A Short Overview of English Syntax
A Short Overview of English Syntax
Rodney Huddleston
The University of Queensland
This paper presents a brief account of English syntax based on The Cambridge Grammar of the English
Language,[1] providing an overview of the main constructions and categories in the language. The present
version is intended primarily for members of the English Teachers' Association of Queensland (ETAQ), offering
an alternative approach to that presented in the 2007 volume of their journal Words`Worth by Lenore Ferguson
under the title `Grammar at the Coalface' - in particular the articles `The structural basics' (March 2007) and
`Functional elements in a clause' (June 2007). I make use of concepts discussed in my own Words'Worth
paper `Aspects of grammar: functions, complements and inflection' (March 2008), and take over Functional
Grammar's useful convention of distinguishing between functions and classes by using an initial capital letter
for the former: thus Subject is the name of a function, noun phrase the name of a class.
Note that such an example as We stayed at the hotel which you recommended is also a clausal sentence even
though it contains two clauses. This is because one clause, which you recommended, is part of the other,
rather than separate from it (more specifically, the which you recommended is part of the noun phrase the hotel
which you recommended); the larger clause is thus We stayed at the hotel which you recommended, and this
does constitute the whole sentence, like that in [i].
The fact that the two types of sentence are distinguished in terms of clauses implies that we take the
clause to be a more basic unit than the sentence, which reflects the fact that in speech it tends to be more
difficult to determine the boundaries between sentences than the boundaries between clauses. For most of this
overview we will focus on clauses: we return to coordination in Section14.
There are two further points that should be made at this point.
(a) In all the above examples the non-canonical clauses differ in their structure from canonical clauses, but this
is not always so. In [iiib] the subordinate clause is introduced by that but we could omit this, giving She said
they knew the victim, where the underlined clause is identical with [iiia]; nevertheless it is still subordinate and
hence non-canonical. It is subordinate by virtue of being Complement of the verb said, but the subordination
happens not to be marked in the internal grammatical structure of the clause itself.
(b) A clause is non-canonical if it lacks at least one of the above properties. It may of course lack more than
one of them. Thus Wasn't the key taken by the secretary? has three non-canonical properties: it is negative,
interrogative and passive. In the discussion below we will take the non-canonical properties in turn with the
understanding that they can combine.
Note that we use `determinative' as the name of a class and `Determiner' as the name of a function;[2] we need
to invoke the class vs function distinction here to cater for the construction illustrated in the doctor's car. Here
the doctor's has the same function, Determiner, as the in the car, but it is not a word and hence not a
determinative: as far as its class is concerned it's a noun phrase.
The above scheme differs from that of traditional grammar in three respects:
o We take pronouns to be a subclass of nouns, not a distinct primary class.
o Traditional grammars generally take our determinatives to be a subclass of adjectives, though some
recognise a class of articles consisting of the and a. Our determinative class is much larger, containing
not just the and a, but also words like some, any, all, each, every, no, etc.; these are very different from
words like those underlined in [iii].
o We have coordinator and subordinator as distinct primary classes, whereas traditional grammar has a
primary class of conjunctions subdivided into coordinating and subordinating conjunctions.
4 PHRASES
For each of the first six of the word classes in [3] there is a corresponding class of phrases whose Head
belongs to that class. In the following examples, the phrase is enclosed in brackets and the Head underlined:
In canonical clauses describing an action the Subject will be associated with the semantic role of actor, or
agent, as in [5i]. But many clauses don't express actions: we heard an explosion, for example, describes a
sensory experience, and here the Subject is associated with the role of experiencer. There are numerous
different kinds of semantic role that can be associated with the Subject: what the role is in a particular instance
will depend on the meaning of the clause, especially of the verb.
Meaning therefore does not provide a reliable way of identifying the Subject. But this function has a good
few distinctive grammatical properties which together generally make it easy to identify. Here are some of
them.
(a) Position. Its default position - the one it occupies unless there are special reasons for placing it elsewhere -
is before the Predicate.
(b) Formation of interrogatives. You can generally change a declarative clause into an interrogative by inverting
the Subject with the first auxiliary verb; if there is no auxiliary in the declarative you need to insert the
appropriate form of do.[3] In either case the Subject ends up following the auxiliary verb:
(c) Interrogative tags. To seek confirmation of a statement you can add an interrogative tag, consisting of an
auxiliary verb and a personal pronoun Subject which relates back to the Subject of the clause to which the tag
is attached: The boss is in her office, isn't she?; Everyone signed the petition, didn't they?
(d) Subject-verb agreement, Where the verb has person-number properties (in the present tense and the past
tense of be), they are normally determined by agreement with the Subject:
[7] a. Her son plays the piano. b. Her sons play the piano.
Here the lawn is admissible because the verb mow (unlike disappear, for example) allows a Dependent of this
kind, so the lawn is a Complement. But a Dependent indicating time can occur with any verb, so before it
started to rain is an Adjunct.
We will look further at Complements in the next subsection. As for Adjuncts, they are usually realised by
adverb phrases, preposition phrases, subordinate clauses, or a very narrow range of noun phrases. They can
be divided into various semantic subtypes, such as Adjuncts of time, place, manner, etc., as illustrated in [9]:
While thousands of verbs license an Object, only a fairly small number license a Predicative Complement, and
of these be is by far the most common: others include become, remain, appear, seem, etc. The term
`Predicative Complement' is most easily understood by reference to the construction with be: the verb has little
meaning here (it is often called just a `linking verb'), so that the main semantic content of the Predicate is
expressed by the Complement.
