Proc, SWB PDF
Proc, SWB PDF
Proc, SWB PDF
Kelly Binder
A thesis submitted to
in partial fulfillment of
Master of Arts
Carleton University
Ottawa, Ontario
September 1,2000
The author has granted a non- L'auteur a accordé une licence non
exclusive licence allowing the exclusive permettant à la
National Library of Canada to Bibliothèque nationale du Canada de
reproduce, loan, distribute or sell reproduire, prêter, distribuer ou
copies of this thesis in microform, vendre des copies de cette thèse sous
paper or electronic formats. la forme de microfiche/fïlm, de
reproduction sur papier ou sur format
électronique.
receiving Personal Projects Analysis (PPA) but no treatment (0=17) and a comparison
group receiving neither PPA nor treatment (11'1 8). The results from the Procrastination
Procrastination State Inventoty (APSI; Schouwenburg; 1994) found that, at the end of
treatment, procrastination decreased significantly for the treatment group relative to the
comparison groups. Sirnilarly, within-subjects analysis revealed that appraisals for the
project dimension procrastination were significantiy lower for the treatment group. only.
Separate analysis of the PPA project factors (Little, 1983) found no significant
project structure (Le., project dimension control and tirne adequacy). project comrnunity
(Le., project dimension orher's view)and project eficacy (Le., project dimension
Contrary to what was expected, thtre was no significant change in scores for the
Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS; Diener, Emmons, Larsen & Griffin, 1985) and both
positive and negative affect scales (Diener & Emmons, 1984) for any of the three groups.
The implications of these results are discussed dong with future directions for research in
Fïnally, my thesis on procrastination! 1know 1am not unique when 1Say that 1
had to overcome many obstacles to get to this point. Doing the necessary work needed to
successfully compIete a thesis inevitably involves personal struggle and perseverance for
any graduate student. However, doing this monumental task w M e pregnant posed a
whole new set of challenges! Thankfûlly, I survived but were it not for the support of
many individuais, 1do not think 1would have made it to this point.
First and foremost, 1 wish to thank my advisor, Dr. Tim Pychyl, for his guidance,
support and insight. 1 still recall your cornments when 1 first came to see you regarding
my research ideas. You told me you needed another student Like you needed a hole in the
head. However, you did agree to accept me as a rnember of the Procrastination Research
Group and 1am tmly gratefbl for that. Well, after three long years, I just hope you did not
regret your decision! Although, 1am sure you are breathing a sigh of relief now that 1 am
finally finished. Thank you, Tim. for allowing me to pursue research of a clinical nature
1 would like to thank al1 my friends for their continuous support; particularly,
Sheri and Kathy for our 3-way daily emails - they helped to keep me sane! Thank God
there was someone else out there who knew what 1 was going through and aHowed me to
vent about it! Also, thank you to rny "across-the-miles" friends: Arsalan, Kylie and Dil.
1 also want to thank my family for al1 their love and support but especially to my
partner, Roger, who has stuck with me through it dl. Thank you for your understanding,
encouragement, patience, humour and iove. Words cannot express how much you mean
to me,
Finally, to rny unborn child, please forgive me for dl the stress 1 put both of us
under- It brought me great comfort knowing that you were always there with me during
these past eight months. 1 can hardly wait to hold you in my arms!
Table of Contents
Paoe
__e
Introàuction ............................................................................................1
Method ............................................................................................... 27
Results ................................................................................................ -4
.
Page
Discussion ..........................................................................................
..63
Appendices
Appendix A2:Informed Consent Form for Cornparison Group 1 (with PPA) ......-1OS
Appendix A3: Inforrned Consent Fonn for Cornparison Group 2 (without PPA)...107
Appendix B2: Debriefing Sumrnary for Cornparison Group 1 (with PPA) ..........-111
Appendùr B3: Debriefing Summary for Cornparison Group 2 (without PPA) ......-113
Page
-
Paoe
Table 6 . Mean SWLS Scores Across Groups Over Time. ............................ -53
Pre- vs. Post- Test Ratings for al1 Significant PPA Factors.. .................59
Pre- vs. Post- Test Ratings for al1 Significant PPA Dimensions.. ..........62
vii
The Effects of an Academîc Procrastination Treatrnent
resulting in a state of unhappiness (Ferrari, Johnson, & McCown, 1995; Milgram, 1991)
a pervasive and potentidly maladaptive behavior for many university and college students
(p-72)-
As many as 90% of students procrastinate and about 25% of this population report
1984). This self-awareness has been demonstrated by McCown and Roberts (1993) who
found that a si,onificant number of students believed their procrastination interfered with
their academic performance and increased their feelings of stress. It is, therefore,
understandable that students may seek clinical treatment for their procrastination.
found in the literature- Perhaps even more surprisingiy, there is a paucity of outcome
Thus, the purpose of this research was to examine the effects of treatment on academic
procrastination. Moreover, the psychological welI-being of students was exarnined over
Definition of Procrastination
affective and cognitive components (Ferrari et al., 1995; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984).
Both behavioral and affective components are reflected in Milgram's (199 1) definition of
important to perform; and 4) resulting in a state of emotional upset" (as cited in Ferrari et
al., 1995, p. 11). However, as Ferrari and his colleagues point out, procrastination does
not always result in substandard behaviors or poor results. Many individuds, in fact,
perform efficiently under time constraints. With respect to behavior, other researchers
tendency to habitually postpone the initiation or completion of a task (Burka & Yuen,
1982; Ferrari, 1993; Lay 1986; Rothblum, Solomon, & Murakami, 1986). For example,
procrastination constitutes more than a reasonable length of time to complete a task, but
must include both fkequent delay and considerable anxiety7'(p. 387). This definition also
includes an affective component which will be discussed fùrther in the section regarding
intentions and actual behavior (e-g., Blunt & Pychyl, 2000; Ferrari, 1994: Lay, 1986).
considered procrastinating if he or she is waiting for fùrther input from the professor, for
defined as "the time past the optimal beginning point for completion of an important task
that has a high probability of needing completion and that does not have unreasonable
demands of personal costs associated with attempted completion" (Ferrari et al., 1995. p.
actions that have a low probability of needing completion or have excessively high costs
associated with personal completion at their optimal time" (Ferrari et al., 1995, p. 12).
Thus, using the above example, the student who delays completing his or her assignment
ahead and complete the assignment it may result in high personal cost to the student if the
professor were to modi@ some aspect of the assignment ofter he or she completed it. In
other words, sometimes procrastinating may prove to be beneficid in particuiar situations
(Birner, 1993).
definition still exists. For instance, Silver (1974) argues that it is difficult to define
procrastination because what one individual may consider delaying a task may be what
another individual considers punctual. Furthemore, sorne theonsts believe that a person
she is engaging in task delay behavior (Ferrari, 199la; Ferrari, 1992; Senécal, Koestner,
& Vallerand. 1995; Senécal, Koestner, & Vallerand, 1997; Silver & Sabini, 1981; Silver
literature also makes note of the various kinds of procrastination. These range from
the present research examined academic procrastination exclusively, therefore, only this
Academic Procrastination
procrastination, which has been descnbed as behavior that is linked to a specific task
(Harris & Sutton, 1983). Burka and Yuen (1983) have said that it is common for college
research by defining academic procrastination as "a) to nearly always or always put off
anxiety associated with this procrastination" (p. 387). In their reference to academic
procrastination, Lay, Knish and Zanatta (1992) expand upon this definition to include
specific behaviors that contribute to student procrastination. The authors state that
academic procrastination stems from, "a lack of practice or preparation, reduced effort,
and perhaps.. ,unfavorable perfomzance settings, but at least the selection of unfavorable
preparation settings. For example, students may choose to study in places that will
promote distraction and delay" (p. 243-244). This last statement is a reflection of self-
For the purpose of this research. academic procrastination may be defined as any
academic task that is delayed or avoided as a result of the discrepancy between intention
and actual behavior to the extent that it produces negative affect in the procrastinator.
procrastination have been noted. The first of these include objective outcornes such as,
lower grades (Burka & Yuen, 1983; Ferrari et A. 1995; Knaus, 1998; Tice & Baumeister,
1997), higher course withdrawals (Semb, GIick & Spencer, 1979; Rothblum, Solomon &
However, it is important to point out that zcademic procrastination does not always result
in these consequences. For instance, Pychyl, Morin and Salmon (2000), found no
health care visits and physical symptoms in college students were gathered over the
course of the semester. Procrastinators reported lower stress and Iess illness compared to
non-procrastinators at the beginning of the semester, but they reported higher stress and
more illness by the end of the semester. Overall, procrastinators were il1 more ofien and
received lower grades on their assi,grnents. The authors concluded that procrastination
emotional consequences of task delay. When individuais are aware that they are
anxiety (Burka & Yuen, 1983). In fact, according to Knaus (1998), "self-efficacy,
personal confidence and productivity are tightly linked" such that "a combination of low
self-efficacy and anxiety can jointly contribute to expectations of failure, which results in
sense of worthlessness, procrastination, and more self-doubts" (p. 43). In h m ,this may
lead to feelings, such as, helplessness, depression, worry, hostility, fnistration, and
irritation. In the case of students, low fnistration tolerance is often the result of fading to
manage oneself in te= of academics especially when, "new responsibilities can also
evoke resistance in those used to a less responsible lifestyle" (Knaus, 1998, p. 13). This
when the mediating variable of stress is taken into account. That is to Say, the stress
resulting fiom task avoidance rnay simply be a catalyst in the breakdown of self-control.
found in the research of Muraven, Baumeister and Tice (1999) who state that self-
regulation "involves altering one's own responses (e-g.,cognitive processes, feelings. and
behaviors)" (p. 446). The authors compare self-replation to that of a muscle which
when strengthened through exercise increases its power and stamina. This analogy
considers that failure to self-regulate "occurred because people have limited resources for
fatigued'' (Muraven, Baumeister, & Tice, 1999, p. 447). Further, Muraven & Baumeister
(in press) have found a pattern of fatigue and depletion in terrns of self-replation that
revealed: "a) coping with stress has after-effects that encompass a variety of self-
remlatory breakdowns.. .[and] b) coping with emotional distress shows sirnilar patterns"
Given that stress and emotiond Oistress have a significant negative impact upon self-
overcorne her or his procrastination. When the associations between these relationships
This section has been divided into three main parts. The first of these descnbes
several outcome research studies- Finally, various treatment research issues are explored.
management issue (Ferrari et al.. 1995), clinical interventions have been designed and
implemented based Iargely on co,onitive-behavioral therapy (Boice, 1996; Burka & Yuen,
1983; Ellis & Knaus, 1977; Knaus. 1998). This therapy originated as a derivative of the
ABC mode1 of emotional distress (see Kuelwein & Rosen, 1993) which States:
...we experience an activating event (A) in a particular way because of Our beliefs
(B) about this event, which cause us to react behaviorally and emotionally with
rather than the events themselves, that detennine Our ernotions and behaviors.
(P.2 )
Due to the fact that one is often unaware of one's own core beliefs, it is typically the
event itself that is often blamed for one's emotional distress instead. Therefore, the main
that they may be challenged and modified to reflect more accurate, adaptive, and reality-
based thinking (e-g., Beck, 1976; Ellis, 1991). As a result, making overgeneralizations
approach with clients in his own clinical practice. He observed that clients' progress
occurred when cognitive changes were made and then developed his own technique
called rational-ernotive therapy (RET) (see Ellis, 1980, 1985, 1991). According to
Kuelwein & Rosen (1993), cognitive therapy is collaborative and empincaily focused
because it allows for: 1) an agenda to be set between client and therapist each session, 2)
feedback to be given on behalf of the client to the therapist, 3) therapist and client to act
as a tearn to investigate the client's cognitions and test their accuracy and adaptiveness,
and 4) empirical evidence to be gathered by the client from outside of the sessions. The
third and fourth tenet, namely, testing one's beliefs, mây be explained as folIows:
Because the results are often at odds with those the client expected, a discrepancy
is produced within the client's own rnind, thus dnving home both the inaccuracy
disequilibrîum and a nse of affect. The client is not oniy primed by discornfort or
curiosity for cognitive change but the increased affect also often serves to
punctuate and intensiw the new learning. (Kuelwein & Rosen, 1993, p. 10)
These four tenets have been incorporated into some of the programs designed to
by Johnson and McCown (see Ferrari et al., 1995, chap. 9) referred to as the "Doing It
Now" @IN) program. This therapeutic intervention involves 10 sû-uctured sessions
applying techniques such as self-monitoring and relaxation to overcome dysfunctional
cognitions and anxiety. According to these authors, there are two main characteristics of
in impukivity.
In their DIN program, Johnson and McCown include strategies to help both types
are implemented to help those students suffenng with overarousal. For the low-
clients with respect to estimated time of task completion. However, the main strategy of
or irrational feus are perhaps the most important aspect of treatment for the typically
anxious procrastinator" (p. 201)- During specific sessions of the DIN, atternpts to modify
Participants were divided into groups and asked to identify potential cognitions that the
subjects of these vignettes may hold, and then speculate as to how these cognitions may
interfere with completing the tasks. Although not explicitly stated, it is assumed that this
types of interventions as well. For example, Jason and Burrows (1983) ùnplernented a 6-
week program designed to help high school students cope with the developmental
moving away from home. Similar to the DIN, this program incorporated anxiety-
the program, students were found to have better scores on measures of self-efficacy and
rational beliefs, compared to controls- When instmcted to role-play while presented with
Other sources of treatrnent for procrastination have been found in the self-help
literature. For example, Knaus (1998) has written books on the subject of procrastination
and suggests various cognitive-behaviord techniques to help his readers become more
reasonable things, in a reasonable way, within a reasonable rime" (p. 14). What Knaus
considers "reasonable" is defined by common sense such that one's actions remain
rational and non-extreme, hence, providing one with a sense of balance and control over
one's life.
