Developmental Psychology - The Growth of Mind and Behavior by Frank Keil
Developmental Psychology - The Growth of Mind and Behavior by Frank Keil
Developmental Psychology - The Growth of Mind and Behavior by Frank Keil
Psychology
Developmental
Psychology
The Growth of Mind and Behavior
Frank Keil
YALE UNIVERSITY
W. W. NORTON & COMPANY has been independent since its founding in 1923, when William Warder
Norton and Mary D. Herter Norton first published lectures delivered at the People’s Institute, the adult
education division of New York City’s Cooper Union. The firm soon expanded its program beyond the
Institute, publishing books by celebrated academics from America and abroad. By midcentury, the two
major pillars of Norton’s publishing program—trade books and college texts—were firmly established.
In the 1950s, the Norton family transferred control of the company to its employees, and today—with
a staff of four hundred and a comparable number of trade, college, and professional titles published each
year—W. W. Norton & Company stands as the largest and oldest publishing house owned wholly by its
employees.
W. W. Norton & Company, Inc., 500 Fifth Avenue, New York, NY 10110-0017
wwnorton.com
W. W. Norton & Company Ltd., Castle House, 75/76 Wells Street, London W1T 3QT
1234567890
About the Author
Frank C. Keil is the Charles C. and Dorathea S. Dilley Professor of Psychology and Linguistics
and chair of the Psychology Department at Yale University. Keil received his B.S. in Biology
from M.I.T. in 1973, an M.A. in Psychology from Stanford University in 1975, and a Ph.D. in
Psychology from the University of Pennsylvania in 1977. He was a faculty member at Cornell
University from 1977 to 1998 and has been at Yale since 1998. For over 35 years, he has taught
an undergraduate lecture course in developmental psychology, as well as advanced seminars at the
undergraduate and graduate level on topics in cognitive development and cognition.
Keil has published extensively on topics concerned with many areas of the development of
cognition and language. He wrote two books on aspects of conceptual development and, with
the philosopher Robert Wilson, he edited the MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences, which
was selected as the Outstanding Book in Psychology by the Association of American Publishers
in 1999. Keil served as president of the Society for Philosophy and Psychology and has received
numerous awards for his scholarship, including the Boyd R. McCandless Award (Developmental
Psychology), the Distinguished Scientific Award for an Early Career Contribution to Psychology,
a Guggenheim Fellowship, a fellowship at the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral
Sciences, a MERIT Award from the National Institutes of Health, and the Ann L. Brown
Award for Excellence in Developmental Research. Keil served as Master of Morse College at Yale
University from 2001 until 2012. Keil and his wife Kristi Lockhart, a clinical and developmental
psychologist, are parents of three sons who are now in their twenties and thirties.
Contents in Brief
PART I Framing Psychological Development
Chapter 1 Approaching Psychological Development 2
Chapter 2 The Biology of Development 33
PART II Origins
Chapter 3 Coming to Perceive the World 76
Chapter 4 The Emergence of Action 115
Chapter 5 Coming to Understand the Physical World 145
Chapter 6 Connecting with the Social World 187
Chapter 7 The Origins of Emotion, Temperament,
and Personality 227
vi
Contents
ABOUT THE AUTHOR v
PREFACE xvii
Psychoanalytic Perspectives 20
CHAPTER 1
Cognitive Science Perspectives 20
Approaching Psychological Studying Psychological Development 22
Development 2 Observational Studies 22
Experimental Studies 23
Why Study Development? 4 Longitudinal Approaches 26
Development as a Key to Understanding Children’s Cross-Sectional Approaches 27
Capabilities 4
Converging Methods 28
Development as a Means for Insight into the Mature Form 5
Designing a Sound Study 28
Development and Social Policy 6 Reliability and Validity
The Nature of Development 6 Within-Subjects and Between-Subjects Designs
Categorizing Developmental Phenomena 7 Conclusions 30
Periods of Development Summary 31
Areas of Development
Basic Questions about Psychological Development 9
Is Development Stage-like or Continuous?
CHAPTER 2
Is Development Global or Local?
How Do Nature and Nurture Shape Development?
The Biology of Development 33
The Basis of Development 35
Perspectives on Development 14
Inputs to the Biological System 36
Empiricist and Nativist Perspectives 14
The Genes
Empiricism
The Environment
Nativism
Interactions between Genes and Environment
Comparative and Evolutionary Perspectives 17
Constraints on Development 38
Cross-Cultural Perspectives 18 Viability
Neuroscience Perspectives 19 Differentiation of Cells and Structures
Behaviorist Perspectives 19 Timing and Sequencing
vii
The Beginnings of Development 41 Brain Development 56
Meiosis and Fertilization 41 Major Changes to Brain Structures 57
Meiosis and Mitosis Neurons and Neurotransmitters 58
Fertilization Development of Neurons 60
The First Patterns of Differentiation 43 Proliferation
Migration and Synaptogenesis
Anatomical Development 44
Consolidation
Structures and Systems in the Embryo and Fetus 44
Myelination
The Embryonic Period
The Fetal Period Experience and Brain Development 63
Preterm Births 47 Puberty and Brain Development 64
Why Does Anatomical Development Progress Behavioral Genetics 67
As It Does? 49 Heritability 67
Diversity out of Uniformity 49 Behavioral Genomics 68
Sexual Differentiation 52
Conclusions 70
Adverse Influences on the Developing
Embryo and Fetus 53
Summary 72
PART II Origins
Hearing 100
CHAPTER 3
Noticing and Remembering Sounds 100
Coming to Perceive the World 76 Locating Sounds 101
Perceiving Complex Sound Patterns 104
Vision 78
Perceiving Differences in Brightness and Acuity 79 The Chemical Senses: Taste and Smell 107
Visual Acuity Taste 107
Visual Experience and Brain Development Smell 109
Color 84
Intermodal Perception 109
Perceiving Hue and the Clustering of Hues
Categorical Perception of Color Conclusions 112
Depth Perception 86 Summary 112
Cues to Depth
Which Depth Cues Predominate? Gibson’s Visual
Cliff Studies
Functional Significance of the Depth Perception System
CHAPTER 4
Perceiving Patterns and Recognizing Objects 91 The Emergence of Action 115
Pattern Perception in Other Species
Plasticity in Perceptual Development Foundations of Motor Development 116
Perceptually Grouping the World Reflexes in Infancy 117
Face Perception 95 Theories of Motor Development 118
Development of Face Perception: A Two-System Model The Maturational Account
Facial Attractiveness Dynamic Systems Theory
viii CONTENTS
Perception and Action 122 CHAPTER 6
Coordination in Changing Bodies 122
Reaching 123
Connecting with the Social
Empiricist View of Eye-Limb Coordination
Constraints on Links between Eye and Limb
World 187
Navigating Space 125 Developing Bonds between Infants and Caregivers 189
Active versus Passive Visual Experience and Action
Early Perspectives on Infants’ Bonds with Others 190
Walking and Seeing
Psychoanalytic Approaches to Infant-Parent Bonds 190
Learning to Engage in Specific Actions 131 Learning Theory Approaches 191
Early Imitation 131 Bowlby’s Ethological Approach 192
The Process of Imitation
The Development of Birdsong: Variations on the The Underpinnings of Attachment 194
Imitation Theme Components of Attachment 194
Imprinting, Action, and Critical Periods 135 Smiling
Conditioning 136 Clinging and Touching
Classical Conditioning Cuteness
Operant Conditioning Bases of Social Interactions 199
Constraints on Learning Contingent Responding
Social Referencing
Conclusions 142 Joint Attention and Gaze Following
Summary 143
Individual Differences in Attachment Style 201
Studying Attachment Styles in the Strange Situation 202
Causes of Different Attachment Styles 203
CHAPTER 5 Parent Effects on Insecure Attachment
Coming to Understand the Child Effects on Insecure Attachment
Interactions of Parent Effects and Child Effects
Physical World 145 Consequences of Different Attachment Styles 208
Correlations between Infant Attachment Styles and Children’s Social Interactions
Piaget’s Approach 146 Long-Term Links between Infant Attachment Styles and Adult Relationship
Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development 147 Styles
The Sensorimotor Period in Infancy 149 Cross-Cultural Differences in Attachment Styles 210
Stage 1: Use of Reflexes
Stage 2: Emergence of Primary Circular Reactions
Effects of Early Social Deprivation 214
Stage 3: Appearance of Secondary Circular Reactions Social Deprivation in Humans 214
Stage 4: Coordination of Secondary Circular Reactions Deficits in Socially Deprived Infants
Stage 5: Appearance of Tertiary Circular Reactions Untangling Causation
Stage 6: Invention of New Means through Mental Deprivation Studies in Nonhuman Primates 218
Combinations Tactile Stimulation and Physiological Needs
Key Aspects of Piaget’s Theory 154 Peer-Raised Infants
Examining the A-Not-B Error 155 Critical-Period Effects? 222
CONTENTS ix
Emotional Regulation in Infancy 237 Determining Differences in Temperament 249
Situational Factors The New York Longitudinal Study
Attentional Deployment Modern Approaches to Studying Temperament
Response Modification Linking Temperament to Personality Development
Evolutionary Preparedness and Emotional Development 242 Child–Environment Interactions and Goodness of Fit 252
Preparedness for Acquiring Certain Fears Environments for “Squeaky Wheels”
Preparedness and the Development of Disgust Parental Influences on Temperament
Preparedness and the Development of Other Emotions
Conclusions 255
Temperament and the Origins of Personality 247 Summary 256
Temperament-Based Components of Personality and Early
Development 248
x CONTENTS
A Developmental Disorder in Attention: ADHD 363
CHAPTER 9 Biological Bases of ADHD
The Growth of Knowledge 303 Misdiagnosis of ADHD
CONTENTS xi
Schooling 409 Implementing Cognitive Science Ideas in the
Classroom
Characterizing Schools 409
Aptitude-Treatment Interactions
Two Prominent Models of Education
What Schools Do 411 Creativity 417
Schools as Socializing Agents Measures of Creativity 417
Schools as Cultural Institutions The Development of Extraordinary Creativity 420
Schools as Vehicles of Values
Schools and Thought 412 Conclusions 421
Cognitive Science and Teaching Summary 422
Three Cognitive Science Themes for Education
xii CONTENTS
Developing a Sense of Self-Worth 474 Working Parents and Child Care 529
Self-Esteem 474 Changes in Family Structures 530
Self-Efficacy 476 Single-Parent Families
Same-Sex Parents
Self-Regulation 477 Divorce
Brain Maturation and the Development of Self-Regulation 480 Blended Families
Contextual Influences on Self-Regulation 481 Child Abuse 536
Developing a Theory of Mind 482 Effects of Abuse 537
Early Aspects of Theory of Mind 483 Explaining the Cycle of Abuse 537
Comparative Perspectives on Theory of Mind 484 Foster Care 538
Understanding False Beliefs 487 Conclusions 539
Evaluating the False-Belief Task
Summary 540
A Theory of Mind Module? 490
Beyond False Beliefs 492
Making Attributions about Ourselves and Others 493 CHAPTER 15
Emerging Explanations of Behavior 493 Becoming Part of the
Traits and Optimism 496
Attribution, Motivation, and Creativity 497 Community 543
Conclusions 499 Levels of Affiliation 544
Summary 501 The Changing Nature of Social Interactions 545
Developing Relationships 546
Friends
CHAPTER 14 Imaginary Companions
CONTENTS xiii
PART V Broader Developmental
Context
Schizophrenia 614
CHAPTER 16
Features of Schizophrenia 614
Psychopathology in Childhood 586 Causal Factors 616
Early-Onset Schizophrenia 619
Autism 588
Features of Autism 588 Treatment of Childhood Psychopathologies 619
Diagnosis of Autism 589 Behavioral Therapies 619
Incidence of Autism 589 Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy 620
Causal Factors 590 Psychoanalytic Therapies 621
Family Therapies 622
Anxiety Disorders 592
Pharmacological Treatments 622
Incidence of Anxiety Disorders 592
Evaluating Therapies 624
Causal Factors 593
Cognitive Factors Conclusions 625
Biological Factors Summary 625
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder 595
Types of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder
Incidence of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder CHAPTER 17
Diagnosis of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder
Causal Factors Development after Childhood
Eating Disorders 598 and Adolescence 629
Anorexia Nervosa 598
Incidence of Anorexia
Physical and Physiological Changes in Adulthood 630
Cognitive Distortions in Anorexia Cognitive Changes in Adulthood 635
Genetic and Environmental Causal Factors Reaction Time and Speed of Processing 635
Bulimia Nervosa 602 Memory, Higher Cognitive Functions, and Reasoning 635
Incidence of Bulimia
Daily Activities and Cognitive Aging 639
Causal Factors
Circadian Rhythms, Cognition, and the Lifespan 641
Depression 603
Changes in Personality in the Lifespan: Individual and
Unipolar Depression 604
Biological Causes
Group Changes 643
Social Causes Changes in Individuals 643
Cognitive Causes Stressful Life Events 644
Bipolar Disorder 607 Group Changes 645
Causal Factors
Stages of Life? 648
Diagnosis of Bipolar Disorder
Erik Erikson’s Approach 648
Conduct Disorder: The Case of Psychopathy 609 Stage 1: Trust versus Mistrust
Features of Psychopathy 610 Stage 2: Autonomy versus Shame/Doubt
Diagnosis of Psychopathy 611 Stage 3: Initiative versus Guilt
Stage 4: Industry versus Inferiority
Causal Factors 612
xiv CONTENTS
Stage 5: Identity versus Role Confusion EPILOGUE 659
Stage 6: Intimacy versus Isolation
Stage 7: Generativity versus Stagnation GLOSSARY G-1
Stage 8: Integrity versus Despair REFERENCES R-1
Understanding Stages of Adult Development 652
CREDITS AND ACKNOWLEDGMENTS C-1
What Does It Mean to Be Old? 652
NAME INDEX N-1
Stereotypes and Ageism 654
Cultural Variation in Stereotypes and Ageism? 655 SUBJECT INDEX S-1
Conclusions 656
Summary 657
CONTENTS xv
Boxed Features
Development and Social Policy A Language Gene? 287
Biological Knowledge and Exposure to Nature 333
What Kinds of Experimental Research Are Justified? 25
The Genetics of Dyslexia 375
Visiting Nurses, Prenatal Care, and Child Development 51
A g for Emotional Intelligence? 395
Legislating Early Stimulation 106
Moral Dumbfounding: Judgment or Intuition? 446
Can a Toddler’s Motor Development Be Accelerated? 137
Theory of Mind in Dogs? 486
The Myth of the First 3 Years 181
Epigenetics and Families 512
Day Care and Attachment 212
The Development of Racial and Ethnic Occupational
The Causes and Consequences of Problems in Emotional
Stereotypes 578
Regulation 244
Co-rumination, Gender, and Depression 608
The Cognitive Effects of Bilingualism 296
Why Do We Age and Why Do We Live as Long as We
Sputnik and Intuitive Science 328
Do? 632
Children as Witnesses 360
Computers and e-Learning in the Schools 418
Scientific Method Boxes
At What Age Should an Offender Be Treated as an
Adult? 457 Diet and Methylation 40
Potential Drawbacks of Self-Esteem 478 Intersensory Perception at Birth 111
China’s One-Child Policy 522 The Genetics of Early Handedness 124
Free Speech and Children’s Rights 570 Agents and Order 182
Prescribing Psychoactive Medications to Young Internal Working Models and Attachment Styles 209
Children 623 Inferring Actions from Emotions 238
When Is It Acceptable to Deprive the Elderly of Some Early Use of Syntax to Guide Learning New Words 276
Rights? 650
Early Motivation to Learn about Kinds over Individuals 318
xvi
Preface
have been deeply involved in teaching introductory making some progress in linking scholarly work to my own
xvii
reinforce each other. These themes and principles serve emotional states have functional values both for themselves
as constant touchstones for organizing the material in the and for their interactions with others, and while their
book. Seven themes stand out as recurring most frequently. emotions change considerably in the first year of life,
One key theme is the centrality of feedback loops, both infants cannot simply shut down their emotional systems
positive and negative. Development is rarely, if ever, a one- when transitioning to new kinds of emotions or emotional
way process in which one factor causes another to happen interaction patterns. The same is true for everything from
without some kind of feedback. Parents influence their language to friendship and is closely related to the idea that
children, who in turn influence their parents, in cycles the most functionally vital aspects of systems often appear
of interaction that can reinforce desirable outcomes or earliest in both development and in the evolutionary history
aggravate undesirable ones. The simple acts of learning of organisms.
to walk and successfully reaching toward objects involve A sixth theme is the ever-present links and interdependencies
constant feedback loops between what a child sees and between different areas of development. It simply isn’t possible
how she acts. Even at the neural level, the growth of neural to consider cognitive development without also considering
circuits can depend on feedback from experience, which in social and emotional development, just as understanding
turn can change how future events are experienced. In all social development depends on related changes in cognitive
areas of development, we will see the central importance of development, and a full account of emotional development
feedback loops. depends on understanding both social and cognitive
A second major theme is that development is a dynamic development. We will often focus primarily on one area at a
exploratory process in which the child is more than a passive time, but we will always take into account how the different
recipient of experiences and information. When children are areas of development also interact and must be understood
viewed as actively engaging with the world, it is much easier in their entirety to obtain a full picture of what develops.
to understand development, whether it is development of A final theme is that developmental psychology is not
perception, cognition, motivation, or morality. just a human-based process. All organisms with significant
A third theme is to see that development is constrained behavioral components grow from immature states to
by factors that arise both internally from within the child and mature states and show fascinating patterns of change
externally from the environment. While constraints may and constancy. Comparisons across these organisms and
seem to somehow hinder development they may actually especially with humans are not just interesting in their own
foster development by acting as scaffolds and guiding right, they can also greatly help us to understand what is
frameworks that make otherwise overwhelming problems of unique about human psychological development, as well as
learning and skill development manageable. We will see the telling us about what kinds of developmental problems tend
value of constraints and how they can guide development to converge on one common class of solutions and what
in many areas, while still allowing for enormous diversity kinds of problems embrace an enormous diversity of possible
of outcomes. Discussions of constraints also allow for a solutions. This comparative perspective also highlights the
balanced treatment of the tradeoffs between nativist and ways in which evolutionary considerations can inform
empiricist views of development and the related, often thinking about development. These seven themes and
needlessly contentious, nature/nurture debate. principles recur throughout this book and provide a way
A fourth theme is to focus on what children can do and of weaving together all of the book’s content to create an
how their basic capacities emerge and serve as foundations for integrated and more memorable whole that reveals larger
later development, as opposed to focusing on what children developmental patterns.
cannot do compared with older children and adults. While
younger children and infants certainly have limitations that
are not found in later years, it is almost always more fruitful
to avoid “deficit models” of development and instead ask
how children progress as they grow.
Linking Theory and Evidence
A related fifth theme that occurs in all areas of development Developmental psychology has emerged robustly in the
is that infants and children usually have some viable version of past few decades as a full-fledged science in which scholars
a skill throughout development; they rarely go “offline” as they propose theories and models and then test them rigorously.
retool or improve on a system. Just as the human heart must This book celebrates the ways in which ideas, many of
beat continuously as it goes through remarkable anatomical which have been with us for centuries, are now being tested
and physiological changes from its early embryonic stages in carefully designed experiments or through powerful new
to birth and beyond, most perceptual, cognitive, emotional, statistical analyses of large data sets. It is fascinating to see
and social systems must have some working functional how classic issues are now coming into much clearer focus
capacity throughout development. For example, infants’ as a result of clever new studies. Throughout this text, we
xviii PREFACE
will encounter theories, sometimes conflicting theories, and discussion of psychopathology in development. More than
ask how they can be tested and what the current evidence just a review of the psychopathology literature, Chapter 16
tells us. In many cases, the debate continues with more than shows how aspects of development that are covered in earlier
one view remaining viable, but also with a much better chapters, ranging from brain development to cognitive
understanding of the research path forward. executive functioning to socialization, come together in
I am convinced that even the most complex and intricate an interwoven manner to contribute to the emergence of
theories of development, ranging from neural growth various forms of psychopathology. Students will see how all
patterns to changing social network analyses, can be that they have learned bears on powerful and compelling
explained in such a way that any introductory student can problems. They will then experience a different form of
appreciate their key dimensions and how to evaluate them integration of the same range of topics in the final chapter,
in light of the right kinds of studies and evidence emerging which considers development after childhood and in which
from those studies. All the theories and studies in this text there is a fascinating set of questions that can have some
have been written up, reviewed, and rewritten several times striking resonances to earlier developmental questions and
to ensure that they are clear and accessible and that the themes. I have taken great pains to present the full spectrum
links to empirical research are compelling. I want all the of theories and approaches but always with an eye toward
readers of this text to be able to step into the shoes of leading how they relate to each other and to larger developmental
investigators in developmental psychology so that they can issues that transcend any one school of thought. In doing
understand what drove the researchers both in terms of so, I have immersed myself in reading the literature on
theory and their passion to design and execute studies to different aspects of development, as well as engaging in
answer developmental questions. many conversations and corresponding with experts across
many disciplines, far more deeply than I ever could have
imagined I would do when I first started on this project.
The entire process has fed back into this book, and it has
Integrated Treatment been great fun as well!
PREFACE xix
important and socially relevant real-world issues. A second of developmental psychology leads to the theories students
box in each chapter is concerned with an example of an are learning about in class.
exciting new direction of developmental research so students The Developmental Psychology Lecture PowerPoints were
can see how the field continues to evolve through innovative written by David Barner and Jessica Sullivan, both of
research initiatives. It is an important way of illustrating whom are at University of California, San Diego.
how developmental psychology is a dynamic, active field. • All of the art in the book is available as PowerPoints and
A third box in each chapter illustrates in more detail the JPEGs to make creating custom presentation materials
methodology of scientific research relevant to the topics easy.
of that chapter. The purpose here is to show in a manner • Presentation tools are downloadable at wwnorton.com/
closely analogous to posters at scientific conferences how instructors.
an experiment is organized in terms of hypothesis, method,
results, and conclusion, and to include illustrations of the
experimental setup and results. These boxes are somewhat
simpler than most posters at meetings, but they very much Videos
capture their spirit. • Chapter Opening Videos from Frank Keil himself serve as
Throughout the book, there are brief historical discussions an introduction to the chapter topics and help convey his
of research topics that make clear how current ideas and enthusiasm and voice to the reader.
research emerged from older traditions. There is also careful • Classroom Videos with discussion questions feature
documentation of cutting-edge research that serves not only enactments of classic experiments and demonstrate a
to ground the research in particular studies but also serves variety of characteristics from varying developmental
as a rich source of references for further explorations by periods, showing developmental psychology as a science in
students, references that are also available on the instructor action. The Developmental Psychology videos were curated
Web site and in prepared LMS materials available free of by Tasha Howe of Humboldt State University.
charge to every instructor using the book. • Videos are available at wwnorton.com/instructors as part
In the end, these pedagogical devices all serve the of the Interactive Instructor’s Guide and as part of our
overarching goal of this book, which is to get to the heart LMS-ready materials.
of developmental psychology. My aim is to expose readers
to developmental psychology’s big questions in an engaging
and compelling way that invites students to join researchers
in one of the most lively and fascinating areas of intellectual Interactive Instructor’s Guide
inquiry that also has immense personal relevance and social • The searchable Interactive Instructor’s Guide offers class-
importance. room activity suggestions with print-ready handouts, chapter
opening videos, and classroom videos with discussion
questions. The Developmental Psychology videos were curated
by Tasha Howe of Humboldt State University, and the
Supplementary Materials activities were written by Melissa Barnett at the University
of Arizona.
Our supplements package was developed based on data from • The Interactive Instructor’s Guide is downloadable from
a survey of 60 instructors currently teaching developmental wwnorton.com/instructors.
psychology at a wide variety of schools. Our book-specific
resources are easy to access in one place through wwnorton
.com/instructors. They are also searchable on the Interactive
Instructor’s Guide, making lecture planning easy. Test Bank
• The Test Bank for Developmental Psychology is designed to
help instructors prepare their exams. The Test Bank has
been developed using the Norton Assessment Guidelines,
Presentation Tools and each chapter of the Test Bank consists of five
• Lecture PowerPoints offer images and instructor-only question types classified according to the first five levels
lecture notes that include additional examples and of Bloom’s taxonomy of knowledge types: remembering,
teaching suggestions, which will help ease the transition understanding, applying, analyzing, and evaluating.
to using a new textbook. Each chapter also includes an Questions are further classified by section and difficulty,
optional set of slides that cover the research behind a making it easy to construct tests and quizzes that are
concept covered in the chapter to illustrate how the science meaningful and diagnostic.
xx PREFACE
• The Test Bank questions were written by Thompson Davis of the print book and allow students to highlight and take
of Louisiana State University and Lisa Rosen of Texas notes with ease, print chapters as needed, and search the text.
Woman’s University, with Tasha Howe of Humboldt State Norton eBooks are available online and as downloadable
University. PDFs. They can be purchased directly from our Web site, or
• The Test Bank is available in paperback and on disk; it is with a registration folder that can be sold in the bookstore.
also downloadable in PDF, RTF, or ExamView formats
from wwnorton.com/instructors.
Acknowledgments
Norton Coursepacks: Our Content, My deepest debt goes to my wife Kristi Lockhart, who was
Your Course assigned to the same office as me on our first day of graduate
school and who has been my one true love and colleague ever
• Easily add high-quality Norton digital media to your
since. Kristi has made major career sacrifices for the sake of
online, hybrid, or lecture course—all at no cost. Norton
our family and my career and has always done so with great
Coursepacks work with and leverage your existing Learning
generosity and good humor, spreading that spirit of generosity
Management System, so there’s no new system to learn, and
and good humor to our children as well. As a parent and partner
access is free and easy. Comprehensive coursepacks are ready
she has been loving, supportive, and playful in ways that have
to use, right from the start, but they are easy to customize,
sustained and inspired all of us. I am infinitely fortunate that
using the system you already know and understand. The
she is the love of my life and marvel every day that she was
Developmental Psychology Coursepack includes classroom
willing to spend her life with me. She has taught me more
videos with discussion questions, Quiz+ review questions,
about children, parenting, and family dynamics than all other
and flashcards available for the students (or assignable as
sources combined and has been the best parent ever. Through
instructors see fit), as well as the Test Bank.
countless discussions with her about almost all the topics in
• Quiz questions were written by Thompson Davis of
this book and through listening to her many observations
Louisiana State University and Lisa Rosen of Texas
about children, I have been a most grateful recipient of her
Woman’s University, with Tasha Howe of Humboldt State
wisdom. I owe her everything. I have also shared with Kristi in
University.
the delight of watching our three sons grow, each in their own
• All of these materials are downloadable in Blackboard,
ways, into remarkable young men. From their first moments
Moodle, D2L, Angel, and Canvas formats.
as newborns to their lives as young adults, Derek, Dylan, and
Martin have taught us both many volumes about development
and continue to do so, as well as providing us with the most
eBook rewarding and meaningful experiences of our lives. Kristi,
• Same great book, a fraction of the price. Derek, Dylan, and Martin have also been incredibly patient
• An affordable and convenient alternative to the printed and tolerant of my obsession with this book and the ways it
textbook, Norton eBooks retain the content and design has hovered over all of us for decades. These two photos of our
PREFACE xxi
family cover only part of the entire period of writing this book, evolution, sometimes sending me extensive comments on
but they show the passage of time during which I was writing earlier drafts, other times providing confirmatory reviews to
it. The many hours I have spent working on this book have Norton editors about the near final drafts. I have included
been all too evident to every member of my family, and they the affiliations of the reviewers at the time that they wrote
all have been tremendously gracious about its intrusions into their reviews, and some may since have moved on to other
their lives. I thank all of them from the bottom of my heart colleges and universities. I deeply appreciate all the work
for their support, their inspiration, and their forbearance and that the reviewers have done and thank the following:
I thank the heavens that I was lucky enough to be part of this
family. Joseph Allen, University of Virginia
Certain people at W. W. Norton also deserve extraordinary Richard Aslin, University of Rochester
thanks, starting with Don Lamm and Don Fusting, Terry Au, University of Hong Kong
who originally signed me up to write the book years ago. Melissa Barnett, University of Arizona
I appreciate the support I have received from the Acquisitions Kymberley Bennett, Indiana State University
Editors Sheri Shavely, Roby Harrington, Jon Durbin, Cathy Paul Bloom, Yale University
Wick, and, especially recently, Aaron Javsicas, who made Amanda Brandone, Lehigh University
sure the whole project met critical deadlines and who has Sara Broaders, Northwestern University
been very helpful in selecting art. I have also been blessed Gwen Broude, Vassar College
with brilliant copy editing by Janet Greenblatt and project Jean Burr, Hamilton College
editing by Rachel Mayer. I thank Vanessa Drake-Johnson, Joseph Campos, University of California, Berkeley
who served as an early Developmental Editor of the book, Carol Cheatham, University of North Carolina
and Photo Editor Mike Fodera, Photo Researchers Donna Judith Danovitch, Michigan State University
Ranieri and Julie Tesser, Media Editor Callinda Taylor, Daniel Dickman, Ivy Tech Community College of Indiana
Editorial Assistant Shira Averbuch, Production Manager Ann Edworthy, Swansea Metropolitan University
Eric Pier-Hocking, and Marketing Manager Lauren Leanne Franklin, Cardiff Metropolitan University
Winkler. I also would like to thank Jonathan Kominsky Janet Frick, University of Georgia
who worked on the art manuscript in New Haven. Jeffrey Gagne, University of Texas at Arlington
But I must especially acknowledge the massive contribu- Susan Gelman, University of Michigan
tions made by two Developmental Editors, Sarah Mann and Gilbert Gottlieb, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill
Sandy Lifland. Sarah became involved in the project in 2007 Joan Grusec, University of Toronto
and went through the entire text and asked me probing ques- Amy Halberstadt, North Carolina State University
tions about what I really wanted to say and if I was saying it Jacqui Harrison, University of Bolton
in the best and briefest ways possible. She helped enormously Brett Hayes, University of New South Wales
to shape what had become a far too long body of text into Fay Julal, Southampton Solent University
something much more workable. Even more critically, Sandy Robert Kavanaugh, Williams College
Lifland has had a profound influence. She is a legendary edi- Rachel Keen, University of Massachusetts, Amherst
tor who early on gave me challenging feedback on the crude Debby Kemler Nelson, Swarthmore College
first drafts of this book and made me realize just how much Susan Kemper, University of Kansas
more I had to do if I wanted to write the book I really envi- Katherine Kinzler, University of Chicago
sioned. She then rejoined the project in 2011 and worked Kristin Lagattuta, University of California, Davis
full time on the book until its publication. We studied, con- Alan Leslie, Rutgers University
sidered, and often extensively discussed every word of text, Robert Lickliter, Virginia Tech
every line in every figure and graph for ways that they could Gary Marcus, New York University
be clearer, better connected to the big ideas of the book and Ellen Markman, Stanford University
to the basic scientific literature. Sandy herself read hundreds Lori Markson, University of California, Berkeley
of original articles to understand more deeply what I had in Carol Murphy, National University of Ireland, Maynooth
mind and to ensure that I was getting across to the reader Simone Nguyen, University of North Carolina, Wilmington
exactly what I really intended. She also kindly pointed out Bjorn Nilsson, Skovde University College
to me several cases where I still wasn’t clear in my own head Samuel Putnam, University of Oregon
about what I actually wanted to say. I honestly cannot imag- Philippe Rochat, Emory University
ine a better editor who cared more about creating a product Lisa Rosen, Texas Women’s University
of real intellectual and aesthetic value. Karl Rosengren, Northwestern University
This book has been reviewed through the years by Paul Rozin, University of Pennsylvania
many scholars who have been involved in all stages of its Jenny Saffran, University of Wisconsin, Madison
xxii PREFACE
Rose Scott, University of California, Merced I am also grateful to those scholars who have written the
Meghan Sinton, College of William and Mary ancillaries: Tasha Howe, Humboldt State University, who
Susan Sonnenschein, University of Maryland, Baltimore worked on the Test Bank, Student Study Quizzes, and Video
County Curation; Thompson Davis III, Louisiana State University,
Hiroko Sotozaki, Western Illinois University and Lisa Rosen, Texas Woman’s University, both of whom
Caroline Stanley, Wilmington College of Ohio worked on the Test Bank and Student Study Quizzes; Melissa
Michael Steele, University of Utah Barnett, University of Arizona, who worked on the Classroom
Catherine Tamis-LaMonda, New York University Activities; and David Barner, University of California, San
Ross Thompson, University of Nebraska Diego, who with the help of Jessica Sullivan, worked on the
Elliot Turiel, University of California, Berkeley Lecture PowerPoints.
Eva Twetman, Halmstad University College Finally, I thank those many colleagues at Cornell and
Mike vanDuuren, University of Winchester Yale who have inspired and educated me on a daily basis.
Kristy vanMarle, University of Missouri
Peter Vishton, William & Mary College Frank Keil
Maria Wong, Idaho State University August 2013
Fei Xu, University of California, Berkeley
PREFACE xxiii
PA R T I
Framing Psychological
Development
1
Approaching Psychological
Development
Why Study Development? Perspectives on Development Studying Psychological
• Development as a Key to Understanding • Empiricist and Nativist Perspectives Development
Children’s Capabilities • Comparative and Evolutionary • Observational Studies
• Development as a Means for Insight into Perspectives • Experimental Studies
the Mature Form • Cross-Cultural Perspectives • Longitudinal Approaches
• Development and Social Policy • Neuroscience Perspectives • Cross-Sectional Approaches
The Nature of Development • Behaviorist Perspectives • Converging Methods
• Categorizing Developmental • Psychoanalytic Perspectives • Designing a Sound Study
Phenomena • Cognitive Science Perspectives
Conclusions
• Basic Questions about Psychological
Development Summary
baby is born and seems totally helpless. He can’t
hormone levels
FSH ity to see patterns in high resolution, which is called visual
Increases in DHEA acuity, gradually sharpens from birth through the first year
Androstenedione
of life and seems to be a clear case of quantitative change.
Estradiol
As we consider developmental patterns during particular
periods of life and within different domains, we will often
2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 ask if the changes seem to be continuous and quantitative
Age (in years) or if they seem more qualitative and therefore suggestive
FIGURE 1.7 Increases in hormone levels and qualitative of distinct developmental stages. In some cases, as you will
changes during puberty. There is a surge of blood hormone see, experts disagree about which kind of change is occur-
levels during puberty in both girls and boys that produces dramatic ring, and a series of experiments can often help us choose
qualitative physical changes in their bodies. LH (luteinizing hor- between the alternatives. These debates are important not
mone) stimulates ovulation in girls and growth of testes in boys; FSH just for understanding what kind of change is happening;
(follicle-stimulating hormone) stimulates the formation of ova in girls
they can also, for example, help teachers design strategies
and sperm in boys; DHEA (dehydroepiandrosterone) and androstene-
dione are involved in the production of testosterone and estrogen;
that suit children’s emerging abilities.
estradiol is produced as estrogen and affects breast development
and body shape in girls and bone maturation and growth in boys.
Adapted from Gordon and Laufer (2005). Q: What is the difference between stage-like
tative changes would differ from one involving qualitative
and continuous change?
changes. Imagine a child who simply grows larger and larger,
never changing in body proportions and never acquiring
any of the secondary sex characteristics that develop during
puberty. In an extreme case of purely quantitative change, Is Development Global or Local? In some cases, devel-
this child would continue growing at the same rate through- opmental change sweeps across many types of processes or
out his lifespan and never show other signs of aging. abilities at once. When such global changes occur, similar
This distinction between qualitative and quantitative developments occur at roughly the same time in very differ-
change also applies in the psychological realm. Consider, for ent areas of thought. Global changes are therefore general
example, the consistent finding that infants who are younger changes that apply across all situations or domains. For exam-
than 6 months old are unable to see the depth relations con- ple, during middle childhood, children seem to show strong
veyed in pictures, but after the first 6 months of life, they improvement in the ability to focus on the information most
perceive these relations easily (Yonas et al., 2002). Their relevant to a task and ignore irrelevant or distracting informa-
visual systems change qualitatively at around 6 months of tion (Houde et al., 2010). This new ability to sustain atten-
age, acquiring a whole new kind of functional capacity in a tion could support improvements in a variety of areas where
the ability to stay focused is critical. Thus, a child at this age
might show gains in making moral judgments, reasoning
about video game puzzles, and solving math problems.
Other times, particular skills and mental competencies
each show their own unique sets of developmental changes.
When development is characterized by such local changes,
different kinds of psychological capacities develop relatively
independently of each other. Local changes are therefore
specific changes that occur in restricted areas or domains.
In this case, a change in patterns of moral judgment would
not necessarily coincide with or resemble a change in under-
standing mathematical concepts. Consider an example in
which a researcher carefully tracks a child’s patterns of both
moral and mathematical reasoning from age 5 to 10. Dur-
ing this period, the child seems to undergo a major shift in
FIGURE 1.8 Qualitative change in depth perception. Because
3-month-old infants cannot yet see depth as it is depicted in two-
how she thinks about moral dilemmas. Perhaps in the early
dimensional pictures, they would not find this “impossible” figure years she has difficulty understanding immoral acts that
troubling. In contrast, a 7-month-old can see depth in pictures and cause no physical harm but may cause psychological harm,
might well find this illustration odd. but in later years she becomes attuned to the psychological
Strong
nurture
case
and every chapter that follows. In studying development, it helps to frame a set of phenomena in a manner that
is always useful to consider how these three questions reflect emphasizes some aspects over others. When different per-
on a particular aspect of development. The answers are not spectives are applied to the same phenomena, they often
always easy to determine, but just posing these questions raise different kinds of questions and therefore yield dif-
can guide further exploration of developmental phenomena. ferent answers. Because each perspective invariably leaves
out some aspects of a problem and emphasizes others, you
might think of them as different lenses for examining
patterns of change, each one bringing certain issues into
Perspectives on Development sharp focus. For these reasons, we will often find it useful
to consider how multiple perspectives provide their own
The fundamental questions just described are usually unique insights into a developmental pattern. It is also
posed within a particular psychological perspective on the important to keep in mind which perspectives are in play
process of development. These different perspectives— as you pose questions about psychological development.
which include comparative and evolutionary perspectives, We start by considering a pair of broad philosophical per-
cross-cultural perspectives, neuroscience perspectives, behav- spectives on development commonly known as empiri-
iorist perspectives, psychoanalytic perspectives, cognitive cism and nativism.
science perspectives, and more broadly empiricist and nativ-
ist perspectives—are each based on specific intellectual
traditions. They are each associated with certain research Empiricist and Nativist Perspectives
methods, styles of argument, and theoretical positions.
Thus, each perspective tends to approach developmental By the time psychology emerged as a separate discipline in
questions somewhat differently. the late nineteenth century, philosophers, biologists, and
Rather than contradicting each other, these perspec- others had been actively discussing many of its central issues
tives usually offer complementary approaches. Each one for centuries, and some of the most persistent, intriguing
A B C
FIGURE 1.11 The early empiricists. Empiricist philosophers (A) John Locke, (B) George Berkeley, and (C) David Hume supported the
idea that all knowledge is based on learned associations. This view of how the human mind comes to perceive the world, understand causality,
and have new ideas is still with us today.
PERSPECTIVES ON DEVELOPMENT 15
was impossible to account for the enormous differences in ing a mental map of your neighborhood in memory. These
people’s specific talents, like musical talent, mathematical nativist approaches suggest that we use specialized capacities
ability, or athleticism, only in terms of their prior “associa- for processing much more than just sensory input and that
tive” experiences. these specializations may include higher mental functions that
Instead, the nativists held that certain aspects of men- are far “downstream” from the perceptual system.
tal life were intrinsic to being human and that these Nativism is commonly focused on the influences of
capacities were present at birth. The nativists also agreed “nature” on development, while empiricism is linked to
that these inborn capacities were more specialized and “nurture” (Cowie, 1999), but these links can be misleading.
more complex than the general associative mechanism In fact, both empiricism and nativism are based on the idea
proposed by the empiricists. Beyond these points of that organisms must be born with initial capacities that help
agreement, however, there was—for good reason—enor- them perceive and understand their physical and social envi-
mous diversity among the nativists about the nature of ronment (see Figure 1.13). Sensory organs such as the eyes
the human mind. Without the insights of modern experi- and ears gather and process information right from birth,
mental psychology and cognitive science, they could only and they relay that information about the world to the brain,
speculate based on dissatisfaction with the empiricists’ which processes and interprets that information. Thus, both
much simpler model. traditions emphasize that nature endows the newborn with
The empiricist and nativist perspectives still influ- sensory organs and a brain with the capacity to process infor-
ence the study of development today, but at a much more mation from the start.
sophisticated level (Carruthers et al., 2007; Pinker, 2002, Empiricists and nativists disagree, however, on the rela-
2004). The nativists no longer have to rely on intuitions tive roles of nature and nurture in the particular ways we
that certain kinds of knowledge—of God, or morals, or process and interpret information as it moves “downstream”
mathematics—seem inborn. Instead, they now have exper- from the sensory organs to the highest levels of thought. For
imental data that they can analyze. They propose specific empiricists, once sensory information has undergone some
models of mind that make different predictions about such initial perceptual processing, nature’s only role is to provide
experimental data than do the models proposed by the for a general, all-purpose associative mechanism that allows
empiricists. the mind to link together any kind of inputs—whether
Empiricist models of psychological development typically they are visual or auditory, social or physical, or of any other
involve general learning mechanisms that apply in pretty sort—based on how often they co-occur.
much the same way to all sorts of content and experience. In In contrast, a nativist perspective tends to favor preex-
these models, the mind’s only specializations for handling dif- isting specializations not just for the simplest processing of
ferent kinds of information occur at early stages in the pro- incoming information but for all levels of the mind. Nativ-
cess, such as in the way we perceive the initial input from our ists believe that infants come into the world with mental
different sense organs. Nativist approaches, on the other hand, structures tailored for learning specific kinds of informa-
focus on learning mechanisms tailored for processing particu- tion, such as language, moral principles, or number con-
lar kinds of information, such as learning language or retain- cepts. These theories consider the brain’s specializations
A B C
FIGURE 1.12 Intrinsic behavioral tendencies. People have recognized for centuries that different dog breeds can have distinct disposi-
tions and reasoning capacities in areas like (A) retrieving (as in this Labrador retriever), (B) herding (as in this sheepdog), and (C) attacking
(as in this Doberman). These kinds of strong, intrinsic behavioral differences among animals provided early support for a nativist view of
development.
FIGURE 1.14 Comparative and evolutionary perspectives. These perspectives often examine a comparable developmental process
across several species, as shown in these examples of mothers caring for their young. Researchers may then ask if these behaviors are alike
at a deeper psychological level—perhaps because they have evolved to serve similar functions.
PERSPECTIVES ON DEVELOPMENT 17
FIGURE 1.15 Cross-cultural perspectives. Developmental researchers often ask if developmental patterns unfold in the same ways across
cultures. For example, do friendships form in similar ways in all these groups of children? Are the roles of boys and girls the same in all cultures?
fragile young survive to an age of self-sufficiency. A true already own more than things you are about to acquire, a
appreciation and a genuine understanding of developmen- bias known as the endowment effect. Comparative research
tal psychology must keep in mind the common problems has demonstrated that monkeys show similar biases even
that many organisms confront, even as they sometimes solve without observing similar behaviors in others (Lakshmi-
them quite differently. naryanan et al., 2008). Those results suggest that future
Closely related to comparative perspectives, evolution- developmental studies of the endowment effect in humans
ary perspectives ask how and why a particular trait, whether may find its earliest traces even before culture has much of
it is a body part or a behavior, emerged over successive an influence.
generations of a population through the process of natural
selection. Ethology is the study of traits from an adaptive
evolutionary perspective that usually involves comparisons
across species. Thus, ethologists examine how certain traits Cross-Cultural Perspectives
improve a species’ fitness within its specific environment, Before making general statements about the development
conferring advantages that make members of the species of any process, whether it is a child’s attachment to a
that have these traits more likely to survive and produce parent, the concept of self, or almost any other aspect of
viable offspring. Understanding how particular traits psychological development, it is crucial to gain a sense
affect a species’ ability to reproduce in its environment also of how that process develops in different cultures (see
requires closely analyzing the local environment itself in Figure 1.15). All too often, we take our own culture’s pat-
terms of its physical features and the other organisms that terns as the obvious default options, only to be surprised
inhabit it. that these familiar tendencies actually may be quite rare
Evolutionary and comparative perspectives, however, in other cultures.
do far more than describe the range of patterns of devel- The cross-cultural perspective on development focuses on
opment and how they relate to a population’s particular two main issues. First, how do cultural variations influ-
environment. Using these perspectives, we can pose specific ence patterns of development? Research addressing this
questions about the origins of various psychological capaci- question has become increasingly sophisticated in recent
ties, including our earliest emotions. For example, feelings years, demonstrating how development in such diverse
of disgust at bodily excretions do not seem to be present areas as moral reasoning, folk biology (intuitive patterns
at birth, but they emerge in all cultures during the third of reasoning about the living world), and religious beliefs
year of life. From an evolutionary perspective, minimiz- differ between cultures. The most insightful approaches
ing contact with bodily excretions makes sense, since this have developed models in which specific aspects of cul-
would reduce the spread of disease. But perhaps there are tures, such as cultural values or norms, are thought to
also evolutionary reasons that disgust doesn’t appear until have distinct psychological outcomes. For example, some
an age when the child is actually able to do something to cross-cultural researchers have argued that growing up in
avoid the contamination by means of toilet training and a collectivist culture, where the group is considered more
hygiene. Researchers are currently trying to learn whether important than the individual, results in different patterns
both factors—the desire to avoid contamination and the of reasoning than growing up in a more individualist cul-
inability to do so before about the age of 3—affect the ture (Nisbett et al., 2001).
way the emotion of disgust develops. Comparative analyses The cross-cultural perspective’s second main concern is
often pose similar questions about the roots of particular with consistency across cultures: What aspects of behavior
human tendencies, such as the tendency to value things you or mind, if any, develop in the same way throughout the
PERSPECTIVES ON DEVELOPMENT 19
FIGURE 1.17 Behaviorist perspectives. Behaviorist perspectives
on development ask how various external reinforcements, such as
rewards and punishments, can shape a child’s behavior. Here, stick-
ers seem to be a strong reward for a recent behavior.
A
cross fields of psychology, researchers are studying critical questions about early changes in brain functions and
brain activity to shed light on the processes underly- psychological processes (Minagawa-Kawai et al., 2011; Wilcox
ing thought and action. One commonly used set of et al., 2005).
techniques, called functional neuroimaging, involves One interesting use of NIRS has been to explore whether
taking precise measurements of neuronal activity and repre- infants’ brains have regions specialized for processing numbers
senting them as images that show different levels of activation long before the child can speak. Researchers monitored infants’
in specific brain regions. In studies of older children or adults, brain activity with NIRS as the infants repeatedly viewed groups
a technique known as functional magnetic resonance imaging of objects. Most of these groupings had 16 items, but occasion-
(fMRI) has become the dominant method of brain imaging. This ally the infants saw an “oddball” array with a different number.
method takes advantage of the finding that when neurons in Intriguingly, 6-month-olds reliably showed increased activation
a specific area of the brain are active, the amount of oxygen- of a particular region of the brain’s right hemisphere when a
ated blood flowing to that area increases. Thus, fMRI scanners deviant number appeared in the display (Hyde et al., 2010).
work on the principle that at a molecular level, oxygenated blood Further research will be needed to determine whether this pat-
reacts differently in a magnetic field than deoxygenated blood. tern of results is specific to observing a change in number. By
By measuring this difference in the brain of a volunteer while helping researchers to address questions like this and to make
he performs a particular mental task, researchers are able to inferences about the mental capacities of preverbal infants,
see which brain areas receive greater blood flow—and to infer NIRS is likely to be an especially useful tool for studying early
that those areas are the most active. This kind of neuroimag- brain development.
ing can capture detailed images of activity in particular brain
areas, but it requires that the participant be very still inside a
scanner, usually for 15 minutes or longer. During the scan, the
machine’s powerful magnets also cause sudden, loud pulsing
noises. For these reasons, the technique is difficult to use with
young infants.
Recently, a new silent brain-scanning technique known as
near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) has been developed that can
tolerate substantial head movements without compromising the
resulting images, and it seems to be safe for use with infants
(see Figure 1.19). NIRS involves shining a low-energy band of
near-infrared light through the infant’s scalp and skull and mea-
suring the infrared light that is reflected back. Based on the
reflection patterns, researchers can determine blood oxygen-
ation levels in different areas of the brain (Aslin & Mehler, 2005;
Hoshi, 2007; Meek, 2002; Sakatani et al., 1999; Villringer & FIGURE 1.19 Near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) and brain
Chance, 1997). Thus, like fMRI, NIRS is a way of measuring activity. This infant’s brain activity is being measured with NIRS.
which brain regions are more active based on the increased Low-energy near-infrared pulses of light are transmitted through
the scalp and skull onto brain regions, then reflected back onto
flow of oxygenated blood to those areas. Using precise sensors,
sensors near the light transmission points. This procedure,
these measurements can be taken with relatively low levels of which is as harmless as a few minutes of bright sunlight, allows
infrared light, comparable to a few minutes of bright sunlight. researchers to measure blood flow changes in various parts of the
This technique offers an important new way of addressing infant’s brain.
PERSPECTIVES ON DEVELOPMENT 21
Studying Psychological
Psychology
Development
Not surprisingly, it takes a wide range of approaches to study
topics as diverse as a newborn infant’s ability to see color and
an adolescent’s ways of relating to her peers. Throughout
Computer
Linguistics this book, we will encounter various methods of investigat-
science
Cognitive
science
ing particular questions about development. At the outset,
it is useful to survey the range of research techniques that
we’ll encounter to provide a methodological road map and
to explain some key terms.
Philosophy Neuroscience
Observational Studies
FIGURE 1.20 Cognitive science perspectives. Cognitive science
The oldest, and seemingly simplest, method of research
perspectives often ask about how people represent and use informa- is the observational study, in which children are simply
tion. Cognitive scientists draw on the theories and methods of several observed in a natural setting and their behaviors are carefully
disciplines to see how each one offers unique insights into common recorded (see Figure 1.21). These records may take the form
questions. of trained observers’ written notes or collections of audio or
video recordings. But this method is not always as simple
change over the course of development (see Figure 1.20). To as it sounds. Suppose you are interested in researching how
fully address these questions, they seek to integrate many children’s personalities develop between ages 3 and 5. Per-
ways of studying information processing, including formal haps you plan to observe a classroom of 20 preschoolers to
computation (computer science), biological bases (neurosci- test some ideas about how the traits of shyness, aggression,
ence), logical consistency (philosophy), expression in language and leadership develop and how they relate to the child’s pre-
(linguistics), and the way these different types of informa- school experiences. (For example, shy children may seek out
tion processing take place in a real organism in real time different environments within the classroom than extroverted
(psychology). children.) What observational data would you collect? You
In practice, a cognitive science researcher may focus could potentially measure thousands of different variables:
within one discipline, but will nonetheless consider how the number of seconds between each conversation for each
the progress in other relevant disciplines informs her work. child, the direction and duration of each eye gaze, a vast array
For example, a cognitive scientist interested in how lan- of facial expressions or body postures, the objects a child
guage is acquired might conduct experiments on language
acquisition in children (psychology), but when designing
experiments, she might also look at formal descriptions of
language capacity (linguistics), conceptual analyses of the
relationship between language and thought (philosophy),
models of language acquisition in computer systems (com-
puter science), and relations between brain development
and language development (neuroscience). In the study of
development, questions about the origins of thought and
knowledge and about the ways that skills and abilities
change over time naturally lead to considerations of what
kinds of information processing the developing organism
is doing.
We will frequently consider the cognitive science approach
in this book—not only in areas of development traditionally
associated with it, such as visual perception and logical rea-
soning, but also in areas such as social and emotional devel- FIGURE 1.21 Observational studies. Videos of children in
opment. In almost all areas of psychology, it is useful to everyday settings offer rich opportunities for data collection, but
consider how people represent and use relevant information. coding and interpreting the footage also create challenges.
E
xperimental techniques are invaluable in developmental Another critical issue in the IRB’s review, and one that is
research, but unlike observational studies, experiments especially important in studies involving infants or children, is
bring children and infants into environments they would the participants’ ability to consent to take part. Before they con-
not otherwise encounter. How do researchers justify sent, potential participants should be told as much about the
manipulating children’s experience? experimental procedures and goals as possible without com-
Clearly, not all experiments are justified. Few would approve promising the research. The researcher must also be sure that
of an experiment that caused children to feel intense shame and all participants understand that they are free to discontinue the
discomfort in order to study their coping strategies. It strikes study at any time and are under no obligation to finish a session
us as simply wrong to purposely upset children or make them or a task. In the United States, a child under age 18 is not con-
uncomfortable, regardless of the value of the scientific insights sidered fully capable of consenting to participate in research.
that such an experiment might yield. But what about a more Along with the child’s own consent, a parent or legal guardian
benign experiment in which an infant stares at a stimulus for must give written consent as well. In addition, researchers who
10 minutes until he is bored? And is it acceptable to pay a work with children must watch them carefully throughout the
6-year-old a large sum of money to participate in an extremely experiment for any signs that they would like to discontinue the
tedious task? Researchers must carefully consider the risks and study, and their wishes must be honored. During and after a
benefits for participants in their studies—especially when work- study, researchers typically are also obligated to protect partici-
ing with children and infants, since they are much less able to pants’ privacy, and the records are usually coded to make sure
advocate for themselves than are most adults. The researcher’s no one reading about the research can identify the participants.
goal, as in all research on humans, is to minimize any risks to Because experiments involve studying people outside of
participants and to directly benefit both society at large and the normal daily circumstances, they often raise the clearest ques-
participants themselves to the greatest extent possible. tions about potential risks, but concerns about costs and ben-
In most countries with large-scale human research programs, efits to participants are also relevant to observational studies.
every university, hospital, or other institution that undertakes such Many observational studies risk intruding in children’s lives, as
research is legally required to have an institutional review board, the observers may be in plain view, unintentionally influencing
commonly called an IRB. An IRB typically consists of research- the children just by being present. Even so, most researchers
ers from a wide range of fields who are engaged in studies of assume that children who have (with parental approval) con-
humans, and it often also includes members of the community sented to participate will fairly quickly become accustomed to
outside the research institution. On behalf of the research institu- being observed and will behave normally. Concerns about confi-
tion, IRB members scrutinize all research proposals that involve dentiality are as important in observational studies as they are in
human subjects to ensure that the planned studies do not pose experimental studies—maybe more so, since the natural behav-
unreasonable or unnecessary risks for those who participate. ior being observed may be more revealing of the child’s true
Ideally, researchers should always analyze the costs and ben- nature. A child who cheats in a contrived laboratory card game
efits of their own studies and strive to ensure that their benefits may not be nearly as stigmatized as one observed cheating in
outweigh their potential risks. Risks to participants might range the classroom. For all these reasons, observational studies usu-
from discomfort in doing an experimental task to self-esteem ally require participants’ consent, but researchers may opt not
problems if a child finds out that her peers outperformed her. to reveal the focus of their research until after the observation is
On the other hand, participants can also benefit from taking complete to avoid influencing children’s behavior.
part; in fact, some tasks might be intensely engaging for young Many important research questions in developmental psy-
participants. More broadly, a study’s benefits to society include chology, such as how children deal with failure, are particularly
any information the research yields that furthers understanding difficult to study because of ethical concerns. Children are an
of development, so as to better inform decisions about chil- especially vulnerable group, so it is only reasonable that devel-
dren’s education, care, and welfare. opmental research should be conducted extremely carefully.
outdated or flawed by the time the study is over. Retaining to experimental studies. Unlike the longitudinal approach,
a large enough group of participants over time is also chal- which requires the same participants to repeat the experimen-
lenging, as families move or no longer want to participate. tal tasks at different ages, the cross-sectional approach involves
Finally, a subtle problem can confound the more experi- studying a different group of children in each age group. As
mental longitudinal studies. Suppose you assessed under- a result, each child is assessed only once, and there is no need
standing of fractions by repeatedly presenting children to worry about the experimental tasks accidentally becoming
with a series of problems to answer. If you do this every a form of training. Most of the experimental studies discussed
6 months for 6 years, the experimental assessments them- in the chapters that follow will be cross-sectional.
selves can become a form of training that affects how the The largest drawback of cross-sectional studies concerns
participants’ understanding of fractions develops. In this their inability to follow specific individuals’ development over
case, you would be studying the effects of participating in the time, which makes them much less sensitive than longitudi-
experiment rather than the natural course of development. nal studies to individual differences in developmental paths.
For that reason, experimental longitudinal studies are often For that reason, psychologists who are interested in how indi-
used to study the effects of training on the development of vidual differences develop over time, such as how personality
particular skills or capacities. traits emerge, tend to use longitudinal approaches. Nonethe-
less, cross-sectional studies can shed light on other kinds of
individual differences, such as how a behavior manifests at
Q: What are some drawbacks of longitudinal particular points in development. For example, a researcher
studies? might be interested in how shy children cope with large
groups at the ages of 5, 10, and 15. A cross-sectional obser-
vational study could enroll 20 shy children at each of these
ages and then observe the children’s interactions within large
groups to see how the behaviors of each age group differed.
Cross-Sectional Approaches By comparing the coping methods of the 5-year-olds, the
A study that examines developmental change by compar- 10-year-olds, and the 15-year-olds, the researchers could learn
ing groups of children at different ages is known as a cross- how those behaviors varied with age.
sectional approach (Figure 1.22B). To use this approach to Aside from these specific strengths and weaknesses, a dif-
study how children learn fractions, you might select six groups ferent sort of factor related to broad historical patterns can
of 20 children each, taking a group of kindergartners, a group also influence whether a researcher uses a cross-sectional
of second-graders, and so on, through a group of tenth- or longitudinal design. If the changes within a culture over
graders, and then test each group’s abilities to solve prob- time significantly alter children’s experiences, research that
lems using fractions. With sufficient research staff, the entire compares children of different ages can be subject to cohort
study could be conducted in just a few weeks, rather than the effects, in which a given age group (or cohort) might dif-
10 years that a longitudinal design would require. The cross- fer in important ways from people in the same culture who
sectional approach provides another advantage when it comes are somewhat older or younger. Suppose you were interested
Converging Methods
Developmental psychologists, like other psychologists, often
use several different ways to explore a particular problem. As
Designing a Sound Study
we have seen, each method has its own advantages and limita- Beyond the tradeoffs involved in different types of devel-
tions. As researchers apply more methods to the same prob- opmental studies, several concerns about research methods
lem, the different approaches tend to complement each other apply to almost all psychological studies. These concerns
in ways that create a more accurate overall picture. This pro- are relevant regardless of whether the research participants
cess is known as using converging methods. As researchers are adults or children. At the most general level, researchers
use various approaches to probe a phenomenon from different need to make sure their findings are as reliable, valid, and
vantage points, the results of the different types of studies tend replicable as possible.
to converge on a more accurate, objective answer than any
single method could produce. For example, we might observe Reliability and Validity The issue of reliability refers to
that certain forms of parent-child interactions are correlated whether researchers would obtain consistent results if they
with the children having earlier autobiographical memories or others repeated the research study in the future using
when they grow up. But to be more confident that the interac- the same types of participants. More precisely, researchers
tion is having a causal effect on earliest memories, we might often talk about the reliability of a specific way of measur-
also conduct an experimental study in which we taught ran- ing behavior, such as a test or an experimental task, rather
domly assigned parents to interact in a particular way with than the study as a whole. A highly reliable measure will
their young children and then later asked those children if consistently yield the same pattern of results, as long as
they had earlier memories than those of the children in a con- investigators have controlled for other possible influences.
trol group whose parents had not been taught to interact in Such a measure is analogous to a tool that works the same
this particular way. Similarly, if we perform a cross-sectional way every time rather than working only sometimes.
study of children between the ages of 2 and 7 who are sam- Suppose researchers are testing a theory that young chil-
pled 1 year apart and observe that the correlation emerges at dren remember events they see in videos more accurately
around age 7, we might then do a longitudinal study to see how than events described in words. The researchers develop a
early parent training has to occur to have a maximal effect. way of testing both kinds of memory, and they design a
Consider, therefore, the ways in which the advantages and study using this memory test at several different ages. The
disadvantages of observational and experimental studies and measure has reliability problems if it yields different results
longitudinal and cross-sectional approaches overlap. Table 1.1 when repeatedly used with children in the same age group
illustrates the main strengths and weaknesses of each and how who have the same demographic characteristics.
they intersect in different types of studies. These tradeoffs There are two forms of reliability: test-retest reliability
illustrate the importance of using converging methods. and interrater reliability. Test-retest reliability is an indica-
Observational + Rich data may allow for + May uncover long-term patterns of + Reveal distinctive patterns for each
Studies unanticipated insights. change in individuals. age group.
− Support correlational arguments − Require a long time commitment − Questions about individual
but cannot determine causal from research staff. differences are more difficult to
relationships. address.
− Behaviors of interest may not
appear without experimenter
intervention.
Experimental + Allow exploration of cause-and- + Good for studying long-term effects + Enable quick assessment of
Studies effect relationships. of training. hypothesized differences between age
+ Researchers can design focused − Repeated assessments may groups.
assessments of specific variables. become an unwanted form of − May miss key transitions as well as
− Create possibilities of ecologically training. individual developmental patterns.
invalid measures.
TABLE 1.1 Tradeoffs of different developmental research methods. The advantages (+) and disadvantages (−) of each kind of study
and each kind of approach are shown here, as well as the advantages (+) and disadvantages (−) of using converging methods—longitudinal
observational studies, cross-sectional observational studies, longitudinal experimental studies, and cross-sectional experimental studies.
tion of how consistently a measure yields the same result of children’s verbal and visual memories could easily get
when it is used by the same researcher. If a given researcher consistent results, which would also closely resemble the
finds large variability in results when repeatedly measuring results obtained from using the same methods with new
the quality of children’s memories, that would be a case of groups of similar children. Nonetheless, these highly reli-
low test-retest reliability. Interrater reliability describes how able measures may be assessing something very different
consistently two different researchers get the same results from what the investigators intended to measure—if the
when they use the same measure with the same child. To measures themselves are flawed. That is, children’s mem-
have high interrater reliability on a test of children’s memo- ory scores may not really reflect their ability to recall the
ries of videos and verbal descriptions, it would be impor- videos and verbal descriptions. Perhaps the younger chil-
tant for the test to provide scoring guidelines describing dren are more easily distracted, which causes them to
how much detail is required to consider a child’s recall fully underperform unless they are tested in an especially quiet,
“accurate.” Otherwise, individual researchers could make focused setting. In this case, this particular memory mea-
different judgments of what constitutes an “accurate” mem- sure would consistently suggest that young children have
ory. In a well-designed study, the measures should show poorer memories than they actually do. The study would
both test-retest reliability and interrater reliability. have reliability, but it would not be valid for the skill that
Reliability is a necessary part of research in developmen- the researchers intended to measure.
tal psychology, but reliability alone is not enough to con- Like reliability, validity comes in different forms: internal
clude that research findings are sound. Even highly reliable validity and external validity. Internal validity is especially
measurements can be misleading if it turns out that you relevant to experimental studies. It concerns whether the
are not really measuring what you intended to measure. changes observed in the dependent variable are really due to
The criterion of validity refers to whether measurements the experimenter’s manipulation of the independent variable
accurately reflect what a researcher means to study. Return- or whether the changes are caused by other extraneous influ-
ing to the example that compared visual and verbal memory, ences. If, for example, a young child performed poorly on a
suppose the researcher develops a set of measures showing memory test because she was distracted (rather than due to an
very high test-retest and interrater reliability. Thus, differ- inability to remember), the study would lack internal valid-
ent researchers using these methods to test the accuracy ity. External validity refers to whether the research findings
STU DY AN D REVI EW
A B
FIGURE 2.1 Extreme premature birth and development. (A) Madeline Mann was born at 27 weeks and had an extremely low birth weight
because of complications of her mother’s pregnancy. After her birth, she was hooked up to a breathing tube and kept alive through extraordi-
nary measures. (B) Madeline survived and today is a college student without the physical or neurological impairments suffered by most infants
born at extremely low birth weights.
Results:
There was an extensive increase in activation levels of genes levels of activity of cholesterol synthesis genes, which might
related to cholesterol synthesis in the offspring of males fed possibly be related to increases in heart disease.
a low-protein diet. In general, there was more methylation of
many genes in the low-protein groups, especially of a gene Conclusion:
that regulated fatty acid synthesis. When methylation led this Feeding males a low-protein diet from birth to maturity can
gene to be turned off, it may in turn have led to the increased result in gene-regulated metabolic changes in their offspring.
Source study: Carone et al. (2010).
Meiosis Mitosis
Maternal Paternal
Chromosome 1 Chromosome 2
chromosome chromosome
DNA replication
Duplicated
DNA replication
chromosomes
Crossing–over
Duplicated
chromosomes
Segregation
of crossed–over
pairs of chromosomes
Segregation into
single chromosomes
Further segregation
into single
chromosomes
A B
FIGURE 2.5 Meiosis and mitosis. (A) Meiosis is a special kind of cell division that prepares cells for sexual recombination. Normal
human cells have 23 pairs of chromosomes. One chromosome of each parent goes to the sperm and egg cells that will be part of reproduction.
(B) Mitosis is the process in which cell division occurs, but the chromosomes from both parents are copied and appear in all the new cells.
A B
FIGURE 2.6 Trisomy 21 and Down syndrome. (A) A normal karyotype and a trisomy 21 karyotype. The upper panel shows a karyotype in
which there is a pair for each of the 23 chromosomes. The lower panel shows a karyotype leading to Down syndrome, in which there are three
copies of chromosome 21. (B) People with Down syndrome often have characteristic facial features that make them visually identifiable. A small
amount of extra genetic material in part of the twenty-first chromosome causes a large set of effects, ranging from these facial features to heart
defects and a variety of cognitive problems.
Q: What are some examples of the effects of FIGURE 2.7 Fertilization. In the process of fertilization, 50 to
100 sperm, out of an original group of millions, finally reach the egg.
having extra or missing genetic material in the The thread-like projections are the tails of sperm trying to enter the
human genome? large, spherical egg.
Fertilization During sexual intercourse, many millions of and eggs, they are only as genetically similar as any two
sperm are released and swim in search of an egg to fertil- siblings—that is, they have half their genes in common.
ize. Out of the millions that start the journey, only 50 to
100 sperm reach the egg and attempt to penetrate it (see
Figure 2.7). When one sperm finally penetrates the egg,
it triggers a series of chemical events that prevent all other The First Patterns of Differentiation
sperm from entering, as well as allowing the penetrating Around 2 hours after fertilization, the zygote makes the
sperm to move farther into the egg and fuse with the egg’s first of many cell divisions. Initially, as the cells divide rap-
genetic material (Gilbert, 2010). At this point, the fertilized idly by mitosis, they get smaller with each division, such
egg is known as the zygote. If the zygote has an X chromo- that the overall mass of the zygote does not increase. When
some and a Y chromosome, a male is created; if the zygote the cell divisions result in a 16-cell body at day 4, this com-
has two X chromosomes, a female is created. pact, solid ball of cells is known as a morula, and it shows
Conception can result in twins or other kinds of multiple the first signs of cell differentiation: its outer cells are larger
births. If the fertilized egg splits into two separate cells, each than its inner ones. At around day 5, the larger outer cells
of which continues to develop into individuals, identical of the morula become clearly distinct from the inner ones.
twins result. They are also known as monozygotic (MZ) These outer cells (the trophoblast) will eventually become
twins because the two babies came from the same zygote, part of the placenta (an organ that will make possible
which is also the reason for their nearly identical genotypes. the transfer of nutrients and oxygen from the mother and
Biologists used to think that MZ twins had exactly the same wastes and carbon dioxide from the fetus). The encircling
genotype, but it turns out that subtle epigenetic effects can outer cells trigger a group of the smaller inner cells to clus-
arise during in utero development and affect one or more ter in a ball-like structure, which eventually will become
sections of DNA, which can cause differences in the twins’ the embryo (see Figure 2.8). This overall structure—the
susceptibility to certain diseases or subtle physical differences outer ring of cells and the inner cell mass—is known as the
(Bruder et al., 2008; Ollikainen & Craig, 2011). By contrast, blastocyst.
when two different sperm fertilize two different eggs, dizy- In humans, about 16 days after fertilization, the blasto-
gotic (DZ) twins (or twins from two zygotes; also called cyst’s inner cell mass undergoes an important cell differen-
fraternal twins) result. Because DZ twins develop from two tiation process known as gastrulation, which is oriented
separate conceptions involving genetically different sperm relative to the point where the sperm entered the egg. During
Anatomical Development
A 2–cell stage B 4–cell stage C 8–cell stage So far, we have focused on changes at the cellular level, many
of which are visible only with a microscope. Within just a
Inner cell few weeks after conception, however, noticeable anatomical
mass
features start to appear, and they change very rapidly in the
following months.
Trophoblast
D Morula E Early blastocyst F Late blastocyst Structures and Systems in
FIGURE 2.8 The formation of the blastocyst. In the earliest the Embryo and Fetus
stages of growth, the fertilized egg progresses from apparently undif-
ferentiated cells to the beginnings of specialization. After fertilization and cell divisions that make the zygote into
a multicelled organism, rudimentary nervous and circulatory
systems and body structures appear and grow rapidly, becom-
gastrulation, a cascade of changes in chemical concentrations ing specialized for particular functions (see Table 2.1). We
relative to this orientation point soon leads to bilateral sym- will briefly discuss the development of systems and structures
metry, a hallmark of most complex organisms. An organism from 2 to 8 weeks after conception (during the embryonic
that shows bilateral symmetry has identical structures on period) and from 9 to 36 weeks after conception (during the
both sides of a center line (right and left kidneys, arms, legs, fetal period).
eyes, ears, and so on).
In the process of gastrulation, cells and cell clusters move The Embryonic Period At about 2 weeks after concep-
within the blastocyst to form three distinct layers (see Fig- tion, the fertilized egg attaches to the uterine wall (see
ure 2.9). The outermost layer of cells, the ectoderm, even- Figure 2.10). After this implantation, it becomes more com-
tually forms the skin and, perhaps more surprisingly, the mon to refer to the fertilized egg as an embryo (the embry-
nervous system. The middle layer, the mesoderm, becomes onic period lasts from the middle of the second week until
internal tissues, such as the skeleton, internal organs, and the end of the eighth week after conception). The first signs
muscles. And the blastocyst’s innermost cell layer, the endo- of legs and arms are visible at about the third week. The heart
derm, develops into some of the glands, such as the thy- starts to beat at around 18 days, pumping blood around a rap-
roid, and the specialized tissues lining organs such as the idly expanding circulatory system. Limb buds appear as small
lungs. A matrix of fine fibers within the blastocyst directs bumps in the arm region, and similar buds emerge later in
the cells’ migrations, much like a set of guide rails along the leg region, following the head-to-toe principle of develop-
which the cells move. A great deal is happening during ment in which structures tend to develop earlier in the more
these migrations. Specific sequences of genes become acti- anterior regions of the embryo (situated toward the head or
vated and deactivated as cells in the different layers start to front). Development in which the more anterior structures of
develop specialized features. The identities of these cells soon an organism mature earlier than structures in the posterior
become nearly irreversible (Cyranoski, 2007; Jackson et al., sections is known as cephalocaudal development (cephalo =
2010; Verfaillie, 2002). head, caudal = tail).
How do the cells on the inside of the blastocyst, which In humans and all other mammals, as well as birds and
eventually become parts of the embryo, differentiate from the other reptiles, the key event in gastrulation, during which
blastocyst’s external cells, which will form the placenta? The the blastocyst starts to develop distinct layers of cells,
relative positions of these cells in the blastocyst seem to trigger is the formation of the primitive streak, which occurs at
different sets of genes within them, leading the cells to special- the end of week 2 in humans. The primitive streak is a
ize for different functions. Presumably, the cells in the inner ridge on one side of the blastocyst through which cells
layer and outer layer receive different kinds of chemical signals pass to become the mesoderm and endoderm. Where and
(Surani et al., 2007; Thomas et al., 2004). If an outer-layer cell when they pass through the streak influences whether the
is moved to an inner position at an early enough point, it will cells in each layer will become part of the head, the gut,
genetically and functionally develop like an inner-layer cell, or the tail (if the species has a tail). Cells within the streak
becoming part of the embryo rather than the placenta. The itself appear to produce chemical signals that guide other
cells to migrate toward the streak (Alev et al., 2010; Keller, head (see Figure 2.11). This head-to-toe progression is a
2005; Spagnoli & Hemmati-Brivanlou, 2008). Early- powerful organizing principle of embryonic development.
entering cells migrate toward one end of the streak, which Some cells that migrate into the streak end up forming the
may produce stronger signals, and become the head. Later- notochord, which signals ectodermal cells above it to form
entering cells, as well as those that pass through the streak the neural plate, which lengthens and drops down into the
farther down, become other types of structures. Thus, the surface to form a neural groove (at around day 18 in humans)
streak, which is one of the first signs of bilateral symmetry, with ridges on the sides. Eventually, these grooves fold in
also triggers a very early bias toward development in the toward each other, join, and form a hollow neural tube.
TABLE 2.1 Milestones of prenatal development. The timetable of gross anatomical and physiological development of the human embryo
and fetus is highly regular in normal cases. Although many structures are present in the first 5 months, not until well into the sixth month are
babies at all likely to survive birth.
ANATOMICAL DEVELOPMENT 45
After 3 weeks, the embryo’s cells begin to differentiate.
Cells that have identical genetic material take on different
roles that support the development of a well-formed body.
How can cells with the same genetic sequence become fin-
gers, kidneys, brain cells, and muscles?
One key principle is that of successive differentiation.
As specific cells develop, they narrow down their func-
tional specialization in a step-by-step fashion, as shown in
Figure 2.9. Thus, certain cells might first differentiate into
three general groups: cells that can form muscles, organs, or
the skeleton (bones). At first, the cells in the muscle group
can become any kind of muscle, but at a later stage, most of
those cells further differentiate into one of two main types
of muscle. At an even later point, they specialize to form
FIGURE 2.10 Implantation of the embryo. At 6 days, the blasto-
particular parts of specific muscles.
cyst starts to implant on the uterine wall, as shown in this image. By
2 weeks, it is fully implanted and is about 0.01 inch long and is now
Dramatic changes in body structure occur between
known as the embryo. 4 and 8 weeks. At 4 weeks, the embryo is about 3 milli-
meters (about 0.125 inch) long. By the end of week 8, the
embryo is well over 25 millimeters (about 1 inch) long. Even
This process of initial brain formation, called neurulation, though it is only a little over an inch long, it already can be
is completed in week 4. The neural tube eventually becomes clearly seen as having human form.
both the brain and the spinal cord. The notochord acts as a
guiding framework for the spinal cord, with its cells eventu- The Fetal Period From the ninth week until birth, the
ally becoming parts of the vertebrae of the spinal column in growing human organism is referred to as the fetus. Over
humans. If the neural tube does not close properly, however, the next several weeks, the fetus changes dramatically (see
neural tube defects (for example, spina bifida, in which part Figure 2.12). It shows increasingly well-articulated fingers
of the spinal cord protrudes) will result. and toes. At 9 weeks, its four-chambered heart is clearly seen
A B C
FIGURE 2.11 The embryonic period. (A) A 3-week-old embryo. The embryo’s anterior structures, such as the head, tend to develop sooner
than the posterior areas. (B) A 4-week-old embryo. By 4 weeks of age, the human embryo has a visible beating heart and the beginnings of
limb buds, as well as a distinct head region. The heartbeat can be detected through ultrasound. (C) A 6-week-old embryo. The principle that
structures near the head develop before those near the tail is complemented by the principle that structures near the body’s midline tend to
develop before those farther out to the sides.
FIGURE 2.12 Prenatal development from 8 weeks to 36 weeks. From 8 weeks, both external and internal anatomical structures become
further differentiated and more human in appearance. Development typically occurs earlier in the head region than in regions closer to the toes.
in ultrasound, as are visible facial features. Some of its neu- At 36 weeks, the baby weighs about 6.5 pounds and is
ral connections are functioning, allowing it to show some nearly full term. At this point, its movement in the womb
reflexes. is restricted because the baby is so tightly packed into the
By 12 weeks, it is quite easy to see most of the same ana- space. After its birth at 38 weeks of development, a newborn
tomical features in the fetus that are visible in newborns. baby immediately breathes, cries, shows a range of behaviors
The fetus at this point is physiologically much more sophis- and reflexes . . . and starts to learn about the world.
ticated than it was as an embryo, and most of its major
bodily systems are partially functional. The fetus swallows,
its muscles cause joint movements, and its kidneys now
make urine. After 16 weeks, the fetus weighs 6 ounces and Preterm Births
its external body parts are much more distinct. At 20 weeks, A significant number of babies are born less than 38 weeks
the fetus weighs about 1 pound, its hair is more evident, and after conception. When babies are born 3 or more weeks
its heartbeat can be detected with a stethoscope. early, they are known as preterm (or premature) infants and
At 22 weeks, the fetus weighs about 1.25 pounds; this is typically weigh less than 5.5 pounds, or 2.5 kilograms (see
the earliest stage at which a small number of premature new- Figure 2.13). In the United States, roughly 12.5 percent of
borns may survive birth, although this is possible only with births are preterm, while in Europe, the rate is approximately
the intensive medical care available at major hospitals, and the 7 percent (Goldenberg et al., 2008). About 5 percent of pre-
child is still likely to face many long-term disabilities. At 28 term births occur before 28 weeks gestational age and are
weeks, the fetus weighs close to 3 pounds. Its lungs typically known as extremely preterm births.
have developed enough to be able to breathe air if birth is Several forms of physical and psychological stress can
premature. By 6 months, the fetus is sufficiently well formed increase the likelihood of a preterm birth. One major cause
and physiologically mature that roughly 50 percent of infants appears to be infections in either the mother or the fetus,
born at that stage will survive if given intensive medical care. which can indirectly cause either the onset of early labor or
By 8 months, most infants will survive birth without major breaks in the membranes protecting the fetus (Goldenberg
medical interventions. Once the fetus can survive on its own et al., 2008). Even infections far removed from the uterus,
in the outside world, it is often called a baby. such as dental and gum infections, can increase the chances
ANATOMICAL DEVELOPMENT 47
(Volpe, 2009). The many mechanisms of damage are not well
understood, making it difficult for physicians to reduce the
rate of complications associated with extreme preterm births,
even as infants are increasingly able to survive them.
Preterm births are also associated with a range of cogni-
tive difficulties in memory, attention, and language skills, as
well as with mental retardation in very severe cases (Allen,
2008; Anderson & Doyle, 2008; Rijken et al., 2007). These
problems can persist long after infancy. School-age children
who were born extremely preterm show a higher incidence of
mild or severe cognitive impairments (Anderson & Doyle,
2008). Even among adults, those born extremely preterm
show, on average, lower rates of school completion, lower
levels of employment, and greater difficulty living indepen-
FIGURE 2.13 Extremely premature infant. Recent medical dently (Allen, 2008; Moster et al., 2008).
advances have made it possible for premature infants to survive at
How can the negative effects associated with preterm births
ages considered impossible a few decades ago. As younger and
younger babies are able to survive, however, the incidence of severe,
be reduced? Interventions can aim to either reduce the rate of
lifelong complications rises. preterm births or minimize their negative effects. Reducing
the preterm birthrate turns out to be a challenge. As doctors
become better able to recognize the need to induce early labor,
of a premature birth (Jeffcoat et al., 2001). Smoking, alco- preterm birthrates are rising. Technologies such as in vitro fer-
hol, and illicit drugs also increase the chances, and in com- tilization for couples with fertility problems can also increase
bination they raise the odds still further (Dew et al., 2007). the odds of multiple fetuses and preterm births (Goldenberg
In addition to possible toxic effects, smoking, drinking, and et al., 2008). Nonetheless, medical advances are enabling doc-
drug abuse may put infants more at risk simply by exposing tors to prolong the gestational period without harming the
them to the increased odds of being born very early. Along mother or the fetus. Such interventions can range from con-
the same lines as these physical stressors, mothers who expe- vincing mothers to stop smoking, to screening for and treating
rience intense stress and hardship are more likely to have infections promptly, to careful monitoring of the mother’s diet
preterm infants. Psychological stress may cause inflam- and nutritional supplements (Iams et al., 2008).
mation and immune responses, which in turn increase the Even with the most attentive prenatal care, however, pre-
odds of preterm birth (Wadhwa et al., 2001). term births still occur, and so the second form of interven-
Preterm infants, and especially those that are extremely tion aims to help children who are born preterm. Studies
premature, are a cause of concern for physicians and psychol- suggest that among these children, those who experience
ogists because these infants are at increased risk for a host of more parental involvement and stimulating, active play in
complications later in development. As we consider the con- infancy and educational support in the early school years
sequences of preterm birth, however, it is critical to keep in show cognitive and motor improvements (de Kieviet et al.,
mind that these problems take the form of increased risks— 2009; Koldewijn et al., 2010). Preterm infants and preschool
not certainties. Preterm infants may have no negative medi- children have also shown early benefits from additional
cal or psychological conditions at any point in their lives, as practice with motor skills and supportive educational pro-
we saw in the unusual case we described at the beginning of grams, but these gains do not seem to be sustained through
the chapter, but the closer to full term an infant is at birth, elementary school (Orton et al., 2009). Health care profes-
the smaller the chances of these kinds of problems occurring. sionals, teachers, and psychologists still have much to learn
The more immature the preterm infant is at birth, the about what kinds of interventions can provide long-term
higher the risk of medical and psychological problems, not benefits for children born preterm. At this point, improving
just in infancy but also in childhood and beyond (Korven- prenatal care to reduce the number of extremely preterm
ranta et al., 2010). At the level of neuroanatomy, preterm births is likely to be the most effective form of intervention.
birth is associated with greater risk of a number of irregu-
larities in brain development (Allen, 2008; Chuang et al.,
2007; Volpe, 2009). This damage appears to happen in two
ways: (1) premature birth seems to disrupt patterns of neural Q: What difficulties reduce survival, and what
growth that normally occur in the last few months before long-term complications are likely when a fetus
birth, and (2) the problem that may have triggered a prema- is born preterm?
ture birth, such as an infection, can damage the brain directly
Lampreys
Rays
Sharks (regenerating teeth)
Ray–finned fishes
Coelacanths
Lungfishes
Caecilians
Salamanders
Frogs
Platypi
Elephants (trunk)
FIGURE 2.14 Evolution as
Bats (echolocation)
a branching tree. At each
Wolves
Cows branch in an evolutionary tree, a
Mice and rats species acquires distinctive new
Lemurs traits, as shown in this small
Chimpanzees section of the evolutionary tree
Humans covering many vertebrates. As
Turtles (hard shell) the branching structure shows,
Snakes and lizards human development does not
Crocodiles and alligators involve a progression through
Birds (hollow bones) the properties of so-called
“lower species.” Adapted from
C. Loeb, Macalester College.
ANATOMICAL DEVELOPMENT 49
understanding of how the body’s overall structure devel-
ops, as well as how specific features, like limbs, emerge Anterior Posterior
(Gilbert, 2010; Marcus, 2004; Mark et al., 1997). Special
kinds of ordered clusters of master-switch genes known as Fruit fly embryo Fruit fly
homeobox genes activate or repress gene expression and
bod,
thereby play an important role in defining the body plan. Fruit fly lab pb zen Dfd Scr ftz Antp Ubx abd-A Abd-B
These homeobox genes contain the homeobox, a 180-base- chromosome
pair sequence in the DNA of many different species of
animals—from fruit flies to mice to humans. The homeo-
box encodes homeodomain proteins, which regulate genetic Hypothetical Hox1 Hox2 Hox3 Hox4 Hox5 Hox6 (central) Hox7 (posterior)
ancestral
switches affecting the general structure and organization chromosome
of certain body sections. First, the homeobox genes regu-
late the processes that lead to the formation of the ante-
A1 A2 A3 A4 A5 A6 A7 A9 A10 A11 A13
rior (front) and posterior (rear) sections of the organism’s HOXA
Human chromosome
body (see Figure 2.15). Then, acting as switches at the top B1 B2 B3 B4 B5 B6 B7 B8 B9 B13
of large gene regulation cascades, they interact with other HOXB
genes to code for species-specific appendages and other fea- C4 C5 C6 C8 C9 C10 C11 C12 C13
HOXC
tures. More specifically, when a homeobox gene is switched
D1 D3 D4 D8 D9 D10 D11 D12 D13
on, it sends chemical signals that reach many sections of HOXD
the developing body. In response to these signals, genes
throughout the body turn on or off as necessary to direct
the growth of species-specific body parts, such as legs. The
homeobox genes that govern the body plan in vertebrates
are often called Hox genes.
Anterior Posterior
Q: What are homeobox genes? In what ways
are their functions similar across a wide range of
organisms?
Human embryo
T
he city of Elmira, New York, sits near the Pennsylva- nurse visits and others in similar circumstances in the same
nia border in a region that has been economically area receive no visits. By randomly assigning mothers in com-
depressed for decades, as its manufacturing economy parable situations to either participate in the visiting nurse pro-
has withered. Elmira’s economic peak was in the late gram or not, it is possible to compare outcomes between the
1940s and early 1950s. Today, its population is half of what it groups to see whether the visits are effective. (Without random
was 60 years ago, and almost one-third of Elmira’s children under assignment, there would be no way to know whether group
18 live in poverty. In the 1970s, David Olds, a graduate student differences were due to the nurses’ visits or to other factors,
at nearby Cornell University, set out to make a difference in the such as higher levels of motivation among mothers who seek
lives of Elmira’s children, who at that time had the highest rates the nurses’ help.) In general, these careful studies often show
of abuse and child neglect in the state (Boo, 2006). Olds started positive effects for newborn infants. The effects are not always
the Nurse-Family Partnership Program, a program in which large, but the number of successes has encouraged agencies
nurses visited disadvantaged pregnant women to advise them throughout the country to fund such programs.
during their pregnancy and then after the birth of their child. Since Several studies have shown that nurse-visited mothers, espe-
that time, Olds’s program has been extended to sites throughout cially the youngest mothers, have fewer medical complications
the United States. during pregnancy and are less likely to have premature babies
Nurses in the program visit first-time mothers in their homes, (Baldwin et al., 1998; Olds, 2006). In one study, the babies of
beginning during pregnancy and continuing through their child’s nurse-visited mothers were almost a full pound heavier than the
second birthday. During their visits, they offer guidance about babies delivered by mothers in the control group, who were not
prenatal health and infant care in a nonthreatening, nonjudg- visited by nurses (Olds et al., 1986). Nurse-visited mothers also
mental way. They provide information about prenatal nutrition, tended to have fewer episodes of high blood pressure while
explain the threats that smoking and alcohol pose to the fetus, pregnant and lower rates of cigarette smoking (Olds, 2006).
and convey to the mothers the importance of avoiding behaviors Given the negative effects of fetal nicotine exposure on later
that could compromise their own health or harm their infant brain development (Ernst et al., 2001; Thomas et al., 2000),
(Olds, 2006; Olds et al., 2007). They often emphasize the these changes in the mothers’ behavior represent an important
immense promise and potential of the child—if only he or she medical treatment for the growing fetus.
receives the right kind of care and support (Boo, 2006). The benefits of these programs extend far beyond the direct
The nurse’s job can be extremely challenging. Members impact on brain growth. Nurse-visited mothers interact with
of the household, sometimes including the mother, may be their babies in a host of more positive ways and are less likely
hostile about taking an outsider’s advice. One account of so- to abuse their children (Olds et al., 2007). After working with a
called “swamp nurses” who visited pregnant mothers in rural nurse named Luwana, a mother named Alexis observed of her
Louisiana discussed the challenges of an African-American new son Daigan, “He’s not as cranky as he was. And one thing
nurse visiting white teenage mothers who often faced a host I learned already is how he cries different when he’s hungry
of problems, ranging from drug addiction to sexual abuse (Boo, than when he’s wet.” As Luwana explains to Alexis, “Making
2006). To work well with these mothers, the nurses had to that distinction is important. . . . You’re listening to him, and in
have an extraordinary combination of compassion, sensitivity, his own way he’s explaining what he needs” (Boo, 2006). Chil-
and firmness. dren of nurse-visited mothers show continuing benefits on into
The Nurse-Family Partnership Program is evaluated repeat- adolescence (Fisch, 2005; Kitzman et al., 2010; Olds, 2006).
edly to make sure it is effective. Wherever possible, Olds has In many cases, these effects arise from a continuing pattern
used randomized assignment designs, meaning that some of positive interactions between mother and child (Leckman &
mothers in a particular area are randomly selected to receive Mayes, 2007).
ANATOMICAL DEVELOPMENT 51
the cells have begun to differentiate along female lines,
that embryo can acquire many aspects of male anatomy,
including sex-specific brain differences. The same kind of
effect happens when undifferentiated embryonic males are
exposed to higher levels of estrogen.
One example of early sexual differentiation can be
seen in Figure 2.17. Initially, the human embryo has both
Wolffian ducts and Müllerian ducts in the lower abdomen,
as well as bipotential gonads (gonads that could develop
into female or male structures). At this point, either male
or female urogenital structures could emerge, but this
changes as soon as the tissues are exposed to higher lev-
els of a particular sex hormone. High testosterone and low
estrogen cause the Wolffian ducts and testes to grow into
a male urogenital system and cause the Müllerian ducts to
wither away. High estrogen and low testosterone cause the
Müllerian ducts and follicle cells in the ovary to grow into
FIGURE 2.16 Finger number. Although infants are occasionally the female urogenital system and cause the Wolffian ducts
born with extra toes or fingers, as shown here, it is impressive how
to degenerate (see Table 2.2).
often the gene sequence results in the standard number. There is no
specific part of the genome that codes for five digits per hand and The adults of some species retain the capacity to change
foot. Instead, interactions between many genes result in overlapping their phenotypic sex as a function of social dominance.
chemical gradients that, together, usually result in this number.
TABLE 2.2 Changes in sexual characteristics in early prenatal development. The presence of higher levels of testosterone or estrogen
affects the development of sexual structures in males and females.
This phenomenon is most prevalent among reptiles, male characteristics. This theory suggests that the moth-
amphibians, and fish. There may be only one phenotypic er’s immune response builds over the course of multiple
male in the group whose role is stimulating ovulation in pregnancies with male children, so that later sons would
the phenotypic females. When that male dies, the dom- more likely be affected than earlier sons. Yet, since many
inant female undergoes a variety of changes that make firstborns can be homosexual and many lastborns are not,
her a phenotypic male, both behaviorally and anatomi- this pattern is only a small part of any account of how
cally. In these cases of environmental sex determination, homosexual and heterosexual choices emerge in develop-
all group members usually have the same genotype, and ment. Much more research remains to be done before we
the specific male or female gene pathways are triggered can have a clear sense of the many factors contributing to
by changes in the social environment (Francis & Barlow, homosexuality.
1993; Fricke & Fricke, 1977; Godwin, 2009; Robertson, Sexual differentiation continues well after birth and on
1972; Ross, 1990). into puberty, and changing concentrations of hormones
In humans, social context may cause much more subtle continuously influence the process. During this process,
changes in hormonal balances, and if these changes occur genes also interact with many kinds of environments, rang-
during pregnancy, they could potentially have long-term ing from the hormones in the womb to the child’s experi-
effects on the fetus. For example, many studies of prena- ences after birth. All along, interactions between genes and
tal development have demonstrated that during pregnancy, the environment contribute to the ways that biological and
maternal stress levels can affect the timing of the fetus’s behavioral traits emerge.
exposure to different levels of testosterone. One potential
effect of these shifts in timing is to cause less sexual differ-
entiation of the male brain; indeed, some researchers have Adverse Influences on the
claimed that this effect may be a biological basis of homo-
sexuality (LeVay, 1993), although these claims remain con- Developing Embryo and Fetus
troversial. The extent to which maternal hormones and the Given the complexity of development, it is remarkable that
prenatal environment influence human sexual orientation all of the body’s structures form and grow normally and at
remains unclear (Gooren, 2006). appropriate times in the vast majority of cases. But some-
A different aspect of the environment—namely, the times problems do appear, often because of harmful envi-
number of older male siblings a male has—also seems ronmental influences. Between 1957 and 1962, thousands
to influence the frequency of homosexuality among of pregnant women in Europe and North America were
men (Gooren, 2006; Puts et al., 2006). One controver- given what appeared to be a new wonder drug for morning
sial theory hypothesizes that the more older brothers a sickness: thalidomide. The drug has since become infa-
male child has, the greater the probability that child will mous for its adverse effects on developing fetuses. Its most
develop homosexual attractions (Dawood et al., 2009). obvious effects involved severely stunted limb growth and
Because this effect only holds for older siblings with the sometimes an absence of limbs (see Figure 2.18). It also
same mother, not stepchildren or adopted children, it may caused serious defects in internal organs, genitalia, and
be due to an immune response in the mother against some the nervous system. More recent research suggests that
aspect of the Y chromosome or the hormones that trigger thalidomide probably interfered with the activity of genes
ANATOMICAL DEVELOPMENT 53
lowed by the heart, then the limbs, the eyes, and finally
the external genitalia, which do not seem to be influenced
until after 6 weeks.
Thalidomide was a new, poorly understood drug when
it was widely prescribed to pregnant women, as was dieth-
ylstilbestrol (DES), which was used to prevent miscarriage
and led to increased risk for cancer in adolescents and adults
who had been exposed to DES in the womb. In addition,
some common drugs can also act as teratogens. For example,
several anti-acne medicines are part of a family of chemical
compounds known as retinoids. If a pregnant woman takes
even a small amount of these medications, a wide range of
birth defects become more likely. Depending on when in
her pregnancy she takes the medication and for how long, it
can cause hydrocephaly (swelling of the fluid-filled areas of
the brain), microcephaly (undersized brain), heart defects,
ear and eye defects, and cleft lip and palate (Durston et al.,
1989; Maden, 2000). Vitamin A in its various forms is also
FIGURE 2.18 Thalidomide and pregnancy. The tragedy of the a retinoid, and high doses of vitamin A during pregnancy
babies whose mothers were prescribed thalidomide led to increased can cause comparable defects—even though low doses are
sensitivity to the dangers of teratogens and how the timing of expo- essential for normal development (Collins & Mao, 1999).
sure influences their effects. A different drug, valproic acid, which is used to treat con-
vulsive disorders (and sometimes bipolar disorder), can also
cause birth defects ranging from neural problems to facial,
that code for normal limb growth or perhaps with the Hox heart, and skeletal malformation (Duncan, 2007; Finnell &
genes’ timing signals (Marcil et al., 2003; Wolpert, 1999; Burn, 2001; Finnell et al., 2002). Even some drugs with
Zeller, 2010). minimal or no apparent side effects in adults can endanger
Since the thalidomide disaster, doctors and the public a fetus.
have become more aware of possible negative influences on
embryos and fetuses and better able to guard against them.
Called teratogens, these hazardous internal or external
environmental factors can potentially cause problems with
Q: How does the timing of exposure to a
development during the prenatal period. They can be clas- teratogen affect its consequences for the
sified into the categories shown in Table 2.3. developing fetus?
Teratogens illustrate a developmental theme discussed
earlier: the importance of timing and sequencing in the
developmental process. Specific teratogens tend to have Teratogens are not limited to drugs; they can be part of
especially strong harmful effects during specific times the mother’s diet as well. It is well known that drinking
in prenatal development (see Figure 2.19). Thalidomide, alcohol during pregnancy can be hazardous to the fetus,
for example, usually had its disastrous effects from 2 to and its effects can be extensive. Depending on the age
8 weeks after conception. Because of the sequence of of the fetus, alcohol can cause smaller brain volume (see
anatomical development, the specific effects of thalido- Figure 2.20) and impaired development of neurons and
mide depend on the point during pregnancy when it is supporting cells (Archibald et al., 2001; Riley & McGee,
used. Using it only during a brief, early period in prena- 2005; Sowell et al., 2008; Spadoni et al., 2007).
tal development, say at 3 to 6 weeks, might have impacts Hazards posed by foods are often less obvious than the
mostly on the arms and other anterior structures, whereas risks of drugs and alcohol. In some cases, foods can be
using it at a later period, say at 7 to 8 weeks, would more contaminated with a trace amount of a teratogen that,
likely impair growth of the legs and more posterior struc- while mildly harmful in adults, might be devastating
tures. Generally, teratogens have the worst effects dur- for prenatal development. Many fish that are high in the
ing the embryonic period, from about 2 to 8 weeks after food chain, such as swordfish and tuna, retain relatively
conception, when the most dramatic qualitative changes high concentrations of mercury in their tissues from eat-
take place. The central nervous system tends to be most ing mercury-contaminated fish lower in the food chain.
vulnerable in the first few weeks after fertilization, fol- Because mercury can have strong teratogenic effects,
TABLE 2.3 Teratogens and development. Teratogens are internal or external hazardous environmental factors that can affect the develop-
ing organism in utero and can result in birth defects. They are diverse both in kind and effects. Some of the effects can be remarkably specific,
while others can be more global.
expectant mothers should limit their consumption of fish. Garza, 2006; Tamashiro & Moran, 2010). Once again,
Even more subtly, under some conditions, prenatal diets genetic and environmental influences interact to produce
free of known teratogens can still have harmful effects developmental outcomes.
on some fetuses. For example, during pregnancy, diabetic Although this book focuses on developmental psychol-
mothers must manage their blood level of insulin even ogy, a brief survey of biological development of the body
more carefully than usual. Abnormally high or low blood as a whole has brought to light many principles that also
insulin levels may lead to a kind of misprogramming of pertain to neuropsychological development, ranging from
the fetal systems that regulate weight and metabolism canalization to the importance of when an organism
(Plagemann, 2006). In addition, some maternal alleles encounters particular environments or events. We have also
appear to increase the risk of miscarriage or birth defects seen how prenatal development can affect postnatal func-
unless the expectant mother takes a certain dietary supple- tions and development, a theme we will carry forward in
ment, such as vitamin B12, to offset the problem (Stover & our discussion of brain development as well.
ANATOMICAL DEVELOPMENT 55
Dividing Embryonic period Fetal period
zygote/implantation (weeks) (weeks)
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 16 32 38
CNS Eyes Ears Palate Ears Brain
Heart
Arms
FIGURE 2.19 Timing and teratogens’ influences. This chart shows the periods in which different body systems and organs are most
susceptible to teratogens and other external influences, as well as the periods when they are less sensitive but when exposure to teratogens
can still cause damage. Adapted from Moore et al. (2013).
BRAIN DEVELOPMENT 57
occipital lobe processes visual information. These descrip- form a groove, which will become the neural tube. By the
tions of the different functions of the lobes are approximate; time the ectoderm starts to close into a tube at 4 weeks, the
in fact, the lobes interact extensively with one another and structure has three distinct bumps: the forebrain, the mid-
with other brain regions. brain, and the hindbrain, also known as cerebral vesicles. At
Brain structures and the pathways between regions 5 weeks, the forebrain and hindbrain vesicles split and form
mature at different times and rates. As we have seen, evo- several distinct brain regions. Three of the most important
lutionarily more ancient structures that are required for regions are the telencephalon, the diencephalon and mid-
basic survival, such as the spinal cord, medulla, and basal brain, and the brainstem.
ganglia, tend to mature earliest because they support such The telencephalon eventually becomes the cerebral hemi-
activities as breathing, heart rate, and appetite. Areas of the spheres, the areas that support the highest levels of psycho-
cortex that process sensory information, such as the visual logical functioning. The diencephalon and midbrain give rise
cortex and the somatosensory cortex, mature somewhat to the basal ganglia and other structures that reside between
later and continue to mature for some time after birth as the cerebral hemispheres and above the brainstem. These
higher and higher levels of processing become more refined. include structures that integrate sensory information, as well
Finally, areas that process more abstract information and as the cerebellum. By 10 weeks after conception, most of the
regulate other areas, such as the prefrontal cortex, tend to brain’s major regions are visible without use of a microscope.
mature last. This sequence is only a crude characterization At 20 weeks, the cerebral hemispheres are clearly visible,
of a much more complex interplay between brain regions but they are smooth rather than fissured. This begins to
over the course of development, an interplay that also helps change at around 24 weeks. At that point, the cortical surface
drive development. Thus, a circuit of mutual interactions develops the folds and fissures that are essential for supporting
between two areas may foster development of both. the complex processing of the human brain. These changes
Thinking about the brain in terms of regions with spe- are summarized in Table 2.4 and Figure 2.22.
cific functions is helpful for understanding some aspects of As noted earlier, the infant’s brain grows quite a bit after
its structure and capacities. But when it comes to under- birth. Weighing roughly 350 to 400 grams at birth, the
standing brain development, it is important to keep in mind brain’s weight more than doubles in the first year of life,
that the brain is a highly interactive system in which each and it weighs about 1,250 grams (a little under 3 pounds)
area depends on inputs from the others. When you recall by 5 years. These large increases in brain mass in the first
the flowers at your cousin’s wedding, you access and inte- 5 years of life are assumed to increase its processing capac-
grate memory traces from many different parts of the cere- ity. After age 5, brain volume stays relatively constant until
bral cortex. In this process, you use each of the four lobes some shrinkage begins in old age.
in operations such as knowing where the wedding was held,
constructing a mental image of the bouquets, remembering
their sweet smell, visualizing how they were displayed, and
knowing the names of roses, lilies, and orchids. Neurons and Neurotransmitters
How does the embryo develop this rich array of brain Along with these large-scale changes, the brain also
structures and abilities? As mentioned earlier, in the first changes dramatically at the microscopic level, both before
few weeks after conception, part of the ectoderm starts to and after birth. Since much of the change centers around
TABLE 2.4 Timetable of prenatal brain development. Major neural changes occur in a regular sequence of steps representing an extraor-
dinary amount of change in a short period of time.
BRAIN DEVELOPMENT 59
The speed with which the action potential travels down has not yet been shown definitively in the human cortex,
the axon partly depends on whether the axon is wrapped some positive reports suggest that with better measures it
with myelin, a fatty substance that acts somewhat like the may be demonstrated there as well (Gould, 2007).
rubber insulation that directs electrical conduction along
a wire. Myelination occurs when the axons are enclosed
in a myelin sheath, which speeds the movement of the
action potential along the axon. Myelin is produced by Q: Is there growth of new neurons after birth?
glial cells, which, generally speaking, serve to support
the neurons by providing nutrients, producing myelin,
and providing other kinds of structural support. The glial Migration and Synaptogenesis New nerve cells
cells’ importance and number are often underappreciated. develop near the center of the brain and then move through
In the human brain, there are roughly 100 times as many older nerve cells to the outermost layers of the growing
glial cells as neurons. neural network, where they will ultimately become parts
Although electrical signals play a key role in communica- of various brain structures (see Figure 2.24A). Most of this
tion between neurons, it is important to remember that the migration is thought to happen either before birth or around
action potential is not the same as an electrical current passing the time of birth, but some migration is also thought to occur
through a wire, which can travel at 300 million meters per in adult human brains (Cayre et al., 2009; Ghashghaei et al.,
second. In contrast, the action potential involves a series of 2007). Nerve cells appear to migrate by following the paths
electrochemical charges that travel down the axon much more laid down by certain radial glial cells that serve as guides
slowly, ranging from 100 meters per second to a relatively (Hatten, 1990, 1999; Rakic, 1972, 1981, 1988). Chemical
sedate 1 meter per second. In development, neurons gradually signals in the brain also guide nerve cell migration by pro-
become myelinated at different times and rates. As we will see viding different “instructions” for different cell groups about
shortly, the myelination process is thought to play a significant where in the brain to go and when to stop (Dodd & Jessell,
role in the development of various psychological capacities. 1988; Ghashghaei et al., 2007; Tessier-Lavigne & Goodman,
1996; Tessier-Lavigne & Placzek, 1991). Indeed, researchers
can predict where axons will grow within tissue clusters based
on which tissues are present and which chemicals they emit
Development of Neurons (Heffner et al., 1990).
The four main processes involved in the development of After a nerve cell migrates toward the brain’s outermost
neurons are proliferation, migration, consolidation, and level, its axon starts to grow and interconnect with other
myelination. Proliferation involves the creation of nerve cells neurons. As the axon grows, it finds its way to its appro-
and dendritic branches at different times of development. priate destination, which can be more than a meter away.
Migration refers to the remarkable ways that developing The end of the growing axon has a growth cone, which
neurons move through brain tissue to their final positions. enables it to push its way through other tissues and track
Consolidation refers to reducing the number of connections chemical signals that tell it in which directions to move
between neurons as a way of fine-tuning their structure and (see Figure 2.24B and C).
function. Finally, myelination helps speed up the transmis- When the branching ends of the axon arrive at their des-
sion of information along the axonal shaft of a neuron. tination, they form the bulb-like structures that release neu-
rotransmitters across the synaptic gaps between cells. The
Proliferation During the prenatal period, the produc- neuron also grows thousands of smaller dendritic branches
tion of new neurons, or neurogenesis, occurs on a massive from its cell body. This is where the brain’s intricate intercon-
scale. After the neural tube forms, roughly 250,000 new nectedness becomes clear. Each dendritic branch can make
nerve cells are produced every minute until 100 billion are synaptic connections with different neurons, so that just a
present. For many years, researchers thought that no new few neurons can make hundreds of thousands of connec-
neurons were produced after birth, but postnatal neurogen- tions in a very small space. Across the entire human brain,
esis has been documented in several brain areas, including the total number of connections is truly massive, somewhere
the hippocampus, which is involved in memory and spa- around 1015 (that’s 1,000 trillion, or a quadrillion!).
tial knowledge (Abrous et al., 2005; Gould, 2007; Pujadas
et al., 2010; Rakic, 2002; Richardson et al., 2007; Zhao et
al., 2008). Although many fewer neurons are produced after
birth than during the prenatal period, the neurons gener- Q: What factors influence the direction in which
ated after birth can become fully integrated into existing individual neurons grow?
circuitry (Zhao et al., 2008). While postnatal neurogenesis
3
Nucleus Repel
Migrating Nucleus
neuron Attract Attract
1
2
Axon
Radial
glial cell
Neuron Growth Guide Guide Target
Inner layer of brain cone cell a cell c tissues
A B C
FIGURE 2.24 Nerve cell migration and axonal guidance toward target cells. (A) New nerve cells migrate from the inner layer of the
brain through other layers of the cortex toward the outermost layer of the brain, traveling along radial glial cells that act as guides. Adapted
from Rakic (1972, 1990). (B) Once a nerve cell reaches the outermost layer, it extends its axon by producing growth cones that are guided
toward the target cell by chemical signals. These signals, made up of proteins, come from guide cells. Some proteins spur the axon to grow
toward them by emitting attractant chemicals (as shown by guide cells a and c); other proteins repel the axon by emitting chemicals that trigger
the axon to grow in the other direction (as shown by guide cell b). These signals affect the growing axon, causing the growth cone to twist and
turn toward its target. (C) This series of three photomicrographs shows the path of a single axon as its growth cone moves toward the circular
guide cell, reaches it, and then moves beyond it.
Synaptogenesis, the process of forming new synapses, pruning, a process that reduces the number of neurons and
creates the vast number of connections between neurons; synaptic connections. Early on in childhood, the human brain
each of our 100 billion neurons is associated with 10,000 has approximately 100 billion (1010) nerve cells and as many
synapses. In contrast to neurogenesis, which mostly takes as 1,000 trillion (1015) synapses, but through the pruning pro-
place in the prenatal period, synaptogenesis remains an cess, both numbers decrease after childhood. This develop-
important process throughout the lifespan. Functionally, mental pattern of an early excess of connections is sometimes
synaptogenesis seems to be involved in fine-tuning neural called “initial overexuberance.”
networks during learning. It creates new synapses in the Neural structures can be pruned through programmed
course of certain experiences and eliminates synapses in the cell death, or apoptosis. In apoptosis, cells do not simply
course of others (Waites et al., 2005), and it can be quite sen- die randomly; instead, they die in highly specific patterns
sitive to variations in experience and environment (Flavell & that help shape the resulting neural circuits. This type of
Greenberg, 2008). In other mammals, such as rats, just a few cell death is first observed at around 24 weeks after con-
days of experience can create new synaptic connections (Ge ception. Cell death is a natural pattern of development in
et al., 2007; He et al., 2010; Waites et al., 2005). which neurons are pruned out of a system as a consequence
Although newborns do have neurons and synapses, their of less use, less environmental stimulation, or less internal
neurons are simpler in structure and make far fewer inter- stimulation or because a maturational instruction shuts
connections. In addition, around the time of birth, some down cell function. When a neuron undergoes apoptosis, it
neurons’ growth cones are still seeking their target locations necessarily eliminates all of that cell’s synaptic connections.
within the brain. Newborns’ neurons are also much less But another, more fine-grained form of synaptic pruning is
myelinated than adult neurons (see Figure 2.25A). In fact, also possible. In this case, the neuron stays alive, but some of
myelination continues well into adulthood in some areas of its synapses are selectively eliminated. Both forms of prun-
the brain. Development therefore proceeds both at the cel- ing seem to be completed by adolescence (Huttenlocher,
lular level of individual neurons and in terms of their inter- 1979, 2002). It is not yet clear whether these two forms—
connections with other neurons (see Figure 2.25B and C). apoptosis and synaptic pruning—serve different functions.
It seems plausible, though, that apoptosis is associated with
Consolidation During development, the number of syn- larger-scale changes in neural circuits, while synaptic prun-
aptic connections does not steadily increase. Instead, both ing may be more specifically localized and more sensitive to
prenatally and postnatally, there are periods of considerable environmental factors.
BRAIN DEVELOPMENT 61
Dendrites
Cell body
6 months 1 year
Cell body
Axon B
Myelin sheath
Axon
Axon terminal
Growth
cone
FIGURE 2.26 Myelination. (A) The wrapping of a fatty substance known as myelin several times around an axon is a critical developmental
process that speeds up the conduction of nerve impulses along the axon. (B) The myelination process continues into early adulthood. Sensory
areas are myelinated before birth. Myelination of the motor systems also starts before birth and continues past the first year of life. Finally, the
integrative systems show little myelination until after birth, and the process continues into adulthood.
BRAIN DEVELOPMENT 63
brain’s association areas (Amedi et al., 2005; Merabet & be malleable, or “plastic,” and physically change as a result
Pascual-Leone, 2010; Röder et al., 2002). of experience (Greenough et al., 1987).
We have considered the effects of a specific type of depri- In humans, it is not possible to directly measure experience-
vation, such as loss of vision, in an otherwise normal envi- dependent changes in the number of dendritic branches or
ronment. What happens at the neural level when a child synapses. Indirect measures using neuroimaging suggest
grows up in a much more problematic environment with that these changes may occur during the process of acquir-
massive deprivation of all forms of inputs? Several studies ing and honing a skill. For example, when people learn to
have looked at orphans and abandoned children who grew juggle, there is an increase in gray matter (where most neu-
up in deprived and highly stressful conditions—similar in ral interconnections occur) in the brain region associated
some respects to conditions that inhibit neurogenesis and with the visual processing of motion (Draganski et al., 2004).
synaptogenesis in animals (Chu & Lieberman, 2010; Gun- Similarly, years of intensive music performance training is
nar, 2001; Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007). A variety of mea- associated with increased myelination of the relevant motor
sures suggest that such deprivation can cause changes in neuron pathways (Bengtsson et al., 2005; Hyde et al., 2009).
brain development. The deprived children showed differ- Finally, compared with other adults, veteran taxi drivers
ences in overall brain size and physiological activity (Nel- have been shown to have enlarged hippocampal regions—
son, 2007) as well as indications of lesser myelination of areas involved in spatial reasoning and maintaining mental
the connections between major brain regions (Eluvathingal maps (Maguire et al., 2000). Together the findings from
et al., 2006). Some scholars have suggested that extremely human and nonhuman animal studies strongly suggest that
deprived children may experience excessive pruning and the brain restructures itself as a result of focused activity
have less organized synaptic connections (Nelson, 2007). As (Dayan & Cohen, 2011; Johansen-Berg, 2007). It remains
noninvasive measures of brain structure improve, it should controversial, however, whether commercial infant enrich-
be possible to document these differences more precisely ment programs are any more beneficial than normal (non-
and to determine whether they are sometimes irreversible. deprived) infant experience.
Since deprivation can cause apoptosis and pruning of syn-
apses, many have wondered whether an enhanced environ-
ment could have the opposite effect on brain development.
Could engaging stimulation lead to more complex synaptic
connections and dendritic branches? Many eager parents Puberty and Brain Development
have attempted to accelerate their baby’s brain development As children enter puberty, their behavior often changes sig-
through such environmental “enhancements” as classical nificantly. Even the ancients took note. According to the
music, visually challenging mobiles, and intensive language Greek philosopher Aristotle, “Youth are heated by Nature as
exposure. In a broad sense, this question remains open, but drunken men by wine” (Dahl, 2004, p. 8). His observation
it cannot be addressed experimentally in humans given the is borne out by statistics concerning reckless driving, unpro-
obvious ethical problems of assigning infants to “deprived,” tected sex, substance abuse, and a host of other dangerous
“normal,” or “enriched” environments. behaviors carried out by adolescents (Steinberg, 2007).
By posing more focused questions, however, researchers Besides risk taking, adolescents show a variety of behavioral
have found some effects of enrichment (Kempermann et al., changes, including a surge of interest in sex and romantic
1997), many of which result from the learning experiences relationships, changed sleep patterns, and increased appe-
that take place in an enriched environment. Of course, when tite. Several kinds of psychopathology are also much more
an organism learns, the brain changes. If rats are required to common among adolescents than among younger children.
learn a new skilled task, that challenging experience will cause Many of these changes will be discussed in the book’s final
them to grow new synapses in the cerebellum (Black et al., chapters. Here we focus on some of the changes in the brain
1990). Similarly, when monkeys are taught to use a new tool, that relate to the upheaval of adolescence.
their neurons in the brain region involved in grasping and tool As mentioned in Chapter 1, puberty is a time of pro-
use become more interconnected (Hihara et al., 2006). nounced qualitative biological change. Males develop
Aside from growing new synapses, another subtle but facial hair and broader shoulders, and females develop
important effect of learning is the strengthening or weaken- breasts and wider hips. Male voices become lower pitched,
ing of existing synaptic connections, a process that happens a phenomenon that led some overzealous medieval choir
continuously in adult organisms. As we have already begun directors to castrate choirboys shortly before puberty, pre-
to see, experience can also affect the number of synapses, serving their ability to hit very high notes (Rosselli, 1988).
the neuronal patterns of connection, and the birth or death Given these dramatic bodily changes, it is natural to ask
of neurons. These kinds of changes demonstrate the brain’s about corresponding changes in the brain—especially
experience-dependent plasticity, the ability of the brain to since brain signals trigger puberty’s hormone surges (Dahl,
BRAIN DEVELOPMENT 65
NEW DIRECTION S IN DEVELOPMENTAL RESEARCH
M
any of us know someone who has suffered the why some glial cells promote nerve cell regeneration and others
effects of brain damage as an adult. The damage can block it (Muir, 2010).
range from a shrapnel wound that impairs speech, a An especially effective way to repair nerve cell damage
stroke that hampers vision, or the more widespread would be to have a population of cells in the adult brain that
neuronal death that causes the motor problems involved in could differentiate into particular kinds of neurons. They could
Parkinson’s disease. For many years, research suggested that then take over functions that were disrupted by the death of
humans couldn’t develop new nerve cells after birth, so these other nerve cells. Such cells are known as adult stem cells.
conditions were considered incurable. In the last decade or It was previously thought that the adult brain did not contain
so, however, considerable evidence has shown that even adult stem cells and that the only means of nerve regeneration was
brains sometimes generate new nerve cells (Arvidsson et al., to regrow parts of existing neurons. Recently, however, it has
2002; Bjorklund & Lindvall, 2000; Muir, 2010). It appears that become clear that the brain does have adult stem cells capable
the different types of myelin cells that ensheathe many axons of becoming new, functional nerve cells, especially when they
play a critical role in either inhibiting or promoting regrowth are surrounded by an appropriate scaffold of cells that doesn’t
of nerve cells, depending on the type of myelin involved. In inhibit nerve growth (Ma et al., 2009; Teixeira et al., 2007; Teng
rats, the myelin cells that normally ensheathe the optic nerve et al., 2002).
inhibit nerve cell regrowth. But in one study, researchers cut Moreover, some investigators have suggested that the brain’s
the optic nerves of adult rats and then ensheathed the sev- stem cells are part of an inherent developmental program that
ered optic nerve cells with myelin cells transplanted from the allows the brain to adapt to injury (Imitola et al., 2004). This
olfactory bulb, because they were thought to promote nerve capacity seems to diminish with age, however, which is why
regrowth (Li et al., 2003; Raisman & Li, 2007). Indeed, the children tend to recover from brain damage more easily than
transplanted myelin cells promoted the growth of new cells in adults do (Sun & He, 2010). Nonetheless, adults with brain
the severed optic nerves. Researchers have not yet managed to damage can experience some recovery and neural adaptation,
bring about enough nerve regrowth to restore the rats’ vision, and adult stem cells may be part of this process.
but that eventual outcome seems likely. In similar experiments Along similar lines, animal research into many neurodegen-
aimed at regrowing motor neurons that have been cut, research- erative diseases, including Parkinson’s disease, Lou Gehrig’s
ers are making progress restoring the cut nerves’ functionality. disease (amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, or ALS), and multiple
Myelin appears to affect nerve regrowth in interesting sclerosis (Rice et al., 2003; Silani & Corbo, 2004; Singec et
ways. In the central nervous system, the glial cells that pro- al., 2007), has demonstrated that neural stem cells can repair
duce myelin also produce three distinct proteins that become brain damage and sometimes restore function (Akerud et al.,
embedded in myelin and then directly inhibit nerve growth. 2001; Arenas, 2010; Isacson et al., 2003; Redmond, 2002).
These growth-inhibiting proteins may help set normal limits on Of course, many questions about these processes remain unan-
healthy nerves’ growth—but they also block regeneration when swered, and adapting these treatments for use in humans poses
nerves are damaged (Woolf & Bloechlinger, 2002). Changing significant risks. Even so, the possibility that neural stem cells
the material that ensheathes nerve cells by transplanting glial could be used to repair brain damage that was long consid-
cells from olfactory nerves that have growth-promoting proteins ered irreversible has generated enormous interest. In this case,
could become a valuable method of repairing nerve damage in a deeper understanding of developmental processes is yielding
adults. Researchers are actively trying to understand how and valuable progress on addressing problems in adults.
Adolescents
differences in behavior relate to genetic differences between
individuals or populations. Because this kind of research
PFC predated our fairly detailed biochemical understanding of
Striatum DNA, behavioral geneticists have historically used statis-
Nucleus
accumbens tical techniques to infer the degree to which individuals
Amygdala are related genetically—that is, the proportion of their
genes that are the same. Researchers can then compare and
correlate this level of genetic similarity with variations in
Adults behavior.
One of the most common research techniques in behav-
ioral genetics involves studying monozygotic (MZ) and
dizygotic (DZ) twins. Studying twins allows researchers to
PFC
keep either the participants’ genes (in the case of MZ twins)
Striatum
Nucleus or their environment (twins raised together) relatively con-
accumbens
stant across experimental conditions. Because monozygotic
Amygdala
twins have nearly identical genetic material, many research-
ers have studied MZ twins who were raised apart in attempts
to examine the effects of experiences or environment while
holding genes as constant as possible. Dizygotic twins, on
connections in all of these areas may allow more precise the other hand, are only as genetically similar as any two
regulation of emotions (Steinberg, 2007). Thus, changes siblings. Researchers have often studied DZ twins raised in
at the neural level may result in better integration of the the same household as a way to minimize environmental
cognitive control system, reward system, and emotional differences in the children’s experiences while examining
arousal system. the effects of their genetic differences.
BEHAVIORAL GENETICS 67
When heritability measures are applied to behavior, This point is often overlooked and can lead to false and
they are used to describe the degree to which behavioral inflammatory claims about the genetic bases for differences
differences among individuals in a population, such as among groups. As we will see in several later chapters, the
variations in the traits of shyness or excitability, are due behavioral genetics approach has helped researchers under-
to those individuals’ genetic differences. As an example stand the relative contributions of genetic and environ-
of a trait with low heritability, consider the particular mental factors to a wide range of traits, although complex
language that individuals speak. Our native tongue is gene-environment interactions can often make interpreta-
solely a consequence of the language (or languages) we tions difficult.
are exposed to while growing up, not the genes inher-
ited from our biological parents. On the other hand, a
trait with a very high heritability in most environments is
height, which runs strongly in biological families. Still, it Behavioral Genomics
is critical to remember that heritability measures pertain Behavioral genetics still has a role in helping us understand
only to populations, not to individuals. A heritability of .9 individual differences (Plomin et al., 2000). But the field
for height does not indicate that 90 percent of a specific is rapidly changing in light of new knowledge about vari-
person’s height is caused by his or her genes. Rather, it ous species’ genomes and increasingly sophisticated ways of
means that across the whole population being measured, measuring differences in phenotype (Haworth & Plomin,
the differences in height are 90 percent attributable to 2010; Kendler & Greenspan, 2006; Plomin & McGuffin,
genetic variations between that population’s members. 2003; Toga & Thompson, 2005; Visscher et al., 2008). This
A single person’s height reflects a complex interaction new perspective is sometimes called behavioral genomics
between his genotype and many environmental factors, to emphasize the role of understanding a species’ genome
including diet, exercise, and even stress. It doesn’t make and its effects at the molecular level.
sense to try to describe “how much” of a single person’s One way to understand this change in methodology is to
height is attributable to genes. A heritability measure is see the shift that has occurred historically, starting with the
meaningful only in describing a population. early approach to human differences (Galton, 1883), when
Heritabilities depend not only on the range of genotypes researchers could only compare the behaviors they saw with
in a population but also on the environment in which a pop- the levels of genetic variation they inferred by comparing
ulation is studied. In certain environments, heritabilities twins, siblings, and other individuals. In the 1960s, this
can be greatly reduced. For example, some forms of diabe- approach to research had shifted only modestly. Research-
tes have substantial heritabilities, but if a population tends ers knew that differences in DNA were responsible for all
toward a diet that minimizes sugars and carbohydrates, it variances in genotype, but they did not understand how the
is likely that fewer people in that population will become DNA molecule gave rise to particular physical features, let
diabetic. Thus, in that population, the heritability for the alone its mechanisms for affecting behaviors. Over the next
disease would be correspondingly lower, reflecting a stron- few decades, researchers began to understand how genes
ger environmental influence. direct the production of proteins and how multiple genes
The environment’s influence on heritabilities has sig- can interact. Finally, in the last 10 years or so, an extraordi-
nificant consequences when it comes to interpreting or nary surge of technology has made it possible to construct
comparing these measures. Even when the heritability of a gene expression profiles in which a gene’s expression (whether
trait is high for two separate populations, each in its own it is turned on or off) can be measured in thousands of tis-
environment, it’s important not to assume that differences sue samples at various times and under different conditions.
between the two populations are also due to genetic influ- In addition, researchers can investigate how one gene’s
ences. Suppose, for example, that height is highly heritable expression influences other genes. It is now also possible to
in two groups of children, one in suburban New York and study how changes in gene regulation affect the proteins
another in a famine-stricken area of the Sudan. Even if that cells make.
height has a heritability of .9 in both groups, this does not Taken together, these molecular approaches have begun
mean that differences in height between the two popula- to suggest mechanisms for linking genetic variation to
tions are largely genetic. The Sudanese group would likely behaviors (Plomin & McGuffin, 2003). For example, when
be shorter than the New Yorkers on average for reasons a certain gene is expressed, or “turned on,” it may direct
having nothing to do with genetics. Instead, malnutrition cells to create a protein that causes another gene to be
due to famine would likely decrease heights in the entire expressed at several locations in the brain. That second gene
Sudanese population—even though, as a heritability of .9 may then direct the assembly of proteins that form recep-
tells us—differences in height within that group mostly tors for neurotransmitters. Some of the neural circuits that
could be explained by its members’ genetic differences. influence human emotions may work in this way. Thus,
BEHAVIORAL GENETICS 69
closely linked than ever before. While a great deal of work
remains to be done, researchers have already uncovered pat-
terns in the way structure and function develop. These pat-
terns of change are informative at one level as the necessary
underpinnings for behavioral and psychological changes.
At another level, they illustrate how developmental systems
emerge while simultaneously, and continuously, solving
problems of growth and form.
Perhaps the fundamental principle of the biology of
development is that the most basic functions for survival
tend to mature first. Initially, this means that the growing
embryo must be able to receive nourishment and eliminate
waste. Before long, though, the emerging organism needs
more structure and function to survive, including a basic
circulatory system and early segmentation of the body into
FIGURE 2.30 Heritabilities of gray matter. As neuroscientists compartments, which promotes further cellular differen-
develop more precise ways of studying the brain, they can ask how tiation. More broadly, the different tissues must be able
individuals’ differences in gray matter relate to their genetic differ- to communicate so that cell migration, specialization, and
ences. As this computer-generated image shows, the proportion of other patterns of change can proceed. As we have seen, these
gray matter in the frontal cortex (shown on the right) has a much
lines of communication are set up by chemical changes trig-
higher heritability (indicated by pink and red shading) than the
proportion of gray matter in posterior regions (shown on the left and
gered by the cells themselves.
indicated in blue). From Toga & Thompson (2005). The clearest pattern in biological development is the
hierarchical nature of cell differentiation by which cells
that are initially similar can become different kinds of
turn, influence related functions, such as attention and rea- specialized cells at later stages. The pattern is clearly
soning: with better memory, the outputs of attention and sequential; cells pass through stages that are mostly, but
reasoning systems could be held in mind longer. In the perhaps not strictly, irreversible. Very early in the blastula
most complex cases, a pleiotropic gene affects several differ- stage, cells show considerable flexibility in terms of their
ent brain regions, and these regions also interact with each function; when certain cell groups are moved at an early
other. Most researchers who examine relations between stage, the organism can adjust to continue normal devel-
genes and behavior think that the most complex case is also opment. Thus, a cell’s final fate is not prespecified in its
the most common (Visscher et al., 2008). first few steps of differentiation. Instead, specializations
In short, researchers are working “bottom-up,” starting emerge later, as a function of the cell’s surrounding ana-
from the molecular level, and “top-down,” starting from tomical and physiological context.
the behavioral level, to go beyond correlating genotypic These patterns in biological development suggest some
variations directly with behavioral variations. Their goal is similar principles for how psychological processes develop
to eventually explain all the specific interactions between and differentiate. As you will see in later chapters, simpler
genes and behaviors and the results of those interactions. emotions, such as joy, fear, anxiety, and anger, are thought
Rather than simply asking which genes are “responsible for” to provide a necessary context for the development of more
particular behaviors, researchers are exploring the richly complex emotions, such as guilt and pride. Similarly, some
interconnected system of relationships between genes, pro- argue that a child’s early emerging understanding of her
teins, various neural structures, and many facets of cogni- physical and social worlds influences the way she later
tion, perception, and emotion—as well as their interactions comes to understand the biological world.
with a wide range of environmental factors, from diet to As researchers seek to understand the origins of psycho-
visual stimulation. logical systems, they must confront questions about the
different kinds of influences that shape those systems. The
powerful influences of “nature” and “nurture” have been
discussed for millennia, and in the past few decades, the
Conclusions “either/or” arguments about them have given way to an
account of their interactions. Genes are one component of
Developmental biology is one of the most exciting and rap- interrelated biological processes that are also subject to envi-
idly changing areas of research in the life sciences. Genetic ronmental influences at many levels. The local environment
mechanisms and anatomical structures are being more of a particular cell in the body, for example, might activate a
Cognitive
function 3
Cognitive Mechanism 3
function 1
A gene influences
1
several areas of the
brain, and each area
Gene 3
Cognitive
2 affects several
function 2
cognitive processes.
3
Cognitive
function 3
gene in that cell, causing it to produce a new protein. When length of a critical period can determine whether a neck like
the new protein becomes part of the cellular environment, a giraffe’s or a neck like an antelope’s will result. Likewise,
it can cause chemical changes that subsequently turn other the effects of a teratogen depend on when it is introduced
genes on or off. In this manner, one gene can interact with into the prenatal environment.
many others. A more global environmental factor, such as Sequencing, which is intimately related to timing,
a toxin in drinking water, might also affect gene activities, also exerts its own powerful influences. If, for example,
often causing harm. Likewise, a deprived environment or the sequence of development of the basic body plan
one that is especially stimulating may affect brain growth specified by homeobox genes is disrupted, we can see a
through gene regulation pathways. proliferation of incoherent structures in the first most
In all of these interactions, timing—as they say in anterior compartment. The sequence of development is
vaudeville—is everything. We have seen how timing influ- also crucial when particular structures, such as the neu-
ences many patterns of development. During the blastula ral plate, must serve as the foundation or the scaffold for
stage, even the timing of an individual cell’s migration later-developing structures. The earlier structures guide
through the primitive streak influences its differentiation growth of what comes later and even influence patterns
and development. During embryonic development, the of cell specialization.
CONCLUSIONS 71
STU DY AN D REVI EW
Pupil
Optic
Iris
nerve
We begin our discussion of perceptual development with tem consists of two eyes, each with a cornea, a lens, an iris,
vision, often thought to be the most complex perceptual sys- a retina with a two-dimensional, light-sensitive surface, and
tem in development. Vision also nicely illustrates the con- an optic nerve that seems to carry visual information from
trast between sensation and perception. Sensation involves the retina to the brain (see Figure 3.2). In short, they realized
registering sensory information as it is initially taken in— that the visual system has extensive anatomical specializa-
for example, by the retina of the eye, the hair cells of the tions tailored to receiving information in the form of light.
auditory system, or the taste buds of the tongue. Perception Given the easily observable structures and specializations
involves interpreting sensory input, such as understanding of the eye, an empiricist would argue that these are simply
that certain shapes and contours make up a face, knowing sensory constraints that affect the initial processing of visual
that one object appears farther away than another, or recog- information. In this view, these sensory specializations are
nizing particular sound patterns as a voice. We will examine largely separate from the more impressive perceptual abilities—
such topics as the sharpness of vision, color vision, depth such as seeing objects in depth, differentiating colors, and
perception, and face perception. recognizing patterns and objects—which are thought to arise
From vision we move on to hearing, asking how infants from an all-purpose associative learning system. By contrast,
notice and locate sounds and how they pick up on more the nativist would argue that specializations similar to the
complex patterns, such as those of speech. We then consider eye’s facility for processing light might well occur at every level
the chemical senses of taste and smell, asking whether new- of perceptual processing—from the most peripheral sensory
borns seem to distinguish different tastes using the same receptors in the eye all the way “upward” to the highest-level
categories that adults do and whether smells help them conscious impressions of visual perception. Over the centu-
respond more effectively to the world. Finally, we consider ries, both points of view flourished and attracted many adher-
how infants integrate information across these different ents, largely because there was no experimental evidence that
avenues of perception—for example, how a baby links his seemed to strongly favor one view over the other.
sister’s voice with her face. In all these explorations, we will Starting in the 1960s, however, a rapidly growing body of
return to the questions raised earlier about the blind girl research on young infants’ capabilities started to reveal the
whose sight was restored and the newborn arriving home. extent to which visual perception requires specialized process-
ing capacities. One way to approach the current state of this
research is to consider the sensory and perceptual capacities
of the young infant as we move “upstream” from the most
Vision peripheral, basic, and sensory visual input to more central,
complex, and perceptual visual experiences. For example,
Thinkers in several fields have long been interested in our at a peripheral, sensory level, much of the ability to register
understanding of the visual world and how it develops from light at different levels of brightness involves processing that
infancy. In fact, seventeenth-century scholars already had a takes place in the retina, at the back of the eye. The more
fairly detailed understanding of the anatomy of the visual “midstream” level of visual processing, which involves much
system. They knew that from birth, the human visual sys- of the processing of color, happens in midbrain structures.
VISION 79
Video camera
circles. If this procedure were repeated with many infants in essence, creates preferences. Most of us become bored if
of a particular age, the aggregated results would suggest a we see the same thing again and again. The habituation
threshold for visual acuity at that age, which could be com- method purposely creates this kind of boredom as a way to
pared with the threshold values for adults. create a preference for a new stimulus. At first, a gray panel
As simple as it sounds, there are several challenges in might be interesting enough to an infant that she would
making this technique work reliably. Researchers must be look at it for some time. But if it is shown and removed
vigilant that some other factor besides acuity isn’t causing repeatedly, even an infant reaches a point when she would
infants to look at one pattern more than another. For exam- rather look at almost anything else. When the infant seems
ple, researchers must ensure that the panels differ only in the completely bored with the panel, so that she hardly looks
acuity level required to see a difference between them, and at it when it is presented, she is said to experience habitua-
not in other ways, such as overall brightness. In addition, if tion to that stimulus. Habituation is used to study not just
the infant sits in a parent’s lap (which is common) and the vision, but also audition, olfaction, and taste.
parent can see the stimuli, the parent might unknowingly The experimenter then starts the critical second phase of
hold the infant a bit differently when the infant looks at the habituation method by introducing a second panel and
the gray panel. (This can be overcome by blindfolding the watching to see whether the infant shows dishabituation—
adult.) Moreover, the researchers collecting the data must be that is, renewed interest in response to the new panel. In a
“blind” to the experimental condition—that is, they should study of infants’ visual acuity, this new panel might have very
be unaware of which panel the infant is viewing. Other- fine-grained black and white stripes. If the infant doesn’t per-
wise, their own biases might influence the process. Finally, ceive the new panel as different from the original gray one,
a challenge involved in interpreting data from preferential then she should not dishabituate—that is, her looking time
looking paradigms is the possibility that an infant who can should not increase in response to the new panel. But if the
easily tell two displays apart visually might not always show infant does see a difference, she should initially look much
a preference for one over the other. longer when the new panel is presented, followed by a gradual
A second approach to measuring infants’ visual acuity decline in looking at the panel as she becomes bored with
involves the habituation method. Rather than hoping that it. To find a threshold measure of acuity, an experimenter
infants’ preferences will be strong enough to provide evi- might start with the second panel with the finest-grained pat-
dence that they can discriminate between stimuli, as the tern and then gradually substitute panels with more coarse-
preferential looking method does, the habituation method, grained patterns until dishabituation occurs. Changes in
looking times at a stimulus in a standard habituation/disha- In young infants, VEP measurements reveal sharper
bituation sequence are shown in Figure 3.5. levels of acuity than preferential looking or habituation
A third way of testing acuity is by the optokinetic nys- methods do. This discrepancy raises a fascinating question:
tagmus method. Optokinetic nystagmus is the jumping of Do some parts of the infant’s visual system start to detect
the eyes as they track a continuous succession of objects that fine-grained patterns before the infant is able to act on such
stream by, such as watching telephone poles stream by while information? That is, the infant’s brain may be registering
looking out of a moving train. In this method, researchers a certain level of acuity during initial perceptual process-
drag a panel of vertical stripes from right to left in front of ing, but it may not be able to translate that information
an infant to determine how fine-grained the stripe pattern into actually “seeing” at that level of acuity and using it as
must be before the infants’ eyes stop jumping from stripe to a basis for action. It may take time for those initial stages
stripe and behave as if it were one homogeneous gray pat- of perceptual processing to become fully integrated with
tern moving from right to left. higher levels of perception. The difference between VEP
Finally, acuity can be tested by measuring the electrical measures of acuity and those from habituation or preferen-
activity in the infant’s brain that is evoked by visual displays. tial looking highlights an important point about studying
This method, known as the visually evoked potential preverbal infants. Older children and adults can describe
(VEP) method, involves attaching electrodes to the baby’s how they are experiencing a visual display, but even with
scalp to measure brain activity and to determine at what careful, well-designed measurement techniques, it is still
point the changing striped patterns that the infant is watch- much more difficult to know exactly what preverbal infants
ing on a screen no longer cause corresponding changes in are experiencing.
the brain’s electrical activity. Thus, as an infant watches the Even if we assume that measures based on habituation
screen, the researcher might present a sequence of patterns, and preferential looking are the best indicators of what very
alternating between the same all-gray screen and different young infants actually see, they still show that infants have
black and white striped patterns, while monitoring the elec- enough acuity to perceive some lines, edges, and outlines.
trical activity in the infant’s brain. At the beginning of the Over the first year of life, acuity improves considerably
sequence, the striped pattern would be coarse-grained with (Norcia et al., 2005). Figure 3.6 summarizes the changes in
a few wide stripes. As the sequence progressed, the infant acuity in the first 3 years, based on average measurements
would be shown finer and finer-grained stripes. Initially, from visually evoked potentials, optokinetic nystagmus, and
every time the image on the screen switched from gray to preferential looking techniques (Courage & Adams, 1990;
striped, the researcher would measure a related change in Kellman & Banks, 1998; Teller & Movshon, 1986). Averag-
brain activity. But when the striped pattern became too ing across these measures, by age 6 months, the infant has
fine-grained for the infant to tell it apart from the solid gray nearly 20:100 vision, meaning she can see patterns at a dis-
pattern, the brain activity corresponding to the switch to tance of 20 feet about as well as an adult with normal vision
the striped pattern would not occur. could see the same patterns at 100 feet. Many nearsighted
VISION 81
50
Visual acuity (cycles/deg)
VEP
10 OKN
FPL
5
20/20
1
20/25
Visual acuity
20/50
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35
Age (in weeks) 20/100
A
20/200
FIGURE 3.6 Changing visual acuity in infants. 20/400
Acuity clearly improves in the first 3 years of 20/800
life regardless of the particular measure used.
(A) Here increased visual acuity is shown using 0 3 6 9 12 24 36
visually evoked potentials (VEP), optokinetic Age (in months)
nystagmus (OKN), and (forced choice) preferential
looking (FPL) methods. By 6 months or so, the
infant’s acuity is sharp enough that it would
rarely limit other perceptual tasks. Adapted from
Kellman & Arterberry (2006). (B) The same photo
is superimposed on a preferential looking methods
curve to show how it would look to the infant at
different ages as acuity increases. B
adults without glasses cope with this kind of modest differ- These changes in the eye’s shape do not happen auto-
ence in acuity throughout their lives. matically according to a timetable of biological matu-
ration. Instead, the eye’s growth depends on whether it
receives focused input—that is, whether the light patterns
falling on the retina are sharply focused by the lens of the
Q: What are three different ways of measuring eye. If the light patterns on the retina are out of focus,
visual acuity in the human infant? the eye continues to grow until the patterns become more
focused (see Figure 3.7). Apparently, the eye “assumes”
that if the pattern is out of focus, it is because the eye is
Many other acuity-related factors also change during too small and the image is falling behind the retina, so
the first year of life, especially astigmatism. In astigma- the eye grows to adjust. This is generally true; a newborn’s
tism, stimuli at different orientations are perceived with eyes are almost always too small to project images opti-
different levels of acuity. Thus, a vertical line might be mally on the retina.
in sharp focus while an otherwise similar horizontal line Humans are not the only species in which the growth of
would be blurry. The perceptual differences caused by the eyes depends in part on how sharply the retinal images
astigmatism are dramatic in very young infants, but only are focused. Researchers have found that manipulating the
minor in most 1-year-olds and adults (Mutti et al., 2004). degree of focused input to the retinas of newly hatched
When adults experience astigmatism, it is typically due chicks affects the growth of the chicks’ eyes. If, from birth,
to distortions in the lens of the eye or the cornea, but in a baby chick wears contact lenses that keep its retinal images
young infants astigmatism is normal. It happens because blurry, its eyeball and retina will continue to grow far larger
the eyeball is irregularly shaped in early infancy, and than normal as the visual system keeps trying to expand to
consequently, the retinal surface is irregular as well. As the size that will properly focus the image. Conversely, if the
a result, only a small portion of the light that reaches the chicks experience sharply focused light on large regions of
retina can be in focus at one time. As the infant matures, the retina unusually early in their development, the eyeball
the eyeball grows and develops a more spherical form, and retina will stop growing at a smaller size than normal.
which reduces the astigmatism. Soon after the infant These environmental effects on the growth of the chicks’
is 1 year old, his eyeball has become roughly spherical, eyes start to happen almost immediately after the image
such that the light passing through the lens now lands, in on the retina goes either out of or into focus (Judge, 1990;
focus, on the whole retina. Wallman et al., 1987; Zhu et al., 2005). Of course, there are
VISION 83
Color
Color is a vivid part of most people’s lives and may have
important uses in all cultures, such as picking out a brightly
colored object from a cluttered background, determining
the ripeness or poisonousness of various fruits, and perceiv-
ing changes in the weather. Newborn infants have quite
impressive color vision. They not only are able to tell apart A
different wavelengths of light (called hue), but also can
cluster wavelengths into categories very much like the ones
that adults use. Although it might seem that hue is simply
a formal word for color, it actually refers only to the aspect B
of color that depends on wavelengths of light. Our impres- FIGURE 3.8 Light categories and continua. (A) The light coming
sions of color are also influenced by the ratio of color to out of a prism (or rainbow), which is a smoothly varying continuum of
white light (saturation) and the display’s overall brightness. light frequency, appears to have “bands” corresponding to such easily
labeled colors as “red,” “blue,” and “green”; these bands are the con-
For the most part, when researchers discuss color vision in
sequence of the categorical perception of color. (B) By contrast, light
infants, they are talking about hue. intensity for continuously varying shades of gray varies in a smooth,
gradual manner from dark to light with no obvious “bands.”
Perceiving Hue and the Clustering of Hues Early on,
researchers used the techniques of preferential looking and
habituation to test infants’ color vision. In many of these
experiments, the investigators showed the infants pairs to cluster stimuli that vary along a continuum into discrete
of different-colored panels of equal brightness and then categories. Even though the stimuli differ only in small,
observed whether infants could distinguish between them. consistent increments, those that are grouped together in a
The studies suggested that infants have color vision very category seem highly similar to each other, while those in
early and that it improves significantly in the first 4 weeks of different categories seem markedly different (Goldstone &
life as infants develop the ability to distinguish ever-smaller Hendrickson, 2010; Harnad, 1987).
differences in hue (Peeples & Teller, 1975; Teller, 1998). Figure 3.9 illustrates the process of categorical percep-
These improvements are thought to result from increases in tion of color. The difference between the wavelengths of
the processing abilities of the whole visual system, not just blue 1 and blue 2 is the same magnitude as the difference
from parts specialized for color (Kellman & Banks, 1998). between the wavelengths of blue 2 and green 1. Yet, blue
Perceiving differences between hues is one thing; cluster- 2 and green 1 appear much more distinct from each other
ing sets of hues into color categories is quite another. When than do blue 1 and blue 2. The same pattern holds for other
you look at Figure 3.8A, it seems to have red bands, yellow color boundaries, such as green/yellow and yellow/red. Even
bands, and green bands, among others—but these bands when we’re looking at identical differences in physical wave-
of color are actually psychological constructs. The light lengths of light, we don’t always perceive the same degree of
pattern depicted in the figure, like the patterns created by difference between hues. Across most of the continuum of
prisms and rainbows, is actually a smooth continuum of wavelengths, a difference of 35 nanometers between hues
hues that runs from reds (with wavelengths of approximately is hardly noticeable. But at certain critical boundaries—
700 nanometers, or 0.0000007 meter) to blue-purples (with compare 470 and 505 nanometers—the 35-nanometer inter-
wavelengths of about 400 nanometers). All the colors of the val appears to separate quite different shades of blue and green
rainbow are situated at different points along this contin- (Boynton, 1979; Wandell, 1985).
uum of wavelengths. As observers, however, we do not expe- These differences in our discrimination abilities sim-
rience a world of gradually shifting colors; instead, we tend ply mean that we cluster groups of light wavelengths into
to see classes of colors that seem to have distinct boundar- classes, which we know as familiar colors such as red, green,
ies. This is in marked contrast to how we see a continuum blue, and yellow. One benefit of this perceptual tendency to
of different levels of brightness, as in the panel shown in categorize is that it simplifies our interactions with the world
Figure 3.8B. If shades of gray were perceived in the same and with others. Communicating the color of an object is
manner as hue, we would see bands reflecting different much easier if a relatively large range of wavelengths can
classes of grayness from white to black. be treated as roughly equivalent—as members of the same
This process of clustering wavelengths of light into class. Consider how much simpler it is to convey a color
familiar colors is one aspect of categorical perception. In than a specific shade of gray on the smooth continuum
its broadest sense, categorical perception refers to a tendency between black and white.
VISION 85
ing closer. When infants as young as 1 month observe such
Q: What evidence supports the claims that expanding patterns, their reactions indicate that they perceive
very young infants can see color and that they depth. For example, if the looming object appears to be on a
trajectory to collide with them, they will pull their heads back.
perceive colors categorically? Alternatively, if the looming object is a doorway-like open-
ing, they will not try to move out of its path but instead will
seem to anticipate moving through the opening (Carroll &
An important principle emerges here that will extend Gibson, 1981; Schmuckler & Li, 1998; Yonas, 1981). Thus,
to our later discussions of cognitive development as well: not only are very young infants able to use looming as a depth
the ability to perceive and understand the world requires clue, they are also able to interpret looming objects differently
categorization. There is simply too much raw information from similarly sized looming openings.
for any organism to master unless it simplifies and treats Another type of dynamic cue, called motion paral-
the members of some large classes of stimuli, such as colors lax, tracks the different ways that near objects and far
or shapes, as equivalent for certain purposes. The ability objects appear to move relative to the motion of the eye. As
to categorize is one of the most important ways in which Figure 3.10 shows, when an observer sees an object, an image
infants and young children gain early footholds in percep- of the object falls on the observer’s retina. When the observ-
tual and cognitive development. er’s eyes move, the images of those objects on the retina move
Depth Perception
When a mountain goat is born in an especially steep area
of the Sierra Nevada Mountains, it needs to be able to tell
the difference between a small step down and a deep abyss
as soon as it can walk. Likewise, it is highly uncommon
for a baby squirrel to leap too far and miss the next branch
in its sights or for a puppy to fail to catch the ball you’ve
tossed. Animals that move about on their own would not
survive long if they could not sense the distance to objects
and surfaces. As adults, we use depth information whenever
we reach out and grasp something and whenever we move
through the world; we use it to drive, walk through crowds,
and avoid dangerous drop-offs. But despite the obvious evi-
dence that mature members of most visual species can per-
ceive depth, the question of whether human infants can do
so proved difficult to answer.
VISION 87
the way infants learn to perceive objects in depth (Berkeley, the inability to fuse the right and left eyes’ images becomes
1709/1901; Pastore, 1965). More specifically, Berkeley sug- irreversible, even if the problem with the eye muscles is later
gested that the infant is born with only two simple depth- surgically corrected (Fawcett et al., 2005). Thus, a child as
related sensations—the convergence feedback from the eye young as 6 years old who has surgery to correct a cross-eyed
muscles when focusing on the object and the sensation of condition is unlikely to fully recover stereopsis, even after
the degree of reaching or movement required to touch the several years of normal visual experience. In such cases,
object—and an ability to associate these two sensations. the child often comes to rely on one eye much more than
Simply put, infants would begin to associate a strong sense the other to avoid seeing double. This critical-period effect
of binocular convergence with a near reach and weaker feel- is thought to apply to stereopsis in particular, and not to
ings of convergence with longer reaches. convergence, because children with misaligned eyes might
Berkeley’s empiricist model of learning to perceive depth still register the muscle sensations associated with eye move-
makes some general predictions. First, if this model is cor- ments toward greater or lesser degrees of convergence.
rect, the very young infant should simply be unable to per-
ceive depth, since she has not had time to learn the sensory
associations that Berkeley describes. Second, depth percep-
tion should emerge gradually, based on the amounts and Q: What is some evidence for a critical period
kinds of visual experiences the infant has had. The biggest for binocular vision in humans?
problem with Berkeley’s view is the evidence that many spe-
cies, including humans, have excellent depth perception
abilities in the absence of any learning experiences. Infants’ The critical period for developing binocular depth percep-
eyes will converge automatically to fixate on a close object tion has been documented more precisely in monkeys and
as early as 1 month (Hainline & Riddell, 1996), and infants cats. Like humans, monkeys and cats have certain neurons in
between 5 and 8 months old can use convergence informa- the visual cortex that fire only when both eyes receive input
tion as a cue to depth. simultaneously. These cells seem to be involved in comput-
It is more difficult to tell whether infants use binocular ing the disparity between the two images and fusing them
parallax to perceive depth with normal objects because this into one image. In a newborn kitten or monkey, however,
ability involves a precise mental comparison of the tiny dif- these binocularly driven cells tend to be less responsive than
ferences in the images that the two eyes see. But it is possible they are in older animals. They become more fine-tuned only
to artificially generate images that can only be seen when with considerable experience. If the animal wears eye patches
they are fused by the two eyes using parallax cues. (With on alternating sides to prevent that experience, the number
each eye individually, the viewer sees only a seemingly ran- of binocularly driven cells starts to diminish and eventually
dom pattern of dots.) To see whether infants use binocular die off (Freeman & Ohzawa, 1992). When this happens, no
parallax cues, researchers present such images to infants on amount of binocular visual experience later on can resurrect
a computer screen. When the images are set in motion on them (Crawford et al., 1996). This level of neural analysis
the screen, the researchers monitor whether the infant’s eyes concretely illustrates how critical-period effects are manifested
track the images’ movements. This technique has suggested in the nervous system. It shows how some neural circuits are
that stereopsis emerges in about the fourth month of life attuned to certain stimuli during a critical period and then
and becomes increasingly sophisticated over the next few become much less changeable, allowing them to operate more
months (Fox et al., 1980; Granrud, 1986; Wattam-Bell, efficiently (Hensch, 2004; Wang et al., 2010).
2003). One possible reason that younger infants do not use Pictorial cues can convey depth even in two-
stereopsis is that it may require greater visual acuity than dimensional scenes like paintings and photographs and can
they have yet developed (Brown & Lindsey, 2009). If young do so even when seen with only one eye. Figure 3.12 illus-
infants can’t see patterns below a certain level of resolution, trates three different types of pictorial cues: interposition,
as shown in the blurriest images in Figure 3.6B, they may texture gradients, and convergence in the distance. Interpo-
not be able to detect disparities in highly similar images. sition occurs when one shape overlaps another, suggesting
The development of stereopsis presents another example that it must be in front of the other. Texture gradients serve
of a critical-period effect. Some children are born with eye as cues to depth when a repeating pattern on a surface,
muscles that make their eyes excessively converge or diverge, such as the rough grid of a brick path, gets smaller and
resulting in either a “cross-eyed” or “wall-eyed” condition. smaller the farther away the surface is. Convergence in the
As a result, the separate images that their right and left eyes distance describes the way parallel lines, such as straight
see are too different for the brain to fuse them into a single railroad tracks, seem to meet as they extend toward the
image or to use the disparity between the images to provide horizon and thus show linear perspective, or the appear-
depth information. If this situation persists for several years, ance of distance as the lines converge. Adults in all cultures
can use these pictorial depth cues, but infants younger et al., 2002). These findings suggest that at around 6 months
than about 6 months do not seem to use them (Kellman & of age, a capacity emerges for using and integrating different
Banks, 1998). Thus, only humans older than 6 months are kinds of pictorial cues. It is interesting that in some tasks,
likely to be fooled by the illusion in Figure 3.12D, in which 5-month-old infants can readily see depth with two eyes
identical-size cylinders in fact look very different in size by using binocular cues, but they cannot see it with one
because of surrounding pictorial cues. eye when only pictorial cues are available (Hemker et al.,
One technique that researchers have used extensively to 2010). This pattern of results does not necessarily mean that
investigate when infants use pictorial depth cues relies on the ability develops solely as a consequence of maturation;
the simple fact that when young infants are presented with
two objects, they consistently reach for the nearer one. A
clever experimenter can use this reaching preference to find
the conditions under which the infant sees a difference in
depth. In one study, researchers showed 5- and 7-month-old
infants a “window” like the one in Figure 3.13, which con-
tained several pictorial cues to depth (Yonas et al., 1978).
They then observed whether the infants more often reached
toward the side of the window that would appear closer
if the infants were relying on pictorial depth cues. The
5-month-olds showed no preferential reaching for the edge
that would seem nearer based on pictorial cues. Instead, A B
they responded to the surface as if it had no patterns on it FIGURE 3.13 Sensitivity to pictorial depth. (A) A simple trap-
at all, reaching for various parts seemingly at random. In ezoid conveys an illusion of depth, but it is not as compelling as
contrast, the 7-month-olds consistently reached for the edge (B) the illusion conveyed by an Ames window, where the right side
that would have looked closer to someone using pictorial seems farther away because of pictorial cues to depth. These cues
cues. Recall from the visual acuity studies discussed earlier are convergence in the distance (the top and bottom lines of the
that 5-month-olds could easily see the lines and shadows window become closer to each other) and texture gradients (the
that serve as the pictorial cues. Nonetheless, they did not window panes become smaller in the distance). Young infants who
cannot use pictorial cues to depth will reach equally for the right and
use them to see depth.
left sides of the Ames window or in the middle, while older infants
Studies of many other pictorial depth cues have found will reach more for the left side, thinking it is closer. This is one case
this same time frame to be significant: 5-month-olds typi- where younger infants are more accurate; they are not misled by
cally do not use pictorial cues effectively, but 7-month-olds an illusion of pictorial depth. Adapted from Ames (1951) and
do (Hemker et al., 2010; Kellman & Banks, 1998; Yonas Yonas et al. (1978).
VISION 89
learning experiences could be important as well. It is not yet
clear why the ability to use pictorial cues develops later in
infancy than the ability to use dynamic cues or binocular
cues. One possible reason is that getting depth information
from pictorial cues may require more learning or cognitive
interpretation than the other types of cues. Another possi-
bility is that pictorial cues may require more advanced levels
of visual acuity.
2
Q: In what ways does depth perception in
young infants differ from depth perception in 1
Binocular cues
older children and adults? (e.g., disparity)
0
Dynamic cues
(e.g., motion parallax)
Functional Significance of the Depth Perception Sys-
tem As we have seen, the development of depth perception FIGURE 3.16 Three different sets of cues specify depth for
reveals a consistent pattern (see Figure 3.16): The use of the infant. Dynamic cues such as motion parallax appear to be
dynamic cues emerges possibly as early as the first month used first, probably right at birth. Binocular cues start to emerge as
partially effective at around 1 month (as depicted by the dashed
of life. Then the effective use of binocular cues develops
lines), becoming much more effective at 4 months. Pictorial cues are
around 4 months, followed by the effective use of pictorial not effectively used until around 6 months of age, although some
cues around 6 months. From an evolutionary perspective, sensitivity to pictorial relations may appear as early as 4 months
what might account for this sequence? (as depicted by the dashed lines).
One important advantage for the early emergence
of dynamic cues such as motion parallax is that they are
almost always reliable in the natural world. By contrast,
later-developing pictorial cues can be misleading in many pretations of developmental patterns. Ideally, they also help
optical illusions that show contradictory representations predict new patterns and motivate new research.
of depth (see Figure 3.17). In addition, dynamic cues do
not require good visual acuity, but both pictorial cues and
binocular cues do. Under blurred conditions and with Q: Why might infants be able to use dynamic
only one eye, it is still possible to use dynamic cues such
as motion parallax or looming to infer depth relationships,
cues to depth before binocular and pictorial
even while it is not possible to use the other sorts of cues. cues?
Thus, dynamic cues are most useful to infants who initially
have poor acuity and who have to judge depth in conditions
of reduced acuity, such as in low light or with tears in the
eyes. The key point is that natural selection favors the most Perceiving Patterns and
robust and reliable early system for depth perception, the
dynamic cues, and these are the dominant cues in young Recognizing Objects
infants of most species. We will see that these sorts of evo- Do infants perceive meaningful patterns in the same ways
lutionary considerations, especially in comparative research that adults do? Do they, for example, devote more attention
across several species, can often provide meaningful inter- to human faces than to other recurring patterns in their
VISION 91
to yield all the more complex outcomes. A purely empiri-
cist account of using visual primitives to perceive patterns
would claim that no prior biases influence the ways we form
associations between primitives. Instead, the only factor
determining which primitives become most closely linked
in the mind would be the frequency with which we see them
together in the environment. Thus, if an infant more often
A B
encountered two dots stacked vertically and enclosed by an
oval shape rather than two dots side by side within an oval,
that infant should become more attentive to vertical-dot
ovals than to pattern configurations that resemble human
faces. Alternatively, if infants do have a built-in bias to pre-
fer face-like stimuli, they would prefer to look at patterns
that resemble faces, even if they encountered those patterns
less frequently than other patterns.
pattern moving in a certain direction relative to its shape. It might seem as if this wide array of built-in mechanisms
This remarkably sophisticated ability shows that not all pat- for picking out specific environmental patterns would pro-
tern perception requires learning from experience. vide powerful evidence against empiricist accounts of the
A simpler example in the same species has to do with development of pattern perception. But the empiricists have
the way chicks peck at their food. Newborn chicks will not a strong reply when it comes to using these kinds of findings
peck the same amount at all seed-size objects; their pecking about nonhuman animals to theorize about pattern percep-
rate strongly depends on the object’s shape. In general, they tion in humans. Consider again the frog’s bug detector.
peck more at rounded objects than at those with straight Although it is marvelously efficient, it has one potentially
edges and sharp angles (Fantz, 1957; see Figure 3.19). This devastating drawback for organisms that live in varied and
preference makes sense, given how rarely natural foods— changing environments. It is so “hard-wired” in the frog’s
or other things in the natural world—have straight, angu- visual system that it is inflexible and unable to deal with
lar contours. This might seem strange, since chicks didn’t environmental changes. When a new type of bug that is
evolve in an environment that would have required them much easier to catch and more nutritious enters the environ-
to distinguish natural from human-made objects. But their ment, the frog can only benefit if this new food source has
perceptual bias is better understood not as a tendency to the right properties to trigger the bug detector. Some species
avoid non-natural things, but as a tendency to better per-
ceive and act on natural ones. These built-in preferences are
not for specific foods. Rather, these broad predispositions
provide a “head start” in perceptual learning about natural
foods that makes the learning process easier. Later work has
suggested that, beyond foods, newborn chicks have quite A B
general perceptual preferences toward displays that are FIGURE 3.19 Perceptual bias for natural shapes. Newborn
physically possible as opposed to impossible (Regolin et al., chicks prefer to peck at objects like those shown in (A) as opposed
2011). As we will see in later chapters as well, rather than to those in (B). In this way, they show built-in constraints on pattern
placing limits on what the developing organism can per- perception. Adapted from Fantz (1957).
VISION 93
of frogs will starve in a cage full of nutritious, anesthetized see that this plasticity initially develops within a set of per-
bugs just like those they normally catch in the wild because ceptual constraints that are critical for the earliest pattern
the bugs are not moving and the frogs don’t recognize them perception.
as food. Thus, as remarkable as these early emerging exam-
ples of pattern recognition are in other species, we need to
look beyond these fixed and inflexible pattern recognition Q: What are the tradeoffs between a pattern
abilities to fully understand the development of pattern recognition ability that is fully functional at
perception in humans. Like many animals, humans have birth and one that requires considerable
the capacity to learn to recognize many novel patterns after
having some exposure to them. environmental input?
inflexible systems of behavior, while others come into the FIGURE 3.20 Perceiving object unity and common fate. When
world with fewer initial competencies but with considerable 4-month-old infants see two rod segments moving behind an occlud-
plasticity. More complex organisms usually develop more ing box (A), they infer that one unbroken rod is present (B) and not
two fragments (C), even though the fragments are in some respects
plasticity. For example, mammals seem able to respond to
more similar to the original display. When the initial display is static,
their environments with much more plasticity than insects the same infants tend to prefer the broken rod display. Younger
can. We will see that human infants show great plasticity infants prefer the broken rod even in the dynamic condition. Newborn
in the ways they learn to perceive not only visual patterns, chicks, however, prefer the unbroken rod in the moving condition.
but auditory patterns as well. At the same time, we will also Adapted from Kellman & Spelke (1983).
Face Perception
As infants use various perceptual principles and strategies
to begin to perceive and understand distinct objects, one of
the objects they encounter earliest and most frequently is the
human face. The ability to recognize a caregiver’s face enables A B
an infant to know where to direct his efforts for comfort or
FIGURE 3.21 Normal versus scrambled faces. From 3 months
protection. The ability to tell a happy face from an angry
on, humans immediately see a dramatic difference between faces in
face enables an infant to know when to approach or avoid which features are normally configured (A) as opposed to scrambled
another person. The salience of human faces in infants’ lives (B). Although it is much more difficult to show such preferences in
has led researchers to ask whether young infants, and per- newborn infants, most researchers today would agree that such pref-
haps even newborns, are predisposed to process the features erences are present at birth. Adapted from Bhatt et al. (2005).
VISION 95
sions from fearful ones, showing a preference for happy faces
(Farroni et al., 2007). They also prefer face-like stimuli over
other kinds of stimuli when the face’s eyes and mouth are
dark relative to the background (as is true of humans from
all backgrounds), but they do not show this preference when
the eyes and mouth are lighter than the background (Far-
A B
roni et al., 2005). As we will see shortly, they also show
preferences for attractive faces over unattractive ones (Slater
FIGURE 3.22 Scrambled but symmetrical faces. Even when
scrambled faces are symmetrical around a vertical centerline (the et al., 1998, 2000a).
way normal faces are), infants prefer (A) normal faces to (B) scram- Despite newborns’ impressive face perception abilities,
bled but symmetrical faces. Adapted from Easterbrook et al. (1999). more subtle analyses of faces seem to take a few months to
develop (Lee et al., 2013). For example, although newborns
prefer normal faces to scrambled ones, they do not show
to look at ovals with “top-heavy” patterns—that is, arrange- a preference for normal faces over highly distorted faces
ments with more dark features in the upper half of the in which the eyes are much too far apart, looking almost
oval—rather than “bottom-heavy” ovals (Macchi Cassia et rabbit-like. In contrast, 3-month-olds clearly prefer normal
al., 2004; Turati, 2004). Thus, even when the stimuli look faces over the faces that have extreme distortions (Bhatt et
nothing like a face, infants prefer patterns with more fea- al., 2005). These findings suggest that newborns may have
tures packed into the top half of the figure than the bottom some sense of how facial features are laid out, but they have
half. The top-heavy bias cannot account for all the ways in not yet developed a sensitivity to the normal spatial relations
which newborns and young infants prefer faces (Farroni et between those features, a factor that may also explain why
al., 2007), but it does raise an important caution. The stud- they initially show no preference for human faces over mon-
ies of other species are not definitive for humans. It remains key faces. Visual experience in infancy and early childhood
a possibility that human infants are unique among primates also seems to be important to developing some of the more
in the extent to which they must learn about faces dur- subtle sensitivities involved in normal face perception. The
ing the first few months of life. In addition, the preference beginning of the chapter described the remarkable recovery
for faces by human newborns does not seem to just be for of a girl who had minimal visual experience before having
human faces. Thus, newborn infants show no preference for congenital cataracts removed. Studies of face perception
human faces over monkey faces while nonetheless being able in adults who had similar minimal visual experiences in
to tell them apart (Di Giorgio et al., 2012). In addition, they infancy and childhood suggest that these individuals do not
prefer to look at upright monkey faces over inverted faces, show some of the heightened responsiveness to facial con-
just as they prefer upright human faces. Taken together, figurations that is typical of people with normal vision in
these findings suggest a quite general face preference system infancy and childhood. These adults can learn to recognize
that is somewhat abstract in nature; initially it doesn’t even faces (Ostrovsky et al., 2006), but they may show perma-
involve a preference for human faces but instead emphasizes nent, subtle deficits in face-specific aspects of processing,
a more schematic face-like pattern. suggesting that there may be an early critical period for
Although they show a preference for faces over many developing these nuanced perceptual abilities (Le Grande et
other stimuli, newborns probably perceive faces quite differ- al., 2004; Putzar et al., 2010).
ently from the way older children and adults do. Moreover, Another way in which newborns differ from 3-month-
they likely do a great deal of learning about faces in the first olds relates to biases toward faces of their culture. Newborns
few months of life (Balas et al., 2010). Some of this learning show no preferences for faces of their own race. But by
is especially early and rapid. For example, within hours of 3 months of age, Caucasian infants prefer to look at Cau-
birth, newborns start to show a visual preference for their casian faces, African-American infants prefer to look at
mother’s face compared with the faces of other women African-American faces, and 3-month-olds of other racial
(Bushnell et al., 1989; Pascalis et al., 1995). Remarkably, groups also show an own-race preference (Kelly et al., 2005,
they can recognize their mother’s face from its outer features 2007a, 2009). Apparently, in their first few months, infants
(hairstyle and head shape) without the inner features (facial are learning to recognize the patterns of facial features that
expression) and also from the inner features without the they see most often. These familiar patterns seem to provide
outer ones (Turati et al., 2006, 2008). These abilities must them with a kind of race-specific template for interpreting
be based on learning because newborns couldn’t possibly faces and facial expressions, one that works especially well
know before birth how their mothers would look. for novel faces of a familiar racial group.
Beyond their ability to distinguish scrambled faces from This process of becoming attuned to same-race faces is
normal ones, newborns can also discriminate happy expres- part of a more general pattern of developmental change.
VISION 97
when the faces are moving, but show a weaker preference
when they are still.
The distinctive pattern of infants’ face-tracking abilities
is one of the earliest examples in development of a U-shaped
developmental curve. In this developmental pattern, X
Transition between
degree of specialization for processing faces is present in
systems the newborn’s brain from the start, perhaps in both face-
processing systems.
It is not yet clear exactly what is present in the initial
Increasing age
“rough and ready” face-processing system. As we will see
FIGURE 3.24 U-shaped curves. U-shaped developmental curves again in Chapter 4, there seems to be more at work than
often suggest to researchers the operation of two systems. The first
simply a detector for upside-down triangles or top-heavy
may be cruder but easier to initially employ. Then during the transition
to the second more complex system, there is a temporary drop in per- patterns, but the two-system model still provides a use-
formance before an even higher level is achieved. Although suggestive ful developmental account of the way a brain region could
of two systems, it is important to note that some models can create become specialized for one kind of information without
U-shaped patterns without having to invoke two different systems. having such a bias from the start. Regardless of the details
FIGURE 3.26 Facial attractiveness. Most adults agree that some of these faces are more attractive than others. These particular faces are
computer-synthesized images that blend together attributes of the faces of many individuals. The computer generation of images allows for
systematic manipulation of facial properties. When these sorts of faces are shown to infants as young as 6 months of age, the infants show
preference patterns comparable to those of adults. One property of the images that predicts preference is the degree to which all facial features
and distances between them approach an average for all faces. A face that is average in all respects turns out not to be “average-looking” at all
but very attractive, even for infants. In this illustration, the most average faces are those constructed from the largest number of faces (32-face
composites).
VISION 99
This provocative work on attractiveness may tie into make sense of an extraordinary flux of auditory informa-
deeper issues about the origins of face perception in general. tion. Not every splash or bark should be a totally novel
If infants have an innate preference for particular feature rela- auditory event. At some point, infants need to recognize a
tions and proportions, it must be abstract enough to apply to sound as an instance of a broader category of similar sounds
faces of all ages and backgrounds. The preference for symme- that share a common meaning. Before considering sound
try and average features fits the evolutionary argument that categorization, however, we must establish how and when
members of a species should be more attracted to potential infants notice and remember sounds.
mates who appear especially healthy and therefore more likely
to produce healthy offspring. According to this argument,
individuals with average, symmetrical bodies and faces are Noticing and Remembering Sounds
more attractive because they appear to be less likely to have Just as with vision, newborns appear ready and eager to
biological problems (Thornhill & Moller, 1997). Related notice the sounds they hear and to try to make sense of
processes may also motivate parents to be more attracted to them. As already mentioned, their sound perception abilities
healthy-looking newborns. Several studies show that adults are already at work before birth. The sounds a fetus hears
tend to interact more positively with attractive infants (Hil- while in the mother’s uterus can instill auditory preferences
debrandt & Fitzgerald, 1983; Langlois et al., 1995; Sternglanz that are manifested after birth (DeCasper & Fifer, 1980;
et al., 1977). At the same time, there is now some evidence DeCasper & Spence, 1986; Kisilevsky et al., 2009). For this
that relevant experiences with unusual faces may fine-tune reason, newborns already prefer their own mother’s voice
attractiveness preferences, even in adults (Principe & Lang- over the voice of a stranger, and they prefer a story that
lois, 2012). Thus, even when there is an innate set of biases to their mother read aloud during the last weeks of pregnancy
favor certain patterns, those biases still may be modified by over an unfamiliar story. Newborns probably recognize the
experience or result in multiple sets of preferences. distinctive patterns of low-pitched sounds in the story that
they heard before birth, since low-pitched sounds penetrate
most effectively into the uterus. In fact, while still in the
womb, fetuses show different heart rate patterns in response
Q: Why might “average” faces be more to nursery rhymes that they have heard their mother read
attractive, in evolutionary terms? aloud repeatedly as opposed to unfamiliar nursery rhymes
(DeCasper et al., 1994).
In the womb, the fetus may also start to become attuned to
some of the distinctive properties of the mother’s language. For
example, if French newborns hear two recordings of a woman
Hearing speaking, one in French and the other in Russian, they can
tell the two recordings apart. A newborn whose mother speaks
A young infant is exposed to an enormous array of sounds neither French nor Russian cannot make the same discrimi-
as well as sights. From the first moments of life, babies hear nation and apparently has not yet learned to hear a difference
the excited voices of parents and the calmer voices of medical (Mehler et al., 1988; see also Kisilevsky et al., 2009).
professionals. Recall the newborn baby’s first trip home from This ability to detect contrasts between languages may
the hospital that was described at the beginning of Chapter arise from an evolutionarily ancient ability to detect differ-
1. In the next few weeks, this infant will hear the voices of ent rhythmic patterns in sounds. Thus, Dutch and English
adults, the cries of other babies, water splashing, music play- have very similar rhythmic patterns, while Japanese, Polish,
ing, and dogs barking, among thousands of other sounds, and English are all quite different from each other. Young
often several at once. Although the study of vision has dom- infants have difficulty distinguishing languages with similar
inated research on infant perception, we also have a good rhythmic patterns and find it easier to tell apart those with
understanding of the early development of the other senses, different rhythms (Ramus, 2002). This skill might seem to
especially hearing. In fact, infants start perceiving with the be uniquely human, since it relates to the human capacity
other senses, including hearing, while still in the womb, giv- for language, but cotton-top tamarins, a relatively primitive
ing these modalities a sensory head start over vision. primate, show the same ability to distinguish between lan-
Even more important, the senses show some strikingly guages with different rhythmic patterns and the same ten-
similar developmental patterns. Hearing offers some of the dency to confuse languages with similar rhythms (Ramus et
most interesting parallels with vision because using these al., 2000; Tincoff et al., 2005). Apparently, as the language
two senses requires confronting similar kinds of perceptual capacity evolved, it incorporated this more general audi-
problems. Just as we saw with visual information, infants tory skill to help organize linguistic patterns. In addition,
must learn to categorize sounds into groups in order to infants also show early abilities to learn rhythmic patterns
HEARING 101
NEW DIRECTION S IN DEVELOPMENTAL RESEARCH
A
lthough it has been known for some time that aspects auditory perception. This critical-period effect may be largely
of the visual system show critical-period effects, it caused by the brain’s compensation plasticity (see Chapter 2).
has been more difficult to document such effects in Recall that in blind people, areas of the cortex used for visual
hearing. Thanks to improving technology, research- processing can become devoted to other sensory processes
ers are now making advances in understanding critical-period instead, resulting in more acute sensation in other modalities
effects on hearing. At the forefront of this research is work (such as hearing) to help compensate for the loss of vision. Or
involving cochlear implants for very young children. the visual cortex will show activation when subjects who were
In response to incoming sound, the organ of corti within the blind from birth or who became blind at a very early age read
inner ear stimulates the nerve cells that initially process auditory Braille by touching their finger to the letters (Gizewski et al.,
information. In most cases of total deafness, the organ of corti 2003). Similarly, if the auditory cortex does not receive auditory
has been damaged. Cochlear implants are electronic devices input, it can be “colonized” by other sensory functions, such as
that can be implanted in the ears to bypass the damaged organ vision and touch, which help compensate for the hearing loss.
of corti and cause stimulation of the auditory nerve fibers. The By providing a different route for auditory input to reach the
implant creates a form of hearing, the nature of which varies, brain, cochlear implants may preserve the function of the audi-
depending on the age of the recipient. tory cortex for processing sounds (Teoh et al., 2004b).
As implants have been approved for younger and younger Evidence is accumulating that the earlier the patient receives
individuals, researchers have begun to ask whether there is a the cochlear implants, the more normal will be both her abil-
critical period for developing the ability to interpret the firing pat- ity to distinguish between various speech sounds and her abil-
terns of neurons as meaningful auditory input. Older recipients ity to articulate clearly; in fact, some neural circuits may show
of implants are able to detect sounds through those implants, permanent declines in auditory and speech-processing abili-
but can they learn to interpret those sounds in as sophisticated ties if implants are not present before roughly 4 years of age
a manner as younger recipients? Or, analogous to the case of (Eisenberg et al., 2012; Kral & Sharma, 2012). (This connection
the girl whose vision was restored following cataract surgery, between speech perception and articulation illustrates the kind
when the ability to receive auditory input is restored after early of link between perception and action that we will see in more
deafness, are there permanent effects of being deprived of detail in Chapter 4.) Two concerns remain, however. First, the
auditory experience early in life? incidence of medical complications rises with implant surgery in
In studies of cats that were born deaf, cats that received younger and younger babies (Johr et al., 2008). Second, some
cochlear implants early in development showed better results members of the deaf community feel that any increased risk
than those that received implants when they were older. Those associated with the surgery is unacceptable, given that there is
that got early implants gained higher levels of functioning in the a rich and vibrant deaf culture that offers an alternative devel-
auditory cortex, the brain area that processes sounds (Kral et opmental path to many deaf children (Sparrow, 2010). Thus,
al., 2002). Similarly, people who get cochlear implants in the even as the technology evolves and the critical-period effects
first years of life can better distinguish between sounds than are better documented, not all parents choose surgery for their
those who get the implants several years later. infants.
Children who receive cochlear implants at younger ages usu- Even adults who receive cochlear implants show some
ally have better outcomes (Pulsifer et al., 2003; Teoh et al., improvements that suggest an expansion of neural function-
2004a). “Younger” initially meant around 2 years of age, but ing in the auditory cortex. This pattern has been found both in
more recent studies of children who get implants before age 1 adult cats (Klinke et al., 1999) and in adult humans (Teoh et al.,
suggest even better results than with 2-year-olds (Colletti et al., 2004a). Thus, although it remains ideal to receive the implants
2005; Dettman & Dowell, 2010; Niparko et al., 2010), sug- as early in development as possible, they can still confer ben-
gesting a critical period in the development of some aspects of efits to adults.
HEARING 103
Blue Green
Q: How does the development of auditory 35 nm 35 nm 35 nm
“ba” “pa”
30 msec 30 msec 30 msec
B1 B2 P1 P2
Perceiving Complex Sound Patterns ⫺0.05 0.0 ⫹0.05 ⫹0.10 ⫹0.15
Some sounds, such as a bell or a snap of a finger, are relatively Voice onset time (time between lip release and voicing)
simple in acoustic terms. Others, such as human speech, are B
remarkably complex. A simple empiricist view of auditory FIGURE 3.28 Categorical perception of speech. The categorical
development might propose that babies learn to perceive perception of speech can be understood the same way as the cat-
egorical perception of color (A), except that, in this case, the physical
the sounds that are acoustically simplest first. If this were
continuum for perceiving speech (B) is the amount of time delay
true, then to young infants, all speech sounds would ini- between lip release and voicing, called the voice onset time (VOT).
tially sound the same: just an overwhelming confusion of Equal intervals of 30 milliseconds along this auditory physical
sound components. Several decades ago, however, research- continuum are not seen as the same psychologically. B1 and B2, both
ers learned that young infants seem to perceive quite a large in the “ba” category, and P1 and P2, both in the “pa” category, are
amount of structure in speech sounds. Most dramatically, seen as much more similar to each other than B2 and P1, since they
infants do not treat every different nuanced speech sound straddle the “ba” and “pa” categories. Even though physical distances
as unique. Instead, they tend to perceive discrete categories are the same, the psychological distances are much shorter when they
stay within categories rather than cross them. Even newborn infants
of sounds. Even though sounds’ physical properties vary
show these categorical effects in their perception of speech.’
smoothly along a continuum, infants regard all the sounds
within a category as very similar to each other and very
different from the sounds in different categories. In short,
infants are capable of categorical perception of speech.
Many elements of speech primarily differ from one
another in terms of their values on a single dimension. For
Q: How does the categorization of speech
example, the sounds “ba” and “pa” only differ in terms sounds help infants to perceive speech?
of voice onset time (VOT), which describes the time lag
between releasing your lips to define the sound and the
beginning, or onset, of voicing the sound. A speaker saying Studies of infants of all ages have attempted to under-
“ba” voices the “ah” sound almost immediately after releas- stand more clearly how categorical perception of sound
ing the lips, but for “pa” there is a longer delay before voic- emerges and changes over the course of infant develop-
ing occurs. The average VOTs for “ba” and “pa” are shown ment. The first breakthrough study on the topic repeat-
in Figure 3.28, which also shows the similar wavelength edly presented the infants with an artificially synthesized
continuum for color vision that we considered earlier. “ba” sound (with a consistent VOT) whenever they sucked
The VOTs range along a continuum—from negative hard enough on a special pacifier that measured sucking
VOTs, with voicing before lip release, to positive VOTs, intensity (Eimas et al., 1971). Over time, their sucking for
which occur as much as 0.2 second after lip release—but that particular “ba” sound declined, presumably because
psychologically, we do not perceive these sounds as varying they had become habituated to it. The infants would then
gradually. Instead, we group similar sounds into categories hear one of two artificially synthesized sounds that were
and treat the sounds within each category as basically the identical to the original “ba” sound except for differences
same. This means that to differentiate between “ba” and in VOT. The two sounds differed from the original “ba”
“pa,” we hear a fairly large range of VOTs as “ba” and then, by an equal amount of VOT, but in opposite directions as
past a certain point on the continuum, we hear a large range shown in Figure 3.28. To adults, the sound with a shorter
as “pa.” As Figure 3.28 shows, sounds that are within the VOT would still sound like “ba,” while the sound with
“ba” region sound very similar despite having voice onset the longer VOT would sound like “pa” because it falls on
times 30 milliseconds apart. Yet, if two sounds on opposite the other side of the category boundary. If infants had no
sides of the “ba”/“pa” boundary are 30 milliseconds apart, preexisting biases to categorically perceive speech sounds,
they sound very different, at least to adults. then both would sound equally distinct from the original
HEARING 105
DEVELOPMENT AND SOCIAL P OLICY
I
n 1998, following Governor Zell Miller’s enthusiastic recom- music (Rideout et al., 1998), even though pop music does not
mendation, Georgia’s state legislature voted to supply a free seem to share the rich mathematical structure that supposedly
recording of classical music to every child born in the state boosts aspects of reasoning. Even worse, other researchers’
(Sack, 1998). The policy aimed to improve children’s intellec- attempts to replicate the initial findings of a “Mozart effect” gen-
tual development through early stimulation. Miller was respond- erally have not succeeded (Chabris, 1999; McCutcheon, 2000;
ing to research findings interpreted as showing that exposing McKelvie & Low, 2002; Nantais & Schellenberg, 1999; Steele
infants to Mozart’s music increased their intelligence and cogni- et al., 1999). These simple failures to replicate the findings, as
tive functioning (Rauscher et al., 1993, 1995). As other states well as demonstrations of a number of problems with the initial
took an interest in adopting similar programs, it became clear studies’ experimental procedures, have repeatedly raised ques-
that these programs offered suppliers of the recordings sub- tions about whether the effect ever existed at all (Pietschnig et
stantial opportunities to profit. As a result, several companies al., 2010; Schellenberg, 2005).
sprang up offering musical recordings allegedly designed to Although several U.S. states and some other countries pro-
enhance an infant’s or toddler’s intelligence with repeated lis- posed programs of distributing free classical music to newborns
tening. One enterprising company even trademarked the name (see Figure 3.29), the proposals were rarely implemented, and
“The Mozart Effect”—a reference to the research that inspired Zell Miller’s vision seems to no longer be part of Georgia’s offi-
the trend—in an attempt to dominate the market. cial activities. Yet, sales of music and videos touting the Mozart
The so-called Mozart effect had considerable intuitive appeal. effect remain brisk, and many people firmly believe in the phe-
It seemed appropriate that classical music, which most people nomenon. It might seem harmless, but from a public policy point
view as cultured and sophisticated, would actually be good for of view, such misunderstandings incur real costs. Funds used to
the developing brain. Proponents of the effect even offered an provide these materials to infants and toddlers might be better
explanation for the mechanism of improvement. Mozart’s pieces spent on more proven ways to improve their health. In addi-
had a rich mathematical structure (Hetland, 2000; Rauscher tion, attempts to improve children’s abilities through such “quick
et al., 1993, 1995), and hearing the music’s complex rhythms fixes” tend to give short shrift to educational problems that actu-
and patterns “exercised” brain circuits associated with spatial ally require much more long-term, structural support (Jones &
reasoning during a critical period of brain development. If these Zigler, 2002).
theories were correct, parents who deprived their children of
such rich perceptual stimulation might be impairing their chil-
dren’s cognitive development, while parents and states that
supplied Mozart recordings to all infants might have noticeably
smarter children in later years. Even many years later, a cursory
Internet search turns up all sorts of products making claims
along these lines.
It might sound like a developmental psychologist’s dream to
see a policy designed and implemented in accord with develop-
mental research. This research, however, was far less convinc-
ing than its advocates claimed. If listening to classical music has
any effect on infants’ cognition, it is small and short-lived, and it
likely works through simply increasing the infant’s alertness and
attention—a property shared by lots of other stimuli, musical
FIGURE 3.29 Newborns and music. Newborns listened to
and otherwise. In fact, the claim that classical music, especially
classical music over headphones as researchers tested whether
Mozart’s, was unusually effective at enhancing intelligence was hearing music would stimulate the newborns’ cognitive develop-
undercut by enthusiastic supporters of the effect. They claimed ment. But early exposure to classical music did not lead to a
to find it even after exposing infants to sentimental popular significant increase in cognitive abilities.
Even in the context of these evolutionary similarities, fed more fruits early on show much more tolerance for sour
however, newborns’ different facial responses to tastes juices in their second year (Blossfeld et al., 2007). Above
should not be interpreted as evidence that links between and beyond experience, babies also have some genetic dis-
tastes and expressions are fixed from infancy. Young infants positions either to tolerate or to reject some bitter substances
can acquire new tastes just as adults can after experienc- such as alcohol or quinine. These dispositions interact with
ing new flavors. Thus, infants who drink a bitter form of experience and may also be reflected in some cultural dif-
synthetic formula in the first few months of life are likely ferences in diet (Mennella et al., 2005). Once again, now in
to show a normal facial expression when they drink it at the realm of taste, the complex interactions between initial
7½ months. By contrast, infants who encounter that for- genetic predispositions and the environment are essential to
mula for the first time at 7½ months show a strong bitter understanding development.
reaction (Mennella et al., 2004; see Figure 3.31). Babies’
ability to acquire new tastes from early experiences also
suggests one possible consequence of feeding infants syn-
thetic formulas. These formulas tend to have only one dis- Q: To which of the basic tastes do infants
tinct taste, while breast milk has many tastes. As a result, seem especially responsive? Why might this
formula-fed babies may become far less tolerant of alterna- preference have adaptive value?
tive tastes (Mennella et al., 2004). Similarly, babies who are
A B
FIGURE 3.31 Acquired tastes. Quite young babies can come to have acquired tastes through early exposure. Beginning when they were
2 weeks old, infants were fed either a bitter-tasting synthetic protein formula or a milk-based formula. They were both given the bitter-tasting
formula in a test procedure when they were 7 months old and had clearly different reactions. (A) The infant who had been fed the bitter-tasting
formula over 7 months accepted the bottle without a problem. (B) The infant who was encountering the bitter formula for the first time made a
face and pushed the bottle away.
Method:
1. Newborn infants (1 to 3 days old) sat on an experimenter’s lap and watched
two videos of monkey faces without sound.
2. Infants then saw each of the videos with either the sound made by the monkey
in that video (sound matched the video) or the sound made by the monkey in
the other video. The sound came from a speaker between the two videos.
3. Experimenters recorded the infants’ looking preferences when looking at the 0.8
Results: 0.6
In experiment 1, newborn infants showed no significant looking preferences for
either video without sound. But with sound, they looked longer at the video that 0.5
matched the sound they heard. In experiment 2, they heard artificial sounds that
0.4
were in synchrony with the facial expressions of one monkey and matched those
as well.
0.3
Conclusion:
0.2
From the moment of birth, infants are able to map facial expressions onto sounds
made by those expressions, even when they are nonhuman, suggesting a very
0.1
general mechanism for matching visual events to sounds.
Source study: Lewkowicz et al. (2010). 0
Experiment 1 Experiment 2
together, take much longer to develop. They often do not expectations about how objects’ properties go together and
appear until near the end of the first year of life (Bahrick, how an object perceived in one modality should seem when
1983). With more meaningful stimuli, however, such as the it is perceived in another. This initial integration ability also
mother’s face and voice, even newborns seem to engage in provides an essential foundation for learning more complex
intermodal perception. Thus, when a mother’s voice accom- relations across senses. By the time they are 6 months old,
panies her own face as opposed to the face of an unfamiliar for example, infants seem to know which kinds of noises an
female, after just a few minutes of seeing and hearing the face, aggressive-looking dog might make as opposed to a nonag-
an infant will orient more to the image in which the mother’s gressive dog (Flom et al., 2009). In some ways, newborns
face and voice match (Sai, 2005). do understand and use aspects of the intricate relationships
between touch and sight or between sight and sound, yet
they clearly cannot perceive all of these relationships. A key
question for future research is to specify the range of these
Q: When do infants first seem to be able earliest abilities more precisely and to learn how they are
to link emotional expressions in faces to the amplified and extended during early development.
corresponding emotional sounds?
From the broad range of examples given here, we see that Q: What is the value of intermodal perception
even newborns integrate information across sense modali- in newborns?
ties. Based on the information they receive, they form
STU DY AN D REVI EW
Hearing
● Development of the ability to perceive sounds has many
parallels to vision. Very young infants do not start out per- KEY TERMS
ceiving only the simplest, most basic auditory “primitives” astigmatism (p. 82) optokinetic nystagmus
and then combining them to perceive complex patterns, method (p. 81)
binocular cues (p. 87)
such as speech. Rather, infants can process complex speech
binocular disparity (p. 87) perceptual narrowing (p. 97)
sounds from the start, and their perceptual abilities become
increasingly attuned to the types of sounds they typically binocular parallax (p. 87) pictorial cues (p. 88)
hear. categorical perception (p. 84) plasticity (p. 94)
● At birth, infants also have a crude system for localizing the
convergence (p. 87) preferential looking
sounds they perceive. This rudimentary early system is later method (p. 79)
dishabituation (p. 80)
supplanted by a much more precise system in yet another sensitive period (p. 103)
instance of U-shaped developmental changes. dynamic cues (p. 86)
stereopsis (p. 87)
feedback loop (p. 77)
The Chemical Senses: Taste and Smell U-shaped developmental
Gestalt principles (p. 94)
● Young infants can clearly distinguish between various tastes
curve (p. 98)
habituation (p. 80)
and odors, but the research in these areas has been less exten- visual acuity (p. 79)
habituation method (p. 80)
sive than for vision or hearing. visual cliff (p. 90)
● Taste perception begins in the womb and may affect infants’
hue (p. 84)
visually evoked potential
preference for certain tastes after birth. Newborns and intermodal perception (VEP) method (p. 81)
infants show a clear preference for sweet substances, which (p. 109)
calm them and can have painkilling effects. motion parallax (p. 86)
A B C D E
FIGURE 4.1 Reflexes in newborns. Healthy young infants show a wide range of reflexes. Some of these reflexes appear to vanish after the
first few months of life, but in many cases they are still present in altered forms that are the result of changing anatomy. (A) Patellar reflex. (B)
Rooting reflex. (C) Grasp reflex. (D) Stepping reflex. (E) Moro reflex.
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16
Age (in months)
movements. The first report along these lines was made by extensive training before a child was “ready” had little or no
William Preyer, who, like many overeager parents, spent a effect. These sorts of findings led many researchers in the 1930s
great deal of time trying to teach his own son to walk before and 1940s to assume that motor development was largely a
his son was a year old. Unlike most parents who have made preprogrammed affair governed by maturational timetables
similar attempts, Preyer wrote up the results of his labors, and unaffected by experience.
sadly reporting that not only did his son not walk any earlier
than normal, but his final date of walking was actually a bit
later than that of most other children (Preyer, 1888).
The proponents of the maturational account of motor Q: What was the conclusion drawn from
development generally held that motor development depends Gesell’s stair climbing studies with infants?
on the increasing sophistication of the brain’s motor pro-
grams for guiding more and more complex actions. They
argued that a baby cannot walk before 1 year of age because Additional evidence for the maturational account came from
the brain structures involved in guiding motor action are observational studies of infants who showed relatively normal
still too immature to coordinate the movements that enable motor development despite their motor deprivation—that
walking. Training supposedly doesn’t have much of an is, their limited motor experience. One well-known study
effect before a certain age because it has no functional corti- examined motor development in Hopi infants who were
cal circuits to rely on. bound to a cradleboard for parts of the day, especially when
The most influential advocate of the maturational they were asleep (Dennis & Dennis, 1940; see Figure 4.5).
account was Arnold Gesell. His landmark study involved Although these infants had less motor experience than oth-
the emergence of stair climbing ability in identical twin girls ers, they showed almost no motor development delays. Sub-
(Gesell & Thompson, 1929; see Figure 4.4). At the age of sequent to those studies, researchers have found that truly
46 weeks (about 11 months), one twin began daily practice devastating environments, such as understaffed orphanages
at climbing five stairs. She made little progress for the first where infants are left lying in cribs all day (see Chapter 6),
few weeks, but at around 50 weeks she quickly became able can cause extensive delays in motor development (Dennis &
to climb the stairs, and by 52 weeks she could do it in less Najarian, 1957), but even these early impairments are typi-
than half a minute. During this same period, the other twin cally reversible. In fact, there are some indications that tightly
had no practice. She was introduced to stairs for the first swaddling infants, a practice found across an enormous range
time at the end of 52 weeks—at which point she immedi- of cultures over the course of history, may help them to sleep
ately climbed the stairs unaided, almost as rapidly as her sis- and protect them from the cold without negatively affecting
ter. Similar studies on other early motor skills showed that motor development (van Sleuwen et al., 2007).
Dynamic Systems Theory Since the 1970s, a new wave joints work, and how different parts move together. In this
of research has largely replaced the maturational account. account, the changes that occur in the shift from walking to
The new research was partly inspired by two new perspec- running involve more than just implementing a different set
tives on movement. The first of these was developed by of instructions from the brain. Instead, Bernstein argued,
Eleanor Gibson (whose visual cliff studies were discussed in as the limbs move faster, the pattern of movements used to
Chapter 3) and her husband, James Gibson. The Gibsons walk becomes unstable while the pattern of movements in
emphasized the constant interplay between perception and a typical run becomes stable. There are strong constraints
movement and the importance of this link for carrying out on the nature and organization of limb motion and on
real-world actions. Thus, they believed that perception pro- the speeds at which particular kinds of motions can stably
vides a context and a goal for an action, and the action then occur. Notice, for example, the difficulty of running very
leads to the modification of a perception. You perceive your slowly or walking very fast.
environment and then walk with the goal of maneuvering These two perspectives on movement led to a series of
through that environment, so negotiating the environment innovative experiments designed to explore how percep-
is the action to be explained, not simply walking for the sake tion and action might be interwoven into one “dynamic
of walking. Similarly, you perceive a person who is running, system,” in which each continues to affect the other, and
and whether you are fleeing or pursuing that person signifi- how the parts of the body itself physically constrain and
cantly affects your own running. The Gibsons put motor organize actions. Some developmental psychologists have
development into the larger context of related changes in therefore come to embrace what is known as dynamic
perception, attention, and the physical body, which work systems theory, the idea that the development of complex
together to support more sophisticated actions. behaviors should be understood in terms of the interactions
The second perspective that helped launch the new wave among all the changing components involved in execut-
of research arose from the work of the Russian researcher ing the behaviors, and not just as sets of instructions sent
Nikolai Bernstein (Bernstein, 1967). Bernstein suggested from the cortex and carried out by the body. Thus, we need
that our actions are complex and orderly not just because to take into account anatomy at the levels of individual
of the brain’s well-calibrated control of the body, but also muscles and bones, limbs, and the body as a whole, as well
because of the physical properties of our body parts and their as the involvement of the peripheral and central nervous
interactions, including the size of the limbs, the way the systems, and finally the broader context in which an action
A B C
FIGURE 4.7 Perceptual-motor coordination. (A) A squirrel jumping from branch to branch in a tree, (B) a frog catching a fly with its tongue,
and (C) a toddler catching a ball are all examples of perceptual-motor coordination.
Results:
C
Although there was a preponderance of right-handed reaching
in all three groups, there was also some left-handed reaching,
TABLE 1 Correlations between members of MZ and
especially for tubes in the left and middle locations. Significantly
DZ twin pairs and handedness biases
higher correlations for the use of the same hand were found
between MZ twins than between DZ twins in either same-sex
or opposite-sex groups, especially when the tube was in the All
middle position (see Table 1). Left Middle Right Positions
Figure 4.9). In reaching for stationary objects, even new- If even newborns are able to guide their limbs toward
borns seem to have a sense of where to move their hands. the objects they perceive, then simple empiricist accounts
They reach in roughly the right direction, although they appear to be in trouble. These very early abilities suggest
very rarely touch the object and are not able to easily grasp that links between eye and limb that are already present
it (Bertenthal & Clifton, 1998). at birth serve as a framework that guides learning during
While newborn infants may have difficulty grasp- perceptual-motor development (von Hofsten, 1980, 2004).
ing objects, they nonetheless can guide their arms in an The critical question concerns how those links are influ-
intended direction. In one study, researchers passed a beam enced by experience.
of light in front of infants in a darkened room. The light
source was arranged so that each infant could see the beam
only if her hand passed in front of the light, revealing a well- Q: To what extent are newborns capable of
illuminated hand (van der Meer, 1997). The infants initially guiding their limbs toward a desired object?
moved their hands into the ray of light at random, but they How does that ability change in the first few
quickly learned to adjust the direction and speed of their
hand movements to bring their hands into the light and
months of life?
keep them there. Similarly, newborns will move their arms
toward objects they find appealing, even if they cannot grab Within a perceptual-motor framework that is present
them (von Hofsten, 1984). from birth, one powerful way that human infants show
View from
video
camera 2
learning involves coming to understand that objects beyond Later, we will see that these kinds of peripheral limb-based
a certain distance are not worth reaching for. Infants seem constraints, so called because they involve bodily features
to understand this by the time they are at least 4 months rather than instructions to the body by the brain, may be
old, as they are less likely to reach for more distant objects especially prominent in learning to crawl and walk.
(Yonas & Granud, 1985). By 5 months of age, infants also
modify the shapes of their hands to fit best with the shapes
or orientations of objects that they are about to grasp (von
Hofsten & Fazel-Zandy, 1984; see Figure 4.10). The pur- Navigating Space
pose of reaching—to retrieve an object—is therefore inte- Parents are often surprised at just how fast an infant can
grated into an infant’s perception and action from an early move, especially, it seems, when they look away for a moment.
age. Another link between reaching and perception of As infants grow older, they become increasingly adept at get-
distance also occurs with hearing. By 6 months, infants ting around, usually progressing from creeping to crawling
will reach less for objects they hear (in the dark) that are to various stages of walking. During this progression, just as
too far away (Clifton et al., 1991). Even more impressively, in the development of reaching, their movements are con-
10-month-old infants will adjust how they move their
hands toward an object, depending on what they plan to
do with it. Thus, they will reach faster to grasp a ball they
want to throw than a ball they are going to stuff into a
tube. Presumably the difference arises because the stuff-
ing requires more dexterity and a more precise initial grip
(Claxton et al., 2003).
Developmental improvements in reaching clearly are
related to two kinds of changes in the brain: (1) changes
to the neural circuits that directly enable action to become
more sophisticated as the circuits mature, and (2) changes
to the brain in response to learning new forms of actions.
In addition, changes outside the brain are also important. In
some cases, the infant’s own body and posture may impose
limitations, a pattern that fits with the tenets of dynamic
systems theory. For example, 5-month-olds, who are not
able to sit on their own, also seem to be quite poor at FIGURE 4.10 Reaching in older infants. This 8-month-old has
reaching for objects. But when these infants are propped become quite adept at both anticipating the trajectory of this rapidly
up carefully with cushions, suddenly their reaching abili- moving object and also at adjusting her hand orientation and grasp
ties become more sophisticated (Rochat & Goubet, 1995). width in such a way as to make effective contact with the object.
30
(max 60 sec)
25
20
15
10
5
0
Lab 1 Home 1 Home 2 Home 3 Home 4 Lab 2
CC Visit
all respects tend to start walking several months later than by them, as assessed by having wireless eye-tracking sys-
their sighted peers. Interestingly, the blind children’s devel- tems attached to their heads as they walk and run about
opment of other motor behaviors before walking, includ- (Franchak & Adolph, 2010). We are normally not aware
ing the ability to crawl, is not nearly as delayed (Adelson & of using this visual support while walking, but it quickly
Fraiberg, 1974). This suggests that vision is even more impor- becomes obvious when we walk with our eyes closed. A
tant to walking than to other motor activities. series of studies vividly illustrated the extent to which tod-
dlers depend on information from the visual flow field to
maintain balance. In the studies, an unsuspecting new
Q: Why might blind children be delayed in walker was placed in a room and allowed to stand for a few
moments, at which point, even though the floor remained
walking? stationary, all the walls started to move in one direction as
shown in Figure 4.14. Quite remarkably, even though the
One important way in which vision helps us walk is by infant himself did not move, slow movements of the walls
providing us with information about how our body is mov- could “knock” the infants over by disrupting the coordi-
ing through the environment. When we walk at different nation between their actions and their visual information
speeds, we come to expect certain visual patterns known (Bertenthal & Bai, 1989; Lee & Aronson, 1974; Stoffregen
as visual flow fields to stream past at appropriate speeds, et al., 1987). Thus, if the wall was moving toward them,
and we use this visual feedback to adopt a well-balanced they felt as if they were moving toward the wall, tried to
posture. Vision, in particular the visual flow fields around compensate by leaning backward, and lost their balance,
us, provides us with what amounts to visual “hand rails” rather than literally being knocked over by the walls. The
that make it easier to get about in a steady and sure-footed same effect can be produced if an infant is walking instead
manner. These flow fields can work even when children of just standing still and the surrounding walls start to move
and adults do not fixate on any objects in particular and in a manner no longer concordant with the walking. (Older
just observe the overall stream of the environment passing children and adults are susceptible to these effects, too.)
Early Imitation FIGURE 4.18 Imitation by infants. Infants between 2 and 3 weeks
of age will imitate particular facial expressions (tongue protrusion,
Imitating others seems to come naturally—sometimes even mouth opening, and lip protrusion) of an adult model. Normally, the
unintentionally—whether it is imitating someone’s ges- adult must repeat the expression several times in succession with
tures or even their manner of speaking (Pardo & Fowler, clear pauses between each display. From Meltzoff & Moore (1977).
The fact that imitation is possible almost immediately to associate imitative actions with particular individuals.
after birth naturally raises questions as to its underlying In one study, infants imitated an action by a specific adult
mechanisms. Meltzoff and Moore suggest that for many whom they had seen performing that action 24 hours earlier.
human actions, including both facial expressions and hand They readily imitated the familiar model, but were much
gestures, the ability to perceive the action and the ability less likely to imitate a new model. This is quite remarkable,
to produce it might be mentally represented in the same given some claims that infants cannot recognize individual
neural code. Thus, when an infant perceives an action, the faces at that age. It also displays an impressive memory skill,
code for the related motor sequence is activated as well. which extends to several weeks in older infants (Meltzoff &
Although this specific proposal is still being investigated Moore, 1994).
(see New Directions in Developmental Research box), it is Newborns’ ability to imitate facial expressions and actions is
clear that from a very early age, infants have a strong link not uniquely human. Rather, imitation has a long evolutionary
between their perceptions and their ability to reproduce cer- history in primates as a way of interacting that tends to bring
tain kinds of actions. This has been shown in studies that individuals into similar emotional states (for example, feel-
measure brain activity while infants are reaching and while ing afraid). Thus, like human mothers, mothers of newborn
perceiving others reaching for an object (see Figure 4.19). macaques engage in making and imitating facial expressions
Early imitation is not restricted to things that infants see, with their infants (Ferrari et al., 2009a). This mother-infant
of course. They also frequently imitate sounds, in some cases facial feedback loop may help regulate their emotions so they
as early as 10 weeks of age (Kuhl & Meltzoff, 1996). More- can experience a shared emotional state and thus sense what
over, imitation is a form of social activity; 5- to 8-week-old the other is feeling. As Figure 4.20 shows, newborn macaque
infants are much more likely to imitate humans than inani- monkeys can also imitate human facial expressions, such as
mate objects (Legerstee, 1991). For example, infants are sticking out the tongue or showing a wide open mouth (Fer-
more likely to imitate an adult sticking out her tongue than rari et al., 2006). This result is striking, given that the macaque
a long red object moving out of a white tube. Meltzoff and newborns must be perceiving a representation of the human
Moore (1994, 1999) argue that the various forms of imita- face that is generalized and abstract enough that they can use
tion may also help infants recognize people, as if by imitat- it to know which of their own facial muscles to move. Other
ing the other person the infant is saying, “I know you. You studies of rhesus macaque newborns have shown that those
are the person who does this” (that is, sticks out his tongue that imitate most often also tend to develop more sophisti-
or wiggles his arm a certain way). Indeed, infants will reen- cated motor skills later, such as those involved in grasping and
act an imitated action shown by a specific model when reaching (Ferrari et al., 2009b). Mirror neurons are neurons
encountering that model in the future or when exposed to a in the brain that are activated both when performing actions
cue (for example, hearing the model) that reminds them of and when perceiving them being performed by others. They
the model (Meltzoff, 2005). Even 6-week-old infants seem are thought to support imitation and may be involved in the
T
he remarkable ability of even newborns to imitate a variety of noninvasive measures of brain activity, ranging from
the actions of others has led many researchers to electroencephalograms (EEGs; Altschuler et al., 1997) to func-
investigate the mechanisms that could support these tional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI; Grèzes et al., 2003),
surprisingly sophisticated behaviors. A recent series suggests that the same brain regions responsible for object-
of findings in neuroscience on mirror neurons has resulted in directed actions are also active when people observe similar
some provocative suggestions about the neural basis of imita- actions being performed by others. Studies involving people
tion (Lyons et al., 2006; Meltzoff & Prinz, 2002; Nakahara & with intractable epilepsy have also offered more direct confirma-
Miyashita, 2005; Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004). Some of the tion. These individuals already had deep electrodes implanted in
most intriguing findings initially came from studies of the neural regions of their cortex to help guide neurosurgeons attempting
basis of imitation in monkeys. Neuroscientists implanted elec- to treat the epilepsy. They were shown to have mirror neurons
trodes in the monkeys’ premotor cortex, the brain area involved in several locations, some of which were analogous to the brain
in guiding actions, to monitor the firing of individual neurons. areas where they have been found in monkeys (Keysers & Gaz-
They recorded the firing patterns of premotor neurons, both zola, 2010). Other studies with 9-month-old infants used a net
when monkeys performed object-directed actions (including of scalp electrodes to detect mirror neuron–like processes in
grasping, placing, and manipulating objects with their hands) the motor control regions of the brain. These areas show similar
and when they observed other monkeys performing similar electrical activity patterns when infants grasp an object them-
actions. In several cases, the same neuron fired both when a selves and when they anticipate that someone else’s hand is
monkey was performing a specific action on an object, such as about to grasp the object (Southgate et al., 2009, 2010). Thus,
grasping a block on a table, and when the monkey was watch- the motor region is activated when thinking about another’s goal
ing another monkey engaging in the same action (Gallese et al., of grasping, not just by observing actual grasping.
1996, 2004). The neurons that fired when the monkey grasped It has been argued that the sort of imitation found in humans
the block did not fire when the monkey saw the block when it and higher primates is unique and that it represents a highly
was not being grasped by a hand or when the monkey watched advanced evolutionary achievement (Rizzolatti & Craighero,
the experimenter simulate grasping the block when it was not 2004). Mimicry in other sorts of animals is argued to involve
present. Because these same neurons fired when the monkey different kinds of processing and no mirror neurons. If this
either performed or observed a particular action on an object, claim is correct, it raises the question of whether newborns’
they came to be called mirror neurons. ability to imitate might arise from the presence of functional
Interestingly, while the mirror neurons associated with a par- mirror neurons at birth. This question can only be answered
ticular object-directed action did not fire when the action was with a detailed understanding of mirror neuron function, which
performed in the absence of the object, they did fire when the can then be contrasted with other systems that might support
monkey watched a robot perform the action. This suggests that mimicry. The more precise functions of mirror neurons remain
the activity of mirror neurons is associated with the overall goal a topic of considerable debate. Besides their role in imitation
of the action rather than with the necessary limb movements (Jeannerod, 1994), they may contribute significantly to attending
(Gazzola et al., 2007; Rizzolatti & Sinigaglia, 2010). There may to the details of others’ actions (Wilson & Knoblich, 2005), to
be a common code both for perceiving and for performing many understanding the meanings of those actions (Gallese et al.,
kinds of actions. Researchers have documented what seem to be 2004), and to social understanding and empathy (Baird
different populations of mirror neurons for hand movements, for et al., 2011; Meltzoff & Decety, 2003). It remains unclear, how-
facial movements, and for eating (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004). ever, whether the activity of mirror neurons provides a basis for
It is relatively easy to document the activity of mirror neu- inferring others’ thoughts, feelings, and intentions by neurally
rons in monkeys, but it is far more difficult to do so in humans, “simulating” their actions (Saxe, 2005). Thus, imitation may be
because implanting electrodes in human brains solely for the the mirror neurons’ core purpose or only a kind of side effect
purpose of experiments is unethical. Nonetheless, evidence from of their activity.
S
ome affluent parents worry about how to give their the Power Toddler program could really provide long-lasting
children a competitive edge at the earliest age. In benefits. Based on the information in this chapter, what does
response to these kinds of concerns, enrichment pro- research on the development of action suggest about whether
grams for preschoolers, including programs to boost Power Toddler is using false advertising?
motor skills, have become commonplace in many cities and In considering this question, think back to Gesell’s classic
towns. But can these programs really deliver on their promises? studies, such as the 11-month-old twin sisters who learned to
Imagine that a nationwide chain called Power Toddler claims climb stairs. These experiments involved younger children and
that its 2-year program enhances the athletic skills of children far shorter durations of training than Power Toddler’s 2-year pro-
aged 1 to 3 years old through early practice with motor skills. gram, but their findings suggested that extra training made no
For a hefty fee, retired gymnasts, basketball players, and other difference—all that seemed to matter was maturation (Gesell &
athletes coach the toddlers for 2 hours each day, training the Thompson, 1929). Here the question is: Do findings like Gesell’s
children to balance on beams, perform simple gymnastic move- extend to later ages and longer periods of training?
ments, catch and throw balls, and do a variety of other simple Also consider how the evidence cited in Power Toddler’s
athletic activities. Power Toddler’s brochure says that perceptual- brochure relates to its promises of lifelong athletic benefits.
motor development largely depends on prior experience and While those studies and others—including the one described
that by enriching this experience for its students early on, its earlier that utilized Velcro mittens to accelerate infants’ grasping
program confers a lifelong athletic advantage. It even cites some and reaching (Libertus & Needham, 2010)—may well show
studies in which perceptual-motor skills seemed to be influenced how specific perceptual-motor experiences can improve early
by training (Boucher & Doescher, 1991; Laszlo & Sainsbury, motor skills, this is not the same as providing evidence of more
1993). The brochure goes on to refer to studies in Kenya that general, long-term benefits. Perhaps Power Toddler’s program
suggest that certain kinds of parenting practices might boost temporarily speeds up the development of perceptual-motor
children’s early motor development (Super, 1976). It even men- abilities but then other children catch up within a few months.
tions some research arising from Esther Thelen’s studies that Or perhaps Power Toddler’s students regress back to normal
shows that changes in muscles and other physical properties of levels when the program of constant practice ends.
the limbs can bring dramatic changes in motor abilities (Thelen, This imaginary case is not far from real ones, and it illustrates
1995; Vereijken & Thelen, 1997). On this basis, Power Toddler how research on motor development might influence legal deci-
argues that through early exercise, toddlers can strengthen their sions about certain advertisements. In the hypothetical case of
muscles, which will enable them to achieve impressive new lev- Power Toddler, the value of the program for physical prowess is
els of motor activity. probably quite modest. As long as young children have ample
As Power Toddler centers spring up across your state, the opportunities to engage in a variety of gross and fine motor
Consumer Protection Agency becomes concerned that the activities, it is unlikely that special programs provide any addi-
chain may be fraudulently promising parents more than it can tional and lasting athletic advantage. Participating in group activi-
deliver. The agency then turns to developmental psycholo- ties with other children may confer some social benefits, but as
gists for advice about the effects of intensive perceptual-motor a general rule, there is no evidence that toddlers who receive
instruction for children aged 1 to 3. It wants to know whether professional coaching are more likely to become skilled athletes.