There are several grammatical properties that distinguish Objects from Predicative Complements, of
which the two most important ones are illustrated in [11]:
[11] i a. Ed blamed the minister. [Object] b. The minister was blamed by Ed.
ii a. Ed was a minister. [Pred Comp] b. *A minister was been by Ed.
iii a. Ed was innocent. [Pred Comp] b. *Ed blamed innocent.
o The Object of an active clause can usually become the Subject of a corresponding passive clause, but a
Predicative Complement never can. Thus the Object of active [ia] corresponds to the Subject of passive
[ib], whereas [iib] is not a possible passive version of [iia]. (Here and below the asterisk indicates that
what follows is ungrammatical.)
o A Predicative Complement can be realised not only by a noun phrase, as in [iia], but also by an adjective
phrase, as in [iiia], whereas an Object cannot be realised by an adjective phrase, as evident from the
ungrammaticality of [iiib].
[12] i He gave the prisoner some water. [Indirect Object (recipient) + Direct Object]
ii She baked me a cake. [Indirect Object (beneficiary) + Direct Object]
In the representations of the structures, S stands for Subject, P for Predicator, PCs for Subjective Predicative
Complement, Od for Direct Object, PCo for Objective Predicative Complement, and Oi for Indirect Object. The
names reflect the fact that there are two dimensions of contrast:
o One has to do with Objects: an intransitive clause has no Object, a monotransitive clause has a single
Object, and a ditransitive clause has two Objects.
o The other has to with Predicative Complements: if a clause contains a Predicative Complement it is
complex, otherwise ordinary, though the latter term is often omitted (as it is in [v], since there is no
possibility of adding a Predicative Complement to a ditransitive clause).
The names apply in the first instance to the clause constructions, and then derivatively to the verbs that
appear in these constructions. Thus disappear is an (ordinary) intransitive verb, be a complex-intransitive one,
and so on. But it must be borne in mind that the majority of verbs can appear in more than one of them, and
hence belong to more than one class. Find, for example, commonly appears in [iii] (We found the key), [iv] (We
found her co-operative), and [v] (We found her a job).
In the [a] examples here the underlined preposition phrase ([i-ii]) or subordinate clause ([iii-iv]) is the only
Complement, while in the [b] ones it follows an Object. We look at different kinds of subordinate clause in
Section13, but there is one point to be made here about the prepositional constructions. In [i] to contrasts with
other prepositions such as over, from, via, beyond, etc., but in [ii] on is selected by the verb: any adequate
dictionary will tell you (if only by example) that rely takes a Complement with on, consist with of, refer with to,
and so on. Verbs like these that take as Complement a preposition phrase headed by some specified
preposition are called `prepositional verbs'. Most ditransitive verbs also belong to this latter class by virtue of
licensing a preposition phrase with to or for instead of the Indirect Object: compare He gave some water to the
prisoner and She baked a cake for me with [12] above.
6 VERBS
6.1 Verb inflection
The most distinctive property of verbs is their inflection: they have a number of inflectional forms that are
It will be noticed that although we have distinguished six different inflectional forms, there are only four
different shapes: checked, checks, check and checking. By `shape' we mean the spelling or pronunciation.
Thus the preterite and past participle of the lexeme check have the same shape, as do the plain present tense
and the plain form. The same applies to all other regular verbs, i.e. verbs whose inflectional forms are
determined by general rules. But there are a good number of irregular verbs where the preterite and past
participle do not have the same shape: take, for example, has took as its preterite and taken as its past
participle.
This means that it is very easy to decide whether any particular instance of the shape check is a
preterite form or a past participle. What you need to do is ask which form of a verb like take would be needed
in the construction in question. Consider, then, the following examples:
If we substitute take for check in [i] the form we need is the past participle taken: She may have taken a break.
So this checked is likewise a past participle. And if we make the substitution in [ii] we need the preterite form
took: I'm not sure whether she took a break or not. So the checked of [ii] is the preterite form. Note that when
making the substitution you need to keep constant what precedes the verb (e.g. She may have in [i]) since this
is what determines the inflection that is required: what follows the verb is irrelevant and hence can be changed
to suit the verb you are substituting.
Let us now briefly review the six forms.
(a) Preterite. This is a type of past tense: the type where the past tense is marked inflectionally rather than by
means of an auxiliary verb. Many grammars use the more general term `past tense': we prefer the more
specific term to distinguish it from the construction where the auxiliary have marks the other kind of past tense,
as in She has checked the proofs.
(b)-(c) The present tense forms. There are two present tense forms, one which occurs with a 3rd person
singular subject, and one which occurs with any other subject: 1st person (I check), 2nd person (you check) or
plural (they check). We could call this latter form `non-3rd person singular', but `plain present' is simpler. `Plain'
indicates that it is identical with the morphological base of the lexeme, i.e. the starting-point for the rules that
produce the various inflectional forms by adding a suffix, changing the vowel, and so on.
(d) The plain form. This is also identical with the base, but it is not a present tense form. It is used in three
constructions:
The infinitival construction is very often marked by to, but it is also found without to after such verbs as can,
may, will, do (She didn't check the figures herself), make (They made me check the figures myself), etc. The
(e) The gerund-participle. This form always ends with the suffix @ing. Traditional grammar distinguishes two
forms with this suffix, the gerund and the present participle:
The idea was that a gerund is comparable to a noun, while a participle is comparable to an adjective. Thus in
[i] checking the figures is comparable to such checks, where checks is a noun; in [ii] checking the figures is
Modifier to people and was therefore considered adjective-like since the most common type of Modifier to a
noun is an adjective.[4] There is, however, no verb in English that has distinct forms for the constructions in
[19], and so there is no basis for making any inflectional distinction here in Present-day English: we thus have
a single form and the name `gerund-participle' indicates that it covers both traditional categories.