Based upon the ABC method of Ellis (1985, 1991), ffiaus recommends to his
readers and clients that they identify their own false beliefs that lead to self-defeating
feelings, then dispute these irrational ideas. To help, Knaus suggests to his readers to ask
themselves the following questions: How c m 1be absolutely sure 1 will fail at task A? If
I do not make an effort to attempt to do task A, what will the consequences be? How will
1feel about it? Knaus notes that another way to challenge one's îrrational thoughts may
be to think of what one would tell one's friend if he or she was in a similar situation. In
short, making conscious effort to be aware and then correct self-defeating thoughts is a
true for people who engage in catastrophic or black-or-white thinking, for example, "If I
Some of us fall into the extremist thinking trap when we exaggerate the
beyond self-induced catastrophic threats, redefine the threat experience and make
failed," what does failure mean? What does destroy mean? Who would destroy
you? Would you make Iife miserable for yourself? How? For what purpose?
example, Boice (1 996) highlights ten fundamental principles of efficacy that have been
developed and tested against matched controls to help writers who chronically
procrastinate. They are as folIows: 1) wait, be calm, and develop patience instead of
rushing before writing; 2) begin before feeling ready instead of collecting, filing,
rearranging, and oudining ideas; 3) work in bnef, daily sessions; 4) stop and take breaks
when needed; 5) balance prelirninaries such as collecting, organizing, and outlining with
actuai writing; 6 ) habitually monitor for negative self-talk and practice redirecting self-
defeating thinking and habits; 7) manage emotions by working at a moderate Pace such
that writing is not rushed and superficial; 8) moderate attachments and reactions to
wrïting by remaining Iess attached to work emotionally; 9) let others, even crïûcs. do
some of the work; and 10) fimit wasted effort such as rushing work until fatigue sets in
Boice's research and treaunent suggestions, it is important to note that much of academic
procrastination invoIves writing tasks set in the academic environment, hence Boice's
(1983) have d s o enlisted several time management strategies for helping individuais
overcome their procrastination. The authors refer to these strategies as behavioral goals
that include: 1) making tasks that need to get done observable to others, and 2) rnaking
these actions specific, concrete, and broken down into smdl steps. The authors also
suggest procrastinators seiect a particular goal over the course of two weeks and monitor
when they make progress and when they procrastinate in addition to the thoughts and
feelings they experience. They encourage visualizing progress, optirnizing the chance of
success, sticking to a time iirnit, starting before feeling in the mood to start, avoiding
excuses, focusing on one step at a time, getting beyond the first obstacle, being flexible
about altering the goal if necessary, eliminating the need for perfection, and rewarding
progress made.
In sum, based upon past intervention approaches and techniques, it would seem as
consistently challenge and dispute their own negative self-taik that has been derived from
their irrationd beliefs. Theoreticdy, the reshaping of these distorted cognitions serves to
regdate the negative thoughts and emotions that sustain procrastination behavior.
Overall, the treatments techniques reviewed have their roots fimily planted in
Outcome research- To date, there are only a handful of published studies on the
al. (1995) argue that barely meaningful clinical results (Le., according to Jacobson and
Truax (1991), less than -5 standard deviations) are yielded in much of the research that is
available. For instance, Ferrari and his co1leagues (cited in Ferrari et al., 1995) conducted
their own meta-analysis which was based on a compilation of fifteen outcome studies
involving 234 high-school and college students. The authors found the treatment effect
completed the "Do It Now" (Dm) stmctured program. Participants in the DIN program
also fared better than individuals who received ten sessions of brief psychodynarnic
psychotherapy @=15) yielding an effect size of -21 for the variable change in
students in a study skills group and 19 students in a discussion group about the comrnon
transition issues of university students, The therapeutic mode1 for the procrastination
and change behaviors- The techniques employed in the treatment included: 1) helping
each student identify her or his own pattern of procrastination and the fears associated
with it and; 2) helping students acquire and practice various cognitive. behavioral, and
motivational coping strategies that would enable them to develop a more constructive and
productive pattern of managing their academic lives. The results indicated that the
cornparison groups. The success of this treatment may be explained through its objective;
namely, by irnproving one's own sense of personal power, one's self-worth and self-
treatments have shown to be effective to some extent. However, there are a number of
there remains a paucity of research studying the efficacy of existing treatments for
procrastination (Boice, 1996;Ferrari et al., 1995). However, actual reports of effect sizes
appropriate if they are guided by reasonable psychological theory" (Ferrari et al., 1995, p.
188). On the other hand, this position may be viewed as being a superficial and
inadequate explanation for such a complex issue. Given the lack of consensus regarding
199l), it is not surprising that this issue remains unresolved. Possible explanations for
the lack of agreement on this issue may be due to the difficulty of finding a good
theoretical fit among a diverse range of disorders "from marital problems, antisocial
children, headaches, anxiety disorders, depressions, to chronic pain, and with therapies as
diverse as cognitive behavioral and psychodynamic" and that certain "criterion may be
considered too stringent by some (cg., researchers who work with schizophrenic adults or
been put forth. According the Jacobson and Tmax (199 1):
score fails within (closer to the mean of) the functional population on the variable
of interest. When the score satisfies this criterion, it is statistically more likely to
be drawn from the functional than from the dysfunctional population. (p. 13)
among peers and significant others (Kazdin, 1977; Wolf, 1978 as cited in Jacobson &
Truax, 1991; Kendall & Grove, 1988)-an improvement by the end of therapy such that a
"normative" level of functioning is observed (Kendall & Norton-Ford, 1982; Nietzel &
Tnill, 1988), or "changes that significantly reduce one's risk for various health problems"
However, not ail researchers have the opinion that conventional statistical analysis
Jacobson. Follette, & Revenstorf, 1984; Smith, Glass, & Miller, 1980). For example, the
use of the effect size statistic may not be the most appropriate indicator of meaningful
..,the effect size statistic is a measure of the magnitude of the treatment effect,
which could be very large although trivial from a clinician's standpoint- Since the
treatinent for obesity produces an average weight loss of eight pounds, whereas a
amount of variability, the effect sizes will be huge but the clinical significance of
the differences could be negligible (e.g., a group averaging 250 pounds at pretest
and 242 pounds at posttest). Whatever benefits of meta-analysis using the effect
size statistic, such analyses cannot be used to determine the efficacy of any type of
(like their peers who do not seek therapy) when therapy is over. (p. 138)
In short, the issues mentioned above that pertain to the "ciinical significance" of
treatment effects still remains open to debate and are important to consider when
(Cook & Campbell, 1979) may not be present. For example, random assignment is not
researchers work with existing samples in treatment. In this instance, control groups are
characteristically referred to as "cornparison" groups because they are not the typical
controls of a pure experimental design (Cook & Campbell. 1979: Posavac & Carey.
1992).
However, even in the event that random assignment may occur, withholding
bendicial treatments are withheld from persons who might need or deserve them" (Cook
scare and it is simply not possible to provide everyone with the treatment. In this
case, the investigator knows that there wilI have to be no-treatment controls. But
he or she has no parantee that these controls will be equivalent to the treatment
group, since in many settings there will be pressure to distribute the treatment to
those who are thought to merit or need it most rather than to a randomly selected
group. (p.349-350)
Another solution to address the ethical dilemma associated with withholding
treatment may include offering controls treatment at a later date if it is indeed successful
beneficial to incorporate other design elements to improve upon the validity and
Cook and Campbell (1979), is to include pre- versus post-testing as well as intermediary
testing. Posavac and Carey ( 1992) agree with these recornmendations and inciude other
c m be increased by: (1) observing the participants at additiond times before and
afier the program; (2) observing additional people who have not received the
Ferrari et ai., 1995) has not addressed these issues. For example, many of the studies
(e-g., Schubert Walker) used only pre- and post-measures, not additional intermediary
testing. Similady, the outcome variables in the studies were often lirnited to pre- and
procrastination (e-g., Burka & Yuen, 1983; Knaus, 1998). Therefore, in order to fully
only the behavioral aspects of procrastination but changes in individual's subjective well-
being (SWB).
such that it is not regarded seriously enough to be considered problematic (Ferrari et al.,
1995). However, the tmth of the matter is that task avoidance may produce a state of
emotional upset arnong many individuals (Milgram, 1991). The negative psychological
States arising from procrastination can be exarnined most generally in the context of
SWB.
components. The cognitive aspect of SWB refers to one's perceived quality of life and
may be measured by using a life satisfaction scale (see Diener, Emmons, Larsen &
Griffin, 1985); while positive and negative affect scales have been used as outcome
measures for the affective aspects of SWB (Pavot & Diener, 1993a, 1993b). In some
cases, the affective component of SWB has been shown to be an important component in
relation to procrastination. For example, research done by Pychyl, Lee, Thibodeau, and
Blunt (2000) have found guilt (negative affect) and procrastination to be positively
procrastination produced negative affect including guilt, anxiety, and stress. In another
instance, Ferrari (1994) found that feelings of guilt were related to behavioral
procrastination. However, there is virtually no research measuring the level of SWB
before and afier procrastination treatment. Therefore, one goal of this research was to
examine the impact academic procrastination treatment had upon the SWB of students in
is personal project analysis (PPA) (Little, 1983, 1989). PPA has been defined, in part, as,
"an integrated set of assessment components which allow the counsellor to snidy the
content, structure, dynamics, and impact of the everyday pursuits and goals of their
clients" (Little, 1986, p. 596). Little's PPA methodology provides a means of temporally
tracking the daily projects (in this case, academic on-going activities) of respondents
(procrastinators) and dlows each project to be rated alone a number of dimensions, some
of which have been correlated with well-being. In other words, PPA provides knowledge
of what a person is doing and how rheyfeel about what they are doing (e.g Little, 1983).
personal projects were perceived as meaningfu1, structured, not overly suessful and
supported by others (Little, 1989). Pdys and Little (1983) found that projects deemed
supported by others were associated with an increased sense of Iife satisfaction. Past
research has also indicated a relationship between projects that have been self-initiated
and Iife satisfaction in that individuals with higher life satisfaction are more likely to
engage in projects that are self-initiated compared to individuds with lower life
satisfaction (Yetim, 1993). There are d s o numerous other studies exarnining persona1
projects that found significant correlations between specific project dimensions and SWB
(see Pychyl& Little, 1998 for a review). For exarnple, research done by Brunstein (1993)
(opportunity, control and support) and project progress to be highly predictive of SWB
over time.
Although these past studies involving PPA did not examine S W B with respect to
procrastination explicitly, they are important because they provide a basis for cornparison
for outcome research exaMning the effects of treatment for procrastination in relation to
SWB, in general. In other words, past literature exarnining PPA and SWB may help to
procrastination treatmen t.
between SWB and procrastination. For exarnple, research done by Pychyl and Little
Personal projects research has also found a significant relationship between project
efficacy and SWB. For exarnple, project outcome - the extent to which individuals
strongest predictor of life satisfaction (Little, 1989). Other researchers have found
1990). In a recent study done by McGregor and Little (1998), goal eficacy (i.e., how
Iikely one's projects are perceived to be successfùl) was significantly associated with
participants (Le., those with identities that revealed a propensity towards fun and
pleasure) were happiest if they were accomplishing their goals. Specifically, McGregor
and Littie found that the project dimensions difficulty, stress, challenge, time pressure,
and outcome to aIl have a loading greater than -50on the factor efficacy. These findings
are relevant in terms of procrastination if one considers that the PPA factor efficacy
relates directly to task completion. As Knaus (1998) notes, "...people who believe that
they are generally ineffectual, or ineffectual at a specific task, are more inclined to
As can be seen from the preceding cursory review of the research literature
treatment effects. This is reflected in Little's (1998) belief that, "if.. .features of personal
project systems influence well-being, then dinical, counseling, and organizational efforts
to improve project systems should result in increases in well-being" (p. 204). For
increase over time. At the end of treatment, the individual would rate his or her project as
higher on the probability of a successful outcome, and, as we have seen, this is a strong
The main purpose of the present research was to determine whether an academic
treatment is effective, most of this research is limited by the research design, particularly
the Iack of measures of SWB. In this regard, the second purpose of this study was to
participants receiving treatment would have increased scores with respect to life
satisfaction and positive affect, and decreased negative affect scores at the post-test
relative to the pre-test. No sie~ficantchanges in SWB scores were expected for the
cornparison groups.
With respect to PPA and S m ,project factors structrtre, eflcacy, memzing and
corn muni^ were anticipated to increase while stress was expected to decrease. The
Given that much of the treatment literature involves specific techniques and time
management strâtegies (e-g., breaking tasks down into smailer steps) for overcorning
procrastination (e.g., Burka & Yuen, 1983; Boice, 1996; Knaus, 1998), it is conceivable
that participants may perceive their projects to have a certain amount of structure, such
that they are considered manageable (e-g., Little, 1998). To that end, it was hypothesized
that, over the course of treatment, project structure wouId increase significantly for the
regulatory strategies into treatment (e-g., addressing the self-handicapping statements that
sustain irrational thoughts), it is anticipated that participants will become more self-
efficacious with respect to project completion. Thus, by the end of the treatrnent,
marnent participants' appraisais of project efficacy were expected to be higher than no-
Sirnilarly, to the extent that students are progressing on their projects (Le., nor
procrastinating) it is expected that they will find their projects more enjoyable and, hence.
more meaningful (e-g., Little, 1986, 1989; Palys Br Little, 1983). Therefore. project
procrastination.
community would occur for the treatment group compared to the no-treatment group.
This is a logical assumption given the arnount of sharing and persond disclosure that
occurs within the setting of group treatment. That the therapeutic milieu is expected to
foster an increased sense of cornrnunity in its participants may be due, in part, to the fact
that as project goals are discussed, they become more visible tu others. As Little (1998)
States, "project cornmunity can be enhanced by helping individuals talk about the projects
that are deeply important to them with key individuds in their families or workplace so
that they are able to gain support for projects about which others may have been unaware"
(Little, 1998, p. 209). It may further be specuiated that in the context of treatment,
increased visibility of projects may, in turn,increase the social support needed to make
the treatment a success. Given that the academic environment is common to al1 students
may also be one reason as to why, "group therapy ...seems particularly helpful for coIIege
students and not particularly useful for nonstudents" (Ferrari et al, 1995, p. 190).
stress will be sia&ficantiy reduced. This is expected to occur given the relationship
between stress and the procrastinator as Burka and Yuen (1983) write, "Contrary to the
myth that they are reiaxed, easygoing, and lazy, we have seen that most procrastinators
are in fact Iikely to be beset with fear, worry, and tension" (p. 175). Therefore, the
1998) dong with the other previously mentioned procrastination treatment strategies rnay
help students to feel that they can effectively manage their workloads and feel less
these participants belonged to the treatment (or workshop) group. The remaining 32
students were randomly selected from the Procrastination Research Group mass testing
pool based upon scoring in the top 30% on the Aitken Procrastination Inventory (API)
procrastinators relative to the rest of the students who completed the mass testing
questionnaire. These students in the top 3 0 ' ~percentile were then contacted by telephone
and, upon volunteering to participate in this study, were randornly assigned to one of two
comparison groups. The first cornparison group @=17) received Personal Projtcts
Andysis (PPA) while the second comparison group (g=18) did not receive PPA or any
experiment course credits were awarded to any student who was eligible.