Probability of EMR
air to their eyes. They soon learn
0.6
to blink when they hear the tone
alone. (A) By monitoring sleeping
infants’ eye movement responses 0.4
(EMRs) during this kind of classical
conditioning, (B) researchers have 0.2
found that sleeping infants who were
conditioned show more EMRs than
0
infants who were not conditioned.
0 1 2 3 4 5
Those who experienced the classical
Trial block
conditioning also showed more brain A B
activity suggestive of learning the
association compared to infants in
the control group. Adapted from Fifer
et al. (2010).
learn how stimuli are related; this level of learning is seen Interestingly, infants can learn from classical condition-
in classical conditioning (or Pavlovian conditioning). In the ing even when they are asleep. (It is not uncommon for
final level of learning, the organism learns about the con- newborns to sleep 18 hours a day, which would leave less
sequences of its actions, where the stimulus usually takes than 6 hours for learning if the infant needed to be wide
on the role of a positive or negative reinforcer. This level of awake to learn.) When a tone is consistently paired with an
learning is known as operant conditioning. Let us now turn air puff delivered to sleeping newborns (see Figure 4.23),
to classical and operant conditioning in more detail. they will soon show more eye movement in response to the
tone than other newborns who hear tones at random that
Classical Conditioning For over 100 years, since Pavlov’s are not paired with air puffs. In addition, only the infants
groundbreaking studies on dogs, it has been known that who experience the air puff–tone pairings show increases in
an enormous range of organisms can be classically condi- cortical activity that suggest that they are processing new
tioned. In classical conditioning, an initially neutral stim- memories (Fifer et al., 2010). The ability of infants to be
ulus that does not normally elicit a response comes to do so classically conditioned whether they are awake or asleep
through the repeated pairing of the neutral stimulus with a enables them to learn as much as possible about important
stimulus that would naturally elicit the response. To begin patterns in their world. In contrast, sleeping adults are not
with, an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) has a preexist- thought to be capable of classical conditioning. The ability
ing relationship to an unconditioned response (UCR). In to learn this way while sleeping may be a special adapta-
Pavlov’s dogs, meat powder (UCS) led to salivation (UCR). tion of infancy because of the enormous amount of time
In a young infant, a puff of air to the face (UCS) will elicit infants spend asleep. Forming associations through classical
an eyeblink (UCR) without any prior learning. If a tone conditioning is one way that infants begin adapting to their
is repeatedly paired with the puff of air, infants as young environments right from birth.
as 10 days will learn to blink (conditioned response, or
CR) when they hear the tone by itself (conditioned stimu-
lus, or CS), having learned the association between the two
stimuli through classical conditioning (Little et al., 1984). Q: What are some examples of classical
Similarly, when infants are given a sweet solution (UCS), conditioning in infants?
they will automatically show increased sucking (UCR). If
a gentle stroking of the forehead, the conditioned stimu-
lus (CS), is repeatedly paired with a sweet solution, even Early in the study of classical conditioning in infancy,
newborns will soon start to engage in anticipatory suck- J. B. Watson and his associates conducted a dramatic demon-
ing (CR) in response to stroking alone (Blass et al., 1984), stration of this form of learning (Watson & Raynor, 1920).
just as Pavlov’s dogs came to salivate (CR) at the sound of a A 9-month-old infant referred to as “Little Albert” was
bell (CS) alone after the bell had previously been repeatedly brought into the laboratory, where he eagerly approached
paired with the meat powder (UCS). furry animals, such as rabbits. The child was then shown
As
sim
Ac
ilat
co
io
m
n
m
od
at
io
n
B C
FIGURE 5.2 Equilibration and its two components: assimilation and accommodation. (A) Equilibration is seen as composed of two
complementary processes of assimilation and accommodation. With assimilation, the child interprets new and unfamiliar things in terms of exist-
ing mental structures and concepts. With accommodation, the child adjusts her behaviors and concepts to better fit the environment. (B) When
an infant first encounters a TV remote control, she may cognitively assimilate it into the more developed mental representation that she has for
a wireless phone. (C) Somewhat later, she may accommodate her mental structures and behaviors for the remote control in ways that more
closely relate to the unique properties of the remote control.
Sensorimotor period Birth–2 years Infants learn to distinguish their own bodies and actions from the external world
around them.
Preoperational period 2–7 years Children can use symbols to mentally represent objects, but they fail to use mental
operations that enable them to see the quasi-logical relations governing phenomena.
Concrete operational period 7–12 years Children can apply quasi-logical operations to concrete information, but they fail to
think abstractly or in hypothetical terms.
Formal operational period 12 years on Children are able to think logically about things that are not immediately present and
about abstract and hypothetical ideas.
TABLE 5.1 Piaget’s four periods of cognitive development. Each of Piaget’s four periods describes a qualitatively distinct way of
understanding the world. The associated ages provide rough guidelines and can vary considerably across children.
Where would Piaget fall along the nativist/empiricist sensorimotor period (Piaget, 1952b). His evidence for these
continuum? His view of infants and children as striving stages was especially personal, coming almost exclusively
to understand the world better through assimilation and from detailed observations of his own three children.
accommodation stresses an active, exploratory developmen- Piaget saw a great deal of activity going on in infants
tal process. While he clearly disagreed with the empiricist during each of the six sensorimotor stages. This overview
view of the infant as a passive vessel or blank slate, his empha- briefly characterizes each stage, focusing on the infant’s
sis on the domain generality of cognitive development and understanding of physical objects, a form of understanding
his notion that all knowledge is built up through experience that Piaget called the object concept, an idea central to his
made him an empiricist. Although he certainly was con- overall theory of the sensorimotor period. Piaget’s account of
cerned with the mind (unlike a behaviorist) and with com- the object concept focuses on object permanence—how the
plex cognitive structures (rather than the simple networks infant comes to think of objects as things that still exist even
of a classic associationist), he was nonetheless deeply at odds when they are out of sight and not being used or acted on.
with the nativists’ central tenets. Unlike the nativists, he
never proposed that the mind has any innate biases toward Stage 1 (Birth to 1 Month): Use of Reflexes Piaget
particular areas of thought. Thus, while Piaget believed in believed that the very young infant interacts with the envi-
innate schemes that work in ways that go beyond mere asso- ronment by means of relatively fixed, innate routines, or
ciations, he did not believe in the nativist view that there are reflexes, which Piaget viewed as the simplest early ver-
innate domain-specific constraints for understanding such sions of schemes. Among Piaget’s examples of these infant
areas of knowledge as spatial layout, number, or the proper- reflexes, the most commonly mentioned are those related to
ties of physical objects. To better understand the details of breastfeeding, such as the sucking reflex, which is present
Piaget’s approach to infancy, we will turn to his description from birth. In Piaget’s view, these reflexive behaviors are
of the sensorimotor period. not completely fixed, however. Rather, by interacting with
the world, infants learn—through the processes of assimila-
tion and accommodation—to modify the simple schemes
that influence their reflexive behaviors. For example, Piaget
Q: Is Piaget’s approach more compatible with repeatedly observed that his own children became increas-
nativism or empiricism? ingly proficient at finding their mother’s nipple and at
maintaining active sucking while nursing (thus modifying
the sucking reflex through accommodation). The suck-
ing reflex changed in ways that were directly related to the
object it acted on, the breast. Thus, the infant “fine-tuned”
The Sensorimotor Period in Infancy the preexisting sucking reflex to make it more optimal for
Piaget called the first 2 years of life the sensorimotor recovering milk from the breast. Similarly, one of our sons
period. As he did for each of the four periods of cogni- initially had trouble breathing while he was nursing, since
tive development, Piaget further divided the sensorimotor he would bury his face in the breast and cover up his nose
period into smaller stages that give it texture and depth. while sucking vigorously until he would come up for air. As
He proposed that there are six distinct stages within the the days passed, however, he began to show accommodation
sight. For example, if an infant playing with her favorite the infant’s own body. Although younger infants do act on
rattle accidentally dropped it between the folds of a blan- objects other than their own bodies, Piaget held that in Stage 3
ket, she would not search for it, since she would not real- they develop new schemes specifically for interacting with
ize that the rattle continued to exist when she lost sight of external objects. This is the first small step toward developing
it. For the same reason, Piaget believed that infants would limited notions of an external world. But at this point, infants
not understand that an object’s properties can change. For still recognize objects only in terms of the schemes in which
example, if an object is moving, the infant would watch it they are embedded, rather than viewing them independently.
with a “tracking scheme”; but if the object suddenly stopped Like the primary circular reactions, secondary circular
moving, the infant would continue tracking along the path reactions initially happen by chance; then, if the behavior is
the object would have taken if it had continued to move. pleasing, the infant reproduces it. The key advance in Stage 3
Piaget based this prediction on the idea that because a mov- is that the infant acts on something other than his own body
ing object and a stationary object require different schemes, (see Figure 5.5). He might shake a rattle and find the noise
the infant is unaware that the stationary object is the same interesting, then shake it again to repeat the sound. Over
one that was moving a moment before. time, he would become more efficient at making the sound,
producing a new rattle-shaking scheme. All three of Piaget’s
children showed secondary circular reactions to mobiles sus-
pended over their bassinets. Each one discovered by chance
Q: According to Piaget, how do infants in that leg movements caused the mobile to move. As they
Sensorimotor Stage 2 understand objects? sought to repeat this effect, they became increasingly efficient
at integrating schemes: moving their legs while simultane-
ously watching the mobile. With time, such schemes become
Stage 3 (4 to 9 Months): Appearance of Secondary increasingly sophisticated. For example, an infant might
Circular Reactions In Sensorimotor Stage 3, the infant fur- modify the scheme of the mobile by performing an abbrevi-
ther expands and modifies his set of schemes, most notably ated and simplified version of the full scheme as a kind of
through the process of secondary circular reactions. Sec- shorthand way of recognizing it. Thus, an infant might give
ondary circular reactions represent a major advance over the a brief perfunctory leg shake on recognizing a mobile, a leg
primary circular reactions of the previous stage because they shake that is a shortened and smaller version of the original
involve applying new schemes to external objects, not just to that actually moved the mobile and made it exciting.
1. Use of reflexes Birth–1 month The infant’s actions are mostly reflexive, but even at this stage, the infant
learns to modify actions that began as reflexes.
2. Emergence of primary 1–4 months The infant discovers by chance the application of motor schemes to self and
circular reactions modifies schemes. The first acquired adaptations appear.
3. Appearance of secondary 4–9 months The infant now applies schemes more directly to other objects and modifies
circular reactions schemes accordingly. The infant discovers procedures designed to make
interesting sights last.
4. Coordination of secondary 9–12 months The infant now links schemes together and starts to get a more objective
circular reactions sense of objects, but still interprets objects in terms of each specific scheme
and not as objects per se. The infant now applies schemes to new situations.
5. Appearance of tertiary 12–18 months The infant now uses schemes intentionally to actively explore the world, and
circular reactions schemes are now truly subordinate to objects. The infant discovers new ways
of acting on objects through active experimentation.
6. Invention of new means 18 months–2 years The child can symbolically represent objects and their relations.
through mental combinations
TABLE 5.2 Piaget’s six stages of the sensorimotor period. Piaget’s theory of six sensorimotor stages forms an essential backdrop for all
later research in infant cognitive development. His ideas motivated many studies on infant cognition, the majority of which focused on infants’
understanding of objects, and much of that work eventually challenged Piaget’s theory.
Possible
event
Q: How might a preverbal infant’s behavior be
understood either in terms of a sophisticated
internal set of theory-like beliefs or in terms of
interactions “below” the level of beliefs?
Impossible
event
A Habituation event
B Possible event
C Impossible event
FIGURE 5.13 Thinking about interactions between objects that are out of sight. (A) In the habitu-
ation event, a screen is placed in front of a track and then is raised to show that there is nothing behind it.
When a toy car is placed on the ramp, it rolls down the ramp and behind the screen until it emerges on the
other side of the screen. (B) In a possible event, a box is placed behind the track so that when a car is rolled
down the ramp, it is able to roll between the screen and the box and emerge on the other side of the screen.
(C) In an impossible event, a box is placed on top of the track, but it appears as if the car is still able to roll
to the other side of the screen. Yet even 3-month-old infants seem to understand that the car cannot pass
through a box that is on the track, and thus they dishabituate if it reappears on the opposite side of the
screen. They show much less dishabituation when the car reappears on the other side of the screen when
the box has been placed behind the path of the car. Adapted from Baillargeon (1986).
FIGURE 5.16 Early understanding of object properties. (A) A hand raises a cover to reveal a duck
and then lowers the cover over the duck and slides it to the right. When the cover is lifted on the right, the
duck has magically disappeared. (B) After lifting a cover to reveal a duck and then lowering the cover over
the duck, a hand slides the duck behind a screen that is taller than the duck, lifts the cover when it is behind
the screen (presumably leaving the duck behind), and then slides the cover out from behind the screen.
When the cover is lifted, a duck has magically appeared. Infants as young as 2½ months are surprised at
both event outcomes because of their assumptions about how objects interact with other objects. In particu-
lar, these event outcomes violate the principle of persistence. Adapted from Wang & Baillargeon (2008).
Liquid
act in the same ways on hidden objects, then we may need understand invisible displacements. Although the basis
to deflate the level of cognitive sophistication that we attri- for this difference has yet to be fully explained, primates
bute to human infants. We do not yet have a thorough, sys- may share a particular way of encoding and remembering
tematic comparison of how primates and other mammals actions that differs from the way other species encode and
perform on all the tasks previously used to assess reasoning remember actions and which, early on, causes the A-not-B
about physical objects in young human infants, but some error. Another factor that may differ across species is the
researchers have done comparative studies of related tasks. influence of intentional communication (see p. 156). While
Their results are critical to understanding what sorts of many great apes, such as gorillas and chimpanzees, are able
beliefs about the physical world guide infants’ behaviors— to understand when others direct their attention to an object
and if they should even be thought of as beliefs at all. (Pitman & Shumaker, 2009), humans may be the only spe-
Consider the A-not-B task, which requires an under- cies with a well-developed sense of when another person is
standing of object permanence. It turns out that a wide explicitly demonstrating an action (Gergely, 2011). That is,
range of species can find the hidden object in the A-not-B when a person demonstrates an action to a human infant,
task, and some, such as dogs and cats, never commit the the infant may automatically encode the action as some-
A-not-B error. Figure 5.18 shows the age at which different thing important that the other person wants to teach. That
species first pass the A-not-B task, as well as the age at which interpretation may cause human infants to fail the A-not-
gorillas and humans pass the invisible displacements (“shell B task more often than other primates, despite being their
game”) task (the other species never pass it). All the primates cognitive equals or superiors in other respects.
initially fail the A-not-B task but then later pass it. In con- Moreover, when we consider tasks like Baillargeon’s and
trast, dogs and cats always pass the A-not-B task but never Spelke’s for assessing knowledge of solidity, it seems that
Pass invisible
displacements task Gorilla
Macaque Human FIGURE 5.18 The object concept across species. Most primates
Dog
Pass A-not-B task fail the A-not-B task in early infancy and then pass it later. Members
of the ape family also later come to pass the invisible displacements
task. Dogs, cats, and some other animals always pass the A-not-B
Fail A-not-B task
task and never pass the invisible displacements task. These results
suggest that dogs, cats, and other nonprimate animals may be solv-
ing the A-not-B task in ways that don’t create a foundation for being
4 8 12 16 able to later solve the invisible displacements task. Adapted from
Age (in months) Gomez (2005).
Sequence of events 1 + 1 = 1 or 2
1. Object placed on stage 2. Screen comes up 3. Second object added 4. Hand leaves empty
FIGURE 5.22 Addition in infants. A toy mouse is placed on a stage. The mouse is then blocked by a screen, and a second toy mouse is
placed behind the screen. The screen drops to reveal either one or two toy mice. Five-month-old infants dishabituated when they saw just one
mouse display but not when they saw two mice. Adapted from Wynn (1992).
Number awareness and mathematical skills in infants Q: How is the preverbal infant’s numerical
may not all arise from the same cognitive ability. There
is growing evidence that numerical abilities depend on
ability different from that of a 5-year-old? How
several distinct cognitive systems. One striking finding does comparative research across species shed
along these lines is that the ability to estimate differences light on this question?
in quantity involves separate areas of the brain from the
areas of the brain involved in the ability to perform numer-
ical calculations. In particular, estimation heavily involves
the parietal regions of the brain, while calculations involve
more frontal regions as well as areas that are also associ- Spatial Knowledge in Infants
ated with language abilities (Cohen et al., 2000; Dehaene, Throughout this book, we have seen that infants are more
2009; Stanescu-Cosson et al., 2000). Different regions likely to be proficient at skills that are needed early on.
may be responsible for these abilities because the parietal These skills include perceiving depth, perceiving faces, cat-
regions emerged earlier in the course of evolution than the egorizing color, and tracking quantities. Another central
frontal regions, so that the abilities associated with earlier- skill involves knowing where one is. This ability requires
emerging regions may be more common across species and creating mental maps of different environments, retain-
more developmentally primitive. Thus, the estimation abil- ing them outside those environments, and being able to
ity is thought to be evolutionarily very old, early to develop use those maps later to find one’s way or to find an object.
in the infant, and common to many species, whereas the Spatial knowledge involves spatial representation—that is,
calculation ability is possibly specific to humans, later to knowing where objects are in relation to other objects or to
develop, and dependent on language (Cohen et al., 2000; the self and knowing where landmarks are so as to be able
Dehaene, 2009; Dehaene et al., 2004; Stanescu-Cosson to find objects or to navigate from one place to another. For
et al., 2000). Considerable debate remains on just how spatial knowledge to be used successfully, spatial represen-
important language is to the emergence of numerical skills tations must be abstract and flexible, going beyond simple
(Brannon, 2005; Carey, 2001; Gelman & Butterworth, storage of past action patterns.
2005; Wynn et al., 2002; Xu et al., 2005b). The ability to use spatial knowledge might seem too com-
In addition to an early estimation system, there is also plex and sophisticated for infants, and indeed a number of
evidence for an early-emerging system for processing dis- early researchers, including Piaget, proposed that children
crete small numbers. When infants compare amounts such younger than about 1 year had no such abilities. Nevertheless,
as two versus four, they may be using a distinct cognitive Piaget acknowledged that children younger than 2 could suc-
ability arising from a different brain region from that used ceed at some spatial tasks. But recall that in his view, infants’
for estimation. When electrical signals known as event- spatial skills were based entirely on sensorimotor knowledge,
related potentials (ERPs) are measured on 6-month-old which could only be used in motor schemes for performing
infants’ scalps, distinct electrical patterns suggest that large- simple actions (Piaget, 1952b, 1954; Piaget & Inhelder, 1967).
number estimations occur in different regions of the cortex One way to understand the limitations that Piaget attributed
from those associated with small-number operations such as to infants revolves around how infants encode objects in rela-
addition and subtraction (Hyde & Spelke, 2011). This more tion to themselves and to other objects.
primitive addition and subtraction ability, possibly one that Spatial relations can be encoded egocentrically or allo-
involves mentally representing objects, does not involve lan- centrically. An egocentric representation simply notes
guage and may be shared with other species. where things are relative to one’s own body. A baby might
As one leading researcher in the field put it, “Our arith- remember that a chair last appeared on her right. An allo-
metic intuition consists of a complex web of knowledge” centric representation notes where things are relative to
(Dehaene, 2009, p. 254). Several distinct components to other landmarks in the world. You might remember that
this intuition are present well before the first year of life and a chair was just below the right-hand corner of a window.
before the emergence of language, and they may function Egocentric representations might seem simpler, but they are
F
or many years, researchers have been measuring eye suggestions about what he is thinking. Eye-tracking evidence
gaze as a means of making inferences about infant suggests that when infants as young as 4 months old watch an
perception and cognition (Salapatek & Kessen, 1966). object move for a few seconds, then disappear behind a screen,
Major changes in the technology of tracking eye move- they actually visualize the unseen object moving behind the
ments have provided important new ways of conducting such screen, as shown by their anticipatory eye movements toward
studies with infants (Aslin & McMurray, 2004). Sophisticated where the object will reemerge (von Hofsten et al., 2007). Sub-
eye tracking has been performed on adults for quite some time, tle factors may influence when infants can successfully show
but eye tracking in infants presents special challenges. In many predictive eye movements that anticipate an object’s reappear-
cases, the tracking requires adults to sit still and keep their head ance, and here, too, eye tracking may help reveal how young
immobile, resting their chin on a support and holding their fore- infants’ prior experiences—for example, repeated viewing of
head against a brace. Other systems allow somewhat freer head the object as it moves along a path before the screen is put in
movements, but larger head and body movements will compro- place to hide the moving object—facilitate this ability (Johnson &
mise accuracy. In addition, calibrating most adult eye-tracking Shuwairib, 2009). By the time they are 6 months old, infants can
systems requires the participant to look at a number of spe- accurately envision the object moving behind the screen and
cific objects. Because infants can’t be told to sit still or to focus can even seem to envision one object hitting another behind a
intently on command, measuring infant eye tracking requires a screen and this second object then moving out from behind the
way of compensating for frequent head movements. screen (Woods et al., 2010).
Technological improvements and advanced software now Eye tracking can go beyond exploring infants’ understand-
make it quite easy to calibrate eye-tracking systems in preverbal ing of objects’ motions to probing their thinking about objects’
infants and to compensate for some degree of head movement. functional properties—that is, knowing what an object can be
In addition, some eye-tracking systems simply consist of hav- used for. Infants usually do not show consistent, correct motor
ing an infant wear special glasses that contain both a camera uses of simple everyday objects, such as hairbrushes, cups,
that records what she sees and infrared sensors that constantly and phones, until well after they are 12 months old. But do they
track where she is looking within that scene. The systems are encode objects’ functional uses at an earlier age, before they
often calibrated by showing the infant bright dots that become are able to act them out? To measure this early ability, we can
larger at various locations on a screen. The dots naturally attract track infants’ eye movements while they watch brief video clips
the infant’s attention, so no explicit training is needed. of actors using objects either correctly or incorrectly. One study
Eye tracking is increasingly being used to explore how infants focused on infants at ages 6, 8, 12, 14, and 16 months while
think about objects and their properties. For example, when an they watched adult actors use a brush, or a telephone, or a cup
infant sees an object go behind a barrier, eye tracking provides either correctly or incorrectly (Hunnius & Bekkering, 2010). For
biases to think about some categories—biases that can We know from the research on number that infants are
range from simply understanding a particular concept from surprised when number changes. But what about when
the start to more abstract constraints on classes of concepts. object properties change? Since infants base categories on
For example, a nativist might claim that there is a bias to their recognition of objects’ shared properties, the stability
favor functional features (an object serves as a container) of those properties is an important part of categorization.
over perceptual ones (an object is a cylinder) when both If a toy cat disappears behind a barrier and immediately
are equally salient and in competition with each other. In thereafter a toy dog appears at the other end of the bar-
Chapter 7, we will consider some special categories to which rier, adults will assume that two objects are involved because
young infants seem to have fear responses, and in Chapter 9, they believe that objects’ properties are stable and cannot
we will consider in more detail how older children come to change while moving briefly behind a barrier. Do young
understand concepts, but we will begin here with a discus- infants have similar ideas about what properties, such as
sion of some work on young infants’ abilities to think about size, shape, and color, remain stable over time (Xu, 2003;
categories. Xu & Carey, 1996)?
example, when the adult used the cup correctly, infants at all These results show that by 6 months of age, well before
ages would look “ahead” from the moment the adult grasped most infants show consistent use of such objects, infants have
the cup until it reached the adult’s mouth, anticipating correctly developed expectations about specific motor actions associ-
that the cup would soon be at the adult’s mouth. When the adult ated with these objects. These anticipations may not constitute
moved the cup to her ear instead, as if to use it as a phone, a full functional understanding, but they are at least an impor-
infants of all ages still looked expectantly at the mouth, having tant precursor. To be sure, eye-tracking systems, no matter how
trouble anticipating this atypical action even as they saw the sophisticated, are only one window on infant cognition. The
adult’s hand continue to move toward her ear (see Figure 5.24). largest benefits of the new technology will lie in how information
In contrast, adults quickly readjusted and tracked the object as gathered from eye-tracking studies converges with data from
it moved to an atypical location. other methods.
In one study that examined this question, 9-month-olds These results suggest that 9-month-olds do not realize
watched as a toy truck disappeared behind one side of a bar- that objects, like the truck and the duck, keep most of their
rier, then saw a toy duck emerge soon after from the other properties over time. Some researchers have suggested that
side before going back behind the barrier. When the bar- as children’s early language skills emerge (at about 1 year),
rier was lifted to reveal only one object, the infants did not their way of representing category knowledge changes,
dishabituate. But when the raised barrier revealed both a resulting in properties being more stably attached to objects
truck and a duck, the infants dishabituated; they appeared (Xu, 2002; Xu et al., 2005a). The labels provided by
surprised to see two objects. In another experimental con- words may help infants direct their attention to an object’s
dition, the infants saw both the truck and the duck at the properties so as to know how a category contrasts with other
same time on either side of the barrier before both disap- categories. An alternative interpretation is that younger chil-
peared behind it. In this condition, the infants seemed to dren’s memories of properties may fade more quickly in cer-
expect to see both objects when the screen was raised (Xu & tain kinds of tasks, but that with categories that have more
Carey, 1996). memorable properties or tasks that reduce the demands on
Social Agents
(People and Animals) Inanimate Things
Action–at–a–distance event
inferences about what other social beings are perceiving and FIGURE 5.29 Interpreting actions in terms of goals. After
where they are directing their attention. Beyond the directly watching a hand reach for an object (A), infants as young as 5 months
observable differences between social agents and inanimate are more surprised to see the hand move to the same location to
objects, some of the most profound differences concern social retrieve a different object (B) than they are when the hand moves to a
different location to retrieve the same object as before (C). Apparently,
agents’ intentions, goals, and beliefs. Although these distinc-
they interpret the hand as having the goal of obtaining a specific object.
tions are not measurable the way a change in trajectory is, By contrast, after seeing a stick “reach” for an object, the infant appears
adults and older children can infer them immediately—and surprised to see the stick go to a new location to retrieve the same
we often make inferences automatically about another per- object. They appear to view the stick as simply moving to a set location
son’s goals and intentions. We seem to understand imme- without having a particular goal. Adapted from Woodward (1998).
Green-box event
such as this one, they look longer at expected events. The key
finding is that they showed a clear difference in looking time Q: How do studies on infant imitation reveal
between events in which the ball approached helpers versus differences in how infants see social and
when it approached hinderers.) Remarkable as this finding is
with 12-month-olds, it was then replicated in an even more nonsocial agents and their actions?
striking way with 6-month-olds (Hamlin et al., 2007). In this
version of the task, infants watched as a display of real three-
dimensional objects either helped or hindered another object’s What is it about watching social agents’ actions that
path up a ramp. The infants were then allowed to choose triggers infants to attribute goals to them? Are human-
(indicated by reaching) which object they wanted to hold, the like features, such as a face or a hand, essential? That view,
helper or the hinderer. They consistently preferred the object although long popular, seems unlikely, given that infants
that helped. Attributing goals to social agents therefore leads will attribute goals to simple geometric shapes after watch-
infants to make further inferences about how those same ing them chasing and fleeing. In fact, just the appearance
agents will behave in the future and whether it is desirable to of self-propelled motion in any sort of object can trigger
be in close proximity to them. infants to attribute goals to the object.
By 18 months, infants understand human actions in In a study similar to one discussed earlier, in which a
very different terms from those of objects, and they become hand or a stick retrieved a teddy bear or a ball (Woodward,
increasingly able to differentiate between a true social agent 1998), when 5-month-olds saw a small block seemingly
with intentions and goals and an object that is merely behav- move on its own toward an object, they assumed that it
ing like one. For example, if infants watch someone repeat- had a goal. Much as infants in the earlier study assumed
edly attempting an action, such as putting a ring on a hook that a hand moving an object was a social agent and that
or pulling the cap off one side of a bar, and failing, they will it had a goal, the infants in the second study assumed that
not imitate the failed action they repeatedly observed but the self-propelled block was also a social agent and that it
instead will imitate the action they assume was intended would have the goal of “seeking out” and approaching the
(Meltzoff, 1995; see Figure 5.31; see also Johnson, 2000). same object, even when that object was moved to a new
By contrast, after watching an inanimate object produce the location. In contrast, if the block had a long handle that
same “failed action,” the infants do not infer a goal—they seemed to be manipulated by something out of view, the
simply reproduce the action they saw. infants inferred that it was an inanimate object and that it
B
FIGURE 5.31 Imitation of intended goal. (A) When a social agent is viewed as engaging in an action,
18-month-old infants imitate the intended action (even if the agent is not able to achieve the goal) rather
than the real action, inferring a goal to act on. In this case, the human actor is repeatedly trying to pull
the cap off a small bar. The infant imitates actually taking the cap off instead of the failure to take it off.
(B) When the same action is performed by a machine (which also is not able to pull the cap off the end of
the bar), the infant does not imitate the goal. Adapted from Meltzoff (1995).
would return repeatedly to the same location, even when a perceptual cues of apparent self-directed movement (see
different object was located there, since they assumed that also Luo, 2011). The more cues there are indicating that
an inanimate object would not have a goal (Luo & Baillar- something is a social agent, such as action at a distance,
geon, 2005; see Figure 5.32). Thus, even before their first contingent responding, and self-propelled movement, the
6 months of life, infants attribute goals based on simple easier it is for younger infants to infer that a social agent,
B Test events: approaching a novel object at the old object’s previous location
FIGURE 5.32 Context and goal attribution. When infants saw a block with a small handle as moving by itself, they attributed goals to it. Dur-
ing the familiarization event, the infants observed the block with the short handle contact the cone (and since it seemed to be self-propelled, they
perceived the cone as its goal). When the location of the cone and cylinder were switched, the infants expected the block to contact the cone at its
new location, and they dishabituated when the block seemed to have a new goal (approaching a new object—the cylinder—at the previous location
of the cone). When they saw the block being moved by an off-screen agent holding a longer handle, they no longer attributed goals to the box and
did not dishabituate when the box approached the cylinder at the previous location of the cone. Adapted from Luo & Baillargeon (2005).
D
o children’s experiences in the first 3 years of life have it, “neuroscience cannot currently tell us much about whether
an especially powerful impact on the development of we can, let alone how to, influence brain development” during
brain structures and functions that then has long-term the first few years (Bruer, 1999). Bruer has also pointed out
consequences for psychological abilities long after several reasons to be cautious about neuroscience-based claims
that period is over? Both proponents of early education and about critical learning periods: (1) Bruer says that most criti-
intervention programs and purveyors of products like “educa- cal-period effects that have been clearly demonstrated pertain
tional toys” that are said to maximize early learning experiences to quite focused abilities, such as binocular fusion of the eyes’
in infants frequently refer to neuroscientific evidence, such as the separate images or the ability to see with high acuity. He states
formation of an especially large number of synaptic connections that there is no evidence for broad, general critical-period effects
in the first few years of life, to suggest that children must develop on perception and cognition. Even the critical periods for visual
cognitive skills when these new neural circuits are forming or risk development can happen at different times, depending on the
having insufficient neural growth later on to support rapid learn- visual task involved. (2) Bruer points out that most of the pat-
ing. These changes are described as evidence for developmental terns of synaptic overexuberance and pruning in the brains of
“windows,” or critical periods, during which essential learning infants and young children may be largely determined by genes
must take place if it is to happen at all. Other discussions refer to in a way that is not influenced very much by the normal range of
the pruning of synaptic connections, which starts to occur in tod- environmental experiences, even if there may be effects in cases
dlers, arguing that this process shapes the brain’s architecture in of more extreme deprivation. (3) He argues that the detailed
particular ways. Certain forms of pruning are said to occur only in timing of major early brain developments, even in the areas of
early development, so that if a young child misses out on certain neural tissue that would be most relevant for such abilities as
early experiences, optimal pruning will not occur. executive processes in memory and reasoning, doesn’t closely
These neurodevelopmental patterns are used to argue that coincide with the time frames or critical periods considered most
early intervention programs for disadvantaged children are espe- important for psychological change (Bruer, 1999, 2002).
cially important because early childhood is a time when environ- Clearly, popular claims about development can have a huge
mental inputs sculpt the brain. Others outside of psychology, effect on public policy. To evaluate these kinds of claims, we
such as economists, have used these claims to support argu- need to have a detailed understanding of early cognitive devel-
ments for targeting most funding for disadvantaged children to opment and how its patterns relate to what is known about early
the first few years because they believe it will have the greatest brain development, including experimental studies showing how
impact at that time (Doyle et al., 2009; Heckman, 2006). experience might be related to changes in cognition and the
Accepting such claims is tempting, given the many signifi- brain. The kind of interesting, stimulating environments that Early
cant brain changes that occur during the first few years. But Head Start and similar programs promote surely do provide
even though the patterns of brain change in early childhood are experiences that are valuable for children’s cognitive develop-
remarkable and widespread, anatomical development doesn’t ment. The problem lies in the policy choices that parents and
provide supporting evidence for benefits from a specific kind of politicians make when they are convinced that the first few years
early intervention program. are all that really matters (Heckman, 2006). If all of cognitive
John Bruer, a philosopher of science with expertise in neu- development was constrained by such a powerful, fixed critical
roscience, has examined how well the neuroscience data on period, then communities would devote all of their resources and
development support the popular claims that there are critical energies to infants and toddlers, leaving little or no resources
learning periods in early development. It is especially important for the later years. When bad science causes even modest
to put these claims to the test, as they are used to justify social changes in spending along these lines, older children may suffer.
policies that allocate vast resources for enriched experiences Advances in cognitive neuroscience are shaping how we under-
in the first 3 years of life (Bruer, 1999, 2006, 2008). In short, stand cognitive development in infancy and early childhood. It is
the strong neuroscientific claims about critical learning periods important to keep these exciting findings from being distorted in
before age 3 do not hold up (with the important exception of ways that might hurt rather than help most children (Goswami,
cases in which children are severely neglected). As Bruer puts 2006; Munakata et al., 2004).
Method:
Ball order
1. Twenty-four 12-month-old infants watched either a video of a disordering
event, in which the blocks went from order to disorder, or a video of an
ordering event, in which a randomly arranged group of blocks became
neatly ordered.
2. For both kinds of events, infants saw (a) the blocks in the initial state, (b) Agent disorder
a screen in front of the blocks, and (c) the blocks in the end state after
the screen had been removed.
3. After the screen was in place, either an animated, self-moving object or
an inanimate object went behind the screen and stayed there after the
screen was removed. Agent order
Results:
Infants looked longer at an ordering event that was accompanied by an 10.0
inanimate agent than they did at a disordering event that was accompanied
by an inanimate agent. They did not show a looking time difference between
Mean looking time (in sec)
Conclusion:
By 1 year of age, infants expect only animate agents to be capable of 6.0
producing order from disorder. They expect that both animate and inanimate
entities can produce disorder from order.
4.0
Source study: Newman et al. (2010).
2.0 Disordering
event
Ordering
event
0.0
Agent Ball
12-month-olds
CONCLUSIONS 183
STU DY AN D REVIEW
● Even the youngest infants seem to know there is a world “out ent principles governing interactions, and are heavily influ-
there” that is independent of their actions. enced by inferences about goals when they believe that an
● Young infants also appear to have several interlinked expec- entity is animate.
tations about the nature of physical objects and how they ● There is increasing evidence that infants can also attribute
behave. These expectations suggest that infants may have false beliefs to social agents and use those attributions to
an intuitive theory of the mechanics of bounded physical anticipate different kinds of behaviors.
objects, but there is a great deal of uncertainty as to what it
means for an infant to have such a theory.
● Studies seem to suggest that young infants understand
THOUGHT QUESTIONS
that there is object permanence or continuity, that objects 1. While chimpanzees and other great apes often appear quite
are solid and cannot interpenetrate other objects, and that close to human infants in how they reason about physical
objects must make contact to influence other objects. mechanical relations, there appears to be a major contrast
STUDYCONCLUSIONS
AND REVIEW 185
6
Connecting with the Social World
Developing Bonds between Infants Individual Differences in
and Caregivers Attachment Style • Deprivation Studies in Nonhuman
Primates
• Studying Attachment Styles in the
Early Perspectives on Infants’ Strange Situation • Critical-Period Effects?
Bonds with Others • Causes of Different Attachment Styles Conclusions
• Psychoanalytic Approaches to Infant- • Consequences of Different Attachment
Parent Bonds Styles Summary
• Learning Theory Approaches • Cross-Cultural Differences in Attachment
• Bowlby’s Ethological Approach Styles
The Underpinnings of Attachment Effects of Early Social Deprivation
• Components of Attachment • Social Deprivation in Humans
• Bases of Social Interactions
nna Mae He was born in Memphis, Tennessee, Moving beyond the complexities of this particular case, it is
Developing Bonds between tating and often lasting effects. The first such bond is gen-
erally between parent and infant, but infants also develop
Infants and Caregivers strong bonds with others, including siblings and other
family members and caregivers. Nonetheless, much of the
Virtually all human infants, like the young of many other theory and research we will explore in this chapter involves
species, form bonds to those who take care of them. These the infant-parent bond and especially the infant-mother
bonds are difficult to measure in their own right, but they bond. The most typical sequence for developing bonds is
can be inferred through certain sets of behaviors that serve summarized in Table 6.1.
to set up and maintain proximity between human care- A newborn does not seem to have specific bonds—that
givers and their children (see Figure 6.2). Disrupting these is, bonds to particular individuals. As we have seen in ear-
bonds or preventing them from forming can lead to devas- lier chapters, this does not mean that newborns aren’t sensi-
tive to social stimuli as being somehow different, but they
show no evidence of a strong desire to be around particular
people. Even though very young infants may be able to rec-
ognize their parents through sound, smell, and sight and
may find that recognition pleasing, they do not show dis-
tress if a parent is not there. Infants’ lack of specific bonds
early on has been a cornerstone assumption of adoption
agencies for centuries. Those agencies typically would like
an adoption to occur as early in the infant’s life as possible
to avoid separation pains in both the infant and the bio-
logical parents.
Infants first show specific bonds at around 6 or 7 months.
Prior to that age, they may well prefer to interact with their
parents over others, but this is largely because the parents
know best what makes the baby happy. A socially adroit
stranger can usually be just as effective. At 6 months, infants
become more skilled at being social partners and seem to
FIGURE 6.2 The origins of attachment. Both parent and child
display behaviors that any observer would recognize as the begin-
shift from being junior partners to full partners in the social
nings of an attachment. Here, for example, a Mongolian mother coos interaction. As such, they are more sensitive to others’ social
to her very young baby. But can a newborn infant have a specific and emotional states and are more adept at influencing and
attachment to her mother? responding to those states.
Psychoanalytic Approaches
to Infant-Parent Bonds
Early in the twentieth century, Sigmund Freud changed how
we view our mental lives. Freud is best known for his theories
of mental illness and the unconscious and for launching the
discipline of psychoanalysis, but some of his most important
contributions concern the emergence of social relationships
and personality (Freud, 1905/1976, 1925/1976).
Freud’s theory of bonds between infants and parents
emerged out of a broader account of psychosexual devel-
opment, a proposed series of stages of development related
to drives, instincts, and sources of pleasure, with a partic-
ular focus on sexual desire. According to Freud, all boys
and girls progress through these stages in ways that are
intimately related to the kinds of bonds they form to oth-
ers. Freud placed an extraordinary emphasis on the unique
FIGURE 6.3 Separation distress. At 6 months of age, babies nature of the early mother-infant bond, which he believed
begin to form specific attachments to their primary caregivers and stems from breastfeeding, a source of oral gratification that
within a month or two typically show distress at separation, often becomes associated with the mother. Although Freud’s
clinging to an attachment figure who is about to depart. focus on breastfeeding has been criticized by later writers, it
attach to caregivers. He saw complex attachment behaviors a particular person by using more effective signals (such
as being built up out of specific behavioral elements, such as protesting if a particular person leaves) and by behav-
as smiling, crying, and clinging. Across species, the par- iors to maintain contact with that person (such as crawling
ticular combinations of these elements varies considerably; toward her and clinging to her). Infants use the caregiver
what species have in common is the way they integrate as a secure base for exploration.
these elements into coordinated behaviors that set up and ● Reciprocal relationships phase (18 months–2 years and
maintain attachments. One of Bowlby’s most important older). During this final phase, children are better able
contributions was to focus attention on these attachment to take into account the parent’s needs and adjust their
behaviors as an essential means of binding the caregiver behavior accordingly, such as being able to wait for the
to the infant—thereby creating attachment itself. For any parent to return without protesting. Thus, the child and
species whose young are relatively helpless early on, attach- the parent become more like partners solving a problem to
ment is a crucial way to make sure that parents take care help them get to a common goal.
of their infants. For Bowlby, attachment has the evolution-
ary function of protecting infants from threats to their But Bowlby may have underestimated how even new-
survival, ranging from predators to physical hazards to borns can quickly recognize their mother based on her smell
starvation. Across a wide variety of organisms, it is adap- and can use that information to have specific kinds of inter-
tive for vulnerable offspring to engage in behaviors that actions with her. Newborns may not really be attached, but
increase the odds of staying in proximity to parents, and it they may begin tailoring interactions with specific people
is adaptive for parents (in terms of passing on their genes) right from the start (Winberg, 2005).
to be responsive to those behaviors in ways that maintain Bowlby stressed that although attachment behaviors
proximity to the offspring. As such, caregivers become safe are crucial for creating and maintaining attachment, they
havens in situations that are threatening or frightening, should not be equated with attachment itself. He believed
thereby offering a secure base from which the young off- that careful analysis of attachment behaviors could yield
spring can explore. insights into the nature of the underlying attachment but
As noted earlier, the young infant does not appear to have that assuming a simple linkage between attachment and
a unique bond to a specific caregiver. Bowlby built on the attachment behaviors could be misleading. For example,
observations of many others by noting four phases in the he insisted that the strength of an attachment behavior
development of attachment (Bowlby, 1969; see also Ains- does not necessarily indicate the strength of the attach-
worth et al., 1978): ment itself. Some of the strongest and healthiest attach-
ments provide infants with such trust in their caregiver
● Preattachment phase (birth to 6 weeks). Infants in this that they have no need to cling to the person constantly
phase display attachment-related behaviors but do not to guarantee that they will not be separated. If a child is
seem to target them to particular individuals. anxious about whether the mother will always be there,
● Attachment-in-the-making phase (6 weeks to 6–8 months). however, he might show extremely strong attachment
During this phase, infants start to use signals such as smil- behaviors, such as clinging, crying, and following, in an
ing and crying to focus on specific people. attempt to stay near the mother at all times. The strength
● Clear-cut attachment phase (6–8 months to 18 months– of attachment behaviors can also depend on the particular
2 years). During this phase, infants more actively stay near situation, whereas the nature of the underlying attachment
Smiling at internal Smiling at tactile Smiling at external Smiling at external Smiling at faces Smile of
states stimulation visual stimuli auditory stimuli recognition
Birth 6 months
FIGURE 6.6 Stimuli that elicit smiles. In Wolff’s classic account, stimuli that elicit an infant’s smile progress from internal states to external
stimulation to social stimulation and finally to unique individuals. Today we regard the young infant as having greater sensitivity to social entities,
but Wolff’s general pattern does capture changes in smiling in the first months of life.
Smiling Infants’ smiles are among their most endearing feel that they can withstand anything their adorable bundle
behaviors. The first smile clearly meant for a specific par- throws at them.
ent brings great pleasure. But smiling does not emerge as a The smile is a classic example of an attachment behavior
mature behavior in the newborn (see Figure 6.6). Indeed, that serves a signaling function in a feedback loop between
most parents quickly realize that they are very ineffective at parent and infant (see Figure 6.7). The mother rocks the
making their newborn smile, no matter how many silly faces baby gently, causing the infant to give a small smile. This
and sounds they make. It is not that newborns are incapable
of smiling, but what makes them smile may be quite dif-
ferent than at later ages. The first researcher to study these
developmental changes systematically was Peter Wolff, who
investigated which stimuli elicit smiles most effectively
at different ages in infancy (Wolff, 1963). He found that
as infants get older, they increasingly prefer auditory and
visual social stimuli.
The infant initially appears to smile in response to her
own internal state, such as just after a feeding or after a
particularly good burp. A little later, the best stimuli to elicit Mother smiles
and rocks baby
a smile are likely to be external, primarily those involving
touch and motion, such as hugging and rocking—which
give way to other kinds of external stimuli, such as inter-
esting sights and sounds. Then, the most pleasing stimuli
become specifically social, as the infant smiles at the sound
of a person’s voice or on seeing her face. Finally, at about
Baby’s smile produces Mother’s smile and
6 months of age, the best elicitor of a smile is the face of a more smiling and rocking produces
particular known individual, producing the “smile of rec- rocking in mother larger smile in baby
ognition.” Countless parents would agree that at around
5 or 6 months, something changes: the infant starts to clearly
look into your eyes and smile at you because she knows and
likes you and not simply because some other stimulus has
made her feel good. Those endearing first smiles of recogni-
tion often seem to appear just in the nick of time for parents Baby smiles
who have been lurching through months of sleepless nights.
FIGURE 6.7 Contingent responding. The infant and the mother
In fact, this smile of recognition might serve an important often become partners in a positive-feedback loop in which smiling
adaptive function for the human infant. Just when caregiv- and rocking on the mother’s part produces more smiling in the baby,
ers feel completely overwhelmed and exhausted, the smile of which in turn produces more smiling in the mother. Through such a
recognition appears, and the invigorated parents suddenly loop, close bonds are developed and maintained between the two.
help pups learn whom to attach to. Touch of the sort nor- most richly with their caregivers through visual and auditory
mally received from a pup’s mother can set up a positive means, often several feet away from the target of affection.
bond with a smell that is experienced at the same time, By contrast, most primates other than the great apes (who
presumably a mechanism through which infant rats learn engage in direct eye contact as do humans) appear to need
to form bonds to their mother (Kojima & Alberts, 2011). more immediate physical contact to build and maintain
In human infants, too, gentle touching seems to reduce attachments. Thus, both of these systems help to develop
stress. Thus, when infants are put in a situation in which and maintain social interactions, working through different
their mothers do not respond to them, touching can reduce routes in humans and most other primates.
the infants’ typical physiological stress response to the situ-
ation (Feldman et al., 2010; Jean & Stack, 2009; Jean
et al., 2009; Moszkowski et al., 2009). Even in newborns, Q: Compare and contrast smiling and clinging
skin-to-skin contact has been shown to have an analge- in terms of their functions in setting up and
sic, or pain-reducing, effect (Gray et al., 2000). There are maintaining attachments.
also indications that human infants who sleep in physical
contact with their mothers breastfeed more often through
the night and may have lower rates of sudden infant death Cuteness Even “cuteness” seems to be a critical component
syndrome as well as lower stress levels (McKenna et al., of attachment across several species. The so-called “kewpie
2007). While this is not to suggest that this should be the doll” configuration of more rounded faces, larger eyes rela-
only sleep pattern for an infant, it is yet another indication tive to the face, and smaller noses, chins, and limbs is found
of the value of physical contact between mother and infant in the infants of an enormous range of species and is thought
(Waynforth, 2007). to evoke affection from mature members of the species. Bio-
An infant’s touch also helps to create responses in parents logical models of growth help to explain the developmen-
that strengthen the infant-parent bond. Much as infants’ tal patterns that give rise to these features, which have been
smiles lead to increases in oxytocin in their mothers, their shown to be extremely appealing to humans (Alley, 1988).
touching and stroking of the mother’s breast also increases Even 5-month-old infants prefer looking at infantilized faces
the mother’s oxytocin levels (Matthiesen et al., 2001). More- rather than looking at more mature faces (Van Duuren et al.,
over, it appears that affectionate contact between fathers 2003). Konrad Lorenz was one of the first ethologists to pro-
and infants can also cause the father’s oxytocin levels to rise pose such a pattern, as shown in Figure 6.9. He suggested
(Gordon et al., 2010). These findings suggest that there are that certain juvenile features automatically activate a kind
some common physiological pathways in both mothers and of “baby schema” in much the same way that other stimuli
fathers for forming and maintaining attachments. trigger responses.
Comparing the ways that humans and other primates use It is intriguing how often toy developers, cartoonists,
touch and facial expressions to develop early bonds illustrates and other kinds of artists greatly exaggerate baby-like fea-
both common themes and species-specific adaptations. As we tures in portraying older children to make the images more
have seen, various forms of tactile contact play an important appealing. Although the effects of cuteness have not been
role in many species, and clinging is often especially signifi- studied systematically in many species, it appears that at
cant among primates. Nonetheless, while touch remains very least some species may share the human predisposition to
important for human infants, they often come to interact respond positively toward members of their species with
A B
FIGURE 6.12 Joint attention. (A) In joint attention, a triangle is set up between two individuals and a referent in the world. The two individu-
als look at each other and at the object in a reciprocal fashion that suggests that each wants to make sure that the other knows they are refer-
ring to the same object. (B) Pointing often accompanies these interactive looking behaviors, as it focuses attention on an object in a way that
is both a social act and a communicative one. Joint attention emerges at around 9 months of age and reflects a more sophisticated way for the
infant to link to the social world.
A B C
FIGURE 6.13 The Strange Situation. In the Strange Situation, researchers assess the infant’s attachment to the mother by watching
(A) whether the infant plays with toys when the mother is in the room, (B) how the infant reacts to a stranger in the room, and (C) how the
infant reacts when the mother returns to the room after having left it for awhile.
U
ntil fairly recently, almost all attachment studies in central Africa, stay within an arm’s reach of their children
focused on mothers and infants, relegating fathers to roughly 50 percent of the time. In contrast, Kipsigis fathers,
a marginal or nonexistent role. Although the major- who are part of a pastoral culture in Kenya, spend very little
ity of attachment studies still focus on mother-infant direct time with their children until around the child’s fifth year
bonds, research on father-infant relations is providing a more (Hewlett, 1991; Hewlett & Macfarlan, 2010). More broadly,
comprehensive account of attachment and a better understand- fathers tend to be more involved in child rearing in hunter-
ing of how maternal and paternal roles often differ. gatherer cultures than in pastoral-agrarian cultures, although
Historically, fathers have been less involved in child rear- specific cultures deviate from this pattern (Gettler, 2010). This
ing than mothers (Barry & Paxson, 1971; Hewlett & Macfarlan, wide variation has led some researchers to argue that it is mis-
2010; Munroe & Munroe, 1992). The question of why this is so leading to focus on differences between mothers and fathers
has been considered extensively from an evolutionary perspec- as intrinsic and unchangeable (Lamb, 2004). Culture seems to
tive (Gettler, 2010; Munroe & Munroe, 1992). One evolution- have an enormous influence on how much time fathers spend
ary argument revolves around uncertainty about the paternity with their children and the kinds of activities that they engage
of a child—the notion that, unlike mothers, fathers can rarely in with their children.
be 100 percent sure that they are the child’s biological parent. Fathering may be associated with certain biological changes
This argument suggests that fathers are less invested in child in fathers, especially in those who spend more time with their
care because there is some doubt about whether their efforts infants and children. When they become fathers, males in many
would be promoting the survival of their own genes (Dawkins, species show a drop in testosterone. Testosterone is thought
1976). Along these lines, some have suggested that even to foster both mate seeking and competition between males
when fathers do devote time and care to their offspring, they for the best mate, while a reduction of testosterone may facili-
could be doing so to curry favor with the mother and thereby tate parenting, in that the males with lower testosterone levels
increase the odds of future mating and more offspring (Geary, may spend less time seeking mates and more time with their
2000; Gettler, 2010). Nonetheless, it is critical to remember offspring (Muller et al., 2009; Wingfield et al., 1990, 2000).
that human fathers tend to spend far more time taking care of Just being a father does not necessarily lead to sustained low
infants than the fathers in virtually any other primate species levels of testosterone; the kind of fathering matters as well. This
(Geary, 2000). was convincingly demonstrated in two groups living next to
Research suggests that the differences in how human each other in Tanzania who came from nearly identical biologi-
fathers and mothers bond with their children are primarily a cal backgrounds (Muller et al., 2009). One group, the Datoga,
consequence of dominant cultural practices rather than a result was pastoral and mostly made up of herders, whereas the other
of intrinsic differences between fathers and mothers (Lamb, group, the Hazda, consisted of foragers in the hunter-gatherer
2004; Russell & Saebel, 1997). In some cultural contexts, the tradition. Following the general correlation that we noted earlier,
differences can be very large. For example, while mothers and the Hazda fathers were much more involved parents who spent
infants sleep in the same bed across many cultures, father-infant more time than the Datoga fathers in close proximity to their
co-sleeping has never been a strong cultural tradition (Ball infants. When researchers measured testosterone levels in the
et al., 2000; Weisner, 2009). Yet, the more dramatic cross- fathers’ saliva, they found that the Hazda fathers had consis-
cultural finding is the enormous variation in the level of father tently lower levels of testosterone than did the Datoga fathers.
involvement with their children (Lamb, 2004). For example, In contrast, men in both groups who were not fathers showed
Aka fathers, who are part of a nomadic hunter-gatherer culture higher testosterone levels and no group difference. Thus, dif-
What causes those behaviors? Early on, Ainsworth and her can lead to more insecure attachments (Belsky, 2005;
colleagues proposed that some parents may have difficulty Braungart-Rieker et al., 2001).
understanding the infant’s needs (Ainsworth et al., 1978). One set of studies in this area examined mothers’ abil-
More recent research has supported the notion that par- ity to accurately perceive negative emotional states, not
ents’ problems in perceiving and responding to social cues only in their own 4-month-old infants, but also in their
partners and their own parents before the babies were born. Thus, a mother’s ability to understand when another per-
The less accurate a mother-to-be was at perceiving distress- son is truly distressed, without being insensitive or over-
related emotions, the more likely her infant was to show sensitive, is related to developing an appropriate interactive
insecure attachments at age 12 months or older (Braungart- relationship with her infant. Although the precise mecha-
Rieker et al., 2001; De Wolff & Van IJzendoorn, 1997). nisms of the parent’s influence may be complex and indirect
A B C
FIGURE 6.15 Child effects on attachment. Some infants seem to be intrinsically less happy and more sensitive to adverse events than oth-
ers. These differences in temperaments may well lead to different social interaction patterns with parents and caregivers that in turn help create
different attachment styles. Parents are likely to react differently to (A) an irritated baby, (B) a neutral baby, and (C) a happy baby.
Results:
4
Secure infants looked longer when the “mother” moved away from the
distressed “baby,” while insecure infants looked longer when the “mother” 2
moved closer.
0
Conclusion: Secure Insecure
Infants’ cognitive models of how caregivers and infants interact vary Final Responsive Unresponsive
with their attachment styles, suggesting that the models may further entrench habituation caregiver caregiver
those styles, which in turn reinforce those models. Other
studies in this set show that infants also have different models of how babies
will respond to caregivers, further supporting the idea of a feedback loop.
Source study: Johnson et al. (2010).
others’ perception that children with insecure attachment socializing might share a common cause, arising out of
styles in infancy are less desirable (Jacobson & Wille, 1986). infant temperament.
Despite all the evidence of an association between A second interpretation for the link between infants’
infants’ attachment styles and their abilities and social attachment styles and the nature of their childhood rela-
tendencies as children, these studies do not unequivocally tionships suggests that certain environmental conditions
show that attachment style differences in infancy directly can foster insecure attachments and also affect later social
cause those later abilities and social tendencies. There are patterns. For example, if a family is under extreme stress—
two alternative ways of interpreting this evidence. First, say, from marital discord or poverty—the constant pressure
some infants’ temperamental features may elicit ineffective could lead to insecure attachments that could last for several
patterns of social interaction from others. These features years. The effects could work through both children and
might hamper the typical back-and-forth rhythms that parents. Stressed parents might be less able to fully engage
help build ways for two individuals to come into better with their infants and less sensitive to their needs, leading
emotional and social synchrony. In infancy, these prob- infants to see the parents as less reliable sources of comfort
lems might lead to insecure attachments, and later they and safety. Infants in stressful environments might be poorly
might cause more strained interactions with peers and nourished or sleep deprived and less adept at communicat-
other social difficulties. In this view, children’s social dif- ing their states to their caregivers, which could lead to both
ficulties are not caused by insecure attachment in infancy; less responsiveness from the caregivers and insecure attach-
instead, insecure attachment and later problems with ment in infants and older children.
I
n many societies, the percentage of two-parent households in Some researchers argue that spending a lot of time in day
which both parents work outside the home increased greatly care, usually defined as being cared for by individuals other
in the last few decades of the twentieth century. In 1975, than the parents for 20 or more hours a week, poses some
39 percent of American mothers with children under 6 years risks for children even when the care is high quality. The most
of age worked outside the home. That percentage increased consistently negative research assessments of the effects of
continuously up to 59 percent in 1998, and in 2006 it was long hours in day care initially came from the Pennsylvania
roughly 65 percent (Coontz, 2010; Cotter et al., 2008). These Infant and Family Development Project (Belsky, 1986, 2001,
changes are coupled with other changes in Western families, 2005; Belsky & Rovine, 1987, 1988). In that project, research-
especially because grandparents and other relatives are less ers measured Strange Situation responses in 1-year-olds who
likely to live nearby and so are not available to take care of the spent large amounts of time in day care during their first year of
children when the parents go to work. In this context, day care life and found that 43 percent of the infants were classified as
can be viewed as either a liberating source of social support or insecurely attached to their mothers, as opposed to 28 percent
a necessary compromise for dealing with economic necessity. of infants who experienced little or no early day care. Other
Sometimes these views can coexist in the mind of a parent who studies in the 1980s reported similar effects (Barglow et al.,
is concerned about the impact of day care on his or her children. 1987; Gamble & Zigler, 1986).
It is important to understand that the range of day care set- These early studies treated day care as the same across
tings and programs is enormous. Day care providers differ in all of the studies and did not take into account other factors,
their philosophies, caregiver-to-child ratios, training methods such as the quality of the day care (for example, the ratio of
for staff, rates of staff turnover, and physical facilities. Given infants to caregivers and caregiver training) and the differences
this wide variety of forms of day care and given the general in the children (temperament and gender) and in their families
trend toward increasing use of some form of day care in many (for example, sensitivity and responsiveness of the parents to
cultures, the most pressing social policy questions revolve the child and stressors experienced by working parents). Some
around what kinds of day care are associated with the most researchers expressed concern that these other factors may
positive outcomes for children (see Figure 6.16). have also played a role in attachments between mothers and
their infants who were in day care (Clarke-Stewart, 1989).
Moreover, using the Strange Situation in these kinds of stud-
ies may have introduced problems similar to the ones intro-
duced in the cross-cultural studies. Children who have spent
much of their infancy in a day care center with strangers com-
ing and going might have quite different adaptations to what is
“strange” than infants who spent more time close to a parent.
Extensive day care can thus be thought of as a kind of “culture”
quite different from extensive home care, so it is important to
be cautious in interpreting these differences in Strange Situation
classifications, especially in light of later studies.
In the early 1990s, a massive U.S. study was launched to
look more systematically at how both the quality and amount of
nonmaternal child care related to many aspects of development.
That study, known as the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and
Youth Development, started with over 1,300 infants and contin-
FIGURE 6.16 Effects of day care. Many studies have
ued to study over 1,000 of those children well into adolescence
examined the effects of day care on attachment style. The most
investigated factors for links to attachment style are (1) the qual-
(NICHD, 2005). Although the results are complex and nuanced,
ity of the care itself, which can vary enormously across different several general patterns have been found consistently and rep-
day care settings, and (2) the amount of time spent in day care. licated in other countries.
and showed great distress when one of the six was absent. opportunities for social give-and-take with willing partners.
They also were quite wary of adults and took a long time Although infants can attend to the special distinctive prop-
to trust adults and show any attachments to people outside erties of some social stimuli very early on, it seems to take
their group. Even with all their hardships, however, these time and practice to learn the nuances of those stimuli that
six children seemed considerably better off than children form the foundation for specific, focused attachments.
who underwent more complete social deprivation. They Unfortunately, children still suffer from social deprivation
were less withdrawn, did not show self-mutilation, and had throughout the world, and its effects remain as devastating
relatively normal social relations among themselves except as they were in Spitz’s time. In Romania, a large number of
for being extremely “clingy” with each other. Apparently, children were abandoned to state-run orphanages during a
the opportunity to interact and bond with peers was enough period when Nicolae Ceausescu headed the country. When
to protect these children from the intense emotional prob- he fell from power in 1990, foreign researchers started to
lems observed in more fully deprived infants. This study visit the orphanages and found horrendous conditions.
suggested that infants’ need for nourishment might not be There were often more than 10 children for each caregiver,
the most important factor motivating their attachments. It making meaningful interactions impossible. Children who
also showed that the role of nurturant older individuals in were younger than 2 were left lying in their cribs without
early social development is not as absolutely critical as most stimulation for up to 20 hours each day. In recent years, one
parents might think. group of Romanian infants has been studied extensively—
Later studies on deprivation have led researchers to sug- not to document deprivation effects, but to understand how
gest that in the first few months of life, interacting with any the children responded when they were removed from their
responsive social beings may be enough to put infants on harsh environment and adopted into loving families.
a normal trajectory for attachment. Only later, after about This group of severely deprived children showed the
6 months, does the infant seem to need to form specific same tendencies toward vacant expressions and rocking
attachments to individuals. Thus, children isolated from back and forth that were seen in the deprived children in
their parents early on show few or no deficits as long as other earlier studies. As American and Canadian families began
responsive caregivers fill in during the first few months adopting these children, researchers started to ask about
of life (Schaffer & Callender, 1959; Schaffer & Emerson, the developmental outcomes and adjustment of the chil-
1964). At that age, interacting with a variety of people is dren who had been adopted. One relevant study followed
as beneficial for the infant as the same level of interaction three groups of infants in British Columbia (Mainemer et
with one person. Even if very young infants are able to rec- al., 1998). One group had been in Romanian orphanages
ognize individuals and develop specific interaction patterns for at least 8 months before adoption. A second group had
with them, they may not need such interactions with the been adopted from Romanian orphanages at an earlier age,
same individuals at an early age. Instead, they simply need in the first 4 months of life. A third group, which served as
Any observational studies on the effects of institutionaliza- Q: How has random assignment been used to
tion run the risk of confusing correlation with causation.
Thus, perhaps children who leave institutions early are
study the effects of social deprivation?
selected for adoption because they seem psychologically
healthier. Once again, researchers have tried to control for
such factors, but it is difficult to do so without an experi- Deprivation Studies
mental study using random assignment. It is notable, then,
that a series of studies that did use random assignment sup- in Nonhuman Primates
ported the earlier conclusions, reporting strong, unambig- The earliest work systematically exploring nonhuman
uous causal effects of institutionalization (Bos et al., 2010; attachments was conducted by Harry Harlow and his asso-
Nelson et al., 2007; Zeanah et al., 2009). In one study, ciates in the 1950s and 1960s (Harlow, 1958; Harlow &
researchers followed 136 infants who had all initially been Harlow, 1962, 1969; Harlow & Suomi, 1971; Harlow &
in an institution. At about 22 months of age, half of these Zimmerman, 1958). In his classic experiments, Harlow
infants were randomly assigned to home foster care and studied rhesus monkeys either in conditions that were
the other half stayed at the institution. The researchers analogous to human situations or in conditions that would
assessed all the children at around 22 months and then not be possible or ethical to study in humans. Many of his
again at 30, 42, and 54 months. Children who stayed in studies with monkeys involved controlled simulations of the
the institution showed higher than normal levels of some type of deprivation experienced by the Romanian orphans
disorders, such as high anxiety and depression, whereas chil- and by the infants Spitz had studied in the foundling home.
dren who were assigned to foster homes showed a dramatic The effects on Harlow’s monkeys were often so devastating
drop in levels of anxiety and depression, which continued that today it would be exceedingly difficult to justify fur-
to decrease toward normal levels over time (Zeanah et al., ther research that would inflict such suffering on individu-
2009). A more outward behavioral hallmark of institutional- als of any species.
ized children, random repetitive movements, remained high
in the institutionalized children, but dropped dramatically Tactile Stimulation and Physiological Needs Initially,
in children in the foster homes (Bos et al., 2010). Finally, Harlow simply wanted to know if severe deprivation had
children assigned to foster homes showed markedly better effects on monkeys analogous to those in humans, with the
cognitive outcomes than those who remained in the institu- obvious research advantage of being able to conduct true
tion (Nelson et al., 2007; Smyke et al., 2012). random-assignment experiments from birth. In several
A B
FIGURE 6.20 Bonding with the terry-cloth mother. (A) Infant monkeys in the Harlow studies seemed to form strong bonds to inanimate
wire-frame mothers wrapped in a furry terry cloth. Harry Harlow’s studies on various deprivation conditions and their effects on attachment
behavior served to strongly support the ethological perspective. (B) Harry Harlow is pictured here with one of the infant monkeys on the terry-
cloth mother.
Cloth mother
Mean hours per day
18 18
Wire mother
Cloth mother
12 Wire mother 12
6 6
0 0
1–5 6–10 11–15 16–20 21–25 1–5 6–10 11–15 16–20 21–25
A B Age (in days) Age (in days)
FIGURE 6.21 Contact comfort. (A) When monkeys are given the choice between a wire-frame “mother” and one covered in terry cloth, they
not only show a strong preference for the terry-cloth “mom,” but also tend to be less devastated by the effects of deprivation. (B) The monkeys
went to the wire mother only to feed; they went to the terry-cloth mother, whether or not she provided them with milk, for the contact comfort
she provided.
CONCLUSIONS 223
STU DY AN D REVIEW
SUMMARY ing, and merely being cute. They can also include actions,
such as following or clinging or touching.
Developing Bonds between Infants and Caregivers ● All the attachment behaviors are embedded in loops of inter-
● Human infants and the infants of other species form bonds
actions between caregivers and infants that serve to bring
to those who take care of them and behave in ways that set them into close proximity and then maintain that proximity.
● Several cognitive systems seem to undergird the ability to
up and maintain proximity to their caregivers.
● Bonds between infants and caregivers develop out of an
form attachments. One particularly important ability is a
ability to pick out and partially understand social beings, sensitivity and preference for contingent responding, with
with human infants normally developing specific bonds to parents and children in different cultures having different
particular individuals soon after the sixth or seventh month response routines.
● Social referencing, the use of others’ expressions and behav-
of life.
● Shortly after infants seem to have a bond to specific individ-
iors to make inferences about how to behave in a situation,
uals, they also show marked separation distress (also called is an important way to build bonds between infants and
separation anxiety) when one of those individuals leaves caregivers.
● Joint attention and gaze following, in which a child and
them alone or with a stranger.
another person simultaneously attend to and follow the same
object or event, are critical mechanisms for developing a set
Early Perspectives on Infants’ Bonds with Others
of shared experiences between children and caregivers, which
● Psychoanalytic views of infant bonds overemphasized the
in turn helps each partner better understand and empathize
importance of breastfeeding and the centrality of the infant’s with the other.
bond with the mother as opposed to bonds that the infant
forms with others. Freud’s theory of these bonds emerged out Individual Differences in Attachment Style
of his account of psychosexual development based on drives, ● There are striking individual differences in attachment style
instincts, and sources of pleasure. While many of Freud’s between infants and their parents that are based on how they
specific theories have not held up, he and other psychoana- relate to each other and on expectations about how they will
lytic theorists did raise enduring questions about the impor- respond to each other in particular situations. Mary Ains-
tance of early experiences, sex differences, and the relevance worth developed a method known as the Strange Situation
of an infant’s beliefs about others. for measuring differences in attachment style. Four types of
● Learning theory approaches to parent-child bonds focused
attachment styles are most commonly discussed: Type A, or
on how environmental contingencies reinforced or dis- insecure/avoidant attachment; Type B, or secure attachment;
couraged certain behaviors and how infants formed bonds Type C, or insecure/resistant (or anxious) attachment; and
to positively reinforcing stimuli. They neglected possible Type D, or disorganized attachment.
roles of infants’ mental states and beliefs and had difficulty ● There are several possible causes of different attachment
explaining how bonds endured when children are in abusive styles. These include parental behaviors, children’s effects on
families. parents, interactions between parent and child effects in vari-
● John Bowlby’s ethological approach has become the domi-
ous forms of feedback loops, and genetic influences.
nant perspective for studying how infants form bonds with ● Early attachment styles may have long-term consequences
others. It considers such bonds as part of an attachment for a child’s development. There are associations between
complex and focuses on the attachment behaviors that set early attachment styles and both social and cognitive abili-
up and maintain bonds between offspring and caregivers. It ties and tendencies in childhood and adolescence, but the
also considers analogous systems at work in many other spe- causal account is difficult to untangle. The child’s own tem-
cies and examines the similarities and differences in the ways perament may elicit certain kinds of responses in others, a
that offspring in other species attach to their caregivers. constant stressful environment may cause problems at mul-
tiple ages, and temperament and environment may interact
The Underpinnings of Attachment to amplify or reduce effects.
● Attachment behaviors ensure that infants and parents ● There are substantial cross-cultural differences in attachment
become linked together through strong affectional bonds. style as measured by the Strange Situation, but it is difficult
They can include signaling behaviors, such as smiling, cry- to know whether these differences translate directly into dif-
STUDYCONCLUSIONS
AND REVIEW 225
7
The Origins of Emotion,
Temperament, and Personality
Emotional Development Temperament and the Origins of Conclusions
• Approaches to Emotional Development Personality
Summary
• Differentiation of Emotions in Infancy • Temperament-Based Components of
• Perceiving and Thinking about Emotions Personality and Early Development
• Emotional Regulation in Infancy • Determining Differences in Temperament
• Evolutionary Preparedness and • Child-Environment Interactions and
Emotional Development Goodness of Fit
ot long ago, in a supermarket on a busy Satur- how the infant’s ability to shift attention and the tendency
Disgust Avoid contamination or illness Prevent substances from entering the body or coming into close
contact with the body.
Fear Maintain integrity of self Engage in behaviors (flight or withdrawal) that enable us to avoid
danger, monitor danger, or escape from danger.
Anger Any end state that the child wants Communicate desires or display power or dominance.
Sadness Any end state that the child wants Conserve energy by disengaging or withdrawing.
TABLE 7.1 Emotions and goals. Emotions can be thought of as ways of mobilizing ourselves to achieve certain goals. Here four basic
emotions—disgust, fear, anger, and sadness—are described in terms of the kinds of goals and actions involved when we feel each emotion.
Adapted from Saarni et al. (1998).
6 months
Fear
Anger
tive states through smiles, which can be interpreted as hap- something more like general negative emotion (Camras
piness, contentment, or satisfaction with the current state et al., 1993, 2007; Oster et al., 1992). If a newborn receives
of affairs. As we will see, very early smiles may relate most a shot from a doctor, becomes uncomfortably tangled up in
often to internal states, such as feeling full, warm, and com- bedding, or is simply hungry, a negative facial expression,
fortable. As mentioned in Chapter 6, the stimuli that elicit often accompanied by crying and postural cues to discom-
the most smiles change with development, from mostly fort, is an unmistakable sign of distress. Caregivers usually
internal cues in newborns, to general social stimulation in feel strongly compelled to respond, and in that sense, even
the first few months, to the smile of recognition of specific a general, undifferentiated negative emotional state fulfills
individuals at around 6 months. a signaling function.
Infants’ positive emotions can vary from gentle content- By age 2 months or so, general negative emotions start to
ment to exuberant joy. These emotions can be expressed differentiate into at least the categories of sadness and anger
when an infant achieves a simple form of mastery, like (Izard et al., 1995). Sometimes the same situation may elicit
shaking a rattle to make a sound, or engages intensely with different emotions in different infants. One infant who is
another person, leading to a rapt state of focused attention. frustrated may display anger and may attempt to change the
In each of these situations, the infant’s smile may show sub- environment to attain her goal, whereas another infant may
tle variations, such as an open or closed mouth and a strong show sadness and give up. As infants encounter more and
or weak raising of the cheeks (Messinger, 2002), so consis- more situations in which they have goals and desires that
tently coding each of these variations of smiles and linking are not instantly met, they come to show both sadness and
them to particular forms of happiness is a challenge for even anger responses more frequently. An infant can also oscillate
the most careful researchers. Even in adults, different forms quickly between sadness and anger in the same situation or
of happiness seem to fade gradually into each other rather may show ambiguous negative states that are hard to tell
than being sharply defined. In contrast, negative emotions apart (Camras & Shutter, 2010).
are often seen as more clearly distinct from each other in Fear, which will be discussed more extensively later
adults, but less differentiated in infants. in this chapter, does not seem to be clearly evident until
Just as it is usually obvious when an infant is in a positive around the sixth month of life, typically several months
emotional state, it is also clear when an infant experiences after sadness and anger (Izard, 2007). Infants around 6 or
negative emotions. Initially, however, it is difficult to distin- 7 months old most often show fear in response to strangers,
guish specific negative emotions, as all negative emotions in and it can be quite a shock for parents to find their infant
infancy seem to be variations of distress (Camras & Shut- suddenly terrified when someone unfamiliar drops by.
ter, 2010; Matias & Cohn, 1993). In fact, some research- Infants at this age also typically show fear when faced with
ers argue that negative emotions, such as fear, sadness, and other kinds of unknown things, such as an unusual zoo
anger, cannot truly be distinguished in newborns and very animal or even an unfamiliar toy (Bronson, 1972). They
young infants because these infants actually experience may also begin to display fear in laboratory settings, such
D E F
FIGURE 7.3 Facial expressions of basic emotions in infants. Facial coding systems have identified emotions based on infants’ facial
muscular movements and the positioning of their forehead, eyebrows, eyes, cheeks, and mouth as they are experiencing particular emo-
tions. Facial expressions are social signals to others that enable infants to communicate what they are feeling. Pictured here are infants
showing (A) joy (indicated by narrowed eyes and widened mouth with corners raised), (B) anger (indicated by brows drawn together and
lowered and wide-open square mouth), (C) sadness (indicated by brows raised and angular, narrowed eyes, downturned mouth, and pro-
truding lower lip), (D) disgust (indicated by gaping mouth and lower lip and flattened protruding tongue), (E) surprise (indicated by raised
and prominently arched brows, widened eyes, and gaping mouth in an O shape), and (F) fear (indicated by raised and straightened brows,
widened eyes with tense lower eyelids, and horizontally retracted lips).
as when they are on the visual cliff, although this usually arousal, or apprehension, which can be seen as an initial
emerges at somewhat older ages (Campos et al., 1978; Hiatt emerging divide between the positive and negative emo-
et al., 1979; see Chapter 6). tions (Nesse, 2004). From this point, the positive emo-
Why would fear appear later than anger and sadness? tions, or those based on desire, differentiate into narrower
The answer is not yet clear. One possibility is that figuring categories, such as physical pleasure and acquisitive plea-
out that a situation is potentially threatening may require sure derived from material gains, as well as love, friendship,
more complicated mental representations compared with and pride. The negative emotions, or those based on fear,
simply noticing feelings of discomfort or recognizing that a similarly differentiate into pain, sadness, grief, anger, and
goal is being blocked. Assessing the threat level that a situa- shame. Although these are not precisely the same catego-
tion poses may involve interpreting it in some detail, which ries of emotion normally identified in infants, and their
may be beyond the ability of a very young infant. order of emergence is a bit different from the developmen-
Interestingly, the developmental sequence in which the tal sequence in infants (fear may have appeared earlier than
basic emotions appear in infancy roughly corresponds to anger and sadness in evolutionary history), the broad paral-
the order in which these emotions are thought to have lel between development and evolutionary history is clear.
evolved. Thus, using the terms in Figure 7.4, the most When a developmental progression corresponds to the
primitive kind of emotion might simply be arousal that is course of evolution in this way, the common sequence is
neither positive nor negative. Even the emotions of human often explained by considering how the earliest-emerging
newborns do not seem to be this undifferentiated, but some components are necessary for the organism’s survival.
very simple organisms, such as insects, might just be expe- This kind of explanation can easily be applied to both
riencing different levels of arousal. Figure 7.4 then shows a the development of basic emotions at the individual level
split between positive arousal, or excitement, and negative and to the evolution of these emotions at the level of a
Triangle helps
1 2 3
Method:
1. In a familiarization trial, 14-month-old infants sat on their mother’s
lap and watched an actor repeatedly show either a joyous reaction
or a disgust reaction to one of two cups.
2. Infants then saw the actor with a neutral face either reach into the
cup (attended cup) the actor had looked at on prior trials or reach
into the other cup (unattended cup) he had not looked at before.
3. Experimenters recorded how long infants looked at the events when
the actor’s hand went into the attended cup and when it went into
the unattended cup.
Results:
Conclusion: 5
Even older infants seem to have difficulty inferring actions of others from 0
their emotional states and instead seem to assume that people will act on Reach into Reach into
attended cup unattended cup
things that they have previously attended to, even if they showed nega-
Type of test event seen first
tive emotions while attending. (happy condition)
20
15
10
0
Reach into Reach into
attended cup unattended cup
Type of test event seen first
Source study: Vaish & Woodward (2010). (disgust condition)
regulate positive emotions as well as negative ones. You developmental perspective, three aspects of emotional regu-
might, for example, attempt to dampen your joy at win- lation are especially important in infants: situational fac-
ning a contest when you realize that your best friend lost tors, attentional deployment, and response modification. In
and is terribly disappointed. Finally, emotional regulation addition, for younger infants, the parents play an especially
can involve external factors, such as a parent soothing an important role in regulating infants’ emotions, as infants
infant, or internal factors, such as an infant purposely shift- under 6 months have a relatively limited repertoire of ways
ing his attention away from a frightening image (Eisen- to regulate their own emotions.
berg et al., 2010; Fox & Calkins, 2003; Thompson, 1994).
These facets of emotional regulation, among others, have Situational Factors One way of regulating infants’ emo-
been explored extensively in adults (Gross, 1998). From a tions is to change the situation in which they are immersed,
E
arly interactions between parents and their children may eyebrows, which are typical expressions of anger) than did non-
have a major influence on how children interpret what abusive mothers when they were angry. Even more important,
others are feeling. Important recent research is investi- the children of abusive mothers showed corresponding deficits
gating whether growing up with abusive or emotionally in their ability to process their mother’s angry expressions, hav-
inappropriate parents influences children’s ability to perceive and ing more difficulty in sensing their mother’s anger. Moreover,
interpret the emotions of others. One study examined the abil- the children of abusive mothers were more likely to be anx-
ity of 9-year-olds to detect emotional facial expressions, com- ious, depressed, and aggressive than were the children of
paring the emotional sensitivity of children of abusive parents nonabusive mothers, possibly because of the greater uncer-
with that of children whose parents were not abusive. To test tainty about their mother’s emotional state. Other scholars
the children’s sensitivity to even the earliest traces of particular have likewise found relationships between mothers who are
expressions, the researchers showed them a digitally altered chronically depressed and their children’s abilities to identify
series of images of the same face, with the images gradually and describe the emotions of others (Raikes & Thompson,
transforming from a face with a neutral expression to a face with 2006). Measures of children’s neural activity associated with
a strong emotion, such as extreme anger (Pollak et al., 2009). interpreting emotion also show differences related to their
When viewing the photo series that progressed from neutral early experiences. In one study, infants and toddlers who
to angry faces (see Figure 7.6), children of abusive parents had been institutionalized in orphanages in Romania viewed
identified anger at an earlier point in the series than the children emotional faces while researchers monitored the electrophysi-
whose parents were not abusive. But the abused children and ological activity in their brains. Compared with children who
those in the control group were equally sensitive to sad, fear- grew up in normal homes, they showed much less response
ful, happy, and surprised expressions. These findings suggest in a component of electrical waves called P1 (Moulson et al.,
that abused children are especially prone to identifying ambigu- 2009). It also appeared that leaving the institution and spend-
ous expressions as angry ones and therefore are more likely to ing time in foster care could gradually lessen these effects (see
interpret other people’s expressions as being angry in real-life Chapter 6). Several researchers are now documenting a diverse
situations. set of such biomarkers (ranging from electrophysiological to
In another study, researchers looked at the kinds of facial hormonal measures) that are associated with particular social
and vocal emotional expressions produced by mothers who experiences with caregivers and the ability to process emotions
were either abusive or nonabusive (Shackman et al., 2010). (Strang et al., 2012).
When researchers rated their expressions, the abusive mothers As in so many other cases, it is difficult to be sure if abusive
showed facial expressions that were less typical of anger (for or depressed parents actually cause the changes in how chil-
example, showing less intensity in lowering and contracting their dren perceive and think about emotions. An alternative expla-
contrast the emotional expressions of children who grow general suppression of negative emotions, may begin to
up in agrarian farming families in the Nso culture in be instilled during infancy in many cultures, the more
northwest Cameroon with those of urban, middle-class nuanced ways of following cultural display rules in
children growing up in northern Germany. In the Nso specific contexts continue to develop well into middle
culture, children are socialized early to suppress any signs childhood.
of negative emotions. By comparison, German children Thus, well before the first year of life, situational selec-
are socialized to express emotions readily, especially tion, attentional deployment, and response modification
positive emotions that may be seen as markers of their each play a role in regulating infants’ emotions. All three
autonomy and individuality (Keller & Otto, 2009). Yet, processes are present early on, and they become more
while some forms of response modification, such as a sophisticated in the months and years that follow. In addi-
1
0.8 Fear
Judges (%)
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
1
Sadness
0.8
Judges (%)
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
1
Happiness
0.8
Judges (%)
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
1
Surprise
Judges (%)
0.8
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
1
Anger
0.8
Judges (%)
0.6
0.4
0.2 Control
Abused
0
0 20 40 60 80 100
Image (%)
A B
FIGURE 7.6 Abused children and emotion perception. (A) The faces shown here range from neutral to more and more emotional for
fear, sadness, happiness, surprise, and anger. (B) The accompanying graphs show when control children and abused children recognized
emotional expressions. Abused children tended to see anger earlier, as expressions varied from neutral to angry faces (bottom panel), but
they showed no higher sensitivity for fearful, sad, happy, or surprised expressions (top four panels). Adapted from Pollak et al. (2009).
tion, these processes interact from a very early age. An or reappraisal (Gross, 1998), which consists of rethinking
infant may modify a response in a way that also deploys a situation to interpret its negative aspects in less upset-
her attention in a different manner and that consequently ting or more positive ways. There is not yet any evidence
modifies her situation. As each of these processes becomes that infants are able to use this kind of cognitive reframing,
more sophisticated and interacts with the others, they col- but such a process may yet be shown to have precursors in
lectively offer many ways for cultures to exert their own infancy. Interestingly, the neural circuits involved in distrac-
influences and create culturally unique patterns of emo- tion and reappraisal during emotional regulation have several
tional modulation that go beyond these early forms. Finally, distinct components, which may help explain why these two
as children grow older, they develop new emotional regula- forms of regulation have different developmental trajectories
tion processes. One example involves cognitive reframing (McRae et al., 2010).
S
ome young children seem to lose control of their emo- difficult time regulating their emotions seem to be more likely
tions whenever they get upset, spiraling into fits of than other children to experience behavioral problems later in
despair, anger, or both. These children are not just life, including having greater anxiety, depression, and difficulty
touchy or easily distressed; instead, their emotions interacting with peers.
seem to run away from them. Nonetheless, not all children with emotional regulation prob-
Some children who have difficulty regulating their emotions lems go on to have these difficulties. Rather, the level of stress
seem to have problems in the domain of emotional intelligence. in the child’s environment seems to influence the outcome. We
That is, they have difficulty inferring others’ emotional states have seen this kind of interaction between a child’s own ten-
from their expressions and behavior, and they do not seem to dencies and his environment in other domains of development
understand how specific situations are likely to lead to certain as well; this is another instance in which a group of infants or
emotions (Salovey & Sluyter, 1997; see also Chapter 11). These children are not predetermined to have a particular kind of prob-
difficulties also seem to extend to their perceptions of them- lem, but are at risk for doing so if they also have certain kinds of
selves. They have difficulty recognizing and understanding their experiences. For example, one study compared two groups of
own emotions (Shields et al., 2001). As a result, they may have children who were determined to be genetically at risk for emo-
trouble keeping their feelings in check as a highly emotional sit- tional regulation problems but had experienced different kinds
uation starts to ramp up. A child with low emotional intelligence of parenting styles. One group of at-risk children who had a
may fail to notice his own emotional response early enough to short form of a serotonin-related gene (see Chapter 6) grew up
regulate it effectively. with parents who were not fully responsive and sensitive to their
Difficulties with emotional regulation can begin to cause needs, and these children did indeed develop poor emotional
other problems for these children quite early. Thus, young chil- regulation skills (Kochanska et al., 2009b). The other group
dren who have trouble keeping their own emotions in check or of at-risk children had the same genetic allele associated with
understanding their own emotions and those of others tend to emotional regulation problems, but the children in this second
have more difficulty than their peers adjusting to the first year group had more responsive parents. Interestingly, the children
of preschool (Shields et al., 2001). Despite the challenge of in this second group did not develop any more emotional prob-
precisely defining and measuring emotional regulation problems lems than children who showed no genetic risks.
(Cole et al., 2004; Eisenberg & Spinrad, 2004), research sug- Much as in our discussion of parenting styles and attachment
gests that toddlers and young children who have an unusually in Chapter 6, we should not assume that parenting styles directly
objects such as flowers, suggesting an early-emerging bias where snakes are presented so briefly that we are not even
to monitor certain kinds of potential threats (Rakison & aware of them (Mineka & Ohman, 2002b; Ohman &
Derringer, 2008). Finally, at least one study suggests that Mineka, 2003). These patterns of fear conditioning follow
female infants may be more predisposed than male infants some of the usual laws of associative learning, but they
to associate fear with snakes and spiders, a pattern that is also go beyond them in ways that reflect strong preparedness
also found in adults (Rakison, 2009) and that raises ques- effects (Mineka & Ohman, 2002a). In short, many spe-
tions about origins of sex differences in some phobias. cies seem to share evolutionarily ancient predispositions
The evolutionary roots of phobias serve to remind us to develop fears of snakes, spiders, and even heights and
that although the same situation can elicit different emo- other categories of stimuli (Barrett, 2005).
tions in different people, the relation between situations
and emotions is not arbitrary (Ohman & Mineka, 2001). Preparedness and the Development of Disgust We
We easily learn to fear potentially dangerous natural stim- have seen that some fears are rooted in prepared learning
uli like snakes, and we find it hard to extinguish those and do not depend purely on experience. Is the development
fears. Moreover, we can even acquire those fears in cases of other emotions also influenced by adaptive prepared-
ness? Disgust may well be another emotion that occurs to a disgusting substance), even when there is no way the
in response to particular stimuli within the constraints of physical connection could contaminate the food. Similarly,
prepared learning. This idea dates back to Charles Darwin if an object has previously come into contact with offensive
(1872/1975), who noted that disgust seems to consist of the material, such as feces, people often react to that object with
specific emotion and facial expression that relate to poten- disgust, as if it has been contaminated, even if there is no
tially ingesting something offensive (see Figure 7.11). Emo- rational way that any contamination could survive.
tional responses that are universal to humans are the ones Although disgust, like some fears, shows evidence of
most likely to have roots in evolutionarily prepared reac- preparedness, its development shows different patterns of
tions. In the case of disgust, prepared reactions seem most change and cross-cultural variability than we saw with pre-
likely in response to ingesting either living things or their pared fears. For example, disgust toward feces appears to
by-products (hair, body excretions, and the like). In addi- be universal to adults in all cultures (Rozin et al., 1986,
tion, there is a universal bias toward reactions of disgust 2000, 2008), but it is not usually present in toddlers in any
to food that is physically connected with something offen- culture, at least not until the children are toilet trained.
sive (for example, a long string connecting a plate of food Instead, it typically develops sometime between 3 and
2.5
Camera
Average looking time (in sec)
2.3
2.0
1.8
1.5
1.3
1.0
Snake Nonsnake
Fearful Happy
A B
FIGURE 7.10 Learning to associate snakes with fear. (A) In this experiment, infants sit on their mother’s lap and watch videos showing
hippos and snakes accompanied by fearful or happy voices. (B) Infants as young as 7 months will look longer at a brief video of a snake when
they hear a fearful voice, but not when they hear a happy voice. They look equally long at brief videos of hippos when they hear a happy voice
as when they hear a fearful voice (the nonsnake differences shown are not statistically significant). Adapted from DeLoache & LoBue (2009).
types of immoral actions; and choice of sexual partners We have seen that certain kinds of emotions seem to be
and behaviors. One major remaining challenge is to under- linked, quite possibly through natural selection, to particu-
stand how and why people seem to develop a common lar kinds of stimuli and to certain response routines. Over
emotional reaction to offenses within these particular and above these deeply rooted linkages, emotional develop-
diverse domains. Other questions that are still being inves- ment involves constant refinement, fine-tuning, and exten-
tigated have to do with the reasons women tend to react sion of prepared emotions to broader classes of situations,
more strongly than men to most disgust-provoking stimuli resulting in more sophisticated and nuanced responses.
(Björklund & Hursti, 2004; Haidt et al., 1994; Marzillier & Ultimately, our understanding of these prepared emotional
Davey, 2004, 2005). systems needs to be integrated into broader accounts of how
all emotions and our ways of regulating them develop. In
addition, such an integration may bring new developmental
insights into the close relationship between emotions and
Q: How might irrational thought related to personality.
disgust be evidence for disgust as a biologically
prepared emotion?
FIGURE 7.13 Linking infant temperament to personality. Linking measures of infant temperament to later personality
measures is difficult. This figure illustrates one proposal about how such linkages might be organized, where black lines repre-
sent positive correlations and dashed red lines represent negative correlations. The multiple effects and possible negative and
positive influences help illustrate why it is so difficult to translate infant measures into adult ones. Adapted from Caspi (1998).
Conclusions
While the focus on the early development of emotions and
personalities may seem very different from the discussion in
previous chapters of how we perceive and think about the
world, strikingly similar themes emerge here as in earlier
chapters. Research on the development of emotions, tem-
Subjective pain perament, and personality is undergoing dramatic change.
Infants are increasingly viewed as active seekers and cre-
ators, not only of their environments, but also of their own
emotions, temperaments, and personalities. Rather than
simply responding emotionally to a particular event or hav-
Irritability ing features of temperament and personality impressed on
them from the outside, they are constantly interpreting the
world and using those interpretations to act on it. These
Increases subjective pain interpretations and actions give rise to emotions and also
Parental empathy and contribute to aspects of temperament and personality.
Reduces subjective pain
positive interactions
Emotions also have an adaptive value that allows infants
FIGURE 7.14 Interaction patterns. Two different parents might
have dramatically different interactions with an infant who has an ear-
to regulate their own responses and their interactions with
ache. A parent who finds the infant’s irritability aversive may withdraw others. Along these lines, different personalities and temper-
and react negatively, exacerbating the subjective pain of the earache, aments have different advantages and drawbacks, depending
which increases the infant’s irritability, causing even more negative on the environment. This has fostered the idea of goodness
reactions from the parent in a downward spiral. A different parent of fit—namely, that individual infants and children may
might react in a manner that reduces (dotted red line) the infant’s flourish optimally in quite different environments. There
subjective pain and the level of irritability. is no such thing as a standard, ideal environment in which
to raise children. The adaptive orientation has also led to
a shift toward describing infants’ behaviors, thoughts, and
“fiery” and “spirited” disposition. In countless subtle but emotions in terms of their goals and how they perceive their
important ways, she helped to channel these aspects of her ability to pursue those goals in specific situations, which is
son’s temperament into behaviors that were inspirational a key part of both the functional and evolutionary perspec-
and exciting rather than antagonistic—and that boy is now tives on emotions.
a successful, charismatic adult. The kinds of behavior that Emphasizing the adaptive value of early emotions and
caused such problems early on were turned into passion, traits also leads developmental researchers to consider the
enthusiasm, and, as his mother always saw it, “fiery” and extent to which emotions and temperamental styles might
“spirited” behavior. be targets of natural selection, preparing humans and other
CONCLUSIONS 255
species to link specific emotions to certain kinds of stimuli and attentional factors can help to explain why many facets
or situations. Similarly, some dimensions of temperament, of an individual’s early temperament do not translate directly
such as inhibition and wariness, may help infants learn the into later personality traits but instead interact with many
subtle tradeoffs between being cautious and exploring new developing cognitive and attentional skills that affect the
opportunities. individual’s developing personality.
There seem to be more and more suggestions that cogni- Finally, an understanding of how emotions and tempera-
tive and attentional factors play a central role in evoking and ment develop early in life offers the exciting possibility of
possibly even forming many emotions as well as qualities of uncovering relationships between various components of
temperament and personality. Along these lines, developmen- emotions and personality in adults. Developmental studies
tal changes in cognition and attention may help to explain can demonstrate how cognitive and attentional strategies
many aspects of the development of emotion, temperament, can powerfully influence emotions, and they may help us to
and personality. Exploring these cognitive and attentional understand how sophisticated processes of emotional regu-
contributions may bring insights into the distinctly human lation, such as reframing and reappraisal, work in adults.
aspects of temperament and personality, as opposed to those Similarly, developmental research that reveals the order in
shared across many species. They can also help us to under- which particular emotions emerge can also suggest what
stand how the more complex emotions might emerge from kinds of situations elicit the later- versus earlier-emerging
the basic ones present in early infancy. Moreover, cognitive emotions in adults.
STU DY AN D REVI EW
of linguistic form
a minority of utterances, it is at least a clear rule. The dis-
covery of this rule apparently suggests a broader pattern of
use to the children than is seen in adult usage. Only with
time and new insights into exceptions to the rule are the
18 months 54 months
irregular forms once again mastered. The extent of the dip
FIGURE 8.4 An idealized U-shaped developmental curve. These
in this curve is often exaggerated, but even a small dip
are some of the most intriguing patterns found in the developing illustrates the classic developmental pattern.
child. They are often interpreted as reflecting the emergence of a When children as a group show a drop in performance,
new cognitive structure or a new rule that, in its initial stages, is used it is often taken as a strong sign of a developmental reorga-
too crudely, and then becomes more carefully tuned. In the case of nization and the emergence of a new psychological system.
past tense forms, the emergence of a general “past = verb + ed” We saw several clear cases in perceptual development; use
rule was thought to be initially overused with some irregular verbs of the past tense seems to be a case in language. This effect
and then gradually narrowed down to the right set. Later approaches
can occur in several other areas of language acquisition as
tried to argue that U-shaped patterns could be modeled in distributed
networks in which no specific rules were represented. Adapted from
well. The pattern is so compellingly explained by the notion
Marcus et al. (1992) and Marcus (1993). of discovery and initial overuse of a rule that it is difficult
to imagine that the children are not mentally representing
abstract categories such as verb and past in a rule-like man-
Figure 8.4 shows an idealized version of this pattern, which ner. As we will see later, however, this interpretation has
holds true for other forms as well, such as regularizing been challenged by theories of learning that try to avoid any
irregular plurals (for example, “foots,” “sheeps”; see Marcus, representations of abstract rules.
1995). This kind of U-shaped developmental curve is one of
the most intriguing and classic findings in developmental
psychology (see Chapter 3). Such patterns have launched
many debates about the underlying changes occurring in
Q: What have U-shaped developmental
the child’s mind (Strauss, 1982). patterns traditionally been assumed to
To understand this pattern, keep a few points in mind. represent?
First, the input does not change radically when children
start to overregularize. Children of all ages are hearing
essentially the same distributions of verbs and tenses. Sec-
ond, irregular verbs are among the most frequently used
in English, with the consequence that the majority of past Child-Directed Speech
tense verbs heard by children in normal speech are irregular Adults do not talk to young children in the same way
ones with idiosyncratic past tense forms. When they start to they speak to older children or adults. This special type of
overregularize the past tense “-ed” rule, they are therefore speech, which was previously called motherese (Newport
taking a pattern observed in a minority of instances and et al., 1977) and is now known as child-directed speech
applying it to a broader range of cases. Third, parents rarely or infant-directed speech, differs from other speech in
provide feedback to children about the correctness of such many ways. When talking to young children, parents tend
forms. They may laugh from time to time at the mistakes, to enunciate their words very clearly and to speak more
but parents are generally unlikely to correct ungrammati- slowly and at a higher pitch. These differences tend to
cal early utterances (Braine, 1971; Brown & Hanlon, 1970). accentuate the boundaries between individual words and
Fourth, children usually have never heard these incorrect between phrases. Child-directed speech also uses simpler
overregularizations in the speech of others, so they are pro- words and fewer words per sentence, and it tends to be
ducing novel, incorrect forms on their own. more grammatically correct (Snow & Ferguson, 1977; see
Given these considerations, this particular U-shaped also Golinkoff & Alioto, 1995). When speaking to chil-
curve has been said to represent children’s discovery of dren, parents more often repeat and expand on the child’s
the past tense “-ed” rule. Younger children may only be previous utterance, or respond by recasting, in which they
remembering individual verbs and their different forms paraphrase what the child has said (Bohannon & Stanow-
on a case-by-case basis. Children might hear verbs such as icz, 1988). In recasting, adults are unlikely to directly cor-
“go” and “went” and store them separately in their mental rect a child’s utterance by saying it is wrong, but will use
dictionary (Ullman, 1999; Ullman et al., 2005), perhaps the correct form of a verb or noun in their responses. In
even noting that “went” is used more in situations talk- general, child-directed speech is modified in many ways
ing about the past. As they grow older, however, children that highlight critical elements in the language.
Theories of Language
Acquisition
Over the years, there have been many competing models
offering starkly different accounts of how children acquire
language. Perhaps the most intuitively appealing model is
that children simply imitate the language that they hear. As
we saw in Chapter 4, even very young infants are capable
of imitation. But there are two reasons why language is not
simply learned through imitation. First, if what we mean
by imitation is reproducing bits of what we hear, as a par-
rot does, then imitation clearly will not work as a model.
Children are not at all like parrots or other animal mimics.
If they were, they would not show the common problem of
overregularization, and they would not be able to produce
sentences they had not already heard. In fact, they would
not be able to produce novel sentences at all if they simply
stored parts of utterances and played back the most appro-
priate ones later.
FIGURE 8.5 Child-directed speech. Parents adjust their speech in
Second, if we suggest that imitation is more flexible and
many ways when speaking to young children. These adjustments may that rather than imitating specific strings of words, infants
help make some aspects of language structure more apparent to the imitate intentions, goals, and perhaps even rules, this expla-
child, but they do not explain how the child acquires language. nation adds little to a model of language acquisition. If the
Method:
1. Seventy-two 19-month-olds were given practice watching videos
with background narrations and choosing videos in response to
questions.
2. In a test condition, children saw side-by-side videos in which an
actor performed a novel action while another person stood by
(intransitive action) or in which an actor performed some action
on another actor (transitive action).
3. While looking at the videos, children heard either a two-noun
sentence (“She is gorping her”), a one-noun sentence (“She is
gorping”), or a neutral sentence (“Look here”).
to the two-participant event
Proportion of looking time
Results:
Infants looked longer at the transitive video after hearing a sentence
with two nouns. They had no strong bias to prefer either
video in the neutral condition without nouns.
Conclusion:
Well before 2 years of age, infants will use the presence of one versus
Transitive Intransitive Neutral
two syntactic categories (nouns) in a sentence to infer the meanings of
novel verbs.
Pragmatic Constraints Along with perceptual and con- FIGURE 8.11 Using the speaker’s gaze. Children younger than
ceptual constraints, the goals and beliefs attributed to a 18 months old know that the direction in which a speaker is looking
provides information about what she is speaking about. In this study,
speaker may guide a child’s understanding of what a word
the experimenter used a mirror to covertly watch where the child was
means. Such pragmatic constraints on word meaning come looking while the experimenter looked into a bucket and declared
into play when the child infers what the speaker is refer- “It’s a blicket.” Even though the child did not know she was being
ring to, thereby limiting where the child directs his atten- observed, she checked the direction of the speaker’s gaze to decide
tion (Tomasello, 2000). One pragmatic constraint, known what the person was calling a “blicket.”
as mutual exclusivity, refers to the assumption that each
object in a language has only one label (Clark, 1988; Mark-
man, 1989; Markman & Hutchinson, 1984; Markman & children as young as 2 years old tended to choose the
Wachtel, 1988; Merriman & Bowman, 1989). That is, if object whose name they did not already know. They appar-
something is called a “hammer,” then it is unlikely to be ently assumed that each object had only one label, so that
called by another name as well. To study how children use the new label did not refer to an object whose name they
this constraint, researchers presented children with two already knew.
objects—one that they were already able to name and one Other kinds of pragmatic cues also help children nar-
that they could not yet label (see Figure 8.10). While look- row down possible word meanings. For example, where
ing at both objects, the experimenter asked the child for parents look and the emotions they show influence how
an object by using an unfamiliar label. In this scenario, children interpret the things parents say (see Chapters 6
and 7). A series of elegant experiments examined how
children as young as 1 year use the direction of an adult’s
gaze to help them understand what is being talked about
(Baldwin, 1991, 1993). In one setup, at the same time that
Key =
Magnifier the child was looking at a toy, the experimenter would look
Key = Magnifier into a bucket and utter “It’s a blicket” (see Figure 8.11).
The child would often look away from the toy to focus on
Magnifier =
the adult to see where his gaze was directed. Later, when the
child was shown both the toy she had been looking at and
the object that was in the bucket and asked to “point to the
blicket,” she would point to the object that had been in the
bucket. These results argue strongly against the notion that
children merely associate the words they hear with what-
ever they are looking at. Instead, even the youngest children
took into account where the adult was looking. Children
also rely on a speaker’s mental and emotional state. Thus,
if a speaker says, “Let’s find the blicket” and frowns while
FIGURE 8.10 Mutual exclusivity. This bias associates new words
with objects that do not already have labels. If the child is con-
looking at and touching two objects, but smiles while
fronted with two objects and knows the name of only one (“key”), looking at and touching a third object, a 2-year-old will
she will assume that a new name (“magnifier”) applies to the other assume that the third object is the “blicket” (Tomasello &
object. Barton, 1994).
The Nature of Constraints on Word Meanings We Overextensions and Underextensions The studies
have seen several examples of constraints on word meanings. of developmental changes in children’s understanding of
Although the nature of these constraints remains controver- word meanings have focused on two common patterns
sial, it appears that they may reflect broad cognitive biases that children show as they acquire new words. The first of
that extend beyond the process of word learning (Bloom, these tendencies, called overextension, involves applying
2000). Consider the whole-object bias, for example—children’s a word too broadly. For example, a toddler who calls any
strong preference to attach new words to whole objects rather large, four-legged animal a “cow” has extended this label
than to objects’ parts or properties (Woodward & Markman, far beyond its true boundaries. A child might happily use
1998). This same bias is also involved in tasks that have this label for horses, pigs, and large dogs, and on a trip to
nothing to do with word learning (Bloom, 2000). In count- the zoo might call zebras, elephants, and antelopes “cows”
ing, for example, children show a strong bias to count whole as well. This pattern is especially apparent in observational
objects as opposed to objects’ properties (Dehaene, 1997). studies in which caregivers or other observers write down
Like words, numbers are typically associated with whole children’s notable utterances each day. Those records often
objects. document overextensions because they are obvious and often
The mutual exclusivity bias may also have a much amusing—as when a toddler starts calling all men “Daddy.”
broader basis. We do tend to think that if one object already On the other hand, the related pattern of underextension,
has a label, then a new label must apply to something else. in which a child incorrectly applies a word too narrowly, is
But more broadly, when we are engaged in conversation, we much less noticeable, though also very common (Anglin,
assume that the speaker will indicate shifts in topic. Thus, 1970). A child who underextends the word “doggie” by
if the speaker introduces a new label rather than using a applying it only to dogs that look like the family pet might
familiar one, we tend to assume that the label must apply to say “doggie!” when he sees a Labrador retriever or another
an object not yet named and that the new label is serving as kind of large dog but might say nothing in the presence
a topic-shifting device (Clark, 1995). of small dogs (see Figure 8.12). This underextension would
Children could not learn word meanings without power- probably go undetected by his parents and others.
ful ways of limiting the number of possible meanings. Several
different combinations of constraints provide these limiting
guidelines, and they appear to apply not just to words but Q: Why might it seem that children mostly
to many aspects of cognition and communication. If there overextend word meanings when in fact they
is a language acquisition device, it does not seem to be spe-
cialized for learning most word meanings. Instead, children also underextend them?
usually acquire specific word meanings through using more
general learning strategies (Bloom, 2000).
Linking Features with Meanings One way to think
about semantic development focuses on the features associ-
Q: What is the evidence against the proposal ated with word meanings. In particular, most researchers
assume that people use certain perceptual and conceptual
that constraints on word meanings are active
features of objects and events to help link words to relevant
only in the context of word learning? objects and events. For example, the meaning of “dog” is
typically said to rely on both perceptual features of dogs,
such as certain sizes, textures, and shapes, as well as con-
ceptual features of dogs, such as being an animal, a living
Developmental Changes thing, and a physical object. Researchers who are studying
semantic development have investigated how these semantic
in Word Meanings links between features and words develop, and they have
Beyond the constraints that enable children to narrow asked: Are the developmental changes in children’s under-
down the vast number of possible word meanings, children’s standing of word meanings linked to changes in the way
understanding of word meanings seems to change in fun- they think about semantic features?
Overextension Underextension
One proposal suggests that the types of features chil- explain younger children’s greater use of over- and under-
dren use to represent word meanings change with devel- extensions of word meanings. If a younger child puts a
opment. For example, younger children may focus on greater emphasis on the characteristic features of the most
perceptual features, such as color, size, and surface texture, typical jails, she might call a well-fortified bank a “jail”
rather than functional or conceptual features, such as an (overextension), while not recognizing a more upscale jail
object’s use or whether it is dangerous. This view assumes as such (underextension).
that younger children are either unable to use some kinds In an experiment designed to discover whether children
of features or, at the least, have a strong bias against using considered characteristic or defining features more impor-
them (Gentner & Rattermann, 1991; Landau et al., 1998; tant to various word meanings, researchers told preschoolers
Nelson, 1973). and elementary school children several pairs of descrip-
A second view suggests that the ways that children inter- tive stories created to pit an object’s characteristic features
nally represent semantic features change. For example, against its defining features. For example, in one story, the
younger children might take note of as many features of children were told:
an object as possible to determine what its name means,
There is a beautiful building with columns. Mr. Johnson
whereas older children weigh only the few features they
lives there, but he has a big problem. There are these cracks
have determined are central to the meaning. To see how
in his floors and walls. So, he covers them with paintings
these theoretical approaches are translated into experimen-
and statues, and he never lets anyone see them. Could that
tal studies, we will take a closer look at an example of this
be a museum?
second type of proposal.
One way of envisioning how mental representations of This building has several characteristic features of a museum
word meanings develop is based on the idea that word (it is beautiful, has columns, and is filled with art), but it
meanings are made up of two kinds of features (Smith lacks an important defining feature: no one is allowed to
et al., 1974). Characteristic features are the properties see the art inside. In contrast, the second story in this pair
most typically associated with members of a category. describes a building with the defining features of a museum,
For example, characteristic features of jails include that but not its characteristic features:
they have bars on the windows and are highly secure.
There is this small wooden shack in the countryside. People
Defining features are the properties that pertain to what
come from all over and pay 50 cents to get inside and see the
the word really means and how adults would tell whether
interesting display of dirty shirts with rings around the col-
it applies in a particular case. Thus, a defining feature
lar and spots and stains. Could that be a museum?
of jails is that they are places where criminals are held.
Not all words’ characteristic and defining features can If younger children understand word meanings only in
be easily distinguished, but for those that can, a possible terms of characteristic features, they should identify the
developmental model emerges (Keil, 1989; Keil & Bat- first example as a museum, but not the second. And if older
terman, 1984). Perhaps younger children initially rely on children and adults base word meanings on defining fea-
characteristic features as the basis for word meanings, and tures, they should do the opposite.
then, as they get older, they shift to understanding word The results suggested that preschoolers more often base
meanings in terms of defining features. Thus, they may their notions of word meanings on characteristic features,
undergo a shift from relying on characteristic features to and elementary school children more often use defining fea-
emphasizing defining features for learning word mean- tures. Nonetheless, this characteristic-to-defining shift does
ings. This characteristic-to-defining shift could also help not occur for all words at the same time. Rather, depending
A Language Gene?
F
or some time, researchers have suspected that cer- site of the FOXP2 gene. Other studies have since found that
tain locations on the human genome might be closely other abnormalities in the FOXP2 gene are also associated with
and specifically related to language abilities. In fact, language deficits (Fisher & Scharff, 2009; MacDermot et al.,
many spoken and written language abilities seem to 2005; Peter et al., 2011; Zhao et al., 2010) and with abnormal
have genetic components. And different aspects of language, patterns of functioning in language-related brain areas (Lieg-
such as lexical versus syntactic abilities (that is, knowledge eois et al., 2003; Vernes & Fisher, 2011).
of words versus understanding of how to put words together Do these findings provide evidence for a “language gene”?
into meaningful sentences), seem to be associated with One critical question concerns whether the gene’s effects are
distinct genetic loci (Stromswold, 2001). The most power- language specific or whether its mutations actually affect a
ful evidence for genes related to language abilities comes broader range of cognitive functions. The affected members
from studies designed to pinpoint specific genes associated of the KE family scored below normal on intelligence tests,
with language deficits. The findings point to a gene known as but so did the unaffected members. This pattern suggests
FOXP2, which is located on chromosome 7 and produces a that the affected relatives’ genetic difference at the FOXP2
protein that affects the formation of brain regions and circuits site was not necessarily a factor in their low scores. Even so,
associated with motor control. In humans, it also affects brain testing all the family members to compare their performance
regions associated with planning and ordering in speech and on more linguistic and nonlinguistic tasks would help clarify
language. whether their deficit really is restricted to language-related
This finding emerged during a study of three generations learning and skills.
of the KE family in England (Fisher et al., 1998; Fisher & A second finding also relates to the question of whether
Scharff, 2009; Lai et al., 2001, 2003; Vernes & Fisher, 2011). FOXP2 specifically affects language. This same gene is found
Roughly half the family members showed significant spoken in many species as a regulatory gene that activates other genes
language difficulties, while the other half were unaffected. The involved in the formation of brain structures and circuits during
affected relatives had severe articulation problems, so that the embryonic period and in learning to sequence movements.
their speech seemed extremely effortful. They also had enor- The chimpanzee FOXP2 gene differs from the human version
mous difficulty producing ordered speech sounds and could in only 2 of the 715 amino acids it produces. The version in
barely be understood. Overall, these individuals seemed to mice differs by 3. Moreover, the best estimates of when in evo-
show severe disruptions of the planning processes and move- lution humans and chimpanzees diverged—that is, when the
ments necessary for clear speech. A closer look revealed that uniquely human gene emerged—point to between 120,000
the affected members also had difficulties interpreting some and 200,000 years ago (Enard et al., 2002). Thus, around
grammatical aspects of language. 10,000 generations ago, a very small mutation in FOXP2 may
It was clear that there was a genetic component to the KE have spread quickly and conferred a strong selective advan-
family’s language disorder, so the researchers started to look for tage on a human-like species, possibly leading to the ability to
a mutation that may have led to the disorder (Lai et al., 2000). rapidly articulate strings of speech-like sounds.
Because relatives share more of their genes than strangers do, Even if the FOXP2 gene in humans turns out to be involved
it was not easy to isolate the relevant gene that might have only in language abilities (and deficits), the mechanism under-
led to the mutation by comparing the family members’ genetic lying its effects still remains unclear, and the current evidence
sequences. A major advance occurred when researchers dis- in no way suggests a single “language gene” or even a single
covered a boy with highly similar language problems who was gene for a particular component of language (Marcus & Fisher,
unrelated to the KE family (Lai et al., 2001). When his genetic 2003; Vargha-Khadem et al., 2005). It remains to be seen
sequence was compared with that of the KEs, it corresponded whether FOXP2 is necessary for language, but it is clearly not
to the genetic sequence in every one of the affected family sufficient. A full account of how humans are able to acquire a
members—but not in the unaffected members—in a crucial natural language with all its astonishing complexity will depend
way. Like the genes of the affected KEs, his genes showed a on the interaction of many genes with an elaborate set of envi-
distortion of 1 amino acid out of the 715 produced at the ronmental inputs.
Q: What aspects of language seem to be FIGURE 8.18 Comparing language acquisition abilities. The
affected in the disorder known as specific chimpanzee Gua and the human child Donald were raised together as
language impairment? if they were sister and brother in one of the first naturalistic studies
comparing language acquisition in a human and a chimpanzee.
pendent of the languages they speak, resulting instead show more mastery of the same color terms do so in the left
from varying experiences. It is clear that irrefutable sup- hemisphere (Franklin et al., 2008).
port for the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis will require more Even in adults, learning new terms for unfamiliar color
powerful and carefully collected evidence. More recent categories can quickly redirect which parts of the brain
research examining linguistic influences on thought has are dedicated to color processing, shifting the processing
focused on more subtle effects that are often only evident of the new categories from the right to the left hemisphere
in carefully controlled experimental settings. We now turn (Clifford et al., 2010; Zhou et al., 2010), but it is not yet
to a few such cases. understood how this change happens. Researchers are also
exploring whether learning the names of objects might exert
Color and Number If two languages’ color words divide comparable effects on the way the brain processes objects.
the spectrum of light differently, perhaps speakers of those Along these lines, one study found that when 1-year-olds
languages actually perceive different sets of color catego- view objects for which they have learned labels, they show
ries. Because infants show categorical perception of color increased levels of visual object processing than they do
many months before any language skills (see Chapter 3), while viewing equally familiar objects for which they do
we know that language is not necessary for categorical per- not have labels (Gliga et al., 2010). Certainly, not all cross-
ception effects. Nonetheless, color words might subtly tune language labeling differences directly affect object percep-
color perceptions in ways that resemble the effects of spoken tion and cognition (Barner et al., 2010), but there are some
language on categorical perception of speech sounds. Some indications that language can modulate and redirect the way
linguistic influences have indeed been found in languages children process some aspects of colors and perhaps objects.
that require two different color words for a category that Specific languages also appear to affect mathematical
has only one color word in English. For example, Russian thought, as demonstrated in one remarkable study of bilin-
speakers use different words for dark blue and light blue, gual children who spoke both Welsh and English fluently
while English speakers can simply use “blue” for both. (Ellis & Hennelly, 1980; see also Baddeley et al., 1975). The
Native Russian speakers do seem to show a subtle advantage researchers utilized the fact that there is a difference in the
over English speakers in the ability to discriminate between length of the words that each language uses for numbers.
blues that take different labels in Russian (Winawer et al., Welsh number words are longer, and as a result, they make
2007). Apparently, their need to constantly monitor differ- greater demands on memory and cause more difficulty in
ent hues of blue to know which label to use may sharpen timed calculations like those on intelligence tests. Thus,
perceptual processes related to the discrimination of those the researchers found that the same child who was fluent
colors. in both languages took longer to do some mathematical
Language development may also affect where in the brain calculations in Welsh than in English. Similar effects have
categorical perception of color is processed (Kay & Regier, been found when comparing mathematical calculations
2006; Regier & Kay, 2009). Preverbal infants are better at done in other languages with those done in English (Chan &
discriminating between color categories when the colors are Elliott, 2011; Pixner et al., 2011). Thus, a child’s specific
presented to the left visual field, and therefore processed language may modestly influence the development of her
primarily in the brain’s right hemisphere. In contrast, chil- mathematical thought. These effects may also be quite spe-
dren and adults show better color discrimination when the cific. For example, if Russian-English bilinguals learn to do
colors are presented to the right visual field, and processed a particular calculation in one language, they will later be
primarily in the left hemisphere. Moreover, toddlers who able to do that calculation more quickly in that language
are just beginning to learn color words seem to process color than in the other language in which they are equally fluent
category names in the right hemisphere, whereas those who (Spelke & Tsivkin, 2001b).
One of the most eloquent and forceful proposals thought. These general influences of language on thought
about how the emergence of language reshapes thought may be analogous to the influences of gesture on thought,
came from the Russian developmental psychologist Lev where nonverbal gestures often provide critical additional
Semyonovich Vygotsky (Vygotsky, 1934/1962, 1978). support to children to focus their memory and attention in
Vygotsky’s approach to development was wide-reaching complex tasks, boosting cognitive performance above levels
and influential, and we will consider other aspects of his found with language alone (Goldin-Meadow, 2011).
approach later in the book (see Chapters 9 and 11). In this Several scholars have converged on the idea that the
chapter, we will discuss his view of the influence of lan- emergence of language allows the child to bring together
guage on thought. Vygotsky believed that over the course different domains of thought and to combine them in
of early development, the child internalizes his own lan- novel ways (Carey, 2009; Carruthers, 2002, 2004; Gentner,
guage, making it a true vehicle or tool for thinking about 2003; Inagaki & Hatano, 2003; Mithen, 1996; Spelke,
the world. He held that before language is internalized, the 2003; Spelke & Tsivkin, 2001a). They argue that preverbal
child’s thoughts are entirely concrete and tied to the here children might understand concepts in separate domains
and now. Language, Vygotsky claimed, liberates the child’s but might need language to bring them together. Some
thoughts by making them more abstract and therefore a examples include using language to unite knowledge of
more powerful, principled way of understanding meanings an environment’s geometric features with its landmarks,
and categories. Vygotsky described phenomena very much to unite number estimations with more precise numerical
like the characteristic-to-defining shift that we discussed knowledge, and to unite the domains of plants and animals
earlier in the chapter, and he assumed that such shifts are into a common domain of living things. Language is con-
a consequence of internalizing language. He believed that sidered a domain-general common ground within which
language enables the child to formulate mental definitions to combine elements from separate domains. A key chal-
instead of mere associationistic impressions. Others have lenge for this proposal is to demonstrate that language is the
since argued that when children begin talking themselves dominant vehicle for these integrations, rather than just one
through difficult tasks, this is evidence that they have of many ways of uniting elements of thought (Atran, 2002;
internalized their language as a vehicle of thought (Soko- Hampton, 2002). To resolve this question, researchers are
lov, 1972). increasingly conducting experiments to carefully contrast
In Vygotsky’s view, language may facilitate thought in linguistic influences with those of other kinds of represen-
both children and adults. It can provide additional memory tations, such as symbols and pictures.
and attentional support for complex tasks and can sometimes In the end, some effect of emerging language use on
foster more analytical problem solving. Sometimes the effects thought seems inevitable, if only because speaking our
of language may result from the ways in which grammati- thoughts aloud so often brings new insights. But even after
cal structures and interpretations of words focus thought in years of intensive research on how verbalizing while thinking
particular domains like number (Carey, 2009) and theory of relates to basic cognitive skills, many aspects of the relation-
mind (Astington & Baird, 2005; de Villiers & de Villiers, ship are still not understood (Bloom & Keil, 2001). At pres-
2009). Other times language may help integrate domains of ent, we simply do not know how much of an effect language
Dispenser
Language and Communication
Opaque screen
Stacking peg As our discussion of egocentrism in communication shows,
Blocks the development of language skills goes beyond seman-
tics and grammar. Another important aspect of language
development involves the ability to convey and interpret
additional layers of meaning in an utterance, depending
on its context. That context can include the surrounding
sentences, the physical situation, and the listener’s infer-
ences about the speaker’s (and other listeners’) mental state.
Adults can use and understand metaphors, irony, and sar-
Communicator Listener casm (Gibbs, 1994, 1999). They can decide when to dis-
FIGURE 8.22 The referential communication task. Children fail to count a speaker’s credibility, and they can understand the
realize what the communicative common ground is and make highly
unstated norms of conversation. These pragmatic aspects of
idiosyncratic references to the blocks in front of them. These
references make it difficult or impossible for the listener to know
language—ways of using language to relay and infer mean-
which block the communicator is referring to. As a result, the listener ings within a particular context and to understand and use
is unable to pick out the blocks described by the communicator to indirect language—have roots early in the preschool years,
stack them on the peg. Adapted from Krauss & Glucksberg (1969). but they can take many years to fully master.
E
arly views of bilingualism seemed to suggest that chil- Lambert’s results were striking. The bilingual children per-
dren who faced the “chore” of learning two or more formed at least as well on intelligence tests and in academic
languages would certainly fare worse—in both linguis- settings as the monolingual children, and in several cases they
tic and cognitive skills—than their peers who learned performed better. Being immersed in two languages seemed to
just one language (Haugen, 1956). But these early studies help the bilingual children become more aware of language itself
almost always confounded the effects of bilingualism with as a system of arbitrary symbols. They developed this aware-
the effects of specific social disadvantages. They frequently ness earlier than the children who spoke just one language, and
involved the bilingual children of poor, immigrant families who it seemed to also make them more aware of their other cognitive
were often enrolled in inferior schools. Within a few decades, skills, such as their memory abilities. Since Lambert’s studies,
however, a series of landmark studies led to a major turning hundreds of other studies worldwide have documented a similar
point in our understanding of the effects of bilingualism on cog- pattern: speaking more than one language is associated with
nitive development. several possible cognitive advantages and no known deficits
Consider the situation in Montreal, Canada. Montreal is in (Bialystok & Barac, 2012; Diaz, 1983, 1985; Hakuta, 1986).
the province of Quebec, which is officially French speaking, but Bilingual and multilingual individuals consistently show
where many people are bilingual. For the first half of the twen- advantages in tasks that require a more objective understanding
tieth century, the city’s wealth and power were largely concen- of language and its structures. In one group of studies, monolin-
trated in certain English-speaking groups, such that those who gual and bilingual children were shown drawings of objects with
only spoke English often had an economic advantage over bilin- word labels below them. Although the children could not yet
gual speakers of both French and English. Not surprisingly, the read, some of them had a sense that words carried meaning. And
children from economically disadvantaged backgrounds, many when the labels were moved to accompany different pictures,
of whom were bilingual, often performed worse than monolin- these children also knew that the words retained their mean-
gual children on a variety of cognitive tasks. The breakthrough ings, despite being placed under different objects. The bilingual
on the effects of bilingualism occurred when Wallace Lambert children understood at much earlier ages than the monolingual
and his colleagues conducted much more careful studies of chil- children that words have fixed meanings and that words’ physi-
dren from Montreal, balancing the socioeconomic status of the cal characteristics are not linked directly to objects—that is, that
monolinguals and bilinguals (Lambert, 1975, 1985; Lambert & bigger words do not necessarily correspond to bigger objects
Tucker, 1972). (Bialystok, 1997). Moreover, the awareness of language as a
Some forms of indirect language (language that is be obvious to people familiar with the circumstances.
not literal, such as complicated metaphors, sarcastic or Examples like this make it clear that some aspects of indi-
ironic comments, or double entendres) may hinge criti- rect speech take years to understand and require that chil-
cally on cognitive capacities that develop during the dren become more knowledgeable about social situations
school years, such as the child’s understanding of the and the mental states that specific situations elicit in the
intricacies of others’ mental states. (We will look more people involved. Thus, while a young child may easily
closely at these abilities in the next few chapters.) For our understand metaphors such as “The car is hungry” (for a
purposes here, the key point is that preschool children car that needs gas) or “The dog is talking” (for a barking
may have difficulty going beyond a literal understand- dog) or “The house needs a new hat” (for a house that
ing of utterances that depend on a rich interpretation of needs a new roof ), that same child may have great dif-
the speaker’s beliefs and desires. Younger children might ficulty understanding a wry comment about someone’s
not have a sufficiently developed model of other people’s mental abilities when phrased as “not the sharpest tack
mental states and how they interact with situations to be in the box.” Understanding this metaphor requires know-
able to go beyond the literal. Moreover, even adults who ing what it means to be a “sharp tack in a box” as well as
are, for example, oblivious to the underlying hostility in inferences about relative mental states in others. Children
a situation may not notice sarcasm or irony that would are able to understand metaphors when they link domains
that they understand (for example, linking vehicles with said that he had lost (Mills & Keil, 2005). Apparently,
eating), but they don’t understand metaphors in which children take people’s intentions and motivations into
they don’t understand one of the domains (Keil, 1986). account when they are quite young as a way of judging
We also learn when to doubt what others say. We are, their truthfulness.
for example, often cynical about politicians’ seemingly
self-serving statements. More broadly, we are inclined to
doubt a statement when it seems that the speaker is mak-
ing it merely for personal gain. Although this cynicism Q: What are some reasons why indirect
seems like a fairly sophisticated interpretive skill, young speech might take longer to learn to understand
elementary school children actually seem to know when than direct speech?
to take a statement with a grain of salt. In one study, chil-
dren were told about a race in which two runners finished
so close to each other that it was hard to tell who won. A different aspect of the social uses of language involves
When children as young as 7 years old were told that one learning how to be a conversational partner—how to create
of the runners said he had won the race, they thought he a clear narrative and how to use and interpret the pragmatic
was less likely to be telling the truth than a runner who devices that enhance communication. Research suggests two
CONCLUSIONS 299
with the environment. Language is also an intrinsically Grammar, word meanings, and pragmatics each have their
social enterprise, and yet its acquisition depends on the own distinctive developmental stories and mechanisms,
cognitive and representational capacities of each individual and while they do intersect in interesting ways, it is clear
that make learning possible. Finally, we have seen that lan- that no one overall account of learning works for these
guage is not just one thing with one pattern of acquisition. areas collectively.
STU DY AN D REVIEW
Foundational versus
Emergent Constraints
As we have seen, development always involves interactions
between genetic and environmental factors. Keeping this
perspective in mind, however, it is also increasingly clear
FIGURE 9.2 Global versus local patterns of change. When that the role that each of these factors plays varies greatly
young children like those at this fountain undergo a change in their
between different aspects of development. One way to
concepts about animals, do their other concepts—such as how to
count the number of ducks or boats in the water or the way the stick
think about these influences is in terms of how and when
they are holding can act as a lever—also change in roughly the same the developmental path is narrowed, or constrained, toward
ways? Or does their intuitive understanding of biology, number, and a particular outcome (see Figure 9.3). Thus, some kinds of
physics each show distinct developmental patterns? knowledge seem to emerge in ways that are innately con-
Sensorimotor Birth–2 years The infant represents the world in terms of her own sensations Object permanence
and reflexes.
Preoperational 2–7 years The child is egocentric, ignores all but one aspect of a problem, Symbolic representations
fails to use basic operators such as compensation, reversibility,
and identity.
Concrete operational 7–12 years The child can apply operators to concrete information, but fails Classification, conservation,
to apply them to abstract or hypothetical information. seriation, transitive
reasoning
Formal operational 12 years on The child is able to think logically about things that are not Scientific reasoning tasks
immediately present and can engage in hypothetico-deductive
reasoning.
TABLE 9.1 Piaget’s four periods of cognitive development. According to Piaget’s theory, transitions between these periods involve
sweeping, across-the-board transformations in the nature of thought. Although children show reliable patterns of failures and successes on the
experimental tasks that Piaget devised, psychologists are still debating how accurate he was in interpreting what kinds of cognitive changes
underlie these patterns.
isolate the relationship between the ball’s width and its other with each row having the same number of coins with equal
physical properties. Thus, the child could not understand spaces between them. The experimenter then asks the child
that the increase in its width must be accompanied by a cor- whether both rows have the same number of coins or if one
responding decrease in its height. More generally, because row has more coins. When the child responds that they
the preoperational child lacks the ability to think about rela- have the same number of coins, the experimenter asks the
tions between concrete properties such as height and width, child to watch carefully, then spreads out one row so that
he cannot see how a change in one property might necessar- the coins in that row now form a much longer line. Again,
ily imply a change in another property. By contrast, older the experimenter asks the child whether one row has more
children realize that the ball must logically become shorter coins or if they both have the same number. Many pre-
in a way that compensates for its increasing width. schoolers will now answer that the longer row has more
Piaget’s theory predicted that children in each period coins.
except the last one are largely unable to grasp the kinds Part of Piaget’s genius lay in his ability to devise simi-
of concepts and problems that require cognitive abilities lar tasks to test children’s understanding of conservation
developed only in the next period. In his research, Piaget with many different kinds of quantities, such as number,
observed how children of different ages fared on specific liquid volume, mass, and length (see Figure 9.6). Piaget
types of tasks, and he made strong claims about their abili- argued that children fail at conservation tasks because they
ties based on their performance on these tasks. The most lack the critical mental operators, or formal mental tools,
famous and best studied of these tasks are the conservation needed to consider the relations between sets of properties.
tasks, in which children are asked to judge whether certain Operators are therefore logical, rule-based ways of compar-
physical properties of an object, such as its size or amount, ing mental representations of situations before and after
are “conserved” (remain unchanged) when the object is transformations.
transformed along different sorts of dimensions. Our ball In particular, Piaget believed that preoperational children
of dough is one such example—that is, an increase in width lack the logical operators of compensation, reversibility, and
must be associated with a decrease in height because the identity. The operator of compensation involves noting that
amount of dough remains the same. a change in one dimension compensates for a change in
To make the concept of conservation more explicit, con- another dimension. In the example of the two rows of coins,
sider one of the tasks used to test whether children have the children notice that one row is longer than the other
mastered the conservation of number. In this type of con- after the experimenter spreads out one of the rows of coins.
servation task, an experimenter shows the child two rows But if they lack the operator of compensation, they do not
of objects: let’s assume they are coins. Initially, as shown understand that the increase in length is due to an increase
in Figure 9.5, the coins are arranged in two identical rows, in the space between the coins. The operator of reversibility
C
FIGURE 9.5 Piaget’s number conservation task. Children in the Task Transformation Question
preoperational stage seem to see the change in the length of a row Number ¢ ¢ ¢ ¢ ¢ ¢ ¢ Are they the
of coins as relevant to the number of coins in the display, therefore same number?
failing to “conserve” number.
¢ ¢ ¢ ¢ ¢ ¢ ¢
Basic-level
shirts pants tables chairs horses dogs
categories
Subordinate
T-shirts ski pants coffee tables kitchen chairs Arabians collies categories
FIGURE 9.13 Basic-level categories. The basic level is an intermediate level of categorization that seems to be especially salient, psycho-
logically natural, and easy to think about. Preschoolers are much better at sorting objects into basic-level categories than they are at using other
category levels. This finding suggests that young children’s failures at class-inclusion reasoning may have more to do with their difficulties think-
ing about the relevant categories than with missing the mental operators pertaining to classification.
It seems that in Piaget’s original task, the child might be task variation. In this variation, the younger children often
thinking, “I just told him the number of coins in each row answer correctly when the question about number is asked
is the same. Why is he asking me the same question again? only once, which avoids any pragmatic confusion (Rose &
He must have changed the number somehow, and he wants Blank, 1974; Samuel & Bryant, 1984).
to see if I can tell. Since one row is longer than it was before, Donaldson’s critique of pragmatically odd components in
I guess he must have added something and that’s why he is Piaget’s number conservation task that might mislead young
asking.” In Donaldson’s pragmatically different variations, children can also be extended to Piaget’s other conserva-
however, the experimenter never intentionally changes the tion tasks (Donaldson, 1985; Gelman & Baillargeon, 1983).
rows of objects and seems to be asking the question a sec- All of the classic versions of these tasks, whether they test
ond time simply to make sure nothing has changed (see conservation of number, weight, liquid volume, or another
Figure 9.14). Donaldson’s claim that young children often property, involve the odd practice of asking a child the same
fail at the original task because they misinterpret the experi- question twice, before and after intentionally manipulating
menter’s intentions gained further support from another the materials. Donaldson’s studies show that young children
who are especially good at estimating gain that insight about them to see more complex and systematic numerical rela-
counting at an earlier age (Wagner & Johnson, 2011). tionships (Carey, 2001, 2009). Somehow, children with
A more nuanced perspective arises from work on the “number-rich” languages manage to learn that numbers
understanding of number by Nicaraguan home-signers, above 3 continue to each have a discrete value (Huang
deaf people who use systems of invented hand gestures to et al., 2010). In addition, the more parents talk to their chil-
communicate with hearing friends and relatives (see Chap- dren about number-related topics in these languages, the
ter 8). They grow up in a culture where number is clearly earlier the children make the transition to understanding
important, but their home-created language has no number that the number 4 corresponds to exactly four things, even
symbols, and they have difficulty thinking about precise when socioeconomic factors and other aspects of parents’
number values much higher than 3 (Spaepen et al., 2011). language use are controlled (Levine et al., 2010). Children
Thus, even though they know numbers are important and may achieve this insight by noticing patterns in the way
understand discrete numbers up to 3, they tend to switch to small numbers are used in their language, and then see-
a more approximate estimation system when they encounter ing how those patterns apply to larger numbers as well. In
even modestly large numbers. Above and beyond the debate this form of learning, which is often called bootstrapping,
about how much language contributes to understanding children appear to build on their early insights about small
number, however, it is clear that preschoolers’ numerical abili- numbers, using these patterns as a foundation for under-
ties surpass what Piaget envisioned, since he argued that real standing new and quite different kinds of number con-
numerical understanding was not present until the transition cepts (Carey, 2009; Negen & Sarnecka, 2012; Slusser &
to concrete operational thought around age 7 (Piaget, 1952). Sarnecka, 2011).
As with spatial knowledge, research on the development
Developmental research on number skills has also raised
of mathematical thought suggests several important themes
questions about how children’s intuitive concepts of num-
about how this area of knowledge begins to take shape in
ber change between infancy and middle childhood (Carey,
childhood. Three of these themes are most prominent:
2009; Wynn, 1995, 1998). Older children develop a new
system that is responsible for more sophisticated math- 1. The principles that govern our ways of thinking about
ematical thought and complex computations, and as the numbers and mathematical relations share some com-
study of the Nicaraguan home-signers suggests, language mon properties across all cultures. In particular, almost
may be central to this second system. A child may take every culture’s number system shares fundamental
several years, from age 3 to roughly 7, to fully benefit from assumptions about what numbers are, how they are con-
the subtle but powerful clues that language can provide nected to countable objects, and what sorts of operations
about more sophisticated aspects of mathematics. This gap can be performed on the numbers derived from counting
of several years between the point when a child can first representations.
use number terms to count and the time she is able to talk 2. The development of mathematical thought involves
about mathematical relationships may help explain why changes in several kinds of knowledge systems, includ-
early counting often doesn’t result in knowing how many ing a system for estimating large numbers and a system
things there are. for precise operations on small number values, such as
One line of research suggests that children’s increasing 1 and 2. Some forms are nonverbal and relatively inflex-
understanding of number words and plural forms helps ible, like the information handled by the estimation sys-
O
n October 5, 1957, U.S. citizens were shocked to learn
that the Soviet Union had launched Sputnik, the world’s
first artificial satellite (see Figure 9.21). In response to
this achievement, the United States undertook an all-
out effort to make its children more scientifically literate. Large
resources were devoted to enhancing science education at all lev-
els, from elementary school to graduate school, with the National
Science Foundation playing a particularly central role (Atkinson,
1999). The program succeeded in some respects; technological
and scientific innovation in the United States continues to outpace
that of many other countries. Yet, on tests of scientific knowledge
and reasoning, U.S. children continue to be outperformed by chil-
dren in other countries. In the late 1990s, the Third International
Math and Science Study revealed that after the fourth grade, chil- FIGURE 9.21 Sputnik. The launching of the world’s first artifi-
dren in the United States started to fall dismally behind students cial satellite by the Soviet Union in 1957 set off a wave of initia-
in other industrialized countries (Schmidt et al., 1997, 1999). In tives focused on improving science education in the United States.
response to these findings, congressional hearings examined
the weaknesses in U.S. science education, and President Clinton 2009). But it is not clear what the U.S. students’ persistently
vowed to address the crisis head-on. Yet, a similar 2009 study by poor performance on science-related standardized tests really
the Program for International Student Assessment (PISA) showed means for the future of science education and innovation in the
that U.S. students ranked no higher than 17th in the world in their country. In short, we don’t know which aspects of science educa-
knowledge of science (National Center for Education Statistics, tion matter most for increasing the number of leading scientists
How do children develop these biological beliefs, and what actions between objects. Applying these ideas to children’s
mistakes do they make as they acquire adult knowledge? biological knowledge, proponents mostly assume that chil-
dren explain animal properties in psychological terms and
explain plant properties in simple physical terms (Carey,
Q: What are some universal core biological 1985, 1995). According to these views, as children develop,
beliefs? they come to realize that neither the psychological nor
the physical system of explanation is fully adequate for
explaining the properties of living things, and they begin to
Here we will consider in detail two different sets of pro- develop a new, specifically biological system of explanation.
posals that describe how children learn about living things. This biological explanatory system is based on the idea that
The first set of views, which we will refer to as late-emerging living things are systems made up of smaller systems, such
biology views, assumes that even very young children’s as the muscles and the digestive system. Their biological
impressions of living things are embedded within larger thinking also takes into account the ways that these smaller
explanatory systems that provide them with a sense of why systems serve specific functional roles that help an organism
those things are the way they are. In these views, children survive in its environment.
use a theory for explaining the social world called naive The second set of views, which we will call early compe-
psychology and a theory for explaining the physical world tence accounts, holds that even very young children think
called naive physical mechanics. Thus, these views propose of biological things as different from other kinds of things
that young children can interpret and explain things only and accordingly explain their properties and actions as dis-
in psychological terms or in terms of simple physical inter- tinctive, too. According to these views, most developmental
100
Percent of
children 9-year-olds
saying an
animal-related
50
property FIGURE 9.23 Attributing biological properties. In
pertained Carey’s original studies, younger children seemed to decide
to the 5-year-olds whether animal-related biological properties pertained to
object different categories of living and nonliving things based
on the extent to which those things seemed behaviorally
0
Human Dog Mouse Bird Worm Flower Chair similar to humans.
M
ost developmental studies assume that the out- Different amounts of exposure to nature may play a role,
come or “end state” of development is the level as may the different ways that members of a culture talk about
of knowledge common among college students or humans and other living things (Anggoro et al., 2010; Taverna
other adults in mostly urban, technological societ- et al., 2012). Cultures may also differ in how they expose children
ies. But what happens when that end state is neither the normal to animals, both in real life or indirectly through various media.
state of mature knowledge nor especially sophisticated? This is When children from urban, rural, and Native American communi-
arguably the case with the development of biological knowledge ties were asked to name all the animals they could, urban chil-
(Wolff et al., 1999). After all, the most salient living things that dren more often named exotic animals they had learned about
the average city dweller encounters are probably other humans. from picture books and other media (such as lions, tigers, and
By contrast, an inhabitant of a rural area experiences a much elephants) than common examples from their urban environ-
broader range of biological forms (see Figure 9.24). In the ments (such as squirrels and pigeons). Rural children, and to a
realm of naive biological thought, people who grow up in urban greater extent Native American children, named more local ani-
cultures may have a relatively impoverished understanding. mals (Winkler-Rhoades et al., 2010). Thus, while young children
Work with children in “biology-deprived” environments and often benefit from learning through others (S. Gelman, 2009;
with children in “biology-enriched” environments suggests that Keil et al., 2008), direct experience is also important.
lack of experience with nature affects biological understand- Because urban adults may have such limited knowledge of
ing (Atran et al., 2001, 2004; Herrmann et al., 2010; Medin & biology, examining urban adults to learn about the develop-
Waxman, 2007; Ross et al., 2003). Compared with adults who ment of biological knowledge may produce a skewed account.
regularly encounter many diverse forms of nature, adults in In most cultures, the normal developmental path may lead to
biology-deprived environments show greater difficulty attributing the early emergence of rich biological knowledge. Younger chil-
biological properties, categorize the living world at less specific dren in urban or suburban areas may tend to interpret biologi-
levels, reason in less sophisticated ways about biological prob- cal properties in psychological ways because they have grown
lems, and think about biological essences differently. Their chil- up in a biology-deprived culture. More broadly, the study of
dren also show comparable difficulties (Atran et al., 2004). The how knowledge develops in any domain risks telling a distorted
influence of specific cultures on this domain of thought seems story if the mature state of knowledge is measured according
to emerge around age 5, suggesting a more universal, common to cultural norms that do not represent most cultures (Medin
framework in younger children (Herrmann et al., 2010). et al., 2010; Waxman et al., 2007).
FIGURE 9.24 Developing biological thought in urban and rural youth. It now appears that urban children may have a different
and simpler set of beliefs about biological properties than children raised in rural environments.
FIGURE 9.29 Scaffolding. Parents, teachers, and other adults often help children to attain new skill levels by providing support in subtle
ways that help give the child a sense of achievement, a process called scaffolding. In some cases, adults can provide scaffolding without giving
the child verbal instructions.
CONCLUSIONS 339
STU DY AN D REVIEW
SUMMARY that is, from their own point of view. But later studies showed
that in familiar and accessible tasks, children are able to
Dimensions of Cognitive Development overcome the egocentric perspective. Even young children
● Most of the theories of children’s knowledge of the world can
might use a method that involves noticing the contour of
be contrasted in terms of where they stand on three dimen- the environment, an ability that emerges early and that can
sions: qualitative versus quantitative development, global override other spatial information, such as landmarks.
● The child’s understanding of number undergoes considerable
versus local patterns of development, and foundational ver-
sus emergent constraints on development. change in preschool and middle childhood. The systems that
enable infants to detect number and engage in some forms
A Closer Look at Piaget’s Theory of subtraction and addition remain in place, but they are
● Jean Piaget’s theory of cognitive development argued for
supplemented over time with new systems that enable more
global, qualitative changes in cognitive structure that have complex computations and a deeper understanding of num-
sweeping influences on all areas of cognition. Piaget’s view bers and their manipulations. Studies of children’s counting
was dominant for much of the second half of the twentieth behaviors have been especially revealing in this area.
● In the case of biology, one of the major patterns of change
century, and its influence has only recently diminished.
● Piaget believed that preoperational children (ages 2 to 7) lack
may be from abstract senses of how biology is distinct from
the ability to think about the world flexibly. He tested their psychology and physical mechanics to more mechanistic
cognitive understanding by observing how they performed understandings of how biological processes work. Researchers
on conservation tasks. He believed that they failed on these have asked whether young children come to initially under-
tasks because they lack the mental operators of compensa- stand the living world based on intuitive psychology or if a
tion, reversibility, and identity. Moreover, he said that they distinct intuitive biology emerges early on in a manner that
focus excessively on one dimension of a transformation (cen- is more independent of intuitive psychology.
● Several other domains have also been investigated. These
tration) and ignore relevant other dimensions.
● According to Piaget, children in the concrete operational
include children’s understanding of physical mechanics, their
period (ages 7 to 12) can reason about objects that are in notions of substance and material transformation, and their
their immediate surroundings. They acquire the mental sense of cosmology (beliefs about the earth, sun, and stars).
operators of compensation, reversibility, and identity. They In all cases, preschoolers’ intuitions in these domains repre-
are able to use transitive reasoning and begin to succeed on sent an advance over infancy. But this early knowledge will
conservation, seriation, and classification tasks. still undergo considerable growth and change in later child-
● Piaget believed that the formal operational period emerges
hood. Moreover, some domains, such as physical mechanics,
when children are about 12 years old. During this period, may have a more common core set of beliefs across cultural
they can think logically about objects or ideas that are not groups than other domains, such as cosmology.
physically present or are contrary to fact.
● Later research has suggested that the difficulty that younger Sociocultural Views of the Development of
children have in conservation tasks may not really reflect a Knowledge
qualitatively different cognitive structure but rather differ- ● Vygotsky’s theory of sociocultural development emphasized
ent ways of understanding task demands and the influences the ways in which a child’s immersion in a larger culture has
of cognitive factors (for example, memory or attention) not important supporting influences on cognitive development.
central to the task at hand. ● All development occurs in specific sociocultural contexts
that enable the child to make cognitive progressions that
Domains of Knowledge might otherwise not be possible. Social interactions between
● Recent studies have focused more directly on development of parents and their children, as well as between other mentors
cognition in specific domains, such as spatial layout, num- and children, help children get over some cognitive develop-
ber, and biology. In each of these domains, younger children ment hurdles more easily.
seem to recognize some fairly abstract, domain-specific pat- ● Adult mentors enable children to move into zones of proxi-
terns even before gaining detailed knowledge of the domains. mal development, which are new levels of cognitive compe-
● In the case of spatial layout, there are several ways in which a tence that are just beyond a child’s current abilities. This is
child can know her location in space. Piaget believed that young often achieved when adults engage in scaffolding, providing
children can only think of spatial layouts egocentrically— supporting environments that ease a child’s cognitive load.
KEY TERMS
animism (p. 330) classification (p. 310)
basic level of categorization class-inclusion relations
(p. 316) (p. 315)
bootstrapping (p. 326) cognitive map (p. 321)
centration (p. 310) compensation (p. 309)
MEMORY 345
Working memory, which is sometimes called short- Hippocampal damage can block the ability to transfer
term memory, involves specifically attending to and pro- information from short-lived working memory into more
cessing the incoming information from sensory memory. permanent long-term memory (Squire, 1987). An adult with
Working memory has a limited capacity, which was clas- substantial hippocampal damage might seem to perceive
sically described as seven pieces of information, plus or and experience the world normally, but much of what he
minus two (Miller, 1956). More recent accounts suggest experiences will not be transferred out of working memory.
that the typical capacity may be smaller and that seemingly He might, for example, introduce himself to someone,
larger working memory capacities are achieved by chunk- shake his hand, learn his name and repeat it back, but then
ing (Cowan, 2001), or linking together separate pieces of 20 minutes later, he might repeat the exact same sequence,
information into a larger unit that can be treated as one as if he had never met the other person at all (Scoville &
item in memory. (Think of remembering 2, 4, and 0 in a Milner, 1957). If, as some have argued, very young children
phone number as “240” to make three pieces of informa- have especially immature hippocampal structures, they may
tion into one chunk.) Information in working memory show some similar memory problems.
tends to last about 15 seconds before it either decays or is
“pushed out” of working memory by new, incoming infor-
mation, or processed and transferred into long-term mem-
ory. We can maintain information in working memory for Types of Long-Term Memory
much longer by consciously attending to it and perhaps by Understanding long-term memory is critical to under-
repeating or verbally rehearsing it, as we all do when try- standing current proposals about how memory develops.
ing to keep a phone number or an address in mind. These Three distinctions are typically discussed: explicit versus
working memory processes involve complex interactions implicit memory, procedural versus declarative memory,
between several brain regions (D’Esposito, 2007). Most and semantic versus episodic memory. These distinctions
notably, the prefrontal cortex helps to actively manage do not represent rigid boundaries, as the different ways
what information is attended to and the way it is main- of characterizing memory often overlap. As we will see, a
tained in working memory. Verbal rehearsal, for example, single memory can be explicit, declarative, and semantic.
involves interactions between the prefrontal cortex and But each of these different aspects of long-term memory
language-related brain areas. Similarly, for visual images, contributes somewhat differently to understanding how
a complex cortical network that includes frontal and tem- memory develops.
poral areas maintains mental images in visual working
memory by sustaining neural activity until the images Explicit Memory versus Implicit Memory One way
can be processed into long-term memory (Palva et al., to distinguish between types of long-term memory is to
2010). Working memory is often characterized as the consider the difference between explicit and implicit mem-
“bottleneck” of the memory process, where information ory. Explicit memory is memory for information that is
capacity is severely constrained before information passes consciously recalled and can often be stated verbally, like
through to long-term memory. a new acquaintance’s name or a historical fact. Implicit
Long-term memory has a vast capacity and can last a memory, on the other hand, may influence behavior but
lifetime. In contrast to working memory, in which informa- is usually outside of conscious awareness (see Figure 10.2).
tion’s sensory qualities predominate, information in long- Some patients with hippocampal damage who cannot form
term memory tends to be organized in terms of its meaning. new explicit memories still seem to be able to form implicit
Thus, the sound similarities between words often seem memories. Thus, even though a patient might introduce
most prominent in working memory, whereas similarities in himself to you repeatedly as if you were meeting for the
meanings are more salient in long-term memory. Long-term first time, he might retain some memory traces about you
memories seem to take some time to consolidate before they in other implicit ways. If, for example, you shook hands
are robustly stored. They can therefore be quite fragile at first with him once while hiding a pin in your hand, he might
and easily lost when events such as a severe concussion occur. later avoid shaking your hand. He might remain unaware
Unlike information in working memory, which remains of having met you and of having been pricked, but none-
in conscious awareness, much information in long-term theless behave in a way that showed that he had retained
memory can exist outside of awareness until it is accessed. some information (Milner et al., 1968). As we will see,
Long-term memory storage is distributed throughout many given that explicit memories are often recalled verbally and
regions of the cortex and involves other brain regions, such given that implicit memories can be demonstrated outside
as the thalamus. Most critically, it involves the hippocam- of awareness and without language, some researchers sus-
pus, which is responsible for helping to transfer information pect that very young, preverbal children may rely more on
from working memory to long-term memory. implicit forms of memory. In addition, as described later
in this chapter, some have suggested that we cannot recall between forms of memory. Overall, however, there is a sig-
memories from very early in life because these memories nificant overlap between explicit and declarative memories
may be stored implicitly. and between procedural and implicit memories.
Some theorists have suggested that the ability to form
procedural memories develops before the capacity to form
Q: How are explicit and implicit memories declarative memories (Mandler, 1998, Nelson, 1995).
different? Infants and young children can clearly learn to carry out
simple processes that certainly involve procedural memory.
It is less clear whether preverbal children have declarative
Procedural Memory versus Declarative Memory memories, and earlier researchers often assumed that these
Closely related to the contrast between implicit versus memories were linked to the onset of language. Many cur-
explicit memory is the contrast between declarative and rent researchers, however, tend to think that children can
procedural memory. Declarative memory can be described form declarative memories well before the onset of language,
as “knowing that” memory, as in knowing that an event has and some have investigated this possibility using elicited
occurred or that a particular fact is true. In contrast, proce- imitation tasks. In these tasks, a child is taught an ordered
dural memory involves “knowing how” to perform actions series of actions that involves manipulating objects—such
or procedures, such as tying your shoes or riding a bike. as turning a knob, pulling a lever, and sliding a latch to
As a rule of thumb, declarative memories typically involve open a door—then asked to reproduce it after a substan-
information that can easily be verbalized (or “declared”), tial delay. Six-month-olds have great trouble recalling the
though this is not a requirement. Procedural memories, on actions in the correct order, but 20-month-olds can do so
the other hand, are often implicit, like a sense of how to ride effortlessly. This ability has been interpreted as indicat-
a bike. Despite their overlaps, though, these two pairs of ing that by 20 months, children can form a declarative
categories (explicit/implicit and declarative/procedural) are (and explicit) memory for their actions (Bauer, 2002a, 2005,
not precisely the same. Thus, a procedural memory can be 2006, 2007; Mandler, 2004; McDonough et al., 1995).
explicit—for example, you can be quite consciously aware of It may seem that recalling and reproducing the sequence
remembering the procedure for baking a cake. And a declar- of actions would draw on procedural memory, but the need
ative memory can be implicit—for example, if you read a to recall the actions in the correct order is key to the argu-
passage in which a certain word is consistently misspelled, ment that these tasks require declarative memory as well.
you may implicitly remember that misspelling later by Reproducing each action relies on procedural memory,
being more accepting of it (Jacoby & Hollingshead, 1990). which is available to younger infants, but recalling the order
These unusual examples illustrate the nuanced distinctions of the sequence is thought to be declarative. If that logic
MEMORY 347
is correct, there is evidence for declarative memory well touched the water may still have a memory of experienc-
before the first year of life (Bauer, 2005; Hartshorn & ing the pool, but because she remembers using a differ-
Rovee-Collier, 1997). Using related tasks and similar lines ent sense, she is likely to confuse the memory with other
of reasoning, some have argued that young infants also have memories about water. Thus, the specific episodic memory
explicit memories (Rovee-Collier, 1997). Indeed, it may of the experience will become fragile in contrast to a stron-
be that rudimentary versions of both declarative and pro- ger semantic memory of the typical temperature of pools of
cedural memory (and of explicit and implicit memory) are water. Young children have greater difficulty with source
present quite early in infancy and develop in parallel, rather monitoring, often remembering only the event itself and
than implicit and procedural memory serving as the precur- much less about their own experience of acquiring particu-
sor for explicit and declarative memory (Bauer et al., 2010; lar knowledge (Roberts, 2002; Roberts & Blades, 2000).
Howe & Courage, 1993). Evidence also suggests that the late-developing prefrontal
cortex is intimately involved in source monitoring (Mitch-
Semantic Memory versus Episodic Memory Mem- ell et al., 2004). Since this area of the brain continues to
ory researchers also distinguish between two facets of develop well into adolescence (Casey et al., 2005; Gogtay
declarative memory. The first, semantic memory, refers et al., 2004; Huttenlocher & Dabholkar, 1997), it is not
to knowledge of facts about the world, without necessarily surprising that it has been implicated in childhood diffi-
remembering how or when the information was learned. culties with source monitoring.
The second, episodic memory, refers to memories of Children’s limited source monitoring abilities also affect
specific events that have been experienced at a particular experimental tasks that involve remembering experiences
time and place (Tulving, 1972). For example, after attend- over long intervals. In one study, children ranging from
ing a birthday party, a child might recall, as a semantic 3 to 8 years of age participated in some hands-on demonstra-
memory, that birthday parties have lots of presents and a tions with an adult called “Mr. Science” (Poole & Lindsay,
cake. Recalling the party as an episodic memory, she may 2001). When the children were asked to recall aspects of the
remember that the party happened on a rainy day, that she demonstrations immediately afterward, they did very well.
played particular games there, and that she ate too much Then, 3 months later, the researchers had the parents read
cake. Many episodic memories are also autobiographical the children a book about Mr. Science on 3 consecutive days.
memories—memories of one’s own specific experiences as The book described two real events that had occurred when
a participant in an event. the children participated in Mr. Science’s demonstrations, as
Many scholars believe that children’s semantic memory well as two fictitious events. After listening to the descrip-
emerges earlier than their episodic memory (Nelson, 2001; tions, the children were later asked about what had really
Perner, 2001). Thus, a preverbal child who has been to sev- happened, and 35 percent of them reported that the ficti-
eral birthday parties may have developed a strong semantic tious events had really happened. Finally, the children were
sense of what birthday parties involve while remembering reminded of all the stories read from the book and were told
relatively little about the specific birthday parties she has that only some of the events had actually occurred. They
attended, perhaps blending several parties together. Simi- were instructed to think about what had really happened,
larly, a toddler may have trouble recalling a specific visit to as opposed to things in the stories that were made up. These
the zoo despite being able to describe the zoo in great detail. instructions considerably improved accuracy for the older
Episodic memory may have evolved relatively recently, and children but not for the 3- and 4-year-olds (Poole & Lindsay,
some researchers argue that it exists only in humans (Tulv- 2002). These younger children seemed unable to access their
ing, 1985, 2001; Wheeler et al., 1997). memories in a way that identified the sources and allowed
Early episodic memories are typically fragile, as evi- them to evaluate their accuracy. Children who are 3 and
denced in the ways that children often mistake how 4 years old also easily confuse events they have actually seen
they learned a piece of information. Rather than keeping with televised versions of similar events, while older children
track of where the new information comes from, a pro- are more able to distinguish these two kinds of information
cess known as source monitoring (Johnson et al., 1993; (Roberts & Blades, 1999).
Mitchell & Johnson, 2000; see also Atance & O’Neill, Thus, source monitoring is an important cognitive pro-
2005), they often fail to notice its source. After touching a cess that supports the development of episodic memory. In
pool of water to determine its temperature, for example, a addition, source monitoring helps to bind features to specific
child may claim to have learned this information by smell- memories (for example, a child might bind the facts that the
ing the water (O’Neill & Chong, 2001). Source monitoring day was warm and noisy to a particular birthday party) and
is necessary to build up episodic memories, which include a contributes to skills that guide information flow and manage-
great deal of source-specific information about experiences ment (Raj & Bell, 2010; Shing & Lindenberger, 2011). In turn,
and situations (Johnson, 2005). For example, the child who such skills can enhance source monitoring, and ultimately epi-
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Memory Strategies Serial digit position
By early adolescence, we have acquired many ways of FIGURE 10.3 Serial position curves and age. These curves rep-
improving the power of memory. Memory strategies are resent children’s typical recall patterns for a sequence of seven ran-
explicit techniques we use to enhance the encoding, storage, dom numbers. Older children recall the first few items well (primacy
effect) and the last few items well (recency effect). Primacy effects
or retrieval of information. For example, if you are trying to
reflect the spontaneous use of strategies, such as verbal rehearsal,
remember a set of objects to take on a trip, you might try to while recency effects reflect the last contents of working memory.
enhance your encoding by carefully examining each item Younger children do not show strong primacy effects, presumably
and noticing its distinctive features. You could also try to because they do not spontaneously use memory strategies. Adapted
improve storage of memories by repeating the names of the from Sarver et al. (1976).
MEMORY 349
The higher recall rate for items early in the series, shown What happens when younger children are shown how
at the front of the curve, is known as the primacy effect, to use a memory strategy and told to use it in a particular
and it is largely caused by the use of memory strategies. As task? In most cases, younger children’s performance does not
the numbers keep coming at a pace of two per second, your improve much until they have used the strategy on several
rehearsal falls behind. It becomes impossible to keep rehears- occasions (Bjorklund & Harnishfeger, 1987; Miller et al.,
ing the first numbers while incorporating later numbers. As 1994). This initial lack of benefit from a new strategy, known
a result, these later numbers tend to get “bumped” out of as a utilization deficit, tends to occur only in children
working memory and displaced before they can be encoded younger than about age 7, whereas older children often show
and stored. This “bumping” explains the drop in memory more immediate benefits (Coyle & Bjorklund, 1996). Inter-
performance around the middle of the serial position curve. estingly, even when younger children have benefited from
The end of the curve shows that recall improves for the using a particular strategy, they typically will not employ it
last few items on the list, a pattern known as the recency later when they are asked to do the task on their own. This
effect. This effect occurs because the last few items to be pattern of results struck early researchers as almost paradoxi-
remembered remain in working memory when the list is cal. Why wouldn’t younger children use the strategies when
finished. Because there are no later items to displace the they had been trained to do so and when doing so made them
items at the end of the list, people can retrieve them from perform better? It wasn’t a motivational problem; the children
working memory directly. Preventing adults from rehears- clearly wanted to do well, and sometimes there was even a
ing while the list is presented reveals the contrast between large reward at stake. Rather, the children did not seem to
the primacy effect and the recency effect. Without rehearsal, understand when it was appropriate to use the strategy.
performance on the first part of the curve drops to the same
level as the middle section, while performance at the end of
the curve remains high.
If younger children are less likely to use strategies like Development of Metamemory
rehearsal, we might expect them to perform worse at the A closer look at young children’s failures to use strategies sug-
beginning of the serial position curve, which is shown in gests that one important cause of the problem is that they do
the results for first-graders and third-graders in Figure 10.3 not really understand what memory is or have a sense of how
(Sarver et al., 1976). The clearest pattern in these graphs is that their own memory functions (Flavell 1987; Flavell & Well-
the younger children show only a very limited primacy effect, man, 1977). This awareness of our own memory processes,
although they show a recency effect similar to that shown by abilities, and limitations is known as metamemory, and it is
older children and adults. This pattern suggests that, before one facet of metacognition, the broad ability to think about
age 7, children not only remember less, they seem to use strat- our own mind and thoughts, a topic that we will examine
egies less, leading to qualitatively different patterns of recall. later in the chapter. The two aspects of metamemory are:
Children’s spontaneous use of other memory strategies, (1) knowing roughly how memory works—that is, how infor-
such as clustering items in meaningful ways, associating mation is encoded, stored, and retrieved, and (2) monitoring
items with images, and relating the items to one another, how effectively these processes are working to know when to
shows similar age-related changes (Shin et al., 2007). A rely on strategies. The two facets of metamemory are inter-
large body of research has demonstrated that children linked: monitoring the effectiveness of memory processes
under about age 7 do not spontaneously draw on a variety requires a basic sense of what memory is and how it works.
of strategies that might help them encode, store, and recall Many studies have shown that younger children have
information (Camos & Barrouillet, 2011). Such a failure to weak metamemory skills. Consider, for example, how a
spontaneously use strategies to improve memory has been 4-year-old might approach an experimental task in which
called a production deficit (Kail, 1990; Schneider, 1999). she has to estimate how many picture cards she will be able
Young children sometimes do show signs of using simple to remember (see Figure 10.4). At first, researchers show the
strategies, such as putting an item they need to remem- child a paper strip that has just one card with one picture on
ber in a prominent position, but they generally do not use it, and they ask the child whether she thinks she will be able
memory strategies nearly as effectively as older children. to remember it when it is covered. Then researchers present
the child with a new paper strip that has two cards, and they
ask the child whether she thinks she will be able to remember
Q: Why do younger children tend to show both pictures when they are covered. As this process contin-
robust recency effects but only small primacy ues, each new strip shows a larger number of pictures, and
effects in the serial position curve? the child is again asked whether she thinks she will be able to
remember all the pictures when they are covered. Children in
Yes.
Yes.
FIGURE 10.4 Failure of metamemory in young children. Through the early elementary school years, children
are often quite poor at estimating how much they can remember. They usually are overconfident and think they can
remember far more than they can. This mismatch is thought to be caused by a failure to examine their own memory
processes, a failure of metamemory. Adapted from Yussen & Levy (1975).
MEMORY 351
the later elementary school years, like adults, tend to say that these cases, their expertise seems to enhance their memory
their memory capacities have been reached at about five or six abilities not only within their knowledge base but in other
pictures, but younger children seem much less aware of the areas as well. To investigate how and why expertise affects
limits of their memory. Many 4-year-olds happily predict that memory development, researchers studied child experts by
they will remember as many as ten pictures. Yet, when they redesigning some classic experiments that had been used to
are asked to recall the pictures later, they typically remember test adult expertise and memory.
only two or three (Flavell et al., 1970; Yussen & Levy, 1975). The original studies of adults tested the assumption that
Given that younger children more often think that they the superior chess skills of grandmasters were based on their
will be able to remember without any help, they may not exceptional memory capacities (De Groot, 1965). Research-
realize when they would benefit from strategies like verbal ers hypothesized that grandmasters excelled at chess because
rehearsal. Furthermore, if they try to use strategies but still they had a general ability to store far more information
fail to remember, they may disregard the strategies with- than other players. The studies tested both grandmasters
out realizing that they can become more beneficial with and novices in their ability to remember configurations of
repeated use. Thus, children younger than about age 7 chess pieces after a brief glimpse at the board. When the
may show a production deficit partly because of a failure of pieces were arranged as they would be in the middle of a
metamemory—that is, a lack of understanding that many game, the assumption seemed to hold. Grandmasters often
strategies yield more benefits with practice. remembered perfectly the positions of 20 pieces after only
Following the pattern we have seen in many other aspects a few seconds of viewing, whereas novices and intermediate
of cognition, young children do not completely lack the abil- players usually made more than a dozen mistakes. But the
ity to use memory strategies or an awareness of what they can crucial experimental manipulation involved testing both
remember. Some studies have found that even 2- and 3-year- groups’ memories for chess pieces arranged at random, so
olds have some capability to use strategies and some basic that the positions had no possible relation to an ongoing
sense of their own memory functions, but children become game. In these cases, the grandmasters hardly did better
far more knowledgeable and effective in these areas as they get than the novices at remembering the positions of the chess
older (Bjorklund & Douglas, 1997; Joyner & Kurtz-Costes, pieces (De Groot, 1965; see also Chase & Simon, 1973).
1997; Kail, 1990; Miller et al., 1994). What develops seems to Thus, grandmasters performed well in the first task
be a group of related abilities, including an understanding of because they could use their rich knowledge of chess and
the idea of memory, familiarity with relevant memory strate- of familiar game patterns to store many positions in terms
gies, and an awareness that practicing these strategies helps of the meaningful relations between pieces—that is, they
make them more effective. In addition, the capacity to form could use chunking to remember where the chess pieces
richer memories with more details and internal structure may would be in a typical game. When the pieces were arranged
help foster metamemory in its own right by providing cues randomly, the grandmasters could not exploit their back-
to the quality of the memory and how it was acquired (Ceci ground knowledge, and they performed much like novices.
et al., 2010). Finally, as discussed next, expertise in a specific These findings revealed that grandmasters’ skills are not
knowledge area can strongly influence memory as well. based on broadly superior memory capacities but on their
deep knowledge base, which helps them store information
relevant to their area of expertise much more efficiently.
This finding raised the question of whether very young
Q: How is the development of metamemory children who are chess experts, some of whom have
related to the development of memory played over 1,000 hours of chess by age 5 or 6, would
strategies? show similar memory skills within their area of expertise
(see Figure 10.5). A series of studies used memory tasks to
compare the memory skills of these precocious chess players
with those of other children their age (Chi, 1977, 1978). In
Memory and Expertise fact, the child experts differed from child novices in the
While young children typically perform worse than adults same way that adult grandmasters differed from adult nov-
in experimental tasks that require remembering arbitrary ices. Child experts significantly outperformed their peers
lists, they may have less difficulty learning and remem- at remembering arrangements of chess pieces that would be
bering information that they find more meaningful and meaningful in a game, but they did the same as child nov-
interesting. With practice, some children can gain truly ices when remembering randomly arranged pieces. They
impressive expertise in specific areas of knowledge such as also performed at novice level in memory tasks unrelated
chess. They may then become able to recognize patterns to chess, including remembering simple strings of num-
and contexts, which can greatly improve their memory. In bers or configurations of game pieces on unfamiliar board
MEMORY 353
basic code or format of memory is more difficult to prove, Neural Change Hypothesis The second explanation for
but in a revised form, it may be critical to understanding why we lack memories for early experiences is the neural
infantile amnesia. change hypothesis. As described earlier, the hippocam-
pus plays a role in transferring information from working
memory into long-term memory, as well as helping set up a
Q: What are three different accounts of infantile system for retrieving and integrating memory traces distrib-
uted throughout the cerebral cortex. These processes occur
amnesia? even without explicitly using strategies, and they presum-
ably occur throughout our adult lives. The neural change
Memory Format Change Hypothesis The first hypoth- hypothesis suggests that some brain structures involved in
esis is the memory format change hypothesis, which says memory, including the hippocampus and certain frontal
that the memory format or code changes, so that memories lobe regions, must mature before they can set up and main-
formed very early in life become inaccessible to older children tain permanent memory stores, particularly those that sup-
and adults. This hypothesis seems to fit well with some of the port episodic memories. Especially in the first year of life,
classic stage theories of cognitive development. Recall that some hippocampal regions seem to be quite immature and
many of these stage theories held that a child’s way of think- perhaps unable to help form stable memories of conscious
ing undergoes a global shift during the preschool years—for experiences (Nelson, 1997; Serres, 2001). This may help
example, from preverbal visual imagery to thought processes explain why there is infantile amnesia for episodic memories
embedded in language. Imagine, for example, that a 2-year- before the hippocampus matures.
old represents the experience of a party in a purely image-like Frontal lobe development may continue to influence
way (for example, remembering a set of images of balloons memory functions in children well past the age of 2 because
and clowns and cake at a party) and then as a 4-year-old shifts frontal regions are involved in many strategic aspects of
to more sentence-like thoughts (for example, remembering memory, such as monitoring the contents of working
stories about what happened at a party) as her language skills memory and knowing when to use strategies. The prefron-
improve. If development follows this course, then when she tal regions and other areas of the frontal lobes continue to
is older, she may not be able to use her language-bound way mature during the elementary school years and even into
of thinking to recall her early image-based memories, since adolescence. The development of frontal regions during the
these images would be stored in a different format that she preschool years may help support source monitoring, narra-
would no longer be able to access. Indeed, if such a dramatic tive language skills, and the ability to focus on relevant cues
shift in thought and memory occurs, it could potentially in ways that create more robust episodic memories that can
explain amnesia for early experiences. As we have now seen overcome infantile amnesia.
in several chapters, however, children do not seem to undergo
the kind of across-the-board mental metamorphoses that ear-
lier theorists envisioned. Q: What role might the frontal lobes play in the
Nonetheless, the memory format change hypothesis may development of memory skills?
still be useful for understanding cases in which memory
encoding involves language. Some current theorists argue that
Cueing Hypothesis The third explanation, the cueing
language development might bring with it a new way of for-
hypothesis, emphasizes how the ability to cue, or trig-
matting knowledge and structuring recall and that these new
ger, memories changes with age in ways that may make
ways of thinking might make preverbal memories extremely
very early memories inaccessible. Effective cues often take
difficult to access (Fivush & Schwarzmueller, 1998; Nelson,
the same form as the memory itself: verbal memories are
1993). This language-based proposal is similar to the mem-
generally cued by words, phrases, and stories, while non-
ory format change hypothesis, but it does not suggest such an
verbal memories might be cued by a taste, a smell, or an
all-or-none shift in recall, especially given that language skills
image. There is ample evidence that young infants register
do not develop abruptly, as in a stage change, but become
memories of experiences in nonverbal forms and can recall
gradually more elaborated over time (Fivush, 2011).
these memories months later if the right cues are present.
In one study, researchers assessed infants’ long-term non-
verbal delayed recall through their deferred imitation of
Q: What is the main problem with the original actions they had observed in the past. In the study, 14- and
memory format change explanation of infantile 16-month-old infants observed an actor perform particular
amnesia? actions on four objects and then were presented with these
objects (the cues) after a 2-month or 4-month delay. After
MEMORY 355
interactions focused on narrating experiences, the research- ing improvements in their recall of experiences that they had
ers predicted that Koreans would have later first memories several months earlier (Boland et al., 2003; McGuigan &
than Americans. The same study also tested predictions Salmon, 2004; Reese & Newcombe, 2007; see Scientific
about the effects of birth order and gender. The research- Method box). These experimental studies therefore strongly
ers predicted that firstborns, who receive more undivided support the idea that cultural differences in the ways par-
parental attention, would develop narrative skills earlier ents reminisce with their children contribute to differences
and thereby retain earlier first memories. Similarly, they in early recall.
expected that girls, who tend to have more verbal inter-
actions with parents, would have earlier first memories
than boys. The cross-cultural prediction was borne out— Q: How do early interactions with parents
Americans’ first memories tended to be earlier than those affect the development of long-term conscious
of Koreans—and there was some evidence for the expected memory for experiences?
birth order and gender differences as well. More recent
studies have also supported these cross-cultural and gender
differences in the age of earliest memories (Conway et al.,
2005; Fiske & Pillemer, 2006; MacDonald et al., 2000;
Wang, 2003, 2007; Wang et al., 2000, 2004, 2011). The Emergence of Long-Term
effects also hold when older children, rather than adults, Autobiographical Memory
are asked to recall their earliest memories. Here, too, North
Current researchers believe that explaining the emer-
American children report earlier memories than Chinese
gence of long-lasting episodic memories (and the dis-
children (Peterson et al., 2009), suggesting that the effects
appearance of infantile amnesia) requires taking into
of cultural differences do not arise from changes to early
account multiple factors (Nelson & Fivush, 2004). The
memories in adulthood.
best available evidence suggests that beyond the first year
Besides the tendency of American parents to talk with
of life, the brain is mature enough to avoid imposing
their children in detail about their experiences, a second,
absolute limits on episodic memory, rendering the neural
broader cross-cultural influence on early memories relates
systems hypothesis less important (Ceci et al., 2010;
to the ways children learn to see themselves. Western
Nelson, 1997). Starting at around age 1 and continu-
cultures tend to have an individualistic view of identity,
ing for several years, some closely related neural changes
emphasizing people’s individuality and autonomy more
support the emergence of long-lasting episodic memory
than their group membership. This tendency may make
by influencing the ability to encode and cue memories.
children in Western cultures more inclined to view them-
As long-lasting episodic memories emerge, they also
selves as autonomous early on and may lead them to pay
support stronger autobiographical memories, which
more attention to their own memory timelines, rather than
incorporate episodic memories of one’s journey through
thinking of themselves first and foremost as members of
life as well as linking those episodic memories to seman-
a larger collective with shared group memories (Cohen &
tic memories to make them more richly textured. Four
Gunz, 2002; Ross & Wang, 2010). Thus, the greater indi-
especially important influences on the emergence of epi-
vidualist tendencies in Western cultures may cause an
sodic memory and autobiographical memory need to be
earlier sensitivity to the autobiographical sense of self in
considered:
memory.
Correlational studies, like the one comparing Koreans 1. Explicit rehearsal of past events. Between the ages
and Americans, show that young children with more expo- of about 3 and 5, children become more inclined to
sure to adults’ narratives also tend to retain earlier long-term spontaneously recount past experiences, either to oth-
memories of experiences, but they cannot explain the reason ers or to themselves. Such rehearsals are less common
for the difference. Further evidence from experimental stud- in younger children. As children recount experiences
ies suggests that increased exposure to narrative structures more often, they show greatly improved memory for
can actually cause early improvements in memory. These the events they have talked about. In contrast, newly
causal effects have been demonstrated in studies that assign verbal children rarely engage in such rehearsals sponta-
young preschoolers’ mothers either to an experimental neously, and this lack could contribute significantly to
group, in which they are trained to elaborate on their recol- infantile amnesia.
lections of past events with their child, or to a control group, 2. Development of narrative skills. In the same time
in which they receive no training. Even when the mothers frame, between about ages 3 and 5, children’s narra-
receive only brief training, they spend more time talking tive skills also greatly improve. More specifically, chil-
about events with their young preschoolers, who show last- dren become increasingly attuned to the three aspects
of narrative structure shown in Table 10.1 (Haden better narrative skills. Social skills for sharing memo-
et al., 1997; see also Fivush, 2011). They become bet- ries develop dramatically between the ages of 3 and 5 as
ter able to use orienting information to provide con- children become more adept at communicating ideas to
text, such as where an event happened or who was others.
present. They incorporate more referential detail, such 4. Development of a sense of self. A child’s emerging sense of
as information about the color or size of an object or self may provide important support for the development of
animal, to describe entities in the narrative. And they long-term episodic memories and the emergence of auto-
include more evaluative information that conveys their biographical memory. When children are able to think
own responses to events, such as mentioning that an about their own past as situated on a continuous time-
incident was funny or frightening. Using these narra- line, they can see how personal events along that timeline
tive features to recount an event soon after it happens relate to each other and to experiences shared with oth-
improves memory encoding, and it also aids retrieval ers. Nonetheless, a sense of self like this takes consider-
by providing a more elaborate, detailed framework for able time to develop over the course of the preschool and
later cueing. elementary school years. As we will see in Chapter 13,
3. Social sharing of memories. Talking about past events with the development of the self occurs on several interrelated
others makes those events easier to recall (Fivush, 2011; dimensions, rather than as one discrete achievement.
Nelson, 1993). Moreover, conversations create another
means of cueing, since others can now remind you These four influences together support a complex abil-
of the events and thereby trigger more rehearsals and ity: a narrative way of encoding and remembering. These
MEMORY 357
Narrative Feature Function Elaborated Examples Unelaborated Versions
Orienting information Provides context for where an “I was in the hospital with my parents and “I saw my sister.”
event happened and who was my Uncle Bob and saw my sick sister.”
involved “I was at the zoo with my preschool group “I saw a monkey.”
in the morning and saw a monkey.”
Referential detail Describes physical properties of “It was a big black horse covered with “It was a horse.”
important entities in the narrative sweat.”
“He had a bright red shiny truck that made “He had a truck.”
a squeaking sound.”
Evaluative information Conveys the narrator’s own “We walked into a very scary and creepy “We were in a graveyard.”
response to an event graveyard.”
“I saw a very, very funny movie.” “I saw a movie.”
TABLE 10.1 Narrative skills and the development of episodic and autobiographical memories. Older children tend to elaborate much
more than younger children when describing their experiences. And when children elaborate, they tend to have much more accurate, detailed
recall of the events. Adapted from Haden et al. (1997) and Nelson & Fivush (2004).
Processes
internal to
the child
Semantic memory Episodic memory
Autobiographical
memory
Semantic memory Episodic memory
External
socio-
cultural
processes
Social sharing and
Narrative skills:
conversations about
Use of various techniques to
the past, present, and future:
describe the setting, provide
Initially are simple,
details about referents, and
unelaborated minimalist
offer evaluative information
descriptions that are hard
about aspects of an event
to elicit
ATTENTION 359
DEVELOPMENT AND SOCIAL P OLICY
Children as Witnesses
S
ome crimes involve children, either as victims or as 1993; Ceci et al., 1987; Goodman & Clarke-Stewart, 1991; Prin-
witnesses. In both cases, the child may have memories cipe & Schindewolf, 2012).
of the event that either the defense or the prosecution In one study, the way in which 5-year-olds were asked
may want to use to support their case. To what extent about their visit to the pediatrician for a tetanus shot led them
are these memories reliable? Are young children’s memory abili- to develop false memories of the encounter (Bruck et al.,
ties so different from those of older children and adults that they 1995). Children who were told that the shot hurt were more
should be prevented from testifying in court? These are high- likely to falsely remember experiencing pain and engaging in
stakes questions for anyone who takes part in legal proceedings actions that never actually occurred, such as crying after the
involving children (Ceci & Bruck, 1995; Klemfuss & Ceci, 2012). shot. Misleading information about what the pediatrician and
One crucial question concerns children’s suggestibility. How the office assistant did during the visit also resulted in false
easily can adults alter children’s memories or even implant false memories. Similar effects of suggestibility happen in more
ones? A child’s responses may be based on either compliance casual, everyday circumstances as well. Thus, as a group of
(when faced with an interviewer who engages in very directive preschoolers start to talk among themselves about a recent
or forceful questioning) or on the creation of false memories experience, their conversations may suggest false events that
(Fivush, 1994b). For example, in response to questions that lead to distorted memories (Principe & Ceci, 2002). Children
are directed toward eliciting particular answers, a child might ranging from preschool age to adolescence are therefore often
say something happened simply to please an interviewer with- quite susceptible to the implantation of false memories (Bruck
out having any new memories of what happened. Later, in a et al., 2002; Ceci & Bruck, 1993; Goodman & Melinder, 2007;
less stressful setting, the child might indicate that the story she Holliday et al., 2002). Interestingly, in at least some instances,
agreed to earlier was not what she really remembered. In con- memories created by suggestions can be more detailed and
trast, it is possible to implant truly novel false memories, so that elaborate than real memories (Bruck et al., 2002). Such dif-
a child believes that she has had an experience that she never ferences in detail, however, are only somewhat more likely for
actually had. false memories, so the amount of detail should not be used
In various studies, researchers have demonstrated this to discount or accept the validity of any particular memory in
implantation effect (Brainerd & Reyna, 2005; Ceci & Bruck, the courtroom.
2004; Rueda et al., 2004). These three networks show differ- ter acetylcholine. Developmentally, the orienting network
ent developmental patterns, are based in different parts of the is fully operational in infancy and changes little thereafter.
brain, tend to use different neurotransmitters, and are related
to different loci on the human genome (Fosella et al., 2002; Alerting Arousing the attentional system through a cue that
Rueda et al., 2004; see Figure 10.7). We will briefly consider both indicates that a target stimulus is about to occur and
orienting and alerting first, before looking in more detail at that gives some information about the target is known as
the complex developmental story of executive functioning. alerting. Alerting occurs, for example, when a child notices
that a changing traffic light predicts when and where cer-
Orienting Drawing attention to a particular region is called tain cars will start to move and then looks to that location in
orienting. Thus, orienting occurs when a baby moves her anticipation of the move at an appropriate time. The alert-
eyes and turns her head to look at a blinking light or stays ing network tends to be more prominent in the brain’s left
oriented in that direction in anticipation of another blink. hemisphere, especially in the frontal and parietal areas of the
Orienting appears to be the most primitive attentional net- cortex, and in the thalamus (Petersen & Posner, 2012). It is
work and the earliest to develop. The orienting network associated with the neurotransmitter norepinephrine.
involves a large number of brain regions at both the cortical The alerting system is present in infancy, but it under-
level (the superior parietal lobes and the frontal eye fields) goes considerable refinement during the early elementary
and the subcortical level (the superior colliculus and the school years, with corresponding improvements in a range
pulvinar nucleus). It is associated with the neurotransmit- of alerting tasks (Ahissar, 1999). In one study, children were
presented with a cue that indicated the location where a Executive Functioning In its broadest sense, execu-
target figure would appear. The researchers then measured tive functioning is defined as the collection of cognitive
children’s speed at detecting the object when it appeared. activities involved in goal-directed tasks and problem solv-
When there were valid cues indicating where a target would ing. Descriptions of executive functioning often emphasize
appear, such cues speeded up performance equally well in three component processes: inhibitory control, error correc-
both 8- and 10-year-olds. The ability to shift attention when tion/shifting, and working memory. All three work together
cued seemed to emerge during the early elementary years in the development of attention, but inhibitory control and
and to heavily involve the parietal cortex, which matures error correction/shifting are especially important. The exec-
relatively early in development (Goldberg et al., 2001). By utive functioning network tends to involve frontal areas and
the time children are 8 or older, the cueing system seems the middle of the brain, including such areas as the anterior
to be functioning very effectively, although the ability to cingulate cortex and the lateral prefrontal cortex, as well as
ignore distractors takes longer to develop. including the basal ganglia. The relatively late maturation
of the prefrontal cortex is thought to explain the later devel-
opment of executive functioning (Goldman-Rakic et al.,
Q: What are three distinct components to 1997). It is associated with the neurotransmitter dopamine.
attentional abilities that seem to show their own Executive functioning develops significantly during the
developmental trajectories? elementary and middle school years (Best & Miller, 2010;
Diamond, 2013; Garon et al., 2008; Klenberg et al., 2001,
ATTENTION 361
Anterior Frontal
Frontal cingulate eye field
area gyrus
Superior
Prefrontal parietal lobe
cortex
Posterior
area
Alerting network
Thalamus
Temporoparietal
Pulvinar junction
Superior
colliculus
Alerting structures
Orienting network
Orienting structures
Executive structures
A
FIGURE 10.7 Three attentional networks in the brain. (A) The
anatomy of the orienting (red), alerting (blue), and executive
functioning (green) networks, as shown in a left view of the brain. (B)
These three networks show different developmental trajectories, dif-
ferent neurotransmitter usage, and different patterns of brain activa-
tion, as shown in these fMRI scans of the brain. Adapted from Posner Executive network
& Rothbart (2007) and Fan et al. (2005). B
Zelazo & Mueller, 2002). We can see the influence of execu- Younger children did not systematically search correspond-
tive functioning most clearly in studies on the development ing windows on the two displays, which made them much
of attentional strategies. For example, in tasks that require less efficient and more error-prone. Older children quickly
judging whether two complex visual patterns are identical, realized that it was best to look at one pair of corresponding
it is useful to divide each pattern into subregions and then windows at a time, and their eye movements revealed this
compare corresponding subregions sequentially. But some- strategy. This study provides interesting information about
times a strategy may be simple, such as turning our head the development of attention as well as the planning skills
so that a distracting display is no longer in view. As with that are often a key part of attention.
memory, using these strategies effectively partially depends These sorts of attentional tasks often show dramatic
on an increasing awareness of how our own mind works— developmental changes; indeed, younger children seem
in this case, learning what attention is and how it can be to have a much weaker understanding of what attention
overwhelmed. is and how to monitor it (Miller & Bigi, 1979; Miller &
In one classic study of the development of attentional Weiss, 1982; Miller & Zalenski, 1982). In contrast, older
strategies, Elianne Vurpillot asked children to compare two children and adults know that certain distracting environ-
pictures of houses and to say if they were identical (Vurpil- ments are poor places to do challenging work. They also
lot, 1968). The two houses had similar layouts, but they had can usually tell when their attentional capacities are being
different features in some windows. Vurpillot monitored the taxed too heavily and will change the task or use strategies,
children’s performance by tracking their eye movements. if possible, to reduce the load. With age, children develop
With development, children showed strong improvement a richer understanding of the aspects of their mind that
in both their speed and accuracy in performing the task. attention involves. As they gain this understanding, their
Most interesting, however, was the difference in children’s use of attentional strategies also advances (Welch-Ross &
use of planning strategies for examining the pictures. Miller, 2000).
ATTENTION 363
was later published and translated into many languages and cally has trouble doing well in class, even though teachers
is often regarded as the first published description of what may feel that he is quite bright. He seems unable to concen-
is now called attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder trate on the task at hand or to stop himself from looking
(ADHD). In Hoffmann’s poem, Philip ignores adults’ scold- elsewhere (American Psychiatric Association, 1999; Bark-
ing as he “wriggles and giggles” at the dinner table, “swings ley, 1990). Along with these difficulties in paying attention,
backwards and forwards and tilts up his chair” until it starts children with ADHD may also have trouble moderating
to fall over, then “catches at the cloth” and crashes to the or managing their attention. In some cases, a child who
floor, pulling the tablecloth and the glasses and dishes down usually has trouble focusing may become too obsessively
with him (Hoffmann, 1845/1995; see Figure 10.9). The focused on a task and may be unable to disengage to
uncanny resemblance of these behaviors to those observed respond to parents or teachers (Barkley, 1990; Neuwirth,
by modern clinicians suggests that ADHD may have a long 1994). ADHD is normally classified into three subtypes:
history, especially among young boys (Barkley, 1998b). Yet, inattention, hyperactivity, or combined—a combination
diagnoses of ADHD have greatly increased only recently, of symptoms of the first two types (Dalsgaard, 2013;
as has the use of drugs to treat it. Today in North America, Frick & Nigg, 2012). ADHD is diagnosed if the symptoms
ADHD is diagnosed more often than any other childhood are impairing and occur in two or more settings. Children
psychological problem, and by some accounts, more than need not have every possible symptom of inattention or
all other childhood psychological problems combined. In hyperactivity to be diagnosed into a subtype, but they must
some communities and groups, more than 20 percent of display a substantial and sustained set of symptoms (at least
children have this diagnosis (Carey, 2003). ADHD is most six out of nine possible symptoms) to be so classified. Many
often diagnosed as children progress through the early years studies have repeatedly found evidence for the two sub-
of schooling (ages 5 to 10), but it is also diagnosed in older types of inattention and hyperactivity, as well as evidence
children and adults. In fact, the College Board, which pre- that they are often correlated in a manner that suggests a
pares the SAT, reported that the number of high school common core deficit (Toplak et al., 2012; Wilcutt et al.,
students diagnosed with ADHD—who thereby qualified 2012). Distinct genetic pathways also seem to be associated
for extra time on standardized tests—doubled in one year with the two subtypes (Nikolas & Burt, 2010). Both boys
(Gross, 2002; Vatz & Weinberg, 1994). and girls are diagnosed with ADHD, but the core behaviors
ADHD is defined by several clusters of behaviors that and the diagnosis are more common in boys than in girls
may begin as early as infancy and can continue into adult- (Gershon, 2002; Rucklidge, 2010).
hood. The core symptoms are inattention, hyperactivity,
and impulsivity. The child is fidgety, often running around
the classroom, and seems to have poor impulse control. He Q: What are the typical characteristics of a
is more likely to speak without thinking and to interrupt child with ADHD?
others. Teachers often consider him to be problematic, very
messy, “all over the place,” and difficult to handle. He typi-
Researchers have described potential precursors of
ADHD, including links to some aspects of infant tempera-
ment (see Chapter 7). For example, it has been reported that
children with ADHD are more likely to have been “dif-
ficult” infants who had irregular daily routines and sleep
habits, strong emotional responses to events, and high lev-
els of activity (Nigg et al., 2004; Olson et al., 2002). The
neurological bases of this relationship are not yet clear,
although it has been noted that children with ADHD have
had higher instances of birth trauma and serious illnesses,
including high fevers, during their infancy (Banaschewski
et al., 2005; Coghill et al., 2005).
As toddlers, children with ADHD are highly distract-
ible and have difficulties with self-regulation, includ-
ing problems inhibiting impulsive behavior and related
problems with executive functioning (Barkley, 1997). But
FIGURE 10.9 An early illustration of ADHD. This drawing shows these behaviors are usually less problematic for the child
the consequences of Fidgety Philip’s hyperactivity, as described in in the less structured environment of home or day care
the poem by Heinrich Hoffmann. From Hoffmann (1845/1995). than they will become in the more structured setting of
ATTENTION 365
inappropriate behavior (Lee et al., 2012). Child behavioral children (those who were not diagnosed with ADHD) just as
training generally focuses on developing conflict manage- much as those diagnosed with ADHD (Lewandowski et al.,
ment skills and problem-solving skills (Webster-Stratlon 2007). This finding raises questions about whether granting
et al., 2011). The younger the child, the more effective behav- some students extra time really addresses their specific deficits
ioral interventions seem to be, whereas pharmacological inter- (Franek, 2006; Freedman, 2003; Leong, 2005; Lerner, 2004).
ventions often are not effective and increase the likelihood Research has also shown higher rates of diagnosis of ADHD
of adverse effects in younger children (Charach et al., 2013). and other learning disabilities in wealthy communities
compared with other communities (Allen, 2006; Getahun
Misdiagnosis of ADHD ADHD is not a single problem. et al., 2013; Leong, 2005; Lerner, 2004). These data are
Rather, this label is often applied to a number of related prob- cause for concern, especially considering that large-scale
lems that have different underlying causes (Weinberg & Brum- assessments of young children have not shown higher inci-
back, 1992). Some people diagnosed with ADHD may have dences of ADHD in the children of more affluent families.
smaller working memories; others may have trouble trans- In fact, the incidence of ADHD in younger children is usu-
ferring information from working memory into long-term ally higher in poor communities and among disadvantaged
memory; still others may have more limited metacognitive minorities (Biederman & Faraone, 2005).
skills, making them less efficient at using attentional strategies. The many potential causes for the misdiagnosis of ADHD
Each component of attention, including the subcomponents of should not be taken as arguments against the existence of
executive functioning, is a potential source of attentional diffi- real neurocognitive and attention-related deficits that make
culties. Moreover, when there is hyperactivity, it may arise from it much more difficult for some children to function effec-
a huge number of variables that lead to high levels of activity tively in cognitively challenging settings. And children with
and not just from ADHD. Thus, there is great potential for these deficits typically need quite different interventions
misdiagnosis of ADHD because some factors that really should than children who are having problems for other reasons.
not be considered part of an attentional deficit can cause prob- It is essential not to leap to one diagnosis, such as ADHD,
lems similar to those associated with ADHD. At the cognitive without closely examining the evidence and other possible
level, for example, a child may process information slowly, lead- causes. Interventions can then be tailored to be more sensi-
ing him to fall behind in class and then have trouble focusing tive to the needs of each child.
because he does not understand the material. Thus, even with
well-functioning attentional systems, he may still fail to attend
to information presented in class. Misdiagnosis of ADHD can
also arise from social and emotional factors. A child who is Reasoning and Thinking about
having problems at home may be preoccupied and distractible
in school. Any of these difficulties (and many others) can cause Knowledge
children to exhibit behaviors that may result in their being clas-
Improving at a particular cognitive task often depends on
sified as having ADHD.
mastering its unique features. Developing a chess strategy, for
A final reason that ADHD may be misdiagnosed is that
example, requires learning to apply the rules of the game in
a child with this diagnosis may qualify for special consider-
ways that help anticipate an opponent’s moves. Similarly, in
ations that would not be available if he were simply having
the development of reasoning and the ability to think about
academic problems (Horowitz et al., 1993; Reid & Katsiyan-
our own knowledge (that is, metacognition), domain-specific
nis, 1995). One such case involves special accommodations for
properties of the information involved can also have impor-
high-stakes tests. For example, between 1987 and 2001, there
tant cognitive effects. Nonetheless, there are some broad, gen-
was a 300 percent increase in the number of students who
eral principles that apply to the development of reasoning and
qualified for extra time on the SAT, even though the num-
thinking about knowledge in many different domains. We will
ber of students taking the test increased by only 18 percent
explore both the broad principles and some domain-specific
(Franek, 2006; Freedman, 2003). In many cases, the extra-
knowledge here, focusing on analogical reasoning, scientific
time requests were granted for learning disabilities far broader
reasoning, and judgments about the nature of knowledge.
in nature than ADHD, even though ADHD was most often
listed as the problem (Lerner, 2004). The extra time also
resulted in higher scores, especially on the mathematics part
of the test (Mandinach et al., 2005; Wainer et al., 2004). This Analogical Reasoning
makes sense, given that students with learning disabilities may Analogical reasoning is a way of comparing things
require extra time during testing to demonstrate their true that on the surface seem quite different in order to see
capabilities with the material. But receiving extra time on a deeper-level similarities between them. For example, anal-
large set of mathematics problems seems to benefit control ogies likening the atom to the solar system or the world
F
or over a century, it has been noted that dyslexia seems Meng et al., 2005; Schumacher et al., 2006). A second gene
to run in families (Smith et al., 1983). Twin studies known as KIAA0319 also seems to affect neural migration dur-
and other methods have found high heritability rates ing brain development (Fisher & Francks, 2006; Schumacher
(DeFries & Gillis, 1993). In addition, as children pro- et al., 2007). The story becomes more complicated, however,
gress from learning to read to increasingly using reading to learn, as the involvement of genes on several other chromosomes
shared genotypes are by far the strongest predictors of levels has also been suggested (Gayán et al., 1999; Giraud & Ramus,
of reading ability (Harlaar et al., 2007). With advances in our 2013; Grigorenko et al., 1997; Hannula-Jouppi et al., 2005;
understanding of the human genome, it has been possible to find Kere, 2011; Nopola-Hemmi et al., 2001; Scerri et al., 2010). It is
specific gene loci that are significantly correlated with dyslexia. too early to tell whether these different genetic loci correspond
The genetic studies clearly indicate that there is not a simple to different subtypes of dyslexia or whether different combina-
gene locus for dyslexia. As in almost all cases of genetic influ- tions of genetic loci produce the subtypes.
ences, several gene loci and their interactions seem to be impli- Of course, the involvement of specific genes in dyslexia does
cated. In general terms, the finding that multiple gene locations not mean that everyone with these genes has dyslexia; many
are involved in dyslexia offers some biological support for the do not. Nor does it mean that people with dyslexia cannot learn
idea that there are several distinct types of dyslexia, as well as to read well. With the right instruction, most people with dys-
several interacting biological components that support normal lexia can develop strategies that help make reading easier and
reading (Castles et al., 1999; Scerri et al., 2010; Shaywitz et more fluent (Shaywitz et al., 2003). Interestingly, many people
al., 2003; Shaywitz & Shaywitz, 2005). One key question is with dyslexia seem to recruit brain regions other than the ones
how closely various reading problems—such as problems with used by nondyslexic readers to compensate for their reading
phonological awareness, short-term verbal memory, or rapid difficulties. For example, reading processes and dyslexic defi-
naming of pictures, colors, and letters, among others—are cits usually occur in the left hemisphere, but in some people
associated with specific genetic signatures. with dyslexia, right hemisphere areas may be used during
In the search for genes associated with dyslexia, research- reading to compensate for the deficits (Gabrieli, 2009). Cer-
ers have found one especially prominent locus in a gene called tain approaches to reading instruction may help accelerate this
DCDC2 on chromosome 6. This gene seems to influence how neural compensation process (Shaywitz et al., 2003). Some
new neurons move to their target locations during brain devel- researchers hope that specific genetic signatures will corre-
opment. A DCDC2 variant that causes abnormal neuron migra- spond closely enough with particular reading difficulties that
tion in which neurons end up in inappropriate cortical locations these genetic differences may even provide guidelines for tailor-
is unusually common among people with dyslexia (Kere, 2011; ing individualized approaches to teaching reading.
and instruction. By one estimate, a fifth-grader who does guistic codes that correspond to letters and words (Snowl-
not have dyslexia may read outside of the classroom in 2 days ing, 2001; Ziegler & Goswami, 2005). Children and adults
as many words as a fifth-grader with dyslexia reads in a year who have such difficulties may have less-developed capaci-
(Gabrieli, 2009). ties in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. This region
A large proportion of children with dyslexia do espe- shows no increased activation in people with dyslexia
cially poorly on phonological awareness tasks (Snowling, when they engage in word rhyming tasks, while the same
2001; Ziegler & Goswami, 2005). Some people with dys- region does show more activation when typically develop-
lexia may have difficulty correctly perceiving sounds that ing children engage in the same tasks (Kovelman et al.,
occur close together in time, as they have deficits in detect- 2012). As the New Directions in Developmental Research
ing rapidly changing sensory stimuli (Tallal, 2000). But in box describes, genetic and neuropsychological models of
most cases, the problem on phonological awareness tasks dyslexia are starting to support the idea that there may
does not seem to be with initial perceptual processing of also be distinct types of dyslexia that arise from different
sounds, but with translating sounds into the efficient lin- mechanisms.
strategies
Reconstructing
with development in the area of mathematical
strategies reasoning skills?
Counting-on
Counting-all strategies
strategies
As we have seen, the development of mathematical reason-
Small Age ing involves the emergence of several different component
amount skills, and the best math teachers must address all these com-
FIGURE 10.13 Mathematical strategies in development. As ponents at the same time. Even within a single component
a child develops mathematical problem-solving strategies, several skill, such as choosing between problem-solving strategies,
possible approaches to any given task are available in his repertoire. teachers can foster understanding at several levels, rang-
Here we see different strategies for addition problems. The child uses
ing from rote strategy performance to more sophisticated
a series of overlapping strategies on any given problem, but the ten-
dency to use particular approaches or strategies changes with age, conceptual understanding of the different approaches to
typically shifting toward more efficient strategies. Nonetheless, even a problem. Teaching math well is challenging, and there
adults sometimes use early-developing strategies, such as counting- are considerable variations in teacher effectiveness, not only
on strategies. Adapted from Hopkins & Lawson (2002). across schools, but also across countries.
A B C
FIGURE 10.14 Studying model use with the “shrinking room.” (A) After observing an object being hidden in a small model of a room,
children younger than about 3 often have difficulty locating a corresponding object in the real room. When 3-year-old children are told that the
model is the real room after it was “shrunk” by (B) a “shrinking machine,” they can better understand the relation between the model and the
real room and locate the object in the real room (C).
STU DY AN D REVIEW
dren and adults have biologically based attention-related dis- oped through extensive exposure to the links between the
orders, but because of the many reasons that ADHD may be sound patterns of a language and the visual symbols that rep-
misdiagnosed, there may be considerably fewer people with resent those patterns.
the disorder than the number currently diagnosed and medi- ● Deficits in phonological processing, despite normal intelli-
cated for it. gence, good motivation to read, and early exposure to read-
ing, account for most cases of dyslexia. But many children
Reasoning and Thinking about Knowledge with dyslexia can learn to read well, even though they typi-
● Many kinds of reasoning abilities undergo important devel- cally find the process more difficult than do children who do
opmental changes during the elementary and middle school not have dyslexia.
years. These changes do not cause qualitative differences in ● The development of mathematical reasoning seems best
the types of reasoning children can use. Instead, develop- understood not as a series of stages of mathematical ability,
ment seems to proceed more on a domain-by-domain basis, but as the development of more flexible and adaptive ways of
as increasingly sophisticated knowledge in one area can then using mathematics-related strategies.
support more advanced forms of reasoning. ● The ability to use maps and models as representations of lay-
● Early views held that young children could not reason in out develops a great deal during the preschool and early ele-
terms of analogies, but later studies showed clear evidence mentary school years. Younger children seem to have trouble
of analogical reasoning skills in preschoolers when they were understanding how a small model of a room can represent
asked about areas in which they had well-developed knowl- the room and yet be different from it.
edge. In contrast, when children (or adults) know very little ● Children’s drawing ability also undergoes major changes
about a particular area, they will be unable to reason about during the preschool and elementary school years. Improve-
it by using analogies, a skill that depends on understand- ments depend on developments in children’s motor abilities,
ing important relationships between different domains and sense of how to represent three-dimensional images, detailed
using information about one domain to understand corre- knowledge about the things they draw, and sense of cultural
sponding information in another domain. norms.
B
Opposition to the Eleven Plus grew over time, and peo-
Plus exam provoked waves of anxiety in generations ple began to question whether it provided an equally accu-
of 11-year-olds until its widespread use was discon- rate measurement of the abilities of children from different
tinued in 1976. The Eleven Plus exam was a screen- socioeconomic classes and minority groups. Some also wor-
ing test used to determine what kind of school a ried that the test favored children who matured early over
child would attend thereafter (see Figure 11.1). Moreover, it those who did not hit their stride academically until their
was clearly meant to be a test of intelligence. One of its chief teen years. As a result of this opposition, the Eleven Plus
designers, the British educational psychologist Cyril L. Burt, was scrapped as a national test in the United Kingdom in
was obsessed with the idea of measuring intelligence to iden- 1976. Since then, the distinctions between types of British
tify people of different intellectual abilities. Burt was accused schools have become less rigid, and most British school sys-
of fabricating data to support his views that intelligence is tems now use a variety of measures to assess children over a
inherited (Gould, 1981; Kamin, 1974), an accusation that longer period.
was subsequently contested (Mackintosh, 1995). The Eleven Plus story is not over, however. The test is
The Eleven Plus worked much like a real-life version still used in several UK counties and in much of Northern
of the Sorting Hat used in Harry Potter books. The test Ireland. Many countries that were members of the British
could heavily influence a child’s future even before the child Commonwealth, such as those in the Caribbean, also still
entered middle school. Depending on test scores, children give most students a version of the Eleven Plus, often under
were “streamed” either into an elite educational track or different names, such as the Common Entrance Exami-
into tracks leading toward much less prestigious futures. nation. When members of those communities are polled
Children who scored in approximately the top 20 percent about whether the test should be discontinued, as it was in
on the Eleven Plus were sent to grammar school—the first the United Kingdom, many people favor its preservation.
step leading to a top university and a high-ranking profes- They argue that it helps identify talented children, regard-
sion. The other 80 percent of children soon came to be seen less of class or background, and guides them toward needed
as having “failed” the Eleven Plus, and from age 11 on, they educational resources (Times of London, 2005). The Eleven
were likely destined for blue-collar or other nonprofessional Plus story powerfully demonstrates how views of intelli-
jobs. Although it was possible for a child who “failed” the gence can be translated into public policies that influence
Eleven Plus but performed exceedingly well in school to be the education and intellectual development of millions of
moved up to the grammar school track after age 11, this children.
rarely happened. In several earlier chapters, we considered cognitive devel-
opment primarily in terms of common developmental pat-
terns, focusing on how all children come to understand and
think about the world. Similarly, in this chapter, we will con-
sider some common patterns that theorists have proposed
about how intelligence develops. Some of these ideas, like
the notion that children’s intelligence increases continuously
with age, often turn out to be misleading. Others, such as
ways of encouraging active learning in the classroom, seem
to yield impressive results. For much of this chapter, how-
ever, we will shift emphasis to the development of thought
and knowledge, with a focus on the differences between
individuals. Individual differences are central to the study of
intelligence, as the very notion of intelligence assumes that
people have different levels or kinds of intelligence. In devel-
opmental research, there are two ways to compare the intel-
ligence of different individuals. We can ask how age groups
as a whole differ in terms of facets of intelligence, and we
can ask how differences in intelligence between individuals
within any age group occur. We will consider both ques-
FIGURE 11.1 The Eleven Plus exam. For these young students tions but will tend to focus on the factors that are associated
in 1950, the Eleven Plus exam, which had many of the hallmarks of with differences among children in the same age group. This
intelligence tests, could have an enormous influence on their future focus, however, raises further questions about the develop-
school choices and careers. mental processes that lead to such differences, both within
What is a taxi What is wrong with this picture [of Look through these two pages Bill is 3 years older than Bob and
used for? a girl combing her hair with a comb with images of different objects 6 years younger than Fred. If Fred
that has no teeth]? and animals and tell me how is 12, how old is Bill?
many times you see a lion.
A shoe is to a foot Can you put these 4 pictures Here is a shape. Is there another Mary won 10% more marbles
as a glove is to [showing a person getting a flat tire shape [from a sheet with many than Sue. Sue won 30 marbles.
what? and fixing it ] in the correct order? different shapes] like this one? How many marbles did Mary win?
TABLE 11.1 Items like those on the WISC-IV test. These items are similar to items used to assess children’s performance on the four
components of the WISC-IV.
68% with many other measures over the lifespan and in many
14% 14%
cases provide more accurate predictors than any other avail-
able measures. As always, “prediction” simply refers to a
population-wide correlation over time. It should not be con-
2% 2%
0.1% 95% 0.1% fused with cause, since many other variables are also at work.
One of the driving forces behind the development and
55 70 85 100 115 130 145 IQ scores
85 116 137 158 179 200 300 Weights
use of intelligence tests has been the desire to predict indi-
(in pounds) vidual success in school. Intelligence tests predict academic
−3 −2 −1 0 1 2 3
Standard deviations success, measured in terms of grades, with a correlation
FIGURE 11.3 The normal distribution curve, or bell curve. The
of approximately .50 (McGrew & Knopik, 1993; Neisser
bell curve shown here is an idealized model showing standard devia- et al., 1996; Sternberg et al., 2001). Although this correlation
tions from a mean. The numbers in gold indicate IQ scores, and the is significant, it still means that most of the individual
numbers in green indicate the weights of a group of college freshman. variation in school grades is accounted for by factors—for
Gv Gs Gm Gr Ga
Gc Gf
Broad Broad General Broad Broad Stratum II
Crystallized Fluid
visual cognitive memory and retrieval auditory (Broad Abilities)
intelligence intelligence
perception speediness learning ability perception
Lexical knowledge
Visual memory
Spatial relations
Sequential reasoning
Spatial scanning
Ideational fluency
Attention and general information
Naming facility
Perceptual speed
Memory span
Working memory
Associative memory
Speech-sound discrimination
FIGURE 11.4 Carroll’s three-stratum theory. In Carroll’s theory (CHC theory) and in related approaches, g is simply a part of a larger
system of three levels of abilities. Only part of the narrowest level is shown here. Adapted from Keith & Witta (1997).
Cattell’s Theory of Fluid and Crystallized Intelli- Fluid intelligence and crystallized intelligence reflect dis-
gence Cognitive abilities that make up intelligence can tinct types of abilities, but they are not completely indepen-
also be divided into the categories of fluid intelligence and dent. Someone with a high level of fluid intelligence is likely to
crystallized intelligence (Cattell, 1963). As originally pro- learn factual information rapidly and figure out fairly quickly
posed by the personality psychologist Raymond Cattell in how to do various tasks. Hence, he is also likely to have a
the early 1960s, fluid intelligence refers to the ability to high level of crystallized intelligence. This close relationship
think flexibly and solve novel problems. It is thought to between the two has led some to argue that the distinction is
be largely independent of acquired knowledge. In contrast, unnecessary (Johnson & Bouchard, 2005). But from a devel-
Cattell described crystallized intelligence as the ability to opmental perspective, it can be useful for describing how
use specific skills and knowledge gained through experi- intelligence changes over a person’s lifespan (Blair, 2006).
ence. Hierarchical models often treat both of these forms of Fluid intelligence is thought to develop earlier than crystal-
intelligence as parts of g. On traditional intelligence tests, lized intelligence, as the young child has not had enough
fluid intelligence is considered more closely related to test time to gain specific skills or acquire much knowledge. From
items such as novel math problems and puzzles (not simply about the late 20s onward, fluid intelligence starts to dimin-
memorized procedures or tables), while crystallized intel- ish, while crystallized intelligence continues to develop well
ligence relates more to vocabulary items and questions that into the sixth decade of life and often longer (Blair, 2006).
test general knowledge (Carroll, 1993). These changes are considered in more detail in Chapter 17.
Linguistic Good with words, language Through verbal discussions, Author, trial lawyer, journalist
play, linguistic patterns reading and writing, debates
Mathematical Good with mathematics, logic, Through quantitative tasks, Accountant, scientist,
discovering formal patterns experiments, classification computer programmer
systems
Spatial Good with visualization, maps, Through diagrams, images, Architect, artist, cartographer,
3-D models maps filmmaker
Musical Good with melodies, rhythms, Through musical media, song Musician, singer, music critic
complex musical patterns
Bodily/kinesthetic Good with complex motor Through manipulating objects, Dancer, athlete, martial arts
skills, manipulating objects acting out relations physically, performer, stage performer
hands-on learning
Interpersonal Good at sensing others’ Through rich social Community organizer,
thoughts and feelings, interactions, cooperation, politician, manager,
resolving conflicts sharing information salesperson
Intrapersonal Good at recognizing own Through setting individual Therapist, author, philosopher
abilities and accomplishments, goals, pacing oneself, often
setting reasonable goals working alone
Naturalistic Good at understanding plants, Through situations involving Park ranger, biologist,
animals, ecological relations in living things, nature webs, conservationist
nature ecological systems
TABLE 11.2 Multiple intelligences. Gardner’s eight forms of intelligence. (Based on Gardner, 1983, 2006).
