWRRD ON QUIRED ON
IN HOURS DAILY PRACTICE LEARNKD FIRST DAY MOSTFFI-
TABLE 43
Based upon published scores for the various tests
notion on the part of the learner may be formed of the precise bonds
and connections to be made.
.
e.JDejlnite Knowledge of Success and Error. Much experimental
work implies that the feeling of satisfaction resulting from success-
ful trials of a task facilitates the formation of the connections con-
cerned. It seems obvious therefore as a practical matter that pre-
cise knowledge of the success or failure on the part of the learner
is
exceedingly important. It will not only serve to arouse the
feeling of satisfaction but also help to define the particular bonds
to be established. Feelings of dislike on the part of the learner
toward the material to be learned undoubtedly interfere with the
rapid formation of the connections, and frequently the feeling
of dislike isaccompanied by an attitude of unwillingness or stub-
bornness indicated by such statements as "I know I can't learn
languages; I never could." "I never was able to get mathematics."
"I can't memorize anything." A concrete case that came under
the writer's observation was that of a man considerably older than
the average university student, who in the experiment on the trans-
ference of training (Chapter XI, Experiments in Educational
1
The tests were made in The Alice School, Hibbtng, Minnesota, by Principal L. J.
Coubal, and reported in an unpublished thesis in the library of the University of Wis-
consin. The tests were carried out under the direction of Professor V. A. C. Henmon.
THE RATE AND PROGRESS OF LEARNING I6S
each time; in spelling, the author's six lists were used in rotation,
one at a time; and in arithmetic the three sets in the Courtis Series
A were used and rotated so that there was a recurrence of the same
40
ao
10
test every three months, but very unlikely that this contributed
it is
FIG. 50. Progress in spelling (Starch test) of 3id grade class. Other facts
the same as for Fig. 48.
.10
Addition
Subtraction
a
Si
Multiplication
Division
During the last six years several hundred students have performed
the imagery tests outlined in Chapter VII of the author's Experi-
ments ('17). Among this entire number not more than two or three
persons were found whose images either were practically all of
one type, or who had one or another commonly prevalent class
almost entirely missing. The facts for 95% of all persons are sub-
stantially as set forth on page 45 (Experiments), namely, that nearly
all persons have all types of images which are combined in different
individuals in varying proportions. Mankind as a whole does not
fall into sharply or even vaguely divided groups of visuals, audiles,
"In the visual presentation the subjects read the stimuli directly from
the rotating drum and immediately wrote down as many members as
could be recalled and in the order presented. The subjects were asked
to repress movements of articulation. In the auditory presentation the
experimenter read the stimuli from the drum, the subject keeping his
THE RATE AND PROGRESS OF LEARNING 169
ness. Much of our thinking about the problem has been confused
1
An unpublished study reported by Thorndike. ('14, III, p. 93).
THE RATE AND PROGRESS OF LEARNING 173
ing continuous mental work have been the various types of mental
calculations, addition and multiplication.
particularly These
methods have been used by Krapelin, Thorndike and Arai, Starch
and Ash, and others. As an illustration of one type of mental
addition, the writer has used a method consisting in the mental
addition of 6, 7, 8, and 9 in rotation by beginning with a given
number and adding each of these numbers in turn to the answer
last obtained, as described in Chapter XVI, Experiments ('17).
The advantage form of calculation is that it affords suf-
of this
ficient difficultyand thus fully taxes the efforts of the individual
and makes possible a minute record of the amount and accuracy of
work done during succeeding short intervals of time. Figure 52
shows a curve obtained by this method, covering a period of con-
tinuous work of two hours.
As an illustration of mental multiplication, we may cite the
experiment carried out by Miss Arai under the direction of Thorn-
dike. She used the method of multiplying mentally four place
numbers by four place numbers, as 4,962 times 7,584. She trained
herself for a considerable period of time in this type of mental mul-
* 1 IK 2^
Hours
FIG. 52. Mental work curve. Upper curve shows number of additions
made per half minute period. Lower curve shows number of errors made.
Work was continued for two hours. After Starch and Ash ('17).
work. Thus in the curve, Figure 52, the reduction in the number of
additions made per thirty seconds, was only from 14.0 down to
13.4, or a loss of only 4.3%. Arai found even in the course of 12
hours of such difficult mental multiplication as she carried out,
that her efficiency was reduced only by about one-half. Other
investigators have shown in general the same facts.
Seashore and Kent ('05) measured continuously, for as long as
two hours, the threshold of hearing by recording the audibility and
inaudibility of a sound varied about the limen. The intensity of
the sound was changed at a uniform rate. As soon as it became too
faint tobe heard the subject gave a signal to the experimenter who
at once increased the strength of the sound. As soon as it could be
heard again the subject again responded. Then the sound was de-
creased again, and so on without break. A sample curve is shown
in Figure 53. Ten records were obtained which showed that "con-
THE RATE AND PROGRESS OF LEARNING I7S
ficiency.
CHAPTER XII
HOW TO STUDY
Waste in Studying. Since studying is learning under school
conditions, it would seem worth while to make such suggestions
as can be made concretely to assist pupils in this important phase
of the psychology of learning. It may seem preposterous to give
advice about something concerning which each pupil is presumably
proficient after years of practice in it, and furthermore to attempt
to give suggestions on studying may seem to many to be nothing
more than an "unprofitable delineation of the obvious." It is,
however, very certain that there is an uncalculated waste of energy
and a still more prodigal waste of time in so-called studying. If
we may judge from the possibility of improvement in reading ca-
pacity alone, and from the larger accomplishments attained under
favorable conditions of work, we may venture to guess that the
average student could accomplish his work just as efficiently or
more efficiently, in two-thirds, or less, of the amount of time
ordinarily consumed, by developing more economical methods
and habits of studying. Improvements in proper procedure in
studying have shown how much more may be accomplished in the
same length of time or even in a shorter period of time. Vicious
habits of dawdling in school work are acquired, which may have
their permanent effect throughout the individual's life.
Is Studying Worth While? This question is worth raising in
view of the belief, prevalent among students, parents, and grad-
uates, that after all it does not matter much whether a pupil does
well in his studies or not, that the boy who does poorly in the
grammar grades or the high school will outgrow his negligence and
come into his own when he gets into his college or professional
course, or that when he gets into the real business of life he will
outstrip his more studious mates. To what extent are these beliefs
true or false? To what extent is early scholastic performance
indicative of similar or different performance later on? To what
extent is scholastic performance prophetic of performance in life?
A considerable amount of statistical material has been accumu-
lated in the attempt to answer these questions. Some of this
176
HOW TO STUDY 177
The 250 Yale men who graduated from the Harvard Law School
in 1900-1915 were divided into nine groups according to their
scholarship at Yale. These nine groups, with the exception of one,
finished the Harvard Law School in the same relative order of
scholarship that they had held at Yale.
To many persons a more important problem is the relationship
between scholastic attainment and success in business or profes-
sional work. Foster ('16) has summarized in an interesting manner
much of the evidence pertaining to this problem. He made a
study of the Harvard College class of 1894. He asked three men,
the dean of the college, the secretary of the alumni association,
and a member of the class, to name the most successful men of the
class. They were free to use their own interpretation of success
except that they were not to include men whose success appeared
to be due chiefly to family wealth or position. The three judges
agreed on twenty-three men. Foster then obtained their records
178 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
are in one group, the five Greek words in the second group, and
the five sums in the third group. The time required by ten sub-
jects for memorizing the first set was an average of 14 minutes
and 3 seconds; the time required for memorizing the second series,
which was arranged in order, was on the average 9 minutes and n
seconds. The comparison shows a very decided advantage in
favor of learning the material in organized form.
(4) Recall at brief intervals the essential ideas of what you have
read. Stop at the end of each paragraph or two, shut your book
or your eyes, and recall the essential ideas you have read. Say to
yourself "What did I read about?" Then try to answer the ques-
tion. Note here what was snid about forgetting in the last chapter.
The is the occasion and stimulus which
chief value of examinations
by laboratory experiments.
Then each day or two, relate the recent material in a given
13. Get rid of the idea that you are working for the teacher.
14. Don't apply for help until you have to.
15. Have a clear notion of the aim.
1 6. Before beginning the advance work, review rapidly the previous
lesson.
17. Make
a rapid preliminary survey of the assigned material.
Find out by trial whether you succeed better by beginning with the
1 8.
hardest or with the easiest task when you are confronted with several
tasks of unequal difficulty.
19. In general, use in your studying the form of activity that will
later be demanded when the material is used.
20. Give most time and attention to the weak points in your knowledge
or technique.
21. Carry the learning of all important items beyond the point neces-
sary for immediate recall.
22. You must daily pass judgment as to the degree of importance of
items that are brought before you, and lay special stress on the per-
manent fixing of those items that are vital and fundamental.
23. When a given bit of information is clearly of subordinate im-
portance and useful only for the time being, you are warranted in giving
to it only sufficient attention to hold it over the time in question.
24. Make the duration of your periods of study long enough to utilize
"warming-up" but not so long as to suffer weariness or fatigue.
25. When drill or repetition is necessary, distribute over more than
one period the time given to a specified learning.
26. When you interrupt work, not only stop at a natural break, but
also leave a cue for its quick resumption.
27. After intensive application, especially to new material, pause for a
time and let your mind be fallow before taking up anything else.
28. Use various devices to compel yourself to think over your work.
29. Form the habit of working out your own concrete examples of all
general rules and principles.
30. Form the habit of mentally reviewing every paragraph as soon as
you have read it.
31. Don't hesitate to mark up your own books to make the essential
ideas stand out visibly.
32. Whenever your desire is to master material that is at all extensive
and complex, make an outline of it. If you also wish to retain this
material,commit your outline to memory.
33. In all your work apply your knowledge as much as possible and as
soon as possible.
34. Do not hesitate to commit to memory verbatim such materials as
definitions of technical terms, formulas, dates and outlines, always pro-
vided, of course, that you also understand them.
35. When the material to be learned by heart presents no obvious
l88 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
Total 31
effect; two said that only the slow students had been helped, the brighter
ones were not; and two had no data on which to base their opinions.
"Wherever the plan had been in use long enough to make possible the
compiling of statistics as to the effect of supervised study on scholarship,
there was practically unanimous agreement that the number of failures
had been reduced and the standards of scholarship had been raised. The
high school at Snokomish, Washington, reports that the average per-
centage of failures in elementary algebra for the two years prior to the
adoption of supervised study was 28%. But for the two-year period
following the adoption of supervised study the failures in the same
subject were reduced to 17%. Hoquiam, Washington, reports that the
average marks of the students range 10% higher and that the number of
honor pupils has been doubled since supervised study was introduced.
The principal of the Arcata high school, California, reports that the
average mark of the freshman class has been raised from 78% to 82JHz%
during the first year of supervised study. Santa Cruz, California, com-
paring the year 1914-15, the last under the old plan, with the year
1916-17, the second year under supervised study, finds that the increase
in the total number of high marks has been 157%; the decrease in low
failures, 188%. Reno, Nevada, reports a decrease of 45% in the number
of failures, and an increase of 24% in the number of students making
excellent marks."
TABLE 46. After Brown and Hall-Quest ('17, p. 386). Supervision of study
apparently was begun in 1912 although I have not been able to find a
definite statement by Hall-Quest to that effect.
90
I
3-
75
1 2 8 4 56 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
Weeks
FIG. 54. The broken line represents the supervised group. The continuous
line represents the unsuperviscd group. After Minnick ('13).
TABLE 48
The spread of improvement in reacting to various sensory stimuli. After
Gilbert and Fracker ('97)
1
Average of J. A. C. and G. C. F. only.
J. C. P. was practiced only in reaction time, while the other two were
practiced in both reaction and reaction with discrimination and choice. All
figures of the above table represent per cent of gain by practice.
Each of the forms of reaction shows on the whole a distinct gain
in the second end tests. How much of this gain is actually due to
the training series cannot be definitely determined. of the Many
earlier investigators did not make control tests, that is, they did not
repeat the end tests on another group of subjects who did not take
the practice series but who took only the end tests separated by an
interval equal to that consumed by the practice series. It is obvious
that a certain portion of the gain in the end tests is due to the fact
that when the second end tests are made, some advantage is derived
from the familiarity or practice in having done the end tests once
TRANSFERENCE OF TRAINING 195
due to was only 44% as great as that for areas of the same
this training
shape and For areas of the same shape, but from 140-300 sq. cm.
size.
in size, the improvement was 30% as great. For areas of different shape
and from 140-400 sq. cm. in size, the improvement was 52% as great.
"Training in estimating weights of from 40-120 grams resulted in
only 30% as much improvement in estimating weights from 120 to 1800
grams. Training in estimating lines from .5 to 1.5 inches long (resulting
in a reduction of error to 25% of the initial amount) resulted in no im-
provement in the estimation of lines 6-12 inches long.
"Training in perceiving words containing V
and 's' gave a certain
amount of improvement in speed and accuracy in that special ability.
In the ability to perceive words containing 'i and 't/ 1
and 'p/ 'c' andV
'a,' V
and 'r,' 'a' and n,' c
T
and V, mispelled words and A's, there
was an improvement in speed of only 39% as much as in the ability
specially trained, and in accuracy of only 25% as much. Training in
perceiving English verbs gave a reduction in time of nearly 21% and in
omissions of 70%. The ability to perceive other parts of speech showed a
reduction in time of 3%, but an increase in omissions of over ioo%."
"Two n
days in marking out words con-
reagents were trained for
*
jects. This loss was due to the very large loss of one subject which
far outweighed the gain of the other subject. The gain in the
estimation of the smaller weights was greater than in the training
series itself. Thorndike and Woodworth's experiments showed
a gain of 45% in the training series and of 38% in the end tests
with the smaller weights and of 16% with the larger weights.
Kline had nine persons practice for fourteen days from 30 to 45
minutes daily in canceling e's and t's on pages of prose. Before
and after the practice he tested them in canceling nouns, verbs,
prepositions, pronouns, and adverbs. Eight other persons were
tested ig like manner without doing the practice series. Kline
found that the practiced group did not gain as much as the un-
practiced group. This he explains by the introspective statements
of his subjects that "there was a tendency to cross out words
containing e's and t's rather than the required part of speech."
The detailed results follow:
TABLE 49
The spread of improvement in marking letters. After Kline ('09, p. 10)
PRACTICED
GROUP
After practice 14.0 6.0 28.00.5 2 3 6.3 0.6
Before practice 28.6 17.3 4 4 4 5
Differences. . 7.4
. 4.7 -2.0' 2 1 2.5 2 1 -1.3 3.0
UNPRACTICED
GROUP
Second period 30.4 1.3 6.0 7.0 26.6.1.7 5.0 0.6 4.0 0.7 7.0
First period . . 23.5 5.0 16.62.6 10.5 4.6 0.3 13.7 2.013.0
Differences... 6.9 6.7 2.61.0 -2.0> 10 00.9 0.4-0.3 9.7 6.0
1
sign indicates loss at second period.
in the pitch of tones before and after training twice a week for five
months in discriminating shades of blue. The accuracy in the
four end tests showed the following gains:
"(i) Six keys of a typewriter are labeled with six symbols (letters or
figures). Fifty-five of these letters or figures, in chance order, are now
shown one by one, and the subject on seeing one taps the corresponding
key. The time taken to tap out the series is recorded. Six different
symbols are then used with a new series composed of them, and the sub-
ject's time record is taken as before. This is continued until twenty
different sets of symbols have been used. Although the symbols have
been changed each time, there is a steady improvement, ranging for the
four subjects in the following decrease in time: 62 to 52, 95 to 85, 71.5 to
58, 65 to 56. The major part of this gain could not have been due to
merely getting used to the machine or to the general features of the
experiments, for the fourth subject was already used to these and still
gained about nine-tenths as much as the other three.
"(2) The other experiment consisted in taking daily records for
twenty days, by means of a stop-watch, of the time required to repeat
the alphabet from memory. Each day's experiment was as follows:
First, the alphabet was repeated as rapidly as possible forward; sec-
ond, the letter n was interpolated between each of the letters; third,
the alphabet was repeated backward interpolating n between each two
of the letters. At the end of twenty practices in each order the subject
TRANSFERENCE OF TRAINING 199
"Each set of 102 cards contained six colors, or six designs, was shuffled
so that no color or device repeated itself, and was sorted into six com-
partments. The first six cards of the pack determined the order of colors
in the compartments according to which the rest of the pack was to be
sorted. Nine reagents took part and the experiment continued two
semesters. There was transference of practice-effect from one set of
colors to the other set of colors, and to the geometric forms; and from one
set of geometric forms to the other and to the colors. Increased powers
of discrimination and attention were thought to be the causes of trans-
ference."
ment made in the practice and that made in the test series; occasionally
a larger percentage of gain is made in the latter than in the practice itself.
This again indicates the presence of direct practice in the test scries.
"Some at least of the remaining general improvement found is to be
explained simply in terms of orientation, attention, and changes in the
technique of learning.