Materials and Procedure
Materials
Ail participants completed a battery of measures across four tirne periods. The
(PASS; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984), the Academic Procrastination State Inventory
Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al., 1985) and positive and negative affect
scaies @iener & Emrnons, l984)], and a modified version of Personal Projects Andysis
(PPA; Little, 1983). These measures are described in detail below and are provided in
vdid (Solomon & Rothblum. 1984, 1988), two-pan, 44-item scde developed to measure
The fxst haif of the test lists six academic pursuits including writing a term pzper,
administrative tasks, attending meetings and perfomiing other general acadernic tasks.
Each of the six tasks are rated on the frequency of, reasons for and desire to stop
procrastinating using three 5-point Likert scales (ranging from "a" = never procrastinate,
nor a t al1 a problem, and do not want tu decrease, respectively, to 'e" = ahvays
score is obtained by first assigning a numerical value to the 5-point Likert scale (Le., a=l,
b=2, c=3, etc.) and then summing the fnst two questions of each of the six procrastination
areas. The higher the score the greater the degree of self-reported procrastination.
involving the recollection of a term paper that has been delayed by the respondent.
Twenty-six potential reasons for procrastinating on this task are provided (e-g., "Really
disliked writinp term papers.") and students are asked to rate each reason on a 5-point
Likert scale ranging frorn not at all reflects why I procrastilzated to definitely reflects why
Iprocrastinated. For al1 of these items, two statements are used to describe each of the
9) fear of success, 10) tendency to feel overwhelmed and poorly manage tirne, 1 1)
rebellion against control, 12) risk-taking, and 13) peer influence. From these individual
items, Solomon and Rothblum (1984) have created seven factors with factor loadings
ranging from 0.56 to 0.98. They include fear of failure, aversiveness of task, difficulty
According to Solomon and Rothblum ( 1994), test-retest reliability for the PAS S
correlations for the prevalence of procrastination was -74 and -56 for the reasons for
procrastinating. In addition, tests for reliability using split-half (odd versus even)
reasons for procrastinating (Ferrari et al., 1995). The former reliability coefficient may
be considered low and is due perhaps to the fact that it included six different target areas
(Fermi et al., 1995). However, the procrastination coefficient for "writing a t e m pape?
was moderately high (-81) (Ferrarî et al., 1995). Cronbach alphas for the present research
were -79 for the prevalence of procrastination and -83 for the reasons for procrastinating.
This measure was useful in two ways. First, it provided a general level of overdl
students procrastinated (e-g., "You were womed you would get a bad grade."),
Furthermore, the latter hdf of the PASS may be of particular importance in tenns of
treatment because it "is useful in both identiming potential focal areas for intervention,
and in tracking changes in procrastination over time" (Solomon & Rothblum, 1994, p.
446).
procrastination behavior and thoughts (see Appendix D). It contains three valid and
reliable subscales (academic procrastïnation, fear of failure, and lack of motivafion) with
coefficient alphas of -90, -85,and .79, respectively (Ferrari et al., 1995). Cronbach alphas
for the present research were -77, -78, and .78, respectively. Respondents are asked to
rate the frequency of engaging in the items during the last week dong a 5-point scale. It
should be noted, however, that the APSI was originally written in Dutch and when it was
translated to English the rating scde was not consistent with other scales (Le., 1 = not, 2 =
con,gruity and cIarity, the wording of the rating scale was slightly modified to include 1 =
no? at d l , 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, 4 = open, and 5 = al1 the rime. Two exarnples of
items include "Gave up studying because you did not feel well." and Lbhterrupted
studying for a while in order to do other things." Ferrari referred to the APSI as a "newer
and promising measure" that was appropnate for this research (personal communication,
October 17, 1997). A total score was obtained by summing aii items for each subscale,
separately. Only the frrst factor (Le., the procrastination subscale) had an item that
needed a reverse score. The item in question was "Studied the subject matter that you
had planned to do." Finally, it should be noted that the order of the items were
randomized in the present study such that the specific subscales were not discemible to
both cognitive and affective aspects of subjective well-being (SWB). The first measure
(see Appendix El), the 5-item Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS: Dieneret al., 1985),
was used to assess the cognitive aspects of SWB. Ratings were made d o n g a 7-point
Liken scale ranging from 1 (srrongly agree) to 7 (srrongly disagree). Scores were
obtained by summing d l five items (e-g.. "1 am satisfied with my life."). Diener et al.
coefficient alpha of -87. The Cronbach alpha for the present research was found to be -88.
The SWLS is a robust measure onginally designed to tap the global judgment of quality
of life and has been suggested to be useful for clinical application (Diener, et al.. 1985).
Specifically, Pavot and Diener (1993b) state "the SWLS has shown sufficient sensitivity
to be potentially valuable to detect change in life satisfaction during the course of clinical
interventions" (p. 164). Hence, this scale was chosen as an appropriate outcome rneasure
The second measure (see Appendix EZ),a 9-item positive and negative affect
scale @iener & Emmons, 1984) was used to assess the affective aspects of SWB. Both
scales have demonstrated temporal reliabiiity and intemal consistency with coefficients
approaching -90(Diener & Emmons, 1984). Alpha coefficients for both positive and
negative affect scdes have been found to be high at -89 and -84,
respectively (Emmons,
1991). The present research found the Cronbach alphas to be -88 and -72 for both
positive and negative affect scales, respectively. The positive items consisted of four
adjectives (Le., happy, joyful, pleased and enjoyment/fun) while the negative items
be noted that the additional item of guilr was included in the negative affect scale. Past
research (e.g.. Pychyl et al., 2000) has indicated that guilt is a sipificant positive
considered that changes in an individual's guilt rnight be an important index for clinical
SWB and. as such, was used as an OuiCome variable to reflect changes in smdent life
individual's everyday pursuits and goals and as an indirect measure of SWB (cg., Little,
1983,1986). With PPA,typicaIly respondents are asked to write down as many
personally salient projects as they can think of in ten minutes in what is cailed the
eliciration list or "project d m p - Participants then choose ten projects from this list,
"
write them down in the PPA matrix and appraise each project dong a number of
dimensions (see Appendix FI). The PPA instnictions were modified to obtain academic
projects only. In other words, participants were asked to list only their academic projects.
The projects are appraised on an 11-point scale from zero to ten where a zero indicates a
low rating and a ten indicates a high rating on a particular project dimension.
outcome, self-identity, other's view, positive impact, negative impact, progress, challenge
and absorption. Additional relevant dimensions were also included in the PPA rnatrïx
including procrasrinarion and guilr. This was based upon previous research that found a
significânt correlation between procrastination and the standard project dimensions (Blunt
& Pychyl, 2000; Pychyl, 1995) as well as guilt (Blunt & Pychyl, 2000; Pychyl. 1995:
The mean score for each of Little's five project factors was then obtained by collapsing
across the corresponding project dimensions (see Table 1). These five factors included
the project appraisal dimensions grouped by project factor are presented in Table 1.
In sum, two measures of procrastination and three measures of subjective weii-
being were used in this study. The former included both the degree to which students
procrastinated, as well as their reasons for delaying academic tasks. The Iatter contained
both cognitive and affective aspects of SWB includinp the Ievei of students' life
satisfaction (cognitive), their general positive and negative feelings (affective) and their
appraisals of the academic projects that they were working on (cognitive and affective).
Taken together, these variables were used to detelnine the extent to which treatment
Table 1.
All participants were asked to read and sign an infonned consent form before
questionnaires received during any given period depended upon the group and time
interval. A summarized testing schedule indicating what measures were given, when they
availability, to meet on campus to complete the questionnaires. As such, the testing for
rnost of the students belonging to the comparison groups did not coincide with the exact
day of testing for the treatment group. However, d l the testing for these students
occurred within the same week as the treatment group. An informa1 debriefing was
provided for al1 participants at the end of Time 4 data collection (Appendices B 1, B2, and
B3).
Tabie 2.
Surnrnarized Testino Schedule for Al1 Groups Across Al1 Time Intervals
Note. The order of al1 measures was randornized within each session.
Academic Procrastination Workshop and Research Procedure
The following section provides the content and format f o r both the workshop and
research procedure. The research component descnbes the procedure involving both the
treatment group and cornparison groups, 1t should be noted t h a t rnuch of the content
overlapped between workshop sessions. This was unavoidable and, in fact, necessary
considering the very nature of group therapy. When ideas and concepts are shared, it is
not uncornmon to revisit previously discussed topics. Some m a y argue that this is
inevitable with respect to cognitive-behavioral approaches givern that the aim is to modi@
pre-existing beliefs that are often resistant to change (Kuehlweul& Rosen, 1993).
The treatment group participated in six, two-hour, workshop sessions (one pre-
session and five treatment sessions) that took place once a week at Carleton University.
The sessions and data cotlection were conducted in the winter term of 1998. Former
university counsellor, Margarer Delicate, facilitated the workshop. The experimenter co-
The day before each session, the experimenter called t h e students to remind them
about the workshop. For those students who missed sessions, a telephone check-up was
Pre-session
participants.
The cognitive-behavioral approach of this workshop was based upon a previous
treatment program oflered at Carleton University with the purpose of providing increased
insight, skills and strategies to help students overcome their academic procrastination. As
Delicate (1998) outlined in her presentation for the Canadian Association of College and
University Student Services (CACUSS, June 1998), her goals in working with
procrastination were to: 1) change the thinking patterns that sustain procrastination; 2)
reduce the often paralyzing feelings of guiIt and anxiety; and 3) help students manage
their workload by breaking down large tasks into smaller ones and, thus, prevent feelings
of being overwhelmed. These aspects of the treatment were sirnilar to various other
program, see Ferrari et al., 1995) as well as treatment strategies and techniques mentioned
Research component. This was the first testing interval (T 1) (see Table 2 ) . Every
participant was required to complete a consent form (Appendices A 1, A2, and A3). For
those students attending the workshop, a description about the research study was
participants left the group. The package contained al1 five rneasures as described in the
previous section. The students in the first comparison group received the same package
of questionnaires while the students in the second comparison group received al1
Treatment comDonent. The purpose and goals of the workshop were explained
explored together with the pros and cons of task delay. Participants were asked to share
procrastination. The issue of taking responsibility for one's own behavior was addressed.
From this, discussions surrounding students' thoughts and feelings about academia
Rothblum, 1990) and irrational thoughts were discussed in an attempt to dispel any
cognitive myths (e-,o., "1 should be productive d l the time"), as well as challenge existing
maladaptive thoughts that sustain procrastination (e-g., "If 1start earlier, then 1 will have
less or no time to do enjoyable things."). Students were asked to write down their
feelings about procrastination and replace their pre-existing, unfavorable thoughts with
more helpful ones using exercise sheets that were handed-out (Appendix G). On the first
sheet, students were asked to document their thoughts and behaviors and rate their
feelings (on a ten-point scale) associated with academic procrastination before. during
and after a chosen task or activity. However, the results of these exercises were not a part
of this study. The second sheet was the sarne as the f-rstexcept, in this instance, students
were asked to replace their previous thoughts with more helpful ones that they generated
on their own based upon some tips given in the session (see Appendix G). Next, they
were asked to rate their feelings and behaviors associated with these new thoughts
relative to the chosen task or activity. At the end of the session, the implications of
anxiety upon procrastination were raised, and participants were taught relaxation
exercises (involving visualization) to help rninimize this anxiety. Assignments for the
following session were to practice the relaxation technique and to continue to use the
exercise sheets. It should be noted that thoughu, feelings, and behaviors pertaining to
Session 2
were discussed further (e-g., perfectionisrn: "1 should be perfect, if not 1 am worthless.").
Issues around getting started were brought up. Reinforcing the content from the previous
session, students were asked to brainstorm on how to change their "cognitive script" (Le.,
new statements to replace the old less helpfùl statements). Subsequently, students broke
off into dyads to help each other find solutions to their individual problems and suggest
alternatives to their irrationd thinking (e-g. irrational thought: "If 1 like [a particular
subject] I should be good at it and it should corne easy to me."). Another exercise sheet
adapted from psychologist Neil Fiore (as cited in Burka & Yuen, 1983) was handed-out
called "the un-schedule." Here, students were supplied with a blank timetable for the
week (weekends inciuded) that divided each day into 30 minute intervals. Students were
asked to fil1 in all timeslots that pertained to routine activities, such as getting ready in the
morning, transportation to and frorn university, scheduled classes, etc. This included
uncertain when exactly a particular activity would be done (e-g., grocery shopping),
students were asked to estimate the time and day they might do it. Findly, they were
instructed to not record doing any acadernic work until afier they had done some. At this
point, they were asked to take the un-schedule home and, over the course of the week,
indicate the actual time spent on any coursework by writing it in the schedule. The
purpose of the un-schedule was to provide students with a more realistic view of what
their individual schedules were like. This exercise allowed students to make several
observations inchding when they were studying, how long they were studying and where
else in their schedules they c o d d make tirne to study. This helped students: 1) Iook ahead
to see how much of their time was dready cornmitted and, hence, see the maximum
amount of time left to cornplete tasks: and 2 ) look back to see where their time had gone.
Research component. This was the second testing interval (T2). Participants who
previousIy completed the PPA (the treatment group and first cornparison group) were
asked to complete the PPA again (see Appendix F2 for rnodified instructions). Projects
were rated dong al1 project dimensions as previously done in the pre-session. No
Session 3
introduced in conjunction with feedback from using the un-schedule given in the previous
session. Goal-setting was mentioned in the context of breaking down projects into
manageable parts. Each student was asked to choose one project to work on and to team
up with a partner to plan how she or he were to go about working on it. Al1 participants
were instructed to take turns listening to and strategizing with her or his paxtner about a
specific academic task. In addition, students were encouraged to consider possible self-
sabotage statements or circurnstances that may be preventing them from reaching their
goal. A discussion concemïng choice brought up issues regarding control (or fear of
losing it), self-actualization, societd expectations and resentment from the group. The
counsellor then addressed the issue of low fnistration tolerance, and she also elaborated
M e r upon concepts such as fear of success as welI as failure (e-g., Rothblum, 1990),
the distinction between the two and comfort zone issues (Le., using procrastination to be
sometimes paralyzing guilt students may be feeling. For example, students were
encouraged to avoid using words like "should" or "must" with themselves when uying to
overcome procrastination. Another dyad exercise gave students the chance to correct this
"lecturing or parental style" that rnany participants had embedded as part of their
cognitive script. Students were encouraged to monitor and modify their cognitive scripts
and irrational negative self-talk over the course of the week as well as continue to work
on the task they chose to focus on with their partner at the beginning of the session.
