Annual Reviews Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics
Annual Reviews Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics
Annual Reviews Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics
Author(s): M. P. Austin
Source: Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, Vol. 16 (1985), pp. 39-61
Published by: Annual Reviews
Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/2097042
Accessed: 28-01-2019 19:30 UTC
REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
https://www.jstor.org/stable/2097042?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms
Annual Reviews is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Annual
Review of Ecology and Systematics
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 1985. 16:39-61
Copyright (? 1985 by Annual Reviews Inc. All rights reserved
CONTINUUM CONCEPT,
ORDINATION METHODS,
AND NICHE THEORY
M. P. Austin
INTRODUCTION
Major similarities exist between the role of the continuum concept in plant
community ecology and that of niche theory in animal community ecology.
Identical techniques have been used to study each idea. This article reviews the
adequacy of the current use of the continuum concept and associated ordination
techniques and briefly discusses the implications for niche studies.
The concept of vegetation as a continuum with a changing species composi-
tion along environmental gradients arose in antithesis to the community-unit
theory which stated that plant communities are natural units of coevolved
species populations forming homogeneous, discrete, and recognizable units.
Goodall (49) provides a very clear statement of the issues. Two reviews (76,
117) and responses (29) summarize the controversy well. Ecologists now
accept the continuum concept and incorporate it into textbooks (e. g. 68, 81, 97,
118), though questions remain as to its adequacy (e.g. 68).
The need for quantitative procedures for examining vegetation as a con-
tinuum (27, 114) prompted development of ordination techniques. Goodall
(48) introduced the term "ordination" for methods that arrange samples (or
species) in relation to "a multidimensional series." Excellent reviews survey the
history of ordination techniques and the theory and application of current
methods (37, 54, 119). However, problems of interpreting the results of these
methods continue, as well as questions about whether their mathematical
assumptions are compatible with ecological theory (8, 87).
39
0066-4162/85/1120-0039$02.00
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
40 AUSTIN
CONTINUUM CONCEPT
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CONTINUUM, ORDINATION, AND NICHE 41
Current Issues
The modern argument for the existence of the continuum is presented graphical-
ly (118) for a single environmental gradient (Figure la, b). The community-unit
(Figure la) is composed of dominant species, e.g. trees, plus other species
(shrubs, herbs) that through selection have adapted to live in association with
the dominants and with each other. Competition between dominants results in
sharp boundaries between the communities. The individualistic continuum
(Figure lb) has neither sharp boundaries between species nor well-defined
groups of species with similar distributions. In contrast to Whittaker (118),
Gleason in his original paper (45) made almost no mention of competition as
determining species distribution.
Resource partitioning and associated competition, as usually considered in
niche studies (94), would be expected to lead to an even distribution of species
along an environmental gradient (Figure ic). If species belong to different
strata or functional guilds, then each stratum may partition the gradient in-
dependently of the other strata (Figure Id). With more than three independent
strata, such an arrangement would be difficult to distinguish from the in-
dividualistic continuum (Figure lb).
Whittaker examined his data for evidence of environmental niche partition-
ing (120; Gauch & Whittaker 39). He suggested that where species are domi-
nant, few in number, and close ecological equivalents to each other, they may
be regularly distributed along an environmental gradient. He quoted an ex-
ample of a replacement series for coniferous tree species in relation to elevation
on north-facing slopes in the Santa Catalina and Pinaleno Mountains, Arizona.
Where the species departed from even partitioning of the elevational gradient,
they were distinguishable on a second environmental gradient, "topographic
moisture" (120, 122).
The current statement of the continuum concept supported by the "Cornell
school" (37, 118, 119) is set out in a paper published in 1972 (39). Gauch &
Whittaker (39) put forward nine propositions regarding species behavior along
environmental gradients. The propositions, based on an informal graphical
examination of vegetation data, without any statistical analysis, were used in an
algorithm for generating artificial data sets for comparative studies of ordina-
tion methods. The two major propositions are: 1. Species response curves
approximate normal (Gaussian) curves, i.e. symmetric bell-shaped curves
similar in appearance to a normal error probability distribution function, and 2.
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
42 AUSTIN
(a)
C.)
(C)
Environmental gradient
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CONTINUUM, ORDINATION, AND NICHE 43
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
44 AUSTIN
Present Position
No formal statistical or experimental tests of either the continuum concept or
the individualistic hypothesis have been attempted (8, 37, 39, 48, 54, 119).
