Smart Meters
Smart Meters
Smart Meters
discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/319597907
CITATIONS READS
3 63
13 authors, including:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Sylwia Hyniewska on 13 September 2017.
PII: S0360-1323(17)30422-5
DOI: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2017.09.008
Reference: BAE 5087
Please cite this article as: Mogles N, Walker I, Ramallo-González AP, Lee J, Natarajan S, Padget J,
Gabe-Thomas E, Lovett T, Ren G, Hyniewska S, O'Neill E, Hourizi R, Coley D, How smart do smart
meters need to be?, Building and Environment (2017), doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2017.09.008.
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to
our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo
copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please
note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all
legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
PT
RI
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
EP
C
AC
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
PT
1
Department of Computer Science, University of Bath, UK.
RI
2
Department of Psychology, University of Bath, UK,
SC
3
Faculty of Computer Science, Universidad de Murcia, Spain.
4
Department of Architecture and Civil Engineering, University of Bath, UK,
U
d.a.coley@bath.ac.uk
AN
*
Corresponding author.
M
Graphical Abstract
D
TE
C EP
AC
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
PT
RI
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
EP
Abstract
C
Governments across the world are investing in smart metering devices that report
AC
energy use to the user with the aim of reducing consumption. However, the
effectiveness of such In-Home Displays (IHDs) has been questioned, since savings are
small. This is possibly because informing the consumer of their consumption in kWh,
consumption. We investigate, for the first time, the effect of replacing the simple
statement of energy use an IHD gives, with a detailed array of information specifically
2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
designed to improve consumer energy literacy and suggest behaviour change through
for context, and which can identify potential profligacy. The results from a carefully
controlled field experiment show: 1) value framing and action prompts have a
PT
significant effect on occupants' behaviour, with the mean temperature of homes being
reduced from 22.4°C to 21.7 °C, and a marked reduction in gas consumption—22.0%
RI
overall and 27.2% in high consumers; 2) energy literacy increasing from 0.52 to 1.28
SC
(on a 0-4 scale); 3) it is possible to target potentially profligate households, without
U
finding the system useful. These results emphasize the necessity of improving energy
AN
literacy when encouraging energy efficient behaviours and point to a new generation
of smart meters with the potential to increase energy literacy, make much greater
M
3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 Introduction
The residential energy sector accounts for 23% of total energy consumption
worldwide, placing it third after industry at 37% and transportation at 28% [1]. In
developed countries the sector is even more important; in the US, for example,
PT
residential consumption represents 25% of total energy use [2] and in the UK 29% [3].
RI
projected that residential consumption worldwide will grow by an average of 1.4% a
year from 2012 to 2040 [5]. Thus the sector is critical to both national energy policy
SC
and international climate change policy, and many attempts are being made to reduce
U
residential consumption by influencing the behaviour of occupants.
AN
Residential energy consumption is however a multidimensional phenomenon
embedded within a socio-cultural and infrastructure context, and for this reason
M
research reported here lies in inducing behavioural change with the help of energy
demand feedback via smart meters or ambient displays. Opinions regarding the
TE
effectiveness of such solutions to date are unfortunately not unanimous [6, 7, 8, 9, 10,
EP
11, 12].
solutions; however, with new developments in energy data management [13], more
AC
advanced feedback is now possible. The main contribution of the current paper to
digital energy feedback research is twofold: i) in broad terms, it tests the effectiveness
in an experimental setting for the first time; ii) in more specific terms, it evaluates the
effect of two new approaches to feedback – internal values and tailored action
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
prompts, delivered via a computer tablet. It replaces the normal user- and building-
blind smart meter concept with one that not only reports the energy used but
recommends specific actions and works with the personal values of the user. For
example, it might recommend turning the heating thermostat down one degree-
PT
centigrade, and explain this not just in terms of kWh or financial savings, but with
respect to environmental gain or other personal values. The system tested here is a
RI
simplified version of a future building- and user-conscious approach that would report
SC
complex and tailored information to the user in written or spoken sentences. For
example, “I note that the heating turns off at 9am each weekday, yet your home
U
appears to be unoccupied from 7am; would you like me to change the heating timing?
AN
This might save you £47 per year and the stop the emission of 218 kg of carbon
dioxide”. Or, “I note that your home might be over-ventilated when the heating is on,
M
losing £98 of heat per year. You might like to keep most of your windows closed when
the heating is on”. Such an approach, which we term intelligent smart metering (or
D
ISM), requires knowledge of which values are most likely to prompt the user to act.
TE
accurate savings based on the building and its use, rather than on inaccurate typical
AC
from a minimum of sensor information so as not to overly increase the cost of the
smart meter, for example a mix of utility meter data, room temperature, sub-circuit or
high-frequency electricity data to infer occupation and maybe home CO2 concentration
(as a proxy for ventilation rate). The sensors needed and the accuracy of such an
5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
approach, which uses inverse modelling to obtain an accurate thermal model from a
time series of data, have been reported by the authors in [14] and [15]. By creating
such a model, the financial, and other, impact of any suggestions, for example turning
down the thermostat by one degree, can be calculated for the specific home and
PT
reported to the user. In addition, inappropriate suggestions, for example suggesting a
RI
One can imagine other advantages of the ISM approach, for example, the
SC
reporting of the presence of high U-Values in the fabric to utility companies for
targeted intervention.
