Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

Tarone 2010

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Language Teaching

http://journals.cambridge.org/LTA

Additional services for Language Teaching:

Email alerts: Click here


Subscriptions: Click here
Commercial reprints: Click here
Terms of use : Click here

Second language acquisition by low-literate learners: An


under-studied population

Elaine Tarone

Language Teaching / Volume 43 / Issue 01 / January 2010, pp 75 - 83


DOI: 10.1017/S0261444809005734, Published online: 24 March 2009

Link to this article: http://journals.cambridge.org/abstract_S0261444809005734

How to cite this article:


Elaine Tarone (2010). Second language acquisition by low-literate learners: An under-studied
population. Language Teaching, 43, pp 75-83 doi:10.1017/S0261444809005734

Request Permissions : Click here

Downloaded from http://journals.cambridge.org/LTA, IP address: 193.255.193.162 on 30 Sep 2013


Lang. Teach. (2010), 43:1, 75–83 
c Cambridge University Press 2009
doi:10.1017/S0261444809005734 First published online 24 March 2009

Second language acquisition by low-literate learners:


An under-studied population

Elaine Tarone University of Minnesota, USA


etarone@umn.edu

Many adolescent and adult L2 learners in language classrooms, both in the US and other
countries, have little or no alphabetic print literacy. Language teachers may turn to SLA
research for assistance, yet almost all research on oral SLA has focused on educated,
highly-literate learners (Bigelow & Tarone 2004; Tarone, Bigelow & Hansen 2009). The
assumption seems to have been that the findings of this research hold for ALL learners,
including learners with little to no literacy. However, research in cognitive and experimental
psychology shows that the acquisition of grapheme–phoneme correspondence – the ability to
associate a phoneme and a visual symbol – changes the way oral language is processed. The
present paper shows the relevance of these findings for SLA. It summarizes a three-part study
on oral L2 processing, a partial replication of previous SLA research, carried out in a
population of low-literate adolescent Somali learners of L2 English. The findings confirm that
alphabetic print literacy level had a significant impact on oral L2 processing. The paper
concludes with a call to replicate current SLA studies and findings with populations of learners
who have little or no alphabetic literacy.

1. Introduction

Increasingly, language teachers both in the US and other countries encounter adolescent and
adult L2 learners who have little or no alphabetic print literacy. These teachers may turn to
SLA research for assistance, yet almost all research on oral SLA has focused on educated,
highly-literate learners (Bigelow & Tarone 2004; Tarone, Bigelow & Hansen 2009). The
assumption seems to have been that the findings of this research hold for ALL learners,
including learners with little to no literacy. But we must now question this basic assumption.
I will review research findings in cognitive psychology that show that the acquisition of
grapheme–phoneme correspondence – the ability to associate a phoneme and a visual symbol
– changes the way oral language is processed. SLA researchers must now seriously consider
the consequences: alphabetic literacy must also change the way L2 learners process oral L2
input. Particularly in view of the fact that large numbers of immigrant L2 learners in the US
have little to no alphabetic literacy, it is urgent that we replicate standard SLA studies within

A revised version of a plenary paper presented on March 25, 2008 as part of the Language Institute Lecture Series,
University of Wisconsin–Madison.

http://journals.cambridge.org Downloaded: 30 Sep 2013 IP address: 193.255.193.162


76 PLENARY SPEECHES

this population, to gain a better understanding of the way they process and acquire oral L2
English. This understanding may then enable us to develop a more effective pedagogy for
low-literate immigrant adult L2 learners.
This paper reviews published research in cognitive psychology comparing the way
alphabetically literate and illiterate adults perform oral language processing tasks in their
native language, and represent it in short-term memory. The paper then extends these
findings to the field of SLA, summarizing a three-part study (Tarone et al. 2009) on oral L2
processing. This study was a partial replication of previous SLA research, carried out in a
population of low-literate adolescent Somali learners of L2 English. The findings of this study
confirm that alphabetic print literacy level has a significant impact on oral L2 processing.
Only with such replication studies in SLA can we possibly reach an understanding of the
language learning processes of low-literate adults, thereby providing a reliable foundation
upon which language teachers can build a more effective pedagogy.