"These results seem to render unnecessary the hypothesis proposed by
Ebert and Meumann to account for the large extent of the general in-
fluence of special practice, which their experiments seem to indicate."
1
Quoted by permission from an unpublished table prepared by Dearborn.
TABLE 52
Transference of training in memory. After Fracker ('08)
22% 6% 16%
End Tests:
Unlike Training Series:
Geometrical Figures, 8 trained subjects, 5%
Nine Numbers, 8
8
"
"
"
"
44"
13%
04
4 untrained,
"
8
o 4%
i%
Movement,
Poetry, 8 74" 2
i
5%
6% 3% 3%
An interesting result emphasized by these data is the fact that
the transfer to the types of memory similar to that involved in the
training series is considerably greater than the transfer to the
memory functions unlike the training series. The average residual
gain in the four similar memory processes is 16%, whereas in the
four unlike memory processes it was only 3%.
Sleight made a careful and extensive investigation on transfer-
ence of training in one sort of memory to other sorts of memory.
He believed that previous researches had not used enough subjects
to be statistically reliable. He therefore carried out his first re-
search with 84 pupils from three girls' schools, averaging 12 years
and 8 months old. Ten cross sectional tests were made before,
in the middle, and after the training series, as follows: (i) Re-
given in the last column. Sleight has not made such a percentage
comparison, but has used a different, and possibly fairer, plan of
computing the data. I have, however, made this computation in
terms of percentages as these will be more intelligible to the reader
unfamiliar with statistical methods. The average percentages at
the bottom of the table show only slight gains on the part of the
trained groups, 2, 3, and 4, over the untrained group. The average
gain of group 2, trained in poetry, over group i, untrained, was
3.3%; of group 3, trained in arithmetical tables, over group i was
2.6%; and of group 4, trained in prose, over group i was 4.0%.
The amounts of transfer are very small. Sleight failed to indicate
the improvement in the training series themselves so that it is im-
possible to compare the transferred amount with it.
TABLE 53
The numbers in the following table are the average scores made by each
group in each test. Group i had no special practice, Group 2 was practiced in
learning poetry, Group 3 in learning tables, and Group 4 in learning prose
substance.
The column under Section I gives the scores before the training, under
Section II about the middle of the training, under Section III after the training.
After Sleight. ('11,^413.)
TRANSFERENCE OF TRAINING 20$
TABLE S3 Continued
u tt u
17.0
TABLE 54
the end tests. The results in each case showed a greater gain in the
practiced group.
The results from the author's class experiments (Chapter XI V
208 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
I. Reaction
1. Simple sensory to sound (50) i
2. Compound
a. With discrimination
(1)Marking out small a's (100) 2
a. Successive presentation
(1) Memory of sound intensities (50) 8
(2) Memory of consonants (50) 9
(3) Memory of Arabic numerals (50) 10
(4) Memory of visual signs (10) n
(5) Memory of associated pairs (50) 12
b. Simultaneous presentation
(i) Learning i2-letter-rectangles
(a) Free (10) 13
(b) With distraction (10) 14
Equivocal Word-completion
2. (10) 15
Free 2-minute trains of ideas
3. (3) 16
IV. Extensive threshold of visual attention
1. Free (15) *7
2. With distraction (10) 18
V. Maximum voluntary activity tapping (5 30") 19
(The figures in parenthesis indicate the number of reactions, memory units
or experiments, in the test.)
"During the 55-day interval between the tests, two reagents (MN.,
Le.) took training 18 days on Test 17; 25 1 2-letter-rectangles were pre-
sented daily, making in all 450 experiments each. Two reagents (Rt. and
SI.) took training 18 days on Test 13; 20 1 2-letter-rectangles were pre-
sented daily, aggregating 360 experiments each. One reagent (Ly.) took
training in simple reaction to sound for ndays, 1,100 reactions in all.
(Le., who took training on test 17, also took training in this simple re-
action to the extent of about 500 reactions.) Two reagents (He., Cr.)
took training on memory schemes for about 14 days. And one reagent
(al.) took training on Test 17 for 8 days, almost consecutive, to the ex-
tent of 200 experiments."
The results obtained from the various end tests are rather intri-
cate and difficult to present in tabular form and somewhat doubtful
as to their meaning so far as improvement in attention is concerned.
Coover attempted to interpret their meaning from the standpoint
of control of attentionby comparing the variability in the perform-
ance of the persons before and after training on the assumption that
reduction in variability indicated better attention. For a detailed
consideration, Coover's original report must be consulted. His
general conclusion was that "as a measure of attention our tests
are inadequate, and the question of transference of improved con-
"
ditions of attention remains open (page 183).
g. Analysis and Ingenuity. Ruger ('10), in connection with his
ing, so similar to the training series that they were all but identical
with it,was about 75% as much as that made in the training
series;while the improvement made in the forms of memory rather
different from the training series was only about 15% as much
as that in the training series. Up to about 1890 when James re-
ported the first investigation on the problem of transference, it
was tacitly assumed by many writers that a very large share, if
not all, of the training derived from one sort of exercise was carried
over to other sorts of exercise. After the first investigations be-
came generally known, many writers went to the other extreme
and assumed that all training is entirely specialized and that
nothing carries over from one kind of practice to any other kind
of practice. As a general estimate, on the basis of experimental
work done thus far, the amount of transference between the ex-
tremes of 100% and o% of transfer lies nearer to the zero end and
is probably in the neighborhood of 20% to 30% of transfer to
other functions, how may the change in these other mental func-
tions be explained? How does change in one function carry over
to others? Two general theories have been proposed: (i) The
theory of identical elements or special connections, and (2) the
theory of generalization or common capacities.
The theory of identical elements has been advocated by Thorn-
dike and may best be stated in his own words:
"The answer which I shall try to defend is that a change in one func-
tion alters any other only in so far as the two functions have as factors
identical elements. The change in the second function is in amount that
due to the change in the elements common to it and the first. The
change is simply the necessary result upon the second function of the
alteration of those of its factors which were elements of the first function,
and so were altered by its training. To take a concrete example, im-
provement in addition will alter one's ability in multiplication because
addition absolutely identical with a part of multiplication and because
is
certain other processes, e. g., eye movements and the inhibition of all
the necessary movements to open one door, but he never has the ability
to generalize this experience. He cannot sec that the same method of
opening doors is applicable to many other latches. The result is that the
animal goes through life with one particular narrow mode of behavior,
and exhibits his lack of intelligence by his inability to carry this single
type of skill over to the other cases which arc very familiar to the trained
human intelligence.
"James goes on to say that the same distinction appears when we con-
trast a trained scientific mind with the ordinary mind. The ordinary
thinker docs not see how to deal with a situation in terms of scientific
principles. James cites the his own experience with a smoking
example of
student-lamp. He discovered accident that the lamp would not
by
smoke if he put something under the chimney so as to increase the air
current, but he did not realize that what he had done was only one par-
ticular example of the general principle that combusion is favored by a
large supply of oxygen. The general principle and its useful application
belong to a sphere of thinking and experience which the untrained lay-
man has not yet mastered." (Judd, '15, pp. 413-414.)
A
"The first and most striking fact which is to be drawn from school
experience that one and the same subject-matter may be employed
is
with one and the same student with wholly different effects, according to
the mode of presentation. If the lesson is presented in one fashion it
will produce a very large transfer; whereas if it is presented in an entirely
different fashion it will be utterly barren of results for other phases of
mental life. It is quite possible to take one of the objects of nature study,
for example, and to teach it in such a way that it becomes an isolated and
utterly formal possession of the student. This has been illustrated time
and time again by the instruction which has been given in birds and
plants. A teacher can teach birds and plants in such a way as to arouse a
minimum of ideas in the student's mind. The training may be as formal
in these content subjects as it ever was in language instruction. On the
other hand, the same subject-matter may be taken by a different teacher,
and under other methods can be made vital for the student's whole
thinking. Thus the teacher who is dealing with birds as a subject of
nature study and secures an interest on the part of his students for the
world in which these birds live, through an examination of the structures
and habits of the birds, will have in this subject-matter one of the most
broadly interesting topics that can be taught. In exactly the same way a
teacher who knows how to make use of the materials given in a Latin
course may render this subject very broadly productive, as contrasted
with the teacher who merely gives the formal aspects of the subject.
Formalism and lack of transfer turn out to be not characteristics of sub-
jects of instruction, but rather products of the mode of instruction in
these subjects." (Judd, '15, pp. 412-413.)
217
218 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
side, and English composition are thinking subjects of very high educa-
"
tional value." (Thomas, M. C., Old Fashioned Disciplines," Journal
of the Association of Collegiate Alumna, May, 1917, p. 588.)
"I think everyone realizes as he grows older that he has his limitations.
I,for one, regret very keenly that I took a great deal of Latin and Greek
and did not spend far more time on advanced mathematics and physics.
I am, however, not now wasting any time in vain or useless regrets on
this account,but simply doing the best I can with the knowledge that I
have acquired."
"The mere routine labors of the translation of Greek and Latin authors
into one's vernacular, the effort to ascertain their exact meaning and the
TRANSFERENCE OF TRAINING 219
TABLE 55
The residual gain on the average was 29%. The average gain
made by the trained group in the practice series, comparing the
first day with the i4th day, was 112%. Hence the gain trans-
ferred to the allied arithmetical operations was only 26% of the
gain in the practice series itself. From one point of view, this
seems to be a very considerable amount of transfer, but when we
note that some of the end tests were as similar to the training
series as they could be without being identical with it the transfer
is small. We might expect almost a complete carrying over to
the closely similar operations but the largest amount of residual
gain took place in the multiplying of two-place numbers by a
one-place number and in the adding of three-place numbers; but
even there it was only slightly larger than the transfer to the other
operations.
Winch conducted a series of experiments to determine the amount
of transfer from improvement
in numerical computation to arith-
metical reasoning. In each experiment the class was divided into
two groups of approximately equal ability as shown by a previous
test in arithmetical reasoning. Then one-half of the class was
trained in "rule" sums after which a final test in arithmetical
reasoning was given alike to both groups.
The first class, composed of 1 3-year-old girls from a poor neigh-
borhood, showed improvement in numerical accuracy but no
transfer to arithmetical reasoning. The second class, composed of
"One-half of the following z6 words are alike in one respect and in that
226 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
respect unlike all the others in the list. Find these eight words and mark
them with a check (V)."
biscuit pirate mountains men
oxen geese fathers-in-law factory
scholars knives vessel table
pole frame children mice
(8 are plurals)
TABLE 58
YXA&S or LATIN NUMBER OF PUPILS AVERAGE SCORES
UNIVERSITY STUDENTS
o 47 IO.I
1-6 99 10.2
example, the upper part of Table 57. The students who had
10 to 15 years of foreign languages made a score in grammatical
knowledge of 63, as compared with a score of 47.8 made by the
students who had years of foreign languages, a difference of
2 to 5
made for the value of general mental training to be derived from the
study of languages. Thus Lodge states the value of the study of
Latin as follows:
"
Far above every other subjectit trains (i) the process of observation,
(2) the function of correct record, (3) the reasoning power and general
intelligence in correct inference from recorded observation. To this
should be added its great value in developing the power of voluntary
attention.
"The value of Latin as a practical subject has to do particularly with
the effect of the language in the cultivation of English style. In the
English vocabulary a very large proportion of words in everyday use are
of Latin origin, and it has been estimated that two-thirds of the Latin
vocabulary of the classical period has in some form or other come over
into English speech. For the correct use of synonyms in English and
the habit of expressing one's thoughts clearly, concisely, and cogently, a
discriminating knowledge of Latin is indispensable, and while not every
pupil in the school may be expected to develop a good style, nevertheless
he should be given the necessary foundation for it.
"When we turn to literature, we find that Latin is influential every-
where particularly in our classical authors by allusions, by quota-
tions, by actual domestication. Many of our great English writers are
permeated with Latin. We cannot expect that all will desire to feed
230 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
100%
oo
80
bo 70
M 60
60
A -One Year of Latin and German.
B-One Year of Latin.
C- Spanish Only.
2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 16
Weeks
FIG. 55. Progress of pupils in learning Spanish. After Swift ('06).
those who had only one year of Latin, and those who had never
studied a foreign language before. The "Latin" and the "Latin
and German " groups stood considerably higher at the beginning
than the "Spanish only" group, but the "Spanish only" group
gained gradually so that at the end of the 15 weeks it had made up
about two- thirds of the difference. Swift concludes:
"The number included in these tests was too small to serve as a basis
for anything more than tentative conclusions, but the results certainly
open the question whether the advantage to beginners of a new language,
so generally thought to accrue from the study of Latin, may not be due,
chiefly if not solely, to grammatical information that would be carried
over from one language to another, and which would naturally help
enormously at the start. In acquiring facility in the use of the Spanish
gender, to cite one example, Latin would aid materially, since the ma-
jority of Latin feminincs are feminine in Spanish, and a large part of
Latin masculines and neuters become masculine in Spanish. The de-
clension of Spanish adjectives for gender and number, and their agree-
ment, in these respects, with their nouns, would give Latin students a
further advantage. The teacher of the Spanish classes noted that more
frequent and detailed explanations of case were needed by those who
had not studied Latin. The order of words, also, was more readily
mastered by those familiar with the Latin arrangement. Finally, in
learning the conjugations and in understanding the significance of tenses,
the assistance of the information acquired under these topics in Latin
was found to be especially great. The indications, however, are that the
higher records made by the Latin and German pupils were the result of
the substance of language information obtained from these studies rather
1 "
than of so-called
any 'mental
'language* or discipline. (Swift, '06,
pp. 250 ff.)
o 25 81.8
1-2 224 81.9
3-4 195 83-05
5 6 155 84.0
The entire 783 papers divided on a basis of the number of years Latin was
studied
The pupils with no Latin may have had one or more years of
other languages and consequently Partridge presents the following
table for students who had studied Latin only:
1
It is obvious that the o columns in these two tables will contain record of exactly
the same pupils.
TRANSFERENCE OF TRAINING 235
the four-year Latin students showed a marked lead, and in all but
the spelling which I have considered above there is a steady re-
trogression although for the practical purposes the one-and-two-
year Latin students might be classed together."
The interpretation of these figures is by no means so obvious.
Harris has made no allowance for the native superiority of the stu-
dents with more years of Latin study. In fact, the probability is
that, if we may infer from other studies in which such a deduction
has been made, a large part of the superiority is due to original
nature. Harris's results as they stand prove little or nothing con-
cerning the effect of training in Latin.
F. M. Foster performed a similar experiment at the University of
Iowa; 503 freshmen, about equally divided between the sexes, were
given a spelling test of forty words of Latin derivation. The results
are given in the following table:
spent in the study of Latin and the ability to spell words of Latin
derivation for the two extreme Latin groups.
Best 14 of 4 year Latin girls, mental ability av. 25.8, spelling av. 5% error
This table shows clearly that students who chose to study Latin
had on the average a distinctly better native intelligence than the
non-Latin students and that the ability to spell words of Latin der-
ivation was to a considerable extent due to this superior intelligence
rather than to the study of Latin.
The secretary of the College Entrance Examination Board made
an extensive tabulation of the records of the classical and the non-
classical students who took the examinations in 1914, 1915, and
1916. The classical students are theones who offered Latin or
Greek, or both; non-classical students are those who offered neither
Latin nor Greek. A total of 21,103 candidates are concerned in the
following table which is based on the marks in all subjects except
Latin and Greek (reported in Value of Classics, 1917, p. 366):
"The combined data from the nineteen high schools and academies
reporting yield the following results:
"Students receiving High Honors at Graduation were 18% of all the
classical students, but only 7.2% of all the non-classical students.
"That is: the classical students show a superiority ol 150%.
"Students receiving Honors at Graduation were 32.1% of all tha
classical students, but only 30.8% of all the non-classical students.
"That is: the classical students show a superiority of 36.7%.
"Students receiving Honors or Prizes for Debating, Speaking or
Essay-writing were 8.8% of all the classical students, but only 3.5% oj
all the non-classical students.
"That is: the classical students show a superiority of 150%."
"The combined data from the seventeen colleges and universities
reporting yield the following results:
"Students receiving High Honors at Graduation were 17.3% of all
the classical students, but only 6.6% of all the non-classical students.
"That is: the classical students show a superiority of 162%.
"Students receiving Honors at Graduation were 46.5% of all the
classical students, but only 38.5% of all the non-classical students.
"That is: the classical students show a superiority of 20.7%.
"Students elected to Phi Beta Kappa were 16.8% of all the classical
students, but only 8.9% of all the non-classical students.
"That is: the classical students show a superiority of 88.8%.
"Students winning Prizes or Honors for Scholarship in Other than
Classical Subjects were 13.5% of all the classical students, but only
9.3% of all the non-classical students.
"That is: the classical students show a superiority of 45.2%.