(f) The past participle. This is used in two main constructions, the perfect and the passive:
The perfect is a past tense marked by the auxiliary verb have, while the most straightforward cases of the
passive involve the auxiliary verb be. We retain the traditional term `past participle', though the `past'
component of meaning applies just to the perfect construction.
o In [i] we see the basic use, indicating past time. The event of his arriving took place in the past, and the
state of her knowing him well obtained in the past (it may still obtain now, but I'm talking about some time
in the past). This is much the most frequent use, but it's important to be aware that the preterite doesn't
always have this meaning.
o Example [iia] could be used to report Ed's saying `I am ill': present tense am is shifted back to preterite
was under the influence of the preterite reporting verb said. In [iib] my original thought was `It starts
tomorrow': again present tense starts is shifted back to preterite started. This example shows very
clearly that the backshift use is not the same as the past time use, for clearly the starting is not in the
(b) The present tense. The two most important uses are seen in [22]:
[22] i Present time a. I promise I'll help you. b. She lives in Sydney.
ii Future time a. Exams start next week. b. I'll go home when it gets dark.
o In [i] we again have the basic and much the most common use: to indicate present time. In [ia] the event
of my promising is actually simultaneous with the utterance, for I perform the act of promising by saying
this sentence. In [ib] we have a state, and the present tense indicates that the state obtains at the time of
speaking.
o In [ii] the time is future. In main clauses this is possible only when the event is in some way already
scheduled, as in [iia]. But this constraint does not apply in various kinds of subordinate clause such as
we have in [iib].
[23] i Modal auxiliaries can, may, must, will, shall, ought, need, dare
ii Non-modal auxiliaries be, have, do
(Could, might, would and should are the preterite forms of can, may, will and shall respectively, though they
differ considerably from other preterites, as we shall see.)
(a) Subject-auxiliary inversion. We have seen that in canonical clauses the Subject precedes the verb whereas
in most interrogative main clauses the Subject follows the (first) verb. The verb that precedes the Subject,
however, must be an auxiliary verb: only auxiliaries can invert with the Subject. Compare:
If the declarative doesn't contain an auxiliary, as in [ib], it is necessary to insert the auxiliary do so that
inversion can apply: Did she take the car? This do has no meaning: it is simply inserted to satisfy the
grammatical rule requiring an auxiliary.
(b) Negation. The construction where not is used to negate the verb likewise requires that the verb be an
auxiliary:
Again, if there is no auxiliary in the positive, do must be inserted to form the negative: She did not take the car.
A further, related, point is that auxiliaries, but not lexical verbs, have negative forms ending in the suffix
n't: a more informal variant of [25iia] is She hasn't taken the car.
The particular type of non-finite clause that is used depends on the Head verb, whether auxiliary or lexical.
Ought and intend license infinitivals with to, can and help infinitivals without to; be, in one of its uses, and
begin license a non-finite clause with a gerund-participle form of the verb; be, in a second use, and get license
one with a past participle form of the verb.
Note, then, that the verb phrase in [iiia], say, is divided into was + checking the figures, not was checking
+ the figures, just as that in [iiib] is divided into began + checking the figures, not began checking + the figures.
And similarly with the other examples.
[27] i Progressive marker a. They are watching TV. b. I've been working all morning.
ii Passive marker a. It was taken by Jill. b. He may be arrested.
iii Copula a. She was a friend of his. b. That is very likely.
o In [i], where be is followed by a verb in the gerund-participle form, it is a marker of progressive aspect. It
generally serves to indicate that the situation - the action, event, state, or whatever - was, is or will be in
progress at the time in question.
o The clauses in [ii] are passive; [iia] is the passive counterpart of active Jill took it, the presence of be
being one of the major differences between the two forms. There is no active counterpart of [iib] because
the latter has no by phrase (cf. Section15).
o In [iii] be is the only verb, but it still behaves as an auxiliary. Thus the interrogative of [a] is Was she a
friend of his? and the negative of [b] is That isn't very likely. In these examples the auxiliary has as its
Complement not a non-finite clause but a noun phrase (a friend of his) and an adjective phrase (very
likely).
(b) Have. This verb belongs to both lexical and auxiliary classes. In She had a swim it is a lexical verb, for the
[28] i Perfect marker a. He has broken his leg. b. He may have taken it yesterday.
ii Static have a. She has enough credit. b. We have to invite them all.
o The perfect is marked by auxiliary have + a past participle. It is best regarded as a secondary past tense
- the primary past tense being the inflectional preterite. Note, for example, that the preterite is found only
in finite constructions such as He took it yesterday, so it can't occur after may (cf. *He may took it
yesterday: may takes an infinitival clause as Complement), and perfect have is then used instead, as in
[ib]. Since have itself can inflect for tense, [ia] is doubly marked for tense: it is `past in present', the past
being marked by the lexeme have and the present by the inflection on have. This reflects the fact that
while the event of his breaking his leg is located in past time it is seen as having relevance to the
present. The most likely scenario is that his leg has not yet healed, so that he is at present incapacitated.
The present tense component also explains why it is not normally possible to add an Adjunct like
yesterday: *He has broken his leg yesterday.
o Have in [ii] denotes a state, unlike that of the above She had a swim, which is dynamic, denoting an
event. Usage is divided as to whether static have is an auxiliary or a lexical verb. Those who say She
hasn't enough credit or Have we to invite them all? and the like are treating it as an auxiliary, while those
who say She doesn't have enough credit or Do we have to invite them all? are treating it as a lexical
verb. Many people use both constructions, though the lexical verb treatment has been gaining ground for
some time. Note that in [iia] have, like be in [27], doesn't have a non-finite clause as Complement.
