Objective Personality Test
Objective Personality Test
Objective Personality Test
This chapter aims to provide an overview of current developments and research on so-called
Objective Personality Tests (OPTs). After a short introductory overview of their history, the
specifications and definitions of OPTs are discussed. We further present a system that aims to
distinguish between three different categories of OPTs. We then outline the state of research
on the psychometric properties of OPTs, especially with regard to findings on their reliability
and validity. Finally, we make suggestions for future research and comment on the
The practice of obtaining information about personality from sources that go beyond self-
reports by including samples of test takers overt behavior has a history in psychological
measurement as long as psychological testing itself. In fact, some initial concepts of OPTs
were introduced long ago by the pioneer James McKeen Cattell as mental tests. In 1890, he
proposed a series of 10 tests based on experiment and measurement, thus aspiring to apply
the exactness of the physical sciences. His battery contained heterogeneous tasks, including
measures of personality. For example, the Dynamometer Pressure Test assessed a persons
Objective Personality Tests
maximum possible squeeze of the hand and was supposed to measure effort and volitional
Decades later, documentation on psychological testing during the Second World War
indicated the use of objective personality testing-like procedures: For example, tests of
perceptual abilities were employed in the German military in order to tap aspects of
character and personality structure (see Fitts, 1946). During that time, the Office of
American Strategic Services (OSS) also reported that emotional stability and tolerance for
frustration were assessed via observation in standardized problem solving tasks (OSS
The current concepts of OPTs can be traced back to a more recent history of Objective
Personality Tests (OPTs) that began in the second half of the last century. These can be
divided into two periods: Tests of the first generation were a result of Raymond Bernard
Cattells comprehensive theoretical approach that was based on the idea that a complete
B. Cattell, 1946; R. B. Cattell & Kline, 1977) described tests (T-data) as one of the three
data, Cattell and his collaborators developed the first generation of OPTs with the aim of
designing Cursive Miniature Situations (R. B. Cattell, 1941, 1944) that simulate the
Within his prolific and decades-long period of test development, Cattell had already proposed
no fewer than 500 tests by the mid-1960s. These were characterized by great variation with
reference to materials and scoring methods (e.g., R. B. Cattell, 1968; Schuerger, 2008). In
order to assess T-data, he included behavior samples, physical performance checks, and
psychophysiological data as well as so-called projective techniques and creative design tasks.
2
Objective Personality Tests
At the beginning of his research, the variables he included were related to Eysencks research
variables (see Hundleby, Pawlik, & Cattell, 1965; Schmidt, 2006). In his research, he also
incorporated questionnaire-like tests that were supposed to be able to hide what was being
measured. He investigated the relations between these measures as well as the common
structure that supported them (R. B. Cattell, 1948). Efforts by Cattell and his colleagues also
led to the publication of the well-known Objective-Analytic Test Batteries (R. B. Cattell &
Schuerger, 1976, 1978); for an overview, see Kline and Cooper (1984) and Schuerger (2008).
Tests labeled as the second generation of OPTs emerged in the 1990s. They benefitted
from the development of computer technologies that can provide highly flexible methods of
item presentation and precise registration of a persons behavior (Ortner et al., 2007). Tests of
the first generation were mostly designed in a bottom-up approach that aimed to provide
additional information within a holistic approach and referred to personality factors after
empirical analyses in line with Cattells theoretical framework. Tests of the second
motivation, reflexivity versus impulsivity, the ability to work under pressure, or vocational
interests (for an overview, see Ortner et al., 2007). New technologies and opportunities in the
measurement domain also allowed for broader varieties of task presentations, variables
presented stimuli (e.g., Proyer, 2007; Proyer & Husler, 2007), viewing times, reaction times
(e.g., Proyer, 2007), reaction speed (Schmidt-Atzert, 2007), or other variables related to test
performance (e.g., orientation of a figure in a maze; (Ortner, Kubinger, Schrott, Radinger, &
Litzenberger, 2006). In line with these technical advances, contemporary OPTs were
new OPTs overcame the criticism that examiners had to put forth enormous effort and face
3
Objective Personality Tests
certain challenges during the administration of at least some tests. This second generation of
In recent decades, several names and definitions for the group of tests addressed as OPTs
have been proposed. Objective-Analytic Tests (R. B. Cattell, 1955) or simply Objective Tests
(e.g., R. B. Cattell, 1946) are well known terms and were suggested in a framework that also
assigned standardized ability and achievement tests to this group. Performance Tests of
Personality (Cronbach, 1970) was also suggested as a name. More recently, Kubinger (2009)
introduced the term Experiment-Based Assessment for measures that infer characteristics of
However, the term experiment is not meant to imply that OPTs are experiments in a narrow
sense since the measurement procedure has to fulfill common psychometric standards.