Acceptance appears to have been based on accumulating circumstantial evi-
dence provided by ordination methods (15, 26, 39, 76, 90, 91, 117). The
individualistic hypothesis has been qualified by Gauch & Whittaker's (39)
proposition that major species are regularly distributed along environmental
gradients (120).
A continuing source of difficulty in testing these models will be the nature of
the gradient used to examine the species response. Is it appropriate to use a
compositional gradient based on multivariate ordination methods the ecological
meaning of which may be obscure? What credence can be given to an environ-
mental gradient when its relevance to the growth of the species concerned is
unlikely to be causal or even remotely related to the proximate factors (8)? If an
axiomatic definition of an appropriate environmental gradient and the units in
which it is to be measured are provided, then each of the propositions (39) can
potentially be falsified. The continuum concept makes far fewer assumptions
about species behavior than does the community concept, but it requires
additional assumptions about environment.
The continuum concept has played an important part in changing plant
ecologists' perception of vegetation. Modem statements of the concept do not
provide adequate mechanisms to explain the occurrence of a continuum. The
methods quoted to provide evidence for the concept require greater scrutiny.
Higher standards of hypothesis testing, beyond that of graphical interpretation
of one- or two-dimensional gradients are required. Multidimensional analyses
cannot be accepted unless they account for the major proportion of the variation
in species abundance; otherwise there is no guarantee that the dimensions are
relevant to the species populations studied.
ORDINATION METHODS
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CONTINUUM, ORDINATION, AND NICHE 45
Vegetational Ordination
The appeal of indirect compositional techniques is that they make no assump-
tion about the important environmental gradients. The results from these
techniques should indicate graphically which samples occupy the extremes of
the major underlying gradients determining the vegetation composition and
which samples are intermediate. Trial and error plotting of possible environ-
mental variables or use of more explicit statistical techniques may then indicate
which environmental variables are important and are correlated with the com-
positional factors (54).
EARLY ORDINATION METHODS The continuum index method (27) was soon
superseded by the Bray & Curtis (or polar) ordination technique (18), as this
allowed more than a single compositional gradient to be estimated. Early
interpretation of the Wisconsin ordination results (18, 19, 26, 27) did not
clearly distinguish whether the principal variation was due to environmental
factors or whether it was due to successional trends. It was only later, when a
clearer perception of the methodology was available, that Peet & Loucks (93)
were able to show the relative importance of environment and succession in
determining forest composition. This confounding could contribute to the
appearance of a continuum. Controversy has continued over the original Bray-
Curtis or polar ordination method. Beals (17) provides an extensive review,
arguing that the method is still useful. Earlier work (12) that criticized the use of
the Bray-Curtis coefficient and suggested the use of Euclidean distance instead
was incorrect, but other features of the method (e.g. axis positioning) remain
controversial.
Principal components analysis superseded polar ordination because it made
use of all the information contained in the similarity matrix to determine the
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
46 AUSTIN
component axes (12, 89). PCA has since become widely used
method in ecology in areas very different from the vegeta
was originally introduced (48, 54, 89). See Green (52) and Gauch (37) for
examples. The components, axes or dimensions, are orthogonal mathematical
constructs. This does not mean they are necessarily ecologically independent
(4, 13). The orientation of the components is sensitive to the distribution of
samples in the multidimensional species space; thus, there is no necessary
ecological interpretation for components (4, 54).
The mathematical methods used in vegetational ordination imply particular
vegetation models concerning the shape of species response curves. This was
first shown by Swan (108) for Bray-Curtis ordination using artificial data, then
for PCA by Noy-Meir & Austin (86). The problem had previously been
anticipated by Goodall (48). PCA specifically assumes a linear model of
relationships measured in terms of covariance or correlation coefficients. If the
species response curves are bell-shaped as indicated in earlier continuum
studies, then a linear model is inapplicable and a one-dimensional gradient as
shown in Figure 1 becomes distorted into a "horse-shoe" in two or more
dimensions (5, 6, 54, p. 265). This has since been confirmed by many workers
(e.g. 35, 39,42, 87, 121). Some workers (1, 36, 84, see also 54) still claim that
PCA is a viable procedure for vegetation studies. This may be appropriate under
special conditions (13, 87, 99), e.g. for short compositional gradients or for
structural or other attributes which are monotonic along gradients. However,
two- or three-dimensional data using bell-shaped species response curves can
produce complex distorted "flasks" (13, 54, 121) in which the underlying
gradient structure is impossible to recognize without prior knowledge. Thus,
although no assumptions are made about the number or environmental nature of
the underlying gradients, the biological implications of the mathematical model
mean that neither the number nor the orientation of these gradients may be
detectable.
