Chapter 8: Jury Selection and Research
Chapter 8: Jury Selection and Research
Chapter 8: Jury Selection and Research
-jury system is founded in English traditions and practises from the United States.
However, in Canada that kind of litigation consulting and jury “manipulation” that is
witnessed to our south does not occur.
Case-Specific Approaches:
-if the broad-attitude/trait approach may be said to approach jury selection with a
preconceived theory about how certain traits of jurors relate to their verdicts, the case-
specific approach works in the opposite way. In this approach the litigation consultant
looks at the particular facts and issues of the case and then develops a list of measurable
jurors who will decide one way or the other. In its purest form, the case specific approach
is coldly empirical; it uses the reactions of mock jurors and focus groups to identify the
variables likely to be important in the actual juror’s decisions. In the end the trial
consultant develop a list of characteristics that they hypothesize to be important.
Criminal Trials:
-two concepts have provided the structure for the measure of criminal juror bias-
authoritarianism and the distinction between a pro-prosecution and a pro-defence
orientation. Attitude scales have been developed to measure each.
The Legal Attitudes Questionnaire:
-LAQ was the first systematic measure developed to assess juror’s biases; it was
published by Virgainia R.Boehm in 1968.
-It contained 30 statements, arranged in 10 sets of three items. In each of these triads one
statement reflected authoritarianism, one reflected equalitarianism, and one reflected
antiauthoritarianism.
-this was the first instrument to attempt systematic measurements of jurors’ general
predispositions. However, it was cumbersome to complete and score.
-According to Boehm, the authoritarian items reflected one of three topics; they either
“expressed right-wing philosophy, endorsed indiscriminately the acts of constituted
authority, or were essentially punitive in nature.” In contrast, anti-authoritarian items
“expressed left-wing sentiments, implied that the blame for all antisocial acts rested with
the structure of society, or indiscriminately rejected the acts of constituted authority.” The
more moderate third type, equalitarian items, “endorsed traditional, liberal, nonextreme
positions on legal questions or were couched in a form that indicated the questions
reasonably could have two answers.”
-Every respondent could have a score ranging from 30(high) to 10(low) on each of the
three dimensions. Boehm theorized that jurors with high scores on authoritarianism had a
tendency to convict, that high score on anti-authoritarianism were associated with a
verdict of acquittal, and that scores on equalitarianism were not related to verdicts.
-more recently researchers have systematically examined the validity of the LAQ and
have proposed revisions. As part of a meta-analysis of the effects of authoritarian
attitudes on mock jurors’ verdicts, Narby, Culter, and Moran reviewed studies using the
original LAQ; results are as follows:
-Boehm gave the LAQ to 151 undergraduate students and then presented each
with one of two written versions of a murder trial. In a case in which the weight of
the evidence leaned toward innocence, the respondents who voted guilt had
significantly higher authoritarian subscale scores than did respondents who voted
not guilty. In a case in which the evidence was in the direction of guilt, the anti-
authoritarian subscale score for individual’s subjects predicted the verdict for the
group of adult respondents (age 21 and older). Among younger respondents the
difference was in the same direction but was not statistically significant.
-Jurrow asked 211 employed adults to complete the original LAQ and then listen
to tapes of two simulated murder cases. Those who voted to convict the defendant
scored higher on the authoritarian subscale. Scores on the anti-authoritarian
subscale distinguished between mock jurors who voted guilty and not guilty for
one case, but not the other.
-Cowan, Thompson, and Ellsworth had 288 jury-eligible adults complete the LAQ
and then watch a videotaped re-enactment of a murder trial. The choice of verdict
options by individual mock jurors was related to their scores on the authoritarian
subscale; the authors did not report whether the other two subscales had any
predictive validity.
**These studies indicated that subscale responses (at least for authoritarian subscale)
had predictive validity; that is, were related to eventual verdicts. But this conclusions
reflected group differences, not results that were so precise that you could, with
assurance, predict an individual’s verdict on the basis of his of her authoritarian score.