2. Impact of alphabetic literacy on adults’ oral language processing

Research in the realm of native-language cognitive processing has amassed solid evidence
that illiterate adults have significant difficulty with oral language tasks requiring a focus on
segments of language (Morais, Cary, Alegrı́a & Bertelson 1979; Morais, Bertelson, Cary &
Alegrı́a 1986; Adrian, Alegrı́a & Morais 1995; Reis & Castro-Caldas 1997; Petersson et al.
2000; Dellatolas et al. 2003; Loureiro et al. 2004; Tarone & Bigelow 2005). The simple
tasks in question require an awareness of, and sometimes an ability to manipulate, individual
language segments – phonemes, syllables or words. In a typical example of such a task, the
listener is asked to delete or reverse the order of individual phonemes or syllables that are
presented orally. For example, I might say the word stan and ask you to say that word without
the ‘s’ in front (tan). I could do the same task with syllables and ask you what remains if I
take the ‘ka’ off of a pseudoword kade (de). Reversal tasks can also be performed with both
phonemes and syllables: tan reversed is nat; now tell me what is los reversed (sol)? Or, with
syllables, kade backwards is deka; now tell me what is mepa backwards (pame)? And so on. Such
tasks are significantly harder for adults who are not alphabetically literate.
We see similar differences in performance on ‘phonological fluency tasks’, which require
an individual to list all the words they can think of in one minute that begin with the sound
‘s’, or ‘t’, ‘p’. Illiterate individuals are worse at this than literate ones. In contrast, there is
no difference between literate and illiterate adults in their performance of ‘semantic fluency
tasks’, where the individual is asked to list all the ‘animals’ or all the ‘foods’ they can think
of in one minute. So alphabetic literacy affects the processing of oral language in terms of
linguistic segments, but not its processing in terms of meaning.
We see the same distinction between results for literate vs. illiterate adults in oral word
repetition tasks – either real words they know and can process in terms of meaning, or ‘pseudo’
words have no meaning (made up from real words by changing consonants – so, ballpark might
become nallkark). There is no difference between literate and illiterate adults in ability to repeat
real words, but illiterate adults have a significantly harder time repeating pseudowords. These
differences have a physical reality in the brain. Castro-Caldas et al. (1998: 1057) display brain

http://journals.cambridge.org Downloaded: 30 Sep 2013 IP address: 193.255.193.162


ELAINE TARONE: SLA AND LOW-LITERATE LEARNERS 77

scans of literate and illiterate adults as they repeated meaningful vs. pseudowords. There were
no differences in the scans when meaningful words were repeated, but dramatic differences
between the literate and illiterate brains during pseudoword repetition.
So literacy appears to have a dramatic impact on the cognitive processing of oral language
in terms of its linguistic segments (phonemes, syllables and words) rather than its meaning. In
the 1980s, a Wisconsin–Madison scholar, Charles Read, headed an important project in this
line of research. Observing that all the previous studies had involved literacy in alphabetic
scripts, Read wondered whether it was literacy itself that had this impact, or whether the root
cause was ALPHABETIC literacy, specifically. Read, Zhang, Nie & Ding (1986) administered
the same oral tasks as those described above to older adults in China. Although all the
participants were well-educated, half of them had only learned to read Chinese characters,
which are logographs, while the other half had also learned to read Chinese in Hanyun
Pinyin, which is an alphabetic script. Read et al. found the same differences between these
two groups that we have already seen in comparisons of adults who were literate or illiterate
(in alphabetic scripts). The group that could read an alphabetic script did significantly better
than the character-only group on the oral tasks requiring awareness and manipulation of
linguistic segments. The Read et al. study shows that it is specifically the ability to decode
ALPHABETIC script that improves one’s ability to perform phonological awareness tasks. In
alphabetic literacy we learn to use a visual symbol to represent a phoneme, and then learn
to organize these into groups of symbols to represent words. Reis & Castro-Caldas (1997)
conclude that the ability to represent sound segments with visual symbols affects short-term
memory, where those visual symbols can be manipulated in cognition, changed in order,
added or deleted, and then rendered back into oral form. This ability constitutes a powerful
tool for processing language in terms of its form, rather than just in terms of its meaning.
Consider the implications of these conclusions for current research on adult SLA, which
has been carried out almost exclusively with learners who are literate – typically, college or
university students who can be assumed to be highly literate – and most of these individuals
have been literate in alphabetic scripts (Bigelow & Tarone 2004). But if, as we have seen,
alphabetic literacy significantly affects the cognitive processing of oral NATIVE languages, we
have to assume that it also affects the way oral SECOND languages are processed by learners
who are not literate. The unavoidable conclusion is that current SLA research findings on
the way adult learners process oral L2 input, as well as current SLA theories that are based
on those findings, may not apply to illiterate L2 learners. And there are increasing numbers
of illiterate and low-literate L2 learners, not just in third world countries, but coming as
immigrants and refugees to first-world countries. We need a better understanding of the
language processing limitations and abilities of these learners.