1234
Years of High School
Median grades in English ofCedar Rapids High School students who studied
Latin, German or no foreign language
Here again the people with four years of Latin maintain their
"Obviously, the first step was to select two sets of pupils of equal
ability,one set in the second year of Latin, and the other in the second
year of a modern language. Accordingly, we chose pupils such that each
group had virtually the same average mark in Latin, on the one hand,
and modern language, on the other, and also in English, with the result,
in actual figures, that the non-Latin group in the two studies averaged
0.5 of i% the higher. To make doubly sure that the Latin pupils were
not favored, the non-Latin group were taken from the section of Mr. Mur-
dock, a classical scholar, who in his English teaching emphasizes the
Latin element in the language. There were twenty-one pupils in each
set, all in the second year class of the school.
"Five measurements were made, one in spelling, one of the use of
words in sentences, the third in definitions and parts of speech, the
fourth in the meaning of words and spelling, and the fifth in excellence
in vocabulary.
"Miss Humphrey selected the words in Nos. 1-4, and the subject in
No. 5. In Nos. i and 2 the words were taken from the 600 or 800 deriva-
tives in the notebooks of a fourth-year pupil of the class, who was ex-
cluded from the measurements. Moreover, to be fair to the non-Latin
TRANSFERENCE OF TRAINING 241
group, care was taken not to select words too difficult. In No. 3 the
words were taken from the 'Tale of Two Cities' which the pupils of both
groups were reading at the time in connection with their work in Eng-
lish II. Of the twenty words in No. 4, ten were taken ficm the 'Tale of
Two and ten from other sources. The subject in No. 5 was,
Cities'
1
'What do best. The papers were marked by teachers in the
I like to
English department and the results given to me. Altogether, six teachers
of English assisted in the measurements.
"To these five measurements is added a sixth in my opinion most
impressive of all. This test was made last June, shortly after I had
received Professor Holmes's letter, by Miss Gormley, with her pupils in
English II. As it happened, Miss Gormley, who was also the 'home-
room* teacher of all the pupils and consequently had access to their
marks, in making up the two groups to be composed of pupils of equal
ability,took into account not only foreign language and English II, as
was the case in measurements 1-5, but also the studies the pupils had
taken during the year. Hence we have even more reason in this case
than in the others to assume that the pupils were of equal ability. In
each set there were seventeen second-year students. The words were
taken entirely from Franklin's Autobiography and Silas Marner which
all were reading at the time. The Latin pupils were selected from the
first class Ihad had in the subject, just as they were completing the
course at the end of the second year.
"The result of the six measurements were as follows:
AVERAGES
LATIN NON-LATIN
PER CENT PER CKNT
January and February, 1914:
1. Spelling 82.5 72.6
2. Use of words in sentences 57.5 40 6
.
367.8 193-1
Averages 61 .3 32 18
.
32.18
Difference 29. 12%
"In No. i, the spelling measurement, the words were not difficult,
but such as ordinary pupils of sixteen should know something about,
' *
whether they had studied Latin or not ^as valedictory/ competition,'
'occurrence,' 'benevolence,' 'legible.'
"In No. 2, the pupils composed
sentences containing the derivatives,
some of which, in this measurement also, ought not to be unfamiliar to
242 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
1
non-Latin pupils in their second year of English, as 'impediment,
'advocate/ 'reference/ 'anticipate/ 'subside.'
"In the third measurement, the difference in the averages of the two
groups 69.5% and 33.3% was so great that Miss Humphrey thought
that perhaps too difficult words had been selected, or at least words which
placed the non-Latin students at an unreasonable disadvantage. Cu-
riously enough, in this measurement the words were taken, not from the
notebooks of a Latin pupil, as in the first two tests, in which the difference
between the two groups was much less, but, as stated above, from The
Tale of Two Cities. Furthermore, in No. 3, the non-Latin pupils were
so far afield in giving accurate definitions, and so confused in classifying
the words as to parts of speech, that it was decided to give another test
in which they should be asked, not to define words, but to give their
meanings, with the parts of speech omitted entirely. The results in this
measurement 57% and 27.5% were virtually the same as in No. 3.
"
Since practically every second-year pupil could write at least passably
on such a subject as 'What do I like to do best* it was decided to make
the basis of comparison in No. 5, not the average of the two groups, but
the percentage of rating above the passing mark. Moreover, in this
vocabulary test, emphasis was laid, not merely upon words of Latin
origin, but upon any words out of the ordinary, from whatever source.
The wide difference in the results from the view-point of excellence in
vocabulary 36.0% and 6.8% shows clearly what I have always be-
lieved and maintained, namely, that the work in commercial Latin
necessarily gives the pupils the dictionary habit, the results of which
extend far beyond the Latin derivatives actually studied.
"Of all the measurements, No. 6, was perhaps the most convincing.
In this test, the Latin pupils, unlike those in Nos. 1-5, had had during
the last six months of the two years' course the benefit of drill in a vocab-
ulary not in the commonest use and yet valuable and even necessary to
educated people. The list of words was taken entirely from Franklin's
Autobiography and Silas Marner which the pupils had just read, and
was not of unusual difficulty, consisting of such words, for example, as
asperity, promiscuous, mortuary. Yet by referring to the results it will
be seen that to the non-Latin group of pupils such words were practically
meaningless.
"An examination of the marks on these tests may prove of interest.
Among the seventeen non-Latin students the highest grade was 30%, and
five zeroswere recorded. In the Latin group, on the other hand, the
lowest mark was 30%, while one pupil received 100%, two 90%, two 80%,
five 70%, and only three had below 50%. The difference in averages of
the two groups was 53%." (Perkins, '14, pp. 11-14.)
by the Latin pupils. Even when the words are selected from Eng-
lish sources such as Silas Marner and Franklin's Autobiography
there is still the question as to the particular words chosen for the
test. It is obviously unfair to select words which are relatively rare
and whose meaning may readily be inferred from their origin. To
what extent the words were selected fairly cannot be judged since
Perkins docs not give the lists of words used.
The writer ('17) undertook a study to determine as precisely
as possible, the relative shares contributed by language training
and by original ability toward proficiency in English composition.
A series of tests was carried out with a group of 177 university
students. These tests together with their findings are given in
Table 74.
No. 3 consisted in writing an extemporaneous composition within
a limited time. These compositions were rated by three judges by
the Hillegas Scale.
No. 4 gives the average number of words written by each group
of students.
No. 5 gives the average number of different words used in each
composition.
No. 6 was a test in speed of reading. The numbers refer to the
words read per second.
No. 7 gives the number of words written in reproducing the
thought of the passage read in No. 6.
No. 8 gives the number of A's canceled in one minute in the well-
known A-test.
No. 9 gives the scores made in canceling in one minute a certain
geometrical form on a page of similar forms.
No. 10 consisted in reading to the persons a series of ten words
to see how many they could recall immediately afterwards.
No. ii was carried out by giving a stimulus word and having the
persons write as many associated words as they could in thirty
seconds.
No. 12 consisted in giving a series of ten words and allowing
fifteen seconds to each word for writing as many synonyms as
possible.
. No. 13 was a set of tests in imaging geometrical forms.
244 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
guages for a total of 9-15 years and those who pursued languages
for a total of 1-2 years could certainly not be due to language train-
ing.
Row 1 6 gives for the different groups the average scholarship
grades during the first year of the high school. It will be noted that
there is a steady increase from group to group. The 9-15 year
group had an average grade of 88.0, or five points higher than the
1-2 year group.
The next problem was to compare in common terms the five
points of difference in scholarship on the percentage scale with the
difference of 10.6 in quality of composition as measured by the Hille-
gas scale. To reduce these two types of measurements to commen-
surate units, fifty-eight compositions were rated by four persons
both by the percentage method and by the Hillegas scale. By a
process of equating values it was found that i.o point on the per-
Coefficients of correlation
TRANSFERENCE OF TRAINING 247
These correlations are so low that, with the exception of the ones
for grammar, no significance can be attached to them except to say
that there is practically no correlation between the various com-
parisons made and that the Latin group shows no superiority over
the modern language group in spelling, reproduction, definitions,
and a doubtful superiority in dictation, derivations, compositions,
and grammar. The differences that are shown are non-committal
and so small that they would have to be substantiated with other
groups to be conclusive.
d. Science. Claims of general training to be derived from the
One of the important values claimed for the sciences is the general
training of accuracy and fidelity in observation and the transfer-
ence of this particular type of training to other types of observation.
Miss Hewins ('16) made an attempt to measure the extent to which
this improvement is general or carries over to other types of ob-
servation. She divided each of three classes in botany, composed
of 34 boys and 50 girls, in the first year of a New York high school,
EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
into two groups, and gave them a series of tests in various kinds of
observation of a biological and non-biological nature as follows:
Series i
Practice series
MATERIAL RECORDING
1 May 15 Description of
" "
lilac flower
2 16 box-elder leaf 10 minutes
317 tt tt
jj lc s t em>
of gill-run-over-the-ground
\ eil
^ ancj flower for each
test
Scries 2
TRANSFERENCE OF TRAINING 249
The practice series was continued for a period of ten days with
one section of each of the three classes. The other section of each
class answered questions from books on the material of the lessons.
At the end of the period, she gave the tests in Series 2 to both
sections of each class and obtained the following gains in scores
over Series i:
"Table 77 shows that in the biological tests, the average gain of each
practisedboy was 8.06 per test for the 5 tests while the unpractised
showed a gain of 3.03. The practised girls averaged 6.41 gain per test,
while the unpractised lost 1.24 per test. In the non-biological, the prac-
tised girls gained 6.2 per test for the 6 tests while the unpractised gained
5.6; the practised boys gained 8.97 per test
and the unpractised boys
gained 5.27 per test."
"Feeling that the balance of arguments and scientific proofs were
against formal discipline when this investigation was begun, I am forced
by the results obtained to admit that in this experiment the proof seems
to be on the affirmative side.
"A valuable lesson, I think, can be drawn from one phase of this
investigation. By consulting the tables and summaries, it will be i
250 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY"
that sometimes one division does not fall in line with the general trend,
but that a larger number outweighs the negative and shows positive re-
sults." (Hewins, pp. in and 113.)
"
Moreover, the science (Geometry) has indirect effects which are not
small.
" ' '
Of what kind? he said.
"'There arc the military advantages of which you spoke,' I said;
and in all departments of knowledge, as experience proves, any one who
has studied geometry is infinitely quicker of apprehension than one who
has not.
" ' '
Rugg (
Test x.
i. Divide eighty-one by seven. .
Test 2.
"
TABLE 78. Representing the results for Rights." Adapted from Rugg's
table, p. 123 ('16)
TEST 1 TEST 2 TEST 3 TEST 4 TEST 5
TRAIN- CON- TRAIN- CON- TRAIN- CON- TRAIN- CON- TRAIN- CON-
ING TROL ING TROL ING TROL ING TROL ING TROL
GROUP GROUP GROUP GROUP GROUP GROUP GROUP GROUP GROUP GROUP
Feb.scores 20.0 1738 20.00 19.3 4.67 450 059 0.28 2.32 1.92
Junescores 22.81 2006 23.00 19.15 5.66 4.58 1.50 0.68 333 1.81
Grossgain 2.81 2.68 3.00 -.15 .99 0.083 .91 0.40 1.01-0.10
% ga in 14.5 15.6 15.0 -.78 21.2 1.8 157.0 143.0 43.5 -5.0
Residual
difference.... -1.10 15.78 19.4 14.0 48.5
These results are not very different from those surveyed in the
preceding chapter. Rugg states the results perhaps as favorably as
the data permit, perhaps too favorably. At any rate we ought to
note that the gain in the non-geometrical tests, Nos. i and 2, is
only about one-fourth or one-fifth as much as in the strictly geomet-
rical tests,Nos. 4 and 5. We also should note, as Rugg himself
points out, that only about two-thirds of the persons in the train-
ing group gained; the remaining one- third did not gain or show
transfer.
General Interpretation. The transference of training of the capa-
citiesinvolved in the learning of school material is very small so far
as present partial data indicate. This seems to be equally true
of all school subjects, the sciences as well as the languages and
mathematics. If we represent the possible transfer effect as ranging
from o%, or none, to 100%, or an improvement in other capacities
equal to that in the capacity trained, then the amount of transfer
is much nearer to the o%
end than to the 100% end. It probably is
o or very nearly o, for all capacities which are not distinctly similar
or related to the capacities specifically trained. Thus in the author's
experiment, practice in mental multiplication improved other forms
of mental calculations about one-fourth as much, but had no effect
upon immediate memory of numbers or words. In Winch's ex-
periment, practice in arithmetical computations had either no effect
or a doubtful effect upon arithmetical reasoning. In Rugg's in-
vestigation, practice in visualization ordinarily done in a course
in descriptive geometry had only a moderate effect upon visuali-
zation of other sorts. In Miss Hewin's study, improvement in
TRANSFERENCE OF TRAINING 253
as, for example, in the case of the effect of the study of Latin upon
the study of Spanish, or upon the knowledge of English grammar.
The fact of identity of material or similarity of procedure makes
254 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
have been made, the general disciplinary effect has been found to be
much less, or, in many instances, even non-existent. To argue that
because certain great leaders of men had a certain type of education,
it must have produced their greatness does not
prove the point.
They probably would have achieved distinction if they had had any
other sort of education. If the chief argument for pursuing a given
subject is that it selects the more able pupils, it would be much more
economical to do so by a shorter and more certain method. Almost
any fifteen or twenty mental tests that can be applied in a psycho-
logical laboratory in two hours would separate much more ac-
curately the gifted from the stupid.
Finally, the upshot of the experimental and statistical inquiries
into the transference of training is that effects of training are trans-
ferred in smaller amounts and within much narrower limits than has
commonly been assumed. This does not mean that there is no gen-
eral mental discipline in any form of training, nor that the doctrine
of formal discipline has been "exploded" but rather that the actual
limits of general discipline have been more accurately defined.
These limits, to be sure, seem to be much narrower than many are
inclined to believe. So far as the value of school subjects is con-
cerned, it means that the content value of a subject must be the
prime reason and the general disciplinary value the secondary
reason for pursuing it.
Before leaving this discussion, two further points should be borne
in mind: The first is that any effect of transfer, even if very slight,
would probably be worth while if it extended to all or to a large
number of capacities. If training in botanical observation improved
all forms of observation in life ever so little, it might still be the
best form of training in observation. But the implication of the
evidence thus far at hand is that the spread seems to extend only to
rather narrow limits. The second point is, that while the trend of
the arguments here presented would be to reduce the time devoted
to some subjects, particularly in high school and college, we must
be sure that we put something better in their places. The advan-
tage of some of the subjects that would suffer reduction is that they
are well organized for teaching purposes. Some of the new sub-
stitutes are not well organized and offer neither form nor content.
Transitions should be made gradually so that the new branches may
become organized and extended, and the teachers properly trained.
PART III
auditory and the visual centers whereby the sound and the sight
of the word become associated. Silent reading involves only
the first six steps except in so far as incipient speech move-
ments accompany it, in which case the remaining steps enter
in part.
Such an analysis as this may seem detailed to an unprofitable ex-
tent; but, in fact, it might be made even more detailed, depending
upon the extent to which we are able to discern and describe the
minuteness of the neural and mental functions involved. The
more complete and accurate our analysis and description of the
steps is, the more sure our knowledge for managing these
processes will be; and ultimately, that is what teaching amounts
to: The efficient management of the psychophysical processes
concerned.
The next problem is, How does each of the elements in the
reading process operate? The truth is that concerning many of
them we know at the present time very little or nothing with cer-
tainty or completeness. Concerning some of them, however,
considerable definite knowledge has been accumulated in recent
years. What the differences between an efficient and an inefficient
reader are, or what the difficulties in learning to read are at each
of the steps can be inferred partly, but only partly, from our
present knowledge about these factors. It is, however, certain
that the differences and difficulties are to be found in these and
possibly additional processes. We
shall examine each of these
the space around them. We do not know what the most ad-
vantageous size of younger children who are beginning
type is for
to learn to read. We feel that it ought to be larger than for older
children or for adults, but we do not know definitely how much
larger.
So far as the inertia of the retina to the reception of the stimuli
goes, we may
infer that it varies in different individuals probably
paper in such a way that the successive pages followed one another
on a single band of paper. These were placed upon special reels
in such a way that the pages would be at a uniform distance
from the eyes, at a uniform angle and would have uniform
illumination.
Three measures of the changes in the eyes produced by reading
were taken:
1. The number of lines read during a i5-minute period.
TABLE 70,. After Hull anil Ames. From a Thesis in the Library of the Uni-
versity of Wisconsin, 1917
placing a mirror on one page while some one reads the opposite
page, and then observing the action of the eyes as reflected in the
mirror. Javal ('79) counted the pauses of the eyes by means of a
sound attachment to the eyelids. Laudolt ('91) counted them
by direct observation. Erdmann and Dodge ('08) counted them
by observing the eyes of their subjects in a mirror. They found
more pauses with difficult than with easy reading material, and
also more pauses in reading a foreign language than in reading
one's native language.
The first successful attempt to record eye movements was made
by Huey ('98-^00). He
attached a plaster of Paris cup to the
cornea which was connected with a light aluminum pointer. This,
in turn, rested on the smoked drum of a kymograph on which the
movements were registered. In this manner he attempted to
study the nature and rapidity of eye movements and the nature
and length of successive pauses. Dodge then developed a falling
plate camera which photographed a beam of light reflected from
the cornea of the eye. This method was also used by Dearborn.