Furthermore, the original version has several problems, one of which was the
cumbersome scoring structure, in which the three forced choice response format
prevented an independent assessment of the dimensions. The format and instructions
were also difficult for some respondents to understand and follow, leading to frequent
invalid responses. So, a revised version of the LAQ was developed**
Civil Trials:
-Unlike criminal cases, in civil cases it is necessary for both the plaintiff and the
defendant to be reasonably assured of a favourable decision. A litigant who is not so
assured will, most likely, settle the issue out of court. Given this aspect of civil jury trials
in many cases the amount of evidence favouring each side will be nearly equal.
-Though, such trials can differ in the nature of the claim, the types of parties involved,
and other specifics, it may be useful to examine some general attitudes.
General Attitudes:
-Attitudes about the “Litigation Explosion:” Some prospective jurors- believing media
claims of a litigation explosion- may have adopted beliefs that too many lawsuits are
frivolous and that people are too quick to sue, this reflecting an anti-plaintiff bias.
-Attitudes about Risk Taking: In law risk means a danger of harm of loss from a
plaintiff’s action or behaviour. Traditionally, the law has said that “a plaintiff who
voluntarily encounters a known risk cannot recover.” But in real life things are not that
simple, as demonstrated by the attempts to classify the allocation of blame implicit in
contributory negligence. Jurors can differ in their attitudes toward the assumption of
risk. Assumption of risk can be thought of as a continuum ranging from no risk to 100
percent risk. Particular actions by plaintiffs can be assigned values along this continuum.
-Attitudes about Standard of Care:
-Attitudes about Personal Responsibility: The public has stereotyped civil juries as
pro-plaintiff- that is, sympathetic to claims of misfortune and willing to tap into the deep
pockets of rich defendants. The empirical evidence challenges this view and even leads
to a conclusion that an anti-plaintiff bias often emerges in jury decisions. Several causes
for this doubtless exist. When talking to jurors after civil trials, this text’s first author for
the impression that they had a strong belief in personal responsibility; these jurors lack
sympathy for people with unhappy outcomes and (sometimes justified) grievances
against a manufacturer, physician, or governmental organization. Feigenson, Park, and
Salovery noted “evidence of a specifically antiplaintiff bias in responsibility judgments
and refer to interviews with actual jurors and experimental research supporting a
conclusion that jurors often attribute the behaviour of plaintiffs to undersirable motives,
such as greed, rather than to legitimate grievances.
Corporate Responsibility:
-Some potential jurors are anti-business, standing up for the powerless individual against
the monolithic corporation. But others believe businesses are hampered too much by
government regulations.
-Hans and Lofquist constructed an attitude scale to measure potential juror’s attitudes
toward business regulation. The 16 items on this scale tap attitudes about civil litigation,
the benefits of costs of government regulation of business, and standards for worker
safety and product safety. After reviewing this work, Wrightsman and Heili formulated
additional items that might reflect jurors’ biases in civil trials. These items, called the
Civil Trial Bias Scale, were administered along with Hans and Lofquist’s items, to 204
undergraduates and the responses were factor analyzed to determine what constructs
underlay the responses. The first factor that emerged in this analysis was to favour
business and the easing of stringent requirements for safety. For example, the higher
loading item (the one that correlated the highest with the factor), #16 from the Hans and
Lofquist set, states: “Requiring that products be 100 percents sage before they’re sold to
the public is just too expensive.” The other factors emerging from this analysis also
covered a variety of attitudes.
-A separate analysis of the Hans and Lofquist items produced clearer results than the
factor analysis of the two scales together. What emerges is one set of attitudes opposed to
government regulation and dealing with what are the proper safety standards. But other
dimensions may also be present; the separate factor analysis of the Civil Trial Bias Scale,
not detailed here, found that jurors differed on assigning responsibility for bad outcomes,
the inexplicability of bad events and the value of risk taking.
-A recent instrument that shows promise here is the Attitude Toward Corporations (ATC)
scale. It contains five subscales that measure product safety, government regulation,
treatment of employees by corporation, and anti-plaintiff and anti-corporation attitudes.
The original pool of items from which the ATC emerged capitalized on the items
developed by Hans and Lofquist, described above, but other items were constructed, and
then the early versions of the scale were subjected to item analyses so that a 150item
scale resulted.
Case-Specific Approaches:
-some litigation consultants seek to determine the criteria for jury selection by “working
from the inside out” rather than beginning with hypothetical determinants. The goal is to
determine characteristics of the ideal jurors or, if that is not possible, the ideal juror to
avoid.