A
s researchers look for intellectual abilities that are ability to perceive and identify emotions, (2) the ability to use
not measured by traditional intelligence tests, they emotions to aid thought processes, (3) the ability to understand
have shown great interest in emotional intelligence how conflicting emotions can lead to other emotions, and (4) the
(Brackett et al., 2013; Mayer et al., 2008; Salovey & ability to use emotions to achieve social goals. This description of
Mayer, 1990). Emotional intelligence is the ability to evaluate emotional intelligence carries with it a developmental hypothesis
both our own emotions and the emotions of others and to use that younger children have mastered only the less complex lev-
those evaluations to guide our thoughts and actions. Although els. In a broad sense, younger children do seem much less adept
emotional intelligence is a relatively recent concept, it has quite at understanding the nuances of social situations and knowing
old roots. In 1920, the American psychologist Edward Thorndike how to act appropriately. Nonetheless, controversy remains over
argued that a broad view of intelligence must include the notion of the best ways to break down the components of emotional intel-
social intelligence, the ability to smoothly manage social interac- ligence in children and over how to describe their developmental
tions (Thorndike, 1920). As Thorndike noticed, developing tests trajectories (Mathews et al., 2003; Zeidner et al., 2003).
for social intelligence was much more difficult than developing As noted earlier, for most individuals, intelligence test scores
tests for more analytical thinking (Thorndike, 1936; Thorndike & show a significant positive correlation between different analyti-
Stern, 1937). This difficulty has persisted (Kihlstrom & Cantor, cal abilities, which some have cited as support for the existence
2000), but there are now more attempts to develop reliable and of g, or general intelligence. This raises the question of whether
valid tests of both social and emotional intelligence. the distinctive abilities that make up emotional intelligence would
As with analytical intelligence, researchers have devised also be positively correlated. Is there a g factor for emotional intel-
different ways of subdividing emotional intelligence into more ligence analogous to that proposed for analytical intelligence?
specific abilities. In one prominent view, emotional intelligence One research project examined scores on a wide range of tasks
has five distinct components that fit into two larger catego- related to emotional intelligence, looking for positive correlations
ries of intrapersonal intelligence and interpersonal intel- (Mayer et al., 1999). These ranged from perceptual tasks, such
ligence (Salovey & Mayer, 1990). Here are the components of as the ability to recognize emotions in faces, to cognitively com-
intrapersonal intelligence: plex tasks, such as the ability to think about how emotions com-
• Self-awareness. Keeping track of our own emotions and emo- bine and lead to new emotions. When the scores of hundreds of
tional states as they occur. adults and adolescents were analyzed, there was a clear positive
• Ability to manage emotions. Knowing how to handle our emo- correlation of all the abilities. This analysis also suggested that a
tional states in an appropriate manner and understanding the single underlying factor was responsible for most of the patterns
factors that give rise to our emotional states. in the data. This evidence suggests a g-like factor, raising the pos-
• Motivation. Using our emotional states to help achieve our sibility of an “essence” for emotional intelligence that manifests in
goals and deal with impulses; may involve knowing when to a wide range of tasks (Mayer et al., 2001, 2004).
delay gratification for larger, long-term gains. Just as in the case of analytical intelligence, controversies
Here are the components of interpersonal intelligence: remain about the extent to which these positive correlations
• Empathy. Having a sense of how others might feel in a situ- definitively indicate a single, general emotional intelligence
ation, especially when their feelings might be quite different (Goldenberg et al., 2006; Palmer et al., 2005; Pérez et al.,
from our own; requires the ability to put ourselves in their 2005; Roberts et al., 2010). Nonetheless, that possibility high-
shoes. lights a developmental story in which there could be powerful
• Ability to handle relationships. Understanding the emotional individual differences in emotional intelligence from an early
tenor of a social situation and being able to respond in a man- age, perhaps arising from a core skill like identifying emotional
ner that manages emotional states in others in an effective states, which then influences all other emotional intelligence
and useful manner. tasks in the course of development. The early roots of emo-
A second view subdivides emotional intelligence into four lev- tional intelligence, as well as the way it develops; are likely to
els of increasing complexity (Mayer & Salovey, 1997): (1) the be an exciting area of research in future years.
scores. Surprisingly, several research groups have found The Heritability of Intelligence
such relationships in longitudinal studies (Bornstein et al., Long before Alfred Binet began designing the first IQ
2006; Colombo, 1993, 2004; Colombo et al., 2004; Fagan, tests, people believed that differences in intelligence were
1990; McCall & Carriger, 1993; McCall & Mash, 1995; largely the result of innate differences in intellectual pow-
Rose & Feldman, 1995; Rose et al., 2009; Slater et al., ers, even if they had no idea how those innate differences
1989). In one line of work, children’s habituation measures were physically realized in the body. More recently, those
in infancy correlated significantly with their intelligence inborn differences have been attributed to genes. There are
test scores at age 11 (Rose & Feldman, 1995; Rose et al., many strands of support for the claim that genes are one
1997, 2005, 2012). A related finding showed that children source of the differences in intelligence. But it is critical
who were more inclined to explore and seek stimulation at to remember that environmental factors are also at work,
age 3 scored significantly higher on intelligence tests at age including a variety of prenatal and postnatal biological
11 (Raine et al., 2002). influences. Genes that influence intelligence do not deter-
These studies reported correlations ranging between mine a specific level of ability; instead, they can be thought
.35 and .50, which are statistically significant predictors, of as specifying a range of skill levels that vary as a function
although the relationship is not especially strong. In statisti- of the environment. As an analogy, consider that a child of
cal terms, a correlation of .50 between infant habituation short parents might grow to heights ranging from very
measures and IQ scores at age 11 means that the correlation short to average height, depending on his diet, exercise, and
with infant measures accounts for only about 25 percent of other factors, whereas a child of tall parents might have a
the variation in childhood IQ scores. This leaves 75 percent height ranging from average height to very tall, depending
of the variation to be explained by other factors. on the same set of environmental influences.
Correlations as high as .50 are certainly interesting and As described in Chapter 2, behavior geneticists talk of the
provocative. They suggest that some key parts of intelligence heritability of a trait as a way of describing the extent to which,
may be present in young infants. Nonetheless, it would be within a particular population, the variations in that trait are
a serious mistake to conclude from these correlations that due to genetic differences rather than environmental ones.
habituation rates in infancy predetermine later intelligence The more the variations in a particular trait among a group
test scores or that intelligence is largely unchangeable over of people can be accounted for by differences in their genes,
the course of development. In addition, while there are dif- the closer that trait’s heritability approaches 1 for that popula-
ferences in intelligence scores between ethnic, racial, and tion in that environment. On the other hand, the more the
socioeconomic groups of older children and adults (as dis- trait’s variations within the group are due to environmental
cussed shortly), no such differences are found in any of the variations, the closer its heritability approaches 0. Height, for
infant measures (Fagan, 2011). This suggests that those example, has a high heritability of roughly .80 in most popu-
group differences must arise from factors other than those lations and environments, while hair length has a heritability
that are present in infants. Interestingly, stronger corre- close to 0. Thus, individual variations in height tend to be
lations with later test scores appear around the age when closely linked to genetic variation, whereas differences in hair
A B
be at the root of this mathematics disparity, since men are the social influences on sex differences in intelligence
more likely to use visual/spatial strategies on complex math should not be underestimated (Hines, 2010a, 2011; Spelke,
tasks (Halpern & LaMay, 2000). 2005). Parents interact with girls and boys quite differently,
In other memory tasks, women tend to show consistent and parents, peers, and the surrounding culture generally
advantages, outperforming men at recalling lists of words, provide girls and boys with very different environments.
numbers, and faces, among many other items. Women Starting when their children are very young, parents tend
also tend to do better on language-based tasks, whether to offer them different kinds of toys depending on their
they involve fluency, spelling, or reading comprehension sex (see Figure 11.10). If a boy is more frequently offered a
(Halpern, 2004). In addition, girls tend to start speaking truck and a girl a doll, these choices create different kinds
earlier than boys and have larger vocabularies at comparable of play experiences. Parents also tend to approve of differ-
ages (Halpern & LaMay, 2000). ent kinds of play activities for boys and girls, encourag-
There is tremendous interest in trying to link brain ing boys to engage in sports and girls to engage in more
differences to cognitive differences between the sexes social activities that might develop verbal skills (Leaper &
(Hampson & Moffet, 2004). A number of biological dif- Friedman, 2006). Of course, parents and cultures can vary
ferences between male and female brains have been docu- considerably in the extent to which they treat boys and girls
mented, many of which appear in infancy and persist differently, a topic explored in more depth in Chapters 13,
throughout the lifespan (Hines, 2011). They include a higher 14, and 15, but there are clearly many social and cultural
rate of blood flow saturated with oxygen in females (Gur & influences that could contribute to sex differences in cogni-
Gur, 1990), overall larger brains in males, different ratios of tion (Lawton, 2010).
white and gray brain matter, larger orbitofrontal regions rela- In sum, sex differences on the various components of
tive to other brain regions in females (Gur et al., 2002), and intelligence tests are attributable to a wide range of causes
differences in the thickness of the cerebral cortex in specific (Halpern et al., 2007; Lawton, 2010). Biological factors may
regions (Sowell et al., 2007). play a role, but given the potential for abuse based on such
It is difficult to decipher the connection between these alleged differences, it is critical to remain skeptical and care-
sex-related differences in brain anatomy and differences in fully consider socialization explanations as well. That is,
brain function (Cosgrove et al., 2007; Halpern et al., 2007; even if some biological sex differences can be clearly linked
Lawton, 2010). More certain, however, is the existence of to cognitive differences, this tells us little about the effects
cognitive sex differences in their own right, several of which of different learning experiences. Even if girls devote less
have been reliably demonstrated across cultures. One broad, of a particular brain region than boys to, say, visual/spatial
well-supported difference lies in the distribution of test tasks, they may simply use other regions to support the
scores between the sexes. There tend to be more males at the same level of visual/spatial skills as boys, given comparable
extremes of performance, both at the highest and the lowest encouragement and instruction. We simply know far too lit-
levels (Halpern et al., 2007; Johnson et al., 2008; Strand tle about brain structure and function to make any stronger
et al., 2006). inferences.
The emergence of some cognitive differences between Finally, it is important to remember that the research
the sexes early in life has led several researchers to think shows sex differences between population averages, not
that these differences are partially influenced by hormones between individuals. The range of variation in cognitive
(Bull et al., 2010; Levine et al., 1999). At the same time, skills among women or among men is far larger than the
differences between men and women. Therefore, it is not it becomes clear that some alleles that are more common in
possible to make any statements about any individual’s cog- certain groups are related to a greater susceptibility to cer-
nitive skills based on his or her sex. tain diseases and different responses to specific treatments
(Bamshad, 2005; Bamshad at al., 2007; Helgadottir et al.,
2006; Mountain & Risch, 2004; Rotman, 2003).
Ethnic and “Racial” Differences
The bases of sex differences in intelligence subtest scores
are controversial, but this controversy pales next to the Explaining Group Differences
heated debate over ethnic differences in test scores. Never- What do group differences in test scores mean? Some people
theless, some facts are clear. There are large differences in simply deny the results, arguing that they are fabrications
average IQ scores across ethnic groups: as much as 15 points of racist testers and researchers. This criticism seems prob-
between some samples of European Americans and African lematic, since researchers from all political persuasions and
Americans, where European Americans may have a mean backgrounds tend to find similar results with the same tests.
centering around 100 and African Americans may have a Nonetheless, it is still possible that implicit racial attitudes
mean centering around 85. Despite above-average academic are influencing test construction and design. More detailed
performance, Asian Americans have only slightly higher criticisms focus on how well intelligence tests really measure
intelligence test scores than European Americans, a finding what we think of as intelligence and whether they provide
that runs contrary to some stereotypes. Finally, Hispanic equally accurate measurements for different groups. The
Americans perform roughly in between the European- issues are complex, and much more extensive treatments
American and African-American levels. can be found elsewhere (Ceci, 1990; Fish, 2002; Gould,
It is critical to use the term ethnic group and not race 1981; Hunt & Carlson, 2007; Nisbett, 1998, 2005; Nisbett
for groups such as European Americans, African Ameri- et al., 2012; Rushton & Jensen, 2005; Sternberg, 1994). In
cans, and Asian Americans. The reason is simple: the entire the main, there are four types of explanations for the sig-
notion of racial groups is biologically suspect (Hirschfeld, nificance of group differences in IQ scores: irrelevance, bio-
1996). For example, in many analyses, African-American logical differences, socioeconomic factors, and sociocultural
groups have been shown to be made up of heterogeneous stereotypes.
peoples with much larger genetic differences among them
than there are between them and European Americans Are Group Differences Irrelevant? One frequent argu-
(Cavalli-Sforza et al., 1994; Olson, 2001). Despite com- ment is that IQ tests are largely irrelevant and that the group
mon beliefs and prejudices, ethnic groups rarely map in a differences they uncover are not meaningful for any other
straightforward way onto biological groups of humans. At kind of setting. For example, perhaps members of economi-
the same time, as we learn more about the human genome, cally disadvantaged groups are less familiar with the content
Sociocultural Stereotypes The most plausible expla- category. He contends that members of this third kind of
nations for ethnic group differences in IQ scores seem to minority group have little confidence that their academic
involve people’s views of their roles relative to the dominant efforts will be appropriately rewarded, and they may come
culture in which they live. The anthropologist John Ogbu to regard school as a futile waste of time. Perhaps the most
has offered a cross-cultural explanation emphasizing the revealing aspect of Ogbu’s analysis is that when precisely
ways that minority groups differ, not just from the majority the same racial group that constitutes this kind of trapped
group but also from each other. His theory describes three minority in one culture constitutes a more advantaged
distinct types of minority groups that tend to show sharply minority in another culture, the minority members’ aca-
different outcomes in both intelligence testing and school demic performance rises as well.
achievement (Ogbu, 1978; Ogbu & Stern, 2001). One type Social psychologists Claude Steele and Joel Aronson
of minority group chooses, on its own initiative and with- have made a related argument concerning how some groups
out economic or social penalties, to be distinct from the are influenced by their beliefs about society’s view of their
majority culture. American examples would include vari- intellectual capacities. Steele and Aronson refer to the phe-
ous religious groups, like the Amish, or those who live in nomenon of stereotype threat, in which the very fear of
communes. The second type of minority group seeks out confirming a negative stereotype leads to reduced perfor-
new opportunities through migration, despite temporarily mance on a task or test. They argue that African Americans
being at an economic disadvantage in the new culture. Such face powerful stereotypes about their not doing as well on
groups of immigrants tend to see themselves as better off for intellectual tasks. These stereotypes are not only entrenched
having decided to leave their home culture and join a new in the dominant European-American culture but well
one, in a subordinate status, with the goal of working their known to African Americans as well.
way up to affluence. In one series of studies, Steele and Aronson (1995) var-
Finally, there are minority groups whose members are ied the extent to which African-American students saw a
put in a subordinate, disadvantaged class without any challenging verbal test as truly indicative of intellectual
choice, usually from birth. They often have the feeling of ability and then compared their performance with that of
being trapped in that class. Ogbu argues that such groups European-American students with matching SAT scores.
include African Americans, lower caste members in India, Thus, some students were told that the test was a very reliable
the Maori of New Zealand, and any other groups with a measure of intellectual skills, whereas others were informed
similar sense of being forced against their will into a lower that it was simply a difficult verbal puzzle. Steele and Aronson
SCHOOLING 409
A B
FIGURE 11.17 Differences among schools. Schools throughout the world, such as (A) this madrassa in Pakistan and (B) this Montessori
school in London, can vary radically in their structure, curriculum, class size, and many other factors.
The younger boys’ instruction largely consists of hear- ments than schools in affluent suburbs. It is therefore
ing passages of the Koran read in Arabic, even though not surprising that many U.S. civil rights activists in the
the boys do not understand Arabic. Instead, they speak 1960s worked to implement forced busing, which allowed
Pashto, a local language in Pakistan. The curriculum underprivileged children access to schools in affluent
is meant to be absorbed without question by those who communities.
learn it (Goldberg, 2000). After several years, many of the
students can recall and recite long passages of the Koran Two Prominent Models of Education In the United
perfectly. States, two dramatically different models of education
By contrast, consider private Western schools such as emerged early in the twentieth century (Scribner, 1988).
the Montessori school shown here (see Figure 11.17B). The first came from the American philosopher, psycholo-
In such a school, teachers help children explore in more gist, and educator John Dewey. Expanding on a tradition
individualistic ways. Children are encouraged to question eloquently described by the French philosopher Jean-Jacques
everything, and they feel free to challenge every statement. Rousseau (Dewey, 1902/1991), Dewey’s approach to educa-
They are, in many ways, in charge of their own learning, tion was designed around the child’s natural mental abilities
with teachers serving as guides and facilitators of the stu- and interests and focused on material relevant to the child’s
dents’ curiosity. daily activities. Building on this “natural” foundation,
Finally, consider a school in one of the wealthiest suburbs teachers could introduce the basic concepts of mathematics,
of New York City. Even though it is an elementary school, literacy, geography, and science. Dewey’s emphasis was on
the school and the children’s parents are already thinking the student’s mind, not on the subjects to be taught, and
about which colleges will admit these students. The teachers he favored projects that require integrating many kinds of
test the children frequently with standardized tests to make knowledge. The Dewey model led to a child-centered form
sure the children are comfortable with that format. Exten- of instruction, one that viewed the child as a spontaneous,
sive homework is assigned that is geared toward improving active individual who brings many abilities and interests to
performance on those tests. So that students will stay ahead the learning situation. These abilities and interests would
of statewide norms, the school helps arrange for tutors to then serve as raw materials and points of departure for fur-
meet with their students outside the classroom. By the sec- ther education.
ond grade, students are already intensely competitive and see The second approach to schooling began with an Indiana
every activity as a way to succeed and surpass others—or to school superintendent, William Wirt, who in 1910 devel-
fail and fall behind. oped the idea of a platoon school. This was a school in
As these examples show, the nature of classroom expe- which every room was used to maximum efficiency, and
riences varies widely. Some of these variations correlate children moved from classroom to classroom to pick up
highly with the affluence of the neighborhoods in which different intellectual skills, such as reading or mathematics
the schools are located. On average, schools in impov- (Scribner, 1988). Similarities between Wirt’s approach
erished areas are much less effective learning environ- and the assembly-line model of industrial manufacturing
SCHOOLING 411
contrast, the dominant U.S. culture, including the schools, ences are discussed more broadly, in contexts other than
tends to emphasize affect-free problem solving, individual schools, in the next few chapters.
effort, sitting still, competition, and written performance
and literacy. Thus, a child from the dominant culture enters
school with all the values from home dovetailing nicely with Q: What are some ways in which schools
the school’s structure, whereas some African-American chil- influence children besides educating them in the
dren enter school at odds with much of the implicit structure
(Boykin et al., 2005; Marryshow et al., 2005). curriculum?
This kind of conflict between schooling patterns of the
dominant culture and the values of local subcultures occurs
elsewhere in the world as well. For example, consider the
conflict between the Inuit community members of Baffin
Schools and Thought
Island, Arctic Bay, and the “southern” school systems of When we look to the more explicit influences that schools have
Canada (Douglas, 1994; McGregor, 2010). In many Inuit on thought and intelligence, there is little doubt that school-
communities, children are expected to help out during the ing increases intelligence in all the ways it is normally assessed.
day in such activities as taking care of younger children or A variety of studies have shown that the number of years of
helping to unload fish from a large catch. When a child schooling children receive seems to directly influence their
helps out in this way, however, the “southern schools” mark performance on intelligence tests (Ceci, 1991, 1999; Falch &
down the child as “truant.” This practice creates consider- Sandgren Massih, 2011; Teasdale & Owen, 2000; Winship &
able tension for the children both with their families and Korenman, 1997). Furthermore, between June and September,
their schools. In almost all such cases, children in the non- intelligence test results tend to drop for children who are on
dominant group become alienated from the schools and do summer vacation. Just 2½ months out of school seems to be
not perform as well. These effects are often related to schools enough to cause a decline in performance equivalent to roughly
that see the traditional culture as somehow deficient rather a month of progress (Cooper et al., 1996). One remarkable
than merely different. demonstration of these effects occurred when schools in parts
of Virginia were closed in the 1960s to avoid racial integra-
Schools as Vehicles of Values Finally, the idea that schools tion. Some children were deprived of school for a year or more.
teach children certain cultural values or, conversely, erode Compared with children whose schooling was not interrupted,
those values has become increasingly prominent in the United those unable to attend lost roughly 6 points on intelligence test
States. In this view, schools are seen as helping to instill a moral scores for each year of school they missed (Green et al., 1964;
sense or a moral compass. This can be seen in efforts to make Neisser et al., 1996).
children sensitive to the needs of others, to ethnic and group Besides imparting information, schools teach analytical
differences, and to certain ethics of work and self-discipline. skills, test-taking skills, and certain ways of thinking. In this
Whether schools should play such a role is highly controversial, respect, the effects of schooling on intelligence test scores
and many parents feel that in doing so, schools undermine the resemble the effects of practice and training on athletic skills.
parents’ own attempts to teach their children values. Although people’s athletic abilities differ, everyone can ben-
In the United States, this conflict has repeatedly emerged efit from good coaching, training, and practice. If you haven’t
in the controversy over whether prayer should be allowed in done the right exercises to develop your muscles, your per-
the public schools. The U.S. Constitution, which requires formance will suffer, and appearing “coordinated” in more
separation of religion and state, has been interpreted as complex motor tasks is as much a product of practice as it is
disallowing any religious practices in schools that are sup- of native abilities. Practice and training appear to be at least as
ported by public funds. But many religious parents feel that important for intelligence as they are for motor skills.
depriving children of exposure to prayer at school compro-
mises a parent’s ability to instill important religious values.
Thus, they see schools as central to instilling values in their Q: In what ways do schools have an influence
children. We will consider in much greater detail the ques- on intelligence test scores?
tion of moral values and their emergence in children in
Chapter 12.
All three types of influences—socialization, encultura- In addition to improving intelligence test performance,
tion, and the instillation of moral values—overlap and work schooling has broad effects on thought that differ from the
together; they are not easy to keep separate. Nonetheless, it effects of simply growing older. In one study of the ways
is useful to see them as three different, interrelated sources schooling and age each affect mathematics and numerical
of schools’ influence on children. All three types of influ- reasoning, researchers compared performance within three
SCHOOLING 413
SCIENTIFIC METHOD: Direct versus Inquiry-Based Methods of Instruction
Hypothesis:
Although inquiry is thought to capture naturalistic ways of learning that lead to
deeper conceptual insights, in many situations direct instruction may be more
effective.
Method:
1. A total of 57 children (mean age 9 years) participated.
2. Children were randomly assigned to one of four groups: (a) high guidance
followed by high guidance (High + High), (b) high guidance followed by
low guidance (High + Low), (c) low guidance followed by high guidance
(Low + High), and (d) low guidance followed by low guidance (Low +
Low).
3. All children were pretested on their ability to design a simple physics
experiment.
4. Experimenters taught children with direct instruction and/or unguided
discovery in one of the four combinations.
.8
Results: .7
High + High and High + Low guidance conditions were far more effective than .6
the other two conditions in instilling a deep conceptual insight into how to design .5
.3
Phase 2: Ramps
pre-training
Phase 3:
Training 1
Phase 4:
Training 2
Phase 5:
Springs transfer
Phase 6:
Car transfer
Phase 8:Story
post-training
Phase 9:
Remote story
Phase 10:
Remote ramps
Phase 7: Ramps
post-training
direct instruction, even if the direct instruction involves only half as many
manipulations of physical systems. Direct instruction plus discovery was
almost as effective.
Source study: Matlen & Klahr (2013).
students will likely “discover” many falsehoods, especially Brown and Campione stressed that each child always has zones
in science education (Klahr & Nigam, 2004; Klahr, 2013; of proximal development in many different areas of learning.
Matlen & Klahr, 2013). For example, in guided discovery Good teachers try to make as many of them available as possible
activities about evolution, children often “discover” the so the child can actively choose her own route to knowledge.
misconception that organisms evolve in their own life- Active learning and zones of proximal development can
times, unless they are guided toward understanding how also influence children’s relationships with peers who are
natural selection happens over successive generations. In somewhat more competent. In many cases, student collabo-
learning how to design the best ways to understand simple rations can provide opportunities for active learning, since
physical mechanical systems, more explicit guidance not they give students with different strengths the chance to
only provides better learning with fewer trials, but also teach one another (Tudge, 1992). More broadly, Vygotsky
promotes deeper conceptual understanding (Matlen & saw these educational interactions with both adults and
Klahr, 2013; see Scientific Method box). peers as part of the process of internalizing the knowledge
Teaching methods that foster active learning are also linked and practices of the students’ own culture. Schools play an
to Vygotsky’s notion of the zone of proximal development important part in this process, both in terms of the skills
(Vygotsky, 1978), described in Chapter 9. The zone of proximal and information they teach and in terms of the cultural val-
development represents the next level of ability or understand- ues and norms they convey in less explicit ways (Davydov &
ing that a child can learn with appropriate, supportive teaching. Kerr, 1995).
Knowledge Test, Unit 1 (First semester) Knowledge Test, Unit 2 (Second semester) Knowledge Test, Unit 3 (Second semester)
80 80 80
Pre Pre Pre
70 Post Post 70 Post
70
60 60
Test score
Test score
Test score
60
50 50
40 50 40
30 30
40
20 20
10 30 10
CLP all CLP half No CLP CLP all CLP half No CLP CLP all CLP half No CLP
year group year group group year group year group group year group year group group
A
A B C
FIGURE 11.19 Effects of the Community of Learners program of instruction. The first group (CLP all year) was exposed to the Com-
munity of Learners program methods for a full year. The second group (CLP half year) was taught with these methods during the first semester,
then switched to regular instruction for the second semester. The third group (no CLP) had regular classroom experience all year. The Commu-
nity of Learners program clearly enhanced student test performance. (A) Unit 1 test results during the first semester when both the CLP all year
group and the CLP half year group did research, but the no CLP group did not do research. (B) Unit 2 test results during the second semester
when only the CLP all year group did research. (C) Unit 3 test results during the second semester when again only the CLP all year group did
research. Adapted from Brown (1992).
SCHOOLING 415
100 FIGURE 11.20 Teaching children how to
Master science teacher’s classroom
90 think about science. When sixth grade students
Regular science teacher’s classroom taught by a master science teacher were com-
80
pared with students taught by a regular science
Percent of sample
70
teacher, there were striking differences in the sorts
60
of questions the students asked in discussions
50 about science. For example, among the master
40 teacher’s students, metacognitive questions—
30 that is, questions about the thought processes
20 involved in doing science—were seven times
more common than they were among the regular
10
science teacher’s students (the comparison class-
0 room). By contrast, more descriptive questions
Doing Gathering Thinking Finding Testing Under- Developing
things information explanations ideas standing ideas were three times more common in the comparison
ideas classroom. Adapted from Smith et al. (2000).
CREATIVITY 417
DEVELOPMENT AND SOCIAL P OLICY
C
omputers have had a dramatic impact on many cul- cational goals (Roschelle et al., 2000). Rather than simply
tures. Many children in industrialized countries spend providing positive and negative reinforcements in automated
more time looking at computers or cell phone screens drills, computers can be used to improve students’ access to
than they do watching television (Taylor & Keeter, information, to further communication, and to provide some
2010). Using the Internet to search for information, play online domain-specific assessments of the child. Computers can be
games, or interact on social networks has become a common- most effective in the classroom when teachers have clear, spe-
place event. But before these uses became commonplace, chil- cific goals for incorporating them into lessons and evidence
dren were using computers in schools. Today, the vast majority that these goals are feasible. Too often, school districts buy
of schools in developed countries make Internet-linked comput- a large number of computers, set them up in the classrooms,
ers available to their students (see Figure 11.21).
and then are dismayed to find that they have little impact on
The use of computers in the schools on such a large scale
learning. But when computers have been used in the context
since the 1980s raises questions about how they affect educa-
of cognitive theories of learning, there have been some strik-
tion. Do they support more efficient learning? Are they more
ing successes (Sandholtz & Reilly, 2004; Tamim et al., 2011).
helpful in some academic disciplines than in others? Does
Brown and Campione’s Community of Learners program used
access to the wealth of information on the Internet contribute to
computers to help teachers hone in on students’ zones of
children’s conceptual growth, or does it overwhelm them with
proximal development (see Chapter 9). Software designed for
too much information, much of it dubious? Computer use in the
schools varies so widely that there are no easy answers to these classroom use can help teachers get more precise moment-to-
questions, and so controversy remains regarding their academic moment snapshots of a child’s progress on an academic task.
value (Cuban, 2001; Hew & Brush, 2011; Joshi et al., 2010; This can help teachers determine which new topics would be
Sandholtz & Reilly, 2004; Tamim et al., 2011). Nonetheless, challenging but still within the student’s capability.
some general messages are emerging about the most effective Computers and other modes of Internet access can also
ways of using computers to enhance learning. enhance education by supporting collaborative learning. With
The cognitive science approach to educational practice computer networks, students can collaborate over long dis-
has helped promote the use of computers to further edu- tances and large time frames by posting their work in a com-
FIGURE 11.21 Computers in the schools. Although computers are now used in virtually all schools in developed countries, contro-
versy remains concerning their usefulness as supplements to more traditional forms of education.
In fact, an individual’s performance on common creativity learning and practicing specific ways of thinking (Murdock,
tests remains fairly stable over time, but there is less consis- 2003; Sternberg et al., 2004). These techniques include using
tency on creativity tests than on intelligence tests (Simonton, mental images to explore new possibilities (Smith et al., 1995),
2000). To the extent that creativity test performance is less exploiting the power of analogies (Holyoak & Thagard,
stable than intelligence test performance, it supports the view 1995), and radically reframing problems (Polanyi, 1966). In
that anyone can become at least somewhat more creative by addition, simply focusing on developing task-relevant cogni-
tive skills and heuristics can lead to more creative thought is an intrinsic component as well. Some researchers suggest
(Hennessey & Amabile, 2010). that because creativity appears to run in families, there may
These kinds of training do seem to improve certain even be a substantial genetic contribution (Eysenck, 1995).
aspects of creative thinking, but it is not clear whether simi- It is difficult, however, to disentangle genetic effects from
lar experience-dependent effects account for most people’s those that might occur from growing up in an environment
creativity. Certainly, many people like to think that there fostered by highly creative parents.
CREATIVITY 419
out people who exemplified creativity in seven of the
Wall eight multiple intelligences he had defined in his earlier
research: linguistic, logical/math, musical, spatial, bodily/
kinesthetic, interpersonal, and intrapersonal intelligence.
As Table 11.4 shows, each kind of intelligence lends itself
A B to particular professions, and Gardner studied creative
FIGURE 11.22 Functional fixedness. In this classic problem stud- geniuses in each one.
ied by Karl Duncker in 1945, creativity was measured by the extent Gardner emphasized that most outstanding people spent
to which participants could avoid being fixated on (A) the standard many thousands of hours of practice or sustained effort in
functions of matches, candles, and thumbtacks and (B) come up with their chosen domain. Thus, creativity rarely just appeared
novel and creative uses. Adapted from Duncker (1945). without a passion to invest time and effort in a domain
(Ericsson, 1996). In each area, Gardner chose people who
had continually excelled in their domain by solving prob-
The Development of Extraordinary lems, fashioning products, or raising issues. Moreover, their
contributions had to be recognized as new and different and
Creativity yet be broadly accepted in a way that had dramatic influence
Howard Gardner (1994, 1997), whose theory of multiple intel- on the culture at large. Thus, the recognition of extraordi-
ligences was discussed earlier in the chapter, has advocated a nary creativity must always be seen as a social judgment rel-
different approach to studying creativity. Gardner argues evant to a particular cultural group. People are not creative
that the common psychometric approaches to defining and in isolation, but only in terms of how their accomplishments
measuring creativity have yielded several important insights, relate to the society in which they live (Csikszentmihalyi,
but they largely fail to truly identify the people who are most 1997; Harrington, 1990). This observation also resonates
creative in everyday life. Instead, Gardner advocates studying with Vygotsky’s ideas about how children come to internal-
extraordinarily creative people as a way to uncover creativity’s ize the sociocultural context in which they grow up. Simi-
critical components. larly, creativity emerges as an individual becomes embedded
Two approaches best capture Gardner’s proposals for within a set of cultural and social structures.
how to study creativity. The first of these, the idiographic Although Gardner’s sample of seven people is too small to
approach, involves studying case histories of highly cre- form strong conclusions about how creativity develops, he was
ative individuals to see whether they seem to cluster into nevertheless struck by the many ways in which these people’s
different types. The second method, known as the nomo- life experiences differed from what we might expect from
thetic approach, involves comparing the lives of especially a random sample of the population. Taking a nomothetic
creative individuals to look for common patterns or prin- approach, Gardner looked for similarities between their lives
ciples (Simonton, 2000). Thus, we might ask if extraordi- and began to uncover some common principles. The follow-
nary creativity typically emerges late in life, or we might ing patterns hold true for most, if not all, of the seven geniuses:
ask whether more highly creative people tend to live during
times of dramatic cultural change. 1. Most were born near great cultural centers or cities.
Gardner has often taken the idiographic approach, 2. Most came from middle-class backgrounds, rather than
studying in detail the lives of creative geniuses. He used from the intellectual elite, wealthy families, or poor
several criteria to choose these individuals. First, he sought families.
CONCLUSIONS 421
these social influences on the mind and behavior in much acquire knowledge. They rarely, if ever, were concerned
greater depth. with the kinds of individual differences that are a major
Schooling provides the most vivid example of how social focus of intelligence testing. (Unfortunately, the term nativ-
and environmental factors can influence intelligence. Not ist has sometimes been used to describe those who believe
only do children with high IQ scores tend to stay in school for that differences in intelligence have a strong genetic basis.
more years, but the longer children of various IQ levels stay This sense of the word, which focuses on how people differ
in school, the more their intelligence rises on a wide variety of from each other, is completely separate from the meaning
fronts, ranging from transitive reasoning to storytelling and we have been using in this book, which is instead concerned
comprehension skills. As schools bring children within the with what is common to all humans in comparison with
framework of the dominant culture, the effects on patterns of other species. The two meanings should not be confused.)
thought and cognitive performance can be profound. More commonly, controversies about how differences in
Schools need to be sensitive to the individual child intelligence develop are referred to as the nature/nurture
and the ways in which a child’s particular aptitude inter- debate, where differences among people are seen as largely
acts with instructional techniques. Consideration of such a consequence of their intrinsic natures or of how they are
“learner characteristics” also reinforces the view of devel- nurtured. By contrast, both sides of the nativist/empiricist
opment as an active exploratory process and not simply debate have classically focused on how to characterize pat-
one of the child acting as a passive recipient of experiences. terns of development that are common to all people.
More broadly, as the rate of technological change acceler- In several places in this chapter, we have discussed the
ates, schools must confront the problem of teaching only advantages of viewing developmental psychology as the
a fixed set of skills. Rather, students must hone the abil- study of emerging competencies and abilities rather than
ity to acquire new sets of skills as the world around them the gradual disappearance of cognitive deficits that plague
changes. If the curriculum becomes focused on training the younger child. Likewise, this perspective has been valu-
students for high-stakes standardized tests, teachers may able in thinking about intelligence in relation to models of
not have the chance to prepare students to be adaptive, education. It is far less helpful to ask what younger children
flexible learners. The problem is not with the tests them- are “missing” with respect to intelligence and creativity
selves, but with designing tests that go beyond simply than to explore the nature of the earliest roots of cognitive
assessing factual knowledge and that do not cause high abilities—roots that can stretch back to infancy. We have
levels of test anxiety. seen that schooling is more effective when it focuses on
Clearly, the research on the development of intelligence abilities that children bring to the classroom and works on
draws together several familiar developmental themes. With developing those abilities further while linking them to
respect to the nativist/empiricist debate, the links are more other skills in an integrated fashion. Fortunately, this kind
remote than they first appear. The classical nativists and of focus on early competencies is what the best teachers do
empiricists debated about human nature and how people naturally.
STU DY AN D REVIEW
Debating the Origins of Moral Thought FIGURE 12.1 Are children inherently antisocial? In his novel
For hundreds of years, philosophers and other scholars have Lord of the Flies, William Golding suggested a view of morality in
debated about the moral characteristics of young children which children, once removed from the constraints of society, develop
into cruel antisocial beings. Shown here is a film still from the 1963
and how morality develops. Two broad views have charac-
film based on the book.
terized much of that debate: (1) the idea that young children
have some kind of inherent moral nature, and (2) the con-
trasting notion that they have no inherent moral character- This account suggests that children are filled with base
istics and must learn about morality from their social and impulses that need to be controlled and suppressed by
cultural experiences. Within these views there can also be civilization.
strong variations. The other view of moral development sees no moral
Consider the view that the young child comes into the capacity and no inclination toward either good or bad in
world with certain moral propensities. The eighteenth- infants and young children. Many who hold this view
century French philosopher Jean-Jacques Rousseau, for argue that very young children are no more moral than
example, proposed that young children are intrinsically animals because they lack the cognitive capability to have a
good. He believed that infants and young children are moral sense. They hypothesize that young children are ini-
pure in heart and mind and would always behave in mor- tially amoral and that they learn morality as a consequence
ally appropriate ways if not for the corrupting influences of of development. In general, proponents of this view believe
society. In this view, a primary goal of child rearing should that infants and young children are not cognitively and
be to shield children from society’s corrupting influences emotionally capable of moral thought, so they are not fit to
so that their intrinsic moral sense has time to mature and be judged as either good or bad. In this view, it makes no
become less vulnerable to such influences. The French phy- more sense to think of a young child as good or bad than it
sician Jean Marc Gaspard Itard undertook a dramatic and does to judge a tiger’s morality in its behavior toward prey.
disturbing “test” of this premise by administering random In short, there is a long history of thinking about chil-
beatings to Victor, the “wild child” discussed in Chapter 8. dren’s moral development as if they were primarily bad or
Itard wanted to see whether Victor would show outrage at good from the start, or as if they only gradually acquired
the abuse, even though he had spent much of his child- moral capabilities after being exposed to the values of their
hood alone and isolated from social rules and norms. When society. The views favoring an inherent moral nature did
Victor did show outrage, Itard reasoned that this response not ignore society’s effects; they just saw them as interacting
revealed the boy’s deep-seated, unlearned sense of justice. with preexisting moral natures. The views that saw children
Others who believed in an inherent moral nature took a as amoral held that society’s effects are largely responsible
darker perspective. They saw children as selfish creatures for children’s morality, much like the empiricists’ view of
who need to be civilized by society. The same negative view the child as a blank slate.
persists today in more subtle forms. For example, William More recently, these two types of views have been com-
Golding’s classic novel Lord of the Flies describes how a group plemented by perspectives arising from evolutionary and
of children stuck on a deserted island become unmoored cultural psychology. Although these modern perspectives
from society’s social and moral constraints and descend acknowledge many kinds of influences on the development
into antisocial and even violent actions (see Figure 12.1). of moral thought, they often disagree about which influences
FIGURE 12.4 Cognitions versus emotions as the basis for moral judg-
ments. For centuries, scholars have disagreed as to whether moral judgments
are grounded in rational thought about principles, as in (A) Star Trek’s fictional
Spock, or in moral emotions, as in (B) the obvious empathy shown by St. Francis
of Assisi. Related questions ask about the relative roles of emotions and cogni-
tion at various points in a child’s development. B
Most of the foundational work on children’s moral develop- tions. These early views include stages of moral development
ment emphasizes the cognitive component of moral thought in which children’s thinking about rules and moral judgments
and the child’s growing understanding of rules and principles changes with age; they were first proposed by Jean Piaget in
for guiding moral reasoning. These views often carry the devel- his theory of moral development, followed by the stages pro-
opmental implication that moral judgment is the highest form posed by Lawrence Kohlberg and Carol Gilligan. We will dis-
of morality and that the most virtuous people are those most cuss each of these theories in turn and ask how they differ.
able to rely on cognitive factors and not on other factors in Recent research has stressed a pluralistic approach in which
making judgments (Kohlberg, 1984). In these early theories, an individual draws on different forms of moral thought in
moral development consists of increasingly being able to act different contexts at all ages and may not pass through stages
on the basis of reasoned moral judgments, with the youngest as such. Moreover, today scholars have shown that emotions
children being most ruled by other factors, such as their emo- continue to have a central role throughout development.
0 Premoral development Up to 4 years There is no explicit awareness of rules, no use of moral principles
or notions of justice.
1 Heteronomous stage 4 to 10 years Rules are seen as unchanging and external, like physical laws.
Judgments of culpability are based on the act’s consequences
rather than the actor’s intentions. There is little sense of what
punishment is appropriate for what degree of transgression.
2 Autonomous stage 10 to 11 years and Rules are seen as human agreements that can be changed if all
older parties consent. Judgments of culpability are based in part on
intentions, and punishment should be appropriate to the severity
of the transgression.
TABLE 12.1 Piaget’s stages of moral reasoning. In Piaget’s theory of moral development, children progressed through three stages of
moral reasoning, with the first stage supposedly devoid of any awareness of moral rules, principles, or ideas of justice.
A B
FIGURE 12.6 Scenarios used by Piaget to elicit children’s moral reasoning. In the heteronomous
stage, children tend to judge consequences as more important than intentions. Thus, they judge (A) a child
who accidentally causes 15 glasses to break as more guilty than (B) a child who accidentally causes 1
glass to break, despite the fact that the first child did not intend to do anything wrong, but the second child
did have a negative intention (reaching for jam that was deliberately placed out of his reach).
A B C
FIGURE 12.7 Social conventions versus moral laws. Driving on a particular side of the road is an arbitrary social convention established
by a society, as shown by (A) British drivers, who drive on the left side of the road, and (B) American drivers, who drive on the right side of the
road. (C) Prohibitions against stealing would be considered moral laws in all societies.
Kohlberg’s Theory of Moral In Europe, a woman was near death from a special kind of
cancer. There was one drug that the doctors thought might
Reasoning save her. It was a form of radium that a druggist in the
same town had recently discovered. The drug was expen-
Lawrence Kohlberg approached the study of moral devel- sive to make, but the druggist was charging ten times what
opment with close attention to Piaget’s work. Kohlberg the drug cost him to make. He paid $200 for the radium
believed that one mistake that trapped both Freudians and and charged $2,000 for a small dose of the drug. The sick
behaviorists was to not think seriously about what morality woman’s husband, Heinz, went to everyone he knew to bor-
itself is. There was, for example, almost no attention from row the money, but he could only get together about $1,000
either of those camps to thousands of years of philosophical which is half of what it cost. He told the druggist that his
discourse as to what constitutes true virtue. For Kohlberg, wife was dying and asked him to sell it cheaper or let him pay
beliefs about what is moral are not incidental, but rather later. But the druggist said: “No, I discovered the drug and
central to the proper study of moral development and how I’m going to make money from it.” So Heinz got desperate
children become moral thinkers—or what he called “moral and broke into the man’s store to steal the drug for his wife.
philosophers.” Should the husband have done that? (Kohlberg, 1963, p. 19)
Kohlberg believed that children construct morality by
developing a system of beliefs about concepts like justice For this dilemma, many arguments could support moral
and individual rights. He thought that it takes a long time approval or disapproval of Heinz’s action. According to
for children to develop accurate beliefs and reasoning pat- Kohlberg, certain kinds of arguments, both for and against
terns about morality and that early on, they confuse moral his choice, typify each stage of moral development.
issues with other issues, such as power, coercion, and author- In Kohlberg’s first level of moral reasoning, the precon-
ity. This process of discovering which issues are truly moral ventional level (Stages 1 and 2), a child believes that some-
formed the basis for Kohlberg’s model. one is virtuous if she is not punished much for her actions
or does not suffer much compared with her gains. Thus, a
child in Stage 1 might say that it is not appropriate to steal
the drug because Heinz will be punished harshly for theft.
Kohlberg’s Stages of Moral Reasoning Another child in Stage 1 might say that it is appropriate to
Kohlberg initially proposed six stages of moral development steal the drug because Heinz will get a much worse punish-
to describe how children’s basic concept of virtue changes. ment if he lets his wife die. Both responses are considered
He then clustered these six stages into three larger levels of typical of Stage 1, regardless of the particular moral choice,
moral reasoning—preconventional, conventional, and post- because in both cases avoiding punishment is the key con-
conventional reasoning—which loosely resonate with the cern. Children in Stage 2 might say that it is acceptable to
stages of moral development that Piaget proposed. Both of steal the drug because it is worth spending some time in
their theories capture the idea of the child moving through jail to keep your wife alive. Or they might say that it is not
stages of being unaware of rules, of using rules too literally, acceptable to steal the drug because the jail time would not
and of engaging in more abstract thought. be worth it given that his wife is so sick that she might die
Kohlberg assessed children’s level of moral development anyway.
by using a method that he devised known as the moral To a child in the conventional level of moral reasoning
dilemma. Kohlberg’s moral dilemmas were short descrip- (Stages 3 and 4), being virtuous means doing what we are
tions of morally ambiguous situations in which a char- told or conforming to group norms. Children in Stage 3
acter must make a difficult moral choice. After hearing might say that it is acceptable to steal the drug because a
the dilemma, the study participant is asked which course nice person does not let his wife die, or they might say that
of action is morally superior and why. Piaget and earlier it is not acceptable because only bad people steal things.
TABLE 12.2 Kohlberg’s stages of moral development. Each of Kohlberg’s three levels of moral reasoning is divided into two stages.
Kohlberg argued that each of these six stages shows distinctive patterns of moral reasoning. Although Kohlberg later revised his theory to
add and later eliminate a stage, we present here his original six stages, which are the most widely known version of his theory. Adapted from
Kohlberg (1976).
Adaptive Protecting and Reaping benefits Reaping benefits Negotiating Avoiding microbes
challenge caring for young, of mutual of group hierarchy, deferring and parasites
vulnerable, or cooperation with cooperation to authority
injured kin nonkin
Proper domain Suffering, distress, Cheating, Threat or challenge Signs of Waste products,
(adaptive triggers) or threat to kin cooperation, to group dominance and diseased people
deception submission
Actual domain Baby animals, Marital fidelity, Home sports Bosses, respected Taboo ideas
(commonly cartoon characters broken vending teams professionals (communism,
encountered machines racism)
triggers)
Characteristic Compassion Anger, gratitude, Group pride, Respect, fear Disgust
emotions guilt belongingness,
rage at traitors
Relevant virtues Caring, kindness Fairness, Loyalty, patriotism, Obedience, Temperance,
[and vices] [cruelty] justice, honesty, self-sacrifice deference chastity, piety,
trustworthiness [treason, [disobedience, cleanliness [lust,
[dishonesty] cowardice] uppitiness] intemperance]
TABLE 12.4 Moral modules. One way of presenting the pluralistic view of morality argues that humans have five distinct moral modules that
are shared with evolutionary ancestors. Human cultural and cognitive processes have elaborated on these basic modules, giving rise to moral
emotions and to our notions of virtues and vices. Adapted from Haidt & Joseph (2007).
university administration became unhappy that students Thus, many decisions and actions can be justified at vari-
were distributing leftist political pamphlets on-campus, ous levels of moral reasoning or in terms of several differ-
and it attempted to ban these materials, claiming that ent moral belief systems, making it quite difficult to use
the tables set up to distribute them were slowing cam- beliefs to predict behaviors (Keniston, 1970). People who
pus traffic. In response, the students conducted a traf- consciously want their moral beliefs to be consistent with
fic flow study documenting no such problems. When the their actions may show more consistency (Blasi, 1980), but
administration did not back down, the incensed students even this desire does not always result in consistency. Pre-
insisted that their civil rights had been violated and even- dicting moral behavior from beliefs may also be possible
tually held a sit-in at one of the university buildings—a in studies that set up situations in which certain forms of
highly provocative act in which participants risked not
only their academic standing but their future careers (see
Figure 12.9).
Was it possible to predict who engaged in the sit-in
based on their stages of moral reasoning? Haan and her
colleagues assessed many of the demonstrators, using
Kohlberg’s moral dilemmas to determine their levels of
moral reasoning. We might expect that only students
at the highest levels would have engaged in an action
that would put them at such risk solely on the basis of a
relatively abstract principle such as free speech. In fact,
students at several levels of moral reasoning were well
represented. Although there were proportionally more
protesters at Kohlberg’s two highest levels of moral rea-
soning (Stages 5 and 6) than in the student body at large,
there were also some protesters who were at Stage 2. Even
as some students were demonstrating as a way of honor-
ing principles, others were doing so apparently for purely
hedonistic reasons. These students saw the sit-in as great
fun, and their enjoyment outweighed the risks.
FIGURE 12.9 Moral judgments and moral actions. Although
students who participated in the Free Speech Movement on the
Q: Why might it be problematic to use Berkeley campus in the 1960s were more likely to show higher levels
of moral reasoning on Kohlberg’s stages, there were still many other
someone’s tested level of moral reasoning to protesters from lower stages and many who did not participate from
predict his moral behavior? higher stages, revealing that there is no simple mapping from levels
of moral reasoning to kinds of moral behaviors.
T
he idea that moral intuitions might be rooted in emo- there is just one person who will be killed (see Figure 12.10). Is
tions is an old one, going back at least as far as the it right to throw the switch? Most people say it is, and they give
eighteenth century. Recently, researchers have pro- thoughtful explanations about sacrificing one person to save
posed a new view of the role that emotions play in five people. Now consider a second situation that is similar to
moral thought, spurred by the phenomenon of moral dumb- the first, except that instead of throwing a switch, you can shove
founding. This term refers to the finding that for some moral a large, curly-haired man with glasses from a footbridge onto
situations, people have strong convictions about what is right the tracks in front of the trolley, knowing that sacrificing this
and wrong but are unable to explain why. The phenomenon of one man’s life will save the other five people, as the large man’s
moral dumbfounding builds on an apparent disconnect between body will stop the trolley before it reaches the other five people.
having strong “gut feelings” about what is right and being able Most people find the second action unacceptable. But what
to justify those feelings with higher-order reasoning. The phe- is the difference? The first scenario describes an impersonal
nomenon does not exist for all moral situations, only for certain moral situation, one that is abstract and does not directly
ones that most people immediately consider wrong, although involve specific individuals. In this case, it is relatively easy to
they cannot explain why. distance yourself from the situation and consider the tradeoffs
In many cases, moral dumbfounding reactions fit within involved in either throwing the switch or not. The second sce-
an evolutionary psychology perspective. One such scenario nario describes a personal moral situation because it involves
describes an adult brother and sister who have consensual sex the individual in the situation in a direct, immediate way (Greene
(Haidt, 2003). The scenario painstakingly describes how the et al., 2001; Singer, 2005). Thus, in the switch-throwing (imper-
brother and sister decided to do it only once, used contracep- sonal) case, it is relatively easy to distance yourself from the situ-
tives, and had no adverse emotional reactions. The vast majority ation and consider the tradeoffs in abstract terms. In the shoving
of adults find the scenario shocking and instantly condemn it as (personal) case, it is impossible not to encode the effects of
wrong. Yet, when asked to explain why they think it is wrong, this action on a specific person (which is why the description
most adults either give incorrect reasons (for example, “the includes personal characteristics), and it is difficult not to be hor-
couple might have a child with genetic defects,” even though rified at the thought of harming an innocent person.
they know the couple used contraceptives) or say they don’t Moral intuitions in impersonal versus personal situations
know why. People judge quite a large number of scenarios in appear to show different patterns of activation in the brain
this way. For example, even if they are not religious, people (Cushman et al., 2010). In fMRI studies of increased blood
think it is wrong to use sacred articles to, say, clean out a toilet flow in brain regions that occurs while engaging in judgments
bowl, whether or not anyone else even knows about it, and yet about the two kinds of situations, different sets of brain regions
they are “dumbfounded” when asked to explain why (Haidt & become activated; for example, areas associated with emotional
Bjorklund, 2008; Haidt & Joseph, 2004). processing are significantly more activated in the personal moral
Moral judgments of this sort may be like judgments of aes- situations than in the impersonal moral situations (Greene et al.,
thetics. Most people who find a sunset beautiful would be hard 2001, 2004). In addition, people who suffer brain damage to the
pressed to explain why it is more beautiful than any other. Peo- prefrontal cortex, a key region associated with personal moral
ple seem to perceive the beauty directly. Similarly, some moral intuitions, tend to make “utilitarian” moral judgments in which
intuitions may be triggered by emotional reactions rather than they treat both trolley scenarios as a simple weighting of the
being a result of deliberative rational thought. These cases that total number of lives saved (Greene, 2007; Koenigs et al., 2007;
rely on gut feelings about what is right might be quite distinct Young & Koenigs, 2007).
from cases that require careful reasoning. Moral dumbfounding arises from powerful emotions; there is
One way researchers have studied these kinds of situations an immediate sense of knowing what is right and wrong without
is by presenting participants with two moral situations, only one reflection, based on a link between an emotion and a moral
of which draws on emotion-based intuitions. For example, imag- evaluation (Dwyer, 2009). The social intuitionist model of
ine that you are at a trolley stop, and you see that a runaway moral judgment suggests that in these cases, emotions like
trolley is about to kill five people on the track. You can pull a anger or contempt can be triggered automatically by perceived
switch that will send the trolley onto a different track on which violations of norms and rights. Thus, people can have moral
A B
FIGURE 12.10 Moral dumbfounding. (A) In the first scenario, there is a runaway trolley and five people are on one track and one per-
son is on another track. Participants are asked if they would throw a switch that would shift the trolley from the track on which five people
would be killed to the track on which one person would be killed. (B) In the second scenario, participants are asked if they would push
one person onto the track to save the five people on the track.
A B
FIGURE 12.12 Cooperation versus competition. Shown here is an apparatus used to measure cooperation and competition among
children. The goal is for each child to retrieve a marble from within the container. Competitive participants simply pull on the strings, causing the
breakup of the container (which is held together by magnets), and thereby causing the marble to roll away, so that neither child gets the marble.
Cooperative participants agree to let each other get a marble on successive terms. Adapted from Madsen & Connor (1973).
Hypothesis:
When a situation clearly shows that a person is in need of help to achieve a
goal, even young toddlers will behave altruistically.
Method:
1. Twenty-four 14-month-old toddlers participated in the experiment.
2. All the children saw an experimenter (1) drop a clothespin while hanging
up clothes and then try to retrieve it, (2) drop a marker while making a
drawing and then try to retrieve it, and (3) try to pick up paper balls with
tongs when the balls were out of reach for the experimenter but not for
the child.
3. In a control condition, the experimenter intentionally threw away the
clothespin/marker or did not try to move the paper balls to a new spot.
4. The researchers recorded the incidence of instrumental helping by the
toddlers.
Results:
In all three tasks, but especially the paper ball task, toddlers helped more in
the experimental condition.
Conclusion:
In tasks that involve instrumental helping of another person to achieve a
goal, very young children will altruistically offer assistance at no benefit to
themselves.
100
Mean percentage of trials
Experimental condition
80
Control
60
40
20
0
Clothespin Marker Paper ball
Source study: Warneken & Tomasello (2007). Helping to reach out-of-reach objects
S
everal high-profile cases of prosecuting juveniles as How does research on the development of moral reasoning
adults have raised questions about the age at which and moral behavior bear on the question of trying a 13-year-old
children should be held morally responsible for their killer as an adult? Despite the thousands of studies on moral
actions. No one would think that a 4-year-old who development, the implications are unclear. Given the difficulties
kills an older sibling with his parent’s gun should be held as of demonstrating clear stages in the development of moral rea-
responsible as an adult, but what about an 11-year-old or a soning, it is not an easy matter to declare that children at a given
14-year-old? age simply lack a certain level of moral reasoning. Moreover,
Consider the case of Nathaniel Abraham, who in 1997, at even if it were possible to create a scale of moral maturity, it
the age of 11, shot and killed a stranger with a .22 caliber rifle. would be difficult to ensure that the courts applied it consis-
Nathaniel, who was 13 at the time of his trial, was prosecuted tently to all offenders. We know that a fair number of adults
in court as an adult. Many observers were shocked at the show moral reasoning patterns that seem to be more common
thought of a 13-year-old being treated as an adult, but the among children. Should those people, who are not necessarily
prosecutors insisted that Abraham was no ordinary 13-year- mentally deficient, be held less responsible for their actions than
old (see Figure 12.17). At the time of the crime, he had a other adults?
long history of problems with the police, many of them involv- A second issue concerns whether the justice system
ing violence against others (Bradshaw, 1999). Convicted of should be more concerned with protecting the safety of oth-
second-degree murder, he avoided imprisonment in an adult ers or with punishing wrongdoers. It would be possible to
facility only because the law grants judges the discretion of make the case that a child who has committed a violent crime
sentencing children younger than 14 to juvenile facilities. is likely to commit other violent acts in the future, but this
does not necessarily mean that the child should be punished
as an adult. In fact, imprisoning the child with adult criminals,
who may exert an even more antisocial influence on him, may
increase (rather than diminish) the chance that he will com-
mit future violent crimes when he is released from prison.
Instead of putting the child in prison, another approach might
be to find a way to monitor the child so as to minimize his risk
to others without treating him like an adult offender in other
respects.
At present, perhaps the most that developmental research
can offer is evidence for the wide variety of ways in which
moral reasoning and judgment develop. Even if there are no
clear moral stages, the many documented developmental differ-
ences in such areas as emotional regulation, forms of empathy,
and understanding of consequences all support arguments for
treating children differently from adults in the criminal justice
FIGURE 12.17 Treating youthful offenders as system (Whaley & Koenen, 2001). We all believe that young
adults. Nathaniel Abraham, shown here in court at age 13, children differ from adults in their abilities to appreciate the con-
was tried as an adult for murder. A recent surge of such trials in
sequences of their actions and to inhibit their impulses. Still, we
several countries raises profound questions about how scientific
research on the moral development of children should be used to need to better understand how these abilities emerge before
evaluate the appropriate age at which juveniles might be tried as developmental differences should be brought to bear on public
adults. policy decisions.
accompanied by both a reason and a genuine concern for ments have made prominent efforts to raise public aware-
the child’s welfare, may not have worse effects than non- ness of the effects of physical abuse on children (Gracia &
physical forms of punishment (Benjet & Kazdin, 2003; Herrero, 2008). Socioeconomic strain and the lack of
Larzelere & Kuhn, 2005). In addition, the negative effects social support structures can also cause very substantial
of physical punishment may vary across cultures and even variations in the prevalence of physical punishment within
among subgroups within a culture (MacKenzie et al., 2011). a culture (MacKenzie et al., 2011). For example, in the
The negative associations with physical punishment sum- state of Kentucky, physical punishment in the school is
marized in Table 12.5 tend to hold for European-American more prevalent in counties that have higher incidences of
families in the United States, but physical punishment does families living in poverty and lower incidences of involve-
not always show such undesirable associations in African- ment in community groups such as churches (McClure
American families. Indeed, careful, appropriate physical & May, 2008). In addition, males, single parents, and
punishment is more associated with positive outcomes than those associated with some fundamentalist religions are
with negative outcomes in certain cultures (Lansford et al., more likely to use physical punishment (Xu et al., 2000).
2004; Larzelere, 2000). These patterns of variation make it clear that social poli-
Examining punishment across a wider range of cultures, cies designed to ameliorate the negative effects of physical
including those of China, India, Italy, Kenya, the Philip- punishment must be very sensitive to the social contexts in
pines, and Thailand, reveals quite substantial variations in which it occurs.
the amount of physical punishment parents use (Lansford
et al., 2005). For example, parents in Thailand are much less Modeling Theory and Moral Behaviors We have so
likely to use physical punishment than parents in Kenya. far largely focused on the punishment of antisocial behav-
Moreover, in cultures where physical punishment is com- iors. It is more difficult and rare to punish a child for not
mon, it is not nearly as strongly related to childhood behav- being especially altruistic. There is, however, another form
ior problems as it is in cultures where physical punishment of social influence on morality that seems to apply equally
is less common (Lansford et al., 2005). to both antisocial and prosocial behaviors. Known as mod-
Even in relatively similar cultures, there are substantial eling theory (or social learning theory), it relies on the
variations in the acceptability of physical punishment. For likelihood that people tend to reproduce behaviors that they
example, its acceptability varies quite dramatically within see others performing (Bandura, 1971).
the countries of the European Union, and in general it Hundreds of studies have shown that both children and
is considered less acceptable in countries whose govern- adults become more likely to perform a wide variety of
A B
FIGURE 12.19 Modeling aggressive behavior. Children have repeatedly been shown to reproduce quite specific behaviors modeled by
adults, whether it is the somewhat bizarre act of physically assaulting a Bobo doll or the verbal assault of younger, more helpless individuals.
From Bandura (1971).
parental interaction styles might lead to different self-con- also contribute to the correlation—instead of, or in addition
scious moral emotions (Lagattuta & Thompson, 2007). to, parents’ effects on their children. We will return to these
Parents can also influence how sensitive their children issues in more detail in Chapters 13 through 16.
are to different aspects of moral behaviors and their conse-
quences. One study examined these effects by asking moth-
ers of 4-year-olds to talk about their child’s good and bad
actions in the prior week. The researchers then analyzed the
mother’s comments to determine how often she referred to
Conclusions
the child’s feelings and intentions, rules, or the consequences Morality is a complex, multifaceted aspect of being human.
of the child’s actions. The extent to which the mother made We reason in moral ways that are distinct from reasoning
comments focusing on such themes predicted the child’s about such things as social conventions and physical laws,
conscience development, which was measured in terms of and these distinctive ways of reasoning seem to appear quite
compliance and internalization of certain values (Laible & early in development. Within the realm of morality itself,
Thompson, 2000). In other words, the way a mother talks there are further differences. When we are quite young, we
about her child’s actions, both to the child and to others, can empathize with others in need, and yet that empathy
may affect the way the child thinks about moral issues and relates only indirectly to altruistic reasoning and to proso-
acts on them (Grusec et al., 2000). More generally, parents cial actions. Similarly, aggressive feelings are related to anti-
who adopt a noncoercive, highly interactive, and sensi- social reasoning and behaviors, but they are not the same
tive parenting style that is infused with positive emotions thing. We can have hostile and aggressive feelings but not
and that uses very gentle discipline strategies tend to fos- necessarily act on them. Moreover, the reasoning associated
ter an earlier development of conscience in their children with such feelings may change over development with the
(Kochanska & Aksan, 2006). emergence of more sophisticated ways of understanding sit-
Developmental psychologists have learned a great deal uations and other agents. We can reason about many moral
about how patterns of child rearing and socialization can domains, including sharing resources, fairness and justice,
affect a child’s moral development (Bugental & Good- and care, in ways that show their own developmental trajec-
now, 1998; Eisenberg, 2000; Kochanska & Aksan, 2006; tories and that can be relatively compartmentalized (Killen,
Shweder et al., 1997). Much of that research is also closely 2007). Finally, all kinds of moral reasoning are linked in
connected to the broader issues of socialization discussed subtle and complex ways to behavior, although there is no
in Chapters 14 and 15, concerning how the child becomes easy, straightforward way of translating moral beliefs into
part of a family and society. Throughout these discussions, actions.
one important caution to keep in mind is that correlations Despite this pluralism and the rapid state of change in
between parenting styles and children’s behaviors are just research on moral development, some general themes in
that, correlations. Explaining these patterns requires a care- this area do emerge and reflect themes we have discussed
ful investigation of whether shared genetic factors, similar in earlier chapters. First, the idea that the child as an active
environments, and the children’s effects on their parents may constructor of morality rather than a passive recipient is
CONCLUSIONS 463
intrinsic to Kohlberg’s approach and to many other theories of everyone everywhere evaluating certain kinds of moral
of moral reasoning, such as Gilligan’s, Damon’s, and Eisen- dilemmas in the same way. At the same time, however,
berg’s. It is less intrinsic to many older theories of social- cultures do not differ so dramatically that we cannot find
ization and to current political arguments about instilling common ground with other groups. There may be a uni-
moral values in children. But most current, sophisticated versal collection of moral reasoning systems, and cultures
views of socialization do seem to recognize that the child is may emphasize (or downplay) different moral components,
an active participant in the socialization process. depending on which situations and social structures are
A second familiar theme is that patterns of moral rea- most pervasive in those cultures.
soning and behavior show less dramatic developmental Finally, as we look back to the two views of morality
shifts than were formerly assumed to be the case, and many introduced at the beginning of this chapter, it is now clear
aspects of moral development are significantly influenced that young children are not amoral in the manner we assume
by situational factors. Of course, in some situations, older holds for most animals. Within their first 2 years, children
children tend to use more complex patterns of moral reason- show compassion, selfishness, and a range of other behaviors
ing than do young children, but even younger children can indicating an early moral sense. It is also clear that young
grasp these patterns when they are carefully simplified and children are neither primarily moral nor immoral. At all
made relevant to them. ages, children show both prosocial and antisocial behaviors.
On a related note, cross-cultural research suggests that Children share with adults a tension between good and bad
there may not be universals of moral reasoning in the sense impulses.
STU DY AN D REVIEW
Ecological self Birth onward Sense of where we are as we move through the world
Perceptual and motor awareness
Automatic, immediate, nonverbal
Interpersonal self Shortly after birth onward Interacting with other intentional agents
Automatic, without reflection
Inherently social
Extended self Age 4 onward Autobiographical timeline
Understanding that we are linked to the past and having the ability to
think about the future
Private self Age 4 onward Understanding that we have privileged experiences no one else has
Knowledge, dreams, perspectives
Conceptual self Age 4–5 onward, with major growth in Most influenced by others and by ambient culture
middle childhood and beyond Social roles (gender roles, family roles)
Able to articulate who we are relative to others
TABLE 13.1 Neisser’s five senses of the self. Neisser (1988) argued that there are five distinct senses of the self rather than a single
unitary concept.
A B C
FIGURE 13.4 The mirror test. In the mirror test, a visible mark is put on a participant’s face and is only visible in a mirror. When the partici-
pant sees the mark in the mirror, the critical question is whether he will touch the mark on the mirror or the mark on his own body. The relative
rarity of this behavior across species has led some to argue that it is evidence for a true concept of self as well as the ability to put oneself in
others’ shoes empathically (Gallup, 1998). But it may be that the ability to pass the mirror test is only one part of the full self-concept. Pictured
here are mirror tests with (A) a chimpanzee, (B) a bottle-nosed dolphin, and (C) an 18-month-old human infant.
TABLE 13.2 Developmental changes in self-esteem. As children grow older and engage in more sophisticated social comparisons, their
level of self-esteem tends to drop from early overconfidence to a more realistic level, as well as becoming more intricately structured.
SELF-REGULATION 477
DEVELOPMENT AND SOCIAL P OLICY
T
he influence of self-esteem on behavior has become In retrospect, these findings should not be that surprising.
increasingly controversial in the realm of social policy Children tend to compare themselves with the peers they typi-
(Twenge, 2006). For years, the conventional wisdom cally encounter in order to build up norms on which to make a
was that the more self-esteem that could be instilled self-esteem judgment. Most African-American children are there-
or encouraged in children, the better. In addition, some argued fore going to compare themselves with other African-American
that children from some disadvantaged groups might not be children in the same situation. While far more African-American
doing as well in school and on standardized tests because of children live in poverty than European-American children (by
lower self-esteem. Many schools in inner-city neighborhoods some estimates the likelihood that an African-American child
put enormous efforts into treating self-esteem problems under lives in poverty is almost four times greater (U.S. Bureau of
the assumption that raising self-esteem would solve problems the Census, 2010), that difference would not affect self-esteem
of academic underachievement (Jackson, 2009). judgments nearly as much as how well-off each child perceives
Yet, the empirical data has not been so clear on the link between himself relative to his immediate peers.
belonging to a disadvantaged group and having low self-esteem. The strong influence of immediate peers is borne out by a
An extensive analysis of 261 studies, which included a combined related finding that wealthy African-American children who live
total of over half a million participants, concluded that self-esteem in upscale suburban communities, typically among even more
scores for African-American children were not lower, but actually affluent European-American children, do tend to show lower self-
slightly higher, than those for European-American children (Gray- esteem ratings than European-American children in the same
Little & Hafdahl, 2000). In particular, compared with their European- communities, presumably because they are comparing their situ-
American peers, African-American children under age 10 had ation to their even more affluent neighbors (Gray-Little & Hafdahl,
slightly lower scores, while those over age 10 had higher scores. 2000). The implications of this research are complex. Group
However, when children of all ages were taken together in a single identification seems to help minority children’s self-esteem when
comparison, the African-American children had higher self-esteem it focuses their comparisons on children living in similar circum-
scores than the European-American children. This difference has stances. However, it can hurt their self-esteem when their group
appeared in subsequent analyses as well (Eccleston et al., 2010) is juxtaposed with another group that is better off.
and raises questions about whether efforts to raise self-esteem Contrary to earlier ideas, low self-esteem may also not
would be effective ways to close academic achievement gaps be among the main reasons that some individuals transgress
between disadvantaged groups and more advantaged groups. against society. Low self-esteem was, for a time, a popular
children often prey on their younger siblings’ inability to developmental changes not only in cognition and attention
delay gratification by dangling in front of them an immedi- but also in the structure and functioning of the brain.
ately available toy or snack as an alternative to a much more Many studies have examined which factors influence the
desirable one that requires waiting. What 3-year-old can ability to self-regulate and thereby delay gratification. In
resist an older brother’s offer of a pretty good toy that is here one classic study, 4-year-olds were asked which snack food
now in exchange for a much better one due to arrive by mail they preferred: marshmallows or pretzels. The experiment-
in a few days? ers then told the children that they could have the less pre-
Self-regulation and the ability to delay gratification are ferred food immediately, or if they waited for a longer time
recurring topics in this book. We saw early elements of these (up to 15 minutes), they could receive the preferred food
processes in infants in Chapter 7, and we also considered (Mischel & Ebbeson, 1970; Mischel et al., 1972). Despite
them in Chapter 12 in the context of how they might con- the relatively short wait time, younger children clearly had
tribute to antisocial behavior. In this chapter, we want to great difficulty delaying gratification, opting instead for the
focus on how the processes of self-regulation and the abil- readily available, smaller gain.
ity to delay gratification change over the course of normal One variable influencing the ability to wait was whether
child development. We will address how they are related to the children were left alone during the 15-minute delay
with the reward in front of them, which made it harder to develop a wide range of strategies, from verbally reminding
resist immediate gratification than when the reward was not themselves of the long-term goal, to diverting their atten-
present. This result is not surprising; adults, too, are much tion, to changing their attitude toward the various reward
more tempted by visible rewards. A subtler finding is that factors (Mischel, 1996).
the ability to delay gratification may involve an attentional A common method of distancing ourselves from tempta-
component that changes over the course of development tion involves mentally replacing desired objects with symbols
(Mischel & Ebbeson, 1970; Mischel et al., 1972, 1973). for those objects. In one study of this strategy, researchers
As children grow older, they seem more able to direct their showed 3- and 4-year-olds two piles that contained differ-
attention to things other than the immediately appealing ent numbers of jelly beans: a pile of three and a pile of eight
object by focusing on either the other things in the room (Carlson et al., 2005). They were taught that if they pointed
or various things in memory as a purposeful distraction. at one pile, a puppet would get to take that pile, and they
As adults, we use these strategies so automatically that we would receive the other pile. Although they clearly under-
may not realize how common they are. (They are perhaps stood the instructions, the children had trouble learning to
more obvious in attempts to deal with pain.) To overcome inhibit pointing to the pile that they wanted. They often
immediate impulses in favor of longer-term gains, children pointed to the larger one and then unhappily received the
SELF-REGULATION 479
smaller one for themselves. However, when the piles were Brain Maturation and the
replaced with symbols for larger and smaller amounts (for
example, a picture of an elephant and a picture of a mouse) Development of Self-Regulation
and the researchers explained the relationships between the Along with the cognitive strategies for delaying gratification
symbols and jelly beans, the children could much more eas- that emerge with development, there are good reasons to
ily point to the mouse symbolizing the smaller amount in suspect that some patterns of brain maturation also con-
order to receive the larger pile. The symbols distanced the tribute to this ability. When a child chooses to eat pretzels
children from the temptation and allowed “cooler heads” to immediately rather than wait for the marshmallows she
prevail over the immediate desire (Garon & Moore, 2007). really wants, it may be because the child is unable to inhibit
The effects of symbols suggest that the abilities to inhibit the desire for the pretzels long enough to wait for the larger
responses and delay gratification are linked to both atten- reward. Whenever the notion of inhibition is raised, the
tional abilities and the ability to use mental representations brain’s frontal lobes are potentially involved. As discussed
to regulate behaviors (Garon et al., 2008). This complex in Chapter 2, the frontal lobes, which are part of the fron-
interplay of factors, some of which continue to develop tal cortex, lie behind the forehead at the front of the brain.
throughout childhood, helps explain why the ability to Among their many capacities, one key role involves execu-
delay gratification continues to improve gradually for many tive functions, the cognitive activities involved in goal-
years after the preschool period. directed tasks and problem solving. These abilities allow us
Children show substantial individual differences in the to optimize not just immediate payoffs but also long-term
ability to delay gratification. Some 4-year-olds could wait benefits (see Chapter 10).
much longer than others for the snack they badly wanted. One of the most famous cases in neuropsychology, which
Interestingly, when the researchers followed up with those involved massive frontal lobe damage, began to reveal these
same groups of children more than 10 years later, they structures’ crucial contributions to self-regulation, espe-
found that the ability to delay gratification at age 4 was cially emotional regulation. Phineas Gage, a 25-year-old
correlated with many later aspects of cognitive and social railroad worker, was a victim of an unfortunate accident
behavior, including SAT scores (Mischel et al., 1989). Some in 1848 in which a large iron rod entered his head at such
researchers have also argued that a strong ability to delay an angle that it caused massive damage to the frontal lobes
gratification, or exercise “effortful control,” as a toddler is (see Figure 13.11). Quite remarkably, Gage survived the
a personality trait that remains stable for several years, and trauma and lived for 12 more years, but his personality had
it manifests in such areas as having a stronger conscience changed dramatically. He no longer seemed to have much
(Kochanska & Knaack, 2003). control over his emotions; he had become impulsive and
more prone to unnecessary risks. The injury impaired his
ability to inhibit many aspects of his behavior. The rela-
Q: How can strategies influence the tionship between frontal lobe damage and difficulties in
development of self-regulation? self-regulation has been repeatedly documented for over
150 years (Heatherton, 2011).
SELF-REGULATION 481
through various tasks as the school day unfolds. These skills Our discussion of self-regulation has focused on the
gradually improve to a threshold that is sufficient for success child’s cognitive and emotional states and the interactions
in the classroom and do not seem to represent a qualitative between them. These factors are critical for understanding
shift. Yet, children vary considerably in when they achieve how self-regulation develops, but it is in no way sufficient
such levels of self-regulation, with boys typically developing for a full understanding of self-control and moderation of
later than girls (Mathews et al., 2009). children’s behavior. Parents, peers, and the culture at large
There are many ways to measure the different dimen- also affect how children internalize norms, attitudes, and
sions of self-regulation, but one simple task, known as the ways of expressing emotions. These topics, however, are bet-
Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders (HTKS) task, offers an inte- ter discussed in Chapters 14 and 15, which deal with the
grated assessment of many components of self-regulation, influences of the family and of the larger culture.
including attention, memory, executive skills, and impulse
control. The task involves having children initially touch
the body part mentioned with their hand. It then progresses
to asking the children to touch “the opposite” body part (for
example, the toes when the child hears “head” or the shoul-
Developing a Theory of Mind
ders when the child hears “knees”). This second part of In the course of a day of running errands, working, and
the task requires attention, working memory, and response interacting with family members, a typical adult makes
inhibition—all components of self-regulation (McClelland many inferences about his own mind and the minds of
& Cameron, 2012; Wanless et al., 2011). Moreover, the task others. Consider a middle-aged father arising at 6:00 a.m.
is easy to use in real-world settings and predicts success in As the alarm goes off, he wakes and momentarily consid-
the classroom. In addition, it has been used cross-culturally ers what a strange dream he was having. His wife starts to
and lends further support to findings of cross-cultural differ- stir, and he waits to ask her a question until she sits up in
ences in the development of self-regulation skills. Research bed. He then asks her if she should pick up pizza on the
using the HTKS task has shown repeatedly that children way home that evening. She replies that she will be com-
from Asian countries tend to have higher levels of self- ing home early that day and will have time to do some
regulation early on than children from Western countries, shopping. He nods. Eating breakfast, he hears a radio com-
such as the United States (Oh & Lewis, 2008; Sabbagh mentator railing against a politician he has not heard of
et al., 2006; Wanless et al., 2011; see Figure 13.12). The rea- before. He assumes that the criticism is probably suspect.
sons for the cross-cultural and gender differences are unclear, His teenage son sits down to breakfast and says, “Great,
but there are correlations with differences in socialization I love eating Brand X cornflakes five days in a row.” The
across the cultures. For example, teachers in Asian cultures father acknowledges that he has been too busy to go shop-
tend to give more instructions concerning behavioral regula- ping recently. Driving to work, the father notices a street
tion (Lan et al., 2009). Thus, a child’s local cultural context sign that was struck by a car the night before and is now
may help accelerate the development of self-regulation skills. pointing the wrong way. He realizes that drivers who do
A B
FIGURE 13.12 Cross-cultural differences in self-regulation. Numerous studies have reported that (A) 5-year-olds in Asian cultures have
higher average levels of self-regulation than (B) 5-year-olds in North American cultures. Such differences may arise from differences in socializa-
tion practices.
Method:
1. Twenty 12-month-old infants, twenty 18-month-old infants, and
nineteen great apes participated.
2. The experimenter “looked” at an object using only his eyes, his eyes
together with his head, or his head with his eyes closed, or various
controls.
3. The experimenter recorded the mean proportion of looks by the
participants to the target.
The whites of human eyes make eye gaze direction
Results: much easier to discern.
Human infants looked at the target mostly as a consequence of where Age Eyes
Yes No
the experimenter’s eyes looked, since the whites of human eyes make
Head Yes 12 0.20 0.06
eye gaze direction much easier to discern. Great apes were influenced 18 0.33 0.05
by both eyes and the head, but the head was more important.
No 12 0.09 0
18 0.24 0.08
Conclusion: Human infants use eye direction mostly.
Humans from at least 1 year of age are especially skilled at using eye Eyes
gaze direction to infer targets of mutual interest when in the company Yes No
of another person. In contrast, great apes rely more on the much Head Yes 0.53 0.35
cruder, and potentially misleading, cue of head direction. No 0.13 0.06
Great apes used head direction mostly.
Source study: Tomasello et al. (2007).
M
ost research comparing social cognition in humans
and other species has been with nonhuman primates,
such as chimpanzees, gorillas, and various species
of monkeys, since their genomes are most closely
related to that of humans (chimpanzee DNA shares 96 percent
of its sequence with human DNA). Yet, from an evolutionary per-
spective, it does not make sense as a research strategy. While
primates vary dramatically in their degree of sociability—some
species are loners and others are “groupies”—some nonpri-
mate species are also highly social, sometimes in ways that
are closely linked to human activities. It is especially interesting
to ask how these other species—which do not share as many
broad cognitive abilities with humans—might nonetheless have
sophisticated social cognitive skills. Such a focus starts to reveal
just how important general cognitive capacities might be to the
FIGURE 13.15 Dog social cognition. Recent evidence sug-
development of social skills in humans (as opposed to more
gests that dogs may have been selected for a set of specific
specific adaptations for social cognition). perceptual and cognitive skills for detecting social information
Few groups are more appropriate for such questions than and intentions in humans, often in ways that exceed those found
dogs (see Figure 13.15). Dogs started to differentiate from in great apes. These findings are important for understanding the
wolves roughly 15,000 years ago (Savolainen et al., 2002), and origins of our own species’ abilities to negotiate our social world.
certain traits were selected for as these animals adapted to a
special social niche with humans. Dogs may have been selected eye gaze and sensing human goals (Miklósi & Topál, 2012).
for their ability to partially perceive mental states in humans and The fascinating question revolves around how simple the “seed”
work with them cooperatively rather than competitively in ways social cognitive skills need to be in young dogs to enable them
that maximize mutual benefit (Hare & Tomasello, 2005), and to to later be such adept social companions to humans. Answering
excel at social as opposed to causal thinking (Brauer et al., 2006; this question will help us better understand what might be the
Miklósi & Topál, 2012). Thus, although dogs do far worse than minimal amount of social learning biases we might want to grant
chimpanzees at using nonsocial cues to find the location of hid- to human infants. It is impressive that higher-order social cogni-
den food, they do better than chimpanzees in locating food on the tive behaviors, such as the human tendency to “overimitate” the
basis of pointing gestures made by humans (Brauer et al., 2006). intentional acts of others (Lyons et al., 2011), are also found in
Developmental studies have been particularly informative as dogs but not in apes (Miklósi & Topál, 2012).
to how dogs might differ from wolves. When dogs and wolves Dogs and humans may also provide an example of conver-
are both hand reared in equivalent socially rich human envi- gent evolution of a set of social skills. Because dogs are in many
ronments, young puppies show more advanced social cogni- ways more socially skilled and socially perceptive than most
tion skills than young wolf pups. When hand-raised dogs and nonhuman primates, it seems that they developed these skills
wolves were given pointing tasks, older wolves could use point- long after any common mammalian ancestor to both dogs and
ing cues to find hidden food, but younger ones (4-month-olds) primates. Thus, they must have developed their social skills
could not, while 4-month-old puppies did just fine (Gácsi et al., independently from those adaptations that humans may have
2009). Young dogs actually prefer human companions to other acquired to be socially skilled. That convergence raises two
dogs, while young wolves do not (Morell, 2009). Dogs seem questions: Have dogs adapted to the social niche in subtly dif-
prepared to think more adeptly about social states, especially ferent ways than human infants? Do dogs use different mecha-
those in humans. Wolves can be gradually trained to be more nisms than humans in developing social skills, or is there only
social, but they do not seem to have the head start that dogs do. one straightforward way to build social skills, such that both
There is active debate on just how rich a theory of mind to species independently evolved essentially the same building
grant to dogs, but it does seem that they are adept at following blocks of social cognition and perception?
Pencils Smarties
Step 3 for a child at 3 years 4 months Step 3 for a child at 3 years 11 months
in the basket (Frith & Frith, 1999). Thus, across the different behavioral orientation toward reducing an uncomfortable
types of false-belief tasks, children younger than about 3½ physiological state or increasing a pleasant one. This kind of
fail to see how a person’s beliefs might cause them to act in drive or “desire” is much simpler and more immediate than
a way that is contrary to the facts. These younger children a goal-driven behavior. For example, a fly lands on sugary
seem to view human beliefs as if people were all-knowing soft drink residue because it “wants” to eat sweet things.
(Barrett & Driesenga, 2001). Of course, the sense of “want” that we attribute to the fly
is not the same as our understanding of what it means for
a 4-year-old to want another child’s toy. In the child’s case,
Q: Describe two different types of false-belief we rely on our own mature theory of mind and assume that
tasks. the child’s wanting is associated with a rich set of beliefs
about what the toy is like, who owns it, and what owning it
would be like. On the other hand, a child who has not yet
How, then, do younger children understand beliefs if passed the false-belief task may not have that level of insight
they are unable to see them as fallible? One proposal is that about the kinds of thoughts and feelings a playmate associ-
before about age 3½ children tend to think about beliefs as ates with wanting a toy and may see other people’s behaviors
if they were desires (Wellman, 1990). To understand this in ways that more closely resemble how we see the fly.
frame of mind, consider that when we observe simpler ani-
mals, explanations based on drives (internal physiological
states like hunger or thirst) are sometimes the most plausible
way of explaining their behaviors. We do not think of a fly Q: How might a 2-year-old’s theory of mind be
as having beliefs, but we can imagine it having something different from a 4-year-old’s theory of mind?
that might be construed as a desire in the sense of a drive or
That That
person sure curb made
is clumsy. me trip.
A B
FIGURE 13.20 The fundamental attribution error. The fundamental attribution error leads people to (A) attribute negative behaviors in
others to internal trait-like dispositions but (B) the same behavior in themselves to situations. Researchers have asked how the relative empha-
ses on dispositions and situations change with development.
Traits and Optimism TABLE 13.3 Youthful optimism about negative traits. This
table shows the three types of negative traits that children and adults
Your view of yourself clearly influences your view of your were asked about in the study of children’s optimism about the
future. If you believe you are a powerful, competent per- future. The younger the child, the more likely he was to see negative
son, you are inclined to think the future bodes well for traits as likely to improve. Adapted from Lockhart et al. (2002).
you. If you see yourself as helpless and incompetent, you
will more likely believe that uncontrollable events are in
store and that you will not manage them well. Two key child was then asked what the child in the story would be
questions help frame these issues with respect to develop- like at various times in the future, up to young adulthood.
ment: (1) Are there any general patterns concerning how Even the youngest children expected more positive change in
children at different ages view the future? (2) What kinds psychological attributes than physical ones. But remarkably,
of individual differences develop among children in this they also expected that the physical traits, some as permanent
respect? as a missing finger, would dramatically improve in the future.
Regarding general developmental patterns, younger chil- This youthful optimism reflects a positive attitude
dren seem to be more optimistic about the future than older toward others’ traits and presumably toward their own,
ones (see Figure 13.22). In one set of studies, each child was since the protagonists were of the same age and sex as
told about another child of his or her same sex and age who the participants. Younger children’s optimism may help
had a negative attribute that was either physical (such as an protect them from getting discouraged by the inevitable
allergy) or psychological (such as being messy) or a blend failures that come from being unskilled in so many areas
of the two (such as being physically aggressive; Lockhart (Lockhart et al., 2002). Interestingly, a study comparing
et al., 2002). Table 13.3 shows several sample attributes. The young Japanese children’s and adults’ responses to the
same type of stories about children found the same general
pattern as in the original U.S. study: the Japanese chil-
dren showed much more optimism about improvement of
No change negative traits than did the adults (Lockhart et al., 2008).
.7 Average change However, the Japanese data suggested a somewhat differ-
Extreme change
.6
ent developmental trajectory, in which Japanese adults
tended to be more optimistic about some scenarios than
Percentage of responses
12
0
Expected-reward Unexpected-reward No-reward
condition condition condition
B
made them discount their earlier, intrinsic motivation for it, a for drawing were later asked to draw to earn a reward, they
phenomenon called the overjustification effect. After draw- tended to make a larger number of drawings that were of
ing to earn the certificate, they seemed to say to themselves, lower quality. Some children even set up little assembly lines
“I was doing this because I knew I’d get a reward, not because to churn out lots of drawings that were each much simpler
I liked doing it.” Thus, their self-concept mediated the way the than the ones they produced before being rewarded. This
reward influenced their behavior. new approach to the task fit their interpretation of working
Later studies have shown that if a child simply knows for the reward (Loveland & Olley, 1979).
that an adult is carefully watching her behavior, it can Nonetheless, some have argued that extrinsic rewards
be enough to reduce intrinsic motivation for the behav- need not undermine creativity if they are handled carefully.
ior (Lepper & Green, 1975). In at least some situations, a These researchers suggest that if you fine-tune the system
child who knows she is being monitored may reframe the to reward only high-quality work and explicitly convey this
situation in such a way that she believes that her behavior criterion, the rewards can enhance creativity (Eisenberger
is motivated by an external factor. Even the simple act of & Cameron, 1998). Yet, these manipulations can be subtle
praising a child’s actions can have a similar effect, under- and difficult to implement with young children. In the long
mining motivations for those actions (Henderlong & run, the rewards may still subtly undermine motivation
Lepper, 2002). Yet, not all praise has negative effects; prais- and creativity if they are removed. For example, one study
ing internal factors like effort, rather than actions, is less on educational video games that teach mathematical skills
likely to decrease motivation. found that games designed around intrinsic motivational
These kinds of interactions and individual responses are factors (such as embedding math problems in the most
cognitively complex. The child must assess her own mental exciting parts of the games) were seven times as likely to
and motivational states, take into account the context in be played in the future as those that were designed around
which she is acting, weigh the external conditions influenc- extrinsic factors (such as embedding math problems in tests
ing her actions, and then reassess the relationships between at the end of the game, where achieving a good score was the
the situational and dispositional factors that drive her behav- reward; Habgood & Ainsworth, 2011).
iors. Thus, the presence of the overjustification effect in pre- In research that rewarded children with stickers for eating
schoolers suggests that they clearly can take into account vegetables they disliked, the studies reported an increased
both situational and dispositional factors when interpreting preference for the rewarded vegetables more than 3 months
their own behaviors. later (Cooke et al., 2011). The success may be because the
One of the most insidious ways that extrinsic rewards can vegetables were initially strongly disliked, and by being
influence self-perception and motivation comes from how rewarded for eating them, the children learned that they
children change their approach to a task when the rewards were not so bad. On the other hand, if the children’s ini-
are still present. In the example of drawing with markers, if tial dislike for the vegetables had been mild, the result of
the children who initially expected (and received) a reward reinforcement could have been negligible. Thus, when there
CONCLUSIONS 499
that become more sophisticated over time, again support- false-belief tasks tell only part of the story of how theory of
ing a recurring theme in this book of the value of focusing mind develops. More recent research is beginning to link
on emerging abilities as opposed to disappearing deficits. In simple patterns of goal attribution in infancy to more com-
addition, the different facets of the self interact and support plex patterns of thought about second-order beliefs in late
growth in each other over the course of development. Thus, childhood. It is therefore misleading to think that there is
an increasing sense of the private self supports the develop- a particular age at which a child acquires a theory of mind.
ment of the extended self, and the conceptual self interacts Instead, it makes more sense to think of infants and young
with the autobiographical self. In fact, there are interactions children as using a rich array of cognitive and perceptual
between almost every pairing of the dimensions of self that tools to gain insight into the behaviors of social agents. Even
reinforce each other and that may ultimately help create a an understanding of false beliefs now seems to be within
more integrated sense of self. the conceptual reach of infants, at least at an implicit level.
In the development of self-esteem and self-efficacy, Again, emerging competencies is a powerful theme. Younger
which add an evaluative component to self-knowledge children and infants do not have as many of these tools, and
and understanding, we see once again how basic cognitive they use some of them in more limited ways. Nonetheless,
skills influence a child’s social interactions and emotional they are sensitive from a very early age to some special prop-
states. Contrary to popular accounts, however, low self- erties of social agents, and they benefit from ways in which
esteem may not be the prime cause of various acting-out all these tools work together to help them think about the
behaviors, and overly high self-esteem can pose its own set minds of others.
of problems. Thoughts about the self are also related to A critical part of thinking about others involves infer-
emotional intelligence and self-regulation. The ability to ences about dispositions and traits and how they might
regulate ourselves by deferring gratification may heavily vary or stay the same over time. Children, much more than
involve attentional skills that enable us to focus on other adults, tend to believe that negative traits in younger peo-
objects rather than the most salient goal. Self-regulation ple will improve and that positive traits will endure. This
ability is also influenced by context: whether those influ- youthful optimism may be a way of protecting themselves
ences are local, such as the presence of a mirror, or cultural, against the failures that younger children inevitably experi-
reflecting the way a culture values strong self-regulation ence as they mature. These attributions are critical, as well,
skills early on. In this way, we see that self-regulation can to understanding motivation and creativity in the young
be amplified or reduced by the environment in which a child. More broadly, these developmental patterns and the
child develops. related idea of self-determination reinforce the theme that
The child’s understanding of other people’s minds shows for optimal development, children need to be active agents
several major developmental changes. Based on the much- in their own developmental journeys and not just passive
studied false-belief tasks, earlier researchers believed that recipients of environmental inputs and contingencies. Of
children younger than about 3½ cannot think of others course, external inputs and encouragement are important,
as having false beliefs about a current situation. But more but they should always occur in ways that are sensitive to a
recent studies suggest that young children’s failures on child’s intrinsic interests and curiosities.
● Self-efficacy, the belief about our capacity to achieve goals, iors is critical to functioning as a social individual. Younger
is a more focused kind of assessment often tied to a particu- children have rudimentary attributional skills, but they often
lar situation. Feelings of low self-efficacy are often easier to seem to miss the subtler connections between traits, situa-
change in a child than low self-esteem. tions, and behaviors. They may be more likely to use evalu-
ative reasoning, making global evaluations of others as good
Self-Regulation or bad and missing the more subtle aspects of how traits can
● Younger children are more likely to show difficulties in predict behaviors.
self-regulation and hence have problems in delaying grati- Younger children show greater optimism than older chil-
fication. Difficulties may frequently arise from inadequate dren and adults, leading them to believe that negative traits
strategies for diverting their attention from a desired object change for the better as people age. This optimism may have
or from an inability to think of ways to make the desired adaptive value if it shields the child from discouragement
object less salient or accessible. Some of these problems may when she faces failures associated with being young and
arise from relatively late maturation of regions of the frontal inexperienced.
lobes. Damage to these regions in adult brains can also cause ● Attributions about the self can powerfully influence motiva-
self-regulation problems. Self-regulation levels vary across tion and creativity. Even for preschool children, an intrinsi-
both genders and cultures. They seem to be influenced by cally interesting form of play can become much more like
surrounding cultural and local contexts as well as socializa- work if they perceive that they are doing it merely to earn
tion practices. external rewards.
● It is possible to motivate children without undermining
Developing a Theory of Mind intrinsic motivation. This can be done by reminding chil-
● Young children are certainly aware of the distinctive nature dren of the intrinsic values of their actions as well as enabling
of social beings, but at first they may understand other social feelings of choice and control when using more external
beings’ behavior only in terms of their goals and desires. forms of reinforcement.
between parents and their children, and the effects of dif- potential tradeoffs associated with placing children in foster
ferent environments on both children and their parents. We care as opposed to keeping them in their own problematic
will also explore how all of these factors interact in contrib- home setting?
uting to long-lasting characteristics in children. In all of these cases, it is important to carefully monitor
We will then consider the relations between siblings, the kinds of methods that researchers use and the extent
including whether a child’s position relative to other siblings to which these methods can support clear conclusions
has a systematic effect on development: Does birth order about causal patterns. We will also want to keep in mind
matter, and does it matter more in some kinds of families the cycles of interactions that can happen in a family and
than in others? We will also examine whether siblings try to the challenges of disentangling causes from effects. At the
differentiate themselves from one another. And we will con- broadest level, we will consider families in terms of two
sider the other social and cognitive effects that sibling rela- complex webs of interactions, one that occurs among family
tionships may have on the children themselves and within members and another that occurs between the family and
the family system. the larger culture in which it is situated. Our goal will be
We will then turn to the developmental consequences to discern some reliable patterns that hold across these two
of changing family demographics. In the United States webs of interactions.
and many other developed countries, the trends in recent
decades point toward older parents, smaller families, and
more families in which both parents work full-time out-
side the home. Each of these trends can change the family
dynamic, influencing both parents and children. We will Parenting
also consider some of the changes to family structure that
Parenting can take several forms that serve different func-
have become more prevalent in recent decades, including
tions for the child. One review described seven important
single-parent families, families with same-sex parents, and
components of parenting that guide a child’s development
families in which parents divorce and perhaps remarry,
(Brooks-Gunn & Markman, 2005). Each of these compo-
blending two previously separate families. There is now
nents forms a continuum with positive and negative forms
extensive research on what kinds of effects these different
of parental behaviors at opposite ends. The positive and
family structures have on children and how, in some cases,
negative behaviors show dramatic differences in their preva-
they seem to vary, depending on the children’s age and
lence and their effects across different socioeconomic and
gender.
cultural groups, and these patterns are correlated with dif-
Finally, we will consider the prevalence and effects of
ferent outcomes for children.
child abuse. Such cases can range from physical and verbal
abuse to incest. These tragedies raise significant concerns ● Nurturance involves showing children warmth and
about the long-term consequences for children. In addition, responding to their needs and changing emotions (see
they demonstrate potential effects across generations, as Chapter 6). When parents fail to nurture, they may be
abused children become abusive parents themselves, creat- either disengaged or inappropriately intrusive.
ing a “cycle of abuse.” We will also ask about the potential ● Discipline methods that attempt to explain the reason for
effects of foster care on children. For example, what are the a punishment are generally considered more effective than
PARENTING 505
punishments that are coercive, physical, or insufficiently children and the level of parents’ demands or control. This
explained (see Chapter 12). theory holds that a parent can be either high or low on each
● Teaching a child ideally involves questioning a child care- dimension and that a parent’s particular combination of
fully to find out where she is coming from cognitively. warmth and control ends up setting the tone for most inter-
Parents who are less effective teachers may instead take actions between parent and child. Simplifying each of these
over the task for the child without considering the child’s dimensions into high and low values yields a two-by-two
own mental state, or they may not offer any help. table showing the four most commonly discussed parenting
● The language that parents use to engage the child can be styles (see Table 14.1).
adjusted to suit his age and situation, and it can include Table 14.1 reflects one of the most active areas of research
questions, expansions of the child’s utterances, and a rich on parenting (Maccoby & Martin, 1983). The authoritative,
narrative structure (see Chapter 8). Alternatively, a parent permissive, and authoritarian styles were originally proposed
may talk to the child only rarely or may talk “at the child” by developmental psychologist Diana Baumrind in 1966
without encouraging interaction. (Baumrind, 1966). (The fourth style, neglectful / uninvolved,
● The materials that parents provide to foster creative play— logically results from a low level of both warmth and con-
like books, toys, and games—enable a child to imagine, trol, but it was not a major component of Baumrind’s origi-
explore, and interact more fruitfully with others. Other nal account.) Although threads of these ideas extend back
parents may provide minimal support or prefer materials though earlier studies (Grusec, 2011; Hess & Shipman,
more useful for “managing” the child, such as sitting him 1965; Lewin, 1946; Sears et al., 1957), Baumrind initially
in front of a television for long periods. developed these categories after observing preschool and
● Monitoring includes being aware of a child’s activities school-age children in school, in lab settings, and interacting
throughout the day, such as where the child is and what with their parents at home. She also interviewed the parents
she is reading or playing with or watching on television. about how they interacted with their children. The parent-
A low-monitoring parent may be barely aware of a child’s ing styles that Baumrind proposed appeared quite distinct
activities and may not really have any sense of how a child and were associated with different behaviors and traits in the
spends her time. children.
● Parents manage their children’s time by arranging play Over several decades of research and analysis, Baumrind’s
dates or other recurring activities that give a child’s life parenting styles have endured remarkably well (Baumrind,
structure and regularity. Parents who fail to adequately 1991). Given that versions of these types do seem to exist, at
manage a child’s time might not plan for the days or weeks least among middle-class North American parents, the obvi-
ahead, leaving the child with no routines and no sense of ous question is whether they directly influence aspects of
what is coming next. the child. Researchers have frequently addressed this ques-
tion by looking for correlations between parenting styles
Across these seven dimensions of parental behavior, some
and aspects of children’s behavior and personalities (see
common aspects distinguish the more positive, balanced
Figure 14.3). Any significant correlations were then assumed
ways of interacting with children from the negative ways,
to result from the parenting style—the classic fallacy of
which can be either quite rigid or disengaged. In particular,
equating correlation with causation. This is an important
parents tend to vary in the level of warmth and responsive-
concern to keep in mind as we look at each parenting style
ness they show to their children and in the level of demands
and the patterns of correlation with developmental outcomes
or control they place on them. Variations in these broader
and consider what sorts of causal influences are suggested.
aspects of parental behavior form the foundation for one of
Our first examples are limited to middle-class, white, North
the most influential theories about parenting effects.
American children. They mostly pertain to children in early
and middle adolescence, as many researchers assume that
parenting effects may be the most dramatic after several
years of parenting.
Parenting Styles
For several decades, researchers have examined parenting
styles and the possible influences of such styles on children.
High Control Low Control
We define parenting styles as the ways in which parents
engage in behaviors and have attitudes toward their children High Warmth Authoritative Permissive
that create a particular parenting environment or climate.
Low Warmth Authoritarian Neglectful/uninvolved
As already noted, the most prominent theory of parenting
styles is based on two main ways that parental behaviors TABLE 14.1 Parenting styles. Each of the four parenting styles
vary: the warmth and responsiveness of parents toward their involves a different combination of warmth and control.
PARENTING 507
a child’s behavior by manipulating his feelings. These Children of neglectful/uninvolved parents tend to fare
manipulations can include withdrawing affection from the badly. One especially clear pattern is their tendency to
child or inducing guilt or shame. Psychological control can be especially susceptible to peer pressure and therefore to
include a parent’s intrusions into a child’s deeply personal engage more often in behaviors that do not conform to adult
domains, in which she feels she should have autonomy, espe- norms (Bednar & Fisher, 2003; Durbin et al., 1993; Shuck-
cially as an adolescent (Steinberg, 2005). Psychological con- smith et al., 1995). Some studies have suggested that girls
trol, which is more typical of authoritarian parents, is often with neglectful/uninvolved parents are more likely than
associated with more depressive and anxious symptoms in other girls to become involved in peer groups whose norms
children (Steinberg, 2005). Behavioral control, in contrast, are clearly different from those of the dominant adult cul-
involves attempting to regulate a child’s behavior by setting ture (Brown et al., 1993; Lamborn et al., 1991). Table 14.2
guidelines that follow the norms and values of the family summarizes the features of each parenting style and some of
or society—perhaps by requiring certain homework hours, the associated developmental outcomes.
a set bedtime, or appropriate ways to speak to older adults.
Compared with psychological control, behavioral control is
more common among authoritative parents and is generally Q: What are Baumrind’s three parenting styles,
associated with fewer negative outcomes in children.
Finally, while Baumrind’s initial focus was not on the and how are they associated with psychological
fourth cell, it is important to mention the neglectful / unin- differences in children? When and how is
volved parents, who seem to ignore and not care about their gender of the child relevant?
children. They do not interact much with their children,
which makes their style almost a form of nonparenting.
They might be oblivious to the child’s hopes, desires, and
fears and uninterested in monitoring the child’s behaviors.
This category is less discussed than the others, but may be Parenting Contexts
more likely among parents who struggle with many prob- Many of these differences among children seem to fit with
lems of their own, making them unable to function well causal explanations of how parents might influence their chil-
as parents. Imagine, for example, a single mother who is dren. But accounting for cross-cultural and social class differ-
deeply clinically depressed. She has little motivation to do ences inevitably complicates those causal stories because the
anything more than the bare minimum required to get associations between parenting styles and children’s behaviors
through the day. Her child is largely irrelevant to her. In differ across cultural and socioeconomic groups.
other cases, a parent may actually develop feelings of hos-
tility toward a child that consequently contribute to a ten- Parenting across Cultures Even when a parenting
dency to be neglectful and uninvolved. style appears to be the same across several cultures, it can
Authoritarian Low warmth, high control Dependent, low social Hostile Set low goals
(overbearing, rigid, competency, look to
psychological control) authority
Permissive Low warmth, low control Immature, low impulse No obvious gender No obvious gender
control, not inclined to differences differences
take responsibility or act
independently
Authoritative High warmth, high control Self-reliant, self-controlled, Responsible Independent and socially
(balanced, flexible, behavioral willing to explore responsible
control)
Neglectful/ Low warmth, low control Strongly influenced by May show a greater
uninvolved peers tendency to join peer
groups that depart from
adult norms
TABLE 14.2 Effects of parenting styles Four different parenting styles have been consistently associated with particular kinds of behaviors
and traits in middle-class North American children.
PARENTING 509
diapers (which were neglected at day care), and help the specifically targeting groups that are more likely to show
oldest child with his homework. It is hardly surprising that authoritarian or neglecting/uninvolved parenting styles
this parent would lack the energy to have rich, interactive (Cowan et al., 1997). These interventions are based on the
conversations with the children. It would be easier, at least belief that even in stress-ridden families, the authoritarian
at first, to give clear orders and back them up with threats style is still not best for either the children or the parents.
of punishment. In the short term, authoritarian parenting A number of these interventions have shown positive effects
styles can often produce quick behavioral changes more for both parents and children. The most compelling studies
easily. In the long term, however, those styles may become randomly assigned parents to either a group that received
less effective, resulting in the range of negative behaviors instruction on parenting skills or a control group that was
more commonly seen among children of authoritarian involved in an activity unrelated to parenting (Cowan &
parents. Cowan, 2002). This experimental approach enabled the
researchers to disentangle mere correlations from causal
The Ecological Systems Approach The influence influences. In several such studies, the children of parents
of context has been described in a broader sense as the who were coached to be less coercive and more interactive
ecological systems approach to families, which empha- showed corresponding improvements in behavior (Spoth
sizes that contextual factors, including culture and social et al., 2001). Training generally focused on creating
class, among others, are crucial to understanding the opportunities for positive family interactions, establishing
child’s development within a family (Bronfenbrenner, appropriate expectations about children and appropriate
1977, 1979, 1986). This approach, which was devised by monitoring and discipline, teaching parents how to coach
developmental psychologist Urie Bronfenbrenner, consid- their children to resist peers when needed, reducing and
ers the child’s environments at several scales, ranging from managing instances of family conflict, and emphasizing the
the family itself, to the school, to the larger culture. In addi- value of displaying positive emotions.
tion, other environments, such as the parents’ workplaces, The issue of parenting interventions becomes especially
may indirectly affect the child by limiting the parents’ important in the context of large-scale programs such as
time for interacting with the child or through the child’s Head Start. As described in Chapter 11, Head Start targets
experiences in day care. According to ecological systems millions of lower-income children throughout the United
accounts, attempts to characterize how families are impor- States in an effort to increase school readiness. A related
tant to a child’s development must consider the child’s program, Early Head Start, focuses on children between
role in each of these “social ecosystems” and how that role birth and 3 years. One facet of both programs is to teach
interacts with other participants in the systems. Ecologi- parenting skills to lower-income disadvantaged parents. It
cal approaches attempt to shed light on the influences of was assumed that such interventions might also promote
a child’s many social contexts by drawing parallels to the school readiness by fostering more learning-rich interac-
ways that organisms interact in biological systems. We will tions between young children and those around them.
return to Bronfenbrenner’s ecological systems approach in There have been many analyses of the outcomes of such
Chapter 15 as part of our consideration of children’s roles programs, and a consensus seems to be emerging that par-
in social systems outside the family. These other systems enting skills can be successfully taught in such large-scale
include peer relationships, social groups, the media, and programs (Brooks-Gunn & Markman, 2005; Zhai et al.,
the larger culture in which a child develops. 2011); and, in at least some cases, interventions in ran-
dom assignment studies have been linked to higher levels
of school readiness (Sheridan et al., 2011). Thus, parents
Q: How do different parenting contexts like can, with guidance, become more attentive, less coercive,
and more interactive with their children. Interestingly, the
culture and SES affect the associations between effectiveness of these interventions differs, depending in
parenting styles and children’s psychological part on the parents’ race. These interventions tend to have
outcomes? a large effect with African-American parents, but they
often show no effect with European-American parents
(Sheridan et al., 2011). The reasons for these group differ-
ences are not yet clear.
Will the parents who undergo training maintain a
Interventions to Improve Parenting warmer, more interactive parenting style in the face of con-
Over the past few decades, researchers in North America tinuing life stresses? And do the effects on children’s behavior
and elsewhere have offered courses in parenting skills, with their parents have a positive influence on relationships
A B
FIGURE 14.4 Child effects. Although researchers traditionally tended to emphasize the effects of parental behavior on children, the children
themselves may have a powerful influence on their parents. For example, (A) a child who repeatedly throws tantrums may elicit very different
behaviors in his parents than (B) a child who is excessively shy.
PARENTING 511
NEW DIRECTION S IN DEVELOPMENTAL RESEARCH
A
s described in Chapter 2, epigenetic regulation is
one way that environmental influences can affect the
activity of genes. This kind of gene regulation occurs
when the chemicals surrounding a particular cell
type affect which of the cells’ genes are active or inactive, such
that when the cells divide, the genes of all the cells that they
create can have the same pattern of active and inactive genes.
As epigenetics becomes an increasingly important aspect of
the study of development, it is beginning to illuminate some of
the surprisingly strong ways that parents, children, and siblings
influence each other. FIGURE 14.5 Epigenetics and parental care. When rat mothers
For some time, researchers have been able to demonstrate lick their offspring more, that increased care can turn on gene
powerful epigenetic effects in animals, most notably in rats and pathways that produce more neurotransmitters involved in reducing
mice. For example, when a rat pup is frequently licked by its anxiety. Thus, a rat mother who is not stressed and who is able to
devote more extensive care to her offspring can cause the genes
mother, chemical changes to a certain gene related to hippo-
correlated with lower anxiety to switch on in her offspring.
campal function cause other genes to trigger the production
of higher levels of neurotransmitters, such as serotonin, in the
young rat (see Figure 14.5). That increase in neurotransmitter gene expression was “fixed” early in life. However, follow-up
levels in turn seems to bias the rat pup to be less fearful and studies have shown that these maternal effects on offspring’s
less reactive in stressful environments. Rat pups whose moth- genetic activity can be reversed through drug treatments in adult
ers lick them less often do not undergo these genetic changes, rats (Champagne et al., 2008; Weaver et al., 2005, 2006).
which results in lower levels of neurotransmitter production and It is, of course, a great leap from mother rats licking their pups
more fearful and reactive behavior (Caldji et al., 1998; Meaney, to the possibility of epigenetic effects of parenting in humans.
2001; Meaney & Szyf, 2005; Weaver et al., 2004). Thus, early But researchers in both animal systems and human develop-
maternal behavior can affect the activation or deactivation of a ment have drawn parallels, and work with nonhuman primates
particular set of genes, resulting in a long-term effect on the off- has strengthened those parallels. In some studies, an important
spring’s physiological and psychological responses to stresses. manipulation has been to compare peer-raised and mother-raised
This kind of effect initially was thought to be irreversible after monkeys (Suomi, 1997, 2005). As mentioned in Chapter 6,
either encourage approach and discourage withdrawal—or, In general, the same adult would much more likely slant
on other occasions, encourage the inhibition. The child’s toward an authoritarian style with a less cooperative child.
shyness affected the parents by making his interaction with A more naturalistic, longitudinal study of children with
(or withdrawal from) peers a more central issue in parent- conduct disorders found similar patterns: when difficult
child interactions (Rubin et al., 1998). children misbehave, that bout of misbehavior triggers more
The clearest way to study child effects is to set up an authoritarian behavior in parents, who then exercise more
interaction that can be repeated by many parent-child control and use more negative coercions (Dodge, 2002).
pairs. Researchers can experimentally “manipulate” the Other studies suggest that negative temperaments in chil-
child’s behavior by randomly assigning children to behave dren cause negative reactions in their parents, although
in particular ways and then compare the effects of the these studies are not experimental and depend on complex
child’s behavior on parents’ responses. In one study, chil- statistical analyses to infer child temperament effects on
dren were randomly assigned to be either compliant and parenting as opposed to effects in the opposite direction
cooperative or resistant and noncooperative while perform- (Ganiban et al., 2011; South et al., 2008). At least one study
ing a task with an adult stranger (Bugental et al., 1980). has shown that difficult temperament-related behavior in
PARENTING 513
Although some behavioral geneticists have focused on benefit some kinds of children much more than others, and
understanding genetic influences on either the parent’s for some it may even be detrimental. Similarly, some parent-
behavior or the child’s behavior, another approach offers ing effects may depend on aspects of the child that change
an account distinct from parental influences and child over the course of development, initially exerting little imme-
behavior. Instead of a one-way influence of the child on the diate sway, then strongly influencing the child for a period of
parent (or vice versa), the parent and a child might share a time before waning.
genetic disposition toward a particular trait, such as shyness Consider one example of how an interaction pattern
or irritability. That shared disposition could then cause a between parent and child might change with development
strong correlation between a particular style of parenting because of both parent and child effects. A parent who is
and a particular kind of behavior from the child. Likewise, prone to ordering children about might do so to a greater
if withdrawn parents tend to have withdrawn children, it extent with smaller and less vocal children who cannot
may not be the parents’ behavior or the children’s behav- respond adequately to this behavior. Thus, authoritarian
ior that causes the correlation, but the third factor of their parenting might wax and wane as a child develops. This
shared genotype. It is important to remember, however, case illustrates how more subtle differences in temperament
that even with shared genotypes, phenotypic expressions of and personality of children and parents can create quite
those genotypes may be quite different in young children intricate patterns of interaction. There are now “biopsycho-
and adults. Thus, a toddler who has a genotype that makes social” approaches that see certain combinations of biologi-
him irritable and therefore prone to crying fits and physical cal disposition, psychological tendencies, and sociocultural
tantrums when frustrated is not likely to have a parent who contexts as likely to either increase or decrease children’s
behaves in the same way, even if the genotype for their irri- risk of some adverse outcomes (such as becoming aggressive
tability is shared. That parent may be volatile and reactive toward others) early in life. These approaches also seek to
but is unlikely to plop down on the grocery store floor, burst account for ways that experiences with parents, peers, and
into tears, and have a physical tantrum. the broader social world either increase or reduce that risk
Behavioral genetic studies cover more than shared geno- (Dodge & Pettit, 2003). Figure 14.6 shows one model of the
type effects and accounts of how certain genotypes are related ways these interactions could contribute to conduct disor-
to child and parent effects. In animal studies, researchers der, which is a behavior pattern in children and adolescents
are also making headway in understanding the epigenetic that involves repeated incidents of actions that infringe on
mechanisms through which some parental behaviors can the rights of others. It is often called “antisocial behavior.”
affect genetic activity in offspring (see the New Directions The interactionist approach is valuable for its broad
in Developmental Research box). As more sophisticated yet nuanced view of the many influences affecting both
techniques emerge for studying the entire human genome parents and children, but its critics have cited one serious
and looking at complex interactions between sets of genes, a
much more diverse set of causal pathways between behaviors
and genes is likely to be part of any discussion of parenting.
Biological Biological
predisposition predisposition
Q: How can a behavioral genetic approach (genes) (prenatal environment)
Sociocultural Sociocultural
The Interactionist Approach context context
PARENTING 515
Conversational Interactions Manner of Play Reactions to Emotions
Boys Both parents talk to boys somewhat Both parents play more roughly with boys Fathers react negatively to crying,
less and focus more on nonpersonal and offer them toy vehicles and sports fearfulness, or other expressions that
or intrapersonal topics. equipment to play with. could signify weakness in boys.
Girls Mothers talk to girls more overall and Both parents play more gently with girls Both parents are more tolerant of
specifically about emotions. and offer them dolls and props for playing displays of distress and fear-related
house. emotions in girls.
TABLE 14.3 Some differences in how parents socialize boys and girls. The effects of parental socialization vary as a function of the
gender of the parent and that of the child; usually, same-gender pairs have the strongest influence. Nonetheless, these differences are often
more modest than expected, especially when children are younger than 6 years of age. From Maccoby (2003).
Gender biases in adults also seem to be more tolerated in children, it is difficult to prove that parenting actually
by children than other forms of biases. Thus, in one study caused the differences in children. To understand why it is
in Denmark, when children were asked to make judgments so hard to show a causal effect, we will now consider some
about exclusion from activities based on gender as opposed to models of parenting influence.
ethnicity, they thought that exclusion based on ethnicity was Even though studies of parent effects have tended to
less acceptable and more morally inappropriate (Moller & dominate the research on parenting and gender, children’s
Tenenbaum, 2011). This pattern follows earlier studies in behaviors can also strongly influence their parents. For
the United States showing a greater willingness to tolerate example, girls and boys often have strong toy preferences
exclusion based on gender than on ethnicity (Killen, 2007). well before 2 years of age, before they experience gender-
Thus, parents may get less negative feedback or resistance stereotyped toys or related interactions with their parents
when they show gender bias as opposed to other forms of (Maccoby, 2003). That is, they bring to their early interac-
group biases. tions certain behavioral patterns that may lead the parent to
In general, even though parents interact differently conclude that the child prefers one kind of toy to another
with their sons and their daughters across a wide spec- and then offer what the child seems to want. This pattern
trum of behaviors and situations (see Figure 14.7), like so sometimes surprises parents who avoid gender-stereotyped
many other aspects of socialization, it is difficult to show toys so as not to impose gender roles on their children. More
how these gender-related differences in parenting actually formally, researchers have long known that even when we
affect the children. Even when there are strong associations factor out differences in parents’ behaviors in terms of the
between parents’ behaviors and gender-related differences types of toys they provide or offer to their sons and daugh-
ters, the children still show gender-based differences in toy
preferences (Calders et al., 1989). If parental socialization
cannot fully account for young children’s early gendered
preferences, what other explanations are possible?
One alternative approach suggests that children have bio-
logically determined biases that influence their preferences
in ways that are initially independent of socialization, biases
that may have counterparts in other primates. It may seem
implausible that a child’s biological sex might somehow
directly bias him or her toward gender-stereotyped toys, but
several studies have supported that possibility. For example,
the extent to which a female fetus is exposed to testosterone
seems to relate to the likelihood that she will later show a
tendency to like toys that boys normally prefer. This asso-
ciation has been shown in rare cases where a female fetus
has the inherited adrenal gland disorder known as congeni-
A B
tal adrenal hyperplasia (CAH). CAH results in exposure
FIGURE 14.7 Gender and parenting. There are modest but con-
to higher than normal levels of masculinizing hormones,
sistent differences in how parents of each sex interact with children such as testosterone, in utero. Girls born with CAH show
of each sex. The impact of these differences on the children is less increased preferences for playing with gender-atypical toys
clear. (Berenbaum & Beltz, 2011; Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005;
Girl-typical behaviors
ters, influencing their toy preferences (Cohen-Bendahan
et al., 2005: Jordan-Young, 2011). Additional indirect effects 20
could occur through the intensive medical and psychiatric
attention that girls with CAH receive and expectations by
parents and others that their daughters will be “masculine”
(Jordan-Young, 2011). Such indirect effects, however, are
0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50 2.00 2.50
not likely to provide a full explanation for the different
Fetal testosterone level (nmol/L)
behaviors in girls who have higher prenatal levels of mascu-
FIGURE 14.8 Prenatal testosterone levels and sex-stereotyped
linizing hormones. In fact, when mothers of girls with CAH
behaviors. Higher levels of prenatal testosterone in girls are associ-
are studied, they do not treat their daughters in ways that ated with greater frequencies of childhood behaviors favored by
favor masculine behaviors and, if anything, show more of a boys. Adapted from Auyeung et al. (2009).
bias toward feminine behaviors (Hines, 2010).
Some researchers have examined testosterone levels
in the mother’s blood during pregnancy and then ana- (Hassett et al., 2008; Hines & Alexander, 2008; Williams &
lyzed their children’s behaviors, including toy pref- Pleil, 2008; see Figure 14.9). It is not yet known which
erences (Auyeung et al., 2009; Hines et al., 2002; see features of the toys drive these preferences, as monkeys
Figure 14.8). They found that the higher the testoster- clearly have no evolved tendencies to interact with any of
one levels, the greater the children’s tendency to play these objects. Future studies that carefully and systemati-
with stereotypically male toys. Thus, there was a “dose- cally vary the perceptual features of different categories
dependent” relationship between levels of testosterone of toys will help researchers understand the real nature of
and the extent to which male activities were preferred. the effect.
The mothers in these studies were unaware of their
own testosterone levels, making parental attitude and
behavior effects less plausible in this case. In addition,
some studies show that “antiandrogen” compounds that
reduce prenatal testosterone levels in boys are associ-
ated with decreased levels of masculine play (Swann
et al., 2010). At the same time, it is important to note
that, in statistical terms, variations in testosterone levels
in these studies could account for only part of the varia-
tion in toy preferences, leaving room for parental influ-
ences as well (Jordan-Young, 2011).
The effects of prenatal testosterone could still be inter-
acting with differences in parenting if parents notice subtle
differences in body shape or activity level in girls who have
been exposed to more testosterone and then treat them
differently. But this account cannot explain the striking
findings of toy preferences in other primate species that
show no parental socialization related to toy preferences. FIGURE 14.9 Toy preferences and gender in vervet monkeys.
Studies have repeatedly found that young male monkeys For reasons that still need to be understood, even vervet monkeys,
prefer playing with trucks instead of dolls or plush animals, which have no reason to play with toy trucks or dolls, show gender-
and the females sometimes show the opposite preferences based toy choices.
PARENTING 517
Parental socialization practices can certainly influence parental tensions less intensely, and they can learn much
toy preferences in boys and girls. Yet, it now seems clear that from their older siblings.
parents’ behaviors are likely to interact with aspects of toy In examining sibling effects, the critical question is
preferences that are present before the occurrence of social- whether these kinds of differences in family structure actu-
ization. In some cases, these presocialization toy preferences ally influence development. Like the debates about parent-
can even compete with parental biases. Most of the time, ing effects, there has been considerable contention, in recent
however, parents’ gender socialization behaviors and chil- years, as to whether sibling effects are real and substantial
dren’s early biases probably reinforce each other. (Harris, 2000). The true debate, however, seems to have
If we take a wider view of familial influences on gender, more to do with the degree of sibling effects than with
siblings, too, seem to influence one another’s gender role their existence. When considered in detail, family structure
qualities. One study examined families over a 3-year period seems to have a modest but broad, multifaceted influence
to determine how siblings’ and parents’ gender role qualities on children.
interacted. To learn whether children influenced the extent
to which their siblings adopted gender-stereotyped views and
roles, the researchers analyzed the effects of siblings on leisure
activities, toy preferences, and ways of approaching social situ- Birth Order Effects
ations (McHale et al., 2001). They found that older siblings’ Of the many factors proposed to be influenced by birth
gender role qualities predicted similar qualities in younger sib- order, the most heavily studied has been intelligence. In
lings 2 years later, even when parents’ and younger siblings’ 1975, a provocative paper was published showing what
initial roles were controlled for statistically. Moreover, the seemed to be a correlation between intelligence, as measured
older siblings influenced younger siblings’ gender role qualities by intelligence tests, and birth order. The data indicated
more than parents did, and their influence was significant in that firstborns had the highest intelligence and that intel-
both same-sex and mixed-sex sibling pairs. The older siblings, ligence consistently dropped the later a child’s position was
in contrast, were most influenced by their parents. Thus, par- in the birth order (Zajonc, 1976; Zajonc & Markus, 1975;
ents seem to influence gender roles most strongly in their first- see Figure 14.10). The documented drop in scores is very
born children, who then propagate those roles with younger modest, sometimes as little as 1 percent of the score of the
siblings. This may also interact with the finding, noted earlier, next oldest sibling, and large population samples are usually
that children are more tolerant of gender biases shown by par- needed to find a significant, reliable effect (Kristensen &
ents and peers than they are of other sorts of biases, such as Bjerkedal, 2007).
those involving ethnicity (Killen, 2007; Moller & Tenebaum, This pattern immediately raises the question of whether
2011). By acquiescing to, or even endorsing, such biases, sib- the birth order effect actually causes the difference in scores.
lings and peers may reinforce parental behaviors. One objection is that larger families tend to be more com-
mon in lower socioeconomic classes, who also, for a variety
of other reasons, tend to score lower on intelligence tests. But
when socioeconomic status is held constant, the birth order
Q: Describe at least three different interacting effect on intelligence remains significant in some popula-
influences that contribute to children’s gender tions. More subtle statistical issues have kept debates over
socialization. the magnitude of the effect alive (Armor, 2001; Rodgers
et al., 2000; Wichman et al., 2007; Zajonc, 2001). But despite
these disagreements, the current consensus still favors an
effect (Black et al., 2011; de Haan, 2010). Moreover, another
analysis has suggested that firstborns also tend to complete
Siblings and Family more years of schooling (Black et al., 2005).
Two potential causes for the birth order effect on intel-
Dynamics ligence have been frequently discussed. The first, resource
theory, holds that the more children in the family, the less
We have all heard people explain the behavior of another attention parents can devote to each child. The second expla-
on the basis of that person’s place in the family. Firstborn nation, confluence theory, argues that as families get larger,
children, at least early on, frequently have the undivided their average overall intellectual climate drops (Zajonc &
attention of both parents, whereas later-borns always have Markus, 1975). Thus, the firstborn child is more likely to be
to compete for parental resources. In other respects, though, exposed to the parents’ more adult-centered discourse, while
later-born children may have the advantage. They benefit a fourth-born will more likely be exposed to conversations
from more experienced parenting, they tend to experience about toilet training and various cartoon characters.
Sibling Relationships
Q: What is sibling differentiation? How can it and Socialization
be used to explain the socialization patterns in a
Aside from birth order effects, which remain somewhat
family? controversial, several other patterns of sibling interactions,
including siblings’ jealousy and conflict but also their degree
of social understanding, may have long-term developmen-
An Interactionist Approach to Birth Order Effects tal outcomes. These interactions often involve parents and
Even if these theories of birth order effects tend to hold true other important figures in a child’s life as well.
on average across large population samples, they undoubtedly
interact with many other aspects of family life, producing enor- Jealousy and Conflict between Siblings The birth of a
mous variations. As just one example, we saw at the beginning new child can be especially upsetting to firstborns who are
of this chapter that siblings’ development can vary considerably close in age to the new baby. This effect makes sense, given
depending on the socioeconomic status of the family (Conley, that parents do tend to devote less attention to the first-
2004). That is, siblings’ future outcomes will be more similar born after the new arrival, try as they might to do otherwise
when they grow up in families of high socioeconomic status, (Dunn & Kendrick, 1982). Firstborns tend to react to this
where parents do not have to make difficult choices about change by becoming more demanding of their parents. Sib-
resource allocation (Conley & Glauber, 2005). ling jealousy is also common and seems to vary in degree,
C
hildren who are “only children” and have no siblings Chinese politicians and social planners, and many articles in the
have been stereotyped in a variety of ways, some popular press have expressed worries about a new generation
of them contradictory. They are sometimes seen as of “little emperors”—terribly spoiled, overindulged only children
spoiled, unusually mature (or unusually immature), (Dean, 1992). Even in recent years, employers have posted
dependent, selfish, and socially incompetent (Silverman & job openings with the restriction that single children need not
Silverman, 1971). But are they really at greater risk for develop- apply (Chang, 2008). In addition, 75 percent of China’s urban
ing these largely negative traits? Although some early studies couples reported that they would have preferred to have larger
suggested problems in only children that accord with stereo- families, more like the ones they grew up in, and that they
types, later reviews of the body of research found that only regard the one-child policy as a considerable sacrifice. Parents
children show no systematic deficits (Falbo & Polit, 1986). This also worried that their children would be lonely without siblings
finding is reassuring given that in developed countries, many and would miss out on critical social interactions (Tao & Chiu,
more families are now choosing to have just one child compared 1985; Tung, 1997). A Chinese version of Sesame Street even
to a decade ago (Frejka & Sardon, 2007). included special segments aimed at teaching only children how
But what about cases where having an only child is not a vol- to get along with peers (Tung, 1997).
untary decision but a nationwide decree? In 1979, the People’s Early studies of China’s new generation of only children
Republic of China laid down a set of regulations known collec- reported differences that followed the “little emperor” stereo-
tively as the “one-child policy” (see Figure 14.11). These laws type. Only children were said to be more spoiled and demand-
stated that every family was to have not more than one child. The ing (Jiao et al., 1986). These studies, however, were challenged
goal was to prevent the widespread famines that had resulted in by later ones that found no major differences in the behaviors
the past when China’s food supplies fell short of its large popula- or personalities of only children and other children (Chen et al.,
tion. Some exceptions to the one-child policy were granted, espe- 1994; Falbo & Poston, 1993). Whether a child was the firstborn
cially in more rural areas, but it generally accomplished its goal: of several siblings or later-born, that child did not seem to differ
China’s birth rate fell and its incidence of single-child families in any systematic way from only children. In fact, in some stud-
greatly increased (Doherty et al., 2001). From 1980 to 2000, ies, only children were found better adjusted than other children
China’s birth rate declined from roughly 35 children per 1,000 (Liu et al., 2010). Incidentally, these children with siblings were
people each year to roughly 16 children per 1,000 people. quite easy to find in China because certain regions were permit-
Have such dramatic and rapid changes in the structure of ted fairly widespread exceptions to the one-child policy.
Chinese families caused developmental changes in children One explanation for this shift in research findings is that par-
and young adults? This question has been of great concern to ents and grandparents concerned about having spoiled children
some of the effects of being only children take many years to Rural area
100 female births
120
manifest and may only be found in adults. This was the argument
of one large-scale study of 412 Chinese adults who were born 117
just a year or two before or after the one-child policy was imple- 114
mented in 1979 (Cameron et al., 2013). The adults were studied 111
in experimental tasks designed to assess trust, risk, and perfor-
108
mance in competitive tasks. Children born in the first year of the
one-child policy performed in these tasks in ways that were less 105
1982 1987 1990 1995 2000 2005 2006 2007 2008 2009
trusting, less trustworthy, and less willing to take risks. Of course,
FIGURE 14.12 Sex ratios and the one-child policy. One effect
the presence of such traits in laboratory tasks might not translate of the one-child policy, especially in rural areas, is a huge shift in
into real-world behaviors. Somewhat more naturalistic question- the ratio of boys to girls, which may well have a profound impact on
naires after the experiments, however, also found strong effects, social relations among peers. Adapted from Hvistendahl (2010).
Method:
1. Forty-eight families with children ranging in age from 5 to 10 years old were
randomly assigned to either mediation or control groups.
2. In the mediation group, parents were trained in a series of formal mediation
techniques.
3. In the control group, parents were told that the study examined sibling conflict
and parental interventions and were told to intervene normally.
4. Parents’ reports of subsequent conflict episodes between siblings were
recorded.
Results:
1. Let’s set some ground rules about
Siblings whose parents were assigned to the mediation group showed better how to resolve your disagreements.
conflict resolution strategies, compromised more frequently, and managed the
outcomes of conflicts better. They also showed less negativity in recurrent con- 2. Each of you should say what you
flicts, more nuanced assessments of blame, and greater ability to appreciate their think was happening and how you
disagree or agree.
siblings’ points of view (see table).
3. Each of you should say what you
Conclusion: want and how you feel.
Training parents in well-proven mediation strategies for adults can reduce the
intensity and tone of sibling conflicts both in terms of behaviors and in terms of 4. You should both try to find a way to
resolve your disagreement and
social cognitive styles. check to see if your solution is feasible.
Mediation Control
Condition Condition
Resolutions
Compromise 74 40
Win/lose 6 27
Reconciliation 18 9
No Resolution 10 52
Who Resolves?
Parent 11 74
Children 46 30
Children with parent’s help 50 26
26 26.9
25 France 24.4
27.8
24 Canada 23.7
23 28.0
Finland 24.4
22 28.0
21 Denmark 23.8
28.4
20 Greece 25.0
28.5
1900 1920 1940 1960 1980 2002 Ireland 25.3
A 28.7
Italy 25.0
FIGURE 14.13 Median age of marriage and birth of first child 28.7
Sweden 25.9
over the last century. (A) The age of parents, which is related 28.8
to the age of marriage, has risen dramatically in the past 40 years, Netherlands 24.8
29.0
but much of that rise is related to a return from a dramatic drop in Japan 25.6
29.2
the age of marriage that occurred after World War II. Adapted from Switzerland 25.3
29.4
Cherlin (2005). (B) Over the past four decades, however, whether
the parents are from North America, Europe, or Japan, the mean age 0 20 25 30
of mothers at the time of the birth of their first child has increased Age of mother (in years)
B
dramatically. Adapted from Mathews & Hamilton (2009).
support for the family, and parents’ ability to provide more 200 40
resources for the children (Gustafsson, 2005; Martin, 2004;
Miller, 2011). Older parents also tend to interact with their 150 30
children verbally in more sophisticated ways and to be less
100 20
reactive. As always, though, it can be difficult to distinguish
these effects from socioeconomic ones, since older parents 50 10
are more likely than younger ones to be of higher socioeco-
0 0
nomic status. 20–24 25–29 30–34 35–39 40–44 ≥45
The downside of delayed childbearing is less apparent, Maternal age (in years)
but some effects seem likely. First, many children of older FIGURE 14.15 Age and motherhood. When women choose to
parents may have grandparents so old that they play only a have their first child at later and later ages, fertility rates and spon-
marginal role, if any, in their lives, depriving these children taneous abortions can change dramatically. Adapted from Heffner
of the benefits of a close relationship with their grandparents. (2004).
These families may also be deprived of the kind of support
that grandparents can provide to a family, such as provid-
ing day care, helping out in times of crisis, or simply being pregnancy complications increases greatly in the third and
a mentor and source of expertise (Hayslip & Kaminski, fourth decades of a woman’s life (Heffner, 2004). This, in
2005). Second, children report that they identify less with turn, can have a huge influence on family planning as well
older parents, see them as more remote from their own lives, as creating additional stressors. In short, it will be important
and feel less in tune with them. Research also suggests that for future studies to systematically investigate the negative
older parents have less energy and more limited ability to effects of late childbearing, as the answers concern increas-
engage in physical activities with their children (Martin, ing numbers of families.
2004). Finally, as seen in Figure 14.15, there are clear bio- Older parents are only part of the story of the changes
logical factors to consider, as the incidence of fertility and in parental age in the last several decades. At the same time
that parents in two-parent families are getting older, there
has been a substantial increase in the number of single-
parent families (U.S. Census Bureau, 2012a). Some of
these single-parent families are headed by teenage mothers,
although the rate of teenage pregnancies and the percentage
of young teen mothers has decreased since the 1990s (Ham-
ilton et al., 2012; Martin et al., 2012; see Figure 14.16). The
net effect of these changes regarding parent age is to create
an increase in two kinds of parents: those who are young
and single and those who are older and have a partner.
What are the developmental effects of having very young
parents—what some call the phenomenon of “children hav-
ing children”? Several studies have reported that very young
teenage mothers are at greater risk for depression, and their
children are more likely to have academic and behavioral
problems, such as acting out in school (Beers & Hollo, 2009;
Pogarsky et al., 2006; Shaw et al., 2006; Sullivan et al.,
FIGURE 14.14 Older parents. In many countries, parents are 2011). In these cases, however, disentangling causation from
older when they have their first child. Being an older parent presents correlation is difficult. Very young mothers tend to be from
a number of tradeoffs in terms of its impact on children. lower income brackets than older mothers, are more likely
75 20
15–19 years
50
25 15–17 years 15
0
1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010
0
FIGURE 14.16 Teen pregnancy rates. Teen pregnancy and birth Asian or Non- Total Hispanic Non- American
Pacific Hispanic Hispanic Indian or
rates have been steadily declining for the last four decades. Adapted
Islander white black Alaska
from Martin et al. (2012). Native
Race and Hispanic origin
FIGURE 14.18 Average age of mother at first birth for various
to be single parents, and tend to be less educated (see groups. For a diverse set of groups, the average age of mothers
Figure 14.17). When researchers try to account for these has increased in recent years. Adapted from Mathews & Hamilton
other correlated influences, the effect of being a teenage (2009).
mother per se often becomes smaller. However, when these
other factors are controlled, children—especially boys—of
teenage mothers still face a greater risk of negative outcomes teenage mothers. In short, although the number of very
(Olausson et al., 2001; Pogarsky et al., 2006). In many young mothers may be dropping overall, they still represent
other cases, factors such as the mother’s education level a large proportion of parents in some groups.
are so strongly linked to maternal age that it is difficult to
examine the effects of maternal age separately from these
associated factors.
Although the incidence of teenage pregnancy has Changes in Family Size
declined in recent years in the United States and other In countries where the average parental age is rising, one
countries, the statistics vary widely among racial and socio- consequence is a decline in family size. In addition, in many
economic groups. The mean age of U.S. mothers when they countries, factors independent of parents’ age also contrib-
first give birth ranges from roughly 29 for Japanese Ameri- ute to smaller family size. Parents are choosing to have fewer
cans and Chinese Americans to roughly 22 for African children partly because of the economic burden of support-
Americans, Mexican Americans, and Native Americans (see ing more children and because the demands of caring for
Figure 14.18). This increasing age for first births has larger families is more likely to impinge on the parents’
been found in many parts of the world. In some groups, career aspirations (Miller, 2011). Average family size varies
a younger mean age reflects a disproportionate number of considerably in different countries, as Figure 14.19A shows.
Most developed countries have seen a pattern of decreas-
ing family size over the years. Yet even countries that are
culturally very similar can show striking differences, both
in average family size and in family size trends over time
(see Figure 14.19B). In addition, within any given country,
family size can vary considerably as a function of race and
ethnicity (see Table 14.4). For these reasons, any statements
about the developmental influence of changing family sizes
have to consider the local cultural context.
It might seem that children in larger families would tend
to have worse outcomes. After all, the more children there are
in a family, the less economic resources and parental time and
energy can be spent on each child, an argument that has been
used to explain some sibling and birth order effects. On the
whole, children from larger families do tend to receive fewer
FIGURE 14.17 Teenage mothers. One major challenge, being years of education and earn less money, and they are more likely
met here by these teenage mothers in Brazil, is to continue schooling. to have disciplinary problems (Loeber & Stouthamer-Loeber,
N landia
Sw F rw s
itz ranay
F rla e
G inl nd
O Lux en and
EC e m y
D mb ark
er rg
e
Ire re
u d
e c
ag
ze LCa de
ak ic
ew G an
D rm n
o n
un r
r n
a
av ou
Sl Murk
at
Ki st
e a
d It
he to
m
o
s
T
ni
ni
U
C
A
2.2
Canada
U.S.
2
1.8
1.6
1.4
1.2
1975 1977 1979 1981 1983 1985 1987 1989 1991 1993 1995 1997 1999 2001
B
1986). But a closer look at these statistics reveals that fam- up as an only child does not seem to make children more
ily size is unlikely to be the main reason for these patterns. spoiled or demanding, despite popular stereotypes (Chen
Family size is generally correlated with socioeconomic status: et al., 1994; Falbo & Posten, 1993; Mancillas, 2006).
poorer families tend to have more children. Thus, in many Another way of thinking about family size and struc-
cases, negative outcomes associated with large families may ture involves the difference between nuclear families and
also be due to socioeconomic status. As mentioned earlier, extended families. Nuclear families generally consist of the
family size may have negative effects primarily on poor fam- mother and father as heads of the household, along with their
ilies. For families that are not so poor, family size does not children. Extended families reach across generations and
seem to negatively influence educational outcomes (Black sometimes incorporate aunts, uncles, and cousins. Often a
et al., 2005). At the other end of the continuum, growing grandfather or grandmother may be head of the extended
Total Number of Average Household Size People under 18 in Household People over 18 in
Households (millions) Household
TABLE 14.4 Family size and ethnicity in the United States. From U.S. Census Bureau (2012b).
Percent
45
to the parents. But extended families may encounter more 44.6
43.3
disruptive transitions than nuclear families as grandparents Germany
age and family structure changes. 40
38.4
38.3 38.2
Netherlands
Italy
35
Working Parents and Child Care
Sometimes, cultural influences on families reflect chang-
30
ing socioeconomic realities. In Chapter 6, we considered the
28.5
ways that day care arrangements might influence a child’s
26.4
attachment to his parents. In many countries, this issue is 25
part of a much larger change in the number of families in 1970 1975 1980 1985 1990 1995 2000 2005
which both parents work full-time outside the home. The FIGURE 14.20 The rise of women in the workplace. Over the
incidence has increased in several Western countries over past three decades, there has been a sharp rise in the percentage
the past few decades (see Figure 14.20). For example, in of women in the workplace, although differences between countries
the United States in 2012, 58 percent of mothers with a remain very large. Adapted from the Bureau of Labor Statistics
child under 2 years of age were employed outside the home (2011).
(Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2013). In contrast, in the early
1950s, only 18 percent of mothers with a child under age 6
were employed outside the home (Halpern, 2005). The that children’s cognitive development suffers when mothers
shift has primarily occurred among married women with work outside the home (Burchinal & Clarke-Stewart, 2007).
a husband present in the household, as single mothers have There are several reasons why the effects of having two
worked at higher levels all along. working parents seem small or nonexistent. Although some
How has such a dramatic shift influenced the children in researchers found that employed mothers spend less time
those families? Mothers who work outside the home might with their children (Gottfried & Gottfried, 2006), others
be spending less time with their children, which could found that by sacrificing leisure time, working mothers
have some negative effects. A few researchers have reported usually manage to spend just as much time with their chil-
negative socioemotional effects, such as more difficulty in dren as those who are not employed. One line of research,
interpersonal relationships (Belsky, 2002; Belsky & Johnson, in which mothers recorded their activities in detailed diary
2005; National Institute of Child Health and Human entries, carefully documented how these working mothers
Development, 2003), but the vast majority of studies do not engaged in more multitasking and cut back on time doing
find any consistent effects, and when effects are found, they housework in order to spend as much time with their chil-
tend to be small. dren as mothers did in prior generations (Bianchi, 2011).
In short, the shift to working outside the home by moth- Indeed, one study found that working mothers during
ers does not seem to have had any consistently measurable the period from 2003 to 2008 spent an hour more per week
negative impact on their children (Gottfried & Gottfried, with their children than did stay-at-home mothers in 1975
2006; Halpern, 2005). At least one study has shown that (Bianchi et al., 2006; Fox et al., 2013). In addition, fathers
children of mothers working outside the home have no dif- are spending more time with their children as mothers spend
ferences in educational achievements or in social or behav- more time working outside the home. As a result, today’s
ioral problems (Kalil & Ziol-Guest, 2008). One long-term children may actually spend more time with their parents
study followed children up to the age of 24 and found no than children did in prior generations (Halpern, 2005).
differences in occupational status at that age (Gottfried & Another reason why children of working mothers fare as
Gottfried, 2006). Likewise, there is little reliable evidence well as other children may relate to the consistent finding
Canada Ireland
1981 12.7 1981 7.2
1991 16.2 1991 10.7
2001 23.5 2002 17.4
2006 24.6 2006 22.6
Japan Netherlands
1980 4.9 1988 9.6
1990 6.5 2000 13.0
2000 8.3 2009 16.0
2005 10.2
Denmark Sweden
1980 13.4 1985 11.2
1990 17.8 1995 17.4
2001 18.4 2000 21.4
2009 21.7 2008 18.7
TABLE 14.5 Single-parent families across cultures. The number of single parents has been increasing throughout the world to a degree
that far exceeds the rate of population growth in those countries. Source: U.S. Census Bureau (2012b).
et al., 2002; Chan et al., 1998; Patterson, 1992, 2013; Perrin, for most same-sex couples, the process of giving birth to or
2002; Siegel et al., 2013; Tasker, 2002, 2005). Children adopting a child is necessarily quite deliberate and involved,
who grow up in gay and lesbian families do not differ from so same-sex parents have fewer unplanned or unwanted
other children in their cognitive and social development or children. Moreover, same-sex couples, like all other couples
their incidence of behavioral problems. They also show the who adopt children, have to pass assessments by adoption
same profiles of sexual preferences, gender identities, and agencies and other social service organizations of their fit-
gender role behaviors. Some reviews suggest that children ness as parents before they are allowed to adopt. Finally,
in gay and lesbian families may have more negative experi- many lesbian partners exercise choice in their selection of
ences than their peers associated with others’ homophobia donors for insemination and may therefore be selecting for
and stigmatizing of their parents, but even these negative certain positive behavioral traits in fathers that are passed
experiences do not appreciably change child outcomes on to their children.
(Tasker, 2005). Indeed, at least one study reports higher In short, although there are many popular stereotypes
levels of cognitive and social functioning and lower levels about problems associated with single-parent families and
of behavioral problems among children of lesbian parents same-sex parents, the negative effects of growing up in
(Gartrell & Bos, 2010). One factor to keep in mind is that either of these kinds of families are usually nonexistent. In
35
2000
2009
30
25
Percent
20
15
FIGURE 14.28 Foster care families. Throughout the world, many
10 children spend part of their childhood growing up in foster care
families, which may consist solely of foster children or be a blend of
the foster parents’ biological children and foster children. Foster care
5 families vary greatly in terms of the kind of environment they provide,
and there is ongoing concern for those children being raised in sub-
0 standard foster care settings.
White/Non- Black/Non- Hispanic Other races/
Hispanic Hispanic Multiracial
FIGURE 14.27 Race and ethnicity of children in foster when placed in different foster homes (Linares et al., 2007).
care. The incidence of children in foster care in the United States Several additional, carefully designed studies will be needed
has increased over the past decade in all groups except African
to understand when it is better to place a child in foster care
Americans, who have shown a substantial decline. Adapted from
Child Welfare Information Gateway (2011).
rather than allowing him to stay in a problematic home.
Clearly, another central issue in this area of research is the
need to develop better ways of characterizing different kinds
At this point, both groups of maltreated children—those of foster care situations to understand which types will more
who would enter foster care and those who would remain often lead to developmental problems and how those situ-
at home—showed similarly high levels of many kinds of ations interact with the internal dynamics of the families
behavior problems compared with the control children. at risk.
After spending time in foster care, that group of chil-
dren showed more behavioral problems than the children
who remained with a maltreating parent. These problems
continued for many years after the children left foster care
and as they became young adults.
Conclusions
This study has its limitations, however, even with the An understanding of the child’s role in the family seems to
considerable methodological care taken by the researchers. invoke a different sort of approach from those used in earlier
For example, when maltreated children stayed in their par- chapters. Many different theoretical approaches can be used
ents’ homes rather than entering foster care, there was likely to examine the associations between behavioral and psycho-
something about their home life or their family relation- logical patterns in children and those in their parents and
ships that convinced social workers that they could safely siblings. The focus of these theories ranges from social learn-
stay in their homes—something that was not true of the ing effects, to behavioral genetic forces, to attachment-based
families whose children were placed in foster care. The dynamics, to environmental influences. As we have seen,
preplacement data showed comparable levels of problems in many cases, all of these factors are at work, making the
between the two groups of maltreated children, but these job of sorting out causes from mere correlations extraordi-
more subtle family differences may still have affected the narily complex. Given the numerous interacting influences
children’s development. that affect family dynamics, we cannot simply ask a ques-
As seen earlier in this chapter, the effects of foster care tion such as how much parents matter. Instead, we must ask
also interact with how siblings get along with each other, how parental influences interact with the child’s disposition,
such that low-conflict siblings should be kept together in the local environment, and the particular aspects of child
foster placements, but high-conflict siblings are better off development under investigation. The surrounding culture
CONCLUSIONS 539
can be critical as well. What constitutes harsh, unyielding The social policy implications of research on the fam-
parenting in one culture may be seen as a normal, caring ily are far too important to let larger patterns of cause and
style in another. effect go unexplained. We see once again how viewing
This complex web of interactions can be difficult to development as an active exploratory process is so impor-
disentangle, but the best work manages to conceptualize tant for understanding many aspects of the role of the
and empirically uncover stable, widely applicable patterns. child in the family. For example, the idea of child effects as
These patterns often take the form of interactions between explaining links between parenting and children’s behav-
types of parents, types of children, and types of behaviors. iors is closely linked to a perspective that sees the child as
For example, we saw that research on the effects of divorce an active, influential participant in the process of socializa-
points to a pattern of effects that interacts with the gender tion in the family (Grusec, 2011). It also fosters a view of
of both the parents and the children. That same pattern parents and children as frequently becoming intertwined
of effects is also influenced by the children’s age—both in interactive feedback loops, rather than seeing the child
when the divorce occurred and the age of the child when as a passive recipient of parental influences. We also see in
the potential effects of the divorce are studied. From this many ways how social, cognitive, and personality develop-
set of interactions, reliable patterns have emerged that can ment are interwoven in understanding many causal patterns
be used to make interventions more effective for children within the family. For example, gender roles are related to
struggling to cope with their parents’ separation. Simi- children’s beliefs about themselves and others, to interaction
larly, some studies suggest that teaching parenting skills patterns between parents and children, and to the particular
can have positive outcomes for children, although it is not personalities of all participants. Similarly, sibling conflicts
always easy to obtain long-term positive effects from such and jealousy within the family can only be understood by
interventions. Finally, research on foster care is starting considering cognitive capacities, social interactions, temper-
to help social workers and other professionals understand ament, and personality in all their interactive richness. As
when and why it is important to keep siblings together research models become better able to take all these causal
in foster family placements and when it is better to keep patterns into account, social policy decisions will be corre-
them apart. spondingly better informed.
STU DY AN D REVI EW
Developing Relationships
All relationships serve essential functions for the developing
child. They serve to transfer information between individu-
als and help set up stable, enduring patterns of interaction,
FIGURE 15.2 Negative interactions. When 2-year-olds get into
whether those interactions are between friends and mentors,
fights, they most often fight about toys. When older preschoolers
fight, the conflicts are more often around ideas, such as which super-
or bullies and enemies. Relationships can allow individuals
hero is stronger, or around games, such as who gets to go first. to provide mutual support for each other—both social and
physical support. In other, more harmful relationships, there
may be patterns of dependency and control between individ-
In the elementary school years, children experience a uals. All kinds of relationships, however, can serve to create
huge surge in social interactions. The networks beyond stable social links, and structured sets of links that are larger
the family, arising from sports teams, the school, and the than individuals.
neighborhood, for example, enable children’s social lives to
flourish. Peer group sizes increase throughout this period, Friends With respect to the earliest relationships,
going from groups of two or three early on to social circles infants show unique, repeated interaction patterns with
of a dozen or more. During this period, children require certain individuals, such as with a parent or a sibling, but
less and less adult supervision and can sometimes be found these interactions are not true friendships. Friendships
playing in the neighborhood or on school grounds for hours are ongoing, positive reciprocal relationships with cho-
with little or no adult oversight. During this period, most sen companions. As described in Chapter 6, infants have
children also shift markedly toward interacting with peers of particular expectations of certain specific interactional
their own sex. Suddenly, sometimes seemingly inexplicably, partners, but these interactions largely lack the reciprocal
girls and boys who have played together for years sort them- exchanges (for example, sharing toys or projects) that are
selves into same-sex groups and engage in predominately essential to friendships. In addition, these relationships
single-sex interactions (Fabes et al., 2003; Martin & Fabes, develop largely as a matter of convenience, depending on
2001; Strough & Covatto, 2002). According to the psycho- who is accessible, and less because of a desire to be with
analytic approach to development, this takes place during a specific peer. Some infants are naturally more friendly
what is referred to as the latency period of middle to late and outgoing than others and seem to enjoy being around
childhood, when sexuality is said to be largely dormant and others as opposed to being on their own, but they still
does not overtly influence interactions. During this period, do not seem to build structured social connections with
children also show a decline in pretend play and become others.
more likely to use verbal or physical aggression in conflicts, Toddlers, on the other hand, develop clear friendships
sometimes with great cruelty. In a similar vein, gossip (Howes, 1996; Newcomb & Bagwell, 1995), which seem
about others also increases (Crick et al., 2001; Crozier & to consist mostly of animated, positive interactions while
Dimmock, 1999). playing (Gifford-Smith & Brownell, 2003). Toddlers
purposefully choose their friends, usually picking the
children most like themselves. They begin to show more
prosocial behaviors—for example, sharing and empa-
Q: How do patterns of play between the sexes thy—with friends during this period, but they also begin
change during early and later childhood, and showing more hostility than before with both friends and
what classic construct in psychology has been enemies.
used to explain these changes? School-age children develop more sophisticated notions
of friendship. For example, they gain a clearer idea of reci-
imaginary companions
in the past
Percent of reported
40
30
20
10
0
5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
Age (in years)
A B
FIGURE 15.4 Imaginary companions. (A) Imaginary companions are quite common, not only in elementary school but even in middle
school. When the entire span of childhood is taken into account, the majority of children report having had an imaginary companion sometime
during that period. (B) The graph shows how the incidence of having an imaginary companion changes with age. In this study, over 50 percent
of children at certain ages reported either having an imaginary companion or having had an imaginary companion. Older children either forgot
having had an imaginary companion earlier or chose not to disclose that fact. Adapted from Pearson et al. (2001).
Imaginary companions also serve several other purposes referential communication skills—that is, the ability to
for children. They may allow the child to act out various direct others’ attention to objects or tasks (Roby & Kidd,
social scenarios in a nonthreatening manner, they may alle- 2008). Thus, children with imaginary companions tend to
viate loneliness or boredom that occasionally afflicts virtu- show more advanced communication skills in experimen-
ally all children, and they may allow children to deal with tal tasks in which they have to communicate to someone
frightening situations or work out problems. In almost all behind a screen how to identify the correct picture from a
cases, however, children do realize that their imaginary set. (They are not better at understanding instructions in
companions are not real (Taylor, 1999). They may attri- these tasks, just at giving them.)
bute to their imaginary companion a personality, as well as Why do imaginary companions disappear as the child
beliefs, desires, and other emotions, but they do all of this grows older? One likely reason is that, as the child experi-
with full knowledge that their imaginary companion is not ences a wider range of real relationships, the imaginary ones
really “there” in the sense that other people are. become less necessary. Many cultures also stigmatize adults
Children with imaginary companions tend to have with imaginary companions, and children may choose to
somewhat higher levels of some cognitive and social skills. censor their own imaginations as they become aware of this
They tend to be less shy and are more able to consider situ- stigma. It is interesting to note that imaginary compan-
ations from another’s point of view, a valuable skill in many ions become much more acceptable for adults when they
social situations (Taylor, 1999; Taylor et al., 2004). Some- are in extreme isolation. Consider, for example, the main
times, the imaginary companion can be a vehicle for a character in the 2000 film Cast Away, who is marooned on
child to tell his parent about a problem he is encountering. a desert island and develops an imaginary friendship with
For example, a child might tell a parent that his imaginary a volleyball. The ball acquires a distinct character that is
companion is being picked on at school as a way of relating deeply missed when it is lost at sea. It is easy to empathize
his own experience. (This practice is not so different from with the need for a companion in times of isolation, suggest-
the adult technique of broaching an awkward topic by say- ing that, even as adults, we retain vestiges of the cognitive
ing, “I have a friend who [has the following problem].”) and emotional roots that give rise to children’s imaginary
An imaginary companion also enables a relatively helpless companions.
child to feel more powerful or competent as he acts out
roles with his companion that might not yet be feasible in Bully-Victim Relationships Some relationships have
daily life. More generally, imaginary companions increase decidedly negative consequences for one or both of the indi-
social skills and social understanding (Taylor et al., 2004) viduals involved. One especially vivid and much-studied
and provide a better sense of self-knowledge (Davis et al., case involves relations between bullies and victims of bully-
2011). They also have been shown to increase children’s ing. Bullying, which is also known as peer victimization,
groups, there are indications that children as young as 7 years with the broader social patterns in these groups. To do this,
old reflect the adult pattern of showing stronger out-group we will consider a wide range of research that measures chil-
biases when they are members of a majority group as opposed dren’s social status in groups. Many of these studies inves-
to a minority group (Newheiser & Olson, 2012). tigate the factors that lead children to hold certain social
positions and what the developmental consequences might
be of occupying these positions.
Q: When, in the course of development, do
cliques tend to first appear? Why might they not Sociometric Status and Social Interaction For more
than 75 years, researchers have been interested in devising a
appear until that age? typology that describes how peers interact with each other,
which could then be used to classify children into different
Finally, in adolescence, cliques start to decline in impor- kinds of roles within groups. In 1934, the psychiatrist Jacob
tance. Although some adolescents still try to maintain cliques L. Moreno developed a method of discussing social status
and reap the benefits of belonging to an exclusive group, most in groups known as sociometry. Moreno believed that ana-
adolescents are less focused on cliques (Adler & Adler, 1998). lyzing a group’s internal structures and the particular posi-
The makeup of social groups also changes, shifting toward tion that an individual occupies in these structures—that
more mixed-sex groups in which both sexes interact and col- is, the person’s sociometric status—could help predict the
laborate. There is also a rise in what are sometimes called person’s psychological well-being (Moreno, 1951). Moreno
“crowds” in adolescence, where stereotyped identities, values, determined sociometric status based on how much peers
and activities tend to define a particular crowd and where liked or disliked others in their group. Moreno’s sociometric
a particular adolescent might get a reputation (perhaps not method has become a common tool for understanding chil-
fairly) that links him to one crowd. Thus, in high school, dren’s development within the context of social groups. Over
it is common to talk of groups such as nerds, jocks, Goths, the years, a typology of five types of children has developed:
stoners, punks, and preppies. For many high schoolers, affili- popular children, rejected children, controversial children, aver-
ations with such crowds can have enormous influences on age children, and neglected children (Newcomb et al., 1993).
their friendships and daily activities (Brown & Klute, 2003). The most common sociometric method for determining
Any careful observer of children at different ages would sociometric status and sorting a group of children into these
notice many of these patterns. The more challenging five types is the peer nomination technique. Each child in
research questions center around the different ways that chil- the group is individually asked to name the children in the
dren come to occupy specific niches or social roles within group whom they like the most and those they would most
groups. As we examine the wide variety of social roles that like to play with (Coie et al., 1982). They are also asked to
children occupy in groups, we will also keep in mind ques- nominate the children whom they like the least and those
tions about how developmental change interacts with the they would least like to play with. Computing the “like”
different social roles children play in their peer groups and and “dislike” ratings enables researchers to create the five
Interactions Not really social; more More elaborate Large increase in Peer interactions more
simply joint arousal patterns of play frequency of social influential and more
interactions frequent
Little reciprocal give- Increase in sharing,
and-take pretense, and orderly Increase in size of peer More cross-sex
interactions groups interactions
High conflict
Less adult supervision
More single-sex
interactions
More interactions
outside the home
More verbal aggression
and gossip; less
pretend play
Relationships Unique patterns of Children purposefully Bullying Recognition of
sustained interactions picking friends, usually nonexclusivity of
between pairs, but not Broader view of
choosing those most friendships and
really friendships friendship, allowing
like themselves granting of more
for a more diverse
autonomy to friends;
More prosocial with range of friends and
thus, less jealous
friends but also more friendships
of friends’ other
hostile
relationships
Increased intimacy with
friends
Groups Little evidence of group Dominance hierarchies; Stable cliques; Decreasing importance
behavior less overall aggression, popularity hierarchies of cliques
but little explicit
Social comparison more Increasing importance
awareness of own
frequent and influential of integration of sexes
status in hierarchy
TABLE 15.1 Developmental changes in social affiliations. Children’s social relations show distinct patterns of change during different
developmental periods and across various levels, from interactions to relationships to groups. Adapted from Rubin et al. (1998).
but closer analyses do not strongly support a causal link. For FIGURE 15.15 Violent television programming. News reports
example, children from disadvantaged backgrounds tend to are heavily skewed toward reporting violent events. Adapted from
Johnson (1996).
spend more time watching television, but a host of other
factors associated with disadvantaged backgrounds may be
influencing academic performance and not television per se.
Again, without additional experimental studies, it is diffi- Does all this violence matter? Some writers have gone so
cult to find strong evidence for a causal effect. Similarly, it far as to suggest that the violence in television is actually
is difficult to find clear evidence that entertainment televi- good because it gets everyday violence out of children’s sys-
sion displaces time spent on reading and thereby reduces tems through the psychological mechanism of catharsis. The
literacy. Although some older studies seem to suggest that catharsis hypothesis, which was initially proposed in a much
such an effect occurred when television was introduced into broader and potentially plausible account by Freud, argues
one town and not into another, recent studies do not con- that experiencing violence in a pretend setting is a form of
sistently show a causal relation between amount of enter- release that makes it less likely that aggressive impulses will
tainment television watched and reduced literacy (Moses, be released in real life. The catharsis hypothesis for televi-
2008). While there is a positive relation, there is no evidence sion, however, has been repeatedly discredited (Bushman
supporting claims by some in the entertainment industry et al., 1999; Geen & Quanty, 1977). It is safe to say that vio-
that entertainment television increases literacy. lent television does not normally reduce aggression in viewers
of any age. Moreover, many people believe, on some intuitive
Negative Social Effects of Television A great deal of level, that violence on television increases aggression.
violence occurs in television shows, and there are no indica-
tions that it is on the decline. One analysis compared the
number of antisocial acts per hour on television in contem- Q: What is the catharsis hypothesis, and what
porary programming with that occurring 25 years earlier is the nature of the evidence about the effects of
(Potter & Vaughan, 1997). There was a slight increase from
40.8 antisocial acts per hour to 42.1 antisocial acts. In addi-
violent television programming on children?
tion, the content of violent programming is very different
from reality. In the television world, violent acts are punished The question of whether violence on television actually
much less often, and violent actors are often more attractive does increase aggression has been addressed in experimental
and heroic than in the real world (Potter et al., 1995). Even ways, as well as in naturalistic studies, with the limitations
the news portrays violence at far higher rates than it actually inherent in both kinds of studies. As the total number of
occurs in most people’s daily lives (Anderson et al., 2003; studies on the influences of television is well over 1,000,
Johnson, 1996; see Figure 15.15). By some estimates, a child it is not possible to survey these studies with any degree of
at the end of elementary school is likely to have witnessed completeness. It is possible, however, to describe the over-
roughly 8,000 murders and over 100,000 other violent acts whelming patterns found in both naturalistic studies and in
on television (Anderson et al., 2003, Condry, 1989; Huston experimental settings.
et al., 1992). Ironically, cartoon shows for children show In naturalistic settings, the evidence normally must be cor-
three times as much violence as violent entertainment tele- relational in nature. It is extremely difficult to make strong
vision for adults (Wilson et al., 2002). inferences about causation when experimental control is
TABLE 15.2 Violent television viewing in childhood and adult aggression. Significant positive correlations are found between various
measures of watching and identifying with television violence as a child and displaying both physical and more indirect forms of aggression as
adults, with men showing more physical aggression and women showing more indirect aggression. Adapted from Huesmann et al. (2003).
lacking. Nonetheless, the naturalistic studies do show consis- designed to tease out evidence for causal effects do suggest
tent correlations between higher levels of watching violent tele- that watching violence on television causes more aggressive
vision and higher levels of aggression. For example, Table 15.2 behavior (Anderson et al., 2003; Huesmann et al., 2003).
shows one analysis of how viewing television in child- An alarming pattern occurs across all the nonexperi-
hood was correlated with aggressive behavior 15 years later mental studies: the correlation between violent television
(Huesmann et al., 2003). watching and aggression seems to be rising. As seen in
Unlike the experimental studies, naturalistic studies often Figure 15.16, the rise has been most dramatic since 1995.
have the appeal of looking at patterns of television viewing One possible explanation for the rising correlation is simply
that occur over the course of several years, as opposed to that the quality of studies is improving and that the relation
an hour or two in a laboratory. Summarizing across many is being measured with increasing precision and is coming
naturalistic studies, one meta-analysis of 400 studies on the closer to the correlations found in carefully controlled exper-
relation between violence and television found a positive imental studies (Bushman & Anderson, 2001). An alterna-
correlation of roughly .2 (Paik & Comstock, 1994; see also tive explanation is that somehow aggression on television is
Hogben, 1998). As we will see shortly, even though a cor- becoming more influential on children’s behavior, perhaps as
relation of .2 means that most of the variations in violent expertise in programming and special effects increases.
behavior are not accounted for by variations in how much
violent television is watched, it still represents a strongly sig-
nificant effect for a population as a whole. .22
A
discussion of children and the media should consider protection of free speech and that the high school’s principal
not just the media that children are exposed to but was allowed to forbid publication in the school newspaper
also the media that children produce. One of the most of items that he thought might embarrass members of the
controversial topics in social policy concerning school community (for example, the effects of divorce on
children involves restraints on what children are able to families) or that might be inappropriate for younger readers
say and write. This topic brings into play issues concern- of the paper (for example, discussions of birth control and
ing the moral development of children, the development of sexual activity).
impulse control, and peer aggression, among many others. Why was free speech not guaranteed in a high school
It also raises questions about whether children should be newspaper, given that it is usually allowed on college cam-
restricted in their own speech so as to protect others from pus newspapers? Why should high school students be forced
that speech, and it thereby again introduces issues concern- to “shed their constitutional rights to freedom of speech or
ing the influence of media on children, even when it is pro- expression at the schoolhouse gate” (Tinker v. Des Moines
duced by peers. Independent Community School District, 1969)? Some cases
In many countries, free speech is one of the most cher- are related to concerns that the school newspaper might
ished rights of its citizens. It may therefore seem ironic that contain material that would be inappropriate for younger
free speech is not guaranteed for children and adolescents in readers at the school, but many other cases simply involve
some contexts, most notably in the schools. The legal rationale speech that is uncomfortable for the faculty and administra-
against granting free speech to children is based on theories tion of a school or that is perceived as “disruptive to the edu-
about children’s cognitive and social development as well as cational mission of the school” (Garnett, 2008). The notion
ideas of promoting the educational mission of schools. In 1988, of disruption, however, can be difficult to define and often
the U.S. Supreme Court decided in Hazelwood School District seems to involve assumptions about how the immaturity of
et al. v. Kuhlmeier et al. that a group of high school students minors makes them more easily influenced. In one highly
who were members of the Hazelwood East High School, in publicized case (Morse v. Frederick, 2007), a student in a
St. Louis County, Missouri, did not automatically have the public high school in Juneau, Alaska, was suspended from
game experience are now being found in other domains as course airplane piloting. There are few popular video games
well, such as learning how to remotely operate drone aircraft that focus on prosocial behavior and even fewer studies of
(McKinley et al., 2011). the positive effects of such games. But based on findings
Given that many children start playing video games about the effects of prosocial television shows, researchers
before they even enter school, the cognitive effects of play- expect to find comparable if not even stronger effects of
ing such games that are seen in adolescents may be building video games that reward prosocial, nonaggressive behavior
on many years of prior experience. Any adult without video (Wilson, 2008). In fact, such effects are now being uncov-
game experience who tries to learn a new game along with ered by researchers. For example, one study randomly
a child rapidly realizes how much he is at a disadvantage as assigned college students either to a prosocial video game
a learner. Children may have years of expertise gained from in which their goal was to take care of small creatures or
playing a wide variety of video games that gives them a huge to a neutral game involving falling shapes (Tetris). Later,
head start even for learning a completely novel game. the experiments staged an aggressive encounter between
Many video games are designed to teach specific skills, people in the laboratory and measured the likelihood of the
such as mathematics skills (Lopez-Morteo & Lopez, 2007) game players to intervene. Those who had previously played
and various science topics (Minton, 2008). When the games the prosocial version intervened more than half the time,
are well designed, they usually have strong educational ben- while those who had played the neutral game intervened only
efits (Swing & Anderson, 2008). In addition, video games one-fifth of the time (Greitemeyer & Osswald, 2010). Other
have successfully been used to facilitate instruction in mili- studies have shown that even when playing violent video
tary strategy and operations, disease management, and of games, if the players form cooperative teams, some prosocial
behaviors may also be promoted in later tasks (Greitemeyer where the causal influence is much clearer but the effects
et al., 2012). The authors of these studies strongly suggest are more likely to be measured only on a short-term basis
that such positive effects of prosocial games should also be (Anderson & Bushman, 2001; Gentile & Anderson, 2006;
found in quite young children, but large-scale experimental Sherry, 2001; Swing & Anderson, 2008). Researchers suggest
studies still need to be done with younger populations. that the same clusters of factors that mediate the influence of
When video games contain violence, they may facilitate violent television on aggressive behavior are at work in video
aggression even more than television programs, as video games as well, including desensitization, arousal, priming of
games foster and train active aggression as part of the experi- certain actions and response tendencies, and learning new
ence rather than offering passive viewing, as is the case with ways to aggress (Anderson et al., 2010; Swing & Anderson,
television. A child learning to play a video game becomes 2008). In addition, researchers have argued that video games
more and more skilled at killing, maiming, and brutalizing have properties that may cause greater effects than televi-
other agents, many of which can look like normal humans. sion. These properties include increased identification with
Although the body of research on video games and aggres- the aggressor (in many games the player can pick an aggres-
sion is less extensive than that on television, most reviews sor that he identifies with), active participation, rehearsal
of the research conclude that, similar to the findings with of aggressive actions from start to finish, direct rewards for
television, there is a relation between the time spent playing engaging in violent behavior, and a much higher incidence of
violent video games and increased levels of aggression. More- violent episodes per hour. Heavy involvement in aggressive
over, the relation is found in both naturalistic studies, where role-playing games on the Internet may have similar effects
causation is more difficult to infer, and experimental studies, (Holtz & Appel, 2011; Krahé et al., 2011; Lee et al., 2010). In
Percentage of computer
100
aggressive cognition (speed of accessing violent concepts) and
TABLE 15.3 Internet use patterns (shown as percentages) and socioeconomic status. Usage patterns of the Internet are quite similar
across various socioeconomic groups, with a tendency toward higher usage rates in most categories for more affluent children. Adapted from
DeBell & Chapman (2003), based on data from the U.S. Census Bureau (2001).
ents of their friends). In contrast, higher levels of face-to- longer periods by posting hurtful messages or graphics on
face communication were associated with more positive Web pages or bulletin boards. Cyberbullying raises concerns
social well-being (Pea et al., 2012). because of the ways it departs from real-world bullying (Men-
Other studies suggest that a primary focus with online esini & Spiel, 2012). Cyberbullies can be anonymous, they
friends can lead to a form of “Internet addiction” in which can search out a wider range of vulnerable targets, and they
children of both sexes seem to become intensely preoccupied can more easily find willing accomplices. The potential to
with online activities as opposed to face-to-face interactions embarrass or humiliate another person can be greatly exacer-
(Smahel et al., 2012). Most children, however, seem to use bated by having a much larger audience to witness the event.
the Internet in a manner that helps promote and maintain In addition, cyberbullying tends to be even less monitored by
offline relationships. In addition, only a relatively small num- adults, who may be largely oblivious to harsh bullying that
ber of children seem to engage in Internet activities that are would be noticed in the real world. This may be a particularly
unsafe with respect to exposure to either inappropriate con- acute problem because incidences of bullying can spiral out
tent or individuals (Valcke et al., 2011). For parents who are of control in a community when parents and school authori-
concerned about the time their children are spending on the ties have little or no awareness that it is happening. Finally,
Internet, it is clearly important to get a sense of what activities any feelings of empathy that might be triggered when a vic-
their children are engaging in to assess whether they might tim is being bullied face-to-face are not likely to be triggered
be harmful. As noted earlier, the vast majority of parents on the Internet (Li, 2007; see also Blais, 2008; Patchin &
find it difficult to monitor Internet activity at that level of Hinduja, 2006; Smith et al., 2008, for related discussions of
detail. Fortunately, the early fears of children being exposed the unique aspects of cyberbullying). Moreover, cyberbullies
to nearly continuous hazards seem to be heavily exaggerated. report lower feelings of remorse for their actions than do bul-
One troubling pattern in Internet activity among children lies in the physical world (Slonje et al., 2012).
and adolescents is known as cyberbullying, a form of aggres- It appears that bullies on the Internet are often the same
sion over the Internet, often by a group against an individual individuals who engage in bullying in the real world (Blais,
(Li, 2007; Ybarra & Mitchell, 2004, 2008). Cyberbullying 2008; Gradinger et al., 2009; Olweus, 2012; Vazsonyi
can occur in real time as users gang up on one another in an et al., 2012). The effects of cyberbullying seem quite simi-
instant message board or chat room. It can also occur over lar to those of real-world bullying, with victims showing
Rigidity
younger and older children the status questions and inter-
est questions. The older children showed a clear gender-
consolidation
linked status rating, judging exactly the same occupation as
flexibility
being of higher status when modeled by males. The younger
learning
children showed no significant difference in their ratings
based on whether the job was modeled by a male or female;
low
there were no differences in their interest in doing the jobs, 3 12
and the gender of the children did not influence how they Age (in years)
judged the status of the jobs (Liben et al., 2001). FIGURE 15.23 Gender stereotype rigidity and age. The rigidity
The results suggest that for familiar jobs, gender stereo- of gender stereotypes, as learned from a culture, reaches a peak at
types emerge quite early and that jobs typically associated roughly 5 years of age. Adapted from Martin & Ruble (2004).
with males are valued more highly. Apparently, this value
structure then becomes an interpretive schema for prejudg- groups is further enhanced when the culture at large tends
ing the status of totally novel occupations as a function of to label them, as when a teacher says, “Good morning boys
the gender that models that occupation. One obvious ques- and girls.” Quite remarkably, negative attitudes toward a
tion for future research is whether there might be dramatic group can be produced in just a matter of a few weeks. In
cross-cultural differences in the extent to which occupa- one study, researchers randomly assigned children as young
tions gain or lose status merely as a function of which gen- as 6 to either a blue-colored T-shirt group or a green-colored
der typically holds that kind of job. T-shirt group, and teachers had the children form lines or be
One intriguing finding on the emergence of gender stereo- seated or do other tasks based on whether they were wearing
types is that as younger children learn and consolidate a stereo- a blue or green T-shirt. Even though both groups were treated
type and before they learn to be more flexible, they may hold equally, within a few weeks, the children in each group had
stronger stereotypes than older children (Martin & Ruble, increased negative attitudes toward the members of the other
2004). It seems that initially, at around age 3, they struggle group (Bigler et al., 1997). If bias can be found in such artifi-
to learn a stereotype and that during that period, they apply it cial cases with no real differences in treatment, it is easy to see
unevenly to a sex. Then, when they clearly master the stereo- how much stronger the effects can be in the real world. Later
type, they tend to apply it far too rigidly as an essentially all-or- studies have shown that 5-year-olds will quickly adopt nega-
none statement about appropriateness of an activity for males tive biases to an out-group that is set up on clearly arbitrary
or females. Thus, a 5-year-old might be observed to vehemently grounds and will distort information so that it is positively
insist that police officers can only be men, even if that child has construed for the in-group and negatively construed for the
regularly observed female police officers in his neighborhood. out-group (Dunham et al., 2011; see Scientific Method box).
This rigidity may be related to an early tendency to see gender More naturalistically, even preschoolers show group favorit-
roles as closely linked to biological essences (Taylor et al., 2009). ism at an implicit level that is just as strong as it is in later
As the stereotype is mastered in later childhood and becomes childhood and adulthood. The preschool children seem to be
easy to deploy, children tend to become increasingly flexible in “prepared” to develop such implicit prejudices and to quickly
the way they use it (see Figure 15.23). Even younger children, adopt them at full strength (Dunham et al., 2013).
however, can show some flexibility when they think about the Biases can also emerge from other superficial markers of
context in which a gender norm is observed, such as when it out-group status, such as accent. For example, when two novel
occurs in a different culture (Conry-Murray & Turiel, 2012). speakers were observed by 2-year-olds, both U.S. and French
This may provide ways of helping younger children abandon children preferred to give a “present” to the individual who
rigid stereotypes. was a native speaker as opposed to giving it to someone who
The general process of forming group-based stereotypes was speaking with a foreign accent (Kinzler et al., 2012). Even
has been examined within the framework of what is known younger infants (10-month-olds) preferred toys that were
as developmental intergroup theory (Bigler & Liben, modeled by people with native accents as opposed to foreign
2007). According to this theory, at least as early as age 4, accents. Children also preferred to associate with those who
children start to notice perceptually salient group differences had their same native accent (Kinzler et al., 2009).
and attach these to stereotypes. (This especially happens The use of stereotypes to predict the behaviors of oth-
in regard to “minority groups,” groups that are proportion- ers does not require a rich sense of the mental lives of oth-
ally smaller in an overall population.) The salience of such ers, which is part of the reason stereotypes are so pernicious.
F
or many years, it has been known that prejudice against culture stereotypes about the relation of job status to the domi-
disadvantaged ethnic and racial groups is present by nant ethnic and racial groups, assuming that European Ameri-
at least the elementary school years (Clark, 1955). The cans were more likely to dominate in the higher-status jobs and
nature of prejudice and discrimination has changed African Americans in the lower-status jobs. This difference was
in the past few decades, however, from explicit and willful just as strong in African-American children whose parents were
attacks on other groups to more implicit forms of discrimina- from high-SES backgrounds and thus occupied high-status jobs.
tion in which the actor often believes that he is not prejudiced Evidently, counterexamples from their own family were not suf-
(Dovidio et al, 1997; Fazio & Olson, 2003; Wittenbrink et al., ficient to block the cultural stereotype. At the same time, and
1997). Interestingly, the attitudes of parents and peers do contrary to gender results, the children all indicated a prefer-
not always predict those of children (Aboud & Doyle, 1996; ence to have the higher-status jobs, even though they perceived
Stephan, 1999). Children seem to be most influenced by those such jobs as being dominated by a racial group different from
peers and adults whom they most respect and who discuss their own.
prejudice in ways that touch on topics that the children care The most compelling result, however, came in a study mod-
most about (Aboud & Fenwick, 1999). In addition, some forms eled closely after the gender study described in Figure 15.24.
of prejudice may arise from observations of how the culture at Novel occupations were pictured with either European-American
large seems to treat and value different groups. In the United models or African-American models doing the same jobs. For
States, it has repeatedly been shown that both European- exactly the same occupation, the race of the models had a large
American and African-American children tend to develop nega- impact on status ratings by both first- and sixth-graders, with
tive stereotypes regarding African Americans (Aboud, 1988; the occupation being granted higher status when modeled by
Spencer & Markstrom-Adams, 1990), assuming that positive European Americans. The children also tended to show a ten-
traits, such as being intelligent or nice, are more likely to be dency to have a greater interest in pursuing a job when it was
seen in European Americans and that negative traits are more modeled by European Americans. This result has been echoed
likely to be seen in African Americans. by other findings, including the finding that children in South
One line of work asks how children’s views of occupations Africa tend to associate racial groups with social class (Olson
might be influenced by these ethnic and racial stereotypes et al., 2012).
(Bigler et al., 2003). This research follows the same approach as This constellation of results illustrates the challenges of reduc-
that used in studies of gender stereotypes and children’s evalu- ing stereotypes about occupations and roles and reducing the
ations of occupations (Liben et al., 2001). African-American use of those stereotypes to drive prejudices about types of peo-
children from the first and sixth grades and from higher and ples. Sadly, it seems that even if an underprivileged group makes
lower socioeconomic status (SES) groups were asked about great strides by moving into a profession previously populated by
the status of familiar and novel occupations and also about their a dominant group, unless those strides are made in many fields at
interest in engaging in those jobs themselves. For the famil- the same time, those advances may be undercut by a reduction
iar jobs, even the first-graders had already learned from their of the perceived status of the profession.
Method:
1. Sixty-four 5- and 6-year-olds were randomly assigned either to a
group that wore green T-shirts or to a group that wore blue T-shirts,
with assignment based on the children choosing a coin from the
experimenter’s closed hand.
2. Children heard one story about a child in a picture who wore the
same-colored T-shirt as the participant and another story about a
child who wore the other-colored shirt. Both stories assigned an
equal number of positive and negative behaviors to the child in the
picture (four each).
or
3. All children were asked to recall details of the stories and to choose
which child protagonist they wanted to play with.
4. Researchers coded memory of positive and negative behaviors and
playmate choice.
Results:
Participants recalled significantly more positive behaviors about
in-group characters than about out-group characters and showed a 1.6
tendency to recall more negative behaviors about out-group characters. 1.4 In-group
Number recalled
They also strongly preferred in-group characters as potential playmates. 1.2
Out-group
1.0
0.8
Conclusion: 0.6
Even though membership in the group was clearly arbitrary, children 0.4
as young as 5 processed information differently about characters that 0.2
matched their in-group versus those in an out-group. 0
Positive Negative
Action valence
Source study: Dunham et al. (2011).
We only need to associate negative behaviors with a certain negative bias toward those out-groups. Apparently, having
group to predict how they will behave. Those predictions additional information on how the two groups potentially
may be more wrong than right, but they are often made in differed enabled children to build up stronger reasons for
cases of racist and gender-based stereotypes. Because we do their biases (Abrams, 2011).
not need to take into account another’s beliefs and desires As children begin to enter middle school, they start to
when using a stereotype to predict behavior, even individuals realize the inappropriateness of holding negative stereotypes.
with autism can use stereotypes even though they are unable This emerging awareness can in some cases cause older chil-
to pass traditional theory of mind tasks (Hirschfeld et al., dren to do worse than younger children on tasks that would
2007). It could be that sophisticated insight into another be most easily solved by explicitly acknowledging race. For
person’s mind would lead to empathy and thereby blunt example, when 8- and 9-year-olds were shown pictures of
the bias. But unfortunately, sophisticated insight into other 40 faces that varied on several dimensions, including race,
minds does not guarantee open-mindedness toward other and had to guess which one the experimenter had in mind,
groups. In fact, in one study, elementary school children who they solved the task more quickly and with fewer questions
were less egocentric and more aware of how an out-group’s than 10- and 11-year-olds (Apfelbaum et al., 2008). The
preferences might differ from their own also showed more older children were more reluctant to use explicit references
CONCLUSIONS 581
STU DY AN D REVI EW
hood and early adolescence. Indeed, some have argued that, of the most powerful influences worldwide is that of televi-
as humans mature, they have an evolutionary bias to attend sion, which has become a major part of the daily lives of
more to peers than to parents. people, not just in the developed world but also in the devel-
● Peer relations can be thought of as occurring at three levels: oping world. Many children in developed countries spend
interactions, relationships, and groups. more time watching television than doing any other activity,
● Interactions are the simplest forms of relations, ones that may including time spent learning in school.
well be transient. In younger children and infants, interactions ● While infants may not be influenced much by the content of
may be little more than mutual arousal of excitement or dis- television, by 2 years or so, children start to become engrossed
tress. Older children show notions of reciprocity and sharing with the content of television that is age appropriate.
and start to engage in play and pretense. ● Educational programming has been shown to have both
● Relationships are unique and repeated patterns of interac- positive cognitive and positive social effects on children.
tions between two people, such as a child’s relationships with Unfortunately, it is not the most frequently watched form of
peers, parents, or siblings. Infant relationships may include television.
strong bonds of affection and expectations of classes of ● Entertainment television tends to contain high levels of vio-
behaviors, but they do not contain the reciprocal give-and- lence, levels that have remained equally high for decades. The
take found in toddlers and older children and which is so vast majority of research on the influences of television on
essential to friendships. As children approach and enter ado- children has focused on the question of whether violence on
lescence, friendships take on more intensity, and gender dif- television leads to aggression in children. Across several hun-
ferences in friendship patterns become more prominent. dred studies, both of a naturalistic and experimental nature,
● In addition to real friends, many children also have imagi- it is clear that exposure to violent programming is correlated
nary companions. They may also have enemies. Children also with higher levels of aggression and probably causes it.
can become involved in bully-victim relationships. Bullies ● The correlation between violent television watching and
often have an inflated sense of self-esteem rather than low self- aggression is not massive, and it is certainly not able to pre-
esteem, contrary to popular lore. The victims of bullies tend dict a high likelihood of aggression by any child based on her
to be repeated targets of bullying who suffer considerably from viewing habits, but when taken for a population as a whole,
the experience. the risk of increased aggression as a result of viewing violent
● While friends and even imaginary companions are usually television may represent a health hazard of a magnitude close
associated with positive outcomes, enemies and both par- to that of the relation between smoking and lung cancer. It
ties in bully-victim relationships are associated with negative has been difficult to address this potential health problem
outcomes. through public policy. A large array of other potential effects
● Because the formation of groups may require somewhat more of television is increasingly receiving attention.
cognitive complexity, groups are not voluntarily formed by ● As computers become ever more prevalent in the home
human infants. Preschoolers form ad hoc groups, but in ele- and video games become more complex, comparable ques-
mentary school, children start to form groups that are more tions have arisen about their influences. Although a smaller
stable, often exclusive cliques. number of studies have been conducted in this area, the
● Sociometric status is a measure of a child’s standing in a group. effects appear to be similar to those produced by television,
It is usually measured by asking peers which children in the at least in the area of violence. Nonetheless, positive cogni-
relevant group are the most liked and which ones are the least tive benefits can be found for educational video games, and
liked. There seem to be five different kinds of children as even prosocial effects may be found for the right kinds of
revealed by such ratings: popular children, rejected children, games.
Features of Autism
Autism was first diagnosed by Leo Kanner in the 1940s
(Kanner, 1943), when it was often known as Kanner’s
syndrome. Before Kanner’s work, individuals with autism
were often lumped together with children who were labeled
“retarded.” Kanner noticed a set of problems that occur
together and that are now considered the central features of
autism: emotional aloneness or remoteness, obsessions with
preservation of sameness, and a tendency toward elective
mutism, in which children capable of speaking seem to have
no desire to do so, especially in a communicative manner.
Children diagnosed with autism can range from those who
show massive cognitive deficits in all areas and require assis- A
tance with even the simplest daily functions to those who,
in rare cases, can exhibit remarkable skills in specific areas. Toddler with autism
Percent of visual fixation time
For that reason, any description of the features of autism 80 Typically developing toddler
must be understood as only approximate for a given child
with that diagnosis. Indeed, it is common to talk of “autism 60
spectrum disorders” so as to make clear how much the vari-
ous deficits and atypical behaviors can vary (Klin et al., 40
2004; Lord & Jones, 2012; Rapin & Tuchman, 2008a;
Volkmar et al., 2007). For the same reason, most current
20
researchers tend to think that there is not just one cause of
autism and that, instead, different kinds of “autisms” may
0
arise through different causal pathways (Betancur, 2011; Eyes Mouth Body Object
Rapin & Tuchman, 2008b). B
One set of symptoms revolves around problems with FIGURE 16.1 Visual fixations by toddlers with autism and
social skills. Children with autism may not respond to social toddlers without autism. Toddlers with autism and typically
overtures by others. For example, they may not respond developing toddlers watched a video of an adult, and researchers
when spoken to, sometimes even if their own name is called. tracked where the toddlers directed their eyes. (A) Stills of the video
They often seem to lack empathy for others (see Chapter 12) indicate the eye scan patterns and show striking differences between
the scans of typically developing toddlers (in yellow) and the scans
and seem to have trouble understanding the mental states of
of toddlers with autism (in green). Note in particular how typically
others, such as their beliefs or feelings (Rapin & Tuchman, developing toddlers focus on the eyes to a vastly greater extent.
2008a; Volkmar et al., 2007). A related set of problems con- (B) The graph shows the percent of visual fixation time on different
cerns difficulties in “reading faces.” One sign of this is shown body parts by the toddler with autism and the normally developing
in the very different ways in which people with autism scan toddler. Adapted from Jones et al. (2008).
AUTISM 589
of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders reasoning (see Chapter 13). At the neural level, some fMRI
(DSM), the manual used by clinical psychologists and psy- studies have suggested that individuals with autism show
chiatrists to diagnose mental disorders. In May 2013, the less activity in brain regions associated with social cognition
DSM-V revision replaced prior versions that had appeared (Philip et al., 2012). The theory suggests that without these
in 1994 (DSM-IV) and 2000 (DSM-IV-TR). DSM-V listed social skills, people with autism tend to rely on other more
the same diagnostic features of autism as earlier manuals, analytical and systematized ways of understanding, which is
but it increased the strictness of the diagnostic criteria (for an extreme form of the usual male analytical tendency. This
example, requiring more symptoms for a diagnosis), which observation also fits with the tendency for many individuals
may result in the exclusion of less severe cases. As part of the with autism to have an intense interest in orderly routines,
changes, it also combined autistic disorder, Asperger’s disor- arrangements of objects, and the like. It may also explain
der, childhood disintegrative disorder, and pervasive devel- why autism has been found to be more common in fami-
opmental disorder not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS) lies of physicists, engineers, and mathematicians, groups
into one larger category called autism spectrum disorder, or that tend to represent stereotypical male cognitive styles
ASD, arguing that the divisions into four related disorders (Baron-Cohen et al., 1998). Yet, it is not clear how such sex
had been artificial and that the disorders occurred in more differences are affected by biological mechanisms—that is,
of a continuum. It is not yet clear how much the changes will how the presence of a Y chromosome could lead to behav-
affect the number of diagnosed cases of autism, but some ioral differences related to autism. There are some indica-
researchers project quite substantial drops in those receiving tions of links of subsequent autistic traits (such as reduced
ASD diagnoses, especially among those who were formerly eye contact, less empathy, and more systematizing) to fetal
diagnosed with Asperger’s or PDD-NOS (McPartland testosterone levels (see Figure 16.2). Higher levels of fetal
et al., 2012). More generally, however, the shifting boundary testosterone in either sex are correlated with reduced social
of criteria for diagnosis with ASD is not likely to influence skills and increased systematizing tendencies (Auyeung &
psychological theories of autism as much as debates about Baron-Cohen, 2008; Auyeung et al., 2009, 2010; Mills
availability of resources for people with disabilities and con- et al., 2007). Normally, of course, higher fetal testosterone
clusions as to whether there is a true “autism epidemic.” levels are much more common in males. Somewhat more
Autism is diagnosed four times as frequently in boys as in surprisingly, even in adults, testosterone can have an influ-
girls. This is a pronounced sex difference, but this kind of ence on empathy-related brain regions that presumably had
disparity is not unusual for incidences of mental illness. The been influenced prenatally by sex hormones. Thus, when
difference may be even larger for milder cases of autism, such
as Asperger’s disorder, which has been reported to have a male
to female ratio of 8:1 (Auyeung & Baron-Cohen, 2008). One 50 Girls
hypothesis aimed at explaining why autism is more common Boys
in boys suggests that the psychological features of autism
resemble an extreme version of the male social brain (Baron- 40
Q-CHAT score
events (Lai et al., 2012). The ability to systematize is more 0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50 2.00
explicitly defined as “the drive to analyze a system in terms Fetal testosterone level (nmol/L)
of the rules that govern the system, in order to predict the FIGURE 16.2 Fetal testosterone and subsequent autism.
system” (Baron-Cohen et al., 2005, p. 820). For both boys and girls, there is a significant positive relationship
between the amount of fetal testosterone and later diagnosis of
autism-related symptoms. On the graph, the y-axis is a measure
of degree of autism symptoms, known as Q-CHAT (Quantitative
Causal Factors CHecklist for Autism in Toddlers), in which higher scores represent
more symptoms. The x-axis depicts the amount of fetal testosterone
As we have noted, autism has been hypothesized to involve as measured in the mother’s amniotic fluid. Higher levels of testoster-
a severe impairment of the ability to empathize and the one are related to higher Q-CHAT scores, especially in boys. Adapted
related ability to “read” others by engaging in theory of mind from Auyeung et al. (2010).
AUTISM 591
autism (or their mothers) have had a higher incidence of having briefly discussed phobias in Chapter 7 in the context
various infections early in development, although it is also of prepared fears. We will then discuss obsessive-compulsive
possible that they may have more active immune responses disorder as an anxiety-related disorder that has some dis-
to infections or that autism early on could lead to greater tinctive components that do not affect all people with anxi-
susceptibility to infections. Other work suggests that autis- ety problems.
tic individuals may have more immune-related diseases,
such as autoimmune disorders (Patterson, 2011; Trottier
et al., 1999). Indeed, one view suggests that as many as one-
third of the cases of autism may result from problems of Incidence of Anxiety Disorders
immune system regulation in the mother, sometimes as a Considered collectively, anxiety disorders are the most prev-
side effect of an infection, but other times simply because alent mental illness in children, with incidences approaching
a poorly regulated immune system results in inflammatory 15 percent of children between ages 9 and 17 (Cartwright-
disease that influences fetal brain development (Velasquez- Hatton et al., 2006; Costello et al., 2005; Muroff & Ross,
Manoff, 2012). 2011; Southam-Gerow & Chorpita, 2007; Velting et al.,
More broadly, a wide array of environmental factors, rang- 2002). Anxiety disorders seem to be about equally common
ing from infectious diseases to exposure to insecticides, has among boys and girls in childhood, but they become more
been seen as potentially related to the onset of autism. In common among girls in adolescence (Kashani & Orvaschel,
addition, when such effects do occur, they may be produced 1990; Kubarych et al., 2008; Nielsen et al., 2000). Anxi-
through gene-environment interactions. For example, a par- ety disorders are among the earliest psychopathologies to
ticular kind of environmental exposure might influence a emerge, with patterns of anxious behavior in infancy pre-
genetic mechanism regulating a specific aspect of neuronal dicting social anxiety many years later—probably even in
development (Pessah & Lein, 2008). Environmental factors adulthood. Moreover, when anxiety problems are identified
on their own may not be sufficient to trigger the onset of in children, they are highly likely to be predictive of long-
autism in most individuals, but these factors may have such term problems. For example, the child who is excessively
effects in a special subset of individuals who are at risk because anxious about school tests, parties, and athletic events is
they have a certain genotype. Such gene-environment inter- more likely to have severe anxiety problems as an adult, and
action patterns are often described in terms of the diathesis- often depression as well (Bernstein et al., 1996; Brady &
stress hypothesis, which proposes that some individuals have Kendall, 1992; Southam-Gerow & Chorpita, 2007).
a genetic or biological vulnerability (a “diathesis”) that causes
them to be more likely to show a disease when immersed in a
stressful, negative environment. At the present time, however,
the research community has not achieved much consensus on
Q: Describe the prevalence of anxiety
the links between specific environmental factors and autism disorders in children.
spectrum disorders.
Causal Factors
FIGURE 16.3 Social anxiety disorder. Most people feel some There have been several theories about how and why anxi-
anxiety in front of groups, but when the anxiety becomes so strong ety disorders emerge in children, ranging from those that
that the individual cannot even look at others, it may be a sign of focus on the biological components, like the genetic basis
social anxiety disorder. Social anxiety disorder often becomes most
for cortical circuits that inhibit behaviors (see Chapter 7), to
salient when children are in middle school.
accounts that refer to a child’s early experiences, such as pat-
terns of parenting (see Chapter 6). Freud described cases of
situations. A child with such a disorder might worry exces- anxiety arising out of internal conflicts. In one of his most
sively about an upcoming trip to the zoo, whether she will colorful accounts, he described the case of “Little Hans,”
have to use the toilet at school, and whether her school bus who supposedly acquired a phobia of horses because of an
will crash on a field trip. If the worries are more focused on Oedipal conflict in which he saw the horse as proxy for a
just social situations, the disorder is a social anxiety disor- castrating father (Freud, 1909). By contrast, we saw in Chap-
der. If the worries are primarily about a particular fear, such ter 4 how John Watson instilled anxiety in a child through
as a fear of large dogs, the disorder is a phobia. classical conditioning, a technique that tried to deny any
Developmentally, the earliest signs of anxiety are seen in value to considering mental states in the child (Watson &
the form of separation anxieties (discussed as separation dis- Rayner, 1920). Here, however, we focus mostly on general-
tress in Chapter 6), with some children showing excessively ized anxiety disorder, in which a child seems to be anxious
fearful and anxious responses upon separation from a par- about a wide range of events and possible futures and not
ent or other adult caregiver. Specific phobias are usually the about a highly specific object, as in the case of phobias.
next disorder to appear, often before 7 years of age. General- Insecurely attached infants and children are more likely
ized anxiety disorder doesn’t usually appear until around age to show anxiety problems later on (Brumariu & Kerns,
9 or 10 (Muroff & Ross; 2011; Southam-Gerow & Chorpita, 2011; Cassidy & Berlin, 1994; Colonnesi et al., 2011; Esb-
2007). Social anxieties tend to become more prominent jørn et al., 2012; Thompson, 2001). As is often the case with
around age 12, while panic disorder (a disorder in which attachment-related research, however, it is difficult to assess
panic attacks occur frequently and for no obvious reason) causal directions. While it might be that parents who are
doesn’t usually occur until around age 15 (Southam-Gerow & insensitive to children’s needs may cause children to become
Chorpita, 2007). With the exception of children with panic more anxious, it is also possible that anxious children cause
disorder, children who show one kind of anxiety disorder parents to behave in ways that make the parents appear to
are more likely to show other kinds of anxiety disorders as be insensitive. Moreover, there are associations between
well, suggesting some shared underlying causes (Muroff & depression in mothers and increased anxiety in children
Ross; 2011; Wood et al., 2003). (Feng et al., 2008; Pilowsky et al., 2006). Such associations
Taking a closer look at the anxiety disorders, we observe may reflect the influences of unresponsive mothers or the
that anxiety manifests itself differently throughout develop- modeling of anxiety by depressed mothers (Gerull & Rapee,
ment. Most notably, for both normal and abnormal cases 2002). In fact, researchers have shown in experimental stud-
of anxiety, the objects that produce anxiety tend to change ies that when parents are told to model anxiety-reducing
with age. Younger children tend to worry more about behaviors, children show less anxiety toward potentially
did children who were not anxious. It therefore appears that Interpretation
socially anxious children form different cognitive interpre- Memory
tations of social situations, even when they are bystanders, bias
Biological Factors Across many twin and adoption stud- Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder
ies, researchers have consistently found evidence for genetic As seen at the beginning of this chapter, some children can
influences on anxiety (Gregory & Eley, 2007; Rapee et al., develop a series of behaviors that involve a high level of anxi-
2009; Sakolsky et al., 2012). Most studies have focused on ety as well as high incidences of checking various things, per-
genes associated with the production and regulation of the sistent, intrusive thoughts that cannot be ignored or set aside,
neurotransmitter serotonin, but other studies have suggested and highly repetitive behaviors (American Psychiatric Associ-
links to genes associated with other neurotransmitters, ation, 2000; Leckman et al., 1997). This pattern of behaviors
such as dopamine, and genes associated with stress-related is known as obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD).
hormones. In addition, the association between genes and
anxiety disorders can be stronger in some environments Types of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder Let’s consider
than in others (Fox et al., 2005; Sakolsky et al., 2012). For in more detail the four main types of obsessive-compulsive
example, variations in genes that regulate serotonin show disorder that researchers have repeatedly proposed (Bloch et
significant relations to anxiety when children are in families al., 2008; see Figure 16.5). One cluster of behaviors revolves
Symmetry
Ordering
Aggressive Symmetry
Repeating
Sexual
Counting
Religious Forbidden thoughts
Checking
Somatic
A B
FIGURE 16.5 Clusters of symptoms in obsessive-compulsive behaviors. Obsessive-compulsive behaviors are often classified into four
types or factors, with characteristic obsessions and compulsions, which can overlap somewhat: cleaning and contamination fears, a desire to
order things, hoarding of objects, and forbidden thoughts (usually aggressive, blasphemous, or sexual).The diagram shows solid lines for links
between obsessions or compulsions and factors that can be found in both adults and children, and dashed lines for links between obsessions or
compulsions and factors found only in children. Adapted from Bloch et al. (2008).
100 100
80
150
Number of cases
Number of cases
60
100
40
50
20
0 0
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 0 10 20 30 40 50 60
A Bulimia Age at onset (in years) B Anorexia Age at onset (in years)
FIGURE 16.8 Age of onset of bulimia and anorexia. Despite being quite distinct diseases with different etiologies, the typical age of
onset for bulimia is very similar to that for anorexia. Adapted from Favaro et al. (2009).
Incidence of psychoses during Half as common as in bipolar Twice as common as in unipolar Large
depression
Number of lifetime episodes of Somewhat less frequent Somewhat more frequent Moderate
depression
Longest episodes of depression Much longer Much shorter Large
Excessive self-reproach More extensive Less extensive Large
Difficulty sleeping Less difficulty More difficulty Large
Loss of energy Greater loss Less significant loss Large
Diminished libido More diminished Less diminished Large
TABLE 16.2 Bipolar disorder versus unipolar depression. Even in their depressive periods, people afflicted with unipolar depression have
different symptoms and time courses than those with bipolar disorder. People with bipolar disorder have more frequent incidences of psychoses,
more frequent episodes of depression, and shorter episodes of depression, among other differences. From Forty et al. (2008).
DEPRESSION 603
has inflated self-esteem, grandiose plans, increased talkative- 30
ness, and a diminished amount of sleep (American Psychi- Total
Q: What are some differences in how with reduced intensity, increases markedly after 15 years of age
depression is manifested in people with bipolar (Hankin et al., 1998; see also Ge et al., 1994; Lakdawalla et al.,
versus unipolar versions of the disorder? 2007; Petersen et al., 1993).
Why is there such a dramatic increase in the rate of
depression after puberty? Several possible reasons have
been proposed: more stressful life events during and after
puberty (Ge et al., 2001), increased interpersonal vulner-
Unipolar Depression ability (Gotlib & Hammen, 1992), physiological changes
It was traditionally held that young children simply didn’t get produced by puberty, and changes in cognition (Lakdawalla
depressed. More recently, there seems to be a consensus that et al., 2007; Lockhart et al., 2002; Rutter, 1987). These
young children can develop true depressions that are similar accounts also attempt to explain why the surge in incidence
to both the unipolar and bipolar types in adults (Coyle et al., coincides with the quite sudden onset of a large gender dif-
2003). This opinion is based on the ability to reliably use assess- ference. Each of these factors is considered in exploring the
ment questionnaires for childhood depression and because of etiology of childhood and adolescent depression.
convergences between what parents say about the behaviors of
their children and the children’s own self-reports (McClellan & Biological Causes Unipolar depression and its weaker
Werry, 2000). The childhood assessments focus on factors variants tend to run in families, suggesting a genetic compo-
that tend to be more behavioral in nature, such as problems in nent. Indeed, in one study of severe depression among mono-
school and in social interactions and the presence of behaviors zygotic and dizygotic adolescent twins, the much higher
that are disruptive and aggressive. There are also indications concordance rate among MZ twins suggested a heritability
of unpleasant or uncomfortable moods and an undervaluing of roughly 40 percent (Glowinski et al., 2003; Lau & Eley,
of the self relative to others (Carle et al., 2008). Some children 2010; Rice & Thapar, 2009). Thus, even in cases of relatively
do seem to enter sustained periods of reduced affect and have early onset of the disorder, the heritabilities are as high as in
some of the other symptoms found in adults, such as feelings adults, suggesting that the biological basis in adolescence may
of hopelessness about the future, excessive rumination about be similar to that found in older individuals. Moreover, if one
negative events, and suicidal thoughts. It is far less clear, how- individual in a family experiences depression in adolescence,
ever, that these children experience the full range of adult or the likelihood that a relative will also experience depression
adolescent symptoms. In fact, the recent flurry of attention in adolescence increases based on how closely related the two
to the presence of depression in young children runs the risk individuals are (for example, MZ twins, DZ twins, non-twin
of overlooking much more dramatic developmental facts: the siblings, or cousins), suggesting that genetics plays an impor-
incidence of unipolar depression skyrockets during adolescence tant role in the onset of the disorder.
and swings from an even distribution among the sexes to a Genetic factors may often work in combination with
huge preponderance among females (see Figure 16.9). Unipolar daily life stressors, such that adolescents who have a par-
depression, with its somewhat muted forms of negative affect ticular genetic vulnerability may be much more likely to
DEPRESSION 605
major depression have different cognitive schemas for think- arguments mentioned earlier to tendencies to appraise many
ing about the world and interpreting events. For example, life events in more negative terms. But it is not always the
Aaron Beck proposed that depressed individuals have a case that the environments faced by teenagers are that much
negative style of thought that leads them to make different more stressful than those encountered a few years earlier.
sorts of attributions than nondepressed individuals and that In fact, as seen in Chapter 15, bullying and other forms
this difference in attribution style leads to depression (Beck, of peer aggression often reach their peak before puberty.
1967). Beck hypothesized that depressed individuals have Nonetheless, adolescence may pose its own uniquely stress-
a mutually reinforcing set of negative beliefs about them- ful challenges, and with respect to sex differences, there are
selves, about the world, and about their future that causes a consistent reports that girls both report and actually experi-
negative schema to automatically be used in most situations. ence more negative life events in late childhood and adoles-
A key part of Beck’s approach was the idea that various bio- cence (Hyde et al., 2008).
logical factors, such as a certain genotype, place a child at We can also look beyond the impact of stressful life
risk and then a particular kind of stressful environment trig- events on cognition and instead directly consider the child’s
gers the negative cognitive style. Thus, not all children with cognitive development. Are there more general changes in
a genetic vulnerability to depression will become depressed how children think about the world that might be related
and not all children growing up in highly stressful environ- to the surge of depression in teenagers? Such a change does
ments will become depressed, but when the two factors are seem to occur in the extent to which children are optimistic
both present, depression may emerge. about whether current negative traits will change into more
positive versions of those traits in the future. As described in
Chapter 13, there is a major shift during the elementary and
middle school years in how children regard the malleability
Q: How might stress and genetic factors of negative traits (Lockhart et al., 2002). Younger children
interact to create depression? assume that negative traits, even physical ones like missing
a finger, are likely to change for the better with maturity.
Older children and adults think that such change toward
A related theory proposes that depression grows out of the positive is much less likely. In addition, younger chil-
feelings of hopelessness that are usually engendered by spe- dren tend to see more of a role for their own efforts in bring-
cific life experiences (Hyde et al., 2008; Rose & Abramson, ing about change, hence inoculating them from learned
1992). A child might be ill treated, and in her attempts to helplessness. Children’s views of the role of effort also seem
figure out why, she might develop a cognitive style in which to vary across cultures, which may explain the lower rates
she assumes that her misfortune is because of something of depression in cultures such as Japan, where traits are seen
about her rather than about the world. When she starts as more changeable through effort (Lockhart et al., 2008).
to see this negative feature in global terms (that is, across Although a decrease in cognitive optimism may offer
many different contexts) and as stable over time, she may insight into the increase in depression in adolescence, it does
fall into a state of cognitive hopelessness in which her future not in itself explain sex differences. Future studies need to
looks very bleak. This is because she thinks that something examine more systematically whether children’s diminished
intrinsic about her, which is very wide ranging in effects and optimistic bias as they age coincides with the onset of depres-
which will always be present, will doom her to a future of sion on an individual basis. As with other models, some
negative events. This view evolved out of a theory known as interaction with a biological risk factor or social experiences
learned helplessness, in which animals (including humans) more common to girls is also needed to explain why some
are put in a series of uncontrollable negative situations from individuals develop depression and others do not. It is also
which they cannot learn a way to escape, leading them to important to keep in mind that changes during puberty do
“learn to be helpless.” For example, a rat might be put into not seem to be correlated with the rate of cognitive develop-
a cage in which it receives shocks that it can neither escape ment during the same period (Litt, 1995). Thus, cognitive
nor have any control over. When animals or people are put development is likely to make an independent contribution
in these situations, they seem to learn a helpless style that is to depression that is above and beyond any direct or indirect
thought to be closely related to depression (Seligman, 1975). effects of puberty (see also Lakdawalla et al., 2007).
Yet, neither of these cognitive approaches on their own
can explain either the huge surge of depression in adoles-
cence or the sex differences. We can argue that transitions Q: How might patterns of cognitive
during puberty result in changes in learning experiences development explain the increase in depression
in school, peer groups, and the like, and that girls have with age?
more stressful transitions. This links the social influence
Pubertal hormones:
Biological Genetic
pubertal timing
Vulnerability vulnerability
and development
FIGURE 16.10 Factors
Gender contributing to the emergence
Affective Depressogenic difference in of depression in adolescence.
Temperament
Vulnerability vulnerability depression It now appears that biological,
(age 13–15) affective, and cognitive factors
can create a high level of
Negative Objectified “depressogenic vulnerability”
Cognitive
cognitive body Rumination
Vulnerability that interacts strongly with
style consciousness
negative life events to create
depression in adolescence and
that seems to converge more
Negative Peer sexual Other negative
Life Events harassment life events strongly for girls. Adapted from
Hyde et al. (2008).
DEPRESSION 607
NEW DIRECTION S IN DEVELOPMENTAL RESEARCH
A
s researchers attempt to explain the large gender dif- outgrowth of girls’ tighter social relations and higher levels of
ference in depression and its onset in puberty, one self-disclosure? Is it a side effect of socialization differences
new variable of interest is that of co-rumination. Co- that tend to push girls toward thinking more about internal
rumination occurs when a group tends to dwell on states and personal attributions? Or does it reflect something
negative events in a mutually reinforcing manner. Like rumination, deeper about the cognitive styles of girls and boys? Whatever
it seems to maintain and perhaps exacerbate depression, and it the answers, the co-rumination hypothesis does suggest that
occurs more frequently among girls and women than among boys girls who are depressed might be helped by interventions that
and men. Yet, it may seem paradoxical that girls, who are known focus on the social groups in which they are embedded and
to have generally closer and more extensive social relationships how the members of those social groups interact. More broadly,
than boys, are more depressed, as social bonds are thought to the social interaction patterns that distinguish boys and girls,
normally alleviate depression (Bukowski et al., 1996). But if co- especially shortly before and during adolescence, may be criti-
rumination is often a consequence of those bonds, it may help to cal to understanding the emergence of gender differences in
explain the higher rates of depression among girls. psychopathology (Crick & Zhan-Waxler, 2003).