Session 4
students were asked for feedback as to how they were feeling, what they were doing, what
obstacles or self-sabotaging (if any) had occurred in the past week. Negative self-
appraisals (e-g., "l'm too stupid to finish this."), as well as problem-orïented solutions
were addressed as needed. The group, as a whoie, offered solutions for specific students
in need of help. The issue of project management was revisited. To assist students in
their progress, the counsellor presented an outiine of seven guidelines towards meeting
goals. These objectives were as follows: 1) make your task specific and concrete; 2 )
break your task into small steps; 3) start up ;4) visualize your progress; 5) optimize your
chances of completing your task; 6 ) stick to a time lirnit; and 7) do not wait unid you feel
Idce it. The notion of cornrnitrnent was introduced dong with the costs and benefits of
procrastinating. For example, it was pointed out that some students may be feeling
"stuck" because they perceived becoming more productive a threat (e-g., "No longer
Session 5
Treatment component. Dunng this session, overall feedback about the workshop
was discussed within the group and, with the permission of al1 participants, was audio-
recorded by the experimenter. Students were asked whether they felt that they had
benefited frorn the treatrnent and in what way. They were also asked to comment on what
they disliked about the workshop and made suggestions on how to improve it. Specific
feedback comments are provided in the Discussion section. It is important to note that
these tape-recordings were used to help understand the numerical data and were not
systematically quantified or analyzed in any other marner as the focus was on the
questionnaire package was given to dl three groups. However, only the groups that
Follow up session
Research component. This was the fourth testing interval (Time 4). A follow-up
Al1 participants were asked to complete the questionnaire package. The PPA was
completed by the treatment g o u p and the first cornparison group in the sarne manner as
in Session 5 . Similarly, feedback was audio-recorded for those participants who did not
Of the total sample of 50 students, 62% (n=3 1) were enrolled in the Facuity of
Arts and Social Sciences (FASS), 14%@=7) in Science, 10%(11=5)in Public Affairs and
With respect to the treatrnent group, three students did not complete the workshop
and, as such, did not complete the questionnaire throughout d l of the testing sessions.
Therefore, any data pertaining to these participants were eliminated from the anaiysis.
This reduced the original nurnber of participants in the treatment group frorn 18 to 15,
From this remaining group, the majority of students (n=13) attended at least 4 out of 6
workshop sessions whiIe the rest of the participants b=2) attended half of the workshop
sessions- The most cornmon reason given by students for missing a workshop session
In response to the PASS question, "To what extent do you want to decrease your
tendency to procrastinate on this task?" the mean pre-test scores for each of the three
groups, across al1 six academic domains, were 4.17 (SD = .57), 3.87 (So = -68) and 4.07
groups at the pre-test for this item suggesting that al1 three groups appeared to be
equivalent in their desire to decrease their procrastination across d l six acadernic domains
on the PASS.
Similady, al1 three groups were equivalent in terms of age (see Table 3). With
respect to gender, however, fernales were overrepresented in the treatrnent group. This
Boice, 1996, p. 30) who found procrastination to be "more common among women-"
Furthermore, research done by Jourard (197 1) has found that females are often socialized
to self-disclose more than mâles. Zn addition, females often connect with others when
they are in need of support. For example, a study examining socid support during a
transition into a new school found that, "Girls rnay be more Iikely than boys to seek out
other sources of support when their peers do not provide it" (Dunn, Putdlaz, Sheppard, &
considering that many students in the treatment group were in their first year of
university. Hence, these reasons rnay have motivated more femde than male students to
Table 3.
Age
M
-
-
SD
Gender
Fernale
Male
Note. Comparison Group 1 received PPA. Comparison Group 2 did not receive PPA.
AnaIysis Overview
All statisticd andyses were performed using SPSS 7.5. Due to the small
sample size for each of the three groups, it was decided that Tl outcome variable scores
would be used as a covariate in order to adjust for any pre-existing differences between
groups and, hence, maintain a sufficient arnount of power needed to detect any significant
changes (Keppel, 1991)- Separate ANCOVAs were used to analyze each of the
dependent variables. Follow-up pianned comparisons were performed for pre- and post-
correction was made due to the fact that these were planned comparisons (Le., a priori)
The results of the PASS are divided into two sections. The first sectiori presents
the results pertaining to the overall ievei of self-reported academic procrastination. The
second section presents the results pertaining to the reasons for procrastinating across
procrastination would occur over time for the treatrnent group relative to the cornparison
groups, the first section of the PASS provided a total procrastination score and was
analyzed using a repeated measures ANCOVA with T 1 PASS scores as a covariate. The
PASS scores for each group at each time interval are presented in Table 4. The Levene's
account for this small violation in the assumptions. As expected, an omnibus F test
Table 4.
Time Interval
Group Tl T3 T4
Treatment
-
M 44.33 38.33 40.73
-
SD 5.23 9.83 6-10
Comparison 1 (w/PPA)
-
M 44-58 44.47
-
SD 8 .O6 7 -33
-- ~~~~~
-
M 43.44 42.67
-
SD 5 -62 6.27
significant reduction in procrastination for the treahnent group relative to the comparison
groups, both after treatment had ended and at the follow-up. There was no significant
vs. T4 (t(14) = 3.89, < -002). There were no significant differences within-subjects for
the rernaining two groups. Taken together, these results suggest that the workshop had
some effect in lowering the level of procrastination for the treatment group over and
The second section of the PASS provided possible reasons for procrastinating and
was analyzed using a repeated measures ANCOVA with Tl scores as the covariate.
Overall mean factor scores were found by collapsing the items across seven categories: 1)
yielded a sipifkant difference between groups (F(2,41) = -3.63, p < -04)for the factor
comparison groups were significantly different (D = -.8 1 , < ~.02)with the second
comparison group reporting significantly more difficulty making decisions than the first
cornparison group, None of the six remaining PASS factors demonstrated statistically
group (i.e., did not receive PPA) revealed a significant increase in the dzflculzy rnaking
< -05). There were no significant
decisiuns factor for Tl vs. T3 (t(17) = - 2 . 1 5 , ~
On the whole, the results fiom the second part of the PASS seem to suggest that
regardless of whether they were receiving treatment for their procrastination or not.
research yielded similar findings when compared to normative data. Two comrnonly
endorsed reasons were: "Just felt too lazy to write a term paper'' (42.78) and "Really
Rothblurn's sample (1984) self-reported: "Just felr too lazy to &te a term paper"
procrastination, an ANCOVA was used to analyze APSI scores. Table 5 presents the
mean APSI scores between groups across al1 three time intervals for al1 three of
revealed that the treatment group engaged in significantly less thoughts and behaviors
compared to the fxst cornparison group. The results suggest that, over time, students in
the treatment group perceived thernselves to be engaged in significanùy less thoughts and
significant difference was found ktween the treatment group and the second comparison
group. However, a significant difference was found between the cornparison groups at
T3 (o = .37, p < -01) with the second comparison group indicating a decrease in their
was found between comparison goups on their procrastination scores at T4. Neither of
the two remaining APSI factors demonstrated statistically significant differences within
or between groups.
revealed significant decreases in APSI factor procrastination scores for T l vs. T3 (t(I4) =
4.13,E < -001) and Tl vs. T4 (t(14) = 2.48, < .03).In other words, the treatment group
attending the workshop (post-test) cornpared to before attending the workshop (pre-test).
Similady, the second comparison group's procrastination scores decreased for TI vs. T3
(t(17) = 2.50,p < -02) and Tl vs. T4 G(17) = 2.37-2 < -03). Thus, it would appear PPA
significant within-subjects differences were found for the rernaining cornparison group.
Table 5.
APSI: Means and Standard Deviations for Three Factors Across Time and Grouv
Time Interval
Factor TI T3 T4
Procrastination
Fear of Failure
Lack of Motivation
In the sections that follow, measures of SWB were andyzed separately. The fust
section provides the results of the cognitive component to SWB, namely, the Satisfaction
With Life Scale (SWLS). The second section provides the results for both positive and
S m Scores- The overall mean score on the SWLS was 21.32, with a standard
deviation of 7.29. This mean score was lower than that of undergraduates in the
study done by Diener et al. (1985) that found the mean on the SWLS to be 23.5, with a
standard deviation of 6.43. Table 6 presents al1 group means and standard deviations
across time for students in the present study. In order to investigate whether mean
satisfaction with life scores changed, over time, a repeated measures ANCOVA was used
to detect any significant differences between groups. The analysis revealed no significant
difference in S'WLS scores between groups. These results suggest that none of the
participants differed in how they cognitively perceived their satisfaction with Me.
Sirnilarly, a within-subjects analysis revealed no si,@icanr differences over time for any
of the three groups. This is contrary to previous research that found the life satisfaction
of clients in therapy changed significantly from 14.1 (SD = 1.9) at the start of therapy, to
26.9 (So = 3.6) after one month of therapy (Friedman, 1991, cited in Pavot and Diener,
1993b)-
Table 6 ,
Time Interval
Treatrnent
M
-
SD
-
Cornparison 1 (wPPA)
-
M 2 1.53 23.88
-
SD 7.80 7.65
-
M 19.67 2 1.22 2 1.39
Affect Scales. Using a repeated measures ANCOVA, both affect scales were
analyzed separately across groups and time. The means and standard deviations are
presented in Table 7 below. Despite participants rating their overall positive feelings
higher than their negative feelings, no significant differences between groups were found
suggesting that participants were similar and consistent in how they felt over time.
Similarly, a within-subjects analysis revealed no significant differences over time for any
of the three groups. These results were contrary to what was originally expected as it was
hypothesized that the treatment group wodd show increased positave and decreased
negative affect.
Table 7.
Time IntervaI
Positive Affect
Negative Affect
Note. The affect scale was not adrninistered at T2. Standard deviztions are in
parentheses.
Personal Proiects Analvsis
The results of PPA are divided into two main sections. The fxst section presents
the results for the five original factors of PPA. The second section presents the results for
selected project dimensions within PPA. Both sections include separate results for
For the purposes of this research, the project dimensions positive impact and
negnrive impact were not analyzed as they do not fa11 under any particular PPA factor (see
Table 1). Twenty represcntative examples of the projects students listed are provided in
Table 8. These projects were randornly selected with the purpose of demonstrating the
Proiect Factors
Within-subjects Analysis. Scores for each of the first 16 PPA dimensions were
surnrned across d l of the projects and their respective means were grouped into Little's
( 1983, 1989) original five factors: rneaning, strzrcture, comrnrrniîy, eficacy and stress.
The factor means and standard deviations for al1 four time intervals are presented in Table
9. Using paired-saniples tests for both groups separately, only the treatment group
revealed within-subjects significance over time for three PPA factors: srructztre,
cornmiiniiy, and eficacy. The treatment group's appraisal of project structure increased at
the end of treatment (f( 14) = -2.76, E < -02)as well as at the follow-up (t(14) = - 3 . 8 2 , ~
<
-002) relative to before treatrnent began. Sirnilarly, this group reported project
Raise my GPA
Research on Web
1 1) Drop geography
15)Find a tutor
Means and Standard Deviations Between Groups Over Time for Five Factors of PPA
Time Interval
Factor Tl T2 T3 T4
Meaning
Cornparison 1 (w/PPA) 6-02 (1-54) 6.30 (1-20) 6.29 (1-23) 6-20 (1 -45)
Structure
Cornparison 1 (wPPA) 5.22 (1.78) 5.50 (1-55) 5.63 (1J O ) 5.86 (1 -92)
Community
Cornparison 1 (w/PPA) 4.90 (1.57) 5.12 (1.54) 4.92 (124) 5.49 (1.78)
Efficacy
Cornparison 1 (w/PPA) 5.24 (1.43) 5.17 (1.26) 5.38 (1.37) 5.16 (1.41)
Stress
Compatison 1 (w/PPA) 5.15 (0.78) 5.15 (1.20) 5.27 (1.21) 4.88 (1.60)
the end of treatment G(14) = -2.21, E < -04)but not at the follow-up, compared to before
treatment. These pre- vs. post-test results for the treatment group are presented in Table
10. It was decided that no family-wise error correction was needed due to the theoretical
rationde that the comparison between pre- and post-test scores was planned for ail
In general, these results suggest that project systems for those students receiving
treatment for theù- procrastinarion were more stmctured and visible to others. It also
suggests that these students perceived themselves to be more self-eff~caciousby the end
Between-.xoups
- Analvsis. A repeated measures ANCOVA was performed using
T l project appraisd ratings as a covariate, for each of the PPA five project factors
Proiect Dimensions
Within- subiects Andysis. Only the underlying PPA dimensions of the three
significant factors (Le., structure, community and efficacy) from the within-subjects
project dimension appraisals, across these three factors, was found for the comparison
group.
Table 10.
Structure
Cornmunity
Efficacy
different when mean pre- vs. post-test scores were compared; these included: orctcome,
rime adeqrracy, control and other's view. The project dimension procrastination was also
examined given the focus of this study. The means and standard deviations for each of
these PPA dimensions are presented in Table 11 alont with those of the cornparison
group. The results of the 1tests for these project dimensions are presented in Table 12.
decreased by the end of treatment (1(14)= 2.15, E < .05). Interestingly, this result was not
significant by the follow-up, despite that the overall procrastination rating at the follow-
up was lower than before treatment began. In other words, it would appear that the
adequacy, control, and other's view improved over time. Specifically, in terms of the
ueatrnent began (t(14) = -3.00, E, < -01). However, an improved appraisal of the project
dimension outcome was not observed at the follow-up. Similady, in terrns of time
adequacy, students considered the arnount of time they spent on their projects to be more
adequate. but only at T4 relative to T l (t(14) = -2.55, g < -02) rzot T3 relative to Tl. The
same was found with respect to how important projects seemed to be by relevant people
(Le.. other's view) (t(l4)= -2.88, g < -01). In other words, the treatment group
considered that other people perceived their projects to be more important at the follow-
up than at any other time. However, in the case of project contml, appraisals were
significantly higher at both the post-test (t(l4) = -2.56, p < -02) and the follow-up
(r(14) = -2.75, Q c .Oz), relative to the pre-test. This would seem to suggest that treatment
Means and Standard Deviations for Signïficant Proiect Dimensions Across G r o u ~ and
s
Time
Time Interval
- - - - -
Factor Tl T2 T3 T4
Procrastination
Cornparison 1 (w/PPA) 7.76 (1-53) 7.59 (1.65) 7.52 (1.41) 7.27 (1.53)
Outcome
Cornparison 1 (wPPA) 7-15 (1.39) 7.12 (1.03) 7.38 (1.05) 7.03 (1.82)
Time Adequacy
Cornparison 1 (w/PPA) 3.49 (2.03) 3.74 (2.17) 4.25 (2.21) 4.41 (2.79)
Control
Cornparison 1 (w/PPA) 6.78 (2.33) 7.26 (1.52) 7.04 (2.18) 7.23 (1.79)
Other's View
Cornparison 1 (w/PPA) 5.91 (2.39) 5.60 (2.30) 5.54 (2.21) 6.47 (2.00)
Table 12.
designed to treat acadernic procrastination had an effect upon students' overall self-
The data collected in this quasi-expenmentai design yielded several significant and
interesting findings with the main result being that treatment decreased participants'
perceptions of their level of procrastination, but did not have an impact on their SWB.