U
This paper attempts to discover if this new approach can be applied to a group of
AN
homes and if it generates changes in behaviour.
M
2 Background
Some research findings [16] suggest that continuous energy feedback might be
D
an effective driver of energy-related behaviour change. For example, Barbu et al. [17]
TE
suggest that energy feedback provided to users via smart meters could save 5-15% of
total energy costs. Similarly, [18, 19, 20] suggest that energy feedback through
EP
advanced in-home displays (IHDs) could help to save up to 20% of energy costs, either
for electricity or total energy bills. However, reality seems to fall short of such
C
reported across multiple utility pilot programs aimed at electrical energy conservation
Current technological solutions for real time energy feedback suffer from
multiple issues [22, 23], for example: unengaged users; failure to address users’
personal motivations and needs embedded in daily routines and social practices;
6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
financial costs; and inattention to users’ personal characteristics [24]. It seems clear
that users need something more to motivate and engage them than plain energy
feedback in kWh or cost if we are to get energy reductions of 10% or more. Some
PT
research indicates that intelligent energy feedback that offers different feedback
RI
Chiang et al. [26] have calculated that smart meters can pay back their
SC
installation costs in 4 years if the energy savings are 3% or more, although the
observation that the utility company no longer need to visit the home to read the meter
U
might currently be the main economic driving force for their installation. It is hence
AN
interesting to ask whether we might move the focus from a device that is mainly of use
to the energy company to one that has equal utility to the occupant.
M
attention from energy behaviour researchers, with varying success being noted. For
TE
have not achieved notable success [25, 27, 28, 29]. One possible reason for this is the
EP
so-called boomerang effect [30], whereby when households are told they are using less
energy than average they start to use more when they see what is ‘normal’ or
C
‘permissible’. This suggests that if messages are to be sent to households, only those
AC
where there is some evidence of potential energy profligacy should be targeted with
certain messages, and this is a key aspect of our study, and the one reason for its
success.
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
and their role in pro-environmental behaviour is studied within climate change risk
PT
perception [32]. In social psychology, Schwartz [33] identified a number of personal
values universal for all cultures and nations. From this perspective, personal values are
RI
defined as superordinate goals that serve as enduring guiding principles in peoples’
SC
lives [33]. Common to all these approaches is the idea that values are conceptually
different from goals, opinions and attitudes: values reflect broad long-term preferences
U
and so provide unity across a broad range of behaviours (a person who above all
AN
values their own self-interest will be self-interested in most settings); goals, opinions
and attitudes, on the other hand, are much more situation-specific and changeable over
M
time [34]. In the current study, we test personal values as motivators to energy-saving
respectively, concern for others, the self and the natural environment. These values
TE
were chosen from the various Schwartz’s personal value sets based on research by
DeGroot and Steg [35, 36], who identified altruistic, egoistic, biospheric and hedonic
EP
(pleasure-seeking) values as the key value orientations highly correlated with pro-
C
environmental behaviour. In this study, hedonic values were dropped from this set
AC
after pilot testing (with an opportunistic sample of 30 UK adults) found much lower
engagement with such messages compared to the other three value orientations (which
pilot participants found useful, and more engaging than energy messages expressed in
standard kWh).
The final facet of this study concerned the nature of the action prompts. The
8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
literature hints that energy feedback may be effective when it prompts concrete
personalised actions (for example, “Please switch off unused appliances”, “Adjust your
solving from the householder, as would a more general action prompt like “Reduce
PT
your energy consumption”. However, at the moment we lack the empirical quantitative
data needed to substantiate these claims [24]. Research on the effect of general action
RI
prompts has shown that it has limited influence on behaviour, but with increased
SC
possibilities for specificity and context detection offered by modern technologies it
might substantially improve the effect of feedback [37]. Some findings also indicate
U
that for the energy information to be useful, it should be tailored to personal contexts
AN
[38]. Thus, specific action prompts, tailored to specific households, might have a more
profound effect on energy related behaviour, and this idea is tested here.
M
related to the educational component of energy feedback and the concept of energy
TE
Energy literacy has been called “a broad term encompassing content knowledge as
aspects” [40], although arguably such a definition conflates knowledge about where
AC
energy is consumed with the motivation to reduce this. Cognitive (knowledge, in-depth
that promoting energy literacy can foster a shift in knowledge and perception of energy
and thus will facilitate responsible energy related choices and behaviour [40, 41, 42].
9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
energy feedback systems that use action prompts tailored to users’ behaviour in a
systematic manner. The only work that provides some tailored heating action prompts
is our previous study [43], however we did not aim to test different energy feedback
PT
approaches in an experimental setting. In the work of D’Oca et al. [44] tailored to
users’ contexts, newsletters with action suggestions were sent via email to participants,
RI
though the study only addressed electricity consumption, and action prompts were not
SC
part of an energy feedback display. One work on energy consumption feedback in a
U
action prompts [45], though this study again was focused only on electricity
AN
consumption and the tailored advice was sent by email not a smart meter.
There are a lot of commercially available devices and applications that provide
M
people with simply energy feedback and give general, not occupant- or building-
D
specific tips and suggestions regarding energy saving issues. In other words, the
TE
existing IHDs are not smart enough to address building context or personal
motivations.