It has the important advantage over Huey's method in that it
READING 267
Figure 58. The vertical lines and brackets indicate the locations of the eye
fixations. The numbers give the length of the fixations in loooth of a sec-
ond. After Dearborn ('06).
They are probably caused by the fact that the peripheral percep-
tion of the beginning of the line is not accurate. The exact location
from the beginning and from the end of the line is gotten as a
habit after several lines have been read if the lines are of uni-
form length. For this reason the lines should be of the same
20fc EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
that is, a little distance from the beginning or the end words."
The first fixation in a line is usually longer than the other fixations.
average duration per pause was from 20 to 27% longer, and that
the perception time was from 44 to 64% longer.
By an ingenious combination of camera and phonograph, Gray
determined accurately the relation of the eye to the voice in oral
reading. He found that the eye always precedes the voice, with
some subjects as much as four words and with others much less.
As a general rule a wide eye-voice span was associated with fluent
reading.
(5) Transmission of visual nerve impulses to the visual area of
the brain. Little can be said about this process. All that we know
about it is that the velocity of nerve impulses varies in different
persons, and mny be slow or rapid in slow or rapid readers, but we
do not know definitely. Experimental work is necessary to de-
termine to what extent it may be true.
(6) The arousal of association processes. A word gradually
acquires meaning in the life of a child by its recurrence in numerous
situations and by the connection of the particular significance or
experience with the word. Thus, the child hears the word "chair"
in connection with a certain object on which he sits. He hears it
also after a while in connection with other objects which look
different from the one he is accustomed to using. But new associ-
ative bonds are formed and he knows in general what is meant
when he hears the word "chair." Later on he is shown a certain
combination of visual characters and is told the word "chair."
A new bond is then formed between the sound and the visual
stimulus of the word "chair." An important part of the act of
270 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
(?)> (8)> and (9) Transmission of nerve impulses from the visual
center to the motor speech centers and thence to the speech organs.
The motor speech centers concerned in the control of the speech
organs are highly specialized. In right-handed persons, they are
located in the left hemisphere of the brain in the region of the fis-
sure of Rolando. These processes obviously will be involved in
oral reading and in speech, but they are also active in silent reading
in the form of incipient speech movements, particularly in the
tongue and, to a less extent, in other speech parts. They are
active in the in speaking, only on a much smaller
same manner as
scale. To whatextent this inner speech is an important part in
the reading process is somewhat uncertain. It is believed by
some investigators to be an important agency for maintaining
the continuity of the thought aroused by the successive visual
glimpses of the printed line.
Disturbances in the motor speech centers are known under the
names of various types of aphasia which will not be considered here.
But even in normal persons the rapidity and facility with which
the neural centers act may have an effect upon the efficiency of
reading.
Much valuable information could undoubtedly be obtained
from a careful laboratory study of individuals with language de-
fects, such as persons who have difficulty in learning to read or
individual, and the control of his speech organs was rapid and
accurate. The inference by a process of elimination was that the
chief difficulty in his reading ability lay in the central assimilation
or association processes. It seemed that visual impressions were
brought into the brain centers with normal speed and facility,
but that there was, for some reason, extreme slowness in the
mental interpretation of these stimuli. A test in silent reading
and in oral reading showed him to be equally slow in both cases.
His rate of reading was approximately that of a child at the end
of the first grade, namely, 1.5 words per second. His compre-
hension of what he read was good. His general intelligence, as
shown by other tests, was normal for his age. His work in other
school subjects was satisfactory. It seemed, therefore, probable,
although not absolutely certain in the absence of further tests, that
his difficulty lay in the central interpretation processes. This case,
similar to others, shows in an interesting manner the great intricacy
READING 273
TABLE 81
Correlations between reading ability and various elements entering into reading
ability
1
Reported in an unpublished thesis in the library of the University of Wisconsin, 1918.
274 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
arily.
Gray has devised a test for oral reading consisting of a
('17)
series oftwelve graded passages which are used to determine the
rate of oral reading and the number of errors in the pronuncia-
1
tion.
GRADES
Speed of reading
12345678
TABLE 82
(words per second) 1.5 1.8 2.1 2.4 2.8 3.2 3.6 4.0
Comprehension
(words written) 15 20 24 28 33 38 45 So
1
Further discussion of these tests may be found in the original sources in which these
tests were reported, or in the writer's Educational Measurements, or in Monroe's Edu-
cational Tests and Measurements,
276 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
Speed
Fro. 59. The continuous lines represent the standard attainments in read-
ing. The broken lines represent the attainments in a certain school.
others because the terms in which his abilities are expressed are
accurately defined. Measurements of this sort have disclosed
enormous ranges of ability in the various school subjects as
pointed out in a preceding chapter on individual differences.
They have shown that at the present time pupils are not
promoted according to ability, but rather according to the num-
ber of years they have attended school. Thus, for example, the
READING 277
pupils in a fifth grade range all the way in reading ability, from
the second or third grade on up to the 8th grade. (See Figure 16
in Chapter III.)
Another interesting comparison at this juncture is the reading
ability of the pupils of various ages in each grade. Such a study
\\ as made of the
pupils in one school by the writer, and is shown in
the following table:
TABLE 83
Speed
Grades
FIG. 60. The continuous lines represent the standard attainments in reading.
The broken lines represent the attainments in a certain school.
"Silent Reading: During the study hour or seat work period, silent
reading isconducted by means of single copies of books containing in-
teresting material. Every child is given a book, and he reads as many
as he can during the period. When the silent reading period
pages silently
is finished, a mark is placed where the reading ended. At the next silent
reading period the pupil continues his reading, and so the work progresses
until he has finished the book. The silent period may be continued as long
as the teacher wishes. A record is kept of each child's reading by checking
off a book as soon as it is finished. All through the year, a unit book is
used during the regular recitation period for the drill that is necessary.
"Books are placed in the hands of the children on the first day of
school, and they are allowed to keep books at their desks to read in
school or at home as they desire.
"With this system, each child can go his own gait, reading as many
books in a year as he can. The best readers will read from thirty-five to
forty books. The average is about twenty books each." [Report of Read-
ing in Dodgeville (Wisconsin) Public Schools, by Supt. H. W. Kircher].
TABLE 84
Attainments in reading in a certain school
.................. . . .. . ..
Average age of pupils ............. 6. .7.1. .8.3. .9.2 10.3. 11 4. 12. 2.. 13.1
(Dodgeville Report, page 11.)
READING 261
"The groups which were not to have the speed drills, and which were
to be used as a basis for comparison with those which did have, were
dealt with after the usual fashion in teaching reading. The other groups,
in addition to their oral reading, were given daily speed drills, without,
however, giving a total of any more time to their reading than the other
group received. So far as feasible both groups were taught reading at
about the same time of day, or else at equally desirable periods. They
used the same books and the same degree of enthusiasm was expected
to be put into both. The drills were, of course, conducted by the teacher
in charge of the class, and ran from November 7th to June 2nd. They
were on relatively easy reading matter, and mostly interesting narrative.
They occupied ordinarily from five to ten minutes of the reading period.
The group as a whole was told explicitly where to begin and how far to
read, and were then all set to silent reading at the same time with the
exhortation to see who could get it read first. After all, or nearly all, had
finished someone was asked to tell the substance of what he had read. If,
in this reproduction, he omitted anything he was questioned on it as a
guarantee against skimming."
Tests were given to both groups at four points during the school
year, comparing the drill groups with the no-drill groups as a
base. From the first test to the last, the results showed a gain in
speed of 18.7% and the trifling loss of 1.1% in quality of com-
prehension.
Freeman ('16) reports a series of tests made by K. D. Waldo
on the possibility of increasing speed. The lower grades particu-
larly made a very large gain in speed which was accompanied by a
parallel gain in amount reproduced, as indicated in Table 86.
READING 283
TABLE 86
l
Improvement in reading from September to March
ington, and divided the pupils of each grade into six groups ac-
cording to their speed of reading, putting the slowest sixth together
and the next sixth together, and so on to the last sixth, consisting
1
From an unpublished master's thesis by K. D. Waldo, on eie in the library of the
University of Chicago.
284 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
of the most rapid readers. The results are exhibited in the follow-
ing table. The first column gives the average number of words read
by each group, the second column the average number of words
written representing a correct report of the thought, the third
column gives the speed of reading in terms of the number of
words read per second, the fourth column gives the percentage
of words read in relation to the number of words written.
TABLE 87
These results indicate ina striking manner that the rapid reader
comprehends relatively almost as much out of what he reads as the
slow reader, and, absolutely, he grasps nearly as many more ideas
in a given period of time as is proportional to the extra ground
covered. Specifically, the table shows that the average speed of
reading of the slowest group was 1.2 words per second, whereas
the speed of reading of the fastest group was 4.9 words per second.
The percentage of comprehension in relation to the amount read
was 46% for the slowest group and 37% for the fastest group. In
other words, the percentage of comprehension is almost as large for
the fast group as for the slow group; or, comparing the first and
second columns, we note that the fast group read almost exactly four
times as fast as the slowest group and wrote three and one-third
times as much as the slowest group. In other words, the ratio of
the speed of the reading between the fast and slow group is one to
comprehension is one to three and one-third.
four, while the ratio of
The inference is then that the rapid reader derives relatively almost
as much out of what he reads as the slow reader. Absolutely he
obtains several times asmany ideas. Concretely, the comparison
may be made in another way: Of two persons belonging re-
still
spectively to groups one and six, each reading for one hour, the
READING 285
fast reader would cover four times as much ground and derive three
and one-third times as many ideas as the slow reader. The fast
reader, therefore, has an astounding advantage over the slow reader.
These results consequently give no corroboration for the common
belief that an inverse relation exists between speed and compre-
hension in the fast and the slow reader.
Similar results have been presented by Judd. These results
are shown in the following diagram, Figure 61:
"For the purpose of this study of the relation between rate and quality,
all ofthe individual records of Cleveland pupils were divided into classes.
First the speed records were arranged in order from the most rapid to the
slowest. The most rapid of these records \vere designated by the simple
* 1
term 'rapid.' In this class of rapid records were included the most
rapid 25% of all the records. In like fashion the slowest 25% of all the
records were set aside and designated as 'slow.' This left half the rec-
ords, or the middle 50%, which were designated as of 'medium speed.'
In like manner the 25% of all records which were qualitatively the best
were designated 'good'; the 25% which were qualitatively the worst were
designated 'poor,' and the term 'medium' was applied to the middle 50%.
"It becomes a very simple matter to assign all records in each grade
to the appropriate class and determine the percentage of the grade which
falls into this class. Diagram 59 gives the results, the percentages being
in each case the nearest whole number to the calculated figure, and the
size of the circle being proportionate to the size of the class indicated.
"These figures serve to emphasize the fact that good readers are
usually not slow and poor readers are usually not fast. It is evidently
not safe to attempt to lay down any absolute rule. There are good read-
ers who are slow. In some cases such readers may be temperamentally
slow. But even making allowance for such individual peculiarities, the
figures show that good reading and slow reading are not incompatible.
In like manner there are a certain number of children who read rapidly
but retain little of what they read. With the figures in hand a teacher
can profitably study her class and determine somewhat more completely
than it is possible to do for the whole school system what are the special
explanations of each individual type of ability.
"For the purpose of this survey the general fact that high rate and
good quality are commonly related, and that low rate and poor quality
are commonly related, is of great importance."
producer and the best reproducer was one of the fastest readers.
READING 287
pupils. The following figures give the average speed of oral and
silent reading in terms of words read per second:
TABLE 88
3 2.1 2.3
4 2.3 2.6
5 2.4 3.1
6 2.8 3.9
7 3-i 4-7
8 3-9 4-3
These results indicate that in the third grade the speed of oral and
silent reading is very nearly identical, but that silent reading in-
creases thereafter considerably faster from grade to grade, so that
in the eighth grade the rate of silent reading is approximately one
word per second, or about 25% faster than oral reading.
Mead ('15) tested 112 pupils in five classes in the sixth grade in
both oral and silent reading. He made six tests, each two minutes
long, and determined the speed by the number of lines read and the
comprehension by the number of "points" reproduced in writing.
His results are as follows:
TABLE 89
Relative ability in silent and oral reading. After Mead ('15)
Av. No. Av. No POINTS PFR CENT REPRODUCED
LINES READ REPRODUCED OF AMOUNT READ
Silent reading 39-4 16.4 38. 7
Oral reading 33.6 12.1 32.9
Each of the five classes did better in silent reading than in oral
reading. Mead concludes: "From the results of these five classes
288 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
O -f O i-< O OS
0<S
fc 00
3> U
c
Sti>
OJ f fS O t^- 00 O O 00 >
>S*
READING 289
we are more convinced than ever that our schools devote altogether
too much time to oral reading and too little to silent."
Within the following two years, Mead repeated the same tests
with 340 pupilsin grades three to ten, excepting the ninth, and ob-
tained corroborative results. "Fifteen out of seventeen classes
did better by the silent method of reading. Seventy per cent of the
children taken separately did better by this method." The de-
tailed facts are given in Table 90.
Pintner made eight tests, two minutes in length, with 23 pupils
in the fourth grade and found the following results:
TABLE 91
Thus it appears in all of the tests that silent reading has the lead
over oral reading in both speed and comprehension. H. A. Brown
('16) and others believe that there should be no oral reading as
such after the third grade, that silent reading should be empha-
sized instead, that above the third grade teaching to read should be
phonic classes concentrated on the word and the sound at the ex-
pense of the sense, that their reading was less smooth and slower
and that their ideas were confused. On the other hand, they re-
ported that the no-phonics classes enjoyed their reading, that they
read more swiftly, more expressively, and more for the sense of the
material but that they did not read quite so accurately. The
ability to attack new words was about the same. Experiments
of this sort ought to be carried out more extensively and compari-
sons of the results should be made by means of more precise, quan-
titative measures.
publishers and educators, but no one knows with certainty the com-
parative merits of these methods nor which ones are most economic-
ally productive of the best development in reading ability.
Superintendent Harris ('16) of Dubuque, Iowa, in conjunction
with H. W. Anderson, undertook an experiment to determine the
relative merits of three systems of teaching reading. The teachers
had felt for some time that they were not securing the results in
reading that they might reasonably expect. The experiment is
described thus:
the various systems of primary reading out of the realm of mere opinion
and place upon bed rock by scientific evaluation of results actually
achieved.
"In order to accomplish this it was decided to test:
"i. The mechanics of oral reading in the second half of Grade I (lA)
and in the two divisions of Grade II (2B and 2A).
"
2. The silent reading in Grade II: for (a) rate; (b) comprehension.
"These tests were given in the following schools:
"It was believed that these tests would show the results obtained by
the Beacon system and the Horace Mann system during the first two
years of their use, and offer an opportunity for comparison with each
other and with the Aldine system."
Oral reading was tested with Gray's oral reading scale, and silent
reading was tested by Starch's tests measuring the speed and com-
prehension of reading. The results obtained are given in the fol-
lowing tables.
TABLE 92
Results of the oral reading test
employed.
"The results of the Oral Reading Tests seem to show conclusively that
the pupils trained by the Beacon System are very greatly superior in the
mechanics of oral reading to those trained under the Aldine or the Horace
Mann systems of reading."
2Q2 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
C D E F
.8 .9 1.7 2.
.8 .9 1.7 1.9
where the average number of words was 24.3. The results obtained
through the Horace Mann system seem to be below the average, this
class reproducing only 15.1 words.
"The results of the Silent Reading Tests seem to show conclusively
that the pupils trained by the Beacon System are far superior to those
trained under the Altline or the Horace Mann systems of reading.
"It was realized that objections might be raised to the results herein
shown, on the ground that the teacher rather than the system was the
strong factor in the results. While it is highly improbable that out of six
different corps of teachers, the group teaching the Beacon System would
be uniformly better, in each section tested ; yet, as an absolute check upon
this phase of the matter, the Silent Reading Tests were given in Grade
IIIB, which entered school before cither the Beacon System or the Horace
Mann System was placed on trial in any school and which therefore could
not have had, in any one of the six schools, its initial training in either of
the two systems named." (Starch's Reading Test, Series A, No. 3, was
used.)
TABLE 95
"This table sho\vs that the pupils at School A read more rapidly than
those at any other school, their median rate being 2.9 words per second,
while the nearest approach to this rate was 2 words per second in School
E. However, comprehension three schools excel Grade IIIB at
in
School A. A reproduced 24 words correctly, while those
Pupils at School
at Schools B, D, and E reproduced 27.5, 29, and 27.5 words respectively.
