Positive Emotions Trigger Upward Spirals Toward Emotional Well-Being
Positive Emotions Trigger Upward Spirals Toward Emotional Well-Being
Positive Emotions Trigger Upward Spirals Toward Emotional Well-Being
http://pss.sagepub.com/
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Additional services and information for Psychological Science can be found at:
Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav
Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Research Report
POSITIVE EMOTIONS TRIGGER UPWARD SPIRALS TOWARD
EMOTIONAL WELL-BEING
Barbara L. Fredrickson1 and Thomas Joiner2
1
University of Michigan and 2Florida State University
Abstract—The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions pre- focus. Negative states—like anxiety, depression, and failure—predict
dicts that positive emotions broaden the scopes of attention and cogni- local biases consistent with narrowed attention, whereas positive states—
tion, and, by consequence, initiate upward spirals toward increasing like subjective well-being, optimism, and success—predict global bi-
emotional well-being. The present study assessed this prediction by ases consistent with broadened attention (Basso, Schefft, Ris, & Dem-
testing whether positive affect and broad-minded coping reciprocally ber, 1996; Derryberry & Tucker, 1994). Other experiments have shown
and prospectively predict one another. One hundred thirty-eight col- that positive emotions produce patterns of thought that are notably un-
lege students completed self-report measures of affect and coping at usual, flexible, creative, and receptive (Isen, 1987). In general terms,
two assessment periods 5 weeks apart. As hypothesized, regression positive emotions “enlarge” the cognitive context (Isen, 1987), an ef-
analyses showed that initial positive affect, but not negative affect, fect linked to increases in brain dopamine (Ashby, Isen, & Turken,
predicted improved broad-minded coping, and initial broad-minded 1999).
coping predicted increased positive affect, but not reductions in nega- If positive emotions broaden attention and cognition, enabling flex-
tive affect. Further mediational analyses showed that positive affect ible and creative thinking, they should also facilitate coping with
and broad-minded coping serially enhanced one another. These find- stress and adversity (Aspinwall, 1998). Indeed, people who experi-
ings provide prospective evidence to support the prediction that posi- ence positive emotions during bereavement tend to develop long-term
tive emotions initiate upward spirals toward enhanced emotional well- plans and goals. Together with positive emotions, plans and goals pre-
being. Implications for clinical practice and health promotion are dis- dict greater well-being 12 months postbereavement (Stein, Folkman,
cussed. Trabasso, & Richards, 1997). One way people experience positive
emotions in the face of adversity is by finding positive meaning in or-
dinary events and within the adversity itself (Folkman & Moskowitz,
Positive emotions feel good. Plus, the balance of people’s positive
2000; Fredrickson, 2000). Finding positive meaning also predicts in-
and negative emotions contributes to judgments of life satisfaction
creases in well-being and health (Davis, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Larson,
(Diener & Larsen, 1993). Are these the only reasons people should
1998). The relation between positive meaning and positive emotions is
care about positive emotions? We think not. We propose that positive
considered reciprocal: Not only does finding positive meaning trigger
emotions not only feel good in the present, but also increase the likeli-
positive emotion, but also positive emotions—because they broaden
hood that one will feel good in the future. That is, we suggest that pos-
thinking—increase the likelihood of finding positive meaning in sub-
itive emotions trigger upward spirals toward enhanced emotional well-
sequent events (Fredrickson, 2000).
being.
These suspected reciprocal relations among positive emotions,
This prediction stems from a new perspective on positive emotions
broadened thinking, and positive meaning underlie our prediction that
offered within Fredrickson’s (1998, 2001) broaden-and-build theory.
positive emotions trigger upward spirals. That is, the effects of posi-
This model posits that, unlike negative emotions, which narrow
tive emotions should accumulate and compound: The broadened at-
people’s thought-action repertoires (e.g., fight or flight), positive emo-
tention and cognition triggered by earlier experiences of positive
tions broaden people’s thought-action repertoires, encouraging them
emotion should facilitate coping with adversity, and this improved
to discover novel lines of thought or action. Joy, for instance, creates
coping should in turn predict future experiences of positive emotion.