EP
energy feedback within field experiments. One exception being the previously
AC
substantially improve users’ engagement and increase their motivation to save energy
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
since multiple values guide people’s lives [34] and focussing only on one value might
messages that refer, and are valid only for, specific households.
PT
The main research questions that we wanted to answer were:
RI
1. What is the overall effect of digital energy feedback interventions?
SC
2. What is the additional benefit of internal values embedded in energy feedback
messages?
U
3. What is the additional effect of personalised messages with action prompts?
AN
4. What effect has a digital energy feedback on cognitive variables, such as energy
11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
4 Method
To answer the research questions defined in Section 3, we designed an energy
feedback system with different types of feedback, we deployed the system in homes in
PT
the UK and performed a field experiment. The experiment had a 2 (Value framed vs.
Non-value framed) by 2 (No action prompt vs. Tailored message with action prompt)
RI
within-subject factorial design resulting in four energy feedback experimental
SC
conditions (see Table 1).
4.1
U
Participants and procedure AN
Our participants were residents of social housing recruited in the first half of
2013 with the help of Exeter City Council. Initially, an information package was sent
M
households. After a consent form was signed by the occupants, the sensors were
D
installed during January-May 2014 in 73 homes. The first phase of the project
TE
established the baseline: sensor data were collected but no energy feedback was
For the second phase of the project, an energy feedback experiment was
C
conducted over three months (January 2016 - March 2016) with the help of a new
AC
energy feedback application (iBert) written by the authors and presented to participants
via a tablet computer. The current paper describes the analysis of this second phase of
the project. Due to the length of the study, and the social group involved, some
households dropped out of the study, moved houses or changed contact details, which
12
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
In this sample, each household had on average 2.8 residents. Each home had a
main contact, and the mean age of these was 50.6 years (with a minimum age of 27
PT
(identified at a national UK level for 2015, with the London area excluded [46]).
Around 25% of households reported difficulties with paying energy bills. Fourteen
RI
percent of households already had a basic smart meter (could correspond to condition
SC
1 in our study).
The focus of the study was in part the response of occupants to feedback from
U
personalised messages. For this reason, individual basic thermal models of each of the
AN
homes were created. It had already been demonstrated by the authors that the sensors
used can auto-generate a suitable dynamic thermal model of each building based only
M
on the data received from the sensors (without the need for information on the size of
D
the homes or their constructions). This model is a lumped parameter model and the
TE
work has been reported in another paper [15] and shown to produce accurate results.
However, to make sure that no extra noise was added to the experiment and ensure the
EP
results reported were not affected by an auto-generated model which might well
change during the experimental cycle, simple (steady-state) models were created by
C
using floor plans and information about the materials and constructions from the city
AC
council and visual inspection. These models consisted of calculating the mean heat
The simple model obtained from the building geometry was used by iBert to
investigate the energy savings of various strategies (turning the thermostat down,
13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
closing windows, turning the heating off when the building was not occupied, turning
the lights and some other electrical items off when the building was not occupied) that
could be applied to each house if the household breached set action levels of: room
temperature, ventilation rate, having the heating on when building was unoccupied;
PT
and high electricity use when the building was unoccupied. Depending on the
condition applied to the home at the time (see Table 1 and 2), this gave iBert the ability
RI
to inform households of: their current energy consumption; the total consumption over
SC
the last week; their consumption expressed in accordance to other values (such as an
environmental cost); the savings that might occur if they took action; and, most
U
importantly, the specific actions they might take.
AN
Due to page limitations, full details of the electronics, sensors and coding that
lie behind iBert will be described in a later paper. But in outline, the system consisted
M
of sensors that measured real-time utility data, air temperature (in up to three rooms),
radiator temperature (to sense if heating was on), humidity (in up to three rooms),
D
light, motion and CO2 levels, with this data collected approximately every 5 minutes
TE
via Raspberry Pi and transmitted via the home’s broadband or 3G network to a secure
cloud store. These data were then used to see if action levels had been triggered,
EP
calculate any savings that might be made if behaviour were to change in the future, and
C
report back to the occupant via the same connection to the Wi-Fi enabled tablet in the
AC
home as a weekly digest. The frequency with which householders interacted with the
iBert app was recorded directly from each tablet and as well as a record of the tailored
PT
Action C3: Standard display + C4: Standard display +
Personalised + tailored action prompts + tailored action prompts +
values
If you reduced the
RI
thermostat temperature in If you reduced the thermostat
your house one degree you temperature in your house one
would save 11 kWh. degree you would save 11
SC
kWh; this is equivalent to
£1.43.
U
AN
The households were cycled through the four conditions shown in Table 1 with
each condition lasting three weeks. Households were randomly assigned to one of the
M
example, during the first three weeks, one-quarter of the homes were under condition
D
C1 of Table 1 and in the following three weeks they had condition C2 applied to them.
TE
In conditions C3 and C4, tailored energy feedback messages are generated once
EP
per week, based on energy-related information aggregated over the previous week, and
were displayed during the subsequent week. If the algorithm for identifying energy-
C
wasting events and variables did not detect any potentially profligate energy use in a
AC
particular household, that household did not receive any tailored messages that week.
Table 2: Latin square counterbalancing design for the four experimental conditions: C1
to C4 across the four temporal phases of the experiment.