"Furthermore, in the tests made in Grade II, while the Beacon group
at School A excelled all other groups in rate and comprehension of silent
reading, several of the other schools excelled the non-Beacon group at
School A in both these points. Thus, in the rate of silent reading, in
Grade IIB, the non-Beacon group in School A was excelled by Schools B,
E, and F, and in Grade IIA, the non-Beacon group at School A was
equalled by School F; and in comprehension, the non-Beacon group at
School A was excelled by School E in Grade IIB, and by Schools C, E, and
F Guide IIA.
in
"These facts show rather conclusively that it was not the superiority
of the teaching which determined the results of the tests, since teachers
in other schools than School A, both in Grade II and in Grade III, with
pupils trained under the old systems, secured results as good as or better
than those secured by the teachers at School A with pupils whose first
training also had been under the old system."
2Q4 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
reading.
Gray made a comparison of the attainments in reading in 44
schools, 26 of which had used the Aldine method, 17 the Ward
method, and one a method of its own. The results showed no con-
sistent or uniform superiority of one method over another. The
Pupils were formerly told that they must not read fast but that
they should read slowly because they would then get the thought so
much better. In the third place, there has come along with these
two changes a shift from the mechanics of reading to the content
of reading. Former aims of reading are fairly represented by the
following answers, given by pupils who had finished the grammar
school, in response to the question, What is your idea as to what
the reading lessons were for? Some of the answers were: "To learn
to pronounce," "To help us in reading before people," "Just to
pass away the time," and "I thought it was to learn us to use better
language." (Briggs '13).
Accordingly then the aim in reading to-day is the development
of speed in reading and a parallel gain in thought-getting. Recog-
nizing this change in aim, what definite suggestions can be made
to facilitate improvement in these aspects of reading ability?
Experimental results are as yet too few to make many specific
recommendations with complete confidence. However, several
important suggestions may be offered.
(a) As to the speed of reading: Force yourself to read more
quent intervals for two reasons. In the first place, they will afford
the pupil himself a definite basis for discovering his reading abil-
ity and, by keeping his own record from test to test, they will
furnish a powerful stimulus to the pupil to surpass his own preced-
ing attainments. This point was elaborated more fully in Chapter
XI. In the second place, intensive tests with emphasis on both
rate and comprehension will give the pupil practice in the phases
of reading in which the school has in the past not furnished ade-
quate training. The great emphasis upon oral reading has tended
to slow habits of reading and placed the primary emphasis
instill
297
298 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
there was a definite relation between the handwriting and the per-
sonality of the writer. The greater pressure exerted by men as a
class suggests that the sex of the writer determines at least certain
characteristics of the writing. This has been investigated inde-
pendently by Binet, Downey, and Starch who find that untrained
subjects can determine the sex of the writer in from 65 to 75% of
the specimens (50% being pure chance). The author made a com-
parison of the writing ability of 2,113 boys and girls in the Madison
schools and found the differences exhibited in Figures 63 and 64.
It appears from these graphs that the median of the girls is above
that of the boys in both speed and quality, but particularly in
quality. The difference in speed is very slight.
Miss Downey ('10) attempted to determine sex differences in
handwriting by selecting from envelopes by a chance method 100
samples of men's writing and 100 samples of women's writing and
by asking thirteen persons to judge whether a given sample had been
written by a man or by a woman. The thirteen persons made re-
spectively the following percentages of correct judgments: 60, 60,
61, 64, 66, 66, 63, 68.5, 70, 70.5, 71.4, 71.5, 77.5. These show
that on the average a judgment of sex as revealed in handwriting
is correct 67% of the times, or two out cf three times. Miss Downey
women showed less variability
also reports that the writing of the
and more conventionality than that of the men. Those samples of
writing by women which were called masculine were generally from
persons accustomed to doing a great deal of writing.
Correlation of handwriting with other traits. A
good deal of mis-
leading character interpretation is based upon various features of
handwriting. Experimental work needs to be done in this field, but
it is probable that there is nothing to the claim of graphologists
-
84
u
FIG. 63.
12345678 -L. -L.
Grades
-L.
FIG. 64. Sex differences in quality of writing. After Starch ('13, p. 461).
model, (3) an extreme slanting model, and (4) a large model with
many flourishes. The purpose of the typewritten passage was to
obtain at the outset a sample of the normal writing of each person.
The other three models were taken from school copy books.
After the experiment was finished, each person was asked whether
he had tried purposely to imitate the various models. Three
persons stated that they had intentionally modified their styles
of writing. Their records were thrown out. The samples pro-
duced by the remaining 103 persons were carefully measured to
ascertain their slant and size. Slant was measured by means of
a specially prepared, transparent device with ruled lines for de-
termining the angle of inclination of certain tall letters, such as 1,
f,and p, with the base line on which the words were written. Size
was measured by determining the horizontal width of letters by
measuring the length of words and dividing by the number of
letters in the word.
These measurements showed that the average tendency for this
group of persons was to make the letters distinctly more vertical
when the vertical model was before them and more slanting when
the slanting model was before them as compared with their nor-
mal styles of writing. They also tended to write slightly larger
when the large model was before them. The amounts of these
changes were as follows:
TABLE 97
"
Average inclination of 1 written from vertical copy 68.8
written from slanting copy
"
Average inclination of 1 61.5
"
Change from normal to vertical 3.7
"
Change from normal to slant 3.6
"
Total range of change 7.3
Average width of letters in normal writing 4.33 mm.
Average width of letters written from large model 4.85 mm.
in the presence of the pupils for the purpose of showing them how
to write, is likely to be more efficacious in securing imitation than
a static model in a copy-book would be. It would seem, therefore,
highly imperative that every elementary school teacher should be
a reasonably good writer.
In connection with the survey of penmanship in the Grand
Rapids, Michigan, schools, Freeman (Judd, '16) reports that
"Grand Rapids adopted about five years ago a new system of pen-
manship. Up to that time the writingwas not regarded as satisfactory.
A part of the difficulty was thought to be due to the inability of the
teachers themselves to write well enough to furnish a good example to
the pupils. Accordingly, by action of the Board of Education, all teachers
in the elementary schools were required, as a condition of promotion, to se-
cure a Palmer certificate. This rule has been recently enforced with strict-
ness and the writing in the schools is reported to be greatly improved."
speed on the one hand and poor quality on the other. That is the
"extreme slant" writers write more rapidly and more poorly than
the "vertical" writers.
(2) Length of Period of Practice. What is the most productive
practice period in learning to write? Even such a question as
this, which is capable of definite experimental solution, has been
answered only in part. The answer given by school programs in
the time allotted therein for writing, is based largely on opinions
instead of facts.
TABLE 99
Quality of handwriting at roughly the same rate in seven school systems. After
Thorndike ('10)
"What these facts do prove is: First, that at least three systems (C,
D, and E) get little or no better results at a time cost of about 75 minutes
a week than two systems (A and B) do at zero time-cost; second, that
one system (F) at no greater time-cost than C, D, and E gets results
about 25% better than they do; and third, that practice for quality may
secure only at the cost of speed. The teachers in A and B are better
it
paid than those in the other cities, so that the success of these schools at
no time-cost might not be generally attainable.
"Leaving F out of account, the differences of these school systems in
the method of teaching handwriting, in the time devoted to it, and in
the ideals of the system in respect to it arc of inconsiderable influence
upon efficiency. One makes its pupils write very well at very slow rates,
the others vary a little in quality with small inverse variations in speed.
On the whole, in spite of the achievement of system F, efficiency in hand-
writing seems, like spelling, and unlike arithmetic to be under present
conditions not very much influenced by the management of the schools."
(Thorndike, "Handwriting," p. 33.)
10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45
Rank
FIG. 65. Relation between attainment in writing and time devoted to writ-
ing. After Freeman ('15).
4 5
Grades
FIG. 66. Average speed and quality of handwriting of the various grades
in a given school. The broken lines represent the school. The continuous lines
are the standard attainments.
8th Grade
7th Grade
6th Grade
5th Grade
80 40 50 60 70 80 90
Quality-Ayers Scale
FIG. 67. Average speed of handwriting at each quality of writing from 20
to 90, 10,528 cases from 5th, 6th, 7th and 8th grades. After Judd ('16, p. 72).
when one tries to write especially well. The school is constantly in the
position of seeking some reasonable balance between speed and quality.
"Diagram 67 gives the facts for the 10,528 specimens carefully studied.
314 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
In the vertical axis of this diagram are represented the different speeds;
in the horizontal axis are the various grades of quality. The results from
each grade are represented separately. Thus, beginning at the extreme
right end of the bottom line, we see from the diagram that for those
writers in the fifth grade who show the highest quality (90) the rate is
on the average 51 letters per minute. Advancing along the line toward
the left, we find that those in the fifth grade who show a quality 80 have
an average speed of 54 letters.
"The diagram shows that there is a general area between qualities
60 and So, and between speeds 60 and 80, where all the grades above the
fifth may be said to reach a level. Greater speed seems to be purchased
at an undue sacrifice of quality, and higher quality seems to result in
much slower speeds. We thus have in our results some indications as
to the probable area within which teachers will find a desirable balance
between speed and quality." (Pp. 70-71.)
"Diagram 68 shows the average results for the four upper grades in
36 schools. The figure is to be interpreted as follows: In the upper
diagram, which gives the results for the fifth grades, there are numerous
small squares, each representing a single fifth grade. In each square is a
number showing the average number of letters written per minute in a
grade. Thus in the square at the extreme left of the diagram is the num-
ber '39.' This means that the average number of letters written per
minute by that fifth grade was 39. In the next vertical column of squares
are numbers ranging from 42 to 49. These indicate that there were fifth
grades showing each of the averages given.
"One of the most impressive facts which is brought out by this com-
parison is that the slowest fifth grade
is only half as fast as its fastest
fifth grade. Like statements can be made regarding the other grades.
These wide differences cannot be attributed to any native characteristics
which the children bring to the school. Such disparities might appear in
individuals, but the figures report whole grades. All the fifth grades are
going through the schools parallel with one another and arc officially
ranked as alike. The same statement can be made regarding the other
HANDWRITING 3*5
Speed Records of 56
36 Fifth Grades 75
64
64
52
139142 61
66
Speed Records of
36 Sixth Grades
60 71
81
r-t^%
47 50 60
1 1 1
88
75
Speed Records of 78
78 85
36 Seventh Grades
73 84
64 72 06
57
145150 3SL
79
79 88
76 87
Speed Records of
36 Eighth Grades 74
72
_7J. 81
3 <W_ _TO 80
64 MtOOl
grades also. Perhaps the most obvious case is that of the eighth grade.
Children will go out of the various eighth grades into high schools with
the official assumption that they arc equally well fitted for advanced
Quality Records of
36 Fifth Grades
Quality Records of
36 Sixth Grades
Quality Records of
36 Seventh Grades
Quality Records of
36 Eighth Grades
work, and yet one eighth grade averages only 46 letters a minute, and
another averages 101. Is it not evident that there must be a difference
in emphasis on speed in writing in different schools?" (Pp. 63-64.)
"Diagram 69 shows in a manner similar to that explained in the
earlier paragraph on speed the results obtained from 36 schools. From
the figure it will be seen that in quality, as in speed, the most striking
variation exists between grades which are officially recognized as parallel.
Furthermore, there is the same overlapping of grades, several of the
fifth grades ranking higher than the average eighth grade." (P. 68.)
FIG. 70. Position of pupil in relation to desk and paper. After Freeman
Ci4>.
(8) Movement
Drills. Special drills in movements such as
ovals, vertical movements progressing to the right, horizontal
movements from left to right and from right to left, have been
advocated by various systems of penmanship with the belief that a
substantial amount of time given to such drills will establish good
form and speed in writing. To what extent such formal drill or
how much of it may actually be profitable, is open to question.
It would be an experiment worth undertaking to teach three
sections of a class of pupils for a year or more by giving to one
section a considerable amount of such drill, to the second section
none, but to devote instead the entire time to drill and practice in
writing the letters themselves, and to the third section a com-
bination of the two types of drill.
320 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
(i) Methods
of Measurement. On the face of it, it would
seem be an easy matter to devise a definite and objective method
to
of testing attainment in spelling. All that would seem to be neces-
sary would be the selection of a series of words and the determina-
tion of the number or percentage of these words that a pupil or
class can spell correctly. But a closer study of the possibility of
TABLE 99
upper grades.
80
70
^60
FIG. 71.
2345678 Grades
Sex differences in spelling as measured by the author's test.
in the preface that "It omits the alphabet and the 'ababV on the
one hand, and on the other, quite a number of sesquipedalian
words common to all old-time spelling books." It further urges
as a vantage point, "The practical character of the work which
aims to set forth, not the tens of thousands of long-tailed words in
osity and ation, but the actual vocabulary used in speaking and
writing." And yet the book contains in one lesson (page 143)
such words as, lethean, pharisee, pentagon, pneumatics, theoc-
racy, anathema, dysentery, etc. In another lesson (page 144) it
contains oleaginous, farinaceous, argillaceous, lachrymose, sacer-
dotal, animadversion.
SPELLING 327
97 seven times
82 eight times
53 nine times
225 ten to nineteen times
224 twenty or more times
is the fact that the sharp bend in both curves occurs between
words whose frequency is between two and three. After three,
the curve shoots up very rapidly. This same breaking point occurs
in the Cook List between seven and eight. It is higher in this list
because Cook tabulated a larger amount of writing. Words
occurring three or more times in the Starch and Eldridge Lists
constitute over nine-tenths of all running words.
This process of selection yielded 2,626 words. This number may
seem small compared with the number of words in former spelling
8000
2700
2400
^2100
* 1800
1500
1200
I
000
600
300
20 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 a 2 1
Number of Times Words Occurred
IMC,. 72. Number of words occurring with various frequencies in the El-
people. Cook found that three out of his thirteen persons who had
each furnished 40,000 running words (the largest number obtained
from any one individual) of family correspondence had used a
vocabulary of 2,575, 1,546, and 2,330 different words respectively.
This would mean that a person could write 100 letters each 400
words in length, making a total of 40,000 running words, and not
use more than 2,500 different words. If we may assume that the
average man or woman, exclusive of persons whose occupation
involves considerable writing, such as novelists, teachers, and
journalists, writes one such letter a month, his entire correspondence
for ten years would not involve more than 2,500 different words.
with reference to any given word final decision was made according
to the third plan.
The 2,626 words were first arranged according to their frequencies
of occurrence and all those words in the list which were also in the
Jones List were then placed into the grades in which the Jones
List shows them to be first used by children. A considerable num-
ber of the 2,626 words were not found in the Jones List. These
words were placed into higher and higher grades according as they
occurred less and less frequently.
After this task was completed the entire list of words was re-
checked according to the third plan. Fifteen different lists of
words which had been prepared by various cities or school systems
for their own uses such as the Boston Minimum List, the Stockton
List, the Santa Cruz List, the Chicago Speller, etc., were used.
After each of the 2,626 words occurring in one or more of the lists,
was written the number of the grade into which the word was
placed by each list. An average of these placements was then
obtained and accordingly the word was finally placed into its
grade. For example, the word "flower" was placed by seven
lists into different grades as follows: 4, 4, 2, 3, 2, 2, 4. This gives
an average grade placement of 3.00. " Cough was assigned by six
"
of 3.66. All of the 2,626 words were thus assigned with the excep-
tion of 126 words which did not occur in any of the lists employed
and 178 words which were found in only one list. In order to make
the grade placement of these 304 words with equal confidence, a
group of seven experienced elementary teachers or supervisors
were asked to assign each of these words to some one grade accord-
ing to their best judgment. An average of these judgments was
then obtained and the words were placed accordingly.
All of the 2,626 words were then assigned to the various grades
according to the average grade placement as follows:
This process gave 360 words in each grade or two for each school
day with the exception of grade one into which the remaining 106
easiest words fell.
That the words in the second grade are not too difficult is shown
by the fact that in several schools the percentages in this grade
averaged as high as 90. The chief reason for the lower percentage
in the first and second grades is the variation in amount of actual
instruction in spelling given in different schools in these grades.
The best school among the 22 averaged around 90% in every grade
and the poorest school averaged around 60% in its various grades.
Aside from this list, numerous special lists have been prepared
partly on the basis of vocabulary studies or partly on the basis of
words commonly misspelled by pupils as reported by teachers.
Illustrations of such lists are the Boston Minimum Spelling List
(1915) consisting of 762 words, the Nicholson List, consisting of
3,070 words, prepared for the State of California, and the Chico
(California) List, consisting of 3,470 words, prepared by Studley
and Ware.
(2) The Influence of Rules in Spelling. Cook ('12) made a
test with 50 words on 70 university freshmen and on 39 high school
seniors and 30 high school freshmen. These 50 words were ex-
TABLE 100
give the rules they had learned only six weeks previously. In the univer-
sity group, those who gave some sort of rule to cover any part of the list
of words, averaged 4% higher in general spelling efficiency than those who
could not give any rule. So it is fair to assume that their better observ-
ance of the rules as shown by Table 100 is the result of their better spelling
ability in general, and not to any conscious application of the rules as
such. Not a single rule tested proved to be of real value, except the one
for the last two words of the list that relating to final ie." (After
Cook.)
made between two groups, one of which had learned and applied
rules while the other one had never had any contact with rules.