Other linear models-principal coordinate analysis (PCoA) (51), canonical
variates analysis (CVA), and canonical correlation analysis (CCA)-suffer
from analogous problems of curvilinear distortion (4, 38, 42, 67, 121) and other
unrealistic statistical assumptions (125). In a monographic treatment of the use
of CCA in ecology, Gittins (44) has argued that ecological information can be
usefully derived from CCA, even though it is a linear method. Animal ecolo-
gists often use PCA and other linear techniques (22, 52, 65, 101, 102, 106)
without discussion of the ecological implications of the linear model (cf
78, 96). There appears to be no justification for using PCA or Bray-Curtis
ordination in preference to the newer, more robust techniques of detrended
correspondence analysis (DCA) or multidimensional scaling (MDS) (cf 37,
52, 54, 80).
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CONTINUUM, ORDINATION, AND NICHE 47
CORNELL TECHNIQUES Reciprocal averaging (59, 60) and its recent variant,
detrended correspondence analysis (61, 62), are currently the most popular
ordination methods. It has been claimed that DCA constitutes a near-optimal
ordination method (37). RA is equivalent to a principal coordinates analysis of a
X2 distance matrix where sites are weighted according to their totals (20). The
ecological model equivalent to this mathematical formulation is not clear to the
reviewer. Analysis using simulated vegetation data has shown that RA pro-
duces an "arch" in two dimensions when there is a one-dimensional gradient
(42). Mathematically, a polynomial relationship exists between higher di-
mensions (axes) and the first (42, 60). When there exist more than one major
ecological axis, the spurious polynomial axes interact with the ecological axes
and confound interpretation. This is a major weakness of RA.
DCA (37, 61, 62) uses a complex algorithm to remove the expected arch by
detrending, i.e. adjusting the higher axes' scores to an average of zero within
segments of the lower axes (37). The adjustment makes no allowance for
interaction between a two- or three-dimensional set of gradients (coenoplane)
and the mathematical distortion (32). A compression problem associated with
the arch distortion at the ends of gradients (37) is further allowed for in DCA by
assuming the species abundances have certain normal-distribution-like proper-
ties and adjusting their values accordingly, in effect assuming species appear
and disappear at a constant rate along the gradient. Numerous applications of
DCA are now being published (e.g. 104, 112), but further evaluation appears
needed (80).
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
48 AUSTIN
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CONTINUUM, ORDINATION, AND NICHE 49
Environmental Ordination
Whittaker (114, 115, 116, 120, 122) pioneered the direct gradient analysis
(DGA) approach to environmental ordination using elevation and topography.
Austin et al (15) discuss the methodology, indicating the close relationship with
Jenny's soil-factor equation (63). Greig-Smith (54: Ch. 9) reviews many of the
possible methods of vegetation/environment correlation. Because of the restric-
tive mathematical assumptions of multivariate statistical methods (PCA, CVA,
CCA) as they may relate to the ecological behavior of organisms, these methods
will not be considered further here (4, 96, 125).
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
50 AUSTIN
sampling strategy; (b) the rejection rate of samples (see Figure 2 in 120); and (c)
the absence of statistical analysis. Recent improvements have been made. Peet
(90, 91) tested the correlation between the weighted average gradient and
independent measures of topographic position (e.g. potential incident solar
radiation) and found it to be very high. In addition, he specified his sampling
frame and intensity and so ensured that at least one sampling unit occurred in
every cell-2 10-of a three-dimensional table of elevation, topographic posi-
tion, and stand age. Such an experimental design is not explicit in the original
studies (115, 116, 122). Comments (115, p. 58) may be found suggesting
perhaps 13% of samples were excluded from the graphical analysis. Recent
graphical DGA studies (e.g. 66) have presented only response isolines without
scatter-plots showing the distribution of data points on which they are based. It
is impossible to evaluate such graphs.
