A Blind Mobility Aid Modeled After Echolocation of Bats: A On in FM A in A Is 70 So As A A in
A Blind Mobility Aid Modeled After Echolocation of Bats: A On in FM A in A Is 70 So As A A in
A Blind Mobility Aid Modeled After Echolocation of Bats: A On in FM A in A Is 70 So As A A in
MAY 1991 46 I
Ims - 1
cl
obstacle 41
E
*
Frequency i n kHr
(a)
Attenuation in dB
Horizontal
(b)
f=SOhHz
li c - ’ J 90
Attenuation in dE U / U
Fig. 2. Block diagram of our niobility aid device Vert ical
(C)
k-2 ~ ” *
Fig. 5 . Arrangement of the transducers
transducers
(a)
0
(C) (d)
Fig. 7 . Arrangements of obstacles and transducers for psychophysical ex-
periment of the discrimination ability of poles.
[see Fig. 7(a)]. The pole was moved manually and slowly
to prevent nondevice stimuli from affecting the judgment.
The order of diameter presentation went from the largest
to smallest.
(b) Experiment 2
Fig. 6 . (a) Relative intensities of the reflected sound as a function of the The angle 8 from the front of the observer was changed
direction of the pole. (b) Arrival time difference of the reflected sounds until the pole was just perceived to be at a different direc-
between two receivers as a function of the directions of the pole.
tion [see Fig. 7(b)].
Next, the ability of a subject to discriminate between
sity and time differences produce a sound image inside the two objects was evaluated by the following three experi-
head through binaural sound perception that changes de- ments. In the experiments, two other kinds of ultrasound
pending on the direction of obstacle, although auditory signals were used besides the downswept FM signal to
localization based on sound intensity differences is best determine the most suitable. These consisted of a 50-kHz
made in a higher range than the output produced by the constant frequency (CF) with 1-ms duration and an up-
device. swept FM signal from 40 to 70 kHz with 1-ms duration.
Each subject was tested using all these signals in the order
111. EVALUATION METHODFOR DISCRIMINATION of CF, upswept, and downswept FM signals. The various
BETWEEN Two OBJECTS frequency variations with time are shown in Fig. 8.
The ability of subjects to perceive an object was eval- In the third and fourth experiments, two poles 70 cm
uated by following two experiments. Two normal-hearing high were placed in front of the observer at an inclination
subjects participated in these experiments. The subjects of 60" [see Fig. 7(c)]. The diameter of the right pole was
ranged in age from 24 to 29 years and had hearing thresh- 30 mm, and during the course of each experiment, the
olds better than 10 dB HL from 125 to 4000 Hz. The diameter of the left was reduced from 30 to 3 mm.
threshold was taken as the geometric mean of the two sub-
jects. In the experiments, a pole 70 cm high was placed Experiment 3
in front of the observer as shown in Fig. 7(a) and (b). The The distance R was changed until both poles were per-
pole was made of acrylic material. The diameter of the ceived to be at the same distance.
pole was reduced from 20 to 2 mm incrementally.
Experiment 4
Experiment 1 R was changed until the two objects could just be per-
The distance R was made large. The pole was then ceived as being separate.
moved continually closer until it could just be perceived In the last experiment, the left pole was replaced by a
464 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING, VOL. 38, NO. 5, MAY 1991
70 Upswept FM
/'
300 4
L
Y
c 60
3
g 50
U
%
i
/'
40 ,'
0 0.5 1 .o
t i m e i n msec
2 5 8 10 12 15 20
Fig. 8. Time spectrum patterns of three orientational sounds used for the d i a m e t e r o f p o l e i n mm
psychophysical experiment.
Fig. 9. Distance ( R ) at which the pole could be perceived at the different
direction from the front of the observer.
steel plate perpendicular to the line between the plate and
observer [see Fig. 7(d)]. The plate was 153 cm high by
124 cm wide. In the course of this experiment, the di-
ameter of the right pole was reduced from 30 to 3 mm.
Experiment 5
R was made large. The pole was then moved closer un-
til it could just be perceived.
c o n s t a n t frequency p
i
Final Rep., 1973.
[7] E. Foulke, “The perceptual basis for mobility,” Amer. Found. Blind
Res. Bull., vol. 23, pp. 1-8. 1971.
[8] L . Kay, “The design and evaluation of a sensory aid to enhance spa-
tial perception of the blind,” Dep. Elect. Eng., Univ. Canterbury,
New Zealand, Rep. 21, 1973.
191 V. Bruns, “Basilar membrane and its anchoring system in the cochlea
of the greater horseshoe bat,” Anat. Embryo/., vol. 161. pp. 29-50,
1980.
[IO] W. A. Wimsatt, Ed., Biology of Bats: Vol. 3. New York: Aca-
demic, 1977.
[ I I ] J. A. Simmons and J . A . Vermon. “Echolocation: Discrimination of
targets by the bat. Eptesicus fuscus,” J. Exp. Zool.. vol. 176, pp.
315-328, 1971.
[ 121 J . A. Simmons. “The resolution of target range by echolocating bats,”
J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., vol. 54. pp. 157-73, 1973.
60
II ”‘-
1
1
1 2 5 10 30
d i a m e t e r o f pole i n m m
Tohru Ifukube was born in Hokkaido, Japan, in
Fig. 13. Distance ( R ) at which the pole was perceived as a separate object 1946. He received the M.S. and Dr. degrees in
as a function of the diameter of the pole. electronics from Hokkaido University in 197 I and
1977. respcctively.
He joined the Research Institute of Applied
most effective for the perception of small obstacles placed Electricity o f Hokkaido University in 1971, where
near a wall. he studied information processing of the auditory
Temporal masking may explain why the downswept FM and tactile senses. He also designed sensory sub-
stitute devices such as a tactile vocorder for the
was more effective in all cases. Sounds that occur earlier deaf. a monosyllabic voice typewriter for the dis-
are not easily masked by later sounds due to temporal abled. and a niobilitv aid for the blind. He was a
masking. With downswept FM, the high-frequency Visiting Associate Professor at Stanford University. Stanford. CA, in 1984.
He is now Associate Professor of the Division of Medical Electronics of
sounds occur first, and these sounds are important to de- the Research Institute of Applied Electricity at Hakkaido University. His
tect smaller objects. Whether this speculation is true or research interests include the analysis of human sensory functions and the
not, our mobility aid modeled after the bat’s echolocation design of sensory substitutes for the disabled.
system seems to be effective in detecting small objects
placed in front of the head.
The test used to evaluate the scheme is of a limited and
Tadayuki Sasaki was born in Hirosaki. Japan, in
preliminary nature. Although the package combined with 1958. He received the B.S. degree in physics and
a microcomputer board and battery pack for the device is the M.S. degree in biomedical engineering from
relatively large, this device can be made in an inconspic- Hokkaido University, Sapporo. Japan. in 1981 and
1984, respectively.
uous package and can incorporate cosmetic features. The Since 1984, he has worked as a researcher at
ears are not completely closed with the earphones for this the Rescarch Institute of the National Rchabilita-
device, so as to not prevent the user from making use of tion Center for the Disabled, Tokorozawa. Japan.
His research includes an analysis of the echolo-
other naturally occurring sounds to aid mobility. The cation mechanism of bats. a design for mobility
presently evaluated device has to be used with a cane or aids lor the blind, and application
.. of an ultrasonic
a guide dog and is only part of the mobility task. The imaging system to robots.
Mr. Sasaki is a member of the Robotics Society of Japan. the Acoustical
essence of mobility is the motion of a person, either body Society of Japan, the Institute of Electronics. Information and Communi-
movement through walking or head movements to local- cation Engineers. the Japan Society of Medical Electronics and Biological
ize sounds. The ultimate test will be the ability of the Engineering, the Socicty of Instrument and Control Engineers. and the Ja-
pan Society of Mechanical Engineers.
device to provide the information needed by the blind pe-
destrians in the real world in which the mobility task is
ordinarily performed.
Chen Peng was born in Harbin. China, in 1963.
REFERENCES He received the B.Eng. degree in acoustic engi-
neering in 1982 from the Harbin Institutc of Ship-
[ I ] S . Shao, “Mobility aids for the blind,” in Electronics Devices f o r building Engineering. Harbin. China.
Rehabilitation. New York: Chapman and Hall, 1985, ch. 3. p. 79. Since graduation. he worked for four years in
[2] L. Kay, “An ultrasonic sensing probe as a mobility aid for the blind,” the Jan-Ning Machinery Plant conducting re-
Ultrasonics, vol. 2, p. 53, 1964. search on computers applied to engineering. He
131 L . Russel, “Travel path sounder,” in Proc. Rotterdam Mobility Res. joined the Nan-jing Institute of Technology. Nan-
Conf. New York: Amer. Foundation for the Blind, 1965. Jing, China. as a graduate student in biomedical
[4] N. Pressey. “Mowat sensor,” Focus, vol. 3, pp, 35-39, 1977. engineering in September 1986. He was a fellow
[5] J. D. Armstrong, “Summary report of the research programme on . . Electricity,
with the Research Institute of Amlied
electronic mobility aids.” Dep. Psychology, Univ. Nottingham, Not- Hokkaido University. Sapporo. Japan. in 1987. His research interests in-
tingham. England, 1973. clude the application of ultrasound engineering to biomedical engineering.