(a) Need and dare. These are auxiliaries only when followed by an infinitival construction without to, as in
Need I bother? and I daren't tell them, etc. Thus in I need a haircut, I need to get my hair cut, I dare you to
repeat that, etc. we have lexical verbs.
Note that although We have to invite them all has essentially the same meaning as We must invite them all,
this have is not a modal auxiliary: it has none of the above three grammatical properties. It is a special case of
the static have, illustrated in [28ii], and as such it is for many speakers not an auxiliary at all, but a lexical verb.
(c) The preterite forms. Could, might, would and should are the preterite forms of can, may, will and shall
respectively, but the use of these preterites differs from that of other preterite forms in Present-day English.
o Only could and would have the basic preterite use of indicating past time: I could do it easily when I was
younger; I asked him to help but he wouldn't.
o The status of might and should as preterites is established by their use in certain conditional
constructions and in those cases of reported speech or thought where present tense forms are excluded.
Thus though we can have may in If you come back tomorrow you may find him in, we need might in If
you came back tomorrow you might find him in.[5] And if at some time in the past I had the thought `I
shall easily finish before she returns' I would report this with should, as in I knew I should easily finish
(d) Types of modal meaning. The modal auxiliaries express a considerable variety of meanings, but they can
be grouped into three major types.
o Epistemic modality. Here we are concerned with what is necessary, likely or possible: He must have
overslept; Dinner should be ready in a few minutes; She may be ill.
o Deontic modality. Here it is a matter of what is required or permitted: You must work harder; You should
be studying for your exam; You can/may go with them if you like.
o Dynamic modality. Here it is a question of properties or dispositions of persons or other entities involved
in the situation: She can speak very persuasively (ability), Will you help me? (willingness). This kind of
meaning is mainly found with just can, will and dare.
In some cases there is a clear ambiguity as to which type of meaning is intended. You must be very tactful, for
example, can be interpreted epistemically (I'm inferring from evidence that you are very tactful) or deontically
(I'm telling you to be very tactful). She can't be serious may be understood epistemically (She is obviously not
being serious) or dynamically (She is unable to be serious).
(a) Inflection. Nouns generally exhibit inflectional contrasts of number and case:
School grammars commonly use the term `possessive' instead of `genitive', but that term is far too specific for
the wide range of relationships covered by this case: compare, for example, Kim's parents, the boys'
behaviour, the train's arrival, the mayor's obituary, the sun's rays, today's news.
(b) Function. Nouns can function as Head in noun phrases that in turn function as Subject or Complement in
clause structure, or Complement of a preposition, as illustrated in [30], where nouns are underlined and noun
phrases bracketed:
(c) Dependents. There are some kinds of Dependent that occur exclusively (or almost exclusively) with a noun
as Head:
[31] i Certain determinatives the student, a school, every book, which exam
ii Pre-head adjectives. mature students, a new book, an easy exam
iii Relative clauses the student who directed the play, a book I'm reading
(a) Determiners. These are found uniquely in the structure of noun phrases. They have the form of
determinatives (or determinative phrases, as in almost all students, not many people, too few volunteers) or
genitive noun phrases (the girl's voice, some people's behaviour, my book).
[32] i Definite the Premier of NSW, the key, this book, both copies, the man's death
ii Indefinite a politician, some keys, any serious book, enough copies, three dogs
We use a definite noun phrase when we assume that its content is sufficient, in the context, to identify the
referent. There's only one (current) Premier of NSW, so the definiteness in the first example is unproblematic,
but with the second example there is of course very heavy reliance on context to make the referent clear. The
is a pure marker of definiteness, known as the definite article. Its use effectively pre-empts a which question:
if I say Where's the key? I assume you won't need to ask Which key? Note that a genitive Determiner confers
definiteness on the noun phrase: the man's death means `the death of the man', and a man's death likewise
means `the death of a man'. Noun phrases like black coffee and friends, which have a common noun as Head
and no Determiner are normally indefinite.
(b) Complements. The clearest cases of Complements involve preposition phrases where the preposition is
specified by the Head noun, and certain types of subordinate clause:
[33] i Preposition phrases her review of the play, a ban on alcohol, his marriage to Sue
ii Subordinate clauses the idea that he might be ill, an opportunity to make friends
Note that nouns, unlike verbs, do not take Objects: we say She reviewed the play, but not *her review the play;
instead we need of the play. With ban and marriage the prepositions required are on and to. The subordinate
clauses in [ii] clearly satisfy the licensing test: only a fairly narrow range of nouns can take Complements like
these.
(c) Modifiers. The typical pre-Head Modifier is an adjective or adjective phrase: a good book, a very serious
matter. But those are not the only possibilities. In particular, nouns can also function as Modifier to a Head
noun: a school play, the unemployment situation, etc. Post-Head Modifiers are typically preposition phrases
and subordinate clauses that occur more freely than Complements in that they do not have to be licensed by
the Head noun: a man of honour, the house opposite the post office, the play that she wrote, the guy who
spoke first.
It is also possible to have Modifiers that precede the Determiner: all the books, both these plays, too
small a car for our needs. Note that adverbs can occur in this position, but not after the Determiner: absolutely
the best solution, but not *an absolutely success. Instead of the latter we need an adjective, an absolute
success.
[34] i Singular-only nouns crockery, dross, harm, nonsense; news, mumps, physics, ...
ii Plural-only nouns belongings, clothes, genitals, scissors; cattle, police, ...