-Recall, that the procedure does not permit one to compose an “ideal jury” without
restrictions;
1. The procedure is to remove the supposedly undesirable jurors rather than to
select the desirable ones.
2. There is no way to prevent the other side from dismissing those whom your
side considers more desirable.
Juror Investigation:
-citizens clearly have their privacy invaded when they become prospective jurors. The
courts have accepted certain procedures because they subscribe to the goal that voir dire
can identify prospective jurors whose biases prevent them from being open-minded.
-trial consultants do, on occasion, use out-of-court investigations to determine the
attitudes and values of prospective jurors. Public records, such as house appraisals, may
be consulted; the trial consultants’ team may drive by the prospective juror’s house, note
its condition and the quality of its neighbourhood, and search for any “diagnostic”
bumper stickers n the juror’s car. Friends and neighbours may be interviewed.
-in the U.S. there are limits to such activities. Prospective jurors cannot be contacted
outside the courtroom; jury tampering is illegal, and the courts have held persons to be
in contempt of court for communicating with jurors even though it was no clear that they
had sought to influence the jurors.
-Out-of-court investigations by psychologists may violate ethical guidelines about
participant’s rights to decline or withdraw from research participation.
Summary:
-In Canada, the way in which juries are selected is governed by the Charter of Rights and
Freedoms, which says that everyone accused of a crime is entitled to a fair trial by an
impartial jury.
-Canadian courts attempt to ensure a jury that is “as random as possible,” while requiring
jurors to be unbiased and competent to do their job.
-We do not see the type of jury manipulation witnessed in other countries, particularly the
U.S.
-Psychologists in Canada have conducted important work on jury decision making, while
working with the constraints of laws preventing jurors from speaking about their
deliberations after trials. As such, most of the research on juries has used a mock jury
approach in which people are asked to act as if they are jurors in an actual case.
-Some American lawyers believe that trials can be won or lost on the basis of the specific
jury “selected” for the trial. In actuality, jury “selection” is a misnomer, because lawyers
on each side can dismiss prospective jurors but cannot ensure that any one juror is
chosen, as the other side also has the opportunity to “strike” or dismiss jurors through the
use of peremptory challenges.
-Trial lawyers are increasingly relying on psychologists as trial consultants. In such
jurisdictions, in advising the lawyer about jury selection, the psychologist uses
information based on mock trials, focus groups, community attitude surveys, and
sometimes out-of-court investigations.
-Two approaches have been used. The use of broad traits or general attitudes reflects an
assumption that certain predispositions of jurors may predict their verdicts in a wide
variety of trials.
-With regard to criminal trials, two attitude scales have some limited general utility in
predicting juror verdicts: the Revised Legal Attitudes Questionnaire Scale, which
measure authoritarianism, and the Juror Bias Scale, which measures biases regarding
probability of commission and reasonable doubt.
-With regard to general characteristics that may predict verdicts in civil suits, measure of
risk taking responsibility, and attitudes toward the “litigation explosion” are promising.
-Psychologists are in disagreement about whether scientific jury selection is appropriate
or useful. One laboratory study found that it was more effective than the traditional
method in some trials, but not all trials. A conservative conclusion is that the use of such
procedures may account for a small degree of variance in the juror’s verdicts- perhaps
10%, thus not enough to conclude that trial consultants can “rig” juries, but enough of an
edge to make them useful to some trial lawyers.
-The second approach works from the inside out, identifying specific aspects of a
particular case and then assessing prospective jurors on those characteristics (such as
racial attitudes or attitudes towards protestors). Two examples, the Harrisburg Seven trial
and Byron de la Beckwith’s retrial, illustrated the procedures used in this approach.
-A number of ethical issues surface when forensic psychologists assist in jury selection.
Investigations of prospective jurors may violate their right to give consent and to privacy.
Another concern is the inequality between prosecution and defence of the utilization of
the trial consultants. Use of supplemental juror questionnaires may lessen this concern.
-In Canada, we have a unique and diverse society. Research examining actual jury
decision making in this context is essential. It remains to be seen whether Canadian laws
will advance to become more flexible in allowing jurors to discuss their experiences with
mental health professionals and researchers.