clarify the meaning of objective in OPTs: In the domain of psychological assessment, this
term is used to indicate whether the result obtained by a test has been influenced by external
conditions such as the setting, testing environment, testing materials, and examiners (see
Pawlik, 2006). Cattell (1968) introduced a second, new higher degree of objectivity to
situationa test. The person reacts to a standard stimulus situation, and his behavior is
4
Objective Personality Tests
measured in ways of which he may not be aware and with interpretations of which he
Cattell wrote about the test taker, [...] even when he wished to, he cannot know
himself objectively enough to give a true picture (Cattell, 1968, p. 53). In line with this
Cattell stated that a dependence on features related to language is present to a lesser extent on
OPTs compared with questionnaires. Only when experimental measurement is made in terms
of behavioral response on exactly reproducible test situations can one have confidence in that
replication of results, in different countries (Cattell & Warburton, 1967, p. 4). Yet, there are
no known newer studies that have compared the cultural sensitivity of scores on OPTs with
characteristics are inferred from their overt behavior in such a standardized setting that lacks
face validity (see Cattell, 1968; Schmidt, 1975). This is done through the application of
achievement-oriented tasks or particular scoring methods (see Cattell & Kline, 1977).1
1
Taking the achievement-task-based definition into account, one could argue that Implicit Association Tests
(IATs; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) and some other indirect measures presented in this volume may
be representative of OPTs. IATs were developed to assess individual differences in the strength of associations
between cognitive representations of at least two concepts (see Perugini, this volume). Although this method has
primarily been designed to measure interindividual differences in attitudes toward objects (e.g., Banse, Seise, &
Zerbes, 2001), there was also a rather quick emergence of IATs that allowed the assessment of attitudes toward
the self (self-esteem) as well as the self-concept of personality, such as anxiety (e.g., Egloff & Schmukle, 2002;
Greenwald & Farnham, 2000). Achievement on an IAT-like reaction-time task serving as an indicator of self-
5
Objective Personality Tests
standardized miniature situation. OPT scores are not based on self-estimation or self-ratings
of the construct of interest. Furthermore, the aims of OPTs are masked and, compared to self-
report measures, OPTs have in fact shown a lower susceptibility to the manipulation and
As mentioned in the introduction, OPTs have been characterized by great variation from the
very beginning (R. B. Cattell, 1968), and several attempts have been made to group these
heterogeneous tests. Several decades ago, Hundleby (1973) proposed a categorization that
differentiated between four groups of tests: (1) The assessment of Expressive Movements
(e.g., characteristic movements of hand and body, allowing only a reduced assessment of very
specific aspects of human personality); (2) Simulated Real-Life data (i.e., the assessment of
behavior in situations that resemble a target situation as much as possible); (3) Physiological
esteem as one example (e.g., Rudolph, Schrder-Ab, Schtz, Gregg, & Sedikides, 2008) would fit the definition
of OPTs given earlier. However, the IAT and other indirect measures that capture representations of the self are
distinguished from OPTs in this chapter even if this distinction may not be very sharp: OPTs are defined as tests
that do not address (implicit) representations but rather provide samples of realistic behavior. Information
assessed by OPTs is not dependent on any mental representation, for example, the representation of the self as an
achievement-motivated person (Pawlik, 2006), because OPTs directly assess the behavior associated with the
construct of interest. Thus, objective and indirect tests will be viewed as different measurement approaches in
this chapter.
6
Objective Personality Tests
preference, response styles, responses to interference when performing an assigned task, and
responses to suggestion) with the last group including most of the tests during this time.
Although current and newly developed OPTs still display great variability in task
concepts, materials, and scoring methods, contemporary OPTs do not represent as great a
distinct today). In the following section, we propose a categorization of OPTs in the light of
1) OPTs masked as achievement tasks: In this category of tests, test takers are required
what is actually being measured or how the instrument is being scored. As a characteristic of
this group of tests, the task is not embedded in a simulated or imagined/suggested real-life
context or real-life situation. Typically, test takers cannot distinguish these tests from classical
(cognitive) performance tests. In most test concepts in this category, higher construct scores
are associated with a more accurate or faster performance on the task. This indicates that an
OPT-scoring procedure may even be independent from the task presented on the test itself. In
earlier approaches, this was operationalized, for example, by asking participants to compare
the lengths of 42 pairs of lines and to indicate whether (a) the one on the left or (b) the one on
the right side is longer or whether (c) both are of equal length. The analysis is based on the
number of comparisons made within a given time span (i.e., 30 s for Part I of the test). This is
one of eight subtests for the measurement for Exvia versus Invia (i.e., T45 in R. B. Cattell &
The Emotional Stroop Task (e.g., Dawkins & Furnham, 1989) can be understood as an
earlier example of such an OPT in this category. Test takers are instructed to name the colors
in which words are printed, whereas the words vary in their relevance to potential emotional
topics. The lower the interference of the emotional stimuli, the faster and more accurately the
7
Objective Personality Tests
colors will be named. Another example from this group is the Objective Achievement
Motivation Test (OAT; (Schmidt-Atzert, 2007): Test takers are instructed to pass fields on the
screen colored in either red or green. Fields are passed by quickly clicking on matching (red
or green) buttons (see Figure 1). The number of fields passed in a given time serves as an
computerized Working Styles battery (Kubinger & Ebenhh, 1996). The test is based on the
T62 test Hesitancy by Cattell and Warburton (1967). Test takers see two figures on the
screen and are instructed to select the larger one; after a decision has been made, a new pair of
figures appears on the screen. The figures differ by only about 10% of their total size but are
rotated to different angles. The number of decisions made within a given time is interpreted as
Another computerized battery of six tests was designed to measure stress resistance,
which occurs when performance is not impaired by typical occupational short-term stressors
(BAcO; see Kubinger, 2009; Ortner, 2012; Ortner, Kubinger, et al., 2006). The tasks assess
pressure task instructs test takers to assign letters to symbols according to a given coding
scheme as quickly and accurately as possible by dragging and dropping. The time available
for assigning letters displayed on the screen is gradually reduced in the second part of the test.