Note that the last three items in [i] end in @s but are nevertheless singular, as evident, for example, from the
agreement in This news is good. Conversely, the last two items in [ii] don't end in @s, but are nevertheless
plural: cf. These cattle are in good health.
However, most nouns can occur with either a count or a non-count interpretation:
The interpretations in [a] allow for a contrast between one and more than one (cf., for example, He pulled out
two white hairs), but those in [b] do not. When we speak of count and non-count nouns, therefore, we are
referring to nouns as used with a count and non-count interpretation. Thus hair is a count noun in [ia], a non-
count noun in [ib], and so on.
(c) Subject-verb agreement. We noted in Section5.1 that where a verb has person-number properties they
normally agree with those of the Subject noun phrase, more particularly with those of the Head noun of that
noun phrase: The dog is barking vs The dogs are barking. There are, however, certain semantically-motivated
types of departure from this pattern, as illustrated in [36]:
[36] i Measure expressions Two hours isn't long enough for such a job.
ii Quantificational nouns A lot of people like it.
iii Collective nouns The jury haven't yet reached a decision.
o In [i] the hours aren't thought of individually but as making up a single period, so the Subject is treated as
singular.
o In [ii] the verb-form is determined not by the Head noun lot but by people, which is embedded within the
Subject noun phrase.
o With collective nouns like jury in [iii] there is divided usage, with singular hasn't also used.
(a) Proper nouns. This subclass includes nouns such as John, Mary, Smith, Beethoven, Sydney, Egypt, Nile,
Easter, Friday, etc. They characteristically function as Head of noun phrases serving as proper names, names
individually assigned to particular people, places, festivals, days of the week, and so on. Note, however, that
they also occur, derivatively, in other kinds of noun phrase: That's not the Smith I was referring to, Let's listen
to some Beethoven. Conversely, not all proper names contain proper nouns: cf. Central Avenue, New Year's
Day, and so on. And some proper names contain more than just a proper noun: the Nile, Mt Everest, King
John.
(b) Pronouns. The grammatically distinctive property of pronouns is that they do not normally combine with
Determiners: He arrived, not *The he arrived. There are several subtypes of pronoun, including:
[37] i Personal pronouns I, we, you, he, she, it, they, one
ii Reciprocal pronouns each other, one another
iii Interrogative or relative pronouns who, what, which, whoever, etc.
We will comment here on only the first of these categories. Personal pronouns are those where we find
contrasts of person. I and we are first person, used to refer to the speaker or a group containing the speaker.
(`Speaker' is to be understood as covering the writer in written texts.) You is second person, used to refer to
[38] i Nominative I, we, you, ... I did it. It was I who did it.
ii Accusative me, us, you, ... It bit me. It was me who did it.
iii Dependent genitive my, our, your, ... My son is here. I saw your car.
iv Independent genitive mine, ours, yours, ... Mine was broken. That's mine.
v Reflexive myself, ourselves, ... I hurt myself. We talk to ourselves.
Nominatives occur mostly as Head of a Subject noun phrase. In formal style they can also occur in certain
types of Predicative Complement, with the accusative as a less formal variant: It was I/me who did it. In other
types, however, only the accusative is possible: The victim was me, not *The victim was I, and the like.
Dependent genitives occur when there is a following Head in the noun phrase, independent ones when there
isn't. Reflexives usually relate back to the Subject noun phrase, as in the above examples.
[39] i Attributive a hot day, some new DVDs, this excellent play, lonely people
ii Predicative It's hot. These look new. I found it excellent. They seem lonely.
Attributive adjectives are pre-head Modifiers in noun phrase structure; predicative adjectives are Predicative
Complements in clause structure (see Section5.5).[6]
There are, however, some adjectives that are restricted to one or other of these functions:
[40] i Attributive-only the main speaker, a mere child, the only problem, my own car
ii Never-attributive I'm afraid. She's asleep. He looks content. It's liable to flood.
[41] i Degree modification very good, quite hot, rather young, too old, incredibly bad
ii Inflection for grade hotter, younger, older, better; hottest, youngest, oldest, best
Gradable adjectives that don't inflect mark comparative and superlative degree by means of the adverbs more
and most respectively: more intelligent, most intelligent.
There are also a good number of adjectives that denote non-scalar properties and hence are non-
gradable: alphabetical order, the chief difficulty, the federal government, her right eye, third place. Some
adjectives, moreover, can be used in two different senses, one gradable, the other non-gradable (and usually
the more basic). In The door is open, for example, open is non-gradable, but in You should be more open with
us it is gradable.
[42] i Complements good at chess, grateful for your help, fond of animals, keen on golf,
glad that you liked it, unsure what had happened, eager to help
ii Modifiers very bad, morally wrong, this good, most useful, much better, two
The Complements are preposition phrases or subordinate clauses; in the former case the adjective selects a
particular preposition to head the Complement: fond takes of, keen takes on, and so on. The Modifiers are
adverbs (e.g. very), determinatives (this), noun phrases (two days) or post-Head prepositional phrases.
Adjective phrases containing post-Head Dependents cannot normally be used attributively: He's good at
chess, but not *a good at chess schoolboy.
In general adverbs that can modify adjectives and other adverbs can also modify verbs, but there are some
exceptions, most notably very and too (in the sense `excessively'). Compare He's very FOND of her and *He
very LOVES her (we need He loves her very MUCH).
A few adverbs inflect for grade (soon, sooner, soonest), but for the most part comparatives and
superlatives are marked by more and most: more carefully, most carefully.
[44] i Complements Luckily for me, it rained. We handled it similarly to the others.
ii Modifiers She sang very well. It won't end that soon. We left a bit late.