Baseline Phase 1 Phase 2 Phase 3 Phase 4 After
14/12/15 04/01/16 25/01/16 15/02/16 07/03/16 28/03/16 18/04/16
15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
C1 C2 C3 C4
C4 C1 C2 C3
Before iBert After iBert
C3 C4 C1 C2
C2 C3 C4 C1
PT
This results is sixteen textual messages in total: four for personal value neutral
RI
biospheric values). A household could receive up to four different messages since a
SC
value message is framed according to one of the three personal values and a value is
selected at random. Messages in conditions C3 and C4 are sent only if there are
U
energy-wasting events identified by the intelligent algorithms.
AN
Participants received an incentive of £150 in the form of supermarket vouchers,
M
sent by post in three instalments of £50 (at the beginning of the project, at the end of
year one and at the end of year two after the completion of the project). In addition,
D
they were allowed to keep the tablet once the study has ended. All participants who
TE
win a £20 supermarket voucher during a prize draw. After the end of the experiment
EP
the energy feedback app was deactivated and sensor equipment was collected from the
C
participating homes four months after the end of the energy feedback interventions.
AC
Prior to both phases of the study participants were given paper information
sheets, consent forms and surveys that contained among others demographics and the
first seven items from the energy literacy survey described in [40]. This is an updated
developed in the by DeWaters and Powers [40]. To the authors’ knowledge, it is the
definition. The seven items used from this questionnaire addressed only the general
knowledge of energy consumption. (The full energy literacy and user experience
PT
Figure 1. The reader can see that almost all houses had substantial level of interactions
with the tablet. Each interaction with the app consisted of a minimum of three recorded
RI
app related events, such as ‘app started’, ‘app resumed’, ‘app closed’ etc.
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
Figure 1: Interactions heat map—the colour indicates the number of interactions. The
C1: Standard energy feedback. For the standard display condition (C1: No
value – No action; see Table 1), we followed the recommendations of the minimal
17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
requirements for an In-Home Display (IHD), i.e. we replicated a typical smart meter in
the UK [47]. This displayed: current gas and electricity consumption; cumulative
consumption for the past week; information in kWh; and a visual (i.e. non-numerical)
PT
C2: Standard feedback with personal values. In a personal value only
RI
condition (C2: Value - No action) the display was the same as in C1, but
SC
complemented the weekly summary information translated into one of the three
U
destroyed; or altruistic – expressed as the cost to society, couched as the number of
AN
minutes with a family doctor that could be provided at the same cost. So, for example,
a household might have the kWh display replaced with information that their energy
M
use in the last week was equivalent in carbon terms as the destruction of 1.7 trees or 20
D
minutes with a doctor. Egoistic, biospheric or altruistic personal values for each
TE
C3: Standard feedback with tailored actions. This condition was the same
EP
as C1 except households also saw a tailored message box with action prompts in text
form about any potentially energy profligate behaviour (room temperatures > 21°C,
C
occupied CO2 levels of <800 ppm (possibly indicating excessive ventilation), mean
AC
was unoccupied) during the previous week. Action prompts described concrete actions
with low personal costs, e.g. lowering a thermostat setting or leaving windows open
less frequently. A participant could receive from zero to a maximum of four messages.
18
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
energy related variables [for example, we have noticed that your thermostat was set to
23°C]; (2) evaluation of the factual information [e.g. this value is unnecessarily high];
over a longer time period (over a whole winter, over a year) [e.g. in your house this
PT
may imply 520kWh use more per winter]; and (4) an advice part which contained an
action prompt [e.g. we recommend that you lower the thermostat to 21 degrees
RI
centigrade and that you check if you are comfortable at that temperature].
SC
The trigger of 21°C was based on national recommendations [48]. The 800 ppm
was chosen by considering EN 15251. This suggests IDA 2 air quality (medium IAQ)
U
is achieved below 1000 ppm. In an effort to be precautionary, a lower value of 800
AN
ppm was used - this was the median value found during the baseline phase. The 600 W
was also derived from data gathered during the baseline phase. This showed that in all
M
houses consumption was bi-modal: low values during unoccupied periods or the night;
D
and high values during occupied periods. The mean figure that separated the two being
TE
600W.
condition C4, households received the same information as in C3, but the kWh part of
the message was translated into one of the three internal personal values just like in
C
information is translated into biospheric value concepts might be: “I think a lot of heat
might be escaping from open windows. The escaping heat results in wasted energy.
This requires roughly 12 more trees to compensate for the extra pollution caused by
your home over a winter. This may be because there are too many windows open, they
are open too wide or for too long. Try changing how many windows you open and for
19
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
how long and you may save energy that over a whole winter is equivalent to planting
12 trees. Try it, and check if this message disappears next week”.
PT
5 Results
In this section, we show the results from the different analyses that
RI
were performed to evaluate the effectiveness of the iBert system.
SC
This analysis tests hypothesis 1, asking if digital feedback in general has an
U
effect on energy-related behaviour compared to a baseline period before the
AN
interventions. The purpose of this is to examine if our findings are in line with those
from other EHD trials. A repeated-measures ANOVA was used to evaluate the effect
M
levels).
D
using Holm-Bonferroni p value correction reveal that home internal temperature before
iBert was active is significantly higher than the internal temperature during the iBert
C
experimental phase (d = 0.24, p < 0.001) and the difference between the internal
AC
temperature during the experiment and after the experiment is not significant either (p
= 0.97)—indicating the intervention had lasting impact (Table 3). We understand that
lowering the temperature can lead to larger issues such as comfort problems or even
health risks in vulnerable households, it is for this reason that this analysis is expanded
later to check if this temperature reduction was from all households, or only from ones
20
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 3: Mean internal temperature (in ◦C) for households before, during and
◦ ◦
Temperature ( C) Standard deviation ( C)
PT
Baseline 20.4 2.07
IBert 19.6 2.50
RI
Phase
After 19.4 2.35
U SC
In addition, we conducted two analyses of covariance to see how often occupants
AN
interacted with iBert and if occupants receiving tailored textual feedback influenced
householders had received at least one tailored textual feedback message, F(2,56) =
D
3.45, p = 0.04, ηp2 = 0.11. This interaction shows that the difference in internal
TE
temperature between homes with and without a tailored textual feedback is not visible
during the digital interventions phase, but it is after the interventions. In the light of
EP
between with-iBert and after-iBert phases for two groups of households: those who did
AC
not receive any temperature-related tailored textual message and those who did.
This difference is significant, t(29) = 2.29, p = 0.03, which indicates that homes
that received tailored textual messages with action prompts had lowered their internal
temperature after the digital interventions; while homes which did not receive
21
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
with frequency of app usage and received textual messages as covariates, would
PT
Analysis 2: Post-intervention effect of messages
RI
assess whether people responded to these, we conducted four follow-up one-sample t-
SC
tests to compare internal temperatures to 21◦C, before and after the intervention, for the
group who received temperature-related action prompts and the group who did not. For
U
the non-message group (the subgroup whose home temperature was lower than 21◦C
AN
and who therefore were never told to change the temperature even under experimental
conditions C3 and C4), internal temperature was significantly lower than 21◦C both
M
before (mean temperature = 18.7◦C, SD = 1.6, t(24) = -6.99, p < 0.001) and after the
D
experiment (mean temperature = 18.7, SD = 1.9, t(16) = -5.09, p < 0.001) – as would
TE
be expected. The group that received the temperature message because their baseline
temperature was over 21◦C (mean temperature = 22.3◦C, SD = 1.1, t(14) = 4.38, p
EP
<0.001), however, had internal temperatures that essentially settled at 21◦C after the
internal temperatures that put their home temperatures almost exactly at the level that
was suggested; a similar change was not seen in those who received no prompts,
suggesting the effect is likely not the result of weather-related changes. Note that both
groups could still see the standard IHD module of the app after the experiment,
although the tailored messages function was deactivated. No messages were triggered
22
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
based directly on gas consumption (but rather on internal temperature, heating times
and excessive ventilation), however the use of iBert reduced the mean household gas
PT
Analysis 3: Effect of values and action prompts
RI
To evaluate our second and third hypotheses, regarding the effect of internal
values framing and tailored action prompts during the energy feedback iBert phase, we
SC
conducted a two-way repeated measures ANOVA on home internal temperature,
electricity consumption and CO2 level data with the factors Personal Values (present or
U
absent) and Action Prompts (present or absent). Baseline measurements of these
AN
dependent variables, taken before iBert deployment, were used as covariates. After
controlling for the baseline temperature, homes in the value framing conditions (C3
M
and C4) have a lower internal temperature (19.23◦C) compared to homes in the
D
An ANOVA on these internal temperature readings during the iBert phase, with
F(1,33) = 5.29, p = 0.028, ηp2 = 0.14. The baseline temperature covariate was also
C
significant, F(1,33) = 4.94, p = 0.033, ηp2= 0.13, suggesting the warmer homes at the
AC
start of the study still tended be the warmer homes at the end.
unlikely that action prompts were forgotten between experimental phases. In light of
this, the data were separated into two groups, before the issuing of action prompts and
post issuing. Action prompts were found to have a statistically significant impact under
a pairwise t-test (p = 0.021) on the heating degree day adjusted mean gas consumption
23
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
PT
RI
Analysis 4: Case studies of subgroups with high and low energy
SC
consumptions
To get more insight into the changes in internal temperature, gas and electricity
U
consumption, and CO2 concentration, across all the stages of the study, we examined
AN
the box plots of the data for two subgroups: one being the households that had high
energy use and the other those homes with low energy use.
M
Figure 2 shows that the median temperature of the houses that had a high
D
thermostat temperature (upper graph) went down during the iBert interventions and
TE
24
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
PT
RI
U SC
AN
M
Figure 2: Home internal temperatures (in °C) before, across the iBert digital
intervention phase and after the feedback. The graph has been separated as houses
D
likely to receive a personalised message (upper graph) and those that did not (lower).
TE
The blue box represents 50% of the data, the median is represented as a horizontal
EP
(red) line within the box and the blue whiskers represent the 97.5% of the data. The
thick green line is at 21°C—the trigger for receiving a message. The individual dots,
C
to 21ºC in the group with temperatures higher than 21ºC in the baseline: with a p-value
rejecting the Null hypothesis (they are equal) of p=0.0039 (<0.05); whereas the
temperatures before and after in the group in which the temperatures were already
25
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
below 21ºC cannot be considered different (p-value of 0.46). Moreover, a t-test was
used to prove if the hypothesis that the temperatures in the baseline in the houses in
which the starting temperature was larger than 21ºC was statistically different to 21ºC
after the study. The test proved that the temperatures of the group which behaviour
PT
needed to be modified were not statistically different to 21ºC after the study with a p-
value of 0.17 (the null hypothesis mean = 21ºC, can not be rejected) meaning that the
RI
behaviour of the occupants had been modified.
SC
A similar evaluation was done with the CO2 concentration. Houses in which the
CO2 levels were less than 800 ppm were considered as potentially over ventilated, and
U
therefore it was assumed that some energy savings could be achieved by reducing
AN
ventilation.
To evaluate this, we again separated the homes into two groups: the ones that
M
showed high levels of ventilation in the baseline (CO2<800ppm) and those that did not
D
(Figure 3). In this case, all the tests show that the CO2 concentrations were not
TE
changed in the intervention; neither in the homes that showed high levels of CO2 nor in
those that showed low levels. This is possibly because changing the ventilation regime
EP
in a house is not a trivial task. Also, it should be noted that CO2 is not only an indicator
of ventilation, but also an indicator of infiltration occurring through cracks and other
C
26
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
PT
RI
U SC
AN
Figure 3: Home CO2 concentrations before, across the iBert digital intervention phase
M
and after the feedback. The graph has been separated into houses likely to receive a
D
personalised message (upper graph) and those that were not (lower). The thick green
TE
With respect to electricity, it also seems that the intervention failed to change
C
the behavioural habits of the occupants, with no statistically significant change seen in
AC
The gas consumption however shows a clear indicator of the effect of iBert.
Although iBert contained no direct messaging with respect to gas consumption, three
statements within iBert were designed to reduce gas consumption: change in the
thermostat temperature, change in the operating scheme (turning heating off when
house unoccupied) and change in the ventilation regime. As there was no direct
27
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
messaging with respect to gas consumption, we cannot separate high and low
Calculating the pre/post iBert household gas consumption for just these high
PT
consumers shows a mean household saving of 27.2%, compared to 22.0% for all
homes.
RI
It is unknown why there was no reduction in electricity use, but the following
SC
are possibilities: (1) for an occupant, reducing electricity consumption is more
complex than simply turning a thermostat down, or ensuring heating is off when
U
leaving the building; (2) rational electricity consumption is more individualistic than
AN
heating consumption, with a multitude of different devices in different homes, this
could lead to high consumers not necessarily being profligate; (3) it is known that
M
people are unsure of which electrical items use the most electricity, and hence which to
D
28
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
PT
RI
U SC
AN
Figure 4: Box plots of daily electricity usage after separation of big consumers (top)
M
and small consumers (bottom). The thick green line is at 600 W—the trigger for
D
receiving a message.
TE
A paired sample t-test was conducted to evaluate the effect of digital energy
indicated that the mean energy literacy score before the interventions (M= 0.52, SD=
AC
0.71) was significantly lower than the mean energy literacy score after the
interventions (M = 1.28, SD =1.06, t(24) = 3.17, p = 0.004). This confirms our fourth
hypothesis that the energy literacy of participants will improve after digital feedback
29
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Three months after the end of the energy feedback study, participants were
contacted by phone to arrange for the equipment to be collected and were asked to
complete the same items from the energy literacy survey described in [50,51], again
PT
along with nine questions related to system usability and user experience: four items
adopted from the System Usability Scale (SUS)[52]) which is the most frequently cited
RI
scale for system usability evaluation, one item addressing participants’ preferences for
SC
different components of iBert and four items on the effect of iBert on participants’
behaviour. In total, 65% of the participants completed the post-study survey. The
U
answers were rated on a 3-point ordinal Likert scale. The usability questions were:
AN
“I think that I would like to use iBert frequently”
“I needed to learn a lot of things before I could get going with the system”
D
The results show that the majority of the participants liked to use iBert
EP
frequently, found the system easy to use and in general found iBert useful. Eighty-two
percent of the respondents found live energy consumption data useful, 77% liked the
C
weekly energy summary and 78% the tailored messages; ANOVA analysis showed the
AC
three components of feedback were found roughly equally useful, F(2, 32) = 1.27, p =
0.30. Some 50% of the respondents took energy saving actions suggested by iBert and
roughly 36% of them took other energy saving actions not suggested by the system.
30
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
PT
Other households members were aware of the iBert
information 86%
Found iBert (partially) useful 82%
RI
SC
6 Discussion
In this section we discuss the findings in the light of the four hypotheses
This hypothesis was confirmed. Homes with high internal temperatures reduced
D
31
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
is observed, the digital interventions will influence energy related cognitive variables,
PT
effect of iBert interventions on energy literacy. One may argue that the effect of
energy literacy could be attributed to other factors and not the tailored educative
RI
messages per se, e.g. by the Hawthorne effect [53]. This is a phenomenon where
SC
people behave differently when they know they are being observed. Given the fact that
the sensors sets were installed in these homes two years earlier and they had been
U
monitored for a long period of time prior to the energy feedback intervention study, we
AN
believe that the Hawthorne effect was minimal in comparison to other studies as many
7 Conclusions
D
The main innovative aspect of this study is that it tests for the first time a
TE
combination of two energy feedback strategies, personal values and tailored action
motivational messages and action prompts tailored to user behaviour and building
characteristics were embedded into an intelligent energy feedback system called iBert.