The given time-per-task screen is displayed in the middle of the screen by a digital clock,
which counts back to zero. The scores assess whether test takers increase or decrease their
coding speed and accuracy (Ortner, Kubinger, et al., 2006). The potential confounding of
achievement and personality components creates a challenge when the accuracy and speed
Some tests postulate only minor effects of ability on test scores as a result of item
selection (e.g., very easy items) and/or exercise trials in order to minimize advancing certain
8
Objective Personality Tests
which can be included in item response theory models that are able to separate these aspects
2) OPTs that aim to represent real-life simulations: In this category of OPTs, test
takers are required to solve a less or more complex task embedded in a real-life situation or
setting. Tests of this kind have not been developed or presented as pure achievement tasks,
although participants work toward such a goal. The trait or state measured is usually not made
A group of tests that were published during the last few years assigned to this category
are experimental games, including, for example, the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART;
Lejuez et al., 2002), the Betting Dice Test, or Roulette Test (see both Rubio, Hernndez,
Zaldivar, Marquez, & Santacreu, 2010). All of these aim to measure constructs related to risk
propensity by putting test takers into a gambling situation and scoring their behavior in the
simulated environment. Test takers can maximize their gains by making less or more risky
choices, where more risky choices are usually associated with higher gains. Another test that
is aimed at assessing risk propensity is the Crossing the Street Test (Rubio et al., 2010;
Santacreu, Rubio, & Hernndez, 2006), which simulates a traffic situation. Test takers are
instructed to navigate a pedestrian as quickly but also as safely as possible to reach the other
side of the road through different traffic situations. Test takers are asked to make a decision
about the position on the street from which a pedestrian should cross from one sidewalk to
another. Safer positions are related to more walking effort, more time, and lower scores.
A series of OPTs allowing for the assessment of vocational interests based on so-
called objective interest indices proposed by Cattell (R. B. Cattell, 1950; R. B. Cattell, 1957)
9
Objective Personality Tests
were developed by Proyer and were set together in the Multidimensional Objective Interest
Battery (Proyer, 2006; Proyer & Husler, 2008). OPT approaches in this domain have a long
tradition (Fryer, 1931; Super & Roper, 1941). One of the subtests of the MOI (Distribution of
money) asks participants to imagine having the responsibility to distribute 100,000 of their
proposals. Test takers are informed that they have the final word in deciding who will receive
the money. The concept is based on the assumption that decisions are based on personal
one of the six dimensions of vocational interest proposed by Holland (1997). A total score can
be computed for money assigned to each of the six dimensions, and larger amounts of
A challenge for test development and the application of tests in this category lies in a
stronger transparency (in comparison with other OPTs) that may also depend on the test
administration context. If tests are, for example, employed within an occupational selection
procedure, OPTs that are aimed at representing real-life situations may not elicit typical
behavior but may be expected to produce socially desirable behavior. In line with more
theoretical approaches, this would especially be the case if the OPTs in question activate more
controlled than spontaneous behavior (see Schmitt, this volume). However, test concepts in
this domain can still be scored in a way that is not transparent. For example, a test taker who
is not familiar with Hollands classification scheme will not be able to correctly recognize or
assign the organizations to the interest dimensions. On the other hand, a person applying for a
job as a bus driver may be able to deduce from the aptitude assessment situation that a
gambling task is being presented not just as a simple game but represents a relevant situation
that is related to the vocational activity at hand. The questions of whether the measurement
aim can be uncovered and the test performance can be influenced in a specific direction by the
10
Objective Personality Tests
test taker also seem to depend on the test situation itself. Furthermore, the question arises as to
whether simulated situations better represent and predict real-life situations and behavior.