However, caution is needed in interpreting the results due to the srnall sample sizes for
each group which reduced the arnount of power and, subsequently, may have increased
This section is divided into two main parts. The first section presents the results
of the outcome variables, narnely, those for procrastination and SWB, respectively. The
second section presents the limitations of the present research dong with several
Procrastination
One of the main objectives of the workshop was to help students decrease their
tendency to procrastinate. The results of this research revealed three key pieces of
evideace, al1 of which were indicative that the treatment had some degree of success in
decline in self-reported academic procrastination for the treament group relative to the
cornparison groups at both the post-test and follow-up testing intervals. Additionally, the
within-subjects analysis for the treatment group indicated that the PASS scores
before treatment began. Secondly, the Academic Procrastination State hventory (APSI)
procrastination scores for the treatment group decreased si,@ficantly relative to the first
comparison group at both tbe post-test and follow-up testing intervals. Similady, a
behaviours perîaining to procrastination, over time, for those participants who received
treatment. Thirdly, the PPA project ratings of procrastination significantly decreased over
time for the ueatrnent group upon examination of within-subjects andysis. This was not
In sum, the combination of these results strengthens the position that the
workshop sessions had a positive effect upon students, reducing their tendency to
procrastinate, according to their self-reports, as one student cornrnented, "It was more of
just having the opportunity to actually look at what I was doing and actuaily examine it,
like you have the cataiyst there to examine it closely that really helped."
noted in the Iiterature (e-g., Schubert Walker, in press) and, therefore, support the results
of the present research. However, it could be argued that the change in procrastination
scores was not ciinicaily significant, based upon the on-going debate with respect to what
constitutes meaninb$ul change (see Jacobson & Revenstorf, 1988). It will be recdled that
was tnily effective may depend more upon other indicators of meaningful change; for
example, a level of change that is recognizable among peers and significant others
(Kazdin, 1977; Wolf, 1978 as cited in Jacobson & Tmax, 1991; Kendall & Grove, 1988).
Other experts believe that the benchmark for meanuigful change in participants is
whether the treatment demonstrated a r e d effect in their daily lives or ". ..if the
participants are better in any practical way for having participated in the prograrnT'
(Posavac & Carey, 1992, p. 143). When asked if they perceived any difference in their
procrastination, one student in the treatment group of the present research comrnented,
"It's [procrastination] decreasing very, very slowly but it's stiil there." Sirnilarfy, another
a Little bit with the way I'm thinking." Therefore, it rnay be argued that although
On the other hand, it may be premature to conclude that these feedback statements
mentioned above suggest that the treatment may not have had a substantial impact upon
statements may simply reflect the fact that: 1) modifying pre-existing irrational beliefs
that sustain procrastination takes considerable time and poses an "intellectual challenge"
to overcome for most individuals (Kuehlwein & Rosen, 1993, p.79); and 2 ) the length of
weatment was too bnef to expect these changes to occur quickly and to their fullest
extent. Yet, despite these restrictions, it is plausible to conclude that the treatment did
behaviorai changes observed (i-e., the decrease in procrastination scores on both the
PASS and APSI), as well as in the comments of one student who began to assume
things anymore. 1 think that was a big realization. So, like.. -before you'd just
for students participating in treatrnent for their procrastination. The hypothesis was that
over the course of treatrnent, satisfaction with life would improve, negative affect would
be argued that this lack of significant change in SWB is conceivable given that some
researchers have stated that "how one feels at the moment and d s o how happy one feels
on average over time - are prirnarily a matter of chance" (Lykken & Tellegen, 1996, p.
189) and that the effects of events may "be transitory fluctuations about a stable
temperamental set point or trait that is charactenstic of the individual" (Myers & Diener,
1995 cited in Lykken & Tellegen, 1996, p. 189) rather than SWB per se. Moreover, the
lack of change in SWB scales, resulting from this research, may be particularly relevant if
one takes into account that both the SWLS and affect scales have been designed to
measure global aspects of qudity of life (e.g., Diener et al., 1985 and Pavot & Diener,
1993b) that are apt to change upon the recent occurrence of major life events (e.g., death
of a loved one) @iener, Suh, & Fujita, 1996). In this study, only the acadernic projects of
students were examined which is merely one aspect of their Iives as a whole. Therefore,
it would be justifiable to argue that these SWB measures may not have k e n sensitive
enough to detect changes in student's well-being if these changes were soIeIy related to
Furthemore, past research, which has tracked changes in SWB over time, have
had longer periods of time between testing intervals (e-g., Diener, Suh, & Fujita, 1996).
Therefore, it may be argued that perhaps these measures of SWB were not sensitive
enough to detect short-rem change. This is further supported by the previous reports of
That said, some researchers have found SWB changes in the short-term. For
exarnple, Diener, Suh, and Fujita (1996) state "at l e s t for a short period of time, recent
life events do affect SWB beyond the influence of stable personality or baseline IeveIs of
SWB" (p. 1097). However, once again. the extent to which academic procrastination
and rnay help to explain the lack of significance over time for this variable.
Perhaps another explanation for why no changes in SWB were observed in the
present research was due to the possibility that not al1 of the scales used forrned
supported by McGregor & Little (1998) who contend that "conventional measures of
(p. 505). For example, procrastinators ofien report high levels of anxiety and Iow
hstration tolerance (Burka & Yuen, 1983; Knaus, 1998; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984),
common emotional consequences to delaying or avoiding tasks. Yet, neither one of these
two constmcts was measured extensively- Thus, it rnay have been beneficial to have
included specific measures of the constmcts most pertinent to procrastination and monitor
In short, despite the fact that some previous research has found significant
changes in SWB using globd rneasures, these outcome variables may not have been
adequate in capturing specific constmcts targeting the most relevant affective components
for procrastinators which, in turn, rnay have been more suited to test the originaI
hypotheses.
as to why no significant change in either positive or negative affect was observed in the
present study. Despite finding a significant correlaùon between negative affect and trait
Prescott, 1977). A lack of a significant decrease in negative affect was explained in terrns
of seif-replation to the extent that "procrastination has been identified as one means to
regulate negative emotions that may accornpany a task, at least in the short term" (Pychyi
et al., 2000, p. 240). Therefore, given this fact, "...when students are procrastinating,
they are not concurrently experiencing negative affect in terms of depression, anger,
fnistration, worry or unhappiness. On the contrary, they are doing something they see as
pleasant" (mchyl et al., 2000, p. 248). h would, therefore, seem reasonable to expect a
significant increase in positive affiect, as a result. However, this was not the case. When
the students in their study were procrastinating they were also reporting significantly
higher levels of guilt and lower levels of motivation. Thus, it was concluded that "...as
247). Hence, despite rating activities they were involved in while procrastinating as more
pleasant than the activities they were avoiding, positive affect may have been offset by
decreases in negative affect occurred with respect to the treatment group of the present
research.
Lastly, the issue of the follow-up (T4) testing penod may provide another
interpretation for the lack of sipificant change in SWB in the present research. During
this time interval. students were in the rniddle of final examinations -- a critical time in
the school year with respect to acadernic performance. Needless to Say, students may
have been feeling an increase in stress overall. As such, no increase in positive affect
would be expected at this time period relative to the pre-test. On the other hand, some of
these students had yet to write one exam at the time of testing, while other students were
either halfway through or had finished al1 of their exarns. In terrns of affect, it would
seem plausible to suggest that a rnix of positive and negative emotions would result and,
hence, not provide a coherent or consistent picture as to how the treatment was affecting
students. In other words, the event of final exams rnay be considered a confounding
variable to the extent that it interfered with the ability of both the satisfaction with life
s c d e (SWLS) and affect scaies to reflect an accurate indication of students7 S m as a
&rOUP-
Despite the absence of any statistically significant results with respect to SWB, a
students felt the workshop helped to decrease their feelings of isolation or aloneness in
their procrastination and that helped them to feel better to some extent. This cornmon
reaction to the treatment is best illustrated by the comment of the following student:
The group discussions 1really liked because you could relate to everyone Iike 1
just thought 1 was the only one who procrastinated but seeing that it's a problem a
.- .somehow it [the treatment] kind of gave me.. .made me feel a little better about
myself knowing that there's other people that have the same problem that I do too.
OveralI, despite the lack of any significant statistical findings with respect to
satisfaction with Iife or positive and negative affect, the treatment appeared to help
cognitive changes, albeit to a lesser extent. However, very littie, if any, evidence of real
It rnay be recalled that McGregor and Little (1998) adapted personal projects
analysis (PPA) in order to examine the relationship between goal appraisals and SWB.
Likewise, the present research used PPA as an outcome measure of SWB in order to
no significant differences between groups across project factors (Le., meaning, structure,
community, efficacy and stress). This non-sipificant finding may have resuited from the
possibility that not enough statistical power was available to detect a significant
With respect to within-subjects analysis for the treatment group only, it should be
recalled that project structure, community and efficacy increased significantly upon
treatrnent. Subsequently, the project dimensions associated with these factors were also
control, other's view and outcome. At the end of treatment, the appraisals of al1 four
project dimensions increased significantly. Given that two out of the four project
dimensions, narnely, time adequacy and control Ioad ont0 the factor structure, the results
would seem to indicate that treatrnent did more for improving project structure than it did
for project community and project efficacy (as indicated by the remaining two project
workshop participant:
...[the workshop content presentatïons] gives you ideas of what you rnight try to
do and if you get different ideas something rnight work for you, some things
won? but at least you have different things to do and different chings to try- So 1
liked that and I'd probably encourage a bit more of that and spending more time
on these exercises here because 1 found it more powerful when I do them here
addition, despite its Iack of significance, the dimension progress was included in this
section due to its direct association with the concept of procrastination. Possible reasons
as to why no significant change was observed in this variable. in the present research, are
discussed.
Time adequacy. As presented in the ResuIts section, the project dimension time
adequacy was found to be significant for the treatment group, within-subjects analysis,
only. There is supporting evidence in the literature (e.g., Pychyl, 1995; Pychyl & Little,
That is to Say, the Iess adequate tirne one feels to have spent on a project, the greater the
likelihood they would have procrastinated on that project. Similarly, in a study done by
Lay (1990), it was found that for short-term projects ( e g , "write essay for rny history
The significant increase in time adequacy ratings for post-test scores relative to
the pre-test suggests that the ueatment may have helped students devote more adequate
amounts of t h e to their projects. Indirectly, this may be a resuIt of reduced
procrastination, on the whole, as seen in the PASS and APSI scores. Specific workshop
content presented by the counsellor, namely, that the arnount of time it takes to complete
a given task is often at least double the predicted time may have aIso been a contributing
consistently and, not to wait until they felt "ready" (Boice, 1996). This last component is
supported by Knaus (1998) who listed, "Focus yourself along an action pathway where
you place your emphasis on your Do It Now! plan - even when you don? feel like it" (p.
procrastination.
On the other hand, the significant difference in time adequacy ratings for the
treatment group occurred between the pre-session (before treatment) and the follow-up
(during examinations) only, This outcome once again implicates the confounding
variable, narnely, the follow-up (T4) testing period, as previously described- In this case,
the extent to which tirne adequacy would have been relevant may have depended upon
where along the continuum a student lies in tenns of project progress. For instance, if
students had already completed al1 of their coursework including examinations before
procrastinating at the time of the follow-up, then the amount of time she or he devoted to
a project "at the last minute" may have been perceived as adequate under the
circumstances.
of procrastination, students who report not having enough tirne to complete their projects
rnay be doing so because they are procrastinating, ûs illustrated in the comments by the
following student:
I have an Economics exam on Friday and I'm fieaking out because I've got an
exam tomorrow that 1should be studying for but 1want to study for my
Econornics because it's more important to me. But 1 need to study for my other
exarn because it's there. It's procrastination because 1should have started
However, the reverse may be true as PychyI (1995) notes in his dissertation on
doctoral students and their personai projects, "some students reported having more than
adequate time, at least in the sense that they found time to procrastinate" (p. 72). This
may have been the case for some of the treatrnent participants, as one student confided:
I've got my procrastination down to a science where if 1say it's going to take me
hours to do an essay and to do a good job and to have it completely finished it'll
take exactly eight hours. So long as I begin when 1Say I'm going to and keep at it,
Although, in terms of the present study, it could be q u e d that given the significant
decrease in procrastination scores for the treatment group, the likelihood of students'
appraising the project dimension time adequacy, in this manner, dirninishes. In the end,
the discussion regarding the confounding variable (Le., testing during the examination
penod) may provide the most likely explanation for why there was a significant increase
in the treament groups' perception that they had adequate time in which to complete their
treatment for their procrastination increased their sense of control over their projects. As
When I came into the workshop I was feeling redly out of control
like.. .broken.. .now 1 feel like I have the tools and have an understanding of
This factor was the only PPA dimension to be si,&ficant at both post-testing intervals
suggesting that perhaps treatment helped students organize their projects - a first step
towards project completion. In their research, Blunt and Pychyl (2000) demonstrated a
correlation between control and task aversiveness during Gollwitzer's (1990) Actional
stage- This relationship is anticipated if it is considered that the actional stage includes
striving towards goal completion through goal-directed behaviors, hence, as Blunt ( 1998)
writes, "we might expect individuals to procrastinate to a greater extent on projects which
are less structured and difficult to coordinate" (p.53). In fact, this negative correlation
between control and procrastination was found by Szawlowski (1987). The results from
his research seem to support this hypothesis, in that, as control increased, procrastination
decreased. It should be noted, however, that this study did not determine whether control
(or, rather, lack thereof) is a cause or an effect of procrastination. It also did not
determine which variable changed as a direct result of treatment. It may have been that
the treatrnent influenced project control which, in turn, helped to decrease procrastination.