Co-rumination is more socially dynamic and interactive than Co-rumination may have costs by inducing depression. But it
rumination. As we first mentioned in Chapter 15, co-rumination also may have benefits by promoting close friendships and net-
is the excessive discussion of personal problems in one-on-one works of social support, which may help explain why it persists
interactions with friends. These discussions may include going even with some negative consequences. Studies of ado-
over the problem again and again, speculating about future lescent girls do indeed suggest such a dual role in which
problems, being encouraged by others to engage in such dis- co-rumination levels are associated both with higher levels of
cussions, and focusing on negative emotions. Such discussions depression and with tighter social bonds (Rose et al., 2007; Starr &
might be about whether a potential boyfriend is about to break Davila, 2009). In addition, co-rumination seems to facilitate
up with a girl or whether a comment made by another girl is contagion of depressive emotions among friends (Schwartz-
really a veiled insult (Rose, 2002). The constant discussion of Mette & Rose, 2012), illustrating how the positive effects of
problems and negative emotions might prevent vulnerable girls friendships may also have negative side effects in vulnerable
from thinking or acting more positively and hence might lead to groups prone to co-rumination. The study of co-rumination rep-
or maintain depression. resents a new direction of research because it takes the process
When a co-rumination scale was developed, it did indeed of rumination beyond that of a lone individual. In considering how
demonstrate more co-rumination among third, fifth, seventh, a social network might cause a kind of positive feedback that
and ninth grade girls than among boys, with the gender dif- increases the depressive effects of rumination while also being
ference much larger among the adolescents in the group reinforced by the ways in which it builds social bonds and mutual
(Rose, 2002). Questions still remain about why co-rumination support, researchers can seek ways to break the loop and reduce
occurs more among girls in the first place. Is it simply a natural depression.
bipolar disorder in both children and adults, except that it is In addition to the behavioral differences, bipolar and
necessary to adjust the ratings of such factors as “grandiose unipolar affective disorders tend to occur in different family
plans” for what is normal for a child of that age (Leibenluft, groups and are reliably distinguished by different assessment
2008; Leibenluft & Rich, 2008). Thus, it might not be that instruments and by different developmental time courses,
unusual for a fourth-grader to think that he might really with bipolar disorders tending to emerge earlier (Leiben-
become an NBA star, despite having limited athletic skills, luft & Rich, 2008; Moreno et al., 2012). Unlike unipolar
but it would be unusual for a fourth-grader to think that he depression, bipolar disorder is not characterized by any dif-
could study hard and get a college degree over the next few ference between the sexes. Finally, the heritability of bipolar
months. disorder seems to be a good deal higher than the heritability
TABLE 16.3 Diagnosing conduct disorder. Conduct disorder is diagnosed when at least three of these behaviors are present within a
1-year time interval. Adapted from American Psychiatric Association (2000).
emotions, since a different basis for these inappropriate class. Harry gives George small gifts and flatters him heavily.
behaviors has been hypothesized (Blair & Blair, 2009). In He finds out about George’s secret crush on Mary, one of the
addition, when we consider conduct disorders as a whole, girls in their class, and he tells George that he knows Mary.
the primary focus will be on treatment methods. He then shows George some alleged excerpts from Mary’s
diary that he says he accidentally found and which say nice
things about George. A few days before a critical take-home
final examination, Harry asks George for help on the exam,
Features of Psychopathy a clear violation of the honor code. When George objects,
Psychopathy is one of the most chilling disorders, and Harry downplays the moral issues and reminds George that
adults afflicted with this disorder often arouse highly nega- he did look at Mary’s diary. In a veiled threat, he asks George
tive reactions in those around them. For centuries, histori- if helping on the exam would be as embarrassing as everyone
ans and writers have discussed individuals who seem to be knowing that George looked at Mary’s diary and had a secret
cold, calculating, and remorseless. In 1941, Hervey Cleck- crush on her. George ends up helping Harry on the exam.
ley crystallized these observations in a compelling account Since Harry has no further use for George, he stops interact-
of the disorder entitled The Mask of Sanity (Cleckley, 1941), ing with him and, to relieve boredom, writes an anonymous
which described the adult with psychopathy as having e-mail telling the entire class about George’s crush on Mary.
no guilt, shame, or emotional attachments, and as highly When George confronts Harry, Harry denies involvement in
manipulative and intelligent. In essence, this characteriza- a completely compelling manner that also casts George’s one
tion remains the same today, with somewhat less emphasis true friend in the class under a pall of suspicion.
on intelligence as a critical factor. A person with psychopa- It is difficult to imagine that people could behave so cal-
thy can be described as someone who is driven by the pursuit lously and ruthlessly to others and without any pangs of
of goals but who does not feel the impediments of morality conscience, but the abundant case reports of such individ-
or empathy toward others. As such, to get to a desired end, uals suggest that they constitute roughly 1 percent of the
a skilled person with psychopathy will manipulate a victim general population and a much higher proportion of inmate
through lies, deception, and distortions. populations. Some of the most successful people with psy-
Imagine a teenager, Harry, who “befriends” George, one of chopathy are so adroit at manipulating others that they
the least popular but most academically talented students in a are rarely recognized as having caused the pain that they
Item Description
1. Impression management Exhibits superficial charm and presents positive attributes to gain trust of others.
2. Grandiose sense of self-worth Is opinionated and domineering and shows inflated sense of self.
3. Stimulation seeking Is prone to boredom, seeks novelty and excitement, and enjoys taking risks.
4. Pathological lying Lies easily and often.
5. Manipulation for personal gain Deceives, manipulates, and cons people.
6. Lack of remorse/guilt Does not feel guilty or care about hurting others.
7. Shallow affect Does not feel deep emotion, but pretends to feel emotion and to care about others.
8. Callousness Lacks empathy, views others as objects, and does not care about others’ feelings and needs.
9. Parasitic orientation Exploits others by taking their money and possessions or by manipulating others to do his work.
10. Poor anger control Is easily offended and provoked to violence.
11. Promiscuous sexual behavior Has many casual sexual encounters, often involving threats and coercion.
12. Early behavioral problems Lies, steals, and sets fires before age 10.
13. Lacks goals Has no interest in education and has unrealistic future goals.
14. Impulsivity Acts out, may quit school or leave home on a whim, and does not consider consequences of
his actions.
15. Irresponsibility Engages in reckless behaviors and does not fulfill obligations or pay back debts.
16. Failure to accept responsibility Blames others for his problems and does not take personal responsibility for his actions.
17. Unstable interpersonal relationships Does not show commitment or loyalty to others and has volatile relationships.
18. Serious criminal behavior Engages in many instances of criminal activity.
19. Serious violations of conditional Commits two or more violations (for example, crimes or drug or alcohol abuse) when released
release on probation from juvenile correctional facilities.
20. Criminal versatility Engages in at least six kinds of criminal behavior.
TABLE 16.4 Symptoms of psychopathy. The Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version rates individuals on 20 items that collectively are quite
effective in identifying adolescents with psychopathy. Adapted from Dolan (2004).
Method:
1. Researchers included in the study 88 boys, ages 7 to 11, who had
often been in trouble at home or at school.
2. All boys were administered the Antisocial Process Screening Device
(APSD), which detects psychopathic tendencies. Higher scores
indicated more psychopathy.
3. All boys sat at a monitor while wearing goggles that quickly flashed
different images to each eye. One eye saw an abstract pattern in
motion that faded; the other eye saw a face (either fearful, happy,
disgusted, or neutral) that also faded. The abstract pattern made it 2000
Results: 0
Children who had lower APSD scores noticed the fearful faces before
other kinds of faces, but children who had higher scores (and more
signs of psychopathy) did not.
-1000
Conclusion:
Children who score high on psychopathy screening tests take longer
to notice fearful faces when they are automatically processing visual -2000
stimuli that are outside of their awareness. This may be because they 0 2 4 6 8 10
are not as sensitive to potential threats in their environment when they APSD score
are engaged in preattentive processing of the world around them. This
early automatic deficit may make it difficult to train them to be more
sensitive to such expressions.
Source study: Sylvers et al. (2011).
inhibit emotions (Glenn et al., 2009; Motzkin et al., 2011; the heritability of psychopathy has not been studied in as
Verona et al., 2012). EEG studies of children who exhibit much detail as the heritability of other mental disorders,
psychopathy-related behaviors when they are as young as there are consistent indications of genetic influences on two
8 years old show that these children have similar difficul- distinct factors, one that is concerned with impulsivity and
ties in modulating negative emotions (Granic et al., 2012). antisocial behavior (especially of a proactive as opposed to
Thus, it may be that early difficulties in appropriately mod- a reactive sort) and another that is associated with callous-
ulating and regulating emotions interact with difficulties ness and “interpersonal detachment” (Bezdjian et al., 2011;
in empathy and emotion perception to create psychopathic Larsson et al., 2006; Liu et al., 2002; Taylor et al., 2003;
behaviors. Viding et al., 2008, 2012). It is not yet clear if one of these
It has also been hypothesized that there is a specific gene factors is more primary and tends to lead to the other or
variant in mice that results in reduced amygdala volume whether both contribute relatively equally to the emergence
and increased aggressive behavior and that it may have a of psychopathy, but there is some sense in the literature that
counterpart in humans (Blair, 2003; Meyer-Lindenberg callousness and interpersonal detachment may be more cen-
et al., 2006; Monaghan et al., 1997; Raine, 2008). While tral to the emergence of psychopathy.
SCHIZOPHRENIA 615
phrenia. In addition to simple delays in motor development,
Q: What is the typical developmental course of the unusual motor patterns include writhing and jerky
movements of the limbs, unusual postures with the arms,
late-onset schizophrenia?
and tics and spasms (MacManus et al., 2012). Such motor
abnormalities have even been reported in infants (Walker
Because of the low incidence of schizophrenia, it is not et al., 1994).
very practical to closely observe a large group of children None of these predictors, however, means that we can
knowing that only 1 percent will end up being the group foretell the future of any individual child. Many children
of interest. It would be much easier to take a group of with these symptoms will not develop schizophrenia,
people who have the disease and somehow observe what and many people who are diagnosed with the disorder as
they were like as children. This idea seemed impossible adults do not seem to have had any history of these pre-
until researchers realized that with the rise of video cam- dictive behaviors in childhood. This raises challenges for
eras and home movie systems, it has become a ritual for interventions, for even if a child exhibits many predictive
many families to record their children at each birthday behaviors, we would not want to intervene in too radical a
party and other events (Grimes & Walker, 1994; Schiff- way to try to reduce the possibility of schizophrenia if the
man et al., 2004; Walker et al., 1993). Could information intervention itself was potentially harmful (for example,
in these home movies suggest precursors of schizophrenia? using strong medications that might have harmful side
Once the idea emerged, the general method was clear. A effects).
group of judges watched large collections of these home
movies of children, some of whom later developed schizo-
phrenia and some of whom did not (usually siblings of the Q: What are some possible signs that young
first group). The judges, who were blind as to the children’s children will develop schizophrenia in the future?
future outcomes, coded a wide range of behaviors in the
children. There were unmistakable indications of differ-
ences. In particular, children who developed schizophrenia
showed more affective blunting—that is, less pronounced
emotions as well as less frequent displays of responses indi- Causal Factors
cating either strong positive or negative emotions. Thus, The genetic basis for schizophrenia has been extensively
while those diagnosed with schizophrenia may not show studied, and a clear relation exists between the likelihood
the symptoms of the disorder as children, a significant of developing the disorder and how closely one is related to
number do tend to show precursor behaviors having to another relative who has the disorder (Cannon et al., 1998;
do with social responsiveness that may reflect social and Jablensky & Kalaydjieva, 2003). The genetic story is com-
cognitive deficits that continue to gradually accumulate plex, involving several different genes and their interactions
throughout adolescence and beyond (Algon et al., 2012; and cases where some of the genes may be present but not
Cannon et al., 2002; Welham et al., 2009). expressed (Claes et al., 2012; Gejman et al., 2011; Gottesman
Beyond social responsiveness deficits, we can also find & Bertelsen, 1989).
communicative and language difficulties and some cognitive The strong genetic component of the disease is unmis-
and attentional deficits (Erlenmeyer-Kimling et al., 2000; takable and might seem to suggest a purely biological expla-
Kates, 2010; Reichenberg et al., 2010). Moreover, when at- nation. But another equally dramatic finding raises further
risk children and adolescents show higher levels of unusual questions: roughly half of MZ twins in which one twin is
thought content, greater tendencies of suspicion/paranoia, diagnosed with schizophrenia do not share the disorder,
and a history of substance abuse (for example, heavy use of even though they have identical genomes, a genetic effect
marijuana), they are substantially more likely to later exhibit that is weaker than that for obsessive-compulsive disor-
psychosis and other symptoms of schizophrenia (Cannon der (see Figure 16.13). Thus, although researchers repeat-
et al., 2008). The association of later schizophrenia with edly find higher heritabilities for schizophrenia in relatives
earlier marijuana use does not mean that heavy marijuana of those with schizophrenia than in the general popula-
use early on causes schizophrenia, but some researchers do tion, it is important to keep in mind that many adults
suspect that it may play a role in aggravating or precipitating with schizophrenia have never had any relatives who were
symptoms (Anglin et al., 2012). diagnosed with the disorder (Gottesman & Erlenmeyer-
Other potential early signs may involve motor movement Kimling, 2001). In addition, the same study that found
difficulties, with children who show a certain class of motor that higher paternal age is linked to higher rates of autism
movements more likely to later develop symptoms of schizo- in children also found a link between higher paternal age
and higher rates of schizophrenia in children, apparently order emerges. One account suggests that children with
resulting from mutations being introduced into the child’s the at-risk gene carried by the Genain sisters will be much
genome when precursors of sperm cells repeatedly divided, worse off if they contract certain infections while in the
and not because of the inheritance of mutations in either womb or shortly after birth (Rentakallio, 1997; Torrey,
parent’s genome (Kong et al., 2012). 1988). Since even Myra had some schizophrenic symp-
The lack of a complete genetic predetermination for toms, it appears that all four sisters were equally at risk,
schizophrenia was most dramatically revealed in a highly but that differences in the severity of an early infection
unusual case of four sisters born in the 1930s. Known as resulted in a difference in the extent to which the schizo-
the Genain quadruplets, they first came to the attention of phrenic effects were amplified. A prenatal infection seems
researchers in the 1950s because one of them was showing more plausible than a postnatal one, because that is the
the symptoms of a full-blown case of schizophrenia. They period during which the brain is especially susceptible to
were all monozygotically related—hence, they had identi- trauma (see Chapter 2).
cal genes—yet they varied strikingly in the extent to which Support for the infection theory was found after the
they manifested the disorder, and they continued to dif- massive 1918 worldwide influenza epidemic. Following
fer from one another at age 66, almost 40 years after they the epidemic, children whose mothers had been victims
were first studied (Rosenthal, 1963; see also Mirsky et al., of the epidemic when they were pregnant with the child
1995, 2000). One sister, known as Myra, had symptoms of showed a higher incidence of schizophrenia (Yudofsky,
schizophrenia, but she was able to manage with them quite 2009). In addition, the incidence of schizophrenia shows a
well, and she married, had children, and successfully raised seasonal variation based on when children are born, with
a family. At the other extreme, a sister known as Hester schizophrenia tending to be more likely in those born in
was never able to make it on her own outside of a sheltered winter or spring months, shortly after periods when infec-
environment such as an institution or a home with exten- tious diseases are more common (Mortensen et al., 1999).
sive care. The other two sisters, called Nora and Iris (all are Of course, many pregnant mothers contract influenza and
pseudonyms to protect privacy), showed moderate levels of have children who never develop schizophrenia. Thus,
symptoms between Myra’s and Hester’s. some researchers have suggested that the disorder develops
because of the combination of being genetically at risk and
contracting an infection by a particular type of virus—
Q: Describe evidence showing how genetic sometimes called a “schizovirus”—that amplifies the
factors cannot provide a full explanation of genetic effects. In some cases, a particularly bad infection
schizophrenia. on its own, such as toxoplasmosis, a disease often passed to
pregnant mothers by house cats, may cause schizophrenia
in those who are not genetically at risk (Torrey & Yolken,
The glaring question was: How could four genetically 1995, 2003).
identical individuals have such a wide range of outcomes The infection theory remains controversial. Higher levels
with a disorder that is often thought to be biologically of antibodies for some viruses have been found in those
based? The answer may lie in the details of how the dis- with schizophrenia, but not for other viruses that are also
SCHIZOPHRENIA 617
thought to be agents that cause schizophrenia (Brown et al., natal events (Marenco & Weinberger, 2000). One neuro-
2004; Karlsson et al., 2001). Because some viruses have been developmental account posits three distinct times when there
known to be dormant for many years before flaring up, the may be influences that increase the odds of an individual
infection account has also been used to explain the surge of developing schizophrenia: conception, early development
schizophrenia in adolescence, but the mechanism remains (prenatal and during and soon after birth), and later devel-
unclear. From a cross-cultural viewpoint, some striking dif- opment (childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood).
ferences in the incidence of the disease, such as a rate that is As seen in Figure 16.14, the influences at conception
four times higher in some regions of Ireland, have also been are largely genetic factors that put a developing human
cited to support a model based on local disease epidemics. at risk (Karlsgodt et al., 2011). Influences during early
Taken as a whole, there seems to be an emerging consensus development involve both prenatal complications aris-
that exposure to influenza in the womb probably can be a ing from factors such as malnutrition and maternal dis-
contributing factor to developing the disorder (Brown et al., eases and complications right at the time of birth, such
2004; Yudofsky, 2009). as excessively low levels of oxygen being supplied to the
An alternative account argues that stress and maltreat- baby, resulting in brain damage. These early complica-
ment early on can amplify the disorder, and once again the tions may combine with certain genetic risk factors to cre-
Genain sisters may offer hints. There is some evidence that ate abnormalities in several brain regions, especially those
the sisters’ abusive father was considerably kinder to Myra involving the frontal and temporal lobes, the limbic system,
and Nora than to Iris and Hester, leading some to argue and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Later devel-
that while stress alone cannot cause schizophrenia, it can opmental factors may involve genetic abnormalities that
greatly amplify the symptoms (Mirsky et al., 2000). There cause excessive pruning of gray matter in frontal and tem-
is also the possibility, however, that Hester and Iris were poral lobes and diminished development of white mat-
treated worse because they were more impaired. ter tracts that connect regions concerned with attention
The most plausible model is one that suggests strong and memory. These later developmental factors are likely
interactions between biological factors, such as genes and to be exacerbated by environmental stresses. A series of
infections, and environmental ones, such as stress (Walker & detailed MRI structural studies looking both at gray mat-
Diforio, 1997; Walker et al., 2008). This “neurodevelop- ter volume and white matter connectivity between brain
mental model,” which emphasizes the critical influence of regions has shown that deterioration resulting from exces-
a biological factor during prenatal development that then sive pruning of neurons and synapses and decreased con-
gradually unfolds for many years after birth, may offer the nectivity often precedes the onset of full-blown symptoms
best explanation of how the disorder gets initially launched, of schizophrenia, strongly suggesting a causal role of these
especially when combined with a genetic factor that makes changes—that is, they are not just consequences of hav-
some fetuses more vulnerable to the effects of those pre- ing the disorder (Karlsgodt et al., 2011).
Synaptic Social
Genomic pruning, white matter environmental factors
development (stress)
Prenatal
and perinatal
complications Later neural system
vulnerabilities
ration
Deterio
Early neural system
FIGURE 16.14 A neurode-
vulnerabilities Symptoms of deterioration
velopmental model of schizo- (e.g., abnormalities
phrenia. According to this in frontal and medial Psychosis onset
model, there are three distinct temporal lobes, limbic Precursor symptoms
periods in development: concep- system, and HPA axis) Functional deterioration
tion, early development, and later Premorbid behavior disturbance
development, at which times Delayed language
different factors may contribute to Motor abnormalities
the later onset of schizophrenia. Conception Birth Infancy Childhood Adolescence Young Adulthood
Adapted from Karlsgodt et al.
Conception Early development Later development
(2011).
Family Therapies
We briefly discussed family-based approaches in the context
Q: What are some of the challenges of using
of anorexia, when we presented Minuchin’s bold ideas on the family therapy techniques?
role of dynamic systems in families that fall into abnormal
interaction patterns (Minuchin & Fishman, 1981). These
approaches are notable because they require far more than
a one-on-one therapist-child relationship; they focus on
engaging the entire family. In many cases, it seems to make Pharmacological Treatments
sense to view the child’s problem as part of a full family In addition to the wide range of therapies based on inter-
system that has dysfunctional interaction patterns that need personal interactions, there are, of course, attempts to treat
to be treated. Family therapy approaches have been shown disorders directly through various medications. The steady
to be effective on problems ranging from infant sleep pat- increase in the use of drugs to treat childhood psychopa-
terns, to conduct disorders, to obsessive-compulsive behav- thology in recent years is a vast and controversial topic (see
iors, to depression, to substance abuse (Asarnow et al., 2001; Development and Social Policy box). Drug treatment tended
Carr, 2009). In some cases, treating the family as a whole initially to be for children with disorders related to ADHD
seems to produce larger and more enduring results. For and most often consisted of doses of methylphenidate (trade
example, in one study comparing treatments for teenag- name Ritalin). As described in Chapter 10, the use of meth-
ers with substance abuse problems, researchers found that ylphenidate and related drugs for ADHD with ever younger
while both CBT and family therapy had similar short-term children has soared, but in many cases, such drug treatment
effects, family therapy had better long-term outcomes, as may be overused, even as it seems to have clear benefits for
well as being more effective for more severe forms of abuse children who are correctly diagnosed with the disorder.
(Liddle et al., 2008). Simply treating the child alone may At present, a much wider range of drugs is being used to
not be as effective in the long term if the parents are also treat adolescents and children (Figure 16.15 illustrates the
engaging in behaviors that tend to aggravate or even pro- increase in use of both stimulants and antidepressants in
mote drug use—for example, belittling a child’s compe- children and adolescents over the past few decades). In addi-
tence or not providing opportunities for rich and rewarding tion to drugs for ADHD, antidepressants such as fluoxetine
social interactions with the rest of the family.
One major challenge of family therapy is that many
members of the family often do not want to acknowledge 5
their role in creating the underlying problems. It is all too Stimulants
Percent of children prescribed drugs
4.5
common for parents to drop their children off at the thera- Antidepressents
4
pist and assume that all of the child’s problems are within
3.5
the child and simply need to be fixed by the therapist. Even Stimulants
if there is some sense of a larger context for the problem, 3
parents and therapists may not agree as to what it is. For 2.5
example, in one study, researchers asked children, parents, 2
and therapists to identify the fundamental problem or prob- 1.5 Antidepressents
lems that they should be working on. More than 75 percent
1
of the child/parent/therapist triads began therapy without
any agreement on even a single problem that they were sup- 0.5
Prescribing Psychoactive
Medications to Young Children
A
lthough most childhood psychopathologies have Chapter 2). How might elevated levels of neurotransmitters and
been treated through different sorts of therapies, in certain firing patterns caused by psychiatric drugs influence
recent years drugs have emerged as a frequent form brain growth? At present, the answers to these questions are
of treatment as well, a phenomenon that has gen- unknown. And even if psychoactive drugs do improve the short-
erated enormous controversy (Morris & Stone, 2011; Whitaker, term mental health of some children, it will be years before the
2010). There has been an extraordinary rise in the use of various long-term developmental implications are fully understood.
psychoactive medications with children, with the age for pre- Some researchers are concerned that the welfare of children
scribing such medications decreasing to the point where even may be in conflict with the profit motive of some pharmaceutical
preschoolers are being given medications originally developed companies (Harris, 2004). In that context, it is notable that the
for adult forms of psychopathology. In fact, the fastest-growing United Kingdom, which has a more socialized form of medicine
rate of prescriptions for antidepressants is in preschoolers, with than the United States, has much more stringent rules against
the rates of prescriptions for some groups of children tripling prescribing antidepressants to children (Satel, 2004). The con-
in the last decade (Brown, 2003; Zito et al., 2003). Over one trast between the two countries reveals how economic factors
recent 4-year period, prescription rates for preschool girls rose have the potential to interact with the health interests of children.
65 percent (Delate et al., 2004; Warner et al., 2004), and the These issues have become especially salient, given reports of
rates have continued to rise significantly (Parens & Johnston, higher levels of suicide among children taking antidepressants
2008). It is true, of course, that these increases were from an as opposed to placebos (Brent & Maalouf, 2009; Jureidini
initially very low rate, but the increases are nonetheless dramatic. et al., 2004). It has even been argued that giving extended high
There is a lack of critical information about the risks of using doses of medications such as Ritalin to young children could
such medications to treat very young children, especially over increase the risk of tipping some children into psychotic states
the long term. In most cases, young children are prescribed (Whitaker, 2010).
drugs that have had little or no testing of their effects in children Some have argued that opposition to using such medica-
(Brown, 2003; Hsia & Maclennan, 2009). Children are being tions with children is “misguided resistance to appropriate treat-
given medications based on the results of studies conducted ment” (Koplewicz, 2004), on the grounds that any increased
on adults and based on doctors’ best guesses about the likely risks posed by the medications for depression, psychosis, and
impacts and best dosages for children. But children absorb conduct disorders are offset by the larger risks associated with
medicine at different rates than adults, and children may metab- harm to the child, such as in adolescent and child suicide, or
olize psychoactive medications differently than adults, which to others through violence by the child. Consider the case of
could change the drug concentrations that would be harmful in depression. If it could be reliably demonstrated that prescribing
the target tissues of children. Moreover, young children might antidepressants to children and adolescents does reduce the
have difficulty expressing whether they are experiencing side incidence of suicide, it would make it easier to evaluate the costs
effects from too large a dosage of a drug. All of these factors and benefits of such prescriptions, but no such demonstration is
make guessing at doses a risky procedure. available (Jureidini et al., 2004; Tsapakis et al., 2008).
When a psychological disorder is diagnosed in preschool- Parents must consider all of these concerns when deciding if
ers, it is exceedingly difficult to know whether its causes might their child should take psychoactive medications. When children
resemble those in an adult or older teenager, raising even more are in a state of clear psychological distress and at great risk
doubts about whether a particular medication designed for to themselves or others, it is inevitable that parents will have
those groups would work in young children. Of special con- to consider the use of medications that are at least sometimes
cern is the effect of medications taken over the long term on effective in bringing short-term improvements in a child’s condi-
a child’s developing brain. We have seen that there are exqui- tion. Given these considerations, there is a clear need for more
sitely sensitive feedback loops between various neural circuits long-term studies of the efficacies and side effects of using
in development that help regulate aspects of brain growth (see psychoactive drugs in children (Rapoport, 2009).
STU DY AN D REVI EW
unipolar and bipolar, have quite different symptoms and pat- developmental status of the child. Cognitive development,
terns of development. Unipolar depression increases markedly in particular, interacts heavily with the ways in which vari-
in females after puberty. Bipolar disorder, which consists of ous forms of treatment might be implemented. For example,
shifts between depressive and manic phases, tends to occur therapies that ask children to think about their own beliefs
equally in males and females, and generally emerges in late must take into account metacognitive development, and
adolescence. therapies that focus on family dynamics must adjust for the
● The late emergence of most cases of unipolar depression may social and communicative abilities of the youngest members
be related to changes in cognitive development, including a of those families.
reduction of an optimistic bias that colors the thoughts of
younger children and an increase in various forms of rumina-
tion. Unipolar depression tends to run in families, as does THOUGHT QUESTIONS
bipolar disorder, which has an even higher genetic heritability. 1. There are several reported cases of identical (monozygotic)
twins who experience very different outcomes with respect to
Conduct Disorder: The Case of Psychopathy psychopathology. Discuss a hypothetical pair of twins where
● Conduct disorder is a broad class of behavioral problems, but one develops a serious mental illness and the other lives a life
one severe form, known as psychopathy, is normally not diag- without any apparent mental illness. Consider at least three
nosed until adolescence or adulthood. People with psychopa- different causal factors that could be responsible for these
thy are extremely self-centered, are superficially charming, different outcomes.
A
basic fact of human life remains a mystery: Why do pairs of the telomeres were not there. But some of the base
we age? Why don’t we reach a peak adult form and pairs of the telomeres are not reproduced, and so the telomeres
then simply maintain that form for perpetuity, con- become a bit shorter with each division, sacrificing themselves
stantly replenishing damaged tissue in the same to save the more critical base pairs. Ultimately, they will become
manner that a salamander regrows its tail? One long-accepted too short and will lose their functional roles, although at least
theory argues that aging is an evolved trait in which organ- some cells in the body, as well as cancer cells, seem to have a
isms are programmed to gradually die after they have created mechanism for re-extending the telomeres through an enzyme
viable offspring so that they can make resources available for known as telomerase (Hastings et al., 2004; Klingelhutz,
the next generation (Weismann, 1889). More recently, the idea 1999; Rubin, 1998). Recent work also suggests that high-stress
of evolutionarily programmed aging and death has been dis- environments, either physiologically in terms of poor nutrition or
counted as implausible when considered in detail (Kirkwood & psychologically in terms of adverse events, may accelerate the
Cremer, 1982; Sutphin & Kennedy, 2009), but the original pro- rate of telomere shortening (Effros, 2009, 2012; Epel, 2009).
posal illustrates how the overall question of how and why we More optimistically, some hypotheses suggest that more benign
age is one of the most central questions in biology and one that environments may halt and even reverse the effect and cause
is still not well understood. More current views argue that aging telomere lengthening and slower aging (Effros, 2009; Epel,
is a side effect of the lack of natural selection for repair mecha- 2009). This work has led to proposals that telomere length
nisms of tissue damage occurring later in life (Partridge, 2010). might be seen as a psychobiomarker—a biological feature
Average lifespans vary considerably across organisms and are that serves as an indicator of psychological functioning—of the
certainly related to their genetic makeup. It has been possible, for quality of aging (Epel, 2009).
example, to selectively breed species of worms (Kenyon, 2001) We can also see how the full life cycle becomes relevant to
and fruit flies (Landis et al., 2004) to obtain ”Methuselahs” with the effects of shortened telomeres. Thus, it may be that stress
far greater lifespans (for example, 75 days in fruit flies rather very early in life starts to prematurely shorten telomeres. One
than 40 days). Such a genetic involvement, however, should not study of chick embryos showed that injecting the eggs with hor-
be taken as evidence for a genetic program that specifically trig- mones related to stress responses resulted in shortened telo-
gers aging. It simply means that the factors that contribute to meres when the chicks were born (Haussmann et al., 2012).
aging are partially related to an organism’s genetic makeup. In humans, children who suffer from maltreatment also show
Several distinct theories of aging remain under debate. One shortened telomeres compared with controls (Price et al., 2013).
posits a gradual accumulation of negative random events, most There are even reports that children from lower socioeconomic
often as a product of metabolic processes such as oxidation, groups have shortened telomeres compared with more affluent
which can leave by-products that decrease the efficiency of pro- children, with calculations that the shortening is “roughly equiva-
teins and metabolic structures such as mitochondria. lent to 6 years of additional aging” (Needham et al., 2012). If
A second view argues that cells can divide only a limited supported by further research, these reports suggest that with-
number of times, roughly 50 in many cases. This cell division out amelioration later, earlier negative life experiences could
limit is known as the Hayflick limit (Hayflick, 1965, 1998). It have negative effects on aging half a century later.
is thought to be related to DNA sequences called telomeres, A third view suggests that the genetic material itself, the ani-
which are located at the ends of chromosomes and protect the mal’s DNA, becomes slowly degraded over time through muta-
chromosomes from deteriorating and from fusing with other tion and other events, such that it becomes more and more
chromosomes. Telomeres prevent the loss of critical base pair likely to produce nonoptimal proteins. If this third view is cor-
sequences at the ends of the chromosomes during cell divi- rect, the greater longevity of some species, such as humans,
sion by acting as protective caps to these base pairs. Because may be related to the higher incidence of redundant strings of
duplication of base pairs cannot occur at the extreme end of DNA, which can provide a kind of backup production of critical
the chromosome, essential base pairs would be lost if the base proteins (Morrish et al., 2002).
1.2
Speed of processing:
Performance declines with increasing pattern comparison task
age for speed of processing, working
memory, and long-term memory Working memory:
reading span task
0.6
Performance on task (z-score)
Long-term memory:
cued recall task
World knowledge:
Shipley vocabulary task
0
−0.6
Performance is preserved
and even increases with age
for world knowledge
−1.2
FIGURE 17.6 Fluid versus crystallized intelligence and aging. On most cognitive measures of what is considered “fluid intelligence,”
there is a marked decline in performance with increasing age. On measures of more “crystallized intelligence,” performance can increase
until a person is well into his 70s. Tasks involving speed of processing, working memory, and long-term memory clearly show declines in
performance, while tasks involving general world knowledge generally show increases in performance. Adapted from Zimerman et al.
(2011).
60
Study 4 as manuscript editing—and they select specific goals within
that area—for example, building up world knowledge rele-
50 vant to the topic areas where they do most of their editing by
reading extensively about those areas. Although the upper
limits of the benefits of such specializations and practice are
40
not yet known, it seems reasonable that, in many cases, a
person may be able to continually optimize performance for
30 20 or even 30 years. Those individuals who cognitively age
in a “successful” manner may be those who develop a skill
that they can continue to fine-tune and improve in decade
20 after decade.
A different phenomenon that mitigates the effects of
aging on performance may be compensation—using aids
10
20 30 40 50 60 70 80 or altering behavior to make up for losses in cognitive or
Age (in years) physical abilities. As people age, they may start to realize
more clearly the limits of their cognitive systems and may
FIGURE 17.7 Consistent cognitive improvement with aging.
One of the most robust examples of a cognitive skill that increases develop ways of working around those limits. This might
throughout the lifespan is that of solving crossword puzzles. This be as simple as learning to use a notepad to help them
graph shows strikingly similar improvements across four different remember novel information or as complex as develop-
studies. Adapted from Salthouse (2004). ing new networks of friends and acquaintances who can
0.8
Control
0.7 Exercise
0.6
Size of effect
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
0
Executive Controlled Spatial Speed
Task type
A B
FIGURE 17.9 Exercise and cognitive aging. (A) Regular aerobic exercise has been causally linked not just to improved physiological health
in the elderly but also to improved cognitive performance. (B) In older adults, exercise is strongly associated with higher scores on a variety of
cognitive tasks, with the strongest effects occurring on executive tasks and more modest effects occurring on speed-of-processing tasks. These
effects are found not only in correlational studies but also in experimental studies that randomly assign elderly individuals to exercise groups
and control groups. This graph summarizes a meta-analysis of a large set of such studies. From Hillman et al. (2008).
Q: What aspects of cognition are influenced Q: What contrasting roles do implicit and
most by daily exercise? explicit memories seem to play in aging
populations?
It has long been thought that there is a “use it or lose
it” rule that governs cognitive aging, because those who
stay more cognitively active show slower rates of cognitive Circadian Rhythms, Cognition,
decline (Hultsch et al., 1999; Schaie, 2008). Many older
adults who believe in this rule will engage in regular cog- and the Lifespan
nitively challenging activities such as word and card games Circadian rhythms—that is, the shifts in biological activ-
as a form of cognitive calisthenics. Again, random assign- ity and functions throughout a daily cycle—typically vary
ment training studies are the best way to make strong in college-aged people and middle-aged people. Most college
inferences about causality. Here the findings are complex students seem to be more alert and cognitively functional
but point toward the need for a good deal of specificity in the late afternoon or evening, while most middle-aged
in the training. Thus, if a group of elderly people practice adults seem to be “morning people” (see Figure 17.10). Inter-
daily on a certain activity such as a video game, they will estingly, children also tend to have peak performance ear-
show improvements in that game and in closely related lier in the day, but a shift toward peak performance later in
tasks but generally not in other less similar cognitive tasks the day happens rather rapidly around puberty. As people
(Mather, 2010; Rabipour & Raz, 2012).
Some accounts of the sparing of cognitive functions in
the elderly refer to the idea of “cognitive reserve” (Stern,
2009). Cognitive reserve is the idea that some people
have more brain capacity to begin with and thereby have
more brain structures “in reserve” to support function-
ing in the face of aging-associated decline or minor brain
injuries. They may also have richer links between memo-
ries, thereby allowing the memories to more robustly rein-
force each other. There is good evidence that people with
above-normal cognitive capacities in college and middle
age do tend to show higher levels of cognitive functioning
in old age (Stern, 2009), but it is not yet known if cogni-
tive reserve is the primary reason or if people with higher
levels of cognitive ability remain active on many fronts
and thereby maintain their abilities through activity, not
because of reserves.
One study of young and old college professors and
young and old people selected from a more general popula-
tion documented less decline in the older professors than
in the general population, but also found interesting differ-
ences across tasks. Simple speed tasks, such as tasks involv-
ing one-choice reaction times, showed similar patterns of
FIGURE 17.10 Time of day and cognitive functioning. Although
decline in the elderly professors and among the elderly in
many college students may find that they study best late in the
the general population. With more cognitively complex afternoon or in the evening, older adults tend to be more effective
tasks, however, such as recall of a prose passage, the elderly cognitively in the early morning and can show marked drops in
professors showed almost no decline in performance, while performance in the late afternoon, often taking naps on purpose or
the elderly people from the general population showed a simply falling asleep unintentionally wherever they are.
Q: How does the time of day tend to interact FIGURE 17.11 Cognitive activities and aging. Many elderly
with patterns of cognitive functioning in older people engage in challenging mental activities, such as playing
and younger adults? bridge, out of the conviction that one of the best ways to hold off the
decline of cognitive skills is by continuing to vigorously exercise their
cognitive faculties. Such activities can indeed help to slow or even
Overall, the range and extent of cognitive changes in the halt decline, especially in tasks that are closely related to the original
elderly are large and often dramatic, making it all the more activity.
2
FIGURE 17.14 A positivity bias in memory. Participants of dif-
ferent ages were shown photos of (A) positive events, (B) negative
1 events, and (C) neutral events. (D) As shown in this graph, older
adults (ages 65 to 80) recalled more positive images than either
neutral or negative ones, while middle-aged adults (ages 41 to 53)
0 and younger adults (ages 18 to 29) recalled both positive and
18–29 41–53 65–80 negative images more than neutral ones. Adapted from Mather &
Age (in years) Carstensen (2005).
D
(Kennedy et al., 2004). The nuns ranged in age from 47 to and list positive and negative features. If they were sim-
102 years at the time of recall. Older nuns showed a strong ply asked to pick an object and then rate satisfaction with
bias toward recalling items in their questionnaires more their choice, there was no difference between the groups.
positively than they had originally reported them 14 years Thus, the positivity bias influences feature choice in the
earlier. Older nuns were seen as more motivated to provide evaluation task, which then influences satisfaction with the
a positive interpretation of past life events, perhaps as a choice. There may be real-world implications concerning
way of regarding their lives, as they neared their ends, as strategies used to influence decision making in older adults
more worthwhile. (Since more than 60 percent of the nuns as opposed to younger adults. Having older adults gener-
who had originally filled out the questionnaire had died ate a list of pros and cons for a decision is more likely to
during this period, the impending end of life was likely to result in their being satisfied with the decision than it is for
be very salient to the older nuns.) Over shorter time inter- younger adults.
vals, older people also show a distinctly better memory of At the neural level, this positivity effect has been dem-
positive pictures compared with negative and neutral ones onstrated by showing that the amygdala, a brain region
(see Figure 17.14). heavily involved in emotions, shows markedly less acti-
Positivity effects also have an influence on decision vation in response to negative stimuli than to positive
making. For example, when college students and people in stimuli in older adults, while showing strong responses to
their 60s were asked to list positive and negative features of both kinds of stimuli in younger adults (Mather et al.,
several common objects (for example, a pen, a mug), those 2004). Because negative events do not elicit as strong an
in the older group tended to list more positive features and emotional response in older adults, they are not nearly as
fewer negative ones. Both groups then got to pick an object motivated to attend to or remember negative information.
to keep, and those in the older group declared more satisfac- It appears that older adults also focus more on emotional
tion with their choice both immediately and after 2 weeks regulation and are often better at it than younger adults,
(Kim et al., 2008). This difference in satisfaction occurred being less quick to feel anger in reaction to negative events,
only when both groups were asked to evaluate each object reacting less strongly, and letting anger and negative affect
Method: Time
1. Fifty-two younger adults (18 to 35 years old) and 52 older adults
(62 to 94 years old) participated.
2. All participants started by looking at a blank screen across the room. What emotional expression was on the
face where the dot now is?
3. Two faces appeared for 1 second: one with a positive or negative
emotion and one that had a neutral expression. A happy
4. A small dot then appeared where one of the faces had been, and face.
participants were asked to say what the emotional expression had
been on that face.
5. The researchers measured the speed of the participants’ response.
dissipate more quickly. One reason for this shift in focus is them to be more optimistic and to show the positivity bias
the related finding that, as people approach the end of life (Samenez-Larkin & Knutson, 2014).
(including younger people with terminal illnesses), they Beyond the preference for noticing and remembering
tend to shift their priorities more to goals related to emo- positively construed information, there are other ways in
tional well-being in the present and less to those related to which personality for an entire age group tends to change
gaining knowledge for future use (Mather & Carstensen, as people age. Again, while a person may still be disagree-
2005). Older adults also show an asymmetry in neural able relative to his same-age peers, he may become much
processing of anticipated gains and losses compared with more agreeable in more absolute terms (comparing across
younger adults. This, too, may be related to the positivity all ages) as he grows older. As seen in Figure 17.15, these
bias. In particular, when anticipating rewards, individuals changes can be very substantial in some areas, such as
from both age groups show equally strong neural responses conscientiousness or agreeableness. Overall, scholars have
in reward-related centers. But when they are anticipating repeatedly noted that these changes tend to be ones that
losses, older adults show considerably lower levels of activ- impartial observers would rate as desirable. Thus, as people
ity in both the dorsal striatum and the anterior insula, grow older, their personalities tend to shift in ways that
which are regions related to processing gains and losses others would see as making them better people (Roberts &
(Samenez-Larkin et al., 2007). This pattern may result Mroczek, 2008).
in older adults having less anxiety associated with wor- Sometimes, apparent age-related shifts in personality
ries about potential future losses, which may thereby lead may simply reflect changing realities. Older individuals are
1 1
Cumulative d value
Cumulative d value
0.8 0.8
0.6 0.6
0.4 0.4
0.2 0.2
0 0
−0.2 −0.2
10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80
Age (in years) Age (in years)
Emotional stability Openness to experience
1.2 1.2
1 1
Cumulative d value
Cumulative d value
0.8 0.8
0.6 0.6
0.4 0.4
0.2 0.2
0 0
−0.2 −0.2
10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80
Age (in years) Age (in years)
FIGURE 17.15 Group changes in personality. While people tend to show consistency in personality types relative to their same-age peers,
age groups as a whole can show quite dramatic changes with increasing age along various personality dimensions. Here we see substantial
rises in three dimensions (agreeableness, conscientiousness, and emotional stability) and relative stability in another (openness to experience).
From Roberts & Mroczek (2008).
Q: What are some ways that older adults as a Erik Erikson’s Approach
group tend to shift in terms of their personality Erikson was initially trained in the psychoanalytic tradition
characteristics, their views of reality, and their and in fact studied under Anna Freud. At the same time, he
remembered experiences? had a strong background in anthropology, which led him
to take into account the effects of culture to a far greater
extent than either Freud or his daughter. Erikson proposed Stage 3: Initiative versus Guilt Preschool children
eight stages of psychosocial development, as summarized develop a better and better sense of their own personal
in Table 17.1, with each stage organized around a type of responsibility. As their circle of relationships extends to the
psychosocial conflict (Erikson, 1950). These conflicts are family as a whole, they become more aware of how their
the challenges people face in relating to others or to society actions influence others. They need to learn to take the
at large. They occur throughout the lifespan and must be initiative as they seek to gain mastery of a wide variety of
addressed in order to establish an identity within the social life tasks, but too much initiative without any guilt may
world. The challenge at each stage may be successfully lead to psychopathic behavior, producing a child without
Stage Approximate Age (in years) Developmental Period Psychosocial Conflict Most Important Relations
8 50 and older Later adulthood Integrity vs. despair Mankind /“my kind”
TABLE 17.1 Erikson’s stages of psychosocial development. Erikson proposed eight stages of psychosocial development, each marked by
a distinctive form of conflict that an individual has to address to help construct his social identity.
A
t first, it might seem outrageous to discuss depriving Legal scholars continuously wrestle with appropriate guide-
the elderly of certain rights, but in fact it happens lines (Frolik & Kaplan, 1999; Schuster, 1998; Walsh et al., 1994).
every day with the implicit consent of most members But wills are only the tip of an ever-growing iceberg as more and
of the larger society. Unfortunately, many people start more people reach advanced age. When should we deprive the
to have diminished psychological capacities toward the end of elderly of the right to drive? We know that past age 65 or so,
their lives. Certainly not everyone, but a large number of people there is an increased risk of accidents. How much of an increase
in their 80s and 90s show serious patterns of mental decline. is too much? Many states recognize this problem by requiring
At some point, that drop in abilities has strong social policy that the elderly have far more frequent renewals of their driver’s
implications. license and that they show up in person rather than renew by
An often-discussed example concerns wills. At what point mail (Grabowski & Morrisey, 2001; McLachlan, 1997).
does a person who is in intellectual decline lose the right to What about the right to vote? That nearly sacred right has not
change her own will? Offspring often contest changes in wills been challenged as much for the elderly, but there is no logical
that deprive them of resources on the grounds that the changes reason not to challenge the right to vote of a person of severely
were made by a parent who was no longer “all there.” When diminished capacity. Consider the case of a man with advanced
wealthy Aunt Hattie changes her will so as to give all her earthly senile dementia whose wife helped him vote in the 2000 Florida
goods to her butler, her heirs may claim that she was no lon- election. The man thought it was 1942 and that Franklin Delano
ger able to think clearly. What counts as sufficient grounds for Roosevelt was president. His wife went into the voting booth
depriving her of the right to change her own will? One well- with her husband and cast the ballot for her husband (voting
known case concerned the hotel owner Leona Helmsley, who for George Bush in an election where a few hundred votes
left $12 million to her Maltese dog “Trouble” and nothing to two decided the U.S. presidency; see Vedantam, 2004). Increas-
of her grandchildren. The grandchildren argued that Helmsley ingly, there has been an interest in the legal basis for allowing
was mentally incompetent when she signed that version of the elderly people with severely diminished capacity to vote, espe-
will, and a judge agreed, cutting back Trouble’s inheritance to a cially since some of the most pivotal “swing states” in elections
mere $2 million and giving some of the remainder to the grand- have large numbers of elderly voters. In Florida, nearly 500,000
children (Roberts, 2009). people had Alzheimer’s disease in 2000 and a sizable number
a conscience. Too much guilt, however, leads to exces- dren become aware of how their performance compares
sive inhibition in the child and trepidation about her own with that of others. The school and neighborhood value
actions. This illustrates a point that Erikson believed was high levels of achievement and reward industry. If goals
true of the conflicts in all of the stages. While one of the and tasks set up by their communities are appropriate,
contrasting traits is usually heavily favored, a healthy indi- children develop a sense of competence as they accom-
vidual has some degree of both traits. Without any ini- plish a task. A feeling of industry emerges. Alternatively,
tiative at all, a child might not be likely to try any new if the school and community ask too much of a child
behaviors or social situations. Without guilt, a child might or do not give the child a fair chance, the child may
be too impulsive, trying new behaviors without regard to develop a sense of inferiority and may see herself as a
how they affect other people. failure.
Stage 4: Industry versus Inferiority An elementary Stage 5: Identity versus Role Confusion Adolescents are
school child is confronted with tasks in which high lev- seen as striving to find their place in the world. If they suc-
els of achievement are valued. As relationships in the ceed, they develop a sense of their role in life and feel that
neighborhood and school increase in importance, chil- they have a future path that will lead them into adulthood.
licensed drivers
old. Indeed, in many prior elections, as many as 70 percent of 150
the people in a population who had severe dementia nonethe-
less voted (Henderson & Drachman, 2002; Vedantam, 2004). 100
Moreover, voting rates are highest among people in the 65- to
50
74-year-old age group (Karlawish et al., 2004).
Some legal scholars contrast driving, which they call a “privi- 0
lege,” with voting, which they call a “right.” But even with that
16
17
18
20 19
25 24
30 29
3 5 34
40 39
45 4
50 49
55 54
60 9
6 5 64
70 6 9
75 4
80 79
4
5
–4
–5
–7
–8
≥8
–
–
–
–
–
–
–
–
–
difference, it is not clear that everyone past age 18 should be
Driver’s age (in years)
voting regardless of their mental status. After all, “moral status”
prevents many felons from voting. Some state laws on voting do FIGURE 17.17 Collision rates by age. Older drivers, even
very old ones, are not nearly as likely to get into an accident as
set restrictions based on criminal records and mental incapacity,
younger drivers. Part of the reason lies in the fewer miles driven
but any limits on voting rights, especially of the sort that gradu- by the elderly. But even taking into account those fewer miles
ally increase with aging, are intensely controversial. One recent driven, people older than 85 years still have a lower accident rate
appeals court outlined the following principle: “A person has the than people in their early 20s.
capacity to vote if he or she understands the nature and effect
of voting and has the capacity to choose among the candidates
and questions on the ballot” (Karlawish et al., 2004). Yet, for the right to drive. Clearly, we need better ways of assessing
that principle to be implemented, there needs to be a standard- patterns of decline for each individual in his own right. More-
ized and fair way of assessing cognitive understanding of the over, a particular person’s pattern of decline may be relevant for
“nature and effect of voting” and “the capacity to choose.” restricting just one narrow range of rights, such as driving, and
Regardless of the details of this debate and how it turns out, not any others. It seems likely that psychologists, the people
one fact is likely to dominate. There is immense variation in pat- most qualified to make such assessments, will be very active
terns of decline among the elderly, and it would be impossible participants in determining when it is appropriate to reduce the
to set up any restrictions of rights solely on the basis of chrono- rights of the elderly (Baker et al., 1998; Ball et al., 1993, 2006).
logical age. For example, a 95-year-old might be vastly more One likely outcome is that quite different profiles of psychologi-
intellectually intact than a 75-year-old. While it might be best to cal capacity will be considered adequate for different sorts of
keep some elderly people from driving, others might actually be legal procedures and public rights, requiring particular tests for
more careful drivers (see Figure 17.17) and still deserve to have each sphere of life potentially affected by aging.
If they fail, they may reject the society at large and the roles Stage 7: Generativity versus Stagnation Generativity
that it offers. They may lack a sense of group identity or consists of making contributions to the world and to the
identify with a group that defines itself as rebellious and next generation. As people go through middle age, gen-
that is against any traditional life roles. erativity may take the form of having children and raising
them. Or it could take the form of making major contri-
Stage 6: Intimacy vs. Isolation In this stage of life, a criti- butions in a career, contributions that enhance the lives
cal task is that of forming close interpersonal bonds, typi- of others and that do not just advance the self. Society as
cally those of a married couple. Intimacy may be a major a whole benefits from the activities of a generative person.
challenge for many young adults, not just because of the A person who shows stagnation, however, tends to be overly
difficulties of forming stable close bonds, but also because concerned with her own selfish needs, making no contribu-
of the dangers of excess intimacy in the form of promiscu- tions to the community at large.
ity. At the other end of the extreme, failure to develop inti-
macy may result in a sense of isolation, leading to feelings of Stage 8: Integrity versus Despair The final stage is a
loneliness and perhaps bitterness toward those who do have potential capstone on a life of positive prior stages or the
intimacy. final act in a tragedy of a life gone wrong. People who achieve
96 99
00
early age.
9 7
1 8
4 8
5 8
6 8
7 8
8 8
9 8
0 9
1 9
2 9
4 9
5 9
6 9
7 9
8 9
8 9
9 9
17 1–1
FIGURE 17.18 Increase in average lifespan. As seen in this graph, developed countries, with the consequence that parents’
the average lifespan in Sweden more than doubled in 250 years. roles outside the home are correspondingly larger. Women
have entered the workforce in unprecedented numbers in
midlife. Moreover, with the average lifespan in some devel- many countries over the past few decades, and the role of
oping countries less than half that in developed countries, homemaker is no longer viewed as the only way to have
the age at which a person is considered old in Swaziland may a productive and meaningful life. There is every reason to
be considered middle-aged in Japan. believe that many equally dramatic changes will happen
Changing views of the roles we should occupy at different over the next few decades as well.
ages are often evident in how visual media portray people of One particularly interesting case of changing perceptions
different ages. Whether it be the cover of a nation’s leading of age-related roles concerns the debate about whether peo-
magazine about senior citizens (see Figure 17.19) or the depic- ple go through midlife crises. The notion of a midlife crisis
tions of 50- and 60-year-old characters in film and art, the has waxed and waned in recent years. It normally refers to
notion of “old” seems to have clearly shifted upward in age. a time when a person in his 40s, more often a male, goes
In addition, older adults are increasingly keeping physically through a period of dramatic self-questioning and reevalua-
fit and engaging in activities that were formerly restricted to tion, sometimes said to be brought on by a relatively sudden
younger adults. At age 70, Mick Jagger continues to perform awareness of an inevitable and not-too-distant future death
as a rock star to sold-out audiences in large stadiums. For- (Jacques, 1965). In reality, however, there is little reliable
mer president George H. W. Bush went skydiving on his 85th evidence that men or women regularly go through such a
birthday, and countless other men and women in their 70s crisis in their 40s more than in any other decade in their
and beyond are skiing, surfing, and doing many other activi- lives (Freund & Ritter, 2009; Lachman, 2004). There are
ties that only a few years ago were thought to be the domain of important transition points throughout adulthood, rang-
adolescents and young adults. Of course, many older adults do ing from the arrival of children, to having an empty nest,
have real limits as to what they can do, but it is very difficult to finding that a career path has reached an apogee lower
to make generalizations about an entire group of older adults. than had been expected, to dealing with retirement. Each of
The age when people marry has also changed consid- these, and many others, can precipitate a crisis of introspec-
erably in developed countries from a time when many tion and reevaluation, but no one decade seems to dominate
SUMMARY abilities) that remain the same or improve in middle and late
life.
Physical and Physiological Changes in Adulthood ● Speed of information processing, such as tasks involving
● Any account of the physical and physiological changes that simple reaction times, generally shows decline. At the same
occur in adulthood must address the ways in which those time, in local areas of high expertise in which tasks are richly
processes are changing over the course of history in light of structured (through chunking) and highly familiar (through
medical advances, lifestyle changes, and new cultural prac- many hours of practice), skill levels may continue to improve
tices. The distinction between successful aging and patho- for much of the lifespan.
● By selecting a smaller subset of tasks that fit well with suc-
logical aging illustrates many ways in which older individuals
can thrive far longer in life than was considered feasible even cessful aging, people can continue to practice and develop
a few decades ago, while others may suffer from irreversible an area of expertise throughout much of their life. People
debilitating diseases. can optimize their selected skills, and they can also learn to
● Pathological aging may also involve several diseases that compensate for losses in one cognitive skill by shifting to a
occur more frequently, but certainly not inevitably in the reliance on another that is less affected by aging.
● Memory declines with age, but not in a global manner. Some
elderly. These include Parkinson’s disease, Alzheimer’s dis-
ease, and other forms of dementia that can rise quite dra- aspects of memory—especially explicit recall of situations—
matically in incidence as some people progress through their show substantial declines, while others—for example, more
80s and 90s. implicit memories driven by frequency and familiarity—
● People can age at different rates and any account of changes stay more constant. Similarly, fluid intelligence—which
in adults must recognize the considerable variances that can involves reasoning and processing new information—often
occur and how they are related to successful and pathological shows declines, but crystallized intelligence—which involves
aging. Chronological age and biological age are increasingly familiar facts and knowledge—tends to remain strong in
distinct concepts. older adults.
● Today, middle age is generally thought to be roughly 40 ● Some aspects of memory, such as prospective memory,
to 60 years of age, and the term elderly typically refers to which is memory to do a task or obtain a goal, actually show
those 65 and older. These ages are quite a bit older than a improvement with age. But older adults may have more diffi-
few decades ago. In another few decades, the ages associated culty than younger adults with source monitoring, often not
with “middle age” and “elderly” are likely to be considerably knowing where they first learned something. This may be
older as well. because of the relative sparing of semantic memory (memory
● After the early 20s, there are multiple patterns of decline in of a fact) but the decline in episodic memory (memory of the
the human body. Muscle strength drops to an ever-increasing event in which a fact was learned).
● Patterns of reasoning show subtle shifts. In tasks that require
extent in the later years, bone density drops, and most physio-
logical organs show a reduction in their functional capacities. equal weighing of several factors or considering additional
Sensory receptors also tend to decline in sensitivity, especially but irrelevant information, older people can sometimes rea-
those of vision and hearing. son more accurately than younger adults. Older adults also
● Several hypotheses as to why we age are still under active can show better decision making in some emotion-laden
investigation, with no clear answer yet emerging. One view is situations.
that metabolic processes may leave by-products that decrease ● Circadian rhythms are cyclical surges in hormones and
the efficiency of proteins and metabolic structures. Another transmitters throughout the day that lead most middle-aged
view is that cells may be able to divide only a limited number and older adults to have optimal cognitive functioning in
of times. A third view is that DNA may slowly degrade over the morning hours and most younger adults to show optimal
time because of mutations. cognitive functioning later in the day.
● Activities, both cognitive and physical, can have a substantial
Cognitive Changes in Adulthood effect on the quality of life as we get older. Physical exercise
● While there are some declines in psychological capacities can greatly slow down many forms of age-related cognitive
that occur in older adults, there are also areas of psychologi- declines, especially those concerned with higher levels of cog-
cal performance (for example, some cognitive and emotional nition. Cognitive activity—for example, doing puzzles and
fter 17 chapters, it is helpful to remind students reasonable to hope that they may remember some larger-
659
4. How a better understanding of children helps create a better in their own right and to see their nobler goals and beliefs.
future. All of us, not just parents, live on in the generations If we can live our own lives in ways that are aligned with
that follow us. Our ideas, our values, and all our positive our children’s optimal futures and if we do our best to help
contributions can only be carried forth after our deaths them achieve those futures, all our good works can be carried
if those who come after us choose to carry them forward. forth long after we are gone. In this way, to better understand
After their parents are gone, most children will not simply children is to give much more meaning to our own lives.
live according to a preordained plan laid down by their par-
If some versions of these four insights resonate with
ents, and most did not do so when their parents were alive.
students who have worked through this text, they will
Moreover, as we have seen, parents do not create or sculpt
have learned a great deal not just about developmental
a child into a desired form. Instead, if things go well, they
psychology but also about its relevance to a much wider
work as mentors, coaches, and partners with children, help-
range of intellectual and social issues. On an almost
ing them develop in the most optimal way possible. To better
daily basis, I am still struck by new ways in which these
understand children is to know their likely trajectories and
insights influence my own interpretations of the world
to know how our own lives may be partially carried forth in
around me.
those trajectories. It is to appreciate children as individuals
660 EPILOGUE
Glossary
accommodation The process by which the child adapts her cognitive disorders include phobias, social anxiety disorder, generalized
and behavioral structures or schemes to more accurately fit with a anxiety disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder.
pattern in the environment. anxious attachment See insecure/resistant attachment.
action potential A wave of changing electrical potentials that travels aphasia A form of brain damage that causes problems in language
down the axon, which is a neuron’s main path of transmission. function. There are different kinds of aphasias that impair different
adaptation In Piaget’s sense, a process by which the child better fits aspects of language.
her schemes with new experiences in the environment. apoptosis Programmed cell death that occurs in the course of normal
affective blunting Showing a low level of expression of emotion, and neural development.
thought to be a developmental precursor of schizophrenia. aptitude-treatment interaction Relationship between the effectiveness
affordances The possibilities for action based on the properties of of a particular teaching method (treatment) and the learner’s specific
the objects or surfaces on which the action will be performed. strengths (aptitudes) and weaknesses; often known as “learner
The same object may have different affordances for different characteristics.”
organisms. assimilation The process by which the child incorporates new infor-
ageism A form of bias against elderly people that can result in unfair mation from the environment into his preexisting mental structures
discrimination against them in a manner analogous to cases of or schemes in a way that usually distorts the new information to fit
racism and sexism. with the preexisting structures.
alerting Arousing the attentional system through a cue that both astigmatism A visual condition that affects the focusing of light and
indicates that a target stimulus is about to occur and that gives results in stimuli at different orientations being perceived with
some information about the target. different levels of acuity.
allele A gene variant. attachment The strong and enduring emotional bond between
allocentric representation A representation of space that is objective a child and a significant other and the processes that create and
and not relative to the position of the viewer. maintain this long-lasting social relationship.
altruism Behavior in which an individual acts for the benefit of attachment complex The set of behaviors and mental states that, taken
another at his or her own personal expense. together, are responsible for setting up and maintaining attachment.
amenorrhea The absence of a menstrual period in women who attachment style A pattern of relating to significant others based on
are of menstrual age. This often occurs in those with anorexia expectations about how they will respond and affecting perceptions,
nervosa. emotions, thoughts, and behaviors in close relationships.
amygdala A brain structure that is located near the brain’s midline attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) A constellation
in the right and left temporal lobes and that is involved in the of problems resulting in difficulty staying on task, often
manifestation and regulation of emotions. accompanied by restless activity.
analogical reasoning A way of comparing things that, on the attentional deployment A method of emotional regulation that
surface, seem quite different in order to see deeper level similarities involves purposely shifting the focus of one’s thoughts in a way
between them. that makes a situation feel less emotionally charged.
animism According to Piaget, the tendency of young children to attentional schema A cognitive structure that is learned through
attribute psychological motivations to nonliving things. experience in response to a particular set of stimuli in a specific
anorexia nervosa An eating disorder characterized by an intentional kind of task. When embedded in such tasks, attentional schema
refusal to eat anything beyond minimal amounts of food as well as form a framework for organizing information and responses to that
by an excessive concern with being thin. information and for recognizing particular patterns of information
anorexic family A family in which a certain pattern of interaction is and filtering out irrelevant information.
thought to foster the emergence of anorexia. attraction effect An effect in judgment and reasoning in which
A-not-B error The tendency of infants to search for an object where people tend to change their estimates of the values of goods in the
it has previously been found and not where it was most recently face of additional, but irrelevant, information.
observed to be hidden. attribution theory A theory that encompasses the way that people
antisocial behavior Behavior that hurts or impairs another individual explain their own behavior and the behavior of others.
or a group. Such behavior can include both verbal and physical authoritarian parent A parent who adopts a stern, coercive mode of
aggression as well as cheating and lying. child rearing.
anxiety disorder A disorder in which anxiety is so strong and authoritative parent A parent who adopts a warm mode of child
persistent that it interferes with normal functioning. Anxiety rearing with clear limits and guidelines for the child’s behavior.
G-1
autism A disorder characterized by difficulty relating to others that high activity, or “mania,” in which the person often has inflated
generally appears before age 3. It is usually associated with a lack self-esteem, grandiose plans, increased talkativeness, and a
of empathy, problems with social responsiveness, language and diminished amount of sleep.
communication deficits, and resistance to changes in routine, and blended family A family consisting of the parents and all children
it often includes repeated stereotyped movements and a narrow from their current relationship as well as children from prior
focus of interest. relationships.
autobiographical memories Explicit memories of specific experiences blocking A phenomenon in classical conditioning in which pre-
in one’s own life. training with one conditioned stimulus can interfere with (block)
autonomous stage In Piaget’s theory of moral development, a stage the formation of an association with a subsequent conditioned
in which children think of rules as social conventions that are stimulus.
created by humans and can be changed by humans. bootstrapping Building new conceptual structures out of earlier ones
avoidant attachment See insecure/avoidant attachment. that serve as a framework for fostering conceptual change and growth.
axon Tubelike projection from the neuron along which the action brainstem A structure beneath the brain and above the spinal cord
potential travels and which ends in an axon terminal containing that is concerned with regulating heart rate, breathing, swallowing,
neurotransmitters that will be transmitted to another neuron. blood pressure, digestion, and other automatic nonvoluntary
processes in the body.
babbling Part of the sequence of prelinguistic vocalizations occurring branching direction Language structure in which the hierarchical
in the first year of life. Children go through increasingly complex embeddings of grammatical categories branch in a particular
patterns of babbling, starting from around 4 months onward. direction. Different languages have strong tendencies to be either
Deaf children show a comparable babbling with sign language. right branching or left branching.
baby sib study A study in which younger siblings of children who bulimia nervosa An eating disorder in which a person eats excessively
have a disorder are studied for signs of early precursors of the and rapidly and then follows those episodes of overeating with
disorder. If there is a substantial heritability to the disease, the some way of purging the food.
younger siblings are much more likely to develop the disorder than bullying Repeated and sustained verbal or physical threats or attacks
the general population of young children, making the odds of against a victim who is unable to defend himself or herself. Also
discovering early precursors higher. known as peer victimization.
base pair A pair of complementary bases (adenine with thymine;
guanine with cytosine) in DNA. canalization The process in which cells get committed to becoming
basic emotion An emotion, such as joy, sadness, disgust, surprise, certain cell types over the course of development.
fear, or anger, that is thought to be present in young infants and catastrophizing Assuming the worst possible outcome for future
that is one of the building blocks for the emergence of later more events, even when those outcomes are highly unlikely or
complex emotions; also referred to as a primary emotion. implausible.
basic level of categorization A level of categorization at which categorical perception The tendency to group stimuli that vary
category members maximally contrast with members of other along a physical continuum, such as light wavelength, in terms of
categories while also having high similarity among members of the categories, such as different colors.
same categories. catharsis hypothesis The hypothesis that experiencing aggression in
behavioral genetics The study of how genetic variation between a pretend setting (such as watching a violent television program)
individuals or populations in a species contributes to behavioral releases aggressive impulses so that such impulses are less likely to
variation. be released later. This hypothesis was popularized by Freud and
behavioral genomics The study of genetic variation in a species’ then extended by those trying to minimize the impact of television
genome and its effects at the molecular level and links between violence. With respect to viewing violence on television, this
those molecular effects and behavioral variations. hypothesis is, with only rare exceptions, false.
behaviorism The view that psychology is best understood as centration A mental operation, according to Piaget, that emerges in
describing the effects of external contingencies on behavior and the preoperational period and that causes the child to focus on
not by referring to unseen mental events and representations. one dimension (for example, height) while ignoring relevant other
bell curve A plot of the distribution of scores in a large population; dimensions (for example, width).
also known as a normal distribution curve. cephalocaudal development Development in which the more ante-
between-subjects design A design in which different but equivalent rior sections mature earlier than the posterior sections (cephalo =
groups are assessed in each experimental condition in a study. head, caudal = tail).
Big Five Relatively independent clusters of five personality traits— cerebellum A brain structure beneath the rear end of the cerebral
extroversion, neuroticism, openness to experience, agreeableness, cortex that is involved in coordination of voluntary movement and
and conscientiousness—that have repeatedly been shown to have integration of some sensory information with action.
strong genetic bases. cerebral cortex The region of the brain associated with high-level
bilateral symmetry Symmetry of structures around both sides of a thought and perception. It is commonly divided into four major
center line, such as right and left kidneys, arms, legs, eyes, and ears. regions on each side of the brain: the frontal, parietal, temporal,
binocular cue A cue to the perception of depth that arises from the and occipital lobes.
use of both eyes. cerebrum A complex of three structures in the brain, consisting of
binocular disparity See binocular parallax. the basal ganglia, the olfactory bulb, and the cerebral cortex.
binocular parallax A binocular cue to depth that arises from small characteristic feature In the study of word meaning, a feature that
disparities between the images that reach the two eyes; also called is typically associated with members of a category but that is not
binocular disparity essential to any members of the category.
bipolar disorder A distinct affective disorder that is characterized by child effect A genetically determined behavior in a child that evokes
swings between periods of lethargy and low affect and periods of a particular kind of behavioral response in the parent.
G-2 GLOSSARY
child-directed speech Speech by adults or older children that is concrete operational period A stage of cognitive development
directed toward younger children and that is altered in a wide occurring after the preoperational period and before the formal
variety of ways (for example, slower, louder, and higher-pitched operational period, roughly from 7 to 12 years. During this
speech that stresses the boundaries between words) that make it stage, children start to use mental operators such as identity and
different from speech directed toward adult peers; also known as reversibility to more flexibly understand the physical world and the
motherese and infant-directed speech. ways in which it can change.
chromosome An organized cluster of genes within the cell nucleus. conditioned response (CR) A response that becomes associated with
The chromosomes contain the cell’s DNA. a stimulus that normally does not elicit that response.
circadian rhythm Daily cycle of biological function and activity conditioned stimulus (CS) A stimulus that becomes associated with
based on the rise and fall of hormones and neurotransmitters in an an unconditioned stimulus (and its response) but that would not
organism over the course of a 24-hour period. normally elicit a response.
classical conditioning A form of learning in which the pairing of an conduct disorder A pattern of frequent and recurring antisocial
unconditioned stimulus with a conditioned stimulus results in a behaviors in children and adolescents, whereby the children
conditioned response. regularly violate the rights of others as well as violating social
classification In Piaget’s theory, the ability to think about groups of norms or rules.
things as forming classes defined by a single property or relation. confluence theory The theory that as family size grows, there is a
class-inclusion relations How different classes of things in a hierarchy corresponding drop in the overall intellectual climate in the family
relate to each other and how broad superordinate categories can because many conversations and interactions would be between
encompass narrower, subordinate categories. children rather than between adults and children.
clique A small group of peers who voluntarily spend a great deal of connectionism An approach to learning and representation that
time together and seem to actively exclude others. posits massively parallel processing of small elements, none of
codominant Referring to a case where two different alleles for a gene which by themselves may be explicitly represented. This approach
are both expressed phenotypically. draws heavily on statistical patterns of feature correlations in the
cognitive empathy A form of empathy that requires social cognitive environment.
skills and the ability to take another’s perspective and that involves conservation task Any of the tasks designed by Piaget in which
understanding and representing the emotional and mental points children are asked to judge whether certain physical properties of
of view of others. an object, such as its size, or aspects of an array, such as its number,
cognitive map A mental representation of the spatial layout, used to remain unchanged when the object or array is transformed along
infer distance, direction, and a way of navigating the environment. different dimensions.
cognitive reserve Psychological capacity that is available to support constraints on word meanings Constraints on the possible meanings
cognitive functioning in the face of age-associated declines or that can be assigned to a given word. These are thought to be
minor brain injuries. necessary to explain how word meanings are acquired because
cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) A therapy that is based on the they narrow down the enormous number of possible meanings to a
idea that a person’s problems stem from how he sees and thinks workable number.
about the world and that the way to change the behaviors is to constructivist approach An approach to moral development that
change the ways the person interprets and thinks about the world sees the child as constructing moral interpretations of a situa-
around him. tion by evaluating the situation, the actors, and the cultural
cohort effect An effect produced in children of a particular age context.
group based on their experiences in the culture that differ from the controversial child When sociometric status is measured, a child clas-
experiences of those who are younger or older. sified as high on both the like and the dislike scales. Controversial
collectivist culture A culture in which the rights of the group are children are noticed by everyone and are seen as sociable and as
deemed to be more important than the rights of the individual. leaders, but they are also more likely to be seen as aggressive and
compensation The process of mitigating the effects of aging on arrogant.
cognitive performance by introducing aids to cognition or altering convergence A binocular cue to depth that arises from the sensation
behavior. that occurs when the two eyes converge to focus on an object.
compensation A mental operation, according to Piaget, that emerges converging methods Using various approaches to study a prob-
in the concrete operational period and that enables the child to lem from different vantage points in order to arrive at a more
understand how a change in one dimension, such as height, might accurate, objective answer than any single method could produce
be compensated for by another change, such as in width, with a alone.
third dimension, such as quantity, remaining invariant. core domain A domain of knowledge and thought that is thought
compensation plasticity The capacity for an area of the brain that to have a privileged role in development, emerging early in
has been deprived of its normal inputs and processing routines to infancy and maintaining a strong influence throughout much of
have heightened functions in other abilities that compensate for development.
the loss. corpus callosum A central pathway made up of a large set of heavily
complex emotion An emotion, such as guilt or pride, that is thought myelinated nerve fibers that transfers information between the two
to emerge later in development and that may build on interactions hemispheres of the cerebral cortex.
of basic emotions and involve supporting cognitions. co-rumination The tendency for a social group to dwell on negative
complex reaction time The time taken to engage in a response to events in a mutually reinforcing manner.
various spatial and temporal configurations of stimuli, such as creativity The ability to come up with ideas or strategies that are
pressing a key when the number following a prior number is twice both novel and truly original as well as being useful or functional.
that prior number. critical period A period in development when a particular experience
conceptual self A view of the self that includes a sense of the roles may have a dramatic impact and during which learning a particular
that we occupy in broader cultural and social contexts. kind of information or skill is possible. Before or after such a
GLOSSARY G-3
period, experience barely seems to have an effect and learning does dishabituation The expression of interest in a new display or scene,
not occur. as shown by increased responses to the novel stimulus.
crossing-over A recombination of genetic material from the father disorganized attachment An attachment style marked by less
and the mother that occurs when part of one chromosome switches consistency across situations and characterized by combinations of
locations with the corresponding part of the other chromosome in both insecure and controlling behaviors. Infants showing this style
the pair. may have a higher incidence of psychopathology in later life.
cross-sectional approach An approach to development in which display rules Norms governing the kinds of emotional expressions
studies look at different groups of participants at different age a particular group considers appropriate in particular situations.
periods. dispositional factor A factor intrinsic to a person, such as a trait or
crystallized intelligence The ability to use specific skills and the person’s temperament.
knowledge gained through experience. dizygotic (DZ) twins Twins from two different zygotes; often
cueing hypothesis An explanation of infantile amnesia that is based referred to as fraternal twins.
on the idea that early memories are present but not available unless DNA Deoxyribonucleic acid, normally found in cell nuclei; contains
linked to the right sorts of cues. the molecular code in the form of sequences of chemical bases
cultural psychology A view of psychology in which cultural (adenine, thymine, guanine, and cytosine) for cellular construction
differences are believed to greatly influence psychological processes and growth.
and representations. This view argues against the existence of domain general Referring to a learning mechanism or representational
intrinsic universals of moral thought. format that applies across all domains of cognition.
culture of honor A cultural style, seen in the American South, domain specific Referring to a learning mechanism or representational
in which there is an endorsement of relatively higher levels of format that applies to a specific domain of cognition.
violence in response to threats to oneself, one’s property, or dominance hierarchy A network of relations between members
one’s background. (It does not endorse more violence in all of a social group, often primates. The structure of the network
situations.) often involves a single powerful individual at the top and groups
cyberbullying A form of aggression over the Internet, usually in of subordinates that get ever larger with decreasing social status.
which a group victimizes an individual. dominant Referring to an allele that is expressed in the phenotype,
cycle of abuse The pattern in which abused children grow up to be both when paired in a homozygous pair of alleles and when paired
abusing parents, whose abused children then grow up to abuse with a recessive allele in a heterozygous pair of alleles.
their children. dual representations Models that can be both physical objects in
their own right and also symbols for other objects.
declarative memory Memory that can be described as “knowing dynamic cue A cue to the perception of depth that arises from moving
that,” such as knowing a fact or that an event has occurred; objects and surfaces.
sometimes used synonymously with explicit memory. dynamic systems theory A theory of motor development that stresses
defining feature In the study of word meaning, a feature that is the importance of the physics of the limb and the environment
necessary to specifying meaning. in which an action is made in addition to central cortical control
delay of gratification The ability to hold off engaging in an action programs.
that will bring a desired reward. dyslexia A learning disability typically involving a deficit in phonological
delusion A false or highly unrealistic belief. Delusions are often processing despite normal intelligence, a good motivation to learn to
evidence of psychosis and frequently a symptom of schizophrenia. read, and ample early exposure to environments that normally foster
dendrite One of many signal-receiving branches extending from a reading.
nerve cell. Dendrites contain terminals that respond to specific
neurotransmitters from other neurons and conduct the information ecological self A sense of the self as moving through the world,
received toward the cell body. As many as 10,000 dendrites may be gained through perception and action.
present per neuron. ecological systems approach An approach to the study of families
dependency A reliance on another for basic physiological needs, such that emphasizes the various contexts in which children develop
as food and shelter and protection from harm. Attachment and and how children fit into those contexts.
dependency can be largely dissociated. ecological validity A quality of a study that captures all the critical
dependent variable The variable that one is measuring in a study to factors to understanding a behavior or ability in a natural context.
see how it varies as a function of different values of the independent ectoderm One of three cell layers at an early stage of development.
variables, which are manipulated as a way of exploring and Cells from this level become skin, hair, the brain, and the spinal
explaining the nature of the dependent variable. cord.
depressogenic vulnerability The convergence of the separate egg cell The female gamete that merges with the male sperm cell
cognitive, biological, and emotional factors to create depression. during fertilization.
desensitization A process in which experiencing continuous high ego One of three components of the mind described by Freud that
levels of violence comes to be accepted as normal and may lead normally emerges in development after the id and before the
to increased tolerance of aggressive behaviors as well as increased superego and that enables the child to channel and direct the id.
willingness to engage in aggressive acts. egocentric representation A representation of space that is relative
developmental intergroup theory A theory that children as young as to the position of the viewer.
4 years of age start to notice perceptually salient group differences egocentrism In cognitive development, a tendency to fail to take into
and to attach these to stereotypes. account the points of views of others.
diathesis-stress hypothesis The proposal that a genetic predisposition elective mutism A phenomenon, often found in autism, in which
for a mental disorder may combine with high levels of environmental a child who is capable of speaking almost never does so in social
stress to cause the disorder to become manifested. interactions.
G-4 GLOSSARY
embryo The developing organism between the middle of the second week the creation or elimination of synapses or the creation or death of
and the end of the eighth week of development. During the embryonic neurons.
period, rudimentary systems, structures, and organs first appear. experimental study A study in which a certain variable (the inde-
emergent constraint A constraint that emerges over the course of pendent variable) is manipulated while holding all other aspects of
development and that reflects the influences of experience and the the experimental situation constant to see how the manipulation
environment. affects the dependent variable.
emotion A transient state that corresponds to physiological and explicit cognition Knowledge or thought processes that are part of
cognitive processes associated with distinct internal sensations, or conscious awareness and that can often be described in words.
feelings. explicit memory Memory for information that involves conscious
emotional contagion A process in which the emotional states of recall.
others, such as laughter or sadness, become manifested in a person extended family A family whose members reach across generations
who is exposed to those states. and sometimes incorporates aunts, uncles, and cousins.
emotional empathy A form of empathy that involves directly extended self A sense of the self as traveling along an autobiographical
witnessing another person’s emotional state and feeling the same timeline.
emotion in a relatively immediate and direct way. externalizing problem A problems that is expressed externally by a
emotional intelligence The ability to recognize, think about, child, such as aggression or sexual promiscuity.
and manage our own emotional states and processes as well as
recognizing the emotional states of others. factor analysis A technique in data analysis that isolates relatively
emotional regulation The ways in which people of all ages influence independent clusters of information that vary along dimensions
the particular emotions they experience, when and how they known as factors.
experience them, and how they reveal these emotions. false-belief task A task that assesses the ability to understand how
empiricism A view of development that stresses the presence, at birth, others might have beliefs that are contrary to a true state of affairs
of a general, all-purpose learning system, with little or no biases to in the world.
acquire particular kinds of information. false-photograph task A task developed to investigate whether
encoding The process of turning information into a mentally useful children who fail false-belief tasks actually misunderstand beliefs
representational format. per se. The task does not involve beliefs, but young children still
endoderm One of three cell layers at an early stage of development. have trouble inhibiting a default (incorrect) response based on the
Cells from this level become glands and the linings of the lungs. objects that are in view when they are questioned.
environmental niche A particular kind of environment that fast mapping The phenomenon of rapidly learning the mapping
an organism lives in and for which the organism has evolved between words and the entities they refer to. While specifically
adaptations that enable it to thrive in that environment. proposed for word learning, it seems to be a broader phenomenon
epigenetic regulation The process in which changes in gene that also applies to other forms of mapping.
expression occur based on internal and external environmental feedback loop In developmental psychology, the constant interplay
signals that affect the DNA. between an infant and his environment, in which an experience
episodic memory Memory of specific events that one has experienced; causes a change in the infant’s state, which in turn influences how
a type of declarative memory. Autobiographical memories are a the infant’s next experiences are encountered.
kind of episodic memory. feral child A child who grows up in the wild and has no contact with
equilibration The process through which a child’s mental structures humans.
come into a better fit with various aspects of the environment fetus The developing organism after the embryonic period, when
through assimilation and accommodation. later prenatal change appears more qualitative. During the fetal
essentialism A tendency to assume that the surface properties of period, which occurs from week 9 until birth, organs, muscles,
entities are caused by deeper essences. structures, and systems grow larger and mature, developing into
ethology The study of traits (either body parts or behavior) from an recognizable forms.
adaptive evolutionary perspective that usually involves comparisons fixed-trait theorist An individual who tends to think that traits are
across species. intrinsic and unchangeable regardless of effort.
evaluative reasoning Thinking about an individual’s qualities and fluid intelligence The ability to think flexibly and solve novel
behaviors in terms of whether the person is entirely good or entirely problems. It supposedly does not depend significantly on acquired
bad and predicting his future behavior on that basis. (Compare knowledge.
trait-based reasoning.) Flynn effect The finding that intelligence test scores have been
evidence-based therapy Therapy for which efficacy has been shown consistently rising throughout the world for the past 60 years, a
by rigorous, carefully controlled studies that compare one therapy pattern that was masked by the changing the norms of the tests.
with another or with no therapy. formal operational period A stage of cognitive development
evolutionary psychology A view of psychology that emphasizes occurring after the concrete stage, at roughly 12 years old but with
evolution of a variety of psychological adaptations that increased considerable variability. In this stage, children start to engage in
the fitness of individuals. In regard to moral development, the deductive and logical reasoning about situations and processes
emphasis is on the universals of moral thought and behavior that and are able to consider logical alternatives and their potential
have been selected as adaptive in all humans. consequences.
executive functioning The collection of cognitive activities involved foundational constraint A constraint that is present from the start
in goal-directed tasks and problem solving, including inhibitory and is usually associated with nature accounts of learning and
control, error correction and shifting, and working memory. development.
experience-dependent plasticity The phenomenon of changes in friendship An ongoing, positive reciprocal relationship with a chosen
brain structure as a result of experience, usually referring to either companion.
GLOSSARY G-5
frontal lobe A lobe in the cerebral cortex involved in planning, habituation method A method to determine if an infant can
problem solving, and the regulation of thought and emotion. More distinguish a new stimulus from a familiar stimulus. An infant’s
posterior parts of the frontal lobe are also involved in aspects of responses (for example, looking time) will decrease with repeated
motor activity and the processing of grammar. presentation of a familiar stimulus, but they will increase if the
functional fixedness A bias to use an object or device in its traditional infant perceives that there is a new stimulus.
manner and not to come up with novel, more creative uses. Hayflick limit The view that human cells can only divide roughly
functionalist approach An approach to emotion that stresses the 50 times, putting an upper limit on the lifespan of an organism.
function of emotional responses—specifically, that emotions help heritability The extent to which variations in a trait within a
mobilize us to take action toward goals. particular population are due to genetic differences rather than
fundamental attribution error The tendency to explain the behavior environmental differences.
of others in terms of dispositional factors as opposed to situational heterochronic gene A gene that is related to the timing of
factors. development, often regulating when other genes turn on or off.
heteronomous stage In Piaget’s theory of moral development, a
gamete A specialized sex cell from either the male (sperm) or the female stage in which children think that rules are fixed and immutable
(egg) that combines with another sex cell during reproduction. realities that are part of the natural world.
gastrulation The process of cell differentiation into three primary heterozygous Having two different alleles for a particular gene.
germ layers: ectoderm, endoderm, and mesoderm. hippocampus Bilateral structures that are involved in developing new
gender identity A sense of whether we are male or female. memories and that bind information from different brain regions.
gender role The behaviors and traits that are considered typical of Bilateral damage may cause difficulty in consciously accessing new
each sex. memories.
gender schema A cognitive structure for interpreting gender-related holophrase A single word that stands for a larger phrase or full sentence
activities and roles. that is mentally present but that the child is unable to produce.
gene A section of genetic material with inherited instructions for homeobox A short stretch of 180 base pairs of DNA that occurs
creating a protein (or part of a protein) and regulating when the in the same precise sequence in many different species and that
protein will be active. encodes the proteins that regulate the formation of body regions
general intelligence, g A common factor that seems to be behind and structures.
performance on the vast majority of items on standardized homeobox genes Master-switch genes that contain the homeobox
psychometric tests of analytical intelligence. and that affect the body plan of many different species by encoding
generalized anxiety disorder A disorder in which there are high proteins and regulating gene expression.
levels of anxiety for a wide range of situations as opposed to the homozygous Having the same alleles for a particular gene.
specific fears in phobias. hostile attribution bias A tendency to interpret ambiguous situations
generativity The degree to which a language is able to generate a as hostile.
large number of novel patterns and structures from a small number Hox genes Homeobox genes in vertebrates. Hox genes encode proteins
of primitives and rules. and regulate gene expression and thereby affect the body plan of
genome The entire body of genetic information for an organism, vertebrates.
often used to describe the most typical profile for a species or hue The aspect of color that is a consequence of wavelengths of light.
subspecies. hypothetico-deductive reasoning The ability to think systematically
genotype The genetic makeup of a specific individual or a specific about different possibilities that might depart from the current
gene, encoded as alleles in the individual’s DNA. reality and to derive predictions given that certain information is
geometric information The information about an environment’s true.
overall shape (its contour) that helps to locate oneself in space.
Gestalt principles Holistic principles of perception that allow viewers id One of three components of the mind described by Freud that
to parse up the world into objects and other meaningful units. emerges first in development and represents an individual’s basic
glial cell A brain cell that provides nutrients, produces myelin, and desires and drives.
provides structural support. Glial cells are roughly 10 times as identity A mental operation, according to Piaget, that emerges
common as neurons. in the concrete operational period and that enables the child to
global change A type of development that occurs at roughly the realize that a single dimension or aspect remains the same despite
same time in very different areas of thought or behavior. dramatic changes in other dimensions.
goodness of fit The idea that infants with different temperaments idiographic approach With respect to creativity, studying case
may thrive in different environments that fit best with those histories of highly creative individuals to see whether they seem to
particular temperaments. cluster into different types.
grammar The structural relations of a language that govern how imaginary companion A fantasized character that a child makes up
words and other syntactic units are organized. or an object that the child treats as an animate creature.
grasp reflex A reflex found in newborns in which they will grasp an immanent justice In Piaget’s theory of moral development, a form
object when it is pressed against their palm. of reasoning about justice in which the child believes that most
growth cone The end of an axon that is migrating through neural events that adversely affect an individual who has previously
tissue to its eventual destination. It guides reactions toward and done something wrong are forms of punishment for the earlier
away from chemical gradients until it arrives at its destination, transgression.
where it forms a synapse with a target neuron. impersonal moral situation A moral situation in which the
individuals involved are not salient and in which it is possible to
habituation The gradual decline of interest in a display or scene, as adopt a moral stance that appeals to abstract moral principles and
shown by a decrease in responses to a repeated stimulus. patterns of reasoning.
G-6 GLOSSARY
implicit cognition Knowledge that exists outside of awareness and joint attention The situation in which two people are jointly
that may be difficult to describe in words. attending to an object and are aware that they are both doing so.
implicit memory Memory for information or actions that occurs
outside of conscious awareness. kinesthetic self-concept A concept of the self that arises from an
imprinting A form of learning that takes place during a critical period awareness of our motor actions as we moves in an environment.
in which young members of a species acquire a behavior or response
pattern that remains permanent after the critical period is over. landmark A large object in the environment that enables an
incremental theorist Someone who views traits as changeable organism to navigate by estimating distance and direction from
through effort. the landmark.
independent variable A variable that one manipulates so as to language acquisition device (LAD) A hypothesized mental system
understand patterns of variation in the dependent variable. in humans that is specialized for the acquisition of language and
indiscriminate attachment An attachment style in which infants not other kinds of knowledge.
are as affectionate and receptive to complete strangers as they are latency period A time in middle to late childhood, posited by Freud,
to their primary caregivers. when children tend to practice strict segregation of the sexes in
individualist culture A culture in which the rights of the individual terms of preferred playmates and friends.
are deemed to be more important than the rights of the group. learned helplessness A learned state in which an organism concludes
infantile amnesia The inability to recall any memories prior to a that escape from an aversive situation is impossible and that it is
certain age, usually around 2½ years of age. helpless and cannot change things for the better.
information integration theory A theory of cognitive development lexicon The set of words that a person knows; often referred to as a
that says that older children integrate different dimensions or com- person’s vocabulary.
ponents of a task into a coherent system for successful reasoning, linguistic determinism The view, first attributed to Whorf and
whereas younger children focus excessively on one dimension or Sapir, that the language that one learns determines the nature of
component of the task at the expense of others. one’s thoughts.
inimical relationship A relationships characterized by conflict, linguistic relativity The view, first attributed to Whorf and Sapir, that
dislike, and aggression in which each person considers the other patterns of thought vary as a function of the language one learns.
to be an enemy. local change A specific psychological capacity that develops relatively
insecure/avoidant attachment An attachment style in which infants independently of other specific capacities.
not only avoid or fail to initiate interactions with the mother after longitudinal approach An approach to development in which studies
a reunion, but also show less visible anxiety in the absence of the look at the same group of participants at different age periods.
mother. long-term memory Memory that endures for extended periods of
insecure/resistant attachment An attachment style in which infants time. Information in long-term memory has often been processed,
show elevated anxiety in the absence of the mother and often try to organized in terms of meaning, and stored for later retrieval.
vigorously prevent her from leaving. They may also show distress
and anger when the mother returns while nonetheless clinging to Machiavellian emotion An emotional expression that is used to
her. In the presence of a mother and a stranger, they may explore produce an effect in others and that does not necessarily reflect the
minimally and be quite clingy. Also known as anxious attachment. emotion actually felt by the agent expressing it.
instrumental conditioning See operant conditioning. maturational account An account of development in which
instrumental helping Prosocial behavior to help someone else achieve developmental change is largely attributed to biological maturation
a goal, even though there is no obvious benefit to the helper. as opposed to experience.
intelligence quotient (IQ ) Originally an index of intelligence meiosis The process in which certain types of cells divide and produce
based on the ratio of a person’s mental age to his chronological cells with single sets of chromosomes. The correct chromosome
age. IQ is now calculated in a different manner and simply refers number is once again restored during sexual reproduction.
to where a specific score stands on a distribution of intelligence memory format change hypothesis An explanation of infantile
test scores. amnesia that is based on the idea that infants and very young
intermodal perception The ability to see objects and events in an children represent the world in a format that is radically different
integrated manner across different sensory systems, such as vision from later formats and is therefore inaccessible at later ages.
and hearing. mesoderm One of three cell layers at an early stage of development.
internal working model A mental representation of the self and Cells from this level become muscles, organs, and skeleton.
others and how they might interact in different circumstances. meta-analysis A technique of data analysis in which data from
internalizing problem A problem that is largely within the individual, multiple prior studies are gathered into a large-scale study and the
such as depression. data are statistically analyzed to determine which treatments are
interpersonal intelligence The ability to have empathy and to think most effective.
about factors governing interactions between people; a form of metacognition Knowledge about the nature of one’s own mind and
emotional intelligence. thoughts and those of others.
interpersonal self A sense of the self as engaged in social interactions metamemory A form of metacognition involving knowledge about
with others. what memory is, how it works, and what its limits are.
intrapersonal intelligence The ability to think about factors methylation A process in which methyl groups become attached to
governing our own mental, social, and emotional states; a form of certain DNA base pairs, usually with the result of turning off genes
emotional intelligence. that follow them in a regulatory sequence.
invisible displacement The movement of an object to a new hiding microgenetic analysis A study in which the researcher monitors the
spot when it is covered by another object and therefore not directly process of development by assessing participants every few days or
visible. weeks rather than every few years.
GLOSSARY G-7
migration The process by which cells in a developing organism move negativity bias A bias to pay more attention to and/or weigh more
to their eventual permanent destinations. heavily negative rather than positive information.
mirror neuron A neuron in the brain that is activated both when neglected child A child who does not appear to be high on either
performing an action and when perceiving it being performed by the like or dislike scale of an assessment of sociometric status.
others and that is thought to support imitation. Neglected children are often shy and withdrawn, and they are
mirror test A test of someone’s sense of self in which a mark is put largely ignored by their peers.
on a subject such that it can only be seen when the subject views neural change hypothesis An explanation of infantile amnesia
himself in a mirror. based on the idea that early memories do not become permanent
mitosis The normal process of cell division in which genetic material because of intrinsic immaturities of the child’s brain that make it
is conserved. initially unable to store information for the long term.
modeling theory An approach to learning and socialization based neural tube The round, enclosed structure that is usually formed in
on the idea that children and adults reproduce behaviors that they the first few weeks of life and that encases the spinal cord and the
observe others performing; also called social learning theory. brain.
module In Fodor’s sense, a functionally specialized cognitive system neurogenesis The making of new neurons, usually prenatally, in
that is tailored for processing certain forms of information in ways humans.
that are not influenced by other cognitive systems. neuron A brain cell containing a cell body, axon, and dendrites; also
monozygotic (MZ) twins Twins from the same zygote; often referred known as a nerve cell. Brain cells communicate with other cells
to as identical twins. both within the brain and from other parts of the body.
moral dilemma Devised by Kohlberg to study moral reasoning, an neurotransmitter A chemical that mediates transmission of impulses
ambiguous situation in which a person in a story must make a across synapses.
difficult moral decision and the participant in the study must say neurulation The formation of the neural tube through closing of the
which choice is better and why. The pattern of reasoning used by neural plate.
the participant is more important than the actual choice selected. nomothetic approach With respect to creativity, comparing the lives
moral dumbfounding The finding that in some moral situations, of especially creative individuals to look for common patterns or
people have strong convictions about what is right and wrong but principles.
are unable to explain why. normal distribution curve A plot of the distribution of scores in a
moral modules Forms of moral reasoning that have evolved to deal large population; also known as a bell curve.
with specific kinds of moral problems encountered by individuals nuclear family A family typically consisting of the mother and father
and groups. as heads of the household, along with their children.
Moro reflex A reflex found in young infants in which an abrupt lack
of support of the head results in an outward and upward movement object concept A concept of the behavior of a bounded physical object,
of the arms. which is generally studied under certain conditions of change, such
morpheme The smallest unit of language that represents a discrete as when it disappears from view.
meaning. Morphemes are not only words but also parts of words object permanence The property that objects continue to exist even
that carry meaning, such as plural endings in English. when they are out of sight.
motherese See child-directed speech. object solidity An object’s property of cohering and taking up space,
motion parallax A cue to depth that arises from the relative motion preventing other objects from passing through it.
of objects at different distances. observational study A study in which the researcher simply observes
motor deprivation A condition under which an organism is either participants acting in various contexts.
prevented from moving or not given the opportunity to pair obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) A disorder involving a
movement with perception or objects acted upon. high level of anxiety about contamination, order, loss of objects,
motor empathy A form of empathy in which a person tends to or aggressive or sexual thoughts and characterized by persistent,
automatically copy the motor actions, postures, and facial intrusive thoughts and highly repetitive checking behaviors.
expressions of another. occipital lobe A lobe in the cerebral cortex that has structures
multiple intelligences The theory that intelligence is divided into primarily concerned with processing visual information at various
eight distinct modalities that often have strong sensory or motor levels of increasing complexity.
links. operant conditioning A form of learning in which desired behaviors
mutual antipathy A pattern found in both bully-victim relationships are gradually approximated in an organism through schedules of
and inimical relationships in which two children intensely dislike positive and negative reinforcement for increasingly accurate versions
each other. of the desired behavior; also called instrumental conditioning.
mutual exclusivity A constraint on word meanings based on the operator A formal mental tool by which people mentally represent
assumption that entities do not tend to have more than one label. states and changes in the world.
myelin A fatty substance that wraps around the axons of neurons and optimization The continued perfection of a skill through hours and
speeds up the transmission of action potentials. hours of diligent practice; often a way in which older adults can do
myelination The process by which the axons of neurons are enclosed better than younger adults, who are faster on simple reaction times
in a myelin sheath. for all components of the skill.
optokinetic nystagmus method A technique used to measure
nativism A view of development that stresses the existence, at birth, of acuity, in which increasingly thin arrays of stripes are moved across
a set of different learning systems, each biased to acquire particular the infant’s visual field and the infant’s spontaneous eye tracking of
kinds of information better than others. the stripes is observed.
natural language A language that can be learned and used easily and orienting Drawing attention to a particular area so that a person can
relatively effortlessly by young humans. recognize when a stimulus occurs.
G-8 GLOSSARY
overextension A phenomenon in semantic development in which the phonology The sound patterns of a language and the rules that
child uses a word to apply to a larger set of entities than is normal govern them.
in adult usage. pictorial cue A cue to the perception of depth that arises from
overjustification effect A drop in spontaneous behavior due to information in two-dimensional pictures.
reduced motivation, produced when we perceive the behavior as place error Another name for the A-not-B error.
having been done for an external award. placenta The organ that is attached to the mother’s uterus and
overregularization The excessive use of a rule so that it applies to connected to the developing fetus by the umbilical cord and
more cases than it actually should. through which nutrients and oxygen are transferred from the
oxytocin A hormone that leads to increased levels of trust and mother and wastes and carbon dioxide from the fetus.
nurturance to members of one’s group. plasticity Flexible abilities that enable a developing organism to be
responsive to changing environmental contingencies.
parenting style The behaviors and attitudes that parents have platoon school A model of education promoted by William
toward their children, which together create a particular parenting Wirt early in the twentieth century that stressed efficiency and
environment or climate. envisioned modeling student instruction in a manner analogous to
parietal lobe A lobe in the cerebral cortex that has several functions, the new technology of assembly lines.
including processing and interpreting touch sensations, integrating pleiotropic Referring to a single gene that affects many traits.
visual and spatial information, and processing some aspects of the polygenic Referring to a trait that is affected by many genes at the
semantic properties of language. same time.
patellar reflex An early-emerging reflex, also known as the “knee popular child When sociometric status is measured, a child who is
jerk reflex,” involving the extension of the lower leg when the nominated as being liked by most children and disliked by few
tendon at the kneecap (the patella) is abruptly pressed. children, often as a result of both appealing physical attributes
pathological aging A pattern of aging that reflects the effects of and social skills. Sociometric popularity contrasts with perceived
other physiological or mental diseases or of extreme environmental popularity, which may be more related to perceived dominance
influences. and power than to kindness and trustworthiness.
peer nomination A process of measuring sociometric status or positivity bias A tendency for older adults to process information in
popularity by asking peers to name other children who they like a more positive manner than younger adults.
the most and the least or want to be friends with the most and the poverty of the stimulus argument The idea that the linguistic
least. Peer nominations are often asymmetrical. information that a child hears is so impoverished that, on its own,
peer victimization See bullying. it is inadequate to explain the ability to learn a language.
perceptual narrowing A process in which infants increase their pragmatics The ways in which language is used to effect successful
processing ability and precision in a narrow category, but often at communication in social contexts, conveying intended meanings
the expense of processing outside that category. such as inferred goals and motivations.
perceptual reasoning index A subcomponent of the WISC-IV that preferential looking method A method to determine whether a
uses pictures and other visual materials and largely avoids using subject can detect a difference between two types of displays,
language to assess intelligence. It includes items testing the ability implied when the subject looks at one display more than the
to (1) complete missing details in pictures, (2) arrange pictures other.
to form a reasonable sequence of events, and (3) use blocks to prefrontal cortex The front-most region of the frontal lobes, which
re-create abstract designs. is often heavily involved in many aspects of memory, including
perceptual-motor development Development that proceeds memory storage and retrieval and the use of various memory
through an integration of action with perceptual feedback. While strategies.
normally discussed with respect to vision, it can also apply to other prelinguistic vocalization Any of a number of sounds made by the
modalities. infant prior to the clear use of language. Vocalizations include
permissive parent A parent who adopts a style of child rearing that is babbling but can also include other earlier sounds, such as cooing
warm but sets few if any guidelines for the child’s behavior. and squealing.
personal moral situation A moral situation in which individuals are preoperational period A stage of cognitive development occurring
depicted in personal ways that invoke strong, immediate judgments after the sensorimotor period, at roughly 2 to 7 years. In this stage,
that often bypass more abstract and reflective moral reasoning. children are able to represent the world in symbolic terms, but
pervasive developmental disorder A disorder in which there is a broad have difficulty thinking nonegocentrically or considering different
spectrum of problems usually associated with delays in socialization dimensions of situations and the relations between them because
and communication. The term is often used to describe individuals they are missing mental operators.
with symptoms similar to those found in autism. prepared Having a tendency to associate a class of responses more
phenotype The physical manifestation of a gene sequence, either with easily with one class of stimuli than with another without having
respect to one trait or with respect to the entire organism, including prior experience with those stimuli.
its anatomical structures, biochemical processes, and behaviors. prereaching Mostly failed attempts to touch objects by very young
phobia An extreme, often irrational fear of a specific thing or situation. infants.
phoneme The smallest unit of a language that makes a difference in preterm infant An infant born 3 or more weeks early; also known as
word or morpheme meaning. a premature infant.
phonics approach An approach to reading that emphasizes the primacy effect When memorizing a list of items, the tendency to
sounds of letters and letter patterns and how they can be combined better recall items near the beginning of the list relative to items
into larger units. in the middle.
phonological awareness An understanding of the sound units and primary circular reaction The discovery by chance of new ways of
sequences in spoken language. acting on one’s own body.
GLOSSARY G-9
primitive streak A ridge of tissue that emerges early in prenatal radial glial cell A special glial cell that acts as a guide for the
development and that forms the basis for an environment that migration of nerve cells in areas such as the cerebral cortex.
induces changes in cells that pass through the streak to become the reactive aggression Aggression that is engaged in as retaliation for
mesoderm and endoderm. aggression against oneself.
principles and parameters approach An approach advocated by recasting Repeating back a child’s utterance to him or her, but with
Noam Chomsky that says that there are a common set of principles expansions of the sentence into a larger, grammatically correct form,
that govern the internal structure of all languages and that, early in or repeating an ungrammatical sentence back in grammatical form.
life, the child’s mind contains a large number of possible linguistic recency effect When memorizing a list of items, the tendency to
forms or alternative parameters that will later become fixed based better recall items near the end of the list relative to items in the
on the specific language the child learns. middle.
private self A sense that we have experiences that no one else has, recessive Referring to an allele that is not expressed in the phenotype
such as dreams, points of view, and thoughts. when paired with a dominant allele; if the alleles in the pair are
proactive aggression Aggression that is engaged in against another recessive homozygous, however, their properties will be expressed.
person without provocation and usually as a way of achieving a referential communications task A task in which a speaker tries to
goal, such as obtaining material goods or causing intimidation or communicate to a listener, who cannot see the speaker, a series of
suffering in a victim. operations that involve reference to a set of materials that both the
procedural memory Memory of how to perform certain actions or speaker and the listener have in front of them.
procedures; “knowing how” memory. Many procedural memories, reflex An innate response that occurs automatically to a particular
but not all, are also implicit memories. form of stimulation.
processing speed index A subcomponent of the WISC-IV that regulatory cascade A series of interactions consisting of sets of genes
measures how quickly a child seems to process information in that turn other genes on or off. When one gene can turn on other
tasks ranging from time spent searching for a target shape among genes that turn on still other genes, a cascade is said to occur.
a much larger set of shapes to tasks identifying all instances of a rehearsal A memory strategy consisting of repeating the information
target image—for example, an animal or a specific number—in a to be remembered.
very cluttered field of diverse objects. rejected child A child who is nominated as being disliked by most
production deficit An apparent inability to spontaneously use a children and liked by few children on an assessment of sociometric
strategy that would clearly improve performance on a memory status. Rejected children often interpret ambiguous situations in a
task. hostile way.
prosocial behavior Behavior that benefits other individuals, groups, relational aggression Aggression that is intended to damage a social
or the society as a whole, often at a cost to the person who engages relationship between a victim and other individuals. Gossip,
in the behavior. rumor, and ridicule are often the weapons used to damage another
prosody The acoustic properties of speech, such as intonation, person’s reputation and that person’s relationship with others.
rhythm, and pitch. reliability The capability of a research design to achieve the same
prospective memory Remembering to do a task or attain a goal in results when repeated with the same kinds of participants.
the future. replicability The ability for others to easily reproduce a study and
psychobiomarker A biological feature, such as telomere length, that find the same results.
can be used as an index or marker of a person’s psychological state, resilience The ability of some children to have very positive
indicating, for example, a recent history of high levels of stress. behavioral and psychological outcomes, despite growing up in an
psychometric approach An approach to the study of intelligence environment that normally is associated with a high incidence of
based on the scores on standardized tests administered to large behavioral and psychological problems.
groups of people. These tests tend to focus on analytical forms of resistant attachment See insecure/resistant attachment.
intelligence. resource theory The idea that the more children there are in a family,
psychopathy A disorder in which a person seems to lack empathy or the fewer resources there are for each child.
a moral sense to guide his or her actions and manipulates victims response modification A form of emotional regulation that involves
through lies, deception, and distortions. managing an emotional reaction by directly influencing the
psychosexual development A series of stages of development response itself or engaging in an activity that indirectly reduces
proposed by Freud and related to drives, instincts, and sources of anxiety or increases arousal.
pleasure, with a particular focus on sexual desire. retrieval The process of finding a memory in storage and bringing it
psychosis A loss of contact with reality, as often shown by to mind for use, usually in a conscious manner.
hallucinations and delusions in a person with schizophrenia. reversibility A mental operation, according to Piaget, that emerges in
psychosocial conflict According to Erik Erikson, a conflict that the concrete operational period and that enables the child to realize
characterizes each stage of psychosocial development and that that certain transformations on the physical world are reversible.
must be successfully resolved to form a good foundation for the riddle of reference The dilemma of knowing whether a word refers
next stage and its challenges. to a whole object, a set of its parts, or an event or action associated
with the object.
qualitative change A stagelike form of developmental change in romantic relationship A mutually recognized voluntary interaction
which a new kind of structure or process emerges that was not that is seen as more intense than other relationships and that is also
present before the change. distinguished by displays of affection, either with actual sexual
quantitative change A developmental change in which the same behavior or the anticipation of such behavior occurring.
structures and processes remain but change in magnitude—for rooting reflex A reflex found in newborns in which a gentle touch on
example, an incremental expansion to a mental process rather than the cheek results in movement of the face so as to bring the mouth
a totally new kind of mental process. around the touching object.
G-10 GLOSSARY
rumination A form of cognition in which the individual dwells sense of self A composite of the various ways an individual thinks
on negative events in ways that may help maintain a depressive about himself and has distinct kinds of self-awareness.
state. sensitive period A period in development when learning a particular
kind of information is much easier. Before or after such a period,
sampling bias A problem in research in which participants are learning is more difficult.
selected in a nonrandom manner that suggests a relationship that sensorimotor period According to Piaget, a period of development
might not really be present in a truly random sample. from birth to 2 years of age, when infants largely understand
Sapir-Whorf hypothesis The idea that specific languages powerfully the world in terms of the way they perceive and act on it and
mold thought, affecting how individuals perceive and understand understand objects primarily as embedded parts of perceptual and
the world. motor activities.
scaffolding A process through which adults and older children sensory memory A brief memory for sensory information, such as
structure the young child’s environment and social milieu in a visual images, sounds, odors, tastes, and touch.
manner that enables the child to move to new levels of cognitive separation anxiety See separation distress.
achievement. separation distress The distress felt by an infant when a specific
scheme A complex of mental structures, processes, and actions that individual with whom the infant has a specific bond leaves. The
are used to interact with the environment. infant wants that particular person—not just any social partner—
schizophrenia A cluster of disorders in which there may be to come back. Also known as separation anxiety.
hallucinations and delusions, disorganized thoughts, chaotic speech serial position curve A record of recall of a list of serially presented
and behavior, and difficulties responding and communicating items, showing different rates of recall as a function of where the
with others. items appear in the list. In adults, the curve usually has a U shape,
scientific reasoning The ability to develop and test hypotheses about with greater recall near the beginning and the end of the list.
some aspect of the world and then efficiently test those hypotheses seriation The ability to order objects according to a shared property,
with relevant data. such as length. This ability is often facilitated by transitive reasoning.
secondary circular reaction A scheme that is applied directly to sexual diffentiation A process in which differences between females
external objects (beyond the infant’s own body) and modified and males are initiated by the X and Y sex chromosomes, which
accordingly. trigger and regulate sex hormones that affect male or female
secure attachment An attachment style in which infants show some characteristics. These differences appear in major anatomical
distress when the mother leaves, but not an excessive amount, and structures, in physiological systems, and in the brain.
show positive affect when she returns. They also show substantial shape bias A putative constraint on word meaning in which labels,
exploratory behavior in the presence of the mother, using her as a when applied to objects, are assumed to apply to other objects of
secure base. the same shape.
secure base The role that a caregiver may play with respect to a shaping The gradual modification of a behavior through operant
securely attached infant, who uses the caregiver as a base from conditioning, by gradually rewarding behaviors as they come
which to explore. closer and closer to resembling a desired behavior.
security motivational system A system that has been selected for sibling differentiation A phenomenon in which siblings strive to
by evolution and that is meant to increase vigilance for future occupy contrasting social and skill niches as a way of developing
threats to one’s well-being, including clues to predators, toxins, their own distinctive identities in a family. This is in contrast to the
and harmful changes in normal routines. case where younger siblings imitate older ones.
self-concept A conscious sense of the self that involves both facts simple reaction time The time it takes to engage in a motor response
about the self and evaluative beliefs about the self. for a stimulus, such as pressing a key whenever a particular stimulus
self-conscious emotion An emotion that requires some degree of appears.
self-awareness and an evaluation of the self as distinct from others situation modification A method of emotional regulation that
and in relation to others. involves changing the circumstances that bring about an emotional
self-determination A sense of having a choice in decisions about our reaction.
life and daily activities, of feeling that we have had an important situation selection A method of emotional regulation that involves
role in determining our behaviors. choosing one’s circumstances based, in part, on the kind of
self-efficacy Our beliefs about our ability to achieve certain goals, emotional reaction they elicit.
often tied to a particular set of circumstances. situational factor A factor that arises from external circumstances
self-esteem The extent to which we value our own worth and the and influences behavior.
emotions that accompany such an assessment. social aggression A kind of aggression that includes such behaviors as
self-regulation The ability to regulate our own emotional states malicious teasing, social exclusion from a group, and humiliation.
and reactions to situations and others. This may also include the social anxiety disorder A disorder in which the individual feels
ability to understand and evaluate our emotional states and the intense anxiety in social situations, often concerning how he or she
consequences of those states for ourselves and others. is viewed by others.
semantic development The process of learning word meanings and social comparison The act of comparing ourselves to another as a way
their interrelationships by linking words to concepts and then of calibrating our standing in a group or relative to another group.
applying those concepts to objects. Such comparisons emerge strongly in the elementary school years.
semantic memory Memory for episode-independent facts, such as social convention A rule of human interaction that is jointly decided
the capital of a country or the meanings of words; a component of upon as a means of achieving some sort of coordinated activity in
declarative memory. which both parties will benefit. Such rules are usually arbitrary,
semantics The ways in which words and the relations between them such as driving on a particular side of the road, but they must be
yield meaning. honored by everyone once they are established.
GLOSSARY G-11
social information processing An approach to encoding, interpret- superego One of three components of mind described by Freud; it
ing, and responding to social cues that is often used to explain peer represents the emergence of a conscience, an internal sense of right
group acceptance or rejection. and wrong.
social intelligence The ability to manage our interactions with others. synapse The gap between two nerve cells where a signal can be
social intuitionist model A model of moral judgment in which transmitted by a neurotransmitter.
perceived violations of norms and rights automatically trigger synaptic pruning The process by which extra synaptic connections
emotions like anger or contempt without consideration of abstract are reduced in normal development.
moral principles. synaptogenesis The process by which new synapses are created.
social learning theory The idea that people tend to copy behavior syntax The ordering relations in a language that yield what is
modeled by others, especially those they like and respect; also normally thought of as a grammar.
called modeling theory.
social referencing The use of the emotional and social states of tabula rasa A “blank writing tablet” or “blank slate,” used as a meta-
others as information on how to interpret a situation. phor by empiricists to describe the newborn’s mind as completely
sociometric status A person’s social standing in a group, often malleable by experience; originally used in a Latin translation of
computed from a combination of ratings of how much the person Aristotle, but attributed to John Locke, who actually used the
is liked and disliked by peers in that group. metaphor “white paper.”
source monitoring The ability to keep track of the sources of our telegraphic speech Speech by young children that is severely
memories, such as whether we experienced them directly, learned truncated by dropping out nonessential elements such as articles,
them from someone else, or acquired them through a dream. prepositions, and pronouns.
specific bond A bond to a particular individual, usually formed at telomerase An enzyme that re-extends telomeres and which may
about 6 to 7 months. Also called specific attachment. be used by cancer cells to keep dividing in ways that exceed the
specific language impairment (SLI) A language impairment in Hayflick limit.
which there are highly specific problems with particular syntactic telomere An ending on a chromosome that is thought to shorten
structures, such as noun-verb agreement and the use of articles and with each cell division and to be the reason for the Hayflick limit.
prepositions. temperament Early emerging behavioral characteristics that
sperm cell The male gamete that combines with a female gamete distinguish one infant or young child from another, typically
(egg cell) during fertilization. understood as precursors to later emerging personality traits.
stages of development Relatively discontinuous changes in thinking temporal lobe A lobe in the cerebral cortex with structures that
and behaving, resulting in new kinds of accomplishments and process auditory information and other aspects of language
patterns of thought that were not present before that stage. meaning and that support crucial components of memory.
standard deviation An indication of how much variation there is teratogen An internal or external hazardous environmental factor
from a mean score—that is, how widely scores are distributed that can potentially cause problems during the prenatal period.
around the mean. One standard deviation encompasses roughly Teratogens include drugs, diseases, malnutrition, and environmen-
68 percent of all scores, two standard deviations encompasses tal toxins.
roughly 95 percent of all scores, and three standard deviations tertiary circular reaction A scheme used intentionally by infants
encompasses roughly 99.7 percent of all scores. to actively explore the world. With such reactions, infants start
statistical learning Learning based on the probability of events co- to recognize objects as entities in their own right, independent of
occurring or occurring in certain orders. specific schemes.
stem cell An undifferentiated cell that can become a cell of any type. theory of mind A well-developed set of untaught, intuitive beliefs about
stepping reflex A newborn reflex of engaging in repetitive stepping others’ mental states and processes and an understanding of how to
motion when held upright. use those beliefs to infer others’ goals and explain their actions.
stereopsis The fusion of an image that is seen with both eyes, a fusion theory of mind module (TOMM) A specialized, autonomous system
that uses binocular disparity to compute depth and give a sense of dedicated to processing the beliefs and mental state processes in
three-dimensionality. others.
stereotype threat The perception that an activity, usually some trait-based reasoning Thinking about an individual’s qualities and
sort of performance, such as taking a test, is governed by a behaviors in ways that focus on specific traits. (Compare evaluative
negative stereotype about the group to which one belongs. Fear of reasoning.)
confirming that stereotype then reduces task performance. transitive reasoning A way of reasoning about known relationships
still face An impassive, straight face shown by a caregiver in the still- to make logical inferences about other unknown or less direct
face task, in which the caregiver abruptly stops interacting with an relations (for example, if A is taller than B, and B is taller than C,
infant and breaks the normal cycle of social interaction by showing then A must be taller than C). Some theories have argued that
unresponsive facial expressions. young children are unable to reason transitively.
storage The process of preserving information in memory over time. trisomy 21 A defect resulting from an error in meiosis that results in
Strange Situation A series of laboratory episodes developed by Mary three chromosomes for chromosome 21 and leads to Down syndrome.
Ainsworth and used to measure the kind of attachment a child
shows to a parent. The method examines an infant’s reactions to unconditioned response (UCR) A response that is normally
unusual, mildly threatening situations in the presence of a parent associated with a stimulus without the need of associative learning.
and when the parent has left the room, as well as how the child unconditioned stimulus (UCS) A stimulus that normally elicits an
responds when the parent returns. unconditioned response.
successful aging A pattern of aging that reflects the adoption of underextension A phenomenon in semantic development in which
strategies and behaviors that optimally adapt an older person’s the child uses a word to apply to a smaller set of entities than is
changing skills and abilities to her daily activities. normal in adult usage.
G-12 GLOSSARY
unipolar depression An affective disorder in which a severe and visual flow fields The streaming visual patterns made by objects and
sustained drop in affect is often accompanied by agitation, feelings terrain over time as they flow by an observer who is either moving
of sadness and worthlessness, insomnia, and loss of appetite. or is having the environment move about her.
universal constraints Limitations on the sets of language rules that visually evoked potential (VEP) method A method of measuring
are cognitively natural, resulting in consistent patterns of linguistic at what point of resolution the changing patterns that an infant is
structure seen in all languages and thereby aiding in language watching on a screen no longer cause corresponding changes in his
acquisition. brain’s electrical activity.
U-shaped developmental curve A developmental pattern in which vitalism The belief that living things are imbued with a vital force
an early, rudimentary ability or capacity initially declines with that is the cause behind growth, movement, and possibly other
increasing age and then increases, becoming more sophisticated than biological activities.
it was at the start. This pattern is associated with the replacement of
an early system with a later-emerging, more mature one. whole language approach An approach to reading that emphasizes
utilization deficit An inability to benefit from a memory strategy reading in the context of stories, narratives, and even pictorial
when it is first used. support.
whole-object bias A bias to assume that labels refer to whole objects
validity The capability of a research design to measure what was rather than their parts, their colors, or other properties.
intended to be measured. Williams syndrome A genetic disorder leading to an elfin appearance
verbal comprehension index A subcomponent of the WISC-IV that and below-average intelligence but in which individuals seem to
measures both comprehension of language-based materials and have intact language skills while having considerable difficulties in
the ability to solve problems posed in words. It includes questions many other areas of cognition.
designed to measure vocabulary, general knowledge about the within-subjects design A research design in which the same
world, and some questions that require abstract reasoning about participants are assessed in different experimental conditions.
similarities. working memory A form of memory in which incoming information
video deficit The relative inability of younger children to learn is attended to and processed in a preliminary or limited way before
as effectively from viewing videos as from watching live it either decays or is further processed into long-term memory; also
demonstrations. referred to as short-term memory.
visual acuity Sharpness of visual perception based on the ability working memory index A subcomponent of the WISC-IV that
to see the contrasts in the world that represent lines, shapes, and assesses performance on tasks that draw on working memory. It
shadows. includes doing simple arithmetic tasks and remembering strings of
visual cliff An apparatus for assessing depth perception in infants. It digits either backwards or forwards.
consists of a large, transparent and nonreflective pane of glass with
two similar patterned displays below it. One-half of the glass pane zone of proximal development A level of cognitive attainment that
has a pattern directly beneath it, so that the surface appears solid, is the next possible achievement by a child, often made possible
and the other half of the glass pane has the same pattern roughly through adult guidance.
3 feet below, giving the impression of a sudden drop-off. If the zygote A fertilized cell formed at conception from the merging of the
infant perceives depth, she will be afraid to crawl over the half of father’s sperm and the mother’s egg.
the glass pane that appears to have a drop-off.
GLOSSARY G-13
References
Aartsen, M. J., Smits, C. H., van Tilburg, T., Knipscheer, K. C., & Abramson, L. Y. (2011). Schools blend computers with classroom
Deeg, D. J. (2002). Activity in older adults: Cause or consequence of learning. National Public Radio, All Things Considered, June 22,
cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study on everyday activities 2011.
and cognitive performance in older adults. Journals of Gerontology, Abramson, L. Y., Alloy, L. B., Hankin, B. L., Clements, C. M.,
Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57B, P153–P162. Zhu, L., Hogan, M. E., & Whitehouse, W. G. (2000). Optimistic
Abbott, R. D., White, L. R., Ross, G. W., Masaki, K. H., Curb, cognitive styles and invulnerability to depression. In J. Gillham
J. D., & Petrovitch, H. (2004). Walking and dementia in physically (Ed.), The science of optimism and hope (pp. 75–98). Philadelphia:
capable elderly men. Journal of the American Medical Association, Templeton Foundation.
292, 1447–1453. Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness
Abecassis, M. (2003). I hate you just the way you are: Exploring the for- depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological
mation, maintenance, and need for enemies. In E. V. E. Hodges & Review, 96, 358–372.
N. A. Card (Eds.), Enemies and the darker side of peer relations: New Abravenel, E., & Sigafoos, A. D. (1984). Exploring the presence of
directions for child and adolescent development (Vol. 102, pp. 5–22). imitation during infancy. Child Development, 55, 381–392.
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Abrous, D. N., Koehl, M., & Le Moal, M. (2005). Adult neurogen-
Abecassis, M., Hartup, W. W., Haselager, G. J. T., Scholte, R., & esis: From precursors to network and physiology. Physiological
VanLieshout, C. F. M. (2001). Mutual antipathies and their signif- Reviews, 85, 523–569.
icance in middle childhood and adolescence. Child Development, Abudy, A., Juven-Wetzler, A., Sonnino, R., & Zohar, J. (2012).
73, 1543–1556. Serotonin and beyond: A neurotransmitter perspective of OCD.
Abed, R. T., & de Pauw, K. W. (1999). An evolutionary hypothesis for In J. Zohar (Ed.), Obsessive-compulsive disorder: Current science and
obsessive-compulsive disorder: A psychological immune system? clinical practice (pp. 220–243). Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
Behavioural Neurology, 11, 245–250. Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working mem-
Abela, J. R., & Hankin, B. L. (2011). Rumination as a vulnerabil- ory and intelligence: The same or different constructs? Psychological
ity factor to depression during the transition from early to middle Bulletin, 131, 30–60.
adolescence: A multiwave longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Acklin, M. W. (2003). Genetics and criminality: The potential misuse
Psychology, 120, 259–271. of scientific information in court. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Abell, F., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2000). Do triangles play tricks? 160, 1202–1203.
Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and Acredelo, L. P. (1978). Development of spatial orientation in infancy.
abnormal development. Cognitive Development, 15, 1–16. Developmental Psychology, 14, 224–234.
Aber, J., Allen, J., Carlson, V., & Cicchetti, D. (1989). The effects Acredelo, L. P. (1987). Early development of spatial orientation in
of maltreatment on development during early childhood: Recent humans. In P. Ellen & C. Thinus-Blanc (Eds.), Cognitive processes
studies and their theoretical, clinical, and policy implications. In of spatial orientation in animal and man: Vol 2. Neurophysiology
D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds.), Child maltreatment: Theory and and developmental aspects (pp. 185–201). Dordrecht, Netherlands:
research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect Martinus Nijhoff.
(pp. 579–619). New York: Cambridge University Press. Acredolo, L. P. (1990). Behavioral approaches to spatial orientation in
Aberg, M. A., Pedersen, N. L., Torén, K., Svartengren, M., infancy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608, 596–612.
Bäckstrand, B., Johnsson, T., Cooper-Kuhn, C. M., Aberg, N. D., Acredelo, L. P., & Evans, D. (1980). Developmental changes in the
Nilsson, M., & Kuhn, H. G. (2009). Cardiovascular fitness is effects of landmarks on infant spatial behavior. Developmental
associated with cognition in young adulthood. Proceedings of the Psychology, 16, 312–318.
National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 106, 20906–20911. Adams, M. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about
Aboud, F. E. (1988). Children and prejudice. New York: Blackwell. print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Aboud, F. E., & Doyle, A. B. (1996). Parental and peer influences Adams, R. J., Courage, M. L., & Mercer, M. E. (1994). Systematic
on children’s racial attitudes. International Journal of Intercultural measurement of human neonatal color vision. Vision Research, 34,
Relations, 20, 371–383. 1691–1701.
Aboud, F. E., & Fenwick, V. (1999). Exploring and evaluating school- Adamson, L. B., & Frick, J. E. (2003). The still face: A history of a
based interventions. Journal of Social Issues, 55, 767–785. shared experimental paradigm. Infancy, 4, 451–473.
Abrams, D. (2011). Wherein lies children’s intergroup bias? Adelman, R. D., Greene, M. G., & Ory, M. G. (2000). Communication
Egocentrism, social understanding, and social projection. Child between older patients and their physicians. Clinics in Geriatric
Development, 82, 1579–1593. Medicine, 16, 1–24.
R-1
Adelson, E., & Fraiberg, S. (1974). Gross motor development in childhood gender atypical behaviour and parent-child relation-
infants blind from birth. Child Development, 45, 114–126. ship. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 214–233.
Adler, P. A., & Adler, P. (1995). Dynamics of inclusion and exclusion Albano, A. M., & Kendall, P. C. (2002). Cognitive-behavioral ther-
in preadolescent cliques. Social Psychology Quarterly, 58, 145–162. apy for children and adolescents with anxiety disorders: Clinical
Adler, P. A., & Adler, P. (1998). Peer power: Preadolescent culture and research advances. International Review of Psychiatry, 14, 129–134.
identity. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Albano, A., March, J., & Piacentini, J. (1999). Cognitive behavioral
Adler-Baeder, F. (2006). What do we know about the physical abuse treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. In R. Ammerman,
of step children? A review of the literature. Journal of Divorce & M. Hersen, & C. Last (Eds.), Handbook of prescriptive treatments for
Remarriage, 44, 67–81. children and adolescents (2nd ed., pp. 193–215). Boston: Allyn &
Adolph, K. E. (1997). Learning in the development of infant locomo- Bacon.
tion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Alberts-Corush, J., Firestone, P., & Goodman, J. (1986). Attention
62 (3, Serial No. 251). and impulsivity characteristics of the biological and adoptive par-
Adolph, K. E. (2008). Learning to move. Current Directions in ents of hyperactive and normal control children. American Journal
Psychological Science, 17, 213–218. of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 413–423.
Adolph, K. E., Eppler, M. A., & Gibson, E. J. (1993). Crawling ver- Albiach-Serrano, A., Call, J., & Barth, J. (2010). Great apes track hid-
sus walking infants’ perception of affordances for locomotion over den objects after changes in the objects’ position and in subject’s
sloping surfaces. Child Development, 64, 1158–1174. orientation. American Journal of Primatology, 72, 349–359.
Adolph, K. E., Karasik, L. B., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2010). Using Alessandri, S. M. (1991). Play and social behavior in maltreated pre-
social information to guide action: Infants’ locomotion over slip- schoolers. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 191–205.
pery slopes. Neural Networks, 23, 1033–1042. Alev, C., Wu, Y., Kasukawa, T., Jakt, L. M., Ueda, H. R., & Sheng, G.
Adolph, K. E., & Kretch, K. S. (2012). Infants on the edge: Beyond (2010). Transcriptomic landscape of the primitive streak. Development,
the visual cliff. In A. Slater & P. Quinn (Eds.), Developmental 137, 2863–2874.
psychology: Revisiting the classic studies. London: Sage Publications. Alexander, G. M., & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differences in response
Adolph, K. E., Vereijken, B., & Shrout P. E. (2003).What changes in to children’s toys in nonhuman primates (Cercopithecus aethiops
infant walking and why. Child Development, 74, 475–497. sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 467–479.
Agras, S., Hammer, L., & McNicholas, F. (1999). A prospective study Alexander, P. C. (1993). The differential effects of abuse characteris-
of the influence of eating-disordered mothers on their children. tics and attachment in the prediction of long-term effects of sex-
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25, 253–262. ual abuse. Special Issue: Research on treatment of adults sexually
Ahissar, M. (1999). Perceptual learning. Current Directions in abused in childhood. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 8, 346–362.
Psychological Science, 8, 124–128. Alexandre, G. C., Nadanovsky, P., Moraes, C. L., & Reichenheim, M.
Ahmed, A., & Ruffman, T. (1998). Why do infants make A not B (2010). The presence of a stepfather and child physical abuse, as
errors in a search task, yet show memory for the location of hidden reported by a sample of Brazilian mothers in Rio de Janeiro. Child
objects in a nonsearch task? Developmental Psychology, 34, 441–453. Abuse & Neglect, 34, 959–966.
Ahnert, L., & Lamb, M. E. (2001). The East German child care sys- Alfano, C. A., Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (2002). Cognition in
tem: Associations with caretaking and caretaking beliefs, children’s childhood anxiety: Conceptual, methodological, and developmen-
early attachment and adjustment. American Behavioral Scientist, tal issues. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 1209–1238.
44, 1843–1863. Alfonso, V. C., Flanagan, D. P., & Radwan, S. (2005). The impact
Ainsworth, M.D.S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant care and the of the Cattell-Horn-Carroll theory on test development and inter-
growth of love. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. pretation of cognitive and academic abilities. In D. P. Flanagan &
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1979). Attachment as related to mother-infant P. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment (2nd ed.,
interaction. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 9, 2–52. pp. 185–202). New York: Guilford Press.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attachment, exploration Algon, S., Yi, J., Calkins, M. E., Kohler, C., & Borgmann-Winter,
and separation: Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a K. E. (2012). Evaluation and treatment of children and adolescents
strange situation. Child Development, 41, 49–67. with psychotic symptoms. Current Psychiatry Reports, 14, 101–110.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Ali, M. M., Amialchuk, A., Gao, S., & Heiland, F. (2012). Adolescent
Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. weight gain and social networks: Is there a contagion effect?
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Applied Economics, 44, 2969–2983.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Wittig, B. A. (1969). Attachment and explor- Allen, A. (2006, May 16). Flag on the field: Should colleges know
atory behaviour of one-year-olds in a strange situation. In B. M. when a student takes the SAT untimed? Slate. http://www.slate
Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 113–136). .com/id/2141820/
London: Methuen. Allen, L. S., Hines, M., Shryne, J. E., & Gorski, R. A. (1989). Two
Akerud, P., Canals, J. M., Snyder, E. Y., & Arenas, E. (2001). sexually dimorphic cell groups in the human brain. Journal of
Neuroprotection through delivery of glial cell line-derived neuro- Neuroscience, 9, 497–506.
trophic factor by neural stem cells in a mouse model of Parkinson’s Allen, M. (2008). Neurodevelopmental outcomes of preterm infants.
disease. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 8108–8118. Current Opinion in Neurology, 21, 123–128.
Akmajian, A., Demers, R. A., Farmer, A. K., & Harnish, R. A. Alley, T. R. (1988). The effects of growth and aging on facial aesthet-
(2001). Linguistics: An introduction to language and communication. ics. In T. R. Alley (Ed.), Social and applied aspects of perceiving faces
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (pp. 51–62). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Akshoomoff, N. A., & Stiles, J. (1995). Developmental trends in Allison, P. D., & Furstenberg, F. F. (1989). How marital dissolu-
visuospatial analysis and planning: I. Copying a complex figure. tion affects children: Variations by age and sex. Developmental
Neuropsychology, 9, 364–377. Psychology, 25, 540–549.
Alanko, K., Santtila, P., Salo, B., Jern, P., Johansson, A., & Sandnabba, Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New
N. K. (2011). Testing causal models of the relationship between York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
R-2 REFERENCES
Almli, C. R., & Finger, S. (1984). Early brain damage. Orlando: Amsterdam, B. K. (1972). Mirror self-image reactions before age two.
Academic Press. Developmental Psychology, 5, 297–305.
Alter, A. A., Aronson, J., Darley, J. M., Rodriguez, C., & Ruble, D. Anastasi, A. (1956). Intelligence and family size. Psychological Bulletin,
(2010). Rising to the threat: Reducing stereotype threat by refram- 53, 187–209.
ing threat as a challenge. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Andersen, S. L. (2003). Trajectories of brain development: Point
46, 166–171. of vulnerability or window of opportunity? Neuroscience and
Altschuler, E. L., Vankov, A., Wang, V., Ramachandran, V. S., & Behavioral Reviews, 27, 3–18.
Pineda, J. A. (1997). Person see, person do: Human cortical elec- Anderson, C. A., Berkowitz, L., Donnerstein, E., Huesmann, R. L.,
trophysiological correlates of monkey see monkey do cell. Society of Johnson, J., Linz, D., Malamuth, N., & Wartella, E. (2003). The
Neuroscience Abstracts, 719.17. influence of media violence on youth. Psychological Science in the
Alvarez, J. M., Ruble, D. N., & Bolger, N. (2001). The role of evalua- Public Interest, 4, 81–110.
tion in the development of person perception. Child Development, Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2001). Effects of violent video
72, 1409–1425. games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive
Amabile, T. M. (2001). Beyond talent: John Irving and the passionate affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: A meta-
craft of creativity. American Psychologist, 56, 333–336. analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychological Science, 12,
Amano, S., Kezuka, E., & Yamamoto, A. (2004). Infant shifting 353–359.
attention from an adult’s face to an adult’s hand: A precursor of Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Media violence and the
joint attention. Infant Behavior and Development, 27, 64–80. American public revisited. American Psychologist, 57, 448–450.
Amato, P. R. (1994). The implications of research findings on chil- Anderson, C. A., Gentile, D. A., & Buckley, K. E. (2007). Violent
dren in stepfamilies. In A. Booth & J. Dunn (Eds.) Stepfamilies: video game effects on children and adolescents: Theory, research, and
Who benefits? Who does not? Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. public policy. New York: Oxford University Press.
Amato, P. R. (1996). Explaining the intergenerational transmission of Anderson, D. R. (1998). Educational television is not an oxymoron.
divorce. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, 628–640. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 557,
Amato, P. R.(2000). The consequences of divorce for adults and chil- 24–38.
dren. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 1269–1287. Anderson, D. R. (2002). Children’s attention to television. In J. R.
Amato, P. R. (2006). Marital discord, divorce, and children’s well- Schement (Ed.), Encyclopedia of communication and information
being: Results from a 20-year longitudinal study of two genera- (pp. 132–136). New York: Macmillan Reference.
tions. In A. Clarke-Stewart & J. F. Dunn (Eds.), Families count: Anderson, D. R., Bryant, J., Wilder, A., Santomero, A., Williams, M., &
Effects on child and adolescent development (pp. 179–202). New Crawley, A. M. (2000). Researching Blue’s Clues: Viewing behavior
York: Cambridge University Press. and impact. Media Psychology, 2, 179–194.
Amato, P. R. (2010). Research on divorce: Continuing trends and new Anderson, D. R., & Hanson, K. G. (2010). From blooming, buzzing
developments. Journal of Marriage and the Family 72, 650–666. confusion to media literacy: The early development of television
Amato, P. R., & Booth, A. (1996). A prospective study of divorce and viewing. Developmental Review, 30, 239–255.
parent-child relationships. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, Anderson, D. R., & Pempek, T. A. (2005). Television and very young
356–365. children. American Behavioral Scientist, 48, 505–522.
Amato, P. R., & Keith, B. (1991). Parental divorce and the well-being Anderson, E. R., & Hope, D. A. (2008). A review of the tripartite
of children: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 26–46. model for understanding the link between anxiety and depression
Ambady, N., Shih, M., Kim, A., & Pittinsky, T. L. (2001). Stereotype in youth. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 275–287.
susceptibility in children: Effects of identity activation on quanti- Anderson, J. R. (1977). Induction of augmented transition networks.
tative performance. Psychological Science, 12, 385–390. Cognitive Science, 1, 125–157.
Ambrose, S. E. (1996). Undaunted courage: Meriwether Lewis, Thomas Anderson, J. R. (1984). The development of self-recognition: A review.
Jefferson, and the opening of the American West. New York: Simon & Developmental Psychobiology, 17, 35–49.
Schuster. Anderson, J. R. (1995). Cognitive psychology and its implications
Amedi, A., Merabet, L. B., Bermpohl, F., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). (4th ed.). New York: Freeman.
The occipital cortex in the blind: Lessons about plasticity and Anderson, K. G. (2011). Step-parenting, divorce, and investment
vision. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 306–311. in children. In C. Salmon & T. Shackelford (Eds.), The Oxford
American Association for the Advancement of Science. (1993). Project handbook of evolutionary family psychology (pp. 97–112). New York:
2061. Benchmarks for Science Literacy. Washington, DC: American Oxford University Press.
Association for the Advancement of Science. Anderson, K. G., Kaplan, H., & Lancaster, J. (1999a). Paternal care by
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical genetic fathers and stepfathers I: Reports from Albuquerque men.
manual of mental disorders (4th ed.) (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 405–431.
American Psychiatric Press. Anderson, K. G., Kaplan, H., Lam, D., & Lancaster, J. (1999b).
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statisti- Paternal care by genetic fathers and stepfathers II: Reports by
cal manual of mental disorders (Text Revision) (DSM-IV-TR). Xhosa High School students. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20,
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. 433–451.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical Anderson, K. G., Kaplan, H., & Lancaster, J. B. (2007). Confidence
manual of mental disorders (5th ed.) (DSM-V). Washington, DC: of paternity, divorce, and investment in children by Albuquerque
American Psychiatric Press. men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 1–10.
Ames, A. (1951). Visual perception and the rotating trapezoidal win- Anderson, L. M., Shinn, C., Fullilove, M. T., Scrimshaw, S. C.,
dow. Psychological Monographs, 65 (7, Whole No. 324). Fielding, J. E., Normand, J., Carande-Kulis, V. G., & the Task
Amso, D., & Johnson, S. P. (2005). Selection and inhibition in Force on Community Preventive Services. (2003). The effec-
infancy: Evidence from the spatial negative priming paradigm. tiveness of early childhood development programs: A systematic
Cognition, 95, B27–B36. review. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 24, 32–46.
REFERENCES R-3
Anderson, N. B., & Nickerson, K. J. (2005). Genes, race, and psychology Anzures, G., Wheeler, A., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M.,
in the genome era: An introduction. American Psychologist, 60, 3–8. Heron-Delaney, M., Tanaka, J. W., & Lee, K. (2012). Brief daily
Anderson, N. H. (1980). Information integration theory in develop- exposures to Asian females reverses perceptual narrowing for
mental psychology. In F. Wilkening, J. Becker, & T. Trabasso (Eds.), Asian faces in Caucasian infants. Journal of Experimental Child
Information integration by children (pp. 1–45). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Psychology, 112, 484–495.
Anderson, N. H. (1981). Foundations of information integration theory. APA Presidential Task Force on the Assessment of Age-Consistent
New York: Academic Press. Memory Decline and Dementia. (1998). Guidelines for the evalua-
Anderson, N. H., & Butzin, C. A. (1978). Integration theory applied tion of dementia and age-related cognitive decline. Washington, DC:
to children’s judgments of equity. Developmental Psychology, 14, American Psychological Association.
593–606. Apfelbaum, E. P., Sommers, S. R., & Norton, M. I. (2008). Seeing
Anderson, P. J., & Doyle, L. W. (2008). Cognitive and educa- race and seeming racist? Evaluating strategic colorblindness in
tional deficits in children born extremely preterm. Seminars in social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95,
Perinatology, 32, 51–58. 918–932.
Anderson, R. (1995). Consumer culture and TV programming. Boulder, Appel, B., Korzh, V., Glasgow, E., Thor, S., Edlund, T., David,
CO: Westview Press. I. B., & Eisen, J. S. (1995). Motoneuron fate specification revealed
Anderson, S. W., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, by patterned LIM homeobox gene expression in embryonic zebraf-
A. R. (1999). Impairment of social and moral behavior related to ish. Development, 121, 4117–4125.
early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2, Appel, M., & Kronberger, N. (2012). Stereotypes and the achievement
1032–1037. gap: Stereotype threat prior to test taking. Educational Psychology
Anderssen, N., Amlie, C., & Ytterøy, E. A. (2002). Outcomes for chil- Review, 24, 609–635.
dren with lesbian or gay parents. A review of studies from 1978 to Apple, M. (1992). Computers in schools: Salvation or social disease?
2000. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 335–351. Education Digest, 57, 47–52.
Andrews, G., & Halford, G. S. (2002). A cognitive complexity metric Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Visuospatial imme-
applied to cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 153–219. diate memory in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech,
Anggoro, F., Medin, D., & Waxman, S. (2010). Language and experi- Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 265–277.
ence influence children’s biological induction. Journal of Cognition Archibald, S. L., Fennema-Notestine, C., Gamst, A., Riley, E. P.,
and Culture, 10, 171–187. Mattson, S. N., & Jernigan, T. L. (2001). Brain dysmorphology in
Anglin, D. M., Corcoran, C. M., Brown, A. S., Chen, H., Lighty, Q., individuals with severe prenatal alcohol exposure. Developmental
Brook, J. S., & Cohen, P. R. (2012). Early cannabis use and schizo- Medical and Child Neurology, 43, 148–154.
typal personality disorder symptoms from adolescence to middle Arenas, E. (2010). Towards stem cell replacement therapies for
adulthood. Schizophrenia Research, 137, 45–49. Parkinson’s disease. Biochemical and Biophysical Research
Anglin, J. M. (1970). The growth of word meaning. Cambridge, MA: Communications, 396, 152–156.
MIT Press. Arikha, N. (2007). Passions and tempers: A history of the humours. New
Anglin, J. M. (1977). Word, object and conceptual development. New York: HarperCollins.
York: W. W. Norton. Armor, D. J. (2001). On family size and intelligence. American
Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological anal- Psychologist, 56, 521–522.
ysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Arnold, A. P., & Gorski, R. A. (1984). Gonadal steroid induction of
58 (Serial No. 238), 1–187. structural sex differences in the central nervous system. Annual
Angoff, W. H., & Johnson, E. G. (1990). The differential impact Review of Neuroscience, 7, 413–442.
of curriculum on aptitude test scores. Journal of Educational Arnold, A., Lewis, J., Maximovich, A., Ickovics, J., & Kershaw, T.
Measurement, 27, 291–305. (2011). Antecedents and consequences of caregiving structure
Angold, A., & Rutter, M. (1992). Effects of age and pubertal sta- on young mothers and their infants. Maternal and Child Health
tus on depression in a large clinical sample. Development and Journal, 15, 1037–1045.
Psychopathology, 4, 5–28. Arnold, L. E., Abikoff, H. B., Cantwell, D. P., Conners ,C. K., Elliott,
Angst, J., & Marneros, A. (2001). Bipolarity from ancient to modern G. R., Greenhill, L. L., Hechtman, L., Hinshaw, S. P., Hoza, B., Jensen,
times: Conception, birth and rebirth. Journal of Affective Disorders, P. S., Kraemer, H. C., March, J. S., Newcorn, J. H., Pelham, W. E.,
67, 3–19. Richters, J. E., Schiller, E., Severe, J. B., Swanson, J. M., Vereen, D., &
Antell, S., & Keating, D. (1983). Perception of numerical invariance Wells, K. C. (1997). National Institute of Mental Health Collaborative
in neonates. Child Development, 54, 695–701. Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (the MTA):
Anthony, J. L., & Francis, D. J. (2005). Development of phonological Design challenges and choices. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54,
awareness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 255–259. 865–870.
Anthony, J. L., Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., Driscoll, K., Phillips, Aronfreed, J. (1968). Conduct and conscience: The socialization of inter-
B. M., & Cantor, B. G. (2002). Structure of preschool phonological nalized control over behavior. New York: Academic Press.
sensitivity: Overlapping sensitivity to rhyme, words, syllables, and Arrindell, W. A., Eisemann, M., Richter, J., Oei, T. P. S., Caballo,
phonemes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 65–92. V. E., van der Ende, J., et al. (2003). Phobic anxiety in 11 nations.
Antinucci, F., & Parisi, D. (1974). Early semantic development in Part I: Dimensional constancy of the five-factor model. Behaviour
child language. In E. H. Lenneberg & E. Lenneberg (Eds.), Research and Therapy, 41, 461–479.
Foundations of language development: A multidisciplinary approach Artman, L., & Cahan, S. (1993). Schooling and the development of
(Vol. 1, pp. 189–201). New York: Academic Press. transitive inference. Developmental Psychology, 29, 753–759.
Antonini, A., & Stryker, M. P. (1993). Development of individual Arunachalam, S., & Waxman, S. R. (2010a). Language and con-
geniculocortical arbors in cat striate cortex and effects of binocular ceptual development. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive
impulse blockade. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 3549–3573. Science, 1, 548–558.
R-4 REFERENCES
Arunachalam, S., & Waxman, S. R. (2010b). Meaning from syntax: Atkinson, L., & Zucker, K. J. (Eds.). (1997). Attachment and psychopa-
Evidence from 2-year-olds. Cognition, 114, 442–446. thology. New York: Guilford Press.
Arvidsson, A., Collin, T., Kirik, D., Kokaia, Z., & Lindvall, O. Atkinson, R. C. (1999). The golden fleece, science education, and
(2002). Neuronal replacement from endogeneous precursors in the U.S. science policy. Proceedings of the American Philosophical
adult brain after stroke. Nature Medicine, 8, 963–970. Society, 143, 407–417.
Asarnow, J. R., Jaycox, L. H., & Tompson, M. C. (2001). Depression Atladottir, H. O., Henriksen, T. B., Schendel, D. E., & Parner,
in youth: Psychosocial interventions. Journal of Clinical Child E. T. (2012). Autism after infection, febrile episodes, and antibi-
Psychology, 30, 33–47. otic use during pregnancy: An exploratory study. Pediatrics, 130,
Asarnow, J. R., Tompson, M. C., & McGrath, E. P. (2004). e1447–e1454.
Childhood-onset schizophrenia: Clinical and treatment issues. Atladottir, H. O., Thorsen, P., Ostergaard, L., Schendel, D. E.,
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 180–194. Lemcke, S., Abdallah, M., & Parner, E. T. (2010). Maternal infec-
Asendorpf, J. B., Warkentin, V., & Baudonniere, P. (1996). Self- tion requiring hospitalization during pregnancy and autism spec-
awareness and other-awareness. II: Mirror self-recognition, social trum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40,
contingency awareness, and synchronic imitation. Developmental 1423–1430.
Psychology, 32, 313–321. Atran, S. (1995). Classifying nature across cultures. In D. N. Osherson
Ashe, V. M., Chiszar, D., & Smith, H. M. (1975). Behavior of aquatic (Series Ed.) & E. E. Smith & D. N. Osherson (Vol. Eds.), An invi-
and terrestrial turtles on a visual cliff. Chelonia, 2, 3–7. tation to cognitive science: Vol. 3. Thinking (2nd ed., pp. 131–174).
Asher, S. R., & Wheeler, V. A. (1985). Children’s loneliness: A com- Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
parison of rejected and neglected peer status. Journal of Consulting Atran, S. (1999). Itzaj Maya folkbiological taxonomy: Cognitive uni-
and Clinical Psychology, 53, 500–505. versals and cultural particulars. In D. L. Medin & S. Atran (Eds.),
Asherson P., Virdee, V., Curran, S., Ebersole, S., Freeman, B., Folkbiology (pp. 119–204). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Craig, I., Simonoff, E., Eley, T., Plomin, R., & Taylor, E. (1998). Atran, S. (2002). A metamodule for conceptual integration: Language
Association study of DSM-IV attention-deficit hyperactivity disor- or theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 674–675.
der (ADHD) and monoamine pathway genes. American Journal of Atran, S., & Medin, D. L. (2008). The native mind and the cultural
Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 81, 549. construction of nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Askew, M., Hodgen, J., Hossain, S., & Bretscher, N. (2010). Values Atran, S., Medin, D. L., Lynch, E., Vapnarsky, V., Ucan Ek’, E., &
and variables: A review of mathematics education in high-performing Sousa, P. (2001). Folkbiology doesn’t come from folkpsychology:
countries. London: Nuffield Foundation. Evidence from Yukatec Maya in cross-cultural perspective. Journal
Aslin, R. N. (1987). Visual and auditory development in infancy. of Cognition and Culture, 1, 4–42.
In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., Atran, S., Medin, D. L., & Ross, N. (2004). Evolution and devolu-
pp. 5–97). New York: Wiley. tion of knowledge: A tale of two biologies. Journal of the Royal
Aslin, R. N., & McMurray, B. (2004). Automated corneal-reflection Anthropological Institute, 10, 395–420.
eye tracking in infancy: Methodological developments and appli- Au, T. K. (1983). Chinese and English counterfactuals: The Sapir-
cations to cognition. Infancy, 6, 155–163. Whorf hypothesis revisited. Cognition, 15, 155–187.
Aslin, R. N., Jusczyk, P. W., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Speech and audi- Au, T. K. (1994). Developing an intuitive understanding of substance
tory processing during infancy: Constraints on and precursors kinds. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 71–111.
to language. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & D. Kuhn & R. Siegler Au, T. K., & Romo, L. F. (1999). Mechanical causality in children’s
(Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 2: Cognition, perception “folkbiology.” In D. L. Medin & S. Atran (Eds.), Folkbiology
and language (5th ed., pp. 147–198). New York: Wiley. (pp. 355–401). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Aslin, R. N., & Mehler, J. (2005). Near-infrared spectroscopy for Audet, K., & Le Mare, L. (2011). Mitigating effects of the adoptive
functional studies of brain activity in human infants: Promise, caregiving environment on inattention/overactivity in children
prospects, and challenges. Journal of Biomedical Optics, 10, 1–3. adopted from Romanian orphanages. International Journal of
Aslin, R. N., Saffran, J. R., & Newport, E. L. (1998). Computation Behavioral Development, 35, 107–115.
of conditional probability statistics by 8-month-old infants. Austin, C., & Edwards, R. G. (Eds.). (1981).Mechanism of sex differ-
Psychological Science, 9, 321–324. entiation in animals and man (pp. 465–547). London: Academic
Astington J. A., & Baird J. (Eds.). (2005). Why language matters for Press.
theory of mind. New York: Oxford University Press. Auyeung, B., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2008). A role for fetal testosterone
Astington, J. W. (1993). The child’s discovery of the mind. Cambridge, in human sex differences: Implications for understanding autism.
MA: Harvard University Press. In A. W. Zimmerman (Ed.), Autism: Current theories and evidence
Astington, J. W. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between (pp. 185–208). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press.
language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., &
Psychology, 35, 1311–1320. Hackett, G. (2009a). Fetal testosterone and autistic traits. British
Astington, J. W., & Gopnik, A. (1991). Theoretical explana- Journal of Psychiatry, 100, 1–22.
tions of children’s understanding of the mind. British Journal of Auyeung, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K.,
Developmental Psychology, 9, 7–31. Hackett, G., & Hines, M. (2009b). Fetal testosterone predicts
Atance, C. M., & O’Neill, D. K. (2005). The emergence of episodic sexually differentiated childhood behavior in girls and in boys.
future thinking in humans. Learning and Motivation, 36, 126–144. Psychological Science, 20, 144–148.
Atkinson, L., Goldberg, S., Raval, V., Pederson, D. R., Benoit, D., Auyeung, B., Taylor, K., Hackett, G., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2010).
Moran, G., Poulton, L., Myhal, N., Zwiers, M., Gleason, K. E., & Foetal testosterone and autistic traits in 18 to 24-month-old chil-
Leung, E. (2005). On the relation between maternal state of mind dren. Molecular Autism, 1, 11–19.
and sensitivity in the prediction of infant attachment security. Aviezer, O. (2003). Bedtime talk of three-year-olds: Collaborative
Developmental Psychology, 41, 42–53. repair of miscommunication. First Language, 23, 117–139.
REFERENCES R-5
Axia, G., & Baroni, M. R. (1985). Linguistic politeness at different Baird, J. A., & Astington, J. W. (2004). The role of mental state under-
age levels. Child Development, 56, 918–927. standing in the development of moral cognition and moral action.
New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 103, 37–49.
Bäckman, L., & Dixon, R. A. (1992). Psychological compensation: Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to
A theoretical framework. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 259–283. people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interactions.
Baddeley, A. D. (1985). Working memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford Child Development, 55, 1278–1289.
University Press. Baker, K. (2009). Conduct disorders in children and adolescents.
Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length Paediatrics and Child Health, 19, 73–78.
and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning Baker, M. (1996). The polysynthesis parameter. New York: Oxford
and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575–589. University Press.
Bagley, C. (1991). The prevalence and mental health sequels of child Baker, R. R., Lichtenberg, P. A., & Moye, J. (1998). A practical guide-
sexual abuse in a community sample of women aged 18 to 27. line for assessment of competency and capacity of the older adult.
Canadian Journal of Community Mental Health, 10, 103–116. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 29, 149–154.
Bagner, D., Pettit, J. W., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Seeley, J. R. (2010). Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Juffer, F.
Effect of maternal depression on child behavior: A sensitive period? (2003). Less is more: Meta-analyses of sensitivity and attach-
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, ment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin, 129,
49, 699–707. 195–215.
Bahrick, L. E. (1983). Infants’ perception of substance and temporal Bakwin, H. (1942). Loneliness in infants. American Journal of Diseases
synchrony in multimodal events. Infant Behavior and Development, of Children, 63, 30–40.
6, 429–451. Balas, B. J., Nelson, C. A., Westerlund, A., Vogel-Farley, V., Riggins,
Bahrick, L. E. (1988). Intermodal learning in infancy: Learning on T., & Kuefner, D. (2010). Personal familiarity influences the pro-
the basis of two kinds of invariant relations in audible and visible cessing of upright and inverted faces in infants. Frontiers in Human
events. Child Development, 59, 197–209. Neuroscience, 4, 1–6.
Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton, P., Simonoff, E., Baldwin, D. A. (1991). Infants’ contribution to the achievement of
Yuzda, E., & Rutter, M. (1995). Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: joint reference. Child Development, 62, 875–890.
Evidence from a British twin study. Psychological Medicine, 25, 63–77. Baldwin, D. A. (1993). Early referential understanding: Infants’ abil-
Bailey, P. E., Henry, J. D., Rendell, P. G., Phillips, L. H., & Kliegel, ity to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental
M. (2010). Dismantling the “age-prospective memory paradox”: Psychology, 29, 832–843.
The classic laboratory paradigm simulated in a naturalistic setting. Baldwin, D. A., Baird, J. A., Saylor, M. M., & Clark, A. (2001).
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 646–652. Infants parse dynamic action. Child Development, 72, 655–948.
Baillargeon, R. (1986). Representing the existence and the location of Baldwin, L. M., Larson, E. H., Connell, F. A., Nordlund, D., Cain,
hidden objects: Object permanence in 6- and 8-month-old infants. K. C., Cawthon, M. L., Byrns, P., & Rosenblatt, R. A. (1998). The
Cognition, 23, 21–41. effect of expanding Medicaid prenatal services on birth outcomes.
Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3½ and 4½ month-old American Journal of Public Health, 88, 1623–1629.
infants. Developmental Psychology, 23, 655–664. Ball, H. L., Hooker, E., & Kelly P. J. (2000). Parent-infant co-sleeping:
Baillargeon, R. (1995). Physical reasoning in infancy. In M. S. Fathers’ roles and perspectives. Infant and Child Development, 9,
Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 181–204). 67–74.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Ball, K, Owsley, C., Sloane, M. E., Roenker, D. L., & Bruni, J. R.
Baillargeon, R. (2002).The acquisition of physical knowledge in (1993). Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes
infancy: A summary in eight lessons. In U. Goswami (Ed.), in older drivers. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 34,
Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 47–83). 3110–3123.
Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Ball, K., Roenker, D., Wadley, V., Edwards, J., Roth, D., McGwin,
Baillargeon, R. (2004). Infants’ physical world. Current Directions in G., Raleigh, R., Joyce, J., Cissell, G., & Dube, T. (2006). Can
Psychological Science, 13, 89 –94. high-risk older drivers be identified through performance-based
Baillargeon, R., & DeVos, J. (1991). Object permanence in young measures in a Department of Motor Vehicles setting? Journal of the
infants: Further evidence. Child Development, 62, 1227–1246. American Geriatrics Society, 54, 77–84.
Baillargeon, R., & Hanko-Summers, S. (1990). Is the top object Ball, W. & Tronick, E. (1971, February 26). Infant responses to
adequately supported by the bottom object? Young infants’ under- impending collision: Optical and real. Science, 171, 818–820.
standing of support relations. Cognitive Development, 5, 29–54. Baltes, P. B. (1993). The aging mind: Potential and limits. Gerontologist,
Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief under- 33, 580–594.
standing in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 110–118. Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontog-
Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E. S., & Wasserman, S. (1985). Object perma- eny: Selection, optimization, and compensation as foundation of
nence in five-month-old infants. Cognition, 20, 191–208. developmental theory. American Psychologist, 52, 366–380.
Baillargeon, R., Stavans, M., Wu, D., Gertner, Y., Setoh, P., Kittredge, Baltes, P. B., & Kliegl, R. (1992). Further testing of limits of cognitive
A. K., & Bernard, A. (2012). Object individuation and physical plasticity: Negative age differences in a mnemonic skill are robust.
reasoning in infancy: An integrative account. Language Learning Developmental Psychology, 28, 121–125.
and Development, 8, 4–46. Baltes, P. B., & Lindenberger, U. (1997). Emergence of a powerful
Baillargeon, R., & Wang, S. (2002). Event categorization in infancy. connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 85–93. adult life span: A new window to the study of cognitive aging?
Baird, A., Scheffer, I., & Wilson, S. (2011). Mirror neuron system Psychology and Aging, 12, 12–21.
involvement in empathy: A critical look at the evidence. Social Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (1993). The search for a psychology
Neuroscience, 6, 327–335. of wisdom. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 75–80.
R-6 REFERENCES
Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Wisdom: A metaheuris- Barber, B. K. (2002). Intrusive parenting: How psychological con-
tic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. trol affects children and adolescents. Washington, DC: American
American Psychologist, 55, 122–136. Psychological Association.
Baltes, P. B., Cornelius, S. W., Spiro, A., Nesselroade, J. R., & Willis, Barber, B. K., Stolz, H. E., & Olsen, J. A. (2005). Parental support,
S. L. (1980). Integration versus differentiation of fluid/crystallized psychological control and behavioral control: assessing relevance
intelligence in old age. Developmental Psychology, 16, 625–635. across time, culture, and method. Monographs of the Society for
Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Lifespan Research in Child Development, 70, 1–137.
psychology: Theory and application to intellectual functioning. Bard, J. B. L. (1977). A unity underlying the different zebra striping
Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 471–507. patterns. Journal of Zoology (London), 183, 527–539.
Bamshad, M. (2005). Genetic influences on health: Does race matter? Bargh, J. A., McKenna, J. A., & Fitzsimons, G. M. (2002). Can you
Journal of the American Medical Association, 294, 937–946. see the real me? Activation and self expression of the “true self” on
Bamshad, M., & Guthery, S. L. (2007). Race, genetics and medi- the Internet. Journal of Social Issues, 58, 33–48.
cine: Does the color of a leopard’s spots matter? Current Opinion Barglow, P., Vaughn, B. E., & Molitor, N. (1987). Effects of maternal
in Pediatrics, 19, 613–618. absence due to employment on the quality of infant-mother attach-
Banaschewski, T., Hollis, C., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., Rubia, K., ment in a low-risk sample. Child Development, 58, 945–954.
Willcutt, E., & Taylor, E. (2005). Towards an understanding of Barkley, R. (1998a). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Scientific
unique and shared pathways in the psychopathophysiology of American, 279, 66–71.
ADHD. Developmental Science, 8, 132–140. Barkley, R. (1998b). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A hand-
Bandi-Rao, S., & Murphy, G. (2007). The role of meaning in past- book for diagnosis and treatment. New York: Guilford Press.
tense inflection: Evidence from polysemy and denominal deriva- Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and
tion. Cognition, 104, 150–162. executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD.
Bandura, A. (1971). Social learning theory. New York: General Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94.
Learning Press. Barna, J., & Legerstee, M. (2005). Nine- and twelve-month-old
Bandura, A. D. (1978). Self efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of infants relate emotions to people’s actions. Cognition and Emotion,
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215. 19, 53–67.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social Barner, D., Li, P., & Snedeker, J. (2010). Words as windows to
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. thought: The case of object representation. Current Directions in
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Psychological Science, 19, 195–200.
Freeman. Barnett, D., Ganiban, J., & Cicchetti, D. (1999). Maltreatment, emo-
Bandura, A., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., & tional expressivity, and the development of Type D attachments
Regalia, C. (2001). Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms from 12 to 24 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research
governing transgressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social in Child Development, 64, 97–118.
Psychology, 80, 125–135. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory
Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1961). Transmission of aggres- of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
sion through imitation of aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal Baron-Cohen, S. (2003). The essential difference: The truth about the
and Social Psychology, 63, 575–582. male and female brain. New York: Basic Books.
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. (1963). Social learning and personality Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D. A., & Crowson, M. (1997). Do chil-
development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. dren with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to
Banerjee, D., & Slack, F. (2002) Control of developmental timing by crack the code of language? Child Development, 68, 48–57.
small temporal RNAs: A paradigm for RNA-mediated regulation Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., Wheelwright, S., Short, L., Mead, G.,
of gene expression. Bioessays, 24, 119–129. Smith, A., & Scahill, V. (1998). Autism occurs more often in
Banerjee, R., Watling, D., & Caputi, M. (2011). Peer relations and the families of physicists, engineers, and mathematicians. Autism, 2,
understanding of faux pas: Longitudinal evidence for bidirectional 296–391.
associations. Child Development, 82, 1887–1905. Baron-Cohen, S., Knickmeyer, R. C., & Belmonte, M. K. (2005).
Bang, M., Medin, D., & Atran, S. (2007). Cultural mosaics and men- Sex differences in the brain: Implications for explaining autism.
tal models of nature. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Science, 310, 819–823.
of the USA, 104, 13868–13874. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic
Bao, A. M. & Swaab, D. F. (2011). Sexual differentiation of the child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.
human brain: Relation to gender identity, sexual orientation, and Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient:
neuropsychiatric disorders. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 32, An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high func-
214–226. tioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and
Bao, X. H., & Lam, S. F. (2008). Who makes the choice? Rethinking Developmental Disorders, 34, 163–175.
the role of autonomy and relatedness in Chinese children’s motiva- Baroni, M. R., & Axia, G. (1989). Children’s meta-pragmatic abili-
tion. Child Development, 79, 269–283. ties and the identification of polite and impolite requests. First
Barbacid, M. (1995). Neurotrophic factors and their receptors. Current Language, 9, 285–297.
Opinion in Cell Biology, 7, 148–155. Baroody, A. J., & Ginsburg, H. P. (1986). The relationship between
Barbaro, J., & Dissamayake, C. (2009). Autism spectrum disorders initial meaningful and mechanical knowledge of arithmetic. In
in infancy and toddlerhood: A review of the evidence on early J. Hiebert (Ed.), Conceptual and procedural knowledge: The case of
signs, early identification tools and early diagnosis. Journal of mathematics (pp. 75–112). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 30, 447–459. Barr, C. L., Wigg, K. G., Bloom, S, Schachar, R., Tannock, R., Roberts, W.,
Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting a Malone, M., & Kennedy, J. L. (2000). Further evidence from hap-
neglected construct. Child Development, 67, 3296–3319. lotype analysis for linkage of the dopamine D4 receptor gene and
REFERENCES R-7
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Medical integrity in healthy aging adults and patients with Alzheimer
Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 96, 262–267. disease: A magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of Neurology,
Barr, C. L., Wigg, K., Malone, M., Schachar, R., Tannock, R., 60, 393–398.
Roberts, W., & Kennedy J. L. (1999). Linkage study of catechol- Barzilai, N., Guarente, L., Kirkwood, T. B., Partridge, L., Rando,
O-methyltransferase and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. T. A., & Slagboom, P. E. (2012). The place of genetics in ageing
American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), research. Nature Reviews Genetics, 13, 589–954.
88, 710–713. Bascandziev, I., & Harris, P. L. (2011) Gravity is not the only ruler
Barr, R. G., Pantel, M. S., Young, S. N., Wright, J. H., Hendricks, for falling events: Young children stop making the gravity error
L. A., & Gravel, R. (1999). The response of crying newborns after receiving additional perceptual information about the
to sucrose: Is it a “sweetness” effect? Physiology & Behavior, 66, tubes mechanism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109,
409–417. 468–477.
Barr, R., & Hayne, H. (1999). Developmental changes in imitation Bates, E. (1976). Acquisition of polite forms: Longitudinal evidence.
from television during infancy. Child Development, 70, 1067–1081. In E. Bates (Ed.), Language and context: The acquisition of pragmat-
Barr, R., Zack, E., Garcia, A., & Muentener, P. (2008). Infants’ atten- ics (pp. 255–294). New York: Academic Press.
tion and responsiveness to television increases with prior exposure Bates, E. (2004). Explaining and interpreting deficits in language
and parental interaction. Infancy, 13, 30–56. development across clinical groups: Where do we go from here?
Barrett, H. C. (2005). Adaptations to predators and prey. In D. M. Brain and Language, 88, 248–253.
Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 200–223). Bates, E., & Elman, J. (1996). Learning rediscovered. Science, 274,
New York: Wiley. 1849–1850.
Barrett, J. R., & Driesenga, A. (2001). God’s beliefs versus mother’s. Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V.
Child Development, 72, 50–65. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in
Barrett, K. C., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Cole, P. M. (1993). Avoiders vs. infancy. New York: Academic Press.
amenders: Implications for the investigation of guilt and shame Bates, E., Elman, J., Johnson, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., &
during toddlerhood? Cognition and Emotion, 7, 481–505. Plunkett, K. (1998). Innateness and emergentism. In W. Bechtel &
Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on G. Graham (Eds.), A companion to cognitive science (pp. 590–601).
Psychological Science, 1, 28–58. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Barrett, L., Henzi, P., & Rendall, D. (2007). Social brains, simple Bates, E., O’Connell, B., & Shore, C. (1987). Language and com-
minds: Does social complexity really require cognitive complexity? munication in infancy: Development of gesture. In J. D. Osofsky
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 149–203).
362, 561–575. New York: Wiley.
Barrett, M. (1995). Early lexical development. In P. Fletcher & Bates, E., Thal, D., & Janowsky, J. S. (1992). Early language develop-
B. MacWhinney (Eds.), Handbook of child language (pp. 362–392). ment and its neural correlates. In I. Rapin & S. Segalowitz (Eds.),
Cambridge, UK: Blackwell. Handbook of neuropsychology: Vol. 6. Child neurology (pp. 69–100).
Barrett, P. M., Duffy, A. L., Dadds, M. R., & Rapee, R. M. (2001). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Cognitive-behavioral treatment of anxiety disorders in children: Bates, J. E., Marvinney, D., Kelly, T., Dodge, K. A., Bennett, D. S., &
Long-term (6-year) follow-up. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Pettit, G. S. (1994). Child-care history and kindergarten adjust-
Psychology, 69, 135–141. ment. Developmental Psychology, 30, 690–700.
Barry, C. T., Frick, P. J., DeShazo, T. M., McCoy, M., Ellis, M., & Bates, J., Pettit, G., Dodge, K., & Ridge, B. (1998). Interaction of
Loney, B. R. (2000). The importance of callous-unemotional traits temperamental resistance to control and restrictive parenting in the
for extending the concept of psychopathy to children. Journal of development of externalizing behavior. Developmental Psychology,
Abnormal Psychology, 109, 335–340. 34, 982–995.
Barry, C. T., Grafeman, S. J., Adler, K. K., & Pickard, J. D. (2009). Bateson, P., & Horn, G. (1994). Imprinting and recognition memory:
The relations among narcissism, self- esteem, and delinquency A neural net model. Animal Behaviour, 48, 695–715.
in a sample of at-risk adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 47, Batsche, G. M., & Knoff, H. M. (1994). Bullies and their victims:
577–582. Understanding a pervasive problem in the schools. School Psychology
Barry, H., & Paxson, L. M. (1971). Infancy and early childhood: Review, 23, 165–174, EJ 490–574.
Cross-cultural codes 2. Ethnology, 10, 466–508. Bauer, P. J. (1996). What do infants recall of their lives? Memory for
Bartlett, F. C. (1977/1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and specific events by one-to two-year-olds. American Psychologist, 51,
social psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. 29–41.
Bartocci, M., Winberg, J., Ruggiero, C., Bergqvist, L. L., Serra, G., & Bauer, P. J. (2002a). Early memory development. In U. Goswami
Lagercrantz, H. (2000). Activation of olfactory cortex in newborn (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development
infants after odor stimulation: A functional near-infrared spectros- (pp. 127–146). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
copy study. Pediatric Research, 48, 18–23. Bauer, P. J. (2002b). Long-term recall memory: Behavioral and neuro-
Barton, B. K., & Morrongiello, B. A. (2011). Examining the impact of developmental changes in the first 2 years of life. Current Directions
traffic environment and executive functioning on children’s pedes- in Psychological Science, 11, 137–141.
trian behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 47, 182–191. Bauer, P. J. (2005). Developments in declarative memory: Decreasing
Bartoshuk, L. M., & Beauchamp, G. K. (1994). Chemical senses. susceptibility to storage failure over the second year of life.
Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 419–449. Psychological Science, 16, 41–47.
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. Bauer P. J. (2006). Constructing a past in infancy: A neuro-
New York: Oxford University Press. developmental account. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 175–181.
Bartzokis, G., Cummings, J. L., Sultzer, D., Henderson, V. W., Bauer, P. J. (2007) Recall in infancy: A neurodevelopmental account.
Nuechterlein, K. H., & Mintz, J. (2003). White matter structural Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 142–146.
R-8 REFERENCES
Bauer, P. J., Hertsgaard, L. A., & Dow, G. A. A. (1994). After 8 Bayley, N. (1935). The development of motor abilities during the
months have passed: Long-term recall of events by 1- to 2-year-old first three years. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
children. Memory, 2, 353–382. Development, 1.
Bauer, P. J., Kroupina, M. G., Schwade, J. A., Dropik, P., & Wewerka, Bayley, N. (1949). Consistency and variability in the growth of intel-
S. S. (1998). If memory serves, will language? Later verbal accessibil- ligence from birth to eighteen years. Journal of Genetic Psychology,
ity of early memories. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 655–679. 75, 165–196.
Bauer, P. J., San Souci, P., & Pathman, T. (2010). Infant memory. Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley scales of infant development. New York:
Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 267–277. Psychological Corporation.
Bauer, P. J., & Wewerka, S. S. (1995). One- to two-year-olds’ recall Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley scales of infant development (2nd ed.). San
of events: The more expressed, the more impressed. Journal of Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 475–496. Bayley, N. (2005). Bayley scales of infant and toddler development
Bauer, P. J., & Wewerka, S. S. (1997). Saying is revealing: Verbal (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: PsychCorp/Pearson.
expression of event memory in the transition from infancy to early Beaton, A. E., Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Gonzalez, E. J., Kelly,
childhood. In P. van den Broek, P. J. Bauer, & T. Bourg (Eds.), D. L., & Smith, T. A. (1996). Mathematics achievement in the middle
Developmental spans in event comprehension and representation: school years: IEA’s third international mathematics and science study
Bridging fictional and actual events (pp. 139–168). Mahwah, NJ: (TIMSS). Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College.
Erlbaum. Beauchamp, G. K., & Mennella, J. A. (2011). Flavor perception
Baugh, J. (1983). Black street speech: Its history, structure and survival. in human infants: Development and functional significance.
Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. Digestion, 83, 1–6.
Baumberger, B., Isableu, B., & Fluckiger, M. (2004). The visual con- Beauchamp, G. K., Cowart, B. J., Mennella, J. A., & Marsh, R. R.
trol of stability in children and adults: Postural readjustments in (1994). Infant salt taste: Developmental, methodological, and con-
a ground optical flow. Experimental Brain Research, 159, 33–46. textual factors. Developmental Psychobiology, 27, 353–365.
Baumeister, R. F. (1990). Suicide as escape from self. Psychological Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia:
Review, 97, 90–113. University of Pennsylvania Press.
Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive
(2003). Does high self-esteem cause better performance, inter- therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press.
personal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychological Becker, A. (2004). Television, disordered eating, and young women
Science in the Public Interest, 4, 1–44. in Fiji: Negotiating body image and identity during rapid social
Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2005, change. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 28, 533–559.
January). Exploding the self-esteem myth. Scientific American, Becker, A. E., Burwell, R. A., Gilman, S. E., Herzog, D. B., &
292, 70–77. Hamburg, P. (2002). Eating behaviours and attitudes following
Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1993). When ego- prolonged exposure to television among ethnic Fijian adolescent
threats lead to self-regulation failure: Negative consequences of high girls. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 509–514.
self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 141–156. Beckers, T., Krypotos, A. M., Boddez, Y., Effting, M., & Kindt, M.
Baumeister, R. F., & Ilko, S. (1995). Shallow gratitude: Public and (2013). What’s wrong with fear conditioning? Biological Psychology,
private acknowledgment of external help in accounts of success. 92, 90–96.
Basic and Applied Psychology, 16, 191–209. Beckett, K., & Baylies, M. K. (2007). 3D analysis of founder cell and
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of fusion competent myoblast arrangements outlines a new model of
threatened egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of myoblast fusion. Developmental Biology, 309, 113–125.
high self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 5–33. Bedford, F. (1989). Constraints on learning new mappings between
Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (Eds). (2004). Handbook of self- perceptual dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
regulation: Research, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford Perception & Performance, 15, 232–248.
Press. Bednar, D. E., & Fisher, T. D. (2003). Peer referencing in adoles-
Baumrind, D, & Black, A. E. (1967). Socialization practices associ- cent decision making as a function of perceived parenting style.
ated with dimensions of competence in preschool boys and girls. Adolescence, 38, 607–621.
Child Development, 38, 291–327. Beers, L., & Hollo, R. (2009).Approaching the adolescent-headed
Baumrind, D. (1966). Effects of authoritative parental control on family: A review of teen parenting. Current Problems in Pediatric
child behavior. Child Development, 37, 887–907. Adolescent Health Care, 39, 216–233.
Baumrind, D. (1967). Child care practices anteceding three patterns Behrman, R. E., & Stith Butler A. (Eds.). (2007). Institute of
of preschool behavior. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 75, 43–88. Medicine Committee on Understanding Premature Birth and
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Assuring Healthy Outcomes Board on Health Sciences Outcomes.
Developmental Psychology Monograph (Part 2), 4, 1–103. Preterm birth: Causes, consequences, and prevention. Washington,
Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parenting style on adolescent com- DC: National Academies Press.
petence and substance abuse. Journal of Early Adolescence, 11, 56–95. Beilin, H. (1980). Piaget’s theory: Refinement, revision, or rejection?
Baumrind, D. (1996). The discipline controversy revisited. Family In R. H. Kluwe & H. Spada (Eds.), Developmental models of think-
Relations, 45, 405–414. ing (pp. 245–261). New York: Academic Press.
Bavelier, D., & Neville, H. J. (2002). Cross-modal plasticity: Where Beilock, S., Rydell, R., & McConnell, A. R. (2007). Stereotype threat
and how? Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 3, 443–452. and working memory: Mechanisms, alleviation, and spillover.
Bavelier, D., Dye, M. W., & Hauser, P. C. (2006). Do deaf individuals Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 256–276.
see better? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 512–518. Beitchman, J. H., Zucker, K. J., Hood, J. E., daCosta, G. A., &
Bayer, S. A., & Altman, J. (1991). Neocortical development. New York: Akman, D. (1991). A review of the short-term effects of child sex-
Raven Press. ual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 15, 537–556.
REFERENCES R-9
Beitchman, J. H., Zucker, K. J., Hood, J. E., daCosta, G. A., Belsky, J., & Steinberg, L. D. (1978). The effects of day care: A critical
Akman, D., & Cassavia, E. (1992). A review of the long-term review. Child Development, 49, 929–949.
effects of child sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 16, 101–118. Belton, E., Salmond, C. H., Watkins, K. E., Vargha-Khadem, F., &
Bell, R. Q., & Harper, R. V. (Eds.). (1977). Child effects on adults. Gadian, D. G. (2003). Bilateral brain abnormalities associated
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. with dominantly inherited verbal and orofacial dyspraxia. Human
Bellgrove, M. A., Hawi, Z., Lowe, N., Kirley, A., Robertson, I. H., & Brain Mapping, 18, 194–200.
Gill, M. (2005). DRD4 gene variants and sustained attention in Belton, E., Salmond, C., Watkins, K., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Gadian,
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Effects of asso- D. (2002). Bilateral grey matter abnormalities in a family with a
ciated alleles at the VNTR and -521 SNP. American Journal of mutation in FOXP2. NeuroImage, 16, 10144.
Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 136, 81–86. Beltz, A. M., Swanson, J. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2011). Gendered
Bellugi, U., & Wang, P. P. (1998). Williams syndrome: From cog- occupational interests: Prenatal androgen effects on psychological
nition to brain to gene. In G. Adelman & B. H. Smith (Eds.), orientation to things versus people. Hormones and Behavior, 60,
Encyclopedia of neuroscience. Amsterdam: Elsevier. 313–317.
Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., Jernigan, T., Trauner, D., & Doherty, S. (1990). Bem, D. J., & Allen, A. (1974). On predicting some of the people
Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile some of the time: The search for cross-situational consistencies in
of Williams syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 37, behavior. Psychological Review, 81, 506–520.
115–125. Bem, D. J., & Funder, D. C. (1978). Predicting more of the people more
Bellugi, U., Marks, S., Bihrle, A. M., & Sabo, H. (1988). Dissociation of the time: Assessing the personality of situations. Psychological
between language and cognitive functions in Williams syndrome. Review, 85, 485–501.
In D. Bishop & K. Mogford (Eds.), Language development in Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex
exceptional circumstances (pp. 177–189). Edinburgh: Churchill typing. Psychological Review, 88, 354–364.
Livingstone. Ben Hamida, S., Mineka, S., & Bailey, J. M. (1998). Sex differences in
Belmont, M., & Marolla, F. A. (1973). Birth order, family size, and perceived controllability of mate value: An evolutionary perspec-
intelligence. Science, 182, 1096–1101. tive. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 953–966.
Belsky, J. (1980). Child maltreatment: An ecological integration. Benedict, R. (1946). The chrysanthemum and the sword: Patterns of
American Psychologist, 35, 320–335. Japanese culture. New York: New American Library.
Belsky, J. (1986). Infant day care: A cause for concern? Zero to Three, Benenson, J. F., & Christakos, A. (2003). The greater fragil-
6, 1–7. ity of females’ versus males’ closest same-sex friendships. Child
Belsky, J. (2001). Developmental risk (still) associated with early child Development, 74, 1123–1129.
care. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 845–860. Benenson, J. F., Markovits, H., Thompson, M. E., & Wrangham,
Belsky, J. (2002). Quantity counts: Amount of child care and chil- R. W. (2011). Under threat of social exclusion, females exclude
dren’s socioemotional development. Journal of Developmental and more than males. Psychological Science, 22, 538–544.
Behavioral Pediatrics, 23, 167–170. Bengtsson, S. L., Nagy, Z., Skare, S., Forsman, L., Forssberg, H., &
Belsky, J. (2005). Family influences on psychological development. Ullen, F. (2005). Extensive piano practicing has regionally spe-
Psychiatry, 4, 41–44. cific effects on white matter development. Nature Neuroscience, 8,
Belsky, J., Friedman, S., & Hsieh, K. (2001). Testing a core emotion- 1148–1150.
regulation prediction: Does early attentional control moderate the Benjet, C., & Kazdin, A. E. (2003). Spanking children: The contro-
effect of infant negative emotionality on later development? Child versies, findings, and new directions. Clinical Psychology Review,
Development, 72, 123–133. 23, 197–224.
Belsky, J., Hsieh, K., & Crnic, K. (1998). Mothering, fathering and Bennett, A. J., Lesch, K. P., Heils, A., Long, J. C., Lorenz, J. G., Shoaf,
infant negativity as antecedents of boys’ externalizing problems S. E, Champoux, M., Suomi, S. J., Linnoila, M. V., & Higley, J. D.
and inhibition at age 3 years: Differential susceptibility to rearing (2002). Early experience and serotonin transporter gene variation
experience? Development and Psychopathology 10, 301–319. interact to influence primate CNS function. Molecular Psychiatry,
Belsky, J., & Johnson, C. D. (2005). Developmental outcome of chil- 7, 118–122.
dren in day care. In J. Murph, S. D. Palmer, & D. Glassy (Eds.), Bennett, M., (Ed.) (1993). The development of social cognition: The
Health in child care: A manual for health professionals (4th ed., child as psychologist. New York: Guilford Press.
pp. 81–95). Elks Grove Village, IL: American Academy of Bennett, M., & Cormack, C. (1996). Age and embarrassment at oth-
Pediatrics. ers’ gaffes. Journal of Social Psychology, 136, 113–115.
Belsky, J., Rha, J., & Park, S. (2000). Exploring reciprocal parent and Berch, D. B. (1977). Absolute judgment of recency: A reexamination
child effects in the case of child inhibition in U.S. and Korean of children’s short-term memory data. Psychological Bulletin, 84,
samples. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 1261–1266.
338–347. Berenbaum, S. A., & Beltz, A. M. (2011). Sexual differentiation of
Belsky, J., & Rovine, M. (1987). Temperament and attachment secu- human behavior: Effects of prenatal and pubertal organizational
rity in the Strange Situation: An empirical rapprochement. Child hormones. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 32,183–200.
Development, 58, 787–795. Berenbaum, S. A., & Hines, M. (1992). Early androgens are related
Belsky, J., & Rovine, M. (1988). Nonmaternal care in the first to childhood sex-typed toy preferences. Psychological Science, 3,
year of life and the security of infant-parent attachment. Child 203–206.
Development, 59, 157–167. Berenbaum, S. A., Martin, C. L., Hanish, L. D., Briggs, P. T., &
Belsky, J., Rovine, M., & Taylor, D. G. (1984). The Pennsylvania Fabes, R. A. (2008). Sex differences in children’s play. In J. B.
infant and family development project, III: The origins of individ- Becker, K. J. Berkley, N. Geary, E. Hampson, J. Herman, &
ual differences in infant-mother attachment: Maternal and infant E. Young (Eds.), Sex differences in the brain: From genes to behavior.
contributions. Child Development, 55, 718–728. New York: Oxford University Press.
R-10 REFERENCES
Berenbaum, S. A., & Resnick, S. M. (2007). The seeds of career Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and lan-
choices: Prenatal sex hormone effects on psychological sex differ- guage (pp. 51–102). New York: Wiley.
ences. In S. J. Ceci & W. M. Williams (Eds.), Why aren’t more Bertin, E., & Bhatt, R. S. (2001). Figural goodness, stimulus hetero-
women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 147–157). geneity, similarity and object segregation in infancy. Developmental
Washington DC: APA Books. Science, 4, 423–432.
Bergan, J. F., & Knudsen, E. I. (2009). Visual modulation of auditory Bertin, E., & Striano, T. (2006). The still-face response in newborns,
responses in the owl inferior colliculus. Journal of Neurophysiology, 1.5-, and 3-month-old infants. Infant Behavior & Development, 29,
101, 2924–2933. 294–297.
Berger, C., Rodkin, P. C., & Dijkstra, J. K. (2011). Antipathetic rela- Bertrand, J., Mars, A., Boyle, C., Bove, F., Yeargin-Allsopp, M., &
tionships among adolescents: Exploring prevalence, gender dif- Decoufle, P. (2001). Prevalence of autism in a United States popu-
ferences, and stability in the United States and Chile. Anales de lation: The Brick Township, New Jersey, investigation. Pediatrics,
Psicologia, 27, 783–790. 108, 1155–1161.
Berkeley, G. (1709/1901). An essay towards a new theory of vision. In Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on
A. C. Fraser (Ed.), The works of George Berkeley, D.D., formerly executive function. Child Development, 81, 1641–1660.
Bishop of Cloyne, including his posthumous works. Oxford, UK: Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Jones, L. L. (2009). Executive functions
Clarendon Press. after age 5: Changes and correlates. Developmental Review, 29,
Berko-Gleason, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. 180–200.
Word, 14, 150–177. Betancur, C. (2011). Etiological heterogeneity in autism spectrum
Berkowitz, M. W., & Oser, F. (Eds.) (1985). Moral education: Theory disorders: More than 100 genetic and genomic disorders and still
and application. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. counting. Brain Research, 1380, 42–77.
Berlin, B. (1992). Ethnobiological classification: Principles of catego- Bethea, C. L., Streicher, J. M., Coleman, K., Pau, F. P., Moessner, R., &
rization of plants and animals in traditional societies. Princeton: Cameron, J. L. (2004). Anxious behavior and fenfluramine-induced
Princeton University Press. prolactin secretion in young rhesus macaques with different alleles
Berlin B., & Kay, P. (1969). Basic color terms: Their universality and of the serotonin reuptake transporter polymorphism (5HTTLPR).
evolution. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press. Behavior Genetics, 34, 295–307.
Bernal, S. (2005). Object lessons: Spelke principles and psychological Bettelheim, B. (1972). The empty fortress: Infantile autism and the birth of
explanation. Philosophical Psychology, 18, 289–312. the self. New York: Free Press.
Berndt, T. J. (2002). Friendship quality and social development. Bezdjian, S., Tuvblad, C., Raine, A., & Baker, L. A. (2011). The
Current Directions in Psychological Sciences, 11, 7–10. genetic and environmental covariation among psychopathic per-
Berndt, T. J. (1996). Exploring the effects of friendship quality on sonality traits, and reactive and proactive aggression in childhood.
social development. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & Child Development, 82, 1267–1281.
W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in child- Bhatt, R. S., Bertin, E., Hayden, A., & Reed, A. (2005). Face pro-
hood and adolescence (pp. 346–365). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge cessing in infancy: Developmental changes in the use of different
University Press. kinds of relational information. Child Development, 76, 169–181.
Bernstein, G. A., Borchardt, C. M., & Perwien, A. R. (1996). Anxiety Bherer, L. (2012). Physical activity and exercise in older adults. In
disorders in children and adolescents: A review of the past 10 years. E. O. Acevedo (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of exercise psychology
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, (pp. 359–384). New York: Oxford University Press.
35, 1110–1119. Bhugra, D. (2005). The global prevalence of schizophrenia. PLOS
Bernstein, N. (1967). The co-ordination and regulation of movement. Medicine, 2, e151.
Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press. Bialystok, E. (1997). Effects of bilingualism and biliteracy on chil-
Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1974). Physical attractiveness. In dren’s emerging concepts of print. Developmental Psychology, 33,
L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, 429–440.
7, 157–215. Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2012). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating
Bertamini, M., Spooner A., & Hecht, H. (2004). The representa- advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control.
tion of naive knowledge about physics. In G. Malcolm (Ed.), Cognition, 122, 67–73.
Multidisciplinary approaches to visual representations and interpreta- Bianchi, S. M. (2011). Family change and time allocation in American
tions. Amsterdam: Elsevier. families. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social
Bertenthal, B. I., & Bai, D. L. (1989). Infants’ sensitivity to opti- Science, 638, 21–44.
cal flow for controlling posture. Developmental Psychology, 25, Bianchi, S. M., Robinson, J. P., & Milkie, M. A. (2006). Changing
936–945. rhythms of American family life. New York: Russell Sage.
Bertenthal, B. I., & Fischer, K. W. (1978). Development of self- Bickerton, D. (1984). The language bioprogram hypothesis. Behavioral
recognition in the infant. Developmental Psychology, 14, 44–50. and Brain Sciences, 7, 173–221.
Bertenthal, B. I., Boker, S., & Xu., M. (2000). Analysis of the Bickerton, D. (1988). Creole languages and the bioprogram. In
perception-action cycle for visually induced postural sway in F. J. Newmeyer (Ed.), Linguistics: The Cambridge survey: Vol. 2.
9-month-old sitting infants. Infant Behavior & Development, 23, Linguistic theory: Extensions and implications (pp. 268–284).
299–315. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Bertenthal, B. I., Rose, J. L., & Bai, D. L. (1997). Perception-action Bickerton, D. (2007). Language evolution: A brief guide for linguists.
coupling in the development of visual control of posture. Journal Lingua, 117, 510–526
of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, Biederman, J., & Faraone, S. V. (2005). Attention-deficit hyperactiv-
1631–1643. ity disorder. Lancet, 366, 237–248.
Bertenthal, B., & Clifton, R. (1998). Perception and action. In Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Keenan, K., Knee, D., & Tsuang, M. T.
W. Damon (Series Ed.) & D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), (1990). Family-genetic and psychosocial risk factors in DSM-III
REFERENCES R-11
attention deficit disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child Bjorklund, D. F., & Douglas, R. N. (1997). The development of mem-
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 526–533. ory strategies. In N. Cowan (Ed.), The development of memory in
Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Milberger, S., Curtis, S., Chen, L., childhood (pp. 201–246). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Marrs, A., Ouellette, C., Moore, P., & Spencer, T. (1996). Bjorklund, D. F., & Green, B. L. (1992). The adaptive nature of cog-
Predictors of persistence and remission of ADHD into adolescence: nitive immaturity. American Psychologist, 47, 46–54.
Results from a four-year prospective follow-up study. Journal of the Bjorklund, D. F., & Harnishfeger, K. K. (1987). Developmental dif-
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 343–351. ferences in the mental effort requirements for the use of an orga-
Biederman J., Faraone S. V., Mick, E., Spencer T., Wilens, T., nizational strategy in free recall. Journal of Experimental Child
Kiely, K., Guite, J., Ablon, J. S., Reed, E., & Warburton, R. (1995). Psychology, 44, 109–125.
High risk for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among chil- Björklund, F., & Hursti, T. J. (2004). A Swedish translation and
dren of parents with childhood onset of the disorder: A pilot study. validation of the Disgust Scale: A measure of disgust sensitivity.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 431–435. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 45, 279–284.
Biederman, J., Milberger, S., Faraone, S. V., Kiely, K., Guite, J., Black, J. E., & Greenough, W. T. (1986). Induction of pattern in neu-
Mick, E., Ablon, S., Warburton, R., & Reed, E. (1995). Family- ral structure by experience: Implications for cognitive develop-
environment risk factors for attention deficit hyperactivity ment. In M. E. Lamb, A. L. Brown, & B. Rogoff (Eds.), Advances
disorder: A test of Rutter’s indicators of adversity. Archives of in developmental psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 1–50). Hillsdale NJ:
General Psychiatry, 52, 464–470. Erlbaum.
Bierman, K. L. (2004). Peer rejections: Developmental processes and Black, J. E., Isaacs, K. R., Anderson, B. J., Alcantara, A. A., &
intervention strategies. New York: Guilford Press. Greenough, W. T. (1990). Learning causes synaptogenesis, whereas
Bierwisch, M. (1970). Semantics. In J. Lyons (Ed.), New horizons in motor activity causes angiogenesis, in cerebellar cortex of adult
linguistics (Vol. 1, pp.166–84). Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin. rats. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 87,
Bigler, R. S., Averhart, C. J., & Liben, L. S. (2003). Race and the 5568–5572.
work force: Occupational status, aspirations, and stereotyping Black, S. E., Devereux, P. J., & Salvanes, K. J. (2005).The more the
among African American children. Developmental Psychology, 39, merrier? The effect of family size and birth order on children’s edu-
572–580. cation. Quarterly Journal of Economics 120, 669–700.
Bigler, R. S., Jones, L. C., & Lobliner, D. B. (1997). Social categoriza- Black, S. E., Devereux, P. J., & Salvanes, K. G. (2011) Older and
tion and the formation of intergroup attitudes in children. Child wiser? Birth order and IQ of young men. CESifo Economic Studies,
Development, 58, 530–543. 57, 103–120.
Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental intergroup the- Blair R. J. R. (2008). The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal
ory: Explaining and reducing children’s social stereotyping and cortex: Functional contributions and dysfunction in psychopathy.
prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 162–166. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 2557–2565.
Binet, A. (1903). L’etude expérimentale de l’ intelligence [Experimental Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion
studies in intelligence]. Paris: Schleicher frères. in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s functioning at
Birch E., & Petrig, B. (1996). FPL and VEP measures of fusion, stereopsis school entry. American Psychologist, 57, 111–127.
and stereoacuity in normal infants. Vision Research, 36, 1321–1327. Blair, C. (2006). How similar are fluid cognition and general intel-
Birch, S. A., & Bloom, P. (2004). Understanding children’s and ligence? A developmental neuroscience perspective on fluid cogni-
adults’ limitations in mental state reasoning. Trends in Cognitive tion as an aspect of human cognitive ability. Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 8, 255–260. Sciences, 29, 109–125.
Birch, S. A. J., & Bloom, P. (2007). The curse of knowledge in reason- Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, execu-
ing about false beliefs. Psychological Science, 18, 382–386. tive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and
Birdsong, D., & Molis, M. (2001). On the evidence for maturational literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78, 647–663.
constraints in second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Blair, R. J. R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality:
Language, 44, 235–249. Investigating the psychopath. Cognition, 57, 1–29.
Biro, S., & Leslie, A. M. (2007). Infants’ perception of goal- Blair, R. J. R. (1997). Moral reasoning and the child with psychopathic
directed actions: Development through cue-based bootstrapping. tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 731–739.
Developmental Science, 10, 379–398. Blair, R. J. R. (1999). Responsiveness to distress cues in the child with
Bisanz, J., Morrison, F. J., & Dunn, M. (1995). Effects of age and psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 27,
schooling on the acquisition of elementary quantitative skills. 135–145.
Developmental Psychology, 31, 221–236. Blair, R. J. R. (2003). Neurobiological basis of psychopathy. British
Bishop, D. V. M. (2006).What causes specific language impairment in Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 5–7.
children? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 217–221. Blair, R. J. R. (2005). Responding to the emotions of others:
Bishop D. V. M. (2009). Genes, cognition and communication: Insights Dissociating forms of empathy through the study of typical
from neurodevelopmental disorders. The year in cognitive neurosci- and psychiatric populations. Consciousness and Cognition: An
ence. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 1–18. International Journal, 14, 698–718.
Björklund, A., Ginther, D. K., & Sundström, M. (2004). Family Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Fine cuts of empathy and the amygdala:
structure and child outcomes in the United States and Sweden. IZA Dissociable deficits in psychopathy and autism. Quarterly Journal
Discussion Papers, 1259. Bonn, Germany: Institute for the Study of Experimental Psychology, 61, 157.
of Labor (IZA). Blair, R. J. R., & Blair, K. S. (2009). Empathy, morality, and social
Bjorklund, A., & Lindvall, O. (2000). Self-repair in the brain. Nature, convention: Evidence from the study of psychopathy and other
405, 892–895. psychiatric disorders. In J. Decety & W. Ickes (Eds.), The social
Bjorklund, D. F. (Ed.). (2000). False-memory creation in children and neuroscience of empathy (pp. 139–152). Cambridge, MA: MIT
adults: Theory, research, and implications. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Press.
R-12 REFERENCES
Blair, R. J. R., & Coles, M. (2000). Expression recognition and behav- Bloom, L., Lightbown, P., & Hood, M. (1975). Structure and varia-
ioural problems in early adolescence. Cognitive Development, 15, tion in child language. Monographs of the Society for Research in
421–434. Child Development, 40, 1–97.
Blair, R. J. R., Colledge, E., Murray, L., & Mitchell, D. G. V. (2001). Bloom, L., Tinker, E., & Margulis, C. (1993). The words children
A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expres- learn: Evidence against a noun bias in children’s vocabularies.
sions in children with psychopathic tendencies. Journal of Abnormal Cognitive Development, 8, 431–450.
Child Psychology, 29, 491- 498. Bloom, P. (1994). Generativity within language and other cognitive
Blais, J. J. (2008). Chatting, befriending, and bullying: Adolescent domains. Cognition, 51, 177–189.
Internet experiences and associated psychosocial outcomes. Ph.D. Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words.
Thesis, Queens University. http://hdl.handle.net/1974/1413. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Blais, J. J., Craig., W. M., Pepler, D. J., & Connolly, J. (2008). Bloom, P. (2005). Word learning, intentions, and discourse. Journal of
Adolescents online: The importance of Internet activity choices to the Learning Sciences, 14, 311–314.
salient relationships. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37, 522–536. Bloom, P. (2010). The moral life of babies. New York Times Sunday
Blakemore, C., & Cooper, G. F. (1970). Development of the brain Magazine, May 5.
depends on the visual environment. Nature, 228, 477–478. Bloom, P. (2011). Family, community, trolley problems, and the crisis
Blakemore, S. J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nature in moral psychology. Yale Review, 99, 26–43.
Reviews: Neuroscience, 9, 267–277. Bloom, P. (2013). Just babies: The origins of good and evil. New York:
Blanchard-Fields, F. (2007). Everyday problem solving and emo- Crown.
tion: An adult developmental perspective. Current Directions in Bloom, P., & German, T. P. (2000). Two reasons to abandon
Psychological Science, 16, 26–31. the false belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition, 77,
Blanchard-Fields, F., & Hess, T. M. (Eds.). (1996). Perspectives on cog- B25–B31.
nitive change in adulthood and aging. New York: McGraw-Hill. Bloom, P., & Gleitman, L. (1999). Language acquisition. In R. A.
Blanchard-Fields, F., Mienaltowski, A., & Seay, R. B. (2007). Age Wilson & F. C. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive
differences in everyday problem-solving effectiveness: Older adults sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
select more effective strategies for interpersonal problems. Journals Bloom, P., & Keil, F. C. (2001). Thinking through language. Mind
of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, and Language, 16, 351–367.
62B, P61–P64. Bloom, P., & Wynn, K. (1997). Linguistic cues in the acquisition of
Blaschke, A. J., Staley, K., & Chun, J. (1996). Widespread pro- number words. Journal of Child Language, 24, 511–533.
grammed cell death in proliferative and postmitotic regions of the Bloome, D., & Lemke, J. (Eds.). (1995). Special Issue: Africanized
fetal cerebral cortex. Development, 122, 1165–1174. English and education. Linguistics and Education, 7, 1–81.
Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1991). Measures of self-esteem. In J. Blossfeld, I., Collins, A., Boland, S., Baixauli, R., Kiely, M., &
Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of Delahunty, C. (2007). Relationships between acceptance of sour
personality and social psychological attitudes (Vol. 1, pp. 115–160). tastes and fruit intakes in 18-month-old infants. British Journal of
San Diego: Academic Press. Nutrition, 98, 1084–1091.
Blasi, A. (1980). Bridging moral cognition and moral action: A critical Blumberg, H. P. (2007). Dimensions in the development of bipolar
review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 1–45. disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 104–106.
Blass, E. M., Ganchrow, J. R., & Steiner, J. E. (1984). Classical con- Boden, M. A. (1989). Artificial intelligence and biological intel-
ditioning in newborn humans 2–48 hours of age. Infant Behavior ligence. In M. Boden (Ed.), Artificial intelligence in psychology
and Development, 7, 223–235. (pp. 125–162). Cambridge, MA: Bradford/ MIT Press.
Blass, E., & Hoppmeyer, L. (1991). Sucrose as an analgesic for new- Boden, M. A. (1992). The creative mind: Myths and mechanisms. New
born infants. Pediatrics, 87, 215–218. York: Basic Books.
Blatt, S. J., Wein, S. J., Chevron, E., & Quinlan, D. M. (1979). Bogdan, R., Williamson, D. E., & Hariri, A. R. (2012). Mineralocor-
Parental representations and depression in normal young adults. ticoid receptor Iso/Val (rs5522) genotype moderates the association
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 388–397. between previous childhood emotional neglect and amygdala reac-
Blazer, D. G. (2003). Depression in late life: Review and commentary. tivity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 515–522.
Journals of Gerontology, Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Bohannon, J. N., & Stanowicz, L. (1988). The issue of negative evi-
Sciences, 58A, M249–M265. dence: Adult responses to children’s language errors. Developmental
Bleakley, A., Jamieson, P. E., & Romer, D. (2012). Trends of sexual and Psychology, 24, 684–689.
violent content by gender in top-grossing U.S. films, 1950–2006. Bokhorst, C. L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Pasco Fearon, R. M.,
Journal of Adolescent Health, 51, 73–79. Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Fonagy, P., & Schuengel, C. (2003).
Bleuler, E. (1911/1950). Dementia præcox or the group of schizophre- The importance of shared environment in mother-infant attach-
nias [Dementia praecox oder Gruppe der Schizophrenien]. New York: ment security: A behavioral genetic study. Child Development, 74,
International Universities Press. 1769–1782.
Bloch, M. H., Landeros-Weisenberger, A., Rosario, M. C., Boland, A. M., Haden, C. A., & Ornstein, P. A. (2003). Boosting
Pittenger, C., & Leckman, J. F. (2008). Meta-analysis of the symp- children’s memory by training mothers in the use of an elaborative
tom structure of obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of conversational style as an event unfolds. Journal of Cognition and
Psychiatry, 165, 1532–1542. Development, 4, 39–65.
Bloom, A. H. (1981). The linguistic shaping of thought: A study in the Bonatti, L. L., Frot, E., & Mehler, J. (2005). What face inversion does
impact of language on thinking in China and the West. Hillsdale, to infants’ counting abilities. Psychological Science, 16, 506–510.
NJ: Erlbaum. Bonatti, L., Frot, E., Zangl, R., & Mehler, J. (2002). The human first
Bloom, L. (1970). Language development: Form and function in emerg- hypothesis: Identification of conspecifics and individuation of
ing grammars. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. objects in the young infant. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 388–426.
REFERENCES R-13
Boney-McCoy, S., & Finkelhor, D. (1995). Psychosocial sequelae Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? Mandarin and
of violent victimization in a national youth sample. Journal of English speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43,
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 726–736. 1–22.
Boney-McCoy, S., & Finkelhor, D. (1996). Is youth victimization Boroditsky, L. (2011, February). How language shapes thought: The
related to trauma symptoms and depression after controlling for languages we speak affect our perceptions of the world. Scientific
prior symptoms and family relationships? A longitudinal, pro- American, 304, 63–65.
spective study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, Boroditsky, L., Schmidt, L.A., & Phillips, W. (2003). Sex, syntax and
1406–1416. semantics. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language
Boo, K. (2006). Swamp nurse. The New Yorker, February 6. in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 61–78).
Boom, J, Brugman, D., & van der Heijden, P. G. M. (2001). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hierarchical structure of moral stages assessed by a sorting task. Bos, A., Huijding, J., Muris, P., Vogel, L., & Biesheuvel, J. (2010).
Child Development, 72, 535–548. Global, contingent, and implicit self-esteem and psychopathologi-
Boorstin, D. (1983). The discoverers. New York: Random House. cal symptoms in adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences,
Booth, A., Waxman, S. R., & Huang, Y. T. (2005). Conceptual 48, 311–316.
information permeates word learning in infancy. Developmental Bos, K. J., Zeanah, C. H. Jr, Smyke, A. T., Fox, N. A., & Nelson,
Psychology, 41, 491–505. C. A. III. (2010). Stereotypies in children with a history of early
Borer, H., & Wexler, K. (1987).The maturation of syntax. In institutional care. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine,
T. Roeper & E. 164, 406–411.
Borkowski, J. G., Ramey, S. L., & Bristol-Power, M. (Eds.). (2002). Bos, P. A., Panksepp, J., Bluthé, R. M., & Honk, J. V. (2012). Acute
Parenting and the child’s world: Influences on academic, intellectual, effects of steroid hormones and neuropeptides on human social-
and social-emotional development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. emotional behavior: A review of single administration studies.
Bornstein, D. (2011). Hard times for recess. New York Times. Retrieved Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 33, 17–35.
April 10, 2013. http://opinionator.blogs.nytimes.com/2011/04/04 Bosacki, S., & Astington, J. W. (1999). Theory of mind in preadoles-
/hard-times-for-recess/ cence: Relations between social understanding and social compe-
Bornstein, M. H. (1975). Qualities of color vision in infancy. Journal tence. Social Development, 8, 237–255.
of Experimental Child Psychology, 19, 401–419. Bostic, J. Q., Muriel, A. C., Hack, S., Weinstein, S., & Herzog, D.
Bornstein, M. H. (1987). Perceptual categories in vision and in audi- (1997). Anorexia nervosa in a 7-year-old girl. Journal of
tion. In S. Harnad (Ed.), Categorical perception (pp. 287–300). Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 18, 331–333.
New York: Cambridge University Press. Bouchard, T. J., & McGue, M. (2003). Genetic and environmen-
Bornstein, M. H. (1994a). Infancy. In R. H. Sternberg (Ed.), tal influences on human psychological differences. Journal of
Encyclopedia of human intelligence (Vol. 1, pp. 570–575). New Neurobiology, 54, 4–45.
York: Macmillan. Boucher, B. H., & Doescher, S. M. (1991). Influencing preschool
Bornstein, M. H. (1994b). Alfred Binet, the origins of mental test- children’s motor development: A comparison of two groups. Early
ing, and the consequences for understanding the development of Child Development and Care, 77, 67–76.
human intelligence. In P. Fraisse & J. Segui (Eds.), Les origines Bouma, G., Malouf, R., & Sag, I. A. (2001). Satisfying constraints on
de la pspychologie scientifique: Centenaire de l’année psychologique extraction and adjunction. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory,
(pp. 145–161). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. 19, 1–65.
Bornstein, M. H. (1997). Selective vision. Behavioral and Brain Bouvier, P., Halperin, D., Rey, H., Jaffe, P. D., Laederach, J.,
Sciences, 20, 180–181. Mounoud, R. L., & Pawlak, C. (1999). Typology and correlates
Bornstein, M. H. (2006). Parenting science and practice. In of sexual abuse in children and youth: Multivariate analyses
W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.) & I. E. Sigel & K. A. in a prevalence study in Geneva. Child Abuse & Neglect, 23,
Renninger (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Child 779–790.
psychology and practice (6th ed., pp. 893–949). New York: Wiley. Bowden, C.L. (2005). A different depression: Clinical distinctions
Bornstein, M. H., Arterberry, M. E., & Mash, C. (2010). Infant object between bipolar and unipolar depression. Journal of Affective
categorization transcends diverse object-context relations. Infant Disorders, 84, 117–125.
Behavior & Development, 33, 7–15. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36,
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C. S., Bell, C., Haynes, O. M., Slater, A., 129–148.
Golding, J. & Wolke, D. (2006). Stability in cognition across early Bower, T. G. (1979). Human development. San Francisco: Freeman.
childhood: A developmental cascade. Psychological Science, 17, Bower, T. G. R. (1971). The object in the world of the infant. Scientific
151–158. American, 275, 30–38.
Bornstein, M. H., Kessen, W., & Weiskopf, S. (1976). Color vision Bower, T. G. R. (1974). Development in infancy. San Francisco:
and hue categorization in young human infants. Journal of Freeman.
Experimental Psychology, 2,115–119. Bowlby, J. (1951). Maternal care and mental health. Bulletin of the
Bornstein, M. H., Suwalsky, J. T. D., & Breakstone, D. A. (2012). World Health Organization, 3, 355–534.
Emotional relationships between mothers and infants: Knowns, Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child’s ties to his mother.
unknowns, and unknown unknowns. Development and International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 39, 350–373.
Psychopathology, 24, 113–123. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. London:
Bornstein, M. N. (1989). Stability in early mental development: From Hogarth.
attention and information processing in infancy to language and Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and
cognition in childhood. In M. H. Bornstein & N. A. Krasnegor anger. London: Hogarth.
(Eds.), Stability and continuity in mental development. Hillsdale, Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds.
NJ: Erlbaum. London: Tavistock.
R-14 REFERENCES
Bowman, C. G., & Mertz, E. (1996). A dangerous direction: Legal Brannon, E. M. (2005). The independence of language and math-
intervention in sexual abuse survivor therapy. Harvard Law ematical reasoning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Review, 109, 549–639. of the USA, 102, 3177–3178.
Boyatzis, R. E., Good, D., & Massa, R. (2012). Emotional, social, and Brannon, E. M., Wusthoff, C. J., Gallistel, C. R., & Gibbon, J. (2001).
cognitive intelligence and personality as predictors of sales leader- Numerical subtraction in the pigeon: Evidence for a linear subjec-
ship performance. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, tive number scale. Psychological Science, 12, 238–243.
19, 191–201. Brans, R. G., Kahn, R. S., Schnack, H. G., van Baal, G. C., Posthuma, D.,
Boykin, A. W. (1994). Harvesting talent and culture: African-American van Haren, N. E., Lepage, C., Lerch, J. P., Collins, D. L., Evans,
children and educational reform. In R. Rossi (Ed.), Schools and stu- A. C., Boomsma, D. I., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2010). Brain plastic-
dents at risk (pp. 116–138). New York: Teachers College Press. ity and intellectual ability are influenced by shared genes. Journal of
Boykin, A. W., Tyler, K. M., & Miller, O. (2005). In search of cultural Neuroscience, 30, 5519–5524.
themes and their expressions in the dynamics of classroom life. Bransford, J. D., Brown, A. L., & Cocking, R. (1999). How people
Urban Education, 40, 521–549. learn: Brain, mind, experience, and school. Washington, DC:
Boynton, R. M. (1979) Human color vision. New York: Holt, National Research Council.
Rinehart, & Winston. Brauer, J., Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006).
Boysen, S. T., & Himes, G. T. (1999). Current issues and emerging the- Making inferences about the location of hidden food: Social dog,
ories in animal cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 683–705. causal ape. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120, 38–47.
Brackett, M. A., Bertoli, M., Elbertson, N., Bausseron, E., Castillo, R., & Braungart, J. M., & Stifter, C. A. (1991). Regulation of negative reac-
Salovey, P. (2013). Emotional intelligence: Reconceptualizing the tivity during the strange situation: Temperament and attachment
cognition-emotion link. In M.D. Robinson, E. R. Watkins, & in 12-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 14,
E. Harmon-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion 349–364.
(pp. 365–379. New York: Guilford Press. Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Garwood, M. M., Powers, B. P., & Wang, X.
Braddick, O., &. Atkinson, J. (1988). Sensory selectivity, attentional (2001). Parental sensitivity, infant affect, and affect regulation:
control, and cross-channel integration in early visual development. Predictors of later attachment. Child Development, 72, 252–270.
In A. Yonas (Ed.), Perceptual development in infancy. Hillsdale, NJ: Brazelton, T. B., & Nugent, J. K. (1995). Neonatal behavioral assess-
Erlbaum. ment scale (3rd ed.). London: MacKeith Press.
Braden, J. (1994). Deafness, deprivation and IQ. New York: Plenum Breedlove, S. M., & Hampson, E. (2002). Sexual differentiation
Press. of the brain and behavior. In J. B. Becker, S. M. Breedlove, D.
Bradley, C. (1937). The behavior of children receiving benzedrine. Crews, & M. M. McCarthy (Eds.), Behavioral endocrinology (2nd
American Journal of Psychiatry, 94, 577–588. ed., pp. 75–114). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bradshaw, K. (1999, November 17). Michigan boy who killed at 11 is Breggin, P. (1998). Talking back to Ritalin. Monroe, ME: Common
convicted of murder as adult. New York Times, p. A1. Courage Press.
Brady, E. U., & Kendall, P. C. (1992). Comorbidity of anxiety and Bremner, J. G. (1985). Object tracking and search in infancy: A review
depression in children and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin, 111, of data and a theoretical evaluation. Developmental Review, 5,
244–255. 371–396.
Braine, M. D. S. (1971). On two types of models of the internaliza- Brendgen, M., Dionne, G., Girard, A., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., &
tion of grammars. In D. Slobin (Ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar Pérusse, D. (2005). Examining genetic and environmental
(pp. 153–186). New York: Academic Press. effects on social aggression: A study of 6-year-old twins. Child
Braine, M. D. S. (1976). Children’s first word combinations. Monographs Development, 76, 930–946.
of the Society for Research in Child Development, 41, pp. 1–104. Brennan, P., & Walker, E. (2000). Vulnerability to schizophrenia in
Brainerd, C. (1978). Piaget’s theory of intelligence. Englewood Cliffs, childhood. In R. Ingram & J. M. Price (Eds.), Handbook of vul-
NJ: Prentice-Hall. nerability to psychopathology: Risk across the life-span. New York:
Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2005). The science of false memory. Guilford Press.
New York: Oxford University Press. Brennan, T. (Ed.). (1989). Between feminism and psychoanalysis. New
Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Ceci, S. J. (2008). Developmental York: Routledge.
reversals in false memory: A review of data and theory. Psychological Brent, D.A., & Maalouf, F.T. (2009). Pediatric depression: Is there
Bulletin, 134, 343–382. evidence to improve evidence-based treatments? Journal of Child
Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Zember, E. (2011). Theoretical and Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 143–152.
forensic implications of developmental studies of the DRM illu- Brentari, D., & Coppola, M. (2013). What sign language cre-
sion. Memory & Cognition, 39, 365–380. ation teaches us about language. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews:
Bramlett, M. D., & Mosher, W. D. (2002). Cohabitation, marriage, Cognitive Science, 4, 117–236.
divorce, and remarriage in the United States. National Survey of Bretherton, I. (1992). The origins of attachment theory: John Bowlby
Family Growth, National Center for Health Statistics. Vital and and Mary Ainsworth. Developmental Psychology, 28, 759–775.