(3) Length of Class Period. Dr. J. M. Rice ('97) tested the
spelling ability of about 33,000 pupils to ascertain the effects of
different factors upon efficiency in the subject, such as methods of
teaching, foreign parentage, home environment, amount of time
devoted to spelling in the school program, and the like. His re-
sults with reference to the factor of time are presented in the
table.
The results as they standwould seem to indicate that length of
class period makes no achievement in
difference in the ultimate
^2 CA3
Si
336 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
SPELLING 337
Records of 1898
"
In brief there
,
is no direct relation between method and results. . . .
The results varied as much under the same as they did under different
methods of instruction. The facts here presented, in my opinion, will
admit of only one conclusion, viz., that the results are not determined
by the methods employed, but by the ability of those who use them. In
other words, the first place must be given to the personal equation of the
teacher, while methods and devices play a subordinate part." (After
Rice.)
"It was decided to abandon the use of the spelling book and home
lessons in the subject, to omit also the period from the school programme
which had been devoted to its study and recitation and to investigate the
effect that the abstraction of these influences might produce upon the
spelling of the pupils of the several school grades. Several methods of
measuring results were devised which will be herein described and statis-
tically reported upon." (After Cornman.)
good as that of the other schools and that the pupils improved
steadily in spelling, even though special instruction in them had
been omitted. Some of his figures follow:
7th grade 73 o 69 .9
6th grade 70.4 65 i
.
The spelling ability of classes in the Northwest School who had for
three years been without specific instruction in spelling compared with
that of corresponding grades of previous years, who had had the full
amount of drill in spelling.
7th grade 99 i
.
98 6
.
6th grade 96 75 99 .o
5th grade 96 95 97 6
.
That is, half of the classes without specific instruction did better and
half of them did worse than the corresponding classes with specific in-
struction.
Cornman concludes:
"
(6) The amount of time devoted to the specific spelling drill bears no
discoverable relation to the result, the latter remaining practically con-
stant after the elimination of the spelling drill from the school programme.
" view of the economy of time, to rely
(9) It is therefore advisable, in
upon the incidental teaching of spelling to produce a sufficiently high
average result.
"
(10) The average result is what can be and is attained, as shown by
statistical evidence, by average pupils under teachers of average pro-
fessional efficiency in classes of average size, i. e., in the elementary
schools of this country as now organized. To remain strictly within the
evidence gathered by this investigation, it must be admitted that there
may be teachers of surpassing ability, who can obtain more than average
results by the method of the specific spelling drill, and other teachers of
meaner ability who need the drill to bring their pupils up to the level of
this average result. It is claimed, however, that there is no evidence
(whatever may be the wealth of opinion) to prove that such teachers
exist or to show where they may be found. Moreover, the evidence
which has been presented in this paper makes their existence at least
improbable."
The extensive work of both Rice and Cornman has been very
valuable in attacking a large educational problem by more exact
methods and in showing that there is undoubtedly an enormous
waste in the teaching of spelling, as there is probably in all school
subjects; but the results are not final proof that length of time or
methods of learning are negligible elements. Indeed both labora-
tory experiments in the learning process and the more recent and
more carefully controlled tests in spelling itself indicate quite
certainly that length of time and manner of learning do make
important differences.
The author found that there are large differences in the average
spelling attainment of the various classes of any given grade in
the same school system. The results for the 10 schools of a Wis-
consin city, Table 104, City J, were as follows:
SPELLING 341
TABLE 104
45678
105.
Combined averages for the composition and column tests, all schools
GRADES AU.GEADIS
Percent 98.40 96.31 96.95 97.03 96.28 97.00
342 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
He states:
"First, the general spelling efficiency for all schools shown (97%) is
striking. It is 12.6% higher than Kratz's results (84.4%, for the fourth
to eighth grades, inclusive), 25% higher than Chancellor's (72%, for
10,000 pupils from the fourth to eighth grade), 25.48% higher than the
results in Rice's column test (71.52%), which consisted of a list of dic-
tated words, and 22.42% higher than the results from his sentence test,
which consisted of dictated sentences containing 50 test words for the
fourth and fifth grades, and 75 words for the sixth, seventh and eighth
grades. It eclipses by 25.7% Cornman's average in three term examina-
tions during three years for eighty Philadelphia schools (71.3%), and
is 27% higher than the results of these examinations in his two experi-
mental schools (70%), in which the spelling instruction was incidental.
In four column tests given to these two schools from September to June
and consisting of lists of fifty words differing from grade to grade, the
averages were 33%, 49%, 50% and 50% respectively for one school, and
49%> 57% 60% and 68% for the other; while the repetition of Rice's
column and sentence tests gave an average efficiency in 1897 of 78.9% in
one school, and 67.1% in the other, and in 1898, 73.1% and 65.4% re-
spectively, for the column test. The corresponding averages for the
sentence test were: 82.3% and 74.6% in 1897, and 76.5% and 77.9% in
1898. It will be observed that there was a loss of efficiency in 1898 except
in the case of the sentence test for one school." (After Wallin.)
Average % 98 96 73 68
Daily results 98 .. 78
SPELLING 343
upon the spelling of words, or (2) by any device that will make
conspicuous the particular part of any word that is most likely to
be misspelled. These points will be considered more fully in the
next three paragraphs.
(6) Careful Attention upon the Successive Letters of a Word.
Pryor ('15) reports "an experiment to determine the value of
'spelling the word through' as an aid to learning. Two divisions
of the fifth grade studied the same list.
Conditions as to time,
length of period, and the like were the same for both divisions.
For one, emphasis was placed on observing carefully the order of
letters while studying. Preliminary and final tests given to both
divisions showed an advance from 50.55% to 83.39%, or an average
gain of 32.84% for the division working under the usual condi-
tions. The other division advanced from 48.58% to 89.14%, an
average gain of 40.56%."
(7) Personal Incentive to Interest and Effort on the Part of the
Learner. Aside from competitive contests in their various forms,
there are at least two ways in which personal effort may be stimu-
lated: (a) By
having at regular intervals definite tests, preferably
by means of some one of the standard spelling scales or tests so
that each pupil may know his attainment from time to time and
measure his progress. This plan will usually arouse very keen
interest in the individual to surpass his own previous record. See
their spelling efficiency in all written work. This plan was em-
loo
80
60
40
20
8 4 5 6 8
Grades
p IG yjj Averages in spelling in grades 5 to 8 in a certain school as measured
by the author's test. The continuous line represents standard attainments.
Wallin ('n) used the same test words both in a column test and in
a dictated composition and found an average spelling effi-
test
of words in the mind, since in the normal child words and meanings
are built up together through oft repeated associations of words
" "
and meanings. Hence we shall use the term word-idea to convey
this union of symbol and meaning. Furthermore, from the psychol-
ogical standpoint, the occurrence in the mind of ideas or words
to be expressed, is fundamentally a matter of the psychology of
association. Where do the ideas come from? What causes them
to arise in themind? Why do certain ones arise rather than others?
To what extent can the occurrence of ideas be controlled? Com-
position, either oral or written, is simply the outward expression of
the ideas that do arise. The occurrence of the ideas and their
precise verbalform takes place in the mind as a part of step (i).
Obviously then, a study of steps (i) and (2) and an attempt to
answer the questions raised thereby, constitute almost entirely
a study in the psychology of association or thinking processes.
Composing fundamentally is thinking.
Let us take a typical example of oral or written composition,
such as a bit of speaking or writing. How do the ideas in this
simple composition arise in the mind? The blunt answer is, they
arise almost entirely in a mechanical manner according to the
established neural or mental connections. The first idea in a chain
arises as the result of a stimulus, either through the senses or
Thinking and language are the two sides of the same shield.
The language used to express ideas depends upon the thinking
that goes on in the mind; and the thinking depends upon the
verbal-ideational connections established in the neural and mental
network by reading and hearing successions of words, phrases and
sentences. Language is not words; it is thinking, thinking by
means of symbols.
55, 65, 75, 85 and 95. A composition is rated by any one of the
scales by comparing its merit with those in the scale, and giving
to it the value of the step on the scale to which it is judged equal.
J
Figure 74, being 4.2 points for Classes I and III, and 5.2 points
for Class II. These graphs indicate a noticeable improvement
100
1 2 45678
'
Weeks of Practice
10 11 ia
80
80
60
I
I
50
H 40
o
5
"2
& 90
20
10
FIG. 74. Median scores in composition for three high-school classes. Con-
tinuous line represents a first-semester freshman class; the dotted line repre-
sents a second-semester freshman class; and the dash-line represents a first-
semester sophomore class. After Brown and Haggerty ( '17, p. 524).
from the first week to the twelfth, but show no difference in ability
between the sophomore class and the freshman classes.
study of other languages aids either very little or nof at all in the
writing of English.
(3) Acquisition of Grammatically Correct English, (a) The
influence of knowledge of grammar. Grammar was introduced into
1
Such a list has been prepared by the writer on the basis of the vo*abulary studies
described in Chapter XVIII. The Starch List of 5,003 different words used by 40
authors was checked up with the Eldricige, Ay res, Jones, and Cook Joists to find all
additional words in them which were not in the Starch List. This made a total of 0079
differentwords found in 40,000 running words (Starch List) of 40 different high-grade
current magazine writers, in some 43,000 running words (Eldridge List) of newspaper
writing, in some 23,000 running words (Ayres List) of correspondence, in 15,000,000
running words (Jones List) of children's compositions, and in 200,000 numinjL words
(Cook List) of family correspondence.
LANGUAGE 357
sinewy and well proportioned; his chest broad, his figure stately, blending
dignity of presence with ease of manner. His robust constitution had
been tried and invigorated by his early life in the wilderness, his habit of
occupation out of doors, and his rigid temperance, so that few equalled
him in strength of arm or power of endurance. His complexion was
florid, his hair dark brown, his head in shape perfectly round. His broad
nostrils seemed formed to give expression and escape to scornful anger.
His dark blue eyes, which were deeply set, had an expression of resigna-
tion and an earnestness that was almost sad."
Let the teacher select all the important descriptive words and
phrases from it and put them in a list, such as this:
The pupils should first study these words and phrases to make
sure that they thoroughly understand them; then they should
write a description of some person of their acquaintance who may
LANGUAGE 359
by the teacher are too vague and indefinite. The pupil is told
that he lacks organization, that he must develop a better vocabu-
lary, or that he lacks imagination. But how is any child specif-
ically and concretely to know how to improve in these respects?
How may a child know
that such words and phrases as "stature,"
"stately dignity," "florid complexion" might be the most appro-
priate to use when describing certain persons? When the child
learns addition or spelling he knows more precisely what he has to
learn. Instruction in composition can possibly not be particular-
ized as fully, at least not as easily, but that should not prevent as
much particularization as possible.
The chief objection by teachers of English to such a procedure
is that they believe it would kill originality and make mere thought-
less, verbal machines out of their pupils. In answer to this point,
however, we must remember that the most original people in the
world are also the ones who use to the fullest extent the work,
methods, and ideas of others. The most original persons are also
the most imitative persons. Ingersoll said of Shakespeare:
"Of course Shakespeare made use of the work of others, and we might
almost say, of all others. Every writer must use the work of others. The
only question is, how the accomplishments of other minds are used,
whether as a foundation to build higher, or whether stolen to the end
that the thief may make a reputation for himself, without adding to the
great structure of literature.
"Thousands of people have stolen stones from the Coliseum to make
360 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
"I read it over and over and was much delighted with it. I thought
the writing was excellent, and wished, if possible, to imitate it. With
that view J took some of the papers and making short hints of the senti-
ments in each sentence, laid them by a. few days, and then, without
looking at the work, tried to complete the papers again, expressing each
hinted sentiment at length, and as fully as it had been expressed before,
in any suitable words that should occur to me. Then I compared my
'Spectator* with the original, discovered some of my faults, and cor-
rected them." To acquire a stock of words and a readiness in recollection
and use of them, he "took some of the tales in the 'Spectator' and turned
them into verse; and after a time when I had pretty well forgotten the
prose, turned them back again." (Quoted by Bolton, F. E., Principles of
Education, p. 421.)
"
Consciously or unconsciously every artist is a debtor to the past.
The most original of innovators has made his originality partly out of
himself, partly out of what he has appropriated and absorbed from those
who practiced his art before him. Only a few of his separate contrivances
are his own, and the most he can claim is a patent on the combination.
"... The young artist is a weakling if he openly robs any single one
of his predecessors; he is a dolt if he does not borrow from as many of
them as may have the separate qualities he is striving to combine.
"The arts arc one in reality; and what is true of painting and sculpture
and architecture is true also of literature, of prose and verse. For exam-
ple, there are few men of letters of our time whose prose has been more
praised for its freshness and its individuality than the late Robert Louis
Stevenson; but his was an originality compounded of many samples.
He confessed frankly that he had sat at the feet of the masters, playing
the 'sedulous ape' to a dozen or more, and at last slowly learning how to
be himself. Again, the verse of Dante Gabriel Rossetti has a note of its
own, a note which many younger poets have delighted to echo and re-
echo; but Rossetti told a friend that the exciting cause of his 'Blessed
Damozcl' was the 'Raven' of Edgar Allan Poc, and Poe's own indebted-
ness to Coleridge is obvious, even if it had not been expressly avowed."
days.
A fourth investigation was made by Starch (unpublished) in
which oral errors of pupils in grades one to eight were collected by
the teachers, and oral errors of university students by a group of
TABLE ioS
Errors in language
3 64 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
These studies show that most of the errors in language are con-
fined to a small number of types. Thus in Charter's tabulation,
71% of all oral errors fall under five types or grammatical .rules,
TABLE 109
CLASSIFICATION OF ERRORS
Verbs
X 4
60.0
5 678
GRADES
TOTAL
1. 44. 2 55 .4 54 o 43 "5 48 2 40 9
2. Pronouns 15.9 14.0 6.7 7.7 12.3 18.8 13.5
3. Negatives 11.5 7.1 202 7.3 15.2 14.0 n.6
4. Syntactical redundance. ... 8.0 6.6 11.2 12.6 16.5 9.6 9.7
5. Mispronunciations 14 7 7.8 2 2 4.9 1.7 8.0
6. Prepositions 3.4 3.2 1.8 5.6 4.1 2.6 3.5
7. Adjectives and adverbs. ... 2.0 0.6 2.2 6.6 8.2 4.8 3.3
8. Ambiguous expressions 0.2 ,2
per thousand words for the latter. The college freshmen are there-
fore distinctly superior. The following table shows the relative
improvement for the various types of errors. Naturally the raw
results of such an experiment somewhat exaggerated the improve-
ment due to four years of training because of the elimination of
the poorer high school students before reaching college. In the
last column of the table this factor has been compensated for.
LANGUAGE 367
TABLE no
"A course for the second semester of the ninth grade, which was to be
taught in two ways; one class would have only written exercises; the
other, a combination of two-thirds oral and one-third written. All
classes taking either course were to be given the same written tests at
LANGUAGE
the beginning, at the middle, and at the end of the semester. All the
papers written by each class, including these tests, were to be forwarded
to the committee in charge of the experiment, accompanied
by a report
from the teacher, stating as accurately as possible how much time he
spent in preparation, in conference, and in correcting papers, and also
his opinion as to the results of the experiment.
"The outcome was decidedly favorable to the use of oral composition.
The sections taking the combined course were better at the end of the
semester in thought-vigor, freedom and interest than the others they were
;
"I know of nothing in the world that illustrates more beautifully the
law of diminishing returns than required courses in composition. A
class of students will never under any circumstances write five times as
well by writing five themes as they will by writing one; but the reading
and correcting of five themes require five times the effort on the part of
the body of teachers." (P. 123.)
rooted in your life. Each lapse is like the fall of a ball of string
which one is carefully winding up; a single slip undoes more than
a great many turns will wind again."
Pupils should be required to be as careful in all their oral and
written work as they are in their classes in English. The organiza-
tion of schools should be so modified that the necessary cooperation
between teachers of English and teachers of other subjects could be
made possible. Why should not all written work in all other sub-
TABLE in
HIGH NUMBER OF MEDIAN SCORE IN COMPOSITION
SCHOOL PUPILS (HILLEGAS SCALE)
1 6 40.5
2 77 47-4
3 l69 47-7
4 IS 52-8
5 41 52.9
6 113 58-5
7 ... 62.3
8 55 63.0
9 261 63 I
.
10 22 77.2
From this table it appears that the pupils in the best school
wrote compositions of approximately twice as good quality as the
pupils in the worst school. Furthermore there is, so far as these
limited data go, no indication of connection between size of school
and quality of composition. The probability is that the most potent
factor in the situation was the teaching ability of the instructor.
CHAPTER XX
ARITHMETIC
celery for yc., and give the clerk a dollar bill, how much change
should you receive? What mental processes are either involved
or presupposed in arriving at the answer, 28c., or rather iri the
clerk'smaking the change up from 72 until he reaches one dollar?