3. A three-part study on literacy level and L2 acquisition

There is therefore an urgent need for replications of SLA studies in populations of illiterate
and low-literate learners. The three-part study described below began as an attempt to take
some central SLA studies that have been originally carried out with highly literate L2 learners,
and replicate them with low-literate L2 learners, to determine whether literacy level seemed

http://journals.cambridge.org Downloaded: 30 Sep 2013 IP address: 193.255.193.162


78 PLENARY SPEECHES

to affect the outcomes. We chose the following three types of SLA studies, two of which
focused explicitly on learners’ processing of oral L2 input:
• corrective feedback study
• elicited imitation study
• interlanguage analysis of narratives

As these three studies are described in detail and readily available elsewhere (Hansen 2005;
Bigelow, delMas, Hansen & Tarone 2006; Tarone, Swierzbin & Bigelow 2006; Bigelow,
Hansen & Tarone 2007; Tarone & Bigelow 2007; Tarone, Bigelow & Hansen 2009), in
this paper I will simply summarize their overall design and briefly present their results and
conclusions.
We gathered the data for all three parts of this study at the same time in 2003–04 from
the same group of 35 low-literate participants. My co-researchers had previously spent a
year engaging in a variety of activities in this community, which included tutoring, support
for a youth soccer team, and a range of community events. Our participants were 15–27
years old, and were members of a large community of Somali immigrants living adjacent
to the University of Minnesota in Minneapolis. All 35 participants were reading English
at a range of levels one would normally expect of elementary school students. Their oral
English was noticeably fluent, confident, and pragmatically adept, in spite of containing a
range of morphosyntactic inaccuracies. To more precisely establish their literacy levels, we
administered the Native Language Literacy Screening Device in both Somali and English,
and assigned each participant a quantitative score ranging from 1 (lowest literacy) to 9 (highest
literacy) in Somali and in English, using a rubric we designed. Based on those scores, we
selected two groups of learners for further study: the four least-literate participants (mean
literacy scores of 3.5–6) and the four most-literate participants (mean literacy scores of 8–9).
These two groups were well balanced in terms of years of schooling, oral scores on the
SPEAK test, and age, and minimally different in terms of gender. It is interesting that years
of schooling did NOT always correspond to measured literacy levels, validating our decision
to empirically establish literacy levels using an objective measure.
We compared the performance of the low-literate and the moderate-literate groups on (i)
corrective feedback tasks focused on English question formation, (ii) elicited imitation tasks
with English questions, and (iii) their use of several interlanguage forms in oral narrative tasks.
As set out above, the overall goal of the three-part study was to obtain initial empirical data
to establish whether overall oral language processing outcomes of low-literate L2 learners
might be different from those of considerably more literate learners performing similar
oral language tasks in published SLA research. In addition, we asked whether within-group
differences in literacy level could be shown to have significant effects on the oral language
processing outcomes of these participants on all three parts of the study. Below we summarize
data collection, data analysis and results for the corrective feedback, elicited imitation, and
interlanguage analysis parts of the study.