(a) Function of prepositions. Prepositions function as Head in preposition phrases, and these in turn function
as Dependent (Complement or Modifier) to any of the four major parts of speech:
(b) Complements of prepositions. Usually (as in all the examples in [45]) prepositions take a noun phrase as
Complement. There are, however, other possibilities:
[46] i Preposition phrase He emerged [from under the bed. I'll stay [until after lunch].
ii Adjective phrase That strikes me [as unfair]. I took him [for dead].
iii Adverb phrase I didn't know [until recently ]. I can't stay [for long].
iv Clause It depends [on what she says]. I told her [before she left].[7]
(c) Preposition stranding. In a number of clause constructions the Complement of a preposition is placed at the
front of the clause or omitted altogether, leaving the preposition `stranded':
[47] i a. What are you looking at? b. It's something [which I can do without].
ii a. This is the book [I was referring to]. b. He went to the same school as [I went to].
The construction is characteristic of relatively informal style, but it is a serious mistake to say that it is
grammatically incorrect.
11 NEGATION
(a) Clausal vs subclausal negation. Negation is marked by individual words such as not, no, never, or by
affixes such as we have in uncommon, non-compliant, infrequent, careless, isn't, won't, etc. We need to
distinguish, however, between cases where the negative affects the whole clause (clausal negation) and
those where it affects just a part of it (subclausal negation):
The clauses in [i] are negative, but those in [ii] are positive even though they contain a negative element within
them. We say this because they behave like obviously positive clauses with respect to the constructions shown
in [49]:
o In [a] we have a clause followed by an interrogative `tag' used to seek confirmation of what has been
said. The usual type of tag reverses the `polarity' of the clause to which it is attached - that is, it is
negative if attached to a positive clause, as in [ia], and positive if attached to a negative clause, as in
[iia]. And we see from [iiia], therefore, that He is unwell counts as positive since the tag is negative: the
clause is no more negative than He is sick.
o In the [b] examples we have added a truncated clause introduced by and so or and nor. We get and so
after a positive clause and and nor after a negative one. And Not surprisingly, he was ill is shown to be a
positive clause because it takes and so.
(b) Non-affirmative items. There are a number of words or expressions that occur readily in negative or
interrogative clauses but generally not in positive declaratives. Compare:
Instead of [iia] we say He found some cracks. Such items as any in [50] are called non-affirmative (with
`affirmative' understood as combining declarative and positive). They include compounds with any@, such as
anybody, anyone, anything, etc., at all, either, ever, yet, budge, can bear, can stand, give a damn, lift a finger,
etc. More precisely, these are non-affirmative in at least one of their senses: some of them also have senses in
which they can occur in affirmative constructions. The any series of words, for example, can occur in
affirmative constructions when the meaning is close to `every', as in Anyone can do that.
We use different terms for the clause types than for the speech acts because the relation between the two sets
of categories is by no means one-to-one. Consider such examples as [52]:
Grammatically, [i] is declarative, but it would be used as a question: a question can be marked by rising
intonation (or by punctuation) rather than by the grammatical structure. Example [ii] is likewise declarative but
again it would be used as a question (perhaps in a court cross-examination): the question force this time
comes from the verb ask, in the present tense with a 1st person Subject. Promise in [iii] works in the same
way: this example would generally be used to make a promise. This illustrates the point that although we have
just a handful of different clause types there are a great many different kinds of speech act: one can apologise,
offer, congratulate, beseech, declare a meeting open, and so on. Finally, [iv] is a closed interrogative but would
characteristically be used to make a request. In this use it is what is called an indirect speech act: although it
is literally a question it actually conveys something else, a polite request.
All canonical clauses are declarative and we need say no more about this type, but a few comments are
in order for the remaining four types.
(a) Closed interrogatives. These are so called because they are typically used to ask questions with a closed
set of answers. Usually these are Yes and No (or their equivalents), but in examples like Is it a boy or a girl?
they derive from the terms joined by or: It's a boy and It's a girl. Grammatically they are marked by Subject-
auxiliary inversion (though such inversion is not restricted to interrogatives: in the declarative Never had I felt
so embarrassed it is triggered by the initial placement of the negative never).
(b) Open interrogatives. These are typically used to ask questions with an open set of answers (e.g. very,
quite, slightly, etc. in the case of [51iii]). They are marked by the presence of an interrogative phrase consisting
of or containing a so-called `wh-word': who, what, when, where, how, etc. This phrase may be Subject (Who
said that?), Complement (What do you want?) or Adjunct (When did he leave?). If it is Complement or Adjunct
it normally occurs at the beginning of the clause, which has Subject-auxiliary inversion, as in the last two
examples. It is possible, however, for it to remain in post-verbal position, as in And after that you went where?
(c) Exclamatives. These have, at the front of the clause, an exclamative phrase containing either how, as in
[51iv], or what, as in What a fool I've been!
(d) Imperatives. The most common type of imperative has you understood, as in [51v], or expressed as
Subject (as in You be careful; Don't you speak to me like that). The verb is in the plain form, but do is used in
the negative: Don't move. We also have 3rd person imperatives like Somebody open the window,
distinguished from the declarative precisely by the plain form verb. 1st person plural imperatives are marked by
let's: Let's go!, Don't let's bother.
13 SUBORDINATE CLAUSES
Subordinate clauses normally function in the structure of a phrase or a larger clause. Whereas main clauses
are almost invariably finite, subordinate clauses may be finite or non-finite.
[53] i She says that he is kept well-informed [tensed: is is present tense verb]
ii She insists that he be kept well-informed [subjunctive: be is plain form]
Subjunctive is thus the name of a syntactic construction, not an inflectional category, as in traditional grammar.
