Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

Inflection in Swahili Language

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 20

Kiswahili Verbs:

A Lexicographical Challenge
Z.s.m. Mochiwa, Department of Kiswahili, The Open University of Tanzania,
Dar Es Salaam, Tanzania (zasamociwa@yahoo.com)

Abstract: This article makes three significant claims about Kiswahili verbs. By investigating the
verbal root pend- 'love' the article claims that the agglutinative nature of Bantu languages is at the
core of the morphological fecundity of Kiswahili verbs. Evidenced both vertically and horizontally,
the fecundity brings in, respectively, extensions and derivations. The article claims further that
each of the extensions of the verb can, theoretically at least, participate in derivational processes.
The second claim is that this fecundity triggers lexicographical problems of choice and semantic
analysis. Specifically, the article underscores the need for selectivity of information to control the
density of the dictionary entry. Yet, selectivity presupposes a thorough analysis of the morpho-
syntactic behaviour of the verb in its many extended and derived forms. Indeed, such an analysis
presupposes, in turn, an eclectic use of many linguistic theories. Bantu lexicography must be up
front in making theoretically sound decisions. Finally, on the basis of linguistic theories, lexicogra-
phy becomes a concretization of the mental lexicon claimed to be part of the competence of the
native speaker.

Keywords: KISWAHILI, VERBS, LEXICOGRAPHY, LEXICON, DICTIONARY, INFLEC-


TION, EXTENSION, DERIVATION, CAUSATIVE, SOUND-MEANING, ROOT, CITATION

Opsomming: Kiswahiliwerkwoorde: 'n Leksikografiese uitdaging. Hierdie


artikel maak drie betekenisvolle bewerings oor Kiswahiliwerkwoorde. Deur 'n ondersoek van die
verbale kern pend- "liefhê" beweer die artikel dat die agglutinerende aard van die Bantoetale aan
die kern lê van die morfologiese vrugbaarheid van Kiswahiliwerkwoorde. Soos sowel vertikaal as
horisontaal getoon, lei die vrugbaarheid tot suffiksering en afleiding onderskeidelik. Die artikel
beweer verder dat elk van die agtervoegsels van die werkwood, teoreties altans, aan die afleidings-
prosesse kan deelneem. Die tweede bewering is dat hierdie vrugbaarheid leksikografiese probleme
van keuse en semantiese ontleding veroorsaak. Die artikel onderstreep veral die noodsaaklikheid
van die selektiwiteit van inligting om die digtheid van die woordeboekinskrywing te beheer. Se-
lektiwiteit veronderstel egter 'n deeglike ontleding van die morfosintaktiese gedrag van die werk-
woord in sy talle gesuffikseerde en afgeleide vorme. Op sy beurt veronderstel so 'n ontleding
inderdaad 'n eklektiese gebruik van talle taalkundige teorieë. Bantoeleksikografie moet die eerste
wees in die maak van teoreties gegronde besluite. Op grond van linguistiese teorieë word die lek-
sikografie uiteindelik 'n konkretisering van die mentale leksikon wat na bewering deel van die
vermoë van die moedertaalspreker is.

Sleutelwoorde: KISWAHILIWERKWOORDE, LEKSIKOGRAFIE, LEKSIKON, WOORDE-


BOEK, VERBUIGING, SUFFIKSERING, OORSAAKLIK, KLANK-BETEKENIS, KERN, AANHA-
LING

Lexikos 17 (AFRILEX-reeks/series 17: 2007): 114-133


Kiswahili Verbs: A Lexicographical Challenge 115

1. Introduction

A dictionary is the result of an all-encompassing sociolinguistic research. Ac-


cording to Gleason (1967: 101), a dictionary is 'the meeting place of all the sys-
tems, linguistic and non-linguistic which bear relevantly on speech behavior'.
The linguistic side of the project reveals all, or, at least, as many as possible of,
the idiosyncrasies of the language system of which the dictionary is compiled.
In the light of this, a dictionary must enable the user to come face to face with
the arbitrary nature of the language. Put somewhat succinctly, a dictionary
reveals the non-correspondence between form and meaning in language.
Looked at from this same angle, a dictionary may be considered as being a
near mirror image of the lexicon claimed to be part of a native speaker's compe-
tence. According to generative grammarians, this lexicon in the native
speaker's mind contains a list of morphemes whose meanings cannot be pre-
dicted by mere inspection of their forms. One is tempted to add here that the
dictionary is in many respects a concretization of the mental lexicon native
speakers are claimed to have. Representing this school of thought, Kenstowicz
and Kisseberth (1979: 3) state:
In generative grammar it is assumed that the morphemes of a language are
stored by the speaker in a special listlike device called a lexicon or dictionary,
which contains all of the truly unpredictable, idiosyncratic information about the
behavior — syntactic, semantic, phonological — of each morpheme known to the
speaker.
Although a parallel can be drawn between the dictionary and the lexicon, the
latter is much more detailed than the former. This means that the compilation
of the dictionary goes hand in hand with some deliberate selectivity. Bearing in
mind the anticipated user's needs of the dictionary being compiled, lexicogra-
phers are forced to select certain types of information. What is more, consid-
erations outside the field have considerable influence on lexicographers' work.
These include usability of the product, cost issues and possibly the size of the
finished product. These important considerations made Zgusta (1971: 16, 17)
sound the following warning to lexicographers:
The lexicographer is doing scientific work, but … he publishes it for users whose
pursuits are always more practical, at least as regarded from his own point of
view … In other words, the user of the dictionary does not wish, at least usually,
to have the purely lexicographic problems presented, but to have them solved.
As has already been pointed out, the density of information included in a dic-
tionary varies, depending on the anticipated user. A learner's dictionary will be
different from one for the use of a native speaker. The lexicographer rightly
assumes that the user, being a native speaker of the target language, has ac-
quired an exhaustive grammatical competence. The user's grammatical com-
petence, for example, obviates the need for recording some of the morphologi-
cal processes characteristic of the language. To the learner, however, the dic-
116 Z.s.m. Mochiwa

tionary is a tool for learning the target language. It must contain information
that will enable him/her to acquire competence in it.
In their bid to control the density of information to be recorded in their
dictionaries, Bantu lexicographers are, time and time again, uncertain of what
to include. As a result they tend to lay undue emphasis on matters of frequency
regardless of the target user. In accordance with the extent to which users try to
acquire the target language exhaustively, lexicographers must ensure that the
morphology of the most frequently used words are given. The overall success
of lexicographers' products depends to a large extent on what they choose to
record.
One is, however, apt to point out that the choice of what to include must
be such that the given information enables the user to acquire an accurate
account of the morphological structure of the language. This is further empha-
sized by Prinsloo and De Schryver (2001: 188) who claim, among others:
The basic aim of the lexicographer is to guide the user in respect of the proper-
ties/features/characteristics/use/meaning of the lemma, i.e., to know the word.
To be more precise, a dictionary representing the native speaker's lexicon must
accurately characterize both the inflections and derivations found in the lan-
guage being described.