OPTs, test takers are instructed to answer items that are similar to questionnaire items or to
make other forms of evaluative decisions. Although these tests appear to exist in the twilight
zone between objective tests and questionnaires, tests included as OPTs assessunlike
Examples of such OPTs are given in Cattells OA-TB (Cattell & Schuerger, 1976,
1978). For example, the T239 test (Decision speed on social problems) asks test takers to
solve specific problems by suggesting a solution (e.g., You are a mile away from the nearest
station when the car breaks down. What would you do? If you know, make a checkmark).
Rather than the answer, the speed with which a decision is made is scored as a sign of
assertiveness and confidence. A different example is the T261 test (Attainability of aims),
which requires test takers to estimate their probability of achieving specific unlikely aims,
such as dating a famous film star. The higher the probability estimate of the occurrence of this
event, the more optimistic the person is evaluated on this test. Test T328 (Word classification)
requires test takers to classify words (e.g., honesty), as personal, emotional, exciting, or
neutral. The words that are included were chosen as the items that had revealed the highest
variability in terms of evaluation in pilot studies. It is assumed that highly anxious persons
classify fewer words as neutral (see R. B. Cattell & Warburton, 1967). A new approach in this
category is the Objective Heuristic Thinking Test (OHTT; Jasper & Ortner, 2014) that aims to
assess thinking biases. Items of the Representativeness Scale demand estimations of the
probability that a described person belongs to a certain group by given base rates for the
membership. The Availability Scale requires the estimation of the probability that a particular
11
Objective Personality Tests
person or a group of persons would encounter an unlikely event (e.g., plane crash). The
Anchoring Scale consists of items including a short statement (e.g., Imagine you have a
bicycle with 7 gears) and a loosely related question (e.g., How many grams does the
bicycles shifter weigh?). Items were designed such that a reasonable answer to the question
would be either much lower or much higher than the actual anchor value.
In sum, these questionnaire-type OPTs, like estimative items, are typically associated
with the first generation of objective personality tests. New technologies allow for more
adaptations and new variations in item material or mode of presentation than merely varying
the scoring procedure. Thus, future successful developments that are based on these early
With regard to the early systematic empirical studies on the psychometric properties and
construct validity of OPTs, Cattell expected to find that all three postulated sources that are
necessary to build a complete picture of personality (Q-, L-, and T-Data) would point to a
Cattell, 1957). However, his own extensive research program did not support this assumption:
The T-data and Q-data sets showed only low convergence. With respect to the construct
It was concluded that different methods assess different aspects of the underlying traits and
that OPTs and self-report questionnaires may thus systematically assess different aspects of
other authors (Hcker, Schmidt, Schwenkmezger, & Utz, 1975; Hcker, Schwenkmezger, &
12
Objective Personality Tests
Utz, 1979; Skinner & Howarth, 1973). Most certainly, it cannot be argued that low or zero
correlations of an objective test with Q-data support the validity of the OPT but rather that
validation strategies need to go beyond simple correlations between a newly developed OPT
with a questionnaire that measures the same underlying construct (see Ortner, Proyer, &
Kubinger, 2006).
validity of second generation OPTs similar to the ones carried out in the early period of OPTs
had not yet been conducted. In addition, most of these measures had been developed without
referring explicitly to a unifying framework like the one that existed in Cattells tradition.
Furthermore, much less research has been published on OPTs in comparison with the large
amount of research published in recent years on indirect measures, most notably on the
Implicit Association Tests (IATs; see Perugini, this volume). Nevertheless, a large number of
studies employing new OPTs also revealed low correlations or a lack of convergence with
self-report measures. For example, studies using the Objective Achievement Motivation Test
(Schmidt-Atzert, 2004) revealed zero correlations with the achievement striving scale of the
NEO-PI-R (r = .02; Ziegler, Schmukle, Egloff, & Bhner, 2010). Analyses of composite
scores for conventional (questionnaire, nonverbal test) and seven OPTs from the initial
version of the MOI (Proyer, 2006; Proyer & Husler, 2008) for the assessment of vocational
interests revealed correlations between -.20 (enterprising interests) and .41 (artistic interests)
with the corresponding scales (see Proyer, 2007). Dislich, Zinkernagel, Ortner, & Schmitt,
(2010) revealed a lack of convergence in the risk propensity domain between the Domain-
Specific Risk-Taking (DOSPERT) Scale (Blais & Weber, 2001) and the BART (r = -.17).