It has a plain form verb and when the Subject is a personal pronoun it appears in nominative case.
We distinguish three main types of finite subordinate clause: content clauses, relative clauses and
comparative clauses.
Like main clauses they select for clause type, except that there are no subordinate imperatives:
o Declaratives are often marked by the subordinator that; and since that occurs in both the tensed clause
and the subjunctive in [53] we include both in the declarative class.
o Closed interrogatives have whether or if instead of the Subject-auxiliary inversion found in main clauses
(compare the main clause counterpart of the subordinate clause in [ii]: Did everybody support the
proposal?).
o Open interrogatives have the interrogative phrase in initial position and normally no Subject-auxiliary
inversion (again compare the main clause counterpart of that in [iii]: Which proposal did everybody
support?).
o Exclamatives mostly have the same form as their main clause counterparts, as with [iv].
[56] i a. I agree with [the guy who spoke last]. b. I agree with [the guy that spoke last].
ii a. He lost [the key which I lent him]. b. He lost [the key I lent him].
Such clauses contain an overt or covert element which relates back to the Head noun, so we understand in [i]
that some guy spoke last and in [ii] that I lent him a key. This `relativised element' is overt in [ia] (the relative
pronoun who) and [iia] (which), but covert in the [b] examples. This is obvious in the case of [iib], and in [ib]
that, although traditionally classified as a relative pronoun, is better regarded as a subordinator, the same one
as is found in declarative content clauses like [55i]; on this analysis there is no overt relativised element in [ib]
any more than in [iib].
The relativised element can have a variety of functions in the relative clause: in [56i] it is Subject, in [56ii]
Object, and so on.
(b) Supplementary relative clauses. The relative clauses in [56] are tightly integrated into the structure of the
sentence, but it is also possible for relative clauses to be set off by punctuation or intonation, so that they have
the status of more loosely attached Supplements, as in:
[57] i I've lent the car to my brother, who has just come over from New Zealand.
ii He overslept again, which made him miss the train.
In this type the relativised element is almost always overt, and doesn't relate back to a noun but to a larger
unit, a whole noun phrase in [i] (my brother) and a clause in [ii], where which is understood as `(the fact) that
he overslept again'.
(c) The fused relative construction. This is structurally more complex than the above constructions:
[58] i a. Whoever wrote this must be very naive. b. You can invite who you like.
ii a. He quickly spent what she gave him. b. What books he has are in the attic.
The underlined sequences here are not themselves clauses but noun phrases: clauses don't denote entities
that can be naive or be invited or spent or located in the attic. Note, moreover, that are in [iib] agrees with a
plural noun phrase Subject, whereas Subjects with the form of clauses take 3rd person singular verbs, as in
[54i]. Whoever in [58ia] is equivalent to the person who and what in [iia] to that which, and so on. This is why
we call this construction `fused': the Head of the noun phrase and the relativised element are fused together,
instead of being separate, as in [56ia/iia].
These constructions may look superficially like open interrogative content clauses. Compare [58iib], for
example, with I asked her what she gave him. The meaning is quite different: the latter, where the underlined
clause is interrogative, can be glossed as `I asked her the answer to the question, `What did she give him?'',
but there is no such question meaning in [58iia]. Similarly compare [58iib], meaning `The (few) books he has
are in the attic', with What books he has is unknown, where the underlined clause is interrogative and the
meaning is `The answer to the question `What books does he have?' is unknown'; note that this time the main
clause verb is singular is, agreeing with the clausal Subject.
[59] i a. I'm as ready as I ever will be. b. As was expected, Sue won easily.
ii a. More people came than I'd expected. b. He has more vices than he has virtues.
The distinctive property of such clauses is that they are structurally incomplete relative to main clauses: there
are elements understood but not overtly expressed. In [ia] and [iia] there's a missing Complement and in [ib] a
missing Subject. Even in [iib] there's a missing Dependent in the Object noun phrase, for the comparison is
Infinitivals contain a plain form of the verb, with or without the special marker to; gerund-participials and past-
participials have verbs in the gerund-participle and past participle forms; for further examples, see [26] above.
Most non-finite clauses have no overt Subject, but all three kinds allow one under certain conditions.
o In infinitivals, it occurs in the to-variant with initial for as subordinator: For them to be so late is very
unusual.
o In gerund-participials a personal pronoun Subject usually appears in accusative case, but genitives are
found in relatively formal style: We objected to them/their being given extra privileges.
o Example [iiia] is a past-participial with an overt Subject.
Infinitivals are much the most frequent of the three classes of non-finite clause, and appear in a very wide
range of functions. These include Subject (To err is human), Complement of a verb (as in [60ia/b]: the Head
verb determines whether to is included), Complement of a noun (I applaud [her willingness to compromise]),
Complement of an adjective (She's [willing to compromise]), Adjunct (She walks to work to keep fit), Modifier of
a noun (I need [an album to keep the photos in]). In general, prepositions take gerund-participials rather than
infinitivals as Complement (He left [without saying good-bye]), but the compound in order and so as are
exceptions (She stayed at home [in order to study for the exam]).
14 COORDINATION
Coordination is a relation between two or more items of equal syntactic status, the coordinates. They are of
equal status in the sense that one is not a Dependent of another.
(a) The marking of coordination. Coordination is usually but not invariably marked by the presence of a
coordinator, such as and, or, nor, but; the first three of these may also be paired with a determinative, both,
either and neither respectively. The main patterns are seen in [61]:
Examples [i]-[iii] illustrate the most usual case: a coordinator in the last coordinate. In [iv] there is a coordinator
in all non-initial coordinates, in [v] a determinative in the first, and in [vi] no overt marking of coordination at all.