Austin et al (15) make the point that environmental variables or gradients are
of various kinds: (a) those which have no necessary physiological influence on
plant growth (indirect gradients such as elevation)-correlation of these with
vegetation patterns is likely to be location specific; (b) those which have a
physiological influence (direct gradients) but are not a resource for plant growth
(e.g. pH), and (c) -those (resource gradients) where the environmental variable
can be directly used as a resource by plants (e.g. available soil nitrate). Clearly a
spectrum of gradient types exists. In principle, DGA should be based on the
most proximal variables that can be measured or estimated. Numerous studies
have devised environmental scalars of increasing relevance to plant growth (70,
1 1 1, 124, 127). Attempts to analyze niche relationships with simple, separate,
and indirect environmental variables like slope and aspect are unlikely to be
profitable, particularly when confounded with differences in location (e.g.
126). More attention to environmental processes for estimating environmental
gradients is necessary.
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CONTINUUM, ORDINATION, AND NICHE 51
6-
8-
10 -
12-
6-
C 0-05~~
c 12-
c 14-
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
52 AUSTIN
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CONTINUUM, ORDINATION, AND NICHE 53
The development of the niche concept has been primarily based on birds (22,
65, 103) and lizards (94, 103), while the continuum concept is based on higher
plants. There has been a common methodology (e.g. use of PCA and CCA),
though few studies recognize the commonality (see though 78, 79, 104).
Zoologists continue to accept PCA. Numerous examples could be cited of
workers who interpret PCA axes as having physical meaning and who display
little appreciation of the problems of curvilinear distortion and independence of
components. As Meents et al (78) point out, few ecological reasons lead us to
expect only linear relationships between organisms and features of their en-
vironment. Nonlinear relationships may also occur between habitat measures.
Relationships between organisms may show very different forms of correlation
depending on their overlap along gradients (54).
One interesting example of the problems is provided by Rotenberry & Wiens
(102), who use PCA to examine the niche distribution of bird species in relation
to habitat at a continental scale in steppe vegetation. They apply PCA to a
correlation matrix between various variables measuring the coverage and the
vertical and horizontal structure of vegetation. The relationships between
variables are likely to be at least monotonic, if not linear. However, variables
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
54 AUSTIN
such as the percentage cover of grass, herbs, and litter are necessarily logically
correlated, and the number of variables in each of such correlated groups will
determine the relative magnitude of the components. The choice of relative
numbers of attributes will determine also the relative orientation of the com-
ponents (4, 54). Rotenberry & Wiens (102) argue that the use of continuously
distributed standardized environmental variables largely circumvents
nonlinearity problems. This does not mean that PCA provides ecologically
independent components to which bird species may respond (54).
The authors use linear correlation to examine the relationship between bird
species abundance and the vegetation habitat components. Although some
show strong correlations with the first component, many do not, but examina-
tion of a figure in Rotenberry & Wiens (102: Figure 5, p.2261) shows a striking
sequence of bird distributions along the first vegetation component, a sequence
which Rotenberry & Wiens do not discuss. The sequence of species is very
clear from Dickcissel, with a negative linear response, through truncated
bell-shaped responses, to clear symmetric bell-shaped responses (e.g. Lark
bunting and western meadow-lark occupying central positions along the com-
ponent), to Sage sparrow showing a positive response. In this case, PCA has
proved robust, but the use of the linear correlation coefficient appears to have
limited the interpretation. On the basis of Rotenberry & Wiens' figure, the
hypothesis can be advanced that plots of species abundance in the three major
dimensions of the PCA will show considerable niche partitioning. In this
example, it is possible to make suggestions of alternative interpretations of the
results; in many uncited examples it is not. Where the possibility of curvilinear-
ity is recognized, it may be ascribed to less important components of the
problem. For example, Miles & Ricklefs (79) suggest that curvilinearity is due
to being constrained to sum to a constant, while Dunn & Everitt (31) suggest it
is due to use of presence/absence data for species abundance (cf 54, 78, 96).
In fauna studies, workers are beginning to recognize the role of MDS (96',
113), though the claim that NM-MDS had not yet been applied in quantitative
ecology in 1980 (113) highlights the lack of communication between plant and
animal ecologists. The absence of any mechanistic model for the shape of the
utilization function curve for habitat gradients argues strongly that as few
assumptions as possible should be incorporated into any mathematical analysis
of niche.