Or, it rnay have been that procrastination decreased, with the help of treatment, which
did not feel in control of their projects, which resulted in feelings of fnistration and
It seems like the steps aren't my goals, it's just the final goal. It's Like of this is
just awful, what 1have to go through, and it won? be until the final stage that it'll
be like, "Now 1 have what 1 want." And the rest was just torture to go through it.
It feels really separated from me because 1 don't have a lot of choices in what
courses to take. There's so many restrictions and there's so many courses that I'd
like to take in philosophy and religion.. .al1 different sorts of things and it's so
lirnited. And it's frustrating because there's courses that you have to take that you
shouldn't,.,I think 1 shouldn't have to take because 1'11 never use it and it's realIy
to fulfill a requirement that someone else has made for me. It just doesn't make
sense to me. 1 don't feel like 1 have a lot of control over my life at this point
In sum, the evidence presented above suggests that the academic procrastination
treatment may have had a significant effect in terms of improving students' perception of
control over their projects, thereby supporting the original hypothesis that overall project
structure would increase by the end of treatment. After all, one of the main goals of the
treatment used in the present research was to help participants manage their workload by
using various strategies (e.g., breaking down large tasks into smaller steps) and
techniques (e.g., relaxation exercises), not to mention the attempts to restructure their
Other's view. From the original hypotheses, it was expected that participation in
the treatrnent would increase the overall sense of community in students given that group
significantly higher for the treatment group reIative to the comparison group. However,
the fact that this significant difference did not occur irnrnediately after treatment (T3) but
rather during the follow-up (T4) is interesting and open to interpretation. It may be the
perceptions, in general, and this, coupled with the fact that exarns are seen as perhaps
more important academic projects, produced this result. In this instance, perhaps the self-
selection for treatment may be an important difference between the groups. The students
who sought out treatment recognized the need for help with their problems regarding
procrastination and others in their lives may have influenced this as well. In any event, it
is impossible to know exactly why this treatment group effect emerged at T4, and it may
only be left to speculation at this point based upon an understanding of the PPA
Outcome. This PPA project dimension was appraised significantly higher for the
treatrnent group at the post-test (T3) relative to the pre-test (Tl) only. By the end of
treatment, students felt that they sipificantly improved their chances of successfully
completing their academic projects, on average, than before the workshop began. This
would suggest that the treatment may have had some effect on improving student's self-
efficacy - the PPA factor compnsed of project progress and outcome dimensions.
positively correlated to time adequacy (Z: = -05, p. c -001) and negatively correlated to
procrastination (g = -.037,Q < -01). However, in this same snidy, project outcome was
not significantly correlated with progress for these students suggesting that perhaps
expectation for success may still remain high despite procrastination on tasks or limited
tirne in which to complete projects. The expectation of project success was also found to
be positiveIy associated to how much control doctord students had with respect to their
projects (r = -33, p c -01) (Pychyl & Little. 1998). These last two research findings relate
progress was found (see the following section) despite the significant increase in project
outcome at the end of treatment (T3) and; 2 ) the treatrnent group perceived themselves at
having significantiy more control over their projects after treatment had ended.
Sirnilar to the time adequacy dimension, caution needs to be taken regarding the
into consideration that T3 occurred near the end of the semester and that many students
rnay have already completed or neared completion of many academic projects they had
(with the exception of final examinations), thus, reflecting a possible positive account in
was found suggesting that perhaps, by then, students were engaged in 1st-minute
cramming and may have felt that they were not going to do well on their upcorning
been a non-event depending upon what stage students were at in their schedules (e.g.,
they bad no more examinations to write). Future research that would take this into
which to complete their projects (Le., time adequacy dimension), students' appraisals of
treatment. This result does not support the original hypothesis of anticipating an increase
found that "the progress [doctoral students] are currently making on their projects rnay be
more related to having adequate blocks of time in which to engage in projects which they
are passionate about and for which they have social support" (p. 185). The lack of
significantly improved progress for students in this study is even more puzzling when one
considers that procrastination decreased over time. In other words, as the project system
suggests, students receiving treatment sense that they have enough time in which to
complete their projects and feel that they are procrastinating less on tasks and, yet, no
significant amount of perceived project progress has been made! One explanation may be
due perhaps to the notion that, at the time of testing, the majority of these projects were
finished, recently assigned, or just started. In other words, the particular stage (Little,
1983; Blunt & Pychyl, 2000) a project belonged to may be important to understanding
progress scores overall. For example, post-testing (T3) may have coincided with many
students subrnitting end-of-tenn assi,onments which, in turn, may bave accounted for
lower progress ratings as they begin the new projects of preparing for exams on which
progress dimension itself. The standard PPA Project Appraisal Mauyi asks respondents
to rate "how successful you have been in a project so fa?. The use of the term
"successful" rnay have been interpreted by students to be at the level of progress "quality"
rather than the degree of completion, For instance, it rnay be the case that a student fails
to produce a large quantity of work (the volume of work generated rnay be thought of as
being directly proportional to finishing a particular task) and, yet, considers the quality of
the work produced t o be quite good. As a result, this rnay translate into a higher project
progress rating. In contrast, it rnay be the case that quantity is high, but the end product
rnay be so poor or trivial that it is not deemed to be a worthy marker of success. Hence, a
student rnay assign a low progress rating despite meeting a deadline. One may further
speculate that the latter project appraisai rnay be typical of the perfectionist. This
personality characteristic is important when one considers that research has found a
made. Firstly, it is important to bear in mind that acadernic pursuits are dmost
exclusively assigned by others (Le., professors) and are often caxied out in isolation.
Hence, despite their importance, students rnay not fînd their academic projects
particularly enjoyable or, to sorne extent, diKicult. This rnay result in decreased project
cornmitment (e-g., Brunstein, 1993; Pychyl, 1995; Fychyl & Little, 1998). Although it
was not measured directly, this lack of project cornmitment rnay have had significant
negative implications in reIation to project progress. This cascade of events rnay be even
more pertinent to the procrastinating student who, for instance, may also be motivated by
fear of negative project evaluation not to take necessary action to complete projects (or
even start them!) in order to avoid criticism (Little, 1983). At any rate, either one of the
above scenarios could have had an impact on progress ratings in such a manner as to fail
Finally in terrns of the PPA, the issue of project meaning in relation to project
would appraise their projects as more meaningful and: 2) even though project meaning
was not found to increase significantly over the course of treatrnent, it may still be
considered a potentially important marker of SWB (e-g., Little. 1986, 1989; McGregor &
Little. 1998). The following section explores a possible rationale as to why project
treatment.
Based upon past literature, project efficacy has been a robust predictor of SWB
(e.g., Little, 1989; Wilson, 1990; Yetim, 1993) to the extent that "people feel better when
they are doing well and when they expect to be doing well in the future" (McGregor &
Little, 1998, p. 505). In light of the results of the PPA from the present study, it would
appear that SWB improved somewhat given that project outcome increased by the end of
treatment. However, it wouId be premature to speculate that project efficacy is the only
significant predictor of SWB. In fact, according to Little (1998) "...mere efficacy, in the
absence of project meaning, is not likely to enhance well-being" (p. 207). And, it may be
argued that this has implication with respect to the project systems of participants in this
study because, "the least meaningful projects are clearly those related to academic tasks"
(Littie, 1998, p. 200). This statement, in and of itself, dong with the fact that students
ofien find academic projects Iess enjoyable compared to other types of personal projects,
of treatment.
In their study, McGregor and Little (1998) considered goal efficacy or "how Iikely
with life, more positive affect than negative affect - the traditional standards of SWB)
and goal integrity or "how consistent one's projects are with core aspects of the self' (p.
These authors claim that a certain "tension" rnay arise between efficacy and integrity to
produce a "meaning and manageability tradeoff'. For exarnple, if the emphasis of project
systems is on manageability (Le., goal efficacy), people rnay feel happy because they are
accornplishing and managing their goals, but it rnay aiso be that they are engaged in
projects that are not particularly meaningful or what is referred to as "trivial pursuits"
(Little, 1989). Additionally, this focus on action rnay be a way for people to avoid
statement:
...irnrnersing oneself in the busy pursuit of efficacy can at least distract one from
is so often met with sincere bewilderment. For busy people, it rnay seem like an
Therefore, it is conceivable that for some individuals, efficacy rnay overshadow the
construct of meaning with respect to SWB. This is particularly relevant for the
participants of the present research where the emphasis of academic procrastination
meaning per se. Thus, it is conceivable that students' appraisais of project structure
would significantly increase but that appraisals of project meaning would not.
Finally, the present research did not examine goal integrity as it was presented
above. Although, the project dimension self-identity (Le., one of the project dimensions
that loads onto the factor meaning) was included in the Project Appraisal Matrix, many
concept to their academic tasks. This is not uncornmon, in fact, McGregor and Little
(1998) cite this dimension as a limitation in their own research, ".. .it is unclear how
accurately participants were able to rate projects, especially on some of the more abstract
dimensions, such as self-identity" (p. 508). In addition, the project dimension value
congruency (one of the original project dimensions of PPA; see Little, 1983) was ornitted
from the Project Appraisal Matrix because its reIevance to academic pursuits was also
questionable. Thus, to avoid confusing students who completed PPA, it was left out of
the Project Appraisal Matrix. Perhaps if these issues were addressed and clarified in
future research, a significant change in project meaning would be revealed and a clearer
There are four key shortcomings in the present research that need to be
considered. These limitations in the study Iead to a number of suggestions for future
size. Despite efforts made in recniiting smdents on a voluntary basis, only a handful of
students ended up participating. This was due, in part, to the fact that the recruitrnent for
the workshop itself was rather brief. This shortcoming rnay have severely hampered
attempts to gain sufficient visibility of the existence of the workshop thereby reducing the
chances of not only obtaining a larger sample, but also obtaining a more diverse group of
students. In addition, it will be noted that the sample size was also Iimited by the fact that
funding for only one treatment group was availabIe. Furtherrnore, at the counsellor's
request, the treatrnent group could not be larger than about 30 participants.
Consequently, this limitation of sample size irnposed serious restrictions upon the
the research design. However, through the usage of ANCOVA to control for individual
differences at the outset, an attempt was made to preserve the statistical power in the
design. Overail, given the lack of power, it is important to be cautious when interpreting
participants or what Cook and Campbell (1979) consider a threat to externai validity. A
way to compensate for this limitation rnay have been to analyze the data of both: 1)
students who dropped out afier registering for the workshop and 2 ) students who made
initial contact in response to the poster announcement, but then never actually registered
to participate in the workshop. Data obtained from these groups of students could then be
compared to those students who participated in the workshop. This is relevant if one
considers that "what appears to be a successful program may only be successful with
some" (Posavac & Carey, 1992, p- 189). It should be noted that five students satisfied the
fxst condition. When contacted by phone to fmd out why they decided to not participate,
students mentioned that they had either school or job commitments that would interfere
with their workshop attendance. One student dropped out of the university aitogether to
work full-time and commented, "Maybe that's why 1 procrastinated, because 1 really
didn't want to be there." In the case of the second condition, by word of mouth, the
experimenter ofien heard remarks by fellow students who said that they "would like to
attend the workshop" but they were "too busy to fit it in rïght now." It would be sheer
speculation at this point to wonder whether these students would have benefited more
from the workshop than the actual treatment group did. Perhaps they would have
exhibited significant SWB improvements over the course of treatment relative to the
comparison groups. In other words, by their very nature, these students may have been
different than the students who actuaily participated. Thus, it rnay have been beneficial if
these students were asked to complete the questionnaire package despite not being able to
attend the workshop, to help improve the extemal validity of the present research.
participants in these two groups were rmdomly assigned, the pool from which they were
drawn may have lacked sufficient representativeness. It will be recalled that the entire
cornparison group sample was randomly selected from the Procrastination Research
Group mass testing pool based upon scoring in the top 30% on a procrastination measure.
However, it was originally intended that al1 participants in this research would have
onginated from the sarne source, in particular, by voluntarily coming forth in search of
help for their procrastination. If enough students responded to the poster announcement
then this could have been a redis5 random assignment wouid have been possible, and all
participants could have k e n considered equivalent across groups. Unfortunately, this did
Campbell, 1979).
In addition, Cook and Campbell (1979) also warn of the difference between
of the present research, these results may only apply to students in an academic setting.
the target population of interest. In other words, these results do not necessarily reflect
Third. another limitation rnay be in the treatrnent design itself. Past research
yet. no universal intervention has been developed that may be applied on a larger scale as
is similar to the 12-step program used by Alcoholics Anonymous (AA), for example.
However, within the context of the treatment used, several suggestions of how to improve
upon the existing program were made by the participants thernselves. These included:
increasing the number and duration of sessions, more emphasis on making specific
projects public to the group such that participants would have a greater sense of
accountability towards meeting their goal, less redundant content information, and having
the workshop commence earlier in the terrn so students could have enough time to apply
what they leamed over the course of the semester. The latter improvement is shared by
Boice ( 1996) who suggests "that timely interventions should occur early ...during the
period when they can more easily set an efficacious course" (p.80).
Fourth, another limitation with respect to the sample in the present research
pertained to the sixdents themselves. Nthough the exact figures were not obtained in this
study, several students (from both treatment and cornparison groups) mentioned privately
to the experimenter that they suffered from Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder
(ADHD). This pre-existing condition may have been a contrîbuting factor to student
or an inability to stay focused on the task at hand for long penods of tirne. Ln other
words, procrastination rnay have been a symptom of having ADHD. Consequently, this
aspect of individual variability is likely to have increased the amount of emor variance
These limitations in the present study, particularly the limitations related to the
research design, lead to a number of suggestions for future research such as tracking
specific projects, categorizing different types of projects, adding other PPA project
method of ''tracking" specific projects or groups of projects over the course of treatment.
For instance, in one of his studies, Lay (1990) asked students to indicate a deadline for
their projects, for exarnple, within 2 months (Le., short-term), beyond 2 months (Le.,
intervals asking them to rate each project, using a version of PPA, across various
instance, analyzing projects based upon their classification with respect to moiarity (e-g.,
Little, 1983, 1988, 1989) (cg., "wnte my English essay" versus "get into graduate
school") as well as content ( e g , reading projects, wnting projects, group projects) may
be worth exploring in the future. In the research done by Elliot, Sheldon and Church
procrastination") and approach goals (e-g., "be more conscientious and efficient") were
created from the lists of personal strivings students provided during testing. The results
revealed that students with a greater proportion of avoidance goals reported Iower global
SWB over the course of the semester. Through path analyses, perceived progress was
found to be a mediator between avoidance regulation and SWB as the authors explain:
...the pursuit of avoidance goals would lead to low perceptions of progress and
goals is likely to induce worry, threat, anxiety, and research has clearly shown that
Further, those students who reported fewer self-regdatory skills were found to have a
and expected progress was also found (r=-.19, p < -05)suggesting that the more
avoidance goals students had, the more students expected to do poorly on their goals.