An analysis will show at least the following steps: (i) The concepts
of numbers and their meaning, (2) the ability (a) to hear, interpret
and speak (if only by inner speech, which accompanies even the
thinking of the numbers) the sounds for the numbers when the
calculation is carried out mentally, or (b) to write and read the
symbols for the numbers when the calculation is done with paper
and pencil; (3) the previously established mechanical associations
among numbers known as addition, subtraction, multiplication
and division; (4) The fact that the various articles are put to-
gether probably suggests the putting together of the numbers
representijig their value. That is, it acts as a stimulus to arouse
the association process of addition rather than subtraction or
multiplication or division; (5) the occurrence of the successive
374
ARITHMETIC 375
"In all probability the errors of the great mind exceed in number those
of the less vigorous one. Fertility of imagination and abundance of
guesses at truth are among the first requisites of discovery."
"The world little knows how many of the thoughts and theories which
have passed through the mind of a scientific investigator have been
crushed in silence and secrecy by his own severe criticism and adverse
examination; that in the most successful instances not a tenth of the
suggestions, the hopes, the wishes, the preliminary conclusions have
been realized." (Quoted from Lindley.)
TABLE H2
TOTAL ERRORS TOTAL ERRORS
12-4 14 12-7 17
12-8 o 12-5 o
I3-S 8 15-8 17
13-8 15 15-7 o
TT-6 TO II-9 15
11-5 O II-2 O
II-7 22 17-9 12
11-4 5 17-8 i
21
-5=514
3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Hi 14
Tvn n
15 16 17 18 19 20
Estimates
FIG. 75. The number of persons whose estimates of the number of steps
fallon various numbers showing a tendency to avoid odd and prime numbers.
1 2 8 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 .15 16 17]
Totals in Addition.
FIG. 76. Showing the relative numbers of addition combinations amounting
to the various totals. Adapted from Table 128.
ARITHMETIC 381
increasing difficulty.
Judd and Counts ('16) prepared for the survey of the Cleveland
schools, fifteen sets of arithmetic tests, four for addition, two for
subtraction, three for multiplication, four for division, and two
for operations with fractions. The various sets for any one type
of operation represented successively more and more complex
stages of the operation.
Standards of attainment have been prepared by the authors of
the various tests so that measurements of the abilities of pupils
and schools can be compared directly with the respective norms for
the various grades. These tests have been very useful in getting at
some real facts concerning progress and attainment in arithmetic.
ARITHMETIC 383
Grade
FIG. 77. Difference between boys and girls in solving arithmetical problems
as measured by the author s scale ('15).
1
i ii in mi inn nun mini mum nniim iiimiiiT*
2 O 00 000 0000 00000 000000 0000000 00000000 000000000 0000000000
[Lay]
(prepared by Born, Busse, and Behme) and (b) those in which the
dots are arranged vertically (prepared by Hentschd, Beetz, So-
belewsky, and Kaselitz). See Figure 78.
Extensive experiments have been made upon school children by
Lay to determine the relative merits of the various devices and
arrangements of the materials used for teaching apprehension of
the number of objects. Lay investigated such problems as whether
it is better for teaching numbers to present groups of stimuli or
a? -Grade
?/-% Mistakes
1JAT
IB 2aT 2B 3AT 3B 4B 6AT
FIG. 79. Curve of error for the number pictures 5 to 12 as a whole. After
Howell ('14, p. 213^.
Comparison of the Born, Beetz, and Lay pictures with one another and with
the Russian Machine in the apprehension of the numbers 5 to 10.
P = chances for mistakes. M = number of mistakes.
3 86 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
ranged in size from three to twelve. Each card was exposed for
5 seconds. The investigation was carried out with pupils in an
elementary school. Howell's results are shown in the curve of
Figure 79. This curve shows a rapid drop in errors from the first
grade to the second, and that pupils reach a high degree of cer-
tainty between the third and fourth grade in apprehending the
numbers. Howell also found that certain numbers are not appro-
80 40 60 60 80 90 100
Per cent,
FIG. 81. Percentage of superintendents in 830 cities who favor elimination
of the various topics represented by checked surface and those who favor "less
attention" arc represented by shaded surface. After Jessup and Coffman ('15).
3 88 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
108080406060708090100
Per cent
FIG. 82. Percentage of superintendents in 830 cities who favor giving more
attention to each of these topics. After Jessup and Coffman ( '15).
paper, the long method of greatest common divisor, longitude and time,
least common multiple, metric system, progression, quarter in avoirdu-
pois table, reduction of more than two steps, troy weight, true discount,
unreal fractions."
Total 1085
390 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
Receipts i
Square Measure 27
Fractions. .
2-5 534 Taxes 6
I
Time Measure 13
6-1 o Buying 3 1 .>8
Selling. .646
10 plus
i plus
86
i plus
5 Plus 60
Total.
"The problems solved in actual usage are brief and simple. They
chiefly require the more fundamental and more easily mastered proc-
esses.
"
In actual usage, few problems of an abstract nature are encountered.
The problems are concrete and relate to business situations. They
require simple reasoning and a decision as to the processes to be em-
ployed.
"The study justifies careful consideration of the following question:
After the development of reasonable speed and accuracy in the funda-
mentals and the mastery of the simple and more useful arithmetical
processes, should the arithmetic work not be centered largely around
those problems which furnish the basis for much business information?"
(Wilson.)
ARITHMETIC 391
"In the first place, out of a total of 1,023 types of practical problems
found in four text-books, 720, or 71%, occur in occupational activities.
"A study of the frequency with which type problems occur reveals
a significant fact; viz., the frequency ranges from one to 434.
"This wide variation in frequency shows that the authors of our text-
books are far from being in agreement on the type problems of arithmetic.
Only one author out of four has recognized 511 out of 1,023 type problems
and 140 type problems have received the recognition of only two authors
out of four." (Monroe.)
"
Gives two averages for each grade as well as for each school as a
whole. Thus, the school at the top shows averages 80.0 and 83.1, and
the one at the bottom, 25.3 and 31.5. The first represents the percentage
of answers which were absolutely correct; the second shows what per
cent of the problems were correct in principle, i. e., the average that
would have been received if no mechanical errors had been made. The
difference represents the percentage of mechanical errors, which, I be-
lieve, in most instances, makes a surprisingly small appearance."
39* EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
-
^QO
O M
OO 00 00
co ^N
t* t-
to to O COOO tooO
f ^\O VO*O^^t't>
M
'^COOO O
OtvO t>OO '
O
'
<O **
I
c,
g
* 6 6 o* * cood d* ** >d T- to to 6
-OO OO
t*
t>>vO t^ ^* to to to ^ ^fod
^*vQ ^4" *O co co
t^.
CM \o
*
r
fl
:s :
:
--, R
. .
^K M W ^, .
i
.
^
s
^ f.-,
> to vo M vO 00
N food od M vd o^ ^' M
to to c -*f o to '
! a
ARITHMETIC 393
ssssssssssssssssss
Q
So >O
<. M tO
^.vo co t
M co w M
co co * *> INO vo >o 10 o i
B
O O O to *
I
s \O M M M VOOO M \O O
-
2
a W ONO O O O" O O* O
PO *cO-<cic<o>-irociHi
^^^^^^i ^^^^^^^^^^^ 1
OOODOOOOOOOOOOOOOO
394 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
the conditions reversed; for while the two schools that gave forty-five
minutes made averages of 64% and 67%, respectively, the school that
gave only twenty-five minutes succeeded in obtaining an average of 69%.
This would appear to indicate that while, on the one hand, nothing is
gained by an increase of time where the instruction in arithmetic is
faulty, on the other hand, nothing is lost by a decrease of time, to a
certain point, where the schools are on the right path in teaching the
subject. Perhaps the most interesting feature of the table is the fact that
the school giving twenty-five minutes a day came out within two of the
top, while the school giving seventy-five minutes daily came out prac-
tically within one of the bottom."
TABLE 116
FIG. 83. Median scores of the $th grades and of the 8th grades in 90 schools
in simple addition. After Judd ( 'i 6, p. 1 1 2) .
that they can be seen in the openings of the card and compared
with the correct answers printed on that side of the card.
This plan of drill work has a number of advantages, such as an
incentive to rapid and accurate work, immediate self-checking ot
the answers* and so on.
398 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
periods. In the second experiment 222 children were used and the
drill was given for twenty periods. The results of the two experi-
ments are given in parallel columns in Table 117. In each case
section I received the drill and section II received the regular class
work. The pupils did not know that any experiment was in
progress.
ARITHMETIC 399
TABLE 117
found that the drilled group was also superior in retention, having
lost.2% while the undrillcd group had lost 2.29%.
The same experiment was repeated by F. M. Phillips. He had
69 children for subjects and gave to one group drill in fundamentals
and in reasoning, both oral and written, for eight weeks. Neither
teachers nor students knew the purpose of the tests. He found
that, "The improvement in fundamentals of the combined drill,
groups was 15% greater than that of the non-drill groups. Inj
reasoning, the drill groups improved 50% more than the non-drill
groups. . . . The greatest gains were made in the sixth grade and
the least in the eighth." Almost all the gain on fundamentals
was in multiplication.
TABLE 118
TABLE 119
Wimmer's results
longer periods.
Superintendent Wimmer, in connection with his drill experiments
previously reported, also investigated the problem of economy of
long and short periods of practice. The drill, three-fifths on rea-
soning and two-fifths on fundamentals, was given to one group
for five minutes at the beginning of each class period. The other
group received one 15-minute period per week. Drill lasted for
six weeks. The results are shown in the following table. They
are distinctly in favor of the longer drill period despite the fact that
only three-fifths as much time was spent by this method.
ARITHMETIC 405
TABLE 123
After Wimmer
ever: Is drill with the Courtis practice pad more or less efficient
than drill as ordinarily given?
days. When tested at the end of the experiment, those, using the
key put down 25.4 answers in two minutes, while the computers
wrote down 44.3 examples, showing a decided advantage for the
computation method. He concludes that tables should be learned
by use rather than by memorizing.
Conrad and Alps ('16) investigated the effect of suppressing artic-
ulatory movements upon the effect of drill in rapid adding. They
divided sixty-four high school students into equal groups of equal
ability. The students were then given eight periods of drill in
rapid addition of columns. The pupils in one group were per-
mitted to add in their ordinary way which involved a great deal
of articulation or inner speech. The other group was cautioned
repeatedly and emphatically to "think results only." The former
was called the traditional method and the latter the economical
method. The percentages of gain by the two methods were as
follows:
The traditional method gained in attempts 8.5% and in rights
2 -5% The economical method gained in attempts 34.4% and in
rights 30.9%. This gave an advantage in favor of the economical
method of 25% in attempts and of 33.4% in rights. These results
came out almost startlingly in favor of "thinking results only."
The evil effects of articulation and lip movements have been no-
ticed in connection with reading (page 287). It is probable that
the cause is the same in both cases.
P. B. Ballard investigated the comparative efficiency of the
"equal addition" method and the "decomposition" method in
subtraction.
oo
00
40
10
"They frequently miss count and get a quotient figure one removed
(say) from the right one.
"8. Making one of the quotient figures the quotient.
"9. Substituting multiplication for division.
"10. Unclassified."
Showing the number of mistakes in the division tables falling into each class
GRADE KINDS OF MISTAKES TOTAL MISTAKES
separately for each of the three processes and for each of the grades
from 4 to 8. It is noticeable that the greatest difficulty was pre-
sented in subtraction by borrowing, in multiplication by the
tables and in division by the remainder. For the most part the
TABLE 127
The per cent of each type of error in the examples of subtraction, multiplica-
tion, and division respectively from the 812 papers from six schools in
Seattle. (After Gist ('17).)
Subtraction:
Borrowing 54 56 52 51 55
Combinations 36 38 45 44 41
Omissions 2 i 2 3 i
Reversions i 2 tf o o
7 o, o, etc 5 3 }A o o
Left-hand digit , o o o o 2
ARITHMETIC 413
Multiplication:
Tables 79 73 73 77 75
Addition 18 20 22 19 20
Cipher in multiplier 1.5 6 5 4 5
Division:
Remainder too large 34 39 27 19 10
Multiplication 22 15 19 37 33
Subtraction TT 14 18 25 23
Last remainder o, and o in dividend ... 7 15 19 7 n
Multiplicand larger than dividend 7 4 i r r
Cipher in quotient, as
of quotient in quo
90898
. .
37847
7 i i 3 3
TABLE 128
Table showing the relative difficulty of the various addition combinations for
the third and eighth grades respectively
HOLLOWAY PHELPS
19 .62
19 -45
18 .67
17 .54
17 2.22
17 .27
16 .50
15 1. 1 6
15 -72
15 i 30
13 -86
13 i 09
13 .88
9 .07
9 -37
8 .12
8 1.46
.62
52
39
.28
25
24
.24
-IS
.14
.05
ARITHMETIC 41$
TABLE 129
12 X 6. .. 361 6 X 3 .. 102 4 X i. 31 . .
9 X 9. .-
263 10 X6 79 9X1 21
12 X 4 ...250 4 X 4-..- 78 6 X i... 21
10 X 10.. ..241 4X3-- --76 12 XL. ..20
8 X 4- ---235 7 X3-." 7i 5 X i... 20
7X 4.... 192 10X5 5S 2XL... 20
12 X 3----i*3 8 X 2 "" S8 2X2.. .18
8 XL 18
10 X i 12
CHAPTER XXI
HISTORY
(3) Processes (i) and (2) applied to events preceding the one in
question.
(4) Processes (i) and (2) applied to events succeeding the one
in question.
The following are the average scores for the ends of the different
grades obtained from approximately 2,000 pupils:
Grade 6 7 8 H. S.
Scores 7 20 38 38
BOYS GIRLS
High School 47 4i 73 3$
8th grade 288 45 352 31
7th grade 94 24 101 J7
420 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
Bell and McCollum applied a history test to 1,500 pupils and found
similar differences. The boys
in the elementary schools did 28%
better and in the high schools better than the girls. This
31%
superiority on the part of the boys may be due to their greater
interest in battles which in turn may be due to the greater strength
of the fighting instinct.
TABLE 130
City A 66 35
"
B 30
"
C 3; 4S
"
I), School i 22
* 4
2 59
"
"3 42
K 17 33
"
*' - 40 -
65
-
40
48
33
39
-
41
29
33
19
~ -,,
3 52
"
4 ... 17 50
"5 -'9
chase; 1807, Fulton's steamboat; 1812, war with England; 1820, Missouri
Compromise; 1823, Monroe Doctrine; 1826, first railroad; 1844, first
telegraph; 1846, sewing-machine invented; 1845, first reaper; 1846-
1848, secession and Civil War; 1863, Emancipation
Mexican War; 1861,
Proclamation; Gettysburg, Vicksburg; 1866, Atlantic cable; 1876, first
telephone, 1878, electric light invented; 1898, war with Spain; 1903,
firstwireless across Atlantic; 1914, world war in Europe.
"Other omissions. Detailed provisions of various tariff acts (but the
meaning of tariff should be understood); details of political campaigns
except Jefferson's, Jackson's, Lincoln's and any current campaign in
progress; critical study of political party principles (but give broad
distinctions between chief rival parties) ; financial panics except those of
7, 1873, 1893." (Pp. 271 and 272.)
by the space given to different topics, and by listing the topics and
names included in all, or in a certain fraction of these texts.
1888, and the tendency has been general and decided since that time.
"
(3) The later books give a perceptibly heavier emphasis to the facts
of economic and industrial development than do the earlier books, al-
though political development still constitutes the essential core of ele-
mentary historical instruction.
"
(4) As regards the treatment of specific eras or epochs, the principal
increases in emphasis are to be noted in connection with: (a) the period
HISTORY 425
426
MARKS AS MEASURES OF SCHOOL WORK 427
Philosophy 55 33 10 2 623
Latin 1 52 42 6 o 130
Sociology 52 30 13 5 958
Mathematics 1 40 31 16 13 208
Economics 39 37 19 5 461
Greek 39 26 24 1 1 287
Latin II 36 40 19 5 577
French 36 29 25 10 295
Political Science .... 34 30 27 9 592
Mathematics IT ... .32 29 23 i^ 145
German 1 30 29 20 1 1
580
Psychology I 30 36 24 10 907
German II 20 38 25 u 94*
Elocution 20 Or 19 o 917
Geology 22 48 22 S 293
History 1 14 53 27 6 779
Zoology 1 21 45 28 6 479
Psychology II 19 47 29 5 238
History of Art 25 40 30 5 685
Bacteriology 20 45 31 4 263
Freehand Drawing . . . 18 47 25 10 506
Chemistry 1 23 40 31 6 205
English 1 21 41 30 8 964
Astronomy 13 40 33 5 22 5
History II n 51 33 5 8o6
Zoology II 24 37 31 8 250
German III 22 37 28 13 44i
Chemistry II 9 48 43 o 21
Education 18 38 35 9 266
Mathematics III 19 36 26 19 182
Mathematics IV 25 29 36 10 380
Physiology 20 33 40 7 426
Anatomy 19 34 3<> Ir 544
Mathematics V 16 34 35 *5 209
Engineering 1 13 36 42 9 813
Mechanical Drawing. , 18 29 41 12 538
Mechanics 18 26 42 14 495
Engineering II 16 26 46 12 826
Chemistry HI i u 60 28 1903
English II 9 ** 35 28 1,098
428 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
TABLE 134
German:
35- 26.3 21. 12.3 5-2
36.. 12. 49.1 24. II. I 3-7
37 .. 34-3 2.9 2.9 67
3... 11.4 22.9 3- 2 61
39- -
17-3 37-3 29 6.6 9 3 75
40. .