Plant ecologists use environmental factor complexes or compositional di-
mensions as their gradients, while animal ecologists usually choose habitat or
food. Plant ecologists tend to ignore functional differences between species
(e.g. tree, shrub, and herbs), and hence often adopt Gleason's individualistic
hypothesis; many animal ecologists, by concentrating on guilds, emphasize
resource partitioning and competition. In many continental scale comparisons
of niche structure (22, 94, 102), the changes in habitat gradients (e.g. shrub
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CONTINUUM, ORDINATION, AND NICHE 55
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
56 AUSTIN
FUTURE PROSPECTS
Observational Analysis
Explicit hypotheses regarding the patterns and shapes of species response to
distal environmental gradients can be derived from the propositions of Gauch &
Whittaker (39) regarding the continuum concept. These can now be tested
statistically with the use of DGA and new statistical procedures such as GLM
(15). The possibility exists that a major environmental variable has been
missed, however. Exploratory techniques, and ordination in particular, will
then be needed to examine whether major axes of variation correspond to DGA
gradients used. Two alternative developments for exploratory ordination exist
at present: to develop (a) methods robust to differences in the underlying
models or (b) methods based on an explicit model of vegetation response.
Controversy exists over whether either DCA or NM-MDS is a sufficiently
robust solution to the first option at present; the second option will not be viable
until we have more conclusive information about species responses.
Theoretical Developments
Vegetation science has no theoretical basis at present, and a satisfactory
synthesis of continuum ideas and niche theory is urgently needed, as are
mechanistic models of how plant species behave along environmental gra-
dients. These models should define a species' fundamental niche and show how
species physiology determines the shape of this niche. Only then will it be
possible to discriminate between models of niche overlap and packing based on
competition, predation, or stochasticity. A possible null model for species is
that the fundamental niche remains unchanged in the presence of other species.
If the controlling influence of competition varies along environmental gra-
dients, more elaborate theories and null models will be required.
Experimental Analysis
In principle, Ellenberg (32, 33) demonstrated an experimental approach to
community ecology incorporating environmental gradients. In practice, the
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CONTINUUM, ORDINATION, AND NICHE 57
Niche Theory
Those plant ecologists whose interests are in community analysis and the
hypotheses associated with the as yet unproven continuum concept are studying
topics very similar to those of animal ecologists interested in niche theory.
Communication and methodological problems are not the only difficulties
inhibiting a synthesis of ideas. Plant ecologists have expressed their hypotheti-
cal patterns in terms of distal factors (e.g. mean annual rainfall) that determine
the broad properties and dynamics of an ecosystem. Niche ecologists have been
more concerned with proximal factors of food, competition, and predation.
Integration of these differing levels of abstraction will be necessary before any
theoretical communication will be possible.
CONCLUSION
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
58 AUSTIN
Literature Cited
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CONTINUUM, ORDINATION. AND NICHE 59
32. Ellenberg, H. 1953. Physiologisches und glesey. II. Experimental studies. J. Ecol.
6kologisches Verhalten derselben Pflan- 61:819-30
zenarten. Ber. Dtsch. Bot. Ges. 65:35 1- 48. Goodall, D. W. 1954. Objective methods
62 for the classification of vegetation. III.
33. Ellenberg, H. 1954. Uber einige Fort- An essay in the use of factor analysis.
schritte der kausalen Vegetationskunde. Aust. J. Bot. 2:304-24
Vegetatio 5/6:199-211 49. Goodall, D. W. 1963. The continuum
34. Ernst, W. 1978. Discrepancy between and the individualistic association. Vege-
ecological and physiological optima of tatio 11:297-316
plant species: A re-interpretation. Oecol. 50. Goodall, D. W. 1970. Statistical plant
Plant. 13:175-88 ecology. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 1:99-
35. Fasham, M. J. R. 1977. A comparison of 124
nonmetric multidimensional scaling, 51. Gower, J. C. 1966. Some distance prop-
principal components and reciprocal erties of latent root and vector methods
averaging for the ordination of simulated used in multivariate analysis. Biometrika
coenoclines and coenoplanes. Ecology 53:325-38
58:551-61 52. Green, R. H. 1979. Sampling Design and
36. Feoli, E., Feoli-Chiapella, L. 1980. Statistical Methods for Environmental
Evaluation of ordination methods Biologists. New York: Wiley
through simulated coenoclines: some 53. Green, R. H. 1980. Multivariate
comments. Vegetatio 42:35-41 approaches in ecology: The assessment
37. Gauch, H. G. Jr. 1982. Multivariate of ecologic similarity. Ann. Rev. Ecol.
Analysis in Community Ecology. Cam- Syst. 11:1-14
bridge: Cambridge Univ. Press 54. Greig-Smith, P. 1983. Quantitative
38. Gauch, H. G., Wentworth, T. R. 1976. Plant Ecology. Oxford: Blackwell. 3rd
Canonical correlation analysis as an ed.