Health Statistics, 23, 1–93. Bretherton, L., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states:
Branchi, I. (2009). The mouse communal nest: Investigating the epi- The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental
genetic influences of the early social environment on brain and Psychology, 18, 906–921.
behavior development. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, Brewer, M. B, & Chen, Y. R. (2006). Where (who) are collectives in
551–559. collectivism? Toward conceptual clarification of individualism and
Brand, C. R., Egan, V., & Deary, I. J. (1994). Intelligence, personal- collectivism. Psychological Review, 114, 131–151.
ity and society: Constructivist versus essentialist possibilities. In Brewer, S., & Williams, T. (2004). Finally, a sense of closure?
D. K. Detterman (Ed.), Current topics in human intelligence: Vol. 4. Animal models of human ventral body wall defects. BioEssays, 26,
Theories in intelligence (pp. 29–42). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. 1307–1321.
REFERENCES R-15
Brewer, W. F., & Samarapungavan, A. (1991). Children’s theories ver- Brooks-Gunn, J., & Warren, M. P. (1989). Biological and social
sus scientific theories: Differences in reasoning or differences in contributions to negative affect in young adolescent girls. Child
knowledge? In R. R Hoffman & D. S. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition Development, 60, 40–55.
and the symbolic processes: Vol. 3. Applied and ecological perspectives Brossard, L. M., & Decarie, T. G. (1968). Comparative reinforcing effect
(pp. 209–232). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. of eight stimulations on the smiling response of infants. Journal of
Brewer, W. F., Chinn, C. A., & Samarapungavan, A. (2000). Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 9, 51–59.
Explanation in scientists and children. In F. C. Keil & R. A. Wilson Brotman, M. A., Guyer, A. E., Lawson, E. S., Horsey, S. E., Rich,
(Eds.), Explanation and cognition (pp. 279–298). Cambridge, MA: B. A., Dickstein, D. P., Pine, D. S., & Leibenluft, E. (2008). Facial
MIT Press. emotion labeling deficits in children and adolescents at risk for
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Gotlib, I. H. (1993). Psychopathology bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 385–389.
and early experience: A reappraisal of retrospective reports. Browder, L. W., Erickson, C. A., & Jeffery, W. R. (1991). Developmental
Psychological Bulletin, 113, 82–98. biology (3rd ed.). Philadelphia: Saunders.
Bridges, K. M. (1932). Emotional development in early infancy. Child Brower, T., & Wilcox, T. (2012). Shaking things up: Young infants’
Development, 3, 324–341. use of sound information for object individuation. Infant Behavior
Bristol, M. M., Cohen, D. J., Costello, E. J., Denckla, M., Eckberg, and Development, 35, 323–327.
T. J., Kallen, R., Kraemer, H. C., Lord, C. Maurer, R., McIlvane, Brown, A. A., Spetch, M. L., & Hurd, P. L. (2007). Growing in circles:
W. J., Minshew, N., Sigman, M., & Spence, M. A. (1996). State of Rearing environment alters spatial navigation in fish. Psychological
the science in autism: Report to the National Institutes of Health. Science, 18, 569–573.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 121–154. Brown, A. L. (1992). Design experiments: Theoretical and method-
Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. London: ological challenges in creating complex interventions in classroom
Pergamon Press. settings. Journal of theLearning Sciences, 2, 141–178.
Brody, G. H. (1998). Sibling relationship quality: Its causes and con- Brown, A. L., & Campione, J. C. (1994). Guided discovery in a
sequences. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 1–24. community of learners. In K. McGilly (Ed.), Classroom lessons:
Brody, G. H. (2004). Siblings’ direct and indirect contributions to Integrating theory and classroom practice (pp. 229–270). Cambridge,
child development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, MA: MIT Press.
124–126. Brown, A. L., & DeLoache, J. S. (1978). Skills, plans, and self-
Brody, G. H., Kim, S., Murry, V. M., & Brown, A. C. (2003). regulation. In R. S. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s learning: What devel-
Longitudinal direct and indirect pathways linking older sibling ops? (pp. 3–35). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
competence to the development of younger sibling competence. Brown, A. L., & Kane, M. J. (1988). Preschool children can learn to
Developmental Psychology, 39, 618–628. transfer: Learning to learn and learning from example. Cognitive
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1970). Two worlds of childhood: U.S. and U.S.S.R. Psychology, 20, 493–523.
New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Brown, A. M., & Lindsey, D. T. (2009). Contrast insensitivity: The
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of critical immaturity in infant visual performance. Optometry and
human development. American Psychologist, 32, 513–531. Visual Science, 86, 572–576.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Brown, A. S., Begg, M. D., Gravenstein, S., Schaefer, C. A., Wyatt,
Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard R. J., Bresnahan, M., Babulas, V. P., & Susser, E. S. (2004).
University Press. Serologic evidence of prenatal influenza in the etiology of schizo-
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human phrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 774–780.
development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22, Brown, A., & Campione, J. (1994). Guided discovery in a commu-
723–742. nity of learners. In K. McGilly (Ed.), Classroom lessons: Integrating
Bronson, G. W. (1972). Infants’ reactions to unfamiliar persons cognitive theory and classroom practice (pp. 229–270). Cambridge,
and novel objects. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child MA: MIT Press.
Development, 37, 1–46. Brown, B. B., & Klute, C. (2003). Friends, cliques, and crowds. In
Bronstein, P. (1999). Differences in mothers’ and fathers’ behaviors G. R. Adams & M. D. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of
towards children: A cross-cultural comparison. In L. A. Peplau, adolescence (pp. 330–348). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
S. C. DeBro, R. C. Veniegas, & P. L. Taylor (Eds.), Gender, culture, Brown, B. B., Mounts, N., Lamborn, S. D., & Steinberg, L. (1993).
and ethnicity: Current research about women and men (pp. 70–82). Parenting practices and peer group affiliation in adolescence. Child
Mountain View, CA: Mayfield. Development 64, 467–482.
Brooks, N., Pogue, A., & Barner, D. (2011). Piecing together numeri- Brown, D. E., & Hammer, D. (2008). Conceptual change in physics.
cal language: Children’s use of default units in early counting and In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research on concep-
quantification. Developmental Science, 14, 44–57. tual change (pp. 127–154). New York: Routledge.
Brooks-Gunn, J., Graber, J. A., & Paikoff, R. L. (1994). Studying Brown, F. R., Voigt, R. G., & Elksnin, N. (1996). AD/HD: A neu-
links between hormones and negative affect: Models and mea- rodevelopmental perspective. Contemporary Pediatrics, 13, 25–44.
sures. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 4, 469–486. Brown, J. R., & Dunn, J. (1996). Continuities in emotion under-
Brooks-Gunn J., Klebanov, P. K., & Duncan, G. J. (1996). Ethnic standing from 3 to 6 years. Child Development, 67, 789–802.
differences in children’s intelligence test scores: Role of economic Brown, K. (2003). Psychopharmacology: The medication merry-go-
deprivation, home environment, and maternal characteristics. round. Science, 299, 1646–1649.
Child Development, 6, 396–408. Brown, L. S. (1994). Subversive dialogues: Theory in feminist therapy.
Brooks-Gunn, J., & Markman, L. (2005). The contribution of par- New York: Basic Books.
enting to ethnic and racial gaps in school readiness. Future of Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in
Children, 15, 139–168. language usage. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
R-16 REFERENCES
Brown, R. (1958a). Words and things. New York: Free Press. Essays in neuroeducation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Brown, R. (1958b). How shall a thing be called? Psychological Review, Press.
65, 14–21. Brumariu, L. E., & Kerns, K. A. (2010). Parent-child attachment and
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: internalizing symptoms in childhood and adolescence: A review
Harvard University Press. of empirical findings and future directions. Development and
Brown, R. D., Goldstein, E., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2000). The history Psychopathology, 22, 177–203.
and zeitgeist of the repressed/false-memory debate: Scientific and Brumberg, J. J. (1988). Fasting girls: The emergence of anorexia nervosa
sociological perspectives on suggestibility and childhood memory. as a modern disease. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
In D. F. Bjorklund (Ed.), False-memory creation in children and Bruner, J. S. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child
adults: Theory, research, and implications (pp.1–30). Mahwah, NJ: Language, 2, 1–40.
Erlbaum. Bruner, J. S. (1976). From communication to language: A psychologi-
Brown, R. T. (1989). Creativity: What are we to measure? In J. A. cal perspective. Cognition, 3, 255–287.
Glover, R. R. Ronning, & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New York:
creativity (pp. 3–32). New York: Plenum Press. W. W. Norton.
Brown, R. W. (1958). Words and things. Glencoe, IL: Free Press. Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
Brown, R., & Hanlon, C. (1970). Derivational complexity and the University Press.
order of acquisition of child speech. In J. R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition Bruner, J. (1995). From joint attention to the meeting of minds: An
and the development of language. New York: Wiley. introduction. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its
Brown, R., & Lenneberg, E. H. (1954). A study in language and cog- origins and role in development (pp. 1–14). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
nition. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 454–462. Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., & Austin, G. A. (1956). A study of think-
Browne, A., & Finkelhor, D. (1986). Impact of sexual abuse: A review ing. New York: Wiley.
of the research. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 66–77. Bruner, J. S., Oliver, R. R., & Greenfield, P. M. (Eds.). (1966). Studies
Brownell, K. D., & Foreyt, J. P. (1986). Handbook of eating disorders: in cognitive growth. New York: Wiley.
Physiology, psychology and treatment of obesity, anorexia and buli- Bruner, J. S., & Olver, R. R. (1963). Development of equivalence
mia. New York: Basic Books. transformations in children. Monographs for the Society of Research
Bruch, H. (1973). Eating disorders: Obesity, anorexia nervosa, and the in Child Development, 28, 125–143.
person within. New York: Basic Books. Bryan, W. L., & Harter, N. (1899). Studies on the telegraphic lan-
Bruck, M., Ceci, S. J., & Hembrooke, H. (2002). The nature of guage: The acquisition of a hierarchy of habits. Psychological
children’s true and false narratives. Developmental Review, 22, Review, 6, 345–375.
520–554. Bryant, P. E., & Trabasso, T. (1971). Transitive inferences and mem-
Bruck, M., Ceci, S. J., Francoeur, E., & Barr, R. (1995). “I hardly cried ory in young children. Nature, 232, 456–458.
when I got my shot!” Influencing children’s reports about a visit to Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (1998). Epidemiology of autism:
their pediatrician. Child Development, 66, 193–208. Prevalence, associated characteristics, and implications for research
Bruder, C. E., Piotrowski, A., Gijsbers, A., Andersson, R., Erickson, S., Diaz and service delivery. Mental Retardation and Developmental
de Stahl, T. D., Menzel, U., Sandgren, J., von Tell, D., Poplawski, A., Disabilities Research Reviews, 4, 97–103.
Crowley, M., Crasto, C., Partridge, E. C., Tiwari, H., Allison, D. B., Bschor, T. (2002). Larvierte depression: Aufstieg und fall einer diagnose
Komorowski, J., van Ommen, G. J., Boomsra, D. I., Petersen, N. L., den [Masked depression: The rise and fall of a diagnosis]. Psychiatrische
Dunnen, J. T., Wirdefeldt, K., & Dumanski, J. P. (2008). Phenotypically Praxis, 29, 207–210.
concordant and discordant monozygotic twins display different DNA Buchanan, C. M., Eccles, J. S., & Becker, J. B. (1992). Are adolescents
copy-number-variation profiles. American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, the victims of raging hormones? Evidence for activational effects
763–771. of hormones on moods and behavior at adolescence. Psychological
Bruer, J. (1999). The myth of the first three years: A new understanding of Bulletin, 111, 62–107.
early brain development and lifelong learning. New York: Free Press. Buchanan, C. M., Maccoby, E. E., & Dornbusch, S. (1991). Caught
Bruer, J. T. (1993a). Schools for thought: A science of learning in the between parents: Adolescents’ experience in divorced homes. Child
classroom. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Development, 62, 1008–1029.
Bruer, J. T. (1993b). The mind’s journey from novice to expert. Buchanan, C. M., Maccoby, E. E., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1996).
American Educator, 17, 6–15, 38–46. Adolescents after divorce. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Bruer, J. T. (1997). Education and the brain: A bridge too far. Press.
Educational Researcher, 26, 4–16. Buckner, J. C., Mezzacappa, E., & Beardslee, W. R. (2003).
Bruer, J. T. (1999). The myth of the first three years: A new understand- Characteristics of resilient youths living in poverty: The role of
ing of early brain development and lifelong learning. New York: Free self-regulatory processes. Development and Psychopathology, 15,
Press. 139–162.
Bruer, J. T. (2002). Avoiding the pediatrician’s error: How neurosci- Bugental, D. B. (2000). Acquisition of the algorithms of social life:
entists can help educators (and themselves). Nature Neuroscience, A domain-based approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 187–219.
5, S1031–S1033. Bugental, D. B., & Goodnow, J. G. (1998). Socialization processes.
Bruer, J. T. (2006). Points of view on the implications of neuroscience In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
research for science teaching and learning: Are there any? A skepti- child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality develop-
cal theme and variations: The primacy of psychology in the science ment (5th ed., pp. 389–462). New York: Wiley.
of learning. CBE Life Sciences Education, 5, 104–110. Bugental, D. B., & Johnston, C. (2000). Parental and child cogni-
Bruer, J. T. (2008). Building bridges in neuroeducation. In A. M. tion in the context of the family. Annual Review of Psychology, 51,
Battro, K. W. Fischer, & P. J. Le?na (Eds.), The educated brain: 315–344.
REFERENCES R-17
Bugental, D. B., Blue, J. B., & Cruzcosa, M. (1989).Perceived con- http://www.bls.gov/news.release/pdf/famee.pdf (Accessed May 14,
trol over caregiving outcomes: Implications for child abuse. 2013)
Developmental Psychology, 25, 532–539. Bureau of Labor Statistics, U.S. Department of Labor. (2011). Women
Bugental, D. B., Blue, J. B., Cortez, V., Fleck, K., Kopeikin, H., at work: Spotlight on statistics. http://www.bls.gov/spotlight/2011
Lewis, J., & Lyon, J. (1993). Social cognitions as organizers of /women/ (Accessed July 16, 2013)
autonomic and affective responses to social challenge. Journal of Burgess, N. (2006). Spatial memory: How egocentric and allocentric
Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 94–103. combine. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 551–557.
Bugental, D., Caporael, L., & Shennum, W. A. (1980). Experimentally Bürglin, T. R. (1996). Homeodomain proteins. In R. A. Meyers,
produced child uncontrollability: Effects on the potency of adult Encyclopedia of molecular biology and molecular medicine (Vol. 3,
communication pattern. Child Development, 51, 520–528. pp. 55–76). Weinheim, Germany: VCH Verlagsgesellschaft mbH.
Bugental, D. B., Lewis, J. C., Lin, E., Lyon, J., & Kopeikin, H. (2000). Burke, A. C., Nelson, C. E., Morgan, B. A., & Tabin, C. (1995).
In charge but not in control: The management of authority-based Hox genes and the evolution of vertebrate axial morphology.
relationships by those with low perceived power. Developmental Development, 121, 333–346.
Psychology, 35, 1367–1378. Burton, R. V. (1963). Generality of honesty reconsidered. Psychological
Bugental, D. B., Lyon, J. E., Lin, E. K., McGrath, E. G., & Bimbela, A. Review, 70, 481–499.
(1999). Children “tune out” in response to the ambiguous commu- Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2001). Media violence and the
nication style of powerless adults. Child Development, 70, 214–230. American public: Scientific facts versus media misinformation.
Buhrmester, M. D., Blanton, H., & Swann, W. B. (2011). Implicit American Psychologist, 56, 477–489.
self-esteem: Nature, measurement, and a new way forward. Journal Bushman, B. J., Baumeister, R. F., & Stack, A. (1999). Catharsis,
of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 365–385. aggression, and persuasive influences: Self-fulfilling or self-
Bukowski, W. (2001). Friendship and the worlds of childhood. In defeating prophecies? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
D.W. Nangle & C.A. Erdley (Eds.), The role of friendship in psycho- 76, 367–376.
logical adjustment: New directions for child and adolescent develop- Bushman, B. J., & Huesmann, L. R. (2006). Short-term and long-
ment (Vol. 91, pp. 93–105). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. term effects of violent media on aggression in children and adults.
Bukowski, W. M., Newcomb, A. F., & Hartup, W. W. (Eds.). (1996). Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 160, 348–352.
The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence. Bushman, B. J., & Huesmann, L. R. (2012). Effects of violent media
New York: Cambridge University Press. on aggression. In D. G. Singer & J. L. Singer (Eds.), Handbook of
Bulik, C. M., Slof-Op’t Landt, M. C. T., van Furth, E. F., & Sullivan, children and the media (2nd ed., pp. 231–248). Thousand Oaks,
P. F. (2007). The genetics of anorexia nervosa. Annual Review of CA: Sage.
Nutrition, 27, 263–275. Bushnell, I. W. R., Sai, F., & Mullin, J. T. (1989). Neonatal rec-
Bulik, C. M., Sullivan, P. F., Carter, F. A., & Joyce, P. R. (1996). ognition of the mother’s face. British Journal of Developmental
Lifetime anxiety disorders in women with bulimia nervosa. Psychology, 7, 3–15.
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 37, 368–374. Busjahn, C. A., Schulz-Menger, J., Abdel-Aty, H., Rudolph, A.,
Bulik, C. M., Sullivan, P. F., Wade, T. D., & Kendler, K. S. (2000). Jordan, J., Luft, F. C., & Busjahn, A. (2009). Heritability of left
Twin studies of eating disorders: A review. International Journal of ventricular and papillary muscle heart size: A twin study with car-
Eating Disorders, 27, 2–20. diac magnetic resonance imaging. European Heart Journal, 30,
Bull, R., & Benson, P. (2006). Digit ratio (2D/4D) and the spatial rep- 1643–1647.
resentation of magnitude. Hormones and Behavior, 50, 194–199. Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing per-
Bull, R., Davidson, W. A., & Nordmann, E. (2010). Prenatal testos- sonality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
terone, visual-spatial memory, and numerical skills in young chil- Buss, D. M., Larsen, R. J., Westen, D., & Semmelroth, J. (1992). Sex
dren. Learning and Individual DifferencesU, 20, 246–250. differences in jealousy: Evolution, physiology, and psychology.
Bullock, M., Gelman, R., & Baillargeon, R. (1982). The development Psychological Science, 3, 251–255.
of causal reasoning. In W. J. Friedman (Ed.), The developmental Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (2008). Attractive women want it
psychology of time (pp. 209–254). New York: Academic Press. all: Good genes, economic investment, parenting proclivities, and
Bumpass, L. L., Raley, R. K., & Sweet, J. A. (1995). The changing emotional commitment. Evolutionary Psychology, 6, 134–146.
character of stepfamilies: Implications of cohabitation and non- Butler, A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman, E. M., & Beck, A. T. (2006).
marital childbearing, Demography, 32, 425–436. The empirical status of cognitive-behavioral therapy: A review of
Burack, C. (1994). The problem of the passions: Feminism, psychoanaly- meta-analyses. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 17–31.
sis, and social theory. New York: New York University Press. Butler, R. (1998). Age trends in the use of social and temporal com-
Burchinal, M. R., & Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (2007). Maternal employ- parison for self-evaluation: Examination of a novel developmental
ment and child cognitive outcomes: The importance of analytic hypothesis. Child Development, 69, 1054–1073.
approach. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1140–1155. Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007).
Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Riggin, R., Zeisel, S., Neebe, E., & Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Developmental
Bryant, D. (2000). Relating quality of center-based child care to Science, 10, F31–F38.
early cognitive and language development longitudinally. Child Butterfill, S. (2007). What are modules and what is their role in devel-
Development, 71, 339–357. opment? Mind & Language, 22, 450–473.
Burdge, G. C., Hanson, M. A., Slater-Jefferies, J. L., & Lillycrop, Butterworth, G. (1977). Object disappearance and error in Piaget’s
K. A. (2007). Epigenetic regulation of transcription: A mechanism stage IV task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 391–401.
for inducing variations in phenotype (fetal programming) by dif- Butterworth, G. (1992). Origins of self-perception in infancy.
ferences in nutrition during early life? British Journal of Nutrition, Psychological Inquiry, 3, 103–111.
97, 1036–1046. Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (1988). Toward the next generation in
Bureau of Labor Statistics, U.S. Department of Labor, The Editor’s data quality: A new survey of primate tactical deception. Behavioral
Desk. (2013). Employment characteristics of families—2012. and Brain Sciences, 11, 267–283.
R-18 REFERENCES
Byrne, R., & Whiten, A. (Eds.) (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: Campbell, D. B. (2010). Advances and challenges in the genetics of
Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and autism. Focus, 8, 339–349.
humans. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Campbell, D. W., & Eaton, W. O. (1999). Sex differences in the activ-
ity level of infants. Infant and Child Development, 8, 1–17.
Cabrera, N. J., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bradley, R. H., Hofferth, Campbell, F. A., & Ramey, C. T. (1994). Effects of early intervention
S., & Lamb, M. E. (2000). Fatherhood in the twenty-first century. on intelligence and academic achievement: A follow-up study of chil-
Child Development, 71, 127–136. dren from low-income families. Child Development, 65, 684–698.
Cacchione, T., Call, J., & Zingg, R. (2009). Gravity and solidity in Campos, J. J., & Barrett, K. C. (1984). Toward a new understanding of
four great ape species (Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus, Pan troglo- emotions and their development. In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. B.
dytes, Pan paniscus): Vertical and horizontal variations of the table Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, cognition, and behavior (pp. 229–263 ).
task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123, 168–180. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Cahyadi, M. V., & Butler, P. H. (2004). Undergraduate students’ Campos, J. J., Barrett, K. C., Lamb, M. E., Goldsmith, H. H., &
understanding of falling bodies in idealized and real-world situa- Stenberg, C. (1983). Socioemotional development. In P. H. Mussen
tions. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 41, 569–583. (Series Ed.) & M. M. Haith & J. J. Campos (Vol. Eds.), Handbook
Cairns, R. B., Cairns, B. D., Neckerman, H. J., Gest, S. D., & Gariepy, of child psychology: Vol. 2. Infancy and developmental psychobiology
J.-L. (1988). Social networks and aggressive behavior: Peer support (4th ed., pp. 783–915). New York: Wiley.
or peer rejection. Developmental Psychopathology, 24, 815–823. Campos, J. J., Frankel, C. B., & Camras, L. (2004). On the nature of
Calamari, J. E., Chik, H. M., Pontarelli, N. K., & DeJong, B. (2012). emotion regulation. Child Development, 75, 377–394.
Phenomenology and epidemiology of obsessive compulsive disorder. Campos, J. J., Hiatt, S., Ramsay, D., Henderson, C., & Svejda, M.
In G. Steketee (Ed.), Oxford handbook of obsessive compulsive and (1978). The emergence of fear on the visual cliff. In M. Lewis &
spectrum disorders (pp. 11–47). New York: Oxford University Press. L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The development of affect (pp. 149–182).
Calders, Y. M., Huston, A. C., & O’Brien, M. (1989). Social inter- New York: Plenum Press.
actions and play patterns of parents and toddlers with feminine, Campos, J. J., Langer, A., & Krowitz, A. (1970). Cardiac responses
masculine, and neutral toys. Child Development, 60, 70–76. on the visual cliff in prelocomotor human infants. Science, 170,
Caldji, C., Tannenbaum, B., Sharma, S., Francis, D., Plotsky, P. M., & 196–197.
Meaney, M. J. (1998). Maternal care during infancy regulates the Campos, J. J., & Stenberg, C. (1981). Perception, appraisal, and emo-
development of neural systems mediating the expression of fearful- tion: The onset of social referencing. In M. E. Lamb & L. R.
ness in the rat. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the Sherrod (Eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical
USA, 95, 5335–5340. considerations (pp. 273–314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Caldwell, B. M. (1970). The effects of psychosocial deprivation on human Campos, J. J., Thein, S., & Owen, D. (2003). A Darwinian legacy
development in infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 16, 260–277. to understanding human infancy: Emotional expressions as behav-
Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A. (1992). The relations among infant tem- ior regulators. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1000,
perament, security of attachment, and behavioral inhibition at 110–134.
twenty-four months. Child Development, 63, 1456–1472. Campos, J. J., Walle, E. A., & Dahl, A. (2010). What is missing in the
Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A. (2002). Self-regulatory processes in study of the development of jealousy? In S. L. Hart & M. Legerstee
early personality development: A multilevel approach to the study (Eds.), Handbook of jealousy: Theory, research, and multidisciplinary
of childhood social withdrawal and aggression. Development and approaches. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
Psychopathology, 14, 477–498. Camras, L. A. (1992). Expressive development and basic emotions.
Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Social cues that chim- Cognition and Emotion, 6, 269–284.
panzees do and do not use to find hidden objects. Animal Cognition Camras, L. A., Malatesta, C., & Izard, C. E. (1991). The development
3, 23–34. of facial expression in infancy. In R. S. Feldman & B. Rime (Eds.),
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior (pp. 73–105). Cambridge, UK:
of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 187–192. Cambridge University Press.
Calvert, C. (2000). Free speech and public schools in a post Columbine Camras, L. A., Oster, H., Bakeman, R., Meng, Z., Ujiie, T., & Campos,
world: Check your speech rights at the schoolhouse metal detector. J. J. (2007). Do infants show distinct negative facial expressions for
Denver University Law Review, 77, 739. fear and anger? Emotional expression in 11-month-old European
Calvert, S. L., & Kotler, J. A. (2003). Lessons from children’s televi- American, Chinese, and Japanese infants. Infancy, 11, 131–155.
sion: Impact of the Children’s Television Act on children’s learn- Camras, L. A., Ribordy, S., Hill, J., Martino, S., Sachs, V., Spaccarelli,
ing. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24, 275–335. S., & Stefani, R. (1990). Maternal facial behavior and the recogni-
Camara, K. A., & Resnick, G. (1988). Interparental conflict and coop- tion and production of emotional expression by maltreated and
eration: Factors moderating children’s post-divorce adjustment. In nonmaltreated children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 304–312.
E. M. Hetherington & J. Aratesh (Eds.), Impact of divorce, single Camras, L. A., & Shutter, J. M. (2010). Emotional facial expressions
parenting, and stepparenting on children (pp. 169–195). Hillsdale, in infancy. Emotion Review, 2, 120–129.
NJ: Erlbaum. Camras, L. A., Sullivan, J. S., & Michel, G. (1993). Do infants express
Cameron, L., Erkal, N., Gangadharan, L., & Meng, X. (2013). Little discrete emotions? Adult judgments of facial, vocal and body
emperors: Behavioral impacts of China’s one-child policy. Science, actions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 17, 171–186.
339, 953–957. Cannon, T. D., Cadenhead, K., Cornblatt, B., Woods, S. W.,
Camos, V., & Barrouillet, P. (2011). Developmental change in work- Addington, J., Walker, E., Seidman, L. J., Perkins, D., Tsuang, M.,
ing memory strategies: From passive maintenance to active refresh- McGlashan, T., & Heinssen, R. (2008). Prediction of psychosis in
ing. Developmental Psychology, 47, 898–904. youth at high clinical risk: A multisite longitudinal study in North
Campbell, A., Shirley, L., & Candy, J. (2004). A longitudinal study America. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 28–37.
of gender-related cognition and behaviour. Developmental Science, Cannon, M., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Harrington, H., Taylor, A.,
7, 1–9. Murray, R. M., & Poulton, R. (2002). Evidence for early-childhood,
REFERENCES R-19
pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform dis- Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1996). Science and core knowledge. Philosophy
order: Results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Archives of General of Science, 63, 515–533.
Psychiatry 59, 449–456. Carlat, D. J., Camargo, C. A. Jr, & Herzog, D. B. (1997). Eating
Cannon, T. D., Kaprio, J., Lonnqvist, J., Huttunen, M., & disorders in males: A report on 135 patients. American Journal of
Koskenvuo, M. (1998). The genetic epidemiology of schizophre- Psychiatry, 154, 1127–1132.
nia in a Finnish twin cohort: A population-based modeling study. Carle, A. C., Millsap, R. E., & Cole, D. A. (2008). Measurement bias
Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 67–74. across gender on the Children’s Depression Inventory: Evidence
Cannon, W. B. (1932). The wisdom of the body. New York: W. W. for invariance from two latent variable models. Education and
Norton. Psychological Measurement, 68, 281–303.
Cantlon, J. F., Platt, M., & Brannon, E. M. (2009). Beyond the num- Carlson, B. (1988). Patten’s Foundations of embryology (5th ed.). New
ber domain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 83–91. York: McGraw-Hill.
Cantwell, D. P. (1996). Attention deficit disorder: A review of the past Carlson, M. J. (2004). Involvement by young unmarried fathers before
10 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent and after their baby’s birth. The Prevention Researcher, 11, 14–17.
Psychiatry, 35, 978–987. Carlson, M., & Earls, F. (1997). Psychological and neuroendocrino-
Canty, N., & Gould, J. L. (1995). The hawk/goose experiment: logical sequelae of early social deprivation in institutionalized chil-
Sources of variability. Animal Behaviour, 50, 1091–1095. dren in Romania. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 807,
Capecchi, M. R. (1997). The role of Hox genes in hindbrain develop- 419–428.
ment (pp. 334–355). New York: Oxford University Press. Carlson, M., McLanahan, S., & England, P. (2004). Union formation
Capranica, R. R. (1978). Auditory processing in anurans. Federation in fragile families. Demography, 41, 237–262.
Proceedings, 37, 2324–2328. Carlson, S. M., Davis, A. C., & Leach, J. G. (2005). Less is more:
Caputi, M., Lecce, S., Pagnin, A., & Banerjee, R. (2012). Longitudinal Executive function and symbolic representation in preschool chil-
effects of theory of mind on later peer relations: The role of proso- dren. Psychological Science, 16, 609–616.
cial behavior. Developmental Psychology, 48, 257–270. Carlson, S. M., Taylor, M., & Levin, G. R. (1998). The influence of
Caramazza, A., McCloskey, M., & Green, B. (1981). Naive beliefs culture on pretend play: The case of Mennonite children. Merrill-
in “sophisticated” subjects: Misconceptions about trajectories of Palmer Quarterly, 44, 538–565.
objects. Cognition, 9, 117–123. Carmichael, L., Hogan, H. P., & Walter, A. A. (1932). An experimen-
Caravita, S. C. S., Di Blasio, P., & Salmivalli, C. (2009). Unique and tal study of the effect of language on the reproduction of visually
interactive effects of empathy and social status on involvement in perceived form. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15, 73–86.
bullying. Social Development, 18, 140–163. Carnagey, N. L., Anderson, C. A., & Bartholow, B. D. (2007). Media
Card, N. A. (2007). “I hated her guts!” Emerging adults’ recollections violence and social neuroscience: New questions and new oppor-
of the formation, maintenance and termination of antipathetic tunities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 178–182.
relationships during high school. Journal of Adolescent Research, Carone, B. R., Fanqueier, L., Habib, N., Shea, J. M., Hart, C. E.,
22, 32–57. Bock, C. Li, C., Gu, H., Zanmore, P. D., Meissner, A., Wang, Z.,
Card, N. A., & Hodges, E. V. E. (2007). Victimization within Hofmann, H. A., Friedman, N., & Rando, O. J. (2010). Paternally
mutually antipathetic peer relationships. Social Development, 16, induced transgenerational environmental reprogramming of meta-
479–496. bolic gene expression in mammals. Cell, 143, 1084–1096.
Carey, B. (2003, June 30). Psychiatry and preschoolers: More young Caronna E. B., Milunsky, J. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2008). Autism
children are being diagnosed with drugs even as doctors grapple spectrum disorders: Clinical and research frontiers. Archives of
with a lack of research. Los Angles Times, p. F1. Disease in Childhood, 93, 518–523.
Carey, G., & Gottesman, I. (1981). Twin and family studies of anxi- Carpendale, J. I. M., & Carpendale, A. B. (2010).The development of
ety, phobic, and obsessive disorders. In D. F. Klein & J. G. Rabkin pointing: From personal directedness to interpersonal direction.
(Eds.), Anxiety: New research and changing concepts (pp. 117–136). Human Development, 53, 110–126.
New York: Raven Press. Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cogni-
Carey, S. (1978). The child as word learner. In M. Halle, J. Bresnan, & tion, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to
G. A. Miller (Eds.), Linguistic theory and psychological reality 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
(pp. 264–293). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Development, 63, 1–143.
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: Carr, A. (2009). The effectiveness of family therapy and systemic
MIT Press. interventions for child-focused problems. Journal of Family
Carey, S. (1991). Knowledge acquisition: Enrichment or concep- Therapy, 31, 3–45.
tual change? In S. Carey & R. Gelman, (Eds.), The epigenesis of Carr, N. (2010). The shallows: What the Internet is doing to our brains.
mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 257–291). Hillsdale, NJ: New York: W. W. Norton.
Erlbaum. Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-
Carey, S. (1995). On the origin of causal understanding. In D. Sperber, analytic studies. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
D. Premack & A. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisci- Carroll, J. B. (1997). The three-stratum theory of cognitive abili-
plinary debate (pp. 268–302). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. ties. In D. P. Flanagan, J. L. Genshaft, & P. L. Harrison (Eds.),
Carey, S. (2001). Cognitive foundations of arithmetic: Evolution and Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues
ontogenesis. Mind and Language, 16, 37–55. (pp. 122–130). New York: Guilford Press.
Carey, S. (2004). Bootstrapping and the origin of concepts. Daedalus, Carroll, J. J., & E. J. Gibson (1981). Differentiation of an aperture
133, 59–68. from an obstacle under conditions of motion by three month old
Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. New York: Oxford University infants. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of The Society for
Press. Research in Child Development, Boston.
Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Carroll, S. B. (1995). Homeotic genes and the evolution of arthropods
Proceedings of the Stanford Child Language Conference, 15, 17–29. and chordates. Nature, 376, 479–485.
R-20 REFERENCES
Carruthers, P. (2002). The cognitive functions of language. Behavioral Cash, T. F., & Deagle, E. A. (1997). The nature and extent of body-
and Brain Sciences, 25, 657–726. image disturbances in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa:
Carruthers, P. (2004). Practical reasoning in a modular mind. Mind & A meta-analysis. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 22,
Language, 19, 259–278. 107–125.
Carruthers, P., Laurence, S., & Stich, S. (2007). Introduction. The Caspari R. (2011, August). The evolution of grandparents. Scientific
innate mind: Vol 3. Foundations and the future (3rd ed., pp. 3–15). American, 305, 44–49.
New York: Oxford University Press. Caspari, R. E., & Lee, S. H. (2004). Older age becomes common late
Carstensen, L. L., & Charles, S. T. (1998). Emotion in the second in human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
half of life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 144–149. of the USA, 101, 10895–10900.
Carstensen, L., Gabrieli, J., Shepard, R., Levenson, R., Mason, M., Casper, R. C., & Troiani, M. (2001) Family functioning in anorexia
Goodman, G., Bootzin, R., Ceci, S., Bronfrenbrenner, U., nervosa differs by subtype. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
Edelstein, B., Schober, M., Bruck, M., Keane, T., Zimering, R., 30, 338 –342.
Oltmanns, R., Gotlib, I., & Ekman, P. (1993, March). Repressed Caspi, A. (1998). Personality development across the life course. In
objectivity. APS Observer, p. 23. W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality develop-
time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American ment (pp. 311–388). New York: Wiley.
Psychologist, 54, 165–181. Caspi, A., Hariri, A. R., Holmes, A., Uher, R., & Moffitt, T. E. (2010).
Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Mayr, U., & Nesselroade, J. (2000). Genetic sensitivity to the environment: The case of the serotonin
Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span. transporter gene and its implications for studying complex diseases
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 644–655. and traits. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 509–527.
Carter, A. S., Garrity-Rokous, F. E., Chazan-Cohen, R., Little, C., & Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W.,
Briggs-Gowan, M. J. (2001). Maternal depression and comor- Harrington, H., McClay, J., Mill, J., Martin, J., Braithwaite, A., &
bidity: Predicting early parenting, attachment security, and tod- Poulton, R. (2003). Influence of life stress on depression: Modera-
dler social-emotional problems and competencies. Journal of the tion by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301, 386–389.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 18–26. Cassibba, R., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bruno, S., & Coppola, G.
Cartwright-Hatton, S., & Murray, J. (2008). Cognitive therapy with (2004). Attachment of mothers and children with recurrent asth-
children and families: Treating internalizing disorders. Behavioural matic bronchitis. Journal of Asthma, 41, 419–431.
and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36, 749–756. Cassidy, J. (1986). The ability to negotiate the environment: An
Cartwright-Hatton, S., McNicol, K., & Doubleday, E. (2006). aspect of infant competence related to quality of attachment. Child
Anxiety in a neglected population: Prevalence of anxiety disor- Development, 57, 331–337.
ders in pre-adolescent children. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, Cassidy, J. (2000). Adult romantic attachments: A developmental per-
817–833. spective on individual differences. Review of General Psychology, 4,
Carver, K., Joyner, K., & Udry, J. R. (2003). National estimates of 111–131.
adolescent romantic relationships. In P. Florsheim (Ed.), Adolescent Cassidy, J. (2001). Truth, lies, and intimacy: An attachment perspec-
romantic relations and sexual behavior: Theory, research, and practi- tive. Attachment & Human Development, 3, 121–155.
cal implications (pp. 23–56). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Cassidy, J., & Berlin, L. J. (1994). The insecure/ambivalent pattern
Cary, G. (1994). Genetics and violence. In A. J. Reiss, Jr., Miczek, of attachment: Theory and research. Child Development, 65,
K. A., & J. A. Roth (Eds.) & Panel on the Understanding and 971–991.
Control of Violent Behaviors, National Research Council, Cassidy, J., & Marvin, R., with the MacArthur Working Group.
Understanding and preventing violence. Biobehavioral influences (1992). Attachment organization in preschool children: Procedures
(Vol. 2, pp. 21–53). Washington, DC: National Academies Press. and coding manual (4th ed.). Unpublished manuscript, University
Case, R. (1972). Learning and development: A neo-Piagetian interpre- of Virginia.
tation. Human Development, 15, 339–358. Cassidy, K. W. (1998). Three- and four-year-old children’s ability to
Case, R. (1984). The process of stage transition: A neo-Piagetian use desire- and belief-based reasoning. Cognition, 66, B1–B11.
view. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Mechanisms of cognitive development Castellanos, F. X., Giedd, J. N., Marsh, W. L., Hamburger, S. D.,
(pp. 19–44). New York: Freeman. Vaituzis, A. C., Dickstein, D. P., Sarfatti, S. E., Vauss, Y. C.,
Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to childhood. New Snell, J. W., Lange, N., Kaysen, D., Krain,A. L., Ritchie, G. F.,
York: Academic Press. Rajapakse, J. C., & Rapoport, J. L. (1996). Quantitative brain
Case R. (1992). The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual under- magnetic resonance imaging in attention-deficit hyperactivity dis-
pinnings of children’s thought and knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: order. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 607–616.
Erlbaum. Castellanos, F. X., Lau, E., Tayebi, N., Lee, P., Long, R. E., Giedd,
Case, R. (1998). The development of conceptual structures. In J. N., Sharp, W., Marsh, W., L., Walter, J. M., Hamburger, S. D.,
W. Damon (Series Ed.) & D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Ginns, E. I., Rapoport, J. L., & Sidransky, E. (1998). Lack of an
Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and lan- association between a dopamine- 4 receptor polymorphism and
guage (5th ed., pp. 745–800). New York: Wiley. attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Genetic and brain mor-
Case, R., & Okamoto, Y. (1996). The role of central conceptual struc- phometric analyses. Molecular Psychiatry, 3, 431–434.
tures in the development of children’s thought. Monographs of the Castellanos, F. X., Lee, P. P., Sharp, W., Jeffries, N. O., Greenstein,
Society for Research in Child Development, 61 (Serial No. 246). D. K., Clasen, L. S., Blumenthal, J. D., James, R. S., Ebens,
Casey B. J., Galvan, A., & Hare, T. A. (2005). Changes in cerebral C. L., Walter, J. M., Zijdenbos, A., Evans, A. C., Giedd, J. N., &
functional organization during cognitive development. Current Rapoport, J. L. (2002). Developmental trajectories of brain volume
Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 239–244. abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/
Casey, B. J., Getz, S., & Galvan, A. (2008). The adolescent brain. hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Medical Association,
Developmental Review, 28, 62–77. 288, 1740–1748.
REFERENCES R-21
Castles, A., Datta, H., Gayan, J., & Olson, R. K. (1999). Varieties of Cerella, J. (1990). Aging and information processing rate. In J. E.
developmental reading disorder: Genetic and environmental influ- Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging
ences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 73–94. (3rd ed., pp. 201–221). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Cattagni, A., & Ferris, E. (2001, May). Internet access in U.S. pub- Cerrelli, E. C. (1995). Crash data and rates for age-sex group of driv-
lic schools and classrooms: 1994–2000. National Center for ers, 1994. Research Note, Technical Report. Washington, DC:
Education Statistics (NCES 2001–07). National Highway Traffic Safety Adminstration.
Cattaneo, A., Monasta, L., Stamatakis, E., Lioret, S., Castetbon, K., Cervera, M. D., & Mendez, R. M. (2006). Temperament and ecologi-
Frenken, F., Manios, Y., Moschonis, G., Savva, S., Zaborskis, A., cal context among Yucatec Mayan children. International Journal
Rito, A. I., Nanu, M., Vignerová, J., Caroli, M., Ludvigsson, J., of Behavioral Development, 30, 326–337.
Koch, F. S., Serra-Majem, L., Szponar, L., van Lenthe, F., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Prelude or requiem for the Mozart effect?
Brug, J. (2010). Overweight and obesity in infants and pre-school Nature, 400, 826–827.
children in the European Union: A review of existing data. Obesity Chae, C. H., Jung, S. L., An, S. H., Park, B. Y., Wang, S. W., Cho,
Reviews, 11, 389–398. I. H., Cho, J. Y., & Kim, H. T. (2009). Treadmill exercise improves
Cattell, R. B. (1943). The measurement of adult intelligence. cognitive function and facilitates nerve growth factor signaling by
Psychological Bulletin, 40, 153–193. activating mitogen-activated protein kinase/extracellular signal-
Cattell, R. B. (1963).Theory of fluid and crystallized intelli- regulated kinase1/2 in the streptozotocin-induced diabetic rat hip-
gence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, pocampus. Neuroscience, 164, 1665–1673.
1–22. Chaiken, M., Bohner, J., & Marler, P. (1994). Repertoire turnover and
Cattell, R. B. (1978). The scientific use of factor analysis in behavioral the timing of song acquisition in European starlings. Behaviour,
and life sciences. New York: Plenum Press. 128, 25–39.
Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: Its structure, growth, and action. New Chall, J. (1983). Stages of reading development. New York: McGraw-Hill.
York: Elsevier. Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2005). Personality and
Caughy, M .O. B., DiPietro, J. A., & Strobino, D. M. (1994). Day- intellectual competence. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
care participation as a protective factor in the cognitive develop- Champagne, F. A. (2010). Epigenetic influence of social experiences
ment of low-income children. Child Development, 65, 457–471. across the lifespan. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 299–311.
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., Menozzi, P., & Piazza, A. (1994). The history Champagne F. A. (2013). Epigenetics and developmental plasticity
and geography of human genes. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University across species. Developmental Psychobiology, 55, 33–41.
Press. Champagne, D. L., Bagot, R. C., van Hasselt, F., Ramakers, G.,
Caviness V. S., Takahashi, T., & Nowakowski, R. S. (1995). Numbers, Meaney, M. J., de Kloet, E. R., Joëls, M., & Krugers, H. (2008).
time and neocortical neurogenesis: A general developmental and Maternal care and hippocampal plasticity: Evidence for experience-
evolutionary model. Trends in Neurosciences, 18, 379–383. dependent structural plasticity, altered synaptic functioning, and
Cayre, M., Canoll, P., & Goldman, J. E. (2009). Cell migration in the differential responsiveness to glucocorticoids and stress. Journal of
normal and pathological postnatal mammalian brain. Progress in Neuroscience, 28, 6037–6045.
Neurobiology, 88, 41–63. Champagne, F. A., & Curley, J. P. (2008). Maternal regulation of estro-
Ceci, S. J. (1990). On intelligence . . . more or less: A bioecological treatise gen receptor alpha methylation. Current Opinion in Pharmacology,
on intellectual development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. 8, 735–739.
Ceci, S. J. (1991). How much does schooling influence general intel- Champagne F. A., & Curley J. P. (2009). Epigenetic mechanisms
ligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evi- mediating the long-term effects of maternal care on development.
dence. Developmental Psychology, 27, 703–722. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 593–600.
Ceci, S. J. (1999). Schooling and intelligence. In S. Ceci & W. Williams Champagne, F. A., Francis, D. D., Mar, A., & Meaney, M. J. (2003).
(Eds.), The nature—nurture debate: Essential readings in develop- Variations in maternal care in the rat as a mediating influence for
mental psychology (pp. 167–175). Oxford: Blackwell. the effects of environment on development. Physiology & Behavior,
Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1993). Suggestibility of the child wit- 79, 359–371.
ness: A historical review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, Chan, M. E., & Elliott, J. M. (2011). Cross-linguistic differences in
113, 403–439. digit memory span. Australian Psychologist, 46, 25–30.
Ceci, S., & Bruck, M. (1995). Jeopardy in the courtroom: A scien- Chan, R. W., Raboy, B., & Patterson, C. J. (1998). Psychosocial adjust-
tific analysis of children’s testimony. Washington, DC: American ment among children conceived via donor insemination by lesbian
Psychological Association. and heterosexual mothers. Child Development, 69, 443–457.
Ceci, S. J., Fitneva, S. A., & Williams, W. M. (2010). Representational Chandler, M., Fritz, A. S., & Hala, S. (1989). Small scale deceit:
constraints on the development of memory and metamemory: A Deception marker of 2-, 3- and 4-year-olds’ early theories of mind.
developmental-representational theory. Psychological Review, 117, Child Development, 60, 1263–1277.
464–495. Chandler, M. J., Greenspan, L., & Barenboim, C. (1973). Judgments
Ceci, S. J., Papierno, P. B., & Kulkofsky, S. (2007). Representational of intentionality in response to videotaped and verbally presented
constraints on children’s suggestibility. Psychological Science, 18, moral dilemmas: The medium is the message. Child Development,
503–509. 44, 315–320.
Ceci, S. J., Toglia, M. P., & Ross, D. F. (Eds.) (1987). Children’s eye- Chandler, M. J., Sokol, B. W., & Wainryb, C. (2000). Beliefs about
witness memory. New York: Springer-Verlag. truth and beliefs about rightness. Child Development, 71, 91–97.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2013). National mar- Chang, C. (2004). Telling stories of experiences: Narrative development
riage and divorce rate trends. CDC/NCHS National Vital Statistics of young Chinese children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 83–104.
System. http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nvss/marriage_divorce_tables.htm Chang, L. T. (2008). Factory girls: From village to city in a changing
(Accessed May 14, 2013). China. New York: Spiegel & Grau.
Cerella, J. (1985). Information processing rates in the elderly. Changeaux, J. P. (1985). Neuronal man: The biology of mind. New
Psychological Bulletin, 98, 67–83. York: Pantheon.
R-22 REFERENCES
Changeux, J.-P., & Dehaene, S. (1989). Neuronal models of cognitive Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., Li, B., & Li, Z. (1999). Adolescent outcomes
function. Cognition, 33, 63–109. of social functioning in Chinese children. International Journal of
Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian par- Behavioral Development, 23, 199–223.
enting style: Understanding Chinese parenting through the cul- Chen, Y., Keen, R., Rosander, K., & von Hofsten, C. (2010).
tural notion of training. Child Development, 65, 1111–1119. Movement planning reflects skill level and age changes in toddlers.
Chao, R. K. (2000). The parenting of immigrant Chinese and Child Development, 81, 1846–1858.
European American mothers: Relations between parenting styles, Chen, Z. (1996). Children’s analogical problem solving: The effects
socialization goals, and parental practices. Journal of Applied of superficial, structural, and procedural similarity. Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 21, 233–248. Experimental Child Psychology, 62, 410–431.
Chao, R. K. (2001) Extending research on the consequences of par- Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1990). Attending to behaviour
enting style for Chinese Americans and European Americans. versus attending to knowledge: Examining monkeys’ attribution
Child Development, 72, 1832–1843. of mental states. Animal Behavior, 40, 742–753.
Chapin, H. D. (1915). A plea for accurate statistics in infants’ institu- Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1991). Reading minds or reading
tions. Archives of Pediatrics, 32, 724–726. behaviour? Tests for a theory of mind in monkeys. In A. Whiten
Charach, A., Carson, P., Fox, S., Ali, M. U., Beckett, J., & Lim, (Ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation
C. G. (2013). Interventions for preschool children at high risk of everyday mindreading (pp. 175–194). Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell.
for ADHD: A comparative effectiveness review. Pediatrics, 131, Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1992). Characterizing the mind of
e1584–e1604. another species. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 172–179.
Charles, S. T., & Carstensen, L.L. (2010). Social and emotional aging. Cheng, K. (1986). A purely geometric module in the rat’s spatial rep-
Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 383–409. resentation. Cognition, 23, 149–178.
Charles, S. T., Mather, M. M., & Carstensen, L.L. (2003). Aging and Cheng, K. (2008). Whither geometry? Troubles of the geometric
emotional memory: The forgettable nature of negative images for older module. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 355–361.
adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 310–324. Cheng, K., & Gallistel, C. R. (1984). Testing the geometric power
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive of an animal’s spatial representation. In H. L. Roitblat, T. G.
Psychology, 4, 55–81. Bever, & H. S. Terrace (Eds.), Animal cognition (pp. 409–423).
Chaudhuri, K. R., Healy, D. G., Schapira, A. H., & National Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Institute for Clinical Excellence. (2006). Non-motor symptoms of Cheng, K., & Newcombe, N. S. (2005). Is there a geometric module
Parkinson’s disease: Diagnosis and management. Lancet Neurology, for spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence. Psychonomic
5, 235–245. Bulletin & Review, 12, 1–23.
Chauvel, G., Maquestiaux, F., Hartley, A. A., Joubert, S., Didierjean, A., & Cherlin, A .J. (1992). Marriage, divorce and remarriage. Cambridge,
Masters, R. S. W. (2012). Age effects shrink when motor learning is MA: Harvard University Press.
predominantly supported by nondeclarative, automatic memory pro- Cherlin, A. J. (2010). Demographic trends in the United States:
cesses: Evidence from golf putting. Quarterly Journal of Experimental A review of research in the 2000s. Journal of Marriage and the
Psychology, 65, 25–38. Family, 72, 403–419.
Chawarska, K., Klin, A., Paul, R., & Volkmar, F. (2007). Autism spec- Cherlin, A. J., Chase-Lansdale, P. L., & McRae, C. (1998). Effects
trum disorder in the second year: Stability and change in syndrome of divorce and mental health through the life course. American
expression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 128–138. Sociological Review, 63, 239–249.
Chawarska, K., & Volkmar, F. (2005). Autism in infancy and early Cherline, A. J. (2005). American marriage in the early twenty-first
childhood. In F. Volkmar, A. Klin, & R. Paul (Eds.), Handbook of century. Future of Children, 15, 33–55.
autism and pervasive developmental disorders (3rd ed.). New York: Cherney, I. D., Seiwert, C. S., Dickey, T. M., & Flichtbeil, J. D.
Wiley. (2006). Children’s drawings: A mirror to their minds. Educational
Cheatham, C. L., & Bauer, P. J. (2005). Construction of a more Psychology, 26, 127–142.
coherent story: Prior verbal recall predicts later verbal accessibility Cheshire, A., Muldoon, K. P., Francis, B., Lewis, C. N., & Ball, L. J.
of early memories. Memory, 13, 516–532. (2007). Modelling change: New opportunities in the analysis of
Chen, X., Cen, G., Li, D., & He, Y. (2005). Social functioning and microgenetic data. Infant and Child Development, 16, 119–134.
adjustment in Chinese children: The imprint of historical time. Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. (1973). Facial expression of emotion in non-
Child Development, 76, 182–195. human primates. In P. Ekman (Ed.), Darwin and facial expression
Chen, D. W, Fein, G. G., & Tam, H. P. (2001). Peer conflicts of (pp. 11–83). New York: Academic Press.
preschool children: Issues, resolution, incidence, and age-related Chi, M. T. H. (1977). Age differences in memory span. Journal of
patterns. Early Education & Development. 12, 523–544. Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 266–281.
Chen, X., & French, D. C. (2008). Children’s social competence in Chi, M. T. H. (1978). Knowledge structures and memory develop-
cultural context. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 591–616. ment. In R. S. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops?
Chen, X., Hastings, P., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Chen, G., & (pp. 73–96). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Stewart, S. L. (1998). Child rearing attitudes and behavioral inhi- Chi, M. T. H. (1992). Conceptual change within and across ontologi-
bition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. cal categories: Examples from learning and discovery in science. In
Developmental Psychology, 34, 677–686. R. Giere (Ed.), Cognitive models of science: Minnesota studies in the
Chen, X., Liu, M., Li, B., Cen, G., & Chen, H. (2000). Maternal philosophy of science (pp. 129–186). Minneapolis, MN: University
authoritative and authoritarian attitudes and mother-child interac- of Minnesota Press.
tions and relationships in China. International Journal of Behavioral Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization
Development, 24, 119–126. and representation of physics problems by experts and novices.
Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., & Li. B. (1994). Only children and sibling Cognitive Science, 5, 121–152.
children in urban China: A re-examination. International Journal Chi, M. T. H., & Koeske, R. D. (1983). Network representation of a
of Behavioral Development, 17, 413–421. child’s dinosaur knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 19, 29–39.
REFERENCES R-23
Chiang, M. C., Barysheva, M., Shattuck, D. W., Lee, A. D., Madsen, S., Chronis-Tuscano, A., Degnan, K. A., Pine, D. S., Perez-Edgar, K.,
Avedissian, C., Klunder, A. D., Toga, A. W., McMahon, K. L., Henderson, H. A., Diaz, Y., Raggi, V. L., & Fox, N.A (2009).
de Zubicaray, G. I., Wright, M. J., Srivastava, A., Balov, N., & Stable early maternal report of behavioral inhibition predicts life-
Thompson, P. M. (2009). Genetics of brain fiber architecture and time social anxiety disorder in adolescence. Journal of the American
intellectual performance. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 2212–2224. Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 928–935.
Chien, S. H. L., Palmer, J., & Teller, D. Y. (2003). Infant lightness per- Chu, A. T., & Lieberman, A. F. (2010). Clinical implications of
ception: Do 4-month-olds show Wallach’s ratio rule? Psychological traumatic stress from birth to age five. Annual Review of Clinical
Science, 14, 291–295. Psychology, 6, 469–494.
Chierchia, G. (1994). Syntactic bootstrapping and the acquisition of Chuang, Y. C., Lee, C., Chiu, N. C., Shu, C. H., Hung, H. Y.,
noun meanings: The mass-count issue. In B. Lust, M. Suner, & Kao, H. A., & Chang, J. H. (2007). Neurodevelopment in very
J. Whitman (Eds.), Syntactic theory and first language acquisition: low birth weight premature infants with postnatal subependymal
Cross-linguistic perspectives: Vol. 1. Heads, projections, & learnability cysts. Journal of Child Neurology, 22, 402–405.
(pp. 301–318). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Chukovsky, K. (1925/1963). From two to five. Berkeley, CA: University
Child Welfare Information Gateway. (2011). Foster care statistics of California Press.
2009. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human CIA World Factbook. (2010). The world fact book: Age structure.
Services, Children’s Bureau. https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/the-world-factbook
Chipungu, S. S., & Bent-Goodley, T. B. (2004). Meeting challenges /fields/2010.html.
of contemporary foster care. Future of Our Children, 14, 75–93. Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S. L., Lewis, B. P., Luce, C., & Neuberg,
Chisaka, O., & Capecchi, M. R. (1991). Regionally restricted devel- S. L. (1997). Reinterpreting the empathy-altruism relationship:
opmental defects resulting from targeted disruption of the mouse When one into one equals oneness. Journal of Personality and Social
homeobox gene hox-1.5. Nature, 350, 473–479. Psychology, 73, 481–494.
Chisaka, O., Musci, T. S., & Capecchi, M. R. (1992). Developmental Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. (1995). Child maltreatment and attach-
defects of the ear, cranial nerves and hindbrain resulting from tar- ment organization: Implications for intervention. In S. Goldberg,
geted disruption of the mouse homeobox gene hox-1.6. Nature, R. Muir, & J. Kerr (Eds.), Attachment theory: Social, developmental,
355, 516–520. and clinical perspectives (pp. 279–308). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
Chisholm, K., Carter, M. C., Ames, E. W., & Morison, S. J. (1995). Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. (2005). Child maltreatment. Annual Review
Attachment security and indiscriminately friendly behavior in of Clinical Psychology, 1, 409–438.
children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Development and Cillessen, A. H. N., & Mayeux, L. (2004). From censure to reinforce-
Psychopathology, 7, 283–294. ment: Developmental changes in the association between aggres-
Choe, K., Keil, F. C., & Bloom, P. (2005). Children’s understanding sion and social status. Child Development, 75, 147–163.
of the Ulysses conflict. Developmental Science, 8, 387–392. Cillessen, A. H. N., & Rose, A. J. (2005). Understanding popularity
Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). Causal attribution in the peer system. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14,
across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin, 102–105.
125, 47–63. Cimpian, A., & Park, J. J. (2013). Tell me about pangolins! Evidence
Chomsky, C. (1969). The acquisition of syntax in children from 5 to 10. that children are motivated to learn about kinds. Journal of
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Experimental Psychology: General. [Epub ahead of print]
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague, The Cirillo, K. J., Pruitt, B. E., Colwell, B., Kingery, P. M., Hurley,
Netherlands: Mouton. R. S., & Ballard, D. (1998). School violence: Prevalence and inter-
Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of B. F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. vention strategies for at-risk adolescents. Adolescence, 33, 319–330.
Language, 35, 26–58. Claes, S., Tang, Y. L., Gillespie, C. F., & Cubells, J. F. (2012). Human
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: genetics of schizophrenia. Handbook of Clinical Neurology, 106,
MIT Press. 37–52.
Chomsky, N. (1966). Cartesian linguistics. New York: Harper & Row. Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006). How native-like is non-native lan-
Chomsky, N. (1975). Reflections on language. New York: Pantheon guage processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 564–570.
Books. Clark, A. (1994). Representational trajectories in connectionist learn-
Chomsky, N. (1981). Principles and parameters in syntactic theory. ing. Minds and Machines, 4, 317–332.
In N. Hornstein & D. Lightfoot (Eds.), Explanation in linguistics: Clark, C. A., Pritchard, V. E., & Woodward, L. J. (2010). Preschool
The logical problem of language acquisition (pp. 32–75). London: executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achieve-
Longman. ment. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1176-1191.
Chomsky, N. (1988). Language and problems of knowledge: The Clark, E. (1988). On the logic of contrast. Journal of Child Language,
Managua lectures. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press. 15, 317–335.
Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New Clark, E. V. (1995). The lexicon in acquisition. New York: Cambridge
York: Harper & Row. University Press.
Christakis, N. A., & Fowler, J. H. (2007). The spread of obesity Clark, E. V. (2009). First language acquisition (2nd ed.). New York:
in a large social network over 32 years. New England Journal of Cambridge University Press.
Medicine, 357, 370–379. Clark, H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
Christakis, N. A., & Fowler, J. H. (2012). Social contagion the- University Press.
ory: Examining dynamic social networks and human behavior. Clark, H. H., & Schunk, D. H. (1980). Polite responses to polite
Statistics in Medicine, 32, 556–577. requests. Cognition, 8, 111–143.
Christian, D. (2011). Dual language education. In E. Hinkel (Ed.), Clark, K. (1955). Prejudice and your child. Boston: Beacon Press.
Handbook of research in second language teaching and learning Clark, L. A. (2009). Stability and change in personality disorder.
(Vol. 2, pp. 3–20). New York and London: Routledge. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 27–31.
R-24 REFERENCES
Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1989). Infant day care: Maligned or malig- bined cognitive, anatomical and fMRI study. Neuropsychologia,
nant? American Psychologist, 44, 266–273. 38, 1426–1440.
Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1991). A home is not a school: The effects of Cohen, N. J., & Corkin, S. (1981). The amnesic patient H. M.:
child care on children’s development. Journal of Social Issues, 47, Learning and retention of a cognitive skill. Society for Neuroscience
105–123. Abstracts, 7, 235.
Clarke-Stewart, K. A., & Hayward, C. (1996). Advantages of father Cohen, R. D., & Mohl, R. A. (1979). The paradox of progressive edu-
custody and contact for the psychological well-being of school-age cation: The Gary plan and urban schooling. Port Washington, NY:
children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 17, 239–270. Kennikat Press.
Clarke-Stewart, K. A., Vandell, D. L., McCartney, K., Owen, M. T., & Cohen, S. M., & Jurgens, G. (1990). Mediation of Drosophila head
Booth, C. (2000). Effects of parental separation and divorce on very development by gap-like segmentation genes. Nature, 346,
young children. Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 304–326. 482–485.
Claxton, L. J., Keen, R., & McCarty, M. E. (2003). Evidence of motor Cohen, S., Janicki-Deverts, D., & Miller, G. E. (2007). Psychological
planning in infant reaching behavior. Psychological Science, 14, stress and disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 298,
354–356. 1685–1687.
Cleckley, H. (1941). The mask of sanity: An attempt to reinterpret the Cohen-Bendahan, C. C., van de Beek, C., & Berenbaum, S. A.
so-called psychopathic personality. St Louis, MO: Mosby. (2005). Prenatal sex hormone effects on child and adult sex-typed
Clement, J. (1993). Using bridging analogies and anchoring intuitions behavior: Methods and findings. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
to deal with students’ preconceptions in physics. Journal of Research Reviews, 29, 353–384.
in Science Teaching, 30, 1241–1257. Cohen-Kettenis, P. (2005). Psychological long-term outcome in inter-
Clements, S. D., & Peters, J. E. (1962). Minimal brain dysfunction in sex conditions. Hormone Research, 64, 27–30.
the school-aged child: Diagnosis and treatment. Archives of General Cohen-Woods, S., Craig, I. W., & McGuffin, P. (2012). The current
Psychiatry, 6, 185–190. state of play on the molecular genetics of depression. Psychological
Clifford, A., Holmes, A., Davies, I. R. L., & Franklin, A. (2010). Medicine, 43, 673–687.
Color categories affect pre-attentive color perception. Biological Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1988). Mother-infant face-to-face inter-
Psychology, 85, 275–282. action: Influence is bidirectional and unrelated to periodic cycles in
Clifford, S. M., & Dissanayake, C. (2008). The early development either partner’s behavior. Developmental Psychology, 24, 386–392.
of joint attention in infants with autistic disorder using home Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. (1989). Specificity of infants’ response to
video observations and parental interview. Journal of Autism and mothers’ affective behavior. Journal of the American Academy of
Developmental Disorders, 38, 791–805. Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 242–248.
Clifton, R., Rochat, P., Litovsky, R., & Perris, E. (1991). Object Cohn, J. F., Zlochower, A., Lien, J., & Kanade, T. (1999). Automated
representation guides infants’ reaching in the dark. Journal of face analysis by feature point tracking has high concurrent validity
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, with manual FACS coding. Psychophysiology, 36, 35–43.
323–329. Cohn, L. D., Macfarlane, S., Yanez, C., & Imai, W. K. (1995). Risk
Cliquet, R. (2003). Major trends affecting families in the new mil- perception: Differences between adolescents and adults. Health
lennium: Western Europe and North America. In Major trends Psychology, 14, 217–222.
affecting families: A background document (pp. 1–40). Report Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1988). Multiple sources of data on social
for the United Nations, Department of Economic and Social behavior and social status. Child Development, 59, 815–829.
Affairs, Division for Social Policy and Development, Program Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). Dimensions
on the Family. http://www.un.org/esa/socdev/family/Publications and types of social status: A cross-age perspective. Developmental
/mtcliquet.pdf Psychology, 18, 557–570.
Closson, L. M. (2009). Status and gender differences in early adoles- Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., Terry, R., & Wright, V. (1991). The role of
cents’ descriptions of popularity. Social Development, 18, 412–426. aggression in peer relations: An analysis of aggressive episodes in
CNN (2003, March 20). Digital divide” shrinks among U.S. kids. boys’ play groups. Child Development, 62, 812–826.
http://www.cnn.com/2003/TECH/internet/03/20/digital.divide Colapinto, J. (2000). As nature made him: The boy who was raised as a
.reut/index.html girl. New York: HarperCollins.
Coghill, D., Nigg, J., Rothenberger, A., Sonuga-Barke, E., & Colby, A., & Damon, W. (1992). Some do care: Contemporary lives of
Tannock, R. (2005). Whither causal models in the neuroscience of moral commitment. New York: Free Press.
ADHD? Developmental Science, 8, 105–114. Colby, A., & Kohlberg, L. (1987). The Measurement of moral judgment
Cohen, D., & Gunz, A. (2002). As seen by the other . . . : Perspectives (Vols. 1 and 2). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
on the self in the memories and emotional perceptions of easterners Colcombe, S. J., Erickson, K. I., Scalf, P. E., Kim, J. S., Prakash, R.,
and westerners. Psychological Science, 13, 55–59. McAuley, E., Elavsky, S., Marquez, D. X., Hu, L., & Kramer, A. F.
Cohen, L. B., & Amsel, G. (1998). Precursors to infants’ perception (2006). Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging
of the causality of a simple event. Infant Behavior and Development, humans. Journals of Gerontology, Series A: Biological Sciences and
21, 713–731. Medical Sciences, 61, 1166–1170.
Cohen, L. B., & Cashon, C. H. (2003). Infant perception and Colcombe, S., & Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cogni-
cognition. In R. Lerner, A. Easterbrooks, & J. Mistry (Eds.), tive function of older adults: A meta-analytic study. Psychological
Comprehensive handbook of psychology: Vol. 6. Developmental Science, 14, 125–130.
Psychology. II. Infancy (pp. 65–89). New York: Wiley. Colcombe, S. J., Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K. I., Scalf, P., McAuley, E.,
Cohen, L. B., & Oakes, L. M. (1993). How infants perceive a simple Cohen, N. J., Webb, A., Jerome, G. J., Marquez, D. X., &
causal event. Developmental Psychology, 29, 421–433. Elavsky, S. (2004). Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and
Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., Chochon, F., Lehericy, S., & Naccache, L. aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA,
(2000). Language and calculation within the parietal lobe: A com- 101, 3316–3321.
REFERENCES R-25
Cole, D. A., Ciesla, J. A., Dallaire, D. H., Danielle, H., Jacquez, Collins, W. A., Welsh, D. P., & Furman, W. (2009). Adolescent
F. M., Pineda, A. Q., LaGrange, B., Truss, A. E., Folmer, A. S., romantic relationships. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 631–652.
Tilghman-Osborne, C., & Felton, J. W. (2008). Emergence of Colom, R., Stein, J. L., Rajagopalan, P., Martínez, K., Hermel, D.,
attributional style and its relation to depressive symptoms. Journal Wang, Y., Alvarez-Linera, Burgaleta, M., Quiroga, M. A., Shih,
of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 16–31. P. C., & Thompson, P. M. (2013). Hippocampal structure and
Cole, D. A., Tram, J. M., Martin, J. M., Hoffman, K. B., Ruiz, M. D., human cognition: Key role of spatial processing and evidence
Jacquez, F. M., & Maschman, T. L. (2008). Individual differences supporting the efficiency hypothesis in females. Intelligence, 41,
in the emergence of depressive symptoms in children and adoles- 129–140.
cents: A longitudinal investigation of parent and child reports. Colombo, J. (1993). Infant cognition: Predicting later intellectual func-
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 17, 16–31. tioning. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Cole, M., McDermott, R. P., & Hood, L. (1978). Ecological niche- Colombo, J. (2004). Visual attention in infancy: Process and product
picking: Ecological invalidity as an axiom of experimental cogni- in early cognitive development. In M. I. Posner (Ed.), Cognitive
tive psychology. New York: Rockefeller University, Laboratory of neuroscience of attention (pp. 329–341). New York: Guilford Press.
Comparative Cognition. Colombo, J., Shaddy, D. J., Richman, W. A., Maikrantz, J. M., &
Cole, M., & Means, B. (1981). Comparative studies of how people think: Blaga, O. (2004). The developmental course of habituation in
An introduction. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. infancy and preschool outcome. Infancy, 5, 1–38.
Cole, M. W., Yarkoni, T., Repovs, G., Anticevic, A., & Braver, T. S. Colonnesi, C., Draijer, E. M., Stams, G. J. J. M., Van der Bruggen,
(2012). Global connectivity of prefrontal cortex predicts cognitive C. O., Bogels, S. M., & Noom, M. J. (2011). The relation between
control and intelligence. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 8988–8999. insecure attachment and child anxiety: A meta-analytic review.
Cole, P. M., Barrett, K. C., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1992). Emotion dis- Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40, 630–645.
plays in two-year-olds during mishaps. Child Development, 63, Coltheart, M. (1999). Modularity and cognition. Trends in Cognitive
314–324. Sciences, 3, 115–120.
Cole, P. M., Bruschi, C., & Tamang, B. L. (2002). Cultural differ- Comeau, L., Genesee, F., & Mendelson, M. (2007). Bilingual chil-
ences in children’s emotional reactions to difficult situations. Child dren’s repairs of breakdowns in communication. Journal of Child
Development, 73, 983–996. Language, 34, 159–174.
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regu- Comings, D., Gade-Andavolu, R., Gonzalez, N., Blake, H., Wu, S., &
lation as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and MacMurray, J. (1999). Additive effect of three noradrenergic genes
directions for child development research. Child Development, 75, (ADRA2A, ADRA2C, DBH) on attention-deficit hyperactivity
317–333. disorder and learning disabilities in Tourette syndrome subjects.
Cole, P. M., Tamang, B. L., & Shrestha, S. (2006). Cultural varia- Clinical Genetics, 55, 160–172.
tions in the socialization of young children’s anger and shame. Compte, A., Brunel, N., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Wang, X. J. (2000).
Child Development, 77, 1237–1251. Synaptic mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial
Coles, R. (1997). The moral intelligence of children: How to raise a working memory in a cortical network model. Cerebral Cortex, 10,
moral child. New York: Random House. 910–923.
Coles, R. (2000). Lives of moral leadership: Men and women who have Comrie, B. (1981). Language universals and linguistic typology: Syntax
made a difference. New York: Random House. and morphology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Colletti, V., Carner, M., Miorelli, V., Guida, M., Colletti, L., & Comstock, G., & Scharrer, E. (2006). Media and popular culture. In
Fiorino, F. (2005). Cochlear implantation at under 12 months: W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.) & K. A. Renninger &
Report on 10 patients. Laryngoscope, 115, 445–449. I. E. Sigel (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Child
Collier, V. P., & Thomas, W. P. (2007). Predicting second lan- psychology in practice (6th ed., pp. 817–863). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
guage academic success in English using the Prism Model. In J. Condry, J. C. (1989). The psychology of television. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cummins & C. Davison (Eds.), International handbook of English Condry, J., & Chambers, J. (1978). Intrinsic motivation and the pro-
language teaching, Part 1 (pp. 333–348). New York: Springer. cess of learning. In M. R. Lepper & D. Greene (Eds.), The hidden
Collings, S. J. (1995). The long-term effects of contact and noncontact costs of reward: New perspectives on the psychology of human motiva-
forms of child sexual abuse in a sample of university men. Child tion (pp. 61–84). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Abuse & Neglect, 19, 1–6. Conel, J. L. (1939–1959). The postnatal development of the human cere-
Collins, M. D., & Mao, G. E. (1999). Teratology of retinoids. Annual bral cortex. (Vol. 1–6). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology, 39, 399–430. Conel, J., & Le, R. (1941). The postnatal development of the human cere-
Collins, R. L. (2011). Content analysis of gender roles in media: Where bral cortex: Vol. 2. The cortex of the one-month infant. Cambridge,
are we now and where should we go? Sex Roles, 64, 290–298. MA: Harvard University Press.
Collins, W. A. (2003). More than myth: The developmental sig- Confer, J. C., Easton, J. A., Fleischman, D. S., Goetz, C. D., Lewis,
nificance of romantic relationships during adolescence. Journal of D. M., Perilloux, C., & Buss, D. M. (2010). Evolutionary psychol-
Research on Adolescence, 13, 1–24. ogy: Controversies, questions, prospects, and limitations. American
Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, Psychologist, 65, 110–126.
E. M., & Bornstein M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on par- Conger, D. (2010). Does bilingual education interfere with English-
enting: The case for nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, language acquisition? Social Science Quarterly, 91, 1103–1122.
218–232. Conley, D. (2004). The pecking order: Which siblings succeed and why.
Collins, W. A., & Sroufe, L. A. (1999). Capacity for intimate rela- New York: Pantheon Books.
tionships: A developmental construction. In W. Furman, B. B. Conley, D., & Glauber, R. (2005). Sibling similarity and difference in
Brown, & C. Feiring (Eds.), The development of romantic rela- socioeconomic status: Life course and family resource effects. Working
tionships in adolescence (pp. 125–147). New York: Cambridge paper 11320. Washington, DC: National Bureau of Economic
University Press. Research.
R-26 REFERENCES
Connor, C. M., Morrison, F. J., Fishman, B., Giuliani, S., Luck, M., Cosgrove, K. P., Mazure, C. M., & Staley, J. K. (2007). Evolving
Underwood, P. S., Bayraktar, A., Crowe, E. C., & Schatschneider, C. knowledge of sex differences in brain structure, function, and
(2011). Testing the impact of child characteristics × instruction chemistry. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 847–855.
interactions on third graders’ reading comprehension by differenti- Coslett, H. B. (2000). Acquired dyslexia. In M. J. Farah & T. E.
ating literacy instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 46, 189–221. Feinberg (Eds.), Patient-based approaches to cognitive neuroscience
Connor, C. M., Morrison, F. J., Fishman, B. J., Schatschneider, C., & (pp. 235–246). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Underwood, P. (2007). The early years: Algorithm-guided indi- Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1994). The evolution of domain specificity:
vidualized reading instruction. Science, 315, 464–465. The evolution of functional organization. In L. A. Hirschfeld &
Connor-Smith, J. K., & Weisz, J. R. (2003). Applying treatment S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cog-
outcome research in clinical practice: Techniques for adapting nition and culture. New York: Cambridge University Press.
interventions to the real world. Child and Adolescent Mental Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Evolutionary psychology and the
Health, 8, 3–10. emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook
Conry-Murray, C., & Turiel, E. (2012). Jimmy’s baby doll and Jenny’s of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 91–115). New York: Guilford Press.
truck: Young children’s reasoning about gender norms. Child Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2013). Evolutionary psychology: New
Development, 83, 146–158. perspectives on cognition and motivation. Annual Review of
Conway, M. A., Wang, Q., Hanyu, K., & Haque, S. (2005). A cross- Psychology, 64, 201–229.
cultural investigation of autobiographical memory: On the univer- Costa, P. T., Jr, & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Set like plaster? Evidence
sality and cultural variation of the “reminiscence bump.” Journal of for the stability of adult personality. In T. F. Heatherton &
Cross- Cultural Psychology, 36, 739–749. J. L. Weinberger (Eds.), Can personality change? (pp. 21–40).
Conway, P. F., & Sloane, F. C. (2006). International trends in post- Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
primary mathematics education: Perspectives on learning, teaching Costanzo, P. R., Coie, J. D., Grumet, J. F., & Farnill, D. (1973). A
and assessment. Dublin: National Council for Curriculum and reexamination of the effects of intent and consequence on chil-
Assessment. dren’s moral judgments. Child Development, 44, 154 –161.
Cook, E. H., Stein, M. A., Krasowski, M. D., Cox, N. J., Olkon, Costello, E. J., Egger, H. L., & Angold, A. (2005). The developmen-
D. M., Kieffer, J. E., & Leventhal B. L. (1995). Association of tal epidemiology of anxiety disorders: Phenomenology, prevalence,
attention-deficit disorder and the dopamine transporter gene. and comorbidity. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North
American Journal of Human Genetics, 56, 993–998. America, 14, 631–648.
Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1989). Observational conditioning of fear Costello, E. J., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., & Angold, A. (2003).
to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 448–459. and adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 837–844.
Cooke, D. J., Michie, C., Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1999). Evaluating Cotman, C. W., & Berchtold, N. C. (2002). Exercise: A behavioral
the screening version of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends in
(PCL: SV): An item response theory analysis. Psychological Neurosciences, 25, 295–301.
Assessment, 11, 3–13. Cotter, D., England, P., & Hermesen, J. (2008). Moms and jobs:
Cooke, L. J., Chambers, L. C., An?ez, E. V., Croker, H. A., Boniface, D., Trends in mothers’ employment and which mothers stay home. In
Yeomans, M. R., & Wardle, J. (2011). Eating for pleasure or profit: S. Coontz (Ed.), American families: A multicultural reader (2nd ed.,
The effect of incentives on children’s enjoyment of vegetables. pp. 379–386). New York: Routledge.
Psychological Science, 22, 190–196. Cotugno, A. J. (1995). Personality attributes of attention deficit
Coon, H., Carey, G., & Fulker, D. W. (1992). Community influences hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) using the Rorschach Inkblot Test.
on cognitive ability. Intelligence, 16, 169–188. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 554–562.
Coontz, S. (2010). Why American families need the Census. Annals of Courage, M. L., & Adams, R. J. (1990). Visual acuity assessment from
the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 631, 141–149. birth to three years using the acuity card procedure: Cross-sectional
Cooper, H., Nye, B., Charlton, K., Lindsay, J., & Greathouse, S. and longitudinal samples. Optometry and Vision Science, 67, 7l3–718.
(1996). The effects of summer vacation on achievement test Courchesne, E., Pierce, K., Schumann, C. M., Redcay, E., Buckwalter,
scores: A narrative and meta-analytic review. Review of Educational J. A., Kennedy, D. P., & Morgan, J. (2007). Mapping early brain
Research, 66, 227–268. development in autism. Neuron, 56, 399–413.
Cooper, R. P., Abraham, J., Berman, S., & Staska, M. (1997). The Cowan, N. (1997). The development of working memory. In N. Cowan
development of infant preference for motherese. Infant Behavior (Ed.), The development of memory in childhood (pp. 163–199).
and Development, 20, 477–488. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1990). Preference for infant-directed Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A
speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61, reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain
1584–1595. Sciences, 24, 87–114.
Corcoran, C., Walker, E., Huot, R., Mittal, V., Tessner, K., Kestler, L., & Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (2002). What an intervention design
Malaspina, D. (2003). The stress cascade and schizophrenia: Etiology reveals about how parents affect their children’s academic achieve-
and onset. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 29, 671–692. ment and behavior problems. In J. G. Borkowski, S. Ramey, &
Cordes, S., & Brannon, E. M. (2009). Crossing the divide: Infants dis- M. Bristol-Powers (Eds.), Parenting and the child’s world: Influences
criminate small from large numerosities. Developmental Psychology, on intellectual, academic, and social-emotional development
45, 1583–1594. (pp. 75–98). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Corrado, R. R., Vincent, G. M., Hart, S. D., & Cohen, I. M. (2004). Cowan, P. A., Powell, D., & Cowan, C. P. (1997). Parenting inter-
Predictive validity of the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version for ventions: A family systems perspective. In I. E. Sigel & K. A.
general and violent recidivism. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, Renninger (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Child psy-
22, 5–22. chology in practice (5th ed., pp 3–72). New York: Wiley.
REFERENCES R-27
Cowan, W. M. (1979). The development of the brain. Scientific of the vulnerable and victimized (pp. 196–214). New York: Guilford
American, 241, 113–133. Press.
Cowan, W. M., Fawcett, J. W., O’Leary, D. D. M., & Stanfield, B. B. Crick, N. R., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2003). The development of psy-
(1984). Regressive events in neurogenesis. Science, 225, 1258–1265. chopathology in females and males: Current progress and future
Cowan, W. M., Jessel, T. M., & Zipursky, S. L. (1997). Molecular challenges. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 719–742.
and cellular approaches to neural development. New York: Oxford Cristofalo, V. J., & Adelman, R. (Eds.). (2002). Annual review of ger-
University Press. ontology and geriatrics, 2001: Vol. 21. Focus on Modern topics in the
Cowie, F. (1999). What’s within? Nativism reconsidered. New York: biology of aging. New York: Springer.
Oxford University Press. Crockenberg, S. (1981). Infant irritability, mother responsiveness,
Cox, C. (1929). The early mental traits of three hundred geniuses and social support influences on the security of the infant-mother
(2nd ed.). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. attachment. Child Development, 52, 857–865.
Cox, M. J., Owen, M. T., Henderson, V. K., & Margand, N. A. Crockenberg, S. C., & Leerkes, E. M. (2003). Infant negative emotion-
(1992). Prediction of infant-father and infant-mother attachment. ality, caregiving, and family relationships. In A. C. Crouter & A.
Developmental Psychology, 28, 474–483. Booth (Eds.), Children’s influence on family dynamics: The neglected
Coyle, J. T., Pine, D. S., Charney, D. S., Lewis, L., Nemeroff, C. B., side of family relationships (pp. 57–78). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Carlson, G. A., Joshi, P. T., Reiss, D., Todd, R. D., & Hellander, Crocker, J., & Knight, K. M. (2005). Contingencies of self-worth.
M. (2003). Depression and Bipolar Support Alliance consensus Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 200–203.
statement on the unmet needs in diagnosis and treatment of mood Crocker, J., & Park, L. E. (2004). The costly pursuit of self-esteem.
disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of the American Psychological Bulletin, 130, 392–414.
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 1494–1503. Crocker, J., & Wolfe, C. T. (2001). Contingencies of self-worth.
Coyle, T. R., & Bjorklund, D. F. (1996). The development of strategic Psychological Review, 108, 593–623.
memory: A modified microgenetic assessment of utilization defi- Crockett, L., Losoff, M., & Peterson, A. (1984). Perceptions of the
ciencies. Cognitive Development, 11, 295–314. peer group and friendship in early adolescence. Journal of Early
Coyle, T. R., Pillow, D. R., Snyder, A. C., & Kochunov, P. (2011). Adolescence, 4, 155–181.
Processing speed mediates the development of general intelligence Cronbach, L. (1975). Beyond the two disciplines of scientific psychol-
(g) in adolescence. Psychological Science, 22, 1265–1269. ogy. American Psychologist, 30, 116–127.
Coyne, S. M., Nelson, D. A., Lawton, F., Haslam, S., Rooney, L., Cronbach, L., & Snow, R. (1977). Aptitudes and instructional meth-
Titterington, L., Trainor, H., Remnant, J., & Ogunlaja, L. (2008). ods: A handbook for research on interactions. New York: Irvington
The effects of viewing physical and relational aggression in the Publishers.
media: Evidence for a cross-over effect. Journal of Experimental Crosnoe, R., & Kirkpatrick Johnson, M. (2011). Research on adoles-
Social Psychology, 44, 1551–1554. cence in the twenty-first century. Annual Review of Sociology, 37,
Craddock, N., & Jones, I. (1999). Genetics of bipolar disorder. Journal 439–460.
of Medical Genetics, 36, 585–594. Crossley, P. H., Martinez, S., & Martin, G. R. (1996). Midbrain
Craik, F. I. M., & Salthouse, T. A. (Eds.) (2000). The handbook of development induced by FGF8 in the chick embryo. Nature, 380,
aging and cognition (2nd ed). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 66–68.
Crain, S., & Lillo-Martin, D. (1999). An introduction to linguis- Crouter, A. C., & Bumpus, M. F. (2001). Linking parents’ work stress
tic theory and language acquisition. Malden, MA: Blackwell to children’s and adolescents’ psychological adjustment. Current
Publishers. Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 156–159.
Crawford, M. L. J., Pesch, T. W., & von Noorden, G. K. (1996). Crowder, R. G., & Wagner, R. K. (1992). The psychology of reading:
Excitatory binocular neurons are lost following prismatic binocu- An introduction (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
lar dissociation in infant monkeys. Behavioral Brain Research, 79, Crowley, K. (2011). Sleep and sleep disorders in older adults.
227–232. Neuropsychology Review, 21, 41–53.
Crawley, S. L., Newcombe, N. S., & Bingman, H, (2010). How Crowley, K., & Galco, J. (2001). Everyday activity and the devel-
focus at encoding affects children’s source monitoring. Journal of opment of scientific thinking. In K. Crowley, C. D. Schunn, &
Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 273–285. T. Okada (Eds.), Designing for science: Implications from everyday,
Creighton, M. R. (1990). Revisiting shame and guilt cultures: A forty- classroom, and professional science (pp. 393–413). Mahwah, NJ:
year pilgrimage. Ethos, 18, 279–307. Erlbaum.
Creswell, C., & O’Connor, T. G. (2011). Cognitive-behavioural Crowley, K., Callanan, M. A., Tenenbaum, H. R., & Allen, E. (2001).
therapy for children and adolescents. In D. Skuse, H. Bruce, L. Parents explain more often to boys than to girls during shared sci-
Dowdney, & D. Mrazek (Eds.), Child psychology and psychiatry: entific thinking. Psychological Science, 12, 258–261.
Frameworks for practice (2nd ed., pp. 265–270). Oxford, UK: Crowther, H. L., Lew, A. R., & Whitaker, C. J. (2000). The devel-
Wiley-Blackwell. opment of beacon use for spatial orientation in 6–8.5-month-old
Crews, F. (1996). The verdict on Freud. Psychological Science, 7, 63–68. infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 41–59.
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation Crozier, W. R., & Dimmock, P. S. (1999). Name-calling and nick-
of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social names in a sample of primary school children. British Journal of
adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74–101. Educational Psychology, 69, 505–516.
Crick, N. R., & Nelson, D. A. (2002). Relational and physical victim- Crystal, D. (1987). Children of the wild. In The Cambridge encyclopedia
ization within friendships: Nobody told me there’d be friends like of language (p. 289). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
these. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 599–607. Crystal, D. S., Parrott, W. G., Okazaki, Y., & Watanabe, H. (2001).
Crick, N. R., Nelson, D. A., Morales, J. R., Cullerton-Sen, C., Examining relations between shame and personality among uni-
Casas, J. F., & Hickman, S. E. (2001). Relational victimization in versity students in the United States and Japan: A developmental
childhood and adolescence: I hurt you through the grapevine. In perspective. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25,
J. Juvonen & S. Graham (Eds.), Peer harassment in school: The plight 113–123.
R-28 REFERENCES
Csibra, G. (2010). Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Dahl, S. G., & Sylte, I. (2005). Molecular modelling of drug tar-
Mind & Language, 25, 141–168. gets: The past, the present and the future. Basic and Clinical
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2005). Social learning and social cognition: Pharmacology and Toxicology, 96, 151–155.
The case for pedagogy. In Y. Munakata & M. H. Johnson (Eds.), Dale, P. S., Jenkins, J. R., Mills, P. E., & Cole, K. N. (2005). Follow-up
Processes of change in brain and cognitive development: Attention and of children from academic and cognitive preschool curricula at 12
performance, XXI (pp. 249–274). Oxford, UK: Oxford University and 16. Special Education, 71, 301–317.
Press. Daley, T. C., Whaley, S. E., Sigman, M. D., Espinosa, M. P., &
Csibra, G., & Johnson, M. H. (1998). Visual processing and action Neumann, C. (2003). IQ on the rise—The Flynn effect in rural
control in 6-month-olds: An HD-ERP approach. International Kenyan children. Psychological Science, 14, 215–219.
Society of Infant Studies Conference, April 2–5, 1998. Dallam, S. J., Gleaves, D. H., Cepeda-Benito, A., Silberg, J. L.,
Csibra, G., & Volein, A. (2008). Infants can infer the presence of Kraemer, H. C., & Spiegel, D. (2001). The effects of childhood
hidden objects from referential gaze information. British Journal of sexual abuse: Comment on Rind, Tromovitch and Bauserman
Developmental Psychology, 26, 1–11. (1998). Psychological Bulletin, 127, 715–733.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of Dalsgaard, S. (2013). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
discovery and invention. New York: HarperCollins. (ADHD). European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 22, 43–48.
Cuban, L. (2001). Oversold and underused: Computers in the classroom. Daly, M., & Wilson, M. I. (1996). Violence against stepchildren.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 77–81.
Cuddy, A. J. C., Norton, M. I., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). This old stereo- Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (2001). An assessment of some proposed
type: The pervasiveness and persistence of the elderly stereotype. exceptions to the phenomena of nepotistic discrimination against
Journal of Social Issues, 61, 265–283. stepchildren. Annales Zoologici Fennici, 38, 287–296.
Cummings, E. M., & Bjork, E. L. (1983). Search behavior on multi- Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the
choice hiding tasks: Evidence for an objective conception of space in human brain. New York: Putnam.
infancy. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 6, 71–87. Damasio, A. R. (2001). Emotion and the human brain. In Annals of
Currin, A., Schmidt, U., Treasure, J., & Jick, H. (2005). Time trends the New York Academy of Sciences: Vol. 935. Unity of knowledge: The
in eating disorder incidence. British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, convergence of natural and human science (pp. 101–106). New York:
132–135. New York Academy of Sciences.
Curtiss, S. (1977). Genie: A psycholinguistic study of a modern-day “wild Damon, W. (1980). Patterns of change in children’s social reasoning:
child.” New York: Academic Press. A two-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 51, 1010–1017.
Cushman, F., & Young, L. (2011). Patterns of moral judgment derive Damon, W. (1990). The moral child: Nurturing children’s natural moral
from nonmoral psychological representations. Cognitive Science, growth. New York: Free Press.
35, 1052–1075. Damon, W., & Hart, D. (1982). The development of self-understanding
Cushman, F. A., Young, L., & Greene, J. D. (2010). Multi-system from infancy through adolescence. Child Development, 53,
moral psychology. In J.Doris (Ed.), The moral psychology handbook. 841–864.
New York: Oxford University Press. Damon, W., & Hart, D. (1988). Self-understanding in childhood and
Custer, W. L. (1996). A comparison of young children’s understand- adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press.
ing of contradictory representations in pretense, memory, and Dan, N., Omori, T., & Tomiyasu, Y. (2000). Development of
belief. Child Development, 67, 678–688. infants’ intuitions about support relations: Sensitivity to stability.
Cutting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (2002). The cost of understanding other Developmental Science, 3, 171–180.
people: Social cognition predicts young children’s sensitivity to Danovitch, J. H., & Keil, F. C. (2007). Choosing between hearts
criticism. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 43, 849–860. and minds: Children’s understanding of moral advisors. Cognitive
Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A. N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2011). Math-gender Development, 22, 110–123.
stereotypes in elementary school children. Child Development, 82, Danovitch, J. H., & Keil, F. C. (2008). Young Humeans: The role
766–779. of emotions in children’s evaluation of moral reasoning abilities.
Cyranoski, D. (2007). Simple switch turns cells embryonic. Nature, Developmental Science, 11, 33–39.
447, 618–619. Danovitch, J., & Bloom, P. (2009). Children’s extension of disgust to
Czeizel, A. E., Puhó, H. E., Ács, N., & Bánhidy, F. (2007). High fever- physical and moral events. Emotion, 9, 107–112.
related maternal diseases as possible causes of multiple congenital Danovitch, J., & Keil, F. C. (2004). Should you ask a fisherman or a
abnormalities: A population-based case-control study. Birth Defects biologist? Developmental shifts in ways of clustering knowledge.
Research, Part A: Clinical and Molecular Teratology, 79, 544–551. Child Development, 75, 918–931.
Darley, J. (1993). Research on morality: Possible approaches, actual
D’Entremont, B., Hains, S. M. J., & Muir, D. W. (1997). A demon- approaches. Psychological Science, 4, 353–357.
stration of gaze following in 3- to 6-month-olds. Infant Behavior Darling, N., Cumsille, P., Caldwell, L., & Dowdy, B. (2006).
and Development, 20, 569–572. Predictors of adolescents’ disclosure to parents and perceived
D’Esposito, M. (2007). From cognitive to neural models of working parental knowledge: Between- and within-person differences.
memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 667–678.
Series B, Biological Sciences, 362, 761–772. Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as context: An
Daffner, K. R., Mesulam, M. M., Scinto, L. F. M., Acar, D., Calvo, integrative model. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 487–496.
V., Faust, R., Chabrerie, A., Kennedy, B., & Holcomb, P. J. (2000). Darlington, R. B. (1986). Long-term effects of preschool programs.
The central role of the prefrontal cortex in directing attention to In U. Neisser (Ed.), The school achievement of minority children
novel events. Brain, 123, 927–939. (pp. 159–167). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dahl, R. E. (2004). Adolescent brain development: A period of vul- Darlington, R., Royce, J., Snipper, A., Murray, H., & Lazar, I. (1980).
nerabilities and opportunities. Keynote address. Annals of the New Preschool programs and later school competence of children from
York Academy of Sciences 1021, 1–22. low-income families. Science, 208, 202–294.
REFERENCES R-29
Darwin, C. (1872/1975). The expression of the emotions in man and De Haan, M., Mishkin, M., Baldeweg, T., & Vargha-Khadem, F.
animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (2006). Human memory development and its dysfunction after
Darwin, C. (1877). A biographical sketch of an infant. Mind, 2, early hippocampal injury. Trends in Neurosciences, 29, 374–381.
286–294. de Hevia, M. D., & Spelke, E. S. (2010). Number-space mapping in
Darwin, C. (1883). The variation in animals and plants under domesti- human infants. Psychological Science, 21, 653–660.
cation (2nd ed., rev.). New York: Appleton and Co. De Jong, P. F., & Van der Leij, A. (2003). Developmental changes
Das, J., de Ruiter, C., Doreleijers, T., & Hillege, S. (2009). Reliability in the manifestation of a phonological deficit in dyslexic children
and construct validity of the Dutch Psychopathy Checklist: Youth learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational
Version: Findings from a sample of male adolescents in a juvenile Psychology, 95, 22–40.
justice treatment institution. Assessment, 16, 88–102. de Kieviet, J. F., Piek, J. P., Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S., & Oosterlaan, J.
Dasen, P. R. (1977). Piagetian psychology: Cross-cultural contributions. (2009). Motor development in very preterm and very low-birth-
New York: Gardner Press. weight children from birth to adolescence: A meta-analysis. Journal
Davis, B. L., MacNeilage, P. F., Matyear, C. L., & Powell, J. K. (2000). of the American Medical Association, 302, 2235–2242.
Prosodic correlates of stress in babbling: An acoustical study. Child de la Haye, K., Robins, G., Mohr, P., & Wilson, C. (2010). Obesity-
Development, 71, 1258–1270. related behaviors in adolescent friendship networks. Social
Davis, J. I., Senghas, A., & Ochsner, K. N. (2009). How does facial Networks, 32, 161–167.
feedback modulate emotional experience? Journal of Research in de Maat, S., de Jonghe, F., Schoevers, R., & Dekker, J. (2009). The
Personality, 43, 822–829. effectiveness of long-term psychoanalytic therapy: A systematic
Davis, P. E., Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (2011). Self-knowledge review of empirical studies. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 17, 1–23.
in childhood: Relations with children’s imaginary companions de Quervain, D. J., & Papassotiropoulos, A. (2006). Identification
and understanding of mind. British Journal of Developmental of a genetic cluster influencing memory performance and hippo-
Psychology, 29, 680–686. campal activity in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Davis, S. F, Grover, C. A, Becker, A. H, & McGregor, L. N. (1992). Sciences of the USA, 103, 4270–4274.
Academic dishonesty: Prevalence, determinants, techniques, and de Vignemont, F., & Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: How,
punishments. Teaching of Psychology, 19, 16–20. when and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 435–441.
Davis-Kean, P. E. (2005). The influence of parent education and de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (1978). Language acquisition.
family income on child achievement: The indirect role of paren- Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
tal expectations and the home environment. Journal of Family de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (2000). Linguistic determinism
Psychology, 19, 294–304. and the understanding of false beliefs. In P. Mitchell & K. Riggs
Davis-Kean, P. E., Huesmann, L. R., Jager, J., Collins, W. A., (Eds.), Children’s reasoning and the mind (pp. 189–226). Hove,
Bates, J. E., & Lansford, J. E. (2008). Changes in the relation UK: Psychology Press.
of self-efficacy beliefs and behaviors across development. Child de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (2009). Complements enable
Development, 79, 1257–1269. representation of the contents of false beliefs: The evolution of
Davydov, V. V., &. Kerr, S. T. (1995). The influence of L. S. Vygotsky a theory of theory of mind. In S. Foster-Cohen (Ed.), Language
on education theory, research, and practice. Educational Researcher, acquisition (pp. 169–195). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
24, 12–21. De Vos, G. (1973). Some observations of guilt in relation to achieve-
Dawel, A., O’Kearney, R., McKone, E., & Palermo, R. (2012). Not ment and arranged marriage. Socialization for achievement
just fear and sadness: Meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion (pp. 144–169). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy. de Waal, F. (1991). The chimpanzee’s sense of social regularity and
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 2288–2304. its relation to the human sense of justice. American Behavioral
Dawes, R. M. (1994). House of cards: Psychology and psychotherapy built Scientist, 34, 335–349.
on myth. New York: Free Press. de Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Good natured: The origins of right and wrong
Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University in humans and other animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press. Press.
Dawood, K., Bailey, J. M., & Martin, N. G. (2009). Genetic and envi- De Waal, F. B. M., Dindo, M., Freeman, C. A., & Hall, M. A.
ronmental influences on sexual orientation. In Y.-K. Kim (Ed.), (2005). Monkey in the mirror: Hardly a stranger. Proceedings of the
Handbook of behavior genetics (pp. 269–279). New York: Springer. National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 102, 11140–11147.
Dawson, G., Panagiotides, H., Klinger, L. G., & Hill, D. (1992). The De Wolff, M. S., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and
role of frontal lobe functioning in the development of infant self- attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant
regulatory behavior. Brain & Cognition, 20, 152–175. attachment. Child Development, 68, 571–591.
Dayan, E., & Cohen, L. G. (2011). Neuroplasticity subserving motor Dean, D. (1992, July 26). The little emperors. Los Angeles Times
skill learning. Neuron, 72, 443–454. Magazine, pp. 17–22.
De Dreu, C. K., Greer, L. L., Handgraaf, M. J., Shalvi, S., Van Kleef, Dearing, E., McCartney, K., & Taylor, B. A. (2009). Does higher
G. A., Baas, M., Ten Velden, F. S., Van Dijk, E., & Feith, S. W. quality early child care promote low-income children’s math and
(2010). The neuropeptide oxytocin regulates parochial altruism in reading achievement in middle childhood? Child Development, 80,
intergroup conflict among humans. Science, 328, 1408–1411. 1329–1349.
De Groot, A. D. (1965). Thought and choice in chess. New York: Basic Deary, I. J. (2012). Intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 63,
Books. 453–482.
De Haan, M. (2010). Birth order, family size and educational attain- Deary, I. J., Corley, J., Gow, A. J., Harris, S. E., Houlihan, L. M.,
ment. Economics of Education Review, 29, 576–588. Marioni, R. E., Penke, L., Rafnsson, S. B., & Starr, J. M. (2009). Age-
de Haan, M., Johnson, M. H., Maurer, D., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). associated cognitive decline. British Medical Bulletin, 92, 135–152.
Recognition of individual faces and average face prototypes by Deary, I. J., Johnson, W., & Houlihan, L. M. (2009). Genetic founda-
1- and 3-month-old infants. Cognitive Development, 16, 659–678. tions of human intelligence. Human Genetics, 126, 215–232.
R-30 REFERENCES
Deary, I. J., Penke, L., & Johnson, W. (2010). The neuroscience of chology (pp.121–146). Washington, DC: American Psychological
human intelligence differences. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, Association.
201–211. Degirmencioglu, S. M., Urberg, K. A., Tolson, J. M., & Richard, P.
Deary, I. J., Spinath, F. M., & Bates, T. C. (2006). Genetics of intel- (1998). Adolescent friendship networks: Continuity and change
ligence. European Journal of Human Genetics, 14, 690–700. over the school year. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 313–336.
Deary, I. J, & Stough, C. (1996). Intelligence and inspection time: DeGrandpre, R. (1999). Ritalin nation. New York: W. W. Norton.
Achievements, prospects, and problems. American Psychologist, 51, Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: How the mind creates mathemat-
599–608. ics. New York: Oxford University Press.
Deater-Deckard, K., & Dodge, K. (1997). Spare the rod, spoil the Dehaene, S. (2009). Origins of mathematical intuitions: The case
authors: Emerging themes in research on parenting and child of arithmetic. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156,
development. Psychological Inquiry 8, 230–235. 232–259.
Deater-Deckard, K., Petrill, S. A., Thompson, L. A., & DeThorne, Dehaene, S., Molko, N., Cohen, L., & Wilson, A. (2004). Arithmetic
L. S. (2005.) A cross-sectional behavioral genetic analysis of task and the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 218–224.
persistence in the transition to middle childhood. Developmental Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., & Tsivkin, S. (1999).
Science, 8, F21–F26. Sources of mathematical thinking: Behavioral and brain-imaging
DeBell, M., & Chapman, C. (2003). Computer and Internet use by chil- evidence. Science, 284, 970–974.
dren and adolescents in 2001 (NCES 2004–014). U. S. Department Delaney, J. (2008, August 6). “Mosquito” prompts teens to buzz off.
of Education. Washington, DC: National Center for Education The Epoch Times.
Statistics. Delate, T., Gelenberg, A. J., Simmons, V. A., & Motheral, B. R.
Decarie, T. G. (1965). Intelligence and affectivity in early childhood: An (2004). Trends in the use of antidepressants in a national sample of
experimental study of Jean Piaget’s object concept and object relations commercially insured pediatric patients, 1998 to 2002. Psychiatric
(E. P. Brandt & Lewis W. Brandt, Trans.). New York: International Services, 55, 387–391.
Universities Press. Del Carmen, R., Pedersen, F. A., Huffman, L. C., & Bryan, Y. E.
Decarie, T. G. (1969). A study of the mental and emotional (1993). Dyadic distress management predicts subsequent security
development of the thalidomide child. In B. M. Moss (Ed.), of attachment. Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 131–147.
Determinants of infant behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 167–187). London: DeLoache, J. S. (1987). Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of
Methuen. very young children. Science, 238, 1556–1557.
DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns DeLoache, J. S. (1989). Young children’s understanding of the cor-
prefer their mothers’ voices. Science, 280, 1174–1176. respondence between a scale model and a larger space. Cognitive
DeCasper, A. J., Lecanuet, J. P., Busnel, M. C., Granier-Deferre, C., & Development, 4, 121–139.
Maugeais, R. (1994). Fetal reactions to recurrent maternal speech. DeLoache, J. S. (1991). Symbolic functioning in very young children:
Infant Behavior & Development, 17, 159–164. Understanding of pictures and models. Child Development, 62,
DeCasper, A. J., & Spence, M. J. (1986). Prenatal maternal speech 736–752.
influences newborns’ perception of speech sounds. Infant DeLoache, J. S. (2000). Dual representation and young children’s use
Behavior & Development, 9, 133–150. of scale models. Child Development, 71, 329–338.
Decety, J. (2010a). The neurodevelopment of empathy in humans. DeLoache, J. S. (2004). Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in
Developmental Neuroscience, 32, 257–267. Cognitive Sciences, 8, 66–70.
Decety, J. (2010b). To what extent is the experience of empathy medi- DeLoache, J. S., & Burns, N. M. (1994). Early understanding of the
ated by shared neural circuits? Emotion Review, 2, 204–207. representational function of pictures. Cognition, 52, 83–110.
Decety, J., & Chaminade, T. (2003). Neural correlates of feeling sym- DeLoache, J. S., Cassidy, D. J., & Brown, A. L. (1985). Precursors of
pathy. Neuropsychologia, 41, 127–138. mnemonic strategies in very young children. Child Development,
Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2006). Human empathy through the lens of 56, 125–137.
social neuroscience. Scientific World Journal, 6, 1146–1163. DeLoache, J. S., Kolstad, V., & Anderson, K. N. (1991). Physical simi-
Decety, J., & Michalska, K. J. (2010). Neurodevelopmental changes larity and young children’s understanding of scale models. Child
in the circuits underlying empathy and sympathy from childhood Development, 62, 111–126.
to adulthood. Developmental Science, 13, 886–899. DeLoache, J. S., & LoBue, V. (2009). The narrow fellow in the grass:
Decety, J., Michalska, K. J., & Kinzler, K. D. (2011). The devel- Human infants associate snakes and fear. Developmental Science,
opmental neuroscience of moral sensitivity. Emotion Review, 3, 12, 201–207.
305–307. DeLoache, J. S., Miller, K. F., & Rosengren, K. S. (1997). The cred-
Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrin- ible shrinking room: Very young children’s performance with
sic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, symbolic and nonsymbolic relations. Psychological Science, 8,
105–115. 308–313.
Deci, E. (1972). Intrinsic motivation, extrinsic reinforcement, and DeLoache, J. S., Pierroutsakos, S., & Uttal, D. H. (2003). The origins
inequity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 113–120. of pictorial competence. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic 12, 114–118.
review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards deLuzuriaga, T. (2008, June 6). Gloucester stymied by rise in teen
on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–668. pregnancy, Boston Globe.
Dediu, D., & Ladd, D. R. (2007). Linguistic tone is related to the Demetras, M. J., Post, K. N, & Snow, C. E. (1986). Feedback to first
population frequency of the adaptive haplogroups of two brain language learners: The role of repetitions and clarification ques-
size genes, ASPM and Microcephalin. Proceedings of the National tions. Journal of Child Language, 13, 275–292.
Academy of Sciences of the USA, 104, 10944–10949. Deming, D. (2009). Early childhood intervention and life-cycle skill
DeFries, J. C., & Gillis, J. J. (1993). Genetics of reading disability. development: Evidence from Head Start. American Economic
In R. Plomin & G. E. McClearn (Eds.), Nature, nurture, and psy- Journal: Applied Economics, 1, 111–134.
REFERENCES R-31
Demo, D. H., & Acock, A. C. (1996). Family structure, family pro- Diamond, A. (1985). Development of the ability to use recall to guide
cess, and adolescent well-being. Journal of Research on Adolescence, action, as indicated by infants’ performance on A-not-B. Child
6, 457–488. Development, 56, 868–883.
Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. London: Penguin. Diamond, A. (1991). Neuropsychological insights into the meaning of
Dennis, W., & Dennis, M. G. (1940). The effect of cradling practices object concept development. In S. Carey & R. Gelman (Eds.), The
upon the onset of walking in Hopi children. Journal of Genetic epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition. The Jean Piaget
Psychology, 56, 77–86. symposium series (pp. 67–110). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dennis, W., & Najarian, P. (1957). Infant development under environ- Diamond, A. (2005). Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/
mental handicap. Psychological Monographs, 71 (7, Whole No. 436). hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologi-
Depp, C. A., & Jeste, D. V. (2006). Definitions and predictors of suc- cally and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit/
cessful aging: A comprehensive review of larger quantitative stud- hyperactivity disorder (with hyperactivity). Development and
ies. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14, 6–20. Psychopathology, 17, 807–825.
Deppe, A. M., Wright, P. C., & Szelistowski, W. A. (2009). Object Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology,
permanence in lemurs. Animal Cognition, 12, 381–388. 64, 135–168.
Derisley, J. (2004). Cognitive therapy for children, young people Diamond, A., Cruttenden, L., & Neiderman, D. (1994). A-not-B with
and families: Considering service provision. Child and Adolescent multiple wells: 1.Why are multiple wells sometimes easier than
Mental Health, 9, 15–20. two wells? 2. Memory or memory + inhibition? Developmental
Descartes, R. (1649/1967). Treatise on the passions of the soul. In Psychology, 30, 192–205.
The philosophical works of Descartes (E. S. Halden & G. R. T. Ross, Diamond, A., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1989). Comparison of human
Trans.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. infants and rhesus monkeys on Piaget’s A-not-B task: Evidence
DeScioli, P., & Kurzban, R. (2009). Mysteries of morality. Cognition, for dependence on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Behavioral
112, 281–299. Neuroscience, 103, 526–537.
Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective Diamond, J. (1996). Why women change. Discover, 17, 130–137.
visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222. Diaz, R. M. (1983). The impact of bilingualism on cognitive devel-
Dettman, S., & Dowell, R. (2010). Language acquisition and critical opment. In E. W. Gordon (Ed.), Review of research in education
periods for children using cochlear implants. In P. E. Nathan (Series (Vol. 10, pp. 23–54). Washington, DC: American Educational
Ed.) & M. Marchark, P. E. Spencer (Vol. Eds.), The Oxford hand- Research Association.
book of deaf studies, language, and education (Vol. 2, pp. 331–342). Diaz, R. M. (1985). Bilingual cognitive development: Addressing
New York: Oxford University Press. three gaps in current research. Child Development, 56, 1376–1388.
Deur, J. L., & Parke, R. D. (1970). Effects of inconsistent punishment Dickens, W. T., & Flynn, J. R. (2001). Heritability estimates versus
on aggression in children. Developmental Psychology, 2, 403–411. large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved. Psychological
Deutsch, C., Swanson, J., Bruell, J., Cantwell, D., Weinberg, F., & Review, 108, 346–349.
Baren, M. (1982). Overrepresentation of adoptees in children with Dickens, W. T. & Flynn, J. R. (2006). Black Americans reduce the
the attention deficit disorder. Behavior Genetics, 12, 231–238. racial IQ gap: Evidence from standardization samples. Psychological
Deveney, C. M., Brotman, M. A., Decker, A. M., Pine, D. S., & Science, 17, 913–920.
Leibenluft, E. (2012) Affective prosody labeling in youths with Diehl, M. (1998). Everyday competence in later life: Current status
bipolar disorder or severe mood dysregulation. Journal of Child and future directions. Gerontologist, 38, 4, 422–433.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 262–270. Diener, E., & Wallbom, M. (1976). Effects of self-awareness on anti-
Devlin, B., Bacanu, S. A., Klump, K. L., Bulik, C. M., Fichter, M. M., normative behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 10, 107–111.
Halmi, K. A., Kaplan, A. S., Strober, M., Treasure, J., Woodside, Diesendruck, G., & Bloom, P. (2003). How specific is the shape bias?
D. B., Berrettini, W. H., & Kaye, W. H. (2002). Linkage analysis Child Development, 74, 168–178.
of anorexia nervosa incorporating behavioral covariates. Human Diesendruck, G., & Markson, L. (2001). Children’s avoidance of lexical
Molecular Genetics, 11, 689–696. overlap: A pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology, 37, 630–644.
Devlin, B., & Scherer, S. W. (2012). Genetic architecture in autism Dillard, J. L. (1972). Black English: Its history and usage in the United
spectrum disorder. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development, States. New York: Random House.
22, 229–237. Diller, L. (1998). Running on Ritalin. New York: Bantam Books.
DeVoogd, T. J. (1991). Endocrine modulation of the development and Diller, L., Tanner, J., & Weil, J. (1995). Etiology of ADHD: Nature or
adult function of the avian song system. Psychoneuroendocrinology, nurture? [Letter to the editor]. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153,
16, 41–66. 451–452.
DeVries, M. W., & Sameroff, A. J. (1984). Culture and temperament: Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is
Influences on infant temperament in three East African societies. good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 285–290.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 54, 83–96. diSessa, A. (1988). Knowledge in pieces. In G. Forman & G. P. Pufall
Dew, P. C., Guillory, V. J., Okah, F. A., Cai, J., & Hoff, G. L. (2007). (Eds.), Constructivism in the computer age (pp. 49–70). Hillsdale,
The effect of health compromising behaviors on preterm births. NJ: Erlbaum.
Maternal and Child Health Journal, 11, 227–233. Dishion, T. J., Andrews, D. W., & Crosby, L. (1995). Antisocial boys
Dewar, K., & Xu, F. (2010). Induction, overhypothesis, and the ori- and their friends in early adolescence: Relationship characteristics,
gin of abstract knowledge: Evidence from 9-month-old infants. quality, and interactional process. Child Development, 66, 139–151.
Psychological Science, 21, 1871–1877. Dixon, R. A., de Frias, C. M., & Bäckman, L. (2001). Characteristics
Dewey, J. (1902/1991). The school and society and the child and the cur- of self-reported memory compensation in older adults. Journal of
riculum. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 650–661.
Di Giorgio, E., Leo, I., Pascalis, O., & Simion, F. (2012). Is the Dixon, S., Snyder, J., Holve, R., & Bromberger, P. (1984). Behavioral
face-perception system human-specific at birth? Developmental effects of circumcision with and without anesthesia. Journal of
Psychology, 48, 1083–1090. Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 5, 246–250.
R-32 REFERENCES
Dobkins, K. R., Bosworth, R. G., & McCleery, J. P. (2009). Effects of Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C., Johnson, B., & Howard, A.
gestational length, gender, postnatal age, and birth order on visual (1997). On the nature of prejudice: Automatic and controlled pro-
contrast sensitivity in infants. Journal of Vision, 9, 1–21. cesses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 510–540.
Dodd, J., & Jessell, T. M. (1988). Axon guidance and the patterning Dovidio, J. F., Piliavin, J. A., Schroeder, D. A., & Penner, L. A. (2006).
of neuronal projections in vertebrates. Science, 242, 692–699. The social psychology of prosocial behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dodge, K. A. (1991). The structure and function of reactive and Doyle, O., Harmon, C. P., Heckman, J. J., & Tremblay, R. E. (2009).
proactive aggression. In D. J. Pepler & K. H. Rubin (Eds.), The Investing in early human development: Timing and economic effi-
development and treatment of childhood aggression (pp. 201–216). ciency. Economics & Human Biology, 7, 1–6.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Drabman, R. S., & Thomas, M. H. (1974). Does media violence
Dodge, K.A. (2002). Mediation, moderation, and mechanisms in how increase children’s tolerance for real-life aggression? Developmental
parenting affects children’s aggressive behavior. In J. Borkowski Psychology, 10, 418–421.
(Ed.), Parenting and the child’s world (pp. 215–229). Hillsdale, NJ: Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., &
Erlbaum. May, A. (2004). Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced
Dodge, K. A. (2006). Translational science in action: Hostile attribu- by training. Nature, 427, 311–312.
tional style and the development of aggressive behavior problems. Draghi-Lorenz, R. (2000). Five-month-old infants can be jealous:
Development & Psychopathology, 18, 791–814. Against cognitivist solipsism. Paper presented in a symposium
Dodge, K. A., Lansford, J. E., Burks, V. S., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., convened for the XIIth Biennial of the International Conference
Fontaine, R., & Price, J. M. (2003). Peer rejection and social on Infant Studies (ICIS), 16–19 July, Brighton, UK.
information-processing factors in the development of aggressive Draghi-Lorenz, R., Reddy, V., & Costall, A. (2001). Rethinking the
behavior problems in children. Child Development, 74, 374–393. development of “nonbasic” emotions: A critical review of existing
Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). A biopsychosocial model of theories. Developmental Review, 21, 263–304.
the development of chronic conduct problems in adolescence. Drake, A. J., Walker, B. R., & Seckl, J. R. (2005). Intergenerational
Developmental Psychology, 39, 349–371. consequences of fetal programming by in utero exposure to glu-
Doherty, J. P., Norton, E. C., & Veney, J. E. (2001). China’s one-child cocorticoids in rats. American Journal of Physiology— Regulatory,
policy: The economic choices and consequences faced by pregnant Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 288, R34–R38.
women. Social Science & Medicine, 52, 745–761. Driscoll, I., Hamilton, D. A., Petropoulos, H., Yeo, R. A., Brooks,
Dolan, M. (2004). Psychopathic personality in young people. Advances W. M., Baumgartner, R. N., & Sutherland, R. J. (2003). The aging
in Psychiatric Treatment, 10, 466–473. hippocampus: Cognitive, biochemical and structural findings.
Dole, A. A. (1995). Why not drop “race” as a term? American Cerebral Cortex, 13, 1344-1351.
Psychologist, 50, 40. Drlica, K. A. (1994). Double-edged sword: The promises and risks of the
Dolinoy, D.C. (2007). Epigenetic gene regulation: Early environmen- genetic revolution. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
tal exposures. Phrarmacogenetics, 8, 5–10. Duboule, D. (Ed.). (1994). Guidebook to the homeobox genes. Oxford,
Domes, G., Heinrichs, M., Michel, A., Berger, C., & Herpertz, S. C. UK: Oxford University Press.
(2007). Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biological Duckworth, A. L., Quinn, P. D., & Tsukayama, E. (2012). What No
Psychiatry 61, 731–733. Child Left Behind leaves behind: The roles of IQ and self-control
Donaldson, M. (1978). Children’s minds. Glasgow: Fontana. in predicting standardized achievement test scores and report card
Donaldson, M. (1983). Children’s reasoning. In M. Donaldson, R. grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 439–451.
Grieve, & C. Pratt (Eds.), Early childhood development and educa- Dueker, G. L., & Needham, A. (2005). Infants’ object category for-
tion. Oxford: Blackwell. mation and use: Real-world context effects on category use in
Donaldson, M. (1985). Children’s minds (2nd ed.). London: Flamingo. object processing. Visual Cognition, 12, 1177–1198.
Donaldson, M. (1987). The origins of inference. In J. Bruner & Duff, S. J., & Hampson, E. (2001). A sex difference on a novel spatial
H. Haste (Eds.), Making sense: The child’s construction of the world working memory task in humans. Brain and Cognition, 47, 470–493.
(pp. 97–108). New York: Methuen. Duit, R., & Treagust, D. F. (2003). Conceptual change: A power-
Donlan, C., Cowan, R., Newton, E. J., & Lloyd, D. (2007). The role ful framework for improving science teaching and learning.
of language in mathematical development: Evidence from children International Journal of Science Education, 25, 671–688.
with specific language impairments. Cognition, 103, 23–33. Dulawa, S. C., Grandy, D. K., Low, M. J., Paulus, M. P., & Geyer,
Doris, J. M. 2002. Lack of character: Personality and moral behavior. M. A. (1999). Dopamine D4 receptor-knock-out mice exhibit
New York: Cambridge University Press reduced exploration of novel stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience, 19,
Dornbusch, S. M., Carlsmith, J. M., Gross, R. T., Martin, J. A., 9550–9556.
Jennings, D., Rosenberg, A., & Duke, P. (1981). Sexual devel- Dunbar, K. (1999). How scientists build models in vivo: Science as a
opment, age, and dating: A comparison of biological and social window on the scientific mind. In L. Magnani, N. J. Nersessian, &
influences upon one set of behaviors. Child Development, 52, P. Thagard (Eds.), Model-based reasoning in scientific discovery.
179–185. New York: Plenum Press.
Dorris, L., Espie, C. A. E., Knott, F., & Salt, J. (2004). Mind-reading Duncan, J. (1996). Cooperating brain systems in selective perception
difficulties in the siblings of people with Asperger’s syndrome: and action. In T. Inui & J. L. McClelland (Eds.), Attention and
Evidence for a genetic influence in the abnormal development performance XVI: Information integration in perception and commu-
of a specific cognitive domain. Journal of Child Psychology and nication (pp. 549–578). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Psychiatry, 45, 412–418. Duncan, J. (2006). Brain mechanisms of attention. Quarterly Journal
Dougherty, D. D., Bonab, A. A., Spencer, T. J., Rauch, S. L., Madras, of Experimental Psychology, 59, 2–27.
B. K., & Fischman, A. J. (1999). Dopamine transporter density in Duncan, S. (2007). Teratogenesis of sodium valproate. Current
patients with ADHD. Lancet, 354, 2132–2133. Opinion in Neurology, 20, 175–180.
Douglas, A. S. (1994). Recontextualizing schooling within an Inuit Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs,
community. Canadian Journal of Education, 19, 154–164. 58 (Whole No. 270).
REFERENCES R-33
Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2011). Consequences of “mini- attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in twins. In R. Plomin &
mal” group affiliations in children. Child Development, 82, 793–811. G. McClearn (Eds.), Nature, nurture, & psychology (pp. 285–303).
Dunham, Y., Chen, E., & Banaji, M. R. (2013). Two signatures of Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
implicit intergroup attitudes: Developmental invariance and early Eberstadt, N., & Torrey, B. B. (2005). The Northern America fertility
enculturation. Psychological Science, 24, 811–812. divide. Policy Review, 132.
Dunn, J. (1988). The beginnings of social understanding. Oxford, UK: Ebert, J. D., & Sussex, I. M. (1970). Interacting systems in development
Blackwell. (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rhinehart and Winston.
Dunn, J. (2001). The development of children’s conflict and prosocial Ebstein, R. P., Novick, O., Umansky, R., Priel, B., Osher, Y., Blaine,
behaviour: Lessons from research on social understanding and gen- D., Bennett, E. R., Nemanov, L., Katz, M., & Belmaker, R. H.
der. In J. Hill & B. Maughan (Eds.), Conduct disorders in childhood (1996). Dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) exon III polymorphism
and adolescence (pp. 49–66). New York: Cambridge University Press. associated with the human personality trait of novelty seeking.
Dunn, J. (2006). Moral development in early childhood and social Nature Genetics, 12, 78–80.
interaction in the family. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Harold, R. D., & Blumenfeld, P. (1993).
Handbook of moral development (pp. 331–350). Mahwah, NJ: Age and gender differences in children’s self- and task perceptions
Erlbaum. during elementary school. Child Development, 64, 830–847.
Dunn, J., & Hughes, C. (2001). “I got some swords and you’re dead!” Eccleston, C. P., Smyth , J. M., & Lopoo, L. M. (2010). Unraveling
Violent fantasy, antisocial behavior, friendship, and moral sensibil- the race paradox of achievement and self-views. Social Psychology of
ity in young children. Child Development, 72, 491–505. Education, 13, 1–18.
Dunn, J., & Kendrick, C. (1982). Siblings: Love, envy and understand- Eckenrode, J., Laird, M., & Dorris, J. (1993). School performance
ing. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. and disciplinary problems among abused and neglected children.
Dupoux, E., & Jacob, P. (2007). Universal moral grammar: A critical Developmental Psychology, 29, 53–62.
appraisal. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 373–378. Eckert, M. A., Galaburda, A. M., Mills, D. L., Bellugi, U.,
Durbin, D. L., Darling, N., Steinberg, L., & Brown, B. B. (1993). Kronenberg, J. R., & Reiss, A. L. (2006). The neurobiology of
Parenting style and peer group membership among European- Williams syndrome: Cascading influences of visual system impair-
American adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 3, 87–100. ment? Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences, 63, 1867–1875.
Durfee, H., & Wolf, K. (1933). Anstaltspflege und entwicklung im Ecuyer-Dab, I., & Robert, M. (2007). The female advantage in object
ersten lebensjahr [Institutional care and development in the first location memory according to the foraging hypothesis: A critical
year of life]. Zeitschrift fur Kinderforschung, 42, 273–320. analysis. Human Nature, 18, 365–385.
Durston, A. J., Timmermans, J. P. M., Hage, W. J., Hendriks, H. F. J., Edelman, G. M., & Jones, F. S. (1993). Outside and downstream of
De Vries, N. J., Heideveld, M., & Nieuwkoop, P. D. (1989). the homeobox. Journal of Biological Chemistry, 268, 20683–20686.
Retinoic acid causes an anteroposterior transformation in the Edvardsen, J., Torgersen, S., Rovsamb, E., Lygren, S., Skre, I., Onstad, S., &
developing central nervous system. Nature, 340, 140–144. Oien, P. A. (2008). Heritability of bipolar spectrum disorders. Unity or
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. heterogeneity? Journal of Affective Disorders, 106, 229–240.
American Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048. Edwards, C. P. (1987). Culture and the construction of moral values:
Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset. New York: Random House. A comparative ethnography of moral encounters in two cultural
Dweck, C. S. (2012). Implicit theories. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A. W. settings. In J. Kagan & S. Lamb (Eds.), The emergence of morality in
Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.). Handbook of theories of social young children (pp. 123–151). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 43–61). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Effros, R. B. (2009). The canary in the coal mine: Telomeres and
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach human healthspan. Journals of Gerontology, Series A: Biological
to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273. Sciences and Medical Sciences, 64, 511–515.
Dwyer, S. (2009). Moral dumbfounding and the linguistic analogy: Effros, R. B. (2012). Stress and immune system aging. In S. C.
Methodological implications for the study of moral judgment. Segerstrom (Ed.), Oxford handbook of psychoneuroimmunology
Mind & Language, 24, 274–296. (pp. 63–76). New York: Oxford University Press.
Dye, M. W., Hauser, P. C., & Bavelier, D. (2009). Is visual selective Egeland, B. Jacobvitz, D., & Sroufe, L.A. (1988). Breaking the
attention in deaf individuals enhanced or deficient? The case of the cycle of abuse: Relationship predictions. Child Development, 59,
useful field of view. PLoS ONE, 4, e5640. 1080–1088.
Dykas, M. J., Ehrlich, K. B., & Cassidy, J. (2011). Links between Egeland, B., & Hiester, M. (1995). The long-term consequences of
attachment and social information processing: Examination of infant day-care and mother-infant attachment. Child Development,
intergenerational processes. Advances in Child Development and 66, 474–485.
Behavior, 40, 51–94. Egeland, B., & Sroufe, A. (1981a). Attachment and early maltreat-
ment. Child Development, 52, 44-52.
Eacott, M. J., & Crawley, R. A. (1999). Childhood amnesia: On answer- Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1981b). Developmental sequelae of
ing questions about very early life events. Memory, 7, 279–292. maltreatment in infancy. In R. Rizley & D. Cicchett (Eds.),
Easterbrook, M. A., Kisilevsky, B. S., Muir, D. W., & Laplante, D. P. Developmental perspectives on child maltreatment (pp. 77–92). San
(1999). Newborns discriminate schematic faces from scrambled Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
faces. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 231–241. Egeland, B., Sroufe, L. A., & Erickson, M. (1983). The developmental
Eaton, W. W., Tien, A. Y., & Poeschla, B. D. (1995). Epidemiology of consequences of different patterns of maltreatment. Child Abuse &
schizophrenia. In J. A. Den Boer, H. G. M. Westenberg, & H. M. Neglect, 7, 459–469.
van Praag (Eds.), Advances in the neurobiology of schizophrenia. Egliston, K. A., & Rapee, R. M. (2007). Inhibition of fear acqui-
Oxford, UK: Wiley. sition in toddlers following positive modelling by their mothers.
Eaves, L., Silberg, J., Hewitt, J. K., Meyer, J., Rutter, M., Simonoff, E., Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 1871–1882.
Neale, M., & Pickles, A. (1993). Genes, personality, and psy- Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. A., Schuster, B. V., Yaghoub-
chopathogy: A latent class analysis of liability to symptoms of Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction
R-34 REFERENCES
helps children learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Eisenberg, N., & Eggum, N. D. (2009). Empathic responding:
Panel’s metaanalysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 250–287. Sympathy and personal distress. In J. Decety & W. Ickes (Eds.),
Eiholzer, U., Haverkamp, F., & Voss, L. D. (Eds). (1999). Growth, The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 71–83). Cambridge, MA:
stature, and psychosocial well-being. Kirkland, WA: Hogrefe & MIT Press.
Huber Publishers. Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In
Eimas, P. D., & Miller, J. L. (1991). A constraint on the discrimina- W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
tion of speech by young infants. Language & Speech, 34, 251–263. child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality develop-
Eimas, P. D., & Miller, J. L. (1992). Organization in the perception of ment (5th ed., pp. 701–778). New York: Wiley.
speech by young infants. Psychological Science, 3, 340–345. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., & Reiser, M. (2000).
Eimas, P. D., Miller, J. L., & Jusczyk, P. W. (1987). On infant speech Dispositional emotionality and regulation: Their role in predict-
perception and the acquisition of language. In S. Harnad (Ed.), ing quality of social functioning. Journal of Personality and Social
Categorical perception: The groundwork of cognition (pp. 161–195). Psychology, 78, 136–157.
New York: Cambridge University Press. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2006). Prosocial behav-
Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, F. R., Jusczyck, P., & Vigorito, J. (1971). ior. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg
Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303–306. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional
Einav, S., & Robinson, E. J. (2011). When being right is not enough: and personality development (6th ed., pp. 646–718). Hoboken, NJ:
Four-year-olds distinguish knowledgeable informants from merely Wiley.
accurate informants. Psychological Science, 22, 1250–1253. Eisenberg, N., Lennon, R., & Roth, K. (1983). Prosocial development:
Ein-Dor, T., Mikulincer, M., Doron, G., & Shaver, P. R. (2010). The A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 19, 846–855.
attachment paradox: How can so many of us (the insecure ones) Eisenberg, N., Miller, P. A., Shell, R., McNalley, S., & Shea, C.
have no adaptive advantages? Perspectives on Psychological Science, (1991). Prosocial development in adolescence: A longitudinal
5, 123–141. study. Developmental Psychology, 27, 849–857.
Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and pro- Eisenberg, N., & Mussen, P. H. (1989). The roots of prosocial behav-
spective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, ior in children, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Memory, and Cognition, 16, 717–726. Eisenberg, N., Shea, C. L., Carlo, G., & Knight, G. P. (1991).
Einstein, G. O., Smith, R. E., McDaniel, M. A., & Shaw P. (1997). Empathy-related responding and cognition: A “chicken and the
Aging and prospective memory: The influence of increased task egg” dilemma. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz (Eds.), Handbook
demands at encoding and retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 12, of moral behavior and development: Vol. 2. Research (pp. 63–88).
479–488. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Eisen, J. S. (1991). Determination of primary motoneuron identity in Eisenberg, N., Smith, C. L., Sadovsky, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004).
developing zebrafish embryos. Science, 252, 569–572. Effortful control: Relations with emotion regulation, adjustment,
Eisenberg , N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. and socialization in childhood. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs
Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 665–697. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applica-
Eisenberg, J., Mei-Tal, G., Steinberg, A., Tartakovsky, E., Zohar, A., tions (pp. 259–282). New York: Guilford Press.
Gritsenko, I., Nemanov, L., & Ebstein, R. P. (1999). Haplotype Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004). Emotion-related regulation:
relative risk study of catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) and Sharpening the definition. Child Development, 75, 334–339.
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Association Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Eggum, N. D. (2010). Emotion-
of the high-enzyme activity Val allele with ADHD impulsive- related self-regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment.
hyperactive phenotype. American Journal of Medical Genetics Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 495–525.
(Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 88, 497–502. Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. (1996). Detrimental effects of reward:
Eisenberg, J., Zohar, A., Mei-Tal, G., Steinberg, A., Tartakovsky, E., Reality or myth? American Psychologist, 51, 1153–1166.
Gritsenko, I., Nemanov, L., & Ebstein, R. P. (2000). A haplotype Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. (1998). Reward, intrinsic interest and
relative risk study of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) exon III creativity: New findings. American Psychologist, 53, 676–679.
repeat polymorphism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder Eisenberger, R., Rhoades, L., & Cameron, J. (1999). Does pay for
(ADHD). American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric performance increase or decrease perceived self-determination and
Genetics), 96, 258–261. intrinsic motivation? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Eisenberg, L. S., Johnson, K. C., Ambrose, S. E., Martinez, A. S., 77, 1026–1040.
Werner, L. A., & Fay, R. R. (2012). Atypical auditory development Ekman, P. (1973). Cross-cultural studies of facial expression. In
and effects of experience. In L. Werner, R. R. Fay, & A. N. Popper P. Ekman (Ed.), Darwin and facial expression: A century of research
(Eds.), Springer handbook of auditory research. Vol. 42: Human in review (pp. 169–222). New York: Academic Press.
auditory development (pp. 255–277). New York: Springer. Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and
Eisenberg, N. (1986). Altruistic emotion, cognition, and behavior. Emotion, 6, 169–200.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ekman, P. (1994). All emotions are basic. In P. Ekman & R. J.
Eisenberg, N. (2005). The development of empathy-related respond- Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotions (pp. 15–19). New York:
ing. In G. Carlo & C. P. Edwards (Eds.), Nebraska symposium Oxford University Press.
on motivation: Vol. 51. Moral motivation through the lifespan Ekman, P. (2009). Darwin’s contributions to our understanding
(pp. 73–117). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. of emotional expressions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Eisenberg, N., Champion, C., & Ma, Y. (2004). Emotion-related regula- Society B, Biological Sciences, 364, 3449–3451.
tion: An emerging construct. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 236–259. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1975). Unmasking the face: A guide to recogniz-
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., & ing emotions from facial clues. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Shepard, S. A. (2005). Age changes in prosocial responding and Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1978). Facial action coding system: A tech-
moral reasoning in adolescence and early adulthood. Journal of nique for the measurement of facial movement. Palo Alto, CA:
Research on Adolescence, 15, 235–260. Consulting Psychologists Press.
REFERENCES R-35
Ekman, P., & Rosenberg, E. L. (Eds.). (1997). What the face reveals: (Eds.), Constructive & destructive behavior: Implications for fam-
Basic and applied studies of spontaneous expression using the Facial ily, school, and society (pp. 147–165). Washington, DC: American
Action Coding System (FACS). New York: Oxford University Press. Psychological Association.
Elder, G. H. (1974). Children of the great depression: Social change in life Eng, T. S. (2005). The impact of ICT on learning: A review of
experience. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. research. International Journal of Education, 6, 635–660.
Elder, G. H., (1998). The life course and human development. In Engen, T., Lipsitt, L. P., & Peck, M. B. (1974). Ability of newborn
W. Damon (Series Ed.) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of infants to discriminate sapid substances. Developmental Psychology,
child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development 10, 741–744.
(5th ed., pp. 939–991). New York: Wiley. Ennis, R. H. (1975). Children’s ability to handle Piaget’s propositional
Elder, G. H. (1999). Children of the great depression: Social change in life logic: A conceptual critique. Review of Educational Research, 45,
experience (25th anniversary ed.). Boulder, CO: Westview Press. 1–41.
Elliott, D. M. (1997). Traumatic events: Prevalence and delayed Enns, M. P., & Hornung, D. E. (1985). Contributions of smell and
recall in the general population. Journal of Consulting and Clinical taste to overall intensity. Chemical Senses, 10, 357–366.
Psychology, 65, 811–820. Epel, E. S. (2009). Telomeres in a life-span perspective: A new “psycho-
Ellis, E. M. (2000). Divorce wars: Interventions with families in conflict. biomarker”? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 6–10.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Epstein, H. T. (1978). Growth spurts during brain development:
Ellis, N. C., & Hennelly, R. A. (1980). A bilingual word-length effect. Implications for educational policy and practice. In J. S. Chall &
Implications for intelligence testing and the relative ease of mental A. F. Mirsky (Eds.), Education and the brain (pp. 343–370).
calculation in Welsh and English. British Journal of Psychology, 71, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
43–51. Erber, J. T., & Long, B. A. (2006). Perceptions of forgetful and slow
Ellis, S., Rogoff, B., & Cromer, C. (1981). Research reports: Age seg- employees: Does age matter? Journals of Gerontology, Series B:
regation in children’s social interactions. Developmental Psychology, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 61, 333–339.
17, 399–407. Erdelyi, M. R. (1993). Repression: The mechanism and the defense.
Ellwood, D. T., & Jencks, C. (2004, February 26). The spread of single- In D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of mental
parent families in the United States since 1960. KSG Working control (pp. 126–148). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Paper No. RWP04-008. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.517662 Erickson, J. E., Keil, F. C., & Lockhart, K. L. (2010). Sensing the
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., coherence of biology in contrast to psychology: Young children’s
Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connec- use of causal relations to distinguish two foundational domains.
tionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Child Development, 81, 390–409.
Elsabbagh, M., Divan, G., Koh, Y. J., Shin Kim, Y. S., Kauchali, S., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2009). Aerobic exercise effects on
Marcín, C., Montiel-Nava, C., Patel, V., Paula, C. S., Wang, C., cognitive and neural plasticity in older adults. British Journal of
Yasamy, M., & Fombonne, E. (2012). Global prevalence of autism Sports Medicine, 43, 22–24.
and other pervasive developmental disorders. Autism Research, 5, Erickson, M. F., Egeland, B., & Pianta, R. (1989). The effects of mal-
160–179. treatment on the development of young children. In D. Cicchetti &
Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., Goldsmith, H. H., & Van Hulle, V. Carlson (Eds.), Child maltreatment: Theory and research on the
C. A. (2006). Gender differences in temperament: A meta-analysis. causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect (pp. 647–684).
Psychological Bulletin, 132, 33–72. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Eluvathingal, T. J., Chugani, H. T., Behen, M. E., Juhasz, C., Muzik, O., Erickson, R. P., & Covey, E. (1980). On the singularity of taste sensa-
Maqbool, M., Chugani, D. C., & Makki, M. (2006). Abnormal tions: What is a primary taste? Physiology & Behavior, 25, 527–533.
brain connectivity in children after early severe socioemotional Erickson, S. J., & Achilles, G. (2004). Cognitive behavioral therapy
deprivation: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Pediatrics, 117, with children and adolescents. In H. Steiner (Ed.), Handbook of
2093–2100. mental health interventions in children and adolescents: An integrated
Elvevag, B., & Goldberg, T. E. (2000). Cognitive impairment in developmental approach (pp. 525–556). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
schizophrenia is the core of the disorder. Critical Reviews in Ericsson, K. A. (Ed.). (1996). The road to expert performance: Empirical
Neurobiology, 14, 1–21. evidence from the arts and sciences, sports, and games. Mahwah, NJ:
Eme, R. F. (1979). Sex differences in childhood psychopathology: A Erlbaum.
review. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 574–595. Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its
Emery, R. E. (1988). Marriage, divorce, and children’s adjustment. structure and acquisition. American Psychologist, 49, 725–747.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional
Emler, N., Renwick, S., & Malone, B. (1983). The relationship performance: Evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints.
between moral reasoning and political orientation. Journal of Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305.
Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 1073–1080. Erikson, E. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: W. W. Norton.
Emmorey, K., Bellugi, U., Friederici, A., & Horn, P. (1995). Effects Eriksson, P. S., Perfilieva, E., Björk-Eriksson, T., Alborn, A. M.,
of age of acquisition on grammatical sensitivity: Evidence from on- Nordborg, C., Peterson, D. A., & Gage, F. H. (1998). Neurogenesis
line and off-line tasks. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 1–23. in the adult human hippocampus. Nature Medicine, 4, 1313–1317.
Enard, W., Przeworski, M., Fisher, S. E., Lai, C. S. L., Wiebe, V., Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., & Jarvik, L. F. (1963). Genetics and intel-
Kitano, T., Monaco, A. P., & Pääbo, S. (2002). Molecular evolu- ligence: A review. Science, 142, 1477–1479.
tion of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature, Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., Rock, D., Roberts, S. A., Janal, M.,
418, 869–872. Kestenbaum, C., Cornblatt, B., Adamo, U. H., & Gottesman, I. I.
Endresen, I. M., & Olweus, D. (2001). Self-reported empathy in (2000). Attention, memory, and motor skills as childhood predic-
Norwegian adolescents: Sex differences, age trends, and rela- tors of schizophrenia-related psychoses: The New York High-Risk
tionship to bullying. In A. C. Bohart, C. Arthur, & D. J. Stipek Project. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1416–1422.
R-36 REFERENCES
Ernst, M., Moolchan, E. M., & Robinson, M. L. (2001). Behavioral and mind, and reasoning: Structure and development (pp. 111–132).
neural consequences of prenatal exposure to nicotine. Journal of the Amsterdam: Elsevier.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 630–642. Fabricius, W. V., & Wellman, H. M. (1983). Memory development.
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1973). Some strategies for the first two years. In T. E. Journal of Children in a Contemporary Society, 16, 171–187.
Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. Fagan, J. F. (1990). The paired-comparison paradigm and infant
New York: Academic Press. intelligence. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608,
Esbjørn, B. H., Bender, P. K., Reinholdt-Dunne, M. L., Munck, 337–357.
L. A., & Ollendick, T. H. (2012). The development of anxiety disor- Fagan, J. F. (2011). Intelligence in infancy. In R. Sternberg &
ders: Considering the contributions of attachment and emotion reg- S. B. Kaufman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of intelligence
ulation. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 15, 129–143. (pp. 130–143). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Espelage, D. L., Holt, M. K., & Henkel, R. R. (2003). Examination of Fagan, J. W. (1993). Reinforcement is not enough: Learned expectan-
peer-group contextual effects on aggressive behavior during early cies and infant behavior. American Psychologist, 48, 1153–1155.
adolescence. Child Development, 74, 205–220. Falbo, T. (1982). The only child in America. In M. E. Lamb & B.
Espelage, D. L., & Swearer, S. M. (2003). Research on school bullying Sutton-Smith (Eds.), Sibling relationships: Their nature and signifi-
and victimization: What have we learned and where do we go from cance across the lifespan (pp. 285–304). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
here? School Psychology Review, 32, 365–383. Falbo, T., & Polit, D. (1986). Quantitative review of the only child
Espinoza, F. (2005). An analysis of the historical development of ideas literature. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 176–189.
about motion and its implications for teaching. Physics Education, Falbo, T., & Poston, D. L. (1993). The academic, personality, and
40, 139–146. physical outcomes of only children in China. Child Development,
Essex, M. J., Thomas, B. W., Hertzman, C., Lam, L. L., Armstrong, 64, 18–35.
J. M., Neumann, S. M., & Kobor, M. S. (2013). Epigenetic vestiges Falch, T., & Sandgren Massih, S. (2011). The effect of education on
of early developmental adversity: Childhood stress exposure and cognitive ability. Economic Inquiry, 49, 838–856.
DNA methylation in adolescence. Child Development, 84, 58–75. Falck-Ytter, T., Fernell, E., Hedvall, A. L., von Hofsten, C., & Gillberg,
Estell, D. B., Farmer, T. W., Pearl, R., Van Acker, R., & Rodkin, C. (2012). Gaze performance in children with autism spectrum
P. C. (2003). Heterogeneity in the relationship between popularity disorder when observing communicative actions. Journal of Autism
and aggression: Individual, group, and classroom influences. New and Developmental Disorders, 42, 2236–2245.
Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, Fall, 75–85. Falk, R. (1986). What is a gene? Studies in the History and Philosophy
Estell, D. B., Farmer, T. W., Pearl, R., Van Acker, R., & Rodkin, of Science, 17, 133–173.
P. C. (2008). Social status and aggressive and disruptive behav- Fan, C. (1994). A comparative study of personality characteristics
ior in girls: Individual, group, and classroom influences. Journal of between only and nononly children in primary schools in Xian.
School Psychology, 46, 193–212. Psychological Science, 17, 70–74.
Evans, D. M., & Martin, N. G. (2000). The validity of twin studies. Fan, J., Fossella, J., Sommer, T., Wu, Y., & Posner, M. I. (2003).
GeneScreen, 1, 77–79. Mapping the genetic variation of executive attention onto brain
Evans, E. D., & Craig, D. (1990). Teacher and student perceptions of activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA,
academic cheating in middle and senior high schools. Journal of 100, 7406–7411.
Educational Research, 84, 44–50. Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., Flombaum, J. I., & Posner,
Exterkate, C.C., Vriesendorp, P. F., de Jong, C. A. (2009). Body attitudes M. I. (2005). The activation of attentional networks. NeuroImage,
in patients with eating disorders at presentation and completion of 26, 471–479.
intensive outpatient day treatment. Eating Behaviors, 10, 16–21. Fantz, L., Fagan, J. F., & Miranda, S. B. (1975). Early visual selectiv-
Eyer, D. (1992). Mother-infant bonding: A scientific fiction. New ity. In L. B. Cohen & P. H. Salapatek (Eds.), Infant perception:
Haven, CT: Yale University Press. From sensation to cognition. Vol. 1: Basic visual processes. New York:
Eyestone, L. L., & Howell, R. J. (1994). An epidemiological study Academic Press.
of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and major depression Fantz, R. L. (1957) Form preferences in newly hatched chicks. Journal
in a male prison population. Bulletin of the American Academy of of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 50, 422–430.
Psychiatry and the Law, 22, 181–193. Fantz, R. L. (1958). Pattern vision in young infants. Psychological
Eysenck, H. J. (1964). Crime and personality. London: Routledge & Review, 8, 43–47.
Kegan Paul. Fantz, R. L. (1961). The origins of form perception. Scientific
Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Genetic and environmental contributions to American, 204, 66–72.
individual differences in the three major dimensions of personal- Faraone, S. V., & Biederman, J. (1998). Neurobiology of attention-
ity. Journal of Personality, 58, 245–261. deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 951–958.
Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of creativity. Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Chen, W. J., Krifcher, B., Keenan, K.,
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Moore, C., Sprich, S., & Tsuang, M. (1992). Segregation analysis
Eysenck, H. J. (1996). Personality and crime: Where do we stand. of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Evidence for single gene
Psychology, Crime & Law, 2, 143–152. transmission. Psychiatric Genetics, 2, 257–275.
Eysenck, R. (1994). Measuring creativity. In M. Boden (Ed.), Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Keenan, K., & Tsuang, M. (1991). A
Dimensions of creativity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. family-genetic study of girls with DSM-III attention deficit disor-
der. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 112–117.
Fabes, R. A., Martin, C. L., & Hanish, L. D. (2003). Young children’s Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Weiffenbach, B., Keith, T., Chu, M. P.,
play qualities in same-, other-, and mixed-sex peer groups. Child Weaver, A., Spencer, T. J., Wilens, T. E., Frazier, J., Cleves, M., &
Development, 74, 921–993. Sakai J. (1999). Dopamine D4 gene 7-repeat allele and attention
Fabricius, W. V., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (1994). The older child’s deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156,
theory of mind. In A. Demetriou & A. Efklides (Eds.), Intelligence, 768–770.
REFERENCES R-37
Faraone, S. V., & Doyle, A. (2001). The nature and heritability of Feldman, N. S., Klosson, E. C., Parsons, J. E., Rholes, W. S., & Ruble,
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Child and Adolescent D. N. (1976). Order of information presentation and children’s
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 10, 299–316. moral judgments. Child Development, 47, 556–559.
Farnill, D. (1974). The effects of social-judgment set on children’s use Feldman, R., Singer, M., & Zagoory, O. (2010). Touch attenuates
of intent information. Journal of Personality, 42, 276–289. infants’ physiological reactivity to stress. Developmental Science,
Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., Menon, E., Zulian, L., Faraguna, D., & 13, 271–278.
Csibra, G. (2005). Newborns’ preference for face-relevant stimuli: Feldman, R., Weller, A., Zagoory-Sharon, O., & Levine, A. (2007).
Effects of contrast polarity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Evidence for a neuroendocrinological foundation of human affili-
Sciences of the USA, 102, 17245–17250. ation. Psychological Science, 18, 965–970.
Farroni, T., Massaccesi, S., Pividori, D., & Johnson, M. H. (2004) Feldman-Summers, S., & Pope, K. S. (1994). The experience of “for-
Gaze following in newborns. Infancy, 5, 39–60. getting” childhood abuse: A national survey of psychologists.
Farroni, T., Menon, E., Rigato, S., & Johnson, M. H. (2007). The Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 636–639.
perception of facial expressions in newborns. European Journal of Feltovich, P. J., Spiro, R. J., & Coulson, R. L. (1993). Learning, teach-
Developmental Psychology, 4, 2–13. ing, and testing for complex conceptual understanding. In N.
Fauber, R., Forehand, R., Thomas, A., & Wierson, M. (1990). A Frederiksen, R. J. Mislevy, & I. I. Bejar (Eds.), Test theory for a new
mediational model of the impact of marital conflict on adolescent generation of tests (pp. 181–217). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
adjustment in intact and divorced families: The role of disrupted Feng, X., Shaw, D. S., & Silk, J. S. (2008). Developmental trajectories
parenting. Child Development, 61, 1112–1123. of anxiety symptoms among boys across early and middle child-
Favaro, A., Caregaro, L., Tenconi, E., Bosello, R., & Santonastaso, P. hood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 32–47.
(2009). Time trends in age at onset of anorexia nervosa and buli- Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J.
mia nervosa. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 70, 1715–1721. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs
Favaro, A., Ferrara, S., & Santonastaso, P. (2003). The spectrum of of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 1–173.
eating disorders in young women: A prevalence study in a general Fenton, N. (1928). The only child. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 35,
population sample. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 701–708. 546–556.
Fawcett, S. L., Wang, Y. Z., & Birch, E. E. (2005). The criti- Ferguson, C., & Slobin, D. I. (Eds.). (1973). Studies of child language
cal period for susceptibility of human stereopsis. Investigative development. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 46, 521–525. Fernald, A. (1984). The perceptual and affective salience of mothers’
Fazio, R. H., & Dunton, B. C. (1997). Categorization by race: The speech to infants. In L. Feagans, C. Garvey, & R. Golinkoff (Eds.), The
impact of automatic and controlled components of racial prejudice. origins and growth of communication (pp. 7–29). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 451–470. Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to moth-
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social erese. Infant Behavior & Development, 8, 181–195.
cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to moth-
Psychology, 54, 297–327. erese. Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 181–195.
Feeney, S. M., & Stiles, J. (1996). Spatial analysis: An examination of Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as bio-
preschoolers’ perception and construction of geometric patterns. logically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In J. H.
Developmental Psychology, 32, 933–941. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind:
Feigenson, L., Carey, S., & Hauser, M. (2002a). The representations Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 391–428).
underlying infants’ choice of more: Object files versus analog mag- New York: Oxford University Press.
nitudes. Psychological Science, 13, 150–156. Fernald, A. (1993). Approval and disapproval: Infant responsive-
Feigenson, L., Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (2002b). Infants’ discrimina- ness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child
tion of number vs. continuous extent. Cognitive Psychology, 44, Development, 64, 657–674.
33–66. Fernald, A., & Hurtado, N. (2006). Names in frames: Infants inter-
Feinberg, M. E., Button, T. M., Neiderhiser, J. M., Reiss, D., & pret words in sentence frames faster than words in isolation.
Hetherington, E. M. (2007). Parenting and adolescent antisocial Developmental Science, 9, F33–F40.
behavior and depression: Evidence of genotype × parenting environ- Fernald, A., & McRoberts, G. (1996). Prosodic bootstrapping: A
ment interaction. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 457–465. critical analysis of the argument and the evidence. In J. Morgan &
Feinberg, M. E., McHale, S. M., Crouter, A. C., & Cumsille, P. K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to
(2003). Sibling differentiation: Sibling and parent relationship tra- grammar in early acquisition (pp. 365–387). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
jectories in adolescence. Child Development, 74, 1261–1274. Fernandez, M., & Bahrick, L. E. (1994). Infants’ sensitivity to arbitrary
Feinman, S., Roberts, D., Hsieh, K., Sawyer, D., & Swanson, D. object-odor pairings. Infant Behavior & Development, 17, 471–474.
(1992). A critical review of social referencing in infancy. In Feron, J., Streri, A., & Gentaz, E. (2002, April). Numerical intermo-
S. Feinman (Ed.), Social referencing and the social construction of dal transfer from touch to vision by 5-month-old infants. Poster
reality in infancy (pp. 15–54). New York: Plenum Press. presented at the Biennial International Conference on Infant
Feiring, C., Taska, L., & Lewis, M. (2002). Adjustment following Studies, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
sexual abuse discovery: The role of shame and attributional style. Ferrari, P. F., Paukner, A., Ionica, C., & Suomi, S. J. (2009a). Reciprocal
Developmental Psychology, 38, 79–92. face-to-face communication between rhesus macaque mothers and
Feldman, H., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Gleitman, L. (1978). Beyond their newborn infants. Current Biology, 19, 1768–1772.
Herodotus: The creation of a language by linguistically deprived Ferrari, P. F., Paukner, A., Ruggiero, A., Darcey, L., Unbehagen, S., &
deaf children. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, symbol, and gesture: Suomi S. J. (2009b). Interindividual differences in neonatal imi-
The emergence of language (pp. 351–414). New York: Academic tation and the development of action chains in rhesus macaques.
Press. Child Development, 80, 1057–1068.
R-38 REFERENCES
Ferrari, F. P., Vanderwert, R., Paukner, A., Bower, S., Suomi, S. J., & Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control
Fox, N. A. (2012). Distinct EEG amplitude suppression to facial and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87,
gestures as evidence for a mirror mechanism in newborn monkeys. 477–531.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 1165–1172. Fischer, K. W., & Bidell, T. (1991). Constraining nativist inferences
Ferrari, P. F., Visalberghi, E., Fogassi, L., Ruggiero, A., & Suomi, S. J. about cognitive capacities. In S. Carey & R. Gelman (Eds.), The
(2006). Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. PLoS Biology, 4, epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and knowledge (pp. 199–235).
1501–1508. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ferrer, I., Soriano, E., Del Rio, J. A., Alcantara, S., & Auladell C. Fischer, K. W., & Bidell, T. R. (1998). Dynamic development of
(1992). Cell death and removal in the cerebral cortex during devel- psychological structures in action and thought. In W. Damon
opment. Progress in Neurobiology, 39, 1–43. (Series Ed.) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psy-
Field, A. E., Javaras, K. M., Aneja, P., Kitos, N, Carmago, C. A., chology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed.,
Taylor, C. B., & Laird, N .M. (2008). Family, peer, and media pp. 467–561). New York: Wiley.
predictors of becoming eating disordered. Archives of Pediatrics and Fischer, P., Fischer, J., Frey, D., Such, M., Smyth, M., Tester, M., &
Adolescent Medicine, 162, 574–579. Kastenmüller, A. (2010). Causal evidence that terrorism salience
Field, T. (1977). Effects of early separation, interactive deficits, and increases authoritarian parenting practices. Social Psychology, 41,
experimental manipulations on infant-mother face-to-face interac- 246–254.
tion. Child Development, 48, 763–771. Fish, J. M. (Ed.). (2002). Race and intelligence: Separating science from
Field, T. (1978). Interaction behaviors of primary versus secondary myth. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
caretaker fathers. Developmental Psychology, 14, 183–184. Fisher, C., Hall, G., Rakowitz, S., & Gleitman, L. R. (1994). When
Field, T. (1979). Visual and cardiac responses to animate and inani- it is better to receive than to give: Syntactic and conceptual con-
mate faces by young term and preterm infants. Child Development, straints on vocabulary growth. Lingua, 92, 333–375.
50, 188–194. Fisher, L., Ames, E. W., Chisholm, K., & Savoie, L. (1997). Problems
Field, T. M. (2000). Infants of depressed mothers. In S. L. Johnson, reported by parents of Romanian orphans adopted to British
A. M. Hayes, T. M. Field, N. Schneiderman, & P. M. McCabe (Eds.), Columbia. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20,
Stress, coping, and depression (pp. 3–22). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 67–82.
Field, T., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Fisher, R. A. (1918). The correlation between relatives on the sup-
Discrimination and imitation of facial expression by neonates. position of Mendelian inheritance. Transactions of the Royal Society
Science, 218, 179–181. (Edinburgh), 52, 399–433.
Fields, J. (2003). Children’s living arrangements and characteristics: Fisher, S. E., & Francks, C. (2006). Genes, cognition and dyslexia:
March 2002. Current Population Reports, P20-547. Washington, Learning to read the genome. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 250–257.
DC: U.S. Census Bureau. Fisher, S. E., Francks, C., Marlow, A. J., MacPhie, I. L., Newbury,
Fields, J., & Casper, L. M. (2001). America’s families and living D. F., Cardon, L. R., Ishikawa-Brush, Y., Richardson, A. J., Talcott,
arrangements: March 2000. Current Population Report, P20-537. J. B., Gayán, J., Olson, R. K., Pennington B. F., Smith, S. D.,
Washington, DC: U.S. Census Bureau. DeFries, J.C., Stein, J. F., & Monaco, A. P. (2002). Independent
Fifer, W. P., Byrd, D. L., Kaku, M., Eigsti, I., Isler, J. R., Grose-Fifer, genomewide scans to identify a chromosome 18 quantitative-trait
J., Tarullo, A. R., & Balsam, P. D. (2010). Newborn infants learn locus influencing dyslexia. Nature Genetics, 30, 86–91.
during sleep. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the Fisher, S. E., & Scharff, C. (2009). FOXP2 as a molecular window
USA, 107, 10320–10323. into speech and language. Trends in Genetics, 25, 166–177.
Finkel, D., Reynolds, C. A., McArdle, J. J., Gatz, M., & Pedersen, N. L. Fisher, S. E., Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K. E., Monaco, A. P., &
(2003). Latent growth curve analyses of accelerating decline in cogni- Pembrey, M. E. (1998). Localisation of a gene implicated in a
tive abilities in late adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 39, 535–550. severe speech and language disorder. Nature Genetics, 18, 168–170.
Finkelhor, D. (1994). The international epidemiology of child sexual Fiske, K. E., & Pillemer, D. B. (2006). Adult recollections of earliest
abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 18, 409–417. childhood dreams: A cross-cultural study. Memory, 14, 57–67.
Finkelhor, D., Hotaling, G., Lewis, I. A., & Smith, C. (1990). Sexual Fiske, S. T. (2012). Warmth and competence: Stereotype content issues
abuse in a national survey of adult men and women: Prevalence, for clinicians and researchers. Canadian Psychologist/Psychologie
characteristics, and risk factors. Child Abuse & Neglect, 14, 19–28. Canadienne, 53, 14–20.
Finkelstein, R., & Perrimon, N. (1990). The orthodenticle gene is Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions
regulated by bicoid and torso and specifies Drosophila head devel- of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive
opment. Nature, 346, 485–488. Sciences, 11, 77–83.
Finlay B. L., & Slattery, M. (1983). Local differences in the amount Fitch, W. T. (2011). Unity and diversity in human language. Philosophical
of early cell death in neocortex predict adult local specializations. Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 366, 376–388.
Science, 219, 1349–1351. Fivush, R. (1991). The social construction of personal narratives.
Finnell, R. H., & Burn, J. (2000). Effect of anti-epileptic drugs on Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 37, 59–81.
intrauterine growth. The Lancet, 356, 1537–1538. Fivush, R. (1994a). Constructing narrative, emotion, and self in
Finnell, R. H., Gelineau-van Waes, J., Eudy, J. D., & Rosenquist T. H. parent-child conversations about the past. In U. Neisser &
(2002). Molecular basis of environmentally induced birth defects. R. Fivush (Eds.), The remembering self: Construction and accuracy
Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology, 42, 181–208. in the self-narrative (pp. 136–157). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
Fisch, S. I. (2005). Nurse home visitation has powerful and enduring University Press.
effects. AAP Grand Rounds, 13, 26–27. Fivush, R. (1994b). Young children’s event recall: Are memories
Fischer, C. S., & Hout, M. (2006). Century of difference: How America constructed through discourse? Consciousness and Cognition, 3,
changed in the last one hundred years. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. 356–373.
REFERENCES R-39
Fivush, R. (1997). Event memory in early childhood. In N. Cowan Flavell, J. H., & Wellman, H. (1977). Metamemory. In R. V. R.
(Ed.), The development of memory in childhood (pp. 139–162). Kail & J. W. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on the development of mem-
Hove, UK: Psychology Press. ory and cognition (pp. 3–33). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Fivush, R. (1998). The stories we tell: How language shapes autobiog- Fleischman, D. A., Wilson, R. S., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Bienias, J. L., &
raphy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 483–487. Bennett, D. A. (2004). A longitudinal study of implicit and explicit
Fivush, R. (2011). The development of autobiographical memory. memory in old persons. Psychology and Aging, 19, 617–625.
Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 559–582. Fleming, J., Mullen, P. E., Sibthorpe, B., & Bammer, G. (1999). The
Fivush, R., Haden, C. A., & Reese, E. (2006). Elaborating on elabo- long-term impact of childhood sexual abuse in Australian women.
rations: Role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socio- Child Abuse & Neglect, 23, 145–159.
emotional development. Child Development, 77, 1568–1588. Fletcher, A., Bonell, C., & Sorhaindo, A. (2011). You are what your
Fivush, R., Hazzard, A., Sales, J. M., Sarfati, D., & Brown, T. (2003). friends eat: Systematic review of social network analyses of young
Creating coherence out of chaos? Children’s narratives of emotion- people’s eating behaviours and bodyweight. Journal of Epidemiology
ally positive and negative events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, and Community Health, 65, 548–555.
1–19. Flinn, M. V., Leone, D. V., & Quinlan, R. J. (1999). Growth and fluc-
Fivush, R., & Nelson, K. (2004). Culture and language in the emer- tuating asymmetry of stepchildren. Evolution and Human Behavior
gence of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 15, 20, 465–480.
586–590. Floeter, M. K., & Jones, E. G. (1985). Transplantation of fetal post-
Fivush, R., & Reese, E. (2002). Reminiscing and relating: The devel- mitotic neurons to rat cortex: Survival, early pathway choices and
opment of parent-child talk about the past. In J. D. Webster & long-term projections of outgrowing axons. Developmental Brain
B. K. Haight (Eds.), Critical advances in reminiscence work: From Research, 22, 19–38.
theory to application (pp. 109–122). New York: Springer. Flom, R., Whipple, H., & Hyde, D. (2009). Infants’ intermodal per-
Fivush, R., & Schwarzmueller, A. (1998). Children remember child- ception of canine (Canis familairis) facial expressions and vocaliza-
hood: Implications for childhood amnesia. Applied Cognitive tions. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1143–1151.
Psychology, 12, 455–473. Flombaum, J. I., Junge, J. A., & Hauser, M. D. (2005) Rhesus mon-
Flanagan, O., & Rorty, A. O., (Eds.) (1991). Identity, character, and keys (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations
morality: Essays in moral psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. over large numbers. Cognition, 97, 315–325.
Flavell, J. (1963). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. Princeton, Floyd, F. J., & Stein, T. S. (2002). Sexual orientation identity forma-
NJ: Van Nostrand. tion among gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths: Multiple patterns
Flavell, J. H. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. of milestone experiences. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 12,
In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), The nature of intelligence (pp. 231–235). 167–191.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Flynn, J. R. (1984). The mean IQ of Americans: Massive gains
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new 1932–1978. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 29–51.
area of cognitive developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests
906–911. really measure. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 171–191.
Flavell, J. H. (1987). Speculations about the nature and develop- Flynn, J. R. (1991). Asian Americans: Achievement beyond IQ. Hillsdale,
ment of metacognition. In F. E. Weinert & R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), NJ: Erlbaum.
Metacognition, motivation and understanding (pp. 21–29). Hillside, Flynn, J. R. (1995). What environmental factors affect intelligence:
NJ: Erlbaum. The relevance of IQ gains over time. In D. K. Detterman (Ed.),
Flavell, J. H. (1988). The development of children’s knowledge about Current topics in human intelligence: Vol. 5. The environment
the mind: From cognition connections to mental representations. (pp. 17–29). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
In J. W. Astington, P. L. Harris, & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing Flynn, J. R. (1998). IQ gains over time: Toward finding the causes. In
theories of mind (pp. 244–71). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge U. Neisser (Ed.), The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related
University Press. measures (pp. 25–66). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Flavell, J. H. (1999). Cognitive development: Children’s knowledge Association.
about the mind. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 21–45. Flynn, J. R. (1999). Searching for justice: The discovery of IQ gains
Flavell, J. H., Beach, D. R., & Chinsky, J. M. (1966). Spontaneous over time. American Psychologist, 54, 5–20.
verbal rehearsal in a memory task as a function of age. Child Flynn, J. R. (2007). What is intelligence? Beyond the Flynn effect. New
Development, 37, 283–299. York: Cambridge University Press.
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., Green, F. L., & Korfmacher, J. E. (1990). Flynn, J. R. (2008). The history of the American mind in the 20th
Do young children think of television images as pictures or real century: A scenario to explain IQ gains over time and a case
objects? Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 34, 399–419. for the irrelevance of g. In P. C. Kyllonen, R. D. Roberts, &
Flavell, J. H., Friedrichs, A. G., & Hoyt, J. D. (1970). Developmental L. Stankov, Extending intelligence: Enhancements and new constructs
changes in memorization processes. Cognitive Psychology, 1, (pp. 207–222). New York: Taylor & Francis.
324–340. Flynn, S., & O’Neil, W. (Eds.). (1988). Linguistic theory in second lan-
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., & Flavell, E. R. (1986). Development of guage acquisition. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic
knowledge about the appearance-reality distinction. Monographs of Publishers.
the Society for Research in Child Development, 51, 1–89. Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1995). DSM-IV field trial: Obsessive-
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., & Flavell, E. R. (1995). Young children’s compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 90–96.
knowledge about thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research Fodor, J. A. (1972). Some reflections on L. S. Vygotsky’s “Thought
in Child Development, 60 (1, Serial no. 243). and Language.” Cognition, 1, 83–95.
Flavell, S.W., & Greenberg, M. E. (2008). Signaling mechanisms Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. New York: Crowell.
linking neuronal activity to gene expression and plasticity of the Fodor, J. (1980). On the impossibility of acquiring “more powerful”
nervous system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31, 563–590. structures. In M. Piattelli-Palmarini (Ed.), Language and learning:
R-40 REFERENCES
The debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky (pp. 142–162). Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (2003). The development of self-control
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. of emotion: Intrinsic and extrinsic influences. Motivation and
Fodor, J. A. (1981). The current status of the innateness controversy. In Emotion, 27, 7–26.
J. A. Fodor (Ed.), Representations: Philosophical essays on the founda- Fox, N. A., Nichols, K. E., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K., Schmidt, L.,
tions of cognitive science (pp. 257–316). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Hamer, D., Ernst, M., & Pine, D. S. (2005). Evidence for a gene-
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT environment interaction in predicting behavioral inhibition in
Press. middle childhood. Psychological Science, 16, 921–926.
Fodor, J. A. (1992). A theory of the child’s theory of mind. Cognition, Fox, R., Aslin, R., Shea, S. L., & Dumais, S. (1980). Stereopsis in
44, 283–296. human infants. Science, 207, 323–324.
Fodor, J. A., Garrett, M. F., Walker, E. C. T., & Parkes, C. H. (1980). Fox, S. E., Levitt, P., & Nelson III, C. A. (2010). How the timing
Against definitions. Cognition, 8, 263–367. and quality of early experiences influence the development of brain
Fogel, A., Nelson-Goens, C., Hsu, H., & Shapiro, A. F. (2000). Do architecture. Child Development, 81, 28–40.
different infant smiles reflect different positive emotions? Social Fraley, R. C. (2002). Attachment stability from infancy to adulthood:
Development, 9, 497–520. Meta-analysis and dynamic modeling of developmental mecha-
Fogel, R. W. (2005). Changes in the physiology of aging during the nisms. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 123–151.
twentieth century. NBER Working Paper 11223. Cambridge, MA: Fraley, R. C., & Roberts, B. W. (2005). Patterns of continuity: A
National Bureau of Economic Research. dynamic model for conceptualizing the stability of individual
Folstein, S. E., & Rosen-Sheidley, B. (2001). Genetics of autism: differences in psychological constructs across the life course.
Complex aetiology for a heterogeneous disorder. Nature Reviews Psychological Review, 112, 60–74.
Genetics, 2, 943–955. Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment:
Fombonne, E. (2001). Is there an epidemic of autism? Pediatrics, 107, Theoretical developments, emerging controversies, and unan-
411–413. swered questions. Review of General Psychology, 4, 132–154.
Fombonne, E., Quirke, S., & Hagen, A. (2011). Epidemiology of per- Franchak, J. M., & Adolph, K. E. (2010). Visually guided navigation:
vasive developmental disorders. In D. G. Amaral, G. Dawson., & Head-mounted eye-tracking of natural locomotion in children and
D. H. Geschwind (Eds.), Autism spectrum disorders (pp. 90–111). adults. Vision Research, 50, 2766–2774.
New York: Oxford University Press. Francis, R. C., & Barlow, G. W. (1993). Social control of primary
Fonagy, P., & Adshead, G. (2012). How mentalisation changes the sex differentiation in the Midas cichlid. Proceedings of the National
mind. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 18, 353–362. Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 90, 10673–10675.
Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (2002). Psychodynamic approaches to child Franek, M. (2006, March 29). Time to think. The New York Times,
therapy. In F. W. Kaslow & J. Magnavita (Eds.), The comprehensive editorial page.
handbook of psychotherapy (Vol. 1, pp. 105–129). New York: Wiley. Frank, M. C., Slemmer, J. A., Marcus, G. F., & Johnson, S. P. (2009).
Fonagy, P., Target, M., Cottrell, D., Phillips, J., & Kurtz, Z. (2002). Information from multiple modalities helps five-month-olds learn
What works for whom? A critical review of treatments for children and abstract rules. Developmental Science, 12, 504–509.
adolescents. New York: Guilford Press. Frank, M. C., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2011). Three ideal observer mod-
Forman, S. F., & Goodman, E. (1999). Characteristics of teens with els for rule learning in simple languages. Cognition, 120, 360–371.
an anorexia nervosa-like syndrome from a recent population-based Frank, R. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emo-
nationally representative survey. Journal of Adolescent Health, 24, 95. tions. New York: W. W. Norton.
Forsyth, D. R., Kerr, N. A., Burnette, J. L., & Baumeister, R. F. Frankenburg, W. K., Fandal, A. W., Sciarillo, W., & Burgess, D.
(2007). Attempting to improve the academic performance of strug- (1981). The newly abbreviated and revised Denver Developmental
gling college students by bolstering their self-esteem: An interven- Screening Test. Journal of Pediatrics, 99, 995–999.
tion that backfired. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 26, Franklin, A., & Davies, I. R. L. (2004). New evidence for infant
447–459. colour categories. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22,
Forty, L., Smith, D., Jones, L., Jones, I., Caesar, S., Cooper, C., Fraser, C., 349–377.
Gordon-Smith, K., Hyde, S., Farmer, A., McGuffin, P., & Franklin, A., Drivonikou, G. V., Bevis, L., Davies, I. R. L., Kay, P., &
Craddock, N. (2008). Clinical differences between bipolar and Regier, T. (2008). Categorical perception of color is lateralized
unipolar depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 192, 388 –389. to the right hemisphere in infants, but to the left hemisphere in
Fossella, J., Sommer, T., Fan, J., Wu, Y., Swanson, J. M., Pfaff, adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA,
D. W., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Assessing the molecular genetics of 105, 3221–3225.
attention networks. BioMed Central Neuroscience, 3, 14. Franklin, A., Drivonikou, G. V., Clifford, A., Kay, P., Regier, T., &
Fowler, J. H., & Christakis, N. A. (2008). Dynamic spread of happi- Davies, I. R. L. (2008b). Lateralization of categorical perception
ness in a large social network: Longitudinal analysis over 20 years of color changes with color term acquisition. Proceedings of the
in the Framingham Heart Study. British Medical Journal, 337, National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 105, 18221–18225.
a2338. Fraser, A., & Evan, G. (1996). A license to kill. Cell, 86, 781–784.
Fox, A. S., Oler, J. A., Shelton, S. E., Nanda, S. A., Davidson, R. J., Freedman, D. G., & Freedman, N. (1969). Behavioural differences
Roseboom, P. H., & Kalin, N. H. (2012). Central amygdala nucleus between Chinese-American and European-American newborns.
(Ce) gene expression linked to increased trait-like Ce metabolism Nature, 224, 1227.
and anxious temperament in young primates. Proceedings of the Freedman, J. (2002). Media violence and its effect on aggression: Assessing
National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 109, 18108–18113. the scientific evidence. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
Fox, L., Han, W. J., Ruhm, C., & Waldfogel, J. (2013). Time for chil- Freedman, M. K. (2003). Disabling the SAT. Education Next, 3,
dren: Trends in the employment patterns of parents, 1967–2009. 36–43.
Demography, 50, 25–49. Freeman, R. D., & Ohzawa,I. (1992). Development of binocular
Fox, N. A. (2003). Not quite ready to invest. Human Development, vision in the kitten’s striate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 12,
46, 104–108. 4721–4736.
REFERENCES R-41
Freitag, C. M. (2007). The genetics of autistic disorders and its clini- Frick, P. J., Bodin, S. D., & Barry, C. T. (2000). Psychopathic traits
cal relevance: A review of the literature. Molecular Psychiatry, 12, and conduct problems in community and clinic-referred samples
2–22. of children: Further development of the psychopathy screening
Frejka, T., & Sardon, J. P. (2007). Cohort birth order, parity progres- device. Psychological Assessment, 12, 382–393.
sion ratio and parity distribution trends in developed countries. Frick, P. J., Cornell, A. H., Bodin, S. D., Dane, H. E., Barry, C. T., &
Demographic Research 16, 315–374. Loney, B. R. (2003). Callous-unemotional traits and developmen-
French, D. C., Chen, X., Chung, J., Li, M., Chen, H., & Li, D. (2011). tal pathways to severe conduct problems. Developmental Psychology,
Four children and one toy: Chinese and Canadian children faced 39, 246–260.
with potential conflict over a limited resource. Child Development, Frick, P. J., & Nigg, J. T. (2012). Current issues in the diagnosis of
82, 830–841. attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant dis-
Freud, A., & Dann, S. (1951). An experiment in group upbringing. order, and conduct disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology,
Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 6, 127–168. 8, 77–107.
Freud, S. (1905/1976). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. In Fricke, H. W., & Fricke, S. (1977). Monogamy and sex change by
J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psy- aggressive dominance in coral reef fish. Nature, 266, 830–832.
chological works (Vol. 7). New York: W. W. Norton. Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view.
Freud, S. (1909). Analysis of a phobia in a five-year-old boy. The stan- San Diego: Academic Press.
dard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud Fridlund, A. J., Beck, H. P., Goldie, W. D., & Irons, G. (2012). Little
(Vol. 10). New York: W. W. Norton. Albert: A neurologically impaired child. History of Psychology, 15,
Freud, S. (1910). The origin and development of psychoanalysis. 302–327.
American Journal of Psychology, 21, 181–218. Friedman, L. J. (1999). Identity’s architect: A biography of Erik H.
Freud, S. (1916/1966). The archaic features and infantilism of dreams. Erikson. New York: Scribner.
In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), Introductory lectures on psychoanaly- Friedrich, L., & Stein, A. (1975). Prosocial television and young chil-
sis. New York: W. W. Norton. dren: The effects of verbal labeling and role playing on learning
Freud, S. (1923/1960). The ego and the id. New York: W. W. Norton. and behavior. Child Development, 47, 27–38.
Freud, S. (1925/1976). Some psychical consequences of the anatomi- Frisco, M. L., Muller, C., & Frank, K. (2007). Parents’ union dis-
cal distinction between the sexes. In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), solution and adolescents’ school performance: Comparing meth-
The standard edition of the complete psychological works (Vol. 19). odological approaches. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 69,
New York: W. W. Norton. 721–741.
Freud, S. (1930). Civilization and its discontents. London: Hogarth Frisén, J., Yates, P. A., McLaughlin, T., Friedman, G. C., O’Leary,
Press. D. D., & Barbacid, M. (1998). Ephrin-A5 (AL-1/RAGS) is essen-
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2002a). Life-management strategies tial for proper retinal axon guidance and topographic mapping in
of selection, optimization, and compensation: Measurement by the mammalian visual system. Neuron, 20, 235–243.
self-report and construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social Frith, C. D. (2007). The social brain? Philosophical Transactions of the
Psychology, 82, 642–662. Royal Society of London, Series B, 362, 671–678.
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2002b).The adaptiveness of selec- Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999, November 26). Interacting minds— a
tion, optimization, and compensation as strategies of life manage- biological basis. Science, 286, 1692–1695.
ment: Evidence from a preference study on proverbs. Journals of Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1995). Autism: Beyond “theory of mind.” In
Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57, J. Mehler & S. Franck (Eds.), Cognition on cognition (pp. 13–30).
426–434. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Freund, A. M., & Ritter, J. O. (2009). Midlife crisis: A debate. Frodi, A. M., Lamb, M. E., Leavitt, L. A., & Donovan, W. L. (1978).
Gerontology, 55, 582–591. Fathers’ and mothers’ responses to infant smiles and cries. Infant
Frey, B. S., Benesch, C., & Stutzer, A. (2007). Does watching TV Behavior and Development, 1, 187–198.
make us happy? Journal of Economic Psychology, 28, 283–313. Frolik, L. A., & Kaplan, R. L. (1999). Elder law in a nutshell (2nd ed.).
Frey, K. S., & Ruble, D. N. (1990). Strategies for comparative evalu- St. Paul, MN: West Group.
ation: Maintaining a sense of competence across the lifespan. In Fromkin, V. A. (Ed.). (2000). Linguistics: An introduction to linguis-
R. J. Sternberg & J. Kolligian Jr (Eds.), Competence considered tic theory. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
(pp. 167–189). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Fromkin, V., Rodman, R., & Hyams, N. M. (2003). An introduction
Frey, M. C., &. Detterman, D. K. (2004). Scholastic assessment or to language (7th ed.). Boston: Thomson Heinle.
g? The relationship between the Scholastic Assessment Test and Frontera, W. R., Hughes, V. A., Lutz, K. J., & Evans, W. J. (1991). A
general cognitive ability. Psychological Science, 15, 373–378. cross-sectional study of muscle strength and mass in 45- to 78-yr-old
Freyd, J. J. (1996). Betrayal trauma: The logic of forgetting childhood men and women. Journal of Applied Physiology, 71, 644–650.
abuse. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Frost, D. M. (2011). Stigma and intimacy in same-sex relationships: A
Freyd, J. J., & Gleaves, D. H. (1996). “Remembering” words not pre- narrative approach. Journal of Family Psychology, 25, 1–10.
sented in lists: Relevance to the current recovered/false memory Fryer, R. G., & Levitt, S. D. (2004). Understanding the black-white
controversy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, test score gap in the first two years of school. Review of Economics
and Cognition, 22, 811–813. and Statistics, 86, 447–464.
Friauf, E., McConnell, S. K., & Shatz, C. J. (1990). Functional synap- Fugelsang, J., Roser, M., Corballis, P., Gazzaniga, M., & Dunbar, K.
tic circuits in the subplate during fetal and early postnatal develop- (2005). Brain mechanisms underlying perceptual causality.
ment of cat visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 2601–2613. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 41–47.
Frick, P. J., & Ellis, M. (1999). Callous-unemotional traits and sub- Funder, D. C. (2006). Towards a resolution of the personality triad:
types of conduct disorder. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Persons, situations, and behaviors. Journal of Research in Personality,
Review, 2, 149–168. 40, 21–34.
R-42 REFERENCES
Fung, H. L., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Sending memorable mes- Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA:
sages to the old: Age differences in preferences and memory for MIT Press.
advertisements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting
163–178. and computation. Cognition, 44, 43–74.
Furman, W., Brown, B. B., & Feiring, C. (Eds.). (1999). The develop- Gallup, G. G. Jr. (1970, January 2). Chimpanzees: Self-recognition.
ment of romantic relationships in adolescence. New York: Cambridge Science, 167, 86–87.
University Press. Gallup, G. G. Jr. (1977). Self-recognition in primates: A comparative
Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and sex differences in per- approach in bidirectional properties of consciousness. American
ceptions of networks of personal relationships. Child Development, Psychologist, 32, 329–338.
63, 103–115. Gallup, G. G. Jr. (1998). Can animals empathize? Scientific American
Furnham, A., & Nederstrom, M. (2010 ). Ability, demographic and Presents, 9, 66–71.
personality predictors of creativity. Personality and Individual Galton, F. (1869). Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and conse-
Differences, 48, 957–961. quences. London: Macmillan/Fontana.
Furstenberg, F. F. (1987). The new extended family: The experience of Galton, F. (1883). Inquiries into human faculty and its development.
parents and children after remarriage. In K. Pasley & M. Ihinger- New York: AMS Press.
Tallman (Eds.), Remarriage and stepparenting: Current research and Gamble, T. J., & Zigler, E. (1986). Effects of infant day care: Another
theory (pp. 42–61). New York: Guilford Press. look at the evidence. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56,
Furstenberg, F. F. (2005). Banking on families: How families generate 26–42.
and distribute social capital. Journal of Marriage and the Family, Ganger, J., & Brent, M. (2004) Reexamining the vocabulary spurt.
67, 809–821. Developmental Psychology, 4, 621–632.
Furstenberg, F. F., Peterson, J. L., Nord, C. W., & Zill, N. (1983). The Gangestad S. W., Thornhill R. & Yeo R. A. (1994). Facial attractive-
life course of children of divorce: Marital disruption and parental ness, developmental stability, and fluctuating asymmetry. Ethology
contact. American Sociological Review, 48, 656–668. and Sociobiology, 15, 73–85.
Furstenberg, F. F., & Teitler, J. O. (1994). Reconsidering the effects of Ganiban, J. M., Ulbricht, J., Saudino, K. J., Reiss, D., & Neiderhiser,
marital disruption: What happens to children of divorce in early J. M. (2011). Understanding child-based effects on parenting:
adulthood? Journal of Family Issues, 15, 173–190. Temperament as a moderator of genetic and environmental contri-
Fusaro, M, Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2011). The good, the butions to parenting. Developmental Psychology, 47, 676–692.
strong, and the accurate. Preschoolers’ evaluations of accurate and Ganong, L., & Coleman, M. (2004). Stepfamily relationships:
strong informants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, Development, dynamics, and interventions. New York: Kluwer
561–574. Academic Press.
Futo, J., Teglas, E., Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2010). Communicative Garber, J., Robinson, N. S., & Valentiner, D. (1997). The relation
function demonstration induces kind-based artifact representation between parenting and adolescent depression: Self-worth as a
in preverbal infants. Cognition, 117, 1–8. mediator. Journal of Adolescent Research, 12, 12–33.
Garcia, J., Hankins, W. G., & Rusiniak, K. W. (1974). Behavioral
Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2009). Dyslexia: A new synergy between education regulation of the milieu interne in man and rat. Science, 184,
and cognitive neuroscience. Science, 325, 280–283. 824–831.
Gabrielli, W. F., & Mednick, S. A. (1983). Genetic correlates of crimi- Garcia, J., Hankins, W. G., Robinson, J. H., & Vogt, J. L. (1972).
nal behavior: Implications for research, attribution, and preven- Bait shyness: Tests of CS-US mediation. Physiology and Behavior,
tion. American Behavioral Scientist, 27, 59–74. 8, 807–810.
Gácsi, M., Gyoöri, B., Virányi, Z., Kubinyi, E., Range, F., Belényi B., & Garcia, J., Lasiter, P. S., Bermudez-Rattoni, F., & Deems, D. A.
Miklósi, A. (2009). Explaining dog wolf differences in utilizing (1985). A general theory of aversion learning. Annals of the New
human pointing gestures: Selection for synergistic shifts in the York Academy of Sciences, 443, 8–21.
development of some social skills. PLoS One, 4, e6584. Garcia-Fernandez, J., & Holland, P. W. (1994). Archetypal organiza-
Gagne, J. R., Saudino, K. J., & Cherny, S. S. (2003). Genetic influ- tion of the amphioxus Hox gene cluster. Nature, 370, 563–566.
ences on temperament in early adolescence. In S. A. Petrill, Gard, M. C. E., & Freeman, C. P. (1996). The dismantling of a
R. Plomin, J. C. DeFries, & J. K. Hewitt (Eds.), Nature, nurture, myth: A review of eating disorders and socioeconomic status.
and the transition to early adolescence (pp. 166–184). Oxford, UK: International Journal of Eating Disorders, 20, 1–12.
Oxford University Press. Gardiner, A., & Björklund, D. F. (2007). All in the family: An
Gagne, J. R., Vendlinski, M. K., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2009). The evolutionary developmental perspective. In C. Salmon & T. S.
genetics of childhood temperament. In Y-K Kim (Ed.), Handbook Shackelford (Eds.), Family relationships: An evolutionary perspective
of behavioral genetics (pp. 251–267). New York: Springer. (pp. 337–358). New York: Oxford University Press.
Gallagher, S. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: Implications Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences.
for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 14-21. New York: Basic Books.
Gallay, M., Baudouin, J.-Y., Durand, K., Lemoine, C., & Lécuyer, R. Gardner, H. (1993). Multiple intelligences: The theory in practice. New
(2006). Qualitative differences in the exploration of upright and York: Basic Books.
upside-down faces in four-month-old infants: An eye-movement Gardner, H. (1994). The creators’ patterns. In M. Boden (Ed.),
study. Child Development, 77, 984–996. Dimensions of creativity (pp. 143–158). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action Gardner, H. (1997). Extraordinary minds: Portraits of exceptional indi-
recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609. viduals and an examination of our extraordinariness. New York:
Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view Basic Books.
of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, Gardner, H. (2006). Multiple intelligences: New horizons in theory and
396–403. practice. New York: Basic Books.
REFERENCES R-43
Gardner, H., & Moran, S. (2006). The science of multiple intel- Ge, X., Conger, R. D., & Elder, G. H. (2001). Pubertal transition,
ligences theory: A response to Lynn Waterhouse. Educational stressful life events, and the emergence of gender differences in
Psychologist, 41, 227–232. adolescent depressive symptoms. Developmental Psychology, 37,
Gardner, R. A., & Gardner, B. T. (1969). Teaching sign language to a 404–417.
chimpanzee. Science, 165, 664–672. Ge, X., Lorenz, F. O., Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H., & Simons,
Gardner, R. A., & Gardner, B. T. (1980). Comparative psychology R. L. (1994). Trajectories of stressful life events and depressive
and language acquisition. In T. A. Sebok & J. U. Sebok (Eds.), symptoms during adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 30,
Speaking of apes: A critical anthology of two-way communication with 467–483.
man (pp. 287–329). New York: Plenum Press. Geangu, E., Benga, O., Stahl, D., & Sriano, T. (2010). Contagious cry-
Gardner, R. M., & Bokenkamp, E. D. (1996). The role of sensory ing beyond the first days of life. Infant Behavior and Development,
and nonsensory factors in body size estimations of eating disorder 33, 279–288.
subjects. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 52, 3–5. Gearhart, J., & Kirschner, M. (1997). Cells, embryos and evolution.
Garmezy, N. (1993). Children in poverty: Resilience despite risk. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Psychiatry, 56, 127–136. Geary, D. C. (2000). Evolution and proximate expression of human
Garner, P. W., & Power, T. G. (1996). Preschoolers’ emotional con- paternal investment. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 55–77.
trol in the disappointment paradigm and its relation to tempera- Geary, D. C. (2013). Early foundations for mathematics learning
ment, emotional knowledge, and family expressiveness. Child and their relations to learning disabilities. Current Directions in
Development, 67, 1406–1419. Psychological Science, 22, 23–27.
Garnett, R. S. (2008). Can there really be “free speech” in public Geary D. C., & Flinn, M. V. (2001). Evolution of human parental
schools? Lewis & Clark Law Review, 12, 45–59. behavior and the human family. Parenting: Science and Practice, 1,
Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive func- 5–61.
tion in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Geen, R. G., & Quanty, M. B. (1977). The catharsis of aggression:
Psychological Bulletin, 134, 31–60. An evaluation of a hypothesis. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
Garon, N., & Moore, C. (2007). Awareness and symbol use improves in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 1–37). New York:
future-oriented decision making in preschoolers. Developmental Academic Press.
Neuropsychology, 31, 39–59. Gehring, W. (1987). Homeo boxes in the study of development.
Gartrell, N., & Bos, H. (2010). U.S. National Longitudinal Lesbian Science, 236, 1245–1252.
Family Study: Psychological adjustment of 17-year-old adolescents. Gehring, W. J., Qian, Y. Q., Billeter, M., Furukubo-Tokunaga, K.,
Pediatrics, 126, 28–36. Schier, A. S., Resendez-Perez, D., Affolter, M., Otting, G., &
Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant tem- Wuthrich, K. (1994). Homeodomain-DNA recognition. Cell, 78,
perament via the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire. Infant 211–223.
Behavior & Development, 26, 64–86. Gejman, P. V., Sanders, A. R., & Kendler, K. S. (2011). Genetics of
Garvey, C. (1984). Children’s talk. Cambridge, MA: Harvard schizophrenia: New findings and challenges. Annual Review of
University Press. Genomics and Human Genetics, 12, 121–144.
Garvey, C., & Berninger, G. (1981). Timing and turn taking in chil- Geldart, S., Mondloch, C. J., Maurer, D., de Schonen, S., & Brent,
dren’s conversations. Discourse Processes, 4, 27–57. H. P. (2002). The effect of early visual deprivation on the develop-
Gathers, A. D., Bhatt, R. S., Corbly, C. R., Farley, A. B., & Joseph, ment of face processing. Developmental Science, 5, 490–501.
J. E. (2004). Developmental shifts in cortical loci for face and Geller, B., Tillman, R., Bolhofner, K., & Zimerman, B. (2008).
object recognition. NeuroReport, 15, 1549–1553. Child bipolar I disorder: Prospective continuity with adult
Gaulden, M. E. (1992). Maternal age effect: The enigma of Down syn- bipolar I disorder; characteristics of second and third episodes;
drome and other trisomic conditions. Mutation Research Reviews in predictors of 8-year outcome. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65,
Genetic Toxicology, 296, 69–88. 1125–1133.
Gay P. (1993). The cultivation of hatred: The bourgeois experience: Geller, B., Tillman, R., Craney, J. L., & Bolhofner, K. (2004). Four-
Victoria to Freud. New York: W. W. Norton. year prospective outcome and natural history of mania in children
Gayán, J., Smith, S. D., Cherny, S. S., Cardon, L. R., Fulker, D. W., with a prepubertal and early adolescent bipolar disorder pheno-
Brower, A. M., Olson, R. K., Pennington, B. F., & DeFries, J. C. type. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 459–467.
(1999). Quantitative-trait locus for specific language and reading Geller, D. A., Biederman, J., Faraone, S., Agranat, A., Cradock, K.,
deficits on chromosome 6p. American Journal of Human Genetics, Hagermoser, L., Kim, G., Frazier, J., & Coffey, B. J. (2001).
64, 157–164. Developmental aspects of obsessive compulsive disorder: Findings
Gay-Andrieu, F., Marty, P., Pialat, J., Sournies, G., Drier de Laforte, T., & in children, adolescents, and adults. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Peyron, F. (2003). Fetal toxoplasmosis and negative amniocentesis: Disease, 189, 471–477.
Necessity of an ultrasound follow-up. Prenatal Diagnosis, 23, 558–560. Geller, D., Biederman, J., Jones, J., Park, K., Schwartz, S., Shapiro, S., &
Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., Wicker, B., & Keysers, C. (2007). The Coffey, B. (1998). Is juvenile obsessive-compulsive disorder a
anthropomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to developmental subtype of the disorder? A review of the pediatric
human and robotic actions. NeuroImage, 35, 1674–1684. literature. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Ge, S., Pradhan, D. A., Ming, G. L., & Song, H. (2007). GABA sets Psychiatry, 37, 420–427.
the tempo for activity-dependent adult neurogenesis. Trends in Geller, D. A., Wieland, N., Carey, K., Vivas, F., Petty, C. R.,
Neurosciences, 30, 1–8. Johnson, J., Reichert, E., Pauls, D., & Biederman, J. (2008).
Ge, X., Conger, R. D., & Elder, G. H. (1996). Coming of age too Perinatal factors affecting expression of obsessive compulsive dis-
early: Pubertal influences on girls’ vulnerability to psychological order in children and adolescents. Journal of Child and Adolescent
distress. Child Development, 67, 3386–3400. Psychopharmacology, 18, 373–379.
R-44 REFERENCES
Gelman, R. (1969). Conservation acquisition: A problem of learn- Gelman, S. A., & Hirschfeld, L. A. (1999). How biological is essential-
ing to attend to relevant attributes. Journal of Experimental Child ism? In D. L. Medin & S. Atran (Eds.), Folkbiology (pp. 403–446).
Psychology, 7, 167–187. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gelman, R. (1972). Logical capacity of very young children: Number Gelman, S. A., & Markman, E. M. (1987). Young children’s induc-
invariance rules. Child Development, 43, 75–90. tions from natural kinds: The role of categories and appearances.
Gelman, R. (1979). Preschool thought. American Psychologist, 34, Child Development, 58, 32–41.
900–905. Gelman, S. A., & Taylor, M. G. (2000). Gender essentialism in cog-
Gelman, R. (1982). Basic numerical abilities. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), nitive development. In P. H. Miller & E. K. Scholnick (Eds.),
Advances in the study of human intelligence (Vol.1, pp. 181–205). Toward a feminist developmental psychology (pp. 169–190). New
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. York: Routledge.
Gelman, R. (1990). First principles organize attention to and learning Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (1991). Insides and essence: Early
about relevant data: Number and the animate-inanimate distinc- understandings of the non-obvious. Cognition, 38, 213–244.
tion as examples. Cognitive Science, 14, 79–106. Gentile, D. A., & Anderson, C. A. (2006). Violent video games:
Gelman, R. (2009). Learning in core and noncore domains. In Effects on youth and public policy implications. In N. Dowd,
L. Tommasi, M. A. Peterson, & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive biology: D. G. Singer, & R. F. Wilson (Eds.), Handbook of children, culture,
Evolutionary and developmental perspectives on mind, brain, and and violence (pp. 225–246). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
behavior (pp. 247–260). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Gentner, D. (1977). If a tree had a knee, where would it be? Children’s
Gelman, R., & Baillargeon, R. (1983). A review of some Piagetian performance on simple spatial metaphors. Papers and Reports on
concepts. In P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.) & J. H. Flavell & E. M. Child Language Development, 13, 157–164.
Markman (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Gentner, D. (1978). On relational meaning: The acquisition of verb
Cognitive development (pp. 167–230). New York: Wiley. meaning. Child Development, 49, 988–998.
Gelman, R., & Butterworth, B. (2005). Number and language: How Gentner, D. (1986). Evidence for a structure-mapping theory of analogy
are they related? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 6–10. and metaphor (Tech. Rep. No. UIUCDCS-R-86-1316). Urbana,
Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. R. (1978). The child’s understanding of IL: University of Illinois, Department of Psychology.
number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Gentner, D. (2003). Why we’re so smart. In D. Gentner &
Gelman, R., & Greeno, J. G. (1989). On the nature of competence: S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the
Principles for understanding in a domain. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), study of language and thought (pp.195–235). Cambridge, MA:
Knowing, learning, and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser MIT Press.
(pp. 125–186). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gentner, D., & Rattermann, M. J. (1991). Language and the career
Gelman, R., & Lucariello, J. (2002). Role of learning in cognitive of similarity. In S. A. Gelman & J. P. Byrnes (Eds.), Perspectives on
development. In H. Pashler (Series Ed.) & R. Gallistel (Vol. Ed.), language and thought: Interrelations in development (pp. 225–277).
Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology: Vol. 3. Learning, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
motivation, and emotion (3rd ed., pp. 395–443). New York: Wiley. Gentner, D., Ratterman, M. J., Markman, A., & Kotovsky, L. (1995).
Gelman, R., & Meck, E. (1983). Preschoolers’ counting: Principles Two forces in the development of relational similarity. In T. J.
before skill. Cognition, 13, 343–359. Simon & G. S. Halford (Eds.), Developing cognitive competence:
Gelman, R., & Spelke, E. S. (1981). The development of thoughts New approaches to process modeling (pp. 263–313). Hillsdale, NJ:
about animate and inanimate objects: Implications for research in Erlbaum.
social cognition. In J. H. Flavell & L. Ross (Eds.), Social cognitive Geoffroy, M. C., Côté, S., Giguère, C., Dionne, G., Zelazo, P. D.,
development: Frontiers and possible futures (pp. 43–66). Cambridge, Tremblay, R. E., Boivin, M., & Séguin J. R. (2010). Closing
UK: Cambridge University Press. the gap in academic readiness and achievement: The role of
Gelman, R., & Williams, E. M. (1998). Enabling constraints for cog- early childcare. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51,
nitive development and learning: Domain specificity and epigene- 1359–1367.
sis. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Georgiou, S. N. (2008). Beliefs of experienced and novice teachers
Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception and lan- about achievement. Educational Psychology, 28, 119–131.
guage (5th ed., pp. 575–630). New York: Wiley. Gerall, A. A., Moltz, H., & Ward, I. L. (Vol. Eds.) & Adler, N. T.
Gelman, S. A. (1988). The development of induction within natural (Series Ed.). (1992). Handbook of behavioral neurobiology: Vol. 11.
kind and artifact categories. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 65–95. Sexual differentiation. New York: Plenum Press.
Gelman, S. A. (1992). Children’s conception of personality traits. Gerard, M., Zakany, J., & Duboule, D. (1997). Interspecies exchange
Human Development, 35, 280–285. of a Hoxd enhancer in vivo induces premature transcription and
Gelman, S. A. (2002). The essential child: Origins of essentialism in anterior shift of the sacrum. Developmental Biology, l90, 32–40.
everyday thought. New York: Oxford University Press. Gergely, G. (2011). Kinds of agents: The origins of understanding
Gelman, S. A. (2009). Learning from others: Children’s construction instrumental and communicative agency. In U. Goshwami, (Ed.),
of concepts. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 115–140. The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development
Gelman, S. A., Coley, J. D., & Gottfried, G. M. (1994). Essentialist (2nd ed., pp. 76–105). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
beliefs in children: The acquisition of concepts and theories. In Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in
L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.
specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 341–365). Cambridge, UK: Gergely, G., &. Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infancy:
Cambridge University Press. The naive theory of rational action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
Gelman, S. A., Collman, P., & Maccoby, E. E. (1986). Inferring prop- 7, 287–292.
erties from categories versus inferring categories from properties: Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Biro, S., (1995). Taking the
The case of gender. Child Development, 57, 396–404. intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition 56, 165–193.
REFERENCES R-45
Gerken, L. (1987). Telegraphic speaking does not imply telegraphic Ghashghaei, H. T., Lai, C., & Anton, E. S. (2007). Neuronal
listening. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 26, migration in the adult brain: Are we there yet? Nature Reviews:
48–55. Neuroscience, 8, 141–151.
Gerken, L. A., & Aslin, R. N. (2005). Thirty years of research on Ghera, M. M., Marshall, P. J., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., Nelson,
infant speech perception: The legacy of Peter W. Jusczyk. Language C. A., Smyke, A. T., & Guthrie, D. (2009). The effects of foster
Learning and Development, 1, 5–21. care intervention on socially deprived institutionalized children’s
German, T. P., & Leslie, A. M. (2001). Children’s inferences attention and positive affect: Results from the BEIP study. Journal
from knowing to pretending and believing. British Journal of of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 246–253.
Developmental Psychology, 19, 59–83. Ghetti, S., DeMaster, D., Yonelinas, A. P., & Bunge, S. A. (2010).
German, T., & Hehman, J. A. (2006). Representational and executive Developmental differences in the contribution of medial tem-
selection resources in “theory of mind”: Evidence from compro- poral lobes to memory formation. Journal of Neuroscience, 30,
mised belief-desire reasoning in old age. Cognition, 101, 129–152. 9548–9556.
Gershoff, E. T. (2002). Corporal punishment by parents and associ- Ghisletta, P., & Lindenberger, U. (2003). Age-based structural
ated child behaviors and experiences: A meta-analytic and theo- dynamics between perceptual speed and knowledge in the Berlin
retical review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 539–579. Aging Study: Direct evidence for ability dedifferentiation in old
Gershon, J. (2002).A meta-analytic review of gender differences in age. Psychology and Aging, 18, 696–713.
ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 5, 143–154. Ghosh, A., Antonini, A., McConnell, S. K., & Shatz, C. J. (1990).
Gersten, R., Woodward, J., & Schneider, S. (1992). Bilingual immer- Requirement for subplate neurons in the formation of thalamocor-
sion: A longitudinal evaluation of the El Paso program. Washington, tical connections. Nature, 347, 179–181.
DC: READ Institute. Gibbs, C., Ludwig, J., & Miller, D. L. (2011). Does Head Start do
Gerull, F. C., & Rapee, R. M. (2002). Mother knows best: Effects of any lasting good? National Labor Bureau of Economic Research,
maternal modelling on the acquisition of fear and avoidance behav- Working Paper No. 17452. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of
iour in toddlers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 279–287. Economic Research.
Gervai, J. (2009). Environmental and genetic influences on early Gibbs, J. C., Basinger, K. S., Grime, R. L., & Snarey, J. R. (2007).
attachment. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 3, Moral judgment development across cultures: Revisiting Kohlberg’s
25–37. universality claims. Developmental Review, 27, 443–500.
Gervai, J., Novak, A., Lakatos, K., Toth, I., Danis, I., Ronai, Z., Gibbs, R. W. (1994). The poetics of mind: Figurative thought,
Nemoda, Z., Sasvari-Szekely, M., Bureau, J. F., Bronfman, E., & language, and understanding. New York: Cambridge University
Lyons-Ruth, K. (2007). Infant genotype may moderate sensitiv- Press.
ity to maternal affective communications: Attachment disorga- Gibbs, R. W. (1999). Figurative language. In R. A. Wilson & F. C. Keil
nization, quality of care, and the DRD4 polymorphism. Social (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences (pp. 314–315).
Neuroscience, 2, 307–319. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gervain, J., & Mehler, J. (2010). Speech perception and language Gibbs, W. W., & Fox, D. (1999). The false crisis in science education.
acquisition in the first year of life. Annual Review of Psychology, Scientific American, October, 87–93.
61, 191–218. Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development.
Geschwind, D. H. (2008). Autism: Many genes, common pathways? New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Cell, 135, 391–395. Gibson, E. J. (1991). An odyssey in learning and perception. Cambridge,
Geschwind, D. H., & Spence, S. J. (2008). Genetics of autism. MA: MIT Press.
Continuum: Lifelong Learning in Neurology: Neurogenetics, 14, Gibson, E. J., & Levin, H. (1975). The psychology of reading.
49–64. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gesell, A. (1945). The embryology of behavior. New York: Harper and Gibson, E. J., Riccio, G., Schmuckler, M., Stoffregen, T. A., Rosenberg,
Row. D., & Taormina, J. (1987). Detection of the traversability of sur-
Gesell, A. (1954). The ontogenesis of infant behavior. In L. Carmichael faces by crawling and walking infants. Journal of Experimental
(Ed.), Manual of child psychology (2nd ed., pp. 335–373). New Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 515–523.
York: Wiley. Gibson, E. J., & Walk, R. D. (1960). The “visual cliff.” Scientific
Gesell, A., & Ilg, F. L. (1949). Child development. New York: Harper. American, 202, 64–71.
Gesell, A., & Thompson, H. (1929). Learning and growth in identi- Gibson, E. J., & Walker, A. S. (1984). Development of knowledge of
cal infant twins: An experimental study by the method of co-twin visual and tactual affordances of substance. Child Development, 55,
control. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 6, 11–124. 453–460.
Getahun, D., Jacobsen, S. J., Fassett, M. J., Chen, W., Demissie, K., & Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston:
Rhoads, G. G. (2013). Recent trends in childhood attention- Houghton Mifflin.
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Medical Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. In R. E. Shaw &
Association Pediatrics, 167, 282–288. J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting, and knowing: Toward an eco-
Gettler, L. T. (2010). Direct male care and hominin evolution: Why logical psychology (pp. 67–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
male–child interaction is more than a nice social idea. American Gifford-Smith, M., & Brownell, C. (2003). Childhood peer relation-
Anthropologist, 112, 7–21. ships: Social acceptance, friendships and peer networks. Journal of
Getzels, J. W., & Jackson, P. W. (1962). Creativity and intelligence. School Psychology, 41, 235–284.
New York: Wiley. Gilbert, A. R., & Maalouf, F. T. (2008). Pediatric obsessive-compulsive
Gewirtz, J. L. (1961). A learning analysis of the effects of normal disorder: Management priorities in primary care. Current Opinion
stimulation, privation, and deprivation on the acquisition of social in Pediatrics, 20, 544–550.
motivation and attachment. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of Gilbert, S. F. (2003). Developmental biology (7th ed.). Sunderland,
infant behavior (Vol. 1, pp. 213–299). London: Methuen. MA: Sinauer Associates.
R-46 REFERENCES
Gilbert, S. F. (2006). Developmental biology (8th ed.). Sunderland, Gleitman, L. R., & Gleitman, H. (1992). A picture is worth a thousand
MA: Sinauer Associates. words, but that’s the problem: The role of syntax in vocabulary
Gilbert, S. F. (2010). Developmental biology (9th ed.). Sunderland, acquisition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 31–35.
MA: Sinauer Associates. Gleitman, L. R., & Landau, B. (Eds.). (1994). The acquisition of the
Giles, H., Ryan, E. B., & Anas, A. P. (2008). Perceptions of inter- lexicon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
generational communication by young, middle-aged, and older Gleitman, L. R., & Newport, E. L. (1995). The invention of lan-
Canadian adults. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 40, guage by children: Environmental and biological influences on the
121–130. acquisition of language. In L. Gleitman & M. Liberman (Eds.), An
Gilger, J., Pennington, B. F., & DeFries, J. C. (1992). A twin study invitation to cognitive science: Vol. 1. Language (2nd ed., pp. 1–24).
of the etiology of comorbidity: Attention-deficit hyperactivity dis- Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
order and dyslexia. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Gleitman, L. R., Newport, E. L., & Gleitman, H. (1984). The current
Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 343–348. status of the motherese hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 11,
Gilinsky, A. S., & Judd, B. B. (1994). Working memory and 43–79.
bias in reasoning across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 9, Gleitman, L. R., & Wanner, E. (1984). Richly specified input to lan-
356–371. guage learning. In O. Selfridge, E. L. Rissland, & M. Arbib (Eds.),
Gill, M., Daly, G., Heron, S., Hawi, Z., & Fitzgerald M. (1997). Adaptive control of ill-defined systems. New York: Plenum.
Confirmation of association between attention deficit hyperactiv- Glenn, A. L., Raine, A., & Schug, R. A. (2009). The neural correlates of
ity disorder and a dopamine transporter polymorphism. Molecular moral decision-making in psychopathy. Molecular Psychiatry, 14, 5–6.
Psychiatry, 2, 311–313. Glenn, C. L. (1997). What does the National Research Council study tell
Gillette, J., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L., & Lederer, A. (1999). Human us about educating language minority children? Washington, DC:
simulations of vocabulary learning. Cognition, 73, 135–176. READ Institute.
Gilligan, C. (1977). In a different voice: Women’s conceptions of self Glicksohn, J. (2002). Criminality, personality and cognitive neurosci-
and of morality. Harvard Educational Review, 47, 481–517. ence. In J. Glicksohn (Ed.), The neurobiology of criminal behav-
Gilligan, C. (1979). Woman’s place in man’s life cycle. Harvard ior (Neurobiological foundation of aberrant behaviors) (pp. 3–24).
Educational Review, 49, 431–446. Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and Gliga, T., Volein, A., & Csibra, G. (2010). Verbal labels modulate
women’s development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. perceptual object processing in one-year-old infants. Journal of
Gillis, J. J., Gilger, J. W., Pennington, B. E., & DeFries, J. C. (1992). Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2781–2789.
Attention deficit disorder in reading-disabled twins: Evidence Glocker, M. L., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J. W.,
for a genetic etiology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, Valdez, J. N., Griffin, M. C., Sachser, N., & Gur, R. C. (2009).
303–315. Baby schema modulates the brain reward system in nulliparous
Gingerich, K. J., Turnock, P., Litfin, J. K., & Rosen, L. A. (1998). women. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA,
Diversity and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of 106, 9115–9119.
Clinical Psychology, 54, 415–426. Glowinski, A. L., Madden, P. A. F., Bucholz, K. K., Lynskey, M. T., &
Ginsburg, H. P., & Opper, S. (1988). Piaget’s theory of intellectual Heath, A. C. (2003). Genetic epidemiology of self-reported life-
development (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. time DSM-IV major depressive disorder in a population-based
Giraud, A. L., & Ramus, F. (2013). Neurogenetics and audi- twin sample of female adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and
tory processing in developmental dyslexia. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 44, 988–996.
Neurobiology, 23, 37–42. Göbel, S. M., Calabria, M., Farné, A., & Rossetti, Y. (2006). Parietal
Giusti, V., Heraief, E., Gaillard, R. C., Burckhardt, P., & Giusti, V. rTMS distorts the mental number line: Simulating “spatial” neglect
(2004). Predictive factors of binge eating disorder in women in healthy subjects. Neuropsychologia, 44, 860–868.
searching to lose weight. Eating & Weight Disorders, 9, 44–49. Gochman, P., Miller, R., & Rapoport, J. L. (2011). Childhood-onset
Gizewski, E. R., Gasser, T., de Greiff, A., Boehm, A., & Forsting, M. schizophrenia: The challenge of diagnosis. Current Psychiatry
(2003). Cross-modal plasticity for sensory and motor activation Reports, 13, 321–322.
patterns in blind subjects. Neuroimage, 19, 968–975. Godfrey-Smith, P. (2000). On the theoretical role of “genetic coding.”
Gladwin, T. (1970). East is a big bird: Navigation and logic on Puluwat Philosophy of Science, 67, 26–44.
Atoll. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Godwin, J. (2009). Social determination of sex in reef fishes. Seminars
Glanzer, M., & Cunitz, A. R. (1966). Two storage mechanisms in in Cell and Developmental Biology, 20, 264–270.
free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D.,
351–360. Vaituzis, A. C., Nugent, T. F., Herman, D. H., Clasen, L. S., Toga,
Glascher, J., Rudrauf, D., Colom, R., Paul, L. K., Tranel, D., A.W., Rapoport, J. L., & Thompson, P. M . (2004). Dynamic map-
Damasio, H., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Distributed neural system for ping of human cortical development during childhood through
general intelligence revealed by lesion mapping. Proceedings of the early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 107, 4705–4709. the USA, 101, 8174–8179.