2. The problem

The agglutinating characteristic of Bantu languages affects verbs more signifi-


cantly than they do other categories. Verbs change morphologically as they
receive affixes either horizontally or vertically. They receive affixes horizontally
during extensions or, more precisely, during their inflections. Kiswahili, for
example, has, according to Kiango (2000: 104), eight verbal affixes1. Among
these, three are less productive while the other five are very much so.
The five productive affixes present formidable problems to the lexicogra-
pher in two ways. On the one hand, a single root of a verb can use a given affix
twice or even more. On the other hand, each root that has acquired a suffix can,
at least theoretically, participate in the vertical morphological process of deri-
vation2. This means, any root that acquires an affix can nominalize or dever-
balize into an adjective.
The theoretical implication of this behaviour is that any verb in these lan-
guages is a 'word factory', so to speak. Many scholars, including Vitale (1981),
Bokamba (1981), Keach (1985), and Prinsloo and De Schryver (2001), among
many others, account for this state of affairs by appealing to the morphology
with which Kiswahili is endowed. All of them claim that its complex morphol-
ogy is entwined with its syntax. According to Givón (1971), the complexity is a
result of the loss of syntactic features which forced otherwise free morphemes
to be bound.
We are apt to ask: How can a Kiswahili verb then be exhaustively treated
Kiswahili Verbs: A Lexicographical Challenge 117

in a dictionary? Put somewhat differently: Can the lexicographer record and


analyze each and every form that derives from the simple root? Is it possible to
use a single verb entry for all its extensions as well as its derivatives such as
nouns and adjectives? In other words: Can the fecundity of a Kiswahili verbal
root such as pend- 'love' fit into a single dictionary entry? If the answer to this
question is "no", how does the lexicographer identify his/her lemmata? De
Schryver and Prinsloo (2002) remind lexicographers that, if the modern Bantu
dictionary is to be really practical and useful, all verbs and their derivations
likely to be looked up need to be included.
The purpose of this article is to explore the morphological and semantic
fecundity of pend- 'love', a simple verb root in Kiswahili. By looking at its hori-
zontal as well as its vertical changes in the acquisition of affixes, our main focus
will be on two important issues. On the one hand, we intend to show the mor-
phological complexity of the forms, both verbal and deverbal, resulting from
the affixation process of the root. On the other hand, we want to study the
semantics of the resulting verbal forms.
The study is carried out in two ways. First, we study the morphosyntactic
patterning of the suffixes. Issues such as which suffix precedes another and
whether or not a single suffix can be used more than once will be within the
interests of the article. Second, we study the type of meanings the resulting
verbal forms express. Specifically, we want to establish whether or not the ver-
bal forms express compositional meanings. Before we embark on this issue, we
need to articulate a few, but fundamental, assumptions.

3. Theoretical underpinnings

A dictionary has been described as a repository of idiosyncracies (Kenstowicz


and Kisseberth 1979). These idiosyncracies explain the arbitrary nature of lan-
guage in general. The most widely acknowledged of these idiosyncrasies is the
form–meaning relation. Meanings of words are, by and large, unpredictable.
We cannot guess the meaning of a given word by inspecting its linguistic form.
Thus, a dictionary is intended to reveal to the user, both the semantic and mor-
phosyntactic peculiarities of words found in the language for which it is com-
piled.
Languages endowed with a complex morphology, as Bantu languages are,
trigger many questions. The most problematic of these in Kiswahili lexico-
graphical projects is the identification of citation forms to be entered in the dic-
tionary. How many extensions deserve entries? How many of the derived
forms must be treated as separate entries? If a single citation form is identified
for a verbal root like pend- 'love' and the rest is left to the user, can he/she,
being a learner, find his/her way through the linguistic maze?
Kiango (2000: 21) who notes this problem, argues that it is compounded
by Western theories of lexicography. His argument is that lexicographers work-
ing on Bantu languages need to be careful with the use of theories which arose
118 Z.s.m. Mochiwa

from typologically different languages. He observes the following:


Lexicographical theories and methods are applied to natural languages which by
their nature, have different phonological, morphological and syntactical struc-
tures.
Although dictionaries differ depending on specific factors, most of which lie
outside the lexicographical field, lexicographers exclude information said to be
derivable from the grammatical competence of the user. In Kiswahili, for exam-
ple, once cheka 'laugh' is semantically analyzed, one can 'safely' assume that the
user can derive chekeka 'be laughable'.
It is, in this vein, that Kamusi ya Kiswahili Sanifu, 'The Standard Kiswahili
Dictionary', henceforth KKS, cites a few verbal extensions after the treatment of
each verb. However, this is fine as long as the dictionary is for the use of native
speakers. Suppose now that the dictionary is for language learners3. They use a
dictionary to learn both the morphosyntactic and semantic features of the tar-
get language.
Compiling a Kiswahili dictionary is a morphosyntactic project. The lexico-
grapher must simultaneously and aptly deal with the morphology of the lan-
guage, which, needless to say, is not only intricate but also triggers syntactic
issues. When it is extended, a verb, for example, acquires new syntactic fea-
tures, which affect the meaning it expresses. It is, as this article wants to argue,
misleading to assume that the user will arrive at the forms omitted.
Although the majority of the languages are yet to be fully analyzed, lexi-
cographers do not take much trouble in their study of the forms they enter into
the dictionaries. More often than not, the user who is at the 'mercy' of the dic-
tionary, is given words falsely claimed to be variants of each other. Yet, a close
look at them reveals that each one is derived from a different verbal root. KKS,
for example, claims that mpotovu and mpotevu are variants of each other.
The morphological histories of the two nouns reveal clearly that they are
derivatives of two different verbs. The noun mpotovu is derived from the verb
potoa 'corrupt' whereas mpotevu is derived from potea 'be lost'. Obviously, the
two derived forms cannot express the same meaning. Mpotovu is 'someone who
corrupts people' whereas mpotevu is 'a prodigal person/someone who tends to
disappear'. Mpotevu also possesses the meaning of 'squanderer'.
That the two derivative nouns are treated as variants is indicative of the
dearth of morphological analysis of the language. Indeed, this is like assuming
on the lexicographers' part that the morphology of the target language can, and
indeed does, produce synonyms. This is again, as this article wants to argue, a
misconception. Lexicographers who have not had both rigorous and vigorous
training in theoretical linguistics lack the apparatus needed to reveal the mys-
teries of words.
What this discussion aims to show is that lexicographers must, first and
foremost, be theoretical linguists. Such lexicographers have the skills needed
for undertaking the project. Given their training, they can resolve issues of a
theoretical nature. According to Zgusta (1971: 19), 'some of the problems of
Kiswahili Verbs: A Lexicographical Challenge 119

lexicographic analysis and presentation are surprisingly general, irrespective of


the language in question'.
Zgusta's observation is crucial despite Kiango's concern about the use of
'imported' theories. Zgusta underscores the theoretical fact that, at a certain
level of abstraction, human languages are similar, if not identical altogether.
Theories that conceptualize language at that level are the ones the lexicogra-
pher needs. Kiango's observation is equally valid, because the lexicographer
needs language-specific parameters (cf. Radford 1997). In other words, both
Zgusta's and Kiango's points of view are important for the lexicographer to
take seriously.
The theoretical linguist is in a better position to conceptualize the nature
of language. He/she is in an informed position to deal with theoretical issues.
A proper theoretical training makes the lexicographer adept at discovering the
rule-governed nature of language.
The theoretical training emphasized here is a fundamental knowledge of
the nature of language that guides the lexicographer in making the right deci-
sions. The lexicographer is not supposed to display his/her linguistic expertise
in the dictionary he/she is compiling. His/her linguistic expertise enables
him/her to accurately characterize the idiosyncracies of the language.
Firmly grounded in theoretical linguistics, lexicographers would not con-
fuse mpotovu and mpotevu. An extension such as chemkika purported to trigger
from chemka 'boil' is another indication of theoretical misconceptions. With a
good theoretical grounding, these misconceptions could be minimized to avoid
the mistakes cited above which have, undoubtedly, far-reaching consequences
for the learner.
A lexicographical theory is much broader than any ordinary linguistic
theory. It covers, according to Zgusta (1971: 15), 'the whole structure of the lan-
guage in question' and 'the culture of the respective community in all its
aspects'. In the first issue of Leksikografičeskij sbornik bratislavský, he states:
The theory of lexicography is connected with all the disciplines which study the
lexical system: semantics, lexicology, grammar, stylistics.
Knowing the importance of language theories, Weinreich (1967: 26) had occa-
sion to lament 'the indifference lexicography displays towards its own meth-
odology'. Clear theoretical and methodological apparatus is a good roadmap to
lexicographical success. With this in mind, we can begin to investigate the ver-
bal root pend- as it acquires suffixes. We will allow the root to acquire one suffix
at a time.

4. Extension suffixes

The number of extension suffixes identified by Kiango (2000) differs from that
of Khamis (1972) who claims that Kiswahili has eleven. According to Khamis,
the suffixes are the mood, i.e. the vowel suffix (vs), the conversive/reversive
120 Z.s.m. Mochiwa

suffix (crs), the intensive suffix (is), the static suffix (sts), the durative suffix
(ds), the potential suffix (pos)4, the stative suffix (ss), the applicative (some-
times referred to as the prepositional/applied) suffix (as), the causative suffix
(cs), the passive suffix (pas) and the associative/reciprocal suffix (rs).
Except for the conversive/reversive and the intensive, all the suffixes
change both the morphosyntactic and semantic patterning of the resulting ver-
bal forms. The conversive/reversive and the intensive suffixes change the se-
mantics of the verbal forms but the morphosyntactic features remain the same.
When acquired by the root, the conversive/reversive suffix, for example,
realized as -u- or -o-, makes it express an antonymic meaning. It changes the
meaning from 'do' to 'undo'. Thus, for example, funga 'close', panga 'arrange'
and choma 'pierce' respectively become fungua 'untie/open', pangua 'disarrange'
and chomoa 'unpierce'.
The intensive suffix, on the other hand, adds vigour and/or continuity as
semantic features to the action of the verb. Without it, the verbal roots pig-,
chek- and end- respectively mean 'hit', 'laugh' and 'go'. On acquisition of the
intensive suffix, however, the new verbal forms pigilia, chekelea and endelea
respectively mean 'hit hard continuously', 'laugh continuously/repeatedly', 'go
unabated/continue without stopping'. Apart from these semantic features
added to them, the verbal forms acquire no syntactic features. Intransitive
verbs, for example, remain intransitive and transitive verbs remain transitive.
For purposes of this article, we investigate five suffixes, namely, the appli-
cative, the causative, the stative, the intensive and the associative/reciprocal.
What we intend to establish is their morphosyntactic patterning as well as their
semantic input. At the same time, we intend to find out whether or not, on
acquisition of the suffixes, the resulting verbal forms express compositional
meanings. This will specifically be our major focus when the verbal forms
result from the acquisition of more than one suffix. To begin with, we follow
the root pend- as it acquires one suffix at a time.

4.1 One extension suffix

The verbal root pend- attracts all the suffixes, except the conversive/reversive,
the durative, and the static. That pend- cannot acquire these suffixes is exempli-
fied by the unacceptability of the following verbal forms, each of which uses
the root.
(1) penda 'love' *pendoa/pendua 'unlove' (conversive)
penda 'love' *pendaa 'love all over' (durative)
penda 'love' *pendama 'stay in love'(?) (static)
Other verbal roots, however, can acquire these suffixes as can be seen from the
following:
(2) funga 'close' fungua 'open/tie – untie' (conversive)
paka 'smear' pakaa 'smear all over' (durative)
tua 'perch' tuama 'perch and stay' (static)
Kiswahili Verbs: A Lexicographical Challenge 121

When the root pend- is allowed to acquire a suffix, the following verbal forms
result:
(3) pend-a 'love/like'
root + vs
pend-an-a 'love/like each other'
root + rs + vs
pend-e-a 'love/like for/on behalf of'
root + as + vs
pend-ek-a 'be lovable/likable'
root + ss + vs
pend-esh-a 'cause to be lovable/likable'
root + cs + vs
All the verbal forms under (3) above result after the root has acquired a certain
suffix in addition to the vowel suffix. The first verbal form, however, is some-
what different from the others, because the root has acquired the vowel suffix
only. The last verbal form does not seem to result from a direct acquisition of
the causative suffix. It seems much more plausible and theoretically tenable to
argue for the acquisition of the stative suffix prior to the causative suffix. Two
reasons are adduced in support of this claim. On the one hand, the meaning
expressed by the verbal form pendesha fits into the semantic frame 'cause to be
verbable' rather than 'cause to verb'.
Thus, pendesha shares the same semantic frame with verbs derived from
adjectives such as the following:
(4) fupi 'short'
kamili 'complete'
bora 'better'
refu 'long'
Such adjectives become verbs after the acquisition of the stative suffix thus:
(5) fupi-k-a 'become short/shorten'
kamili-k-a 'become complete'
bor-ek-a 'become refurbished/better than before'
Like pendeka 'become lovable', the stative verbal forms under (5) can acquire the
causative suffix -sh- as under (6).
(6) fupisha 'cause to become short'
kamilisha 'cause to become complete'
boresha 'cause to become better than'
The causative suffix -sh- expresses the meaning in which someone or some-
thing triggers causation of and/or capability in which someone or something
becomes verbed. The causative sh is specifically found with verbal forms that
have acquired the stative suffix. Verbal forms that use sh for the causative fit
into the semantic frame 'be verbable/become verbable'.
122 Z.s.m. Mochiwa

The claim that the introduction of the causative suffix -sh- is indirect is
supportable on phonological grounds. Verbal forms which take it are those
which, when analyzed, have their roots ending in the velar stop /k/ and, occa-
sionally, the alveolar stop /t/. Such verbal forms almost invariably take the
-sh-. Here are a few examples in addition to the above:
(7) pita 'pass' pisha 'cause to pass'
pata 'get' pasha 'cause to get'
choka 'tire' chosha 'cause to tire'
ruka 'jump/fly' rusha 'cause to fly/jump'
hangaika 'be unsettled/startled/ hangaisha 'cause to be unsettled/star-
troubled' tled/troubled'
Although the article will not go into the details of the morphophonology in-
volved, what can easily be seen happening is that the velar stop is replaced by
the causative. Thus, the conclusion that can be drawn here is that verbal roots
ending in /k/ or /t/ have their causative suffix in the form of a voiceless pala-
tal fricative -sh-.
Some of the verbal forms with roots ending in the velar or alveolar stop
are found to have two causative verbal forms or one but which is acquired after
the stative. Without the stative suffix, the causative suffix derives the following
verbal forms:
(8) (a) pita 'pass' pisha 'cause to be passable'
pika 'cook' *pisha 'cause to be cooking/cooked'
cheka 'laugh' *chesha 'cause to be laughable'
As can be seen, pisha as the causative of both pika and pita is not current in the
language. This is partly due to the morphological clash in which the causative
is forced to serve both verbs. If, however, the three verbs acquire their causa-
tive after their stative, the verbal forms which result are, without exception,
current as becomes evident from the following set of data:
(8) (b) pita pikita itisha 'cause to be passable'
pika pikika pikisha 'cause to be cooking/cooked'
cheka chekeka chekesha 'cause to be laughable'
Thus, both pisha and pitisha are current in the language. The former means
'cause to pass', whereas the latter means 'cause to be passable'. Contrariwise,
chesha does not seem to be current, although chekesha is indeed current. The
stem -cheshi, however, is current because it is found in the noun mcheshi which
means 'a jovial person'.
When the verbal root acquires the suffix, the resulting verbal form has the
semantic frame 'cause to verb'. If, however, the causative is introduced after the
stative suffix, the resulting verbal form means 'to cause to be verbed or
verbable'. Thus, whereas pisha means 'cause to pass', pitisha means 'to cause to
be passable'! The fact that pisha 'cause to cook' does not exist, is probably due to
Kiswahili Verbs: A Lexicographical Challenge 123

a possible clash between pisha from pita 'pass' and the causative of pika (pisha).
Thus, pik-ik- 'be cookable' instead of pik 'cook' is allowed to causativize.
An immediate counterexample to the above claim is the verbal root f- 'die'.
This root extends to fisha 'cause to die'. It cannot be argued that fa 'die' extends
to f-ik-a which in turn, with the introduction of the causative suffix, extends to
fisha. Other verbal forms, which, prima facie, look like possible counterexamples,
are given under (9):
(9) kumbuka 'remember'
anguka 'fall down'
kurupuka 'dart away'
The three forms, however, are not counterexamples. All of them are essentially
stative verbs. For the form kumbuka, for example, the existence of kumbukumbu
'memory' does suggest in a strong way that the true root of kumbuka 'remember'
is not kumbuk-, rather kumbu- and the velar consonant /k/ was introduced by a
stative rule. The other two roots extend as follows:
(10) (a) angu-a 'cause to fall'
angu-k-a 'become falling'
angu-sh-a 'cause to become falling'
(b) kurupu-a 'startle/cause to dart away'
kurupu-k-a 'be in the darting/startled'
kurupu-sh-a 'cause to be in the darting away'
The two examples above give more evidence in support of the claim that -sh- is
a causative suffix specific to stative verbs. The semantic contrast between angua
and angusha lends support to the claim that the morphological engine of a lan-
guage does not produce synonyms. A study of angua shows that it is inherently
a causative verb expressing the meaning 'make to fall' or 'verb the object'.
Likewise, kurupua has the same semantic content as angua, namely, 'make
to dart away'. When these verbs acquire the overt causative, they change their
semantic frame from 'verb the object' to 'cause the object to be verbable'. Thus,
there is no synonymy between angua and angusha or kurupua and kurupusha.
This morphological behaviour is characteristic of many other verbal forms.
Verbal forms such as those under (11) exemplify this.
(11) (a) ondoa 'cause to move/remove'
ondoka 'be in a state of moving'
ondosha 'cause to be in a state of moving'
(b) opoa 'cause to be safe/save'
opoka 'be in a state of safety'
oposha 'cause to be in a state of safety'
The verbal form dondoa 'pick up' allows the formation of dondosha but not
dondoza, as we would expect. We are given to understand that dondoa does not
124 Z.s.m. Mochiwa

participate in the causative suffix introduction rule. Indeed, if it were to apply,


the rule would produce dondoza which means 'make someone verb'. Dondoza is
semantically different from dondosha5 which means 'cause to become dropping'.
The claim here is that the causative meaning inherent in the root itself is
differentiated from the one introduced by the morphological rule. The latter
comes in to express 'a state of becoming verbed' whereas the former, the inher-
ent one, expresses the meaning 'cause to verb'.
We might add here that the inherent causative in ondoa expresses an 'actor'
meaning. The introduced causative suffix, on the other hand, expresses an
'acted upon' meaning. It is plausible to claim that the difference between the
two is that the inherent causative focuses on the subject of the verb, whereas
the other focuses on the object. For the sake of clarification, let us study the
following sentences in which the two verbs angua and kurupua are in use:
(12) Mosi anaangua maembe 'Mosi is making the mangoes fall'
Maembe yanaanguka maembe 'Mangoes are falling down'
Mosi amekurupua sungura 'Mosi has startled the rabbit'
Sungura amekurupuka 'The rabbit has darted away'
Mosi anaangusha maembe 'Mosi is causing the mangoes to be falling'
Mosi anakurupusha sungura 'Mosi is causing the rabbit to dart away'
All these examples support the claim that the stative suffix introduction rule
feeds the causative rule.6 What is also important here is the morphosyntactic
patterning of the suffixes. In a dictionary showing possible extensions, it is
misleading to indicate that the root pend- is immediately followed by a causa-
tive suffix of any kind. It is pendeka 'become/be lovable' which can receive a
causative to produce the verbal form pendesha 'cause to be lovable'.
In other contexts, the causative suffix takes various phonetic forms. A ver-
bal form ending in two vowels has the alveolar fricative /z/ for the causative
suffix. The sequence of vowels comes about in one of two ways. On the one
hand, if the verbal root ends in a vowel, then the vowel suffix becomes the sec-
ond vowel. On the other hand, a verbal root which has acquired a stative suffix,
has the vowel suffix at the end of the verbal form as its second vowel. Thus, on
receiving the applicative suffix, pend- becomes pendea and the causative verbal
form becomes pendeza which means 'cause to love on account of'. That this is
indeed the case, is exemplified by the following set of data.
(13) (a) kaw-a 'delay'
kaw-i-a 'delay someone on account of'
kaw-i-z-a 'cause to delay someone on account of'
(b) chag-a 'work with renewed vigour'
chag-i-a 'work with renewed vigour on account of'
chag-i-z-a 'cause someone to work with renewed vigour on account of'
The introduction of the reciprocal suffix in (14) below makes the preverbal and
postverbal nouns share subjecthood as well as objecthood. The reciprocal suffix
Kiswahili Verbs: A Lexicographical Challenge 125

causes the action of the verb to be bidirectional in which the hitter is simultane-
ously hit. Let us have a look at the data under (14).
(14) Juma anapenda msichana 'Juma loves a girl'
Juma na msichana wanapendana 'Juma and the girl love each other'
Juma anapendana na msichana 'Juma and the girl love each other'
Semantically, on the acquisition of the reciprocal suffix, both the subject and
the object of the first sentences, namely Juma and msichana, do not only share
subjecthood but also objecthood. An intransitive verbal root, however, cannot
acquire the reciprocal suffix, because it has a preverbal noun only. Thus, this
becomes a constraint when the reciprocal suffix introduces the sharing of sub-
jecthood and objecthood. Likewise, the reciprocal suffix cannot be introduced
to a stative verbal form for the same reason. The claim is corroborated by the
set of data below in which intransitive verbs are used:
(15) chemka 'boil'
sita 'hesitate'
lia 'cry'
tokota 'boil (said of heavy liquids)'
Introducing the reciprocal suffix to any of these verbal forms results in unac-
ceptable verbal forms, as can be seen from the examples under (16) below:
(16) *chemkana 'boil each other' 7
*sitana 'hesitate each other'(?)
*liana 'cry each other'(?)
*tokotana 'boil each other'(?)
Any transitive verb, however, can acquire a reciprocal suffix. Indeed, an intran-
sitive verb that has acquired a causative suffix or an applicative suffix, can
acquire the reciprocal suffix. In the following set of data, simple verbal roots
are detransitivized by the stative suffix and then retransitivized by the causa-
tive suffix:
(17) som- 'read'
som-ek-a 'be readable'
som-esh-a 'cause to be readable' 8
sem- 'say'
sem-ek-a 'be sayable'
sem-esh-a 'cause to be sayable'
imb- 'sing'
imb-ik-a 'be singable'
imb-ish-a 'cause to be singable'
The three verbal forms can convert into reciprocals as under (18).
(18) som-esh-an-a 'make reading accessible to each other'
126 Z.s.m. Mochiwa

sem-esh-an-a 'make saying possible for each other'


imb-ishan-a 'make singing possible for each other'
By virtue of the applicative it has acquired, the third verbal form under (3)
above, namely pendea, has become a ditransitive instead of a monotransitive
verb. In other words, instead of a single postverbal noun, the verbal form has
two postverbal nouns. This is readily exemplified by the set of data under (19).
(19) Juma anapenda msichana 'Juma loves a girl'
Juma anampendea nini msichana yule? 'For what reason does Juma like the
girl?'
Juma anampendea tabasamu zake 'Juma loves the girl on account of her
smiles'
The applicative suffix is introduced on both transitive and intransitive verbal
roots. With the introduction of the suffix, intransitive verbal forms become
transitive whereas the transitive ones become ditransitive. Intransitive verbs
such as the following become transitive following the introduction of the suffix:
(20) f-a 'die' f-i-a 'die on account of/against the wish of'
lal-a 'sleep' lal-i-a 'sleep on/for/at'
ja-a 'be/become full' ja-li-a 'be/become full for the sake of'
ka-a 'sit' ka-li-a 'sit for/on behalf of/for the sake of/against
the wish of'
fa-a 'be suitable' fa-li-a 'be suitable for/suit'
A detailed analysis of this type prevents the lexicographer from making wrong
decisions. It is, for instance, misleading to indicate that every verbal root can
directly receive a causative suffix in which the meaning remains to be 'cause to
verb'. Or, for that matter, the user of the dictionary can be misled into believing
that every verbal root can acquire a stative suffix, regardless of whether or not
it is transitive. The lexicographical indications found immediately after verb
entries suggest that each root can acquire a stative suffix. Yet this does not
represent the morphological reality of the language. Intransitive verbs such as
gumi-a 'roar', ja-a 'be full', and ish-a 'be finished/exhausted', are inherently sta-
tive. Allowing them to acquire the suffix results in verbal forms with double
statives that are hard to interpret as can be seen from the following which are
neither current nor potential:
(21) gumi-a 'roar' *gum-ik-a
ja-a 'be full' *jal-ik-a 9
ish-a 'be exhausted' *ish-ik-a
po-a 'be cool/cold' *pol-ek-a 10
As far as their semantic patterns go, it can be claimed that verbal forms with
single suffixes express compositional meanings. Thus, the meaning of a verbal
form carrying a suffix will express the meaning of the root plus that of the suf-
fix. It is however, important to note that, occasionally, a given verbal form may
Kiswahili Verbs: A Lexicographical Challenge 127

carry more than one suffix. Thus, although it is prima facie a single root, a close
analysis will reveal that it has another affix which is somewhat invisible as a
result of morphophonemic processes it has undergone. In such a situation, the
verbal form gives a false reading. The best examples are ondoa and pendea.
Without prior knowledge of the verbal forms from which these are derived,
chances are that the two causative forms are given the reading 'to verb' instead
of 'to be verbed' or 'to be verbing'.
As has been pointed out earlier, each of the verbal forms can convert into a
noun. The language has four nominalizing suffixes, namely -o, -e/-w, -ji, and -i.
Each of these nominalizers has a specific meaning; -o, for example, expresses
the result of the action of the verb, the process or the instrument of the verb. Let
us study the following examples:
(22) pend-o 'the result of the verbing, love'
pend-an-o 'the result of loving each other'
pend-e-o 'the cause/result of being lovable'
pend-ek-o 'the state of being lovable'
pend-e-sho 'the causal reason for being lovable'
Depending on idiosyncratic features of the root, derived nouns such as those
under (22) acquire different noun class prefixes that somewhat modify their
meanings. Thus, upendo and mapendo are different. Upendo is 'the feeling of love'
whereas mapendo is 'all those acts which reveal the feeling of love'. Likewise,
upendano is 'the feeling of loving each other' and mapendano are 'all the acts
which reveal the feelings of people who love each other'.
The nominalizers -i and -ji denote the 'doer' of the verb, i.e. they represent
the subject of the verb. The former bears an added meaning of a professional
verber, a regular doer whereas -ji denotes occasionality, irregularity and lack of
expertise and probably lack of seriousness. Thus, mpenzi is someone who loves
seriously, unswervingly, whereas mpendaji is an occasional, irregular lover,
someone who does not take his/her love seriously. Put differently, although
both express the actor or the verber so to speak, they differ in terms of the level
of commitment. Mpenzi is much more committed than mpendaji. This phenome-
non is not idiosyncratic to the root pend- only, but is also characteristic of the
majority of the verbs as the following set of data shows:
(23) jenga 'build' jenzi 11 'builder'
tunga 'compose' tunzi 'composer'
panda 'plant (seeds)' panzi 'planter of seeds'
unda 'construct' unzi 'constructor'
vua 'fish' vuvi 'fisherman'
lapa 'eat greedily' lafi 'greedy person'
On the other hand, the nominalizer -e is not used, instead, the verb root idio-
syncratically uses the passive marker -w- to form the noun mpendwa 'the be-
loved one'. Another verbal root which behaves alike is tuma 'send'. This verb
128 Z.s.m. Mochiwa

root derives the noun mtumwa which means 'a slave'. Unlike pend- the verbal
root tum- allows the derivation of mtume 'the sent one/prophet'. The verbal
root pend- disallows mpende for the meaning 'the beloved one'. Yet, as a nomi-
nalizer -e is actively used by a number of verbal roots such as the following:
(24) peta 'bend' pete 'ring/something bent'
umba 'create' kiumbe 'something created/creature'
shinda 'win over someone/defeat' mshinde 'someone defeated'
nyonga 'strangle/downtread' mnyonge 'someone downtrodden'
tuma 'send' mtume 'someone sent'
Every verbal root in the language idiosyncratically chooses a number of nomi-
nalizers which participate in derivational processes involving it. The lexicogra-
pher cannot, by mere inspection of the forms, decide on the right nominalizers
for the right verbs. Unfortunately the KKS does not point out which nominal-
izer is used by the root in each of its morphological processes.
What is underscored here is that verbal forms cannot be correctly inter-
preted without knowing their morphological history. Likewise, when suffix
introduction rules are violated, semantic opacity results. When, for example, a
stative suffix is introduced to stative verb roots or those which are intransitive,
the resulting verbal forms are impossible to interpret semantically. The exam-
ples chemkika and gumika given above are good examples of semantic opacity.
Let us now investigate verbal forms with more than one suffix.

4.2 More than one suffix

When a verbal root acquires a suffix, the resulting verbal form may be used as
the input of another suffix introduction rule. Then, once it applies, this rule
feeds another suffix introduction rule. In this way, some verbal forms consist of
two or more extension suffixes. When the verbal forms under (3) above are
allowed to acquire other suffixes, the resulting new forms are those under (25).
(25) pend-e-za 'make someone love someone/something on account of'
pend-an-i-a 'make each other love for/at'
pend-e-an-a 'make each other love for/at'
pend-ek-e-a 'be lovable for/at'
pend-esh-an-a 'make each other lovable'
Except for the last, all these verbal forms make use of two suffixes. The first,
pendeza, has the applicative followed by the causative suffix. The second, penda-
nia, has the reciprocal followed by the applicative suffix. The applicative suffix
follows the reciprocal in the third, pendeana. In the fourth, pendekea, the stative
suffix follows the applicative. In the last verbal form, pendeshana, without going
into the details of the morphophonemics alluded to earlier, the causative fol-
lows the reciprocal suffix. It will be recalled, however, that the introduction of
the causative suffix sets in after the stative suffix has been introduced. Thus, to
Kiswahili Verbs: A Lexicographical Challenge 129

begin with, we have pendeka which feeds the causative introduction rule to
derive pendesha and then the causative is followed by a reciprocal suffix.
When two suffixes are used simultaneously, a number of morphosyntactic
constraints tend to surface. One such constraint is that the applicative suffix
cannot follow the stative. If they are used consecutively, the latter must precede
the former. This claim is confirmed by the fact that the verbal form in (26)(a)
below is unacceptable but (26)(b) is acceptable:
(26) (a) *pend-e-ek-a 'become loving for/on account of'(?)
(b) pend-ek-e-a 'be/become lovable at/on account of'
Likewise, when the causative and the stative are used, the former precedes the
latter. Both the applicative and the stative feed the causative introduction rule.
The applicative suffix and the reciprocal suffix interchange positions without
any consequences, both syntactic and semantic.
In addition to the morphosyntactic constraints, semantic problems surface
as well. When two suffixes are simultaneously used, the semantic processing of
the verbal form become somewhat enigmatic. Let us study the verbal forms
under (25) in the sentences under (27):
(27) (a) Mtoto anapendeza 'The child is attractive'
(b) Watoto wanapendania mali/kwao 'The children like each other on account
of wealth/home'
(c) Watoto wanapendeana mali (as under (27)(b))
(d) Shati linapendekea rangi yake 'The shirt is lovable on account of its colour'
(e) Watoto wanapendeshana 'The children are making each other lovable'
In order to process the above sentences accurately, we need to break them up
into semantic kernels. In sentence (27)(a) Mtoto anapendeza, for example, the
semantic kernels are:
(i) Someone loves the child.
(ii) There is a reason for loving the child.
(iii) It is this reason that causes someone to love the child.
Thus, with these kernels the meaning of the sentence can be couched. Likewise
the sentence (27)(b) Watoto wanapendania mali/kwao carries the following ker-
nels:
(i) The children love each other.
(ii) Wealth is the reason for loving each other.
(iii) Their home is where the loving of each other takes place.
What we need to note here is that the use of the applicative suffix introduces
ambiguity into the sentences. Thus, pendania can mean 'love each other for' or
'love each other at'. The reading to be decided upon depends on what follows
the verbal form. The former reading is arrived at if the verbal form is followed
by mali 'wealth' and the latter is arrived at if kwao 'their home' follows.
130 Z.s.m. Mochiwa

Sentence (27)(c) Watoto wanapendeana mali is synonymous with (27)(b). The


difference is more of a structural nature, because in (27)(b) the reciprocal pre-
cedes the applicative, whereas in (27)(c) the reverse is the case. When we say in
Kiswahili Shati linapendekea rangi yake, sentence (27)(d), we mean to underscore
three facts. First, someone somewhere loves a shirt. Second, the shirt has the
specific feature making it lovable. Third, that specific feature is its colour. By
using the two suffixes, namely the stative and the applicative, the speaker is
able to effectuate the semantic content the sentence carries.
In (27)(e), the stative does not surface so that the causative -sh- precedes
the reciprocal instead. The verbal form pendeshana expresses the stative, the
causative and the reciprocal. Analyzed into its kernels, the sentence Watoto
wanapendeshana means:
(i) Each child is lovable to the other.
(ii) Each child causes the other to be lovable.
When the two are combined, the verbal form expresses 'induced bidirectional
lovability'. This in-depth understanding of the meaning of the verbal form puts
the lexicographer in a better position to define it. Such a definition will be
based on this bidirectional lovability. The definition of the verbal form in isola-
tion can run somewhat as follows: pendeshana 'be involved in being lovable
reciprocally/bidirectionally'.
Although definitions like these may be somewhat abstract, they prepare
the ground for well-couched ones based on a thorough semantic analysis. In
other words, verbal forms of this type cannot be cited in dictionaries without
being adequately characterized.
The fact that verbal forms which combine more than one suffix are se-
mantically intricate, constrains the number of affixes they acquire. On these
grounds, each verbal root can only acquire a certain number of suffixes at a
time. Theoretically, however, a root can acquire any number of suffixes. Some
roots acquire as many as five suffixes as the root pend- does under (28):
(28) pend-ek-e-z-e-an-a
The semantic input of each suffix is as follows:
-ek- 'be/become verbed'
-e- 'for the purpose of/on account/for the sake of'
-z- 'make/cause to become'
-e- 'for the purpose of/on account/for the sake of'
-an- 'each other'
The verbal form analyzed under (28) is a daily-used word in the language. In
order to give a characterization, we need to adduce a principle in which each
suffix has a specific scope. For example, the stative -ek- covers the root. Thus,
pendek-, an extended root, expresses the state of being lovable, i.e. pendek means
'be lovable'. Then the -e- added to pendek- causes pendekea to express the mean-
Kiswahili Verbs: A Lexicographical Challenge 131

ing 'be lovable for the sake of', etc. Let us now derive the meaning of the verbal
form bit by bit:
(29) pend- 'love'
pend-ek- 'be able to be loved/be lovable!'
pend-ek-e- 'be able to be loved for the sake of/on account of'
pend-ek-e-z- 'cause to be able to be loved for the sake of/on account of'
pend-ek-e-z-e- 'cause to be able to be loved for the sake of/at/on account
of'
pend-ek-e-z-e-an- 'cause each other to be able to be loved for the sake
of/at/on account of'
Note, however, that the applicative suffix is repeated. What we are somewhat
uncertain about is whether or not, in each instance of its use, it expresses the
same meaning. In other words, we are not certain whether or not the applica-
tive suffix in pendekea is different from the one in pendekezea.12 We are still
uncertain whether a suffix used twice retains its semantic force even when it is
used for a second time in the same verbal form. Could it be the case that the
next use neutralizes the previous use through a lexicalization process? Al-
though these issues need to be investigated, they are beyond the scope of this
article.
If these verbal forms are allowed to nominalize — and theoretically this is
possible — the verbal root pend- acquires many nominal derivatives. The fol-
lowing are nouns in use by Kiswahili speakers in their daily communication:
(30) pendekezeano
pendekezano
pendekezeanio
From the discussion above, these derivatives may prove to be opaque to a
Kiswahili learner. Yet, these and other derivatives need to be indicated and
characterized in the dictionary. What is more, even for an ordinary dictionary,
the lexicographer is strongly advised to find a way of helping his/her users
who come across forms like these for the first time. One suggestion that can be
given, is to have both the suffixes and the nominalizers characterized some-
where, if it is impossible to characterize every extension and its derivatives.

5. Conclusion

The verbal root pend- is representative of verbs of Bantu origin in Kiswahili. It


has both actual and possible word forms it can generate. A dictionary which
charts out all these word forms goes a long way to prove that the lexical pau-
city purported to be characteristic of the language is more imagined than real.
A well-compiled Kiswahili dictionary, especially for learners, must show this
state of affairs. Indeed, the majority, if not all, Kiswahili speakers, need a dic-
tionary of this type for native speakers per se are, in truth, few.
132 Z.s.m. Mochiwa

In order to achieve this goal, the Kiswahili lexicographer needs to apply


the 'enter-them-all' approach proposed by De Schryver and Prinsloo (2001).
This approach is not only useful but also user-friendly for a dictionary in-
tended for learners and language developers. It is an approach which, accord-
ing to these writers, allows the inclusion of 'all the verbs and their derivations
likely to be looked up'. When, however, costs must be taken into account, the
lexicographer might opt for the 'frequency-based tail slots', or, better still, a
combination of the two proposed approaches.

Endnotes

1. According to Khamis (1972), there are eleven suffixes, two of which are said to be lexical,
because they do not change the syntactic patterning of the verbal forms that result.
2. Kiango (2000) does not seem to differentiate between derivation and inflection. According to
him, inflection is a class maintaining derivation, whereas derivation par excellence is a class-
changing derivation.
3. Kiswahili speakers as a whole are not native speakers of the same level of competence. For
many, Kiswahili is a classroom phenomenon, and, for a few who live in urban areas, it is also
a street as well as a domestic phenomenon. It is important, however, to add here that the
mass-media use of the language has gone a long way in spreading it.
4. This suffix is, according to this article, a variant of the stative suffix. The difference between
bomoka and bomoleka is that the former is derived from bomoa whereas the latter is derived
from the verbal form bomolea which results from the acquisition of the applicative suffix.
5. Dondosha means 'cause something to drop' rather than 'cause something to be picked up'.
With careful interpretation, we arrive at the meaning in which the verbal form calls for an
action in the opposite direction.
6. A rule is said to feed another if its application triggers the structural context needed for it to
apply.
7. 'Boil each other' makes sense in English, because the verb 'boil' can be used both transitively
and intransitively.
8. The way we interpret somesha is 'make someone read' but actually the book is made readable
to someone. Kusomesha mtoto shule is 'to make the school accessible to the child'. It is not, we
contend, 'to make the child read'.
9. When verbal forms with two vowels extend to acquire either the stative or the applicative
suffix, they do so with an introduction of a lateral /l/. This causes them to have two possible
readings: jalika is acceptable if it is derived from jalia meaning 'be full for the sake of'.
10. If the sequencing of the extensions in the KKS is anything to go by, poleka is acceptable, be-
cause it is derived from polea.
11. After nominalization, the derived noun stem acquires a noun prefix. Thus, after it has been
derived, jenzi acquires a noun prefix such as m- for 'the person who verbs' and u- which
denotes the verbing skill or process.
12. The suffix, as has already been pointed out, has a homonymic behaviour. It can mean: 'for the
benefit of', 'to the detriment of', 'at/on/in' or 'with/by'. That is why it is sometimes referred
to as the prepositional suffix.
Kiswahili Verbs: A Lexicographical Challenge 133

References

Bokamba, E.G. 1981. Aspects of Bantu Syntax. Preliminary edition. Urbana-Champaign: University
of Illinois, Department of Linguistics.
De Schryver, G.-M. and D.J. Prinsloo. 2001. Towards a Sound Lemmatisation Strategy for the
Bantu Verb through the Use of Frequency-based Tail Slots — with Special Reference to
Cilubà, Sepedi and Kiswahili. Mdee, J.S. and H.J.M. Mwansoko (Eds.). 2001: 216-242, 372.
Givón, T. 1971. Some Historical Changes in the Noun-class System of Bantu: Their Possible Causes
and Wider Implications. Kim, C.-W. and H. Stahlke (Eds.). 1971. Papers in African Linguistics:
34-54. Edmonton/Champaign: Linguistics Research Inc.
Givón, T. 1971. Historical Syntax and Synchronic Morphology: An Archaeologist's Field Trip. Pa-
pers from the Seventh Regional Meeting, Chicago Linguistic Society, April 16–18, 1971: 394-415.
Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Gleason, H.A. 1967. The Relation of Lexicon and Grammar. Householder, F.W. and S. Saporta
(Eds.). 1967: 85-102.
Householder, F.W. and S. Saporta (Eds.). 1967. Problems in Lexicography. Bloomington: Indiana
University/The Hague: Mouton and Co.
Keach, C.B. 1985. The Syntax and Interpretation of the Relative Clause Construction in Kiswahili. New
York/London: Garland Publishing.
Kenstowicz, M. and C. Kisseberth. 1979. Generative Phonology: Description and Theory. New
York/London/Toronto: Academic Press.
Khamis, A.M. 1972. A Consideration of Grammatical Roles in Swahili. Unpublished M.Phil. Thesis.
York: University of York.
Kiango, J.G. 2000. Bantu Lexicography: A Critical Survey of the Principles and Process of Constructing
Dictionary Entries. Tokyo: Institute for the Study of Language and Cultures of Asia and Africa
(ILCAA).
Mdee, J.S. and H.J.M. Mwansoko (Eds.). 2001. Makala ya kongamano la kimataifa Kiswahili 2000.
Proceedings. Dar Es Salaam: TUKI, Chuo Kikuu cha Dar Es Salaam.
Prinsloo, D.J. and G.-M. de Schryver. 2001. Taking Dictionaries for Bantu Languages into the New
Millennium — with Special Reference to Kiswahili, Sepedi and Isizulu. Mdee, J.S. and H.J.M.
Mwansoko (Eds.). 2001: 188-215.
Radford, A. 1997. Syntactic Theory and the Structure of English: A Minimalist Approach. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Kamusi ya Kiswahili Sanifu. 2004. Dar Es Salaam: Oxford University Press.
Vitale, A.J. 1981. Swahili Syntax. Dordrecht, Holland/Annaminson, U.S.A.: Foris Publications.
Weinreich, U. 1967. Lexicographic Definition in Descriptive Semantics. Householder, F.W. and S.
Saporta (Eds.). 1967: 25-44.
Zgusta, L. 1971. Manual of Lexicography. The Hague/Paris: Mouton.

You might also like