ordination technique. Vegetatio 33:17- 55. Grime, J. P. 1973. Competitive exclu-
22 sion in herbaceous vegetation. Nature
39. Gauch, H. G., Whittaker, R. H. 1972a. 242:344-47
Coenocline simulation. Ecology 53:446- 56. Grime, J. P. 1979. Plant Strategies and
51 Vegetation Processes. Chichester: Wiley
40. Gauch, H. G., Whittaker, R. H. 1972b. 57. Grubb, P. J. 1977. The maintenance of
Comparison of ordination techniques. species-richness in plant communities:
Ecology 53:868-75 the importance of the regeneration niche.
41. Gauch, H. G., Whittaker, R. H., Singer, Biol. Rev. 52:107-45
S. B. 1981. A comparative study of 58. Harper, J. L. 1977. Population Biology
nonmetric ordinations. J. Ecol. 69:135- of Plants. London: Academic
52 59. Hill, M. 0. 1973. Reciprocal averaging:
42. Gauch, H. G., Whittaker, R. H., Went- an eigenvector method of ordination. J.
worth, T. R. 1977. A comparative study Ecol. 61:237-49
of reciprocal averaging and other ordina- 60. Hill, M. 0. 1974. Correspondence analy-
tion techniques. J. Ecol. 65:157-74 sis: A neglected multivariate method. J.
43. Gittins, R. T. 1963. A study of the struc- R. Stat. Soc. Ser. C. 23:340-54
ture of some grassland and heath com- 61. Hill, M. 0. 1979. DECORANA -A FOR-
munities in Anglesey. PhD thesis. Univ. TRAN Program for detrended Corre-
Wales, Bangor spondence Analysis and Reciprocal
44. Gittins, R. 1979. Ecological applications Averaging. Ithaca, NY: Cornell Univ.
of canonical analysis. In Multivariate 62. Hill, M. O., Gauch, H. G. 1980. De-
Methods in Ecological Work, eds. L. trended Correspondence Analysis, an im-
Orloci, C. R. Rao, W. M. Stiteler. Stat. proved ordination technique. Vegetatio
Ecol. No. 7, pp. 309-535. Fairland, Md: 42:47-58
Int. Co-op. 63. Jenny, H. 1980. The Soil Resource: Ori-
45. Gleason, H. A. 1926. The individualistic gin and Behavior. Ecological Studies:
concept of the plant association. Bull. Analysis and Prediction. No. 37. New
Torrey. Bot. Club 53:1-20 York: Springer-Verlag
46. Goldsmith, F. B. 1973a. The vegetation 64. Johnson, R. W., Goodall, D. W. 1982.
of exposed sea cliffs at South Stack, An- Non-linear ordination in several di-
glesey, I. The multivariate approach. J. mensions a maximum likelihood ap-
Ecol. 61:787-818 proach. Vegetatio 48:197-208
47. Goldsmith, F. B. 1973b. The vegetation 65. Karr, J. R., James, F. C. 1975. Ecomor-
of exposed sea cliffs at South Stack, An- phological configurations and con-
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
60 AUSTIN
vergent evolution. See Ref. 23, pp. 258- Field Plateau, Tasmania. PhD thesis.
291 Univ. Tasmania, Hobart
66. Kessell, S. R. 1979. Gradient Model- 81. Mueller-Dombois, D., Ellenberg, H.
ling. New York: Springer-Verlag 1974. Aims and Methods of Vegetation
67. Kessell, S. R., Whittaker, R. H. 1976. Ecology. New York: Wiley
Comparisons of three ordination tech- 82. Nelder, J. A., Wedderburn, R. W. M.
niques. Vegetatio 32:21-29 1972. Generalized linear models. J. R.