Glaser, R., & Silver, E. (1994). Assessment, testing, and instruction: Gogtay, N., Sporn, A., Clasen, L. S., Nugent, T. F., Greenstein, D.,
Retrospect and prospect. Review of Research in Education, 20, Nicolson, R., Giedd, J. N., Lenane, M., Gochman, P., Evans, A., &
393–419. Rapoport, J. L. (2004). Comparison of progressive cortical
Gleitman, L. R. (1990). The structural sources of verb meanings. gray matter loss in childhood-onset schizophrenia with that in
Language Acquisition, 1, 3–55. childhood-onset atypical psychoses. Archives of General Psychiatry,
Gleitman, L., & Bloom, P. (1999). Language acquisition. In R. 61, 17–22.
Wilson & F. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of cognitive science Goldberg, J, (2000, June 25). Inside Jihad U.: The education of a holy
(pp. 434–438). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. warrior. New York Times Magazine.
REFERENCES R-47
Goldberg, J. F., Gerstein, R. K., Wenze, S. J., Welker, T. M., & Beck, in developmental psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 231–283). Hillsdale, NJ:
A. T. (2008). Dysfunctional attitudes and cognitive schemas in Erlbaum.
bipolar manic and unipolar depressed outpatients: Implications for Goldsmith, H. H., & Campos, J. J. (1990). The structure of tempera-
cognitively based psychotherapeutics. Nervous and Mental Disease, mental fear and pleasure in infants: A psychometric perspective.
196, 207–210. Child Development, 61, 1944–1964.
Goldberg, L. R. (1982). From ace to zombie: Some explorations in Goldsmith, H. H., & Gottesman, I. I. (1981). Origins of variation in
the language of personality. In C. D. Spielberger & J. N. Butcher behavioral style: A longitudinal study of temperament in young
(Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (Vol. 1, pp. 203–234). twins. Child Development, 52, 91–103.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Goldsmith, H. H., Lemery, K. S., Aksan, N., & Buss, K. A. (2000).
Goldberg, L. R. (1992). The development of markers for the Big-Five Temperamental substrates of personality development. In V. J.
factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4, 26–42. Molfese & D. L. Molfese (Eds.), Temperament and personality
Goldberg, M. C., Maurer, D., & Lewis, T. L. (2001). Developmental development across the life span (pp. 1–32). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
changes in attention: The effects of endogenous cueing and of dis- Goldsmith, H. H., Lemery, K. S., Buss, K. A., & Campos, J. J.
tractors. Developmental Science, 4, 209-219. (1999). Genetic analyses of focal aspects of infant temperament.
Goldenberg, I., Matheson, K., & Mantler, J. (2006). The assessment Developmental Psychology, 35, 972–985.
of emotional intelligence: A comparison of performance-based and Goldsmith, H. H., Pollak, S. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008).
self-report methodologies. Journal of Personality Assessment, 86, Developmental neuroscience perspectives on emotion regulation.
33–45. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 132–140.
Goldenberg, R. L., Cliver, S. P., Mulvihill, F. X., Hickey, C. A., Goldstone, R. L., & Hendrickson, A. T. (2010). Categorical percep-
Hoffman, H. J., Klerman, L. V., & Johnson, M. J. (1996). Medical, tion. Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 69–78.
psychosocial, and behavioral risk factors do not explain the Golinkoff, R. M., & Alioto, A. (1995). Infant-directed speech facili-
increased risk for low birth weight among black women. American tates lexical learning in adults hearing Chinese: Implications for
Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 175, 1317–1324. language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 22, 703–726.
Goldenberg, R. L., Culhane, J. F., Iams, J., & Romero, R. (2008). Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Bailey, L. M., & Wenger, N. R.
Epidemiology and causes of preterm birth. The Lancet, 371, 75–84. (1992). Young children and adults use lexical principles to learn
Goldfarb, W. (1943). The effects of early institutional care on adoles- new nouns. Developmental Psychology, 28, 99–108.
cent personality. Journal of Experimental Education, 12, 106–129. Golomb, C. (2004). The child’s creation of a pictorial world (2nd ed.).
Goldfarb, W. P. (1945). Psychological privation in infancy and sub- Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
sequent adjustment. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 15, Golombok, S. (2000). Parenting: What really counts? London:
247–255. Routledge.
Goldfield, B. A., & Reznick, J. S. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: Gomby, D. S., Larner, M. B., Stevenson, J. D., Lewit, E. M., &
Rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Language, Behrman, M. D. (1995). The future of children: Long-term outcomes
17, 171–183. of early childhood programs. Los Altos, CA: Center for the Future of
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Learning through gesture. Wiley Children, David and Lucille Packard Foundation.
Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2, 595–607. Gomez, J. C. (2005). Species comparative studies and cognitive devel-
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Mylander, C. (1998). Spontaneous sign sys- opment. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 118–125.
tems creation by deaf children in two cultures. Nature, 391, Good, C., Aronson, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2003). Improving adoles-
279–281. cents’ standardized test performance: An intervention to reduce
Goldman, L. S., Genel, M., Bezman, R. J., & Slanetz, P. J. (1998). the effects of stereotype threat. Journal of Applied Developmental
Diagnosis and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor- Psychology, 24, 645–662.
der in children and adolescents. Journal of the American Medical Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for
Association, 279, 1100–1107. perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15, 20–24.
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987). Development of cortical circuitry and Goodale, M. A., & Westwood, D. A. (2004). An evolving view of
cognitive function. Child Development, 58, 601–622. duplex vision: Separate but interacting cortical pathways for per-
Goldman-Rakic, P. S., Bourgeois, J. P., & Rakic, P. (1997). Synaptic ception and action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 203–211.
substrate of cognitive development: Synaptogenesis in the prefron- Goodglass, H. (1993). Understanding aphasia. San Diego: Academic
tal cortex of the non-human primate. In N. A. Krasnegor, G. R. Press.
Lyon, & P. S. Goldman-Rakic (Eds.), Development of the pre- Goodluck, H. (1991). Language acquisition: A linguistic introduction.
frontal cortex: Evolution, neurobiology, and behavior (pp. 27–47). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Goodman, C. S., & Shatz, C. J. (1993). Developmental mechanisms
Goldman-Rakic, P. S., Iseroff, A., Schwartz, M. L., & Bugbee, N. M. that generate precise patterns of neuronal connectivity. Cell, 72,
(1983). The neurobiology of cognitive development. In P. H. 77–98.
Mussen (Series Ed.) & M. M. Haith & J. J. Campos (Vol. Eds.), Goodman, G. S., & Clarke-Stewart, A. (1991). Suggestibility in chil-
Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Infancy and developmental psy- dren’s testimony: Implications for sexual abuse investigations. In
chobiology (4th ed., pp. 311–344). New York: Wiley. J. Doris (Ed.), The suggestibility of children’s recollections: Implications
Goldsmith, H. H., & Campos, J. J. (1982). Toward a theory of infant for eyewitness testimony (pp. 92–105). Washington, DC: American
temperament. In R. N. Emde & R. J. Harmon (Eds.), The develop- Psychological Association.
ment of attachment and affiliative systems (pp. 161– 193). New York: Goodman, G. S., & Melinder, A. (2007). Child witness research
Plenum. and forensic interviews of young children: A review. Legal and
Goldsmith, H. H., & Campos, J. J. (1986). Fundamental issues in Criminological Psychology, 12, 1–19.
the study of early temperament: The Denver Twin Temperament Goodman, G. S., Ghetti, S., Quas, J. A., Edelstein, R. S., Alexander,
Study. In M. E. Lamb, A. L. Brown, & B. Rogoff (Eds.), Advances K. W., Redlich, A. D., Cordon, I. M., & Jones, D. P. H. (2003).
R-48 REFERENCES
A prospective study of memory for child sexual abuse: New find- Gotestam, K. G., & Agras, W. S. (1995). General population-based
ings relevant to the repressed-memory controversy. Psychological epidemiological study of eating disorders in Norway. International
Science, 14, 113–118. Journal of Eating Disorders, 18, 119–126.
Goodman, N. D., Ullman, T. D., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2011). Gotlib, I. H., & Hammen, C. L. (1992). Psychological aspects of depres-
Learning a theory of causality. Psychological Review, 118, 110–119. sion: Toward a cognitive-interpersonal integration. New York: Wiley.
Goodman, R., & Stevenson, J. (1989). A twin study of hyperactivity—II. Gotlib, I. H., Traill, S. K., Montoya, R. L., Joormann, J., & Chang,
The aetiological role of genes, family relationships and perinatal K. (2005). Attention and memory biases in the offspring of parents
adversity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 691–709. with bipolar disorder: Indications from a pilot study. Journal of
Goodnow, J. J. (1977). Children drawing. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 84–93.
University Press. Gottesman, I. I., & Bertelsen A. (1989). Confirming unexpressed gen-
Gooren, L. (2006). The biology of human psychosexual differentia- otypes for schizophrenia: Risks in the offspring of Fischer’s Danish
tion. Hormones and Behavior, 50, 589–601. identical and fraternal discordant twins. Archives of General
Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1997). Words, thoughts and theories. Psychiatry, 46, 867–872.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Gottesman, I. I., & Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L. (2001). Family and
Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. M. (1994). The theory theory. In L. A. twin strategies as a head start in defining prodromes and endo-
Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds), Mapping the mind: Domain phenotypes for hypothetical early-interventions in schizophrenia.
specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 257–293). New York: Schizophrenia Research, 51, 93–102.
Cambridge University Press. Gottesman, I. I., & Wolfgram, D. L. (1991). Schizophrenia genesis: The
Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. M. (2012). Reconstructing constructiv- origins of madness. New York: W. H. Freeman.
ism: Causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms, and the the- Gottfredson, L. S. (1998). The general intelligence factor. Scientific
ory theory. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1085–1108. American Presents, 9, 24–29.
Gopnik, M. (1990a). Dysphasia in an extended family. Nature, 344, 715. Gottfried, A. E., & Gottfried, A. W. (2006). A long-term investiga-
Gopnik, M. (1990b). Feature blindness: A case study. Language tion of the role of maternal and dual-earner employment in chil-
Acquisition, 1, 139–164 dren’s development: The Fullerton Longitudinal Study. American
Gopnik, M. (1994). Impairments of tense in a familial language disor- Behavioral Scientist 49, 1310–1327.
der. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 8, 109–133. Gottlieb, G. (1983). The psychobiological approach to developmental
Gopnik, M. (1999). Some evidence for impaired grammars. In issues. In P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.) & M. M. Haith & J. J. Campos
R. Jackendoff, P. Bloom, & K. Wynn (Eds.), Language, logic, and (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Infancy and bio-
concepts (pp. 263–283). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. logical bases (4th ed., pp. 1–26). New York: Wiley.
Gopnik, M., & Crago, M. B. (1991). Familial aggregation of a devel- Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science,
opmental language disorder. Cognition, 39, 1–50. 10, 1–11.
Gordon, C. M., & Laufer, M. R. (2005). The physiology of puberty. Gottman, J. M. (1993). The roles of conflict engagement, escalation, and
In S. J. Emans, M. R. Laufer, & D. P. Goldstein (Eds.), Pediatric avoidance in marital interaction: A longitudinal view of five types of
and adolescent gynecology (5th ed., pp. 120–155). Philadelphia: couples. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 6–15.
Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Gough, P. B., Juel, C., & Roper-Schneider, D. (1983). Code and
Gordon, I., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Leckman, J. F., & Feldman, R. cipher: A two-stage conception of initial reading acquisition. In
(2010). Prolactin, oxytocin, and the development of paternal J .A. Niles & L. A. Harris (Eds.), Searches for meaning in reading/
behavior across the first six months of fatherhood. Hormones and language processing and instruction (pp. 207–211). Rochester NY:
Behavior, 58, 513–518. National Reading Conference.
Gordon, P. (2004). Numerical cognition without words: Evidence Gould, E. (2007). How widespread is adult neurogenesis in mam-
from Amazonia. Science, 306, 496–499. mals? Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 8, 481–488.
Goren, C. C., Sarty, M., & Wu, P. Y. K. (1975). Visual following Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge, MA:
and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Belknap Press.
Pediatrics, 56, 544–549. Gould, S. J. (1981). The mismeasure of man. New York: W. W. Norton.
Gosling, S. D. (2001). From mice to men: What can we learn about per- Gould, S. J., (1983). What, if anything, is a zebra? In Hen’s teeth and
sonality from animal research? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 45–86. horse’s toes: Further reflections in natural history (pp. 355–365). New
Goswami, U. (1991). Analogical reasoning: What develops? A review York: W. W. Norton.
of research and theory. Child Development, 62, 1–22. Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco
Goswami, U. (1995). Transitive relational mappings in three- and and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist pro-
four-year-olds: The analogy of Goldilocks and the Three Bears. gramme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 205, 581–598.
Child Development, 66, 877–892. Goy, R. W., & McEwen, B. S. (1980). Sexual differentiation of the
Goswami, U. (2001). Analogical reasoning in children. In D. Gentner, brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
K. J. Holyoak, & B. N. Kokinov (Eds.), The analogical mind: Graber, J. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., Seeley J. R., & J. Brooks-Gunn, J.
Perspectives from cognitive science (pp. 437–470). Cambridge, MA: (1997). Is psychopathology associated with the timing of puber-
MIT Press. tal development? Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Goswami, U. (2006). Neuroscience and education: From research to Adolescent Psychiatry 36, 1768–1776.
practice? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 406–413. Grabowski, D. C., & Morrisey, M. A. (2001). The effect of state regu-
Goswami, U., & Brown, A. L. (1990). Melting chocolate and melting lations on motor vehicle fatalities for younger and older drivers: A
snowmen: Analogical reasoning and causal relations. Cognition, review and analysis. Milbank Quarterly, 79, 517–545.
35, 69–95. Gracia, E., & Herrero, J. (2008). Is it considered violence? The accept-
Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to ability of physical punishment of children in Europe. Journal of
read. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Marriage and Family, 70, 210–217.
REFERENCES R-49
Gradinger, P., Strohmeier, D., & Spiel, C. (2009). Traditional bully- Gredebäck, G., Theuring, C., Hauf, P., & Kenwald, B. (2008). The
ing and cyberbullying: Identification of risk groups for adjustment microstructure of infants’ gaze as they view adult shifts in overt
problems. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 217, 205–213. attention. Infancy, 13, 533–543.
Graesser, A. C., Conley, M. W., & Olney, A. (2012). Intelligent tutor- Gredebäck, G., & von Hofsten, C. (2004). Infants’ evolving represen-
ing systems. In S. Graham & K. Harris (Eds.), APA educational tations of object motion during occlusion: A longitudinal study of
psychology handbook: Vol. 3. Applications to learning and teach- 6- to 12-month-old infants. Infancy, 6, 165–184.
ing (pp. 451–473). Washington, DC: American Psychological Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2003). Action video game modifies
Association. visual selective attention. Nature, 423, 534–537.
Graesser, A. C., Jackson, G. T., & McDaniel, B. (2007). AutoTutor Green, R. L., Hoffman, L. T., Morse, R., Hayes, M. E., Morgan, R. F.
holds conversations with learners that are responsive to their cogni- (1964). The educational status of children in a district without pub-
tive and emotional states. Educational Technology, 47, 19–22. lic schools (Cooperative Research Project No. 23211). Washington,
Graesser, A., & McNamara, D. (2010). Self-regulated learning in DC: Office of Education. U.S. Department of Health, Education,
learning environments with pedagogical agents that interact in and Welfare.
natural language. Educational Psychologist, 45, 234–244. Greenberg, J. H. (1966). Some universals of grammar with particular
Graf Estes, K., Evans, J. L., Alibali, M. W., & Saffran, J. R. (2007). reference to the order of meaningful elements. In J. H. Greenberg
Can infants map meaning to newly segmented words? Statistical (Ed.), Universals of language (2nd ed., pp. 73–113). Cambridge,
segmentation and word learning. Psychological Science, 18, MA: MIT Press.
254–260. Greenberg, J. H. (Ed.) (1978). Universals of human language: Vol. 4.
Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, Syntax. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality Greene, J. D. (2003). From neural “is” to moral “ought”: What are the
and Social Psychology, 101, 366–385. moral implications of neuroscientific moral psychology? Nature
Graham, S. A., & Fisher, S. E. (2013). Decoding the genetics of speech Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 847–850.
and language. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 43–51. Greene, J. D. (2007). Why are VMPFC patients more utilitarian?
Granger, B., Tekaia, F., Le Sourd, A. M., Rakic, P., & Bourgeois J.-P. A dual-process theory of moral judgment explains. Trends in
(1995). Tempo of neurogenesis and synaptogenesis in the primate Cognitive Sciences, 11, 322–323.
cingulate mesocortex: Comparison with the neocortex. Journal of Greene, J., & Haidt, J. (2002). How (and where) does moral judgment
Comparative Neurology, 360, 363–376. work? Trends in Cognitive Science, 6, 517–523.
Granic, I., Meusel, L. A., Lamm, C., Woltering, S., & Lewis, M. D. Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., & Cohen,
(2012). Emotion regulation in children with behavior prob- J. D. (2004). The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in
lems: Linking behavioral and brain processes. Development and moral judgment. Neuron, 44, 389–400.
Psychopathology, 24, 1019–1029. Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., &
Granot, D., & Mayseless, O. (2001). Attachment security and adjust- Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engage-
ment to school in middle childhood. International Journal of ment in moral judgment. Science, 293, 2105–2108.
Behavioral Development, 25, 530–541. Greene, K., Krcmar, M., Walters, L. H., Rubin, D. L., & Hale, J. L.
Granrud, C. E. (1986). Binocular vision and spatial perception on (2000). Targeting adolescent risk-taking behaviors: The contribu-
4- and 5-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: tions of egocentrism and sensation-seeking. Journal of Adolescence,
Human Perception and Performance, 12, 36–49. 23, 439–461.
Granrud, C. E., & Yonas, A. (1984). Infants’ perception of pictorially Greenfield, P. M. (1991). Language, tools, and brain: The ontogeny
specified interposition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior.
37, 500–511. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 531– 595.
Grapin-Botton, A., Bonnin, M.-A., McNaughton, L. A., Krumlauf, Greenfield, P. M., & Smith, J. H. (1976). The structure of communica-
R., & Le Douarin, N. M. (1995). Plasticity of transposed rhombo- tion in early language development. New York: Academic Press.
meres: Hox gene induction is correlated with phenotypic modifica- Greeno, J. G., Riley, M. S., & Gelman, R. (1984). Conceptual
tions. Development, 121, 2707–2721. competence and children’s counting. Cognitive Psychology, 16,
Gray, E. R., Bloomfield, L. L., Ferrey, A., Spetch, M. L., & Sturdy, 94–143.
C. B. (2005). Spatial encoding in mountain chickadees: Features Greenough, W. T., Black J. E., & Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience
overshadow geometry. Biology Letters, 1, 314–317. and brain development. Child Development, 58, 539–559.
Gray, L., Watt, L., & Blass, E. (2000). Skin-to-skin contact is analge- Gregory, A. M., & Eley, T. C. (2007). Genetic influences on anxiety
sic in healthy newborns. Pediatrics, 105, e14–e24. in children: What we’ve learned and where we’re heading. Clinical
Gray, T. A., Hernandez, L., Carey, A. H., Schaldach, M. A., Smithwick, Child & Family Psychology Review, 10, 199–212.
M. J., Rus, K., Marshall Graves, J. A., Stewart, C. L., & Nicholls, Gregory, A. M., Rijsdijk, F.V., Lau, J. Y. F., Napolitano, M.,
R. D. (2000). The ancient source of a distinct gene family encod- McGuffin, P., & Eley, T. C. (2007). Genetic and environmen-
ing proteins featuring RING and C(3)H zinc-finger motifs with tal influences on interpersonal cognitions and associations with
abundant expression in developing brain and nervous system. depressive symptoms in 8-year-old twins. Journal of Abnormal
Genomics, 66, 76–86. Psychology, 116, 762–775.
Gray-Little, B., & Hafdahl, A. R. (2000). Factors influencing racial Greif, M. L., Kemler Nelson, D. G., Keil, F. C., & Gutierrez, F.
comparisons of self-esteem: A quantitative review. Psychological (2006). What do children want to know about animals and arti-
Bulletin, 126, 26–54. facts? Domain-specific requests for information. Psychological
Gredebäck, G., Fikke, L., & Melinder, A. (2010). The development Science, 17, 455–459.
of joint visual attention: A longitudinal study of gaze following Greitemeyer, T. (2011). Effects of prosocial media on social behavior:
during interactions with mothers and strangers. Developmental When and why does media exposure affect helping and aggression?
Science, 13, 839–848. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 251–255.
R-50 REFERENCES
Greitemeyer, T., & Osswald, S. (2010). Effects of prosocial video Gross, E. F., Juvonen, J., & Gable, S. (2002). Internet use and well-
games on prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social being in adolescence. Journal of Social Issues, 58, 75–90.
Psychology, 98, 211–221. Gross, J. (2002, September 26). Paying for a disability diagnosis to
Greitemeyer, T., Traut-Mattausch, E., & Osswald, S. (2012). How to gain time on College Boards. The New York Times, p. A1.
ameliorate negative effects of violent video games on cooperation: Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Kindler, H.,
Play it cooperatively in a team. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, Scheuerer-Englisch, H., & Zimmerman, P. (2002). The unique-
1465–1470. ness of the child-father attachment relationship: Fathers’ sensitive
Grello, C. M., Welsh, D. P., Harper, M. S., & Dickson, J. W. (2003). and challenging play as the pivotal variable in a 16-year longitudi-
Dating and sexual relationship trajectories and adolescent func- nal study. Social Development, 11, 307–331.
tioning. Adolescent and Family Health, 3, 103–112. Grossmann, T., Oberecker, R., Koch, S. P., & Friederici, A. D. (2010).
Grèzes, J., Armony, J. L., Rowe, J., & Passingham, R. E. (2003). The developmental origins of voice processing in the human brain.
Activations related to “mirror” and “canonical” neurones in the Neuron, 65, 852–858.
human brain: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 18, 928–937. Grossniklaus, U., Kelly, B., Ferguson-Smith, A. C., Pembrey, M., &
Grice, D. E., Halmi, K. A., Fichter, M. M., Strober, M., Woodside, Lindquist, S. (2013). Transgenerational epigenetic inheritance:
D. B., Treasure, J. T., Kaplan, A. S., Magistretti, P. J., Goldman, D., How important is it? Nature Reviews Genetics, 14, 228–235.
Bulik, C. M., Kaye, W. H., & Berrettini, W. H. (2002). Evidence Gruber, H. E., & Vonèche, J. J. (1977). The essential Piaget. London:
for a susceptibility gene for anorexia nervosa on chromosome 1. Routledge & Kegan Paul.
American Journal of Human Genetics, 70, 787–792. Grusec, J. E. (1997). A history of research on parenting strategies and
Griffin, T. M., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. P. (2004). Oral children’s internalization of values. In J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski
discourse in the preschool years and later literacy skills. First (Eds.), Parenting and children’s internalization of values: A hand-
Language, 24, 123–147. book of contemporary theory (pp. 3–22). New York: Wiley.
Griffiths, P. E. (2003). Basic emotions, complex emotions, Machiavellian Grusec, J. E. (2011). Socialization processes in the family: Social and
emotions. In A. Hatzimoysis (Ed.), Philosophy and the emotions emotional development. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 243–269.
(pp. 39–67). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Grusec, J. E., & Dix, T. (1986). The socialization of prosocial behav-
Griffiths, T. L., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2009). Theory-based causal ior: Theory and reality. In C. Zahn-Waxler, E. M. Cummings, &
induction. Psychological Review, 116, 661. R. Iannotti (Eds.), Altruism and aggression: Biological and social
Griffiths, T. L., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Kemp, C. (2012). Bayesian origins (pp. 220–246). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
inference. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Oxford Press.
handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 22–35). New York: Oxford Grusec, J. E., & Goodnow, J. J. (1994). Impact of parental discipline
University Press. methods on the child’s internalization of values: A reconceptualiza-
Grigorenko, E. L., Wood, F. B., Meyer, M. S., Hart, L. A., Speed, tion of current points of view. Developmental Psychology, 30, 4–19.
W. C., Shuster, A., & Pauls, D. L. (1997). Susceptibility loci for Grusec, J. E., Goodnow, J. J., & Kuczynski, L. (2000). New direc-
distinct components of developmental dyslexia on chromosomes tions in analyses of parenting contributions to children’s acquisi-
6 and 15. American Journal of Human Genetics, 60, 27–39. tion of values. Child Development, 71, 205–211.
Grimes, K., & Walker, E. F. (1994). Childhood emotional expres- Grusec, J., & Lytton, H. (1988). Social development: History, theory,
sions, educational attainment, and age at onset of illness in schizo- and research. New York: Springer-Verlag.
phrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 784–790. Grych, J. H., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Marital conflict and children’s
Grimshaw, J. (1981). Form, function, and the language acquisition adjustment: A cognitive-contextual framework. Psychological
device. In C. L. Baker & J. J. McCarthy (Eds.), The logical problem Bulletin, 108, 267–290.
of language acquisition (pp. 165–182). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Grych, J. H., & Fincham, F. D. (1992). Interventions for children
Groark, C. J., Muhamedrahimov, R. J., Palmov, O. I., Nikiforova, of divorce: Toward greater integration of research and action.
N. V., & McCall, R. B. (2005). Improvements in early care in Psychological Bulletin, 111, 434–454.
Russian orphanages and their relationship to observed behaviors. Guan, S. S., & Subrahmanyam, K. (2009). Youth Internet use: Risks
Infant Mental Health Journal, 26, 96–109. and opportunities. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 22, 351–356.
Grody, W. W., & Pyeritz, R. E. (1999). Report card on molecular Guasti, M. T. (2004). Language acquisition: The growth of grammar.
genetic testing: Room for improvement? Journal of the American Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Medical Association, 281, 845–847. Guilford, J. P. (1954). Psychometric methods (2nd ed.). New York:
Groesz, L. M., Levine, M. P., & Murnen, S. K. (2002). The effect McGraw-Hill.
of experimental presentation of thin media images on body sat- Guilford, J. P. (1962). Potentiality for creativity. Gifted Child
isfaction: A meta-analytic review. International Journal of Eating Quarterly, 6, 87–90.
Disorders, 31, 1–16. Gulek, C. (2003). Preparing for high-stakes testing. Theory into
Gropen, J., Pinker, S., Hollander, M., & Goldberg, R. (1991a). Practice, 42, 42–50.
Affectedness and direct objects: The role of lexical semantics in the Gullone, E. (2000). The development of normal fear: A century of
acquisition of verb argument structure. Cognition, 41, 153–195. research. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 429–445.
Gropen, J. Pinker, S., Hollander, M., & Goldberg, R. (1991b). Syntax Gunnar, M. (2000). Early adversity and the development of stress reac-
and semantics in the acquisition of locative verbs. Journal of Child tivity and regulation. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on
Language, 18, 115–151. Child Psychology: Vol. 31. The effects of adversity on neurobehavioral
Gross J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An inte- development (pp. 163–200). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
grative review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271–299. Gunnar, M. R. (2001). Effects of early deprivation: Findings from
Gross, A. B., & Keller, H. R. (1992). Long-term consequences of orphanage-reared infants and children. In C. A. Nelson &
childhood physical and psychological maltreatment. Aggressive M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive neurosci-
Behavior, 18, 171–185. ence (pp. 617–629). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
REFERENCES R-51
Gunnar, M., & Quevedo, K. (2007). The neurobiology of stress and from three countries. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological
development. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 145–173. Sciences and Social Sciences, 56, 141–151.
Gunnar, M., & Vazquez, D. M. (2006). Stress neurobiology and Hagen, J. W., & Hale, G. A. (1973). The development of attention in
developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen children. In A. D. Pick (Ed.), Minnesota symposia on child psychology
(Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 2. Developmental neu- (Vol. 7, pp. 117–139). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
roscience (2nd ed., pp. 533–577). New York: Wiley. Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intu-
Gunning, M., Halligan, S. L., & Murray, L. (2013). Contributions of itionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108,
maternal and infant factors to infant responding to the Still Face 814–834.
paradigm: A longitudinal study. Infant Behavior and Development, Haidt, J. (2003). The moral emotions. In R. J. Davidson, K. R.
36, 319–328. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences
Gunter, B. (2008). Media violence: Is there a case for causality? (pp. 852–870). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
American Behavioral Scientist, 51, 1061–1122. Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316,
Gupta, A. R., & State, M. W. (2007). Recent advances in the genetics 998–1002.
of autism. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 429–437. Haidt, J. (2008) Morality. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3,
Gur, R. C., Gunning-Dixon, F., Bilker, W. B., & Gur, R. E. (2002). 65–72.
Sex differences in temporo-limbic and frontal brain volumes of Haidt, J. (2013). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by
healthy adults. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 998–1003. politics and religion. New York: Vintage Books.
Gur, R. E., & Gur, R. C. (1990). Gender differences in regional cere- Haidt, J., & Bjorklund, F. (2008). Social intuitionists answer six ques-
bral blood flow. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 16, 247–254. tions about moral psychology. In W. Sinnott-Armstrong (Ed.),
Gussoni, E., Soneoka, Y., Strickland, C. D., Buzney, E. A., Khan, Moral psychology: Vol. 2. The cognitive science of morality: Intuition
M. K., Flint, A. F., Kunkel, L. M., & Mulligan, R. C. (1999). and diversity (pp. 181–218). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Dystrophin expression in the mdx mouse restored by stem cell Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: How innately pre-
transplantation. Nature, 401, 390–394. pared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus,
Gustafsson, J. (2001). Schooling and intelligence: Effects of track Fall, 55–66.
of study on level and profile of cognitive abilities. International Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2007). The moral mind: How five sets of
Education Journal, 2, 166–186. innate intuitions guide the development of many culture-specific
Gustafsson, S. (2005). Having kids later. Economic analyses for indus- virtues, and perhaps even modules. In P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, &
trialized countries. Review of Economics of the Household, 3, 5–16. S. Stich (Eds.) The innate mind: Vol. 3. Foundations and the future
Gutheil, G., Vera, A., & Keil, F. C. (1998). Do houseflies think? (pp. 367–391). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Patterns of induction and biological beliefs in development. Haidt, J., & Kesebir, S. (2010). Morality. In S. T. Fiske & D. T.
Cognition, 66, 33–49. Gilbert (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (5th ed., pp. 797–832).
Gwiazda, J., Bauer, J., & Held, R. (1989). From visual acuity to Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
hyperacuity: A 10-year update. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43, Haidt, J., Koller, S. H., & Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and
109–120. morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 65, 613–628.
Haan, N., Smith, M. B., & Block, J. H. (1968). Moral reasoning of Haidt, J., McCauley, C. R., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences
young adults: Political-social behavior, family background and in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust
personality correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 701–713.
10, 183–201. Hainline, L., & Riddell, P. M. (1996). Eye alignment and convergence
Haas, S., Schaefer, D. R., & Kornienko, O. (2010). Health and the in young infants. In F. Vital-Durand, J. Atkinson, & O. Braddick
structure of adolescent social networks. Journal of Health and (Eds), Infant vision. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Social Behavior, 51, 424–439. Haith, M. M., & Benson, J. B. (1998). Infant cognition. In W. Damon
Habgood, M. P. J., & Ainsworth, S. E. (2011). Motivating children (Series Ed.) & D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child
to learn effectively: Exploring the value of intrinsic integration in development: Vol. 2. Cognition, language, and perception (5th ed.,
educational games. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 20, 169–206. pp. 199–254). New York: Wiley.
Hack, M., Taylor, H. G., Drotar, D., Schluchter, M., Cartar, L., Wilson- Haith, M. M., Bergman, T., & Moore, M. J. (1977). Eye contact and
Costello, D., Klein, N., Friedman, H., Mercuri-Minich, N., & face scanning in early infancy. Science, 198, 853–855.
Morrow, M. (2005). Poor predictive validity of the Bayley Scales of Hakuta, K. (1986). Mirror of language: The debate on bilingualism.
Infant Development for cognitive function of extremely low birth New York: Basic Books.
weight children at school age. Pediatrics, 116, 333–341. Hakuta, K. (1987). Degree of bilingualism and cognitive abil-
Hada, K., Asahina, M., Hasegawa, H., Kanaho,Y., Slack, F. J., & ity in mainland Puerto Rican children. Child Development, 58,
Niwa, R. (2010). The nuclear receptor gene nhr-25 plays multiple 1372–1388.
roles in the Caenorhabditis elegans heterochronic gene network to Hakuta, K. (1990). Language and cognition in bilingual children. In
control the larva-to-adult transition. Developmental Biology, 344, A. Padilla, H. Fairchild, & C. Valadez (Eds.), Bilingual education:
1100–1109. Issues and strategies (pp. 47–59). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Haden, C. A., Haine, R. A., & Fivush, R. (1997). Developing nar- Hakuta, K., Bialystok, E., & Wiley, E. (2003). Critical evidence: A
rative structure in parent-child reminiscing across the preschool test of the critical-period hypothesis for second-language acquisi-
years. Developmental Psychology, 33, 295–307. tion. Psychological Science, 14, 31–38.
Haeckel, E. (1874). Anthropogenie: Keimes- und Stammes- Geschichte Halbreich, U. (2005). The association between pregnancy processes,
des Menschen. Leipzig: Engelmann. preterm delivery, low birth weight, and postpartum depressions—
Hagberg, B., Alfredson, B. B., Poon, L. W., & Homma, A. (2001). the need for interdisciplinary integration. American Journal of
Cognitive functioning in centenarians: A coordinated analysis Obstetrics and Gynecology, 193, 1312–1322.
R-52 REFERENCES
Hales, C. N., & Barker, D. J. (2001). The thrifty phenotype hypoth- Hamlin, J. K., Newman, G., & Wynn, K. (2009). Eight-month-old
esis. British Medical Bulletin, 60, 5–20. infants infer unfulfilled goals, despite ambiguous physical evi-
Halford, G. S. (1993). Children’s understanding: The development of dence. Infancy, 14, 579–590.
mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hamlin, J. K., & Wynn, K. (2011). Young infants prefer prosocial to
Halford, G. S. (1995). Learning processes in cognitive development: antisocial others. Cognitive Development, 26, 30–39.
A reassessment with some unexpected implications. Human Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by
Development, 38, 295–301. preverbal infants. Nature, 450, 557–559.
Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (1998). Processing Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Mahajan, N. (2011). How
capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for com- infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proceedings of the
parative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 108, 19931–19936.
Brain Sciences, 21, 803–864. Hampson, E. (2002). Sex differences in human brain and cognition:
Halim, M. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2010). Gender identity and stereotyp- The influence of sex steroids in early and adult life. In J. B. Becker,
ing in early and middle childhood. In J. Chrisler & D. McCreary S. M. Breedlove, D. Crews, & M. M. McCarthy (Eds.), Behavioral
(Eds.). Handbook of gender research in psychology (pp. 495–525). endocrinology (2nd ed., pp. 579–628). Cambridge, MA: MIT
New York: Springer. Press.
Hall, G. S. (1904). Adolescence: Its psychology and its relations to physiol- Hampson, E., & Moffat, S. D. (2004). The psychobiology of gender:
ogy, anthropology, sociology, sex, crime, religion, and education (Vols. Cognitive effects of reproductive hormones in the adult nervous
1–2). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. system. In A. H. Eagly, A. E. Beall, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The
Hall-Haro, C., Johnson, S. P., Price, T. A., Vance, J. A., & Kiorpes, L. psychology of gender (2nd ed., pp. 38–64). New York: Guilford Press.
(2008). Development of object concepts in macaque monkeys. Hampton, J. (2002). Language’s role in enabling abstract, logical
Developmental Psychobiology, 50, 278–287. thought. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 688.
Halliday, A. L., & Cepko, C. L. (1992). Generation and migration of Han, W. J. (2005). Maternal nonstandard work schedules and child
cells in the developing striatum. Neuron, 9, 15–26. cognitive outcomes. Child Development, 76, 137–154.
Hallmayer, J., Cleveland, S., Torres, A., Phillips, J., Cohen, B., Hankin, B. L., & Abramson, L. (2001). Development of gender
Torigoe, T., Miller, J., Fedele, A., Collins, J., Smith, K., differences in depression: An elaborated cognitive vulnerability-
Lotspeich, L., Croen, L. A., Ozonoff, S., Lajonchere, C., Grether, transactional stress theory. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 1–40.
J. K., & Risch, N. (2011). Genetic heritability and shared environ- Hankin, B. L., & Abramson, L. Y. (2002). Measuring cognitive
mental factors among twin pairs with autism. Archives of General vulnerability to depression in adolescence: Reliability, validity,
Psychiatry, 68, 1095–1102. and gender differences. Journal of Child and Adolescent Clinical
Hallowell, E. M., & Ratey, J. J. (1994). Driven to distraction: Psychology, 31, 491–504.
Recognizing and coping with attention deficit disorder from childhood Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L. Y., Moffitt, T. E., Silva, P. A., McGee, R., &
through adulthood. New York: Pantheon Books. Angell, K. E. (1998). Development of depression from preado-
Halpern, B. P. (1991). More than meets the tongue: Temporal char- lescence to young adulthood: Emerging gender differences in a
acteristics of taste intensity and quality. In H. T. Lawless & 10-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107,
B. P. Klein (Eds.), Sensory science: Theory and application in foods 128–140.
(pp. 37–105). New York: Marcel Dekker. Hanna, G. L., Veenstra-VanderWeele, J., Cox, N. J., Boehnke, M.,
Halpern, D. F. (2004). A cognitive-process taxonomy for sex dif- Himle, J. A., Curtis, G. C., Leventhal, B. L., & Cook, E. H. Jr.
ferences in cognitive abilities. Current Directions in Psychological (2002). Genome-wide linkage analysis of families with obsessive-
Science, 13, 135–139. compulsive disorder ascertained through pediatric probands.
Halpern, D. F. (2005). Psychology at the intersection of work and American Journal of Medical Genetics, 114, 541–552.
family: Recommendations for employers, working families, and Hannula-Jouppi, K., Kaminen-Ahola, N., Taipale, M., Eklund, R.,
policy makers. American Psychologist, 60, 367–409. Nopola-Hemmi, J., Kaariainen, H., & Kere, J. (2005). The axon
Halpern, D. F., & LaMay, M. L. (2000). The smarter sex: A criti- guidance receptor gene ROBO1 is a candidate gene for develop-
cal review of sex differences in intelligence. Educational Psychology mental dyslexia. PLoS Genetics, 1, e50.
Review, 12, 229–246. Hanscombe, K. B., Haworth, C. M., Davis, O. S., Jaffee, S. R., &
Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R., Hyde, J. S., & Plomin, R. (2011). Chaotic homes and school achievement: A twin
Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). The science of sex differences in science study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 1212–1220.
and mathematics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8, 1–51. Hantouche, E. G., & Akiskal, H. S. (2005). Bipolar II versus unipolar
Hamann, K.,Warneken, F., Greenberg, J. R., & Tomasello, M. (2011). depression: Psychopathologic differentiation by dimensional mea-
Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not in sures. Journal of Affective Disorders, 84, 127–132.
chimpanzees. Nature, 476, 328–331. Happe, F. G. (1995). Understanding minds and metaphors: Insights
Hambrick, D. Z., Salthouse, T. A., & Meinz, E. J. (1999). Predictors of from the study of figurative language in autism. Metaphor and
crossword puzzle proficiency and moderators of age-cognition rela- Symbolic Activity, 10, 275–295.
tions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 131–164. Hardy, S. A., & Carlo, G. (2005). Identity as a source of moral moti-
Hamers, J., & Blanc, M. H. A. (1989). Bilinguality and bilingualism. vation. Human Development, 48, 232–256.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Hardy, S. A., & Carlo, G. (2011). Moral identity: What is it, how
Hamilton, B. E., Martin. J. A., & Ventura, S. J. (2012). Births: does it develop, and is it linked to moral action? Child Development
Preliminary data for 2011. National Vital Statistics Reports, 61 (5). Perspectives, 5, 212–218.
Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics. Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs?
Hamilton, K., & Waller, G. (1993). Media influence on body size Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 439–444.
estimation in anorexia and bulimia: An experimental study. British Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., & Tomasello, M. (2002). The
Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 837–840. domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science, 298, 1634–1636.
REFERENCES R-53
Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised. Toronto, Harris, P. (1995). From simulation to folk psychology: The case for
Ontario: Multi-Health Systems. development. In M. Davies & T. Stone (Eds.), Folk psychology: The
Hare, R. D. (1993). Without conscience: The disturbing world of the theory of mind debate (pp. 207–231). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
psychopaths among us. New York: Simon & Schuster. Harris, P. (2002). What do children learn from testimony? In
Hariri, A. R., & Holmes, A. (2006). Genetics of emotional regulation: P. Carruthers, S. Stich, & M. Siegal (Eds.), The cognitive basis of sci-
The role of the serotonin transporter in neural function. Trends in ence (pp. 316–334). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Cognitive Sciences, 10, 182–191. Harris, P. L., & Corriveau, K. H. (2011).Young children’s selective
Hariri, A. R., Mattay, V. S., Tessitore A., Kolachana, B., Fera, F., trust in informants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of
Goldman, D., Egan, M. F., & Weinberger, D. R. (2002). Serotonin London, Series B, Biological Sciences, 366, 1179–1187.
transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amyg- Harris, P. L., & Kavanaugh R. D. (1993). Young children’s under-
dala. Science, 297, 400–403. standing of pretense. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Harkness, S., Edwards, C. P., & Super, C. (1981). Social roles and Development, 58, 1–107.
moral reasoning: A case study in a rural African community. Harris, P. L., & Koenig, M. A. (2006). Trust in testimony: How
Developmental Psychology, 17, 595–603. children learn about science and religion. Child Development, 77,
Harlaar, N., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2007). From learning to read 505–524.
to reading to learn: Substantial and stable genetic influence. Child Harrison, A. O., Wilson, M. N., Pine, C. J., Chan, S. Q., & Buriel,
Development, 78, 116–131. R. (1990). Family ecologies of ethnic minority children. Child
Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13, Development, 61, 347–362.
673–685. Harrison, R. V., Gordon, K. A., & Mount, R. J. (2005). Is there a
Harlow, H. F. (1969). Age-mate or peer affectional system. In D. S. critical period for cochlear implantation in congenitally deaf chil-
Lehrman, R. A. Hinde, & E. Shaw (Eds.), Advances in the study of dren? Analyses of hearing and speech perception performance after
behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 333–383). New York: Academic Press. implantation. Developmental Psychobiology, 46, 252–261.
Harlow, H. F., & Harlow, M. K. (1962). Social deprivation in mon- Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. 995). Meaningful differences in the everyday
keys. Scientific American, 207, 136–146. experiences of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Harlow, H. F., & Harlow, M. K. (1969). Effects of various mother- Hart, D., & Fegley, S. (1995). Prosocial behavior and caring in adoles-
infant relationships on rhesus monkey behaviors. In B. M. Foss cence: Relations to self-understanding and social judgment. Child
(Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (Vol. 4). London: Methuen. Development, 66, 1346–1359.
Harlow, H., & Suomi, S. (1971). Social recovery by isolation-reared Hart, D. A., Burock, D. A., London, B. E., & Miraglia, A. M.
monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the USA, (2003). Moral development in childhood. In M. H. Bornstein,
68, 1534–1538. L. Davidson, C. L. M. Keyes, K. A. Moore, & the Center for Child
Harlow, H. F., & Zimmerman, R. R. (1958). The development of Well-Being (Eds.), Well-being: Positive development across the life
affectional responses in infant monkeys. Proceedings of the American course (pp. 355–370). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Philosophical Society, 102, 501–509. Hart, H. L. A. (1968). Punishment and responsibility: Essays in the phi-
Harnad, S. (1987). Categorical perception: The groundwork of cognition. losophy of law. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.
New York: Cambridge University Press. Hart, S. L. (2010). The ontogenesis of jealousy in the first year of life:
Harold, G. T., & Conger, R. D. (1997). Marital conflict and adoles- A theory of jealousy and biologically-based dimensions of tempera-
cent distress: The role of adolescent awareness. Child Development, ment. In S. L. Hart & M. Legerstee (Eds.), Handbook of jealousy:
68, 333–350. Theory, research, and multidisciplinary approaches (pp. 57–82).
Harrelson, A. L., & Goodman, C. S. (1988). Growth cone guidance Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
in insects: Fasciclin II is a member of the immunoglobulin super- Hart, S. L., & Carrington, H. (2002). Jealousy in 6-month-old
family. Science, 242, 700–708. infants. Infancy, 3, 395–402.
Harrington, D. M. (1990). The ecology of human creativity: A psy- Hart, S. L., Carrington, H., Tronick, E. Z., & Carroll, S. (2004).
chological perspective. In M. A. Runco & R. S. Albert (Eds.), When infants lose exclusive maternal attention: Is it jealousy?
Theories of creativity (pp. 143–169). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Infancy, 6, 57–78.
Harris J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the Hart, S. L., Field, T., Del Valle, C., & Letourneau, M. (1998). Infants
way they do. New York: Free Press. protest their mothers’ attending to an infant-size doll. Social
Harris Poll. (2003). Barriers to the diagnosis and treatment of attention Development, 7, 54–61.
deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) among African American Hart, S. N., & Brassard, M. R. (1987). A major threat to children’s
and Hispanic children. Harris Interactive Health Care News, 3, 7. mental health: Psychological maltreatment. American Psychologist,
Harris, G. (2004). Experts kept from speaking at antidepressant hear- 42, 160–165.
ing. New York Times, April 16, p. A16. Harter, S. (1983). Developmental perspectives on the self-system. In
Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., & Quinsey, V. L. (1994). Psychopathy as E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4.
a taxon: Evidence that psychopaths are a discrete class. Journal of Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 275–385).
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 387–397. New York: Wiley.
Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child’s environment? A group social- Harter, S. (1993). Causes and consequences of low self-esteem in
ization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102, 458–489. children and adolescents. In R. Baumeister (Ed.), Self-esteem: The
Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the puzzle of low self-regard. New York: Plenum.
way they do. New York: Free Press. Harter, S. (1998). The development of self-representations. In
Harris, J. R. (2000). Context-specific learning, personality, and birth N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emo-
order. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 174–177. tional, and personality development (pp. 553–617). New York: Wiley.
Harris, J. R. (2006). No two alike: Human nature and human individu- Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspec-
ality. New York: W. W. Norton. tive. New York: Guilford Press.
R-54 REFERENCES
Harter, S., & Buddin, B. J. (1987). Children’s understanding of the Hatton, E., & Trautner, M. (2011). Equal opportunity objectifica-
simultaneity of 2 emotions: A 5-stage developmental acquisition tion? The sexualization of men and women on the cover of Rolling
sequence. Developmental Psychology, 23, 388–399. Stone. Sexuality & Culture, 15, 256–278.
Hartshorn, K., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1997). Infant learning and long- Haugen, E. (1956). Bilingualism in the Americas: A bibliography and
term memory at 6 months: A confirming analysis. Developmental research guide. Tuscaloosa, AL: University of Alabama Press
Psychobiology, 30, 71–85. Haugen, E. (1972). Bilingualism, language contact, and immigrant
Hartshorne, H., & May, M. A. (1928). Studies in the nature of charac- languages in the United States: A Research Report, 1956–1970.
ter: Vol. 1. Studies in deceit. New York: Macmillan. In T. A. Sebeok (Ed.), Current Trends in Linguistics (Vol. 10). The
Hartshorne, H., May, M. A., & Shuttleworth, F. K. (1930). Studies in Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton.
the nature of character: Vol. 3. Studies in the organization of charac- Haun, D. B., Rapold, C. J., Janzen, G., & Levinson, S. C. (2011).
ter. New York: Macmillan. Plasticity of human spatial cognition: Spatial language and cogni-
Hartup, W. W. (1996). The company they keep: Friendships and their tion covary across cultures. Cognition, 119, 70–80.
developmental significance. Child Development, 67, 1–13. Hauser, M. D. (1997). Artifactual kinds and functional design fea-
Hartup, W. W. (1999). Constraints on peer socialization: Let me tures: What a primate understands without language. Cognition,
count the ways. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 172–183. 64, 285–308.
Hartup, W. W., & Stevens, N. (1999). Friendships and adaptation Hauser, M. D. (2000). Homologies for numerical memory span?
across the life span. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 127–128.
76–79. Hauser, M. D. (2006). Moral minds: How nature designed our universal
Harvey, E. (1999). Short-term and long-term effects of early parental sense of right and wrong. New York: HarperCollins.
employment on children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Hauser, M. D., & Kralik, J. (1997). Life beyond the mirror: A reply to
Youth. Developmental Psychology, 35, 445–459. Anderson & Gallup. Animal Behavior, 54, 1568–1571.
Harwood, R. L., Miller, J. G., & Irizarry, N. L. (1995). Culture and Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of
attachment: Perception of the child in context. New York: Guilford language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science,
Press. 298, 1569–1579.
Hasher, L., Zacks, R. T., & May, C.P. (1999). Inhibitory control, cir- Hauser, M. D., Kralik, J., & Botto-Mahan, C. (1999). Problem solving
cadian arousal, and age. In D. Gopher & A. Koriat (Eds.), Attention and functional design features: Experiments on cotton-top tama-
and performance XVII: Cognitive regulation of performance: rins (Saguinus oedipus oedipus). Animal Behaviour, 57, 565–582.
Interaction of theory and application (pp. 653–675). Cambridge, Hauser, M. D., Kralik, J., Bott-Mahan, C., Garrett, M., & Oser, J.
MA: MIT Press. (1995). Self-recognition in primates: Phylogeny and the salience
Hashmi, S., Gallaway, M. S., Waller, D. K., Langlois, P. H., & Hecht, of species-typical traits. Proceedings of the National Academy of
J. T. (2010). Maternal fever during early pregnancy and the risk Sciences of the USA, 92, 10811–10814.
of oral clefts. Birth Defects Research Part A: Clinical and Molecular Hauser, M. D., MacNeilage, P., & Ware, M. (1996). Numerical rep-
Teratology, 88, 186–194. resentations in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Hassett, J. M., Siebert, E. R., & Wallen, K. (2008). Sex differences in Sciences of the USA, 93, 1514–1517.
rhesus monkey toy preferences parallel those of children. Hormones Hauser, M. D., Pearson, H., & Seelig, D. (2002). Ontogeny of tool
and Behavior, 54, 359–364. use in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): Innate recognition
Haste, H. E., & Baddeley, J. (1991). Moral theory and culture: The of functionally relevant features. Animal Behaviour, 64, 299–311.
case of gender. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz (Eds.), Handbook Hauser, M. D., & Spelke, E. S. (2004). Evolutionary and develop-
of moral behavior and development: Vol. 1. Theory (pp. 223–249). mental foundations of human knowledge: A case study of math-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ematics. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (Vol. 3,
Hastings, R., Qureshi, M., Verma, R., Lacy, P. S., & Williams, B. pp. 853–864). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
(2004). Telomere attrition and accumulation of senescent cells in Haussmann, M. F., Longenecker, A. S., Marchetto, N. M., Juliano,
cultured human endothelial cells. Cell Proliferation, 37, 317–324. S. A., & Bowden, R. M. (2012). Embryonic exposure to corti-
Hatano, G., & Inagaki, K. (1994). Young children’s naive theory of costerone modifies the juvenile stress response, oxidative stress,
biology. Cognition, 50, 171–188. and telomere length. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London:
Hatano, G., & Inagaki, K. (1999). A developmental perspective on Biological Sciences, 279, 1447–1456.
informal biology. In D. L. Medin & S. Atran (Eds.), Folkbiology Haviland, J. M., & Lelwica, M. (1987). The induced affect response:
(pp. 321–354). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 10-week-old infants’ responses to three emotion expressions.
Hatano, G., Miyake, Y., & Binks, M. G. (1977). Performance of Developmental Psychology, 23, 97–104.
expert abacus operators. Cognition, 5, 57–71. Hawi, Z., McCarron, M., Kirley, A., Daly, G., Fitzgerald, M., &
Hatano, G., & Osawa, K. (1983). Digit memory of grand experts in Gill, M. (2000). No association of the dopamine DRD4 recep-
abacus-derived mental calculation, Cognition, 15, 95–110. tor (DRD4) gene polymorphism in attention deficit hyperactiv-
Hatano, G., & Wertsch, J. V. (2001). Sociocultural approaches to cog- ity disorder (ADHD) in the Irish population. American Journal of
nitive development: The constitutions of culture in mind. Human Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 96, 268–272.
Development, 44, 77–83. Hawi, Z., Millar, N., Daly, G., Fitzgerald, M., & Gill, M. (2000).
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional conta- No association between catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT)
gion. New York: Cambridge University Press. gene polymorphism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
Hatten, M. E. (1990). Riding the glial monorail: A common mecha- (ADHD) in an Irish sample. American Journal of Medical Genetics
nism for glial-guided neuronal migration in different regions of the (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 96, 282–284.
developing mammalian brain. Trends in Neurosciences, 13, 179–184. Hawk, B., & McCall, R. B. (2010). CBCL behavior problems of post-
Hatten, M. E. (1999). Central nervous system neuronal migration. institutionalized international adoptees. Clinical Child and Family
Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 511–539. Psychology Review, 13, 199–211.
REFERENCES R-55
Hawkins, J., Pea, R. D., Glick, J., & Scribner, S. (1984). “Merds that Healey, M. K., & Hasher, L. (2009). Limitations to the deficit attenu-
laugh don’t like mushrooms”: Evidence for deductive reasoning by ation hypothesis: Aging and decision making. Journal of Consumer
preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 20, 584–594. Psychology, 19, 17–22.
Hawley, K. M., & Weisz, J. R. (2003). Child, parent and therapist Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life, and work in
(dis)agreement on target problems in outpatient therapy: The communities and classrooms. New York: Cambridge University
therapist’s dilemma and its implications. Journal of Consulting & Press.
Clinical Psychology, 71, 62–70. Heath, S. B. (1984). Linguistics and education. Annual Review of
Haworth, C. M. A., & Plomin, R. (2010). Quantitative genetics in Anthropology, 13, 251–274.
the era of molecular genetics: Learning abilities and disabilities as Heath, S. B. (1986). Taking a cross-cultural look at narratives. Topics
an example. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent of Language Disorders, 7, 84–94.
Psychiatry, 49, 783–793. Heath, S. B. (1989). Oral and literate traditions among black
Hay, D. F., Nash, A., & Pederson, J. (1981). Responses of six- Americans living in poverty. American Psychologist, 44, 367–373.
month-olds to the distress of their peers. Child Development, 52, Heatherton, T. F. (2011). Neuroscience of self and self-regulation.
1071–1075. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 363–390.
Hay, J. F., & Jacoby, L. L. (1999). Separating habit and recollection Heckman, J. (2006). Skill formation and the economics of investing
in young and older adults. Effects of elaborative processing and in disadvantaged children. Science, 312, 1900–1902.
distinctiveness. Psychology and Aging, 14, 122–134. Heffner, C. D., Lumsden, A. G. S., & O’Leary, D. D. M. (1990).
Hayes, D. (1993). Lexical difficulty of textbooks and declining Target control of collateral extension and directional axon growth
test performance. Paper presented at the Cornell Psychology in the mammalian brain. Science, 247, 217–220.
Department Data Analysis Discussion Group, Ithaca, NY. Heffner, L. J. (2004). Advanced maternal age: How old is too old?
Hayes, D., Gershman, E., & Bolin, L. (1980). Friends and enemies: New England Journal of Medicine, 351, 1927–1929.
Cognitive bases for preschool children’s unilateral and reciprocal Heider, E. R. (1972). Universals in color naming and memory. Journal
relationships. Child Development, 51, 1276–1279. of Experimental Psychology, 93, 10–20.
Hayes, D., Gershman, E., & Halteman, W. (1996). Enmity in males Heider, E. R., & Olivier, D. C. (1972). The structure of color space
at four developmental levels: Cognitive bases for disliking. Journal in naming and memory for two languages. Cognitive Psychology,
of Genetic Psychology, 157, 153–160. 3, 337–354.
Hayes, D. P., & Wolfer, L. T. (1994). Schoolbook simplification, stu- Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York:
dent knowledge and SAT-verbal achievement levels (Tech. Rep. No. Wiley.
94–4). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University, Department of Sociology. Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent
Hayes, D. P., Wolfer, L. T., & Wolfe, M. F. (1996). Schoolbook sim- behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–249.
plification and its relation to the decline in SAT-verbal scores. Heiming, R. S., & Sachser, N. (2010). Consequences of serotonin
American Educational Research Journal, 33, 489-508. transporter genotype and early adversity on behavioral profile—
Hayflick, L. (1965). The limited in vitro lifetime of human diploid pathology or adaptation? Frontiers in Neuroscience, 4, 187–197.
cell strains. Experimental Cell Research, 37, 614–636. Heine, S. H., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999).
Hayflick, L. (1998). How and why we age. Experimental Gerontology, Is there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological
33, 639–653. Review, 106, 766–794.
Hayhoe, M. (2004). Advances in relating eye movements and cogni- Held, R. (1965). Plasticity in sensory-motor systems. Scientific American,
tion. Infancy, 6, 267–274. 213, 84–94.
Hayne, H., Gross, J., Hildreth, K., & Rovee-Collier, C. (2000). Held, R., & Hein, A. (1963). Movement-produced stimulation in the
Repeated reminders increase the speed of memory retrieval by development of visually guided behavior. Journal of Comparative
3-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 3, 312–318. and Physiological Psychology, 56, 872–876.
Hayne, H., Herbert, J., & Simcock, G. (2003). Imitation from televi- Helgadottir, A., Manolescu, A., Helgason, A., Thorleifsson, G.,
sion by 24- and 30-month-olds. Developmental Science, 6, 254–261. Thorsteinsdottir, U., et al. (2006). A variant of the gene encoding
Hayslip, B., & Kaminski, P. L. (2005). Grandparents raising their leukotriene A4 hydrolase confers ethnicity-specific risk of myocar-
grandchildren: A review of the literature and suggestions for prac- dial infarction. Nature Genetics, 38, 68–74.
tice. Gerontologist, 45, 262–269. Helmholtz, H. von. (1866/1962). Treatise on physiological optics (J. P.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). Attachment as an organizational L. Southall, Trans.). New York: Dover. [Originally published as
framework for research on close relationships. Psychological Inquiry, Handbuch der physiologischen Optik. Leipzig: Voss, 1866.]
5, 1–22. Helmholtz, H. von. (1884/1968). The origin of the correct interpreta-
Hazelwood School District et al. v. Kuhlmeier et al., 484 U.S. 260 tion of our sensory impressions. Reprinted in R. M.Warren & R. P.
(1988). Warren, Helmholtz on perception: Its physiology and development.
Hazlett, T. (2004). Requiem for the V-Chip: A relic of the last battle New York: Wiley.
over indecency on TV. Slate, February 13. Helwig, C., Turiel, E., & Nucci, L. (1997). Character education after
He, S., Ma, J., Liu, N., & Yu, X. (2010). Early enriched environment the bandwagon has gone. In L. Nucci (Chair), Developmental
promotes neonatal GABAergic neurotransmission and accelerates perspectives and approaches to character education. Symposium
synapse maturation. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 7910–7916. conducted at the meeting of the American Educational Research
He, Z., Bolz, M., & Baillargeon, R. (2011). False-belief under- Association, Chicago, March 1997.
standing in 2.5-year-olds: Evidence from change-of-location and Helwig, C. C., & Prencipe, A. (1999). Children’s judgments of flags
unexpected-contents violation-of-expectation tasks. Developmental and flag-burning. Child Development, 70, 132–143.
Science, 14, 292–305. Helwig, C. C. & Turiel, E. (2002). Children’s social and moral rea-
He, Z., Bolz, M., & Baillargeon, R. (2012). 2.5-year-olds succeed at soning. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), Blackwell handbook
a verbal anticipatory-looking false-belief task. British Journal of of childhood social development (pp. 475–490). Malden, MA:
Developmental Psychology, 30, 14–29. Blackwell Publishers.
R-56 REFERENCES
Helwig, C. C., Zelazo, P. D., & Wilson, M. (2001). Children’s judg- Hess, E. H. (1956). Space perception in the chick. Scientific American,
ments of psychological harm in normal and noncanonical situa- 195, 71–80.
tions. Child Development, 72, 66–81. Hess, R. D., & Shipman, V. C. (1965). Early experience and the
Hemker, L., Granrud, C. E.,Yonas, A., & Kavšek, M. (2010). Infant socialization of cognitive modes in children. Child Development,
perception of surface texture and relative height as distance infor- 36, 869–888.
mation: A preferential-reaching study. Infancy, 15, 6–27. Hess, T. M., & Blanchard-Fields, F. (1999). Social cognition and aging.
Hemmati-Brivanlou, A., & Melton, D. (1997). Vertebrate embryonic San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
cells will become nerve cells unless told otherwise. Cell, 88, 13–17. Hess, U., & Blairy, S. (2001). Facial mimicry and emotional contagion to
Henderlong, J., & Lepper, M. R. (2002). The effects of praise on chil- dynamic emotional facial expressions and their influence on decod-
dren’s intrinsic motivation: A review and synthesis. Psychological ing accuracy. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 40, 129–141.
Bulletin, 128, 774–795. Hess, U., & Thibault, P. (2009). Darwin and emotion expression.
Henderson, H. A., & Wachs, T. D. (2007). Temperament theory and American Psychologist, 64, 120–128.
the study of cognition-emotion interactions across development. Hetherington, E. M. (1989). Coping with family transitions: Winners,
Developmental Review, 27, 396–427. losers, and survivors. Child Development, 60, 1–14.
Henderson, N. D. (1982). Human behavior genetics. Annual Review of Hetherington, E. M. (1991). Presidential address: Families, lies, and
Psychology, 33, 403–440. videotapes. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 1, 323–348.
Henderson, V. W., & Drachman, D. A. (2002). Dementia, butterfly Hetherington, E. M. (1999). Family functioning and the adjustment
ballots, and voter competence. Neurology, 58, 995–996. of adolescent siblings in diverse types of families. Monographs of the
Hendriks, P., & Spenader, J. (2006). When production precedes com- Society for Research in Child Development, 64, 1–25.
prehension: An optimization approach to the acquisition of pro- Hetherington, E. M., Bridges, M., & Insabella, G. M. (1998). What
nouns. Language Acquisition, 13, 319–348. matters? What does not? Five perspectives on the association
Henggeler, S. W., Schoenwald, S. K., Borduin, C. M., Rowland, between marital transitions and children’s adjustment. American
M. D., & Cunningham, P. B. (1998). Multisystemic treatment of Psychologist, 53, 167–184.
antisocial behavior in children and adolescents. New York: Guilford Hetherington, E. M., Cox, M., & Cox, R. (1985). Long-term effects
Press. of divorce and remarriage on the adjustment of children. Journal of
Henkel, L. A., Johnson, M. K., & De Leonardis, D. M. (1998). Aging the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 24, 518–530.
and source monitoring: Cognitive processes and neuropsychologi- Hetherington, E. M., Henderson, S. H., & Reiss, D. (1999). Adolescent
cal correlates. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 127, 251–268. siblings in stepfamilies: Family functioning and adolescent adjust-
Hennessey, B. A., & Amabile, T. M. (2010). Creativity. Annual Review ment. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
of Psychology, 61, 569–598. 64, 26–49.
Henry, J. D., Macleod, M. S., Phillips, L. H., & Crawford, J. R. Hetherington, E. M., & Jodl, K. M. (1994). Stepfamilies as settings
(2004). A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging. for child development. In A. Booth & J. Dunn (Eds.), Stepfamilies:
Psychology and Aging, 19, 27–39. Who benefits? Who does not? (pp. 55–79). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hensch, T. K. (2004). Critical period regulation. Annual Review of Hetherington, E. M., & Kelly, J. (2002). For better or for worse: Divorce
Neuroscience, 27, 549–579. reconsidered. New York: W. W. Norton.
Hepburn, E. R. (1996). Genetic testing and early diagnosis and inter- Hetherington, E. M., & Stanley-Hagan, M. (2002). Parenting in
vention: Boon or burden? Journal of Medical Ethics, 22, 105–110. divorced and remarried families. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.),
Herbert, J. H., Eckerman, C. O., & Stanton, M. E. (2003). The Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Being and becoming a parent (2nd
ontogeny of human learning in delay, long-delay, and trace eyeb- ed., pp. 287–315). New York: Erlbaum.
link conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 1196–1210. Hetland, L. (2000). Listening to music enhances spatial-temporal
Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1996). Modularity and development: The reasoning: Evidence for the “Mozart effect.” Journal of Aesthetic
case of spatial reorientation. Cognition, 61, 195–232. Education, 34, 105–148.
Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. S. (1994). A geometric process for spatial Hew, K. F., & Brush, T. (2011). Integrating technology into K-12
reorientation in young children. Nature, 370, 57–59. teaching and learning: Current knowledge gaps and recommen-
Hernstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve: Intelligence and dations for future research. Educational Technology Research and
class structure in American life. New York: Free Press. Development 55, 223–252.
Herrmann, P., Waxman, S. R., & Medin, D. L. (2010). Hewlett, B. S. (1991). Intimate fathers: The nature and context of Aka
Anthropocentrism is not the first step in children’s reasoning about pygmy paternal infant care. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan
the world. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the Press.
USA, 107, 9979–9984. Hewlett, B. S., & Macfarlan, S. J. (2010). Fathers’ roles in hunter-
Herrnstein, R. (1990). Levels of stimulus control: A functional gatherer and other small-scale cultures. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.),
approach. Cognition, 37, 133–166. The role of the father in child development (5th ed., pp. 413–434).
Hershenson, J. (1964). Visual discriminations in the human newborn. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 58, 270–276. Heyes, C. M. (1994). Reflections on self-recognition in primates.
Hertenstein, M. J., & Campos, J. J. (2004). The retention effects of an Animal Behavior, 47, 909–919.
adult’s emotional displays on infant behavior. Child Development, Heyes, C. M. (1998). Theory of mind in nonhuman primates.
75, 595–613. Behavioral and Brain Science, 21, 101–134.
Hertzog, C. (2011). Intelligence in adulthood. In R. J. Sternberg & Hiatt, S. W., Campos, J. J., & Emde, R. N. (1979). Facial pattern-
S. B. Kaufman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of intelligence ing and infant emotional expression: Happiness, surprise, and fear.
(pp. 174–190). New York: Cambridge University Press. Child Development, 50, 1020– 035.
Hespos, S. J., Ferry, A. L., & Rips, L. J. (2009). Five-month-old infants Hickling, A. K., Wellman, H. M., & Gottfried, G. M. (1997).
have different expectations for solids and liquids. Psychological Preschoolers’ understanding of others’ mental attitudes toward
Science, 20, 603–611. pretense. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 339–354.
REFERENCES R-57
Hiebert, J., Gallimore, R., Garnier, H., Givvin, K. B., Hollingsworth, H., Hines, M. (2010a). Gendered behavior across the life span. In R. M.
Jacobs, J., Chui, A. M. Y., Wearne, D., Smith, M., Kersting, N., Lerner, M. E. Lamb, & A. Freund (Eds.), The handbook of life-span
Manaster, A., Tseng, E., Etterbeek, W., Manaster, C., Gonzales, P., & development: Vol. 2. Social and emotional development (pp. 341–78).
Stigler, J. W. (2003). Teaching mathematics in seven countries: Results Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
from the TIMSS 1999 video study (NCES 2003-013). Washington, Hines, M. (2010b). Sex-related variation in human behavior and the
DC: U.S. Department of Education. brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 448–56.
Higgins, E. T., & Bryant, S. L. (1982). Concensus information and Hines, M. (2011). Gender development and the human brain. Annual
fundamental attribution error: The role of development and in- Review of Neuroscience, 34, 69–88.
group versus out-group knowledge. Journal of Personality and Hines, M. (2013). Gonadal hormone influences on human neu-
Social Psychology, 43, 889–900. robehavioral development: Outcomes and mechanisms. In D. W.
Higley, J. D., Hopkins, W. D., & Suomi, S. J. (1987). Infantile col- Pfaff & Y. Christen (Eds.), Multiple origins of sex differences in
oration as a releasing mechanism of maternal preference in rhesus brain: Neuroendocrine functions and their pathologies (pp. 59–69).
monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Developmental Psychobiology, 99, 7–13. Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.
Higley, J. D., & Linnoila, M. (1997). Low central nervous system Hines, M., & Alexander, G. M. (2008). Commentary: Monkeys, girls,
serotonergic activity is traitlike and correlates with impulsive boys and toys: A confirmation. Comment on “Sex differences in
behavior: A nonhuman primate model investigating genetic and toy preferences: Striking parallels between monkeys and humans.”
environmental influences on neurotransmission. Annals of the New Hormones and Behavior 54, 478–481.
York Academy of Sciences, 836, 39–56. Hines, M., Golombok, S., Rust, J., Johnston, K., Golding, J., & the
Higley, J. D., & Suomi, S. J. (1996). Effect of reactivity and social ALSPAC Study Team. (2002). Testosterone during pregnancy and
competence on individual responses to severe stress in chil- childhood gender role behavior: A longitudinal population study.
dren: Investigations using nonhuman primates. In C. R. Pfeffer Child Development, 73, 1678–1687.
(Ed.), Severe stress and mental disturbance in children (pp. 3–57). Hinshaw, S. P. (2009). Psychosocial interventions for ADHD
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. and comorbidities. In T. E. Brown (Ed.), ADHD comorbidities
Hihara, S., Notoya, T., Tanaka, M., Ichinose, S., Ojima, H., Obayashi, S., (pp. 385–398). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Fujii, N., & Iriki, A. (2006). Extension of corticocortical afferents Hinton, G. E., & Ghahramani, Z. (1997). Generative models for
into the anterior bank of the intraparietal sulcus by tool-use train- discovering sparse distributed representations. Philosophical
ing in adult monkeys. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2636–2646. Transactions of the Royal Society B, 352, 1177–1190.
Hildebrandt, K. A., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (1983). The infant’s physical Hirsch, H. V. B., & Spinelli, D. N. (1971). Modification of the dis-
attractiveness: Its effect on bonding and attachment. Infant Mental tribution of receptive field orientation in cats by selective visual
Health Journal, 4, 3–12. exposure during development. Experimental Brain Research, 13,
Hill, D. L., & Mistretta, C. M. (1990). Developmental neurobiology 509–527.
of salt taste sensation. Trends in Neuroscience, 13, 188–195. Hirschfield, L. A. (1996). Race in the making: Cognition, culture, and
Hill, J. (2003). Early identification of individuals at risk for antisocial the child’s construction of human kinds. Cambridge, MA: MIT
personality disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, s11–s14. Press.
Hill, J., Inder, T., Neil, J., Dierker, D., Harwell, J., & Van Essen, D. Hirschfeld, L. A., Bartmess, E., White, S., & Frith, U. (2007). Can
(2010). Similar patterns of cortical expansion during human devel- autistic children predict behaviour by social stereotypes? Current
opment and evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Biology, 17, R451–R452.
Sciences of the USA, 107, 13135–13140. Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (Eds.). (1996). The origins of
Hill, J. C., & Schoener, E. P. (1996). Age-dependent decline of atten- grammar: Evidence from early language comprehension. Cambridge,
tion deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, MA: MIT Press.
153, 1143–1146. Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Naigles, L. (1996). Young
Hill, J. P., & Lynch, M. E. (1983). The intensification of gender-related children’s use of syntactic frames to derive meaning. In K. Hirsh-
role expectations during early adolescence. In J. Brooks-Gunn & Pasek & R. M. Golinkoff (Eds.), The origins of grammar: Evidence
A. C. Petersen (Eds.), Girls at puberty: Biological and psychosocial from early language comprehension (pp. 123–158). Cambridge, MA:
perspectives (pp. 201–228). New York: Plenum. MIT Press.
Hilliard, A. G. (2000). Excellence in education versus high-stakes Hirsh-Pasek, K., Nelson, D. G. N., Jusczyk, P. W., Cassidy, K. W.,
standardized testing. Journal of Teacher Education, 51, 293–304. Druss, B., & Kennedy, L. (1987). Clauses are perceptual units for
Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, young infants. Cognition, 26, 269–286.
exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Hirsh-Pasek, K., Treiman, R., & Schneiderman, M. (1984). Brown
Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 58–65. and Hanlon revisited: Mothers’ sensitivity to ungrammatical
Hillman, N., Sherman, H. I., & Graham, C. F. (1972). The effect forms. Journal of Child Language, 11, 81–88.
of spatial arrangement on cell determination during mouse devel- Hirth, F., & Reichert, H. (1999). Conserved genetic programs in
opment. Journal of Embryology and Experimental Morphology, 28, insect and mammalian brain development. Bioessays, 21, 677–684.
263–278. Hirth, F., Therianos, S., Loop, T., Gehring, W. J., Reichert, H.., &
Hiltz, S. R., & Goldman, R. (2005). Learning together online: Research Furukubo-Tokunaga, K. (1995). Developmental defects in brain
on asynchronous learning networks. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. segmentation caused by mutations of the homeobox genes
Hinde, R. A. (1987). Individuals, relationships, and culture. Cambridge, orthodenticle and empty spiracles in Drosophila. Neuron, 15,
MA: Cambridge University Press. 769–778.
Hinde, R. A. (1997). Relationships: A dialectical perspective. Hove, UK: Ho, S., & Saigal, S. (2005). Current survival and early outcomes of
Psychology Press. infants of borderline viability. NeoReviews, 6, 123–132.
Hines, M. (2010). Sex-related variation in human behavior and the Hocevar, D. (1981). Measurement of creativity: Review and critique.
brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 448–456. Journal of Personality Assessment, 45, 450–464.
R-58 REFERENCES
Hochberg, J., & Brooks, V. (1962). Pictorial recognition as an Hogendoorn, S. M., Prins, P. J.., Vervoort, L., Wolters, L. H., Nauta,
unlearned ability: A study of one child’s performance. American M. H., Hartman, C. A., Moorlag, H., de Haan, E. , & Boer, F.
Journal of Psychology, 75, 624–628. (2012). Positive thinking in anxiety disordered children reconsid-
Hodges, E. V., & Card, N. A. (Eds.). (2003). Enemies and the darker ered. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26, 71–78.
side of peer relations: New directions for child and adolescent develop- Hoglund, W. L., & Leadbeater, B. J. (2007). Managing threat: Do
ment. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. social-cognitive errors and skills mediate the links between peer
Hodges, J., & Tizard, B. (1989). Social and family relationships of ex- victimization and adjustment problems in late childhood? Journal
institutional adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, of Research on Adolescence, 17, 525–540.
30, 77–98. Holland, P. W. H., & Hogan, B. L. M. (1988). Expression of homeobox
Hoefer, C., & Hardy, M. C. (1929). Later development of breast fed genes during mouse development: A review. Genes & Development,
and artificially fed infants: Comparison of physical and mental 2, 773–782.
growth. Journal of the American Medical Association, 92, 615–619. Holliday, R. E., Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2011). Developmental
Hoehl, S., Reid, V. M., Mooney, J., & Striano, T. (2008). What are reversals in false memory: Now you see them, now you don’t!
you looking at? Infants’ neural processing of an adult’s object- Developmental Psychology, 47, 442–449.
directed eye gaze. Developmental Science, 11, 10–16. Holliday, R. E., Reyna, V. F., & Hayes, B. K. (2002). Memory pro-
Hoehl, S., & Striano, T. (2010). Discrete emotions in infancy: cesses underlying misinformation effects in child witnesses.
Perception without production? Emotion Review, 2, 132–133. Developmental Review, 22, 37–77.
Hoek, H. W. (1995). The distribution of eating disorders. In K. D. Hollingworth, A., & Luck, S. J. (2008). An overview of visual
Brownell & C. G. Fairburn (Eds.), Eating disorders and obesity: A memory. In S. J. Luck & A. Hollingworth (Eds.), Visual memory
comprehensive handbook (pp. 207–211). New York: Guilford Press. (pp. 3–8). New York: Oxford University Press,
Hoek, H. W., & van Hoeken, D. (2003). Review of the prevalence Holloszy, J. O, Tseng, B. S., Marsh, D. R., Hamilton, M. T., & Booth,
and incidence of eating disorders. International Journal of Eating F. W. (1995). Strength and aerobic training attenuate muscle wast-
Disorders, 34, 383–396. ing and improve resistance to the development of disability with
Hoerandner, C. M., & Lemke, R. J. (2006). Can No Child Left Behind aging. Journals of Gerontology, Series A: Biological Sciences and
close the gaps in pass rates on standardized tests? Contemporary Medical Sciences, 50, 113–119.
Economic Policy, 24, 1–17. Holmes, K. J., & Wolff, P. (2012). Does categorical perception in
Hofer, B., & Pintrich, P. (1997). The development of epistemological the left hemisphere depend on language? Journal of Experimental
theories: Beliefs about knowledge and knowing and their relation Psychology, General, 141, 439- 443.
to learning. Review of Educational Research, 67, 88–140. Holowka, S., & Petitto, L. A. (2002). Left hemisphere cerebral spe-
Hofer, C., & Eisenberg, N. (2008). Emotion-related regulation: cialization for babies while babbling, Science, 297, 1515.
Biological and cultural bases. In M.Vandekerckhove, C. Von Holtgraves, T. M. (2002). Language as social action: Social psychology
Scheve, S. Ismer, S. Jung, & S. Kronast (Eds.), Regulating emotions: and language use. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Culture, social necessity, and biological inheritance (pp. 61–82). Holtz, P., & Appel, M. (2011). Internet use and video gaming predict
Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. problem behavior in early adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 34,
Hofer, M. (2006). Psychobiological roots of early attachment. Current 49–58.
Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 84–88. Holyoak, K. J., & Thagard, P. (1995). Mental leaps: Analogy in creative
Hoff, E. (2005). Language development (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wadsworth/Thomson Learning. Hommel, B., Li, K. Z., & Li, S. C. (2004). Visual search across the life
Hoffman, M. L. (1970). Moral development. In P. H. Mussen span. Developmental Psychology, 40, 545–558.
(Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (Vol. 2, 3rd ed., Honey, R. C., & Bateson, P. (1996). Stimulus comparison and percep-
pp. 261–300). New York: Wiley. tual learning: Further evidence and evaluation from an imprint-
Hoffman, M. L. (1977). Moral internalization: Current theory and ing procedure. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. B,
research. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy- Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 49, 259–269.
chology (Vol. 10, pp. 86–135). New York: Academic Press. Hong, F. (2004). Innocence lost: Child athletes in China. Sport in
Hoffman, M. L. (1982). Development of prosocial motivation: Society, 7, 338–354.
Empathy and guilt. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Development of prosocial Hood, B. M. (1995). Gravity rules for 2- to 4-year-olds? Cognitive
behavior (pp. 281–313). New York: Academic Press. Development, 10, 577–598.
Hoffman, M. L. (1990). Empathy and justice motivation. Motivation Hood, B. M., Wilson, A., & Dyson, S. (2006). The effect of divided
and Emotion, 14, 151–172. attention on inhibiting the gravity error. Developmental Science, 9,
Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications 303–308.
for caring and justice. New York: Cambridge University Press. Hooper, S. R., Wakely, M. B., de Kruif, R. E. L., & Swartz, C. W.
Hoffmann, H. (1845/1995). The story of Fidgety Philip. In Struwwell- (2006). Aptitude-treatment interactions revisited: Effect of
peter [Shockheaded Peter]. Mineola, NY: Dover Publications. metacognitive intervention on subtypes of written expression in
Hofmann, S. G., Asnaani, A., Vonk, I. J. J., Sawyer, A. T., & Fang, elementary school students. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29,
A. (2012). The efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy: A review 217–241.
of meta-analyses. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36, 427–440. Hopkins, S. L., & Lawson, M. J. (2002). Explaining the acquisition
Hogan, M. J., Kelly, C. A. M., Verrier, D., Newell, J., Hasher, L., & of a complex skill: Methodological and theoretical considerations
Robertson, I. H. (2009). Optimal time-of-day and consolida- uncovered in the study of simple addition and the moving-on pro-
tion of learning in younger and older adults. Experimental Aging cess. Educational Psychology Review, 14, 121–154.
Research, 28, 107–128. Horn, G. (1990). Neural bases of recognition memory investigated
Hogben, M. (1998). Factors moderating the effect of television aggres- through an analysis of imprinting. Philosophical Transactions of the
sion on viewer behavior. Communication Research, 25, 220–247. Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 329, 133–142.
REFERENCES R-59
Horn, G. (1998). Visual imprinting and the neural mechanisms of Hsu, L. K. (1996). Epidemiology of the eating disorders. Psychiatric
recognition memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 300–305. Clinics of North America, 19, 681–700.
Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imita- Hu, V. W. (2013). From genes to environment: Using integrative
tion/emulation switching in chimpanzees and children. Animal genomics to build a “systems-level” understanding of autism spec-
Cognition, 8, 164–181. trum disorders. Child Development, 84, 89–103.
Hornig, M., & Lipkin, W. I. (2001). Infectious and immune factors in Hu, W. (2011, January 5). Math that moves: Schools embrace the
the pathogenesis of neurodevelopmental disorders: Epidemiology, iPad. New York Times, p. A16.
hypotheses, and animal models. Mental Retardation and Huang, Y. T., Spelke, E. S., & Snedeker, J. (2010). When is four far
Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 7, 200–210. more than three? Children’s generalization of newly acquired num-
Hornik, R., & Gunnar, M. R. (1988). A descriptive analysis of infant ber words. Psychological Science, 21, 600–606.
social referencing. Child Development, 59, 626–634. Hubbard, J. A., Dodge, K. A., Cillessen, A. H. N., Coie, J. D., &
Hornik, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M. R. (1987). The effects of Schwartz, D. (2001). The dyadic nature of social information-
maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communications processing in boys’ reactive and proactive aggression. Journal of
on infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937–944. Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 268–280.
Horowitz, I. W., King, T. B., & Meyer, E. C. (1993). Legally man- Hubel, D. H. (1988). Eye, brain, and vision. New York: W. H. Freeman.
dated options available to children with ADHD within public edu- Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1963). Shape and arrangement of
cation. Clinical Pediatrics, 32, 702–704. columns in cat’s striate cortex. Journal of Physiology, 165, 559–568.
Horton, M. S., & Markman, E. M. (1980). Developmental differ- Hubel, D. H., Wiesel, T. N., & LeVay, S. (1977). Plasticity of ocu-
ences in the acquisition of basic and subordinate categories. Child lar dominance columns in monkey striate cortex. Philosophical
Development, 51, 708–719. Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological
Horwitz, A.V., Widom, C. S., McLaughlin, J., & White, H. R. Sciences, 278, 377–409.
(2001). The impact of childhood abuse and neglect on adult men- Hudson, D. L. Jr. (2004). The silencing of student voices: Preserving free
tal health: A prospective study. Journal of Health & Social Behavior, speech in American schools. Nashville, TN: First Amendment Center.
42, 184–201. Huesmann, L. R. (2007). The impact of electronic media violence:
Hoshi, Y. (2007). Functional near-infrared spectroscopy: Current sta- Scientific theory and research. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41,
tus and future prospects. Journal of Biomedical Optics, 12, 062106. S6–S13.
Houde, O., Rossi, S., Lubin, A., & Joliot, M. (2010). Mapping numer- Huesmann, L. R., Eron, L. D., Lefkowitz, M. M., & Walder,
ical processing, reading, and executive functions in the developing L. O. (1984). Stability of aggression over time and generations.
brain: An fMRI meta-analysis of 52 studies including 842 chil- Developmental Psychology, 20, 1120–1134.
dren. Developmental Science, 13, 876–885. Huesmann, L. R., Moise-Titus, J., Podolski, C., & Eron, L. (2003).
House, J. D. (2006). Mathematics beliefs and achievement of elemen- Longitudinal relations between children’s exposure to TV violence
tary school students in Japan and the United States: Results from and their aggressive and violent behavior in young adulthood:
the Third International Mathematics and Science Study. Journal of 1977–1992. Developmental Psychology, 39, 201–221.
Genetic Psychology, 167, 31–45. Huey, E. B. (1908/1968). The psychology and pedagogy of reading.
Houser, B. B. (1978). Cheating among elementary grade level stu- Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
dents: An examination. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 5, 2–5. Hughes, M. (1975). Egocentrism in pre-school children. Unpublished
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A. (1998). Innate tal- Ph.D. thesis, University of Edinburgh [cited in Donaldson, M.
ents: Reality or myth? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 399–442. (1985)].
Howe, M. L., & Courage, M. L. (1993). On resolving the enigma of Hughes, V. A., Frontera, W. R., Wood, M., Evans, W. J., Dallal, G. E.,
infantile amnesia. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 305–326. Roubenoff, R., & Fiatarone Singh, M. A. (2001). Longitudinal
Howe, M. L., Courage, M. L., & Rooksby, M. (2009). The genesis and muscle strength changes in older adults: Influence of muscle mass,
development of autobiographical memory. In M. L. Courage & physical activity, and health. Journals of Gerontology, Series A:
N. Cowan (Eds.), The development of memory in infancy and child- Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 56, 209–217.
hood (2nd ed., pp. 177–196). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Hull, C. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-
Howe, N., Rinaldi, C. M., Jennings, M., & Petrakos, H. (2002). “No! Crofts.
The lambs can stay out because they got cozies”: Constructive and Hull, D. (1965). The effect of essentialism on taxonomy: 2000 years
destructive sibling conflict, pretend play, and social understand- of stasis. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 15, 314–326;
ing. Child Development, 73, 1460–1473. 16, 1–18.
Howes, C. (1990). Can the age of entry into child care and the quality Hull, D., & Ruse, M. (Eds.). (1998). The philosophy of biology. Oxford,
of child care predict adjustment in kindergarten? Developmental UK: Oxford University Press.
Psychology, 26, 292–303. Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2013). Learning to read: What we
Howes, C. (1996). The earliest friendships. In W. M. Bukowski, know and what we need to understand better. Child Development
A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Perspectives, 7, 1–5.
Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 66–86). Cambridge, Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Schnack, H. G., Posthuma, D., Mandl, R. C.,
UK: Cambridge University Press. Baare, W. F., van Oel, C., van Haren, N. E., Collins, L., Evans,
Howes, C., & Phillipsen, L. (1992). Gender and friendship: A. C., Amunts, K., Burgel, U., Zilles, K., de Geus, E. J., Boomsma,
Relationships within peer groups of young children. Social D. I., & Kahn, R. S. (2006). Genetic contributions to human
Development, 1, 230–242. brain morphology and intelligence. Journal of Neuroscience, 26,
Hsia, Y., & Maclennan, K. (2009). Rise in psychotropic drug prescrib- 10235–10242.
ing in children and adolescents during 1992–2001: A population- Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J., & Dixon, R. A. (1999). Use
based study in the UK. European Journal of Epidemiology, 24, it or lose it: Engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in
211–216. aging? Psychology and Aging, 14, 245–263.
R-60 REFERENCES
Hume, D. (1739/2007). Treatise of human nature. New York: Oxford Hutchings, J., & Lane, E. (2005). Parenting and the development and
University Press. prevention of child mental health problems. Current Opinion in
Hume, D. (1739–1740/2000). A treatise of human nature (D. F. Psychiatry, 18, 386–391.
Norton & M. J. Norton, Eds.). New York: Oxford University Press. Hutchins, E. (1996). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Hume, D. (1748/1910). An enquiry concerning human understanding, Press.
and selections from a treatise of human nature. Harvard Classics Huttenlocher, P. R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex:
(Vol. 37). New York: PF Collier & Son. Developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163,
Hume, D. (1751/1966). An enquiry concerning the principles of morals. 195–205.
New York: Oxford University Press. Huttenlocher, P. R. (1990). Morphometric study of human cerebral
Hummer, R. A., & Hamilton, E. R. (2010). Race and ethnicity and cortex development. Neuropsychologia, 28, 517–527.
fragile families. Future of Children, 20, 113–132. Huttenlocher, P. R. (2002). Neural plasticity: The effects of the envi-
Hummert, M. L. (2007). As family members age: A research agenda ronment on the development of the cerebral cortex. Cambridge, MA:
for family communication. Journal of Family Communication, 7, Harvard University Press.
3–21. Huttenlocher, P. R., & Dabholkar, A. S. (1997). Regional differences
Hummert, M. L. (2011). Age stereotypes and aging. In K. W. in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. Journal of Comparative
Schaie & S. L.Willis (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging Neurology, 387, 167–178.
(7th ed., pp. 249–262). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Huttenlocher, P. R., & de Courten, C. (1987). The development of
Humphrey, L. L. (1989). Observed family interactions among sub- synapses in striate cortex of man. Human Neurobiology, 6, 1–9.
types of eating disorders using structural analysis of social behav- Huxley, A. (1932). Brave new world. London: Chatto & Windus.
ior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 206–214. Hvistendahl, M. (2010). Has China outgrown the one-child policy?
Hung L. F., Crawford, M. L. J., & Smith E. L. (1995). Spectacle Science, 329, 1458–461.
lenses alter eye growth and the refractive status of young monkeys. Hwang, J., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Behavior genetics studies of
Nature Medicine, 1, 761–765. infant temperament: Findings vary across parent-report instru-
Hunnius, S., & Bekkering, H. (2010). The early development of ments. Infant Behavior & Development, 26, 112–114.
object knowledge: A study of infants’ visual anticipations during Hyde, D. C., Boas, D. A., Blair, C., & Carey, S. (2010). Near-infrared
action observation. Developmental Psychology, 46, 446–454. spectroscopy shows right parietal specialization for number in pre-
Hunnius, S., de Wit, T. C. J., Vrins, S., & von Hofsten, C. (2011). verbal infants. Neuroimage, 53, 647–652.
Facing threat: Infants’ and adults’ visual scanning of faces with Hyde, D. C., & Spelke, E. S. (2011). Neural signatures of number
neutral, happy, sad, angry, and fearful emotional expressions. processing in human infants: Evidence for two core systems under-
Cognition & Emotion, 25, 193–205. lying numerical cognition. Developmental Science, 14, 360–371.
Hunnius, S., & Geuze, R. H. (2004). Developmental changes in Hyde, J. S. (2007). New directions in the study of gender similari-
visual scanning of dynamic faces and abstract stimuli in infants: A ties and differences. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 16,
longitudinal study. Infancy, 6, 231–255. 259–263
Hunt, E., & Carlson, J. (2007). Considerations relating to the study Hyde, J. S., Mezulis, A. H., & Abramson, L. Y. (2008). The ABCs of
of group differences in intelligence. Perspectives on Psychological depression: Integrating affective, biological, and cognitive models
Science 2, 194–213. to explain the emergence of the gender difference in depression.
Hunter, J. (2000). The death of character: Moral education in an age Psychological Review, 115, 291–313.
without good or evil. New York: Basic Books. Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans,
Hupbach, A., & Nadel, L. (2005). Reorientation in a rhombic envi- A. C., & Schlaug, G. (2009). Musical training shapes structural
ronment: No evidence for an encapsulated geometric module. brain development. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 3019–3025.
Cognitive Development, 20, 279–302. Hyman, I. E., Husband, T. H., & Billings, F. J. (1995). False memories
Husky, M. M., Mazure, C. M., Maciejewski, P. K., & Swendsen, J. D. of childhood experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, 181–197.
(2009). Past depression and gender interact to influence emotional Hyman, L. (1987). Epidemiology of eye diseases in the elderly. Eye,
reactivity to daily life stress. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 33, 1, 330–341.
264–271.
Huston, A. C., & Bentley, A. (2010). Human development in societal Iacovou, M. (2001). Family composition and children’s educational
context. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 411–437. outcomes. Institute for Social and Economic Research, Essex
Huston, A. C., Donnerstein, E., Fairchild, H., Fashbach, N. D., Katz, University, Working Paper 2001-12, 1–59. Colchester, UK:
P. A., Murray, J. P., Rubinstein, E. A., Wilcox, B. L., & Zuckerman, Institute for Social and Economic Research. https://www.iser
D. (1992). Big world, small screen: The role of television in American .essex.ac.uk/publications/working-papers/iser/2001-12.pdf
society. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Iams, J. D., Romero, R., Culhane, J. F., & Goldenberg, R. L.
Huston, A. C., & Rosenkrantz Aronson, S. (2005). Mothers’ time (2008). Primary, secondary, and tertiary interventions to reduce
with infant and time in employment as predictors of mother-child the morbidity and mortality of preterm birth. The Lancet, 371,
relationships and children’s early development. Child Development, 164–175.
76, 467–482. Ichise, M., Vines, D. C., Gura, T., Anderson, G. M., Suomi, S. J.,
Huston, A. C., & Wright, J. C. (1998). Television and the informa- Higley, J. D., & Innis, R. B. (2006). Effects of early life stress on
tional and educational needs of children. Annals of the American [11C]DASB positron emission tomography imaging of serotonin
Academy of Political and Social Science, 557, 9–23. transporters in adolescent peer- and mother-reared rhesus mon-
Hutchings, B., & Mednick. S. A. (1977). Criminality in adoptees keys. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 4638–4643.
and their adoptive and biological parents: A pilot study. In S. A. Iiams, T. M. (1976). Assessing the scholastic achievement and cog-
Mednick & K. O. Christiansen (Eds.), Biosocial bases of criminal nitive development of bilingual and monolingual children. In A.
behavior (pp. 127–142). New York: Gardner. Simões (Ed.), The bilingual child: Research and analysis of existing
educational themes (pp. 53–92). New York: Academic Press.
REFERENCES R-61
Imai, M., & Gentner, D. (1997). A crosslinguistic study of early word Iverson, J. M., & Fagan, M. K. (2004). Infant vocal-motor coordina-
meaning: Universal ontology and linguistic influence. Cognition, tion: Precursor to the gesture-speech system? Child Development,
62, 169–200. 75, 1053–1066.
Imitola, J., Park, K., Teng, Y. D., Nisim, S., Lachyankar, M., Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2001). The resilience of ges-
Ourednik, J., Mueller, F. J., Yiou, R., Atala, A., Sidman, R. L., ture in talk: Gesture in blind speakers and listeners. Developmental
Tuszynski, M., Khoury, S. J., & Snyder E. Y. (2004). Stem cells: Science, 4, 416–422.
Cross-talk and developmental programs. Philosophical Transactions Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way
of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 359, for language development. Psychological Science, 16, 367–371.
823–837. Ivkovich, D., Collins, K. L., Eckerman, C. O., Krasnegor, C. O., &
Imura, T., & Tomonaga, M. (2003). Perception of depth from shad- Stanton, M. E. (1999). Classical delay eyeblink conditioning in
ing in infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 6, 4- and 5-month-old human infants. Psychological Science, 10, 4–8.
253–258. Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the value of
Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (1987). Young children’s spontaneous per- choice: A cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. Journal of
sonification as analogy. Child Development, 58, 1013–1020. Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 349–366.
Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (2002). Young children’s naive thinking Izard, C. (1979). The maximally discriminative facial movement cod-
about the biological world. New York: Psychology Press. ing system. Newark, DE: University of Delaware, Office of
Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (2003). Conceptual and linguistic fac- Instructional Technology.
tors in inductive projection: How do young children recognize Izard, C. E. (1994). Innate and universal facial expressions: Evidence
commonalities between animals and plants? In D. Gentner & from developmental and cross-cultural research. Psychological
S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study Bulletin, 115, 288–299.
of language and thought (pp. 313–333). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion sche-
Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (2004). Vitalistic causality in young chil- mas, and a new paradigm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2,
dren’s naive biology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 356–362. 260–280.
Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (2006). Young children’s conceptions of Izard, C. E. (2009). Emotion theory and research: Highlights,
the biological world. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, answered questions, and emerging issues. Annual Review of
177–181. Psychology, 60, 1–25.
Inagaki, K., & Sugiyama, K. (1988). Attributing human character- Izard, C. E., Fantauzzo, C. A., Castle, J. M., Haynes, O. M.,
istics. Developmental changes in over- and underattribution. Rayias, M. F., & Putnam, P. H. (1995). The ontogeny and sig-
Cognitive Development, 3, 55–70. nificance of infants’ facial expressions in the first 9 months of life.
Ingram, D. (1989). First language acquisition: Method, description, and Developmental Psychology, 31, 997–1013.
explanation. New York: Cambridge University Press. Izard, C. E., & Malatesta, C. Z. (1987). Perspectives on emotional
Ingram, D. K., London, E. D., & Reynolds, M. A. (1982). Circadian development I: Differential emotions theory of early emotional
rhythmicity and sleep: Effects of aging in laboratory animals. development. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant develop-
Neurobiology of Aging, 3, 287–297. ment (2nd ed., pp. 494–554). New York: Wiley.
Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from Izard, V., Sann, C., Spelke, E. S., & Steri, A. (2009). Newborn infants
childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic Books. perceive abstract numbers. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1964). The early growth of logic in the child: Sciences of the USA, 106, 10382–10385.
Classification and seriation. New York: Norton.
Innocenti, G. M. (1981). Growth and reshaping of axons in the estab- Jablensky, A. V., & Kalaydjieva, L. V. (2003). Genetic epidemiology of
lishment of visual callosal connections. Science, 212, 824–827. schizophrenia: Phenotypes, risk factors, and reproductive behav-
Inzlicht, M., & Good, C. (2006). How environments threaten aca- ior. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 425–429.
demic performance, self-knowledge, and sense of belonging. In Jackendoff, R. (1993). On the role of conceptual structure in argument
S. Levin & C. van Laar (Eds.), Stigma and group inequality: Social selection: A reply to Edmonds. Natural Language and Linguistic
psychological approaches (pp.129–150). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Theory, 11, 279–312.
Ioannides, C., & Vosniadou, S. (2002). The changing meanings of Jackson, J. H. (1865/1958). Selected writings of John Hughlings Jackson
force. Cognitive Science Quarterly, 2, 5–62. (J. Taylor, Ed.). New York: Basic Books.
Isacson, O., Bjorklund, L. M., & Schumacher, J. M. (2003). Toward Jackson, K. F. (2009). Building cultural competence: A systematic
full restoration of synaptic and terminal function of the dopa- evaluation of the effectiveness of culturally sensitive interventions
minergic system in Parkinson’s disease by stem cells. Annals of with ethnic minority youth. Children & Youth Services Review, 31,
Neurology, 53, S135–S146. 1192–1198.
Isen, A. (1990). The influence of positive and negative affect on cog- Jackson, S. A., Schiesser, J., Stanley, E. G., & Elefanty, A. G. (2010).
nitive organization: Some implications for development. In N. L. Differentiating embryonic stem cells pass through “temporal
Stein, B. Leventhal, & T. Trabasso (Eds.), Psychological and biologi- windows” that mark responsiveness to exogenous and paracrine
cal approaches to emotion (pp. 75–94). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. mesendoderm inducing signals. PLoS ONE, 5, e10706.
Isles, A. R., & Wilkinson, L. S. (2008). Epigenetics: What is it and Jacob, F., & Monod, J. (1961). Genetic regulatory mechanisms in the
why is it important to mental disease? British Medical Bulletin, 85, synthesis of proteins. Journal of Molecular Biology, 3, 318–356.
35–45. Jacobs, A. (2008, May 27). One-child policy lifted for quake victims’
Issacharoff, S., Karlan, P. S., & Pildes, R. H. (2001). The law of democ- parents. New York Times.
racy: Legal structure of the political process (2nd ed.). New York: Jacobson, J. L., & Wille, D. E. (1986). The influence of attachment
Foundation Press. pattern on developmental changes in peer interaction from the
Itard, J. M. G. (1932/1962). The wild boy of Aveyron [Rapports et mem- toddler to the preschool period. Child Development, 57, 338–347.
oires sur le sauvage de l’Aveyron (G. Humphrey & M. Humphrey, Jacobson, M. (1991). Developmental neurobiology. New York: Plenum
Trans.)]. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Press.
R-62 REFERENCES
Jacobson, M. D., Weil, M., & Raff, M. C. (1997). Programmed cell Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (1996). Cognitive factors and fam-
death in animal development. Cell, 88, 347–354. ily structure associated with theory of mind development in young
Jacoby, L. L. (1999a). Deceiving the elderly: Effects of accessibility children. Developmental Psychology, 32, 70–78.
bias in cued-recall performance. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 16, Jensen, A. R. (1993). Why is reaction time correlated with psychomet-
417–436. ric g? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 53–56.
Jacoby, L. L. (1999b). Ironic effects of repetition: Measuring age- Jensen, A. R. (1997). The psychometrics of intelligence. In H. Nyborg
related differences in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: (Ed.), The scientific study of human nature: Tribute to Hans J.
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 3–22. Eysenck at eighty (pp. 221–239). New York: Elsevier.
Jacoby, L. L., & Hollingshead, A. (1990). Reading student essays Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability.
may be hazardous to your spelling: Effects of reading incorrectly Westport, CT: Praeger.
and correctly spelled words. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 44, Jensen, L. A. (2008). Through two lenses: A cultural-developmental
345–358. approach to moral psychology. Developmental Review, 28, 289–315.
Jacques, E. (1965). Death and the mid-life crisis. International Journal Jensen, L. A. (2011). The cultural development of three fundamen-
of Psychoanalysis, 46, 502–514. tal moral ethics: Autonomy, community, and divinity, Zygon, 46,
Jaenisch, R., & Bird, A. (2003). Epigenetic regulation of gene expres- 150–167.
sion: How the genome integrates intrinsic and environmental sig- Jensen, L. A., Arnett, J. J., Feldman, S. S., & Cauffman, E. (2002). It’s
nals. Nature Genetics, 33, 245–254. wrong, but everybody does it: Academic dishonesty among high
Jaffee, S., & Hyde, J. S. (2000). Gender differences in moral orienta- school and college students. Contemporary Educational Psychology,
tion: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 703–726. 27, 209–228.
James, D. K. (2010). Fetal learning: A critical review. Infant and Child Jeon, H., Moulson, M. C., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., & Nelson,
Development. 19, 45–54, C. A. (2010). The effects of early institutionalization on the dis-
James, W. (1890/1950). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover crimination of facial expressions of emotion in young children.
Publications. Infancy, 15, 209–221.
Jan, Y. N., & Jan, L. Y. (1995). Maggot’s hair and bug’s eye: Role Jerison, H. (1985). Brain size. In G. Adelman (Ed.), Encyclopedia of
of cell interactions and intrinsic factors in cell fate specification. neuroscience (pp. 168–170). Boston: Birkhauser.
Neuron, 14, 1–5. Jernigan, T. L., & Gamst, A. C. (2005). Changes in volume with
Jankovic, J. (2008). Parkinson’s disease: Clinical features and diag- age— consistency and interpretation of observed effects. Neurology
nosis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 79, of Aging, 26, 1271–1274.
368–376. Jernigan, T. L., & Tallal, P. (1990). Late childhood changes in brain
Janowsky, J. S., & Carper, R. (1996). Is there a neural basis for cog- morphology observable with MRI. Developmental Medicine and
nitive transitions in school-age children? In A. J. Sameroff & Child Neurology, 32, 379–385.
M. M. Haith (Eds.), The five to seven year shift: The age of reason Jernigan, T. L., Trauner, D. A., Hesselink, J. R., & Tallal, P. A. (1991).
and responsibility (pp. 33–60). Chicago: University of Chicago Maturation of the human cerebrum observed in vivo during ado-
Press. lescence. Brain, 114, 2037–2049.
Janssen, J., & Bodemer, D. (2013). Coordinated computer-supported Jessell, T. M., & Lumsden, A. (1997). Inductive signals and the
collaborative learning: Awareness and awareness tools. Educational assignment of cell fate in the spinal cord and hindbrain. In W. M.
Psychologist, 48, 40–55. Cowan, T. M. Jessell, & S. L. Zipursky (Eds.), Molecular and cel-
Jaswal, V. K., Croft, A. C., Setia, A. R., & Cole, C. A. (2010). Young lular approaches to neural development (pp. 290–333). New York:
children have a specific, highly robust bias to trust testimony. Oxford University Press.
Psychological Science, 21, 1541–1547. Jeste, D. V., Depp, C. A., & Vahia, I. V. (2010). Successful cognitive
Jean, A., & Stack, D. M. (2009). Functions of maternal touch and and emotional aging. World Psychiatry, 9, 78–84.
infants’ affect during face-to-face interactions: New directions for Jiang, S., Xin, R., Wu, X., Lin, S., Qian, Y., Ren, D., Tang, G., &
the still-face. Infant Behavior and Development, 32, 123–128. Wang, D. (2000). Association between attention deficit hyperac-
Jean, A., Stack, D. M., & Fogel, A. (2009). A longitudinal inves- tivity disorder and the DXS7 locus. American Journal of Medical
tigation of maternal touching across the first 6 months of life: Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 96, 289–292.
Age and context effects. Infant Behavior and Development, 32, Jiao, S., Ji, G., & Jing, Q., & Ching, C. C. (1986). Comparative study
344–349. of behavioural qualities of only children and sibling children.
Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain. Neural correlates of Child Development, 57, 357–361.
motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, Johansen-Berg, H. (2007). Structural plasticity: Rewiring the brain.
187–245. Current Biology, 17, 141–144.
Jeffcoat, M. K., Geurs, N. C., Reddy, M. S., Cliver, S. P., Goldenberg, Johansson, C. B., Momma, S., Clarke, D. L., Risling, M.,
R. L., & Hauth, J. C. (2001). Periodontal infection and preterm Lendahl, U., &Frisen, J. (1999). Identification of a neural stem cell
birth: Results of a prospective study. Journal of the American Dental in the adult mammalian central nervous system. Cell, 96, 25–34.
Association, 132, 875–880. Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a
Jencks, C. (1979). Who gets ahead? The determinants of economic success model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–211.
in America. New York: Basic Books. Johnson, E. K., & Tyler, M. D. (2010). Testing the limits of statis-
Jencks, C. (1992). Rethinking social policy: Race, poverty, and the under- tical learning for word segmentation. Developmental Science, 13,
class. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 339–345.
Jenike, M. A. (2004). Obsessive-compulsive disorder. New England Johnson, E. M., Jr., & Deckwerth, T. L. (1993). Molecular mech-
Journal of Medicine, 350, 259–265. anisms of developmental neuronal death. Annual Review of
Jenike, M.A., Baer, L., & Minichiello, W. E. (Eds.) (1998). Obsessive- Neuroscience, 16, 31–46.
compulsive disorders: Practical management (3rd ed.). St. Louis: Johnson, H. D. (2003). Grade and gender differences in the associa-
Mosby. tion between adolescents’ perceptions of boundary violations and
REFERENCES R-63
goal nominations during conflict with cross-sex friends. Child development: Internal working models of attachment in infancy.
Study Journal, 11, 19–38. Cognitive Science, 34, 807–825.
Johnson, J. E., Birren, S. J., & Anderson, D. J. (1990). Two rat homo- Johnson, S. C., Shimizu, Y. A., & Ok, S.-J. (2007). Actors and actions:
logues of Drosophila achaete-scute specifically expressed in neuro- The role of agent behavior in infants’ attribution of goals. Cognitive
nal precursors. Nature, 346, 858–861. Development, 22, 310–322.
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second Johnson, S. P. (2001). Visual development in human infants: Binding
language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisi- features, surfaces, and objects. Visual Cognition, 8, 565–578.
tion of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 60–99. Johnson, S. P. (2004). Development of perceptual completion in
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1991). Critical period effects on infancy. Psychological Science, 15, 769–775.
universal properties of language: The status of subjacency in the Johnson, S. P., & Shuwairib, S. M. (2009). Learning and memory facil-
acquisition of a second language. Cognition, 39, 215–258. itate predictive tracing in 4-month-olds. Journal of Experimental
Johnson, K. A., Robertson, I. H., Barry, E., Mulligan, A., Daibhis, A., Child Psychology, 102, 122–130.
Daly, M., Watchorn, A., Gill, M., & Bellgrove, M. A. (2008). Johnson, S., Slaughter, V., & Carey, S. (1998). Whose gaze will
Impaired conflict resolution and alerting in children with ADHD: infants follow? The elicitation of gaze-following in 12-month-olds.
Evidence from the Attention Network Task (ANT). Journal of Developmental Science, 1, 233–238.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 1339–1347. Johnson, S. P., Slemmer, J. A., & Amso, D. (2004). Where infants
Johnson, M. H. (1993). Constraints on cortical plasticity. In look determines how they see: Eye movements and object percep-
M. H. Johnson (Ed.), Brain development and cognition: A reader tion performance in 3-month-olds. Infancy, 6, 185–201.
(pp. 703–721). Oxford: Blackwell. Johnson, W. (2010). Understanding the genetics of intelligence: Can
Johnson, M. H. (2000a). Cortical specialization in infants: Beyond height help? Can corn oil? Current Directions in Psychological Science,
the maturational model. Brain and Cognition, 42, 124–127. 19, 177–182.
Johnson, M. H. (2000b). Functional brain development in infants: Johnson, W., & Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (2005). The structure of human
Elements of an interactive specialization framework. Child intelligence: It is verbal, perceptual, and image rotation (VPR), not
Development, 71, 75–81. fluid and crystallized. Intelligence, 33, 393–416.
Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & Morton, J. (1991). Johnson, W., Carothers, A. & Deary, I. J. (2008). Sex differences in
Newborns’ preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subse- variability in general intelligence: A new look at the old question.
quent decline. Cognition, 40, 1–19. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 518–531.
Johnson, M. H., & Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive develop- Johnson, W., Penke, L., & Spinath, F. M. (2011). Heritability in the
ment: The case of face recognition. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. era of molecular genetics: Some thoughts for understanding genetic
Johnson, M. H., Munakata, Y., & Gilmore, R. O. (2002). Brain devel- influences on behavioural traits. European Journal of Personality, 25,
opment and cognition: A reader. Oxford: Blackwell. 254–266.
Johnson, M. K. (2005). The relation between source memory and epi- Johnston, T. D., & Edwards, L. (2002). Genes, interactions, and the
sodic memory. Comment on Siedlecki, K. L., Salthouse, T. A., & development of behavior. Psychological Review, 109, 26–34.
Berish, D. E., Is there anything special about the aging of source Johr, M., Ho, A., Wagner, C. S., & Linder, T. (2008). Ear surgery in
memory? Psychology and Aging, 20, 529–531. infants under one year of age: Its risks and implications for cochlear
Johnson, M. K., & Foley, M. A. (1984). Differentiating fact from fan- implant surgery. Otology & Neurotology, 29, 310–313.
tasy: The reliability of children’s memory. Journal of Social Issues, Jones, A. C., & Gosling, S. D. (2005). Temperament and personality
40, 33–50. in dogs (Canis familiaris): A review and evaluation of past research.
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source mon- Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 95, 1–53.
itoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–8. Jones, A. P., Laurens, K. L., Herba, C., Barker, G., & Viding, E.
Johnson, M. K., & Mitchell, K. J. (2002). Source monitoring. In J. H. (2009). Amygdala hypoactivity to fearful faces in boys with con-
Byrne (Ed.), Macmillan psychology reference series: Vol. 2. Learning duct problems and callous-unemotional traits. American Journal of
and memory (2nd ed., pp. 628–631). New York: Macmillan. Psychiatry, 166, 95–102.
Johnson, N., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in sec- Jones, E. G. (1991). The development of the primate neocortex—An
ond language learning: The influence of maturational state on the overview. In S. A. Mednick, T. D. Cannon, C. E. Barr, & M.
acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, Lyon (Eds.), Fetal neural development and adult schizophrenia
21, 60–99. (pp. 40–65). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Johnson, R. L., & Tabin, C. J. (1997). Molecular models for vertebrate Jones, H. E., & Bayley, N. (1941). The Berkeley Growth Study. Child
limb development. Cell, 90, 979–990. Development, 12, 167–173.
Johnson, R. N. (1996). Bad news revisited: The portrayal of violence, Jones, S. M., & Zigler, E. (2002). The Mozart effect: Not learning
conflict, and suffering on television news. Peace and Conflict: from history. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 23,
Journal of Peace Psychology, 2, 201–216. 355–372.
Johnson, S. (2005, April 24). Watching TV makes you smarter. New Jones, W., Carr, K., & Klin, A. (2008). Absence of preferential look-
York Times Magazine. ing to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disabil-
Johnson, S. C. (2000). The recognition of mentalistic agents in ity in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Archives
infancy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 22–28. of General Psychiatry, 65, 946–954.
Johnson, S. C., Bolz, M., Carter, E., Mandsanger, J., Teichner, A., & Jones-Molfese, V. J. (1977). Responses of neonates to colored stimuli.
Zettler, P. (2008). Calculating the attentional orientation of an Child Development, 48, 1092–1095.
unfamiliar agent in infancy. Cognitive Development, 23, 24–37. Joo, M. (2010). Long-term effects of Head Start on academic and
Johnson, S. C., Dweck, C. S., Chen, F. S., Stern, H. L., Ok, S. J., & school outcomes of children in persistent poverty: Girls vs. boys.
Barth, M. E. (2010). At the intersection of social and cognitive Children and Youth Services Review, 32, 807–814.
R-64 REFERENCES
Jordan K. E., & Brannon, E. M. (2006). The multisensory representa- emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 177–237). New
tion of number in infancy. Proceedings of the National Academy of York: Wiley.
Sciences of the USA, 103, 3486–3489. Kagan, J. (1998). Three seductive ideas. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
Jordan-Young, R. M. (2011). Hormones, context, and “brain gender”: University Press.
A review of evidence from congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Social Kagan, J. (2000). Human morality is distinctive. Journal of
Science & Medicine, 74, 1738–1744. Consciousness Studies, 7, 46–48.
Joseph, J. (2000). Not in their genes: A critical view of the genetics Kagan, J. (2008). The biological contributions to temperaments and
of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Review, emotions. European Journal of Developmental Science, 2, 38–51.
20, 539–567. Kagan, J., Arcus, D., Snidman, N., Feng, W. Y., Hendler, J., &
Joseph, J. (2001). Is crime in the genes? A critical review of twin and Greene, S. (1994). Reactivity in infants: A cross-national compari-
adoption studies of criminality and antisocial behavior. Journal of son. Developmental Psychology, 30, 342–345.
Mind and Behavior, 22, 179–218. Kagan, J., & Kogan, N. (1970). Individual variations in cognitive pro-
Josephs, R. A., Markus, H. R., & Tafarodi, R. W. (1992). Gender cesses. In P. A. Mussen (Ed), Carmichael’s manual of child psychol-
and self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, ogy (Vol. 1, pp. 1273–1365). New York: Wiley.
391–402. Kagan, J., & Lamb, S., (Eds.) (1987). The emergence of morality in
Joshi, A. (1999). Computational linguistics. In R. A. Wilson & young children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
F. C. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Gibbons, J. (1989). Inhibited and unin-
(pp. 162–164). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. hibited types of children. Child Development, 60, 838–845.
Joshi, A., Pan, A., Murakami, M., & Narayanan, S. (2010). Role of Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (2004). The long shadow of temperament.
computers in educating young children: U.S. and Japanese teach- Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
ers’ perspectives. Computers in the Schools, 27, 5–19. Kagan, J., Snidman, N., Kahn, V., & Towsley, S. (2007). The preser-
Jouen, F., Lepecq, J. C., & Gapenne, O. (1993). Frames of reference vation of two infant temperaments into adolescence. Monographs of
underlying early movement coordination. In G. J. P. Savelsbergh the Society for Research in Child Development, 72 (Serial No. 287).
(Ed.), Advances in psychology: The development of coordination in Kagan, S., & Madsen, M. C. (1972). Experimental analyses of cooper-
infancy (Vol. 97, pp. 237–263). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science ation and competition of Anglo-American and Mexican children.
Publishers. Developmental Psychology, 6, 49–59.
Joyner, K., & Udry, J. R. (2000). You don’t bring me anything but Kail, R. (1990). The development of memory in children (3rd ed.). New
down: Adolescent romance and depression. Journal of Health and York: W. H. Freeman.
Social Behavior, 41, 369–391. Kail, R. V., McBride-Chang, C., Ferrer, E., Cho, J.-R., & Shu, H.
Joyner, M. H., & Kurtz-Costes, B. (1997). Metamemory develop- (2013). Cultural differences in the development of processing
ment. In N. Cowan (Ed.), The development of memory in childhood speed. Developmental Science, 3, 476–483.
(pp. 275–300). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Kajiura H., Cowart B. J., & Beauchamp G. K. (1992). Early devel-
Judge, S. J. (1990). Does the eye grow into focus? Nature, 345, opmental changes in bitter taste responses in human infants.
477–478. Developmental Psychobiology, 25, 375–386.
Jureidini, J. N., Doecke, C. J., Mansfield, P. R., Haby, M. M., Menkes, Kako, E. (1999). Elements of syntax in the systems of three language-
D. B., & Tonkin, A. L. (2004). Efficacy and safety of antidepressants trained animals. Animal Learning and Behavior, 27, 1–14.
for children and adolescents. British Medical Journal, 328, 879–883. Kaler, S. R., & Freeman, B. J. (1994). Analysis of environmental depri-
Jusczyk, P. W., Pisoni, D. B., Walley, A., & Murray, J. (1980). vation: Cognitive and social development in Romanian orphans.
Discrimination of relative onset time of two-component tones by Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 35,
infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 67, 262–270. 769–781.
Jusczyk, P. W., Pisoni, D. P., Reed, M. A., Fernald, A., & Myers, M. Kalil, A., & Ziol-Guest, K. M. (2008). Parental employment circum-
(1983). Infants’ discrimination of the duration of a rapid spectrum stances and children’s academic progress. Social Science Research,
change in nonspeech signals. Science, 222, 175–177. 37, 500–515.
Jutapakdeegul, N., Casalotti, S. O., Govitrapong, P., & Kotchabhakdi, Kalish, C. W. (1997). Preschoolers’ understanding of mental and
N. (2003). Postnatal touch stimulation acutely alters corticosterone bodily reactions to contamination: What you don’t know can hurt
levels and glucocorticoid receptor gene expression in the neonatal you, but cannot sadden you. Developmental Psychology, 33, 79–91.
rat. Developmental Neuroscience, 25, 26–33. Kalmijn, M. (2013). Long-term effects of divorce on parent–child
Juvonen, J., & Graham, S. (2001). Peer harassment in school: The plight relationships: Within-family comparisons of fathers and mothers.
of the vulnerable and victimized. New York: Guilford Press. European Sociological Review. Epub ahead of print.
Kalthoff, K. (1996). Analysis of biological development. New York:
Kaeberlein, M., & Kennedy, B. K. (2009). Ageing: A midlife longev- McGraw-Hill.
ity drug? Nature, 460, 331–332. Kaltiala-Heino, R., Kosunen, E., & Rimpela, M. (2003). Pubertal
Kagan, J. (1984). The nature of the child. New York: Basic Books. timing, sexual behavior and self-reported depression in middle
Kagan, J. (1989a). Temperamental contributions to social behavior. adolescence. Journal of Adolescence 26, 531–545.
American Psychologist, 44, 668–674. Kalyuga, S. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for
Kagan, J. (1989b). Unstable ideas: Temperament, cognition, and self. learner-tailored instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19,
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 509–539.
Kagan, J. (1994). Galen’s prophecy: Temperament in human nature. Kamawar, D., LeFevre, J., Bisanz, J., Fast, L., Skwarchuk, S., Smith-
New York: Basic Books. Chant, B., & Penner-Wilger, M. (2010). Knowledge of count-
Kagan, J. (1998). Biology and the child. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & ing principles: How relevant is order irrelevance? Journal of
N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 138–145.
REFERENCES R-65
Kamil, A. C., & Balda, R. P. (1990). Spatial memory in seed-caching Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1993). Self-organization and cognitive change.
corvids. In G. Bower (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation: In M. H. Johnson (Ed.), Brain development and cognition: A reader
Advances in research and theory (Vol. 26, pp. 1–25). San Diego, CA: (pp. 592–618). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Academic Press. Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., Berthoud, I., Davies, M., Howlin, P., &
Kamil, A. C., & Jones, J. E. (1997). Clark’s nutcrackers learn geomet- Udwin, O. (1997). Language and Williams syndrome: How intact
ric relationships among landmarks. Nature, 390, 276–279. is “intact”? Child Development, 68, 246–262.
Kamin, L. (1974). The science and politics of I.Q. Hillsdale, NJ: Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klima, E., Bellugi, U., Grant, J., & Baron-
Erlbaum. Cohen, S. (1995). Is there a social module? Language, face-
Kandel, E. R., & O’Dell, T. J. (1992). Are adult learning mechanisms processing, and theory of mind in individuals with Williams
also used for development? Science, 258, 243–245. syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 196–208.
Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessell, T. M. (1991). Principles of Karniol, R. (1978). Children’s use of intention cues in evaluating
neuroscience (3rd ed.). New York: Elsevier. behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 76–85.
Kang, C. (2010). Family size and educational investments in chil- Karno, M., Golding, J. M., Sorenson, S. B., & Burnam, M. A.
dren: Evidence from private tutoring expenditures in South Korea. (1988). The epidemiology of obsessive- compulsive disorder
Oxford Bulletin of Economics and Statistics, 73, 59–78. in five US communities. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45,
Kang, M. J., & Glassman, M. (2010). Moral action as social capital, 1094–1099.
moral thought as cultural capital. Journal of Moral Education 39, Kärtner, J., Keller, H., & Yovsi, R. D. (2010). Mother-infant interac-
21–36. tion during the first 3 months: The emergence of culture-specific
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous contingency patterns. Child Development, 81, 540–554.
Child, 2, 217–250. Kashani, J. H., & Orvaschel, H. (1990). A community study of anxi-
Kanngiesser, P., & Warneken, F. (2012). Young children consider ety in children and adolescents. America Journal of Psychiatry, 147,
merit when sharing resources with others. PloS One, 7, e43979. 313–318.
Kaplan, M. S., & Hinds, J. W. (1977). Neurogenesis in the adult rat: Kasof, J. (1999). Attribution and creativity. In M. A. Runco &
Electron microscopic analysis of light radioautographs. Science, S. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity (Vol. 1, pp. 147–156).
197, 1092–1094. San Diego: Academic Press.
Kappen, C., & Ruddle, F. H. (1993). Evolution of a regulatory Kates, W. R. (2010). Investigating the cognitive antecedents of schizo-
gene family: HOM/HOX genes. Current Opinion in Genetics and phrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 122–124.
Development, 3, 931–938. Katz, J. J. (1972). Semantic theory. New York: Harper & Row.
Kappen, C., Schughart, K., & Ruddle, F. H. (1993). Early evolution- Katz, L. C., & Shatz, C. J. (1996). Synaptic activity and the construc-
ary origin of major homeodomain sequence classes. Genomics, 18, tion of cortical circuits. Science, 274, 1133–1138.
54–70. Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1993) Patterns of marital conflict
Karasik, D., Demissie, S., Cupples, L. A., & Kiel, D. P. (2005). predict children’s internalizing and externalizing behaviors.
Disentangling the genetic determinants of human aging: Biological Developmental Psychology, 29, 940–950.
age as an alternative to the use of survival measures. Journals of Kaufman, J. C. (2009). Creativity 101. New York: Springer.
Gerontology, Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 60, Kaufman, J., & Needham, A. (2011). Spatial expectations of young
574–587. human infants following passive movement. Developmental
Karasik, L. B., Adolph, K. E., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Zuckerman, Psychology, 53, 23–36.
A. L. (2012). Carry on: Spontaneous object carrying in 13-month-old Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Reis, D. L., & Gray, J. R. (2011).
crawling and walking infants. Developmental Psychology, 48, General intelligence predicts reasoning ability even for evolution-
389–397. arily familiar content. Intelligence, 39, 311–322.
Karavasilis, L., Doyle, A. B., & Markiewicz, D. (2003). Associations Kausler, D. H. (1994). Learning and memory in normal aging. San
between parenting style and attachment to mother in middle Diego, CA: Academic Press.
childhood and adolescence. International Journal of Behavioral Kavšek, M., Granrud, C. E., & Yonas, A. (2009). Infants’ respon-
Development, 27, 153–164. siveness to pictorial depth cues in preferential-reaching studies: A
Karlawish, J. H., Bonnie, R. J., Appelbaum, P. S., Lyketsos, C., meta-analysis. Infant Behavior & Development, 32, 245–253.
James, B., Knopman, D., Patusky, C., Kane, R. A., & Karlan, P. S. Kay, P., & Regier, T. (2006). Language, thought, and color: Recent
(2004). Addressing the ethical, legal, and social issues raised by developments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 51–54.
voting by persons with dementia. Journal of the American Medical Kayano, M., Yoshiuchi, K., Al-Adawi, S., Viernes, N., Dorvlo, A. S.,
Association, 292, 1345–1350. Kumano, H., Kuboki, T., & Akabayashi, A. (2008). Eating atti-
Karlsgodt, K. H., Ellman, L. M., Sun, D., Mittal, V., & Cannon, T. D. tudes and body dissatisfaction in adolescents: Cross-cultural study.
(2011). The neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia. In Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 62, 17–25.
A. S. David, S. Kapur, & P. McGuffin (Eds.), Schizophrenia: The Kaye, J. A., del Mar Melero-Montes, M., & Jick, H. (2001). Mumps,
final frontier—Festschrift for R. M. Murray. Hove, UK: Psychology measles and rubella vaccine and the incidence of autism recorded
Press. by general practitioners: A time trend analysis. British Medical
Karlsson, H., Bachmann, S., Schröder, J., McArthur, J., Torrey, Journal, 322, 460–463.
E. F., & Yolken, R. H. (2001). Retroviral RNA identified in the Kaye, W. H., Devlin, B., Barbarich, N., Bulik, C. M., Thornton, L.,
cerebrospinal fluids and brains of individuals with schizophrenia. Bacanu, S. A., Fichter, M. M., Halmi, K. A., Kaplan, A. S., Strober, M.,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 98, Woodside, D. B., Bergen, A.W., Crow, S., Mitchell, J., Rotondo, A.,
4634–4639. Mauri, M., Cassano, G., Keel, P., Plotnicov, K., Pollice, C., Klump,
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental per- K. L., Lilenfeld, L. R., Ganjei, J. K., Quadflieg, N., & Berrettini,
spective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. W. H. (2004). Genetic analysis of bulimia nervosa: Methods and
R-66 REFERENCES
sample description. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35, Keil, F. C. (1986). Conceptual domains and the acquisition of meta-
556–570. phor. Cognitive Development, 1, 73–96.
Kazdin, A. E. (1995). Conduct disorder in childhood and adolescence Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development.
(2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kazdin, A. E. (1997). Practitioner review: Psychosocial treatments Keil, F. C. (1992). The emergence of an autonomous biology. In M. R.
for conduct disorder in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Gunnar & M. Maratsos (Eds.), Modularity and constraints in lan-
Psychiatry, 38, 161–178. guage and cognition (pp. 103–137). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kazdin, A. E. (2002). Psychosocial treatments for conduct disorder in Keil, F. C. (1994). Explanation, association, and the acquisition of
children and adolescents. In P. E. Nathan & J. M. Gorman (Eds.), word meaning. Lingua, 92, 169–196.
A guide to treatments that work (2nd ed., pp. 57–85). London: Keil, F. C. (1995). The growth of causal understandings of natural
Oxford University Press. kinds. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Causal
Kazdin, A. E. (2011). Evidence-based treatment research: Advances, cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 234–262). New York:
limitations, and next steps. American Psychologist, 66, 685–698. Oxford University Press.
Kazdin, A. E., & Benjet, C. (2003). Spanking children: Evidence and Keil, F. C. (1998). Words, moms, and things: Language as a road map to
issues. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 99–103. reality. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
Kazdin, A. E., & Weisz, J. R. (1998). Identifying and developing 63, 149–157.
empirically supported child and adolescent treatments. Journal of Keil, F. C. (2011). Science starts early. Science, 331, 1022–1023.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 19–36. Keil, F. C. (2012). Running on empty? How folk science gets by with
Kazdin, A. E., & Weisz, J. R. (2003). Context and background of less. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 329–334.
evidence-based psychotherapies for children and adolescents. In Keil, F. C., & Batterman, N. (1984). A characteristic-to-defining shift
A. E. Kazdin & J. R. Weisz (Eds.), Evidence-based psychotherapies in the development of word meaning. Journal of Verbal Learning
for children and adolescents (pp. 3–20). New York: Guilford Press. and Verbal Behavior, 23, 221–236.
Keasey, C. B. (1978). Children’s developing awareness and usage of Keil, F. C., Levin, D. T., Richman, B. A., & Gutheil, G. (1999).
intentionality and motive. In H. E. Howe, Jr, & C. B. Keasey (Eds.), Mechanism and explanation in the development of biological
Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1977: Vol. 25. Social cognitive thought: The case of disease. In D. L. Medin & S. Atran (Eds.),
development (pp. 219–260). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Folkbiology (pp. 285–319). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Keating, M. B., McKenzie, B. E., & Day, R. H. (1986). Spatial local- Keil, F. C., Smith, C. S., Simons, D., & Levin, D. (1998). Two dog-
ization in infancy: Position constancy in a square and circular room mas of conceptual empiricism. Cognition, 65, 103–135.
with and without a landmark. Child Development, 57, 115–124. Keil, F. C., Stein, C., Webb, L., Billings, V. D., & Rozenblit, L.
Keel, P. K., Brown, T. A., Holland, L. A., & Bodell, L. P. (2012). (2008). Discerning the division of cognitive labor: An emerging
Empirical classification of eating disorders. Annual Review of understanding of how knowledge is clustered in other minds.
Clinical Psychology, 8, 381–404. Cognitive Science, 32, 259–300.
Keel, P. K., Heatherton, T. F., Dorer, D. J., Joiner, T. E., & Zalta, A. Keil, F. C., Stein, C., Webb, L., Billings, V. D., & Rozenblit, L.
(2006). Point prevalence of bulimia nervosa in 1982, 1992, and (2008). Discerning the division of cognitive labor: An emerging
2002. Psychological Medicine, 36, 119–128. understanding of how knowledge is clustered in other minds.
Keel, P. K., & Klump, K. L. (2003). Are eating disorders culture- Cognitive Science, 32, 259–300.
bound syndromes? Implications for conceptualizing their etiology. Keith, T. Z., & Witta, E. L. (1997). Hierarchical and cross-age con-
Psychological Bulletin, 129, 747–769. firmatory factor analysis of the WISC-III: What does it measure?
Kegl, J. (1994). The Nicaraguan sign language project: An overview. Social Psychology Quarterly, 12, 89–107.
Signpost, 7, 32–39. Keizer, A., Smeets, M. A., Dijkerman, H. C., van Elburg, A., &
Kegl, J. (2002). Language emergence in a language-ready brain: Postma, A. (2012). Aberrant somatosensory perception in anorexia
Acquisition. In G. Morgan & B. Woll (Eds.), Directions in sign nervosa. Psychiatry Research, 200, 530–537.
language acquisition (pp. 207–254). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Kelemen, D. (1999). Function, goals, and intention: Children’s teleologi-
Kegl, J. (2008). The case of signed languages in the context of pidgin cal reasoning about objects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 461–468.
and creole studies. In S. Kouwenberg & J. V. Singler (Eds.), The Kell, H. J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2013). Who rises to the
handbook of pidgin and creole studies (pp. 489–511). Oxford, UK: top? Early indicators. Psychological Science, 24, 648–659.
Wiley-Blackwell. Keller, G. (2005). Embryonic stem cell differentiation: Emergence of
Kegl, J., Senghas, A., & Coppola, M. (1999). Creation through a new era in biology and medicine. Genes and Development, 19,
contact: Sign language emergence and sign language change in 1129–1155.
Nicaragua. In M. DeGraff (Ed.), Language creation and language Keller, H., & Otto, H. (2009). The cultural socialization of emotion
change: Creolization, diachrony, and development (pp. 179–237). regulation during infancy. Journal of Cross- Cultural Psychology, 40,
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 996–2011.
Keil, F. (1979). The development of the young child’s ability to antici- Keller, M. B., McCullough, J. P., Klein, D. N., Arnow, B., Dunner,
pate the outcomes of simple causal events. Child Development, 50, D. L., Gelenberg, A. J., Markowitz, J. C., Nemeroff, C. B., Russell,
455–462. J. M., Thase, M. E., Trivedi, M. H., & Zajecka, J. (2000). A com-
Keil, F. C. (1979). Semantic and conceptual development: An ontological parison of nefazodone, the cognitive behavioral-analysis system of
perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. psychotherapy, and their combination for the treatment of chronic
Keil, F. C. (1981). Constraints on knowledge and cognitive develop- depression. New England Journal of Medicine, 342, 1462–1470.
ment. Psychological Review, 88, 197–227. Kelley, A. E., Schochet, T., & Landry, C. F. (2004). Risk taking and
Keil, F. C. (1983). On the emergence of semantic and conceptual dis- novelty seeking in adolescence: Introduction to part I. Annals of the
tinctions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 357–389. New York Academy of Sciences, 1021, 27–32.
REFERENCES R-67
Kellman, P. J., & Arterberry, M. E. (2006). Infant visual perception. Kendall-Tackett, K., Williams, L. M., & Finkelhor, D. (1993). Impact
In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), of sexual abuse on children: A review and synthesis of recent
Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and lan- empirical studies. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 164–180.
guage (6th ed., pp. 109–160). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Kendler, K. S., & Greenspan, R. J. (2006). The nature of genetic influ-
Kellman, P. J., & Banks, M. S. (1998). Infant visual perception. In ences on behavior: Lessons from “simpler” organisms. American
W. Damon (Series Ed.) & D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1683–1694.
Handbook of child psychology. Vol 2: Cognition, perception and lan- Kendler, K. S., Karkowski, L. M., & Prescott, C. A. (1999). Causal
guage (5th ed., pp. 103–146). New York: Wiley. relationship between stressful life events and the onset of major
Kellman, P. J., & Spelke, E. S. (1983). Perception of partly occluded depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 837–841.
objects in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 483–524. Kendler, K. S., Thornton, L. M., & Prescott, C. A. (2001). Gender
Kellman, P. J., Spelke, E. S., & Short, K. R. (1986). Infant percep- differences in the rates of exposure to stressful life events and sensi-
tion of object unity from translatory motion in depth and vertical tivity to their depressogenic effects. American Journal of Psychiatry,
translation. Child Development, 57, 72–86. 158, 587–593.
Kellogg, W. N., & Kellogg, L. A. (1933/1967). The ape and the child Kendrick, K. M., & Baldwin, B. A. (1987). Cells in temporal cortex
(facsimile of the 1933 edition). New York: Hafner. of sheep can respond preferentially to the sight of faces. Science,
Kelly, D. J., Liu, S., Ge, L., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., Liu, Q, & 236, 448–450.
Pascalis, O. (2007a). Cross-race preferences for same-race faces extend Keniston, K. (1970). Student activism, moral development and moral-
beyond the African versus Caucasian contrast in 3-month-old infants. ity. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 40, 577–591.
Infancy, 11, 87–95. Kennedy, Q., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2004). The role of
Kelly, D. J., Liu, S., Lee, K., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, motivation in the age-related positivity effect in autobiographical
A. M., & Ge, L. (2009). Development of the other-race effect dur- memory. Psychological Science, 15, 208–214.
ing infancy: Evidence towards universality? Journal of Experimental Kennedy, S., & Bumpass, L. (2008). Cohabitation and children’s
Child Psychology, 104, 105–114. living arrangements: New estimates from the United States.
Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., Ge, L., & Pascalis, O. Demographic Research, 19, 1663–1692.
(2007b). The other-race effect develops during infancy: Evidence Kenyon, C. (2001). A conserved regulatory system for aging. Cell,
of perceptual narrowing. Psychological Science, 18, 1084–1089. 105, 165–168.
Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., Gibson, A., Smith, Kere, J. (2011). Molecular genetics and molecular biology of dyslexia.
M., Ge, L., & Pascalis, O. (2005). Three-month-olds, but not WIREs Cognitive Science, 2, 441–448.
newborns, prefer own-race faces. Developmental Science, 8, 31–36. Kerr, J. F. R., Wyllie, A. H., & Currie, A. R. (1972). Apoptosis: A
Kelly, D. L., Mullis, I. V. S., & Martin, M. O. (2000). Profiles of stu- basic biological phenomenon with wide-ranging implications in
dent achievement in mathematics at the TIMSS international bench- tissue kinetics. British Journal of Cancer, 26, 239–257.
marks: U.S. performance and standards in an international context. Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2000). What parents know, how they know
Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College. it and several forms of adolescent adjustment: Further support for
Kelly, E. L., & Conley, J. J. (1987). Personality and compatibility: A a reinterpretation of monitoring. Developmental Psychology, 36,
prospective analysis of marital stability and marital satisfaction. 366–380.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 27–40. Keski-Rahkonen, A., Hoek, H. W., Linna, M. S., Raevuori, A., Sihvola,
Kelly, M. H., & Martin, S. (1994). Domain-specific abilities applied E., Bulik, C. M., Rissanen, A., & Kaprio, J. (2009). Incidence and
to domain-specific tasks: Sensitivity probabilities in perception, outcomes of bulimia nervosa: A nationwide population-based
cognition, and language. Lingua, 92, 105–140. study. Psychological Medicine, 39, 823–831.
Keltner, D., & Anderson, C. (2000). Saving face for Darwin: The Keski-Rahkonen, A., Hoek, H. W., Susser, E. S., Linna, M. S.,
functions and uses of embarrassment. Current Directions in Sihvola, E., Raevuori, A., Bulik, C. M., Kaprio, J., & Rissanen, A.
Psychological Science, 9, 187–190. (2007). Epidemiology and course of anorexia nervosa in the com-
Kemler Nelson, D. G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Jusczyk, P. W., & Cassidy, munity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 1259–1265.
K. W. (1989). How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist Kessel, M., Balling, R., & Gruss, P. (1990). Variations of cervical vertebrae
language learning. Journal of Child Language, 16, 55–68. after expression of a Hox-1.1 transgene in mice. Cell, 61, 301–308.
Kemler, D. G. (1983). Holistic and analytic modes in perceptual Kessel, M., & Gruss, P. (1991). Homeotic transformations of murine
and cognitive development. In T. J. Tighe & B. E. Shepp (Eds.), vertebrae and concomitant alteration of Hox codes induced by reti-
Perception, cognition, and development: Interactional analyses noic acid. Cell, 67, 89–104.
(pp. 77–102). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Kessen, W. (1965). The child. New York: Wiley.
Kemp, C., Goodman, N. D., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2010). Learning to Kessen, W., Haith, M. M., & Salapatek, P. H. (1970). Human infancy:
learn causal models. Cognitive Science, 34, 1185-1243. A bibliography and guide. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s
Kemper, S., & Harden, T. (1999). Experimentally disentangling manual of child psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1). New York: Wiley.
what’s beneficial about elderspeak from what’s not. Psychology and Kewley, G. D. (1998). Personal paper: Attention deficit hyperactiv-
Aging, 14, 656–670. ity disorder is underdiagnosed and undertreated in Britain. British
Kempermann, G., Kuhn, H. G., & Gage, F. H. (1997). More hippo- Medical Journal, 316, 1594–1596.
campal neurons in adult mice living in an enriched environment. Keynes R., & Krumlauf, R. (1994). Hox genes and regionalization of
Nature, 386, 493–495. the nervous system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 17, 109–132.
Kendall, P. C., Choudhury, M., Chung, H., & Robin, J. (2002). Keysar, B. (2008). Egocentric processes in communication and mis-
Cognitive-behavioral approaches. In M. Lewis (Ed.), Child and communication. In I. Kecskes & J. Mey (Eds), Intention, common
adolescent psychiatry (3rd ed.). Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & ground and the egocentric speaker-hearer (pp. 277–296). Berlin:
Wilkins. Mouton de Gruyter.
R-68 REFERENCES
Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2010). Social neuroscience: Mirror neu- Kinney, H. C., Brody, B. A., Kloman, A. S., & Gilles, F. H. (1988).
rons recorded in humans. Current Biology, 20, R353–R354. Sequence of central nervous system myelination in human
Khalid-Khan, S., Santibanez, M., McMicken, C., & Rynn, M. (2007). infancy. II. Patterns of myelination in autopsied infants. Journal of
Social anxiety disorder in children and adolescents: Epidemiology, Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, 47, 217–234.
diagnosis, and treatment. Pediatric Drugs, 9, 227–237. Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2012). “Native” objects
Kiehl, K. A. (2006). A cognitive neuroscience perspective on psy- and collaborators: Infants’ object choices and acts of giving reflect
chopathy: Evidence for paralimbic system dysfunction. Psychiatry favor for native over foreign speakers. Journal of Cognition and
Research, 142, 107–128. Development, 13, 67–81.
Kiehl, K. A. (2010). A paralimbic dysfunction hypothesis of psy- Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., & Correll, J. (2010). Priorities in social cat-
chopathy: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. In D. Barch (Ed.), egories. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 581–592.
Handbook of cognitive and affective neuroscience of psychopathology. Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., DeJesus, J., & Spelke, E. S. (2009).
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Accent trumps race in guiding children’s social preferences. Social
Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Hare, R. D., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B., Cognition, 27, 623–634.
Brink, J., & Liddle, P. F. (2001). Limbic abnormalities in affective Kipke, M. D. (Ed.). (1999). Adolescent development and the biology of
processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional mag- puberty: Summary of a workshop on new research. Washington, DC:
netic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 677 –684. The National Academies Press.
Kihlstrom, J. (1997). Suffering from reminiscences: Exhumed Kirkorian, H. L., Anderson, D. R., & Keen, R. (2012). Age differ-
memory, implicit memory, and the return of the repressed. In ences in online processing of video: An eye movement study. Child
M. A. Conway (Ed.), Recovered memories and false memories Development, 83, 497–507.
(pp. 100–117). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Kirkorian, H. L., Wartella, E. A., & Anderson, D. R. (2008). Media
Kihlstrom, J., & Cantor, N. (2000). Social intelligence. In R. J. and young children’s learning. Future of Children, 18, 39–61.
Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (2nd ed., pp. 359–379). Kirkpatrick, R. M., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2009). Shared-
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. environmental contributions to high cognitive ability. Behavior
Kilbourne, J. (1994). Still killing us softly: Advertising and obsession Genetics, 39, 406–416.
with thinness. New York: Guilford Press. Kirkwood, T. B. L., & Cremer, T. (1982). Cytogerontology since
Killen, M. (2007). Children’s social and moral reasoning about exclu- 1881: A reappraisal of August Weismann and a review of modern
sion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 32–36. progress. Human Genetics, 60, 101–121.
Kim, H. S. (2011). Consequences of parental divorce for child devel- Kirn, J. R., & Nottebohm, F. (1993). Direct evidence for loss and
opment. American Sociological Review, 76, 487–511. replacement of projection neurons in adult canary brain. Journal of
Kim, I. K., & Spelke, E. S. (1992). Infants’ sensitivity to effects of Neuroscience, 13, 1654–1663.
gravity on visible object motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M. J., Brown, C. A., Lee, C. T.,
Human Perception and Performance, 18, 385–393. Cowperthwaite, B., Stutzman, S. S., Swansburg, M. L., Lee, K.,
Kim, I., & Spelke, E. (1999). Perception and understanding of effects Xie, X., Huang, H., Ye, H.-H., Zhang, K, & Wang, Z. (2009).
of gravity and inertia on object motion. Developmental Science, 2, Fetal sensitivity to properties of maternal speech and language.
339–362. Infant Behavior & Development, 32, 59–71.
Kim, J. E., & Moen, P. (2002). Retirement transitions, gender, and Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V.
psychological well-being: A life-course, ecological model. Journals (1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of
of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in
57, 212–222. Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1245–1267.
Kim, J. H., & Huganir, R. L. (1999). Organization and regulation of Kitcher, P. (1993). The advancement of science: Science without legend,
proteins at synapses. Current Opinion in Cell Biology, 11, 248–254. objectivity without illusions. New York: Oxford University Press.
Kim, J. J., Krupa, D. J., & Thompson, R. F. (1998). Inhibitory Kitcher, P. (1995). Freud’s dream: A complete interdisciplinary science of
cerebello-olivary projections and blocking effect in classical con- mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
ditioning. Science, 279, 570–573. Kitsantas, A., & Dabbagh, N. (2010). Learning to learn with integra-
Kim, S., & Hasher, L. (2005). The attraction effect in decision mak- tive learning technologies (ILT): A practical guide for academic
ing: Superior performance by older adults. Quarterly Journal of success. Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.
Experimental Psychology, 58A, 120–133. Kittay, E., & Lehrer, A. (1981). Semantic fields and the structure of
Kim, S., Healey, M. K., Goldstein, D., Hasher, L., & Wiprzycka, U. J. metaphor. Studies in Language, 5, 31–63.
(2008). Age differences in choice satisfaction: A positivity effect in Kitzman, H. J., Olds, D. L., Cole, R. E., Hanks, C. A., Anson, E. A.,
decision making. Psychology and Aging, 23, 33–38. Arcoleo, K. J., Luckey, D. W., Knudtson, M. D., Henderson,
Kim, S., Thibodeau, R., & Jorgensen, R. S. (2011). Shame, guilt, C. R. Jr, &Holmberg, J. R. (2010). Enduring effects of prenatal
and depressive symptoms: A meta-analytic review. Psychological and infancy home visiting by nurses on children: Follow-up of
Bulletin, 137, 68–96. a randomized trial among children at age 12 years. Archives of
Kim- Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Harrington, H., Milne, Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 164, 412–418.
B. J., & Poulton, R. (2003). Prior juvenile diagnoses in Kitzmann, K., Cohen, R., & Lockwood, R. (2002). Are only chil-
adults with mental disorder: Developmental follow-back of a dren missing out? Comparison of the peer-related social compe-
prospective-longitudinal cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, tence of only children and siblings. Journal of Social and Personal
60, 709–717. Relationships, 19, 299–316.
King, L. A., & Hicks, J. A. (2007). Whatever happened to “what Klahr, D. (1984). Transition processes in quantitative develop-
might have been”? Regret, happiness, and maturity. American ment. In R. Sternberg (Ed.), Mechanisms of cognitive development
Psychologist, 62, 625–636. (pp. 101–139). San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
REFERENCES R-69
Klahr, D. (2000). Exploring science: The cognition and development of syndromes in a population-based twin sample. Psychological
discovery processes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Medicine, 31, 737–740.
Klahr, D. (2013). What do we mean? On the importance of not Knobloch, S., Callison, C., Chen, L., Fritzsche, A., & Zillmann, D.
abandoning scientific rigor when talking about science education. (2005). Children’s sex-stereotyped self-socialization through selec-
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 110, tive exposure to entertainment fare: Cross-cultural experiments in
14075–14080. Germany, China, and the United States. Journal of Communication,
Klahr, D., Fay, A. L., & Dunbar, K. (1993). Heuristics for scientific 55, 122–138.
experimentation: A developmental study. Cognitive Psychology, 25, Knudsen, E. (1985). Experience alters the spatial tuning of auditory
111–146. units in the optic tectum during a sensitive period in the barn owl.
Klahr, D., & Li, J. (2005). Cognitive research and elementary sci- Journal of Neuroscience, 5, 3094–3109.
ence instruction: From the laboratory, to the classroom, and back. Knudsen, E. I. (1988). Sensitive and critical periods in the develop-
Journal of Science Education and Technology, 14, 217–238. ment of sound localization. In S. S. Easter, K. F. Barald, & B. M.
Klahr, D., & Nigam, M. (2004). The equivalence of learning paths in Carlson (Eds.), From message to mind: Directions in developmental
early science instruction. Psychological Science, 15, 661–667. neurobiology (pp. 303–319). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.
Klahr, D., Zimmerman, C., & Jirout, J. (2011). Educational inter- Knudsen, E. I. (2004). Sensitive periods in the development of the brain
ventions to advance children’s scientific thinking. Science, 333, and behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1412–1425.
971–975. Knudsen, E., & Knudsen, P. (1986). The sensitive period for audi-
Klaus, M. H., Jerauld, R., Kreger, N. C., McAlpine, W., Steffa, M., & tory localization in barn owls is limited by age, not by experience.
Kennell, J. H. (1972). Maternal attachment: Importance of the Journal of Neuroscience, 6, 1918–1924.
first post-partum days. New England Journal of Medicine, 286, Knudsen, E. I., & Knudsen, P. F. (1990). Sensitive and critical periods
460–463. for visual calibration of sound localization by barn owls. Journal of
Klaus, M. H., & Kennell, J. H. (1976). Parent-infant bonding. St. Neuroscience, 10, 222–232.
Louis, MO: Mosby. Kobayashi, T., Hiraki, K., & Hasegawa, T. (2005). Auditory-visual
Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (2002). Memory and tem- intermodal matching of small numerosities in 6-month-old
poral experience: The effects of episodic memory loss on an amne- infants. Developmental Science, 8, 409–419.
sic patient’s ability to remember the past and imagine the future. Kochanska, G. (1991). Patterns of inhibition to the unfamiliar in chil-
Social Cognition, 20, 353–379. dren of normal and affectively ill mothers. Child Development, 62,
Klemfuss, J. Z., & Ceci, S. J. (2012). Legal and psychological perspec- 250–263.
tives on children’s competence to testify in court. Developmental Kochanska, G. (1997a). Multiple pathways to conscience for chil-
Review, 32, 268–286. dren with different temperaments: From toddlerhood to age 5.
Klenberg, L., Korkman, M., & Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (2001). Differential Developmental Psychology, 33, 228–240.
development of attention and executive functions in 3- to Kochanska, G. (1997b). Mutually responsive orientation between
12-year-old Finnish children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, mothers and their young children: Implications for early socializa-
407–428. tion. Child Development, 68, 908–923.
Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1966). Syntactic regularities in the speech Kochanska, G. (1998). Mother-child relationship, child fearful-
of children. In J. Lyons & R. Wales (Eds.), Psycholinguistic papers ness, and emerging attachment: A short-term longitudinal study.
(pp. 183–207). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. Developmental Psychology, 34, 480–490.
Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Cambridge, Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (2006). Children’s conscience and self-
MA: Harvard University Press. regulation. Journal of Personality, 74, 1587–1617.
Klin, A., Chawarska, K., Rubin, E., & Volkmar, F. (2004). Clinical Kochanska, G., Barry, R. A., Stellern, S. A., & O’Bleness, J. J.
assessment of young children at risk for autism. In R. DelCarmen- (2009a). Early attachment organization moderates the parent-
Wiggins & A. Carter (Eds.), Handbook of infant, toddler, and pre- child mutually coercive pathway to children’s antisocial conduct.
school mental health assessment (pp. 311–336). New York: Oxford Child Development, 80, 1288–1300.
University Press. Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a personal-
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. ity characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and
(2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 1087–1112.
social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals Kochanska, G., & Murray, K. T. (2000). Mother-child mutually
with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816. responsive orientation and conscience development: From toddler
Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., & Sparrow, S. S. (Eds.). (2000). Asperger to early school age. Child Development, 71, 417–431.
syndrome. New York: Guilford Press. Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. L., & Vandegeest,
Kline, M., Tschann, J. M., Johnston, J. R., & Wallerstein, J. S. (1989). K. A. (1996). Inhibitory control in young children and its role in
Children’s adjustment in joint and sole physical custody families. emerging internalization. Child Development, 67, 490–507.
Developmental Psychology, 25, 430–438. Kochanska, G., Philibert, R. A., & Barry, R. A. (2009b). Interplay
Kline, P. (1991). Intelligence: The psychometric view. New York: of genes and early mother-child relationship in the development
Routledge. of self-regulation from toddler to preschool age. Journal of Child
Klingelhutz, A. J. (1999). The roles of telomeres and telomerase in cel- Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 1331–1338.
lular immortalization and the development of cancer. Anticancer Koenig, M. A. (2010). Selective trust in testimony: Children’s eval-
Research, 19, 4823–4830. uation of the message, the speaker, and the speech act. In T. S.
Klinke, R., Kral, A., Heid, S., Tillein, J., & Hartmann R. (1999). Gendler & J. Hawthorne, Oxford studies in epistemology (Vol. 3,
Recruitment of the auditory cortex in congenitally deaf cats by pp. 253–273). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
long-term cochlear electrostimulation. Science, 285, 1729–1733. Koenig, M. A., Clement, F., & Harris, P. L. (2004). Trust in tes-
Klump, K. L., Miller, K. B., Keel, P. K., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G timony: Children’s use of true and false statements. Psychological
(2001). Genetic and environmental influences on anorexia nervosa Science, 15, 694–698.
R-70 REFERENCES
Koenig, O., Reiss, L. P., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1990). The development Kong, A., Frigge, M. L., Masson, G., Besenbacher, S., Sulem, P.,
of spatial relation representations: Evidence from studies of cere- Magnusson, G., Gudjonsson, S. A., Sigurdsson, A., Jonasdottir, A.,
bral lateralization. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50, Jonasdottir, A., Wong, W. S., Sigurdsson, G., Walters, G. B.,
119–130. Steinberg, S., Helgason, H., Thorleifsson, G., Gudbjartsson, D. F.,
Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Cushman, F., Helgason, A., Magnusson, O. T., Thorsteinsdottir, U., & Stefansson,
Hauser, M., & Damasio, A. (2007). Damage to the prefrontal cor- K. (2012). Rate of de novo mutations and the importance of father’s
tex increases utilitarian moral judgments. Nature, 446, 908–911. age to disease risk. Nature, 488, 471–475.
Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. New York: Harcourt, Konishi, N. (1985). Birdsong: From behavior to neuron. Annual
Brace, and Company. Review of Neuroscience, 8, 125–170.
Kohlberg, L. (1963). The development of children’s orientations Koopman, P., Bullejos, M., & Bowles, J. (2001). Regulation of male
toward a moral order: I. Sequence in the development of moral sexual development by Sry and Sox9. Journal of Experimental
thought. Vita Humana, 6, 11–33. Zoology, 290, 463–474.
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental Koopman, R., & van Loon, L. J. (2009). Aging, exercise, and muscle
approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of protein metabolism. Journal of Applied Physiology, 106, 2040–2048.
socialization theory and research (pp. 347–480). Chicago: Rand Koplewicz, H. S. (2004). Misguided resistance to appropriate treat-
McNally. ment for adolescent depression. Journal of Child and Adolescent
Kohlberg, L. (1971). From is to ought: How to commit the natural- Psychopharmacology, 14, 7.
istic fallacy and get away with it in the study of moral develop- Kopp, C. B., & McCall, R. B. (1990). Predicting later mental per-
ment. In T. Mischel (Ed.), Cognitive development and epistemology formance for normal, at-risk, and handicapped infants. In P. B.
(pp. 151–235). New York: Academic Press. Baltes & O. G. Brim, Jr (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior
Kohlberg, L. (1976). Moral stages and moralization: The cognitive- (Vol. 4, pp. 31–61). New York: Academic Press.
developmental approach. In T. Lickona (Ed.), Moral development Kopp, C. B., & Neufeld, S. J. (2003). Emotional development dur-
and behavior: Theory, research, and social issues (pp. 31–53). New ing infancy. In R J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith
York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 347–374). Oxford, UK:
Kohlberg, L. (1984). Essays on moral development: Vol. 2.The psychology Oxford University Press.
of moral development: The nature and validity of moral stages. New Kornack, D. R., & Rakic, P. (2001). Cell proliferation without neuro-
York: Harper & Row. genesis in adult primate neocortex. Science, 294, 2127–2130.
Kohlberg, L. A. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of chil- Korsching S. (1993). The neurotrophic factor concept: A reexamina-
dren’s sex-role concepts and attitudes. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The tion. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 2739–2748.
development of sex differences (pp. 82–173). Stanford, CA: Stanford Korvenranta, E., Lehtonen, L., Rautava, L., Hakkinen, U., Andersson, S.,
University Press. Gissler, M., Hallman, M., Leipala, J., Peltola, M., Tammela, O.,
Kohlberg, L., & Candee, D. (1984). The relationship of moral judg- Linna, M., for the PERFECT Preterm Infant Study Group. (2010).
ment to moral action. In L. Kohlberg (Ed.), The psychology of moral Impact of very preterm birth on health care costs at five years of
development: The nature and validity of moral stages (pp.498–581). age. Pediatrics, 125, e1109–e1114.
New York: Harper & Row. Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E.
Kohlberg, L., Levine, C., & Hewer, A. (1983). Moral stages: A cur- (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435, 673–676.
rent formulation and a response to critics. In J. A. Meacham (Ed.), Koslowski, B. (1996). Theory and evidence: The development of scien-
Contributions to human development (Vol. 10, pp. 1–178). Basel, tific reasoning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Switzerland: Karger. Kosslyn, S. M., Shephard, J. M., & Thompson, W. L. (2007). Spatial
Kohlberg, L., & Turiel, E. (1971). Moral development and moral processing during mental imagery: A neurofunctional theory. In
education. In G. Lesser (Ed.), Psychology and educational practice F. W. Mast & L. Jancke (Eds.), Spatial processing in navigation,
(pp. 410–437). Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. imagery, and perception (pp. 1–16). New York: Springer.
Kohn, A. (2000). Burnt at high-stakes. Journal of Teacher Education, Kost, K., & Henshaw, S. (2010). U.S. teenage pregnancies, births and
51, 315–327. abortions: National and state trends by race and ethnicity—2010.
Kojima, S., & Alberts, J. R. (2011). Warmth from skin-to-skin con- New York: Guttmacher Institute.
tact with mother is essential for the acquisition of filial huddling Kotulak, R. (1996). Inside the brain: Revolutionary discoveries of how
preference in preweanling rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 53, the mind works. Kansas City, MO: Andrews and McNeel.
813–827. Kovács, Á. M., Téglás, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense:
Kolata, G. (1996, May 15). Boom in Ritalin sales raises ethical issues. Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults.
The New York Times, p. C8. Science, 33, 1830–1834.
Kolb, B. (1989). Brain development, plasticity, and behavior. American Kovas, Y., & Plomin, R. (2006). Generalist genes: Implications for
Psychologist, 44, 1203–1212. the cognitive sciences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 198–203.
Koldewijn, K., van Wassenaer, A., Wolf, M. J., Meijssen, D., Kovelman, I., Norton, E. S., Christodoulou, J. A., Gaab, N., Lieberman,
Houtzager, B., Beelen, A., Kok, J., & Nollet, F. (2010). A neurobehav- D. A., Triantafyllou, C., Wolf, M., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., &
ioral intervention and assessment program in very low birth weight Gabrieli, J. D. (2012). Brain basis of phonological awareness for
infants: Outcome at 24 months. Journal of Pediatrics, 156, 359–365. spoken language in children and its disruption in dyslexia. Cerebral
Komuro, H., & Rakic, P. (1993). Modulation of neuronal migration Cortex, 22, 754–764.
by NMDA receptors. Science, 260, 95–97. Kowal, A., Krull, J., & Kramer, L. (2004). How the differential treat-
Kondo-Ikemura, K., & Waters, E. (1995). Maternal behavior and ment of siblings is linked with parent-child relationship quality.
infant security in Old World monkeys: Conceptual issues and Journal of Family Psychology, 18, 658–665.
a methodological bridge between human and nonhuman pri- Krachun, C., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010). A new
mate research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child change-of-contents false belief test: Children and chimpanzees com-
Development, 60, 97–110. pared. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23, 145–165.
REFERENCES R-71
Kraepelin, E. (1899). Psychiatrie (6th ed.). Leipzig: Barth. Krueger, J. I., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2008). Is the allure of
Kraepelin, E. (1919). Dementia praecox and paraphrenia (R. M. self-esteem a mirage after all? [Comment]. American Psychologist,
Barclay, Trans.). Edinburgh: Livingston. 63, 64–65.
Kraepelin, E. (1921). Manic-depressive insanity and paranoia (Classics Krug, E. F. (2006). Law and ethics at the border of viability. Journal
in Psychiatry). Edinburgh: Ayer. of Perinatology, 26, 321–374.
Krahé, B., Möller, I., Huesmann, L. R., Kirwil, L., Felber, J., & Krumlauf, R. (1994). Hox genes and vertebrate development. Cell,
Berger, A. (2011). Desensitization to media violence: Links with 78, 191–201.
habitual media violence exposure, aggressive cognitions, and Kubarych, T. S., Aggen, S. H., Hettema, J. M., Kendler, K. S., &
aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Neale, M. C. (2008). Assessment of generalized anxiety disor-
100, 630–646. der diagnostic criteria in the National Comorbidity Survey and
Kral, A., Hartmann, R., Tillein, J., Heid, S., & Klinke, R. (2002). Virginia Adult Twin Study of Psychiatric and Substance Use
Hearing after congenital deafness: Central auditory plasticity and Disorders. Psychological Assessment, 20, 206–216.
sensory deprivation. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 797–807. Kuhl, P. K. (1986). Theoretical contributions of tests on animals to
Kral, A., & Sharma, A. (2012). Developmental neuroplasticity after the special-mechanisms debate in speech. Experimental Biology, 45,
cochlear implantation. Trends in Neurosciences, 35, 111–122. 233–265.
Kral, K. (2003). Behavioural-analytical studies of the role of head Kuhl, P. K. (1991). Human adults and human infants show a “percep-
movements in depth perception in insects, birds and mammals. tual magnet effect” for the prototypes of speech categories, mon-
Behavioral Processes, 64, 1–12. keys do not. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 93–107.
Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K. I., & Colcombe, S. J. (2006). Exercise, Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech
cognition, and the aging brain. Journal of Applied Physiology, 101, code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 831–843.
1237–1242. Kuhl, P. K. (2010). Brain mechanisms in early language acquisition.
Kramer, L. (2010). The essential ingredients of successful sibling rela- Neuron, 67, 713–727.
tionships: An emerging framework for advancing theory and prac- Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1984). The intermodal representation
tice. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 80–86. of speech in infants. Infant Behavior & Development, 7, 361–381.
Kranzler, J. H., & Jensen, A. R. (1991). The nature of psychometric g: Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). Infant vocalizations in response
Unitary process or a number of independent processes? Intelligence, to speech: Vocal imitation and developmental change. Journal of
15, 397–422. the Acoustical Society of America, 100, 2425–2438.
Krasnegor, N. A., Lyon, G. R., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1997). Kuhl, P. K., & Miller, J. D. (1978). Speech perception by the chin-
Development of the prefrontal cortex: Evolution, neurobiology, and chilla: Identification functions for synthetic VOT stimuli. Journal
behavior. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. of the Acoustical Society of America, 63, 905–917.
Krauss, R. M., & Glucksberg, S. (1969). The development of com- Kuhl, P. K., & Padden, D. M. (1982). Enhanced discriminability
munication: Competence as a function of age. Child Development, at the phonetic boundaries for the voicing feature in macaques.
40, 256–266. Perception and Psychophysics, 32, 542–550.
Kraut, R. E. (1978). Verbal and nonverbal cues in the perception of Kuhlmeier, V., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2003). Attribution of disposi-
lying. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 380–391. tional states by 12-month-olds. Psychological Science, 14, 402–408.
Kraut, R. E., & Johnston, R. E. (1979). Social and emotional mes- Kuhn, D. (2000). Metacognitive development. Current Directions in
sages of smiling: An ethological approach. Journal of Personality Psychological Science, 9, 178–181.
and Social Psychology, 37, 1539–1553. Kuhn, D. (2009). The importance of learning about knowing:
Kraut, R., Patterson, M., Lundmark, V., Kiesler, S., Mukophadyay, T., & Creating a foundation for development of intellectual values. Child
Scherlis, W. (1998). Internet paradox: A social technology that Development Perspectives, 3, 112–117.
reduces social involvement and psychological well-being? American Kuhn, D., Amsel, E., & O’Loughlin, M. (1988). The development of
Psychologist, 53, 1017–1031. scientific thinking skills. San Diego: Academic Press.
Kraut, R. E., & Poe, D. B. (1980). Behavioral roots of person percep- Kuhn, D., & Angelev, J. (1976). An experimental study of the devel-
tion: The deception judgments of customs inspectors and laymen. opment of formal operational thought. Child Development, 47,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 784–798. 697–706.
Krcmar, M., Grela, B., & Lin, K. (2007). Can toddlers learn vocabu- Kuhn, D., & Brannock, J. (1977). Development of the isolation of
lary from television? An experimental approach. Media Psychology, variables scheme in experimental and “natural experiment” con-
10, 41–63. texts. Developmental Psychology, 13, 9–14.
Krebs, D. L. (2008). Morality: An evolutionary account. Perspectives Kuhn, D., Garcia-Mila, M., Zohar, A., & Andersen, C. (1995).
on Psychological Science, 3, 149–172 Strategies of knowledge acquisition. Monographs of the Society of
Krebs, D. L., & Denton, K. (2005). Toward a more pragmatic Research in Child Development, 60 (Serial No. 245), 1–157.
approach to morality: A critical evaluation of Kohlberg’s model. Kuhn, D., Pease, M., & Wirkala, C. (2009). Coordinating the effects
Psychological Review, 112, 629–649. of multiple variables: A skill fundamental to scientific thinking.
Kreukels, B. P. C., & Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. (2012). Male gender iden- Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 268–284.
tity and masculine behavior: The role of sex hormones in brain Kuhn, D., Schauble, L., & Garcia-Mila, M. (1992). Cross-domain
development. In M. Maggi, (Ed.), Hormonal therapy for male sexual development of scientific reasoning. Cognition and Instruction, 9,
dysfunction (2nd ed., pp. 1–10). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. 285–327.
Krist, H. (2000). Development of naive beliefs about moving objects: Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago:
The straight-down belief in action. Cognitive Development, 15, University of Chicago Press.
281–308. Kumpulainen, K., Rasanen, E., & Henttonen, I. (1999). Children
Kristensen, P., & Bjerkedal, T. (2007). Explaining the relation involved in bullying: Psychological disturbance and the persistence
between birth order and intelligence. Science, 316, 1717. of the involvement. Child Abuse and Neglect, 23, 1253–1262.
R-72 REFERENCES
Kumra. S., & Schulz, S. C. (2008). Research progress in early-onset and localization of translocations associated with speech and lan-
schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34, 15–17. guage disorder. American Journal of Human Genetics, 67, 357–368.
Kumsta, R., Kreppner, J., Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Stevens, S., & Lai, C. S. L., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha-Khadem, F., &
Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2010). Deprivation-specific psychological pat- Monaco, A. P. (2001). A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a
terns. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 75, severe speech and language disorder. Nature, 413, 519–523.
48–78. Lai, C. S. L., Gerrelli, D., Monaco, A. P., Fisher, S. E., & Copp, A. J.
Kundey, S. M. A., De Los Reyes, A., Taglang, C., Baruch, A., & (2003). FOXP2 expression during brain development coincides
German, R. (2010). Domesticated dogs’ (Canis familiaris) use of with adult sites of pathology in a severe speech and language disor-
the solidity principle. Animal Cognition, 13, 497–505. der. Brain, 126, 2455–2462.
Kunkel, D. (2001). Children and television advertising. In D. G. Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., Chakrabarti, B., Ecker, C., Sadek, S. A.,
Singer & J. L. Singer (Eds.), The handbook of children and media Wheelwright, S. J., Murphy, D. G. M., Suckling, J., Bullmore,
(pp. 375–393). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. E. T., MRC AIMS Consortium, & Baron-Cohen, S. (2012).
Kurtines, W. M., & Gewirtz, J. L. (Eds.) (1991). Handbook of moral Individual differences in brain structure underpin empathizing-
behavior and development (Vols. 1–3). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. systemizing cognitive styles in male adults. Neuroimage, 61,
Kurtines, W., & Greif, E. B. (1974). The development of moral 1347–1354.
thought: Review and evaluation of Kohlberg’s approach. Laible, D. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2000). Mother-child discourse,
Psychological Bulletin, 81, 453–470. attachment security, shared positive affect, and early conscience
Kwok, V., Niu, Z., Kay, P., Zhou, K., Mo, L., Jin, Z., So, K. F., & Tan, development. Child Development, 71, 1424–1440.
L. H. (2011). Learning new color names produces rapid increase Lakdawalla, Z., Hankin, B. L., & Mermelstein, R. (2007). Cognitive
in gray matter in the intact adult human cortex. Proceedings of the theories of depression in children and adolescents: A conceptual
National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 108, 6686–6688. and quantitative review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology
Review, 10, 1–24.
Laakso, M. P., Vaurio, O., Koivisto, E., Savolainen, L., Eronen, M., Lake, A., Staiger, P., & Glowinski, H. (2000) Effect of western culture
Aronen, H. J., Hakola, P., Repo, E., Soininen, H., & Tiihonen, J. on women’s attitudes toward eating and perceptions of body shape.
(2001). Psychopathy and the posterior hippocampus. Behavioural International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27, 83–89.
Brain Research, 118, 187–193. Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago:
Labov, W. (1970). The study of language in its social context. Studium Chicago University Press.
Generale, 23, 30–87. Lakshminaryanan, V., Chen, M. K., & Santos, L. R. (2008).
Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city: Studies in the Black Endowment effect in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
English vernacular. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
Lachman, M. E. (2004). Development in midlife. Annual Review of 363, 3837–3844.
Psychology, 55, 305–331. Lamb, M. E. (1982). Early contact and maternal-infant bonding: One
Lacy, J. H., & Price, C. (2004). Disturbed families, or families dis- decade later. Pediatrics, 70, 763–768.
turbed? British Journal of Psychiatry, 184, 195–196. Lamb, M. E. (1998). Nonparental child care: Context, quality, cor-
Ladd, G. (2006). Peer rejection, aggressive or withdrawn behavior, relates. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & I. E. Sigel & K. A. Renninger
and psychological maladjustment from ages 5 to 12: An examina- (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Child psychology in
tion of four predictive models. Child Development, 77, 822–846. practice (5th ed., pp. 73–134). New York: Wiley.
LaFontana, K. M., & Cillessen, A. H. (1998). The nature of children’s Lamb, M. E. (2010). How do fathers influence children’s develop-
stereotypes of popularity. Social Development, 7, 301–320. ment? Let me count the ways. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of
LaFontana, K. M., & Cillessen, A. H. (2002). Children’s percep- the father in child development (5th ed., pp. 1–26). Hoboken, NJ:
tions of popular and unpopular peers: A multimethod assessment. Wiley.
Developmental Psychology, 38, 635–647. Lamb, M. E. (Ed.). (2004). The role of the father in child development
LaFreniere, P., & MacDonald, K. (2013). A post-genomic view of (4th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
behavioral development and adaptation to the environment. Lamb, M. E., & Ahnert, L. (2006). Nonparental child care: Context,
Developmental Review, 33, 89–109. concepts, correlates, and consequences. In W. Damon & R. M.
Lagattuta, K. H., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). The development of Lerner (Series Eds.) & K. A. Renninger & I. E. Sigel (Vol. Eds.),
self-conscious emotions: Cognitive processes and social influ- Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Child psychology in practice
ences. In J. L. Tracy, R.W. Robins, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), The (6th ed., pp. 950–1016). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
self-conscious emotions: Theory and research (pp. 91–113). New York: Lamb, M. E., & Lewis, C. (2010). The development and significance
Guilford Press. of father-child relationships in two-parent families. In M. E.
Lagos, M. (2007, November 19). 64 years after childhood friends Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (5th ed.,
parted, they are reunited. San Francisco Chronicle, p. A1. pp. 94–153). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Lahey, B. B., Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (Eds.) (2003). Causes of con- Lamb, M. E., Sternberg, K. J., & Prodromidis, M. (1992). Nonmaternal
duct disorder and juvenile delinquency. New York: Guilford Press. care and the security of the infant-mother attachment: A reanalysis
LaHoste, G. J., Swanson, J. M., Wigal, S. B., Glabe, C., Wigal, T., of the data. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 71–83.
King, N., & Kennedy, J. L. (1996). Dopamine D4 receptor gene Lamb, M. E., Thompson, R. A., Gardner, W. P., Charnov, E. L., &
polymorphism is associated with attention deficit hyperactivity Estes, D. (1984). Security of infantile attachment as assessed in
disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 1, 121–124. the “Strange Situation”: Its study and biological interpretation.
Lai, C. S. L., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Levy, E. R., Hodgson, S., Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 127–147.
Fox, M., Jeremiah, S., Povey, S., Jamison, D. C., Green, E. D., Lamb, N. E., Yu, K., Shaffer, J., Feingold, E., & Sherman, S. L. (2005).
Vargha-Khadem, F., & Monaco, A. P. (2000). The SPCH1 region Association between maternal age and meiotic recombination for
on human 7q31: Genomic characterization of the critical interval trisomy 21. American Journal of Human Genetics, 76, 91–99.
REFERENCES R-73
Lambert, W. E. (1975). Culture and language as factors in learning Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., Casey, R. J., Ritter, J. M., Reiser-
and education. In A. Wolfgang (Ed.), Education of immigrant Danner, L. A., & Jenkins, V. Y. (1987). Infant preferences for
students (pp. 55–83). Toronto: Ontario Institute for Studies in attractive faces: Rudiments of a stereotype? Developmental
Education. Psychology, 23, 363–369.
Lambert, W. E. (1985). Some cognitive and sociocultural conse- Lansford, J. E., Chang, L., Dodge, K. A., Malone, P. S., Oburu, P.,
quences of being bilingual. In J. E. Alatis & J. J. Staczek, (Eds.), Palmérus, K., Bacchini, D., Pastorelli, C., Bombi, A. S., Zelli, A.,
Perspectives on bilingualism and bilingual education. Washington, Tapanya, S., Chaudhary, N., Deater-Deckard, K., Manke, B., &
DC: Georgetown University Press. Quinn, N. (2005). Physical discipline and children’s adjustment:
Lambert, W. E., & Tucker, G. R. (1972). Bilingual education of chil- Cultural normativeness as a moderator. Child Development, 76,
dren: The St. Lambert experiment. Rowley, MA: Newbury House. 1234–1246.
Lamborn, S. D., Mounts, N. S., Steinberg, L., & Dornbusch, S. M. Lansford, J. E., Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., &
(1991). Patterns of competence and adjustment among adolescents Pettit, G. S. (2004). Ethnic differences in the link between physi-
from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful fami- cal discipline and later adolescent externalizing behaviors. Journal
lies. Child Development, 62, 1049–1065. of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 801–812.
Lan, X., Ponitz, C. C., Miller, K. F., Li, S., Cortina, K., Perry, M., & Lany, J., & Saffran, J. R. (2010). From statistics to meaning: Infants’
Fang, G. (2009). Keeping their attention: Classroom practices acquisition of lexical categories. Psychological Science, 21, 284–291.
associated with behavioral engagement in first grade mathematics Lanzetta, J. T., & Englis, B. G. (1989). Expectations of cooperation
classes in China and the United States. Early Childhood Research and competition and their effects on observers’ vicarious emo-
Quarterly, 24, 198–211. tional responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56,
Landau, B. (1994). Object shape, object name, and object kind: 543–554.
Representation and development. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psy- Larsen, W. J., Sherman, L. S., Steven Potter, S., & Scott, W. J. (2001).
chology of learning and motivation (Vol. 31, pp. 253–304). San Human embryology (3rd ed.). New York: Churchill Livingstone.
Diego: Academic Press. Larsen-Freeman, D., & Long, M. (1991). An introduction to second
Landau, B., & Gleitman, L. R. (1985). Language and experience: language acquisition research. London: Longman.
Evidence from the blind child. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Larsson, H., Andershed, H., & Lichtenstein, P. (2006). A genetic fac-
Press. tor explains most of the variation in the psychopathic personality.
Landau, B., Smith, L. B., & Jones, S. S. (1988). The importance of Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 221–230.
shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3, 299–321. Larzelere, R. E. (2000). Child outcomes of nonabusive and custom-
Landau, B., Smith, L., & Jones, S. (1998). Object shape, object func- ary physical punishment by parents: An updated literature review.
tion, and object name. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 1–27. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 3, 199–221.
Landau, B., Spelke, E. S., & Gleitman, H. (1984). Spatial knowledge Larzelere, R. E., & Kuhn, B. R. (2005). Comparing child outcomes of
in a young blind child. Cognition, 16, 225–260. physical punishment and alternative disciplinary tactics: A meta-
Landauer, T. K. (2002). On the computational basis of learning and analysis. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 8, 1–37.
cognition: Arguments from LSA. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychol- Lashley, K. S. (1929). Brain mechanisms and intelligence. Chicago:
ogy of learning and motivation (Vol. 41, pp. 43–84). San Diego: University of Chicago Press.
Academic Press. Laszlo, J. I., & Sainsbury, K. M. (1993). Perceptual-motor development
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s prob- and prevention of clumsiness. Psychological Research, 55, 167–174.
lem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induc- Lau, J. Y., & Eley, T. C. (2010). The genetics of mood disorders.
tion, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 313–337.
211–240. Laurens, K. R., Hodgins, S., Taylor, E. A., & Murray, R. M. (2011).
Landis, G. N., Abdueva, D., Skvortsov, D., Yang, J., Rabin, B. E., Is earlier intervention for schizophrenia possible? Identifying
Carrick, J., Tavaré, S., & Tower, J. (2004). Similar gene expres- antecedents of schizophrenia in children aged 9–12 years. In
sion patterns characterize aging and oxidative stress in Drosophila A. S. David, P. McGuffin, & S. Kapur (Eds.), Schizophrenia: The
melanogaster. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the final frontier. A festschrift for Robin M. Murray (Maudsley Series).
USA, 18, 7663–7668. London: Psychology Press.
Lane, H. (1976). The wild boy of Aveyron. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Laursen, B. (1995). Conflict and social interaction in adolescent rela-
University Press. tionships. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 5, 55–70.
Langley-Evans, S. C. (2007). Developmental programming of health Laursen, B., Bukowski, W. M., Nurmi, J. E., Marion, D., Salmela-
and disease. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society, 65, 97–105. Aro, K., & Kiuru, N. (2010). Opposites detract: Middle school
Langlois, J. H., Ritter, J. M., Casey, R. J., & Sawin, D. B. (1995). peer group antipathies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
Infant attractiveness predicts maternal behaviors and attitudes. 106, 240–256.
Developmental Psychology, 31, 464–472. Lave, J. (1977). Tailor-made experiments and evaluating the intellec-
Langlois, J. H., Ritter, J. M., Roggman, L. A., & Vaughn, L. S. tual consequences of apprenticeship training. Quarterly Newsletter
(1991). Facial diversity and infant preferences for attractive faces. of the Institute for Comparative Human Development, 1, 1–3.
Developmental Psychology, 27, 79–84. Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral
Langlois, J. H., & Roggman, L. A. (1990). Attractive faces are only participation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
average. Psychological Science, 1, 115–121. Lavenex, P., & Banta Lavenex, P. (2006). Spatial relational memory in
Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., & Musselman, L. (1994). What is 9-month-old macaque monkeys. Learning and Memory, 13, 84–96.
average and what is not average about attractive faces? Psychological Lawrence, C. R., Carlson, E. A., & Egeland, B. (2006). The impact
Science, 5, 214–220. of foster care on development. Development and Psychopathology,
Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., & Reisser-Danner, L. A. (1990). 18, 57–76.
Infants’ differential social responses to attractive and unattractive Lawrence, P. A. (1992). The making of a fly: The genetics of animal
faces. Developmental Psychology, 26, 153–159. design. Oxford: Blackwell.
R-74 REFERENCES
Lawrence, P. A., & Morata, G. (1994). Homeobox genes: Their func- Leckman, J. F., & Mayes, L. C. (2007). Nurturing resilient children.
tion in Drosophila segmentation and pattern formation. Cell, 78, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 221–223.
181–189. Leddon, E. M., Waxman, S. R., & Medin, D. L. (2011). What does
Lawrence, P. A., & Struhl, G. (1996). Morphogens, compartments, it mean to “live” and “die”? A cross-linguistic analysis of parent-
and pattern: Lessons from Drosophila? Cell, 85, 951–961. child conversations in English and Indonesian. British Journal of
Lawrence, R. A. (1994). Breastfeeding: A guide for the medical profession Developmental Psychology, 29, 375–395.
(4th ed.). St. Louis, MO: Mosby. LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings
Lawton, C. A. (2010). Gender, spatial abilities, and wayfinding. of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster.
In J. C. Chrisler & D. R. McCreary (Eds.), Handbook of gender LeDoux, J. (1998). Fear and the brain: Where have we been, and
research in psychology (pp. 317–341). New York: Springer. where are we going? Biological Psychiatry, 44, 1229–1238.
Le Bouc, Y., Rossignol, S., Azzi, S., Brioude, F., Cabrol, S., Gicquel, C., & LeDoux, J. (1998). The emotional brain. New York: Weidenfeld &
Netchine, I. (2010). Epigenetics, genomic imprinting, and develop- Nicolson.
mental disorders. Bulletin de l’Académie Nationale de Médecine, 194, LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of
287–297. Neuroscience, 23, 155–184.
Le Corre, M., Van de Walle, G., Brannon, E. M., & Carey, S. (2006). Lee, D. N. (1974). Visual information during locomotion. In R. B.
Re-visiting the competence/performance debate in the acquisition Macleod & H. L. Pick (Eds.), Perception: Essays in honor of James J.
of the counting principles. Cognitive Psychology, 52, 130–169. Gibson (pp. 250–267). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Le Grand, R., Mondloch, C. J., Maurer, D., & Brent, H. P. (2003). Lee, D. N., & Aronson, E. (1974). Visual proprioceptive control
Expert face processing requires visual input to the right hemisphere of standing in human infants. Perception & Psychophysics, 15,
during infancy. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 1108–1112. 529–532.
Le Grand, R., Mondloch, C. J., Maurer, D., & Brent, H. P. (2004). Lee, K. M., Peng, W., & Klein, J. (2010). Will the experience of play-
Impairment in holistic face processing following early visual depri- ing a violent role in a video game influence people’s judgments of
vation. Psychological Science, 15, 762–768. violent crimes? Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 1019–1023.
Lea, S. E. G., Slater, A. M., & Ryan, C. M. E. (1996). Perception Lee, K., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., & Slater, A. (2013). Development
of object unity in chicks: A comparison with the human infant. of face processing ability in childhood. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.),
Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 501–504. Oxford handbook of developmental psychology: Vol I. Body and mind.
Leadbeater, B. J., Ohan, J. L., & Hoglund, W. L. (2006). How chil- New York: Oxford University Press.
dren’s justifications of the “best thing to do” in peer conflicts relate Lee, L. O., & Knight, B. G. (2009). Attentional bias for threat in
to their emotional and behavioral problems in early elementary older adults: Moderation of the positivity bias by trait anxiety and
school. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 52, 721–754. stimulus modality. Psychology and Aging, 24, 741–747.
Leaper, C. (2002). Parenting girls and boys. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Lee, P. C., Niew, W. I., Yang, H. J., Chen, V. C. H., & Lin, K. C.
Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed., (2012). A meta-analysis of behavioral parent training for chil-
pp. 189–225). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. dren with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Research in
Leaper, C. (2011). Research in developmental psychology on gender Developmental Disabilities, 33, 2040–2049.
and relationships: Reflections on the past and looking into the Lee, R., Zhai, F., Brooks-Gunn, J., Han, W. J., & Waldfogel, J. (2013).
future. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 347–356. Head Start participation and school readiness: Evidence from the
Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of early childhood longitudinal study-birth cohort. Developmental
gender effects on parents’ talk to their children: A meta-analysis. Psychology. Epub ahead of print.
Developmental Psychology, 34, 3–27. Lee, S. A., & Spelke, E. S. (2010). Two systems of spatial represen-
Leaper, C., & Friedman, C. K. (2006). The socialization of gender. tation underlying navigation. Experimental Brain Research, 206,
In J. Grusec & P. Hastings (Eds.), The handbook of socialization: 179–188.
Theory and research (pp. 561–587). New York: Guilford Press. Lee, S. A., Winkler-Rhoades, N., & Spelke, E. S. (2012). Spontaneous
Learmonth, A. E., Nadel, L., & Newcombe, N. S. (2002). Children’s reorientation is guided by perceived surface distance, not by image
use of landmarks: Implications for modularity theory. Psychological matching or comparison. PLoS ONE, 7, e51373.
Science, 13, 337–341. Lee, T., Mosing, M. A., Henry, J. D., Trollor, J. N., Lammel, A.,
Lease, A. M., Kennedy, C. A., & Axelrod, J. L. (2002a). Children’s Ames, D., Martin, N. G., Wright, M. J., & Sachdev, P. S. (2012).
social constructions of popularity. Social Development, 11, 87–109. Genetic influences on five measures of processing speed and their
Lease, A. M., Musgrove, K. T., & Axelrod, J. L. (2002b). Dimensions covariation with general cognitive ability in the elderly: The Older
of social status in preadolescent peer groups: Likability, per- Australian Twins Study. Behavior Genetics, 42, 96–106.
ceived popularity, and social dominance. Social Development, 11, Lee, Y. S. (2009). Consequences of childhood obesity. Annals of the
508–533. Academy of Medicine, Singapore, 38, 75–77.
Leatherman, J. L., Kaestner, K. H., & Jongens, T. A. (2000). Leekam, S., & Perner, J. (1991). Does the autistic child have a meta-
Identification of a mouse germ cell-less homologue with conserved representational deficit? Cognition, 40, 203–218.
activity in Drosophila. Mechanisms of Development, 92, 145–153. Leekam, S. R., Prior, M. R., & Uljarevic, M. (2011). Restricted and
Leckman, J. F., & Bloch, M. H. (2008). A developmental and evolu- repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders: A review of
tionary perspective on obsessive-compulsive disorder: Whence and research in the last decade. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 562–593.
whither compulsive hoarding? American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, Legerstee, M. (1991). The role of person and object in eliciting early
1229–1233. imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 423–433.
Leckman, J. F., Grice, D. E., Boardman, M. D., Zhang, H., Vitale, A., Lehr, S. S. (1991). The child’s developing sense of theme: Responses to
Bondi, C., Alsobrook, J., Peterson, B. S., Cohen, D. J., Rasmussen, literature. New York: Teachers College Press.
S. A., Goodman, W. K., McDougle, C. J., & Pauls, D. L. (1997). Lehrer, A. (1985). The influence of semantic fields on semantic
Symptoms of obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of change. In J. Fisiak (Ed.), Historical semantics, historical word-
Psychiatry, 154, 911–917. formation (pp. 283–295). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
REFERENCES R-75
Lehrer, R., Schauble, L., Strom, D., & Pligge, M. (2001). Similarity Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., & Iyengar, S. S. (2005). Intrinsic and
of form and substance: Modeling material kind. In D. Klahr & extrinsic motivational orientations in the classroom: Age differ-
S. Carver (Eds.), Cognition and instruction: 25 years of progress ences and academic correlates. Journal of Educational Psychology,
(pp. 39–74). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 97, 184–196.
Leibenluft, E. (2008). Pediatric bipolar disorder comes of age. Archives Lepper, M., & Greene, D. (1975). Turning play into work: The effects
of General Psychiatry, 65, 1122–1124. of adult surveillance and extrinsic rewards on children’s intrin-
Leibenluft, E. (2011). Severe mood dysregulation, irritability, and sic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31,
the diagnostic boundaries of bipolar disorder in youths. American 479–486.
Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 129–142. Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining
Leibenluft, E., & Rich, B. A. (2008). Pediatric bipolar disorder. children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 163–187. “overjustification” hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social
Leifer, A. D., & Roberts, D. F. (1972). Children’s response to televi- Psychology, 28, 129–137.
sion violence. In J. P. Murray, E. A. Rubinstein, & G. Comstock Lepper, M. R., Henderlong, J., & Gingras, I. (1999). Understanding
(Eds.), Television and social behaviour: Vol. 2. Television and social the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation—uses and
learning (pp. 43–180). Washington, DC: U.S. Government abuses of meta-analysis: Comment on Deci, Koestner, and Ryan
Printing Office. (1999). Psychological Bulletin, 125, 669–676.
Leimgruber, K. L., Shaw, A., Santos, L. R., & Olson, K. R. (2012). Lepper, M. R., & Malone, T. W. (1987). Intrinsic motivation and
Young children are more generous when others are aware of their instructional effectiveness in computer-based education. In R. E.
actions. PloS One, 7, e48292. Snow & M. J. Farr (Eds.), Aptitude, learning and instruction: Vol. 3.
Lejbak, L., Vrbancic, M., & Crossley, M. (2009). The female advan- Cognitive and affective process analyses (pp. 255–296). Hillsdale,
tage in object location memory is robust to verbalizability and NJ: Erlbaum.
mode of presentation of test stimuli. Brain and Cognition, 69, Lerner, C. (2004). “Accommodations” for the learning disabled: A
148–153. level playing field or affirmative action for elites? Vanderbilt Law
Lemaire, P., & Lecacheur, M. (2011). Age-related changes in chil- Review, 57, 1041–1122.
dren’s executive functions and strategy selection: A study in com- Lerner, J. V. (1993). The influence of child temperamental charac-
putational estimation. Cognitive Development, 26, 282–294. teristics on parent behaviors. In T. Luster & L. Okagaki (Eds.),
Lemaire, P., & Siegler, R. S. (1995). Four aspects of strategic change: Parenting: An ecological perspective (pp. 101–120). Hillsdale, NJ:
Contributions to children’s learning of multiplication. Journal of Erlbaum.
Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 83–97. Lerner, R. M., Brentano, C., Dowling, E. M., & Anderson, P. M.
Lemerise, E., & Dodge, K. A. (1993). The development of anger (2002). Positive youth development: Thriving as the basis of per-
and hostile interactions. In L. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), sonhood and civil society. New Directions for Youth Development,
Handbook of emotions (pp. 537–546). New York: Guilford Press. Fall, 11–33.
Lempers, J., Flavell, E., & Flavell, J. (1977). The development in very Leslie, A. (1994). ToMM, ToBY and agency: Core architecture
young children of tacit knowledge concerning visual perception. and domain specificity. In L. A. Hirschfeld (Ed.), Mapping the
Genetic Psychology Monographs, 95, 3–53. mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 119–148).
Lenhart, A., Rainie, L, & Lewis, O. (2001). Teenage life online: The rise Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
of the instant-message generation and the Internet’s impact on friend- Leslie, A. M. (1982). The perception of causality in infants. Perception,
ships and family relationships. Washington, DC: Pew Internet & 11, 173–186.
American Life Project. Leslie, A. M. (1984). Spatiotemporal continuity and the perception of
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New causality in infants. Perception, 13, 287–305.
York: Wiley. Leslie, A. M. (1987a). A “language of thought” approach to early
Lennington, J. B., Yang, Z., & Conover, J. C. (2003). Neural stem pretense. In J. Montangero, A. Tryphon, & S. Dionnet (Eds.),
cells and the regulation of adult neurogenesis. Reproductive Biology Symbolism and knowledge. (Cahiers de la Fondation Archives
and Endocrinology, 1, 99. Jean Piaget, No. 8, pp. 107–118). Geneva: Jean Piaget Archives
Leon, M. (1980). Coordination of intent and consequence informa- Foundation.
tion in children’s moral judgments. In F. Wilkening, J. Becker, & Leslie, A. M. (1987b). Pretense and representation in infancy: The
T. Trabasso (Eds.), Information integration by children (pp. 71–112). origins of theory of mind. Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Leslie, A. M. (1992). Pretense, autism, and the theory-of-mind mod-
Leonard, H. L., Goldberger, E. L., Rapoport, J. L., Cheslow, D .L., & ule. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 18–21.
Swedo, S. E. (1990). Childhood rituals: Normal development or Leslie, A. M. (1995). Pretending and believing: Issues in the theory
obsessive-compulsive symptoms? Journal of the American Academy of ToMM. In J. Mehler & S. Franck (Eds.), Cognition on cognition
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 17–23. (pp. 193–220). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Leonard, H. L., Swedo, S. E., Lenane, M. C., Rettew, D. C., Leslie, A. M. (2000). “Theory of mind” as a mechanism of selective
Hamburger, S. D., Bartko, J. J., & Rapoport, J. L. (1993). A 2- attention. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences
to 7-year follow-up study of 54 obsessive-compulsive children and (2nd ed., pp. 1235–1247). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 429–439. Leslie, A. M., Friedman, O., & German, T. P. (2004). Core mecha-
Leong, N. (2005). Beyond Breimhorst: Appropriate accommodation nisms in “theory of mind.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 528–533.
of students with learning disabilities on the SAT. Stanford Law Leslie, A. M., Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. R. (2008). The generative
Review, 57, 2135–2156. basis of natural number concepts. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12,
Leppänen, J. M., & Nelson, C. A. (2006). The development and neu- 213–218.
ral bases of facial emotion recognition. In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances Leslie, A. M., German, T. P., & Polizzi, P. (2005). Belief-desire rea-
in child development and behavior (pp. 207–246). London: Elsevier. soning as a process of selection. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 45–85.
R-76 REFERENCES
Leslie, A. M., & Keeble, S. (1987). Do six-month-old infants perceive S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychol-
causality? Cognition, 25, 265–288. ogy (pp. 179–202). New York: Guilford Press.
Leslie, A. M., & Polizzi, P. (1998). Inhibitory processing in the false Lew, A. R. (2011). Looking beyond the boundaries: Time to put
belief task: Two conjectures. Developmental Science, 1, 247–253. landmarks back on the cognitive map? Psychological Bulletin, 137,
Leslie, A. M., & Thaiss, L. (1992). Domain specificity in concep- 484–507.
tual development: Neuropsychological evidence from autism. Lew, A. R., Foster, K. A., Crowther, H. L., & Green, M. (2004).
Cognition, 43, 225–251. Indirect landmark use at 6 months of age in a spatial orientation
Lettvin, J. Y., Maturana, H. R., McCulloch, W. S., & Pitts, W. H. task. Infant Behavior and Development, 27, 81–90.
(1959). What the frog’s eye tells the frog’s brain. Proceedings of the Lewandowski, L. J., Lovett, B. J., Parolin, R., Gordon, M., &
Institute of Radio Engineers, 47, 1940–1951. Codding, R. S. (2007). Extended time accommodations and the
Leung, A. K.-Y., & Cohen, D. (2011). Within- and between-culture mathematics performance of students with and without ADHD.
variation: Individual differences and the cultural logics of honor, Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 25, 17–28.
face, and dignity cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Lewin, K. (1946). Behavior and development as a function of the
Psychology, 100, 507–526. total situation. In L. Carmichael (Ed.), Manual of child psychology
LeVay, S. (1993). The sexual brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (pp. 918–970). New York: Wiley.
LeVay, S., Wiesel, T. N., & Hubel, D. H. (1980). The development of Lewis, D. (1969). Convention: A philosophical study. Cambridge, MA:
ocular dominance columns in normal and visually deprived mon- Harvard University Press.
keys. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 191, 1–51. Lewis, M. (1993). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride,
Leve, L., Kerr, D., Shaw, D., Ge, X., Neiderhiser, J., Scaramella, L., shame, and guilt. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland, Handbook of
Reid, J., Conger, R., & Reiss, D. (2010). Infant pathways to exter- emotions (pp. 623–636). New York: Guilford Press.
nalizing behavior: Evidence of Genotype × Environment interac- Lewis, M. (2008). The emergence of human emotion. In M. Lewis,
tion. Child Development, 81, 340–356. J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions
Lever, R., & Sénéchal, M. (2011). Discussing stories: On how a dia- (3rd ed., pp. 304–319). New York: Guilford Press.
logic reading intervention improves kindergartners’ oral narrative Lewis, M., Alessandri, S. M., & Sullivan, M. W. (1992). Differences
construction. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 1–24. in shame and pride as a function of children’s gender and task dif-
Levesque, R. J. R. (2007). Adolescents, media and the law: What devel- ficulty. Child Development, 63, 630–638.
opmental science reveals and free speech requires. New York: Oxford Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisi-
University Press. tion of self. New York: Plenum.
Levine, M. P., & Piran, N. (2005). Eating disorders. In T. P. Lewis, M., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Jaskir, J. (1985). Individual differences
Gullotta & G. R. Adams (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent behavioral in visual self-recognition as a function of mother-infant attach-
problems: Evidence-based approaches to prevention and treatment ment relationship. Developmental Psychology, 21, 1181–1187.
(pp. 363–386). New York: Springer. Lewis, M., Lamm, C., Segalowitz, S., Stieben, J., & Zelazo, P. D.
Levine, S. C., Huttenlocher, J., Taylor, A., & Langrock, A. (1999). (2006). Neurophysiological correlates of emotion regulation in
Early sex differences in spatial skills. Developmental Psychology, 35, children and adolescents. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18,
940–949. 430–443.
Levine, S. C., Jordan, N. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (1992). Development Lewis, M., & Michalson, L. (1983). Children’s emotions and moods:
of calculation abilities in young children. Journal of Experimental Developmental theory and measurement. New York: Plenum Press.
Child Psychology, 53, 72–103. Lewis, M., Ramsay, D. S., & Kawakami, K. (1993). Differences
Levine, S. C., Suriyakham, L. W., Rowe, M. L., Huttenlocher, J., & between Japanese infants and Caucasian American infants in
Gunderson, E. A. (2010). What counts in the development of behavioral and cortisol response to inoculation. Child Development,
young children’s number knowledge? Developmental Psychology, 64, 1722–1731.
46, 1309–1319. Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., Stanger, C., & Weiss, M. (1989). Self-
Levinson, D. J. (1986). A conception of adult development. American development and self-conscious emotions. Child Development, 60,
Psychologist, 41, 3–13. 146–156.
Levinson, S. C. (1997). From outer to inner space: Linguistic catego- Lewis, P., & Walsh, V. (2002). Neuropsychology: Time out of mind.
ries and non-linguistic thinking. In E. Pederson & J. Nuyts (Eds.), Current Biology, 12, R9–R11.
With language in mind: The relationship between linguistic and con- Lewis, T. L., & Maurer D. (2005). Multiple sensitive periods in
ceptual representation (pp. 13–45). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge human visual development: Evidence from visually deprived chil-
University Press. dren. Developmental Psychobiology, 46, 163–183.
Levitt, P., Harvey, J. A., Friedman, E., Simansky, K., & Murphy, Lewis, T. L., & Maurer, D. (2009). Effects of early pattern depri-
E. H. (1997). New evidence for neurotransmitter influences on vation on visual development. Optometry and Vision Science, 86,
brain development. Trends in Neuroscience, 20, 269–274. 640–646.
Levy, F., & Hay, D. A. (Eds.). (2001). Attention, genes and ADHD. Lewkowicz, D. J., Leo, I., & Simion, F. (2010). Intersensory percep-
Philadelphia: Brunner-Routledge. tion at birth: Newborns match non-human primate faces & voices.
Levy, F., Hay, D., McStephen, M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I. (1997). Infancy, 15, 46–60.
ADHD: A category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large- Li, K. Z., Lindenberger, U., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2001).
scale twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Walking while memorizing: Age-related differences in compensa-
Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 737–744. tory behavior. Psychological Science, 12, 230–237.
Levy, N. (2004). What makes us moral? Crossing the boundaries of biol- Li, Q. (2007). New bottle but old wine: A research of cyberbullying in
ogy. Oxford, UK: Oneworld. schools. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 1777–1791.
Levy, S. R., Plaks, J. E., & Dweck, C. S. (1999). Modes of social Li, S. C. (2002). Connecting the many levels and facets of cognitive
thought: Implicit theories and social understanding. In aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 38–43.
REFERENCES R-77
Li, S. C., Hämmerer, D., Müller, V., Hommel, B., & Lindenberger, U. Lillycrop, K. A., & Burdge, G. C. (2011). Epigenetic changes in early
(2009). Lifespan development of stimulus-response conflict cost: life and future risk of obesity. International Journal of Obesity, 35,
Similarities and differences between maturation and senescence. 72–83.
Psychological Research, 73, 777–785. Lin, M. C., Harwood, J., & Hummert, M. L. (2008). Young adults’
Li, Y., Sauve, Y., Li, D., Lund, R. D., & Raisman, G. (2003). intergenerational communication schemas in Taiwan and U.S.A.
Transplanted olfactory ensheathing cells promote regeneration Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 27, 28–50.
of cut adult rat optic nerve axons. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, Linares, L. O., Li, M., Shrout, P. E., Brody, G. H., & Pettit, G. S.
7783–7788. (2007). Placement shift, sibling relationship quality, and child
Liben, L. (1999). Developing an understanding of external spatial outcomes in foster care: A controlled study. Journal of Family
representations. In I. E. Sigel (Ed.), Development of mental repre- Psychology, 21, 736–743..
sentations: Theories and applications (pp. 297–321). Hillsdale, NJ: Lindberg, M. A. (1980). Is knowledge base development a neces-
Erlbaum. sary and sufficient condition for memory development? Journal of
Liben, L. S., & Bigler, R. S. (2002). The developmental course of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 401–410.
gender differentiation: Conceptualizing, measuring, and evaluat- Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (1994). Sensory functioning and
ing constructs and pathways. Monographs of the Society for Research intelligence in old age: A strong connection. Psychology and Aging,
in Child Development, 67 (2, Serial No. 269). 9, 339–355.
Liben, L. S., Bigler, R. S., & Krogh, H. K. (2001). Pink and blue col- Lindsay, D. S., & Read, J. D. (1994). Incest resolution psychotherapy
lar jobs: Children’s judgments of job status and job aspirations in and memories of childhood sexual abuse: A cognitive perspective.
relation to sex of worker. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 281–338.
79, 346–363. Linkenhoker, B. A., von der Ohe, C. G., & Knudsen, E. I. (2005).
Libertus, K., & Needham, A. (2010). Teach to reach: The effects of Anatomical traces of juvenile learning in the auditory system of
active vs. passive reaching experiences on action and perception. adult barn owls. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 93–98.
Vision Research, 50, 2750–2757. Linn, M. C., & Hsi, S. (2000). Computers, teachers, peers: Science
Liddle, H. A., Dakof, G. A., Turner, R. M., Henderson, C. E., & learning partners. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Greenbaum, P. E. (2008). Treating adolescent drug abuse: A ran- Litt, I. F. (1995). Pubertal and psychosocial development: Implications
domized trial comparing multidimensional family therapy and for pediatricians. Pediatrics in Review, 16, 243–247.
cognitive behavior therapy. Addiction, 103, 1660–1670. Little, A. H., Lipsitt, L. P., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1984). Classical
Lidz, J., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. (2001). Kidz in the ‘hood: conditioning and retention of the infant’s eyelid response: Effects
Syntactic bootstrapping and the mental lexicon. IRCS Technical of age and interstimulus interval. Journal of Experimental Child
Reports, 01- 01. Psychology, 37, 512–524.
Lie, N. (1992). Follow-ups of children with attention deficit hyperac- Liu, C.-J., Kemper, S., & Bovaird, J. A. (2009). Comprehension
tivity disorder (ADHD): A review. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, of health-related written materials by older adults. Educational
368, 1–40. Gerontology, 35, 653–668.
Lieberman, A. F. (1977). Preschoolers’ competence with a peer. Liu, D., Diorio. J., Tannenbaum, B., Caldji, C., Francis, D., Freedman,
Relations with attachment and peer experience. Child Development, A, Sharma. S., Pearson, D, Plotsky, P. M., & Meaney, M. J.
48, 1277–1287. (1997). Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and
Lieberman, P. (1991). Uniquely human: The evolution of speech, thought hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science, 277,
and selfless behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 1659–1662.
Liegeois, F., Baldeweg, T., Connelly, A., Gadian, D. G., Mishkin, Liu, D., Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (2007). Components
M., & Vargha-Khadem, F. (2003). Language fMRI abnormali- of young children’s trait understanding: Behavior-to-trait infer-
ties associated with FOXP2 gene mutation. Nature Neuroscience, ences and trait-to-behavior predictions. Child Development, 78,
6, 1230–1237. 1543–1558.
Liew, J., Eisenberg, N., & Reiser, M. (2004). Preschoolers’ effort- Liu, D., Vanderbilt, K. E., & Heyman, G. D. (2013). Selective
ful control and negative emotionality, immediate reactions to trust: Children’s use of intention and outcome of past testimony.
disappointment, and quality of social functioning. Journal of Developmental Psychology, 49, 439–445.
Experimental Child Psychology, 89, 298–313. Liu, H., Heath, S. C., Sobin, C., Roos, J. L., Galke, B. L., Blundell,
Light, L. (2000). Memory changes in adulthood. In S. H. Qualls & M. L., Lenane, M., Robertson, B., Wijsman, E. M., Rapoport,
N. Abeles (Eds.), Psychology and the aging revolution: How we J. L., Gogos, J. A., & Karayiorgou, M. (2002). Genetic variation
adapt to longer life (pp. 73–97). Washington, DC: American at the 22q11 PRODH2/DGCR6 locus presents an unusual pattern
Psychological Association. and increases susceptibility to schizophrenia. Proceedings of the
Lightfoot, C. (1997). The culture of adolescent risk-taking. New York: National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 99, 3717–3722.
Guilford Press. Liu, R. X., Lin, W., & Chen, Z. Y. (2010). School performance, peer
Lightfoot, D. (1991). How to set parameters: Arguments from language association, psychological and behavioral adjustments: A com-
change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. parison between Chinese adolescents with and without siblings.
Liittschwager, J. C., & Markman, E. M. (1994). Sixteen- and Journal of Adolescence, 33, 411–417.
24-month-olds’ use of mutual exclusivity as a default assumption Livingstone, S., Olafsson, K., & Staksrud, E. (2011). Social network-
in second-label learning. Developmental Psychology, 30, 955–968. ing, age and privacy. LSE, London: EUKidsOnline. http://www2
Lillard, A. S. (2001). Pretend play as twin earth: A social-cognitive .lse.ac.uk/media@lse/research/EUKidsOnline/ShortSNS.pdf
analysis. Developmental Review, 21, 495–531. Lizardi, H., Klein, D. N., Ouimette, P. C., Riso, L. P., Anderson,
Lillard, A. S., & Peterson, J. (2011). The immediate impact of dif- R. L., & Donaldson, S. K. (1995). Reports of the childhood home
ferent types of television on young children’s executive function. environment in early-onset dysthymia and episodic major depres-
Pediatrics, 128, 644–649. sion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 132–139.
R-78 REFERENCES
LoBue, V. (2013). What are we so afraid of? How early attention of Chinese and Japanese neonates. Pediatrics International, 47,
shapes our most common fears. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 446–451.
38–42. Loo, S. K., Shtir, C., Doyle, A. E., Mick, E., McGough, J. J.,
LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Detecting the snake in the grass: McCracken, J., Biederman, J., Smalley, S. L., Cantor, R. M.,
Attention to fear-relevant stimuli by adults and young children. Faraone, S. V., & Nelson, S. F. (2012). Genome-wide association
Psychological Science, 19, 284–289. study of intelligence: Additive effects of novel brain expressed
LoBue, V., Rakison, D. H., & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Threat percep- genes. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
tion across the lifespan: Evidence for multiple converging path- Psychiatry, 51, 432–440.
ways. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 375–379. Lopez, R. (1965). Hyperactivity in twins. Canadian Psychiatric
Locke, J. (1690/1964). An essay concerning human understanding. Association Journal, 10, 421–426.
(A. D. Woozley, Ed.). New York: Meridian. Lopez-Morteo, G., & Lopez, G. (2007). Computer support for learn-
Lockhart, K. L. (1981). The development of knowledge about unifor- ing mathematics: A learning environment based on recreational
mities in the environment: A comparative analysis of the child’s learning objects. Computers and Education, 48, 618–641.
understanding of social, moral, and physical rules. Dissertation Lopez-Williams, A., Chacko, A., Wymbs, B. T., Onyango, A. N.,
Abstracts International, 41, 2793. Fabiano, G. A., Seymour, K. E., Chronis, A. M., Pelham, W. E., &
Lockhart, K. L., Abrahams, B., & Osherson, D. N. (1977). Morris, T. L. (2005). Athletic performance and social behavior as
Children’s understanding of uniformity in the environment. Child predictors of peer acceptance in children diagnosed with attention-
Development, 48, 1521–1531. deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral
Lockhart, K. L., Chang, B., & Story, T. (2002). Young children’s Disorders, 13, 173–181.
beliefs about the stability of traits: Protective optimism? Child Lord, C., & Jones, R. M. (2012). Annual research review: Re-thinking
Development, 73, 1408–1430. the classification of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child
Lockhart, K. L., Keil, F. C., & Aw, J. (2013). A bias for the natural? Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 490–509.
Children’s beliefs about traits acquired through effort, bribes, or Lorenz, K. (1943). Innate forms of potential experience. Zeitschrift für
medicine. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1669–1682. Tierpsychologie, 5, 235–409.
Lockhart, K. L., Nakashima, N., Inagaki, K., & Keil, F. C. (2008). From Lorenz, K. (1965). Evolution and modification of behavior. Chicago:
ugly duckling to swan? Japanese and American beliefs about the sta- University of Chicago Press.
bility and origins of traits. Cognitive Development, 23, 155–179. Lorenz, K. (1971). Part and parcel in animal and human societies.
Loeb, S., Bridges, M., Bassok, D., Fuller, B., & Rumberger, R. W. In Studies in animal and human behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 115–195).
(2007). How much is too much? The influence of preschool cen- Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
ters on children’s social and cognitive development. Economics of Losoya, S. H., & Eisenberg, N. (2001). Affective empathy. In J. A.
Education Review, 26, 52–66. Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and
Loeber, R. (1996). Developmental continuity, change, and pathways measurement (pp. 21–43). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
in male juvenile problem behaviors and delinquency. In J. D. Loukusa, S., Leinonen, E., & Ryder, N. (2007). Development of prag-
Hawkins (Ed.), Delinquency and crime: Current theories (pp. 1–27). matic language comprehension in Finnish speaking children. First
New York: Cambridge University Press. Language, 27, 279–296.
Loeber, R., Burke, J., & Pardini, D. A. (2009). Perspectives on oppo- Lourenco, O. (2003). Making sense of Turiel’s dispute with Kohlberg:
sitional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, and psychopathic The case of the child’s moral competence. New Ideas in Psychology,
features. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied 21, 43–68.