Taken together, these resulu help to support the notion that much rnay be gained from the
way of detennîning the change in SWB relative to personal projects in the context of
procrastination.
Funire research might aiso benefit greatiy by expanding upon the PPA project
dimensions which are by no means comprehensive and rnay "not capture dl of the
elements that underlie global appraisals of.. .[project] systems" (Pychyl & Little, 1998, p.
452). Possible dimension additions rnay include dejection, anxiety, and commitment, to
name a few. From his research, Lay (1990) found a repeated link between dejection and
happy, sad, disappointed, and disgusted. Lay concluded that dejection be treated as an
constnict as an added dimension to PPA, or perhaps even in the affect scales of SWB.
Likewise, the constnict anxiev rnay be relevant, given that relaxation techniques were a
part of treatment, and rnay be included as either an added dimension of PPA, an extra
item in the negative affect scale, or even as a separate scde altogether. The inclusion of
by previous research by Pychyl (1995) who concluded that the likelihood of success, with
any given project, rnay depend more upon a student's personal sense of commitment to
that project- Furthemore, "in terms of academic life at least, this seems to be an
important eIement in a student's sense of well-being" (p. 105). The comment from one
of them but just still following through with it, it was kind of a way of like
reaffirming that I'm cornmitted to trying to stop procrastination even though I'rn
still procrastinating.
These ad hoc dimensions, dong with others, may serve to capture significant changes
within workshop participants, over time, which may not be reveded through typicd SWB
measures alone.
part of the testing procedure. For instance. others' ratings of the individual's
completed or are submitted past their due date, over the course of the semester, could be
Conclusions
successful at reducing academic procrastination in the participants, based upon the self-
reported scores for several procrastination rneasures (i-e., PASS, APSI, project dimension
procrastination). This was the main purpose of the present research and provided
differences resulted for any of the PPA project factors. However, within-subjects analysis
these resdts warrants caution given the limitations of the overall research design, for
exarnple, the srnall sarnple size. Therefore, future replication of this study would be
beneficial dong with the recommendations of tracking and categorizing various types of
academic projects, adding other PPA project dimensions, and including more objective
the efficacy of this treatrnent program as well as extend the outcome research, in general,
'A second comparison group not receiving PPA or treatment was included in the
design of the present research to determine whether PPA had some fonn of clinical
impact upon students. The possibiiity that PPA could prove to be therapeutic originated
from Little (1986) who stated that PPA, "...allows the therapist/counsellor to unpack the
dimension into those particular projects and pursuits which generated the client's
interuality score, a tactic which may well have therapeutic or interventional si,onificance"
(p. 603). Further, Little adds that the project dimensions, "...rnap on well to recurring
issues in clinical and counselling psychology [adding] further impetus to treating these as
having potential relevance to diagnostic and therapeutic activities" (p. 607). It was,
therefore, conceivable that by engaging students and helping them to monitor, be aware
of, and identify their feelings about their academic projects, PPA may have had an impact
upon students, clinically, to some degree. Hence, this rnay be considered similar to
receiving treatrnent to some extent. Consequently, in light of the potential for this
differences occurred between the treatment group and the first comparison group, and
significant differences occumd between the first and second comparison group, in terms
of SWB.
References
Barlow, D-H. (198 1). On the relation of clinical research to clinical practice: Curent
issues, new directions. Jortrnnl of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 147-155.
Baumeister, R.F., Heatherton, T.F., & Tice, D.M. (1994). Losing control: How alid why
Beck, A.T. ( 1976). Cognitive therapy and the enzotionnl disorders. Madison. CT:
Blunt, A., & Pychyl, T.A. (2000).Task aversiveness and procrastination: A multi-
Westport. CT.
Bnrnstein, J-C. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-king: A longitudinal study.
Burka, 1.B-, & Yuen, L.M. (1983). Procrastination: Why you do it. What to do about it.
Campbell, D.T. (1957). Factors relevant to the validity of experiments in social settings.
Cook, T.D., & Campbell, D-T. (1979). Quasi-experimentatiorz: Design & analysis
Csikszentmihalyi, M., Larsen, R.J., & Prescott, S. (1977). The ecology of adolescent
the annud conference clf the Canadian Association of College and University
Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984)- The independence of positive and negative affect.
Diener, E., Emrnons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life
Diener, E., Suh, E., & Fujita, F. (1996). Events and subjective well-being: Only recent
events matter. Journal .uf Persortalis. and Social Ps),chology, 70, 1091- 1 102.
Dunn, S., htallaz, M., Sheppard, B-,& Lindstrom, R. (1987). Social support and
Elliot, A.J., Sheldon, KM., & Church, M.A, (1997). Avoidance personal goals and
A. Freeman (Eds.), Cognition and psychotherapy (pp. 3 13-323). New York: Plenum.
Ellis, A., & Knaus, W. J. ( 1977). Overcoming procrastination. New York: Institute for
Rationd Living.
Emmons, R.A. (199 1). Personal strivings. daily life events, and psychological and
12, 1233-1237.
Ferrari, J.R., & Pychyl, T.A. (Eds.). (2000). Procrastination: Current issues and new
Garfield, S.L. (198 1). Evaluating the psychotherapies. Behavior Therapy, 12,295-307-
GoIlwitzer, P.M. (1990). Action phases and rnind-sets. In E.T. Higgins & R.M.
Harris, N.N., & Sutton, R.I. (1983). Task procrastination in organizations: A frarnework
Jacobson, N.S ., & Revenstorf, D. (1988). Statistics for assessing the clinical
Jourard, S.M. (197 1). n i e transparent self (2nded.). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.
Kazdin, A.E. (1977). Assessing the clinical or applied importance of behavior change
Kendall, P.C.,& Norton-Ford, J.D. (1982). Therapy outcome research methods. In P.C.
Keppel, G. ( 1991). Design and a n a k i r : A researcher ' s handboot (3rd ed.) New
Jersey: Prentice-Hall h c .
Kuehlwein, KT.. & Rosen, H. (Eds.). (1993). Cognitive therapies in action: Evolving
Personality, 20,474-495.
Personality, 5 , 9 1- I 03.
Lay, C.H., Knish, S., & Zanatta, R. (1992). Self-handicappers and procrastinators: A
Personali~,26,242-257.
Little, B. R. (1983). Personai projects: A rationale and method for investigation.
Little, B.R. (1988). Personal projects research: Method, theory and research. Final
Ottawa, Canada,
and the search for coherence. In D.M. Buss & N. Cantor (Eds.), Personaliq
psyclrology: Recent trends and emerging directiorrs (pp. 15-3 1). New York:
Springer-Ver1as.
Little. B.R. (1998). Personal project pursuit: Dimensions and dynarnics of personal
meaning. In P.T.P. Wong & P.S.Fry (Eds.), The hrtmnrz qrtest for meaning: A
handbookfor research and clNzical applications. (pp. 193-2 12). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
McGregor, I., & Little, B.R. (1998). Personal projects, happiness, and meaning: On
doing well and being yourself. Journal of Personaliry and Social Psychology, 74,
492-5 12.
Milgram, N. (199 1). Procrastination. In R. Dulbecco (Ed-),EncycZopedia of human
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R.F. (in press). Self-regdation and depletion of limited
Muraven, M., Baumeister, R.F., & Tice, D.M. (1999). Longitudinal improvement of
Myers, D.G., & Diener, E. ( 1995). Who is happy? Psychological Science. 6, 10- 19.
Omodei. M.M., & Wearing, A.J. (1990). Need satisfaction and involvement in personal
Palys, T. S.. & Little, B. R. (1983). Perceived life satisfaction and the organization of
the personal project systems. Jormtal of Personal* and Social PsycIzology, 44.
1221-1230.
Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993a). The affective and cognitive context of self-reported
Pavot, W-, & Diener, E- (1993b). Review of the satisfaction with life scale.
Posavac, E.J., & Carey, R.G. (1992). Progarn evaluarion: Merhods and case studies (4th
Canada
PychyI, T.A,, Lee, J., Thibodeau, R-, & Blunt, A. (2000). Five days of emotions: An
Pychyl, T-A,, & Little, B-R, (1998). Dimensional specificity in the prediction of
Research. 45,423-473.
Pychyl, T-A., Morin, R.W.. & Salmon. B.R. (2000). Procrastination and the planning
Rothblum, E.D- (1990). Fear of failure: The psychodynamic, need achievement. fear of
Rothblum, ED-, Solomon, L.J., & Murakami, J- (1986). Affective, cognitive. and
Semb, G-,Glick, D.M., & Spencer, R.E. (1979). Student withdrawals and delayed work
Senécal, C., Koestner, R., & VaIlerand, R.J. (1995). Self-regulation and academic
Senécal, C.,Koestner, R.,& Vallerand, R.J. (1997). Trait and situational factors in
Personalis; 12,889-903.
Silver, M., & Sabini, J. (198 1). Procrastinating. Jortrnal for rhe T h e o y of Social
Siiver, M., & Sabini, J, (1982, January). When it's not really procrastinating,
Solomon, L. J., & Rothblum, E.D. (1984). Academic procrastination: Frequency and
Solornon, L. J., & Rothblum, E-D- (1988)- Procrastination Assessrnent Scale - Students.
(PASS). In J. Fischer & K. Corcoran (Eds-), Measures for clinicul practice (pp. 446-
performance, stress, and health: The costs and benefits of dawdling. Psychologicnl
Science, 8.454-458 -
Ontario. Canada.
Wolf, M.M. (1978). Social validity: The case for subjective measurement or how
applied behavior analysis is finding its heart. Jortrnal of Applied Behavior Anatysis.
11,203-214.
Appendix A 1
The purpose of an informed consent is to ensure that you understand the purpose of the study and the
nature of your involvement, in order to determine whether you wish to participate in the study.
Research Personnel: The following people are involved in this research project and may be contacted
ar any time: Kelly Binder (Rinciple Investigator, 520-2600 e x t 2705),Dr. T. Pychyl (Faculty Sponsor,
520-2600, e x t 1403). If you have ethical questions/concerns about this shxdy please contact Dr. M.
Gick (Chair, Department of Psychology Ethics Cornmittee, 520-2600 ext. 2664) or Dr. K. Matheson
(Chair, Department of Psychology, 520-2600, ext. 2648).
Purpose: The purpose of this smdy is to investigate the academic procrastination behavior of students
attending an academic procrastination workshop over the course of the semester. No credit will be
offered for participation in this study.
Workshop: You will be asked to attend six group sessions (one per week). Each session will last two
hours. The workshop wiil help you understand your behavior and offer strategies to overcorne your
procrastination. Some between-session " a s s i ~ e n t s "will be given to you throughout the workshop.
Follow-up phone calls may be made to you regarding your progress.
Task Reauirements: You wiil be asked to cornplete several questionnaires at four different times
throughout the semester. The questionnaires wiil include questions asking you about how you feel and
how you manage your tirne, The questionnaires will take 2 hours or less to complete each time,
~nonvmity/~onfidentialik: The data couected in this snidy are confidential. AU data are coded such
that your name is not associated with the data. Any data reported will be based on group data, or by .
code number and will only be made available 20 the researchers associated with this project. Personal
disclosure(s) shared within the group by group members must be kept exclusively within the group.
t Withdraw: Participation in this study is strictly voluntasy. If, for any reason, you wish to
R i ~ h to
withdraw from the study and the workshop, you haveihe right to do so at any rime without academic
penalty.
- - ---
1 have'read the above description of the Academic Procrastination Workshop study and understand
the conditions of my participation. My signature indicates that 1agree to participate in the study.
Date
Appendix A2
The purpose of an informed consent is to ensure that you understand the purpose of the study and
the nature of your involvement, in order to determine whether you wish to participate in the
study.
Research Personnel: The foLlowing people are involved in tbis research project and rnay be
contacted at any time: Keiiy Binder (Principle hvestigator, 520-2600 e x t 2705),Dr. T.Pychyl
(Faculty Sponsor, 520-2600, ext. 1403). If you have ethical questions/concems about this smdy
please contact Dr. M. Gick (Chair, Department of Psych010gy Ethics Committee, 520-2600 ext.
2664) or Dr, K. Matheson (Chair,Department of Psychology, 520-2600, ext. 2648).
Purpose: The purpose of this study is to investigale the academic procrastination behavior of
students over the course of the semester- Your scores on severai questionnaires will be compared
to the scores of other students participating in an academic procrastination workshop,
Task Rmuirements: You will be asked to compfete several questionnaires at four different
times throughout the semester- The questionnaires will include questions asking you about how
you feel and how you manage your time. The questionriaires will take 2 hours or less to
complete each time. Credit(s) may be received for participating in the study depending upon
how many credits you have already.
Anonvmitv/Co~dentialitv:The data collected in this study are cofidential. All data are
coded such that your name is not associated with the data. Any data reported will be based on
group data, or by code number and wiU o d y be made available to the researchers associated with
this project.
Right ta Withdraw: Participation in tiiis study is strictly voluntaq. If, for any reason, you wish
to withdraw Çom the study, you have the nght to do so at any t h e without academic penalty.
I have read the above description of the ~ c a d e m i cProcrastination study and undentand the
conditions of my participation. My signature indicates that I agree to participate in the study.
-p
Date
Appendix A3
The purpose of an infomed consent is to ensure that you understand the purpose of the study and
the nature of your involvement, in order to determine whether you wish to participate in the
study.
Research Personnel: The following people are involved in this research project and may be
contacted at any tirne: Kelly Binder m c i p l e Investigator. 520-2600 ext. 2705). Dr. T. Pychyl
(Facuity Sponsor, 520-2600, ext. 1403). If you have ethical questions/concerns about this smdy
please contact Dr. M. Gick (Chair, Department of Psychology Ethics Committee, 520-2600 ext
2664) or Dr. K. Matheson (Chair,Department of Psychology, 520-2600, ext. 2648).
Pumse: The purpose of this smdy is to investigate the academic procrastïnation behavior of
students over the course of the semester. Your scores on severai questionnaires will be compared
t o the scores of other studenu participahg in an academic procrasruiation workshop.
Task Requirements: You wiiI be asked to complete several questionnaires at three different
times throughout the semester. The questionnaires will include questions asking you about how
you feel and how you manage your time. The questionnaires will take about 2 hours or less to
complete each tirne. Credit(s) may be received for partïcipating in the snidy depending upon
how many credits you have aiready.