17.9 2b 17.1 4.0 123
14-7 29-5 33 18 9 3- 1 95
42. 12 3 27.4 *1 2 r
.9
43- 29 o 30.1 14 4 3 93
44- 21 6 42.0 12 5 2.27 88
45- 22.4 1-7
JL
T.
10
70 75 80 86 90 96 100
FIG. 85. Distribution of all grades in a high school. After Gray ( '13, p. 66).
Ln
T l
u
nu
70 75 80 85 90 86
i
100
FIG. 86. Distribution of all grades in another high school. After Gray (*i3,p. 67).
schools. The two of these schools are shown in
distributions of
Figures 85 and 86. The one grades high and the other grades low.
In one school the great mass of the pupils receive 85 to 100; in the
other they receive 85 to 70.
Table 135 shows the distribution of the grades by the different
teachers in a high school of about 150 pupils.
MARKS AS MEASURES OF SCHOOL WORK 433
TABLE 135
Distribution of grades for the year after the above tabulation was made known
to the teachers. Extreme variations are considerably reduced.
pieces of work.
Starch and Elliott ('12 and '13) made a series of investigations
in which two examination papers in first-year-high-school
final
65 70 75 80 85 90 85
75 80 85 90 95
50 60 65 70
FIG. 88. Distribution of the works assigned by 142 english teachers to an-
other final examination paper in high-school freshman English. After Starch
and Elliott ('12).
28 53 55 60 65 S 55 5 % 3)
i
60 60 70 80 90
Av. 79.4 81.4 79.8 73.7 65.5 78.7 83.8 85.1 79.7 80.2 78.7 5.3 .074
The mean variation of these" marks, comparing the first with the
second for each paper, is 2.2 points. A part of this variation,
however, is due to the slight shift in standard on the part of each
teacher from one grading to the next. By applying the same
process of weighting explained in connection with factor two, the
mean variation drops from 2.2 to 1.75 points. Hence 1.75 repre-
sents the amount of variation contributed by factor four to the
total mean variation and by subtraction we find that factor three
contributes 2.55 points. The four factors therefore contribute
the following amounts:
"
Total mean variation 5 40.
It is obvious then that factors three and four are the most im-
TABLE 139
Distribution of grades at Cornell University for the years 1902, 1903 and 1911.
Adapted from Finkelstein ('13, p. 22), to give the distributions for a five-
point scale, 60 being the passing grade.
NUMBER OF GRADES
0-59 60-69 70-79 80-89 00-100
9.2 22.5 30.0 27.2 II. I 20,348
TABLE 140
Distribution of all grades for two academic years at Harvard College. After
Foster ('n, p. 262)
442 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
TABLE 141
24,979
TABLE 142
the theory rests on the supposition that the pupils are unselected,
chance specimens of mankind as a whole. This supposition, of
course, never obtains absolutely for any group of human beings
brought together anywhere. The very reason that brings any
group together at the same time selects them. Pupils in school are
abrupt cutting off, but a gradual slicing off along a large share of
the distribution surface.
E D C B A
FIG. 93. Distribution of 8,969 grades in elementary courses at Harvard Uni-
'
versity. After Foster ( 1 1) .
E D c B A
FIG. 94. Distribution of 24,979 grades at the University of Missouri. After
Meyer ('08).
sophomore year , , 45
MAKKS AS MEASURES OK SCHOOL WORK 44 j
This table reads that all students whose average grade was
"conditioned" or "failed" dropped out during the freshman year;
52% of those whose grade was "poor" dropped out during the
freshman year and 45% of those remaining whose grade was
"poor" dropped out during the sophomore year, etc. It is obvious,
therefore, that there is elimination from all classes of scholarship
with the exception of the highest from which there is very little or
no loss. The general effect of the actual elimination upon the
distribution curve is to shift the left end of the curve toward the
FIG. 95. The continuous line shows the theoretical distribution of the marks
of students. The upper broken line represents the change in this curve due to
the dropping out of students during the freshman year. The lower broken line
represents the change in the curve due to the elimination during the sophomore
year. After Starch ('13).
grade them according to a five point scale and give the grade of E
to two papers, D to from four to six papers, C to from eight to ten
papers, B to from four to six papers, and A 'to two papers. Even
if the teachers felt, for example, that there were no papers good
enough to receive the grade of A, they were to select the two best
ones and call them A. The outcome was that those teachers who
in their original grades differed most from the combined judgment
of all the teachers were forced to comply more closely to the actual
average marks as given in the first grading. One teacher marked
the highest paper 85 in the original grading, and objected to giving
it a grade of A in the forced distribution on the ground that no
paper in the lot was good enough to receive so high a grade, and
yet the average of the marks given by all the teachers to this
paper was 92.9, the best paper in the entire group.
The theory of the probability distribution of marks should be
observed with sense and reason and not in a purely mechanical
manner. A blind, unintelligent observance of the principle is
bound to lead to injustice, particularly with small classes. In one
such case which came to the author's attention it led to the giving
of a mediocre grade to a pupil of very high ability.
A fourth point frequently raised by teachers to justify unusually
high or low marks is that the particular class in question is an
unusually good one or poor one. Such a claim ought to be allowed
only if it can be justified by good evidence. There are, of course,
differences in classes, but these are almost never as great as we are
inclined to believe. Large differences between successive classes
in the same subject are for the most part illusory for the reason
that the judgment of an individual teacher is more likely to deviate
from a correct estimate than the average ability of a group devi-
ates from the average of other groups. The teacher who says to
each succeeding class that this is the best class he has ever had in
this subject would possess, if this judgment were correct, a magic
ee 75 81 87 93 100
FIG. 96. The continuous line shows the distribution of the marks of a teacher
of Latin and German in a high school. The broken line shows the distribution
of themarks of the same pupils in their other subjects. After an unpublished
report of Supt. J. F. Waddell, Evansville, Wisconsin.
Galton, F. : 1869 and 1892. Hereditary Genius: An Inquiry into its Laws
and Consequences.
Galton, F.: 1883. Human Faculty and its Development.
Inquiries into
Gesell, A. L.: 1906. Accuracy in Handwriting, as Related to School
Intelligence and Sex. American Journal of Psychology, 17 1394-405.
Gilbert, J. A.: 1894. Researches on the Mental and Physical Develop-
ment of School Children. Studies from the Yale Psychological
Laboratory, 11:40-100.
and Fracker, G. C.: 1897. The Effects of Practice in Re-
Gilbert, J. A.,
action and Discrimination for Sound upon the Time of Reaction and
Discrimination for other Forms of Stimuli. University of Iowa
Studies in Psychology, 1 162-76.
Gist, A. S.: 1917. Errors in the Fundamentals of Arithmetic. School
and Society, 7:175-177.
Goddard, H. H.: 1912. The Kallikak Family.
Goddard, H. H.: 1914. Feeble-mindedness: Its Causes and Conse-
quences.
Graves, S. M.: 1917. A Study in Handwriting. J. of Educ. Psych.,
7:483-494.
Gray, C. T.: 1913. Variations in the Grades of High School Pupils.
Gray, C. T.: 1915. A
Score Card for the Measurement of Handwriting.
Bulletin of the University of Texas, No. 37.
Gray, W. S.: 1917. Studies of Elementary School Reading Through
Standardized Tests. Suppl. Educ. Monog., i, No. i.
Gulick, L. H., and Ayres, L. P.: 1908. (Second Edition, 1913.) Medical
Inspection of Schools.
Guyer, M. F.: 1916. Being Well-Born.
456 BIBLIOGRAPHY
Experimental Investigation.
Hillegas, M. B. 191 2. A Scale for the Measurement of Quality in English
:
Jacoby, P.: 1881. (Second Edition, 1904.) fitudes sur la selection dous
ses rapports avec I'heYeclitS chez Thomme.
James, W.: 1890. Principles of Psychology.
Javal, E.: 1878 and 1879. Sur la Physiologic de la Lecture. Annoles d'
Oculistique.
Javal, E.: 1879. Conditions de la lecture facile. Comptes rendus de la
Societie de Biologic, page 8.
Jessup,W. A.: 1915. Current Practices and Standards in Arithmetic.
Fourteenth Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of
Education, 14:116-130.
Johnson, R. I. 1917. The Persistency of Errors in English Composition.
:
etc., 24:401-444-
Ruediger, 1907. The Field of Distinct Vision. Archives of Psychology,
No. 5.
Sackett, L. W. Measuring a
: School System by the Buckingham Spelling
School and Society, 2:860-864, 894-898.
Scale.
Schmidt, W. A.: 1917. An Experimental Study in the Psychology of
Reading. Suppl. Educational Monographs (Chicago), i, No. 2.
Schuster and Elderton: 1907. Inheritance of Ability. Publications of
Eugenics Laboratory, London.
Schuyler, W., and Swift, E. J.: 1907. The Learning Process. Psy-
chological Bulletin, 4:307-310.
Scripture, E. W., Smith, T. L., and Brown, E. M.: 1894. On the Educa-
tion of Muscular Control and Power. Studies from the Yale Psy-
chological Laboratory, 2:114-119.
Sears, J. B.: 1915. Spelling Efficiency in the Oakland Schools. School
and Society, 2:531-537, 569-574-
BIBLIOGRAPHY 461
Review, 18:223-228.
Starch, D.: 1911. The Transfer of Training in Arithmetical Operations.
1
Psychology, 3:209-213.
Starch, D. 1913. The Measurement of Handwriting.
:
Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology, 4:445-464.
Starch, D.: 1913. Reliability and Distribution of Grades. Science
38:630-636.
Starch, D.: 1913. Correlation Among Abilities in School Studies. Jour-
nal of Educational Psychology, 3:415-418.
Starch, D.: 1915. The Measurement of Efficiency in Reading. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 6:1-24.
Starch, D.: 1915. The Measurement of Efficiency in Handwriting.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 6:106-114.
Starch, D.: 1915. The Measurement of Efficiency in Spelling. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 6:127-186.
462 BIBLIOGRAPHY
Uhl, W. T.: 1916. Use of the Results of Reading Tests as Basis for
Planning Remedial Work. Elementary School Journal, 17:266-
275-
University Studies.
Wilcox, M. J.: 1917. Does the Study of High School Latin improve
High School English? School and Society, 6:58-60.
Wilson, G. M.: 1917. A Survey of the Social and Business Use of Arith-
metic. Sixteenth Yearbook of the National Society for the Study
of Education, 1917:128-142.
Wimmer, H.: 1916. An Experimental Study of the Effects of Drill in
Arithmetical Processes under Varying Conditions. Indiana Univer-
sity Studies.
Winch, W. H.: 1904. Immediate Memory in School Children. British
Journal of Psychology, 1:127-134.
Winch, W. H.: 1906. Immediate Memory in School Children: Auditory*
British Journal of Psychology, 2:52-57.
Winch, W. II. :
1908. The Transfer of Improvement in Memory in School
Children: I. British Journal of Psychology, 2:284-293.
Winch, W. H.: 1910. The Transfer of Improvement in Memory in
School Children: H. British Journal of Psychology, 3:386-405.
Hillegas scale, 35, 39, 40, 41, 245. velopment of, i9f.; unrevivability
History: psychological steps involved of, 2 if.
in, 416!.; measurement of attain- Instincts, based upon dy-
theories
ment in, 4i8f.; economic methods lamic theory, 14, isf.; transited-
in learning, 42of.; suggestions for less, 14, 17; recapitulation, 14,
method of study, 42 2f.; essential Z2f.
14.
Hollingworth, H. L., 52. Jacoby, P., 92.
Holloway, H. V., 413, 414. James, W., 21, 23, 138, 139, 193, 2x1.
Horace Mann system of reading, 290, 212, 213, 214, 371.
291, 292, 293. Jastrow, J., 379, 381.
Knight, 346. .
Marks: importance of, 426; variations
Knilling, 384. in distribution of, 426f.; variations
Kraepelin, E., 291,. in evaluation of same school subject,
Kuhlmann, F., 100, 108. 433f.; causes of variations, 435!.;
Language, psychological processes in- evaluation of factors involved, 438.;
volved in, 349!. -~ size of units on marking scale, 438f .;
how distribute marks, 44of.; objec-
Landolt, 266. tions to use of distribution curve
Language scales: in assigning marks, 443 f.; methods
Starch, 352, 353. of reducing variations in grades,
Hilligas-Thorndike, 353, 373. 448f.
Harvard-Newton, 353. Matthews, Brander, 361.
Trabue, 353. Measurement of mental capacities, 97;
Laser, H., 171. value of, 97f.; methods of, 99, 109.
Lathrop, G. C., 378. Mead, C. D., 287, 288, 289, 408.
Latin: general value of, 229; effect of Memorizing ability, regular increase
study of modern language, 23 if.; during school life, igi.
and scholarship, 232; effect on Eng- Memory, see Correlation, Transfer of
lish, 233^, 24if., 246f., 356; on rhet- Training, Learning.
oric, 235; and college honors, 236f.; Mental heredity; problem of, 73;
and original capacity, 238f. methods of studying, 73; views of,
Lay, W. A., 384, 385, 36. 74; Galton's study of, 74f.; Wood's
Learning: problems in rate and prog- study of in royalty, 75; in various
ress of, 141; curve of, 141; early low grade families, 77f.; in the
progress in, 143; various curves of, Jonathan Edwards family, 77; its
142-149; analytic types of, 148; effect on degeneracy and crime, 79;
distribution of practice in, iS3f.; on feeblemindedness, 78, 79f.; gen-
plateaus in, 142, 149, i5of.; see also eral interpretation, 95f.
Phelps, C. L., 412, 413, 414. Reading, steps involved in, 261; re-
Phelps, William Lyon, 369, 370. ception of stimuli, 26 2f.; size of field
Phillips, F. M., 379, 381, 400. of distinct vision, 264; range of at-
Physical defects and school work, tention, 265^; eye movements, 266f.;
i3of. transmission of nerve impulses to
Pintner, R., 107, 130, 289. visual center, 269; arousal of asso-
Plateaus, see Learning. ciation processes, 269^; transmis-
culties in, 180; suggestions for, i83f.; Van Landegend, E., 89.
Whipple's rules for, i86f.; whole and Varying abilities in school subjects;
part method in, i8sf. See also How range, 33, 36; general causes, 33.
to Study. Visual defects, 120, 121; types of, 123;
Supernormal child, need for differen- causes of, i24f.; frequency of, I25f.;
tiated training for, see Overlap- relative frequency in colored and
ping. white children, 125; reasons for
Supervised study, 42, 43, 44, i88f. variations in reports on, 126; in-
'Surface of frequency, 28. crease in higher grades, 127;
Suzzalo, H., 345, 346, 347- methods of avoiding, I27f.
Swift, E. J., 144, 15; i5 2 * 158, 161,
230, 231. Waddell, J. F., 433-
Wagner, 347.
Taussig, A. E., 125, 126, 127, 128. Waldo, K. D., 282, 283, 294.
Telegraphy, i42f. Wallin, J. E. W., 325, 34i, 342,
Terman, L. M., 69, 100, 105, 107, 345-
108. Walsemann, H. J., 384.
Thomas, 64. Ward method in reading, 294.
Thorndike, E. L., i, 10, 16, 40, 50, 55, Warren, H. C., 384.
56, 62, 66, 67, 68, 71, 72, 85, 86, 87, Washington, 358.
89, 148, 150, 155, 157, 161, 165, 172, Waste in education due to lack of
173, 175, 195, 197, 212, 213, 222, exact data on various learning proc-
275, 300* 301, 307, 3o8, 309, 311, esses, 3f.
Wimmer, Herman, 400, 401, 402, Woolley, Helen Thompson, 66, 67.
404. Word lists, see Spelling.
Winch, W. H., 161, 207. Writing, see Handwriting.
Witmer, 122.
Woods, F. A., 75, 76. Yerkes, R! M., 100; and Bridges, 107,
Woodworth, R. S., 195, 197, 210,
212. Zero family, 77.
Woody, C., 382.
This book was distributed courtesy of:
For your own Unlimited Reading and FREE eBooks today, visit:
http://www.Free-eBooks.net
Share this eBook with anyone and everyone automatically by selecting any of the
options below:
COPYRIGHT INFORMATION
Free-eBooks.net respects the intellectual property of others. When a book's copyright owner submits their work to Free-eBooks.net, they are granting us permission to distribute such material. Unless
otherwise stated in this book, this permission is not passed onto others. As such, redistributing this book without the copyright owner's permission can constitute copyright infringement. If you
believe that your work has been used in a manner that constitutes copyright infringement, please follow our Notice and Procedure for Making Claims of Copyright Infringement as seen in our Terms
of Service here:
http://www.free-ebooks.net/tos.html