Disciplines, 50, 133–142. Lourenco, S. F., Addy, D., & Huttenlocher, J. (2009). Location rep-
Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1986). Family factors as corre- resentation in enclosed spaces: What types of information afford
lates and predictors of juvenile conduct problems and delinquency. young children an advantage? Journal of Experimental Child
In M. H. Tonry & N. Morris (Eds.), Crime and justice: An annual Psychology, 104, 313–325.
review of research ( Vol. 7, pp. 29–149). Chicago: University of Lourenco, S. F., & Huttenlocher, J. (2007). Using geometry to specify
Chicago Press. location: Implications for spatial coding in children and nonhu-
Loftus, E. F. (1992). When a lie becomes memory’s truth: Memory man animals. Psychological Research, 71, 252–264.
distortion after exposure to misinformation. Current Directions in Lourenco, S. F., & Longo, M. R. (2010). General magnitude represen-
Psychological Science, 1, 121–123. tation in human infants. Psychological Science, 21, 873–881.
Loftus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal educational
Psychologist, 48, 518–537. and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. Journal of
Loftus, E. F., & Davis, D. (2006). Recovered memories. Annual Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 3–9.
Review of Clinical Psychology, 2, 469–498. Lovejoy, O. C. (1981). The origin of man. Science, 221, 341–350.
Loftus, E. F., Garry, M., & Feldman, J. (1994). Forgetting sexual Loveland, K. A., & Landry, S. H. (1986). Joint attention and language
trauma: What does it mean when 38% forget? Journal of Consulting in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Autism and
and Clinical Psychology, 62, 1177–1181. Developmental Disorders, 16, 335–349.
Loftus, E. F., & Ketcham, K. (1994). The myth of repressed mem- Loveland, K. K., & Olley, J. G. (1979). The effect of external reward
ory: False memories and allegations of sexual abuse. New York: on interest and quality of task performance in children of high and
St. Martin’s Press. low intrinsic motivation. Child Development, 50, 1207–1210.
Loftus, E. F., Milo, E. M., & Paddock, J. R. (1995). The acciden- Lowrey, L. G. (1940). Personality distortion and early institutional
tal executioner: Why psychotherapy must be informed by science. care. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 10, 576–585.
Counseling Psychologist, 23, 300–309. Lu, L. H., Leonard, C. M., Thompson, P. M., Kan, E., Jolley, J.,
Loo, K. K., Ohgi, S., Zhu, H., Howard, J., & Chen, L. (2005). Welcome, S. E., Toga, A. W., & Sowell, E. R. (2007). Normal
Cross-cultural comparison of the neurobehavioral characteristics developmental changes in inferior frontal gray matter are associated
REFERENCES R-79
with improvement in phonological processing: A longitudinal Lynam, D., Moffitt, T. E., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1993).
MRI analysis. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 1092–1099. Explaining the relation between IQ and delinquency: Class, race,
Lucariello, J., & Mindolovich, C. (1995). The development of com- test motivation, school failure, or self-control? Journal of Abnormal
plex metarepresentational reasoning: The case of situational irony. Psychology, 102, 187–196.
Cognitive Development, 10, 551–576. Lynch, W. C., Heil, D. P., Wagner, E., & Havens, M. D. (2008).
Lucy, J. A. (1997). Linguistic relativity. Annual Review of Anthropology, Body dissatisfaction mediates the association between body mass
26, 291–312. index and risky weight control behaviors among White and Native
Ludwig, J., & Phillips, D. A. (2008). Long-term effects of Head American adolescent girls. Appetite, 51, 210–213.
Start on low-income children. Annals of the New York Academy of Lyons, D. E., Damrosch, D. H., Lin, J. K., Macris, D. M., & Keil,
Sciences, 1136, 257–268. F. C. (2011). The scope and limits of overimitation in the trans-
Lumey, L. H., Ravelli, A. C., Wiessing, L. G., Koppe, J. G., Treffers, mission of artefact culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
P. E., & Stein, Z. A. (1993). The Dutch famine birth cohort Society of London, Series B, 366, 1158–1167.
study: Design, validation of exposure, and selected characteris- Lyons, D. E., Santos, L. R., & Keil, F. C. (2006). Reflections of other
tics of subjects after 43 years follow-up. Paediatriac and Perinatal minds: How primate social cognition can inform the function of
Epidemiology, 7, 354–367. mirror neurons. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16, 230–239.
Lumsden, A., & Krumlauf, R. (1996). Patterning the vertebrate neur- Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure
axis. Science, 274, 1109–1115. of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
Lumsden, A., & Massimo, G. (1997). Neocortical neurons: Where do the USA, 104, 19751–19756.
they come from? Science, 278, 402–403. Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics (Vols. 1–2). London: Cambridge University
Lund, J. S., Boothe, R. G., & Lund, R. D. (1977). Development Press.
of neurons in the visual cortex (area 17) of the monkey (Macaca Lyons, M. (2009). Cry baby cry, make your mother buy? Evolution
nemestrina): A Golgi study from fetal day 127 to postnatal matu- of tears, smiles, and reciprocity potential. Behavioral and Brain
rity. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 176, 149–187. Sciences, 32, 399.
Lundberg, S. (2005). Sons, daughters, and parental behaviour. Oxford Lyons-Ruth, K., Alpern, L., & Repacholi, B. (1993). Disorganized
Review of Economic Policy, 21, 340–356. infant attachment classification and maternal psychosocial prob-
Lundgren, B., & Pettersson, B. (2006). The new Swedish public health lems as predictors of hostile-aggressive behavior in the preschool
policy and equity in health. Presentation at the EU Expert Group classroom. Child Development, 64, 572–585.
on Social Determinants and Health Inequalities in Luxembourg, Lyons-Ruth, K., Bureau J. F., Riley, C. D., & Atlas-Corbett, A. F.
March 2-3, 2006. (2009). Socially indiscriminate attachment behavior in the Strange
Luo, Y. (2011). Three-month-old infants attribute goals to a non- Situation: Convergent and discriminant validity in relation to
human agent. Developmental Science, 14, 453–460. caregiving risk, later behavior problems, and attachment insecu-
Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Can a self-propelled box have a rity. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 355–372.
goal? Psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants. Psychological Lytton, H. (1980). Parent-child interaction: The socialization process
Science, 16, 601–608. observed in twin and singleton families. New York: Plenum.
Luo, Y., Kaufman L., & Baillargeon R. (2009). Young infants’ reason- Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents’ differential socialization of
ing about physical events involving inert and self-propelled objects. boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 267–296.
Cognitive Psychology, 58, 441–486. Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1993). Self-perpetuating
Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic properties of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and
Books. Social Psychology, 65, 339–349.
Luria, A. R. (1974). Ob istoricheskom razvitii poznavatel’nykh protsessov
[Historical development of cognitive processes]. Moscow: Nauka. Ma, D. K., Bonaguidi, M. A., Ming, G. L., & Song, H. (2009). Adult
Lust, B. S., Suner, M., & Whitman, J. (Eds.). (1994). Syntactic theory neural stem cells in the mammalian central nervous system. Cell
and first language acquisition: Cross-linguistic perspectives. Hillsdale, Research, 19, 672–682.
NJ: Erlbaum. Ma, L., & Xu, F. (2013). Preverbal infants infer intentional agents
Lutkenhaus, P., Grossmann, K. E., & Grossmann, K. (1985). Infant- from the perception of regularity. Developmental Psychology, 49,
mother attachment at twelve months and style of interaction with a 1330–1337.
stranger at the age of three years. Child Development, 56, 1538–1542. Macari, S. L., Campbell, D., Gengoux, G. W., Saulnier, C. A., Klin,
Luttikhuizen dos Santos, E. S., de Kieviet, J. F., Königs, M., van A. J., & Chawarska, K. (2012). Predicting developmental status
Elburg, R. M., & Oosterlaan, J. (2013). Predictive value of the from 12 to 24 months in infants at risk for autism spectrum dis-
Bayley Scales of Infant Development on development of very order: A preliminary report. Journal of Autism and Developmental
preterm/very low birth weight children: A meta-analysis. Early Disorders, 42, 2636–2647.
Human Development, 9, 487–496. Macchi Cassia, V., Turati, C., & Simion, F. (2004). Can a nonspecific
Lutz, D. J., & Keil, F. C. (2002). Early understanding of the division bias toward top-heavy patterns explain newborns’ face preference?
of cognitive labor. Child development, 73, 1073–1084. Psychological Science, 15, 379–383.
Lynam, D. R. (1997). Pursuing the psychopath: Capturing the Maccoby, E. E. (1990). Gender and relationships: A developmental
fledgling psychopath in a nomological net. Journal of Abnormal account. American Psychologist, 45, 513–520.
Psychology, 106, 425–438. Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming
Lynam, D. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer- together. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.
Loeber, M. (2007). Longitudinal evidence that psychopathy scores Maccoby, E. E. (2000). Parenting and its effects on children: On read-
in early adolescence predict adult psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal ing and misreading behavior genetics. Annual Review of Psychology,
Psychology, 116, 155–165. 51, 1–27.
R-80 REFERENCES
Maccoby, E. E. (2001). The gender of child and parent as factors in fam- MacWhinney, B. (Ed.). (1999). The emergence of language. Mahwah,
ily dynamics. Presented at the 9th National Symposium on Family NJ: Erlbaum.
Issues: Children’s influence on family dynamics: The neglected Maden, M. (2000). The role of retinoic acid in embryonic and post-
side of family relationships. University Park, PA: Pennsylvania embryonic development. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society, 59,
State University. 65–73.
Maccoby, E. E. (2003). The gender of child and parent as factors in Madsen, M. C. (1971). Developmental and cross-cultural differences
family dynamics. In A. Crouter & A. Booth (Eds.), Children’s in the cooperative and competitive behavior of young children.
influence on family dynamics: The neglected side of family relation- Journal of Cross- Cultural Psychology, 2, 365–371.
ships (pp. 191–206). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Madsen, M. C., & Connor, C. (1973). Cooperative and competitive
Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differ- behavior of retarded and nonretarded children at two ages. Child
ences. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press. Development, 44, 175–178.
Maccoby, E. E., & Lewis, C. C. (2003). Less day care or different day Maes, M. (1997). The immune pathophysiology of major depres-
care? Child Development, 74, 1069–1075. sion. In A. Honig & H. van Praag (Eds.), Wiley series on clinical
Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the con- and neurobiological advantages in psychiatry: Vol. 3. Depression:
text of the family: Parent-child interaction. In P. H. Messen & Neurobiological, psychopathological and therapeutic advances
E. M. Hetherington (Eds), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. (pp. 197–215). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Socialization, personality and social development (4th ed., pp. 1–102). Maffei, L., Berardi, N., Domenici, L., Parisi, V., & Pizzorusso, T.
New York: Wiley. (1992). Nerve growth factor (NGF) prevents the shift in ocular
MacDermot, K. D., Bonora, E., Sykes, N., Coupe, A. M., Lai, C. S., dominance distribution of visual cortical neurons in monocularly
Vernes, S. C., Vargha-Khadem, F., McKenzie, F., Smith, R. L., deprived rats. Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 4651–4662.
Monaco, A. P., & Fisher, S. E. (2005). Identification of FOXP2 Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D.,
truncation as a novel cause of developmental speech and language Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Frith, C. D. (2000).
deficits. American Journal of Human Genetics, 76, 1074–1080. Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi
MacDonald, S., Uesiliana, K., & Hayne, H. (2000). Cross-cultural drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA,
and gender differences in childhood amnesia. Memory, 8, 365–376. 97, 4398–4403.
Macdonald-Wallis, K., Jago, R., Page, A. S., Brockman, R., & Maguire, E. A., Woollett, K., & Spiers, H. J. (2006). London taxi
Thompson, J.L. (2011). School-based friendship networks and drivers and bus drivers: A structural MRI and neuropsychological
children’s physical activity: A spatial analytical approach. Social analysis. Hippocampus, 16, 1091–1101.
Science & Medicine, 73, 6–12. Mahajan, N., Barnes, J. L., Blanco, M., & Santos, L. R. (2009).
MacEvoy, J. P., & Asher, S. R. (2012). When friends disappoint: Boys’ Enumeration of objects and substances in non-human primates:
and girls’ responses to transgressions of friendship expectations. Experiments with brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus). Developmental
Child Development, 83, 104–119. Psychology, 12, 920–928.
Maciejewski, P. K., Prigerson, H. G., & Mazure, C. M. (2000). Self- Mahut, H. (1953). Breed differences in the dog’s emotional behaviour.
efficacy as a mediator between stressful life events and depressive Canadian Journal of Psychology, 12, 35–44.
symptoms: Difference based on history of prior depression. British Maia, T. V., Cooney, R. E., & Peterson, B. S. (2008). The neural
Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 373–378. bases of obsessive-compulsive disorder in children and adults.
Maciejewski, P. K., Prigerson, H. G., & Mazure, C. M. (2001). Sex Development and Psychopathology, 20, 1251–1283.
differences in event-related risk for major depression. Psychological Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as
Medicine, 31, 593–604. disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation.
MacKenzie, M. J., Nicklas, E., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Waldfogel, J. In M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds.),
(2011). Who spanks infants and toddlers? Evidence from the Attachment in the preschool years (pp. 121–160). Chicago: University
Fragile Families and Child Well-Being Study. Children and Youth of Chicago Press.
Services Review, 33, 1364–1373. Mainemer, H., Gilman, L.C, & Ames, E. W. (1998). Parenting stress
MacKinnon, D. P. (2006). Introduction to statistical mediation analysis. in families adopting children from Romanian orphanages. Journal
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. of Family Issues, 19, 164–180.
Mackintosh, N. J. (1995). Cyril Burt: Fraud or framed? New York: Majid, A., Bowerman, M., Kita, S., Haun, D. B. M., & Levinson,
Oxford University Press. S. C. (2004). Can language restructure cognition? The case of
Mackintosh, N. J. (1998). IQ and human intelligence. Oxford, UK: space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 108–114.
Oxford University Press. Malakoff, M. (1988). The effect of language of instruction on reason-
MacLean, K. (2003). The impact of institutionalization on child ing in bilingual children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 17–38.
development. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 853–884. Malakoff, M., & Hakuta, K. (1991). Translation skill and metalin-
MacLean, N., & Hall, B. K. (1987). Cell commitment and differentia- guistic awareness in bilinguals. In E. Bialystok (Ed.), Language
tion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. processing and language awareness by bilingual children. Cambridge,
MacLeod, C. (2007). Chinese sports schools feel an urgency to find UK: Cambridge University Press.
gold. USA Today, June 14, p. 1A. Malatesta, C., Culver, C., Tesman, J., & Shepard, B. (1989). The
MacManus, D., Laurens, K. R., Walker, E. F., Brasfield, J. L., development of emotion expression during the first two years of
Riaz, M., & Hodgins, S. (2012). Movement abnormalities and life. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
psychotic-like experiences in childhood: Markers of developing 54, 1–104.
schizophrenia? Psychological Medicine, 42, 99–109. Malatesta-Magai, C. (1991). Emotional socialization: Its role in per-
Macnamara, J. (1982). Names for things: A study of child language. sonality and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti &
Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press. S. L. Toth (Eds.), Internalizing and externalizing expressions of
REFERENCES R-81
dysfunction: Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology Manning, W. D., Giordano, P. C., & Longmore, M. A. (2006).
(Vol. 2, pp. 203–224). New York: Guilford Press. Hooking up: The relationship contexts of “nonrelationship” sex.
Malinosky-Rummell, R., & Hansen, D. J. (1993). Long-term conse- Journal of Adolescent Research, 21, 459–483.
quences of childhood physical abuse. Psychological Bulletin, 114, Mannuzza, S., Klein, R. G., Bessler, A., Malloy, P., & LaPadula, M.
68–79. (1993). Adult outcome of hyperactive boys: Educational achieve-
Man, L. X., & Chang, B. (2006). Maternal cigarette smoking during ment, occupational rank, and psychiatric status. Archives of General
pregnancy increases the risk of having a child with a congenital Psychiatry, 50, 565–576.
digital anomaly. Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, 117, 301–308. Manton, K. C., Gu, X. L., & Ukraintseva, S. V. (2005). Declining
Manago, A. M., Taylor, T., & Greenfield, P. M. (2012). Me and my prevalence of dementia in the U.S. elderly population. Advances in
400 friends: The anatomy of college students’ facebook networks, Gerontology, 16, 30–37.
their communication patterns, and well-being. Developmental Maratsos, M. (1998). The acquisition of grammar. In W. Damon
Psychology, 48, 369–380. (Series Ed.) & D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of
Mancillas, A. (2006). Challenging the stereotypes about only chil- child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language (5th
dren: A review of the literature and implications for practice. ed., pp. 421–466). New York: Wiley.
Journal of Counseling and Development, 84, 268–275. Marcil, A., Dumontier, E., Chamberland, M., Camper, S. A., &
Mandara, J., & Pikes, C. L. (2008). Guilt trips and love withdrawal: Drouin, J. (2003). Pitx1 and Pitx2 are required for development of
Does mothers’ use of psychological control predict depressive hindlimb buds. Development, 130, 45–55.
symptoms among African American adolescents? Family Relations, Marcotte, D., Fortin, L., Potvin, P., & Papillon, M. (2002). Gender
57, 602–612. difference in depressive symptoms during adolescence: Role of
Mandinach, E. B., Bridgeman, B., Cahalan-Laitusis, C., & Trapani, C. gender-typed characteristics, self-esteem, body image, stressful
(2005). The impact of extended time on SAT test performance. life events and pubertal stress. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral
College Board Research Report No. 2005-8. New York: The Disorders, 10, 29–42.
College Board. Marcus, G. F. (1993). Negative evidence in language acquisition.
Mandler, J. M. (1998). Representation. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & Cognition, 46, 53–85.
D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Marcus, G. F. (1995). Children’s overregularization of English plurals:
Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language (5th ed., pp. 255–308). A quantitative analysis. Journal of Child Language, 22, 447–459.
New York: Wiley. Marcus, G. F. (2001). The algebraic mind: Integrating connectionism
Mandler, J. M. (2000). Perceptual and conceptual processes in and cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
infancy. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 3–36. Marcus, G. (2004). The birth of the mind: How a tiny number of genes
Mandler, J. M. (2004a). Thought before language. Trends in Cognitive creates the complexities of human thought. New York: Basic Books.
Sciences, 8, 508–513. Marcus, G. F., Brinkmann, U., Clahsen, H., Wiese, R., & Pinker,
Mandler, J. M. (2004b). The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual S. (1995). German inflection: The exception that proves the rule.
thought. New York: Oxford University Press. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 189–256.
Mandler, J. M. (2012). On the spatial foundations of the conceptual Marcus, G. F., Fernandes, K. J., &. Johnson, S. P. (2007). Infant rule
system and its enrichment. Cognitive Science, 36, 421– 451. learning facilitated by speech. Psychological Science, 18, 387–391.
Mandler, J. M., & Bauer, P. (1989). The cradle of categorization: Is the Marcus, G. F., & Fisher, S. E. (2003). FOXP2 in focus: What can
basic level basic? Cognitive Development, 3, 247–264. genes tell us about speech and language? Trends in Cognitive
Mandler, J. M., & McDonough, L. (1993). Concept formation in Sciences, 7, 257–262.
infancy. Cognitive Development, 8, 291–318. Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen,
Mandler, J. M., & McDonough, L. (1995). Long-term recall of event T. J., & Xu, F. (1992). Overregularization in language acquisition.
sequences in infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57,
457–474. 1–182.
Mandler, J. M., & McDonough, L. (1996). Drinking and driving don’t Marcus, G. F., Vijayan, S., Bandi Rao, S., & Vishton, P. M. (1999).
mix: Inductive generalization in infancy. Cognition, 59, 307–335. Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science, 283, 77–80.
Mandler, J. M., & McDonough, L. (1998). On developing a knowl- Marenco, S., & Weinberger, D. R. (2000). The neurodevelopmen-
edge base in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1274–1288. tal hypothesis of schizophrenia: Following a trail of evidence from
Mandler, J. M., & McDonough, L. (1998). Studies in inductive infer- cradle to grave. Developmental Psychopathology, 12, 501–527.
ence in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 60–96. Mares, M. L., & Woodard, E. (2005). Positive effects of television on
Mangelsdorf, S., McHale, J., Diener, M., Goldstein, L., & Lehn, L. children’s social interactions: A meta-analysis. Media Psychology,
(2000). Infant attachment: Contributions of infant temperament 7, 301–322.
and maternal characteristics. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, Mareschal, D., & Shultz, T. R. (1996). Generative connectionist
175–196. networks and constructivist cognitive development. Cognitive
Manger, T., Eikeland, O., & Asbjornsen, A. (2001). Effects of social– Development, 11, 571–603.
cognitive training on students’ empathy. Swiss Journal of Psychology, Mareschal, D., & Tan, S. H. (2007). Flexible and context-dependent
60, 82–88. categorization by eighteen-month-olds. Child Development, 78,
Mangham, L. J., Petrou, S., Doyle, L. W., Draper, E. S., & Marlow, 19–37.
N. (2009). The cost of preterm birth throughout childhood in Maret, S. M. (1983). Attractiveness ratings of photographs of blacks
England and Wales. Pediatrics, 123, e312–e327. by Cruzans and Americans. Journal of Psychology, 115, 113–116.
Manian, N., & Bornstein, M. H. (2009). Dynamics of emotion regu- Maret, S. M., & Harling, C. A. (1985). Cross-cultural perceptions
lation in infants of clinically depressed and nondepressed mothers. of physical attractiveness: Ratings of photographs of whites by
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 1410–1418. Cruzans and Americans. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 60, 163–166.
R-82 REFERENCES
Marini, A., Martelli, S., Gagliardi, C., Fabbro, F., & Borgatti, R. Marschark, M. (1997). Raising and educating a deaf child (2nd ed.).
(2010). Narrative language in Williams Syndrome and its neuro- New York: Oxford University Press.
psychological correlates. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23, 97–111. Marsh, A. A., Finger, E. C., Mitchell, D. G., Reid, M. E., Sims, C.,
Mark, M., Rijli, F. M., & Chambon, P. (1997). Homeobox genes in Kosson, D. S., Towbin, K. E., Leibenluft, E., Pine, D. S., & Blair,
embryogenesis and pathogenesis. Pediatric Research, 42, 421–429. R. J. (2008). Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions
Markman, A. B. (1997). Constraints on analogical inference. Cognitive in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and
Science, 21, 373–418. disruptive behavior disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165,
Markman, E. M. (1979). Realizing that you don’t understand: 712–720.
Elementary school children’s awareness of inconsistencies. Child Marshall, C. R., & van der Lely, H. K. (2006). A challenge to cur-
Development, 50, 643–655. rent models of past tense inflection: The impact of phonotactics.
Markman, E. M. (1980). Developmental differences in the acquisi- Cognition, 100, 302–320.
tion of basic and superordinate categories. Child Development, 51, Marshall, P. J. (2010). The development of emotion. Wiley
708–719. Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 417–425.
Markman, E. M. (1989). Categorization and naming in children: Marshall, R. E., Stratton, W. C, Moore, J. A., & Boxerman, S. B.
Problems of induction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (1980). Circumcision I: Effects upon newborn behavior. Infant
Markman, E. M. (1991). The whole object, taxonomic, and mutual Behavior and Development, 3, 1–14.
exclusivity assumptions as initial constraints on word meanings. Marsiske, M., Lang, F. R., Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1995).
In S. A. Gelman & J. P. Byrnes (Eds.), Perspectives on language and Selective optimization with compensation: Life-span perspectives
cognition: Interrelations in development (pp. 72–106). Cambridge, on successful human development. In R. A. Dixon & L. Bäckman
UK: Cambridge University Press. (Eds.), Compensating for psychological deficits and declines: Managing
Markman, E. M. (1992). Constraints on word learning: Speculations losses and promoting gains (pp. 35–79). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
about their nature, origins, and domain specificity. In M. R. Martens, M. A., Wilson, S. J., & Reutens, D. C. (2008). Research
Gunnar & M. Maratsos (Eds.), Modularity and constraints in lan- Review: Williams syndrome: A critical review of the cognitive,
guage and cognition (pp. 59–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. behavioral, and neuroanatomical phenotype. Journal of Child
Markman, E. M. (1994). Constraints on word meaning in early lan- Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 576–608.
guage acquisition. Lingua, 92, 199–227. Martin, B., Buffington, A. L. H., Welsh-Bohmer, K. A., & Brandt, J.
Markman, E. M., & Hutchinson, J. E. (1984). Children’s sensitivity (2008). Time of day affects episodic memory in older adults. Aging,
to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic rela- Neuropsychology and Cognition, 15, 146–164.
tions. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 1–27. Martin, C. L., Eisenbud, L., & Rose, H. (1995). Children’s gender-
Markman, E. M., & Siebert, J. (1976). Classes and collections: based reasoning about toys. Child Development, 66, 1453–1471.
Internal organization and resulting holistic properties. Cognitive Martin, C. L., & Fabes, R. A. (2001). The stability and consequences
Psychology, 8, 561–577. of children’s same-sex peer interactions. Developmental Psychology,
Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children’s use of 37, 431–446.
mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Martin, C. L., Fabes, R. A., Evans, S. M., & Wyman, H. (1999).
Psychology, 20, 121–157. Social cognition on the playground: Children’s beliefs about play-
Markovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (1999). The A-not-B error: Results ing with girls versus boys and their relations to sex segregated play.
from a logistic meta-analysis. Child Development, 70, 1297–1313. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 16, 751–772.
Marks, I. M. (1977). Phobias and obsessions: Clinical phenomena in Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic process-
search of a laboratory model. In J. D. Maser & M. E. P. Seligman ing model of sex-typing and stereotyping in children. Child
(Eds.), Psychopathology: Experimental models (pp. 175–213). San Development, 52, 1119–1134.
Francisco: W. H. Freeman. Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. (2004). Children’s search for gender cues:
Marks, I. M. (1987). Fears, phobias, and rituals: Panic, anxiety, and Cognitive perspectives on gender development. Current Directions
their disorders. New York: Oxford University Press. in Psychological Science, 13, 67–74.
Marks, I. M., & Nesse, R. W. (1994). Fear and fitness: An evolutionary Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2010). Patterns of gender develop-
analysis of anxiety disorders. Ethology & Sociobiology, 15, 247–261. ment. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 353–381.
Markson, L., & Bloom, P. (1997). Evidence against a dedicated system Martin, J. A., Hamilton, B. E., Sutton, P. D., Ventura, S. J., Menacker,
for word learning in children. Nature, 385, 813–815. F., & Munson, M. L. (2003). Births: Final data for 2002. National
Marler, P. (1991a). The instinct to learn. In S. Carey & R. Gelman Vital Statistics Reports, 52 (10). Hyattsville, MD: National Center
(Eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition. The for Health Statistics. http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nvsr/nvsr52
Jean Piaget Symposium series (pp. 37–66). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence /nvsr52_10.pdf
Erlbaum Associates. Martin, J. A., Hamilton, B. E., Ventura, S. J., Osterman, M. J. K.,
Marler, P. (1991b). The instinct for vocal learning: Songbirds. In S. E. Wilson, E. C., & Mathews, T. J. (2012). Births: Final data for
Brauth, W. S. Hall, & R. J. Dooling (Eds.), Plasticity of develop- 2010. National Vital Statistics Reports, 61 (1). Hyattsville, MD:
ment (pp. 107–125). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. National Center for Health Statistics. http://www.cdc.gov/nchs
Marlier, L., & Schaal, B. (2005). Human newborns prefer human /data/nvsr/nvsr61/nvsr61_01.pdf
milk: Conspecific milk odor is attractive without postnatal expo- Martin, N. G., Eaves, L. J., Heath, A. C., Jardine, R., Feingold,
sure. Child Development, 76, 155–168. L. M., & Eysenck, H. J. (1986). Transmission of social attitudes.
Marryshow, D., Hurley, E. A., Allen, B. A., Tyler, K. M., & Boykin, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 83,
A. W. (2005). Impact of learning orientation on African American 4364–4368.
children’s attitudes toward high-achieving peers. American Journal Martin, P., Long, M. V., & Poon, L. W. (2002). Age changes and
of Psychology, 118, 603–618. differences in personality traits and states in the old and very old.
REFERENCES R-83
Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Matlen, B. J., & Klahr, D. (2010). Sequential effects of high and
Sciences, 57, 144–152. low guidance on children’s early science learning. In K. Gomez,
Martin, S. P. (2004). Delayed marriage and childbearing: Implications L. Lyons, & J. Radinsky (Eds.), Learning in the disciplines:
and measurement of diverging trends in family timing. In Proceedings of the 9th International Conference of the Learning
K. Neckerman (Ed.), Social inequality (pp. 79–119). New York: Sciences (Vol. 1, pp. 1016–1023). Chicago: International Society of
Russell Sage Foundation. the Learning Sciences.
Martin, S. P. (2006). Trends in marital dissolution by women’s educa- Matlen, B. J., & Klahr, D. (2013). Sequential effects of high and low
tion in the United States. Demographic Research, 15, 537–560. instructional guidance on children’s acquisition of experimenta-
Marx, J. (1991). How embryos tell heads from tails. Science, 254, tion skills: Is it all in the timing? Instructional Science, 41, 621–634.
1586–1588. Matson, J. L. (Ed.). (1993). Handbook of hyperactivity in children.
Marzillier, S. L., & Davey, G. C. L. (2004). The emotional profiling Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
of disgust-eliciting stimuli: Evidence for primary and complex dis- Matsunaga, H., Maebayashi, K., Hayashida, K., Okino, K., Matsui, T.,
gusts. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 313–336. Iketani, T., Kiriike, N., & Stein, D. J. (2008). Symptom structure
Marzillier, S. L., & Davey, G. C. L. (2005). Anxiety and disgust: in Japanese patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. American
Evidence for a unidirectional relationship. Cognition & Emotion, Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 251–253.
19, 729–750. Matsuzawa,T. (2006). Evolutionary origins of the human mother-
Mascalzoni, E., Regolin, L., & Vallortigara, G. (2010). Innate sen- infant relationship. In T. Matsuzawa, M. Tomonaga, & M. Tanaka
sitivity for self-propelled causal agency in newly hatched chicks. (Eds.), Cognitive development in chimpanzees (pp. 127–141). Tokyo:
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 107, Springer-Verlag.
4483–4485. Mattanah, J. F., Pratt, M. W., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (2005).
Mash, E. J., & Barkley, R. A. (Eds.). (1998). Treatment of childhood Authoritative parenting, parental scaffolding of long-division
disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. mathematics, and children’s academic competence in fourth grade.
Massaro, A. N., Rothbaum, A., & Aly, H. (2006). Fetal brain devel- Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26, 85–106.
opment: The role of maternal nutrition, exposures and behaviors. Matthews, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2007). How toddlers and
Journal of Pediatric Neurology, 4, 1–9. preschoolers learn to uniquely identify referents for others: A train-
Massey, C. M., & Gelman, R. (1988). Preschooler’s ability to decide ing study. Child Development, 78, 1744–1759.
whether a photographed unfamiliar object can move itself. Matthews, G., Zeidner, M., & Roberts, R. D. (2003). Development
Developmental Psychology, 24, 307–317. of emotional intelligence: A skeptical—but not dismissive—
Mataix-Cols, D., Nakatani, E., Micali, N., & Heyman, I. (2008). perspective. Human Development, 46, 109–114.
Structure of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in pediatric OCD. Matthews, J. S., Ponitz, C. C., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). Early gender
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, differences in self-regulation: A sixth-sense for academic achieve-
47, 773–778. ment? Journal of Educational Psychology. 101, 689–704.
Mather, M. (2010). Aging and cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Matthiesen, A. S., Ransjo-Arvidson, A. B., Nissen, E., & Uvnas-
Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 346–362. Moberg, K. (2001). Postpartum maternal oxytocin release by
Mather, M., Canli, T., English, T., Whitfield, S., Wais, P., Ochsner, K., newborns: Effects of infant hand massage and sucking. Birth, 28,
Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Carstensen, L. L. (2004). Amygdala responses 13–19.
to emotionally valenced stimuli in older and younger adults. Mattison, J. A., Roth, G. S., Beasley, T. M., Tilmont, E. M., Handy,
Psychological Science, 15, 259–263. A. M., Herbert, R. L., Longo, D. L., Allison, D. B., Young, J. E.,
Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and attentional biases Bryant, M., Barnard, D., Ward, W. F., Qi, W., Ingram, D. K. &
for emotional faces. Psychological Science, 14, 409–415. de Cabo, R. (2012). Impact of caloric restriction on health and
Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Aging and motivated cog- survival in rhesus monkeys from the NIA study. Nature, 489,
nition: The positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends in 318–321.
Cognitive Sciences, 9, 496–502. Matzuk, M. M., & Lamb, D. J. (2008). The biology of infertility:
Mather, M., Gorlick, M. A., & Lighthall, N. R. (2009). To brake or Research advances and clinical challenges. Nature Medicine, 14,
accelerate when the light turns yellow? Stress reduces older adults’ 1197–1213.
risk taking in a driving game. Psychological Science, 20, 174–176. Maurer, D., Lewis T. L., & Mondloch C. J. (2005). Missing sights:
Mathers, C., & Leonard, M. (2000). Global burden of dementia in the Consequences for visual cognitive development. Trends in Cognitive
year 2000: Summary of methods and data sources. Geneva: World Sciences, 9, 144–151.
Health Organization. May, C. P., Hasher, L., & Foong, N. (2005). Implicit memory, age,
Mathers, C. D., Sadana, R., Salomon, J. A., Murray, C. J. L., & and time of day: Paradoxical priming effects. Psychological Science,
Lopez, A. D. (2001). Healthy life expectancy in 191 countries, 16, 96–100.
1999. Lancet, 357, 1685–1691. May, C. P., Zacks, R. T., Hasher, L., & Multhaup, K. S. (1999).
Mathews, J. (2006, February 20). Let’s teach to the test. The Inhibition in the processing of garden-path sentences. Psychology
Washington Post, p. A21. and Aging, 14, 304–313.
Mathews, T. J., & Hamilton, B. E. (2009). Delayed childbearing: More May, J., Evans, J. A., Timmann, C., Ehmen, C., Busch, W., Thye, T.,
women are having their first child later in life. National Center for Agbenyega, T., & Horstmann, R. D. (2007). Hemoglobin variants
Health Statistics, Data Brief No. 21. Hyattsville, MD: National and disease manifestations in severe falciparum malaria. Journal of
Center for Health Statistics. the American Medical Association, 297, 2220–2226.
Mathews, W. W., & Schoenwolf, G. C. (1998). Atlas of descriptive Mayberry R. I. (2010). Early language acquisition and adult language
embryology (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. ability: What sign language reveals about the critical period for lan-
Matias, R., & Cohn J. (1993). Are MAX-specified infant facial expres- guage. In M. Marschark & P. E. Spencer (Eds.), Oxford Handbook
sions during face-to-face interaction consistent with differential of Deaf Studies, Language, and Education (Vol. 2, pp. 281–291).
emotions theory? Developmental Psychology, 29, 524–531. New York: Oxford University Press.
R-84 REFERENCES
Mayberry, R. I., Chen, J. K., Witcher, P., & Klein, D. (2011). Age of McCall, R. B., & Kagan, J. (1970). Individual differences in the infant’s
acquisition effects on the functional organization of language in distribution of attention to stimulus discrepancy. Developmental
the adult brain. Brain and Language, 119, 16–29. Psychology, 2, 90–98.
Mayberry, R. I., Lock, E., & Kazmi, H. (2002). Linguistic ability and McCall, R. B., & Mash, C. W. (1995). Infant cognition and its relation
early language exposure. Nature, 417, 38. to mature intelligence. Annals of Child Development, 10, 27–56.
Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., & Salovey, P. (1999). Emotional intel- McCann, R., & Giles, H. (2002). Ageism in the workplace: A com-
ligence meets traditional standards for an intelligence. Intelligence, munication perspective. In T. D. Nelson (Ed.), Ageism: Stereotyping
27, 267–298. and prejudice against older persons (pp. 163–199). Cambridge, MA:
Mayer, J. D., Roberts, R. D., & Barsade, S. G. (2008). Human abili- MIT Press.
ties: Emotional intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, McCarthy, M. M., Auger, A. P., Bale, T. L., De Vries, G. J., Dunn,
507–536. G. A., Forger, N. G., Murray, E. K., Nugent, B. M., Schwarz,
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In J. M., & Wilson, M. E. (2009). The epigenetics of sex differences
P. Salovey & D. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development and emo- in the brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 12815–12823.
tional intelligence: Implications for educators (pp. 3–31). New York: McCarthy, M. M., Auger, A. P., & Perrot-Sinal, T. S. (2002). Getting
Basic Books. excited about GABA and sex differences in the brain. Trends in
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2004). Emotional intel- Neuroscience, 25, 307–312.
ligence: Theory, findings, and implications. Psychological Inquiry, McCartney, K. A., Burchinal, M., Clarke-Stewart, A., Bub, K. L.,
60, 197–215. Owen, M. T., & Belsky, J. (2010). Testing a series of causal propo-
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D. R., & Sitarenios, G. (2001). sitions relating to time spent in child care to children’s external-
Emotional intelligence as a standard intelligence. Emotion, 1, izing behavior. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1–17.
232–242. McClellan, J. M., & Werry, J. S. (2000). Research psychiatric diagnos-
Mayes, L. C., Klin, A., & Cohen, D. J. (1994). The effect of humour tic interviews for children and adolescents. Journal of the American
on children’s developing theory of mind. British Journal of Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 19–27.
Developmental Psychology, 12, 555–561. McClelland, D. C. (1993). Intelligence is not the best predictor of job
Maylor, E. A. (1990). Age and prospective memory. Quarterly Journal performance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 5–6.
of Experimental Psychology, 42A, 471–493. McClelland, J. L. (1999). Cognitive modeling, connectionist. In R. A.
Mazuka, R. (1996). How can a grammatical parameter be set before Wilson & F. C. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive
the first word? In J. L. Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to sciences (pp. 137–139). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (Eds.). (1986). Parallel dis-
(pp. 313–330). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. tributed processing. Explorations in the microstructure of cognition
Mazuka, R. (1998). The development of language processing strategies: (Vols.1–2). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
A cross-linguistic study between Japanese and English. Hillsdale, NJ: McClelland, M. M., & Cameron, C. E. (2012). Self-regulation in
Erlbaum. early childhood: Improving conceptual clarity and developing
Mazure, C. M., & Maciejewski, P. K. (2003a). A model of risk for ecologically valid measures. Child Development Perspectives, 6,
major depression: Effects of life stress and cognitive style vary by 136–142.
age. Depression and Anxiety, 17, 26–33. McCloskey, M. (1983). Naive theories of motion. In D. Gentner &
Mazure, C. M., & Maciejewski, P. K. (2003b). The interplay of stress, A. L. Stevens (Eds.), Mental models (pp. 299–324). Hillsdale, NJ:
gender and cognitive style in depressive onset. Archives of Women’s Erlbaum.
Mental Health, 6, 5–8. McClure, T. E., & May, D. C. (2008). Dealing with misbehavior at
Mazure, C. M., Maciejewski, P. K., Jacobs, S. C., & Bruce, M. L. schools in Kentucky: Theoretical and contextual predictors of use
(2002). Stressful life events interacting with cognitive/personality of corporal punishment. Youth & Society, 39, 406–429.
styles to predict late-onset major depression. American Journal of McConnell, S. K., Ghosh, A., & Shatz, C. J. (1989). Subplate neurons
Geriatric Psychiatry, 10, 297–304. pioneer the first axon pathway from the cerebral cortex. Science,
McAlister, A., & Peterson, C. C. (2007). A longitudinal study of child 245, 978–982.
siblings and theory of mind development. Cognitive Development, McCormack, T., Brown, G., Maylor, E., Darby, R., & Green, D.
22, 258–270. (1999). Developmental changes in time estimation comparing
McAlister, A. R., & Peterson, C. C. (2013). Siblings, theory of mind, childhood and old age. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1143–1155.
and executive functioning in children aged 3–6 years: New longi- McCoy, C. L., & Masters, J. C. (1990). Children’s strategies for the
tudinal evidence. Child Development, 84, 1442–1458. control of emotions in themselves and others. In B. S. Moore &
McBride-Chang, C. (1995). What is phonological awareness? Journal A. M. Isen (Eds.), Affect and social behavior (pp. 231–268).
of Educational Psychology, 87, 179–192. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
McBurney, D. H., Gaulin, S. J., Devineni, T., & Adams, C. (1997). McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1984). Emerging lives, enduring disposi-
Superior spatial memory of women: Stronger evidence for tions: Personality in adulthood. Boston: Little, Brown.
the gathering hypothesis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1994). The stability of personality:
165–174. Observations and evaluations. Current Directions in Psychological
McCabe, M. P., & Ricciardelli, L.A. (2003). Sociocultural influences Science, 3, 173–175.
on body image and body changes among adolescent boys and girls. McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., & Arenberg, D. (1980). Constancy of
Journal of Social Psychology, 143, 5–26. adult personality structure in males: Longitudinal, cross-sectional
McCall, R. B. (1977). Childhood IQ’s as predictors of adult educa- and times-of-measurement analyses. Journal of Gerontology, 35,
tional and occupational status. Science, 197, 482–483. 877–883.
McCall, R. B., & Carriger, M. S. (1993). A meta-analysis of infant McCrea, S. M., Mueller, J. H., & Parrila, R. K. (1999). Quantitative
habituation and recognition memory performance as predictors of analyses of schooling effects on executive function in young chil-
later IQ. Child Development, 64, 57–79. dren. Child Neuropsychology, 5, 242–250.
REFERENCES R-85
McCrink, K., & Wynn, K. (2004). Large-number addition and sub- McKown, C., & Strambler, M. J. (2009). Developmental antecedents
traction by 9-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 15, 776–781. and social and academic consequences of stereotype-consciousness
McCutcheon, L. E. (2000). Another failure to generalize the Mozart in middle childhood. Child Development, 80, 1643–1659.
effect. Psychological Reports, 87, 325–330. McKown, C., & Weinstein, R. S. (2003).The development and con-
McDonald, A. (2001). The prevalence and effects of test anxiety in sequences of stereotype consciousness in middle childhood. Child
school children. Educational Psychology, 21, 89–101. Development, 74, 498–515.
McDonough, L., Mandler, J. M., McKee, R. D., & Squire, L. R. McLachlan, R. S. (1997). Medical conditions and driving: Legal
(1995). The deferred imitation task as a nonverbal measure of requirements and approach of neurologists. Medicine and Law, 16,
declarative memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 269–275.
of the USA, 92, 7580–7584. McLanahan, S. (1999). Father absence and the welfare of children. In
McDowd, J. M., & Shaw, R. J. (2000). Attention and aging: A func- E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Coping with divorce, single parenting and
tional perspective. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The remarriage (pp. 117–145). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 221–291). Hillsdale, McLanahan, S. (2004). Diverging destinies: How children are faring
NJ: Erlbaum. under the second demographic transition. Demography, 41, 607–627.
McEachin, J. J., Smith, T., & Lovaas, O. I. (1993). Long-term outcome McLaughlin, B. (1984). Second-language acquisition in childhood:
for children with autism who received early intensive behavioral Vol. 1. Preschool children (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
treatment. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 97, 359–372. McLaughlin, B. (1985). Second-language acquisition in childhood:
McGarrigle, J., & Donaldson, M. (1974). Conservation accidents. Vol. 2. School-age children (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cognition, 3, 341–350. McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child devel-
McGinnis, W., & Krumlauf, R. (1992. Homeobox genes and axial opment. American Psychologist, 53, 185–204.
patterning. Cell, 68, 283–302. McMahon, R. (1980). Genetic etiology in the hyperactive child syn-
McGrath, E. P., & Repetti, R. L. (2002). A longitudinal study of chil- drome: A critical review. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 50,
dren’s depressive symptoms, self-perceptions, and cognitive distor- 145–150.
tions about self. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 77–87. McMurdo, M. E. T., & Baines, P. S. (1988). The detection of visual
McGraw, M. (1943). The neuromuscular maturation of the human disability in the elderly. Health Bulletin, 46, 327–329.
infant. New York: Columbia University Press. McNally, R. J. (1987). Preparedness and phobias: A review.
McGregor, H. E. (2010). Inuit education and schools in the Eastern Psychological Bulletin, 101, 283–303.
Arctic. Vancouver, BC: University of British Columbia Press. McPartland, J. C., Reichow, B., & Volkmar, F. R. (2012). Sensitivity
McGrew, K. S., & Knopik, S. N. (1993). The relationship between the and specificity of proposed DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for autism
WJ-R Gf-Gc cognitive clusters and writing achievement across the spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
life-span. School Psychology Review, 22, 687–695. Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 368–383.
McGuffin, P., Rijsdijk, F., Andrew, M., Sham, P., Katz, R., & Cardno, A. McPhedran, S. (2009). A review of the evidence for associations
(2003).The heritability of bipolar affective disorder and the genetic between empathy, violence, and animal cruelty. Aggression and
relationship to unipolar depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, Violent Behavior, 14, 1–4.
60, 497–502. McRae, K., Hughes, B., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J. D., Gross, J. J., &
McGuigan, F., & Salmon, K. (2004). The time to talk: The influence Ochsner, K. N. (2010). The neural bases of distraction and reap-
of the timing of adult–child talk on children’s event memory. Child praisal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 248–262.
Development, 75, 669–686. Mead, M. (1932). An investigation of the thought of primitive
McHale, S. M., Updegraff, K. A., Helms-Erikson, H., & Crouter, children with special reference to animism. Journal of the Royal
A. C. (2001). Sibling influences on gender development in Anthropological Society, 62, 173–190.
middle childhood and early adolescence: A longitudinal study. Meadow-Orleans, K. P., Mertens, D. M., Sass-Lehrer, M., & Scott-
Developmental Psychology, 37, 115–125. Olson, K. (1997). Support services for parents and their children
McKelvie, P., & Low, J. (2002). Listening to Mozart does not improve who are deaf or hard of hearing. American Annals of the Deaf, 142,
children’s spatial ability: Final curtains for the Mozart effect. 278–288.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 241–258. Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated evo-
McKenna, J. J., Ball, H. L., & Gettler, L. T. (2007). Mother-infant lutionary model. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 18, 523–599.
cosleeping, breastfeeding and sudden infant death syndrome: Meaney, M. J. (2001). Maternal care, gene expression, and the trans-
What biological anthropology has discovered about normal infant mission of individual differences in stress reactivity across genera-
sleep and pediatric sleep medicine. American Journal of Physical tions. Annual Review of Neuroscience 24, 1161–1192.
Anthropology, 134, 133–161. Meaney, M. J., & Szyf, M. (2005). Maternal care as a model for
McKinley, R. A., McIntire, L. K., & Funke, M. A. (2011). Operator experience-dependent chromatin plasticity? Trends in Neuroscience,
selection for unmanned aerial systems: Comparing video game 28, 456–463.
players and pilots. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, Meck, W. H., & Church, R. M. (1983). A mode control model of
82, 635–642. counting and timing processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
McKone E., Crookes K., & Kanwisher, N. (2009). The cogni- Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 320–334.
tive and neural development of face recognition in humans. In Medawar, P. B., & Medwar, J. S. (1983). Aristotle to zoos: A philo-
M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences IV (4th ed., sophical dictionary of biology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
pp. 467–482). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/ Bradford Books. Press.
McKown, C. (2004). Age and ethnic variation in children’s think- Medin, D. L., & Waxman, S. (2007). Interpreting asymmetries of
ing about the nature of racism. Journal of Applied Developmental projection in children’s inductive reasoning. In A. Feeney &
Psychology, 25, 597–617. E. Heit (Eds.), Inductive reasoning: Experimental, developmental
R-86 REFERENCES
and computational approaches (pp. 55–80). New York: Cambridge Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1989). Imitation in newborn
University Press. infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underly-
Medin, D. L., & Atran, S. (Eds). (1999). Folkbiology. Cambridge, ing mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 25, 954–962.
MA: MIT Press. Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1994). Imitation, memory, and
Medin, D., Waxman, S., Woodring, J., & Washinawatok, K. (2010). the representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development, 17,
Human-centeredness is not a universal feature of young children’s 83–99.
reasoning: Culture and experience matter when reasoning about Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1999). A new foundation for cog-
biological entities. Cognitive Development, 25, 197–207. nitive development in infancy: The birth of the representational
Mednick, S. A., Gabrielli, W. F., Jr, & Hutchings, B. (1984). Genetic infant. In E. K. Scholnick, K. Nelson, S. A. Gelman, & P. H.
influences in criminal convictions: Evidence from an adoption Miller (Eds.), Conceptual development: Piaget’s legacy. The Jean
cohort. Science, 224, 891–894. Piaget Symposium Series (pp. 53–78). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Meek, J. H. (2002). Basic principles of optical imaging and applica- Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1999). Persons and representation:
tion to the study of infant development. Developmental Science, 5, Why infant imitation is important for theories of human develop-
371–380. ment. In J. Nadel & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Imitation in infancy.
Mehler, J., Bertoncini, J., Barriere, M., & Jassik-Gerschenfeld, D. Cambridge studies in cognitive perceptual development (pp. 9–35).
(1984). Recognition of the mother’s voice by newborns. Cuadernos New York: Cambridge University Press.
de Psicologia, 6, 61–70. Meltzoff, A. N., & Prinz, W. (2002). The imitative mind: Development,
Mehler, J., Dupoux, E., & Southgate, P. (1994). What infants know: evolution, and brain bases. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
The new cognitive science of early development. Cambridge, UK: Press.
Blackwell Publishers. Meltzoff, A., & Moore, K. (1998). Infant intersubjectivity: Broadening
Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P., Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J., & the dialogue to include imitation, identity and intention. In S.
Amiel-Tison, C. (1988). A precursor of language acquisition in Braten (Ed.), Intersubjective communicaton and emotion in early
young infants. Cognition, 29, 143–178. ontogeny (pp. 47–62). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Mehler-Wex, C., Kolch, M., Kirchheiner, J., Antony, G., Fegert, Mendes, D. M., Seidl-de-Moura, M. L., & Siqueira, J. O. (2009). The
J. M., & Gerlach, M. (2009). Drug monitoring in child and ado- ontogenesis of smiling and its association with mothers’ affective
lescent psychiatry for improved efficacy and safety of psychophar- behaviors: A longitudinal study. Infant Behavior and Development,
macology. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 3, 32, 445–453.
14–23. Menesini, E., & Spiel, C. (2012). Introduction: Cyberbullying:
Meier, R. (1991). Language acquisition by deaf children. American Development, consequences, risk and protective factors. European
Scientist, 79, 60–70. Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 163–167.
Meinecke, D. L., & Rakic, P. (1992). Expression of GABA and Meng, H., Smith, S. D., Hager, K., Held, M., Liu, J., Olson, R. K.,
GABAa receptors by neurons of the subplate zone in develop- Pennington, B. F., DeFries, J. C., Gelernter, J., O’Reilly-Pol, T.,
ing primate occipital cortex: Evidence for transient local circuits. Somlo, S., Skudlarski, P., Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A.,
Journal of Comparative Neurology, 317, 91–101. Marchione, K., Wang, Y., Paramasivam, M., LoTurco, J. J., Page,
Meir, I., & Sandler, W. (2008). A language in space: The story of Israeli G. P., & Gruen, J. R. (2005). DCDC2 is associated with read-
Sign Language. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. ing disability and modulates neuronal development in the brain.
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of oth- Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 102,
ers: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. 17053–17058.
Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850. Mennella, J. A., & Beauchamp, G. K. (1997). The ontogeny of human
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). What infant memory tells us about infan- flavor perception. In G. K. Beauchamp & L. Bartoshuk (Eds.),
tile amnesia: Long-term recall and deferred imitation. Journal of Tasting and smelling: Handbook of perception and cognition (2nd ed.,
Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 497–515. pp. 199–221). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Meltzoff, A. N. (1999). Origins of theory of mind, cognition and com- Mennella, J. A., Griffin, C., & Beauchamp, G. K. (2004). Flavor pro-
munication. Journal of Communication Disorders, 32, 251–269. gramming during infancy. Pediatrics, 113, 840 –845.
Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). Imitation as a mechanism of social cogni- Mennella, J. A., Jagnow, C. P., & Beauchamp, G. K. (2001). Prenatal
tion: Origins of empathy, theory of mind, and the representation of and postnatal flavor learning by human infants. Pediatrics, 107,
action. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell Handbook of childhood cog- e88–e93.
nitive development (pp. 6–25). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers. Mennella, J. A., Pepino, M. Y., & Reed, D. R. (2005). Genetic and
Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). Imitation and other minds: The “Like Me” environmental determinants of bitter perception and sweet prefer-
hypothesis. In S. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imi- ences. Pediatrics, 115, e216–e222.
tation: From cognitive neuroscience to social science (pp. 55–77). Merabet, L. B., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2010). Neural reorganization
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. following sensory loss: The opportunity of change. Nature Reviews:
Meltzoff, A. N., & Borton, R. W. (1979). Intermodal matching by Neuroscience, 11, 44–52.
human neonates. Nature, 282, 403–404. Merrick, J., Kandel, I., & Morad, M. (2004). Trends in autism.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2003). What imitation tells us about International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 16, 75–78.
social cognition: A rapprochement between developmental psy- Merriman, W. E., & Bowman, L. L. (1989). The mutual exclusiv-
chology and cognitive neuroscience. Philosophical Transactions ity bias in children’s word learning. Monographs of the Society for
of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358, Research in Child Development, 54 (Serial Nos. 3–4).
491–500. Merriman, W. E., Marazita, J., & Jarvis, L. H. (1993). Four-year-olds’
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and man- disambiguation of action and object word reference. Journal of
ual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78. Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 412–430.
REFERENCES R-87
Merriman, W. E., & Stevenson, C. M. (1997). Restricting a familiar in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child
name in response to learning a new one: Evidence for the mutual Development, 75, 1373–1386.
exclusivity bias in young two-year-olds. Child Development, 68, Mikhail, J. (2007). Universal moral grammar: Theory, evidence and
211–228. the future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 143–152.
Merriman, W. E., & Tomasello, M. (1995). Verbs are words too. In Miklósi, A., & Topál, J. (2012). The evolution of canine cognition.
M. Tomasello & W. E. Merriman (Eds.), Beyond names for things: In J. Vonk & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of
Young children’s acquisition of verbs (pp. 1–18). Hillsdale, NJ: comparative evolutionary psychology (pp. 194–216). Oxford, UK:
Erlbaum. Oxford University Press.
Mervis, C. B., & Crisafi, M. A. (1982). Order of acquisition of Milberger, S., Biederman, J., Faraone, S., Guite, J., & Tsuang, M.
subordinate-, basic-, and superordinate-level categories. Child (1997). Pregnancy delivery and infancy complications and atten-
Development, 53, 258–266. tion deficit hyperactivity disorder: Issues of gene-environment
Mervis, C. S., & Velleman, S. L. (2011). Children with Williams interactions. Biological Psychiatry, 41, 65–75.
syndrome: Language, cognitive, and behavioral characteristics Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of
and their implications for intervention. Perspectives on Language Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 371–378.
Learning and Education, 18, 98–107. Millar, W. S., & Watson, J. S. (1979). The effect of delayed feedback
Merz, E. C., & McCall, R. B. (2010). Behavior problems in children on infant learning reexamined. Child Development, 50, 747–751.
adopted from psychosocially depriving institutions. Journal of Miller, A. R. (2011). The timing of initial family formation and wom-
Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 459–470. en’s wellbeing over the lifecycle. University of Virginia and RAND
Mesman, J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, Corporation, April 22, 2011.
M. J. (2009). The many faces of the still-face paradigm: A review Miller, D. T. (1999). The norm of self-interest. American Psychologist,
and meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 29, 120–162. 54, 1053–1060.
Messaoudi, I., Warner, J., Fischer, M., Park, B., Hill, B., Mattison, J., Miller, E. M. (1994). Intelligence and brain myelination: A hypoth-
Lane, M. A., Roth G. S., Ingram, D. K., Picker, L. J., Douek, esis. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 803–833.
D. C., Mori, M., & Nikolich-Zugich, J. (2006). Delay of T cell Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven plus or minus
senescence by caloric restriction in aged long-lived non-human pri- two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information.
mates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.
103, 19448–19453. Miller, G. A. (1991). The science of words. New York: Freeman.
Messinger, D. (2002). Positive and negative: Infant facial expressions Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development of everyday social
and emotions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 1–6. explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46,
Messinger, D., Fogel, A., & Dickson, L. (1997). A dynamic sys- 961–978.
tems approach to infant facial action. In J. Russell & F. M. Dols Miller, J. G. (2006). Insights into moral development from cultural
(Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 205–226). New York: psychology. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral
Cambridge University Press. development (pp. 375–398). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Metcalfe, J., & Kornell, N. (2007). Principles of cognitive science Miller, K. F. (1992). What a number is: Mathematical foundations
in education: The effects of generation, errors and feedback. and developing number concepts. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), The
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 225–229. nature and origins of mathematical skills (pp. 3–38). Amsterdam:
Metz, K. E. (2004). Children’s understanding of scientific inquiry: North-Holland Elsevier Science Publishers.
Their conceptualization of uncertainty in investigations of their Miller, K. F., & Paredes, D. R. (1990). Starting to add worse: Effects
own design. Cognition and Instruction, 22, 219–290. of learning to multiply on children’s addition. Cognition, 37,
Meyer, J. S., Novak, M. A., Bowman, R. E, & Harlow, H. F. (1975). 213–242.
Behavioral and hormonal effects of attachment object separation Miller, M. N., & Pumariega, A. J. (2001). Culture and eating disor-
in surrogate-peer-reared and mother-reared infant rhesus monkeys. ders: A historical and cross-cultural review. Psychiatry, 64, 93–110.
Developmental Psychobiology, 8, 425–435. Miller, P. H. (1993). Theories of developmental psychology (3rd ed.).
Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L. (2005). Gender identity outcome in female- New York: Freeman.
raised 46, XY persons with penile agenesis, cloacal exstrophy of the Miller, P. H., & Bigi, L. (1979). The development of children’s under-
bladder, or penile ablation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 423–438. standing of attention. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 25, 235–250.
Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F., Dolezal, C., Baker, S. W., Ehrhardt, A. A., & Miller, P. H., & Seier, W. L. (1994). Strategy utilization deficiencies in
New, M. I. (2006). Gender development in women with congeni- children: When, where, and why. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances
tal adrenal hyperplasia as a function of disorder severity. Archives of in child development and behavior (Vol. 25, pp. 107–156). San
Sexual Behavior 35, 667–684. Diego: Academic Press.
Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F.,. Dolezal, C., Baker, S. W., Carlson, A. D., Miller, P. H., Seier, W. L., Barron, K. L., & Probert, J. S. (1994). What
Obeid, J. S., & New, M. I. (2004). Prenatal androgenization affects causes a memory utilization deficiency? Cognitive Development, 9,
gender-related behavior but not gender identity in 5–12-year-old 77–102.
girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Archives of Sexual Miller, P. H., & Shannon, K. A. (1984). Young children’s understand-
Behavior 33, 97–104. ing of the effect of noise and interest level on learning. Genetic
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Buckholtz, J. W., Kolachana, B., Hariri, A. R., Psychology Monographs, 110, 71–90.
Pezawas, L., Blasi, G., Wabnitz, A., Honea, R., Verchinski, B. A., Miller, P. H., & Weiss, M. G. (1982). Children’s and adults’ knowledge
Callicott, J., Egan, M. F., Mattay, V. S., & Weinberger, D. R. about what variables affect selective attention. Child Development,
(2006). Neural mechanisms of genetic risk for impulsivity and vio- 53, 543–549.
lence in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of Miller, P. H., & Zalenski, R. (1982). Preschoolers’ knowledge about
the USA, 103, 6269–6274. attention. Developmental Psychology, 18, 871–875.
Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive atten- Miller, S. A. (1987). Developmental research methods. Englewood
tion: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
R-88 REFERENCES
Mills, C. M., & Keil, F. C. (2004). Knowing the limits of one’s Mischel, W., & Baker, N. (1975). Cognitive transformations of reward
understanding: The development of an awareness of an illusion objects through instructions. Journal of Personality and Social
of explanatory depth. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, Psychology, 31, 254–261.
1–32. Mischel, W., & Ebbeson, E. B. (1970). Attention in delay of gratifica-
Mills, C. M., & Keil, F. C. (2005). The development of cynicism. tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 329–337.
Psychological Science, 16, 385–390. Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., & Zeiss, A. R. (1972). Cognitive and
Mills, J. L., Hediger, M. L., Molloy, C. A., Chrousos, G. P., Manning- attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification. Journal of
Courtney, P., Yu, K. F., Brasington, M., & England L. J. (2007). Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 204–218.
Elevated levels of growth-related hormones in autism and autism Mischel, W., Ebbeson, E. B., & Zeiss, A. R. (1973). Selective attention
spectrum disorder. Clinical Endocrinology (Oxf), 67, 230–237. to the self: Situational and dispositional determinants. Journal of
Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 129–142.
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Mischel, W., & Metzner, R. (1962). Preference for delayed reward as a
Milner, B. (1964). Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In function of age, intelligence, and length of delay interval. Journal
J. Warren & K. Akert (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behav- of Abnormal and Social Psychology 64, 425–431.
ior. New York: McGraw-Hill. Mischel, W., & Moore, B. (1973). Effects of attention to symbolically
Milner, B. (1968). Disorders of memory after brain lesions in man. presented rewards on self-control. Journal of Personality and Social
Neuropsychologia, 6, 175–179. Psychology, 28, 172–179.
Milner, B., Corkin, S., & Teuber, H. L. (1968). Further analysis of the Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Peake, P. (1988). The nature of adolescent
hippocampal amnesic syndrome: Fourteen-year follow-up study of competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal
H.M. Neuropsychologia, 6, 215–234. of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 687–696.
Minagawa-Kawai, Y., van der Lely, H., Ramus, F., Sato, Y., Mazuka, R., & Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratifi-
Dupoux, E. (2011). Optical brain imaging reveals general audi- cation in children. Science, 244, 933–938.
tory and language-specific processing in early infant development. Mishna, F., Saini, M., & Solomon, S. (2009). Ongoing and online:
Cerebral Cortex, 21, 254–261. Children and youth’s perceptions of cyber bullying. Children and
Minati, L., Edginton, T., Bruzzone, M. G., & Giaccone, G. (2009). Youth Services Review, 31, 1222–1228.
Current concepts in Alzheimer’s disease: A multidisciplinary Mitchell, K. J., & Johnson, M. K. (2000). Source monitoring:
review. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Attributing mental experiences. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik
Dementias, 24, 95–121. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 179–195). New York:
Mineka, S., & Oehlberg, K. (2008). The relevance of recent develop- Oxford University Press.
ments in classical conditioning to understanding the etiology and Mitchell, K. J., & Johnson, M. K. (2009). Source monitoring 15 years
maintenance of anxiety disorders. Acta Psychologica, 127, 567–580. later: What have we learned from fMRI about the neural mecha-
Mineka, S., & Ohman, A. (2002). Phobias and preparedness: The nisms of source memory? Psychological Bulletin, 135, 638–677.
selective, automatic, and encapsulated nature of fear. Biological Mitchell, K. J., Johnson, M. K., & Mather, M. (2003). Source moni-
Psychiatry, 52, 927–937. toring and suggestibility to misinformation: Adult age-related dif-
Mineka, S., & Ohman, A. (2002a). Born to fear: Non-associative vs. ferences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 107–119.
associative factors in the etiology of phobias. Behaviour Research Mitchell, K. J., Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., & Greene, E. J. (2004).
and Therapy, 40, 173–184. Prefrontal cortex activity associated with source monitoring in a work-
Mineka, S., & Ohman, A. (2002b). Phobias and preparedness: The ing memory task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 921–934.
selective, automatic, and encapsulated nature of fear. Biological Mitchell, K. J., Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Mather, M., &
Psychiatry, 52, 927–937. D’Esposito, M. (2000). Aging and reflective processes of working
Mineka, S., & Suomi, S. J. (1978). Social separation in monkeys. memory: Binding and test-load deficits. Psychology and Aging, 15,
Psychological Bulletin, 85, 1376–1400. 527–541.
Minton, K. (2008). Teaching on the attack. Nature Reviews Mitchell, P. (1997). Introduction to theory of mind: Children, autism
Immunology, 8, 572. and apes. London: Arnold.
Minuchin, S., & Fishman, H. C. (1981). Family therapy techniques. Mitchell, R. W. (2002). Kinesthetic-visual matching, imitation and
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. self-recognition. In M. Bekoff, C. Allen, & G. M. Burghardt
Minuchin, S., Rosman, B. L., & Baker, L. (1978). Psychosomatic (Eds.), The cognitive animal (pp. 345–351). Boston: MIT Press.
families: Anorexia nervosa in context. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of the mind: The cognitive origins of
University Press. art, religion and science. London: Thames & Hudson.
Mirsky, A. F., Bieliauskas, L. A., French, L. M., Van Kammen, D. P., Miura, I. (1993). Switching pauses in adult-adult and child-child turn
Jonnsson, E., & Sedvall, G. (2000). A 39-year followup of the takings: An initial study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 22,
Genain quadruplets. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26, 699–708. 383–395.
Mirsky, A. F., Kugelmass, S., Ingraham, L. J., Frenkel, E., & Mix, K. S., Huttenlocher, J., & Levine, S. C. (2002). Multiple cues
Nathan, M. (1995). Overview and summary: Twenty-five-year fol- for quantification in infancy: Is number one of them? Psychological
lowup of high-risk children. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 21, 227–239. Bulletin, 128, 278–294.
Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley. Mix, K. S., Levine, S. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (1997). Numerical
Mischel, W. (1970). Attention in delay of gratification. Journal of abstraction in infants: Another look. Developmental Psychology, 33,
Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 329–337. 423–428.
Mischel, W. (1984). Convergences and challenges in the search for Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization
consistency. American Psychologist, 39, 351–364. of individual differences in executive functions: Four general con-
Mischel, W. (1996). From good intentions to willpower. In P. M. clusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8–14.
Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H.,
cognition and motivation to behavior. New York: Guilford Press. Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity
REFERENCES R-89
of executive functions and their contributions to complex “fron- Morata, G., & Lawrence, P.A. (1977). Homoeotic genes, compart-
tal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, ments and cell determination in Drosophila. Nature, 265, 211–216.
49–100. Morell, V. (2009). Going to the dogs. Science, 325, 1062–1065.
Miyake, K., Chen, S. S., & Campos, J. J. (1985). Infant temperament, Moreno, C., Hasin, D. S., Arango, C., Oquendo, M. A., Vieta, E.,
mother’s mode of interaction, and attachment in Japan: An interim Liu, S., Grant, B. F., & Blanco, C. (2012). Depression in bipolar
report. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, disorder versus major depressive disorder: Results from the National
50, 276–297. Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Bipolar
Moffitt, T. E. (2005). The new look of behavioral genetics in develop- Disorders, 14, 271–282.
mental psychopathology: Gene-environment interplay in antiso- Moreno, J. L. (1934). Who shall survive?A new approach to the prob-
cial behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 533–554. lem of human interrelations (with a preface by William A. White).
Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Harrington, H., & Milne, B. J. (2002). Washington, DC: Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing
Males on the life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited anti- Company.
social pathways: Follow-up at age 26 years. Development and Moreno, J. L. (1951). Sociometry, experimental method and the science
Psychopathology, 14, 179–207. of society: An approach to a new political orientation. Beacon, NY:
Mohindra, I., Held, R., Gwiazda, J., & Brill, S. (1978). Astigmatism Beacon House.
in infants. Science, 202, 329–331. Morgan, G., & Woll, B. (Eds.). (2002). Directions in sign language
Moldenhauer, Z., & Melnyk, B. M. (1999). Use of antidepressants in acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
the treatment of child and adolescent depression: Are they effec- Morgan, J. L., & Demuth, K. (Eds.). (1996). Signal to syntax:
tive? Pediatric Nursing, 25, 463–667. Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition. Mahwah,
Moller, S. J., & Tenenbaum, H. (2011). Danish majority children’s NJ: Erlbaum.
reasoning about exclusion based on gender and ethnicity. Child Morgan, S. P. (2003). Is low fertility a twenty-first-century demo-
Development, 82, 520–532. graphic crisis? Demography, 40, 589–603.
Monaghan, A. P., Bock, D., Gass, P., Schwager, A., Wolfer, D. P., Morison, P., White, S. H., & Feuer, M. J. (Eds.) (1996). The use of IQ
Lipp, H. P., & Schutz, G. (1997). Defective limbic system in mice tests in special education decision making and planning. Board on
lacking the tailless gene. Nature, 390, 515–517. Testing and Assessment, National Research Council. Washington,
Mondloch, C. J., Lewis, T. L., Budreau, D. R., Maurer, D., DC: National Academy Press.
Dannemiller, J. L. Stephens, B. R, & Kleiner-Gathercoal, K. A. Morison, S. J., Ames, E. W., & Chisholm, K. (1995). The develop-
(1999). Face perception during early infancy. Psychological Science, ment of children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Merrill-
10, 419–422. Palmer Quarterly, 41, 411–430.
Money, J. (1975). Ablatio penis: Normal male infant sex-reassigned as Moro, E. (1918). Das erste Trimenon. Münchener medizinische
a girl. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 4, 65–71. Wochenschrift, 65, 1147–1159.
Monroe, S. M. (2008). Modern approaches to conceptualizing and Morris, J., & Stone, G. (2011). Children and psychotropic medica-
measuring human life stress. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, tion: A cautionary note. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy,
4, 33–52. 37, 299–306.
Mooney, R. (1999). Sensitive periods and circuits for learned bird- Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American
song. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9, 121–127. and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of
Moore, C., & Corkum, V. (1994). Social understanding at the end of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949–971.
the first year of life. Developmental Review, 14, 349–372. Morrish, T. A., Gilbert, N., Myers, J. S., Vincent, B. J., Stamato,
Moore, D., Benenson, J., Reznick, J. S., Peterson, M., & Kagan, J. T. D., Taccioli, G. E., Batzer, M. A., & Moran, J. V. (2002). DNA
(1987). Effect of auditory numerical information in infants’ look- repair mediated by endonuclease-independent LINE-1 retrotrans-
ing behavior: Contradictory evidence. Developmental Psychology, position. Nature Genetics, 31, 159–165.
23, 665–670. Morse v. Frederick, 551 U.S. 393 (2007).
Moore, K. L. (1988). The developing human: Clinically oriented embry- Mortensen, P. B., Pedersen, C. B., Melbye, M., Mors, O., &
ology (4th ed.). Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders. Ewald, H. (2003). Individual and familial risk factors for bipolar
Moore, K. L., & Persaud, T. V. N. (2003). The developing human: affective disorders in Denmark. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60,
Clinically oriented embryology (7th ed.). Philadelphia: Saunders 1209–1215.
Elsevier. Mortensen, P. B., Pedersen, C. B., Westergaard, T., Wohlfahrt, J.,
Moore, K. L., & Persaud, T. V. N. (2008). Before we are born: Essentials of Ewald, H., Mors, O., Andersen, P. K., & Melbye, M. (1999).
embryology and birth defects (7th ed.). Philadelphia: Saunders Elsevier. Effects of family history and place and season of birth on the risk
Moore, K. L., Persaud, T. V. N., & Torchia, M. G. (2013). Before of schizophrenia. New England Journal of Medicine, 340, 603–608.
we are born: Essentials of embryology and birth defects (8th ed.). Morton, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLERN: A
Philadelphia: Saunders Elsevier. two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review,
Moorman, E. A., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2008). Mothers’ cognitions 98, 164–181.
about children’s self-control: Implications for mothers’ responses Moscovitch, M., Kapur, S., Köhler, S., & Houle, S. (1995). Distinct
to children’s helplessness. Social Development, 17, 960–979. neural correlates of visual long-term memory for spatial location
Morales, M., Mundy, P., Crowson, M. M., Neal, A. R., & Delgado, and object identity: A positron emission tomography study in
C. E. F. (2005). Individual differences in infant attention skills, humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA,
joint attention and emotion regulation behavior. International 92, 3721–3725.
Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 259–263. Moses, A. M. (2008). Impacts of television viewing on young chil-
Morasch, K. C., & Bell, M. A. (2011). The role of inhibitory control dren’s literacy development in the USA: A review of the literature.
in behavioral and physiological expressions of toddler executive Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 8, 67–102.
function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 593–606. Moss, L. (2002). What genes can’t do. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
R-90 REFERENCES
Moster, D., Lie, R. T., & Markestad, T. (2008). Long-term medical Munakata, Y., Casey, B. J., & Diamond, A. (2004). Developmental
and social consequences of preterm birth. New England Journal of cognitive neuroscience: Progress and potential. Trends in Cognitive
Medicine, 359, 262–273. Sciences, 8, 122–128.
Moszkowski, R. M., Stack, D. M., & Chiarella, S. (2009). Infant Munakata, Y., McClelland, J. L., Johnson, M. H., & Siegler, R. S.
touch with gaze and affective behaviors during mother-infant still- (1997). Rethinking infant knowledge: Toward an adaptive pro-
face interactions: Co-occurrence and functions of touch. Infant cess account of successes and failures in object permanence tasks.
Behavior and Development, 32, 392–403. Psychological Review, 104, 686–713.
Motzkin, J., Newman, J. P., Kiehl, K., & Koenigs, M. (2011). Reduced Mundt, M. P. (2011). The impact of peer social networks on adoles-
prefrontal connectivity in psychopathy. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, cent alcohol use initiation. Academic Pediatrics, 11, 414–421.
17348–17357. Munn, P., & Dunn, J. (1989). Temperament and the developing
Moulson, M. C., Westerlund, A., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., & relationship between siblings. International Journal of Behavioral
Nelson, C. A. (2009). The effects of early experience on face rec- Development, 12, 433–451.
ognition: An event-related potential study of institutionalized chil- Munroe, R. L., & Munroe, R. H. (1992). Fathers in children’s envi-
dren in Romania. Child Development, 80, 1039–1056. ronments: A four culture study. In B. S. Hewlett (Ed.), Father-child
Mountain, J. L., & Risch, N. (2004). Assessing genetic contributions relations: Cultural and biosocial contexts (pp. 213–229). New York:
to phenotypic differences among “racial” and “ethnic” groups. Aldine de Gruyter.
Nature Genetics, 36, S48–S53. Muraskas, J. K., Rau, B. J., Castillo, P. R., Gianopoulos J., & Boyd,
Movellan, J. R., & Watson, J. S. (2002). The development of gaze fol- L. A. (2012). Long-term follow-up of 2 newborns with a combined
lowing as a Bayesian systems identification problem. In Proceedings birth weight of 540 grams. Pediatrics, 129, e174–e178.
of the 2nd international conference on development and learning Murdock, B. B., Jr. (1962). The serial position effect of free recall.
(pp. 34–40). Los Alamitos, CA: IEEE Computer Society. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 482–488.
Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can Murdock, M. C. (2003). The effects of teaching programmes intended
undermine children’s motivation and performance. Journal of to stimulate creativity: A disciplinary view. Scandinavian Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 33–52. Educational Research, 47, 339–357.
Muir, D. (2010). The potentiation of peripheral nerve sheaths in Murgatroyd, C., Patchev, A.V., Wu, Y., Micale, V., Bockmühl, Y.,
regeneration and repair. Experimental Neurology, 223, 102–111. Fischer, D., Holsboer, F., Wotjak, C. T., Almeida, O. F. X., &
Muir, D., & Clifton, R. K. (1985). Infants’ orientation to the loca- Spengler, D. (2009). Dynamic DNA methylation programs per-
tion of sound sources. In G. Gotlieb & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), sistent adverse effects of early-life stress. Nature Neuroscience, 12,
Measurement of audition and vision in the first year of postnatal life. 1559–1566.
Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Muris, P., & Field, A. P. (2008). Distorted cognition and pathologi-
Muir, D. W., Clifton, R. K., & Clarkson, M. G. (1989). The develop- cal anxiety in children and adolescents. Cognition & Emotion, 22,
ment of a human auditory localization response: A U-shaped func- 395–421.
tion. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43, 199–216. Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., & Luijten, M. (2002b). The connection
Muir, D. W., & Field, J. (1979). Newborn infants orient to sounds. between cognitive development and specific fears and worries
Child Development, 50, 431–436. in normal children and children with below-average intellectual
Muir, D., & Hains, S. (2004). The U-shaped developmental function abilities: A preliminary study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40,
for auditory localization. Journal of Cognition & Development, 5, 37–56.
123–130. Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., de Jong, P. J., & Ollendick, T. H. (2002a).
Muir, D. W., & Mitchell, D. E. (1973). Visual resolution and experi- The etiology of specific fears and phobias in children: A critique
ence: Acuity deficits in cats following early selective visual depriva- of the non-associative account. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
tion. Science, 180, 420–422. 40, 185–195.
Mullen, M. K. (1994). Earliest recollections of childhood: A demo- Muroff, J., & Ross, A. (2011). Social disability and impairment in
graphic analysis. Cognition, 52, 55–79. childhood anxiety. In D. McKay & E. A. Storch (Eds.), Handbook
Mullen, M. K., & Yi, S. (1995). The cultural context of talk about the of child and adolescent anxiety disorders (pp. 457–478). New York:
past: Implications for the development of autobiographical mem- Springer.
ory. Cognitive Development 10, 407–419. Murphy, G. L. (2002). The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Mullen, P. E., Martin, J. L., Anderson, J. C., Romans, S. E., & Press.
Herbison, G. P. (1994). The effect of child sexual abuse on social, Murphy, G. L., & Lassaline, M. E. (1997). Hierarchical structure in
interpersonal and sexual function in adult life. British Journal of concepts and the basic level of categorization. In K. Lamberts &
Psychiatry, 165, 35–47. D. R. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, concepts and categories: Studies in
Muller, M. N., Marlowe, F. W., Bugumba, R., & Ellison, P. T. cognition (pp. 93–131). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
(2009). Testosterone and paternal care in East African foragers Murphy, L. M. B., Laurie-Rose, C., Brinkman, T. M., & McNamara,
and pastoralists. Proceedings of the Royal Society: Series B, 276, K. A. (2007). Sustained attention and social competence in typi-
347–354. cally developing preschool-aged children. Early Child Development
Mumme, D. L., & Fernald, A. (2003). The infant as onlooker: and Care, 177, 133–149.
Learning from emotional reactions observed in a television sce- Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of inter-
nario. Child Development, 74, 221–237. actions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In T. M.
Mumme, D. L., Fernald, A., & Herrera, C. (1996). Infants’ responses Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 177–197).
to facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing para- Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
digm. Child Development, 67, 3219–3237. Murtha, M. T., Leckman, J. F., & Ruddle, F. H. (1991). Detection of
Munakata, Y. (2004). Computational cognitive neuroscience of early homeobox genes in development and evolution. Proceedings of the
memory development. Developmental Review, 24, 133–153. National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 88, 10711–10715.
REFERENCES R-91
Murton, A. J., & Greenhaff, P. L. (2009). Muscle atrophy in immobi- spent in child care predict socioemotional adjustment during the
lization and senescence in humans. Current Opinion in Neurology, transition to kindergarten? Child Development, 74, 976–1005.
22, 500–505. National Research Council. Kindergarten through Eighth Grade
Mutti, D. O., Mitchell, G. L., Jones, L. A., Friedman, N. E., Frane, Committee on Science Learning. (2007). Taking science to school:
S. L., Lin, W. K., Moeschberger, M. L., & Zadnik, K. (2004). Learning and teaching science in grades K- 8 (R. A. Duschl, H. A.
Refractive astigmatism and the toricity of ocular components in Schweingruber, & A. W. Shouse, Eds). Committee on science
human infants. Optometry and Vision Science, 81, 753–761. learning, kindergarten through eighth grade. Washington, DC:
Myers, N. A., Clifton, R. K., & Clarkson, M. G. (1987). When National Academies Press.
they were young: Almost-threes remember two years ago. Infant National Research Council. (1996). National Science Education
Behaviour and Development, 10, 123–132. Standards. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
National Research Council. (2001). Adding it up: Helping children
Nadder, T. S., Silberg, J. L., Eaves, L. J., Maes, H. H., & Meyer, learn mathematics. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
J. M. (1998). Genetic effects on ADHD symptomatology in 7- National Research Council. (2006). Systems for state science assess-
to 13-year-old twins: Results from a telephone survey. Behavior ment. Committee on Test Design for K-12 Science Achievement.
Genetics, 28, 83–99. M. R. Wilson & M. W. Bertenthal (Eds.), Board on Testing
Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., & Réserbat-Plantey, and Assessment, Center for Education, Division of Behavioral
D. (1999). Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: National
Developmental Science, 2, 164–173. Academies Press.
Nagel, T. (1979). What is it like to be a bat? In T. Nagel (Ed.), Mortal ques- National Research Council. (2007). Taking science to school: Learning
tions (pp. 165–180). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. and teaching science in grades K- 8. Committee on Science
Nakahara, K., & Miyashita, Y. (2005). Understanding intentions: Learning, Kindergarten through Eighth Grade. R. A. Duschl,
Through the looking glass. Science, 308, 644–645. H. A. Schweingruber, & A. W. Shouse (Eds.), Board on Science
Nakamori, T., Sata, K., Atoji, Y., Kanamatsu, T., Tanaka, K., & Education, Center for Education, Division of Behavioral and Social
Ohki-Hamazaki, H. (2010). Demonstration of a neural circuit Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
critical for imprinting behavior in chicks. Journal of Neuroscience, Nawrot, E., Mayo, S. L., & Nawrot, M. (2009). The development
30, 4467–4480. of depth perception from motion parallax in infancy. Attention,
Nakhleh, M., & Samarapungavan, A. (1999). Elementary school chil- Perception and Psychophysics, 7, 194–199.
dren’s beliefs about matter. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, Nedivi, E. (1999). Molecular analysis of developmental plasticity in
36, 777–805. neocortex. Journal of Neurobiology, 41, 135–147.
Naninck, E. F., Lucassen, P. J., & Bakker, J. (2011). Sex differences in Needham, A., Dueker, G. L., & Lockhead, G. (2005). Infants’ formation
adolescent depression: Do sex hormones determine vulnerability? and use of categories to segregate objects. Cognition, 94, 215–240.
Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 23, 383–392. Needham, B. L., Fernandez, J. R., Lin, J., Epel, E. S., & Blackburn,
Nantais, K. M., & Schellenberg, E. G. (1999). The Mozart effect: An E. H. (2012). Socioeconomic status and cell aging in children.
artifact of preference. Psychological Science, 10, 370–373. Social Science and Medicine, 74, 1948–1951.
Nardini, M., Atkinson, J., & Burgess, N. (2008). Children reorient Neff, K. D., & Helwig, C. C. (2002). A constructivist approach to
using the left ? right sense of coloured landmarks at 18–24 months. understanding the development of reasoning about rights and
Cognition, 106, 519–527. authority within cultural contexts. Cognitive Development, 17,
Narvaez, D. (2002). Does reading moral stories build character? 1429–1450.
Educational Psychology Research, 14, 155–171. Negen, J., & Sarnecka, B. W. (2012). Number concept acquisition
Nasby, W., Hayden, B., & DePaulo, B. M. (1979). Attributional biases and general vocabulary development. Child Development, 83,
among aggressive boys to interpret unambiguous social stimuli as 2019–2027.
displays of hostility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 459–468. Neilson, I., Dockrell, J., & McKechnie, J. (1983). Justifying conserva-
Nash, R. J. (1997). Answering the virtuecrats: A moral conversation on tion: A reply to McGarrigle and Donaldson. Cognition, 15, 277–291.
character education. New York: Teachers College Press. Neisser, A. (1990). The other side of silence: Sign language and the deaf
Nasir, N. S., & Kirshner, B. (2003). The cultural construction of moral community in America. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University
and civic identities. Applied Developmental Science, 7, 138–147. Press.
National Center for Education Statistics. (2009). Highlights from Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-
Program for International Student Assessment (PISA) 2009: Century-Crofts.
Performance of U.S. 15-year-old students in reading, mathematics, Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality: Principles and implications of
and science literacy in an international context. Washington, DC: cognitive psychology. New York: W. H. Freeman.
U.S. Government Printing Office. Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical
National Institute of Child Care Health and Human Development, Psychology, 1, 35–59.
Early Care Research Network. (2006). Infant-mother attachment Neisser, U. (1993). The self perceived. In The perceived self: Ecological
classification: Risk and protection in relation to changing maternal and interpersonal sources of self knowledge (pp. 3–21). Cambridge,
caregiving quality. Developmental Psychology, 42, 38–58. UK: Cambridge University Press.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (2000). Neisser, U. (Ed.). (1998). The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ
Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An and related measures. Washington, DC: American Psychological
evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on read- Association.
ing and its implications for reading instruction. Washington, DC: Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Jr, Boykin, A. W., Brody, N.,
U.S. Government Printing Office. Ceci, S. J., Halpern, D. F., Loehlin, J. C., Perloff, R., Sternberg,
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, Early R. J., & Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns.
Child Care Research Network. (2003). Does the amount of time American Psychologist, 51, 77–101.
R-92 REFERENCES
Neisser, U., & Fivush, R. (1994). The remembering self: Construction Nesse, R. M. (2004). Natural selection and the elusiveness of happi-
and accuracy in the self-narrative. New York: Cambridge University ness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series
Press. B, Biological Sciences, 359, 1333–1347.
Neisser, U., & Harsch, N. (1993). Phantom flashbulbs: False recol- Neugarten, B. L., & Neugarten, D. A. (1986). Changing meanings of
lections of hearing the news about Challenger. In E. Winograd & age in the aging society. In A. Pifer & L. Bronte, Our aging society:
U. Neisser (Eds.), Emory Symposia in Cognition: Vol. 4. Affect and Paradox and promise (pp. 33–51). New York: W. W. Norton.
accuracy in recall: Studies of “ flashbulb” memories (pp. 9–31). New Neumann, C. S., Grimes, K., Walker, E. F., & Baum, K. (1995).
York: Cambridge University Press. Developmental pathways to schizophrenia: Behavioral stereotypes.
Neisser, U., & Libby, L. K. (2000). Remembering life experiences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 558–566.
In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of Neuwirth, S. (1994). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. NIH
memory (pp. 315–332). New York: Oxford University Press. Publication No. 96-3572. Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of
Neiworth, J. J., Steinmark, E., Basile, B. M, Wonders, R., Steely, F., & Health.
DeHart, C. (2003). A test of object permanence in a new-world Neville, H. J. (1991). Neurobiology of cognitive and language process-
monkey species, cotton top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Animal ing: Effects of early experience. In K. R. Gibson & A. C. Petersen
Cognition, 6, 27–37. (Eds.), Brain maturation and cognitive development: Comparative
Nell, V. (2002). Why young men drive dangerously: Implications for and cross-cultural perspectives (pp. 355–380). Hawthorne, NY: de
injury prevention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, Gruyter Press.
75–79. Newbury, D. F., Bonora, E., Lamb J. A., Fisher, S. E., Lai, C. S. L.,
Nelson, C. A. (1995). The ontogeny of human memory: A cognitive Baird, G., Jannoun, L., Slonims, V., Stott, C. M., Merricks, M. J.,
neuroscience perspective. Developmental Psychology, 31, 723–738. Bolton, P. F., Bailey, A. J., Monaco A. P, & the International
Nelson, C. A. (1997). The neurobiological basis of early memory Molecular Genetic Study of Autism Consortium. (2002). FOXP2
development. In N. Cowan (Ed.), The development of memory in is not a major susceptibility gene for autism or specific language
childhood (pp. 41–82). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. impairment. American Journal of Human Genetics, 70, 1318–1327.
Nelson, C. A. (2007). A neurobiological perspective on early human Newbury, D. F., Paracchini, S., Scerri, T. S., Winchester, L., Addis, L.,
deprivation. Child Development Perspectives, 1, 13–18. Richardson, A. J., Walter, J., Stein, J. F., Talcott, J. B., & Monaco, A.
Nelson, C. A., Bos, K. J., Gunnar, M. R., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2011). (2011). Investigation of dyslexia and SLI risk variants in reading-
The neurobiological toll of early human deprivation. Monographs and language-impaired subjects. Behavior Genetics, 41, 90–104.
of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76, 127–146. Newcomb, A. F., & Bagwell, C. L. (1995). Children’s friendship rela-
Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H., Fox, N. A., Marshall, P. J., Smyke, tions: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 306–347.
A. T., & Guthrie, D. (2007). Cognitive recovery in socially deprived Newcomb, A. F., Bukowski, W. M., & Pattee, L. (1993). Children’s peer
young children: The Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Science, relations: A meta-analytic review of popular, rejected, neglected,
318, 1937–1940. controversial, and average sociometric status. Psychological Bulletin,
Nelson, D. A., & Marler, P. (1989). Categorical perception of a natural 113, 99–128.
stimulus continuum: Birdsong. Science, 244, 976–978. Newcombe, N. S. (2002). The nativist-empiricist controversy in the
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs context of recent research on spatial and quantitative development.
of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38 (Serial No. 149). Psychological Science, 13, 395–401.
Nelson, K. (1986). Event knowledge and cognitive development. In K. Newcombe, N. S., Drummey, A. B., Fox, N. A., Lie, E., & Ottinger-
Nelson (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in develop- Alberts, W. (2000). Remembering early childhood: How much,
ment (pp. 1–19). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. how, and why (or why not). Current Directions in Psychological
Nelson, K. (1993). The psychological and social origins of autobio- Science, 9, 55–58.
graphical memory. Psychological Science, 4, 7–14. Newcombe, N., Dubas, J. S., & Baenninger, M. (1989). Associations
Nelson, K. (2001). Language and the self: From the “experiencing I” of timing of puberty, spatial ability, and lateralization in adult
to the “continuing me.” In C. Moore & K. Lemmon (Eds.), The women. Child Development, 60, 246–254.
self in time: Developmental perspectives (pp. 15–33). Mahwah, NJ: Newcombe, N. S., & Huttenlocher, J. (2000). Making space: The
Erlbaum. development of spatial representation and reasoning. Cambridge,
Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographi- MA: MIT Press.
cal memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Newcombe, N. S., Huttenlocher, J., & Learmonth, A. (1999).
Review, 111, 486–511. Infants’ coding of location in continuous space. Infant Behavior &
Nelson, K., & Gruendel, J. (1986). Children’s scripts. In K. Nelson Development, 22, 483–510.
(Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development Newcombe, N., Huttenlocher, J., Drummey, A. B., & Wiley, J.
(pp. 21–46). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (1998). The development of spatial location coding: Place learning
Nelson, S. A. (1980). Factors influencing young children’s use of and dead reckoning in the second and third years. Cognitive
motives and outcomes as moral criteria. Child Development, 51, Development, 13, 185–201.
823–829. Newcombe, N. S., Ratliff, K. R., Shallcross, W. L., & Twyman, A. D.
Nelson-LeGall, S. (1985). Motive-outcome matching and outcome (2009). Young children’s use of features to reorient is more than
foreseeability: Effects on attribution of intentionality and moral just associative: Further evidence against a modular view of spatial
judgments. Developmental Psychology, 21, 332–337. processing. Developmental Science, 13, 213–220.
Nesdale, D., & Flesser, D. (2001). Social identity and the development Newcombe, N. S., Sluzenski, J., & Huttenlocher, J. (2005). Preexisting
of children’s group attitudes. Child Development, 72, 506–517. knowledge versus on-line learning: What do young infants really
Nesse, R. M. (1999). Proximate and evolutionary studies of anxiety, know about spatial location? Psychological Science, 16, 222–227.
stress and depression: Synergy at the interface. Neuroscience and Newcombe, N. S., Uttal, D. H., & Sauter, M. (2013). Spatial develop-
Biobehavioral Reviews, 23, 895–903. ment. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of developmental
REFERENCES R-93
psychology: Vol. 1. Body and mind. New York: Oxford University Nicholls, D., & Bryant-Waugh, R. (2009). Eating disorders of infancy
Press. and childhood: Definition, symptomatology, epidemiology, and
Newcomer, V. D., & Young, E. M. (Eds.). (1989). Geriatric dermatol- comorbidity. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North
ogy: Clinical diagnosis and practical therapy. New York: Igaku-Shoin. America, 18, 17–30.
Newcorn, J. H., Halperin. J. M., Healey, J. M., O’Brien, J. D., Nichols, P., & Chen T. (1981). Minimal brain dysfunction: A prospec-
Pascualvaca, D. M., Wolf, L. E., Morganstein, A., Sharma, V., & tive study. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Young, J. G. (1989). Are ADDH and ADHD the same or dif- Nicolson, R., & Rapoport, J. L. (1999). Childhood-onset schizophre-
ferent? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent nia: Rare but worth studying. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 1418–1428.
Psychiatry, 28, 734–738. Niedenthal, P., Mermillod, M., Maringer, M., & Hess, U. (2010). The
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Engle- Simulation of Smiles (SIMS) model: Embodied simulation and
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. the meaning of facial expression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33,
Newell, K. (1986). Constraints to the development of coordination. 417–433.
In M. G. Wade & H. T. A. Whiting (Eds.), Motor development Nielsen, M., Slaughter, V., & Suddendorf, T. (2006). Self-recognition
in children: Aspects of coordination (pp. 341–360). Dordrecht, the beyond the face. Child Development, 77, 176–185.
Netherlands: Matinus Nijhoff. Nielsen, T. A., Laberge, L., Paquet, J., Tremblay, R. E., Vitaro, F., &
Newheiser, A., & Olson, K. R. (2012). White and Black American Montplaisir, J. (2000). Development of disturbing dreams during ado-
children’s implicit intergroup bias. Journal of Experimental Social lescence and their relation to anxiety symptoms. Sleep, 23, 727–736.
Psychology, 48, 264–270. Nigg, J. T., & Casey, B. J. (2005). An integrative theory of attention-
Newman, A., Bavalier, D., Corina, D. P., Jezzard, P., & Neville, H. deficit/hyperactivity disorder based on the cognitive and affective
(2002). A critical period for right hemisphere recruitment in neurosciences. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 785–806.
American Sign Language processing. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 76–80. Nigg, J. T., Goldsmith, H. H., & Sachek, J. (2004). Temperament
Newman, G. E., Choi, H., Wynn, K., & Scholl, B. J. (2008a). The and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: The development of a
origins of causal perception: Evidence from postdictive processing multiple pathway model. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 57, 262–291. Psychology, 33, 42–53.
Newman, G. E., Herrmann, P., Wynn, K., & Keil, F. C. (2008b). Nikolas, M. A., & Burt, S. A. (2010). Genetic and environmental influ-
Biases towards internal features in infants’ reasoning about objects. ences on ADHD symptom dimensions of inattention and hyperac-
Cognition, 107, 420–432. tivity: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 1–17.
Newman, G. E., Keil, F. C., Kuhlmeier, V. A., & Wynn, K. (2010). Early Nilsen, E. S., & Graham, S. A. (2009). The relations between chil-
understandings of the link between agents and order. Proceedings of dren’s communicative perspective-taking and executive function-
the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 107, 17140–17145. ing. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 220–249.
Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learn- Niparko. J. K., Tobey, E. A., Thal, D. J., Eisenberg, L. S., Wang,
ing. Cognitive Science, 14, 11–28. N.-Y., Quittner, A. L., & Fink, N. E. (2010). Spoken language
Newport, E. L., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. R. (1977). Mother, development in children following cochlear implantation. Journal
I’d rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal of the American Medical Association, 303, 1498–1506.
speech style. In C. E. Snow & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to Nisan, M., & Kohlberg, L. (1982). Universality and variation in moral
children: Language input and acquisitions (pp. 109–149). New York: judgment: A longitudinal and cross-sectional study in Turkey.
Cambridge University Press. Child Development, 53, 865–876.
NICD Early Child Care Research Network. (2003). Does amount of Nisan, M., & Kohlberg, L. (1984). Cultural universality of moral
time spent in child care predict socioemotional adjustment during judgment stages: A longitudinal study in Turkey. In L. Kohlberg
the transition to kindergarten? Child Development, 74, 976–1005. (Ed.), Essays on moral development: Vol. 2. The psychology of moral
Nicely, P., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1999). development: The nature and validity of moral stages (pp. 582–593).
Mothers’ attuned responses to infant affect expressivity promote New York: Harper & Row.
earlier achievement of language milestones. Infant Behavior and Nisbett, R. E. (1998). Race, genetics, and IQ. In C. Jencks &
Development, 22, 557–568. M. Phillips (Eds.), Black-white test score differences (pp. 86–102).
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1998). Early child care Washington, DC: Brookings Institution.
and self-control, compliance, and problem behavior at twenty-four Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and
and thirty-six months. Child Development, 69, 1145–1170. Westerners think differently . . . and why. New York: Free Press.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1999). Child out- Nisbett, R. E. (2005). Heredity, environment, and race differences
comes when child care center classes meet recommended standards in IQ: A commentary on Rushton and Jensen. Psychology, Public
for quality. American Journal of Public Health, 89, 1072–1077. Policy, and Law, 11, 302–310.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2001). Child care Nisbett, R. E. (2011). The achievement gap: Past, present & future.
and family predictors of preschool attachment and stability from Daedelus, 140, 90–100.
infancy. Developmental Psychology, 37, 847–862. Nisbett, R. E., Aronson, J., Blair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern,
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2002). Early child care D. F., & Turkheimer, E. (2012). Intelligence: New findings and
and children’s development prior to school entry: Results from the theoretical developments. American Psychologist, 67, 130–159.
NICHD study of early child care. American Educational Research Nisbett, R. E., & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of honor: The psychology of
Journal, 39, 133–164. violence in the South. Denver, CO: Westview Press.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2005). Child care and Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture
child development: Results of the NICHD study of early child care and and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition.
youth development. New York: Guilford Press. Psychological Review, 108, 291–310.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2006). Child-care Njoroge, W. F., & Yang, D. (2012). Evidence-based psychothera-
effect sizes for the NICHD study of early child care and youth pies for preschool children with psychiatric disorders. Current
development. American Psychologist, 61, 99–116. Psychiatry Reports, 14, 121–128.
R-94 REFERENCES
Nolan, D., James, I., & Mallal, S. (2005). HIV: Experiencing the pres- O’Connor, T. G., Caspi, A., DeFries, J. C., & Plomin, R. (2000).
sures of modern life. Science, 307, 1422–1424. Are associations between parental divorce and children’s adjust-
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Women who think too much: How to ment genetically mediated? An adoption study. Developmental
break free of overthinking and reclaim your life. New York: Holt. Psychology, 36, 429–437.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Abnormal psychology (5th ed.). New York: O’Connor, T. G., & Croft, C. M. (2001). A twin study of attachment
McGraw-Hill. in preschool children. Child Development, 72, 1501–1511.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Girgus, J. S. (1994). The emergence of gen- O’Connor, T. G., Deater-Deckard, K., Fulker, D., Rutter, M., &
der differences in depression during adolescence. Psychological Plomin R. (1998). Genotype-environment correlations in late
Bulletin, 115, 424–443. childhood and early adolescence: Antisocial behavioral problems
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Stice, E., Wade, E., & Bohon, C. (2007). and coercive parenting. Developmental Psychology, 34, 970–981.
Reciprocal relations between rumination and bulimic, substance O’Connor, T. G., Heron, J., Golding, J., Glover, V., & the ALSPAC
abuse, and depressive symptoms in adolescent females. Journal of Study Team. (2003). Maternal antenatal anxiety and behavioural/
Abnormal Psychology, 116, 198–207. emotional problems in children: A test of a programming hypoth-
Nopola-Hemmi, J., Myllyluoma, B., Haltia, T., Taipale, M., esis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 1025–1036.
Ollikainen, V., Ahonen, T., Voutilainen, A., Kere, J., & Widen, E. OECD Family Database (2010). SF1.1: Family size and household
(2001). A dominant gene for developmental dyslexia on chromo- composition. Paris: Organization for Economic Co-operation and
some 3. Journal of Medical Genetics, 38, 658–664. Development. http://www.oecd.org/els/soc/41919509.pdf
Norcia, A. M, Pei, F., Bonneh, Y., Hou, C., Sampath, V., & Pettet, Ofen, N., Chai, X. J., Schuil, K. D., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., & Gabrieli,
M. W. (2005). Development of sensitivity to texture and contour J. D. (2012). The development of brain systems associated with
information in the human infant. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, successful memory retrieval of scenes. Journal of Neuroscience, 32,
17, 569–579. 10012–10020.
Nordenström, A., Servin, A., Bohlin, G., Larsson, A., & Wedell, A. Offord, D. R., & Bennett, K. J. (1994). Conduct disorder: Long-term
(2002). Sex-typed toy play behavior correlates with the degree outcomes and intervention effectiveness. Journal of the American
of prenatal androgen exposure assessed by CYP21 genotype in Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33, 1069–1078.
girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Journal of Clinical Ofshe, R., & Watters, E. (1994). Making monsters: False memories,
Endocrinology and Metabolism 87, 5119–5124. psychotherapy, and sexual hysteria. New York: Scribner’s.
Novin, S., Banjeree, R., Dadhkah, A., & Rieffe, C. (2009). Self-reported Ogbu, J. U. (1978). Minority education and caste: The American system
use of emotional display rules in the Netherlands and Iran: Evidence in cross-cultural perspective. New York: Academic Press.
for sociocultural influence. Social Development, 18, 397–411. Ogbu, J. U. (1994). From cultural differences to differences in cultural
Nowakowski, R. S. (1987). Basic concepts of CNS development. frames of reference. In P. M. Greenfield & R. R. Cocking (Eds.),
Child Development, 58, 568–595. Cross-cultural roots of minority child development (pp. 365–391).
Nowakowski, R. S. (1991). Genetic disturbances of neuronal migra- Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
tion: Some examples from the limbic system of mutant mice. In Ogbu, J. U., & Stern, P. (2001). Caste status and intellectual develop-
S. A. Mednick, T. D. Cannon, C. E. Barr, & M. Lyon (Eds.), Fetal ment. In R. J. Sternberg & E. Grigorenko (Eds.), Environmental
neural development and adult schizophrenia (pp. 69–96). New York: effects on cognitive abilities (pp. 1–37). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cambridge University Press. O’Grady, W. (1997). Syntactic development. Chicago: University of
Nucci, L. P. (2001). Education in the moral domain. Cambridge, UK: Chicago Press.
Cambridge University Press. Ogren, M. P., & Lombroso, P. J. (2008). Epigenetics: Behavioral influ-
Nucci, L. P. (2002). The development of moral reasoning. In U. ences on gene function: Part II. Molecular mechanisms. Journal of the
Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive develop- American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 374–378.
ment (pp. 303–325). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. Oh, S., & Lewis, C. (2008). Korean preschoolers’ advanced inhibitory
Nunes, T., Schliemann, A. D., & Carraher, D. W. (1993). Street control and its relation to other executive skills and mental state
mathematics and school mathematics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge understanding. Child Development, 79, 80–99.
University Press. O’Hara, L. A., & Sternberg, R. J. (2000). It doesn’t hurt to ask:
Effects of instructions to be creative, practical, or analytical on
O’Connor, T. G., Rutter, M., & the English and Romanian Adoptees essay-writing performance and their interaction with students’
Study Team. (2000). Attachment disorder behavior following early thinking styles. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 197–210.
severe deprivation: Extension and longitudinal follow-up. Journal Ohman, A., Dimberg, U., & Ost, L. G. (1985). Biological constraints
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, on the fear response. In S. Reiss & R. Bootsin (Eds.), Theoretical
702–712. issues in behavior therapy (pp. 123–175). New York: Academic Press.
O’Sullivan, L. F., & Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L. (2003). African- Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias and preparedness:
American and Latina inner-city girls’ reports of romantic and Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological
sexual development. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, Review, 108, 483–522.
20, 221–238. Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2003). The malicious serpent: Snakes
Oades-Sese, G. V., Esquivel, G. B., Kaliski, P. K., & Maniatis, L. as a prototypical stimulus for an evolved module of fear. Current
(2011). A longitudinal study of the social and academic compe- Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 5–9.
tence of economically disadvantaged bilingual preschool children. Okun, M. A., & Keith, V. A. (1998). Effects of positive and nega-
Developmental Psychology, 47, 747–764. tive social exchanges with various sources on depressive symp-
Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease- toms in younger and older adults. Journals of Gerontology, Series B:
avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 303–321. Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 53, 4–20.
O’Connor, T. G. (2002). The “effects” of parenting reconsidered: Olausson, P. O., Haglund, B., Weitoft, G. R., & Cnattingius, S. (2001).
Findings, challenges, and applications. Journal of Child Psychology Teenage childbearing and long-term socioeconomic consequences:
and Psychiatry 43, 555–572. A case study in Sweden. Family Planning Perspectives, 33, 70–74.
REFERENCES R-95
Oldehinkel, A. J., Ormel, J., Veenstra, R., De Winter, A. F., & O’Neill, D. K. (1996). Two-year-olds’ sensitivity to a parent’s
Verhulst, F. C. (2008). Parental divorce and offspring depressive knowledge state when making requests. Child Development, 67,
symptoms: Dutch developmental trends during early adolescence. 659–677.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 70, 284–293. O’Neill, D. K., & Chong, S. (2001). Preschool children’s difficulty in
Olds, D. L. (2006). The nurse-family partnership: An evidence-based understanding the types of information obtained through the five
preventive intervention. Infant Mental Health Journal, 27, 5–25. senses. Child Development, 72, 803–815.
Olds, D., Henderson, C., Chamberlin, R., & Tatelbaum, R. (1986). Onishi, K., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants
Preventing child abuse and neglect: A randomized trial of nurse understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255–258.
home visitation. Pediatrics, 78, 65–78. Oosterwegel, A., & Oppenheimer, L. (1993). The self-system:
Olds, D. L., Sadler, L., & Kitzman, H. (2007). Programs for parents Developmental changes between and within self-concepts. Hillsdale,
of infants and toddlers: Recent evidence from randomized trials. NJ: Erlbaum.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 355–391. Orobio de Castro, B., Veerman, J. W., Koops, W., Bosch, J. D., &
O’Leary, D. D. M., Schlaggar, B. L., & Tuttle, R. (1994). Specification Monshouwer, H. J. (2002). Hostile attribution of intent and aggres-
of neocortical areas and thalamocortical conections. Annual sive behavior: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 73, 916–934.
Review of Neuroscience, 17, 419–439. Orth, U., Robins, R. W., & Widaman, K. F. (2011). Life-span devel-
O’Leary, D. D. M., & Stanfield, B. B. (1989). Selective elimination opment of self-esteem and its effects on important life outcomes.
of axons extended by developing cortical neurons is dependent on Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 607–619.
regional locale: Experiments utilizing fetal cortical transplants. Orton, J., Spittle, A., Doyle, L., Anderson, P., & Boyd, R. (2009).
Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 2230–2246. Do early intervention programmes improve cognitive and motor
O’Leary, D. D. M., Yates, P. A., & McLaughlin, T. (1999). Molecular outcomes for preterm infants after discharge? A systematic review.
development of sensory maps: Representing sights and smells in Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 51, 851–859.
the brain. Cell, 96, 255–269. Osherson, D. N. (1975). Logical abilities in children: Vol. 3. Reasoning
Ollendick, T. H., King, N. J., & Frary, R. B. (1989). Fears in children in adolescence: Deductive inference. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
and adolescents: Reliability and generalizability across gender, age Oster, H., Hegley, D., & Nagel, L. (1992). Adult judgments and fine-
and nationality. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27, 19–26 grained analysis of infant facial expressions: Testing the validity of
Ollikainen, M., & Craig, J. M. (2011). Epigenetic discordance at a priori coding formulas. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1115–1131.
imprinting control regions in twins. Epigenomics, 3, 295–306. Osterling, J. A., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. A. (2002). Early recog-
Olson, D. (1994). Demythologizing literacy. In D. R. Olson (Ed.), The nition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus
world on paper: The conceptual and cognitive implications of writing mental retardation. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 239–251.
and reading (pp. 1–19). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Ostrov, J. M., Gentile, D. A., & Crick, N. R. (2006). Media exposure,
Olson, D. R., & Torrance, N. (Eds.). (2001). The making of literate aggression and prosocial behavior during early childhood: A longi-
societies. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers. tudinal study. Social Development, 15, 612–627.
Olson, I. R., Zhang, J. X., Mitchell, K. J., Johnson, M. K., Bloise, Ostrovsky, Y., Andalman, A., & Sinha, P. (2006). Vision follow-
S. M., & Higgins, J. A. (2004). Preserved spatial memory over ing extended congenital blindness. Psychological Science, 17,
brief intervals in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 19, 310–317. 1009–1014.
Olson, K. R., & Shaw, A. (2011). “No fair, copycat!” What children’s Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2013). The social side of imitation. Child
response to plagiarism tells us about their understanding of ideas. Development Perspectives, 7, 6–11.
Developmental Science, 14, 431–439. Overton, W. F., & Reese, H. W. (1981). Conceptual prerequisites for
Olson, K. R., Shutts, K., Kinzler, K. D., & Weisman, K. G. (2012). an understanding of stability-change and continuity-discontinuity.
Children associate racial groups with wealth: Evidence from South International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 99–123.
Africa. Child Development, 83, 1884–1899. Oyserman, L., & Lee, S. W. (2008). Does culture influence what and
Olson, S. (2001, April). The genetic archeology of race. Atlantic how we think? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311–342.
Monthly. Özgen, E. (2004). Language, learning, and color perception. Current
Olson, S. L., Bates, J. E., Sandy, J. M., & Schilling, E. M. (2002). Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 95–98.
Early developmental precursors of impulsive and inattentive behav-
ior: From infancy to middle childhood. Journal of Child Psychology Pahlke, E., Bigler, R. S., & Suizzo, M. A. (2012). Relations between
and Psychiatry, 43, 435–447. colorblind socialization and children’s racial bias: Evidence from
Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at school: What we know and what we can European American mothers and their preschool children. Child
do. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Development, 83, 1164–1179.
Olweus, D. (1994). Bullying at school: Long-term outcomes for the Paik, H., & Comstock, G. (1994). The effects of television violence
victims and an effective school-based intervention program. In on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis. Communication Research,
L. R. Huesmann (Ed.), Aggressive behavior: Current perspectives 21, 516–546.
(pp. 97–130). New York: Plenum Press. Pakarinen, E., Lerkkanen, M.-K., Poikkeus, A.-M., Siekkinen, M., &
Olweus, D. (1997). Bully/victim problems in school: Facts and inter- Nurmi, J.-E. (2011). Kindergarten teachers adjust their teach-
vention. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 12, 495–510. ing practices in accordance with children’s academic pre-skills.
Olweus, D. (2011). Bullying at school and later criminality: Findings Educational Psychology, 31, 37–53.
from three Swedish community samples of males. Criminal Paley, W. (1802). Natural theology; or, evidences of the existence and
Behaviour and Mental Health, 21, 151–156. attributes of the deity (12th ed.). London: J. Faulder.
Olweus, D. (2012). Cyberbullying: An overrated phenomenon? Pallanti, S. (2008). Transcultural observations of obsessive-compulsive
European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 520–538. disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 169–170.
Omark, D. R., Omark, M., & Edelman, M. (1975). Formation of Palmer, B. R., Gignac, G., Manocha, R., & Stough, C. (2005). A
dominance hierarchies in young children. In T. R. Williams (Ed.), psychometric evaluation of the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional
Psychological anthropology (pp. 133–156). The Hague: Mouton. Intelligence Test Version 2.0. Intelligence, 33, 285–305.
R-96 REFERENCES
Palmer, B. W., & Dawes, S. E. (2010). Cognitive aging: From basic Parker, J., Rubin, K. H., Erath, S., Wojslawowicz, J. C., & Buskirk,
skills to scripts and schemata. In C. A. Depp & D. V. Jeste (Eds.), A. A. (2006). Peer relationships, child development, and adjustment:
Successful cognitive and emotional aging (pp. 37–54). Washington, A developmental psychopathology perspective. In D. Cichetti &
DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. D. Cohen, Developmental psychopathology: Risk, disorder, and adap-
Palmer, S. E. (1999). Vision science: Photons to phenomenology. tation: Vol. 1. Theory and method (2nd ed., pp. 419–493). Hoboken,
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. NJ: Wiley.
Panagiotaki, G., Nobes, G., & Banerjee, R. (2006). Is the world round Parker, K. L, Schedl, A., & Schimmer, B. P. (1999). Gene interactions
or flat? Children’s understanding of the earth. European Journal of in gonadal development. Annual Review of Physiology, 61, 417–433.
Developmental Psychology, 3, 124–141. Parkhurst, J. T., & Hopmeyer, A. (1998). Sociometric popularity and
Pankratz, M. J., & Jackle, H. (1990). Making stripes in the Drosophila peer-perceived popularity: Two distinct dimensions of peer status.
embryo. Trends in Genetics, 6, 287–292. Journal of Early Adolescence, 18, 125–144.
Pannese, M., Polo, C., Andreazzoli, M., Vignali, R., Kablar, B., Parnavelas, J. G., Papadopoulos, G. C., & Cavanagh, M. E. (1988).
Barsacchi, G., & Boncinelli, E. (1995). The Xenopus homologue of Changes in neurotransmitters during development. In A. Peters & E. G.
Otx2 is a maternal homeobox gene that demarcates and specifies Jones (Eds.), Cerebral cortex (pp. 177–205). New York: Plenum Press.
anterior body regions. Development, 121, 707–720. Parsons, C. (1960). Inhelder and Piaget’s “The growth of logical
Parasuraman, R., Greenwood, P. M., Kumar, R., & Fossella, J. (2005). thinking.” British Journal of Psychology, 51, 75–84.
Beyond heritability: Neurotransmitter genes differentially modu- Partridge, L. (2010). The new biology of ageing. Philosophical
late visuospatial attention and working memory. Psychological Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 365, 147–154.
Science, 16, 200–207. Pascalis, O., de Haan, M., & Nelson, C. A. (2002). Is face processing
Pardo, J. S., & Fowler, C. A. (1997). Perceiving the causes of coar- species-specific during the first year of life? Science, 296, 1321–1323.
ticulatory acoustic variation: Consonant voicing and vowel pitch. Pascalis, O., de Schonen, S., Morton, J., Deruelle, C., & Fabre-Grenet,
Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1141–1152. M. (1995). Mother’s face recognition by neonates: A replication
Parens, E., & Johnston, J. (2008). Understanding the agreements and and an extension. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 79–85.
controversies surrounding childhood psychopharmacology. Child Pascalis, O., & Kelly, D. J. (2009). The origins of face processing
and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 2, 5. in humans: Phylogeny and ontogeny. Perspectives on Psychological
Paris, S. G., & Paris, A. H. (2006). The influence of developmental Science, 4, 200–209.
skill trajectories on assessments of children’s early reading. In W. Pascalis, O., Scott, L. S., Kelly, D. J., Shannon, R. W., Nicholson, E.,
Damon (Series Ed.) & R. Lerner, K. A. Renninger, & I. E. Siegel Coleman, M., & Nelson, C. A. (2005). Plasticity of face process-
(Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Child psychology in ing in infancy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
practice (6th ed., pp. 48–74). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. USA, 102, 5297– 5300.
Park, D., & Schwarz, N. (Eds.). (2000). Cognitive aging: A primer. Pascual-Leone, J. A. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition
Philadelphia: Psychological Press/Taylor & Francis. rule in Piaget’s developmental stages. Acta Psychologica, 32, 301–345.
Park, S. Y., & Jameson, J. L. (2005). Minireview: Transcriptional reg- Pasquinelli, A. E., & Ruvkun, G. (2002). Control of developmental
ulation of gonadal development and differentiation. Endocrinology, timing by microRNAs and their targets. Annual Review of Cell and
146, 1035–1042. Developmental Biology, 18, 495–513.
Parke, R. D. (1969). Effectiveness of punishment as an interaction Passinghma, R. (2006). Cognitive science: Brain development and
of intensity, timing, agent nurturance, and cognitive structuring. IQ. Nature, 440, 619–620.
Child Development, 40, 213–235. Pasterski, V. L., Geffner, M. E., Brain, C., Hindmarsh, P., Brook, C., &
Parke, R. D. (1996). Fatherhood. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Hines, M. (2005). Prenatal hormones and postnatal socialization
Press. by parents as determinants of male-typical toy play in girls with
Parke, R. D. (2002). Punishment revisited— Science, values, and congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Child Development, 76, 264–278.
the right question: Comment on Gershoff (2002). Psychological Pastor, D. L. (1981). The quality of mother-infant attachment and its
Bulletin, 128, 596–601. relationship to toddlers’ initial sociability with peers. Developmental
Parke, R. D. (2004). Development in the family. Annual Review of Psychology, 17, 326–335.
Psychology, 55, 365–399. Pastore, N. (1965). Samuel Bailey’s critique of Berkeley’s theory of
Parke, R. D., & Asher, S. R. (1983). Social and personality develop- vision. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 1, 321–337.
ment. Annual Review of Psychology, 34, 465–509. Pasupathi, M. (2001). The social construction of the personal past
Parke, R. D., & Suomi, S. J. (1981). Adult male-infant relationships: and its implications for adult development. Psychological Bulletin,
Human and nonhuman primate evidence. In K. Immelmann, 127, 651–672.
G. W. Barlow, L. Petrinovich, & M. Main (Eds.), Behavioral devel- Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Robinson, J. C. (2008). The effects of
opment: The Bielefeld interdisciplinary project (pp. 700–725). New choice on intrinsic motivation and related outcomes: A meta-
York: Cambridge University Press. analysis of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 270–300.
Parke, R. D., & Walters, R. H. (1967). Some factors determining the Patchin, J. W., & Hinduja, S. (2006). Bullies move beyond the school-
efficacy of punishment training for inducing response inhibition. yard: A preliminary look at cyberbullying. Youth Violence and
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 32 Juvenile Justice, 4, 148–169.
(Serial No. 109), 1–45. Pate, J., Pumariega, A., Hester, C., & Garner, D. (1992). Cross-
Parker, J. F. (1995). Age differences in source monitoring of performed cultural patterns in eating disorders: A review. Journal of the
and imagined actions on immediate and delayed tests. Journal of American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 802–809.
Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 84–101. Patterson, C. J. (1992). Children of lesbian and gay parents. Child
Parker, J. G., Low, C. M., Walker, A. R., & Gamm, B. K. (2005). Development, 63, 1025–1042.
Friendship jealousy in young adolescents: Individual differences Patterson, C. J. (2013). Family lives of lesbian and gay adults. In G. W.
and links to sex, self-esteem, aggression, and social adjustment. Peterson & K. R. Bush (Eds.), Handbook of marriage and the family
Developmental Psychology, 41, 235–250. (3rd ed., pp. 659–682). New York: Springer.
REFERENCES R-97
Patterson, G. R., Chamberlain, P., & Reid, J. B. (1982). A compara- Pellegrini, A. D., & Long, J. (2002). A longitudinal study of bullying,
tive evaluation of a parent training program. Behavior Therapy, 13, dominance, and victimization during the transition from primary
638–650. to secondary school. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
Patterson, G. R., DeBaryshe, B. D., & Ramsey, E. (1989). A develop- 20, 259–280.
mental perspective on antisocial behaviour. American Psychologist, Pelphrey, K. A., Singerman, J. D., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (2003).
44, 329–335. Brain activation evoked by perception of gaze shifts: The influence
Patterson, P. H. (2011). Maternal infection and immune involvement of context. Neuropsychologia, 41, 156–170.
in autism. Trends in Molecular Medicine, 17, 389–394. Pelphrey, K. A., Viola, R. J., & McCarthy, G. (2004). When strangers
Paul, D. B., & Blumenthal, A. L. (1989). On the trail of Little Albert. pass: Processing of mutual and averted gaze in the superior tempo-
Psychological Record, 39, 547–553. ral sulcus. Psychological Science, 15, 598–603.
Paulhus, D. L., Trapnell, P. D., & Chen, D. (1999). Birth order effects Pelucchi, B., Hay, J. F., & Saffran, J. R. (2009). Statistical learning
on personality and achievement within families. Psychological in a natural language by 8-month-old infants. Child Development,
Science, 10, 482–488. 80, 674–685.
Pauls, D. (1991). Genetic factors in the expression of attention- Pemberton, E. F. (1990). Systematic errors in children’s drawings.
deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Cognitive Development, 5, 395–404.
Psychopharmacology, 1, 353–360. Pempek, T. A., Demers, L. B., Hanson, K., Kirkorian, H. L., & Anderson,
Pauls, D. (1992).The genetics of obsessive compulsive disorder and D. R. (2011). The impact of infant-directed videos on parent–child
Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome. Psychiatric Clinics of North interaction. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 32, 10–19.
America, 15, 759–766. Peng, K., Ames, D. R., & Knowles, E. D. (2001). Culture and human
Pauls, D. L. (2008). The genetics of obsessive compulsive disorder: A inference: Perspectives from three traditions. In D. Matsumoto
review of the evidence. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part (Ed.), Handbook of culture and psychology (pp. 245–264). New
C: Seminars in Medical Genetics, 148C, 133–139. York: Oxford University Press.
Pauls, D. L. (2012). The genetics of obsessive-compulsive disorder: Peng, K., & Knowles, E. (2003). Culture, ethnicity, and the attribu-
Current status. In J. Zohar (Ed.), Obsessive-compulsive disorder: tion of physical causality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Current science and clinical practice (pp. 277–299). Chichester, UK: 29, 1272–1284.
Wiley-Blackwell. Penn, D. C., & Povinelli, D. J. (2007). On the lack of evidence that
Pauls, D. L., Alsobrook, J. P., II, Goodman, W., Rasmussen, S., & chimpanzees possess anything remotely resembling a “theory of
Leckman, J. F. (1995). A family study of obsessive-compulsive dis- mind.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London,
order. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 76–84. Series B, 362, 731–744.
Pavlov, I. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. London: Oxford University Press. Penner, S. (1987). Parental responses to grammatical and ungram-
Payne, D. (2000). Ireland’s measles outbreak kills two. British Medical matical child utterances. Child Development, 58, 376–384.
Journal, 321, 197. Pennisi, E. (2007). Genetics. Working the (gene count) numbers:
Pea, R., Nass, C., Meheula, L., Rance, M., Kumar, M., Bamford, H., Finally, a firm answer? Science, 316, 1113.
Nass, M., Simha, A., Stillerman, B., Yang, S., & Zhou, M. (2012). Peper, J. S., Brouwer, R. M., Boomsma, D. I., Kahn, R. S., & Hulshoff
Media use, face-to-face communication, media multitasking, and Pol, H. E. ( 2007). Genetic influences on human brain structure:
social well-being among 8- to 12-year-old girls. Developmental A review of brain imaging studies in twins. Human Brain Mapping,
Psychology, 48, 327–336. 28, 464–473.
Pea, R. D., Tinker, R., Linn, M., Means, B., Bransford, J., Roschelle, J., Peplau, L. A., DeBro, S. C., Veniegas, R. C., & Taylor, P. L. (Eds.).
Hsi, S., Brophy, S., & Songer, N. (1999). Toward a learning tech- (1999). Gender, culture, and ethnicity: Current research about women
nologies knowledge network. Educational Technology Research and and men. Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.
Development, 47, 19–38. Pepler, D. J., & Craig, W. M. (1999). Understanding bullying and
Pearl, J. (2010). The foundations of causal inference, UCLA Cognitive victimization from a dynamic systems perspective. In A. Slater &
Systems Laboratory, Technical Report (R-355). Sociological D. Muir (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced reader
Methodology, 40, 75–149. (pp. 441–451). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Pearson, D., Rouse, H., Doswell, S., Ainsworth, C., Dawson, O., Pérez, J. C., Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2005). Measuring trait
Simms, K., Edwards, L., & Faulconbridge, J. (2001). Prevalence of emotional intelligence. In R. Schulze & R. D. Roberts (Eds.),
imaginary companions in a normal child population. Child: Care, International handbook of emotional intelligence. Cambridge, MA:
Health and Development, 27, 13–22. Hogrefe & Huber.
Pearson, G. (1983). Hooligan: A history of respectable fears. New York: Perfect, T. J., & Maylor, E. A. (2000). Models of cognitive aging.
Macmillan. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Pedersen, N. L., Plomin, R., & McClearn, G. E. (1994). Is there G Perfetti, C. A. (1999). Cognitive research and the misconceptions of
beyond g? (Is there genetic influence on specific cognitive abili- reading education. In J. Oakhill & R. Beard (Eds.), Reading devel-
ties independent of genetic influence on general cognitive ability?). opment and the teaching of reading (pp. 167–208). Oxford, UK:
Intelligence, 18, 133–143. Blackwell.
Peeples, D. R., & Teller, D. Y. (1975). Color vision and brightness Perls, T. T., & Fretts, R. C. (1998). Why women live longer than men.
discrimination in two-month-old human infants. Science, 189, Scientific American Presents, 9, 100–103.
1102–1103. Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge,
Peets, K., Hodges, E., & Salmivalli, C. (2008). Affect-congruent MA: MIT Press.
social-cognitive evaluations and behaviors. Child Development, 79, Perner, J. (1992). Grasping the concept of representation: Its impact
170–185. on 4-year-olds’ theory of mind and beyond. Human Development,
Pegg, J. E., Werker, J. F., & McLeod, P. J. (1992). Preference for infant- 35, 146–155.
directed over adult-directed speech: Evidence from 7-week-old Perner, J. (1995). The many faces of belief: Reflections on Fodor’s and
infants. Infant Behavior & Development, 15, 325–345. the child’s theory of mind. Cognition, 57, 241–269.
R-98 REFERENCES
Perner, J. (2001). Episodic memory: Essential distinctions and devel- Peterson, D. A., Bjork-Eriksson, T., Alborn, A. M., Nordborg, C.,
opmental implications. In C. Moore & K. Lemmon (Eds.), The self Peterson, D. A., & Gage, F. H. (1998). Neurogenesis in the adult
in time: Developmental perspectives (pp. 181–202). Mahwah, NJ: human hippocampus. Nature Medicine, 4, 1313–1317.
Erlbaum. Peterson, J. L., & Zill, N. (1986). Marital disruption, parent-child rela-
Perner, J., Kloo, D., & Gornik, E. (2007). Episodic memory devel- tionships, and behavior problems in children. Journal of Marriage
opment: Theory of mind is part of re-experiencing experienced and the Family, 48, 295–307.
events. Infant and Child Development, 16, 471–490. Petitto, L. A. (1992). Modularity and constraints in early lexical acqui-
Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds’ dif- sition: Evidence from children’s first words/signs and gestures. In
ficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British M.R. Gunnar & M. Maratsos (Eds.), Modularity and constraints in
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125–137. language and cognition: The Minnesota symposia on child psychology
Perner, J., & Ruffman, T. (2005, April 8). Infants’ insight into the (Vol. 25, pp. 25–58). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.
mind: How deep? Science, 308, 214–216. Petitto, L. A., Katerelos, M., Levy, B., Gauna, K., Tétrault, K., &
Perner, J., Sprung, M., Zauner, P., & Haider, H. (2003). Want that is Ferraro, V. (2001). Bilingual signed and spoken language acqui-
understood well before say that, think that, and false belief: A test of sition from birth: Implications for mechanisms underlying
de Villiers’ linguistic determinism on German- speaking children. bilingual language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 28,
Child Development, 74, 179–188. 1–44.
Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks Petitto, L., & Marentette, P. (1991). Babbling in the manual mode:
that . . .”: Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year old Evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science, 251, 1493–1496.
children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437–471. Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Supportive parent-
Perrett, D. I., Hietanen, J. K., Oram, M. W., & Benson, P. J. (1992). ing, ecological context, and children’s adjustment: A seven-year
Organisation and functions of cells responsive to faces in the longitudinal study. Child Development, 68, 908–923.
temporal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of Pettit, G. S., Laird, R. D., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Criss, M. M.
London, Series B, 335, 23–30. (2001). Antecedents and behavior-problem outcomes of parental
Perrin, E. C. (2002). Technical report: Coparent or second-parent monitoring and psychological control in early adoelscence. Child
adoption by same-sex parents. Pediatrics, 109, 341–344. Development, 72, 583–598.
Perris, E. E., Myers, N. A., & Clifton, R. K. (1990). Long-term Pettmann, B., & Henderson, C. E. (1998). Neuronal cell death.
memory for a single infancy experience. Child Development, 61, Neuron, 20, 633–647.
1796–1807. Philip, R. C. M., Dauvermann, M. R., Whalley, H. C., Baynham, K.,
Peschel, E. R., & Peschel, R. E. (1986). Medicine and music: The Lawrie, S. M., & Stanfield, A.C. (2012). A systematic review and
castrati in opera. Opera Quarterly, 4, 21–38. meta-analysis of the fMRI investigation of autism spectrum disor-
Pessah, I. N., & Lein, P. J. (2008). Evidence for environmental suscep- ders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 901–942.
tibility in autism. What we need to know about gene × environ- Phillips, W., Shankar, M., & Santos, L. R. (2010). Essentialism in
ment interactions In A. W. Zimmerman (Ed.), Autism: Current the absence of language? Evidence from rhesus monkeys (Macaca
theories and evidence (pp. 409–428). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press. mulatta). Developmental Science, 13, F1–F7.
Peter, B., Raskind, W. H., Matsushita, M., Lisowski, M., Vu, T., Piaget, J. (1926/1959). The language and thought of the child. London:
Berninger, V. W., Wijsman, E. M., & Brkanac, Z. (2011). Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Replication of CNTNAP2 association with nonword repetition Piaget, J. (1926/2007). The child’s conception of the world
and support for FOXP2 association with timed reading and motor [J. Tomlinson & A. Tomlinson, Trans.]. Lanham, MD: Rowman &
activities in a dyslexia family sample. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Littlefield.
Disorders, 3, 39–49. Piaget, J. (1932/1965). The moral judgment of the child (M.
Peters, E., Finucane, M. L., McGregor, D. G., & Slovic, P. (2000). Gabain,Trans.). Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
The bearable lightness of aging: Judgment and decision processes Piaget, J. (1936/1953). The origins of intelligence in the child. London:
in older adults. In National Research Council, Committee on Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Future Directions for Cognitive Research on Aging, P. C. Stern, & Piaget, J. (1950). The psychology of intelligence [M. Percy & D. E.
L. L. Carstensen (Eds.), The aging mind: Opportunities in cognitive Berlyne, Trans.]. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
research (Appendix C, pp. 144–165). Washington, DC: National Piaget, J. (1952). The child’s conception of number [C. Gattegno &
Academy Press. F. M. Hodgson, Trans.]. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Petersen, A. C., Compas, B. E., Brooks-Gunn, J., Stemmler, M., Piaget, J. (1952a). Autobiographical essay. In E. G. Boring, H. S.
Ey, S., & Grant, K. E. (1993). Depression in adolescence. American Langfeld, H. Werner, & R. M. Yerkes (Eds.), A history of psychol-
Psychologist, 48, 155–168. ogy in autobiography (Vol. 4, pp. 237–256). Worcester, MA: Clark
Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the University Press.
human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, Piaget, J. (1952b). The origins of intelligence in children. New York:
73–89. W. W. Norton.
Peterson, C. C. (2000). Kindred spirits: Influences of siblings’ per- Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York:
spectives on theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 435–455. Basic Books.
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Piaget, J. (1955). The language and thought of the child. New York:
Three ways of looking at a child’s narrative. New York: Plenum Press. Meridian Books.
Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (2002). Mindreading and moral Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York:
awareness in popular and rejected preschoolers. British Journal of Norton.
Developmental Psychology, 20, 205–224. Piaget, J. (1970). Genetic epistemology [E. Duckworth, Trans.]. New
Peterson, C., Wang, Q., & Hou, Y. (2009). ‘‘When I was little”: York: Columbia University Press.
Childhood recollections in Chinese and European Canadian grade Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1967). The child’s conception of space. New
school children. Child Development, 80, 506–518. York: W. W. Norton.
REFERENCES R-99
Piantadosi, S. T., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Goodman, N. D. (2012). Pinker, S. (2004). Why nature and nurture won’t go away. Daedalus.
Bootstrapping in a language of thought: A formal model of numer- Fall: 5–17.
ical concept learning. Cognition, 123, 199–217. Pinker, S., & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selec-
Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Exact and tion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 707–784.
approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. Science, Pinker, S., & Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of language: What’s
306, 499–503. special about it? Cognition, 95, 201–236.
Pick H. L. (1983). Comparative and developmental approaches to spa- Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1988). On language and connectionism:
tial cognition. In H. L. Pick & L. P. Acredolo (Eds.), Spatial ori- Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language
entation: Theory, research, and application (pp. 73–78). New York: acquisition. Cognition, 28, 73–193.
Plenum. Pinto-Correia, C. (1997). The ovary of Eve: Egg and sperm and prefor-
Pierce, K. A., & Gholson, B. (1994). Surface similarity and relational mation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
similarity in the development of analogical problem solving: Pipe, M.-E., & Wilson, J. C. (1994). Cues and secrets: Influences on
Isomorphic and nonisomorphic transfer. Developmental Psychology, children’s event reports. Developmental Psychology, 30, 515–525.
30, 724–737. Pipp, S., & Harmon, R. J. (1987). Attachment as regulation: A com-
Pierroutsakos, S. L., & DeLoache, J. S. (2003). Why do infants grasp mentary. Child Development, 58, 648–652.
pictured objects? Infancy, 4, 141–156. Pitcher, E. G., & Schultz, L. H. (1983). Boys and girls at play: The
Pierroutsakos, S. L., DeLoache, J. S., Gound, M., & Bernard, E. N. development of sex roles. New York: Praeger.
(2005). Very young children are insensitive to picture—but not Pitchford, I. (2001). The origins of violence: Is psychopathy an adap-
object— orientation. Developmental Science, 8, 326–332. tation? Human Nature Review, 1, 28–36.
Pietschnig, J., Voracek, M., & Formann A. K. (2010). Mozart effect- Pitman, C. A., & Shumaker, R. W. (2009). Does early care affect joint
Shmozart effect: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 38, 314–323. attention in great apes (Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus, Pongo abelii,
Pike, K. M., & Rodin, J. (1991). Mothers, daughters, and disordered Pongo pygmaeus, Gorilla gorilla)? Journal of Comparative Psychology,
eating. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 198–204. 123, 334–341.
Pilgrim, C., & Hutchinson, J. B. (1994). Developmental regulation Pixner, S., Moeller, K., Hermanova, V., Nuerk, H.-C., & Kaufmann, L.
of sex differences in the brain: Can the role of gonadal steroids be (2011). Whorf reloaded: Language effects on non-verbal number
redefined? Neuroscience, 60, 843–855. processing in first grade–A trilingual study. Journal of Experimental
Pilotti, M., Meade, M. L., & Gallo, D. A. (2003). Implicit and Child Psychology, 108, 371–382.
explicit measures of memory for perceptual information in young Pizarro, D. A., & Bloom, P. (2003). The intelligence of the moral
adults, healthy older adults, and patients with Alzheimer’s disease. intuitions: Comment on Haidt (2001). Psychological Review, 110,
Experimental Aging Research, 29, 15–32. 193–196.
Pilowsky, D. J., Wickramaratne, P. J., Rush, A. J., Hughes, C. W., Plagemann, A. (2006). Perinatal nutrition and hormone-dependent
Garber, J., Malloy, E., King, C. A., Cerda, G., Sood, A. B., Alpert, programming of food intake. Hormone Research, 65, 83–89.
J. E., Wisniewski, S. R., Trivedi, M. H., Talati, A., Carlson, M. M., Pleck, J. H. (2010). Paternal involvement: Revised conceptualization
Liu, H. H., Fava, M., & Weissman, M. M. (2006). Children of and theoretical linkages with child outcome. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.),
currently depressed mothers: A STAR*D ancillary study. Journal of The role of the father in child development (5th ed., pp. 58–93).
Clinical Psychiatry 67, 126–136. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Pinderhughes, E. E., Jones Harden, B., & Guyer, A. E. (2006). Plomin, R. (1986). Development, genetics, and psychology. Hillsdale,
Children in foster care. In J. L. Aber, S. J. Bishop-Josef, S. M. NJ: Erlbaum.
Jones, K. T. McLearn, & D. A. Phillips (Eds.), Child development Plomin, R. (1989). Environment and genes. American Psychologist, 44,
and social policy: Knowledge for action. Washington, DC: American 105–111.
Psychological Association. Plomin, R. (1994). Genetics and experience: The interplay between
Pinker, S. (1979). Formal models of language learning. Cognition, 7, nature and nurture. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
217–283. Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McCleam, G. E., & Rutter, M. (1997).
Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Behavioral genetics (3rd ed.). New York: Freeman.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E., & McGuffin, P. (2000).
Pinker, S. (1987). The bootstrapping problem in language acquisi- Behavioral genetics (4th ed.). New York: Freeman.
tion. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition Plomin, R., & McGuffin, P. (2003). Psychopathology in the postgen-
(pp. 399–442). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. omic era. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 205–228.
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argu- Plomin, R., Nitz, K., & Rowe, D. C. (1990). Behavioral genetics and
ment structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. aggressive behavior in childhood. In M. Lewis, & S. M. Miller
Pinker, S. (1991). Rules of language. Science, 253, 530–535. (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychopathology (pp. 119–133).
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York: Plenum Press.
New York: William Morrow. Plotnik, J. M., de Waal, F. B. M., Moore, D., & Reiss, D. (2010). Self-
Pinker, S. (1995). Language acquisition. In L. R. Gleitman & recognition in the Asian elephant and future directions for cogni-
M. Liberman (Eds.), An invitation to cognitive science: Vol. 1. tive research with elephants in zoological settings. Zoo Biology, 29,
Language (2nd ed., pp. 135–182). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 179–191.
Pinker, S. (1995). The language instinct. New York: HarperCollins. Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1991). U-shaped learning and fre-
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: W. W. Norton. quency effects in a multi-layered perceptron: Implications for child
Pinker, S. (2001). Talk of genetics and vice versa. Nature, 413, language acquisition. Cognition, 38, 43–102.
465–466. Pogarsky, G., Thornberry, T. P., & Lizotte, A. J. (2006). Developmental
Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. outcomes for children of young mothers. Journal of Marriage and
London: Penguin/Allen Lane. the Family, 68, 332–344.
R-100 REFERENCES
Polanyi, M. (1966). The tacit dimension. New York: Anchor Books. E. A. Phelps (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of mind: A tribute to
Polgreen, L., & Worth, R. F. (2003, October 27). New Jersey couple Michael S. Gazzaniga (pp. 125–138). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
held in abuse; one son, 19, weighed 45 pounds. New York Times, Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Voelker, P. (2012).
p. A1. Control networks and neuromodulators of early development.
Pollak, S. D. (2008). Mechanisms linking early experience and the Developmental Psychology, 48, 827–835.
emergence of emotions: Illustrations from the study of maltreated Post, R. M., & Leverich, G. S. (2006). The role of psychosocial stress
children. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 370–375. in the onset and progression of bipolar disorder and its comor-
Pollak, S. D., Messner, M., Kistler, D. J., & Cohn, J. F. (2009). bidities: The need for earlier and alternative modes of therapeutic
Development of perceptual expertise in emotion recognition. intervention. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 1181–1211.
Cognition, 110, 242–247. Posthuma, R. A., & Campion, M. A. (2009). Age stereotypes in the
Pollitt, E., Gorman, K. S., Engle, P. L., Martorell, R., & Rivera, J. workplace: Common stereotypes, moderators, and future research
(1993). Early supplementary feeding and cognition. Monographs of directions. Journal of Management, 35, 158–188.
the Society for Research in Child Development, 58 (Serial No. 235). Potash, J. B., & DePaulo, J. R., Jr. (2000). Searching high and low:
Ponitz, C. C., McClelland, M. M., Matthews, J. M., & Morrison, A review of the genetics of bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 2,
F. J. (2009). A structured observation of behavioral self-regulation 8–26.
and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. Developmental Potter, W. J., & Vaughan, M. (1997). Antisocial behaviors in televi-
Psychology, 45, 605–619. sion entertainment: Trends and profiles. Communication Research
Poole, D. A., & Lindsay, D. S. (2001). Children’s eyewitness reports Reports, 14, 116–124.
after exposure to misinformation from parents. Journal of Potter, W. J., Vaughan, M. W., Warren, R., Howley, K., Land, A., &
Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 27–50. Hagemeyer, J. (1995). How real is the portrayal of aggression in
Poole, D. A., & Lindsay, D. S. (2002). Reducing child witnesses’ false television entertainment programming? Journal of Broadcasting
reports of misinformation from parents. Journal of Experimental and Electronic Media, 39, 496–516.
Child Psychology, 81, 117–140. Poulin-Dubois, D., Serbin, L. A., Eichstedt, J. A., Sen, M. G., &
Poole, D. A., Lindsay, D. S., Memon, A., & Bull, R. (1995). Beissel, C. F. (2002). Men don’t put on make-up: Toddlers’ knowl-
Psychotherapy and the recovery of memories of childhood sexual edge of the gender stereotyping of household activities. Social
abuse: U.S. and British practitioners’ opinions, practices, and expe- Development, 11, 166–181.
riences. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 426–437. Poulton, K., Holmes, J., Hever, T., Trumper, A., Fitzpatrick, H.,
Pope, H. G., & Hudson, J. I. (1995). Can memories of childhood McGuffin, P., Owen, M., Worthington, J., Ollier, W.,
sexual abuse be repressed? Psychological Medicine, 25, 121–126. Harrington, R., & Thapar, A. (1998). A molecular genetic study
Pope, K. S. (1995). What psychologists better know about recovered of hyperkinetic disorder/attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
memories, research, lawsuits, and the pivotal experiment. Clinical American Journal of Medical Genetics, 81, 458.
Psychology: Science and Practice, 2, 304–315. Poulton, R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Cannon, M., Murray, R. M., &
Pope, K. S. (1997). Memory, abuse, and science: Questioning claims Harrington, H. (2000). Children’s self-reported psychotic symp-
about the false memory syndrome epidemic. American Psychologist, toms and adult schizophreniform disorder: A 15-year longitudinal
51, 957–974. study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 1053–1058.
Porter, E. (2006, March 2). Stretched to limit, women stall march to Povinelli, D. J. (1998, November). Can animals empathize?—maybe
work. New York Times, pp. A1, C2. not. Scientific American [Special edition], 67–75.
Porter, R. H., & Winberg, J. (1999). Unique salience of maternal Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1996). What young chimpanzees
breast odors for newborn infants. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral know about seeing. Mongraphs of the Society for Research in Child
Reviews, 23, 439–449. Development, 61(2, Serial no. 247).
Posada, G., Longoria, N., Cocker, C. S., & Lu, T. (2010). Attachment Povinelli, D. J., Parks, K. A., & Novak, M. A. (1991). Do rhesus mon-
ties in military families: Mothers’ perception of interactions keys (Macaca mulatta) attribute knowledge and ignorance to oth-
with their children, stress, and social competence. In S. M. ers? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105, 318–325.
Wadsworth & D. Riggs (Eds.), Risk and resilience in U.S. military Povinelli, D. J., Perilloux, H. K., Reaux, J. E., & Bierschwale, D. T.
families (pp. 131–147). New York: Springer. (1998). Young and juvenile chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) reactions
Posner, M. I. (2012). Imaging attention networks. NeuroImage, 61, to intentional versus accidental and inadvertent actions. Behavioral
450–456. Processes, 42, 205–218.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1981). The development of atten- Povinelli, D. J., Rulf, A. B., Landau, K. R., & Bierschwale, D. T. (1993).
tional mechanisms. In J. H. Flowers (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on Self-recognition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Distribution,
motivation (pp. 1–51). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. ontogeny, and patterns of emergence. Journal of Comparative
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2005). Influencing brain networks: Psychology, 107, 347–372.
Implications for education. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 99–103. Povinelli, D. J., & Simon, B. B. (1998). Young children’s reactions to
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Research on attention briefly versus extremely delayed images of the self: Emergence of
networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. the autobiographical stance. Developmental Psychology, 43, 188–194.
Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 1–23. Power, F. C., Higgins, A., & Kohlberg, L. (1989). Lawrence Kohlberg’s
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2009). Toward a physical basis of approach to moral education. New York: Columbia University Press.
attention and self-regulation. Physics of Life Reviews, 6, 103–120. Powers, K. L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., Palladino, M. A., &
Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., & Sheese, B. E. (2007). Attention Alfieri, L. (2013). Effects of video-game play on information pro-
genes. Developmental Science, 10, 24–29. cessing: A meta-analytic investigation. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., & Sheese, B. E. (2010). Genetic Review, 1–25.
variation influences how the social brain shapes temperament and Powers, T. P., & Amemiya, C. T. (2004). Evolutionary plasticity of
behavior. In P. A. Reuter-Lorenz, K. Baynes, G. R. Mangun, & vertebrate Hox genes. Current Genomics, 5, 459–472.
REFERENCES R-101
Prasad, R. (2004). A little miracle called Madeline. The Hindu, Pujadas, L., Gruart, A., Bosch, C., Delgado, L., Teixeira, C. M.,
August 26. Rossi, D., de Lecea, L., Martínez, A., Delgado-García, J. M., &
Prasada, S., & Pinker, S. (1993). Generalizations of regular and irreg- Soriano, E. (2010). Reelin regulates postnatal neurogenesis and
ular morphology. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 1–56. enhances spine hypertrophy and long-term potentiation. Journal of
Pratt, M. W., Green, D., MacVicar, J., & Bountrogianni, M. (1992). Neuroscience, 30, 4636–4649.
The mathematical parent: Parental scaffolding, parenting style, Pullum, G. (1991). The great Eskimo vocabulary hoax and other irrever-
and learning outcomes in long-division mathematics homework. ent essays on the study of language. Chicago: University of Chicago
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 13, 17–34. Press.
Preissler, M. A., & Bloom, P. (2008). Two-year-olds use artist inten- Pullum, G. (1999). Generative grammar. In R. A. Wilson & F. C. Keil
tion to understand drawings. Cognition, 106, 512–518. (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences (pp. 340–343).
Premack, D. (1990). Words: What are they, and do animals have Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
them? Cognition, 37, 197–212. Pulsifer, M. B., Salorio, C. F., & Niparko, J. K. (2003). Developmental,
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a audiological, and speech perception functioning in children after
theory of mind? Behavorial and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526. cochlear implant surgery. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent
Prescott, J., & Wilkie, J. (2007). Pain tolerance selectively increased Medicine, 157, 552–558.
by a sweet-smelling odor. Psychological Science, 18, 308–311. Purves, D., & Lichtman, J. W. (1980). Elimination of synapses in the
Pressley, M., Levin, J. R., Ghatala, E. S., & Ahmad, M. (1987). Test developing nervous system. Science, 210, 153–157.
monitoring in young grade-school children. Journal of Experimental Purves, D., & Lichtman, J. W. (1985). Principles of neural development.
Child Psychology, 43, 96–111. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.
Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and Puts, D. A., Jordan, C. L., & Breedlove, S. M. (2006). O Brother,
proximate bases. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 25, 1–71. where art thou? The fraternal birth order effect on male sexual
Preyer, W. (1888). The mind of the child (H. W. Brown, Trans.). New orientation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
York: Appleton. USA, 103, 10531–10532.
Price, L. H., Kao, H. T., Burgers, D. E., Carpenter, L. L, & Tyrka, Putzar, L., Hötting, K., & Röder, B. (2010). Early visual deprivation
A. R. (2013). Telomeres and early-life stress: An overview. Biological affects the development of face recognition and of audio-visual
Psychiatry, 73, 15–23. speech perception. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 28,
Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (1997). Optimality: From neural net- 251–257.
works to universal grammar. Science, 275, 1604–1610.
Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (2004). Optimality theory: Constraint Quine, W. V. O. (1960). Word and object. Cambridge, MA: MIT
interaction in generative grammar. Malden, MA: Blackwell. Press.
Principe, C. P., & Langlois, J. H. (2012). Shifting the prototype: Quinn, P. C., & Slater, A. (2003). Face perception at birth and beyond.
Experience with faces influences affective and attractiveness pref- In O. Pascalis & A. Slater (Eds.), The development of face process-
erences. Social Cognition, 30, 109–120. ing in infancy and early childhood: Current perspectives (pp. 3–11).
Principe, G. F., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). “I saw it with my own ears”: The Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
effects of peer conversations on preschoolers’ reports of nonexpe- Quinn, P. C., Kelly, D. J., Lee, K., Pascalis, O., & Slater, A. M.
rienced events. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 1–25. (2008b). Preference for attractive faces in human infants extends
Principe, G. F., & Schindewolf, E. (2012). Natural conversations as beyond conspecifics. Developmental Science, 11, 76–83.
a source of false memories in children: Implications for the tes- Quinn, P. C., Uttley, L., Lee, K., Gibson, A., Smith, M., Slater,
timony of young witnesses. Developmental Review, 32, 205–223. A. M., & Pascalis, O. (2008a). Infant preference for female faces
Pringle, R. M., & Martin, S. C. (2005). The potential impacts of occurs for same- but not other-race faces. Journal of Neuropsychology,
upcoming high-stakes testing on the teaching of science in elemen- 2, 15–26.
tary classrooms. Research in Science Education, 35, 347–361. Quinn, P. C., Yahr, J., Kuhn, A., Slater, A. M., & Pascalis, O. (2002).
Prins, P. J. M., & Hanewald, G. J. F. (1997). Self-statements of test- Representation of the gender of human faces by infants: A prefer-
anxious children: Thought-listing and questionnaire approaches. ence for female. Perception, 31, 1109–1121.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 440–447.
Prinz, J. J. (2004). Gut reactions: A perceptual theory of the emotions. Rabipour, S., & Raz, A. (2012). Training the brain: Fact and fad in
New York: Oxford University Press. cognitive and behavioral remediation. Brain and Cognition, 79,
Prinz, J. J. (2007). The emotional construction of morals. New York: 159–179.
Oxford University Press. Racer, K. H., & Dishion, T. J. (2012). Disordered attention:
Proffitt, D. R., & Gilden, D. L. (1989). Understanding natural Implications for understanding and treating internalizing and
dynamics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception externalizing disorders in childhood. Cognitive and Behavioral
and Performance, 15, 384–393. Practice, 19, 31–40.
Provence, S., & Lipton, R. C. (1962). Infants in institutions: A compari- Radhakrishna, A., Bou-Saada, I. E., Hunter, W. M., Catellier, D. J., &
son of their development with family-reared infants during the first Kotch, J. B. (2001). Are father surrogates a risk factor for child mal-
year of life. New York: International Universities Press. treatment? Child Maltreatment, 6, 281–289.
Provine, R. R. (2000). Laughter: A scientific investigation. New York: Raghavan, R., Brown, D. S., Thompson, H., Ettner, S. L., Clements,
Viking. L. M, & Key, W. J. (2012). Medicaid expenditures on psychotropic
Provine, R. R., & Westerman, J. A. (1979). Crossing the mid- medications for children in the child welfare system. Journal of
line: Limits of early eye-hand behavior. Child Development, 50, Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 22, 182–189.
437–441. Rah, Y., & Parke, R. D. (2008). Pathways between parent-child inter-
Pueschel, S. M. (2001). A parent’s guide to Down syndrome: Toward a actions and peer acceptance: The role of children’s social informa-
brighter future (rev. ed.). Baltimore: Paul H. Brooks. tion processing. Social Development, 17, 341–357.
R-102 REFERENCES
Rai, T. S., & Fiske, A. P. (2011). Moral psychology is relationship Rakison, D. H., & Derringer, J. (2008). Do infants possess an evolved
regulation: Moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and pro- spider-detection mechanism? Cognition, 107, 381–393.
portionality. Psychological Review, 118, 57–75. Rakison, D. H., & Krogh, L. (2012). Does causal action facilitate
Raikes, H. A., & Thompson, R. A. (2006). Family emotional climate, causal perception in infants younger than 6 months of age?
attachment security, and young children’s emotion understanding Developmental Science, 15, 43–53.
in a high-risk sample. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, Raley, S., & Bianchi, S. (2006). Sons, daughters, and family processes:
24, 89–104. Does gender of children matter? Annual Review of Sociology, 32,
Raine, A. (2008). From genes to brain to antisocial behavior. Current 401–421.
Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 323–328. Ramachandran, V. S. (2000). Mirror neurons and imitation learning
Raine, A., & Venables, P. H. (1988). Skin conductance responsivity in as the driving force behind “the great leap forward” in human evo-
psychopaths to orienting, defensive, and consonant-vowel stimuli. lution. Edge, no. 69, May 29.
Journal of Psychophysiology, 2, 221–225. Ramus, F. (2002). Language discrimination by newborns: Teasing
Raine, A., Reynolds, C., Venables, P. H., & Mednick, S. A. (2002). apart phonotactic, rhythmic, and intonational cues. Annual Review
Stimulation seeking and intelligence: A prospective longitudinal of Language Acquisition, 2, 85–115.
study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 663–674. Ramus, F., Hauser, M. D., Miller, C., Morris, D., & Mehler, J. (2000).
Raisman, G., & Li, Y. (2007). Repair of neural pathways by olfactory Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top
ensheathing cells. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 8, 312–319. tamarin monkeys. Science, 288, 349–351.
Raj, V., & Bell, M. A. (2010). Cognitive processes supporting epi- Rapee, R., Schniering, C. A., & Hudson, J. L. (2009). Anxiety dis-
sodic memory formation in childhood: The role of source memory, orders during childhood and adolescence: Origins and treatment.
binding, and executive functioning. Developmental Review, 30, Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5, 311–341.
384–402. Rapin, I., & Tuchman, R. F. (2008a). Autism: Definition, neurobiol-
Rajan, V., Cuevas, K., & Bell, M. A. (2013). The contribution of exec- ogy, screening, diagnosis. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 55,
utive function to source memory development in early childhood. 1129–1146.
Journal of Cognition and Development. Rapin I., & Tuchman, R. F. (2008b). What is new in autism? Current
Rakic, P. (1972). Mode of cell migration to the superficial layers of Opinion in Neurology, 21, 143–149.
fetal monkey neocortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 145, Rapoport, J. (1989). The boy who couldn’t stop washing: The expe-
61–84. rience and treatment of obsessive- compulsive disorder. New York:
Rakic, P. (1978). Neuronal migration and contact guidance in the pri- Penguin.
mate telencephalon. Postgraduate Medical Journal, 54, 25–37. Rapoport, J. L. (2009). Personal reflections on observational and
Rakic, P. (1981). Neuronal-glial interaction during brain develop- experimental research approaches to childhood psychopathology.
ment. Trends in Neurosciences, 4, 184–187. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 36–43.
Rakic, P. (1988). Specification of cerebral cortical areas. Science, 241, Rapoport, J. L., Buchsbaum, M. S., Zahn, T. P., Weingartner, H.,
170–176. Ludlow, C., & Mikkelsen, E. J. (1978). Dextroamphetamine:
Rakic, P. (1989). Competitive interactions during neuronal and syn- Behavioral and cognitive effects in normal prepubertal boys.
tactic development. In A. M. Galaburda (Ed.), From reading to Science, 199, 560–563.
neurons (pp. 443–459). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Rapoport, J., & Fiske, A. (1998). The new biology of obsessive-
Rakic, P. (1990). Principles of neuronal cell migration. Experientia, compulsive disorder: Implications for evolutionary psychology.
46, 882-891. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 41, 159–179.
Rakic, P. (1994). Corticogenesis in human and nonhuman primates. Rapoport, J. L., & Frangou, S. (2012). Diagnosis and management
In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 127–145). of child and adolescent onset schizophrenia. In J. A. Lieberman &
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. R. M. Murray, Comprehensive care of schizophrenia: A textbook
Rakic, P. (1995). A small step for the cell, a giant leap for mankind: of clinical management (2nd ed., pp. 37–50). New York: Oxford
A hypothesis of neocortical expansion during evolution. Trends in University Press.
Neurosciences, 18, 383–388. Rapoport, J. L., Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Hamburger, S., Jeffries,
Rakic, P. (1999). Discriminating migrations. Nature, 400, 315–316. N., Fernandez, T., Nicolson, R., Bedwell, J., Lenane, M., Zijdenbos,
Rakic, P. (2002). Adult neurogenesis in mammals: An identity crisis. A., Paus, T., & Evans, A. (1999). Progressive cortical change dur-
Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 614–618. ing adolescence in childhood-onset schizophrenia: A longitudinal
Rakic, P. (2003). Developmental and evolutionary adaptations of cor- magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry,
tical radial glia. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 541–549. 56, 649–654.
Rakic, P., Bourgeois, J.-P., Eckenhoff, M. F., Zecevic, N., & Rapoport, J. L., Swedo, S. E., & Leonard, H. L. (1992). Childhood
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1986). Concurrent overproduction of syn- obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 53,
apses in diverse regions of the primate cerebral cortex. Science, 232, 11–16.
232–235. Rasmussen S. A. (1993). Genetic studies of obsessive compulsive dis-
Rakic, P., Bourgeois, J.-P., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1994). Synaptic order. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 5, 241–247.
development of the cerebral cortex: Implications for learning, Ratcliff, R., Spieler, D., & McKoon, G. (2000). Explicitly model-
memory, and mental illness. In J. van Pelt, M. A. Corner, H. B. M. ing the effects of aging on response time. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Uylings, & F. H. Lopes da Silva (Eds.), Progress in brain research: Review, 7, 1–25.
Vol. 102. The self-organizing brain: From growth cones to functional Rattermann, M. J., & Gentner, D. (1998). More evidence for a rela-
networks (pp. 227–243). Amsterdam: Elsevier. tional shift in the development of analogy: Children’s performance
Rakison, D. H. (2009). Does women’s greater fear of snakes and on a causal-mapping task. Cognitive Development, 13, 453–478.
spiders originate in infancy? Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., & Ky, K. N. (1993). Music and spatial
438–444. task performance. Nature, 365, 611.
REFERENCES R-103
Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., & Ky, K. N. (1995). Listening to Mozart dynamic cognitive deficits in childhood preceding adult schizo-
enhances spatial-temporal reasoning: Towards a neurophysiologi- phrenia: A 30-year study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167,
cal basis. Neuroscience Letters, 185, 44–47. 160–169.
Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Reid, D. J., & Reid, F. J. M. (2007). Text or talk? Social anxi-
University Press. ety, loneliness, and divergent preferences for cell phone use.
Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10, 424–435.
M. S. (2001). How psychological science informs the teaching of Reid, R., & Katsiyannis, A. (1995). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity
reading. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2, 31–74. disorder and Section 504. Remedial and Special Education, 16,
Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, 44–52.
M. S. (2002). How should reading be taught? Scientific American, Reid, S., & Spetch, M. L. (1998). Perception of pictorial depth cues by
286, 84–91. pigeons. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 698–704.
Raz, N. (2000). Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive perfor- Reiger, T., & Kay, P (2009). Language, thought, and color: Whorf
mance: Integration of structural and functional findings. In F. I. was half right. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 439–446.
M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and Reiner, W. (1997). To be male or female—that is the question. Archives
cognition (2nd. ed., pp.1–90). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 151, 224–225.
Reardon, P., & Bushnell, E. W. (1988). Infants’ sensitivity to arbitrary Reiner, W. G. (2005). Gender identity and sex-of-rearing in chil-
pairings of color and taste. Infant Behavior and Development, 11, dren with disorders of sexual differentiation. Journal of Pediatric
245–250. Endocrinology & Metabolism 18, 549–553.
Reavie, L., Della Gatta, G., Crusio, K., Aranda-Orgilles, B., Buckley, Reiss, D. (2001). Child effects on family systems: Behavioral genetic strat-
S. M., Thompson, B., Lee, E., Gao, J., Bredemeyer, A. L., Helmink, egies. Presented at the 9th National Symposium on Family Issues:
B. A., Zavadil, J., Sleckman, B. P., Palomero, T., Ferrando, A., & Children’s influence on family dynamics: The neglected side of
Aifantis, I. (2010). Regulation of hematopoietic stem cell differ- family relationships. Pennsylvania State University, University
entiation by a single ubiquitin ligase-substrate complex. Nature Park, PA, December 6–7, 2001.
Immunology, 11, 207–215. Reiss, D. (2003). Child effects on family systems: Behavioral genetic
Rebelo, E., Szabo, C. P., & Pitcher, G. (2008). Gender assignment strategies. In A. C. Crouter & A. Booth (Eds.), Children’s influ-
surgery on children with disorders of sex development: A case ence on family dynamics: The neglected side of family relationships
report and discussion from South Africa. Journal of Child Health (pp. 3–26). Pennsylvania State University Family Issues Symposia
Care, 12, 49–59. Series. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rector, N, A., & Beck, A. T. (2001). Cognitive behavioral therapy for Reiss, D. (2011). Dolphin research: Educating the public [Letter to
schizophrenia: An empirical review. Journal of Nervous and Mental the editor]. Science, 332, 1501.
Disease, 189, 278–287. Reiss, D., & Leve, L. D. (2007). Genetic expression outside the skin:
Reddy, V. (2000). Coyness in early infancy. Developmental Science, 3, Clues to mechanisms of Genotype × Environment interaction.
186–192. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 1005–1027.
Redmond, D. E. (2002). Cellular replacement therapy for Parkinson’s Reiss, D., & Marino, L. (2001). Mirror self-recognition in the bottle-
disease—Where we are today? Neuroscientist, 8, 457–488. nose dolphin: A case of cognitive convergence. Proceedings of the
Reed, T. E., & Jensen, A. R. (1992). Conduction velocity in a brain National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 98, 5937.
nerve pathway of normal adults correlates with intelligence level. Reiss, D., Neiderhiser, J. M., Hetherington, E. M., & Plomin, R.
Intelligence, 16, 259–272. (2000). The relationship code: Deciphering genetic and social
Reed, T. E., & Jensen, A.R. (1993). Choice reaction time and visual influences on adolescent development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
pathway nerve conduction velocity both correlate with intelligence University Press.
but appear not to correlate with each other: Implications for infor- Reissland, N. (1988). Neonatal imitation in the first hour of life:
mation processing. Intelligence, 17, 191–203. Observations in rural Nepal. Developmental Psychology, 24,
Reese, E., & Newcombe, R. (2007). Training mothers in elaborative 464–469.
reminiscing enhances children’s autobiographical memory and Renshaw, P. D. (1981). The roots of current peer interaction research.
narrative. Child Development, 78, 1153–1170. In S. R. Asher & J. M. Gottman (Eds.), The development of children’s
Regier, T., & Kay, P. (2009). Language, thought, and color: Whorf friendships (pp. 1–25). New York: Cambridge University Press.
was half right. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 439–446. Rentakallio, P., Jones, P., Moring, J., & Von Wendt, L. (1997).
Regolin, L., Rugani, R., Stancher, G., & Vallortigara, G. (2011). Association between central nervous system infections during
Spontaneous discrimination of possible and impossible objects by childhood and adult onset schizophrenia and other psychoses:
newly hatched chicks. Biology Letters, 7, 654–657. A 28-year follow-up. International Journal of Epidemiology, 26,
Reich, S. M., Subrahmanyam, K., & Espinoza, G. (2012). Friending, 837–843.
IMing, and hanging out face-to-face: Overlap in adolescents’ Rentfrow, P. J., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). A theory of the
online and offline social networks. Developmental Psychology, 48, emergence, persistence, and expression of geographic variation in
356–368. psychological characteristics. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
Reichardt, L. F., & Farinas, I. (1997). Neurotrophic factors and 3, 339–369.
their receptors. In W. M. Cowan, T. M. Jessel, & S. L. Zipursky Repacholi, B. M., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires:
(Eds.), Molecular and cellular approaches to neural development Evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology,
(pp. 220–226). New York: Oxford University Press. 33, 12–21.
Reichart, C. G., & Nolen, W. A. (2004). Earlier onset of bipolar dis- Repetti, R. L. (1994). Short-term and long-term processes linking job
order in children by antidepressants or stimulants? An hypothesis. stressors to father-child interaction. Social Development, 3, 1–15.
Journal of Affective Disorders, 78, 81–84. Repetti, R. L., Wang, S., & Saxbe, D. (2009). Bringing it all back
Reichenberg, A., Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Houts, R., Keefe, R. S., home: How outside stressors shape families’ everyday lives. Current
Murray, R. M., Poulton, R., & Moffitt, T. E. (2010). Static and Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 106–111.
R-104 REFERENCES
Repetti, R. L., & Wood, J. (1997). Effects of daily stress at work Richards, N. (2001). Movement in language: Interactions and architec-
on mothers’ interactions with preschoolers. Journal of Family tures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Psychology, 11, 90–108. Richardson, M., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2012). Psychological cor-
Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Behavioral studies of Pavlovian conditioning. relates of university students’ academic performance: A systematic
Annual Review of Neuroscience, 11, 329–352. review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 353–387.
Resnick, L. B. (1989). Developing mathematical knowledge. American Richardson, M. K., Hanken, J., Gooneratne, M. J., Pieau, C.,
Psychologist, 44, 162–169. Raynaud, A., Selwood, L., & Wright, G. M. (1997). There is no
Resnick, L. B., & Resnick, D. P. (1992). Assessing the thinking cur- highly conserved embryonic stage in the vertebrates: Implications
riculum: New tools for educational reform. In B. R. Gifford & for current theories of evolution and development. Anatomy and
M. C. O’Connor (Eds.), Changing assessments: Alternative views of Embryology, 196, 91–106.
aptitude, achievement and instruction. Boston: Kluwer Academic Richardson, S. J., Almeida, O. F. X., & Demeneix, B. A. (2007).
Publishers. Hormones and adult neurogenesis in mammals. Expert Review of
Reynolds, C. R., & Brown, R. T. (Eds.). (1984). Perspectives on bias in Endocrinology & Metabolism, 2, 261–276.
mental testing. New York: Plenum Press. Richland, L. E., Bjork, R. A., Finley, J. R., & Linn, M. C. (2005).
Rhee, S. H., Friedman, N. P., Boeldt, D. L., Corley, R. P., Hewitt, Linking cognitive science to education: Generation and interleav-
J. K., Knafo, A., Lahey, B. B., Robinson, J. A., Van Hulle, C., ing effects. In B. G. Bara, L. Barsalou, & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.),
Waldman, I. D., Young, S. E., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2013). Early Proceedings of the twenty-seventh annual conference of the Cognitive
concern and disregard for others as predictors of antisocial behav- Science Society (pp. 1850–1855). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 157–166. Richland, L. E., Morrison, R. G., & Holyoak, K. J. (2006). Children’s
Rhee, S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmen- development of analogical reasoning: Insights from scene analogy
tal influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 249–273.
adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 490–529. Richters, J. E., Arnold, L. E., Jensen, P. S., Abikoff, H., Conners, C. K.,
Rheingold, H. L., Gewirtz, J. L., & Ross, H. W. (1959). Social condi- Greenhill, L. L., Hechtman, L., Hinshaw, S. P., Pelham, W. E., &
tioning of vocalizations in the infant. Journal of Comparative and Swanson, J. M. (1995). NIMH collaborative multisite multimodal
Physiological Psychology, 52, 68–73. treatment study of children with ADHD: I. Background and
Rheingold, H. L., Hay, D. F., & West, M. J. (1976). Sharing in the rationale. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
second year of life. Child Development, 47, 1148–1158. Psychiatry, 34, 987–1000.
Rhodes, G., Proffitt, F., Grady, J. M., & Sumich, A. (1998). Facial Rickford, J. R. (1999). African American vernacular English. Oxford:
symmetry and the perception of beauty. Psychonomic Bulletin & Blackwell.
Review, 5, 659–669. Rickford, J. R., Bailey, G., & Baugh, J. (1998). African American
Rhodes, M., & Brickman, D. (2008). Preschoolers’ responses to social English: Structure, history, and use. London: Routledge.
comparisons involving relative failure. Psychological Science, 19, Rideout, B. E., Dougherty, S., & Wernert, L. (1998). Effect of music
968–972. on spatial performance: A test of generality. Perceptual and Motor
Rhodes, P., Prunty, M., & Madden, S. (2009). Life-threatening food Skills, 86, 512–514.
refusal in two nine-year-old girls: Re-thinking the Maudsley Rideout, V. J., Foehr, U. G., & Roberts, D. F. (2010). Generation M2:
model. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 14, 63–70. Media in the lives of 8- to 18-year-olds. Menlo Park, CA: Henry J.
Rholes, W. S., & Ruble, D. N. (1984). Children’s understanding of Kaiser Family Foundation.
dispositional characteristics of others. Child Development, 55, Riding, R., & Cheema, I. (1991). Cognitive styles: An overview and
550–560. integration. Educational Psychology, 11, 193–215.
Ricciuti, H. (1965). Object grouping and selective ordering behavior Ridley, M. (1996). The origins of virtue. Harmondsworth, UK: Viking.
in infants 12 to 24 months. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 11, 129–148. Rieser, J. J. (1979). Spatial orientation in 6-month-old infants. Child
Rice, C. M., Halfpenny, C. A., & Scolding, N. J. (2003). Stem cells Development, 50, 1078–1087.
for the treatment of neurological disease. Transfusion Medicine, 13, Rijken, M., Veen, S., & Walther, F. J. (2007). Ethics of maintaining
351–361. extremely preterm infants. Paediatrics and Child Health, 17, 58–63.
Rice, F., Harold, G., & Thapar, A. (2002). The genetic aetiology of Riley, E. P., & McGee, C. L. (2005). Fetal alcohol spectrum disor-
childhood depression: A review. Journal of Child Psychology and ders: An overview with emphasis on changes in brain and behavior.
Psychiatry, 43, 65–79. Experimental Biology and Medicine, 230, 357–365.
Rice, F., & Thapar, A. (2009). Depression and anxiety in child- Rincon-Limas, D. E., Lu, C. H., Canal, I., Calleja, M., Rodriguez-
hood and adolescence: Developmental pathways, genes and Esteban, C., Izpisua-Belmonte J. C., & Botas, J. (1999).
environment. In Y.-K. Kim (Ed.), Handbook of behavior genetics Conservation of the expression and function of apterous orthologs
(pp. 379–396). New York: Springer. in Drosophila and mammals. Proceedings of the National Academy
Rice, M. L., Haney, K. R., & Wexler, K. (1998). Family histories of of Sciences of the U.S.A., 96, 2165–2170.
children with SLI who show extended optional infinitives. Journal Riner, M., Slotta, J. D., Chi, M. T. H., & Resnick, L. B. (2000). Naive
of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 419–432. physics reasoning: A commitment to substance-based conceptions.
Rice, M. L., Huston, A. C., Truglio, R., & Wright, J. (1990). Words Cognition and Instruction, 18, 1–34.
from “Sesame Street”: Learning vocabulary while viewing. Risch, N., Herrell, R., Lehner, T., Liang, K. Y., Eaves, L., Hoh, J.,
Developmental Psychology, 26, 421–428. Griem, A., Kovacs, M., Ott, J., & Merikangas, K. R. (2009).
Richards, J. E., & Rader, N. (1981). Crawling-onset age predicts Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR),
visual cliff avoidance in infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: stressful life events, and risk of depression: A meta-analysis. Journal
Human Perception and Performance, 7, 382–387. of the American Medical Association, 301, 2462–2471.
Richards, M. P. M., Bernal, J. F., & Brackbill, Y. (1976). Early Rising to the threat: Reducing stereotype threat by reframing the
behavioral differences: Gender or circumcision? Developmental threat as a challenge. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46,
Psychobiology, 9, 89–95. 166–171.
REFERENCES R-105
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Roby, A. C., & Kidd, E. (2008). The referential communication skills
Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192. of children with imaginary companions. Developmental Science,
Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the 11, 531–540.
parieto-frontal mirror circuit: Interpretations and misinterpreta- Rocca, W. A., Petersen, R. C., Knopman, D. S., Hebert, L. E., Evans,
tions. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 264–274. D. A., Hall, K. S., Gao, S., Unvarzagt, F. W., Langa, K. M.,
Robbins, R. W., & John, O. P. (1997). Self-perception, visual per- Larson, E. B., & White, L. R. (2011). Trends in the incidence and
spective, and narcissism: Is seeing believing? Pychological Science, prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease, dementia, and cognitive impair-
8, 37–42. ment in the United States. Alzheimer’s and Dementia, 7, 80–93.
Robbins, W. J., Brody, S., Hogan, A. G., Jackson, C. M., & Greene, Rochat, P., & Goubet, N. (1995). Development of sitting and reach-
C. W. (1928). Growth. New Haven: Yale University Press. ing in 5- to 6-month-old infants. Infant Behavior & Development,
Rober, P. (2008). Being there, experiencing and creating space for dia- 18, 53–68.
logue: About working with children in family therapy. Journal of Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (2002). Who’s in the mirror? Self-other dis-
Family Therapy, 30, 465–477. crimination in specular images by 4- and 9-month-old infants.
Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consis- Child Development, 73, 35–46.
tency of personality traits from childbirth to old age: A quantitative Rochat, P., Striano, T., & Morgan, R. (2004). Who is doing what to
review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 3–25. whom: Young infants’ developing sense of social causality in ani-
Roberts, B. W., & Mroczek, D. K. (2008). Personality trait change in mated displays. Perception, 33, 355–369.
adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 31–35. Rochlin, K., Yu, S., Roy S., &. Baylies, M. K. (2010). Myoblast fusion:
Roberts, D. F., Foehr, U. G., Rideout, V. J., & Brodie, M. (1999). When it takes more to make one. Developmental Biology, 341,
Kids & media @ the new millennium. Menlo Park, CA: Henry J. 66–83.
Kaiser Family Foundation. Röder, B., Stock, O., Bien, S., Neville, H., & Rosler, F. (2002). Speech
Roberts, K. P. (2002). Children’s ability to distinguish between mem- processing activates visual cortex in congenitally blind humans.
ories from multiple sources: Implications for the quality and accu- European Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 930– 936.
racy of eyewitness statements. Developmental Review, 22, 403–435. Rodgers, J. L. (2001). What causes birth order-intelligence patterns?
Roberts, K. P., & Blades, M. (1999). Children’s memory and source The admixture hypothesis, revived. American Psychologist, 56,
monitoring of real-life and televised events: Determinants and 505–510.
developmental differences. Journal of Applied Developmental Rodgers, J. L., & Rowe, D. C. (1988). Influence of siblings on adoles-
Psychology, 20, 575–596. cent sexual behavior. Developmental Psychology, 24, 722–728.
Roberts, K. P., & Blades, M. (Eds.). (2000). Children’s source monitor- Rodgers, J. L., Cleveland, H. H., van den Oord, E., & Rowe, D. C.
ing: Current trends and future directions. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (2000). Resolving the debate over birth order, family size, and
Roberts, K. P., & Powell, M. (2006). The consistency of false sugges- intelligence. American Psychologist, 55, 599–612.
tions moderates children’s reports of a single instance of a repeated Rodkin, P. C., Farmer, T. W., Pearl, R., & Van Acker, R. (2000).
event: Predicting increases and decreases in suggestibility. Journal Heterogeneity of popular boys: Antisocial and prosocial configura-
of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 68–89. tions. Developmental Psychology, 36, 14–24.
Roberts, R. D., MacCann, C., Matthews, G., & Zeidner, M. (2010). Rodriguez Mosquera, P. M., Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H.
Emotional intelligence: Toward a consensus of models and mea- (2000). The role of honor-related values in the elicitation, expe-
sures. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 821–840. rience, and communication of pride, shame, and anger: Spain
Roberts, S. (2009, April 22). Trustees begin to parcel Leona Helmsley’s and the Netherlands compared. Personality and Social Psychology
estate. The New York Times, p. A24. Bulletin, 26, 833–844.
Roberts, W. (2002). Are animals stuck in time? Psychological Bulletin, Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories:
128, 473–489. Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental
Robertson, D. R. (1972). Social control of sex reversal in a coral-reef Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814.
fish. Science, 171, 1007–1009. Roeper. T., & Williams, E. (Eds.). (1987). Parameter-setting and lan-
Robertson. J. A. (2004). Extreme prematurity and parental rights guage acquisition Dordrecht: Reidel.
after Baby Doe. Hastings Center Report, 34, 32–39. Rogoff, B. (1981). Schooling and the development of cognitive skills.
Robin, A. L., Gilroy, M., & Dennis, A. B. (1998). Treatment of eating In H. C. Triandis & A. Heron (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural
disorders in children and adolescents. Clinical Psychology Review, psychology: Vol. 4. Developmental psychology (pp. 233–294). Boston:
18, 421–446. Allyn & Bacon.
Robin, D. J., Berthier, N. E., & Clifton, R. K. (1996). Infants’ pre- Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in
dictive reaching for moving objects in the dark. Developmental social context. New York: Oxford University Press.
Psychology, 32, 824–835. Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Goncu, A., & Mosier, C. (1993). Guided partic-
Robins, L., & Rutter, M. (Eds.) (1990). Straight and devious pathways ipation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs
from childhood to adulthood. New York: Cambridge University of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58 (Series No.
Press. 236), 1– 179.
Robinson, J. L, Zahn-Waxler, C., & Emde, R. N. (1994). Patterns of Roisman, G. I. (2009). Adult attachment: Toward a rapprochement
development in early empathic behavior: Environmental and child of methodological cultures. Current Directions in Psychological
constitutional influences. Social Development, 3, 125–145. Science, 18, 122–126.
Robinson, J. P., & Bachman, J. G. (1972). Television viewing hab- Roisman, G. I., & Fraley, R. C. (2006). The limits of genetic influ-
its and aggression. In G. A. Comstock & E. A.Rubinstein (Eds.), ence: A behavior-genetic analysis of infant-caregiver relationship
Television and social behavior: Vol. 3. Television and adolescent quality and temperament. Child Development, 77, 1656–1667.
aggressiveness. Washington, DC: United States Government Roisman, G. I., & Fraley, R. C. (2008). A behavior-genetic study
Printing Office. of parenting quality, infant atttachment security, and their
R-106 REFERENCES
covariation in a nationally representative sample. Developmental Rosen, L. D., Cheever, N. A., & Carrier, L. M. (2008). The impact
Psychology, 44, 831–839. of parental attachment style, limit setting and monitoring on teen
Roisman, G. I., Madsen, S. D., Hennighausen, K. H., Sroufe, MySpace behavior. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,
L. A., & Collins, W. A. (2001). The coherence of dyadic behavior 29, 459–471.
across parent-child and romantic relationships as mediated by the Rosenberg, B. G., & Jing, Q. (1996). A revolution in family life: The
internalized representation of experience. Attachment & Human political and social structural impact of China’s one child policy.
Development, 3, 156–172. Journal of Social Issues, 52, 51–69.
Romaine, S. (1989). Bilingualism. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell. Rosenfeld, M., & Rosenfeld, S. (2004). Developing teacher sensitiv-
Romero, A. P., Baumle, A. K., Badgett, M. V. L., & Gates, G. J. ity to individual learning differences. Educational Psychology, 24,
(2007). Census snapshot: United States. Los Angeles: Williams 465–486.
Institute, UCLA School of Law. Rosenstein, D., & Oster, H. (1988). Different facial expressions to
Ronen, T. (1997). Cognitive developmental therapy with children. four basic tastes in newborns. Child Development, 59, 1555–1568.
Chichester, UK: Wiley. Rosenthal, D. (Ed.). (1963). The Genain quadruplets: A case study and
Rosati, A. G., & Hare, B. (2009). Looking past the model species: theoretical analysis of heredity and environment in schizophrenia.
Diversity in gaze-following skills across primates. Current Opinion New York: Basic Books.
in Neurobiology, 19, 45–51. Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom. New
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes- York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Rosete, D., & Ciarrochi, J. (2005). Emotional intelligence and its rela-
Psychology, 8, 382–439. tionship to workplace performance outcomes of leadership effective-
Roschelle, J., & Pea, R. (2002). A walk on the WILD side: How ness. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 26, 388–399.
wireless handhelds may change computer-supported collabora- Ross, J. R. (1967). Constraints on variables in syntax. Doctoral disserta-
tive learning. International Journal of Cognition and Technology, 1, tion, MIT.
145–168. Ross, J., Anderson, J. R., & Campbell, R. N. (2011). Situational self-
Roschelle, J. M., Pea, R. D., Hoadley, C. M., Gordin, D. N., & awareness influences 3- and 4-year-olds’ self-regulation. Journal of
Means, B. M. (2000). Changing how and what children learn in Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 126–138.
school with computer-based technologies. The Future of Children: Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings:
Children and Computer Technology, 10, 76–101. Distortions in the attribution process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Rose, A. J. (2002). Co-rumination in the friendships of girls and boys. Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 173–220).
Child Development, 73, 1830–1843. New York: Academic Press.
Rose, A. J., Carlson, W., & Waller, E. M. (2007). Prospective associa- Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The person and the situation:
tions of co-rumination with friendship and emotional adjustment: Perspectives of social psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Considering the socioemotional trade-offs of co-rumination. Ross, M. (1989). Relation of implicit theories to the construction of
Developmental Psychology, 43, 1019–1031. personal histories. Psychological Review, 96, 341–357.
Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex differences Ross, M. E. (1996). Cell division and the nervous system: Regulating
in peer relationship processes: Potential trade-offs for the emo- the cycle from neural differentiation to death. Trends in
tional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological Neurosciences, 19, 62–68.
Bulletin, 132, 98–131. Ross, M., & Wang, Q. (2010). Why we remember and what we
Rose, D. T., & Abramson, L. Y. (1992). Developmental predic- remember: Culture and autobiographical memory. Perspectives on
tors of depressive cognitive style: Research and theory. In D. Psychological Science, 5, 401–409.
Cicchetti & S. L. Toth (Eds.), Developmental perspectives on depres- Ross, N., Medin, D., Coley, J. D., & Atran, S. (2003). Cultural and
sion (pp. 323–350). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. experiential differences in the development of folkbiological induc-
Rose, S. A., & Blank, M. (1974). The potency of context in chil- tion. Cognitive Development, 18, 25–47.
dren’s cognition: An illustration through conservation. Child Ross, R. M. (1990). The evolution of sex-change mechanisms in
Development, 45, 499–502. fishes. Environmental Biology of Fishes, 29, 81–93.
Rose, S. A., & Feldman, J. (1995). The prediction of IQ and specific Ross, S. A. (2010). Diet and epigenetics. Nutrition and Health, 1, 101–123.
cognitive abilities at 11 years from infancy measures. Developmental Rossano, F., Rakoczyb, H., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young children’s
Psychology, 31, 685–696. understanding of violations of property rights. Cognition, 121,
Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., Futterweit, L. R., & Jankowski, J. J. 219–227.
(1997). Continuity in visual recognition memory: Infancy to 11 Rossell, C. H., & Baker, K. (1996). The effectiveness of bilingual edu-
years. Intelligence, 24, 381–392. cation. Research in the Teaching of English, 30, 7–74.
Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., & Jankowski, J. J. (2009). Information Rosselli, J. (1988). The castrati as a professional group and a social
processing in toddlers: Continuity from infancy and persistence of phenomenon, 1550–1850. Acta Musicologica, 60, 143–179.
preterm deficits. Intelligence, 37, 311–320. Rosser, J. C., Lynch, P. J., Cuddihy, L., Gentile, D. A., Klonsky, J., &
Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., Jankowski, J. J., & Van Rossem, R. (2005). Merrell, R. (2007). The impact of video games on training sur-
Pathways from prematurity and infant abilities to later cognition. geons in the 21st century. Archives of Surgery, 142, 181–186.
Child Development, 76, 1172. Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In
Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., Jankowski, J. J., & Van Rossem, R. (2012). W. Damon & R. Lerner (Series Eds.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.),
Information processing from infancy to 11 years: Continuities and Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and person-
prediction of IQ. Intelligence. 40, 445–457. ality development (6th ed., pp. 99–166). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Rosen, J. C. (1997). Cognitive-behavioral body image therapy. In Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Hershey, K. (2000). Stability
D. M. Garner & P. E. Garfinkel (Eds.), Handbook of treatment for of temperament in childhood: Laboratory infant assessment to
eating disorders (pp. 188–204). New York: Guilford Press. parent report at seven years. In V. J. Molfese & D. L. Molfese
REFERENCES R-107
(Eds.), Temperament and personality development across the life span Rowe, D. (1994). The limits of family influence: Genes, experience, and
(pp. 85–119). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. behavior. New York: Guilford Press.
Rothbart, M. K., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1985). Three approaches Rowe, D. C., Stever, C., Giedinghagen, L. N., Gard, J. M., Cleveland,
to the study of infant temperament. Developmental Review, 5, H. H., Terris, S. T., Mohr, J. H., Sherman, S., Abramowitz, A., &
237–250. Waldman, I. D. (1998). Dopamine DRD4 receptor polymorphism
Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M.I. (2001). Mechanism and variation and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Molecular Psychiatry,
in the development of attentional networks. In C. A. Nelson & 3, 419–426.
M. Luciana (Eds), Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience Rowe, J. W., & Kahn, R. L. (1987). Human aging: Usual and success-
(Chapter 24, pp. 353–363). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ful. Science, 237, 143–149.
Rothbart, M. K., & Rueda, M. R. (2005). The development of Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quan-
effortful control. In U. Mayr, E. Awh, & S. W. Keele (Eds.), tity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary develop-
Developing individuality in the human brain: A tribute to Michael I. ment. Child Development, 83, 1762–1774.
Posner (pp. 167–188). Washington, DC: American Psychological Rowe, S. M., & Wertsch, J. V. (2007). Vygotsky’s model of cognitive
Association. development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of child-
Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., & Hershey, K. (1995). Temperament, hood cognitive development (pp. 538–554). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
attention and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & Roy, B. C., Frank, M. C. & Roy, D. (2009). Exploring word learning
J. D. Cohen (Eds.), Manual of developmental psychopathology in a high-density longitudinal corpus. Proceedings of the 31st Annual
(Vol. 1, pp. 315–340). New York: Wiley. Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Amsterdam, Netherlands.
Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., & Kieras, J. (2008). Tempera- Royzman, E. B., & Sabini, J. (2001). Something it takes to be an emo-
ment, attention, and the development of self-regulation. In tion: The interesting case of disgust. Journal for the Theory of Social
K. McCartney & D. Phillips (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of early Behaviour, 31, 29–59.
child development. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing. Rozenblit, L., & Keil, F. (2002). The misunderstood limits of folk
Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., Rueda, M. R., & Posner, M. I. (2011). science: An illusion of explanatory depth. Cognitive Science, 26,
Developing mechanisms of self-regulation in early life. Emotion 521–562.
Review, 3, 1–7. Rozin, P. (1976). The evolution of intelligence and access to the cogni-
Rothman, R. H., & Simeone, F. A. (Eds.). (1982). The spine (2nd ed.). tive unconscious. In J. M. Sprague & A. A. Epstein (Eds.), Progress
Philadelphia: Saunders. in psychobiology and physiological psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 245–280).
Rotman, D. (2003, June 1). Genes, medicine, and the new race debate. New York: Academic Press.
MIT Technology Review, pp. 1–7. Rozin, P. (1999). The process of moralization. Psychological Science,
Rousmaniere, K., Dehli, K., & de Coninck-Smith, N. (Eds.) (1997). 10, 218–221.
Discipline, moral regulation & schooling: A social history. New York: Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological
Garland. Review, 94, 23–41.
Rousseau, J. J. (1762/1993). Emile. London: Everyman’s Library. Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2000). Disgust. In
Rovee, C. K., & Fagen, J. W. (1976). Extended conditioning and M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed.,
24-hour retention in infants. Journal of Experimental Child pp. 637–653). New York: Guilford Press.
Psychology, 21, 1–11. Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2008). Disgust. In M. Lewis,
Rovee-Collier, C. (1986). The rise and fall of infant classical condi- J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions
tioning research: Its promise for the study of early development. In (3rd ed., pp. 757–776). New York: Guilford Press.
L. P. Lipsitt & C. Rovee-Collier (Eds.), Advances in infancy research Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2009). Disgust: The body
(Vol. 4, pp. 139–159). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. and soul emotion in the 21st century. In O. Olatunji & D. McKay
Rovee-Collier, C. (1989). The joy of kicking: Memories, motives, (Eds.), Disgust and its disorders: Theory, assessment, and treatment
and mobiles. In P. R. Solomon, G. R. Goethals, C. M. Kelly, & (pp. 9–29). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
B. R. Stephens (Eds.), Memory: Interdisciplinary approaches Rozin, P., Hammer, L., Oster, H., Horowitz, T., & Marmora, V.
(pp. 151–180). New York: Springer-Verlag. (1986). The child’s conception of food: Differentiation of catego-
Rovee-Collier, C. (1997). Dissociations in infant memory: Rethinking ries of rejected substances in the 16 months to 5 year age range.
the development of implicit and explicit memory. Psychological Appetite, 7, 141–151.
Review, 104, 467–498. Rozin, P., & Kalat, J. W. (1971). Specific hungers and poison avoid-
Rovee-Collier, C. (1999). The development of infant memory. Current ance as adaptive specializations of learning. Psychological Review,
Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 80–85. 78, 459–486.
Rovee-Collier, C., & Bhatt, R. S. (1993). Evidence of long-term Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad
memory in infancy. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals of child development hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt,
(Vol. 9, pp. 1–45). London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy,
Rovee-Collier, C., & Giles, A. (2010). Why a neuromaturational divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 574–586
model of memory fails: Exuberant learning in early infancy. Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity domi-
Behavioural Processes, 83, 197–206. nance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5,
Rovee-Collier, C., & Hayne, H. (2000). Memory in infancy and early 296–320.
childhood. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford Rozin, P., & Vollmecke, T. A. (1986). Food likes and dislikes. Annual
handbook of memory (pp. 267–282). New York: Oxford University Review of Nutrition, 6, 433–456.
Press. Rubenstein, A. J., Kalakanis, L., & Langlois, J. H. (1999). Infant pref-
Rovee-Collier, C. K., & Shyi, G. C. W. (1992). A functional and cog- erences for attractive faces: A cognitive explanation. Developmental
nitive analysis of infant long-term retention. In M. L. Howe, C. J. Psychology, 35, 848–855.
Brainerd, & V. F. Reyna (Eds.), The development of long-term reten- Rubenstein, J. L. R., & Beachy, P. A. (1998). Patterning of the embry-
tion (pp. 3–55). New York: Springer-Verlag. onic forebrain. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 8, 18–26.
R-108 REFERENCES
Rubenstein, J. L. R., & Shimamura, K. (1997). Regulation of pat- Ruffman, T., & Langman, L. (2002). Infants’ reaching in a multi-well
terning and differentiation in the embryonic vertebrate forebrain. A not B task. Infant Behavior & Development, 25, 237–246.
In W. M. Cowan, T. M. Jessel, & S. L. Zipursky (Eds.), Molecular Ruffman, T., Perner, J., Naito, M., Parkin, L., & Clements W. (1998).
and cellular approaches to neural development (pp. 356–390). New Older (but not younger) siblings facilitate false belief understand-
York: Oxford University Press. ing. Developmental Psychology, 34, 161–174.
Rubie-Davies, C., Hattie, J., & Hamilton, R. (2006). Expecting the Ruffman, T., Slade, L., Sandino, J. C., & Fletcher, A. (2005). Are
best for students: Teacher expectations and academic outcomes. A-Not-B errors caused by a belief about object location? Child
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 429–444. Development, 76, 122–136.
Rubin, D. C. (1995). Memory in oral traditions: The cognitive psychol- Rugani, R., Fontanari, L., Simoni, E., Regolin, L., & Vallortigara,
ogy of epic, ballads, and counting-out rhymes. New York: Oxford G. (2009). Arithmetic in newborn chicks. Proceedings of the Royal
University Press. Society, B, 276, 2451–2460.
Rubin, H. (1998). Telomerase and cellular lifespan: Ending the Rumelhart, D. E. (1989). The architecture of mind: A connection-
debate? Nature Biotechnology, 16, 396–397. ist approach. In M. Posner (Ed.), Foundations of cognitive science.
Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J. G. (1998). Peer interac- Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
tions, relationships, and groups. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past
N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, tenses of English verbs. In J. L. McClelland & D. E. Rumelhart
emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 619–700). New (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the micro-
York: Wiley. structure of cognition: Vol. 2. Psychological and biological models
Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., Dwyer, K. M., & Hastings, P. D. (2003). (pp. 216–271). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Predicting preschoolers’ externalizing behaviors from toddler Runco, M. A. (2007). Creativity. Theories and themes: Research, devel-
temperament, conflict, and maternal negativity. Developmental opment, and practice. San Diego: Academic Press.
Psychology, 39, 164–176. Rush, A. J. (2003). Toward an understanding of bipolar disorder and
Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R., Chen, X., Buskirk, A., & Wojslawowicz, its origin. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 64, 4–8.
J. C. (2005). Peer relationships in childhood. In M. A. Bornstein & Rushton, J. P., & Jensen, A. R. (2005). Thirty years of research on
M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced text- race differences in cognitive ability. Psychology, Public Policy, and
book (5th ed., pp. 469–512). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Law, 11, 235–294.
Rubin, K. H., Fein, G. G., & Vandenberg, B. (1983). Play. In P. H. Russell, A., & Saebel, J. (1997). Mother-son, mother-daughter,
Mussen (Series Ed.) & E. M. Hetherington (Vol. Ed.), Handbook father-son, and father-daughter: Are they distinct relationships?
of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social devel- Developmental Review, 17, 111–147.
opment (6th ed., pp. 693–774). New York: Wiley. Russell, B. (1948). Human knowledge: Its scope and limits. New York:
Rubin, K. H., Hastings, P., Chen, X., Stewart, S. L., & McNichol, K. Simon and Schuster.
(1998). Intrapersonal and maternal correlates of aggression, con- Russell, D. E. (1984). The prevalence and seriousness of incestuous
flict and externalizing problems in toddlers. Child Development, abuse: Stepfathers vs. biological fathers. Child Abuse & Neglect, 8,
69, 1614–1629. 15–22.
Rubin, K. H., Nelson, L. J., Hastings, P., & Asendorpf, J. (1999). The Russell, G., & Russell, A. (1987). Mother-child and father-child rela-
transaction between parents’ perceptions of their children’s shy- tionships in middle childhood. Child Development, 58, 1573–1585.
ness and their parenting styles. International Journal of Behavioral Russell, J. A., Bachorowski, J. A., & Fernandez-Dols, J.-M. (2003).
Development, 23, 937–958. Facial and vocal expressions of emotion. Annual Review of
Ruble, D. (1983). The development of social comparison processes Psychology, 54, 329–349.
and their role in achievement-related self-socialization. In E. T. Russell, R. B., Green, N. S., Steiner, C. A., Meikle, S., Howse, J. L.,
Higgins, D. N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition Poschman, K., Dias, T., Potetz, L., Davidoff, M. J., Damus, K., &
and social development: A socio-cultural perspective (pp. 134–157). Petrini, J. R. (2007). Cost of hospitalization for preterm and low
New York: Cambridge University Press. birth weight infants in the United States. Pediatrics, 120, e1–e9.
Ruble, D. N., Feldman, N. S., Higgins, E. T., & Karlovac, M. (1979). Russell, V., de Villiers, A., Sagvolden, T., Lamm, M., & Taljaard, J.
Locus of causality and the use of information in the development (1995). Altered dopaminergic function in the prefrontal cortex,
of causal attributions. Journal of Personality, 47, 595–614. nucleus accumbens and caudate-putamen of an animal model of
Ruble, D. N., & Frey, K. S. (1991). Changing patterns of compara- attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: The spontaneously hyper-
tive behavior as skills are acquired: A functional model of self- tensive rat. Brain Research, 676, 343–351.
evaluation. In J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social comparison: Rutter, M. (1987). The role of cognition in child development and
Contemporary theory and research (pp. 79–113). Hillsdale, NJ: disorder. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 60, 1–16.
Erlbaum. Rutter, M. (2005). Incidence of autism spectrum disorders: Changes
Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender development. In W. over time and their meaning. Acta Paediatrica, 94, 2–15.
Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child Rutter, M., Colvert, E., Kreppner, J., Beckett, C. J., Groothues, C.,
psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development Hawkins, A., O’Connor, T., Stevens, S., & Sonuga-Burke, E.
(5th ed., pp. 933–1016). New York: Wiley. (2007). Early adolescent outcomes for institutionally deprived and
Rucklidge, J. J. (2010). Gender differences in attention-deficit/hyper- nondeprived adoptees. I: Disinhibited attachment. Journal of Child
activity disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 33, 357–373. Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 17–30.
Ruddle, F. H., Bartels, J. L., Bentley, K. L., Kappen, C., Murtha, Rutter, M., Giller, H., & Hagell, A. (1998). Antisocial behavior by
M. T., & Pendleton, J. W. (1994). Evolution of Hox genes. Annual young people. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Review of Genetics, 28, 423–442. Ryan, E. B., Jin, Y.-S., & Anas, A. P. (2009). Cross-cultural beliefs
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, about memory and aging for self and others: South Korea and
D. B., Lercari, L. P., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of atten- Canada. International Journal of Aging and Human Development,
tional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1029–1040. 68, 185–194.
REFERENCES R-109
Ryan, K., & Bohlin, K. E. (1999). Building character in schools: Practical Saigal, S., & Doyle, L. W. (2008). An overview of mortality and
ways to bring moral instruction to life. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. sequelae of preterm birth from infancy to adulthood. The Lancet,
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the 371, 261–269.
facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well- Sajwaj, T., Libet, J., & Agras, S. (1974). Lemon-juice therapy: The
being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78. control of life-threatening rumination in a six-month-old infant.
Rymer, R. (1992). Annals of silence: A silent childhood. The New Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 557–563.
Yorker, Part I, April 13, pp. 41–81; Part II, April 20, pp. 43–77. Sakatani, K., Chen, S., Lichty, W., Zuo, H., & Wang, Y. P. (1999).
Cerebral blood oxygenation changes induced by auditory stimula-
Saarni, C., Mumme, D., & Campos, J. (1998). Emotional develop- tion in newborn infants measured by near infrared spectroscopy.
ment: Action, communication, and understanding. In W. Damon Early Human Development, 55, 229–236.
(Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychol- Sakolsky, D. J., McCracken, J. T., & Nurmi, E. L. (2012). Genetics of
ogy: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pediatric anxiety disorders. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics
pp. 237–311). New York: Wiley. of North America, 21, 479–500.
Sabatino, C. P. (1996). Competency: Refining our legal fictions. In Salapatek, P., & Kessen, W. (1966). Visual scanning of triangles by the
M. A. Smyer, K. W. Schaie, & M. B. Kapp (Eds.), Older adults’ human newborn. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 3, 155–167.
decision-making and the law (pp. 1–28). New York: Springer. Salathia, N., & Queitsch, C. (2007). Molecular mechanisms of canal-
Sabbagh, M. A., Moses, L. J., & Shiverick, S. (2006a). Executive ization: Hsp90 and beyond. Journal of Biosciences, 32, 457–463.
functioning and preschoolers’ understanding of false beliefs, false Salmon, W. C. (1989). Four decades of scientific explanation. In
photographs, and false signs. Child Development, 77, 1034–1049. P. Kitcher & W. C. Salmon (Eds.), Minnesota studies in the history
Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Lee, K. of science: Vol. 13. Scientific explanation. Minneapolis: University
(2006b). The development of executive functioning and the- of Minnesota Press.
ory of mind: A comparison of Chinese and U.S. preschoolers. Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination,
Psychological Science, 17, 74–81. Cognition, and Personality, 9, 185–211.
Sachdeva, S., Singh, P., & Medin, D. (2011). Culture and the quest Salovey, P., & Sluyter, D. J. (Eds.). (1997). Emotional development and
for universal principles in moral reasoning. International Journal of emotional intelligence. New York: Basic Books.
Psychology, 46, 161–176. Saltaris, C. (2002). Psychopathy in juvenile offenders: Can tempera-
Sachser, N., & Kaiser, S. (1996). Prenatal social stress masculinizes ment and attachment be considered as robust developmental pre-
the female’s behaviour in guinea pigs. Physiology and Behavior, 60, cursors? Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 729–752.
589–594. Salthouse, T. A. (1984). Effects of age and skill in typing. Journal of
Sack, K. (1998). Georgia’s governor seeks musical start for babies. New Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 345–371.
York Times, January 15, A12. Salthouse, T. A. (1996a) A cognitive psychologist’s perspective
Saetre, P., Strandberg, E., Sundgren, P. E., Pettersson, U., Jazin, E., & on assessment of cognitive competency. In M. A. Smyer, K. W.
Bergström, T. F. (2006). The genetic contribution to canine per- Schaie, & M. B. Kapp (Eds.), Older adults’ decision-making and the
sonality. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 5, 240–248. law (pp. 29–39). New York: Springer.
Safer D. J., & Krager, J. M. (1992). Effect of a media blitz and a Salthouse, T. A. (1996b). The processing-speed theory of adult age
threatened lawsuit on stimulant treatment. Journal of the American differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428.
Medical Association, 268, 1004–1007. Salthouse, T. A. (1999). Cognitive competence and expertise in aging.
Safer, D. J., Zito, J. M., & Fine, E. M. (1996). Increased methylpheni- In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology
date usage for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in the 1990s. of aging (3rd ed., pp. 310–319). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Pediatrics, 98, 1084–1088. Salthouse, T. A. (2004). What and when of cognitive aging. Current
Saffran, J. R. (2003). Statistical language learning: Mechanisms and Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 140–144.
constraints. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 110–114. Salthouse, T. A. (2009). When does age-related cognitive decline
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learn- begin? Neurobiology of Aging, 30, 507–514.
ing by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928. Salthouse, T. A. (2010). Major issues in cognitive aging. Oxford, UK:
Saffran, J. R., Johnson, E. K., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1999). Oxford University Press.
Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Salthouse, T. A., Berish, D. E., & Miles, J. D. (2002). The role of
Cognition, 70, 27–52. cognitive stimulation on the relations between age and cognitive
Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L., & Aslin, R. N. (1996b). Word seg- functioning. Psychology and Aging, 17, 548–557.
mentation: The role of distributional cues. Journal of Memory and Salvas, M.-C., Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Lacourse, E., Boivin, M., &
Language, 35, 606–621. Tremblay, R. E. (2011). Interplay between friends’ aggression and
Saffran, J. R., Pollak, S. D., Seibel, R. L., & Shkolnik, A. (2007). Dog friendship quality in the development of child aggression during
is a dog is a dog: Infant rule learning is not specific to language. the early school years. Social Development, 20, 645–663.
Cognition, 105, 669–680. Salvatore, J. E., Kuo, S. I., Steele, R. D., Simpson, J. A., & Collins,
Saffran, J. R., Werker, J. F., & Werner, J. A. (2006). The infant’s W. A. (2011). Recovering from conflict in romantic relationships:
auditory world: Hearing, speech, and the beginnings of language. A developmental perspective. Psychological Science, 22, 376–383.
In R. Siegler & D. Kuhn (Eds.), Handbook of child development Samanez-Larkin, G. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (2011). Socioemotional
(pp. 58–108). New York: Wiley. functioning and the aging brain. In J. Decety & J. T. Cacioppo
Sagi, A., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Koren-Karie, N. (1991). Primary (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience (pp. 507–521).
appraisal of the Strange Situation: A cross-cultural analysis of pre- New York: Oxford University Press.
separation episodes. Developmental Psychology, 27, 587–596. Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Gibbs, S. E. B., Khanna, K., Nielsen, L.,
Sai, F. Z. (2005). The role of the mother’s voice in developing mother’s Carstensen, L. L., & Knutson, B. (2007) Anticipation of monetary
face preference: Evidence for intermodal perception at birth. Infant gain but not loss in healthy older adults. Nature Neuroscience, 10,
and Child Development, 14, 29–50. 787–791.
R-110 REFERENCES
Samanez-Larkin, G. R., & Knutson, B. (2014). Reward processing R. L. Schiefelbusch, & M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), Biological and
and risky decision making in the aging brain. In V. Reyna & behavioral determinants of language development (pp. 209–234).
V. Zayas (Eds.), The neuroscience of risky decision making. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Shanker, S. G., & Taylor, T. J. (1998). Apes,
Sameroff, A. J., & Chandler, M. (1975). Reproductive risk and language and the human mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
the continuum of caretaking casualty. In F. D. Horowitz, Savelsbergh, G., Rosengren, K., Van Der Kamp, J., & Verheul, M.
M. Hetherington, S. Scarr-Salapatek, & G. Siegal (Eds.), Review (2003). Catching action development. In G. Savelsbergh, K. Davids,
of child development research (Vol. 4, pp.187–244). Chicago: J. Van Der Kamp, & S. J. Bennett (Eds.), Development of movement
University of Chicago Press. co-ordination in children: Applications in the fields of ergonomics,
Samuel, J., & Bryant, P. (1984). Asking only one question in the con- health sciences, and sport (pp. 191–212). New York: Routledge.
servation experiment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Savic, I., Garcia-Falgueras, A., & Swaab, D. F. (2010). Sexual differen-
25, 315–318. tiation of the human brain in relation to gender identity and sexual
Samuels, B. D. (2012). Phonological architecture: A biolinguistic orientation. Progress in Brain Research, 186, 41–62.
approach (Oxford Studies in Biolinguistics). Oxford, UK: Oxford Savin-Williams, R. C. (2006). Who’s gay? Does it matter? Current
University Press. Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 40–44.
Sandanger, I., Nygard, J. F., Sorensen, T., & Moum, T. (2004). Is Savin-Williams, R. C., & Diamond, L. M. (2000). Sexual identity
women’s mental health more susceptible than men’s to the influ- trajectories among sexual-minority youths: Gender comparisons.
ence of surrounding stress? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Archives of Sexual Behavior, 29, 607–627.
Epidemiology, 39, 177–184. Savin-Williams, R. C., Joyner, K., & Rieger, G. (2012). Prevalence
Sandholtz, J. H., & Reilly, B. (2004). Teachers, not technicians: and stability of self-reported sexual orientation identity during
Rethinking technical expectations for teachers. Teachers College young adulthood. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 103–110.
Record, 106, 487–512. Savolainen, P., Zhang, Y. P., Luo, J., Lundeberg, J., & Leitner, T.
Sandoval, W. A. (2005). Understanding students’ practical episte- (2002, November 22). Genetic evidence for an East Asian origin
mologies and their influence on learning through inquiry. Science of domestic dogs. Science, 298, 1610–1613.
Education, 89, 634–656. Saxe, G. (1988). The mathematics of child street vendors. Child
Sann, C., & Streri, A. (2007). Perception of object shape and texture Development, 59, 1415–1425.
in human newborns: Evidence from cross-modal transfer tasks. Saxe, G. (1990). Culture and cognitive development: Studies in math-
Developmental Science, 10, 399–410. ematical understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Santos, L. R. (2004). ‘Core knowledges’: A dissociation between spa- Saxe, G. B., & Posner, J. K. (1983). The development of numerical
tiotemporal knowledge and contact-mechanics in a non-human cognition: Cross-cultural perspectives. In H. P. Ginsburg (Ed.),
primate? Developmental Science, 7, 167–174. The development of mathematical thinking (pp. 291–317). New
Santos, L. R., Flombaum, J. I., & Phillips, W. (2007). The evolution York: Academic Press.
of human mind reading. In S. Platek (Ed.), Evolutionary cognitive Saxe, R. (2005). Against simulation: The argument from error. Trends
neuroscience (pp. 433–456). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 174–179.
Santos, L. R., & Hauser, M. D. (2002). A non-human primate’s Saxe, R. (2006). Uniquely human social cognition. Current Opinion
understanding of solidity: Dissociations between seeing and act- in Neurobiology, 16, 235–239.
ing. Developmental Science, 5, F1–F7. Saxe, R., Carey, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2004a). Understanding other
Santos, L. R., Hauser, M. D., & Spelke, E. S. (2001). Recognition and minds: Linking developmental psychology and functional neuro-
categorization of biologically significant objects in rhesus monkeys imaging. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 87–124.
(Macaca mulatta): The domain of food. Cognition, 82, 127–155. Saxe, R., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Carey, S. (2005). Secret agents:
Santos, L. R., Seelig, D., & Hauser, M. D. (2006). Cotton-top tama- Inferences about hidden causes by 10- and 12-month-old infants.
rins’ (Saguinus oedipus) expectations about occluded objects: A dis- Psychological Science, 16, 995–1001.
sociation between looking and reaching tasks. Infancy, 9, 147–171. Saxe, R., Xiao, D. K., Kovacs, G., Perrett, D. I., & Kanwisher, N.
Sapir, E. (1921). Language: An introduction to the study of speech. New (2004b). A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds
York: Harcourt, Brace & Co. to observed intentional actions. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1435–1446.
Sarkadi, A., Kristiansson, R., Oberklaid, F., & Bremberg, S. (2008). Sayer, L. C., Bianchi, S. M., & Robinson, J. P. (2004). Are parents
Fathers’ involvement and children’s development outcomes: A investing less in children? Trends in mothers’ and fathers’ time
systematic review of longitudinal studies. Acta Paediatrica, 97, with children. American Journal of Sociology, 110, 1–43.
153–158. Scahill, R. I., Frost, C., Jenkins, R., Whitwell, J. I., Rossor, M. N., &
Sarnecka, B. W., & Lee, M. D. (2009). Levels of number knowledge Fox, N. C. (2003). A longitudinal study of brain volume changes
in early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, in normal aging using serial registered magnetic resonance imag-
325–337. ing. Archives of Neurology, 60, 989–994.
Sarnthein, J., Petsche, H., Rappelsberger, P., Shaw, G. L., & von Stein, Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention
A. (1998). Synchronization between prefrontal and posterior asso- in the infant, Nature, 253, 265–266.
ciation cortex during human working memory. Proceedings of the Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990’s: Development
National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 95, 7092–7096. and individual differences. Child Development, 63, 1–19.
Sarver, G. S., Howland, A., & McManus, T. (1976.) Effects of age Scarr, S. (1997). Behavior genetic and socialization theories of intel-
and stimulus presentation rate on immediate and delayed recall in ligence: Truce and reconciliation. In R. J. Sternberg & E. L.
children. Child Development, 47, 452–458. Grigorenko (Eds.), Intelligence: Heredity and environment. New
Satel, S. (2004) Antidepressants: Two countries, two views. New York York: Cambridge University Press.
Times, May 25. Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their envi-
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1991). Language learning in the bonobo: ronment: A theory of genotype-environment effects. Child
How and why they learn. In N. A. Krasnegor, D. M. Rumbaugh, Development, 54, 424–435.