68. Krebs, C. J. 1978. Ecology: the Ex- Stat. Soc. Ser. A. 135:370-84
perimental Analysis of Distribution and 83. Newman, E. I. 1973. Competition and
Abundance. New York: Harper, Row. diversity in herbaceous vegetation. Na-
2nd ed. ture 244:310
69. Levins, R., Lewontin, R. 1980. Di- 84. Nichols, S. 1977. On the interpretation of
alectics and reductionism in ecology. In principal components analysis in ecologi-
Conceptual Issues in Ecology, ed. E. cal contexts. Vegetatio 34:191-97
Saarinen, pp. 107-38. Dordrecht: Reidel 85. Noy-Meir, 1. 1974. Multivariate analysis
70. Loucks, 0. L. 1962. Ordinating forest of semi-arid vegetation in south-eastern
communities by means of environmental Australia. II. Vegetation catenae and en-
scalars and phytosociological indices. vironmental gradients. Aust. J. Bot. 22:
Ecol. Monogr. 32:137-66 115-40
71. MacArthur, R. 1971. Patterns of terres-86. Noy-Meir, I., Austin, M. P. 1970. Prin-
trial bird communities. In Avian Biology, cipal component ordination and sim-
Vol. 1, ed. D. S. Farner, J. R. King, K. ulated vegetational data. Ecology
C. Parkes. New York: Academic 51:551-2
72. Magdych, W. P. 1984. Salinity stress 87. Noy-Meir, I., Whittaker, R. H. 1978.
along a complex river continuum: effects Recent developments in continuous mul-
on mayfly (Ephemeroptera) distribu- tivariate techniques. See Ref. 119, pp.
tions. Ecology 65:1662-72 337-78
73. Mathews, J. A. 1978. An application of 88. Oksanen, J. 1983. Ordination of boreal
non-metric multidimensional scaling to heath-like vegetation with principal com-
the construction of an improved species ponents analysis, correspondence anal-
plexus. J. Ecology 66:157-173 ysis and multidimensional scaling. Vege-
74. Mathews, J. A. 1979. The vegetation of tatio 52:181-89
the Storbreen gletschervorfeld, Jotunhei- 89. Orloci, L. 1966. Geometric models in
men, Norway. II. Approaches involving ecology. 1. The theory and application of
ordination and general conclusions. J. some ordination techniques. J. Ecol.
Biogeogr. 6:133-67 54:193-215
75. May, R. M. 1981. Theoretical Ecology, 90. Peet, R. K. 1978a. Latitudinal variation
Principles and Applications. Oxford: in southern Rocky Mountain forests. J.
Blackwell. 2nd ed. Biogeogr. 5:275-89
76. McIntosh, R. P. 1967. The continuum 91. Peet, R. K. 1978b. Forest vegetation of
concept of vegetation. Bot. Rev. 33:130- the Colorado Front Range: patterns of
87 species diversity. Vegetatio 37:65-78
77. McCormick, J. F., Lugo, A. E., Sharitz, 92. Peet, R. K., Christensen, N. L. 1980.
R. R. 1974. Experimental analysis of Hardwood forest vegetation of the North
ecosystems. In Vegetation and Environ- Carolina piedmont. Veroeff. Geobot.
ment: Handbook of Vegetation Science, Inst. Eidg. Tech. Hochsch. Stift. Ruebel
Part VI, ed. B. R. Strain, J. W. D. Bil- Zuerich 69:14-39
lings, pp. 151-79. The Hague: Junk 93. Peet, R. K., Loucks, D. L. 1977. A gra-
78. Meents, J. K., Rice, J., Anderson, B. dient analysis of southern Wisconsin for-
W., Ohmart, R. D. 1983. Nonlinear rela- ests. Ecology 58:485-99
tionships between birds and vegetation. 94. Pianka, E. R. 1981. Competition and
Ecology 64:1022-27 niche theory. See Ref. 75, pp. 167-196
79. Miles, D. B., Ricklefs, R. E. 1984. The 95. Pickett, S. T. A., Bazzaz, F. A. 1978.
correlation between ecology and Organization of an assemblage of early
morphology in deciduous forest passer- successional species on a soil moisture
ine birds. Ecology 65:1629-40 gradient. Ecology 59:1248-55
80. Minchin, P. R. 1984. A comparative 96. Pimental, R. A. 1981. A comparative
evaluation of techniques for ecological study of data and ordination techniques
ordination using simulated vegetation based on a hybrid swarm of sand ver-
data and an integrated ordination- benas (Abronia Juss.) Syst. Zool.