Anonvmitv/Co~dentialitv:The data coilected in this study are confidentid. Ail data are
coded such that your name is not associated with the data. Any data reponed w i l l be based on
group data, or by code number and WU oniy be made available to the researchers associated with
this project.
Ripht to Withdraw: Participation in this snidy is strictiy voluntary. If, for any reason, you wish
to withdraw nom the study, you have the right to do so at any time without academic penalty.
- - - - -- - - - -
I have read the above description of the Academic Rocrastination study and understand the
conditions of my participation. My signature indicates that 1 agree to participate in the snidy.
Date
Appendix B 1
The purpose of this study was to examine how effective an academic procrastination
workshop was on decreasing the tendency-forstudents to procrastinate on university-related ta&
(e-g., studying for an exarn). Questionnaires were given to you before and after the workshop to
see whether there was an improvement in your procrastinah'on. In other words, by participating
in the workshop, it was expected thar you would procrastinate l e s .
Generally, people procrastinate for a variety of reasons. For instance. the task may be
unpleasant so it is avoided. Another reason someone rnay procrashate may be because they ~e
a h i d they wïii not be able to complete the task successfidiy. This is known as "fear of fidure".
On the other hand, sometimes people may delay doing somethuig (e-g., wxite a term paper)
because they are womed that if they do well (e.g., get a good mark) they wiil have to do just as
weii or even bener the next tirne-
As a result, procrastination often creates feelings of subjective discornfort. In order to
m e s u r e these feelings, a subjective weii-being scde was also inchded in the questionnaire. The
same scale was given to you at two different times. These PNO times were before and afier the
workshop. Ifyour scores on the scale before-the workshop are different fiom your scores on the
scale ufier the workshop it may mean two things: 1) there was a change in your well-being over
time, or 2) the change in your well-being was due to the workshop. In generd, it is hoped that the
workshop helped you to feel better. Therefore, it is expected that your subjective well-being
scores will be higher at the end of the workshop than before the workshop.
Procrastination has also been found to be related to depression and anxiety. Therefore,
you were asked questions about how you felt so that your depression and anxiety levels would be
obtained This information wiii help me to d e out other factors that may affect the results of this
study.
In addition, you were asked to write down your academic "projects" and rate them on
various aspects, for exarnple, how difficult or enjoyable you found these tasks to be. This
information was gathered at several different times throughout the semester in order to provide
me with a means of tracking your on-going academic progress. In other words, it gives me a
record of which projects were carried out by you and how you felt about your projects. In
previous research, a relationship has k e n found between project progress and weLl-being. That is
to Say, people who make progress or complete their projects are usually happier than people who
do not-
Fï~~aily,I do wish to stress that your individual scores/data wiU be kept strictly
confidentid. Any data reported will be based on group data or code number to ensure anonymity.
Thank you for your participation in this research, Your time and efforts were greatly
appreciated. If, for any reason, this experiment has raised issues of a personal nature for you
please contact your family doctor, Health Services (520-6674), or phone the Ottawa Distress
Centre at 238-33 11.
If you wish to discuss any additiond aspects of this research the following people are
available for appointments: Kelly Binder (Principle Investigator, 520-2600 ext. 2705) and Dr. T.
Pychyl (Faculry Sponsor, 520-2600, ext. 1403). If you have ethical questions/concerns about this
study please contact Dr. M. Gick (Chair, Deparment of Psychology Ethics Committee, 520-2600
ext. 2664) or Dr. K. Matheson (Chair,Deparunent of Psychology, 520-2600, ext. 2648).
Appendîx B2
Thank you for your participation in this research. Your time and efforts were greatly
appreciated, If you feel that you need help regarding your procrastination you may contact the
Student Life Center (520-6600), or Health Services (520-6674). If, for any reason, this
experiment has raised issues of a personal nature for you please contact your family doctor,
Health Services (520-6674), or phone the Ottawa Disaess Centre at 238-33 11.
If you wish to discuss any additional aspects of this research the following people are
avaiiable for appointments: KeUy Binder (Prïnciple Znvestigator, 520-2600 ext. 2705) and Dr.
T.Pychyl (Faculty Sponsor, 520-2600, ext. 1403). If you have ethical questions/concerns about
this study please contact Dr. M. Gick (Chair, Department of Psychology Ethics Committee,
520-2600 ext. 2664) or Dr. K. Matheson (Chair, Department of Psychology, 520-2600, ext.
2648).
Appendix B3
The purpose of th% study was to examine how your scores on several
questionnaires wiU compare to students involved in an academic procrastination
workshop. Specifically, 1am interested in knowing how effective this workshop was on
decreasing the tendency for students to procrastinate on university-related tasks (e-g.,
studyinp for an exam). Questionnaires were given to you at different intervals to see
whether there was any change in your tendency to procrastination over the course of the
semester.
Generally, people procrastinate for a variety of reasons- For instance, the task
rnay be unpleasant so it is avoided. Another reason someone rnay procrastinate rnay be
because they are afraid they will not be able to cornplete the task successfulIy. This is
known as "fear of failure". On the other hand, sometimes people rnay delay doing
something (e-g., write a term paper) because they are worried that if they do well (e.g., get
a good mark) they will have to do just as weIl or even better the next tirne.
As a result, procrastination often creates feelings of subjective discornfort. In
order to measure these feelings, a subjective well-being scaie was incIuded in the
questionnaire. The same scale was given to you at different intervals to see whether there
was any change in your feelings over time.
Procrastination has also been found to be related to depression and anxiety.
Therefore, you were asked questions about how you felt so that your depression and
anxiety levels would be obtained. This information will help me to rule out other factors
that rnay affect the results of this study.
Finally, 1do wîsh to stress that your individual scoreddata wiI1 be kept strictly
confidentid. Any data reported w i l be based on group data or code number to ensure
anonymity.
Thank you for your participation in this research. Your time and efforts were
greatly appreciated. If you feel that you need help regarding your procrastination you rnay
contact the Student Life Center (520-6600), or Health Services (520-6674). If, for any
reason, this expriment has raised issues of a personal nature for you please contact your
family doctor, Heaith Services (520-6674), or phone the Ottawa Distress Centre at 238-
3311.
If you wish to discuss any additional aspects of this research the following people
are available for appointrnents: Kelly Binder (Principle Investigator, 520-2600 ext. 2705)
and Dr. T. Pychyl (Faculty Sponsor, 520-2600, ext. 1403). If you have ethical
questions/concerns about this study please contact Dr. M. Gick (Chair, Department of
Psychology Ethics Committee, 520-2600 ext. 2664) or Dr. K. Matheson (Chair,
Department of Psychology, 520-2600, ext. 2648).
Appendix C
Never Al ways
procrastinate Almost never Sometimes Nearly always procrastinate
3. To what extent do you want to decrease your tendency to procrastinate on this task?
Never Aiways
procrastinate Alrnost never ~ometimes Neariy always procrastinate
6 . To what extent do you want to decrease your tendency to procrastinate on this task?
Never Al ways
procrastinate Nmost never Sometimes Nearl y always procrastinate
9. To what extent do you want to decrease your tendency to procrastina.te on this ta&?
Never Always
procrastinate Almost never Sometimes Nearly always procrastinate
12. To what extent do you want to decrease your tendency to procrastinate on this task?
Never Always
procrastinate Almost never Sometimes Nearly always procrastinate
15. To what extent do you want to decrease your tendency to procrastinate on this task?
Never Always
procrastinate Almost never Sometimes Nearly dways procrastinate
18. To what extent do you want to decrease your tendency to procrastinate on this task?
It's near the end of the semester. The term paper you were assigned at the beginning of
the semester is due very soon. You have not begun work on this paper, There are reasons
why you have been procrastinating on this task.
Rate each of the following reasons on a 5-point scale according to how much it reflects
why you procrastinated at the time. Mark your answers by writing the letter "a" to "e" in
the space to the ieft of each statement.
You had a hard time knowing what to include and what not to include in
your paper.
You waited until a classmate did hishers, so that he/she could give you
some advice.
There's some infonnation you needed to ask the professor, but you felt
uncomfortable approaching hirdher.
12. You looked forward to the excitement of doing this task at the last minute.
14. You were concerned that if you did well, your classmates would resent
you.
17. You felt it just takes too long to wnte a term paper.
19. You knew that your classmates hadn't started the paper either.
22. You were concerned that if you got a good grade, people would have
higher expectations of you in the future.
23. You waited to see if the professor would give you some more information
about the paper.
24. You set very high standards for yourself and you womed that you
wouldn't be able to meet those standards.
How frcquemly lut weck did you engage m the fOllowing behaviors or thoughts?
u El
2. The conditions of my life are excellent.
Strongly Siïghtiy Neither Agree Slightly Strongly
Disagree Disagree Disagree not Disagree Ag ree Agtee Agree
17 cl O O O O
3. 1 am satisfied w%hmy Iife.
CI 0 O
the important things I want in life.
4. So far, I have.go~en
n n cl
Frustrated
Not at al! Extremely Much
0
Worried or Anxious
Not al al1
Joyful
Not at al1
Pleased
Not at ail Extremely Much
Angry or hostile
Not al ail Extremely Much
n
Not ai dl Extremelv Much
Depressed
Not at al1 Extremely Much
0 O
Not at al1
Gui IL
Not at a1 Extremelv Much
n
Appendix F1
1am interested in studying the kinds of activities and concerns that university
students have. 1cd1 these personal academicprojects. As students, we al1 have a
number of persond academic projects at any given time that we think about, plan for,
carry out and sometimes (though not aiways) complete.
1am also very interested in finding out how students feel about these personal
academic projects, how enjoyable they are, and so on. 1would appreciate it if you could
begin by writing down in the next ten minutes as many persona1 academic projects as you
can that you are engaged in or thinking about at the present time - remernber these are not
necessarily forma1 projects, or important ones - 1 would prefer you to give me more of the
everyday kinds of activities or concems that characterize your academic life at present.
List of Academic Projects
Please go ahead and wrïte down as many as you can in ten minutes.
Now select ten projects fkom your list on the previous page that you are most
likely to engage in over the next month or so. If you wrote down fewer than ten projects,
see if you can think of several more, or break down some of those you listed into several
projects. It is important that you try to select ten projects.
In rows 1 to 18 please rate each one of your projects using any number from O to
10, inclusively, on the following dimensions:
1. Importance: how important each project is to you at the present time (use 10 if the
project is very important to you and O if it is not at al1 important to you).
2. Enjoyrnent: how you enjoy working on each project (use 10 if you enjoy it a great
d e d and O if you do not enjoy it at dl).
3. Difficulty: how difficuk you find it to carry out each project (use IO for a project
that you find very difficult to carry out and O for one that you do not
find difficult at dl).
4. Visibility: how visible each project is to the relevant people who are close to you,
that is how aware are they that you are engaged in this project (use 10
for a project which is very visible to those around you and O for a
project which is not at al1 visible to those around you).
5. Control: how much you feel you are in control of each project (use 10 for a
project over which you feel in complete control and O for a project over
which you feel you have no control at dl).
6 . Initiation: how much you feel responsible for having initiated each project (use 10
if you feel filly responsible for having initiated a project and O if you
feel you have taken no part whatsoever in initiating a project).
7. Stress: how stressful it is for you to carry out each project (use 10 if a project is
very stressful to carry out and O if a project is very relaxing to carry
out),
8. Time Adequacy: how much you feel that the arnount of time you spend working on each
project is adequate (use 10 if you feel that the amount of time spent on a
project is perfectly adequate and O if you feel, for one reason or another,
that the amount of time you spend working on a project is not at al1
adequate).
9-Outcome: what you anticipate the outcorne of each project to be (use 10 if you
think that a project will be extremely successful and O if you think that a
project will tuni out to be a total failure).
11- Others' view : how important each project is seen to be by relevant people who are
close to you (use 10 if a project is seen by others as very important and
O if it is seen as not important at d).
12. Positive impact: how much you feel that each project helps the others. Don't worry
whether it hinders or not, we'll get to that on the next dimension (use 10
to indicate that a project greatly increases your chances of working on
other projects and O to indicate that a project haç no positive effect).
13. Negative impact: how much you feel that each project hinders other projects (use 10 to
indicate that a project seriously hinders your chances of working on
other projects and O to indicate that it does not have any negative
effect).
14. Progress: how successful you have been in a project so far (use 10 to indicate that
you have been very successful and O to indicate that you have had no
success at dl).
15. Challenge: to what extent each project is demanding and chalIenging to you (use 10
if a project is most challenging and O if it is not challenging at dl).
16- Absorption: to what extent you become engrossed or deeply involved in a project
(use 10 if you generally get absorbed in an activity and O if you ten to be
uninvolved when doing it).
17. Guilt: how much guilt is associated with a project (use 10 if you feel very
guilty in regards to this project and O if you do not feel guilty at dl).
18. Procrastination: to what extent do you delay or put off doing a project (use 10 for a
project that you almost always put off doing and O if you do not put off
this project at ail.
tnriroçtions: 1. Write the names ofthe tm projects yon chose fiom your project lis@) m the
spaces provided below CL^., in the ten ~
M
IO
II~
II under "List of3?rojectsN).
2. Using the giossnry pmvided on the prevïous two pages, rate each of your projects
on the project dimensions in the m&x
Projecî Dimensions l
Appendix F2
Glossaw
In rows I to 18 please rate each one of your projects using any nurnber from O to 10 on
the following dimensions,
1- Importance: how important each project is to you at the present time (use 10 if the
project is very important to you and O if it is not at al1 important to you).
2. Enjoyment: how you enjoy working on each project (use 10 if you enjoy it a great
deal and O if you do not enjoy it at dl).
3- Difficulty: how difficult you find it to carry out each project (use 10 for a project
that you find very dificult to cany out and O for one that you do not
find difficult at dl)-
4. VisibiIity: how visible each project is to the relevant people who are close to you,
that is how aware are they that you are engaged in this project (use 10
for a project which is very visible to those around you and O for a
project which is not at all visible to those around you).
5. Control: how much you feel you are in control of each project (use 10 for a
project over which you feel in complete control and O for a project over
which you feel you have no control at dl).
6. Initiation: how much you feel responsible for having initiated each project (use 10
if you feel fully responsible for having initiated a project and O if you
feel you have taken no part whatsoever in initiating a project).
7, Stress: how stressful it is for you to c a r y out each project (use 10 if a project is
very stressful to carry out and O if a project is very relaxing to carry
out).
Appendix G
Between-session Assignment # l
Record thoughts, feelings (including how intense they are), behaviours, that have to do with
procrastination
AFTER
Appendix H
Poster Announcement