the urge to play, interest creates the urge to explore, and so on. A
As this cycle continues, people build their psychological resilience
key, incidental outcome of these broadened mind-sets is an increase in
and enhance their emotional well-being.
personal resources: As individuals discover new ideas and actions,
We tested these predictions by assessing affect and coping at two
they build their physical, intellectual, social, and psychological re-
times. We spotlighted positive affect (PA) and broad-minded coping,
sources. Play, for instance, builds physical, socioemotional, and in-
characterized by taking a broad perspective on problems and generat-
tellectual skills, and fuels brain development. Similarly, exploration
ing multiple possible solutions to them. Our first hypothesis was that
increases knowledge and psychological complexity (Fredrickson, 1998,
initial experiences of PA predict improvements in broad-minded cop-
2000).
ing over time. Our second hypothesis was that initial levels of broad-
Key to our proposal that positive emotions trigger upward spirals is
minded coping predict increases in PA over time. We also tested
the proposition that positive emotions broaden attention and cogni-
whether similar relations are linked to negative affect (NA), and pre-
tion. Evidence supporting this claim comes from studies that use glo-
dicted the relations are specific to PA. As another test of specificity, we
bal-local visual processing paradigms to assess biases in attentional
tested whether similar relations with PA (and not NA) emerge for
other coping styles. Our third and fourth hypotheses concerned up-
ward spiral relations directly. Our third hypothesis was that initial PA
predicts subsequent PA, in part through changes in broad-minded cop-
Address correspondence to Barbara L. Fredrickson, Department of Psy- ing. Similarly, our fourth hypothesis was that initial broad-minded
chology, University of Michigan, 525 East University Ave., Ann Arbor, MI coping predicts subsequent broad-minded coping, in part through
48109-1109; e-mail: blf@umich.edu. changes in PA. Support for these hypotheses would suggest that PA
172 Copyright © 2002 American Psychological Society VOL. 13, NO. 2, MARCH 2002
Order t for
of entry Predictors F for within-set Partial Model
of set in set set predictors df correlation R2
Dependent variable ! Time 2 broad-minded coping
1 Time 1 coping 71.48* 8.46* 1, 136 .59 .34
2 Main effects 2.99 2, 134 .24 .37
Time 1 PA 2.25* 134 .19
Time 1 NA #0.93 134 #.08
Dependent variable ! Time 2 PA
1 Time 1 PA 39.69* 6.30* 1, 136 .48 .23
2 Time 1 coping 14.65* 3.83* 1, 135 .32 .31
Dependent variable ! Time 2 NA
1 Time 1 NA 26.44* 5.14* 1, 136 .40 .16
2 Time 1 coping 0.66 0.82 1, 135 .07 .17
Note. Coping ! Cognitive Analysis subscale of the Coping Responses Inventory (Moos, 1988), our index
of broad-minded coping; PA ! Positive Affect subscale of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson,
Clark, & Tellegen, 1988); NA ! Negative Affect subscale of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule.
*p " .05.
our fourth hypothesis was that T1 broad-minded coping would predict Step 4 determines whether complete or partial mediation has oc-
T2 broad-minded coping, partly as a function of predicting increased curred; complete mediation is indicated by the effect of the predictor
PA. In each case, supportive findings would suggest an upward spiral, on the outcome being completely removed when the mediator is con-
in which PA and broad-minded coping serially enhance one another. trolled. If Steps 1 through 3 are satisfied, but Step 4 is not, partial me-
To test these hypotheses, we used mediational analyses. Kenny, diation is indicated. In both cases, partial mediation was indicated.