C
The system gives advice according to individual building context and problematic
AC
ventilation, and heating and electricity usage while the home is not occupied.
temperature, and a specific positive effect of internal values and action prompts
incorporated in energy feedback, from digital energy feedback delivered by iBert. This
32
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
is the first time a smart metering approach has been shown to directly influence room
temperature. Unprecedented gas consumption saving were achieved with much of this
PT
substantially greater than the reductions seen in the literature; this provides strong
evidence of the power of personalised feedback, personal values, action prompts and
RI
an active improvement of energy literacy, and could be industry-transformative.
SC
The digital feedback system we have demonstrated in this paper had a marked
U
result is very promising as it indicates that digital feedback (a standard IHD
AN
accompanied by tailored educational messages as in the current study) can improve
first step towards forming new attitudes and foundations for energy-saving behaviour
D
amongst occupants.
TE
about their energy consumption with the purpose of educating users and lowering their
EP
demand. By being building- and occupant-aware, and only reporting messages when
consumption is potentially high, many of the criticisms of smart metering have been
C
addressed. For the first time a smart metering system has been shown to reduce room
AC
temperatures directly, with the temperature chosen by the occupants identical to that
suggested by the algorithm. The gas consumption savings found in this two-year study
have the potential to be transformative for both the occupants and for climate change
policy.
33
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Acknowledgements
This work was done under the auspices of the ENLITEN project, funded by
EPSRC (grant reference EP/K002724/1). We thank Exeter City Council for their
support provided during the project. The internal temperature and energy consumption
PT
data used for the analysis together with the energy literacy and usability surveys data
RI
like to thank the program Saavedra Fajardo (20035/SF/16) funded by Consejería de
SC
Tecnología de la Región de Murcia.
U
AN
References
M
[1] IEA. World Energy Statistics and Balances 2016. Technical report,
[2] EIA. Annual Energy Outlook 2016. Technical report, Energy Information
TE
[4] DECC. 2014 UK Greenhouse Gas Emissions, Final figures. Technical report,
AC
34
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
behavior change and residential energy use: a review of research directions and
PT
8(2):137–147, 2014.
RI
[8] Chris Foulds, Rosalyn A.V. Robison, Rachel Macrorie, Energy monitoring as
a practice: Investigating use of the iMeasure online energy feedback tool, Energy
SC
Policy, 104:194-202, 2017.
U
[9] Thanasis G. Papaioannou, Dimos Kotsopoulos, Cleopatra Bardaki, Stavros
AN
Lounis, Nikos Dimitriou, George Boultadakis, Anastasia Garbi, Anthony Schoofs,
[11] Thorben Jensen, Georg Holtz, Carolin Baedeker, Émile J.L. Chappin,
[12] Xingxing Zhang, Jingchun Shen, Tong Yang, Llewellyn Tang, Luying
Wang, Yingqi Liu & Peng Xu, Smart meter and in-home display for energy savings in
González, Antonio F. Skarmeta, An Open IoT Platform for the Management and
Analysis of Energy Data. Future Generation Computer Systems (In press), 2017.
PT
RI
[14] Lovett, T., E. Gabe-Thomas, S. Natarajan, E. O’Neill and J. Padget. 2013.
SC
for the ‘energy literacy through an intelligent home energy advisor’ (ENLITEN)
U
project.” In Proceedings of the Fourth International Conference on Future Energy
AN
Systems, 279–280. Berkeley, CA: ACM.
inverse modelling for the wide scale characterization of the thermal properties of
[16] Wokje Abrahamse, Linda Steg, Charles Vlek, and Talib Rothengatter. A
through behaviour change: what does it take? Technical report, EEA, 2013.
AC
[18] Sarah Darby. Smart metering: what potential for householder engagement?
[19] Tom Hargreaves, Michael Nye, and Jacquelin Burgess. Making energy
visible: A qualitative field study of how householders interact with feedback from
[20] Vine Desley, Buys Laurie, and Morris Peter. The effectiveness of energy
feedback for conservation and peak demand: a literature review. Open Journal of
[21] Ahmad Faruqui, Sanem Sergici, and Ahmed Sharif. The impact of
PT
informational feedback on energy consumptiona survey of the experimental evidence.
RI
[22] K. Buchanan, R. Russo, and B. Anderson. The question of energy reduction:
SC
The problem(s) with feedback. Energy Policy, 77:89–96, 2015.
U
[23] Geraldine Fitzpatrick and Greg Smith. Technology-enabled feedback on
AN
domestic energy consumption: Articulating a set of design concerns. Pervasive
[24] Kathryn Buchanan, Riccardo Russo, and Ben Anderson. Feeding back about
eco-feedback: How do consumers use and respond to energy monitors? Energy Policy,
D
73:138–146, 2014.
TE
[26] Teresa Chiang, Sukumar Natarajan, and Ian Walker. A laboratory test of the
C
efficacy of energy display interface design. Energy and Buildings, 55:471–480, 2012.