3.1 Corrective feedback


The corrective feedback part of the study (Bigelow et al. 2006) was a partial replication of Philp
(2003), a study that was originally carried out with university students who were learners of

http://journals.cambridge.org Downloaded: 30 Sep 2013 IP address: 193.255.193.162


ELAINE TARONE: SLA AND LOW-LITERATE LEARNERS 79

L2 English. Philp’s study focused on the learners’ responses to corrective feedback provided
in response to their errors in English question formation. Using Pienemann, Johnston &
Brindley’s (1988) proficiency framework to focus on six stages of acquisition of English
questions, Philp studied the factors influencing these learners’ accuracy of recall of recasts
of their questions that were provided in response to their errors. Her findings showed that
the recall accuracy of these highly literate learners was significantly affected by their own
proficiency level, the length of the recast, and the number of changes made in the recast
relative to the original error. The longer the recast, the more changes made by the recast,
and the lower the participant’s proficiency level, the less accurate the participants’ recall
was. Philp concluded that these factors affected the learners’ ability to NOTICE the difference
between their initial questions and the corrections provided in the recasts.
We partially replicated Philp’s study, using its task design and data collection methodology,
which was entirely oral and required absolutely no reading on the part of the participants.
We examined the impact of two of the three variables she did – recast length and number
of changes in the recast – on recall accuracy. All our participants were placed at the same
proficiency level with regard to question formation, following standard procedures used in
Philp’s study, so we could not examine the impact of proficiency level. We added a literacy
variable unexamined in Philp (2003) but important for reasons outlined above – whether the
participants were low-literate or moderately-literate. Because we had few participants, we
measured the statistical significance of the relationships among these variables using the exact
permutation test (Effron & Tibshirani 1993: 210), which is commonly used under similar
conditions in other fields.
An overall comparison of the results of our study with those of Philp (2003) shows some
interesting patterns. Our results were the same as Philp’s in one regard: the number of
changes made in the recast affected the accuracy of the recall of our less literate learners (in
both groups) just as it affected recall by Philp’s participants. However, it is interesting that the
length of the recast was not statistically related to accuracy of recall in our study, in contrast
to Philp’s. We have considered elsewhere the possible reasons for this lack of relationship
between recast length and recall accuracy among our participants, but we feel more research
is needed to find whether these reasons are related to the overall lower literacy level of our
learners.
We found the most interesting results in our within-study analysis examining the impact
of differences in literacy level on accuracy of recall. As noted earlier, our low-literacy group
had mean literacy levels of 3.5–6, and our moderate literacy group had mean literacy levels
of 8–9. The moderate literacy level group performed better in overall accuracy of recall than
the low-literacy group (p = .043). The moderate literacy group also did much better on recall
when the recasts presented two or more changes to their original question form (p = .014).
In Tarone & Bigelow (2007), we used a more qualitative methodology to examine in
detail one of these participants’ responses to our recasts. This analysis shows that the learner
had repeated difficulty noticing and repeating recasts involving changes in word order (e.g.,
changing Why he is mad? to Why is he mad?). Such changes in word order do not change the
meaning of the original utterance. But we also observed that the same learner seemed to
have no difficulty noticing and accurately recalling corrective feedback that supplied a new
vocabulary word (jar). Indeed, he spontaneously used this word later in the conversation,
suggesting that he also may have acquired it. This analysis suggests that recasts focused

http://journals.cambridge.org Downloaded: 30 Sep 2013 IP address: 193.255.193.162


80 PLENARY SPEECHES

on changes to language form were not noticed by this low-literate learner, while corrective
feedback focused on meaning were not only noticed, but learned.
Bigelow et al. (2006) conclude that these findings are consistent with the research and
interpretations articulated in section 2 above – alphabetic literacy appears to improve one’s
short-term memory for language. Alphabetic literacy can help L2 learners to notice language
forms present in oral L2 input that differ from forms they themselves produce, and particularly
to notice formal differences that do not affect meaning. As Reis & Castro-Caldas (1997) state,
alphabetic literacy provides a visual tool that can be used in cognitive processing to enable
individuals to do phonological processing of oral input, in addition to the semantic processing
done by illiterate and literate people alike.