(b) Functional likeness required between coordinates. Coordination can appear at more or less any place in
the structure of sentences. You can have coordination between main clauses (giving a compound sentence, as
in [61i]), between subordinate clauses, between phrases, between words (e.g. Have you seen my father and
mother?). But the coordinates need to be grammatically alike. Usually they belong to the same class, as in all
the examples in [61]. They do not have to be, however: the crucial constraint is that they be alike in function.
Compare, then:
o In [i] we have coordination between an adjective phrase and a noun phrase, and in [ii] between a noun
phrase and a subordinate clause (an open interrogative content clause). These are acceptable because
each coordinate could stand on its own with the same function: in She is very bright and She is a good
leader the underlined units are both Predicative Complements, and in I don't know the cause of the
accident and I don't know how much damage was done they are both Complements.
o But [iii] is unacceptable, even though the coordinates are of the same class, noun phrase, because the
functional likeness condition is not met. The function of Rome in We're leaving Rome is Complement,
whereas that of next week in We're leaving next week is Adjunct.
[63] a. Sam and Pat are a happy couple. b. Sam Pat and Alex like each other.
What is distinctive about this type is that the properties concerned, being a happy couple and liking each other,
apply to the coordinates jointly rather than separately. So we can't say *Sam is a happy couple or *Pat likes
each other. The functional likeness in this type is that the coordinates denote members of a set to which the
relevant property applies. The construction is more restricted than the type illustrated in [61] in that it excludes
determinatives (*Both Sam and Pat are a happy couple), doesn't allow but as coordinator, and does require
likeness of class between the ccoordinates.
15 INFORMATION PACKAGING
The grammar of the clause makes available a number of constructions that enable us to express a given core
meaning in different ways depending on how we wish to to present or `package' the information. For example,
Kim broke the vase, The vase was broken by Kim, The vase Kim broke, It was Kim who broke the vase, What
Kim broke was the vase all have the same core meaning in the sense that there is no situation or context in
which one of them would be true and another false (assuming of course that we are talking of the same Kim
and the same vase). The first of them, Kim broke the vase, is the syntactically most basic, while the others
belong to various information-packaging constructions. The most important of these constructions are
illustrated by the underlined examples in [64]:
In the first three we are concerned simply with the order of elements, while the others involve more
radical changes.
o The basic position for the Complement this one in [i] is after the verb, but in [a] it is preposed, placed at
the front of the clause.
o In [ii] the basic position for the Object, the only copy that has been corrected, is just after the verb but
long or complex elements like this can be postposed, placed at the end.
o In [iii] the positions of the Subject and Complement of the basic version [b] are reversed in the inversion
construction [a]. (More precisely, this is Subject-Dependent inversion, in contrast to the Subject-auxiliary
inversion construction discussed earlier. The Dependent is usually a Complement but can also be an
There are two further comments that should be made about these constructions.
(a) Basic counterpart need not be canonical. For convenience we have chosen examples in [64] where the
basic counterparts are all canonical clauses, but of course they do not need to be. The basic (active)
counterpart of passive Was the car driven by Kim? is Did Kim drive the car?, which is non-canonical by virtue
of being interrogative. Likewise the non-cleft counterpart of It was Sue who had been interviewed by the police
is Sue had been interviewed by the police, which is non-canonical by virtue of being passive: note then that
certain combinations of the information-packaging constructions are possible.
(b) The information-packaging construction may be the only option. The second point is that under certain
circumstances what one would expect to be the basic counterpart is in fact ungrammatical. Thus we can say
There was an accident, but not *An accident was: here the existential construction is the only option. One
difference between actives and passives is that the by phrase of the passive is an optional element whereas
the element that corresponds to it in the active, namely the Subject, is generally obligatory in finite clauses.
Compare, then:
[65] i Passive a. Some mistakes were made by Ed. b. Some mistakes were made.
ii Active a. Ed made some mistakes. b. *Made some mistakes.
Passives like [ib] - called short passives - thus have no active counterpart. They are in fact the more common
type of passive, allowing information to be omitted that would have to be expressed in the active construction.
[1] Written by Rodney Huddleston & Geoffrey K. Pullum in collaboration with a team of thirteen linguists and
published by Cambridge University Press in 2002. A shorter version, designed as an undergraduate textbook,
appeared in 2005 as A Student's Introduction to English Grammar. I am grateful to Geoff Pullum and Anne
Horan for comments on an earlier draft of this paper.
[2] In my Words'Worth paper I relied simply on the convention of upper vs lower case initial to distinguish
`determiner' as a class term and `Determiner' as a function term.
[3] We use bold italics for lexemes; lexeme is a more abstract concept than word as it ignores inflection, so that do,
does, did, etc. are all forms of a single lexeme, do.
[4] Traditional grammar also classifies as participles verb-forms which it regards (mistakenly, in our view) as part
[5] In fact there is divided usage here, and some speakers do allow may in this construction. For them it would
seem that may and might are no longer treated as present and preterite forms of a single lexeme, but as present
tense forms of distinct lexemes.
[6] This is the standard terminology, but note that Functional Grammar uses `Attribute' for the most common type
of Predicative Complement.
[7] In traditional grammar it is not she left but before she left that is analysed as a clause, with before being here a
subordinating conjunction rather than a preposition. We present arguments in favour of our analysis on pp.
1011-14 and 129-30 respectively of the two books mentioned in footnote 1. We also depart from traditional
grammar in treating words like those underlined in Come in or I fell off as prepositions rather than adverbs.