Kashy, and Bolger (1998) described four steps to determine whether That is, T1 PA remained a significant predictor of T2 PA when
mediation occurs. Step 1 is to show a significant correlation between changes in broad-minded coping were controlled (pr ! .45), t(135) !
predictor and outcome (here, between T1 and T2 PA, and between T1 5.82, p " .05. Similarly, T1 broad-minded coping remained a signifi-
and T2 broad-minded coping). Table 1 shows that this step is satisfied. cant predictor of T2 broad-minded coping when changes in PA were
Step 2 is to show a significant correlation between the predictor and controlled (pr ! .54), t(135) ! 7.37, p " .05.
the mediator (here, between T1 PA and changes in broad-minded cop- Last, we calculated the indirect effect sizes of the predictors
ing, and between T1 broad-minded coping and changes in PA). As we through the mediators. The strength of the indirect effect of T1 PA on
have already discussed, this step is also satisfied (see Table 1). T2 PA through changes in broad-minded coping was .16, t(135) !
Steps 3 and 4 are accomplished with one regression analysis, with 3.28, p " .05. The strength of the indirect effect of T1 broad-minded
the outcome as dependent variable and with the mediator and predic- coping on T2 broad-minded coping through changes in PA was .13,
tor entered simultaneously as independent variables. Step 3 is to show t(134) ! 2.77, p " .05.
that the mediator affects the outcome, controlling for the predictor Taken together, this sequence of findings indicates that PA predicts
(i.e., changes in broad-minded coping relate to T2 PA, controlling for itself partly via broad-minded coping, and that broad-minded coping
T1 PA, and changes in PA relate to T2 broad-minded coping, control- predicts itself partly via PA. PA and broad-minded coping, then, mutu-
ling for T1 broad-minded coping). For this step, we created variables ally build on one another.
corresponding to changes in broad-minded coping (the residual of
T2 broad-minded coping regressed on T1 broad-minded coping) and
changes in PA (the residual of T2 PA regressed on T1 PA). Next, we DISCUSSION
computed two regression equations. In the first, with T2 PA as the de- Drawing from the broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 1998,
pendent variable, T1 PA and the variable reflecting changes in broad- 2001), we predicted that positive emotions trigger upward spirals to-
minded coping were entered simultaneously as predictors. Step 3 was ward enhanced emotional well-being. Data from two assessment
satisfied: Changes in broad-minded coping were associated with T2 waves supported our four hypotheses and demonstrated specificity to
PA, controlling for T1 PA (pr ! .20), t(135) ! 2.35, p " .05. In the PA and broad-minded coping. Taken as a whole, our results support
second equation, with T2 broad-minded coping as the dependent vari- the upward-spiral prediction: Positive emotions—through their effects
able, T1 broad-minded coping and the variable reflecting changes in on broadened thinking—predict future increases in positive emotions.
PA were entered simultaneously as predictors. Again Step 3 was satis- Contrast these findings to the downward spirals often associated
fied: Changes in PA were associated with T2 broad-minded coping, with depressed affect. Depressed affect and the narrowed, pessimistic
controlling for T1 broad-minded coping (pr ! .21), t(135) ! 2.44, thinking it engenders influence one another reciprocally, leading to
p " .05. ever-worsening negative emotions, and even clinical levels of depres-
sion (Peterson & Seligman, 1984). In contrast, we have shown that Aspinwall, L.G. (2001). Dealing with adversity: Self-regulation, coping, adaptation, and
health. In A. Tesser & N. Schwarz (Eds.), The Blackwell handbook of social psy-
positive emotions, through incremental processes associated with chology: Vol. 1. Intraindividual processes (pp. 591–614). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
broadened thinking, initiate upward spirals toward emotional well-be- Basso, M.R., Schefft, B.K., Ris, M.D., & Dember, W.N. (1996). Mood and global-local vi-
ing. Individuals who achieve such upward spirals not only enjoy im- sual processing. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 249–
255.
proved emotional well-being, but also build their coping arsenal for Danner, D.D., Snowdon, D.A., & Friesen, W.V. (2001). Positive emotions in early life and
handling future adversities (see also Aspinwall, 1998, 2001). longevity: Findings from the nun study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
These findings have clinical implications as well. Clinicians who ogy, 80, 804–813.
Davis, C.G., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Larson, J. (1998). Making sense of loss and benefit-
induce positive emotions (e.g., by asking depressed patients about ing from experience: Two construals of meaning. Journal of Personality and Social
their past “best of times”), or who time skill-based interventions to Psychology, 75, 561–574.
Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D.M. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In P.M.
correspond to patients’ positive moods, may increase both the chances Neidenthal & S. Kitayama (Eds.), The heart’s eye: Emotional influences in percep-
that patients fully learn therapy-based skills and the speed with which tion and attention (pp. 167–196). San Diego: Academic Press.
they do so (Rudd, Joiner, & Rajab, 2001). Diener, E., & Larsen, R.J. (1993). The experience of emotional well-being. In M. Lewis &
J.M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 405–415). New York: Guilford.
In sum, the data reported here provide prospective evidence that Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J.T. (2000). Positive affect and the other side of coping. Amer-
positive emotions do more than simply feel good in the present. Expe- ican Psychologist, 55, 647–654.
riences of positive emotions also increase the odds that people will Fredrickson, B.L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychol-
ogy, 2, 300–319.
feel good in the future. Moreover, as predicted by the broaden-and- Fredrickson, B.L. (2000, March 7). Cultivating positive emotions to optimize health and
build theory (Fredrickson, 2000), this upward-spiral effect is linked to well-being. Prevention & Treatment, 3, Article 0001a. Retrieved May 8, 2001, from
http://journals.apa.org/prevention/volume3/pre0030001a.html
the broadened thinking that accompanies positive emotions. It may Fredrickson, B.L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The
also explain how positive emotions promote longevity (Danner, Snow- broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218–
don, & Friesen, 2001). Although an isolated experience of positive 226.
Garner, D.M., Olmsted, M.P., & Polivy, J. (1983). The Eating Disorder Inventory: A mea-
emotion is unlikely to increase emotional well-being or longevity, the sure of cognitive-behavioral dimensions of anorexia nervosa and bulimia. In P.L.
broaden-and-build theory predicts that positive emotions accumulate Darby, P.E. Garfinkel, D.M. Garner, & D.V. Coscina (Eds.), Anorexia nervosa: Re-
and compound. The psychological broadening sparked by one positive cent developments in research (pp. 173–184). New York: Alan R. Liss.
Isen, A.M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior. Advances in
emotion increases the odds that an individual will find positive mean- Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 203–253.
ing in subsequent events and experience additional positive emotions. Kenny, D.A., Kashy, D.A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in social psychology. In
D.T. Gilbert, S.T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology
This upward spiral can, over time, build psychological resources and (pp. 233–265). New York: Oxford University Press.
optimize people’s lives. Metalsky, G.I., Joiner, T.E., Jr., Hardin, T., & Abramson, L.Y. (1993). Depressive reactions
to failure in a naturalistic setting: A test of the hopelessness and self-esteem theories
of depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 101–109.
Acknowledgments—Fredrickson’s research on positive emotions is sup- Moos, R.H. (1988). Coping Responses Inventory manual. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford Univer-
ported by grants from the University of Michigan and the National Institute sity and Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Centers.
of Mental Health (MH53971 and MH59615), and by an award from the Peterson, C., & Seligman, M.E.P. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depres-
sion: Theory and evidence. Psychological Review, 91, 347–374.
John Templeton Foundation and the American Psychological Association
Rudd, M.D., Joiner, T., & Rajab, M.H. (2001). Treating suicidal behavior: An effective
(2000 Templeton Positive Psychology Prize). time-limited approach. New York: Guilford.
Stein, N.L., Folkman, S., Trabasso, T., & Richards, T.A. (1997). Appraisal and goal pro-
cesses as predictors of psychological well-being in bereaved caregivers. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 872–884.
Torrance, E.P. (1976). Creativity testing in education. Creative Child & Adult Quarterly, 1,
REFERENCES 136–148.
Watson, D., Clark, L.A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief mea-
Ashby, F.G., Isen, A.M., & Turken, A.U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive sures of Positive and Negative Affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality
affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550. and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
Aspinwall, L.G. (1998). Rethinking the role of positive affect in self-regulation. Motiva-
tion and Emotion, 22, 1–32. (RECEIVED 11/28/99; REVISION ACCEPTED 5/8/01)