3.2 Elicited imitation

The elicited imitation part of the study (fully reported in Hansen 2005) is patterned on
numerous elicited imitation studies in SLA that have heretofore been carried out exclusively
with literate L2 learners (Bley-Vroman & Chaudron 1994). As in the first part of the study,
we used Pienemann et al.’s (1988) proficiency framework to focus on the learners’ production
of various stages of English questions. This part of our study explored the impact of literacy
level on accuracy of recall of English questions in both elicited imitation (EI) and recast tasks.
We speculated that the elicited imitation task would require more phonological processing in
short-term memory than the recast task because the recast task provides a more meaningful
context and more support for semantic processing.
In the EI part of our study, the learners had to recall decontextualized, sentence-level
L2 questions that the researcher read to them. Each learner heard the same 28 questions
(each one eight syllables long, at different question stages), with each question semantically
unrelated to the one preceding it. Because the learners did not know what question to expect
from one to the next, we hypothesized that they would need to rely more on phonological
processing than semantic processing in recalling them. In the recast task – because learners
were recalling corrected forms of L2 questions they themselves had initiated in contextualized,
meaningful interaction – we thought their use of semantic processing might be more helpful
than in the EI task. We predicted that the less literate learner group would have more difficulty
recalling questions in the EI task than the moderate literate learner group would, and that
the performance of both groups would be worse on the EI task than on the recast task.
The results of this part of the study generally upheld our predictions. The moderately
literate group recalled questions more accurately than the low-literate group on both tasks.
Exact permutation analysis showed that the difference in performance between the moderate
and low-level literacy groups approached significance on the EI task at p = .057, and was
highly significant on the recast task at p = .014. Both groups found the EI task significantly
more difficult than the recast task, at p = .008.
We concluded that alphabetic print literacy appeared to promote better recall of oral
L2 question prompts in both recast and EI tasks. The moderately literate group recalled
questions better than the low-literacy group no matter what the task, a difference that
was most pronounced on the recast task. Possible explanations point to an interaction among

http://journals.cambridge.org Downloaded: 30 Sep 2013 IP address: 193.255.193.162


ELAINE TARONE: SLA AND LOW-LITERATE LEARNERS 81

literacy skills, short-term memory (see Baddeley 1986), the impact of literacy on brain activity,
and contextual factors.

3.3 Interlanguage analysis

The third part of our study (fully reported in Tarone, Swierzbin & Bigelow 2006) focused
on morphosyntactic forms produced by the study participants in oral narrative tasks. We
asked whether the two literacy level groups used the same morphemes and syntactic forms
in producing the same narratives. We were particularly interested in their use of semantically
redundant grammatical morphemes, and also in the syntactic complexity evidenced in their
story retells. Would low-literate learners use fewer ‘variational features’? These are the
semantically redundant grammatical morphemes previously identified in large-scale SLA
studies that included unschooled learners (Clahsen, Meisel & Pienemann 1983). This study
did not explicitly measure the literacy levels of these L2 learners, as we did in our study. We
wondered whether the literacy levels of our participants might correspond to their use or
omission of semantically redundant morphemes in English (e.g. plural -s, third person singular
-s, past tense -ed). We also wondered if sentence complexity would be related to literacy level;
more complex syntax might be harder to process without an ability to visualize it.
This third part of our study focused on grammatical forms produced in narrating stories
based on an examination of a series of pictures. To look at the impact of literacy level on use
of semantically redundant morphemes, we compared the two literacy groups in terms of:

• their use of ‘bare verbs’ – that is, verbs with no morphological marking at all – vs. verbs
with morphology (-s, -ed, etc., whether or not accurate)
• their supply of plural -s on regular nouns vs. use of ‘bare nouns’

To look at the impact of literacy level on sentence complexity, we compared the two groups
with regard to number of relative clauses, noun clauses, and clauses expressing causality with
because, so, or since. Because there were such small numbers of all these grammatical forms
produced by both literacy groups, we did not conduct a quantitative analysis to assess the
significance of differences between the groups’ performance. Here we report observable
trends.
Both groups used many ‘bare verbs’, with the low-literacy group appearing to produce
more of these (205/351, 64%) than the moderate literacy group (230/458, 50%). Example
(1) displays this difference in use of ‘bare verbs’:
(1) Najma: (low literacy): Her mother they say, “We going right now. . .”
Faadumo: (moderate literacy): Her mom says, “Come in now, in a car.”
There was considerable difference between the two literacy groups in their use of ‘bare
nouns’, unmarked regular plural nouns. The low-literacy group failed to use add -s to regular
plural nouns considerably more (36/69, 52%) than the moderately literate group (13/57,
23%). But we also noticed considerable individual variation in the use of this morpheme, so
larger-scale studies are needed.

http://journals.cambridge.org Downloaded: 30 Sep 2013 IP address: 193.255.193.162


82 PLENARY SPEECHES

Sentence complexity varied between the two literacy groups as well. For example, the
moderate literacy group used more dependent clauses and clauses with so than the low-
literate group (131 versus 72). Where the moderate literacy group used 28 relative clauses in
their narrations, the low-literacy group used only eight. But the study of sentence complexity
is complex, and we would need a considerably larger group of participants, and possibly a
longer stretch of time, to really tease apart and test these apparent differences in syntactic
complexity related to literacy level. For now, it is enough to say that this part of the study
looked into this aspect of learner language use, and found some trends that are consistent
with our other findings documenting the relationship between alphabetic print literacy and
oral L2 processing. Specifically, if L2 learners with low literacy are less likely to notice
semantically redundant language forms in the input, then they are probably less likely to
acquire those forms over time. And complex syntactic patterns whose processing may benefit
from the ability to visualize their constituent elements may also be acquired more easily by
more literate learners. But conclusions on these last points must await further, more rigorous
study by SLA researchers. The urgency of such studies cannot be overestimated, given the
increasing numbers of illiterate and low-literate L2 learners world-wide who are entering
educational institutions in the developing and developed worlds alike.

4. Conclusion
In this paper, I have reviewed research in cognitive and experimental psychology showing that
the acquisition of grapheme–phoneme correspondence – the ability to associate a phoneme
and a visual symbol – changes the way oral language is processed.
The relevance of these findings for SLA has been established through a summary of a
three-part study on oral L2 processing, a partial replication of previous SLA research in a
population of low-literate adolescent Somali learners of English L2 in the US. The findings of
all three parts of the study support the paper’s contention that alphabetic print literacy level
has such a significant impact on oral L2 processing that existing research cannot be assumed
to apply across the board to illiterate and low-literate L2 learners. The paper concludes with
a call to SLA researchers, at every level, to identify current SLA studies and findings that can
be replicated with populations of learners who have little or no alphabetic literacy, and then
to carry such studies out with the utmost urgency, given the increasing numbers of illiterate
and low-literate L2 learners in our midst.

References

Adrian, J. A., J. Alegrı́a & J. Morais (1995). Metaphonological abilities of Spanish illiterate adults.
International Journal of Psychology 30, 329–353.
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Bigelow, M., B. delMas, K. Hansen & E. Tarone (2006). Literacy and the processing of oral recasts in
SLA. TESOL Quarterly 40, 1–25.
Bigelow, M., K. Hansen & E. Tarone (2007). The impact of alphabetic print literacy on oral second
language acquisition. In L. Condelli & N. Faux (eds.), Research, practice and policy for low-educated second
language and literacy acquisition – for adults: Proceedings of the 2006 LESLLA Conference. Richmond, VA:
Literacy Institute at Virginia Commonwealth University, 99–122.

http://journals.cambridge.org Downloaded: 30 Sep 2013 IP address: 193.255.193.162