AC
[27] Hunt Allcott. Social norms and energy conservation. Journal of Public
[28] Sally Dibb, Marylyn Carrigan, Tim Harries, Ruth Rettie, Matthew Studley,
Kevin Burchell, and Simon Chambers. Is social norms marketing effective? a case
47(9):1458–1475, 2013.
[29] P Wesley Schultz, Mica Estrada, Joseph Schmitt, Rebecca Sokoloski, and
Nilmini Silva-Send. Using in-home displays to provide smart meter feedback about
PT
cost, and social norms. Energy, 90:351– 358, 2015.
RI
[30] P Wesley Schultz, Jessica M Nolan, Robert B Cialdini, Noah J Goldstein,
SC
social norms. Psychological science, 18(5):429–434, 2007.
U
[31] Adam Corner, Ezra Markowitz, and Nick Pidgeon. Public engagement with
AN
climate change: the role of personal values. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate
[34] Linda Steg, Jan Willem Bolderdijk, Kees Keizer, and Goda Perlaviciute. An
C
115, 2014.
[35] Judith IM De Groot and Linda Steg. Value orientations to explain beliefs
38
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
[36] Linda Steg, Goda Perlaviciute, Ellen Van der Werff, and Judith Lurvink.
[37] Jon Froehlich. Promoting energy efficient behaviors in the home through
PT
feedback: The role of human-computer interaction. In Proc. HCIC Workshop, volume
RI
[38] Neil Simcock, Sherilyn MacGregor, Philip Catney, Andrew Dobson, Mark
SC
Ormerod, Zoe Robinson, Simon Ross, Sarah Royston, and Sarah Marie Hall. Factors
[40] Jan DeWaters and Susan Powers. Establishing measurement criteria for an
TE
2013.
EP
[41] Lung-Sheng Lee, Yi-Fang Lee, James W Altschuld, and Ying-Ju Pan.
C
2016.
39
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
[43] Marika Vellei, Sukumar Natarajan, Benjamin Biri, Julian Padget, and Ian
[44] Simona DOca, Stefano P. Corgnati, and Tiziana Buso. Smart meters and
PT
energy savings in italy: Determining the effectiveness of persuasive communication in
RI
[45] Andreas Kamilaris, Jodi Neovino, Sekhar Kondepudi, and Balaji Kalluri. A
SC
case study on the individual energy use of personal computers in an office setting and
assessment of various feedback types toward energy savings. Energy and Buildings,
Financial year ending 2015. Technical report, Office for National Statistics, UK, 2015.
[50] Debby RE Cotton, Wendy Miller, Jennie Winter, Ian Bailey, and Stephen
[51] Jan E DeWaters and Susan E Powers. Energy literacy of secondary students
in new york state (USA): A measure of knowledge, affect, and behavior. Energy
40
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
[52] John Brooke et al. Sus-a quick and dirty usability scale. Usability evaluation
PT
methodological artifact. Journal of applied psychology, 69(2):334, 1984.
RI
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
Part 1
1. How much do you feel you know about energy? (Please circle one)
41
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
A lot (expert)
PT
Nothing
RI
understanding of energy issues? (Please choose one option)
SC
Further or higher education
School
U
AN
Books, newspapers or magazines
Internet
D
3. The term renewable energy resources means (please circle one answer)
42
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Solar
PT
Wind
Landfill gas
RI
Geothermal
SC
Don’t know
U
5. Which of the following actions, if everyone did this all the time, would save the most
AN
energy in the UK? (Please circle the most important )
6. Which kind of lighting uses the least amount of energy? (Please circle one answer)
C
Fluorescent lights
LED lights
Don’t know
43
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Part 2
Agree
PT
Somewhat agree
Disagree
RI
2. I found the system very difficult to use
SC
Agree
U
Somewhat agree AN
Disagree
3. I needed to learn a lot of things before I could get going with the system
M
Agree
D
Somewhat agree
TE
Disagree
EP
4. In which extent did you find the following parts of the iBert system useful:
Useless
Moderately useful
Very useful
Useless
44
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Moderately useful
Very useful
PT
Useless
RI
5. How useful did you find iBert overall?
SC
Useless
U
Moderately useful AN
Very useful
6. Did you pay attention to the text messages that were displayed by iBert?
M
Yes
D
No
TE
Partially
EP
Yes
C
AC
No
8. Did you take any additional energy saving actions NOT suggested by iBert during or
Yes
No
45
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Partially
9. If there are other adults living in your home, did they attend/were aware of the
Not aware
PT
Aware
RI
Partially aware
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
46
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
PT
RI
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
EP
C
AC
47
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
We investigate the effect of replacing the simple statement of energy use a
improve consumer energy literacy and suggest behaviour change through personalised
actionable messages set against a series of psychological value systems for context,
PT
The results from a carefully controlled field experiment show: 1) value framing
RI
and action prompts have a significant effect on occupants' behaviour, with the mean
temperature of homes being reduced from 22.4°C to 21.7 °C, and a marked reduction
SC
in gas consumption—22.0% overall and 27.2% in high consumers; 2) energy literacy
increasing from 0.52 to 1.28 (on a 0-4 scale); 3) it is possible to target potentially
U
profligate households, without inappropriately messaging others;
AN
Engagement is high, with 82% of the participants finding the system useful.
M
meters with the potential to increase energy literacy, make much greater savings and
TE