However, at least a medium convergence was found between the self-reported frequency of
using rational calculation strategies and less risky choices in the Game of Dice task (r = .45;
13
Objective Personality Tests
The lack of convergence with established self-report measures has currently stimulated
the generation of studies including new indirect measures, such as Implicit Association Tests,
as well as studies on the relation between different OPTs that are aimed at assessing the same
construct (Dislich et al., 2010; Ortner et al., 2014). Thus, in line with dual-process theories
(e.g., Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006; Strack & Deutsch, 2004), Schmitt et al. (this volume)
argue that different OPTs may tap structurally more spontaneous or reflective aspects of a
construct and may therefore converge better with indirect measures or questionnaires. Some
OPTs may assess more associative, automatic, and spontaneous dispositions and may
consequently show higher convergence with indirect measures than with direct measures (and
vice versa). These considerations may serve as a way to resolve the so-called method-trait
problem that troubled research in the first generation of OPTs (Pawlik, 2006). The issue of
convergence may be addressed more comprehensively in combination with such new theories.
approach, in which several methods are employed for each trait and several tests are used to
represent each group of methods (objective, indirect, direct), may represent a future approach
for addressing questions of the construct validity of OPTs (see also Ortner & Schmitt, 2014).
Research on indirect measures has revealed that IAT scores and direct measures
Gawronski, Gschwendner, Le, & Schmitt, 2005). This information may also serve as a
starting point for future analyses involving OPTs. For example, with regard to different
convergence was highest for measures that captured the same anxiety content (speech, spider,
general) and the same specificity level (specific anxiety, general anxiety). Systematic
approaches manipulating specificity similarity and content similarity may also help us to
14
Objective Personality Tests
Furthermore, a lack of convergence between OPTs and other measures may also be
objective personality tests measure more situation-specific states than stable traits. To further
explore this possibility, latent state-trait theory (LST-theory; Steyer, Schmitt, & Eid, 1999)
can be employed. Recent research indicates that data obtained by using Experimental Games
with data obtained by a questionnaire and a Single Category IAT by employing a Multi
Cole (2008). This allowed different sources of variance to be separated: stable and momentary
trait influences, stable and momentary method influences, and measurement error influences.
Based on data from 367 students assessed on three different measurement occasions, results
indicated generally lower convergence of the OPTs used with data obtained through other
approaches. Furthermore, analyses revealed that the OPTs that were used assess stable rather
revealed that different methods assess trait components that are not shared between OPTs and
starting point from which to formulate more complex but also more successful hypotheses on
the validity of OPTs (Dislich, Zinkernagel, Ortner, & Schmitt, 2010). Potential moderators of
convergence are personality traits, situational characteristics, attributes of the construct, and
attributes of the measurement procedure (see Schmitt et al., this volume). Some promising
examples of criterion validity of frequently used OPTs are given in Table 1. Results on their
reliability and stability are also summarized for the tests listed in the table. Results given in
15
Objective Personality Tests
the table do not represent the heterogeneity of psychometric properties of available OPTs: For
consistency fails when only one score or item is available on a specific test. Further problems
in interpreting retest correlations occur when test takers receive information during a test trial
that influences his or her attitude or expectations toward forthcoming trials (Kubinger, 2009;
With regard to transparency and the opportunity to manipulate data provided by OPTs,
several studies have indicated that answers are more difficult to fake on OPTs than on
questionnaires. For example, this was shown on tests designed to assess impulsivity (Elliot et
al., 1996; K. Hofmann & Kubinger, 2001), on a test of achievement motivation (Ziegler et al.,
2010), and on a test of risk propensity in the context of driving behavior (Arendasy, Sommer,
Herle, Schtzhofer, & Inwanschitz, 2011). Overall, findings such as these lend support to the
notion that OPTs can truly have advantages in terms of lower social desirability in
Reviewing the number of articles published on indirect measures within the last few years,
especially with regard to the IAT, raises the question of why OPTs, especially of the second
generation, have not yet garnered similar attention and popularity in research. One reason may
be the large heterogeneity of tests in task concepts, scoring, and materials. In addition,
16
Objective Personality Tests
experience has shown that results obtained for a specific measure cannot be transferred to
other measures within this group, which is, to a lesser extent, also the case in research on
With regard to the first generation of OPTs, Pawlik (2006) mentioned that researchers
may tend to rely on questionnaires with a clear and often replicated factor structure (e.g., De
Raad, Perugini, & Szirmak, 1997; McCrae, Costa, Del Pilar, Rolland, & Parker, 1998). This
may have been viewed as an advantage compared with the factor structure and very complex
picture of interindividual differences obtained by including OPTs in early factor analytic work
(Hundleby et al., 1965; Pawlik, 1968). In fact, as data on convergent validity indicate, there is
still a need to investigate what OPTs measure and how they relate to other personality
OPTs are faced with the problem of having to surpass this gold standard. In particular, the
larger temporal stability and stronger correlations with other personality measures are
self-reports are widely acknowledged and have been mentioned; for example, information
obtained through self-reports can easily be distorted so that the accuracy of self-reports can be
Nevertheless, as indicated by the literature, at least some newer OPTs with convincing results
on their psychometric properties have been frequently integrated into research and are
currently gaining popularity, such as the BART (Cazzell, Li, Lin, Patel, & Liu, 2012;
Fukunaga, Brown, & Bogg, 2012; Lahat et al., 2012; Parkinson, Phiri, & Simons, 2012).
future research should involve large-scale approaches that include different methods, and as
17
Objective Personality Tests
approaches modeling OPT data at different points in time should also provide insight into the
different measures that are being used to assess latent trait and state components. These
longitudinal approaches should also allow researchers to take a broader approach, such as the
one provided by modern change models, which allow trait variance to be separated from both
method variance and state variance (Courvoisier et al., 2008; Geiser, Eid, Nussbeck,
However, researchers as well as practitioners have to face the fact that not every
construct can be addressed in a similar way by all methods, and in this case, by OPTs.
considerable amount of variance in Q-data, these domains are very difficult to implement and
assess by using standardized OPTs (Pawlik, 2006). It is more difficult to convert a realistic
in a socially desirable direction might be a part of the construct of interest. Cattell and Scheier
(1963) make such an argument for the measurement of anxiety. They suggest that reaction
anxiety measurement and a questionnaire catches them well (p. 6). Hence, in some cases, the
use of a questionnaire can be helpful for uncovering such aspectsand may be further
What about the use of OPTs in psychological practice? With regard to the earlier
approaches, the low utilization rates of OPTs in practice has been explained by their lower
enormous undertaking to develop, validate and standardize them (Hundleby, 1973, p. 84).
The extensive effort required by examiners to present and score OPTs and even to construct
them are nowadays less of a problem as considerable progress has been made by the
18
Objective Personality Tests
availability of flexible software packages. Also, because OPTs of the second generation are
almost exclusively administered in computerized settings, not only the administration itself
but also the scoring has become much easier for the examiner.
Recent test developments have also inspired OPT use in practice (for an overview, see
Ortner et al., 2007), and new developments have broadened their scope. Whereas clinical
settings were one of the main fields in which OPTs were applied in earlier times (e.g., R. B.
Cattell & Scheier, 1960; Kasielke, Hnsgen, & Strauss, 1985; Schmidt & Schwenkmezger,
1994a), the use of OPTs in the domains of human resources or selection seems to have gained
importance in recent times (see, e.g., the examples of current uses in Ortner et al., 2006).
With regard to the use of OPTs, for example, in the domain of vocational interests in
practice, Proyer (2006; Proyer & Husler, 2008) proposed a multidimensional strategy with
OPTs being one component of a larger test batteryalong with a questionnaire and a
nonverbal test (Proyer, 2007; Proyer, Sidler, Weber, & Ruch, 2012). Proyer argued that the
application of OPTs for counseling is especially useful if (a) a persons differentiation of the
profile(s) is low and/or (b) a persons vocational identity is diffuse. In these cases, nonverbal
tests and OPTs were found to provide additional information beyond pure self-descriptions
that may be distorted for different reasons (e.g., the expectations of others or a lack of
knowledge in the domains that are covered by the conventional questionnaires for vocational
interest). In the domain of vocational interests, this may help researchers to develop
hypotheses about peoples areas of interest that were previously hidden or less well-
cultivated.
To summarize this chapter, research on OPTs has taken considerable steps forward in
the past several years. There has been a transition from OPTs of the first generation to a
second generation, making strong use of the possibilities offered by computerized assessment
procedures. This has allowed researchers to develop new approaches in the design,
19
Objective Personality Tests
presentation, as well as the scoring of the tests. Although there are efforts to further structure
the field, OPTs still remain a rather heterogeneous group of tests. Although this heterogeneity
is a disadvantage in terms of structure, it may offer the field benefits by offering a broad range
of creative processes and ideas and the development of new assessment techniques. Recent
studies give researchers reasons to be optimistic about the future role of OPTs in the standard
repertoire of psychological assessment and their usefulness in both research and practice.
20
Objective Personality Tests
References
Aguado, D., Rubio, V. J., & Luca, B. (2011). The Risk Propensity Task (PTR): A proposal
for a behavioral performance-based computer test for assessing risk propensity. Anales
Arend, I., Botella, J., Contreras, M. J., Hernndez, J. M., & Santacreu, J. (2003). A betting
dice test to study the interactive style of risk-taking behavior. The Psychological
Arendasy, M., Sommer, M., Herle, M., Schtzhofer, B., & Inwanschitz, D. (2011). Modeling
32, 210-218.
Banse, R., Seise, J., & Zerbes, N. (2001). Implicit attitudes towards homosexuality:
Baumert, A., Schlsser, T., & Schmitt, M. (2014). Economic games: A performance-based
Blais, A.-R., & Weber, E. U. (2001). A domain-specific risktaking (DOSPERT) scale for
Brand, M., Fujiwara, E., Borsutzky, S., Kalbe, E., Kessler, J., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2005).
Brand, M., Heinze, K., Labudda, K., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2008). The role of strategies in
159-173.
21
Objective Personality Tests
Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the
Cattell, R. B. (1946). Description and measurement of personality. New York: World Book.
Cattell, R. B. (1948). Primary personality factors in the realm of objective tests. Journal of
(including Adult and Child O-A Batteries). Savoy, IL: Institute for Personality and
Ability Testing.
Cattell, R. B. (1957). Personality and motivation -- Structure and measurement. New York,
Cattell, R. B., & Kline, P. (1977). The scientific analysis of personality and motivation.
Cattell, R. B., & Scheier, I. H. (1960). Handbook for the Objective-Analytic (O-A) Anxiety
22
Objective Personality Tests
Cattell, R. B., & Scheier, I. H. (1963). Handbook fort he IPAT Anxiety Scale Questionnaire
Cattell, R. B., & Schuerger, J. M. (1976). The Objective-Analytic (O-A) Test Kit. Champaign,
IL: IPAT.
Cattell, R. B., & Schuerger, J. M. (1978). Personality in action:hHandbook for the Objective-
Cattell, R. B., & Warburton, F. W. (1967). Objective Personality and Motivation Tests: a
Press.
Cazzell, M., Li, L., Lin, Z.-J., Patel, S. J., & Liu, H. (2012). Comparison of neural correlates
of risk decision making between genders: An exploratory fNIRS study of the Balloon
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.030
Courvoisier, D. S., Nussbeck, F. W., Eid, M., & Cole, D. A. (2008). Analyzing the convergent
10.1037/a0012812
Cronbach, L. J. (1970). Essentials of psychological testing. New York: Harper & Row.
Dawkins, K., & Furnham, A. (1989). The colour naming of emotional words. British Journal
De Raad, B., Perugini, M., & Szirmak, Z. (1997). In pursuit of a cross-lingual reference
23
Objective Personality Tests
Dislich, F. X. R., Zinkernagel, A., Ortner, T. M., & Schmitt, M. (2010). Convergence of
direct, indirect, and objective risk taking measures in the domain of gambling: the
Egloff, B., & Schmukle, S. C. (2002). Predictive validity of an Implicit Association Test for
Elliot, S., Lawty-Jones, M., & Jackson, C. (1996). Effects of dissimulation on self-report and
343.
Fitts, P. M. (1946). German applied psychology during World War II. American Psychologist,
1, 151-161.
Fryer, D. (1931). The measurement of interests in relation to human adjustment. New York,
NY: Holt.
Fukunaga, R., Brown, J. W., & Bogg, T. (2012). Decision making in the Balloon Analogue
Risk Task (BART): anterior cingulate cortex signals loss aversion but not the
infrequency of risky choices. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 479-
Geiser, C., Eid, M., Nussbeck, F. W., Courvoisier, D. S., & Cole, D. A. (2010). Multitrait-
doi: 10.1007/s10182-010-0127-0
Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the implicit association test to measure
79, 1022-1038.
24
Objective Personality Tests
Gschwendner, T., Hofmann, W., & Schmitt, M. (2008). Convergent and predictive validity of
Guttmann, G., & Bauer, H. (2004). RISIKO Risikoverhalten [Risk Behaviour] [Software
Hcker, H., Schmidt, L. R., Schwenkmezger, P., & Utz, H. E. (1975). Objektive Testbatterie,
Hcker, H., Schwenkmezger, P., & Utz, H. (1979). ber die Verflschbarkeit von
Husler, J. (2004). An algorithm for the separation of skill and working style. Psychology
Science, 4, 433-450.
Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., & Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-
analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report
measures. [Proceedings Paper]. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1369-
1385.
25
Objective Personality Tests
Holland, J. L. (1997). Making vocational choices (3rd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources.
Horn, R. (2006). Die OA-TB 75: Neue Skizzen fr die Gestaltung Objektiver
Hundleby, J. D., Pawlik, K., & Cattell, R. B. (1965). Personality factors in objective test
Jasper, F., & Ortner, T. M. (2014). The tendency to fall for distracting information while
10.1027/1015-5759/a000214
Kasielke, E., Hnsgen, K.-D., & Strauss, E. (1985). Probleme und Mglichkeiten der
Psychologie, 6, 89-105.
Kline, P., & Cooper, C. (1984). A construct validation oft he Objective-Analytic Test Battery
Koch, T., Ortner, T. M., Eid, M., Caspers, J., & Schmitt, M. (2014). Evaluating the construct
26
Objective Personality Tests
Lahat, A., Degnan, K. A., White, L. K., McDermott, J. M., Henderson, H. A., Lejuez, C. W.,
propensity for risk taking in childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 847-
Lejuez, C. W., Aklin, W. M., Jones, H. A., Richards, J. B., Strong, D. R., Kahler, C. W., &
Read, J. P. (2003). The balloon analogue risk task (BART) differentiates smokers and
Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., . . .
Matthies, S., Philipsen, A., & Svaldi, J. (2012). Risky decision making in adults with ADHD.
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., del Pilar, G. H., Rolland, J. P., & Parker, W. D. (1998). Cross-
cultural assessment of the five-factor model - The revised NEO personality inventory.
Ortner, T. M. (2012). Teachers' burnout is related to lowered speed and lowered quality for
demanding short-term tasks. Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling, 54, 20-35.
27
Objective Personality Tests
Ortner, T. M., Gerstenberg, F. X., Zinkernagel, A., & Schmitt, M. (2014, submitted).
Ortner, T. M., Horn, R., Kersting, M., Krumm, S., Kubinger, K. D., Proyer, R. T., . . .
Ortner, T. M., Kubinger, K. D., Schrott, A., Radinger, R., & Litzenberger, M. (2006).
Ortner, T. M., Proyer, R. T., & Kubinger, K. D. (Eds.) (2006). Theorie und Praxis Objektiver
Ortner, T. M., & Schmitt, M. (2014). Advances and continuing challenges in Objective
OSS Assessment Staff (1948). Assessment of men: Selection of personnel for the Office of
Parkinson, B., Phiri, N., & Simons, G. (2012). Bursting with anxiety: Adult social referencing
in an interpersonal Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Emotion, 12, 817-826. doi:
10.1037/a0026434
Pawlik, K. (1968). Dimensionen des Verhaltens. Eine Einfhrung in Methodik und Ergebnisse
Introduction into method and results on the factor analytic approach in Psychology].
Bern: Huber.
28
Objective Personality Tests
[Theory and practice of Objective Personality Tests] (pp. 16-23). Bern: Hans Huber.
Pawlikowski, M., & Brand, M. (2011). Excessive Internet gaming and decision making: Do
excessive World of Warcraft players have problems in decision making under risky
Proyer, R. T., & Husler, J. (2007). Assessing behavior in standardized settings: The role of
7, 537-546.
Proyer, R. T., Sidler, N., Weber, M., & Ruch, W. (2012). A multi-method approach to
adolescents. International Journal for Educational and Vocational Guidance, 12, 141-
Rubio, V. J., Hernndez, J. M., Zaldivar, F., Marquez, O., & Santacreu, J. (2010). Can we
predict risk-taking behavior? Two behavioral tests for predicting guessing tendencies
29
Objective Personality Tests
Rudolph, A., Schrder-Ab, M., Schtz, A., Gregg, A. P., & Sedikides, C. (2008). Through a
Santacreu, J., Rubio, V. J., & Hernndez, J. M. (2006). The objective assessment of
personality: Cattells's T-data revisited and more. Psychology Science, 48, 53-68.
GmbH.
GmbH.
30
Objective Personality Tests
Boyle, G. Matthews & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), (Vol. 2, pp. 529-546). Los Angeles,
CA: Sage.
Steyer, R., Schmitt, M., & Eid, M. (1999). Latent state-trait theory and research in personality
Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior.
Super, D. E., & Roper, S. A. (1941). An objective technique for testing vocational interests.
White, T. L., Lejuez, C. W., & de Wit, H. (2008). Test-Retest characteristics of the Balloon
Ziegler, M., Schmidt-Atzert, L., Bhner, M., & Krumm, S. (2007). Faking susceptibility of
Ziegler, M., Schmukle, S., Egloff, B., & Bhner, M. (2010). Investigating measures of
31
Table 1
r rtt
Validity support
(internal consistency) (retest correlation)
Risk Propensity
Crossing the Street Test (CtST; Scores predicted guessing tendencies on a multiple-choice test
Santacreu, Rubio, & Hernndez, .96 (Santacreu et al., 2006) - (1,325 applicants) for an ab initio air-traffic control training
Game of Dice Test (GDT; Brand disorder (ADHD) compared with a control group (n = 31; p = -.01;
.68 (Dislich et al., 2010) -
et al., 2005) Matthies, Philipsen, & Svaldi, 2012).
32
making ability compared to a control group (n = 19) (Pawlikowski
Risk Behaviour Test (RBT; .70 (6 weeks; Guttmann consumption and a parallelized sample of inconspicuous drivers (n
-
Guttmann & Bauer, 2004) & Bauer, 2004) = 214) were correctly assigned at a rate of 77% to the two groups
The Risk Propensity Task (PTR) & Santacreu, 2003; r = .31; n = 59). Scores were revealed to be
.94 (Aguado et al., 2011) -
(Aguado, Rubio, & Luca, 2011) positively associated with a score built by self-reported smoking
2011).
Achievement Motivation
Objective Achievement .95.-.97 (Schmidt-Atzert, .85 (after 29 days; The OAT had relations of r = .24 with the intermediate examination
Motivation Test (OAT; Subtest 2007) Schmidt-Atzert, 2007) grade point average (n = 59) and r = .23 with the school leaving
33
1; (OAT, Subtest 1; Schmidt- examination grade point average (n =100; Schmidt-Atzert, 2004).
2014).
submitted)
Vocational Interests
34