ELAINE TARONE: SLA AND LOW-LITERATE LEARNERS 83

Bigelow, M. & E. Tarone (2004). The role of literacy level in SLA: Doesn’t who we study determine
what we know? TESOL Quarterly 38, 689–700.
Bley-Vroman, R. & C. Chaudron (1994). Elicited imitation as a measure of second-language
competence. In E. Tarone, S. M. Gass & A. D. Cohen (eds.), Research methodology in second-language
acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 245–262.
Castro-Caldas, A., K. M. Petersson, A. Reis, S. Stone-Elander & M. Ingvar (1998). The illiterate brain:
Learning to read and write during childhood influences the functional organization of the adult
brain. Brain 121, 1053–1063.
Clahsen, H., J. M. Meisel & M. Pienemann (1983). Deutsch als Zweitsprache: Der Spracherwerb ausländischer
Arbeiter. Tübingen: Narr.
Dellatolas, G., L. W. Braga, L. N. Souza, G. N. Filho, E. Queiroz & G. Deloche (2003). Cognitive
consequences of early phase of literacy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 9, 771–782.
Effron, B. & R. J. Tibshirani (1993). An introduction to the bootstrap (Monographs on Statistics and Applied
Probability 57). New York: Chapman & Hall.
Hansen, K. (2005). Impact of literacy level and task type on oral L2 recall accuracy. M.A. thesis,
University of Minnesota.
Hill, J. (1970). Foreign accents, language acquisition and cerebral dominance revisited. Language Learning
20, 237–248.
Loureiro, C., L. Willadino-Braga, L. Souza, G. Filho, E. Queiroz & G. Dellatolas (2004). Degree of
illiteracy and phonological and metaphonological skills in unschooled adults. Brain and Language 89,
499–502.
Morais, J., P. Bertelson, L. Cary & J. Alegrı́a (1986). Literacy training and speech segmentation. Cognition
24, 45–64.
Morais, J., L. Cary, J. Alegrı́a & P. Bertelson (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones
arise spontaneously? Cognition 7, 323–331.
Native Language Literacy Screening Device (no date). Glenmont, NY: Hudson River Center for
Program Development. http://www.hudrivctr.org/products_el.htm.
Petersson, K. M., A. Reis, S. Askelof, A. Castro-Caldas & M. Ingvar (2000). Language processing
modulated by literacy: A network analysis of verbal repetition in literate and illiterate subjects.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12, 364–382.
Philp, J. (2003). Constraints on ‘noticing the gap’: Nonnative speakers’ noticing of recasts in NS–NNS
interaction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 25, 99–126.
Pienemann, M., M. Johnston & G. Brindley (1988). Constructing an acquisition-based procedure for
second language assessment. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 10, 217–243.
Read, C., Y. Zhang, H. Nie & B. Ding (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on
knowing alphabetic spelling. Cognition 24, 31–44.
Reis, A. & A. Castro-Caldas (1997). Illiteracy: A cause for biased cognitive development. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society 3, 444–450.
Tarone, E. & M. Bigelow (2005). Impact of literacy on oral language processing: Implications for SLA
research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics 25, 77–97.
Tarone, E. & M. Bigelow (2007). Alphabetic print literacy and oral language processing in SLA. In
A. Mackey (ed.), Conversational interaction in second-language acquisition: A series of empirical studies. Oxford:
Oxford University Press, 101–121.
Tarone, E., M. Bigelow & K. Hansen (2009). Literacy and second language oracy. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Tarone, E., B. Swierzbin & M. Bigelow (2006). The impact of literacy level on features of interlanguage
in oral narratives. In T. Baldwin & L. Selinker (eds.), Interlanguage: Current thought and practices. Special
issue of Revista di Psicolinguistica Applicata 6, 65–77.

ELAINE TARONE is Director of the Center for Advanced Research on Language Acquisition in the
Office of International Programs at the University of Minnesota, and also Distinguished Teaching
Professor in L2 Studies at the same university. She has published extensively since 1972 on research in
L2 acquisition, focusing particularly on the influence of social context and internalized social variables
on various aspects of cognition in SLA. Her new book and DVD, coauthored with Bonnie Swierzbin,
uses videos of learners to introduce classroom level learner language analysis to ESL/EFL teachers,
and will be available from Oxford University Press in summer 2009.

http://journals.cambridge.org Downloaded: 30 Sep 2013 IP address: 193.255.193.162

You might also like