REFERENCES R-111
Scarr, S., Pakstis, A. J., Katz, S. H., & Baker, W. B. (1977). Absence Schloesser, R. J., Huang, J., Klein, P. S., & Manji, H. K. (2008).
of a relationship between degree of white ancestry and intellectual Cellular plasticity cascades in the pathophysiology and treat-
skills within a black population. Human Genetics, 39, 69–86. ment of bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology Reviews, 33,
Scassellati, B. (2002). Theory of mind for a humanoid robot. 110–133.
Autonomous Robots, 12, 13–24. Schlottmann, A., & Surian, L. (1999). Do 9-month-olds perceive
Scerri, T. S., Paracchini, S., Morris, A., MacPhie, I. L., Talcott, J., causation-at-a-distance? Perception, 28, 1105–1113.
Stein, J., Smith, S. D., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., DeFries, Schlottmann, A., Surian, L., & Ray, E. (2009). Causal perception of
J. C., & Monaco, A. P. (2010). Identification of candidate genes for action-and-reaction sequences in 8- to 10-month-olds. Journal of
dyslexia susceptibility on chromosome 18. PLoS ONE, 5, e13712. Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 87–107.
Schaal, B. (1988). Olfaction in infants and children: Developmental Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process
and functional perspectives. Chemical Senses, 13, 145–190. model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological
Schachar, R., & Tannock, R. (1993). Childhood hyperactivity and Review, 115, 336–356.
psychostimulants: A review of extended treatment studies. Journal Schmidt, F. L., & Ones, D. S. (1992). Personnel selection. Annual
of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 3, 81–97. Review of Psychology, 43, 627–670.
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiologi- Schmidt, H. J. (1992). Olfactory hedonics in infants and young
cal determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, children. In S. Van Toller & G. H. Dodd (Eds.), Fragrance: The
379–399. psychology and biology of perfume (pp. 27–35). London: Elsevier
Schacter, D. L., Chiu, C. Y. P., & Ochsner, K. N. (1993). Implicit Science Publishers.
memory: A selective review. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, Schmidt, W. H., McKnight, C. C., & Raizen, S. A. (1997). A splin-
159–182. tered vision: An investigation of U.S. science and mathematics educa-
Schaffer, H. R. (1958). Objective observations of personality devel- tion. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
opment in early infancy. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 31, Schmidt, W. H., McKnight, C. C., Cogan, L. S., Jakwerth, P. M., &
174–183. Houang, R. T. (1999). Facing the consequences: Using TIMSS for a
Schaffer, H. R. (1965). Changes in developmental quotient under two closer look at U.S. mathematics and science education. Dordrecht,
conditions of maternal separation. British Journal of Social and The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Clinical Psychology, 4, 39–46. Schmitt, D. P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., & Allik, J. (2008). Why can’t a
Schaffer, H. R. (1966). The onset of fear of strangers and the incongru- man be more like a woman? Sex differences in Big Five personality
ity hypothesis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 7, 95–106. traits across 55 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Schaffer, H. R. (1990). Making decisions about children: Psychological 94, 168–182.
questions and answers. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Schmitz, J., Blechert, J., Krämer, M., Asbrand, J., & Tuschen-Caffier, B.
Schaffer, H. R., & Callender, W. M. (1959). Psychologic effects of (2012). Biased perception and interpretation of bodily anxiety
hospitalization in infancy. Pediatrics, 24, 528–539. symptoms in childhood social anxiety. Journal of Clinical Child
Schaffer, H. R., & Emerson, P. E. (1964). The development of social and Adolescent Psychology, 41, 92–102.
attachment in infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Schmuckler, M. A., & Li, N. S. (1998). Looming responses to obstacles
Child Development, 29 (serial no. 94). and apertures: The role of accretion and deletion of background
Schaie, K. W. (1984). Historical time and cohort effects. In K. A. texture. Psychological Science, 9, 49–52.
McCluskey & H. W. Reese (Eds.), Life-span developmental psy- Schneider, W. (1999). The development of metamemory in children.
chology: Historical and generational effects (pp. 1–15). Orlando, FL: In D. Gopher & A. Koriat (Eds.), Attention and performance XVII:
Academic Press. Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and appli-
Schaie, K. W. (1994). The course of adult intellectual development. cation (pp. 487–514). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
American Psychologist, 49, 304–313. Schneider, W., Gruber, H., Gold, A., & Opwis, K. (1993). Chess
Schaie, K. W. (2008). A lifespan developmental perspective of psy- expertise and memory for chess positions in children and adults.
chological ageing. In K. Laidlaw & B. Knight (Eds.), Handbook of Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 328–349.
emotional disorders in later life: Assessment and treatment (pp. 3–32). Schneider, W., & Lockl, K. (2002). The development of metacogni-
New York: Oxford University Press. tive knowledge in children and adolescents. In T. Perfect & B.
Schauble, L., Klopfer, L. E., & Raghavan, K. (1991). Students’ transi- Schwartz (Eds.), Applied metacognition (pp. 224–257). Cambridge,
tion from an engineering model to a science model of experimenta- UK: Cambridge University Press.
tion. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 28, 859–882. Schneider, W. J., & McGrew, K. S. (2012). The Cattell-Horn-Carroll
Schellenberg, E. G. (2005). Music and cognitive abilities. Current model of intelligence. In D. Flanagan & P. Harrison (Eds.),
Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 322–325. Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (3rd
Schiffman, J., Walker, E., Ekstrom, M., Schulsinger, F., Sorensen, H., & ed., pp. 99–144). New York: Guilford Press.
Mednick, S. (2004). Childhood videotaped social and neuromotor Schneider, W., Vise, M., Lockl, K., & Nelson, T. (2000).
precursors of schizophrenia: A prospective investigation. American Developmental trends in children’s memory monitoring: Evidence
Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 2021–2027. from a judgment-of-learning task. Cognitive Development, 15,
Schlaggar, B. L., & O’Leary, D. D. M. (1991). Potential of visual cor- 115–134.
tex to develop an array of functional units unique to somatosen- Schneider-Rosen, K., & Burke, P. B. (1999). Multiple attachment
sory cortex. Science, 252, 1556–1560. relationships within families: Mothers and fathers with two young
Schleifer, S. J., Keller, S. E., & Bartlett, J. A. (1999). Depression and children. Developmental Psychology, 35, 436–441.
immunity: Clinical factors and therapeutic course. Psychiatry Schoenfeld, A. H. (2004). The math wars. Educational Policy, 18,
Research, 85, 63–69. 253–286.
Schlessinger, L. (1996). How could you do that?! The abdication of char- Schoenfeld, A. H. (2007). Assessing mathematical proficiency. New
acter and conscience. New York: HarperCollins. York: Cambridge University Press.
R-112 REFERENCES
Scholl, B. J., & Leslie, A. M. (1999). Explaining the infant’s object con- Scientific American. (1896). Is insanity due to a microbe? [editorial]
cept: Beyond the perception/cognition dichotomy. In E. Lepore & Scientific American, 75, 303.
Z. Pylyshyn (Eds.), What is cognitive science? Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Scott, J. P., & Fuller, J. L. (1965). Genetics and the social behavior of the
Scholl, B. J., & Leslie, A. M. (1999). Modularity, development, and dog. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
“theory of mind.” Mind & Language, 14, 131–153. Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after
Scholl, B. J., & Leslie, A. M. (2001). Minds, modules, and meta- bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery &
analysis. Child Development, 72, 696–701. Psychiatry, 20, 11–21.
Scholl, B. J., & Nakayama, K. (2002). Causal capture: Contextual Scribner, S. (1988). Platoon schools. Unpublished paper circulated at
effects on the perception of collision events. Psychological Science, Cornell University, 1988.
13, 493–498. Scullin, M. K., Bugg, J. M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O.
Scholl, B. J., & Nakayama, K. (2004). Illusory causal crescents: (2011). Prospective memory and aging: Preserved spontaneous
Misperceived spatial relations due to perceived causality. Perception, retrieval, but impaired deactivation, in older adults. Memory &
33, 455–469. Cognition, 39, 1232–1240.
Scholl, B. J., & Tremoulet, P. (2000). Perceptual causality and ani- Sears, R. R. (1948). Effects of cup, bottle and breast feeding on oral
macy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 299–309. drive of newborn infants. American Psychologist, 3, 264–266.
Schrijvers, E. M., Verhaaren, B. F., Koudstaal, P. J., Hofman, A., Sears, R. (1975). Your ancients revisited: A history of child devel-
Ikram, M. A., & Breteler, M. M. (2012). Is dementia incidence opment. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Review of child develop-
declining? Trends in dementia incidence since 1990 in the ment research (Vol. 5, pp. 1–73). Chicago: University of Chicago
Rotterdam Study. Neurology, 78, 1456–1463. Press.
Schrum, W., & Cheek, N. H. (1987). Social structure during the Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child
school years: Onset of the degrouping process. American Sociological rearing. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.
Review, 52, 218–223. Seidenberg, M. (1992). Connectionism without tears. In S. Davis
Schull, J. (1990). Are species intelligent? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, (Ed.), Connectionism: Theory and practice (pp. 84–122). New York:
13, 63–108. Oxford University Press.
Schulz-Heik, J. R., Rhee, S. H., Silvern, L., Lessem, J. M., Haberstick, Seidenberg, M. (1993). Connectionist models and cognitive theory.
B. C., Hopfer, C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2009). Investigation of geneti- Psychological Science, 4, 228–235.
cally mediated child effects on maltreatment. Behavior Genetics 39, Seidenberg, M. S., & Petitto, L. A. (1979). Signing behavior in apes: A
265–276. critical review. Cognition, 7, 177–215.
Schumacher, J., Anthoni, H., Dahdouh, F., Konig, I. R., Hillmer, A. M., Seidenberg, M. S., & Petitto, L. A. (1987). Communication, symbolic
Kluck, N., Manthey, M., Plume, E., Warnke, A., Remschmidt, H., communication, and language: Comment on Savage-Rumbaugh,
Hulsmann, J., Cichon, S., Lindgren, C., Propping, P., Zuccheli, M., McDonald, Sevcik, Hopkins, and Rupert (1986). Journal of
Ziegler, A., Peyrard-Janvid, M., Schulte-Korne, G., Nothen, M. M., & Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 279–287.
Kere, J. (2006). Strong genetic evidence of DCDC2 as a susceptibility Seimiya, M., & Gehring, W. J. (2000). The Drosophila homeobox gene
gene for dyslexia. American Journal of Human Genetics, 78, 52–62. optix is capable of inducing ectopic eyes by an eyeless-independent
Schumacher, J., Hoffmann, P., Schmael, C., Schulte-Korne, G., & mechanism. Development, 127, 1879–1886.
Nothen, M. M. (2007). Genetics of dyslexia: The evolving land- Seligman, M. E. P. (1970). On the generality of laws of learning.
scape. Journal of Medical Genetics, 44, 289–297. Psychological Review, 77, 406–418.
Schunk, D. H. (1987). Peer models and children’s behavioral change. Seligman, M. E. P. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behavior
Review of Educational Research, 57, 149–174. Therapy, 2, 307–320.
Schuster, M. (1998). Representing older persons with diminished capac- Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development,
ity: Ethical considerations. 263 Practising Law Institute/Estate and death. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
Planning and Administration, 339. Sellick, C. (2006). From famine to feast: A review of the foster care
Schwartz, C. E, Snidman, N., & Kagan, J. (1999). Adolescent social research literature. Children and Society, 20, 67–74.
anxiety as an outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood. Selman, R. L. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding. New
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, York: Academic Press.
38, 1008–1015. Selman, R. L. (2003). The promotion of social awareness: Powerful les-
Schwartz, C. E., Wright, C. I., Shin L. M., Kagan, J., & Rauch, sons from the partnership of developmental theory and classroom prac-
S. L. (2003). Inhibited and uninhibited infants “grown up”: Adult tice. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
amygdalar response to novelty. Science, 300, 1952–1953. Selman, R. L., & Schultz, L. H. (1990). Making a friend in youth:
Schwartz, D., Hopmeyer Gorman, A. H., Toblin, R. L., & Developmental theory and pair therapy. Chicago: University of
Abouezzedine, T. (2003). Mutual antipathies in the peer group as a Chicago Press.
moderating factor in the association between community violence Seltzer, J. (1994). Consequences of marital dissolution for children.
exposure and psychosocial maladjustment. In E. V. E. Hodges & Annual Review of Sociology, 20, 235–266.
N. A. Card (Eds.), Enemies and the darker side of peer relations: Senghas, A. (1994). Nicaragua’s lessons for language acquisition.
New directions for child and adolescent development (Vol. 102, Signpost, 7, 32–39.
pp. 39–54). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Senghas, A. (2010). The emergence of two functions for spatial devices
Schwartz-Mette, R. A., & Rose, A. J. (2012). Co-rumination mediates in Nicaraguan sign language. Human Development, 53, 287–302.
contagion of internalizing symptoms within youths’ friendships. Senghas, A., & Coppola, M. (2001). Children creating language:
Developmental Psychology, 48, 1355–1365. How Nicaraguan Sign Language acquired a spatial grammar.
Schwebel, D. C., Rosen, C. S., & Singer, J. L. (1999). Preschoolers’ Psychological Science, 12, 323–328.
pretend play and theory of mind: The role of jointly constructed Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2008). Gaze following in human infants
pretense. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 333–348. depends on communicative signals. Current Biology, 18, 668–671.
REFERENCES R-113
Senju, A., Csibra, G., & Johnson, M. H. (2008). Understanding the systems for compensation and persistence: Young adult outcomes
referential nature of looking: Infants’ preference for object-directed of childhood reading disability. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 25–33.
gaze. Cognition, 108, 303–319. Sheridan, M. A., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., McLaughlin, K. A., &
Serbin, L. A., Poulin-Dubois, D., Colburne, K. A., Sen, M. G., & Nelson, C. A. (2012). Variation in neural development as a result
Eichstedt, J. A. (2001). Gender stereotyping in infancy: Visual of exposure to institutionalization early in childhood. Proceedings
preferences for and knowledge of gender-stereotyped toys in the of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 109, 12927–12932.
second year. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, Sheridan, S. M., Knoche, L. L., Kupzyk, K. A., Edwards, C. P., &
7–15. Marvin, C. A. (2011). A randomized trial examining the effects
Serres, L. (2001). Morphological changes of the human hippocam- of parent engagement on early language and literacy: The getting
pal formation from midgestation to early childhood. In C. A. ready intervention. Journal of School Psychology, 49, 361–383.
Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive Sherman, D., Iacono, W., & McGue, M. (1997). Attention-deficit
neuroscience (pp. 45–58). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. hyperactivity disorder dimensions: A twin study of inattention and
Shackman, J. E., Fatani, S., Camras, L. A., Berkowitz, M. J., impulsivity-hyperactivity. Journal of the American Academy of Child
Bachorowski, J., & Pollak, S. D. (2010). Emotion expression among and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 745–753.
abusive mothers is associated with their children’s emotion process- Sherry, J. (2001). The effects of violent video games on aggression:
ing and problem behaviours. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 1421–1430. A meta-analysis. Human Communication Research, 27, 409–431.
Shannon, J. S., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Margolin, A. (2005). Father Sheskin, M. (2013). Unfairness in children and monkeys (Doctoral
involvement in infancy: Influences of past and current relation- dissertation,Yale University, 2013).
ships. Infancy, 8, 21–41. Shields, A., Dickstein, S., Seifer, R., Giusti, L., Magee, K. D., & Spritz, B.
Shantz, C. U. (1983). Social cognition. In J. H. Flavell & E. M. (2001). Emotional competence and early school adjustment: A
Markman (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Cognitive study of preschoolers at risk. Early Education and Development, 12,
development (pp. 495–555). New York: Wiley. 73–96.
Sharp, S. (1996). Self-esteem, response style, and victimization. School Shimamura, A. P., Berry, J. M., Mangels, J. A., Rusting, C. L., &
Psychology International, 17, 347–357. Jurica, P. J. (1995). Memory and cognitive abilities in university
Sharp, S., & Smith, R. (1994). Tackling bullying in your school. A prac- professors: Evidence for successful aging. Psychological Science, 6,
tical handbook for teachers. London: Routledge. 271–277.
Shatz, C. J. (1990). Impulse activity and the patterning of connections Shimizu, H. (2001). Japanese adolescent boys’ senses of empathy
during CNS development. Neuron, 5, 745–756. (Omoiyari) and Carol Gilligan’s perspectives on the morality of care:
Shatz, C. J. (1992a). Dividing up the neocortex. Science, 258, 237–238. A phenomenological approach. Culture & Psychology, 7, 453–475.
Shatz, C. J. (1992b). The developing brain. Scientific American, 267, Shimizu, Y. A., & Johnson, S. C. (2004). Infants’ attribution of a goal
61–67. to a morphologically unfamiliar agent. Developmental Science, 7,
Shatz, C. J. (1997). Neurotrophins and visual system plasticity. In 425–430.
W. M. Cowan, T. M. Jessel, & S. L. Zipursky (Eds.), Molecular Shin, H., Bjorklund, D. F., & and Beck, E. F. (2007). The adaptive
and cellular approaches to neural development (pp. 509–524). New nature of children’s overestimation in a strategic memory task.
York: Oxford University Press. Cognitive Development, 22, 97–212.
Shatz, C. J., Ghosh, A., McConnell, S. K., Allendoerfer, K. L., Friauf, Shing, Y. L., & Lindenberger, U. (2011). The development of epi-
E., & Antonini, A. (1990). Pioneer neurons and target selection sodic memory: Lifespan lessons. Child Development Perspectives,
in cerebral cortical development. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on 5, 148–155.
Quantitative Biology, 55, 469–480. Shirley, M. M. (1931). The first two years: A study of twenty-five babies
Shatz, M., & Gelman, R. (1973). The development of communica- (Vol. 1). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
tion skills: Modifications in the speech of young children as a Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent
function of listener. Monographs of the Society for Research on Child cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay
Development, 38 (Serial No. 152), 1–37. of gratification: Identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental
Shaw, A., Montinari, N., Piovesan, M., Olson, K. R., Gino, F., & Psychology, 26, 978–986.
Norton, M. I. (2013). Children develop a veil of fairness. Journal of Shonkoff, J. P., & Meisels, S. J. (2000). Handbook of early childhood
Experimental Psychology: General. Epub ahead of print. intervention (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Shaw, A., & Olson, K. R. (2012). Children discard a resource to Shore, R. A. (2009). Reframing the first day of school. The School
avoid inequity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, General, 141, Administrator, 66, 22–25.
382–395. Shrum, W., & Cheek, N. H. (1987). Social structure during the school
Shaw, M., Lawlor, D. A., & Najman, J. M. (2006). Teenage children years: Onset of the degrouping process. American Sociological
of teenage mothers: Psychological, behavioural and health out- Review, 52, 218–223.
comes from an Australian prospective longitudinal study. Social Shucksmith, J., Hendry, L. B., & Glendinning, A. (1995). Models of
Science and Medicine, 62, 2526–2539. parenting: Implications for adolescent well-being within different
Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Shaywitz, S. E. (1997). Attention- types of family contexts. Journal of Adolescence, 18, 253–270.
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Advances in Pediatrics, 44, 331–367. Shukla, M., White, K. S., & Aslin, R. N. (2011). Prosody guides
Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). Dyslexia (specific reading the rapid mapping of auditory word forms onto visual objects in
disability). Pediatrics in Review, 24, 147–153. 6-mo-old infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2005). Dyslexia (specific reading the USA, 108, 6038–6043.
disability). Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1301–1309. Shultz, T. R., & Kestenbaum, N. R. (1985). Causal reasoning in chil-
Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P., Mencl, dren. Annals of Child Development, 2, 195–249.
W. E., Constable, R. T., Pugh, K. R., Holahan, J. M., Marchione, Shumow, L., Vandell, D. L., & Posner, J. K. (1998). Harsh, firm,
K. E., Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., & Gore, J. C. (2003). Neural and permissive parenting in low-income families: Relations to
R-114 REFERENCES
children’s academic achievement and behavioral adjustment. Schifter (Eds.), A research companion to principles and standards for
Journal of Family Issues, 19, 483–507. school mathematics (pp. 119–233). Reston, VA: National Council of
Shusterman, A., Lee, S. A., & Spelke, E. S. (2011). Cognitive effects of Teachers of Mathematics.
language on human navigation. Cognition, 120, 186–201. Siegler, R. S., & Jenkins, E. (1989). How children discover new strate-
Shuwairi, S. M., Albert, M. K., & Johnson, S. P. (2007). Discrimination gies. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
of possible and impossible objects in infancy. Psychological Science, Siegler, R. S., & Shrager, J. (1984). Strategy choices in addition and sub-
18, 303–307. traction: How do children know what to do? In C. Sophian (Ed.),
Shweder, R. A. (1982). Beyond self-constructed knowledge: The study Origins of cognitive skills (pp. 229–293). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
of culture and morality. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 41–69. Silani, V., & Corbo, M. (2004). Cell-replacement therapy with
Shweder, R. (1991). Thinking through cultures: Expeditions in cultural stem cells in neurodegenerative diseases. Current Neurovascular
psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Research, 1, 283–289.
Shweder, R. A., & Bourne, E. J. (1982). Does the concept of the per- Silberg, J. L., Pickles, A., Rutter, M., Hewitt, J., Simonoff, E., Maes, H.,
son vary cross-culturally? In A. J. Marsella & G. M. White (Eds.), Caronneau, R., Murrelle, L., Foley, D., & Eaves, L. (1999). The
Cultural conceptions of mental health and therapy (pp. 97–137). influence of genetic factors and life stress on depression among
Boston: D. Reidel. adolescent girls. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 225–232.
Shweder, R. A., Goodnow, J., Hatano, G., LeVine, R. A., Markus, H., & Silberg, J., Rutter, M., Meyer, J., Maes, H., Hewitt, J., Simonoff, E.,
Miller, P. (1998). The cultural psychology of development: One Pickles, A., Loeber, R., & Eaves, L. (1996). Genetic and environ-
mind, many mentalities. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & R. M. Lerner mental influences on the covariation between hyperactivity and
(Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of conduct disturbance in juvenile twins. Journal of Child Psychology
human development (5th ed., pp. 865–937). New York: Wiley. and Psychiatry, 37, 803–816.
Shweder, R. A., & Haidt, J. (1993). The future of moral psychology: Silver, L. B. (1992). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Washington,
Truth, intuition, and the pluralist way. Psychological Science, 4, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
360–365. Silver, M. H., Jilinskaia, E., & Perls, T. T. (2001). Cognitive func-
Shweder, R. A., Mahapatra, M., & Miller, J. G. (1987). Culture and tional status of age-confirmed centenarians in a population-based
moral development. In J. Kagan & S. Lamb (Eds.), The emergence study. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and
of morality in young children (pp. 1–83). Chicago: University of Social Sciences, 56B, P134–P140.
Chicago Press. Silverman, A. (1971). The case against having children. New York:
Shweder, R. A., & Much, N. C. (1991). Determinations of mean- David McKay.
ing: Discourse and moral socialization. In R. A. Shweder (Ed.), Silverman, I. W. (2003). Gender differences in resistance to tempta-
Thinking through cultures: Expeditions in cultural psychology tion: Theories and evidence. Developmental Review, 23, 219–259.
(pp. 186–240). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Simeone, A., Acampora, D., Arcioni, L., Andrews, P., Boncinelli, E., &
Shweder, R. A., Much, N. C., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). The Mavilio, F. (1990). Sequential activation of Hox2 homeobox genes
“big three” of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and the by retinoic acid in human embryonal carcinoma cells. Nature, 346,
“big three” explanations of suffering. In A. M. Brandt & P. Rozin 763–766.
(Eds.), Morality and health (pp. 119–169). New York: Routledge. Simeone, A., Acampora, D., Gulisano, M., Stornaiuolo, A., &
Sidman, R. L., & Rakic, P. (1973). Neuronal migration, with special Boncinelli, E. (1992). Nested expression domains of four homeo-
reference to developing human brain: A review. Brain Research, 62, box genes in developing rostral brain. Nature, 358, 687–690.
1–35. Simeone, A., Acampora, D., Mallamaci, A., Stornaiuolo, A., D’Apice,
Siegal, M., Butterworth, G., & Newcombe, P. A. (2004). Culture and M. R., Nigro, V., & Boncinelli, E. (1993). A vertebrate gene related
children’s cosmology. Developmental Science, 7, 308–324. to orthodenticle contains a homeodomain of the bicoid class and
Siegal, M., Nobes, G., & Panagiotaki, G. (2011). Children’s knowl- demarcates anterior neuroectoderm in the gastrulating mouse
edge of the Earth. Nature Geoscience, 4, 130–132. embryo. EMBO Journal, 12, 2735–2747.
Siegal, M., & Peterson, C. C. (1996). Breaking the mold: A fresh look Simon, T. J., Hespos, S. J., & Rochat, P. (1995). Do infants under-
at children’s understanding of questions about lies and mistakes. stand simple arithmetic? A replication of Wynn (1992). Cognitive
Developmental Psychology, 32, 322–334. Development, 10, 253–270.
Siegel, B. S., Perrin, E. C., & the Committee on Psychosocial Aspects Simon, V. A., Aikins, J. W., & Prinstein, M. J. (2008). Romantic
of Child and Family Health. (2013). Promoting the well-being of partner selection and socialization during early adolescence. Child
children whose parents are gay or lesbian. Pediatrics, 131, 827–830. Development, 79, 1676–1692.
Siegler, R. S. (1976). Three aspects of cognitive development. Cognitive Simons, D. J. (1996). In sight, out of mind: When object representa-
Psychology, 8, 481–520. tions fail. Psychological Science, 7, 301–305.
Siegler, R. S. (1981). Developmental sequences within and between Simons, D. J., & Keil, F. C. (1995). An abstract to concrete shift in the
concepts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child development of biological thought: The insides story. Cognition,
Development, 46 (Whole No. 189). 56, 129–163.
Siegler, R. S. (1988). Individual differences in strategy choices: Simons, L. G., Chen, Y. F., Simons, R. L., Brody, G., & Cutrona, C.
Good students, not-so-good students, and perfectionists. Child (2006). Parenting practices and child adjustment in different types
Development, 59, 833–851. of households: A study of African American families. Journal of
Siegler, R. S. (1989). Mechanisms of cognitive development. Annual Family Issues, 27, 803–825.
Review of Psychology, 40, 353–379. Simonton, D. K. (1991). Emergence and realization of genius: The
Siegler, R. S. (1995). How does change occur: A microgenetic study of lives and works of 120 classical composers. Journal of Personality
number conservation. Cognitive Psychology, 28, 225–273. and Social Psychology, 61, 829–840.
Siegler, R. S. (2003). Implications of cognitive science research for Simonton, D. K. (1994). Greatness: Who makes history and why. New
mathematics education. In J. Kilpatrick, W. G. Martin, & D. E. York: Guilford Press.
REFERENCES R-115
Simonton, D. K. (2000). Creativity: Cognitive, personal, develop- Slater, A., Johnson, S. P., Brown, E., & Badenoch, M. (1996). Newborn
mental, and social aspects. American Psychologist, 55, 151–158. infants’ perception of partly occluded objects. Infant Behavior and
Simpson, J. A., Collins, W. A., & Salvatore, J. E. (2011). The impact Development, 19, 145–148.
of early interpersonal experience on adult romantic relationship Slater, A., Johnson, S., Kellman, P. J., & Spelke, E. (1994). The role
functioning: Recent findings from the Minnesota Longitudinal of three- dimensional depth cues in infant’s perception of partly
Study of Risk and Adaptation. Current Directions in Psychological occluded objects. Early Development and Parenting, 3, 187–191.
Science, 20, 355–359. Slater, A., Morison, V., Somers, M., Mattock, A., Brown, E., &
Singec, I., Jandial, R., Crain, A., Nikkhah, G., & Snyder, E. Y. Taylor, D. (1990). Newborn and older infants’ perception of partly
(2007). The leading edge of stem cell therapeutics. Annual Review occluded objects. Infant Behavior and Development, 13, 33–49.
of Medicine, 58, 313–328. Slater, A., Quinn, P. C., Hayes, R., & Brown, E. (2000b). The role
Singer, D. G., & Singer, J. L. (1990). The house of make-believe: of facial orientation in newborn infants’ preference for attractive
Children’s play and the developing imagination. Cambridge, MA: faces. Developmental Science, 3, 181–185.
Harvard University Press. Slater, A., Rose, D., & Morison, V. (1984). Newborn infants’ per-
Singer, J. L. (1961). Imagination and waiting ability in young chil- ception of similarities and differences between two- and three-
dren. Journal of Personality, 29, 396–413. dimensional stimuli. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
Singer, J., & Singer, D. (1998). Barney & Friends as entertainment 2, 287–294.
education: Evaluating the quality and effectiveness of a television Slater, A., von der Schulenburg, C., Brown, E., Badenoch, M.,
series for preschool children. In J. Asamen & G. Berry (Eds.), Butterworth, G., Parsons, S., & Samuels, C. (1998). Newborn
Research paradigms, television, and social behavior (pp. 305–367). infants prefer attractive faces. Infant Behavior and Development,
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 21, 345–354.
Singer, P. (2005). Ethics and intuitions. Journal of Ethics, 9, 331–352. Slater, E. J., & Haber, J. D. (1984). Adolescent adjustment following
Singer, T., Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). Plasticity of divorce as a function of family conflict. Journal of Consulting and
memory for new learning in very old age: A story of major loss? Clinical Psychology, 52, 920–921.
Psychology and Aging, 18, 306–317. Slater, P. J. B. (1989). Birdsong learning: Causes and consequences.
Singer, T., Verhaeghen, P., Ghisletta, P., Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, Ethology, Ecology, and Evolution, 1, 19–46.
P. B. (2003). The fate of cognition in very old age: Six-year longi- Slaughter, V., Dennis, M. J., & Pritchard, M. (2002). Theory of
tudinal findings in the Berlin Aging Study (BASE). Psychology and mind and peer acceptance in preschool children. British Journal of
Aging, 18, 318–331. Developmental Psychology, 20, 545–564.
Singer, W. (1986). Neuronal activity as a shaping factor in postnatal Slaughter, V., Itakura, S., Kutsuki, A., & Siegal, M. (2011). Learning
development of visual cortex. In W. T. Greenough & J. M. Juraska to count begins in infancy: Evidence from 18 month olds’ visual
(Eds.), Developmental neuropsychobiology (pp. 271–293). Orlando: preferences. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 278, 2979–2984.
Academic Press. Slep, A. M., & O’Leary, S. G. (1998). The effects of maternal attri-
Singleton, J. L., & Newport, E. L. (2004). When learners surpass butions on parenting: An experimental analysis. Journal of Family
their models: The acquisition of American Sign Language from Psychology, 12, 234–243.
inconsistent input. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 370–407. Slining, M., Adair, L. S., Goldman, B. D., Borja, J. B., & Bentley, M.
Sisk, C. L., & Foster, D. L. (2004). The neurobiology of puberty and (2010). Infant overweight is associated with delayed motor devel-
adolescence. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 1040–1047. opment. Journal of Pediatrics, 157, 1, 20–25.
Skilling, T. A., Quinsey, V. L., & Craig, W. M. (2001). Evidence of Sliwinski, M., & Buschke, H. (1999). Cross-sectional and longitu-
a taxon underlying serious antisocial behavior in boys. Criminal dinal relationships among age, cognition, and processing speed.
Justice and Behavior, 28, 450–470. Psychology and Aging, 14, 18–33.
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental anal- Slobin, D. I. (1985). Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making
ysis. New York: Appleton-Century. capacity. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century- acquisition: Vol. 2. Theoretical issues (pp. 1157–1256). Hillsdale,
Crofts. NJ: Erlbaum.
Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York: Knopf. Slonje, R., Smith, P. K., & Frisén, A. (2012). Processes of cyberbul-
Slaby, R. G., Roedell, W. C., Arezzo, D., & Kendrix, K. (1995). Early lying, and feelings of remorse by bullies: A pilot study. European
violence prevention: Tools for teachers of young children. Washington, Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 244–259.
DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children. Sluckin, A. M., & Smith, P. K. (1977). Two approaches to the con-
Slack, J. M. W. (1991). From egg to embryo (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: cept of dominance in preschool children. Child Development, 48,
Cambridge University Press. 917–923.
Slater, A. M., Bremner, J. G., Johnson, S. P., & Hayes, R. A. (2010). Sluckin, W. (1972). Imprinting and early learning (2nd ed.). London:
The role of perceptual and cognitive processes in addition- Methuen.
subtraction studies with 5-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Slusser, E. B., & Sarnecka, B. W. (2011). Find the picture of eight turtles:
Development, 33, 685–688. A link between children’s counting and their knowledge of number-
Slater, A., Bremner, G., Johnson, S. P., Sherwood, P., Hayes, R., & word semantics. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 38–51.
Brown, E. (2000a). Newborn infants’ preference for attractive Sluzenski, J., Newcombe, N., & Ottinger, W. (2004). Changes in
faces: The role of internal and external facial features. Infancy, 1, reality monitoring and episodic memory in early childhood.
265–274. Developmental Science, 7, 225–245.
Slater, A., Cooper, R., Rose, D., & Morison, V. (1989). Prediction of Sluzenski, J., Newcombe, N. S., & Satlow, E. (2004). Knowing where
cognitive performance from infancy to early childhood. Human things are in the second year of life: Implications for hippocampal
Development, 32, 137–147. development. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1443–1451.
R-116 REFERENCES
Smahel, D., Brown, B. B., & Blinka, L. (2012). Associations between Smith, P. (1996). Language and the evolution of mindreading. In
online friendships and Internet addiction among adolescents and P. Carruthers & P. K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind.
emerging adults. Developmental Psychology, 48, 381–388. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Small, B. J., & Bäckman, L., (1999). Time to death and cogni- Smith, P. K., Mahdavi, J., Carvalho, M., Fisher, S., Russell, S., &
tive performance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, Tippett, N. (2008). Cyberbullying: Its nature and impact in sec-
168–172. ondary school pupils. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49,
Smalley, S. L., Bailey, J. N., Palmer, C. G., Cantwell, D. P., McGough, 376–385.
J. J., Del’Homme, M. A., Asarnow, J. R., Woodward, J. A., Smith, S. D., Kimberling, W. J., Pennington, B. F., & Lubs, H. A.
Ramsey, C., & Nelson S. F. (1998). Evidence that the dopamine (1983). Specific reading disability: Identification of an inherited
D4 receptor is a susceptibility gene in attention deficit hyperactiv- form through linkage analysis. Science, 219, 1345–1347.
ity disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 3, 427–430. Smith, S. M., Pang, K., Sundin, O., Wedden, S. E., Thaller, C., &
Smetana, J. G. (1997). Parenting and the development of social Eichele, G. (1989). Molecular approaches to vertebrate limb mor-
knowledge reconceptualized: A social domain analysis. In J. E. phogenesis. Development, 107, 121–131.
Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and children’s internal- Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Finke, R. A. (1995). The creative cogni-
ization of values: A handbook of contemporary theory (pp.162–192). tion approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
New York: Wiley. Smith, W. C., Johnson, S. P., & Spelke, E. S. (2003). Motion and edge
Smetana, J. G., Campione-Barr, N., & Metzger, A. (2006). Adolescent sensitivity in perception of object unity. Cognitive Psychology, 46,
development in interpersonal and societal contexts. Annual Review 31–64.
of Psychology, 57, 255–284. Smith, W. D. (1979). The Hippocratic tradition. Ithaca, NY: Cornell
Smetana, J. G., Killen, M., & Turiel, E. (1991). Children’s reasoning University Press.
about interpersonal and moral conflicts. Child Development, 62, Smolensky, P. (1990). Tensor product variable binding and the repre-
629–644. sentation of symbolic structures in connectionist systems. Artificial
Smink, F. R., van Hoeken, D., & Hoek, H. W. (2012). Epidemiology Intelligence, 46, 159–216.
of eating disorders: Incidence, prevalence and mortality rates. Smyer, M. A. (1996). Decision-making capacity among older adults:
Current Psychiatry Reports, 14, 406–414. Person, process and context. In M. A. Smyer, K. W. Schaie, &
Smith, C., Carey, S., & Wiser, M. (1985). On differentiation: A case M. B. Kapp (Eds.), Older adults’ decision-making and the law
study of the development of size, weight, and density. Cognition, (pp. 283–287). New York: Springer.
21, 177–237. Smyke, A. T., Koga, S. F., Johnson, D. E., Fox, N. A., Marshall, P. J.,
Smith, C. L., Maclin, D., Houghton, C., & Hennessey, G. M. (2000). Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H., & the BEIP Core Group. (2007).
Sixth-grade students’ epistemologies of science: The impact The caregiving context in institution-reared and family-reared
of school science experiences on epistemological development. infants and toddlers in Romania. Journal of Child Psychology and
Cognition and Instruction, 18, 349–422. Psychiatry, 48, 210–218.
Smith, C., Wiser, M., Anderson, C. W., Krajcik, J., & Coppola, B. Smyke, A. T., Zeanah, C. H., Gleason, M. M., Drury, S. S., Fox,
(2004). Implications of research on children’s learning for assessment: N. A., Nelson, C. A., & Guthrie, D. (2012). A randomized con-
Matter and atomic molecular theory. Paper commissioned by the trolled trial comparing foster care and institutional care for chil-
National Research Council’s Committee on Test Design for K-12 dren with signs of reactive attachment disorder. American Journal
Science Achievement. Washington, DC: National Academy of of Psychiatry, 169, 508–514.
Sciences. Snarey, J. R. (1985). Cross-cultural universality of social-moral devel-
Smith, D. (2003). The older population in the United States: March opment: A critical review of Kohlbergian research. Psychological
2002. U.S. Census Bureau Current Population Reports, P20–546. Bulletin, 97, 202–232.
Washington, DC: U.S. Census Bureau. http://www.census.gov Snedeker, J., Geren, J., & Shafto, C. L. (2012). Disentangling
/prod/2003pubs/p20-546.pdf the effects of cognitive development and linguistic exper-
Smith, E. E., Shoben, E. J., & Rips, L. J. (1974). Structure and pro- tise: A longitudinal study of the acquisition of English
cess in semantic memory: A featural model for semantic decisions. in internationally-adopted children. Cognitive Psychology, 65,
Psychological Review, 81, 214–241. 39–76.
Smith, E. L., 3rd, Hung, L.-F., & Huang, J. (2009). Relative periph- Snow, C. E. (1977). Mothers’ speech research: From input to inter-
eral hyperopic defocus alters central refractive development in action. In C. Snow & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children:
infant monkeys. Vision Research, 49, 2386–2392. Language input and acquisition (pp. 31–49). Cambridge, UK:
Smith, J. R., Brooks- Gunn, J., Kohen, D., & McCarton, C. (2001). Cambridge University Press.
Transitions on and off AFDC: Implications for parenting Snow, C. E. (1987). Beyond conversation: Second language learn-
and children’s cognitive development. Child Development, 72, ers’ acquisition of description and explanation. In J. Lantolf &
1512–1533. A. Labarca (Eds.), Research in second language learning: Focus on the
Smith, J., & Ross, H. (2007). Training parents to mediate sibling classroom (pp. 3–16). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
disputes affects children’s negotiation and conflict understanding. Snow, C. E. (1990). Rationales for native language instruction:
Child Development, 78, 790–805. Evidence from research. In A. Padilla, H. Fairchild, & C. Valadez
Smith, L. B., & Samuelson, L. K. (2003). Different is good: (Eds.), Bilingual education: Issues and strategies (pp. 60–74).
Connectionism and dynamic systems theory are complementary Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
emergentist approaches to development. Developmental Science, 6, Snow, C. E. (2000). Comment on “The federal bilingual education
434–439. program” by Christine H. Rossell. In D. Ravitch (Ed.), Brookings
Smith, N., & Tsimpli, I. M. (1995). The mind of a savant: Language papers on education policy (pp. 244–255). Washington, DC:
learning and modularity. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Brookings Institution Press.
REFERENCES R-117
Snow, C. E., & Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.). (1977). Talking to children: Somerville, L. H., Hare, T., & Casey, B. J. (2011). Frontostriatal
Language input and acquisition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge maturation predicts cognitive control failure to appetitive cues in
University Press. adolescents. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 2123–2134.
Snow, R. E. (1989). Aptitude-treatment interaction as a framework Song, K., Hu, X., Olney, A., & Graesser, A. C. (2004). A frame-
for research on individual differences in learning. In P. Ackerman, work of synthesizing tutoring conversation capability with web
R. J. Sternberg, & R. Glaser (Eds.), Learning and individual dif- based distance education courseware. Computers & Education, 42,
ferences: Advances in theory and research (pp. 13–59). New York: 375–388.
W. H. Freeman/Times Books/Henry Holt & Co. Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Brandeis, D., Cortese, S., Daley, D., Ferrin, M.,
Snow, R. E. (1992). Aptitude theory: Yesterday, today, and tomorrow. Holtmann, M., Stevenson, J., Danckaerts, M., van der Oord, S.,
Educational Psychologist, 27, 5–32. Döpfner, M., Dittmann, R. W., Simonoff, E., Zuddas, A.,
Snowling, M. J. (2001). From language to reading and dyslexia. Banaschewski, T., Buitelaar, J., Coghill, D., Hollis, C., Konofal, E.,
Dyslexia, 7, 37–46. Lecendreux, M., Wong, I. C., Sergeant J., & the European ADHD
Snyder, M., Berscheid, E., & Matwychuk, A. (1988). Orientations Guidelines Group. (2013). Nonpharmacological interventions for
toward personnel selection: Differential reliance on appearance ADHD: Systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized con-
and personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, trolled trials of dietary and psychological treatments. American
972–979. Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 275–289.
Sobel, D. M., & Kirkham, N. Z. (2006). Blickets and babies: Sood, E. D., & Kendall, P. C. (2007). Assessing anxious self-talk in
The development of causal reasoning in toddlers and infants. youth: The Negative Affectivity Self-Statement Questionnaire–
Developmental Psychology, 42, 1103–1115. Anxiety Scale. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31, 603–618.
Sobel, D. M., & Kirkham, N. Z. (2007). Bayes nets and babies: Sophian, C., Wood, A. M., & Vong, K. I. (1995). Making num-
Infants’ developing statistical reasoning abilities and their rep- bers count: The early development of numerical inferences.
resentation of causal knowledge. Developmental Science, 10, Developmental Psychology, 31, 263–273.
298–306. Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985).
Sober, E., & Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto others: The evolution and psy- Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior
chology of unselfish behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21, 195–200.
Press. Sourander, A., Brunstein Klomek, A., Ikonen, M., Lindroos, J.,
Sochting, I., & March, J. S. (2002). Cognitive aspects of obsessive Luntamo, T., Koskelainen, M., Ristkari, T., & Helenius, H.
compulsive disorder in children. In R. O. Frost & G. Steketee (2010). Psychosocial risk factors associated with cyberbullying
(Eds.), Cognitive approaches to obsessions and compulsions: Theory, among adolescents: A population-based study. Archives of General
assessment, and treatment (pp. 299–314). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Psychiatry, 67, 720–728.
Pergamon/Elsevier Science. Soussignan, R. (2002). Duchenne smile, emotional experience, and
Sodian, B. (1991). The development of deception in young children. autonomic reactivity: A test of the facial feedback hypothesis.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 173–188. Emotion, 2, 52–74.
Sodian, B. (2011). Theory of mind in infancy. Child Development South, S. C., Krueger, R. F., Johnson, W., & Iacono, W. G. (2008).
Perspectives, 5, 39–43. Adolescent personality moderates genetic and environmental
Sodian, B., Schneider, W., & Perlmutter, M. (1986). Recall, cluster- influences on relationships with parents. Journal of Personality and
ing, and metamemory in young children. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 94, 899–912.
Child Psychology, 41, 395–410. Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Chorpita, B. F. (2007). Anxiety in chil-
Sodian, B., & Thoermer, C. (2008). Precursors to a theory of mind in dren and adolescents. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.),
infancy: Perspectives for research on autism. Quarterly Journal of Assessment of childhood disorders (4th ed., pp. 347–397). New
Experimental Psychology, 61, 27–39. York: Guilford Press.
Sodian, B., Zaitchik, D., & Carey, S. (1991). Young children’s differ- Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Kendall, P. C. (2000). A preliminary
entiation of hypothetical beliefs from evidence. Child Development, study of the emotion understanding of youths referred for treat-
62, 753–766. ment of anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29,
Sokolov, A. N. (1972). Inner speech and thought. New York: Plenum 319–327.
Press. Southgate, V., Johnson, M. H., Karoui, I. E., & Csibra, G. (2010).
Sokolov, Y. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex (S. W. Motor system activation reveals infants’ on-line prediction of oth-
Waydenfeld, Trans.). New York: Macmillan (originally published ers’ goals. Psychological Science, 21, 355–359.
1958). Southgate, V., Johnson M. H., Osborne T., & Csibra G. (2009).
Soley, G., & Hannon, E. E. (2010). Infants prefer the musical meter Predictive motor activation during action observation in human
of their own culture: A cross-cultural comparison. Developmental infants. Biology Letters, 5, 769–772.
Psychology, 46, 286–292. Sovrano, V. A., Bisazza, A., & Vallortigara, G. (2002). Modularity
Solomon, J., & George, C. (Eds.). (1999). Attachment disorganization. and spatial reorientation in a simple mind: Encoding of geomet-
New York: Guilford Press. ric and nongeometric properties of a spatial environment by fish.
Solomon, R. L. (1964). Punishment. American Psychologist, 19, Cognition, 85, B51–B59.
239–253. Sovrano, V. A., Bisazza, A., & Vallortigara, G. (2007). How fish do
Solomon, R. L., Turner, L. H., & Lessac, M. S. (1968). Some effects geometry in large and in small spaces. Animal Cognition, 10, 47–54.
of delay of punishment on resistance to temptation in dogs. Journal Sowell, E. R., Johnson, A., Kan, E., Lu, L. H., Van Horn, J. D., Toga,
of Personality and Social Psychology, 8, 233–238. A.W., O’Connor, M. J., & Bookheimer, S.Y. (2008). Mapping
Somerville, L. H., & Casey, B. J. (2010). Developmental neurobiology white matter integrity and neurobehavioral correlates in children
of cognitive control and motivational systems. Current Opinion in with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Journal of Neuroscience, 28,
Neurobiology, 20, 236–241. 1313–1319.
R-118 REFERENCES
Sowell, E. R., Peterson, B. S., Kan, E., Woods, R. P., Yoshii, J., Bansal, R., Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought
Xu, D., Zhu, H., Thompson, P. M., & Toga, A. W. (2007). Sex (pp. 277–311). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
differences in cortical thickness mapped in 176 healthy indi- Spelke, E. S. (2005). Sex differences in intrinsic aptitude for mathemat-
viduals between 7 and 87 years of age. Cerebral Cortex, 17, ics and science? A critical review. American Psychologist, 60, 950–958.
1550–1560. Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., & Jacobson, K. (1992).
Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Holmes, C. J., Jernigan, T. L., & Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605–632.
Toga, A. W. (1999). In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain Spelke, E. S., & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental
maturation in frontal and striatal regions. Nature Neuroscience, 2, Science, 10, 89–96.
859–861. Spelke, E. S., & Lee, S. A. (2012). Core systems of geometry in animal
Spaccarelli, S. (1994). Stress, appraisal, and coping in child sexual minds. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological
abuse: A theoretical and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, Sciences, 367, 2784–2793.
116, 340–362. Spelke, E. S., Lee, S. A., & Izard, V. (2010). Beyond core knowledge:
Spadoni, A. D., McGee, C. L., Fryer, S. L., &. Riley, E. P. (2007). Natural geometry. Cognitive Science, 34, 863–884.
Neuroimaging and fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Neuroscience & Spelke, E. S., & Newport, E. L. (1998). Nativism, empiricism, and
Biobehavioral Reviews, 31, 239–245. the development of knowledge. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook
Spaepen, L., Coppola, M., Spelke, E. S., Carey, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. of child psychology, Vol. 1: Theoretical models of human development
(2011). Number without a language model. Proceedings of the (5th ed.). New York: Wiley.
National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 108, 3163–3168. Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A. T., & Woodward, A. L. (1995). Infants’
Spagnoli, F. M., & Hemmati-Brivanlou, A. (2008). Guiding embry- knowledge of object motion and human action. In A. J. Premack,
onic stem cells towards differentiation: Lessons from molecular D. Premack, & D. Sperber (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisci-
embryology. Current Opinion in Genetics and Development, 16, plinary debate (pp. 44–77). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.
469–475. Spelke, E. S., & Tsivkin, S. (2001a). Initial knowledge and con-
Spangler, D. L. (1999). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for buli- ceptual change: Space and number. In M. Bowerman & S. C.
mia nervosa: An illustration. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, Levinson (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development
699–713. (pp. 70–97). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Spanos, N. P. (1994). Multiple identity enactments and multiple Spelke, E. S., & Tsivkin, S. (2001b). Language and number: A bilin-
personality disorder: A sociocognitive perspective. Psychological gual training study. Cognition, 78, 45–88.
Bulletin, 116, 143–165. Spelt, D. K. (1948). The conditioning of the human fetus in utero.
Sparrow, B., Liu, J., & Wegner, D. M. (2011). Google effects on mem- Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 338–346.
ory: Cognitive consequences of having information at our finger- Spencer, J. P., Perone, S., & Buss, A. T. (2011). Twenty years and
tips. Science, 333, 776–778. going strong: A dynamic systems revolution in motor and cogni-
Sparrow, R. (2010). Implants and ethnocide: Learning from the tive development. Child Development Perspectives, 5, 260–266.
cochlear implant controversy. Disability & Society, 25, 455–466. Spencer, M. B., & Markstrom-Adams, C. (1990). Identity processes
Spearman, C. (1904). “General intelligence” objectively determined among racial and ethnic minority children in America. Child
and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201–293. Development, 61, 290–310.
Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man: Their nature and measure- Spencer, T. J., Biederman, J., Harding, M., O’Donnell, D., Faraone, S., &
ment. New York: Macmillan. Wilens, T. (1996). Growth deficits in ADHD revisited: Evidence
Speidel, G. E., & Nelson, K. E. (1989). The many faces of imitation in for disorders-associated growth delays? Journal of the American
language learning. New York: Springer-Verlag. Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 1460–1469.
Spelke, E. (1991). Physical knowledge in infancy: Reflections on Spencer, T. J., Biederman, J., Wilens, T., Harding, M., O’Donnell, D., &
Piaget’s theory. In S. Carey & R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of Griffin, S. (1996). Pharmacotherapy of attention-deficit hyperactiv-
mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 133–169). Hillsdale, NJ: ity disorder across the life cycle. Journal of the American Academy of
Erlbaum. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 409–432.
Spelke, E. S. (1987). The development of intermodal perception. In Sperling, L., & Tabor, A. (2001). Twin pregnancy: The role of
P. Salapatek & L. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of infant perception: ultrasound in management. Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica
Vol. 2. From perception to cognition (pp. 233–273). New York: Scandinavica, 80, 287–299.
Academic Press. Spinelli, D. N., Hirsch, H. V. B., Phelps, R. W., & Metzler, J. (1972).
Spelke, E. S. (1988a). The origins of physical knowledge. In L. Visual experience as a determinant of the response characteristics
Weiskrantz (Ed.), Thought without language (pp. 168–184). of cortical receptive fields in cats. Experimental Brain Research, 15,
Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. 289–304.
Spelke, E. S. (1988b). Where perceiving ends and thinking begins: The Spinelli, S., Chefer, S., Carson, R. E., Jagoda, E., Lang, L., Heilig, M.,
apprehension of objects in infancy. In A. Yonas (Ed.), Perceptual Barr, C. S., Suomi, S. J., Higley, J. D., & Stein, E. A.(2010). Effects
development in infancy. Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology of early-life stress on serotonin (1A) receptors in juvenile rhesus
(Vol. 20). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. monkeys measured by positron emission tomography. Biological
Spelke, E. S. (1990). Principles of object perception. Cognitive Science, Psychiatry, 67, 1146–1153.
14, 29–56. Spirduso, W. W. (1980). Physical fitness, aging, and psychomotor
Spelke, E. S. (2002). Developing knowledge of space: Core systems speed: A review. Journal of Gerontology, 35, 850–865.
and new combinations. In A. M. Galaburda, S. M. Kosslyn, & Spitz, R. (1945). Hospitalism: An inquiry into the genesis of psychiat-
Y. Christen (Eds.), The languages of the brain. Cambridge, MA: ric conditions in early childhood. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child,
Harvard University Press. 1, 53–74.
Spelke, E. S. (2003). What makes us smart? Core knowledge and Spitz, R. (1946). Anaclitic depression. Psychoanalytic Study of the
natural language. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Child, 2, 313–342.
REFERENCES R-119
Spitzer, R. L., Devlin, M. J., Walsh, B. T., Hasin, D., Wing, R. R., Social Policy Report: Society for Research on Child Development,
Marcus, M. D., Stunkard, A., Wadden, T. A., Agras, W. S., 12, 1–28.
Mitchell, J., & Nonas, C. (1992). Binge eating disorder: A multisite Stack, C. B. (1986). The culture of gender: Women and men of color.
field trial for the diagnostic criteria. International Journal of Eating Signs, 11, 321–324.
Disorders, 11, 191–203. Stack, C. B. (1990). Different voices, different visions: Gender, cul-
Spitzer, R. L., Yanovski, S., Wadden, T., Wing, R., Marcus, M. D., ture, and moral reasoning. In F. Ginsburg & A. L. Tsing (Eds.),
Stunkard, A., Devlin, M., Mitchell, J., Hasin, D., & Horne, R. L. Uncertain terms: Negotiating gender in American culture (pp. 19–27).
(1993). Binge eating disorder: Its further validation in a multisite Boston: Beacon Press.
study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 13, 137–153. Stanescu-Cosson, R., Pinel, P., van de Moortele, P., Le Bihan, D.,
Spock B. (1957). The common sense book of baby and child care. New Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2000). Understanding dissociations in
York: Pocket Books. dyscalculia: A brain imaging study of the impact of number size
Spoth, R., Redmond, C., & Shin, C. (2001). Randomized trial of brief on the cerebral networks for exact and approximate calculation.
family interventions for general populations: Adolescent substance Brain, 123, 2240–2255.
use outcomes four years following baseline. Journal of Consulting Starkey, P. (1992). The early development of numerical reasoning.
and Clinical Psychology, 69, 627–642. Cognition, 43, 93–126.
Sprengelmeyer, R., Perrett, D. I., Fagan, E. C., Cornwell, R. E., Starkey, P., & Cooper, R. G. (1980). Perception of numbers by human
Lobmaier, J. S., Sprengelmeyer, A., Aasheim, H. B. M., Block, I. M., infants. Science, 210, 1033–1035.
Cameron, L. M., Crow, S., Milne, N., Rhodes, E. C., & Young, Starkey, P., Spelke, E. S., & Gelman, R. (1983). Detection of intermodal
A. W. (2009). The cutest little baby face: A hormonal link to sensi- numerical correspondences by human infants. Science, 222, 179–181.
tivity to cuteness in infant faces. Psychological Science, 20, 149–154. Starkey, P., Spelke, E. S., & Gelman, R. (1990). Numerical abstrac-
Springer, K., & Keil, F. C. (1989). On the development of biologi- tion by human infants. Cognition, 36, 97–128.
cally specific beliefs: The case of inheritance. Child Development, Starr, L. R., & Davila, J. (2009). Clarifying co-rumination: Association
60, 637–648. with internalizing symptoms and romantic involvement among
Squire, L. R. (1987). Memory and brain. New York: Oxford University adolescent girls. Journal of Adolescence, 32, 19–37.
Press. State, M. W., & Levitt, P. (2011). The conundrums of understanding
Squire, L. R. (1994). Declarative and nondeclarative memory: genetic risks for autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience,
Multiple brain systems supporting learning and memory. In D. L. 14, 1499–1506.
Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems (pp. 203–231). Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52,
Srivastava, S., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2003). 613–629.
Development of personality in early and middle adulthood: Set Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellec-
like plaster or persistent change? Journal of Personality and Social tual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality
Psychology, 84, 1041–1053. and Social Psychology, 69, 797–781.
Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Attachment and the roots of competence. Human Steele, K. M., Bass, K. E., & Crook, M. D. (1999). The mystery of
Nature, 1, 50–57. the Mozart effect: Failure to replicate. Psychological Science, 10,
Sroufe, L. A. (1985). Attachment classification from the perspective 366–369.
of infant-caregiver relationships and infant temperament. Child Steffensson, B., Larsson, J., Fried, I., El-Sayed, E., Rydelius, P., &
Development, 56, 1–14. Lichtenstein, P. (1999). Genetic disposition for global maturity:
Sroufe, L. A. (2002). From infant attachment to promotion of adoles- An explanation for genetic effects on parental report on ADHD.
cent autonomy: Prospective, longitudinal data on the role of parents International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 357–374.
in development. In. J. G. Borkowski, S. L. Ramey, & M. Bristol- Stegenga, B. T., King, M., Grobbee, D. E., Torres-González, F.,
Power (Eds.), Parenting and the child’s world: Influences on aca- Švab, I., Maaroos, H. I., Xavier, M., Saldivia, S., Bottomley, C.,
demic, intellectual, and social-emotional development (pp. 187–202). Nazareth, I., & Geerlings, M. I. (2012). Differential impact of risk
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. factors for women and men on the risk of major depressive disor-
Sroufe, L. A., Coffino, B., & Carlson, E. A. (2010). Conceptualizing der. Annals of Epidemiology, 22, 388–396.
the role of early experience: Lessons from the Minnesota Stein, N. L. (1988). The development of children’s storytelling skill.
Longitudinal Study. Developmental Review, 30, 36–51. In M. Franklin & S. Barton (Eds.), Child language: A reader
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Carlson, E. A. (1999). One social world: (pp. 282–297). New York: Oxford University Press.
The integrated development of parent-child and peer relationships. Stein, N. L., & Trabasso, T. (1982). Children’s understanding of sto-
In W. A. Collins & B. Laursen (Eds.), The Minnesota Symposia ries: A basis for moral judgment and dilemma resolution. In C. J.
on Child Psychology: Vol. 30. Relationships as developmental contexts Brainerd & M. Pressley (Eds.), Verbal processes in children (Vol. 2,
(pp. 241–261). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 161–188). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1982). Issues of temperament and attach- Stein, Z. A., Susser, M., Saenger, G., & Marolla, F. (1975). Famine
ment. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 52, 743–746. and human development: The Dutch hunger winter of 1944/45. New
St. Johnston, D., & Nusslein-Volhard, C. (1992). The origin of pat- York: Oxford University Press.
tern and polarity in the Drosophila embryo. Cell, 68, 201–219. Steinbeis, N., & Singer, T. (2013). The effects of social comparison
St. Petersburg–USA Orphanage Research Team. (2008). The effects on social emotions and behavior during childhood: The ontogeny
of early social-emotional and relationship experience on the devel- of envy and Schadenfreude predicts developmental changes in
opment of young orphanage children. Monographs of the Society for equity-related decisions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
Research in Child Development, 73, vii–295. 115, 198–209.
St. Pierre, R. G., & Layzer, J. I. (1998). Improving the life chances Steinberg, L. (1990). Autonomy, conflict, and harmony in the family
of children in poverty: Assumptions and what we have learned. relationship. In S. Feldman & G. Elliot (Eds.), At the threshold: The
R-120 REFERENCES
developing adolescent (pp. 255–276). Cambridge, MA: Harvard Sternberg, R. J., & Zhang, L. F. (2005). Styles of thinking as a basis of
University Press. differentiated instruction. Theory into Practice, 44, 245–253.
Steinberg, L. (2005). Psychological control: Style or substance? In Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., & Bundy, D. A. (2001). The pre-
J. Smetana (Ed.), Changing boundaries of parental authority dur- dictive value of IQ. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47, 1–41.
ing adolescence: New directions for child and adolescent development Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., & Singer, J. L. (2004). Creativity:
(Serial No.108, pp 71–78). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. From potential to realization. Washington, DC: American
Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk taking in adolescence: New perspec- Psychological Association.
tives from brain and behavioral science. Current Directions in Sternberg, R. J., Nokes, K., Geissler, P. W., Prince, R., Okatcha, F.,
Psychological Science, 16, 55–59. Bundy, D. A., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2001). The relationship
Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent between academic and practical intelligence: A case study in Kenya.
risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28, 78–106. Intelligence, 29, 401–418.
Steinberg, L. (2010). A dual-systems model of adolescent risk-taking. Sternberg, R. J., & Wagner, R. K. (1993). The g-ocentric view of
Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 216–224. intelligence and job performance is wrong. Current Directions in
Steinberg, L., Elmen, J., & Mounts, N. (1989). Authoritative parent- Psychological Science, 2, 1–5.
ing, psychosocial maturity, and academic success among adoles- Sternglanz S. H., Gray, J. L., & Murakami, M. (1977). Adult prefer-
cents. Child Development, 60, 1424–1436. ences for infantile facial features: An ethological approach. Animal
Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S. D., Darling, N., Mounts, N. S., & Behaviour, 25, 108–115.
Dornbusch, S. M. (1994). Over-time changes in adjustment and Stevenson, J. (1992). Evidence for a genetic etiology in hyperactivity in
competence among adolescents from authoritative, authoritarian, children. Behavior Genetics, 22, 337–344.
indulgent, and neglectful families. Child Development, 65, 754–770. Stewart, A., & Ostrove, J. (1998). Women’s personality in middle age:
Steiner, J. (1979). Human facial expressions in response to taste and Gender, history, and midcourse corrections. American Psychologist,
smell stimulation. In H. W. Reese & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances 53, 1185–1194.
in child development and behavior (Vol. 13, pp. 257–295). New Stewart, S. E., Rosario, M. C., Brown, T. A., Carter, A. S., Leckman,
York: Academic Press. J. F., Sukhodolsky, D., Katsovitch, L., King, R., Geller, D., &
Steiner, J. E. (1973). The gustofacial response: Observation on normal Pauls, D. L. (2007). Principal components analysis of obsessive-
and anencephalic newborn infants. Symposium on Oral Sensation compulsive disorder symptoms in children and adolescents.
and Perception, 4, 254–278. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 285–291.
Steiner, J. E. (1979) Human facial expressions in response to taste and Stice, E., Burton, E. M., & Shaw, H. (2004a). Prospective relations
smell stimulation. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, between bulimic pathology, depression, and substance abuse:
13, 257–295. Unpacking comorbidity in adolescent girls. Journal of Consulting
Steiner, J. E. (1979). Oral and facial innate motor responses to gusta- and Clinical Psychology, 72, 62–71.
tory and some olfactory stimuli. In J. H. A. Kroese (Ed.), Preference, Stice, E., Fisher, M., & Martinez, E. (2004b). Eating disorder
behavior, and chemoreception (pp. 247–262). London: IRL. diagnostic scale: Additional evidence of reliability and validity.
Steiner, J. E., Glaser, D., Hawilo, M. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2001). Psychological Assessment, 16, 60–71.
Comparative expression of hedonic impact: Affective reactions Stice, E., & Whitenton, K. (2002). Risk factors for body dissatisfac-
to taste by human infants and other primates. Neuroscience and tion in adolescent girls: A longitudinal investigation. Developmental
Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 53–74. Psychology, 38, 669–678.
Stephan, W. G. (1999). Improving intergroup relations in the schools. Stifter, C. A., & Braungart, J. M. (1995). The regulation of negative
New York: Columbia Teachers College. reactivity in infancy: Function and development. Developmental
Sterelny, K., & Griffiths, P. E. (1999). Sex and death: An introduction Psychology, 31, 448–455.
to philosophy of biology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Stigler, J. W. (1984). “Mental abacus”: The effect of abacus training
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view on Chinese children’s mental calculation. Cognitive Psychology, 16,
from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic 145–176.
Books. Stigler, J. W., Gallimore, R., & Hiebert, J. (2000). Using video surveys to
Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2015–2028. compare classrooms and teaching across cultures: Examples and lessons
Sternberg, R. J. (1982). Handbook of human intelligence. New York: from the TIMSS video studies. Educational Psychologist, 35, 87–100.
Cambridge University Press. Stigler, J. W., Lee, S. Y., & Stevenson, H. W. (1987). Mathematics
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intel- classrooms in Japan, Taiwan, and the United States. Child
ligence. Cambridge: University of Cambridge Press. Development, 58, 1272–1285.
Sternberg, R. J. (1988). The triarchic mind: A new theory of human Stiles, J., & Thal, D. (1993). Linguistic and spatial cognitive devel-
intelligence. New York: Penguin. opment following early focal brain injury: Patterns of deficit and
Sternberg, R. J. (1996). Successful intelligence. New York: Simon & recovery. In M. H. Johnson (Ed.), Brain development and cognition:
Schuster. A reader (pp. 643–664). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.). (1994). Encyclopedia of human intelligence. New Stinson, D. A., Logel, C., Zanna, M. P., Holmes, J. G., Cameron,
York: Macmillan. J. J., Wood, J. V., & Spencer, S. J. (2008). The cost of lower self-
Sternberg, R. J., & Detterman, D. (1986). What is intelligence? esteem: Testing a self-and-social-bonds model of health. Journal of
Contemporary viewpoints on its nature and definition. Norwood, Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 412–428.
NJ: Ablex. Stoffregen, T. A. (1986). The role of optical velocity in the control of
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. (Eds.). (1997). Intelligence, heredity, stance. Perception and Psychophysics, 39, 355–360.
and environment. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Stoffregen, T. A., Schmuckler, M. A., & Gibson, E. J. (1987). Use of
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. (Eds.). (2002). The general factor of central and peripheral optical flow in stance and locomotion in
intelligence: How general is it? Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. young walkers. Perception, 16, 113–119.
REFERENCES R-121
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe R. K. Larson, V. Déprez, & H. Yamakido (Eds.), The evolution
contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, of language: Biolinguistic perspectives (pp. 176–193). New York:
10, 640–656. Cambridge University Press.
Stone, V. E., & Gerrans, P. (2006). What’s domain-specific about Stromswold, K. (1997). Specific language impairments. In T. E.
theory of mind? Social Neuroscience, 1, 309–319. Feinberg & M. J. Farah (Eds), Behavioral neurology and neuropsy-
Storey, A. E., Walsh, C. J., Quinton, R. L., & Wynne-Edwards, K. E. chology (pp. 755–772). New York: McGraw-Hill.
(2000). Hormonal correlates of paternal responsiveness in new and Stromswold, K. (2001). The heritability of language: A review and
expectant fathers. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 79–95. meta-analysis of twin, adoption, and linkage studies. Language, 77,
Stotz, K., Griffiths, P. E., & Knight, R. (2004). How scientists concep- 647–723.
tualize genes: An empirical study. Studies in History and Philosophy Strough, J., & Covatto, A. M. (2002). Context and age differences in
of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 35, 647–673. same- and other-gender peer preferences. Social Development, 11,
Stover, P. J., & Garza, C. (2006). Nutrition and developmental 346–361.
biology—Implications for public health. Nutrition Reviews, 64, Stuart, J. J., Brown, S. J., Beeman, R. W., & Denell, R. E. (1991).
S60–S71; discussion S72–S91. A deficiency of the homeotic complex of the beetle Tribolium.
Strachan, T., & Read, A. P. (1999). Human molecular genetics (2nd Nature, 350, 72–74.
ed.). New York: Wiley-Liss. Stucki, M., Kaufmann-Hayoz, R., & Kaufmann, F. (1987). Infants’
Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitat- recognition of a face revealed through motion: Contribution
ing conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial of internal facial movement and head movement. Journal of
feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Experimental Child Psychology, 44, 80–91.
54, 768–777. Subrahmanyam, K., & Greenfield, P. (2008). Online communication
Strand, S., Deary, I., & Smith, P. (2006). Sex differences in cogni- and adolescent relationships. Future of the Child, 18, 119–146.
tive ability test scores: A UK national picture. British Journal of Suddendorf, T., Simcock, G., & Nielsen, M. (2007). Visual self-
Educational Psychology, 76, 463–480. recognition in mirrors and live videos: Evidence for a developmen-
Strang, N. M., Hanson, J. L., & Pollak, S. D. (2012). The importance tal asynchrony. Cognitive Development, 22, 185–196.
of biological methods in linking social experience with social and Sugarman, S. (1983). Children’s early thought: Developments in clas-
emotional development. Monographs of the Society for Research in sification. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Child Development, 77, 61–66. Sugita Y. (2009). Innate face processing. Current Opinion in
Strasburger, V. C., & Donnerstein, E. (1999). Children, adolescents, Neurobiology, 19, 39–44.
and the media: Issues and solutions. Pediatrics, 103, 129–139. Sugita, Y. (2008). Face perception in monkeys reared with no expo-
Strathearn, L., Iyengar, U., Fonagy, P., & Kim, S. (2012). Maternal sure to faces. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
oxytocin response during mother-infant interaction: Associations USA, 105, 394–398.
with adult temperament. Hormones and Behavior, 61, 429–435. Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York:
Strathearn, L., Li, J., Fonagy, P., & Montague, R. (2008). What’s in W. W. Norton.
a smile? Maternal brain responses to infant facial cues. Pediatrics, Sullivan, J. W., & Horowitz, F. D. (1983). Infant intermodal per-
122, 40–51. ception and maternal multimodal stimulation: Implications for
Strauss, S. (Ed.). (1982). U-shaped behavioral growth. New York: language development. In L. P. Lipsitt (Ed.), Advances in infancy
Academic Press. research (Vol. 2, pp. 183–239). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Strayer, F. F., & Santos, A. J. (1996). Affiliative structures in preschool Sullivan, K. (2000). The anti-bullying handbook. Oxford, UK: Oxford
peer groups. Social Development, 5, 117–130. University Press.
Streit, A., & Stern, C. D. (1999). Neural induction, a bird’s eye view. Sullivan, K., Clark, J., Castrucci, B., Samsel, R., Fonseca, V., &
Trends in Genetics, 15, 20–24. Garcia, I. (2011). Continuing education mitigates the negative
Streri, A., & Gentaz, E. (2003). Cross-modal recognition of shape consequences of adolescent childbearing. Maternal and Child
from hand to eyes in human newborns. Somatosensory and Motor Health Journal, 15, 360–366.
Research, 20, 13–18. Sullivan, K., Winner, E., & Hopfield, N. (1995). How children tell a
Strevens, M. (2009). Depth: An account of scientific explanation. lie from a joke: The role of second-order mental state attributions.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 191–204.
Striano, T., & Liszkowksi, U. (2005). Sensitivity to the context of Sullivan, M. W., & Lewis, M. (2003). Emotional expressions of young
facial expression in the still face at 3-, 6-, and 9-months of age. infants and children: A practitioner’s primer. Infants and Young
Infant Behavior and Development, 28, 10–19. Children, 16, 120–142.
Striano, T., & Stahl, D. (2005). Sensitivity to triadic attention in early Sullivan, P. F. (1995). Mortality in anorexia nervosa. American Journal
infancy. Developmental Science, 8, 333–343. of Psychiatry, 152, 1073–1074.
Strober, M. (1991). Family-genetic studies of eating disorders. Journal Sulloway, F. J. (1996). Born to rebel: Birth order, family dynamics, and
of Clinical Psychiatry, 52, 9–12. creative lives. New York: Pantheon Books.
Strober, M., Lilenfeld, L. R., Kaye, W., & Bulik C. (2006). Genetic Sulloway, F., & Zweigenhaft, R. (2010). Birth order and risk taking
factors in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. In P. J. Cooper & in athletics: A meta-analysis and study of major league baseball.
A. Stein (Eds.), Feeding problems and eating disorders in children Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 402–416.
and adolescents (2nd ed., pp. 169–189). London: Routledge. Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Mayer, G. R. (1991). Behavior analysis for lasting
Stromswold, K. (1994). Language comprehension without language change. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich Publishers.
production: Implications for theories of language acquisition. Paper Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Mayer, G. R. (1977). Applying behavior analysis
presented at the 18th Annual Boston University Conference on procedures with children and youth. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and
Language Development. Winston.
Stromswold, K. (2010). Genetics and the evolution of language: Sun, F., & He, Z. (2010). Neuronal intrinsic barriers for axon regenera-
What genetic studies reveal about the evolution of language. In tion in the adult CNS. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20, 510–518.
R-122 REFERENCES
Sun, J., Liu, Y. H., Chen, H., Nguyen, M. P., Mishina, Y., biology: Concept, modeling, and application (pp. 339–360). Berlin
Upperman, J. S., Ford, H. R., & Shi, W. (2007). Deficient and Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.
Alk3-mediated BMP signaling causes prenatal omphalocele-like Sutter, N. B., & Ostrander, E. A. (2004) Dog star rising: The canine
defect. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications, genetic system. Nature Reviews Genetics, 5, 900–910.
360, 238–243. Sutton, J. E. (2006). The development of landmark and beacon
Sun, Y., & Li, Y. (2002). Children’s well-being during parents’ mari- use in young children: Evidence from a touchscreen search task.
tal disruption process: A pooled time-series analysis. Journal of Developmental Science, 9, 108–123.
Marriage and the Family, 64, 472–488. Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999a). Bullying and “the-
Sundet, J. M., Barlaug, D. G., & Torjussen, T. M. (2004). The end of ory of mind”: A critique of the “social skills deficit” view of anti-
the Flynn effect? A study of secular trends in mean intelligence test social behaviour. Social Development, 8, 117–127.
scores of Norwegian conscripts during half a century. Intelligence, Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999b). Social cognition
32, 349–362. and bullying: Social inadequacy or skilled manipulation? British
Suomi, S. J. (1973). Surrogate rehabilitation of monkeys reared in Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 435–450.
total social isolation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 14, Suveg, C., Zeman, J., Flannery-Schroeder, E., & Cassano, M. (2005).
71–77. Emotion socialization in families of children with an anxiety dis-
Suomi, S. J. (1979). Peers, play and primary prevention in primates. order. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 145–155.
In M. Kent & J. Rolf (Eds.), Primary prevention of psychopathol- Suzuki, K., Ando, J., & Satou, N. (2009). Genetic effects on infant
ogy: Social competence in children (pp. 127–149). Hanover, NH: handedness under spatial constraint conditions. Developmental
University Press of New England. Psychobiology, 51, 605–615.
Suomi, S. J. (1997). Early determinants of behaviour: Evidence from Svartberg, K., Tapper, I., Temrin, H., Radesäter, T., & Thorman, S.
primate studies. British Medical Bulletin, 53, 170–184. (2005). Consistency of personality traits in dogs. Animal Behaviour,
Suomi, S. J. (1999). Developmental trajectories, early experiences, 69, 283–291.
and community consequences: Lessons from studies with rhesus Swaab, D. F., & Hofman, M. A. (1995). Sexual differentiation of the
monkeys. In D. P. Keating & C. Hertzman (Eds.), Developmental human hypothalamus in relation to gender and sexual orientation.
health and the wealth of nations: Social, biological, and educational Trends in Neurosciences, 18, 264–270.
dynamics (pp. 185–200). New York: Guilford Press. Swain, I. U., Zelazo, P. R., & Clifton, R. K. (1993). Newborn infants’
Suomi, S. J. (2005). Mother-infant attachment, peer relationships, and memory for speech sounds retained over 24 hours. Developmental
the development of social networks in rhesus monkeys. Human Psychology, 29, 312–323.
Development, 48, 67–79. Swan, S. H., Liu, F., Hines, M., Kruse, R. L., Wang, C., Redmon,
Suomi, S. J., Collins, M. L., & Harlow, H. F. (1973). Effects of per- J. B., Sparks, A., & Weiss, B. (2010). Prenatal phthalate expo-
manent separation from mother on infant monkeys. Developmental sure and reduced masculine play in boys. International Journal of
Psychology, 9, 376–384. Andrology, 33, 259–269.
Suomi, S. J., Collins, M. L., Harlow, H. F., & Ruppenthal, G. C. Swann, C. A., & Sylvester, M. S. (2006). The foster care crisis: What
(1976). Effects of maternal and peer separations on young mon- caused caseloads to grow? Demography, 43, 309–335.
keys. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 101–112. Swann, W. B. Jr, Chang-Schneider, C., & Larsen McClarty, K. 2007.
Suomi, S. J., & Harlow, H. F. (1972). Social rehabilitation of isolate- Do people’s self-views matter? Self-concept and self-esteem in
reared monkeys. Developmental Psychology, 6, 487–496. everyday life. American Psychologist, 62, 84–94.
Suomi, S. J., Harlow, H. F., & Domek, C. J. (1970). Effect of repetitive Swanson, J. M., (1993). Effect of stimulant medication on hyperactive
infant-infant separation of young monkeys. Journal of Abnormal children: A “review of reviews.” Exceptional Child, 60, 154–162.
Psychology, 76, 161–172. Swanson, J. M., Castellanos, F., Murias, M., LaHoste, G., &
Suomi, S. J., Harlow, H. F., & Kimball, S. D. (1971). Behavioral Kennedy, J. (1998). Cognitive neuroscience of attention deficit
effects of prolonged partial social isolation in the rhesus monkey. hyperactivity disorder and hyperkinetic disorder. Current Opinion
Psychological Reports, 29, 1171–1177. in Neurobiology, 8, 263–271.
Suomi, S. J., Mineka, S., & Harlow, H. F. (1983). Social separation Swanson, J. M., Lerner, M., & Williams, L. (1995). More frequent
in monkeys as viewed from several motivational perspectives. In diagnosis of attention deficit- hyperactivity disorder. New England
E. Satinoff & P. Teitelbaum (Eds.), Handbook of behavioral neu- Journal of Medicine, 333, 944.
robiology: Vol. 6. Motivation (pp. 543–583). New York: Plenum Swanson, J. M., Sunohara, G. A., Kennedy, J. L., Regino, R.,
Press. Fineberg, E., Wigal, T., Lerner, M., Williams, L., LaHoste,
Super, C. M. (1976). Environmental effects on motor development: G. J., & Wigal, S. (1998). Association of the dopamine receptor D4
The case of African infant precocity. Developmental Medicine & (DRD4) gene with a refined phenotype of attention deficit hyper-
Child Neurology, 18, 561–567. activity disorder (ADHD): A family-based approach. Molecular
Super, C. M. (1983). Cultural variation in the meaning and uses Psychiatry, 3, 38–41.
of children’s intelligence. In J. B. Deregowski, S. Dziurawiec, & Swarr, A. E., & Richards, M. H. (1996). Longitudinal effects of ado-
R. C. Annis (Eds.), Expiscations in cross-cultural psychology lescent girls’ pubertal development, perceptions of pubertal tim-
(pp. 199–212). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. ing, and parental relations on eating problems. Developmental
Surani, M. A., Hayashi, K., & Hajkova, P. (2007). Genetic and epi- Psychology, 32, 636–646.
genetic regulators of pluripotency. Cell, 128, 747–762. Swearer, S. M., & Doll, B. (2001). Bullying in schools: An eco-
Surian, L., & Caldi, S. (2010). Infants’ individuation of agents and logical framework. In R. A. Geffner, M. Loring, & C. Young
inert objects. Developmental Science, 13, 143–150. (Eds.), Bullying behavior: Current issues, research and interventions
Surian, L., Caldi, S., & Sperber, D. (2007). Attribution of beliefs by (pp. 7–23). New York: Haworth.
13-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 18, 580–586. Swing, E. L., & Anderson, C. A. (2008). How and what do video
Sutphin, G. L., & Kennedy, B. K. (2009). Aging: Evolutionary theory games teach? In T. Willoughby & E. Wood (Eds.), Children’s
meets genomic approaches. In P. Pontarotti (Ed.), Evolutionary learning in a digital world (pp. 64–84). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
REFERENCES R-123
Swinney, D. (1999). Aphasia. In R. A. Wilson & F. C. Keil (Eds.), The Tamashiro, K. L., & Moran, T. H. (2010). Perinatal environment and
MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences (pp. 31–32). Cambridge, its influences on metabolic programming of offspring. Physiology &
MA: MIT Press. Behavior, 100, 560–566.
Sylvers, P. D., Brennan, P. A., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011). Psychopathic Tamim, R. M., Bernard, R. M., Borokhovski, E., Abrami, P. C., &
traits and preattentive threat processing in children: A novel test of Schmid, R. F. (2011). What forty years of research says about the
the fearlessness hypothesis. Psychological Science, 22, 1280–1287. impact of technology on learning: A second-order meta-analysis
Symons, D., & Moran, G. (1994). Responsiveness and dependency and validation study. Review of Educational Research, 81, 4–28.
are different aspects of social contingencies: An example from Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001).
mother and infant smiles. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language
209–214. milestones. Child Development, 72, 748–767.
Szechtman, H., & Woody, E. (2004). Obsessive-compulsive disorder Tang, M., Wang, Z., Qian, M., Gao, J., & Zhang, L. (2008).
as a disturbance of security motivation. Psychological Review, 111, Transferred shame in the cultures of interdependent self and inde-
111–127. pendent self. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8, 163–178.
Szyf, M., McGowan, P., & Meaney, M. J. (2008). The social envi- Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions
ronment and the epigenome. Environmental and Molecular and moral behavior. Annual Review, 58, 345–372.
Mutagenesis, 49, 46–60. Tannock, R. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Advances
in cognitive, neurobiological, and genetic research. Journal of Child
Tabacova, S. A., & Kimmel, C. A. (2001). Enalapril: Pharmacokinetic/ Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 65–99.
dynamic inferences for comparative developmental toxicity. A Tao, K., & Chiu, J. (1985). The one-child-per-family policy: A psy-
review. Reproductive Toxicology, 15, 467–479. chological perspective. In W. Tseng & D. Y. H. Wu (Eds.), Chinese
Tabin, C. J. (1991). Retinoids, homeoboxes, and growth factors: culture and mental health (pp.153–165). New York: Academic Press.
Toward molecular models for limb development. Cell, 66, 199–217. Tasker, F. (2005). Lesbian mothers, gay fathers and their children:
Tai, Y. F., Scherfler, C., Brooks, D. J., Sawamoto, N., & Castiello, U. A review. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 26,
(2004). The human premotor cortex is “mirror” only for biological 224–240.
actions. Current Biology, 14, 117–120. Taverna, A. S., Waxman, S. R., Medin, D. L., & Peralta, O. A. (2012).
Takac, M., Benuskova, L., & Knott, A. (2012). Mapping sensorimotor Core folkbiological concepts: New evidence from Wichi children
sequences to word sequences: A connectionist model of language and adults. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 12, 339–359.
acquisition and sentence generation. Cognition, 125, 288–308. Taylor, B., Miller, E., Farrington, C. P., Petropoulos, M. C., Favot-
Takahashi, T., Nowakowski, R. S., & Caviness, V. S., Jr. (1994). Mode Mayaud, I., Li, J., & Waight, P. A. (1999). Autism and measles,
of cell proliferation in the developing mouse neocortex. Proceedings mumps, and rubella vaccine: No epidemiological evidence for a
of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 91, 373–379. causal association. The Lancet, 353, 2026–2029.
Takano, Y. (1989). Methodological problems in cross-cultural studies Taylor, E. (1989). Prime time families: Television culture in postwar
of linguistic relativity. Cognition, 31, 141–162. America. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Taki, Y., Hashizume, H., Sassa, Y., Takeuchi, H., Asano, M., Asano, K., Taylor, J. L., Lindsay, W. R., & Willner, P. (2008). CBT for people
Kotozaki, Y., Nouchi, R., Wu, K., Fakuda, H., & Kawashima, R. with intellectual disabilities: Emerging evidence, cognitive abil-
(2012). Correlation among body height, intelligence, and ity and IQ effects. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36,
brain gray matter volume in healthy children. Neuroimage, 59, 723–733.
1023–1027. Taylor, J., Loney, B. R., Bobadilla, L., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M.
Talamillo, A., Bastida, M. F., Fernandez-Teran, M., & Ros, M. A. (2003). Genetic and environmental influence on psychopathy:
(2005). The developing limb and the control of the number of dig- Findings from an adolescent male twin cohort. Journal of Abnormal
its. Clinical Genetics, 67, 143–153. Child Psychology, 31, 633–645.
Talens R. P., Christensen K., Putter H., Willemsen G., Christiansen Taylor, L., Faraone, S. V., & Tsuang, M. T. (2002). Family, twin, and
L., Kremer D., Suchiman H. E., Slagboom P. E., Boomsma adoption studies of bipolar disease. Current Psychiatry Reports, 4,
D. I., & Heijmans B. T. (2012). Epigenetic variation during the 130–133.
adult lifespan: Cross-sectional and longitudinal data on monozy- Taylor, M. (1996). A theory of mind perspective on social cognitive
gotic twin pairs. Aging Cell, 11, 694–703. development. In R. Gelman & T. Au (Eds.), Handbook of percep-
Tallal, P. (2000). The science of literacy: From the laboratory to the tion and cognition: Vol. 13. Perceptual and cognitive development
classroom. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the (2nd ed., pp. 283–329). New York: Academic Press.
USA, 97, 2402–2404. Taylor, M. (1999). Imaginary companions and the children who create
Tallal, P., Miller, S. L., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, them. New York: Oxford University Press.
S. S., Schreiner, C., Jenkins, W. M., & Merzenich, M. M. (1996). Taylor, M., Carlson, S. M., & Gerow, L. (2001). Imaginary
Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children companions: Characteristics and correlates. In S. Reifel (Ed.), Play
improved with acoustically modified speech. Science, 271, 81–84. and culture studies: Theory in context and out (Vol. 3, pp.179–198).
Tallal, P., Miller, S., & Fitch, R. (1993). Neurobiological basis of Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing.
speech: A case for the preeminence of temporal processing. Annals Taylor, M., Carlson, S., Maring, B., Gerow, L., & Charley, C. (2004).
of the New York Academy of Sciences, 682, 27–47. The characteristics and correlates of fantasy in school-age children:
Tallal, P., Ross, R., & Curtiss, S. (1989). Familial aggregation in spe- Imaginary companions, impersonation, and social understanding.
cific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, Developmental Psychology, 40, 1173–1187.
54, 167–173. Taylor, M., Esbensen, B. M., & Bennett, R. T. (1994.) Children’s
Tallis R. C., & Fillit, H. M. (Eds.). (2003). Brocklehurst’s textbook understanding of knowledge acquisition: The tendency for chil-
of geriatric medicine and gerontology (6th ed.). London: Churchill dren to report they have always known what they have just learned.
Livingstone/Elsevier Science Limited. Child Development, 65, 1581–1604.
R-124 REFERENCES
Taylor, M. G., Rhodes, M., & Gelman, S. A. (2009). Boys will be Tentori, K., Osherson, D., Hasher, L., & May, C. (2001). Wisdom
boys; cows will be cows: Children’s essentialist reasoning about and aging: Irrational preferences in college students but not older
gender categories and animal species. Child Development, 80, adults. Cognition, 81, B87–B96.
461–481. Teoh, S. W., Pisoni, D. B., & Miyamoto, R. T. (2004a). Cochlear
Taylor, P., & Keeter, S. (2010). Millennials: A portrait of generation implantation in adults with prelingual deafness. Part I. Clinical
next: Confident, connected, open to change. Washington, DC: Pew results. Laryngoscope, 114, 1536–1540.
Research Center. Teoh, S. W., Pisoni, D. B., & Miyamoto, R. T. (2004b). Cochlear
Taylor, S. (2011). Early versus late onset obsessive-compulsive disor- implantation in adults with prelingual deafness. Part II. Underlying
der: Evidence for distinct subtypes. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, constraints that affect audiological outcomes. Laryngoscope. 114,
1083–1100. 1714–1719.
Taylor, S. (2013). Molecular genetics of obsessive–compulsive disor- Terman, L. M., & Oden, M. H. (1947). Genetic studies of genius:
der: A comprehensive meta-analysis of genetic association studies. Vol. 4. The gifted child grows up: Twenty-five years’ follow-up of a
Molecular Psychiatry, 18, 799–805. superior group. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Teasdale, T. W., & Owen, D. R. (2000). Forty-year secular trends in Terrace, H. S. (1983). Apes who “talk”: Language or projection of
cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 28, 115–120. language by their teachers? In J. de Luce & H. T. Wilder (Eds.),
Teasdale, T. W., & Owen, D. R. (2005). A long-term rise and recent Language in primates: Perspectives and implications (pp. 22–39).
decline in intelligence test performance: The Flynn effect in New York: Springer-Verlag.
reverse. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 837–843. Terrace, H. S., Petitto, L. A., Sanders, R. J., & Bever, T. G. (1979) Can
Teixeira, A. I., Duckworth, J. K., & Hermanson, O. (2007). Getting an ape create a sentence? Science, 206, 891–902.
the right stuff: Controlling neural stem cell state and fate in vivo Terwogt, M. M., & Harris, P. (1993). Understanding of emotion. In
and in vitro with biomaterials. Cell Research, 17, 56–61. M. Bennett (Ed.), The child as psychologist: An introduction to the
Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Jr, Wilcox, K. J., Segal, development of social cognition (pp. 62–86). London: Harvester-
N. L., & Rich, S. (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared Wheatsheaf.
apart and together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, Tesar, B., & Smolensky, P. (1998). Learnability in optimality theory.
1031–1039. Linguistic Inquiry, 29, 229–268.
Teller, D. Y. (1998). Spatial and temporal aspects of infant color vision. Tessier-Lavigne, M., & Goodman, C. S. (1996). The molecular biol-
Vision Research, 38, 3275–3282. ogy of axon guidance. Science, 274, 1123–1133.
Teller, D. Y., & Bornstein, M. H. (1986). Infant color vision and color Tessier-Lavigne, M., & Placzek, M. (1991). Target attraction:
perception. In P. Salapatek & L. B. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of Developing axons guided by chemotropism. Trends in Neuroscience,
infant perception (pp. 185–236). New York: Academic Press. 14, 303–310.
Teller, D. Y., & Movshon, J. A. (1986). Visual development. Vision Tessier-Lavigne, M., Placzek, M., Lumsden, A. G. S., Dodd, J., &
Research, 26, 1483–1506. [Graph in that paper is reprinted Jessell, T. M. (1988). Chemotropic guidance of developing axons
from Teller, D. Y., McDonald, M. A., Preston, K., et al. (1986). in the mammalian central nervous system. Nature, 336, 775–778.
Assessment of visual acuity in infants and adults: The acuity Thagard, P. (1990). Concepts and conceptual change. Synthèse, 82,
card procedure. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 28, 255–274.
779–789.] Thagard, P. (1992). Conceptual revolutions. Princeton: Princeton
Telzer, E. H. (2011). Expanding the acculturation gap-distress model: University Press.
An integrative review of research. Human Development, 53, 313–340. Thapar, A., Hervas, A., & McGuffin, P. (1995). Childhood hyper-
Templin, M. C. (1957). Certain language skills in children: Their activity scores are highly heritable and show sibling competition
development and interrelationships. Minneapolis: University of effects: Twin study evidence. Behavior Genetics, 25, 537–544.
Minnesota Press. Thapar, A., Holmes, J., Poulton, K., & Harrington, R. (1999).
Temrin, H., Buchmayer, S., & Enquist, M. (2000). Step-parents Genetic basis of attention deficit and hyperactivity. British Journal
and infanticide: New data contradict evolutionary predictions. of Psychiatry, 174, 105–111.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Thelen, E. (1995). Motor development: A new synthesis. American
Sciences, 267, 943–945. Psychologist, 50, 79–95.
Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2002). Are parents’ gender sche- Thelen, E. (2001). Dynamic mechanisms of change in early
mas related to their children’s gender-related cognitions? A meta- perceptual-motor development. In J. L McClelland & R. S. Siegler
analysis. Developmental Psychology, 38, 615–630. (Eds.), Mechanisms of cognitive development: Behavioral and neural
Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2003). Parent-child conversations perspectives (pp. 161–185). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
about science: The socialization of gender inequities? Developmental Thelen, E., & Fisher, D. M. (1982). Newborn stepping: An expla-
Psychology, 39, 34–47. nation for a “disappearing” reflex. Developmental Psychology, 18,
Tenenbaum, H. R., Snow, C. E., Roach, K. A., & Kurland, B. (2005). 760–775.
Talking and reading science: Longitudinal data on sex differences Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to
in mother-child conversations in low-income families. Journal of the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Applied Developmental Psychology, 26, 1–19. Press.
Teng, Y. D., Lavik, E. B., Qu, X., Ourednik, J., Zurakowski, D., Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1998). Dynamic systems theories. In
Langer, R., & Snyder, E. Y. (2002). Functional recovery follow- W. Damon (Series Ed.) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
ing traumatic spinal cord injury mediated by a unique polymer child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development
scaffold seeded with neural stem cells. Proceedings of the National (5th ed., pp. 563- 634). New York: Wiley.
Academy of Sciences of the USA, 99, 3024–3029. Thelen, E., Fisher, D. M., & Ridley-Johnson, R. (1984). The rela-
Tennenbaum, H. R. (2009). “You’d be good at that”: Gender patterns tionship between physical growth and a newborn reflex. Infant
in parent-child talk about courses. Social Development, 18, 447–463. Behavior & Development, 7, 479–493.
REFERENCES R-125
Thiessen E. D., Hill, E. A., & Saffran, J. R. (2005). Infant-directed Thompson, R. A. (1998). Early sociopersonality development. In
speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy, 7, 53–71. W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emo-
Thiessen, E. D., Kronstein, A. T., & Hufnagle, D. G. (2013). The tional and personality development (pp. 65–76). New York: Wiley.
extraction and integration framework: A two-process account of Thompson, R. A. (2001). Childhood anxiety disorders from the per-
statistical learning. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 792–814. spective of emotion regulation and attachment. In M. W. Vasey &
Thoenen, H. (1991). The changing scene of neurotrophic factors. M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of anxiety
Trends in Neurosciences, 14, 165–170. (pp. 160–182). New York: Oxford University Press.
Thoenen, H. (1995). Neurotrophins and synaptic plasticity. Science, Thompson, R. A. (2011). Emotion and emotion regulation: Two sides
270, 593–598. of the developing coin. Emotion Review, 3, 53–61.
Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New Thompson, R. A., & Calkins, S. D. (1996). The double-edged
York: Brunner/Mazel. sword: Emotion regulation in high risk children. Development and
Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1984). Genesis and evolution of behavioral Psychopathology, 8, 163–182.
disorders: From infancy to early adult life. American Journal of Thomsen, P. H. (1998). From thoughts to obsessions: Obsessive-
Psychiatry, 141, 1–9. compulsive disorders in children and adolescents. London: Jessica
Thomas, A., Chess, S., & Birch, H. (1970). The origins of personality. Kingsley Publishers.
Scientific American, 223, 102–109. Thomson, J. J. (1976). Killing, letting die, and the trolley problem.
Thomas, A., Chess, S., & Birch, H. G. (1968). Temperament and The Monist, 59, 204–217.
behavior disorders in children. New York: New York University Thorndike, E. L. (1920). Intelligence and its use. Harper’s Magazine,
Press. 140, 227–235.
Thomas, A., Chess, S., Birch, H. G., Hertzig, M. E., & Korn, S. Thorndike, E. L., Bregman, E. O., Cobb, M. V., & Woodyard, E.
(1963). Behavioral individuality in early childhood. New York: New (1927). The measurement of intelligence. New York: Bureau of
York University Press. Publications, Teachers College, Columbia University.
Thomas, F. C., Sheth, B., Eckert, J. J., Bazzoni, G., Dejana, E., & Thorndike, R. L. (1936). Factor analysis of social and abstract intel-
Fleming, T. P. (2004). Contribution of JAM-1 to epithelial differ- ligence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 27, 231–233.
entiation and tight-junction biogenesis in the mouse preimplanta- Thorndike, R. L., & Stern, S. (1937). An evaluation of the attempts
tion embryo. Journal of Cell Science, 117, 5599–5608. to measure social intelligence. Psychological Bulletin, 34, 275–284.
Thomas, G. V., Nye, R., Rowley, M., & Robinson, E. J. (2001). What Thornhill, R., & Moller, A. B. (1997) Developmental stability, dis-
is a picture? Children’s conceptions of pictures. British Journal of ease, and medicine. Biological Reviews, 72, 497–548.
Developmental Psychology, 19, 475–491. Thornton, L. M., Mazzeo, S. E., & Bulik, C. M. (2011). The herita-
Thomas, J., Garrison, M., Slawecki, C., Ehlers, C., & Riley, E. bility of eating disorders: Methods and current findings. Current
(2000). Nicotine exposure during the neonatal brain growth spurt Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, 6, 141–156.
produces hyperactivity in preweanling rats. Neurotoxicology and Thorson, H. L., & Simhan, H. N. (2010). Genetics of preterm birth.
Teratology, 22, 695–701. In V. Berghella (Ed.), Preterm birth: Prevention and management
Thomas, M. H., Horton, R. W., Lippincott, E. C., & Drabman, R. S. (pp. 39–49). Oxford: Blackwell.
(1977). Desensitization to portrayals of real-life aggression as a Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary mental abilities. Chicago: University
function of exposure to television violence. Journal of Personality of Chicago Press.
and Social Psychology, 35, 450–458. Times of London. (2005).Grammar lessons: Northern Ireland should
Thomas, M. S. C., & Johnson, M. H. (2008). New advances in keep its academic selection. July 26.
understanding sensitive periods in brain development. Current Timney, B., & Keil, K. (1996) Horses are sensitive to pictorial depth
Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 1–5. cues. Perception, 25, 1121–1128.
Thomas, M., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2005). Can developmental dis- Tinbergen, N. (1951). The study of instinct. New York: Oxford
orders reveal the component parts of the human language faculty? University Press.
Language Learning and Development, 1, 65–92. Tincoff, R., Hauser, M., Tsao, F., Spaepen, G., Ramus, F., & Mehler, J.
Thompson, L. (1997). The development of pragmatic competence: (2005). The role of speech rhythm in language discrimination:
Past findings and future directions for research. In L. Thompson Further tests with a non-human primate. Developmental Science,
(Ed.), Children talking: The development of pragmatic competence 8, 26–35.
(pp. 3–21). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. Tinker v. Des Moines Independent Community School District, 393
Thompson, P. M., Vidal, C., Giedd, J. N., Gochman, P., Blumenthal, U.S. 503 (1969).
J., Nicholson, R., Toga, A. W., & Rapoport, J. L. (2001). Mapping Tizard, B. T. (1977). Adoption: A second chance. London: Open Books.
adolescent brain change reveals dynamic wave of accelerated gray Tizard, B., & Hodges, J. (1978). The effect of early institutional rear-
matter loss in very early-onset schizophrenia. Proceedings of the ing on the development of 8-year-old children. Journal of Child
National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 98, 11650–11655. Psychology and Psychiatry, 19, 99–118.
Thompson, P. T., Giedd, J. N., Woods, R. P., MacDonald, D., Evans, Tizard, B., & Rees, J. (1974). A comparison of the effects of adoption,
A. C., & Toga, A. W. (2000). Growth patterns in the develop- restoration to the natural mother, and continued institutionaliza-
ing brain detected by using continuum mechanical tensor maps. tion on the cognitive development of four-year-old children. Child
Nature, 404, 190–193. Development, 45, 92–99.
Thompson, R. (1998). Early sociopersonality development. In Tizard, B., & Rees, J. (1975). The effect of early institutional rearing
W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of on the behaviour problems and affectional relationships of four-
child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality develop- year-old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16,
ment (5th ed., pp. 25–104). New York: Wiley. 61–73.
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search Tizard, B., & Tizard, J. (1971). Social development of two year old
of definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child children in residential nurseries. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), The ori-
Development, 59, 25–52. gins of human social relations. New York: Academic Press.
R-126 REFERENCES
Toga, A. W., & Thompson, P. M. (2005). Genetics of brain structure Sergeant, J., Sonuga-Barke, E., Steinhausen, H. C., Thompson,
and intelligence. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 1–23. M., Tannock, R., Asherson, P., & Faraone, S. V. (2012). The hier-
Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological archical factor model of ADHD: Invariant across age and national
Review, 55, 189–208. groupings? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 292–303.
Tomarken, A. J., Mineka, S., & Cook, M. (1989). Fear-relevant Torbeyns, J., Verschaffel, L., & Ghesquière, P. (2005). Simple addi-
selective associations and covariation bias. Journal of Abnormal tion strategies in a first-grade class with multiple strategy instruc-
Psychology, 98, 381–394. tion. Cognition and Instruction, 23, 1–21.
Tomasello, M. (1990). Cultural transmission in the tool use and Torrance, E. P. (1962). Guiding creative talent. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
communicatory signaling of chimpanzees? In S. Parker & Prentice-Hall.
K. Gibson (Eds.), “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Torrance, E. P. (1972). Predictive validity of the Torrance tests of cre-
Comparative developmental perspectives (pp. 274–311). New York: ative thinking. Journal of Creative Behavior, 6, 236–252.
Cambridge University Press. Torrance, E. P. (1974). The Torrance tests of creative thinking. Technical-
Tomasello, M. (1999a). The cultural origins of human cognition. norms manual. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Torrance, E. P. (1988). The nature of creativity as manifest in its test-
Tomasello, M. (1999b). The human adaptation for culture. Annual ing. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity. Contemporary
Review of Anthropology, 28, 509–529. psychological perspectives (pp. 43–75). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
Tomasello, M. (2000). The social-pragmatic theory of word learning. University Press.
Pragmatics, 10, 401–413. Torresan, R. C., Ramos-Cerqueira, A. T., de Mathis, M. A., Diniz, J. B.,
Tomasello, M. (2005). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of Ferrão, Y. A., Miguel, E. C., & Torres, A. R. (2009). Sex differences
language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. in the phenotypic expression of obsessive-compulsive disorder: An
Tomasello, M., & Akhtar, N. (1995). Two-year-olds use pragmatic exploratory study from Brazil. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 50, 63–69.
cues to differentiate reference to objects and actions. Cognitive Torrey, E. F. (1988). Surviving schizophrenia. New York: Harper &
Development, 10, 201–224. Row.
Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. (1994). Learning words in nonostensive Torrey, E. F., & Yolken, R. H. (1995). Could schizophrenia be a viral
contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30, 639–650. zoonosis transmitted from house cats? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 21,
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York: 167–171.
Oxford University Press. Torrey, E. F., & Yolken, R. H. (2003). Toxoplasma gondii and schizo-
Tomasello, M., Call, J., & Hare, B. (2003). Chimpanzees under- phrenia. Emerging Infectious Diseases, 9, 1375–1380.
stand psychological states: The question is which ones and to what Toth, S. L., Rogosch, F. A., Sturge-Apple, M., & Cicchetti, D. (2009).
extent. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7, 153–156. Maternal depression, children’s attachment security, and repre-
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). sentational development: An organizational perspective. Child
Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cog- Development, 80, 192–208.
nition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735. Tourangeau, R., & Sternberg, R. (1981). Aptness in metaphor.
Tomasello, M., Hare, B., Lehmann, H., & Call, J. (2007). Reliance Cognitive Psychology, 13, 27–55.
on head versus eyes in the gaze following of great apes and human Tovee, M. J., Benson, P. J., Emery, J. L., Mason, S. M., & Cohen-
infants: The cooperative eye hypothesis. Journal of Human Tovee, E. M. (2003). Measurement of body size and shape percep-
Evolution, 52, 314–320. tion in eating-disordered and control observers using body-shape
Tomasello, M., & Kruger, A. C. (1992). Joint attention on actions: software. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 501–516.
Acquiring verbs in ostensive and non-ostensive contexts. Journal of Townsend, F. (1997). Rebelling against born to rebel. Journal of
Child Language, 19, 311–333. Social & Evolutionary Systems, 20, 191–204.
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural Traub, J. (2002, April 7). The test mess. New York Times Magazine,
learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552. section 6.
Tomasello, M., & Warneken, F. (2008). Share and share alike. Nature, Traugott, E. C. (1976). Pidgins, creoles, and the origins of vernacu-
454, 1057–1058. lar Black English. In D. S. Harrison & T. Trabasso (Eds.), Black
Tomasetto, C., Alparone, F. R., & Cadinu, M. (2011). Girls’ math per- English: A seminar (pp. 57–93). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
formance under stereotype threat: The moderating role of moth- Travis, J. (1997). Brave new egg. Discover, 18, 76–80.
ers’ gender stereotypes. Developmental Psychology, 47, 943–949. Tremblay, L., & Lariviere, M. (2009). The influence of puberty
Tommasi, L., Chiandetti, C., Pecchia, T., Sovrano, A., & Vallorti- onset, body mass index, and pressure to be thin on disordered
gara, G. (2012). From natural geometry to spatial cognition. eating behaviors in children and adolescents. Eating Behaviors, 10,
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 799–824. 75–83.
Tong, F., Irby, B., Mathes, P., & Kwok, O. (2008). Accelerating early Trentacosta, C. J., Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J.,
academic oral English development in transitional bilingual and Gardner, F., & Wilson, M. (2008). The relations among cumu-
structured English immersion programs. American Educational lative risk, parenting, and behavior problems during early child-
Research Journal, 45, 1011–1044. hood. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 49, 1211–1219.
Topál, J., Gergely, G., Erdőhegyi, A., Csibra, G., & Miklósi, A. Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early
(2009). Differential sensitivity to human communication in dogs, infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa
wolves, and human infants. Science, 325, 1269–1272. (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication
Topál, J., Gergely, G., Miklósi, Á., Erdöhegyi, Á., & Csibra, G. (pp. 321–347). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
(2008). Infants’ perseverative search errors are induced by prag- Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO:
matic misinterpretation. Science, 321, 1831–1834. Westview Press.
Toplak, M. E., Sorge, G. B., Flora, D. B., Chen, W., Banaschewski, T., Trickett, P. K., & Putnam, F. W. (1993). Impact of child sexual abuse
Buitelaar, J., Ebstein, R., Eisenberg, J., Franke, B., Gill, M., on females: Toward a developmental, psychobiological integration.
Miranda, A., Oades, R. D., Roeyers, H., Rothenberger, A., Psychological Science, 4, 81–87.
REFERENCES R-127
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Turiel, E. (1998). The development of morality. In W. Damon (Series
Review of Biology, 46, 35–57. Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology:
Troilo, D. T., Nickla, D. L., & Wildsoet, C. F. (2000). Choroidal Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed.,
thickness changes during altered eye growth and refractive state pp. 863–932). New York: Wiley.
in a primate. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 41, Turiel, E. (2002). The culture of morality: Social development, context,
1249–1258. and conflict. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Troilo, D., Li, T., Glasser, A., & Howland, H. C. (1995). Differences Turiel, E., Smetana, J. G., & Killen, M. (1991). Social contexts in
in eye growth and the response to visual deprivation in different social cognitive development. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz
strains of chicken. Vision Research, 35, 1211–1216. (Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development: Vol. 2.
Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in Research (pp. 307–332). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–119. Turkewitz, G., & Kenny, P. (1982). Limitations on input as a basis for
Tronick, E. Z. (1998). Emotions and emotional communication in neural organization and perceptual development: A preliminary
infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–119. theoretical statement. Developmental Psychobiology, 15, 357–368.
Troseth, G. L. (2010). Is it life or is it Memorex? Video as a representa- Turner, O. A. (1950). Some data concerning the growth and development
tion of reality. Developmental Review, 30, 155–175. of the cerebral cortex in man; postnatal growth changes in the corti-
Trottier, G., Srivastava, L., & Walker, C. D. (1999). Etiology of cal surface area. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 64, 378–384.
infantile autism: A review of recent advances in genetic and neu- Turner, P., Gervai, J., & Hinde, R. A. (1993). Gender-typing in young
robiological research. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 24, children: Preferences, behaviour, and cultural differences. British
103–115. Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 323–342.
Tryggvason, M. T. (2006). Communicative behavior in fam- Tversky, A., & Simonson, I. (1993). Context-dependent preferences.
ily conversation: Comparison of amount of talk in Finnish, Management Science, 39, 1179–1189.
SwedishFinnish, and Swedish families. Journal of Pragmatics, 38, Twenge, J. M. (2006). Generation me: Why today’s young Americans are
1795–1810. more Confident, assertive, entitled— and more miserable than ever
Tsapakis, E. M., Soldani, F., Tondo, L., & Baldessarini, R. J. (2008). before. New York: Free Press.
Efficacy of antidepressants in juvenile depression: Meta-analysis. Twyman, A. D., & Newcombe, N. S. (2010). Five reasons to doubt the
British Journal of Psychiatry, 193, 10 –17. existence of a geometric module. Cognitive Science, 34, 1315–1356.
Ttofi, M. M., & Farrington, D. P. (2011). Effectiveness of school- Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes,
based programs to reduce bullying: A systematic and meta-analytic mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional
review. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 7, 27–56. domains of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97,
Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2009). Differentiation of cognitive abilities 103–122.
across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1097–1118. Tyler, D., & McKenzie, B. E. (1990). Spatial updating and training
Tudge, J. (1992). Vygotsky, the zone of proximal development, and effects in the first year of infancy. Journal of Experimental Child
peer collaboration: Implications for classroom practice. In L. C. Psychology, 50, 445–461.
Moll (Ed.), Vygotsky and education: Instructional implications and
applications of sociohistorical psychology (pp. 155–174). Cambridge, U.S. Bureau of the Census (2010). Income, poverty, and health
UK: Cambridge University Press. insurance coverage in the United States: Report P60, Number
Tuholski, S. W., Engle, R. W., & Baylis, G. C. (2001). Individual dif- 238, Table B-2, pp. 68–73. Washington, DC: U.S. Government
ferences in working memory capacity and enumeration. Memory & Printing Office.
Cognition, 29, 484–492. U.S. Census Bureau. (1996). Current Population Reports, Special
Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & Studies, P23-190. 65+ in the United States. Washington, DC: U.S.
W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381–403). New Government Printing Office.
York: Academic Press. U.S. Census Bureau. (2001). Current Population Survey, September
Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there? American 2001: Computer and internet use supplement files. Washington, DC:
Psychologist, 40, 385–398. U.S. Department of Commerce.
Tulving, E. (2001). Origin of autonoesis in episodic memory. In U.S. Census Bureau. (2012). Statistical Abstract of the United States:
H. L. Roediger, III, J. S. Nairne, I. Neath, & A. M. Suprenant 2012. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Commerce.
(Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. U.S. Census Bureau. (2012a). America’s families and living arrange-
Crowder (pp. 17–34). Washington, DC: American Psychological ments—2012. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Association. http://www.census.gov/hhes/families/data/cps2012.html (Accessed
Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual May 14, 2013)
Review of Psychology, 53, 1–25. U.S. Census Bureau. (2012b). Statistical Abstract of the United States:
Tung, L. (1997, August 20). How to get to (China’s) Sesame Street. 2012 (131st ed.). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing
The Wall Street Journal, p. A12. Office. http://www.census.gov/compendia/statab/
Turati, C. (2004). Why faces are not special at birth: An alterna- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for
tive account for newborns’ face preference. Current Directions in Children and Families, Administration on Children, Youth and
Psychological Science, 13, 5–8. Families, Children’s Bureau. (2012). Child maltreatment 2011.
Turati, C., Bulf, H., & Simion, F. (2008). Newborns’ face recognition http://www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/cb/research- data-technology
over changes in viewpoint. Cognition, 106, 1300–1321. /statistics-research/child-maltreatment
Turati, C., Macchi Cassia, V., Simion, F., & Leo, I. (2006). Newborns’ U.S. National Institutes of Health. (1997). Genetic testing for cystic
face recognition: The role of inner and outer facial features. Child fibrosis: Consensus development conference statement. Bethesda,
Development, 77, 297–311. MD: U.S. National Institutes of Health.
Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and Uchikoshi, Y. (2005). Narrative development in bilingual kindergart-
convention. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. ners: Can Arthur help? Developmental Psychology, 41, 464–478.
R-128 REFERENCES
Uher, R., & McGuffin, P. (2010). The moderation by the serotonin Valcke, M., De Wever, B., Van Keer, H., & Schellens, T. (2011). Long-
transporter gene of environmental adversity in the etiology of term study of safe Internet use of young children. Computers &
depression: 2009 update. Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 18–22. Education, 57, 1292–1305.
Uller, C. (2004). Disposition to recognize goals in infant chimpan- Valente, T. W., Fujimoto, K., Chou, C. P., & Spruijt-Metz, D. (2009).
zees. Animal Cognition, 7, 154–161. Adolescent affiliations and adiposity: A social network analysis of
Uller, C., Carey, S., Huntley-Fenner, G., & Klatt, L. (1999). What friendships and obesity. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45, 202–204.
representations might underlie infant numerical knowledge? Valenza, E., & Bulf, H. (2011). Early development of object unity:
Cognitive Development, 14, 1–36. Evidence for perceptual completion in newborns. Developmental
Uller, C., & Nichols, S. (2000). Goal attribution in chimpanzees? Science, 14, 799–808.
Cognition, 76, B27–B34. Valian, V. (1994). Children’s postulation of null subjects: Parameter
Ullman, M. T. (1999). Acceptability ratings of regular and irregular setting and language acquisition. In B. Lust, G. Hermon, &
past tense forms: Evidence for a dual-system model of language J. Kornfilt (Eds.), Syntactic theory and first language acquisition:
from word frequency and phonological similarity effects. Language Cross-linguistic perspectives: Vol. 2. Binding, dependencies, and
and Cognitive Processes, 14, 47–67. learnability (pp. 273–286). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ullman, M. T., Corkin, S., Coppola, M., Hickok, G., Growdon, J. H., Valian, V. (2009). Innateness and learnability. In E. Bavin (Ed.),
Koroshetz, W. J., & Pinker, S. (1997). A neural dissociation within Handbook of child language (pp. 15–34). Cambridge, UK:
language: Evidence that the mental dictionary is part of declarative Cambridge University Press.
memory, and that grammatical rules are processed by the proce- Valian, V., Hoeffner, J., & Aubry, S. (1996). Young children’s imi-
dural system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 266–276. tation of sentence subjects: Evidence of processing limitations.
Ullman, M.T., Pancheva, R., Love, T., Yee, E., Swinney, D., & Developmental Psychology, 32, 153–164.
Hickok, G. (2005). Neural correlates of lexicon and grammar: Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2007). Preadolescents’ and ado-
Evidence from the production, reading, and judgment of inflec- lescents’ online communication and their closeness to friends.
tion in aphasia. Brain and Language, 93, 185–238. Developmental Psychology, 43, 267–277.
Ullman, M. T., & Pierpont, E. I. (2005). Specific language impair- Vallortigara, G., Sovrano, V. A., & Chiandetti, C. (2009). Doing
ment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Socrates experiment right: Controlled-rearing studies of geometrical
Cortex, 41, 399–443. knowledge in animals. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19, 20–26.
Underwood, M. K., Rosen, L. H., More, D., Ehrenreich, S. E., & van Aalst, J. (2006). Rethinking the nature of online work in asyn-
Gentsch, J. K. (2012). The BlackBerry project: Capturing the con- chronous learning networks. British Journal of Educational
tent of adolescents’ text messaging. Developmental Psychology, 48, Technology, 37, 279–288.
295–302. van den Boom, D. C., & Hoeksma, J. B. (1994). The effect of irri-
United Nations (2006). World mortality report, 2005. New York: tability on mother-infant interaction: A growth-curve analysis.
United Nations. Developmental Psychology, 30, 581–590.
Usher, J. A., & Neisser, U. (1993) Childhood amnesia and the begin- van der Lely, H. K., & Christian, V. (2000). Lexical word formation
nings of memory for four early life events. Journal of Experimental in children with grammatical SLI: A grammar-specific versus an
Psychology, 122, 155–165. input-processing deficit? Cognition, 75, 33–63.
Uttal, D. H. (2000). Seeing the big picture: Map use and the develop- van der Maas, H. L. J., Dolan, C. V., Grasman, R. P. P. P., Wicherts,
ment of spatial cognition. Developmental Science, 3, 247–286. J. M., Huizenga, H. A., & Raijmakers, M. E. J. (2006). A dynami-
Uttal, D. H. (2005). Spatial symbols and spatial thought: Cross- cal model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of intel-
cultural, developmental, and historical perspectives on the rela- ligence by mutualism. Psychological Review, 113, 842–861.
tion between map use and spatial cognition. In L. L. Namy (Ed.), van der Meer, A. L. (1997). Keeping the arm in the limelight:
Symbol use and symbolic representation: Developmental, and compar- Advanced visual control of arm movements in neonates. European
ative perspectives (pp. 3–23). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 1, 103–108.
Uzgiris, I. (1989). The links between imitation and social referencing. Van der Ven, S. H., Kroesbergen, E. H., Boom, J., & Leseman, P. P.
In S. Feinman (Ed.), Social referencing and the social construction of (2012). The development of executive functions and early math-
reality. New York: Plenum. ematics: A dynamic relationship. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 82, 100–119.
Vahey, P., Tatar, D., & Roschelle, J. (2007). Using handheld technology Van Duuren, M. A., Kendell-Scott, L., Stark, N. (2003). Early aes-
to move between private and public interactions in the classroom. thetic choices: Infant preferences for attractive premature infant
In M. van ‘t Hooft & K. Swan (Eds.), Ubiquitous computing in edu- faces. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 212–219.
cation: Invisible technology, visible impact (pp. 187–210). Mahway, van Hanswijck de Jonge, P., Van Furth, E. F., Lacey, J. H., & Waller, G.
NJ: Erlbaum. (2003). The prevalence of DSM-IV personality pathology among
Vaish, A., Grossmann, T., & Woodward, A. (2008). Not all emotions individuals with bulimia nervosa, binge eating disorder and obe-
are created equal: The negativity bias in social-emotional develop- sity. Psychological Medicine, 33, 1311–1317.
ment. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 383–403. van Honk, J., Schutter, D. J., Bos, P. A., Kruijt, A. W., Lentjes, E. G., &
Vaish, A., Missana, M., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Three-year-old chil- Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). Testosterone administration impairs cog-
dren intervene in third-party moral transgressions. British Journal nitive empathy in women depending on second-to-fourth digit
of Developmental Psychology, 29, 124–130. ratio. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA,
Vaish, A., & Striano, T. (2004). Is visual reference necessary? 108, 3448–3452.
Contributions of facial versus vocal cues in 12-month-olds’ social Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Juffer, F. (2006). The Emanuel Miller
referencing behavior. Developmental Science, 7, 261–269. Memorial Lecture 2006: Adoption as intervention. Meta-analytic
Vaish, A., & Woodward, A. (2010). Infants use attention but not emo- evidence for massive catch-up and plasticity in physical, socio-
tions to predict others’ actions. Infant Behavior and Development, emotional, and cognitive development. Journal of Child Psychology
33, 79–87. and Psychiatry, 47, 1228–1245.
REFERENCES R-129
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Kroonenberg, P. M. (1988). Cross-cultural Vaux, D. L., & Strasser, A. (1996). The molecular biology of apopto-
patterns of attachment: A meta-analysis of the Strange Situation. sis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 93,
Child Development, 59, 147–156. 2239–2244.
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Ebstein, Vazsonyi, A. T., Macháčková, H., Ševčíková, A., Šmahel, D. & Černá,
R. P. (2011). Methylation matters in child development: Toward A. (2012). Cyberbullying in context: Direct and indirect effects by
developmental behavioral epigenetics. Child Development low self-control across 25 European countries. European Journal of
Perspectives, 5, 305–310. Developmental Psychology, 9, 210–227.
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bard, K. A., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Vedantam, S. (2004, September 14). Dementia and the voter: Research
Ivan, K. (2009). Enhancement of attachment and cognitive devel- raises ethical, constitutional questions. Washington Post, p. A1.
opment of young nursery-reared chimpanzees in responsive versus Veenstra, R., & Dijkstra, J. K. (2012). Transformations in adolescent
standard care. Developmental Psychobiology, 51, 173–185. peer networks. In B. Laursen & W. A. Collins (Eds.), Relationship
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Goldberg, S., Kroonenberg, P. M., & Frenkel, pathways: From adolescence to young adulthood (pp. 135–154).
O. J. (1992). The relative effects of maternal and child problems on Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
the quality of attachment: A meta-analysis of attachment in clini- Velasquez-Manoff, M. (2012). An immune disorder at the root of
cal samples. Child Development, 63, 840–858. autism. New York Times, August 25.
van Praag, H. (2009). Exercise and the brain: Something to chew on. Vellutino, E. R. (1991). Introduction to three studies on reading acqui-
Trends in Neurosciences, 32, 283–290. sition: Convergent findings on theoretical foundations of code-
Van Praag, H., Kempermann, G., & Gage, F. H. (2000). Neural oriented versus whole-language approaches to reading instruction.
consequences of environmental enrichment. Nature Reviews Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 437–443.
Neuroscience, 1, 191–198. Velting, O. N., Setzer, N. J., & Albano, A. M. (2002). Anxiety disor-
van Praag, H., Shubert, T., Zhao, C., & Gage, F. H. (2005). Exercise ders. In D. T. Marsh & M. A. Fristad (Eds.), Handbook of serious
enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. emotional disturbance in children and adolescents (pp. 204–227).
Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 8680–8685. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
van Sleuwen, B. E., Engelberts, A. C., Boere-Boonekamp, M. M., Veraksa, A., Del Campo, M., & McGinnis, W. (2000). Developmental
Kuis, W., Schulpen, T. W. J., & L’Hoir, M. P. (2007). Swaddling: patterning genes and their conserved functions: From model organ-
A systematic review. Pediatrics, 120, 1097–1106. isms to humans. Molecular Genetics and Metabolism, 69, 85–100.
van Soelen, I. L. C., Brouwer, R. M., van Leeuwen, M., Kahn, R. S., Vereijken, B., & Thelen, E. (1997). Training infant treadmill step-
Hulshoff Pol, H. E., & Boomsma, D. I. (2011). Heritability of ver- ping: The role of individual pattern stability. Developmental
bal and performance intelligence in a pediatric longitudinal sam- Psychobiology, 30, 89–102.
ple. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 14, 119–128. Verfaillie, C. M. (2002). Adult stem cells: Assessing the case for pluri-
Vandell, D. L. (2004). Early child care: The known and the unknown. potency. Trends in Cell Biology, 12, 502–508.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 387–414. Verhaeghen, P., & Marcoen, A. (1996). On the mechanisms of plas-
Vandell, D. L., Belsky, J., Burchinal, M., Steinberg, L., & Vandergrift, ticity in young and older adults after instruction in the method of
N. (2010). Do effects of early child care extend to age 15 years? loci: Evidence for an amplification model. Psychology and Aging,
Results from the NICHD study of early child care and youth 11, 164–178.
development. Child Development, 81, 737–756. Verhaeghen, P., Marcoen, A., & Goossens, L. (1992). Improving
Vander Wall, S. B., & Balda, R. P. (1981). Ecology and evolution of memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: A
food-storage behavior in conifer-seed-caching corvids. Zeitschrift meta-analytic study. Psychology and Aging, 7, 242–251.
Fur Tierpsychologie, 56, 217–242. Vernes, S. C., & Fisher, S. E. (2011). Functional genomic dissection of
Vandereycken, W., & Van Deth, R. (1994). From fasting saints to speech and language disorders. In J. D. Clelland (Ed.), Genomics,
anorexic girls: The history of self-starvation. New York: New York proteomics, and the nervous system (pp. 253–278). New York: Springer.
University Press. Vernon, P. A. (1992). Intelligence, reaction time, and peripheral nerve
Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., Copp, A., & Mishkin, M. (2005). conduction velocity. Intelligence, 16, 273–288.
FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language. Nature Vernon, P. A. (1993). Intelligence and neural efficiency. In D. K.
Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 131–138. Detterman (Ed.), Current topics in human intelligence: Vol. 3.
Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., Watkins, K. E., Connelly, A., Individual differences and cognition (pp. 171–188). Norwood, NJ:
Van Paesschen, W., & Mishkin, M. (1997). Differential effects of Ablex.
early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory. Verona, E., Sprague, J., & Sadeh, N. (2012). Inhibitory control and
Science, 277, 376–380. negative emotion processing in psychopathy and antisocial person-
Varley, R., & Siegal, M. (2000). Evidence for cognition without gram- ality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 498–510.
mar from causal reasoning about “theory of mind” in an agram- Viding, E. (2004). On the nature and nurture of antisocial behavior
matic aphasic patient. Current Biology, 10, 723-726. and violence. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1036,
Varnhagen, C. K., Morrison, F. J., & Everall, R. (1994). Age and 267–277.
schooling effects in story recall and story production. Developmental Viding, E., Fontaine, N. M. G., & McCrory, E. J. (2012). Antisocial
Psychology, 30, 969–979. behaviour in children with and without callous-unemotional
Vatz, R. E., & Weinberg, L. S. (1994, July 27) Attention deficit delir- traits. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 105, 195–200.
ium. Wall Street Journal, p. A10. Viding, E., Larsson, H., & Jones, A. P. (2008). Quantitative genetic
Vaughn, B. E., & Langlois, J. H. (1983). Physical attractiveness as a studies of antisocial behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the
correlate of peer status and social competence in preschool chil- Royal Society, B, Biological Sciences, 363, 2519–2527.
dren. Developmental Psychology, 19, 561–567. Villringer. A., & Chance, B. (1997). Non-invasive optical spectroscopy
Vaux, D. L., & Korsmeyer, S. J. (1999). Cell death in development. and imaging of human brain function. Trends in Neurosciences, 20,
Cell, 96, 245–254. 435–442.
R-130 REFERENCES
Vincent-Smith, L., Bricker, D., & Bricker, W. (1974). Acquisition of Von Hofsten, C., Vishton, P. M., Spelke, E. S., Feng, Q., & Rosander,
receptive vocabulary in the toddler-age child. Child Development, K. (1998). Predictive action in infancy: Tracking and reaching for
45, 189–193. moving objects. Cognition, 67, 255–285.
Vinter, A. (1986). The role of movement in eliciting early imitations. Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental models of the earth: A
Child Development, 57, 66–71. study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24,
Vishton, P. M., & Cutting, J. E. (1995). Wayfinding, displacements, 535–585.
and mental maps: Velocity fields are not typically used to deter- Vosniadou, S., & Skopeliti, I. (2005). Developmental shifts in chil-
mine one’s aimpoint. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human dren’s categorizations of the earth. In B. G. Bara, L. Barsalou, &
Perception & Performance, 21, 978–995. M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Conference
Visscher, P. (2012). From Galton to GWAS (and beyond): What of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2325–2330). Stresa, Italy:
we have learned about the genetics of human height? Endocrine Cognitive Science Society.
Abstracts, 29, 1. Vosniadou, S., Skopeliti, I., & Ikospentaki K. (2004). Modes of know-
Visscher, P. M., Hill, W. G., & Wray, N. R. (2008). Heritability in ing and ways of reasoning in elementary astronomy. Cognitive
the genomics era— Concepts and misconceptions. Nature Reviews: Development, 19, 203–222.
Genetics, 9, 255–266. Vossekuil, B., Fein, R. A., Reddy, M., Borum, R., & Modzeleski, W. (2002).
Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Klin, A., Schultz, R., & Cook, E. H. The final report and findings of the Safe School Initiative: Implications for
(2007). Autism and the pervasive developmental disorders. In A. the prevention of school attacks in the United States. Washington, DC:
Martin & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Lewis’s child and adolescent psychia- U.S. Secret Service and U.S. Department of Education.
try: A comprehensive textbook (4th ed., pp 384–400). Philadelphia: Vouloumanos, A., Hauser, M. D., Werker, J. F, & Martin, A. (2010).
Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. The tuning of human neonates’ preference for speech. Child
Volling, B. L. (2012). Family transitions following the birth of a sib- Development, 81, 517–527.
ling: An empirical review of changes in the firstborn’s adjustment. Vouloumanos, A., & Werker, J. F. (2004). Tuned to the signal: The
Psychological Bulletin, 138, 497–528. privileged status of speech for young infants. Developmental
Volling, B. L., Kennedy, D. E., & Jackey, L. M. H. (2010). The Science, 7, 270–276.
development of sibling jealousy. In S. Hart & M. Legerstee (Eds.), Vouloumanos, A., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Listening to language at
Handbook of jealousy: Theory, research, and multidisciplinary birth: Evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. Developmental
approaches (pp. 387–417). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers. Science, 10, 159–164.
Volling, B. L., McElwain, N. L., & Miller, A. L. (2002). Emotion Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex dif-
regulation in context: The jealousy complex between young sib- ferences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of
lings and its relations with child and family characteristics. Child critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270.
Development, 73, 581–600. Voyvodic, J. T. (1996). Cell death in cortical development: How
Volpe, J. J. (1995). Neurology of the newborn (3rd ed.). Philadelphia: much? Why? So what? Neuron, 16, 693–696.
W. B. Saunders. Vreeman, R. C., & Carroll, A. E. (2007). A systematic review of school-
Volpe, J. J. (2009). Brain injury in premature infants: A complex based interventions to prevent bullying. Archives of Pediatrics and
amalgam of destructive and developmental disturbances. The Adolescent Medicine, 161, 78–88.
Lancet Neurology, 8, 110–124. Vurpillot, E. (1968). The development of scanning strategies and their
Von Hofsten, C. (1976). The role of convergence in visual space per- relation to visual differentiation. Journal of Experimental Child
ception. Vision Research 16, 193–198. Psychology, 6, 632–650.
Von Hofsten, C. (1977). Binocular convergence as a determinant of Vyas, N. S., & Gogtay, N. (2012). Treatment of early onset schizo-
reaching behavior in infancy. Perception, 6, 139–144. phrenia: Recent trends, challenges and future considerations.
Von Hofsten, C. (1980). Predictive reaching for moving objects by human Frontiers of Psychiatry, 3, 29.
infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 369–382. Vygotsky, L. S. (1930/1994). The socialist alteration of man. In R. van
Von Hofsten, C. (1982). Eye-hand coordination in the newborn. der Veer & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The Vygotsky reader (pp. 175–184).
Developmental Psychology, 18, 450–461. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Von Hofsten, C. (1983). Catching skills in infancy. Journal of Experi- Vygotsky, L. S. (1934/1962). Thought and language (E. Hanfmann &
mental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 75–85. G. Vakar, Trans. & Eds). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Von Hofsten, C. (1984). Developmental changes in the organization of Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language (E. Hartmann &
prereaching movements. Developmental Psychology, 20, 378–388. G. Vakar, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Von Hofsten, C. (2004). An action perspective on motor develop- Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher
ment. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 266–272. psychological processes (M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, &
Von Hofsten, C., & Fazel-Zandy, S. (1984). Development of visually E. Souberman, Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
guided hand orientation in reaching. Journal of Experimental Child Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (Rev. ed.). [A. Kozulin,
Psychology, 38, 208–219. Ed.] Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Von Hofsten, C., & Lindhagen, K. (1979). Observations on the devel-
opment of reaching for moving objects. Journal of Experimental Waddington, C. H. (1939). An introduction to modern genetics. New
Child Psychology, 28, 158–173. York: Macmillan.
Von Hofsten, C., Kellman, P., & Putaansuu, J. (1992). Young infants’ Waddington, C. H. (1957). The strategy of the genes. London: Allen &
sensitivity to motion parallax. Infant Behavior & Development, 15, Unwin.
245–264. Wadhwa, P. D., Culhane, J. F., Rauh, V., & Narve, S. S. (2001). Stress
Von Hofsten, C., Kochukhova, O., & Rosander, K. (2007). Predictive and preterm birth: Neuroendocrine, immune/inflammatory,
tracking over occlusions by 4-month-old infants. Developmental and vascular mechanisms. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 5,
Science, 10, 625–640. 119–125.
REFERENCES R-131
Wagner, J. B., & Johnson, S. C. (2011). An association between Walker, L. J. (2004). Gus in the gap: Bridging the judgment-action
understanding cardinality and analog magnitude representations gap in moral functioning. In D. K. Lapsley & D. Narvaez (Eds.),
in preschoolers. Cognition, 119, 10–22. Moral development, self, and identity (pp. 1–20). Mahwah, NJ:
Wagner, R., & Sternberg, R. (1985). Practical intelligence in real- Erlbaum.
world pursuits: The role of tacit knowledge. Journal of Personality Walker, L. J. (2006). Gender and morality. In M. Killen & J. G.
and Social Psychology, 49, 436–458. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 93–115).
Wagner, R. K., & Sternberg, R. J. (1991). Tacit knowledge: Its uses Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
in identifying, assessing, and developing managerial talent. In Walker, L. J., Gustafson, P., & Hennig, K. H. (2001). The con-
J. Jones, B. Steffy, & D. Bray (Eds.), Applying psychology in busi- solidation/transition model in moral reasoning development.
ness: The manager’s handbook (pp. 333–344). New York: Human Developmental Psychology, 37, 187–197.
Sciences Press. Walker-Andrews, A. S. (2008). Intermodal emotional processes in
Wainer, H., Bridgeman, B., Najarian, M., & Trapani, C. (2004). How infancy. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. Feldman-Barrett
much does extra time on the SAT help? Chance, 17, 19–24. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 364–375). New York:
Waites, C. L., Craig, A. M., & Garner, C. C. (2005). Mechanisms Guilford Press.
of vertebrate synaptogenesis. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, Wall, P. L., Botly, L. C., Black, C. K., & Shettleworth, S. J. (2004).
251–274. The geometric module in the rat: Independence of shape and feature
Wakefield, J. F. (1991). The outlook for creativity tests. Journal of learning in a food finding task. Learning & Behavior, 32, 289–298.
Creative Behavior, 25, 184–193. Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young chil-
Wakschlag, L. S., Pickett, K. E., Cook, E., Jr., Benowitz, N. L., & dren: A study of the creativity-intelligence distinction. New York:
Leventhal, B. L. (2002). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
severe antisocial behavior in offspring: A review. American Journal Waller, G., & Sheffield, A. (2008). Causes of bulimic disorders.
of Public Health, 92, 966–974. Psychiatry, 7, 152–155.
Walden, T. A., & Kim, G. (2005). Infants’ social looking toward moth- Wallerstein, J. S. (1984). Children of divorce: Preliminary report
ers and strangers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, of a ten-year follow-up of young children. American Journal of
29, 356–360. Orthopsychiatry, 53, 444–458.
Walden, T. A., & Ogan, T. A. (1988). The development of social ref- Wallerstein, J. S. (1985a). Children of divorce: Preliminary report of a
erencing. Child Development, 59, 1230–1240. ten-year follow-up of older children and adolescents. Journal of the
Waldman, I. D., Rowe, D. C., Abramowitz, A., Kozel, S. T., Mohr, American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 24, 545–553.
J. H., Sherman, S. L., Cleveland, H. H., Sanders, M. L., Gard, Wallerstein, J. S. (1985b). The overburdened child: Some long-term
J. M., & Stever, C. (1998). Association and linkage of the dopa- consequences of divorce. Columbia Journal of Law and Social
mine transporter gene and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder Problems, 19, 165–182.
in children: Heterogeneity owing to diagnostic subtype and sever- Wallerstein, J. S. (1991). The long-term effects of divorce on children:
ity. American Journal of Human Genetics, 63, 1767–1776. A review. Journal of the Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Walk, R. D. (1968). Monocular compared to binocular depth percep- 30, 349–360.
tion in human infants. Science, 162, 473–475. Wallerstein, J. S., & Blakeslee, S. (1989). Joint custody: The newest
Walk, R. D., & Gibson, E. (1961). A comparative and analytical study family form. In Second chances: Men, women, and children a decade
of visual depth perception. Psychological Monographs, 75, No. 15. after divorce; who wins, who loses— and why (pp. 256–273). New
Walker, A. S. (1982). Intermodal perception of expressive behaviors York: Ticknor and Fields.
by human infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, Wallerstein, J. S., Lewis, J., & Blakeslee, S. (2000). The unexpected
514–535. legacy of divorce: A 25 year landmark study. New York: Hyperion.
Walker, E. F., & Diforio, D. (1997). Schizophrenia: A neural diathesis- Wallman, J., Gottlieb, M. D., Rajaram, V., & Fugate-Wentzek, L. A.
stress model. Psychological Review, 104, 1–19. (1987). Local retinal regions control local eye growth and myopia.
Walker, E. F., Grimes, K. E., Davis, D. M., & Smith, A. J. (1993). Science, 237, 73–77.
Childhood precursors of schizophrenia: Facial expressions of emo- Walsh, A. C., Brown, B. B., Kaye, K., & Grigsby, J. (1994). Mental
tion. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1654–1660. capacity: Legal and medical aspects of assessment and treatment
Walker, E. F., Mittal, V., & Tessner, K. (2008). Stress and the hypo- (Tax and Estate Planning Series) (2nd ed.). New York: Shepards/
thalamic pituitary adrenal axis activity in the developmental course McGraw-Hill.
of schizophrenia. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 189–216. Walsh, B. T., & Devlin, M. J. (1998). Eating disorders: Progress and
Walker, E. F., Neumann, C. C., Baum, K., & Davis, D. M. (1996). problems. Science, 280, 1387–1390.
The developmental pathways to schizophrenia: Potential moderat- Walters, E. E., & Kendler, K. S. (1995). Anorexia nervosa and
ing effects of stress. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 647–665. anorexic-like syndromes in a population-based female twin sample.
Walker, E. F., & Roberts, M. (Eds.). (2001). Handbook of clinical child American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 64–71.
psychology (3rd. ed.). New York: Wiley. Walton, G. E., Bower, N. J. A., & Bower, T. G. R. (1992). Recognition
Walker, E. F., Savoie, T., & Davis, D. (1994). Neuromotor precursors of familiar faces by newborns. Infant Behavior and Development,
of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20, 441–451. 15, 265–269.
Walker, J., Dale, A., Waran, N., Clarke, N., Farnworth, M., & Wandell, B. A. (1985). Color measurement and discrimination.
Wemelsfelder, F. (2010). The assessment of emotional expres- Journal of the Optical Society of America A, Optics, Image & Science,
sion in dogs using a Free Choice Profiling methodology. Animal 2, 62–71.
Welfare, 9, 75–84. Wang, B. S., Sarnaik, R., & Cang, J. (2010). Critical period plastic-
Walker, L. J. (1991). Sex differences in moral reasoning. In W. M. ity matches binocular orientation preference in the visual cortex.
Kurtines & J. L. Gerwitz (Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior Neuron, 65, 246–256.
and development: Vol. 2. Research (pp. 333–364). Hillsdale, NJ: Wang, Q. (2003). Infantile amnesia reconsidered: A cross-cultural
Erlbaum. analysis. Memory, 11, 65–80.
R-132 REFERENCES
Wang, Q. (2004). The emergence of cultural self-constructs: Autobio- Warner, L. A., Pottick, K. J., & Mukherjee, A. (2004). Brief Reports: Use
graphical memory and self-description in European American and of psychotropic medications by youths with psychiatric diagnoses in
Chinese children. Developmental Psychology, 40, 3–15. the U.S. mental health system. Psychiatric Services, 55, 309–311.
Wang, Q. (2006). Earliest recollections of self and others in European Warren, R. P., Odell, J. D., Warren, W. L., Burger, R. A., Maciulis, A.,
American and Taiwanese young adults. Psychological Science, 17, Daniels, W. W., & Torres, A. R. (1995). Reading disability,
708–714. attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, and the immune system.
Wang, Q. (2007). “Remember when you got the big, big bulldozer?” Science, 268, 786–787.
Mother-child reminiscing over time and across cultures. Social Warren, W. H. Jr., Young, D. S., & Lee, D. N. (1986). Visual con-
Cognition, 25, 455–471. trol of step length during running over irregular terrain. Journal of
Wang, Q., Conway, M. A., & Hou, Y. (2004). Infantile amnesia: A Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12,
cross-cultural investigation. Cognitive Sciences, 1, 123–135. 259–266.
Wang, Q., Hou, Y., Tang, H., & Wiprovnick, A. (2011). Traveling Warshak, R. (1992). The custody revolution. New York: Simon &
backward and forward in time: Culture and gender in the episodic Schuster.
specificity of past and future events. Memory, 19, 103–109. Warshak, R., & Santrock, J. W. (1983). The impact of divorce in
Wang, Q., Leichtman, M. D., & Davies, K. I. (2000). Sharing memo- father-custody and mother-custody homes: The child’s perspec-
ries and telling stories: American and Chinese mothers and their tive. In L. A. Kurdek (Ed.), Children and divorce (pp. 29–46). San
3-year-olds. Memory, 8, 159–177. Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Wang, Q., Leichtman, M. D., & White, S. H. (1998). Childhood Wartenburger, I., Heekeren, H. R., Abutalebi, J., Cappa, S. F.,
memory and self-description in young Chinese adults: The impact Villringer, A., & Perani, D. (2003). Early setting of grammatical
of growing up an only child. Cognition, 69, 73–103. processing in the bilingual brain. Neuron, 37, 159–170.
Wang, Q., & Ross, M. (2005). What we remember and what we tell: Watanabe, D., Savion-Lemieux, T., & Penhune, V. B. (2007). The
The effects of culture and self-priming on memory representations effect of early musical training on adult motor performance:
and narratives. Memory, 13, 594–606. Evidence for a sensitive period in motor learning. Experimental
Wang, S., & Baillargeon, R. (2008). Detecting impossible changes Brain Research, 176, 332–340.
in infancy: A three-system account. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Waterhouse, L. (2006). Multiple intelligences, the Mozart effect, and
12, 17–23. emotional intelligence: A critical review. Educational Psychologist,
Wang, S., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2003). Do child-rearing values 4, 207–225.
in Taiwan and the United States reflect cultural values of collectiv- Waterland, R. A., & Jirtle, R. L. (2004). Early nutrition, epigenetic
ism and individualism? Journal of Cross- Cultural Psychology, 34, changes at transposons and imprinted genes, and enhanced suscep-
629–642. tibility to adult chronic diseases. Nutrition, 20, 63–68.
Wankerl, M., Wüst, S., & Otte, C. (2010). Current developments and Waters, E., Wippman, J., & Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Attachment, positive
controversies: Does the serotonin transporter gene-linked poly- affect, and competence in the peer group: Two studies in construct
morphic region (5-HTTLPR) modulate the association between validation. Child Development, 50, 821–829.
stress and depression? Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 23, 582–587. Watkins, B., & Lask, B. (2002). Eating disorders in school-aged chil-
Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Acock, A. C, Ponitz, C. C., Son, dren. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 11,
S-H., Lan, X., Morrison, F. J., Chen, J-L., Chen, F-M., Lee, K., 185–199.
Sung, M., & Su, L. (2011). Measuring behavioral regulation in Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it.
four societies. Psychological Assessment, 23, 364–378. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.
Wanner, E., & Gleitman L. R. (Eds.). (1982). Language acquisition: Watson, J. B. (1924). Behaviorism. New York: W. W. Norton.
The state of the art. New York: Cambridge University Press. Watson, J. B. (1928). The psychological care of infant and child (with the
Ward, T. B., Becker, A. H., Hass, S. D., & Vela, E. (1991). Attribute assistance of Rosalie Watson). London: Allen.
availability and the shape bias in children’s category generalization. Watson, J. B., & Rayner, T. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions.
Cognitive Development, 6, 143–167. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 1–14.
Wardle, J., & Watters R. (2004). Sociocultural influences on attitudes Watson, J. S. (1972). Smiling, cooing, and “the game.” Merrill-Palmer
to weight and eating: Results of a natural experiment. International Quarterly, 18, 323–339.
Journal of Eating Disorders, 35, 589–596. Watson, J. S., & Ramey, C. T. (1972). Reactions to response-
Ware, E. A., Uttal, D. H., Wetter, E. K., & DeLoache, J. S. (2006). contingent stimulation in early infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
Young children make scale errors when playing with dolls. 18, 219–227.
Developmental Science, 9, 40–45. Wattam-Bell, J. (2003). Motion processing asymmetries and stereop-
Warneken, F., Hare, B., Mellis, A. P., Hanus, D., & Tomasello, M. sis in infants. Vision Research, 43, 1961–1968.
(2007). Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young chil- Watts, R. (2003). Politeness. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
dren. PLoS Biology, 5, e184. Press.
Warneken, F., Lohse, K., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young Waxman, S. R., & Braun, I. E. (2005). Consistent (but not variable)
children share the spoils after collaboration. Psychological Science, names as invitations to form object categories: New evidence from
22, 267–273. 12-month-old infants. Cognition, 95, B59–B68.
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human Waxman, S. R., & Hall, D. G. (1993). The development of a link-
infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311, 1301–1303. age between count nouns and object categories: Evidence from
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Helping and cooperation at fifteen- to twenty-one-month-old infants. Child Development, 64,
14 months of age. Infancy, 11, 271–294. 1224–1241.
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2009a). The roots of human altru- Waxman, S. R., Medin, D. L., & Ross, N. (2007). Folkbiological
ism. British Journal of Psychology, 100, 455–471. reasoning from a cross-cultural developmental perspective: Early
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2009b). Varieties of altruism in chil- essentialist notions are shaped by cultural beliefs. Developmental
dren and chimpanzees. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 397–402. Psychology, 43, 294–308.
REFERENCES R-133
Waynforth, D. (2007). The influence of parent-infant cosleeping, Weisz, J. (2004). Psychotherapy for children and adolescents: Evidence-
nursing, and childcare on cortisol and SIgA immunity in a sample based treatments and case examples. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
of British children. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 640–648. University Press.
Weaver, C. (2002). Reading process and practice (3rd ed.). Portsmouth, Weisz, J. R., & Kazdin, A. E. (2010). Evidence-based psychotherapies
NH: Heinemann. for children and adolescents (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
Weaver, I. C., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., D’Alessio, A. C., Welch-Ross, M., & Miller, P. H. (2000). Relations between chil-
Sharma, S., Seckl, J. R., Dymov, S., Szyf, M., & Meaney, M. J. dren’s theory of mind and a selective attention strategy. Journal of
(2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature Cognition and Development, 1, 281–293.
Neuroscience, 7, 847–854. Welham, J., Isohanni, M., Jones, P., & McGrath, J. (2009). The
Weaver, I. C., Champagne, F. A., Brown. S. E., Dymov, S., Sharma, S., antecedents of schizophrenia: A review of birth cohort studies.
Meaney, M. J., & Szyf, M. (2005). Reversal of maternal program- Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35, 603–623.
ming of stress responses in adult offspring through methyl sup- Weller, A., & Feldman, R. (2003). Emotion regulation and touch
plementation: Altering epigenetic marking later in life. Journal of in infants: The role of cholecystokinin and opioids. Peptides, 24,
Neuroscience, 25, 11045–11054. 779–780.
Weaver, I. C., Meaney, M. J., & Szyf, M. (2006). Maternal care effects Weller, E. B., Danielyan, A. K., & Weller, R. A. (2004). Somatic treat-
on the hippocampal transcriptome and anxiety-mediated behav- ment of bipolar disorder in children and adolescents. Psychiatric
iors in the offspring that are reversible in adulthood. Proceedings Clinics of North America, 27, 155–178.
of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 103, 3480–3485. Weller, E. B., Weller, R. A., & Fristad, M. A. (1995). Bipolar disorder
Webster-Stratton, C. H., Reid, M. J., & Beauchaine, T. (2011). in children: Misdiagnosis, underdiagnosis, and future directions.
Combining parent and child training for young children with Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
ADHD. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40, 34, 709–714.
191–203. Wellington, J. (2005). Has ICT come of age? Recurring debates on
Wehner, R., Michel, B., & Antonsen, P. (1996). Visual navigation the role of ICT in education, 1982–2004. Research in Science &
in insects: Coupling of egocentric and geocentric information. Technological Education, 23, 25–39.
Journal of Experimental Biology, 199, 129–140. Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child’s theory of mind. Cambridge, MA:
Weinberg, W. A., & Brumback, R. A. (1992). The myth of attention- MIT Press.
deficit-hyperactivity disorder: Symptoms resulting from multiple Wellman, H. M., & Bartsch, K. (1988). Young children’s reasoning
causes. Journal of Child Neurology, 7, 431–445. about beliefs. Cognition, 30, 239–277.
Weinberger, D. R. (1996). On the plausibility of “the neurodevelop- Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Bartsch, K. (1986). Infant search and
mental hypothesis” of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, object permanence: A meta-analysis of the A-not-B error. Monographs
14, 1S–11S. of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51, 1–51.
Weinfield, N. S., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2000). Attachment Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of
from infancy to early adulthood in a high-risk sample: Continuity, theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child
discontinuity, and their correlates. Child Development, 71, Development, 72, 655–684.
695–702. Wellman, H. M., & Estes, D. (1986). Early understanding of mental
Weinraub, M., Clements, L. P., Sockloff, A., Ethridge, T. E., entities: A reexamination of childhood realism. Child Development,
Gracely, E., & Myers, B. (1984). The development of sex role 57, 910–923.
stereotypes in the third year: Relationships to gender labelling, Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1992). Cognitive develop-
gender identity, sex-typed toy preference and family characteris- ment: Foundational theories of core domains. Annual Review of
tics. Child Development, 55, 1493–1503. Psychology. 43, 337–375.
Weinreich, U. (1953). Languages in contact: Findings and problems. Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1998). Knowledge acquisition in
The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton. foundational domains. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & D. Kuhn &
Weis, S., Weber, G., Weneger E., & Kimbacher M. (1988). The human R. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2.
corpus callosum and the controversy about a sexual dimorphism. Cognition, perception, and language (5th ed., pp. 523–573). New
Psychobiology, 16, 411–415. York: Wiley.
Weisberg, P. (1963). Social and nonsocial conditioning of infants’ Wellman, H. M., & Inagaki, K. (Eds.). (1997). The emergence of core
vocalizations. Child Development, 34, 377–388. domains of thought: Children’s reasoning about physical, psychologi-
Weismann, A. (1889). Essays upon hereditary and kindred biological cal, and biological phenomena. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
problems. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks.
Weisner, T. S. (2009).Culture, development, and diversity: Expectable Child Development, 75, 523–541.
pluralism, conflict, and similarity. Ethos, 37, 181–196. Wellman, H. M., & Woolley, J. D. (1990). From simple desires to
Weiss, D. J., & Santos, L. R. (2006). Why primates? The importance ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology.
of nonhuman primates for understanding human infancy. Infancy, Cognition, 3, 245–275.
9, 133–146. Welner, Z. (1978). Childhood depression: An overview. Journal of
Weiss, G., & Hechtman, L. T. (1993). Hyperactive children grown up: Nervous and Mental Disease, 166, 588–593.
ADHD in children, adolescents, and adults (2nd ed.). New York: Wender, P. H. (1987). The hyperactive child, adolescent, and adult:
Guilford Press. Attention deficit disorder through the lifespan. New York: Oxford
Weiss, L. G. (2010). Considerations on the Flynn effect. Journal of University Press.
Psychoeducational Assessment, 28, 482. Wender, P. H. (1995). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults.
Weiss, S., Reynolds, B. A., Vescovi, A. L., Morshead, C., Craig, New York: Oxford University Press.
C. G., & van der Kooy, D. (1996). Is there a neural stem cell in the Werker, J. F. (1989). Becoming a native listener. American Scientist,
mammalian forebrain? Trends in Neuroscience, 19, 387–393. 77, 54–59.
R-134 REFERENCES
Werker, J. F., Pegg, J. E., & McLeod, P. J. (1994). A cross-language Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., & Ford, J. M. (2012). Default mode network
investigation of infant preference for infant-directed communica- activity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annual Review of
tion. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 323–333. Clinical Psychology, 8, 49–76.
Werker, J. F., Pons, F., Dietrich, C., Kajikawa, S., Fais, L., & Amano, S. Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Thermenos, H., Milanovic, S., Tsuang, M.,
(2007). Infant-directed speech supports phonetic category learn- Faraone, S., McCarley, R., Shenton, M., Green, A., Nieta-Castanon,
ing in English and Japanese. Cognition, 103, 147–162. A., LaViolette, P., Wojcik, J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Seidman, L.
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (2005) Speech perception as a window for (2009). Hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity of the default net-
understanding plasticity and commitment in language systems of work in schizophrenia and in first degree relatives of persons with
the brain. Developmental Psychobiology, 46, 233–251. schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development (2nd USA, 106, 1279–1284.
ed.). Chicago: Follett. Whiting, J. W. M., & Child, I. L. (1953). Child training and personal-
Werner, H. (1957). The concept of development from a comparative and ity: A cross-cultural study. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
organismic point of view. In D. Harris (Ed.), The concept of develop- Whorf, B. (1956) Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of
ment (pp. 125–148). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Benjamin Lee Whorf (J. B. Carroll, Ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Wertheimer, M. (1958). Principles of perceptual organization. In Press.
D. Beardslee & M. Wertheimer (Eds.), Readings in perception Wichers, M., Schrijvers, D., Geschwind, N., Jacobs, N., Myin-Germeys,
(pp. 115–135). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand. I., Thiery, E., Derom, C., Sabbe, B., Peeters, F., Delespaul, P., &
Wertheimer, M. (1963). Psycho-motor coordination of auditory-visual van Os, J. (2009). Mechanisms of gene-environment interactions
space at birth. Science, 134, 1692. in depression: Evidence that genes potentiate multiple sources of
Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural approach to adversity. Psychological Medicine, 39, 1077–1086.
mediated action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Wichman, A. L., Rodgers, J. L., & MacCallum, R. A. (2006). A mul-
Wertsch, J. V. (1998). Mind as action. New York: Oxford University tilevel approach to the relationship between birth order and intel-
Press. ligence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 117–127.
Westenberg, P. M., Drewes, M. J., Goedhart, A. W., Siebelink, Wichman, A. L., Rodgers, J. L., & MacCallum, R. C. (2007). Birth
B. M., & Treffers, P. D. A. (2004). A developmental analysis of order has no effect on intelligence: A reply and extension of pre-
self-reported fears in late childhood through mid-adolescence: vious findings. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33,
Social-evaluative fears on the rise? Journal of Child Psychology and 1195–1200.
Psychiatry, 45, 481–495. Wichstrom, L. (1999). The emergence of gender differences in
Westlye, L. T., Walhovd, K. B., Dale, A. M., Bjornerud, A., Due- depressed mood during adolescence: The role of intensified gender
Tonnessen, P., Engvig, A., Grydeland, H., Tamnes, C. K., Ostby, Y., & socialization. Developmental Psychology, 35, 232–245.
Fjell, A. M. (2009). Life-span changes of the human brain white mat- Wickramaratne, P., Gameroff, M. J., Pilowsky, D. J., Hughes, C. W.,
ter: Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and volumetry. Cerebral Cortex, Garber, J., Malloy, E., King, C., Cerda, G., Sood, A. B., Alpert,
20, 2055–2068. J. E., Trivedi, M. H., Fava, M., Rush, A. J., Wisniewski, S., &
Whaley, A. L., & Koenen, K. C. (2001). The juvenile-as-adult- Weissman, M. M. (2011). Children of depressed mothers 1 year
criminal debate. Journal of the American Academy of Child & after remission of maternal depression: Findings from the STAR*D-
Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 619–620. Child study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 593–602.
Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a theory Widen, S. C., & Russell, J. A. (2008). Children acquire emotion cat-
of episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoetic conscious- egories gradually. Cognitive Development, 23, 291–312.
ness. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 331–354. Widom, C. S., & Ames, M. A. (1994). Criminal consequences of child-
Whitaker, R. (2010). Anatomy of an epidemic: Magic bullets, psychiat- hood sexual victimization. Child Abuse and Neglect, 18, 303–318.
ric drugs, and the astonishing rise of mental illness in America. New Wieman, C. E. (2010). Science education for the 21st century: Using
York: Crown Publishers. the insights of science to teach/learn science. In Proceedings of the
White, L. K., McDermott, J. M., Degnan, K. A., Henderson, H. A., & 41st ACM technical symposium on computer science education (p.
Fox, N. A.. (2011). Behavioral inhibition and anxiety: The moder- 198). New York: Special Interest Group on Computer Science
ating roles of inhibitory control and attention shifting. Journal of Education, Association for Computing Machinery.
Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 735–747. Wiesel, T. N. (1982). Postnatal development of the visual cortex and
White, P. A. (2009). Perception of forces exerted by objects in colli- the influence of environment. Nature, 299, 583–591.
sion events. Psychological Review, 116, 580–601. Wiesel, T. N., & Hubel, D. H. (1963). Single-cell responses in stri-
White, P. A. (2012). The impetus theory in judgments about object motion: ate cortex of kittens deprived of vision in one eye. Journal of
A new perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 1007–1028. Neurophysiology, 26, 1003–1017.
White, W. A. (1916). Mechanisms of character formation: An introduc- Wiesel, T. N., & Raviola, E. (1977). Myopia and eye enlargement
tion to psychoanalysis. New York: Macmillan. after neonatal lid fusion in monkeys. Nature, 266, 66–68.
Whitehouse, A. J. O., Barry, J. G., & Bishop, D.V. M. (2007). The Wilcox, H. C., Grados, M., Samuels, J., Riddle, M. A., Bienvenu, O. J.,
broader language phenotype of autism: A comparison with specific Pinto, A., Cullen, B., Wang, Y., Shugart, Y. Y., Liang, K. Y., &
language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Nestadt, G. (2008). The association between parental bonding and
48, 822–830. obsessive compulsive disorder in offspring at high familial risk.
Whiteman, S. D., & Buchanan, C. M. (2002). Mothers’ and children’s Journal of Affective Disorders, 111, 31–39.
expectations for adolescence: The impact of perceptions of an older Wilcox, T., & Baillargeon, R. (1998). Object individuation in infancy:
sibling’s experience. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 157–171. The use of featural information in reasoning about occlusion
Whiteman, S. D., McHale, S. M., & Crouter, A. C. (2003). What par- events. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 97–155.
ents learn from experience: The first child as a first draft? Journal of Wilcox, T., Bortfeld, H., Woods, R., Wruck, E., & Boas, D. A. (2005).
Marriage and the Family, 65, 608–621. Using near-infrared spectroscopy to assess neural activation during
REFERENCES R-135
object processing in infants. Journal of Biomedical Optics, 10, of testosterone secretion, mating systems, and breeding strategies.
011010-1–011010-9. American Naturalist, 136, 829–846.
Wilcox, T., & Chapa, C. (2004). Priming infants to use color and pat- Wingfield, J. C., Jacobs, J. D., Tramontin, A. D., Perfito, N., Meddle,
tern information in an individuation task. Cognition, 90, 265–302. S., Maney, D. L., & Soma, K. (2000). Toward an ecological
Willcutt, E. G., Nigg, J. T., Pennington, B. F., Solanto, M. V., Rohde, basis of hormone-behavior interactions in reproduction of birds.
L. A., Tannock, R., Loo, S. K., Carlson, C. L., McBurnett, K., & In K. Wallen & J. E. Schneider (Eds.), Reproduction in context
Lahey, B. B. (2012). Validity of DSM–IV attention deficit/hyper- (pp. 85–128). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
activity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. Journal of Winkler-Rhoades, N., Medin, D. L., Waxman, S. R., Woodring, J., &
Abnormal Psychology, 121, 991–1010. Ross, N. O. (2010). Naming the animals that come to mind: Effects
Willerman, L. (1973). Activity level and hyperactivity in twins. Child of culture and experience on category fluency. Journal of Cognition
Development, 44, 288–293. and Culture, 10, 205–220.
Williams (Eds.), Parameter setting (pp. 23–172). Dordrecht, The Winner, E., & Leekam, S. (1991). Distinguishing irony from decep-
Netherlands: Reidel. tion: Understanding the speaker’s second-order intention. British
Williams, C. L., & Pleil, K. E. (2008). Toy story: Why do monkey and Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 257–270.
human males prefer trucks? Comment on “Sex differences in rhe- Winocur, G., & Hasher, L. (2002). Circadian rhythms and memory
sus monkey toy preferences parallel those of children.” Hormones in aged humans and animals. In L. Squire & D. Schacter (Eds.),
and Behavior, 54, 355–358. Neuropsychology of memory (3rd ed., pp. 273–285). New York:
Williams, H. J., Owen, M. J., & O’Donovan, M. C. (2009). New Guilford Press.
findings from genetic association studies of schizophrenia. Journal Winocur, G., & Hasher, L. (2004). Age and time-of-day effects
of Human Genetics, 54, 9–14. on learning and memory in a non-matching-to-sample test.
Williams, J. P. (1980). Teaching decoding with an emphasis on pho- Neurobiology of Aging, 25, 1107–1115.
neme analysis and phoneme blending. Journal of Educational Winokur, G., Coryell, W., Keller, M., Endicott, J., & Leon, A. (1995).
Psychology, 72, 1–15. A family study of manic-depressive (bipolar I) disease. Is it a dis-
Williams, L. M. (1994a). Recall of childhood trauma: A prospec- tinct illness separable from primary unipolar depression? Archives
tive study of women’s memories of child sexual abuse. Journal of of General Psychiatry, 52, 367–373.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 1167–1176. Winship, C., & Korenman, S. (1997). Does staying in school make
Williams, L. M. (1994b). What does it mean to forget child sex- you smarter? The effect of education on IQ in the bell curve. In
ual abuse: A Reply to Loftus, Garry, and Feldman. Journal of B. Devlin, S. E. Fienberg., D. P. Resnick, & K. Roeder (Eds.),
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 1182–1186. Intelligence, genes, and success: Scientists respond to the bell curve
Williamson, D. E., Birmaher, B., Axelson, D. A., Ryan, N. D., & (pp. 215–234). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Dahl, R. E. (2004). First episode of depression in children at Wisco, B. E., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2008). Ruminative response
low and high familial risk for depression. Journal of the American style. In K. S. Dobson & D. Dozois (Eds.), Risk factors for depres-
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 291–297. sion (pp. 221–236). New York: Academic Press.
Wilson, A. E., Smith, M. D., & Ross, H. S. (2003). The nature and Witherington, D. C., Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Lejeune, L., &
effects of young children’s lies. Social Development, 12, 21–45. Seah, E. (2005). Avoidance of heights on the visual cliff in newly
Wilson, B. J. (2008). Media and children’s aggression, fear and altru- walking infants. Infancy, 7, 285–298.
ism. Future of Children, 18, 87–118. Witherington, D. C., Campos, J. J., Harriger, J. A., Bryan, C., &
Wilson, B. J., Smith, S. L., Potter, W. J., Kunkel, D., Linz, D., Colvin, Margett, T. E. (2010). Emotion and its development in infancy.
C. M., & Donnerstein, E. (2002). Violence in children’s television In G. Bremner & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Wiley-Blackwell handbook
programming: Assessing the risks. Journal of Communication, 52, of infant development. Vol. 1: Basic research (2nd ed., pp. 568–591).
5–35. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Wilson, J. Q., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1985). Crime and human nature: Witt, S. D. (2000). The Influence of television on children’s gender
The definitive study of the causes of crime. New York: Simon & role socialization. Journal of Childhood Education: Infancy Through
Schuster. Adolescence, 76, 322–324.
Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1993). Lethal confrontational violence Witte, A. V., Fobker, M., Gellner, R., Knecht, S., & Floel, A. (2009).
among young men. In N. J. Bell & R. W. Bell (Eds.), Adolescent Caloric restriction improves memory in elderly humans. Proceedings
risk taking (pp. 84–106). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 106, 1255–1260.
Wilson, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1997). Evidence for racial
in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 460–473. prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire
Wilt, F. H., & Hake, S. C. (2004). Principles of developmental biology. measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 262–274.
New York: W. W. Norton. Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. Oxford, UK:
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representations Blackwell.
and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s Wolak, J., Finkelhor, D., Mitchell, K. J., & Ybarra, M. L. (2008).
understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128. Online “predators” and their victims: Myths, realities, and impli-
Winawer, J., Witthoft, N., Frank, M. C., Wu, L., Wade, A. R., & cations for prevention and treatment. American Psychologist, 63,
Boroditsky, L. (2007). Russian blues reveal effects of language 111–128.
on color discrimination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Wolff, P. H. (1963). Observations on the early development of smil-
Sciences of the USA, 104, 7780–7785. ing. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behaviour (Vol. 2,
Winberg, J. (2005). Mother and newborn baby: Mutual regulation pp. 113–138). London: Methuen.
of physiology and behavior—A selective review. Developmental Wolff, P., Medin, D. L., & Pankratz, C. (1999). Evolution and devolu-
Psychobiology, 47, 219–229. tion of folkbiological knowledge. Cognition, 73, 177–204.
Wingfield, J. C., Hegner, R. E., Dufty, A. M., & Ball, G. F. (1990). Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. Stanford, CA:
The “challenge hypothesis”: Theoretical implications for patterns Stanford University Press.
R-136 REFERENCES
Wolpert, L. (1978). Pattern formation in biological development. the security motivation system. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
Scientific American, 239, 154–164. Reviews, 35, 1019–1033.
Wolpert, L. (1999).Vertebrate limb development and malformations. Woolf, C. J., & Bloechlinger, S. (2002). It takes more than two to
Pediatric Research, 46, 247–254. Nogo. Science, 297, 1132–1134.
Wolpert, L., Beddington, R., Brockes, J., Jessell, T., Lawrence, P., & Woolgar, M., Steele, H., Steele, M., Yabsley, S., & Fonagy, P. (2001).
Meyerowitz, E. (1998). Principles of development. Oxford, UK: Children’s play narrative responses to hypothetical dilemmas and
Oxford University Press. their awareness of moral emotions. British Journal of Developmental
Wolraich, M., Wilson, D., & White, W. (1995). The effect of sugar on Psychology, 19, 115–128.
behavior or cognition in children: A meta-analysis. Journal of the Wootton, J. M., Frick, P. J., Shelton, K. K., & Silverthorn, P. (1997).
American Medical Association, 274, 1617–1621. Ineffective parenting and childhood conduct problems: The mod-
Wong, Y., & Tsai, J. L. (2007). Cultural models of shame and guilt. In erating role of callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Consulting and
J. L. Tracy, R. W. Robins, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), The self-conscious Clinical Psychology, 65, 301–308.
emotions: Theory and research (pp. 209–224). New York: Guilford Wozniak, J. (2003). Pediatric bipolar disorder: The new perspective
Press. on severe mood dysfunction in children. Journal of Child and
Wood, D., Bruner, J., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 13, 449–451.
problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, Wozniak, J., Petty, C. R., Schreck, M., Moses, A., Faraone, S. V., &
89–100. Biederman, J. (2011). High level of persistence of pediatric bipo-
Wood, D., & Middleton, D. (1975). A study of assisted problem solv- lar-I disorder from childhood onto adolescent years: A four year
ing. British Journal of Psychology, 66, 181–191. prospective longitudinal follow-up study. Journal of Psychiatric
Wood, J. J., McLeod, B. D., Sigman, M., Hwang, W. C., & Chu, Research, 45, 1273–1282.
B. C. (2003). Parenting and childhood anxiety: Theory, empiri- Wozniak, R. (Ed.). (1995). Mind, adaptation, and childhood: The evo-
cal findings, and future directions. Journal of Child Psychology and lutionary origins of developmental psychology. London: Routledge/
Psychiatry, 44, 134–151. Thoemmes Press.
Wood, J. N., & Spelke, E. S. (2005). Infants’ enumeration of actions: Wray, N. R., & Gottesman, I. I. (2012). Using summary data from
Numerical discrimination and its signature limits. Developmental the Danish National Registers to estimate heritabilities for schizo-
Science, 8, 173–181. phrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depressive disorder. Frontiers
Woodbury, J. (1991). Counting Eskimo words for snow: A citizen’s guide. in Genetics, 3, 118.
http://www.ling.ed.ac.uk/linguist/issues/5/5-1239.html#3 Wright, E., Chisholm, D., MacCannell, J. F., & Whitford, M. (Eds.).
Woodhouse, B. B. (2004). Reframing the debate about the social- (1992). Feminism and psychoanalysis: A critical dictionary. Oxford,
ization of children: An environmentalist paradigm. University of UK: Blackwell.
Chicago Legal Forum, 85, 82–149. Wright, J. C., Huston, A. C., Murphy, K. C., St. Peters, M., Piñon, M,
Woodlee, M. T., & Schallert, T. (2006). The impact of motor activ- Scantlin, R., & Kotler, J. (2001a). The relations of early television
ity and inactivity on the brain: Implications for the prevention viewing to school readiness and vocabulary of children from low-
and treatment of nervous system disorders. Current Directions in income families: The Early Window Project. Child Development,
Psychological Science, 15, 203–206. 72, 1347–1366.
Woodruff, G., & Williams, D. R. (1976). The associative relation Wright, J. C., Huston, A. C., Vandewater, E. A., Bickham, D. S.,
underlying autoshaping in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Scantlin, R. M., Kotler, J. A., Caplovitz, A. G., & Lee, J.
Analysis of Behavior, 26, 1–13. (2001b). American children’s use of electronic media in 1997:
Woods, R. J., Wilcox, T., Armstrong, J., & Alexander G. (2010). A national survey. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,
Infants’ representations of three-dimensional occluded objects. 22, 31–47.
Infant Behavior & Development, 33, 663–671. Wright, J., & Hensley, C. (2003). From animal cruelty to serial
Woodward, A. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an murder: Applying the graduation hypothesis. International
actor’s reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34. Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 47,
Woodward, A. L. (1999). Infants’ ability to distinguish between 72–89.
purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior and Wright, R. (1994). The moral animal: Why we are the way we are: The
Development, 22, 145–160. new science of evolutionary psychology. New York: Vintage Books.
Woodward, A. L. (2005). Infants’ understanding of the actions Wright, R. (2010). The moral animal: Evolutionary psychology and
involved in joint attention. In N. Eilan, C. Hoerl, T. McCormack, & everyday life. New York: Vintage.
J. Roessler (Eds.), Joint attention: Communication and other minds. Wu, C., & Chao, R. K. (2011). Intergenerational cultural dissonance
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. in parent-adolescent relationships among Chinese and European
Woodward, A. L., & Markman, E. M. (1998). Early word learning. Americans. Developmental Psychology, 47, 493–508.
In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Wyllie, J. (2006). Applied physiology of newborn resuscitation.
Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and lan- Current Paediatrics, 16, 379–385.
guage (5th ed., pp. 371–420). New York: Wiley. Wynn, K. (1990). Children’s understanding of counting. Cognition,
Woodward, A. L., Markman, E. M., & Fitzsimmons, C. M. (1994). 36, 155–193.
Rapid word learning in 13- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature,
Psychology, 30, 553–566. 358, 749–750.
Woodward, A. L., Sommerville, J. A. & Guajardo, J. J. (2001). How Wynn, K. (1992). Children’s acquisition of the number words and the
infants make sense of intentional action. In B. F. Malle, L. J. Moses, & counting system. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 220–251.
D. A. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of Wynn, K. (1995). Infants possess a system of numerical knowledge.
social cognition (pp. 149–169). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 172–177.
Woody, E. Z., & Szechtman, H. (2011). Adaptation to potential Wynn, K. (1996). Infants’ individuation and enumeration of actions.
threat: The evolution, neurobiology, and psychopathology of Psychological Science, 7, 164–169.
REFERENCES R-137
Wynn, K. (1998). Psychological foundations of number: Numerical Ybarra, M. L., & Mitchell, K. J. (2004). Online aggressor/targets,
competence in human infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, aggressors, and targets: A comparison of associated youth charac-
296–303. teristics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1308–1316.
Wynn, K. (2008). Some innate foundations of social and moral cogni- Yee, M. D., & Brown, R. (1992). Self-evaluation and intergroup
tion. In P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, & S. Stich (Eds.), The innate attitudes in children aged three to nine. Child Development, 63,
mind: Foundations and the future. Oxford, UK: Oxford University 619–629.
Press. Yelland, N. (2005). The future is now: A review of the literature on
Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Chiang, W. (2002). Enumeration of collec- the use of computers in early childhood education (1994–2004).
tive entities by 5-month-old infants. Cognition, 83, B55–B62. AACE Journal, 13, 201–232.
Wystrach, A., & Beugnon, G. (2009). Ants learn geometry and fea- Yirmiya, N., & Charman, T. (2010). The prodrome of autism:
tures. Current Biology, 19, 61–66. Early behavioural and biological signs, regression, peri- and post-
Wyttenbach, R. A., May, M. L., & Hoy, R. (1996). Categorical per- natal development and genetics. Journal of Child Psychology and
ception of sound frequency by crickets. Science, 273, 1542–1544. Psychiatry, 51, 432–458.
Yonas, A. (1981). Infants’ responses to optical information for col-
Xu, F. (2002). The role of language in acquiring object kind concepts lision. In R. N. Aslin, J. R. Alberts, & M. R. Peterson (Eds.),
in infancy. Cognition, 85, 223–250. Development of perception: Psychobiological perspectives: Vol. 2. The
Xu, F. (2003). The development of object individuation in infancy. visual system (pp. 313–334). New York: Academic Press.
In H. Hayne & J. Fagen (Eds.), Progress in infancy research (Vol. 3, Yonas, A., Cleaves, W., & Pettersen, L. (1978). Development of sensi-
pp. 159–192). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. tivity to pictorial depth, Science, 200, 77–79.
Xu, F., & Carey, S. (1996). Infants’ metaphysics: The case of numeri- Yonas, A., Elieff, C. A., & Arterberry, M. E. (2002). Emergence
cal identity. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 111–153. of sensitivity to pictorial depth cues: Charting development
Xu, F., Cote, M., & Baker, A. (2005a). Labeling guides object individ- in individual infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 25,
uation in 12-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 16, 372–377. 495–514.
Xu, F., Spelke, E. S., & Goddard, S. (2005b). Number sense in human Yonas, A., & Granud, C. E. (1985). Reaching as a measure of
infants. Developmental Science, 8, 88–101. infants’ spatial perception. In G. Gottlieb & N. A. Krasnegor
Xu, F., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Word learning as Bayesian infer- (Eds.), Measurement of audition and vision in the first year of post-
ence. Psychological Review, 114, 245–272. natal life: A methodological overview (pp. 301–322). Norwood,
Xu, X., Tung, Y., & Dunaway, R. G. (2000). Cultural, human, and NJ: Ablex.
social capital as determinants of corporal punishment: Toward an Yoon, C., Cole, C. A., & Lee, M. P. (2009). Consumer decision mak-
integrated theoretical model. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 15, ing and aging: Current knowledge and future directions. Journal of
603–630. Consumer Psychology, 19, 2–16.
Yoon, C., May, C. P., Goldstein, D., & Hasher, L. (in press).
Yakovlev, P. I., & Lecours, A. R. (1967). The myelogenetic cycles of Aging, circadian arousal patterns and cognition. In D. Park &
regional maturation of the brain. In A. Minkowski (Ed.), Regional N. Schwarz (Eds.), Cognitive aging: A primer (2nd ed., pp. 353–363).
development of the brain in early life (pp. 3–70). Oxford: Blackwell. Philadelphia: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.
Yamada, M., Murata,T., Hirose, S., Lavorgna, G., Suzuki, E., & York, G. (2005). In China, cigarettes are a kind of miracle drug. Globe
Ueda, H. (2000). Temporally restricted expression of transcrip- and Mail, June 11, p. A14.
tion factor betaFTZ-F1: Significance for embryogenesis, molting Young, L., & Koenigs, M. (2007). Investigating emotion in moral
and metamorphosis in Drosophila melanogaster. Development, 127, cognition: A review of evidence from functional neuroimaging and
5083–5092. neuropsychology. British Medical Bulletin, 84, 69–79.
Yamamoto, K., & Keil, F. C. (2000). The acquisition of Japanese Young, L., & Saxe, R. (2011). Moral universals and individual differ-
numerical classifiers: Linkages between grammatical forms and ences. Emotion Review, 3, 32–324.
conceptual categories. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 9, 379–409. Young, S. K., Fox, N. A., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1999). The relations
Yang, B., Ollendick, T., Dong, Q., Xia, Y., & Lin, L. (1995). Only between temperament and empathy in 2-year-olds. Developmental
children and children with siblings in the People’s Republic of Psychology, 35, 1189–1197.
China: Levels of fear, anxiety and depression. Child Development, Youngentob, S. L., & Glendinning, J. I. (2009). Fetal ethanol expo-
66, 1301–1311. sure increases ethanol intake by making it smell and taste better.
Yang, H., Ramasubramanian, S., & Oliver, M. B. (2008). Cultivation Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 106,
effects on quality of life indicators: Exploring the effects of 5359–5364.
American television consumption on feelings of relative depri- Youngstrom, E. A., & Goodman, S. (2001). Children’s perception of
vation in South Korea and India. Journal of Broadcasting and other people: Mentalistic versus behavioristic descriptions of peers
Electronic Media, 52, 247–267. and adults. Developmental Science, 4, 165–174.
Yang, J. H., DiSandro, M., & Baskin, L. S. (2010). Gender identity in Ystad, M. A., Lundervold, A. J., Wehling, E., Espeseth, T., Rootwelt, H.,
disorders of sex development. Urology, 75, 153–160. Westleye, L. T., Andersson, M., Adolfsdottir, S., Geitung, J. T.,
Yarasheski, K. E. (2003). Exercise, aging, and muscle protein metab- Fjell, A. M., Reinvang, I., & Lundervold, A. (2009). Hippocampal
olism. Journals of Gerontology, Series A: Biological Sciences and volumes are important predictors for memory function in elderly
Medical Sciences, 58, 918–922. women. BMC Medical Imaging, 9, 17.
Yau, J., & Smetana, J. G. (2003). Conceptions of moral, social- Yu, N., Chen, F. C., Ota, S., Jorde, L. B., Pamilo, P., Patthy, L.,
conventional, and personal events among Chinese preschoolers in Ramsay, M., Jenkins, T., Shyue, S. K., & Li, W. H. (2002). Larger
Hong Kong. Child Development, 74, 647–658. genetic differences within Africans than between Africans and
Ybarra, M. L., & Mitchell, K. (2008). How risky are social network- Eurasians. Genetics, 161, 269–274.
ing sites? A comparison of places online where youth sexual solici- Yuan, S., Fisher, C., & Snedeker, J. (2012). Counting the nouns: Simple
tation and harassment occurs. Pediatrics, 121, 350–357. structural cues to verb meaning. Child Development, 83, 1382–1399.
R-138 REFERENCES
Yudofsky, S. C. (2009). Contracting schizophrenia: Lessons from the Zayas, V., Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Aber, J. L. (2011). Roots of
influenza epidemic of 1918–1919. Journal of the American Medical adult attachment: Maternal caregiving at 18 months predicts adult
Association, 301, 324–326. peer and partner attachment. Social Psychological and Personality
Yuill, N. (1984). Young children’s coordination of motive and out- Science, 2, 289–297.
come in judgments of satisfaction and morality. British Journal of Zeanah, C. H., Egger, H. L., Smyke, A. T., Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A.,
Developmental Psychology, 2, 73–81. Marshall, P. J., & Guthrie, D. (2009). Institutional rearing and
Yuill, N., & Pearson, A. (1998). The development of bases for psychiatric disorders in Romanian preschool children. American
trait attribution: Children’s understanding of traits as causal Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 777–785.
mechanisms based on desire. Developmental Psychology, 34, Zeanah, C. H., Smyke, A. T., & Dumitrescu, A. (2002). Attachment
574–586. disturbances in young children: II. Indiscriminate behavior and
Yurgelun-Todd, D. (2007). Emotional and cognitive changes during institutional care. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
adolescence. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17, 251–257. Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 983–989.
Yussen, S. R., & Bird, J. E. (1979). The development of metacognitive Zeanah, C. H., Smyke, A. T., Koga, S. F., Carlson, E., & the Bucharest
awareness in memory, communication, and attention. Journal of Early Intervention Project Core Group (2005). Attachment in
Experimental Child Psychology, 28, 300–313. institutionalized and community children in Romania. Child
Yussen, S. R., & Levy, V. M. (1975). Developmental changes in Development, 76, 1015–1028.
predicting one’s own span of short-term memory. Journal of Zeidner, M., Matthews, G., Roberts, R. D., & MacCann, C. (2003).
Experimental Child Psychology, 19, 502–508. Development of emotional intelligence: Towards a multi-level
investment model. Human Development, 46, 69–96.
Zagon, I. S., & McLaughlin, P. J. (1993). Receptors in the develop- Zeifman, D., Delaney, S., & Blass, E. M. (1996). Sweet taste, look-
ing nervous system: Growth factors and hormones. New York: ing and calm in 2- and 4-week-old infants: The eyes have it.
Chapman & Hall. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1090–1099.
Zagzebski, L. T. (1996). Virtues of the mind: An Inquiry into the nature Zelazo, P. D., & Cunningham, W. A. (2007). Executive function:
of virtue and the ethical foundations of knowledge. Cambridge, UK: Mechanisms underlying emotion regulation. In J. J. Gross (Ed.),
Cambridge University Press. Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 135–158). New York: Guilford
Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, Press.
M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Zelazo, P. D., Helwig, C. C., & Lau, A. (1996). Intention, act, and
Psychology, 28, 126–136. outcome in behavioral prediction and moral judgment. Child
Zahn-Waxler, C., Schiro, K., Robinson, J. L., Emde, R. N., & Development, 67, 2478–2492.
Schmitz, S. (2001). Empathy and prosocial patterns in young MZ Zelazo, P. D., & Mueller, U. (2002). Executive function in typi-
and DZ twins: Development and genetic and environmental influ- cal and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Handbook
ences. In R. N. Emde & J. K. Hewitt (Eds.), Infancy to early child- of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445–469). Oxford, UK:
hood: Genetic and environmental influences on developmental change Blackwell.
(pp. 141–162). New York: Oxford University Press. Zelazo, P. R. (1971). Smiling to social stimuli: Eliciting and condi-
Zahn-Waxler, C., Shirtcliff, E. A., & Marceau, K. (2008). Disorders tioning effects. Developmental Psychology, 4, 32–42.
of childhood and adolescence: Gender and psychopathology. Zeller, R. (2010). The temporal dynamics of vertebrate limb develop-
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 275–303. ment, teratogenesis and evolution. Current Opinion in Genetics &
Zaitchik, D. (1990). When representations conflict with reality: The Development, 20, 384–390.
preschooler’s problem with false beliefs and “false” photographs. Zentall, T. R., & Edwards, C. A. (1984). Categorical color coding by
Cognition, 35, 41–68. pigeons. Animal Learning & Behavior, 12, 249–255.
Zajonc, R. B. (1976). Family configuration and intelligence: Variations Zentner, M., & Bates, J. E. (2008). Child temperament: An integra-
in scholastic aptitude scores parallel trends in family size and the tive review of concepts, research programs, and measures. European
spacing of children. Science, 192, 227–236. Journal of Developmental Science, 2, 7–37.
Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Birth order debate resolved? American Zentner, M., & Shiner, R. (Eds.). (2012). Handbook of temperament.
Psychologist, 56, 522–523. New York: Guilford Press.
Zajonc, R. B. (2001). The family dynamics of intellectual develop- Zhai, F., Waldfogel, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2011). Estimating
ment. American Psychologist, 56, 490–496. the effects of Head Start on parenting and child maltreatment.
Zajonc, R. B., & Markus, G. B. (1975). Birth order and intellectual Children and Youth Services Review, 1–11.
development. Psychological Review, 82, 74–88. Zhang, Y., & Wang, Y. (2007). Neural plasticity in speech acqui-
Zajonc, R. B., Markus, H., & Markus, G. B. (1979). The birth order sition and learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10,
puzzle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1325–1341. 147–160.
Zajonc, R. B., & Mullally, P. R. (1997). Birth order: Reconciling con- Zhao, C., Deng, W., & Gage, F. H. (2008). Mechanisms and func-
flicting effects. American Psychologist, 52, 685–689. tional implications of adult neurogenesis. Cell, 132, 645–660.
Zajonc, R. B., Murphy, S. T., & Inglehart, M. (1989). Feeling and Zhao, Y., Ma, H., Wang, Y., Gao, H., Xi, C., Hua, T., Zhao, Y., &
facial efference: Implications of the vascular theory of emotions. Qiu, G. (2010). Association between FOXP2 gene and speech
Psychological Review, 96, 395–416. sound disorder in Chinese population. Psychiatry and Clinical
Zajonc, R. B., & Sulloway, F. J. (2007). The confluence model: Birth Neurosciences, 64, 565–573.
order as a within-family or between-family dynamic? Personality Zhou, J., Pfaff, D. W., & Chen, G. (2005). Sex differences in estro-
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1187–1194. genic regulation of neuronal activity in neonatal cultures of ven-
Zapf, J. A., & Smith, L. B. (2007). When do children generalize the tromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus. Proceedings of the National
plural to novel nouns? First Language, 27, 53–73. Academy of Sciences of the USA, 102, 14907–14912.
Zaslavsky, C. (1973). Africa counts: Number and pattern in African cul- Zhou, K., Mo, L., Kay, P., Kwok, V. P., Ip, T. N., & Tan, L. H. (2010).
ture. Boston: Prindle, Weber, & Schmidt. Newly trained lexical categories produce lateralized categorical
REFERENCES R-139
perception of color. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Zimmerman, C. (2000). The development of scientific reasoning
of the USA, 107, 9974–9978. skills. Developmental Review, 20, 99–149.
Zhou, X. D., Li, L., Yan, Z., & Hesketh, T. (2013). High sex ratio Zimmerman, F. J., & Christakis, D. A. (2005). Children’s televi-
as a correlate of depression in Chinese men. Journal of Affective sion viewing and cognitive outcomes: A longitudinal analysis of
Disorders, 144, 79–86. national data. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 159,
Zhou, X. D., Wang, X. L., Li, L., & Hesketh, T. (2011).The very high 619–625.
sex ratio in rural China: Impact on the psychosocial wellbeing of Zimmerman, F. J., Christakis, D. A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2007).
unmarried men. Social Science & Medicine, 73, 1422–1427. Associations between media viewing and language develop-
Zhu, X., Park, T. W., Winawer, J., & Wallman, J. (2005). In a mat- ment in children under 2 years. Journal of Pediatrics, 151,
ter of minutes, the eye can know which way to grow. Investigative 364–368.
Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 46, 2238–2241. Zinbarg, R. E., Barlow, D. H., Brown, T. A., & Hertz, R. M.
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, develop- (1992). Cognitive-behavioral approaches to the nature and
mental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholin- treatment of anxiety disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 43,
guistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29. 235–267.
Zigler, E. (2003). What would draw a basic scientist into Head Zinober, B., & Martlew, M. (1985). The development of communi-
Start (and why would he never leave)? In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), cative gestures. In M. Barret (Ed.), Children’s single word speech
Psychologists defying the crowd: Stories of those who battled the estab- (pp.183–215). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
lishment and won (pp. 273–282). Washington, DC: American Zito, J. M., Safer, D. J., dosReis, S., Gardner, J. F., Magder, L., Soeken,
Psychological Association. K., Boles, M., Lynch, F., & Riddle, M. A. (2003). Psychotropic
Zigler, E., & Muenchow, S. (1992). Head Start: The inside story of practice patterns for youth: A 10-year perspective. Archives of
America’s most successful educational experiment. New York: Basic Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, 157, 17–25.
Books. Zito, J. M., Safer, D. J., Sai, D., Gardner, J. F., Thomas, D., Coombes, P.,
Zigler, E., & Styfco, S. J. (2004). Moving Head Start to the States: Dubowski, M., & Mendez-Lewis, M. (2008). Psychotropic
One experiment too many. Applied Developmental Science, 8, 51–55. medication patterns among youth in foster care. Pediatrics, 121,
Zigler, E., Taussig, C., & Black, K. (1992). Early childhood interven- e157–e163.
tion: A promising preventative for juvenile delinquency. American Zoccolillo, M., Pickles, A., Quinton, D., & Rutter, M. (1992). The
Psychologist, 47, 997–1006. outcome of childhood conduct disorder: Implications for defin-
Zigmond, M., Bloom, F. E., Landis, S. C., Roberts, J. L., & Squire, ing adult personality disorder and conduct disorder. Psychological
L. R. (Eds.). (1999). Fundamental neuroscience. San Diego: Medicine, 22, 971–986.
Academic Press. Zola-Morgan, S., & Squire, L. R. (1993). Neuroanatomy of memory.
Zill, N., Morrison, D. R., & Coiro, M. J. (1993). Long-term effects Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 547–563.
of parental divorce on parent-child relationships, adjustment, and Zurif, E. B. (1998). Aphasia. In G. Adelman & B. Smith (Eds.),
achievement in young adulthood. Journal of Family Psychology, 7, Encyclopedia of neuroscience (2nd ed.). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
91–103. Zurif, E., Swinney, D., & Garrett, M. (1990). Lexical processing and
Zimbardo, P. G. (1977/1990). Shyness: What it is, what to do about it. sentence comprehension. In A. Caramazza (Ed.), Cognitive neu-
Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. ropsychology and neurolinguistics (pp. 123–136). Hillsdale, NJ:
Zimerman, S., Hasher, L., & Goldstein, D. (2011). Cognitive Erlbaum.
aging: A positive perspective. In N. Kapur, A. Pascual-Leone, Zvoch, K. (1999). Family type and investment in education: A com-
V. Ramachandran, J. Cole, S. Della Sala, T. Manly, & A. Mayes parison of genetic and stepparent families. Evolution and Human
(Eds.), The paradoxical brain (pp. 130–150). New York: Cambridge Behavior, 20, 463–464.
University Press. Zweigenhaft, R. L., & von Ammon, J. (2000). Birth order and civil
Zimiles, H., & Lee, V. (1991). Adolescent family structure and educa- disobedience: A test of Sulloway’s “born to rebel” hypothesis.
tional progress. Developmental Psychology, 27, 314–320. Journal of Social Psychology, 140, 624–627.
R-140 REFERENCES
Credits and Acknowledgments
Title page photo: Kim Kozlowski Photography, LLC/Getty Image Karel Navarro; 52 P Barber/Getty Images; 54 © Bettmann/CORBIS;
61 (right) © Jürgen Löschinger/Courtesy Dr. Hans Meinhardt; 65
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J.N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K.M., Greenstein, D.,
Chapter 1 Vaituzis, C., Nugent, T.F., Herman, D.H., Classen, L., Toga, A.W.,
Photos: 1 © PhotoAlto/Alamy; 2 © Jacob Maentz/Corbis; 3 © Rapoport, J.L., Thompson, P.M. (2004). Dynamic Mapping of Human
Rubberball/Rubberball/Corbis; 5 Photoshot Holdings Ltd/Alamy; Cortical Development during Childhood Through Early Adulth; 70
6 Library of Congress; 7 © Andrew Spencer; 9 (top left) Arne Heritability of gray matter figure from Genetics of Brain Structure
Pastoor/Getty Images; (top center) © Mina Chapman/Corbis; and Intelligence. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 1–23. Toga AW,
(top right) Cleve Bryant/Photo Edit; (bottom left) © Purestock/ Thompson PM (2005)
Alamy; (bottom center) Konrad Wothe/Getty Images; (bottom
right) Romana Chapman/Getty Images; 10 Reproduced with Drawn art: Figure 2.29: From “Genes, interactions, and development,”
permission, Arrested development in Xenopus laevis tadpoles: How by Johnston, T.D. & Edwards, L., Psychological Review, 109, pp. 26–34
size constrains metamorphosis. Journal of Experimental Biology, (© 2002). Reprinted by permission of the American Psychological
207, 2133–2145. Irena Rot-Nikcevic* and Richard J. Wassersug. Association.
The Journal of Experimental Biology: jeb.biolo; 15 (left) © Fine
Art Images/SuperStock ; (center) The Granger Collection, NYC;
Chapter 3
(right) With kind permission of the University of Edinburgh/
The Bridgeman Art Library; 16 (left) © Getty Images; (center) Photos: 75 © Florian Franke/Corbis; 76 © moodboard/Alamy; 77
Nick Koudis/Getty Images; (right) Per Breiehagen/Getty Images; Photo courtesy of Pawan Sinha; 82 Courtesy of Ms. Marcela Salamanca &
17 (top) © age fotostock/SuperStock; (bottom left) © KidStock/Blend Dr. Donald Kline, University Of Calgary; 90 Mark Richards/
Images/Corbis; (bottom center) © Mitsuaki Iwago/Minden Pictures/ Photo Edit; 93 (left) Photofees/Getty Images; (right) Patricia Ware/
Corbis; (bottom right) © NHPA/SuperStock; 18 (left) Independent Getty Images; 99 Langlois Social Development Lab; 103 Courtesy
Picture Service/Alamy; (center) Alamy; (right) Karsten Schoene/Laif/ Professor Eric Knudsen/Stanford University School Of Medicine;
Redux ; 19 Dr. April Benasich/Infancy Studies Laboratory/Center for 106 JOE KLAMAR/AFP/Getty Images; 108 (top row) Differential
Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers University; 20 (left) © facial responses to four basic tastes in newborns. Rosenstein D, Oster
Ellen B. Senisi/The Image Works; (right) Stephanie Rausser/Getty H. Child Dev. 1988 Dec;59(6):1555–68.; (bottom row) Flavor
Images; 21 Courtesy Leslie Fabello/Babylab at Birkbeck, University Programming During Infancy. Julie A. Mennella, Cara E. Griffin, Gary K.
of London; 22 Nick Lacy/Eastern Connecticut State University Beauchamp
Drawn art: Figure 3.17: From “Discrimination of possible and
impossible objects in infancy,” by Shuwairi, S. M., Albert, M. K., &
Chapter 2 Johnson, S. P., Psychological Science, 18, pp. 303–307 (© 2007).
Photos: 33 Paul Thompson/Judy Rapoport - UCLA/NIMH; 34 (right) Reprinted by permission of the Association for Psychological Science.
Courtesy A. Hayashi/Loyola University Health System; (left) Courtesy Figure 3.20: From “Infant perception of object unity from translatory
of Loyola University Medical Center; 42 (left) James Cavallini/ motion in depth and vertical translation,” by Kellman, P. J., Spelke, E. S.,
Custom Medical Stock Photo/Newscom; (right) Lauren Shear/ & Short, K. R., Child Development, 57, pp. 72-86 (© 1986). Reprinted
Photo Researchers, Inc.; 43 D. Phillips/Science Photo Library/Photo by permission of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. All rights reserved. Figure
Researchers Inc.; 46 (top left) Dr. Yorgos Nikas/www.eikonika.net; 3.22: From “Newborns discriminate schematic faces from scrambled
(bottom left) ANATOMICAL TRAVELOGUE/SCIENCE PHOTO faces,” by Easterbrook, M. A., Kisilevsky, B. S., Muir, D. W., &
LIBRARY; (bottom center) Science Source/Photo Researchers Inc.; Laplante, D. P., Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53,
(bottom right) Edelmann | Science Photo Library/visualphotos; 47 pp. 231–241 (© 1999). Reprinted by permission of the American
(top left) Anatomical Travelogue/Photo Researchers, Inc.; (top center) Psychological Association. Figure 3.32: From “Intermodal matching by
MedicalRF.com/Visuals Unlimited, Inc.; (top right) Ralph Hutchings/ human neonates,” by Meltzoff, A. N., & R. W. Borton. Nature, 282,
Visuals Unlimited, Inc.; (bottom left) Nestle/Petit Format/Photo pp. 403–404 (© 1979). Copyright © 1979, Nature Publishing Group,
Researchers, Inc.; (bottom center) James Stevenson/Photo Researchers, a division of Macmillan Publishers Ltd. All rights reserved. Reprinted
Inc.; (bottom right) Petit Format/Photo Researchers, Inc.; 48 AP Photo/ with permission.
C-1
Chapter 4 Chapter 6
Photos: 115 Dan Heller; 117 (left) Movies from the NeuroLogic Exam Photos: 187 Alamy; 188 KAREN PULFER FOCHT/The Commercial
and PediNeuroLogic Exam websites are used by permission of Paul D. Appeal/Landov; 189 Danita Delimont/Alamy; 190 PhotoStock-Israel/
Larsen, M.D., University of Nebraska Medical Center and Suzanne S. Photo Edit; 192 (left) Courtesy of Sir Richard Bowlby; (right) © Dr.
Stensaas, Ph.D., University of Utah School of Medicine; (second from Patricia Crittenden; 193 (left) J.-L. Klein & M.-L. Hubert; 193 (center)
left) Spencer Grant/Science Source; (center) Petit Format/Science imagebroker/Alamy; (right) Jonathan Hewitt/Alamy; 197 (left) Tier und
Source; (second from right) BURGER/PHANIE/Science Source; Naturfotografie/SuperStock; (center) DanitaDelimont.com/Newscom;
(right) Catchlight Visual Services/Alamy; 118 Michael Newman/Photo (right) © O. Alamany & E. Vicens/Corbis; 198 Glocker, M. L.,
Edit; 120 (left) Michael Poehlman/Getty Images; (right) The Granger Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J.W.,Valdez, J. N., Griffin, M. C.,
Collection, New York; 125 von Hofsten, C.. (1983). Catching skills Sachser, N., & Gur, R. C. (2009). Baby schema modulates the brain
in infancy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and reward system in nulliparous women; 199 (top) © Ed Tronick/
Performance, 9, 75–85.; 127 © William Vandivert/Estate of William University Distinguished Professor, Department of Psychology,
Vandivert; 128 (top row) Libertus, K., & Needham, A. (2010). Teach University of Massachusetts, Boston Research Associate and Lecturer,
to reach: The effects of active versus passive reaching experiences on Department of Newborn Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital,
action and perception. Vision Research, 50, 2750–2757; 129 Courtesy Harvard Medical School; 200 (right) Philip and Karen Smith/Getty
Bennett I. Bertenthal,Professor of Psychological and Brain Sciences Images; 202 Dr. Robert Marvin; 205 Sozaijiten/Datacraft/Getty Images;
Indiana University; 131 Courtesy of Andrew Meltzoff; 132 © 2009 The 207 (left) © Mike Kemp/Tetra Images/Corbis; (center) © Sarah
Royal Society; 133 Neonatal Imitation in Rhesus Macaques. Ferrari PF, Monte/Westend61/Corbis; (right) © Alan Graf/cultura/Corbis; 212
Visalberghi E, Paukner A, Fogassi L, Ruggiero A, et al. (2006) Neonatal Jim West/Photo Edit; 215 ©Josef Polleross/The Image Works; 216
Imitation in Rhesus Macaques. PLoS Biol 4(9): e302. doi:10.1371/ (left) ullstein bild/The Granger Collection; (right) The Archive of The
journal.pbio.0040302; 135 Brooklyn Museum of Art, New York, USA/ Anna Freud Centre; 219 (left) Harlow Primate Laboratory, University
Museum Collection Fund/The Bridgeman Art Library; 136 Time & Life of Wisconsin-Madison; 220–222 Harlow Primate Laboratory,
Pictures/Getty Image; 138 Newborn infants learn during sleep. Fifer University of Wisconsin-Madison
WP, Byrd DL, Kaku M, Eigsti IM, Isler JR, Grose-Fifer J, Tarullo AR,
Drawn art: Figure 6.21b: Figure 6 from “The Nature of Love,” by
Balsam PD.; 139 Courtesy of Center for the History of Psychology at
Harry F. Harlow, 1958. First published in American Psychologist, 13,
The University of Akron; 140 Science Source/Photo Researchers Inc.;
573–685. Reprinted with permission.
141 (left) © Andy Gehrig /istockphoto; (right) © studio67/Alamy
Drawn art: Figure 4.8: From Growth, by Robbins, W. J., Brody, S., Hogan,
A. G., Jackson, C. M., & Greene, C. W. New Haven: Yale University Chapter 7
Press (1928). Reprinted by permission of Yale University Press. Figure Photos: 227 © Adrianna Williams/Corbis; 229 © English Heritage
4.13: From “Teach to reach: The effects of active versus passive reaching Images; 232 (top left) Pascal Genest/Getty Images; (top center) ©
experiences on action and perception,” by Libertus, K., & Needham, A. Adam Gault/Alamy; (top right) Barbara Taeger Photography/Getty
Vision Research, 50, pp. 2750–2757 (© 2010). Reprinted by permission Images; (bottom left) Eric Bean/Getty Images; (bottom center) ©
of Elsevier, Ltd. All rights reserved. Figure 4.23: From “Newborn infants Dzianis | Dreamstime.com; (bottom right) Beyond/SuperStock;
learn during sleep,” by Fifer, W. P., Byrd, D. L., Kaku, M., Eigsti, I., 241 Pollak, S. D., Messner, M., Kistler, D. J., & Cohn, J. F. (2009).
Isler, J. R., Grose-Fifer, J., et al. Proceedings of the National Academy Development of perceptual expertise in emotion recognition.
of Sciences of the USA, 107, pp. 10320–10323 (© 2010). Reprinted by Cognition, 110, 242–247; 242 (left) © Leila Cutler/Alamy; (right) ©
permission of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. Franco Vogt/Corbis; 243 (left) Stephanie A. Richer; (right) © Stock
Connection Blue/Alamy; 246 (top) From LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S.
Chapter 5 (2008). Detecting the Snake in the Grass: Attention to fear-relevant
stimuli by adults and young children. Psychological Science, 19, 284–289;
Photos: 145 Jessica Lewis/Flickr/Getty Images; 147 ©Archives Jean
(bottom left) Adam Jones/Science Source/Photo Researchers; (bottom
Piaget, Geneva; 148 (center) Raia/Shutterstock; (right) Rubberball/Mike
right) © Derek Croucher/Alamy; 247 (left) © Jon Feingersh Photography/
Kemp/Getty; 151 (left) Bubbles Photolibrary/Alamy; (center) PhotoAlto/
SuperStock/Corbis; (center) David Young-Wolff/Photo Edit; (right) Ryan
Ale Ventura/Getty; (right) Bubbles Photolibrary/Alamy; 152 (left) Marc
Mcvay/Getty Images; 251 (left) Fuse/Getty Images; (right) Valerie Shaff/
Oeder/STOCK4B/Getty Images; (right) Stephanie Deissner/Getty
Getty Images
Images; 156 Topál, J., Gergely, G., Miklósi, Á., Erd_hegyi, Á., & Csibra, G.
(2008). Infants’ perseverative search errors are induced by pragmatic Drawn art: Figure 7.4: From “Natural Selection and the Elusiveness
misinterpretation. Science, 321, 1831–1834; 163 Hespos, S. J., Ferry, A. L., & of Happiness,” by Nesse, R. M. Philosophical Transactions of the
Rips, L. J. (2009). Five-month-old infants have different expectations Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 359, p. 1341 (©
for solids and liquids. Psychological Science, 20, 603–611; 171 Kayte 2004). Reprinted by permission of the Royal Society. Figure 7.5:
Deioma/Photo Edit; 173 Hunnius, S., & Bekkering, H. (2010). The early From “Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds,” by
development of object knowledge: A study of infants’ visual anticipations Kuhlmeier, V., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. Psychological Science, 14,
during action observation. Developmental Psychology, 46, 446–454; 174 pp. 402–408 (© 2003). Reprinted by permission of the Association
Courtesy of Dr. J. M. Mandler; 177 Courtesy of Susan Johnson for Psychological Science.
N-1
Bauer, P. J., 347, 348, 349, 355 Bhugra, D., 615 Bos, H., 531 Brown, R., 263, 267, 269, 553
Baumeister, R. F., 475, 479, 550 Bialystok, E., 296, 297 Bos, K. J., 218, 475 Brown, R. T., 403
Baumrind, D., 506, 507 Bianchi, S. M., 515, 529, 532 Bos, P. A., 591 Browne, A., 537
Bavelier, D., 63, 569 Bickerton, D., 290 Bostic, J. Q., 599 Brownell, C., 546, 548, 555,
Bayley, N., 118, 396 Bidell, T. R., 313 Bouchard, T. J., Jr., 392, 399 557
Baylies, M. K., 40 Biederman, J., 366 Boucher, B. H., 137 Brownell, K. D., 598
Beaton, A. E., 378 Bierman, K. L., 557 Bouma, G., 263 Bruck, M., 360
Beauchamp, G. K., 107, 109 Bigi, L., 362 Bourne, E. J., 441 Bruder, C. E., 43
Beck, A. T., 620 Bigler, R. S., 553, 576, 577, 580 Bouvier, P., 537 Bruer, J. T., 181, 415
Becker, A., 569 Binet, A., 387 Bowden, C. L., 607 Brumariu, L. E., 210, 593
Beckers, T., 620 Birch, S. A., 493 Bower, T. G. R., 95, 155 Brumback, R. A., 366
Beckett, K., 40 Birch, S. A. J., 489 Bowlby, J., 192, 193, 208, 222 Brumberg, J. J., 599
Bedford, F., 127 Birdsong, D., 283 Bowman, L. L., 277 Bruner, J., 200, 264
Bednar, D. E., 508 Biro, S ., 182 Boyatzis, R. E., 390 Bruner, J. S., 338
Beers, L., 526 Bisanz, J., 413 Boykin, A. W., 410, 411 Brush, T., 418
Beilock, S., 405 Bishop, D. V. M., 39, 286 Boynton, R. M., 84, 85 Bryan, W. L., 635
Beitchman, J. H., 537 Bjerkedal, T., 518 Boysen, S. T., 471 Bryant, P., 314, 315, 318, 374
Bekkering, H., 172, 173 Bjork, E. L., 155 Brackett, M. A., 395 Bryant, S. L., 494
Bell, M. A., 348, 481 Bjorklund, A., 66 Braddick, O., 101 Bryant-Waugh, R., 598
Bell, R. Q., 511 Bjorklund, D. F., 350, 352, 536 Bradshaw, K., 457 Buchanan, C. M., 521, 534,
Bell, S. M., 192 Bjorklund, F., 247, 446 Brady, E. U., 592 605
Bellgrove, M. A., 365 Black, J. E., 63, 64 Braine, M. D. S., 267 Buckner, J. C., 453
Bellugi, U., 265, 285, 286, 288 Black, S. E., 518, 528 Brainerd, C., 313 Bugental, D. B., 463, 512, 513
Belmont, M., 408 Blades, M., 348 Brainerd, C. J., 360, 361 Buhrmester, D., 547
Belsky, J., 204, 212, 511, 529 Blair, C., 392, 481 Bramlett, M. D., 532 Buhrmester, M. D., 475
Beltz, A. M., 516, 517 Blair, K. S., 610 Branchi, I., 513 Bukowski, W., 548
Bem, D. J., 441 Blair, R. J. R., 455, 456, 610, Brannock, J., 312 Bukowski, W. M., 608
Bem, S. L., 515 612, 613, 614 Brannon, E. M., 167, 169 Bulik, C. M., 601
Benenson, J. F., 547 Blais, J. J., 572, 573, 574 Brans, R. G., 399 Bull, R., 401
Bengtsson, S. L., 64 Blakemore, S. J., 62 Bransford, J. D., 419 Bullock, M., 335
Benjet, C., 460 Blanchard-Fields, F., 639 Brassard, M. R., 537 Bumpass, L. L., 532, 535
Bennett, A. J., 221 Blank, M., 318 Brauer, J., 486 Bumpus, M. F., 509
Benson, J. B., 165 Blascovich, J., 474 Braungart, J. M., 239 Burchinal, M. R., 529
Bent-Goodley, T. B., 538 Blass, E., 107 Braungart-Rieker, J. M., 204 Burdge, G. C., 40
Bentley, A., 509 Blatt, S. J., 605 Brendgen, M., 454 Bureau of Labor Statistics, 529
Berchtold, N. C., 640 Bleakley, A., 569 Brennan, P., 615 Burgess, N., 322
Berenbaum, S. A., 516 Bleuler, E., 614, 615 Brent, D. A., 621, 623 Burke, P. B., 207
Berenthal, B. I., 122, 124, Bloch, M. H., 595, 596, 597 Brent, M., 273 Burn, J., 54
128, 129 Bloechlinger, S., 66 Brentari, D., 284 Bushman, B. J., 564, 565, 566,
Bergan, J. F., 103 Bloom, L., 265 Bretherton, I., 194 567, 571
Berger, C., 551 Bloom, P., 5, 235, 246, 273, Brewer, M. B., 494 Bushnell, I. W. R., 96
Berkeley, G., 88, 123 278, 283, 293, 299, 379, Brewer, S., 40 Buss, A. H., 248
Berko-Gleason, J., 266 447, 487, 489, 492, 493 Brewer, W. F., 336, 369, 370 Butler, A. C., 620
Berlin, B., 85, 327 Blossfeld, I., 108 Brickman, D., 553 Butler, R., 553
Berlin, L. J., 593 Blumberg, H. P., 609 Bridges, K. M., 230 Buttelman, D., 484
Bernal, S., 158 Blumenthal, A. L., 139 Broadbent, D., 359 Butterfill, S., 491
Berndt, T. J., 548 Bodemer, D., 419 Brody, G. H., 521 Butterworth, B., 169, 325
Berninger, G., 298 Boden, M. A., 157 Bronfenbrenner, U., 441, 510 Butzin, C. A., 436
Bernstein, G. A., 592 Bogdan, R., 222 Bronson, G. W., 231
Bernstein, N., 120 Bohannon, J. N., 267 Bronstein, P., 205 Cahan, S., 413
Berscheid, E., 555 Bokhorst, C. L., 207 Brooks-Gunn, J., 471, 505, Cairns, R. B., 557
Bertamini, M., 335 Boland, A. M., 356 510, 605 Calamari, J. E., 596
Bertelsen, A., 616 Bonatti, L., 174 Brossard, L. M., 140 Calders, Y. M., 516
Bertin, E., 95, 199, 469 Boney-McCoy, S., 537 Brotman, M. A., 609 Caldi, S., 174
Best, J. R., 361, 363 Boo, K., 51 Brower, T., 110 Caldji, C., 512
Betancur, C., 588 Booth, A., 534 Brown, A. A., 171 Calkins, S. D., 207, 238, 239
Bethea, C. L., 221 Borer, H., 283 Brown, A. L., 306, 367, 368, Call, J., 182, 289, 485
Bettelheim, B., 591 Bornstein, D., 410, 411 413, 415 Callender, W. M., 216
Beugnon, G., 170 Bornstein, M. H., 85, 174, Brown, A. M., 88 Callistel, C. R., 168
Bezdjian, S., 613 245, 397 Brown, A. S., 618 Calvert, C., 571
Bhatt, R. S., 95, 96 Boroditsky, L., 292 Brown, B. B., 508, 554, 623 Calvert, S. L., 562
Bherer, L., 640 Borton, R. W., 110 Brown, P., 298 Camara, K. A., 534