classification analysis of the alpine and 30:250-67
subalpine plant communities of the Mt.97. Poole, R. W. 1974. An Introduction to
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
CONTINUUM. ORDINATION. AND NICHE 61
Quantitative Ecology. Tokyo: McGraw- and Baja California and the community/
Hill continuum debate. Vegetatio 52:3-19
98. Prentice, I. C. 1977. Non-metric ordina- 113. Whitmore, R. C., Harner, E. J. 1980.
tion methods in ecology. J. Ecol. 65:85- Analyses of multivariately determined
94 community matrices using cluster analy-
99. Prentice, I. C. 1980a. Multidimensional sis and multidimensional scaling.
scaling as a research tool in Quaternary Biometr. J. 22:715-23
palynology: a review of theory and 114. Whittaker, R. H. 1951. A criticism of the
methods. Rev. Palaeobot. Palvnol. 31: plant association and climatic climax
71-104 concepts. Northwest Sci. 26:17-31
100. Prentice, I. C. 1980b. Vegetation analy- 115. Whittaker, R. H. 1956. Vegetation of the
sis and order invariant gradient models. Great Smoky Mountains. Ecol. Monogr.
Vegetatio 42:27-34 26:1-80
101. Rachel, F. J. 1984. Factors structuring 116. Whittaker, R. H. 1960. Vegetation of the
fish assemblages along a bog lake suc- Siskiyou Mountains, Oregon and Cali-
cessional gradient. Ecology 65:1276-89 fornia. Ecol. Monogr. 30:279-338
102. Rotenberry, J. T., Wiens, J. A. 1980. 117. Whittaker, R. H. 1967. Gradient analysis
Habitat structure, patchiness, and avian of vegetation. Biol. Rev. 42:207-64
communities in North American steppe 118. Whittaker, R. H. 1975. Communities and
vegetation: a multivariate analysis. Ecol- Ecosystems. New York: MacMillan. 2nd
ogy 61:1228-50 ed.
103. Roughgarden, J. 1983. Competition and 119. Whittaker, R. H., ed. 1978a. Ordination
theory in community ecology. Am. Nat. of Plant Communities, Handbook of
122:583-601 Vegetation Science, No. 5. The Hague:
104. Sabo, S. R. 1980. Niche and habitat rela- Junk. 2nd ed.
tions in subalpine bird communities of 120. Whittaker, R. H. 1978b. Direct gradient
the White Mountains of New Hampshire. analysis. See Ref. 119, pp. 7-50
Ecol. Monogr. 50:241-59 121. Whittaker, R. H., Gauch, H. G. Jr. 1978.
105. Salt, G. W. 1983. Roles: their limits and Evaluation of ordination techniques. See
responsibilities in ecological and evolu- Ref. 119, pp. 277-336
tionary research. Am. Nat. 122:697-705 122. Whittaker, R. H., Niering, W. A. 1965.
106. Schum, M. 1984. Phenetic structure and Vegetation of the Santa Catalina Moun-
species richness in North and Central tains, Arizona: A gradient analysis of the
America bat faunas. Ecology 65:1315-24 south slope. Ecology 46:429-52
107. Strong, D. R. 1983. Natural variability 123. Whittaker, R. H., Levin, S. A., Root, R.
and the manifold mechanisms of ecologi- B. 1973. Niche, habitat and ecotope. Am.
cal communities. Am. Nat. 122:636-60 Nat. 107:321-38
108. Swan, J. M. A. 1970. An examination of 124. Wikum, D. A., Wali, M. K. 1974. Anal-
some ordination problems by use of sim- ysis of a North Dakota gallery forest:
ulated vegetation data. Ecology 51:89- vegetation in relation to topographic and
102 soil gradients. Ecol. Monogr. 44:441-64
109. Tilman, D. 1982. Resource Competition 125. Williams, B. K. 1983. Some observa-
and Community Structure. Princeton, tions on the use of discriminant analysis
NJ: Princeton Univ. Press in ecology. Ecology 64:1283-91
110. Van der Maarel, E. 1976. On the es- 126. Yeaton, R. I., Cody, M. L. 1979. The
tablishment of plant community bound- distribution of cacti along environmental
aries. Ber. Dtsch Bot. Ges. 89:415-43 gradients in the Sonoran and Mohave
111. Waring, R. M., Major, J. 1964. Some deserts. J. Ecol. 67:529-41
vegetation of the California coastal red- 127. Zobel, D. B., McKee, A., Hawk, G. M.
wood region in relation to gradients of 1976. Relationships of environment to
moisture, nutrients, light and tempera- composition, structure and diversity of
ture. Ecol. Monogr. 34:167-215 forest communities of the central western
112. Westman, W. E. 1983. Xeric Mediterra- Cascades of Oregon. Ecol. Monogr.
nean-type shrubland associations of Alta 46:135-56
This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Mon, 28 Jan 2019 19:30:01 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms