The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) : A Method of Assessing Executive Function in Children
The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) : A Method of Assessing Executive Function in Children
The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) : A Method of Assessing Executive Function in Children
net/publication/6416206
CITATIONS READS
973 15,214
1 author:
Philip Zelazo
University of Minnesota Twin Cities
206 PUBLICATIONS 18,874 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Philip Zelazo on 07 September 2015.
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, 100 St. George Street, Toronto, Ontario M5S 3G3, Canada. Correspondence should be addressed to P.D.Z.
(zelazo@psych.utoronto.ca).
Published online 27 June; corrected after print 31 August 2006; doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.46
The dimensional change card sort (DCCS) is an easily administered and widely used measure of executive function that is suitable
for use with participants across a wide range of ages. In the standard version, children are required to sort a series of bivalent test
© 2006 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureprotocols
cards, first according to one dimension (e.g., color), and then according to the other (e.g., shape). Most 3-year-olds perseverate
during the post-switch phase, exhibiting a pattern of inflexibility similar to that seen in patients with prefrontal cortical damage.
By 5 years of age, most children switch when instructed to do so. Performance on the DCCS provides an index of the development
of executive function, and it is impaired in children with disorders such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and
autism. We describe the protocol for the standard version (duration = 5 min) and the more challenging border version (duration =
5 min), which may be used with children as old as 7 years.
INTRODUCTION
The DCCS1–3 is an easily administered and widely used (see
refs. 4–10) measure of executive function that is suitable for use
across the lifespan. In the standard version of this task (Fig. 1),
which is usually used with healthy children between the ages of 3
and 5 years, children are shown two target cards (e.g., a blue rab- “Play the color game:
If it’s red, it goes here; Target cards
bit and a red boat) and asked to sort a series of bivalent test cards but if it’s blue, it goes
(e.g., red rabbits and blue boats) according to one dimension there. Here’s a red one.
(e.g., color). During a post-switch phase, they are told to sort Where does it go?”
the same types of test cards according to the other dimension Test cards
(i.e., 3 red rabbits and
(e.g., shape). Regardless of which dimension is presented first, the 3 blue boats presented
majority of normal 3-year-olds perseverate during the post-switch in a quasi-random order)
phase of the standard version by continuing to sort test cards by
the first dimension. Moreover, they do this despite being told the
new rules on every trial, despite having sorted cards by the new
dimension on other occasions, and despite correctly answering
questions about the post-switch rules (e.g., “Where do the rabbits
go in the shape game?”). In other words, most 3-year-olds show a
pattern of inflexibility on this task that resembles that exhibited on
the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) by adult patients with
damage to dorsolateral prefrontal cortex11. By 5 years of age, most
children switch immediately when instructed to do so.
The standard version of the DCCS provides an index of the devel-
opment of executive function during the preschool years, and it has
been useful in the assessment of individual differences in executive
function in this age range; children’s performance on the DCCS
is correlated with their performance on other measures of execu-
tive function as well as measures that have been linked theoreti- “Okay, now we’re not going
to play the color game anymore. Target cards
cally to executive function, such as self- and social-understanding Now we’re going to play
(i.e., theory of mind)1,12,13. Versions of the DCCS have proven sen- a new game—the shape game. Test cards
If it’s a rabbit, it goes here; (i.e., 3 red rabbits and
sitive to disorders that involve impairments in executive function
but if it’s a boat, it goes there. 3 blue boats presented
(e.g., ADHD14 and autism15,16). Research reveals that performance Here’s a rabbit. Where does it go?” in a quasi-random order)
on the DCCS improves with training and that these improvements
generalize to performance on a measure of theory of mind9. The
Figure 1 | Sample target cards and test cards used during the pre- and post-
switch phases of the standard version of the DCCS.
MATERIALS
• For the standard version of the DCCS, suitable participants include children • Fourteen standard test cards for use in the standard version. These are identical
with mental ages between about 2.5 and 5.0 years. For the border version, in style to the target cards, but half of them (n = 7) depict a red rabbit and
participants include children with higher mental ages (between 5.0 and 7.0 years) half (n = 7) depict a blue boat. Two of these standard test cards are used in the
who pass the standard version (see Step 5, below). When used together, these demonstration phase.
two versions provide an index of executive function across the mental age range • In addition to the above materials, seven border test cards are needed for the
of 2.5 to 7.0 years. Parents provide informed consent allowing their children to border version. These are identical in style to the standard test cards except that
participate, and confirm that their children have normal or corrected-to-normal they have a 5 mm black border around them (see Fig. 4). Four of these cards
vision and are not color blind. depict a red rabbit and three depict a blue boat. For the border version, these
• Two sorting trays: each tray is 11.5 cm long, 9.5 cm wide and 2 cm deep, and cards are used together with seven standard test cards (4 red rabbits and 3 blue
there is a display panel behind each tray that supports the target cards and allows boats).
them to be displayed throughout the task. Trays may be made out of wood. • Alternative shapes and colors may be used, as long as the cross-mapped relation
Target cards may be affixed to the display panel by Velcro or tape. between test and target cards is maintained (i.e., each test card matches one
• Two target cards: each card is 10.75 cm × 7 cm and laminated. These cards target card on one dimension and the other target card on the other dimension).
display color drawings on a white background. One target card shows a blue Use shapes and colors that are easily distinguishable and that will be familiar and
rabbit and the other shows a red boat. engaging to the youngest children tested.
PROCEDURE
1| Decide which dimension will be relevant during the pre-switch phase of the standard version. In the examples that follow,
it is assumed that color has been chosen as the pre-switch dimension.
2| During the demonstration phase, place the two sorting trays side by side in front of the subject, ensuring that they are
within reaching distance. Sit beside the child so that you are able to view the display panels. Affix target cards to the display
panels behind the sorting trays (e.g., a blue rabbit behind the tray on the child’s left and a red boat behind the tray on the
child’s right). Label the target cards by both dimensions. Say, “Here’s a blue rabbit and here’s a red boat.” Then say, “Now, we’re
tray, turning the card over if necessary. If the child sorts incorrectly, say, “No, this one’s red, so it has to go over here in the
color game. Can you help me put this red one down?” Ensure that the card is placed face-down in the appropriate tray.
3| Proceed immediately to the pre-switch phase. On the first pre-switch trial, say, “Now it’s your turn. So remember, if it’s
blue it goes here, but if it’s red it goes there.” Randomly select a test card (e.g., a red rabbit), show it to the child, and label
it by the relevant dimension only. Say, “Here’s a red one. Where does it go?” The child may take the card and place it in a tray
or simply point to one of the trays, in which case you may sort the card for them. In either case, ensure that the card is placed
face down in the appropriate tray. Whether or not children sort correctly, simply say, “Let’s do another one” (or, for variety,
“Let’s do it again,” or “How about another one?”), and proceed to the next pre-switch trial; that is, respond to children in a
neutral, non-evaluative, non-corrective fashion (e.g., do not say, “Okay”). On each pre-switch trial, repeat the pre-switch rules,
select a test card (ensuring that the same type of test card, e.g., a red rabbit, is not selected on more than two consecutive
trials), show the card to the child, label
it by the relevant dimension only, and
ask the child where it goes: “Here’s a
red one, where does it go?” or “Here’s a
Target cards
red one, where does this one go?”
6| Children who pass the post-switch phase of the standard version of the DCCS may proceed immediately to the border
version, which uses the same target cards as the standard version. Remove the already-sorted cards from the sorting trays,
reserving four red rabbits and three blue boats. Set the remaining cards aside. Combine the four red rabbits and three blue boats
with the additional Border test cards. Say, “Okay, you played really well. Now I have a more difficult game for you to play. In
this game, you sometimes get cards that have a black border around it like this one [showing a red rabbit with a border]. If
you see cards with a black border, you have to play the color game. In the color game, red ones go here and blue ones go there
[pointing to the appropriate trays]. This card’s red, so I’m going to put it right there [placing it face down in the appropriate
© 2006 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureprotocols
tray]. But if the cards have no black border, like this one [show them a red rabbit without a border], you have to play the shape
game. In the shape game, if it’s a rabbit, we put it here, but if it’s a boat, we put it there [pointing to the appropriate trays].
This one’s a rabbit, so I’m going to put it right here [placing it face down in the appropriate tray]. Okay? Now it’s your turn.”
7| The border version consists of 12 trials. On each trial, repeat the rules (“If there’s a border, play the color game. If there’s
no border, play the shape game”), select a test card (ensuring that the same type of test card—with or without a border—is not
selected on more than 2 consecutive trials), label the card as having a border or not, and ask the child where it goes. After the
child sorts it, simply say, “Let’s do another.” For example, “Remember, if there’s a black border, you have to play the color game.
But if there’s no black border, you have to play the shape game. Here’s one with a black border. Where does it go? [Children sort]
Let’s do another.” As in the standard version, respond to children in a neutral, non-evaluative, non-corrective fashion.
8| Performance on the border version is scored as the number correct out of 12. Children are considered to pass this task if
they sort nine or more cards correctly out of 12 test trials.
● TIMING
Steps 2–4, 5 min; Steps 6+7, 5 min.
? TROUBLESHOOTING
See Table 1.
That is, both the post-switch phase of the standard version and the border version require children to formulate and use a rule
that says, in effect, “If shape game, then if rabbit here and if boat there; if color game, then if red here and if blue there.”
The ability to use higher-order rules may be tied to the development of self-reflection20, and it is hypothesized to rely on the
function of specific regions within the prefrontal cortex21.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Preparation of this article was supported, in part, by a 9. Kloo, D. & Perner, J. Training transfer between card sorting and false belief
grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada and understanding: Helping children apply conflicting descriptions. Child Dev. 74,
by a Canada Research Chair. Many individuals have been involved the creation 1823–1839 (2003).
and refinement of the DCCS and its many versions, but the author would like to 10. Munakata, Y. & Yerys, B.E. All together now: when dissociations between
acknowledge in particular the valuable contributions of D. Frye and T. Palfai, co- knowledge and action disappear. Psychol. Sci. 12, 335-337 (2001).
authors of the article in which the protocol was first described. 11. Milner, B. Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting. Arch. Neurol. 9,
100–111 (1963).
COMPETING INTERESTS STATEMENT The author declares that he has no 12. Carlson, S.M. & Moses, L.J. Individual differences in inhibitory control and
competing financial interests. theory of mind. Child Dev. 72, 1032–1053 (2001).
13. Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K.R., Lee, W. & Zelazo, P.D. Hot and cool
Published online at http://www.natureprotocols.com/ executive function: Age-related changes and individual differences. Dev.
Reprints and permissions information is available online at http://npg.nature. Neuropsychol. 28, 617–644 (2005).
com/reprintsandpermissions/ 14. Mulas, F. et al. Shifting-related brain magnetic activity in attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiat. 59, 373–379 (2006).
1. Frye, D., Zelazo, P.D. & Palfai, T. Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. 15. Zelazo, P.D., Jacques, S., Burack, J. & Frye, D. The relation between theory
Cog. Dev. 10, 483–527 (1995). of mind and rule use: evidence from persons with autism-spectrum disorders.
2. Zelazo, P.D., Frye, D. & Rapus, T. An age-related dissociation between Inf. Child Dev. (Special Issue: Executive functions and development), 11,
knowing rules and using them. Cog. Dev. 11, 37–63 (1996). 171–195 (2002).
3. Zelazo, P.D., Müller, U., Frye, D. & Marcovitch, S. The development of 16. Colvert, E., Custance, D. & Swettenham, J. Rule-based reasoning and theory
executive function in early childhood. Mono. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 68, Serial of mind in autism: a commentary on the work of Zelazo, Jacques, Burack and
No. 274 (2003). Frye. Inf. Child Dev. 11,197–200 (2002).
4. Bialystok, E. Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual 17. Müller, U., Dick, A.S., Gela, K., Overton, W.F. & Zelazo, P.D. The role of
mind. Child Dev. 70, 636–644 (1999). negative priming in preschoolers’ flexible rule use on the dimensional change
5. Bohlmann, N.L. & Fenson, L. The effects of feedback on perseverative errors card sort task. Child Dev. 77, 395–412 (2006).
in preschool aged children. J. Cog. Dev. 6, 119–131 (2005). 18. Zelazo, P.D., Craik, F.I.M. & Booth, L. Executive function across the life span.
6. Carlson, S.M. Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in Acta Psychol. 115, 167–184 (2004).
preschool children. Dev. Neuropsychol. 28, 595–616 (2005). 19. Diamond, A. & Kirkham, N. Not quite as grown-up as we like to think:
7. Dick, A.S., Overton, W.F. & Kovacs, S.L. The development of symbolic parallels between cognition in childhood and adulthood. Psychol. Sci. 16,
coordination: representation of imagined objects, executive function, and 291–297 (2005).
theory of mind. J. Cog. Dev. 6, 133–161 (2005). 20. Zelazo, P.D. The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends Cog.
8. Kirkham, N.Z., Cruess, L. & Diamond, A. Helping children apply their Sci. 8, 12–17 (2004).
knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Dev. Sci. 6, 21. Bunge, S. & Zelazo, P.D. A brain-based account of the development of rule
449–476 (2003). use in childhood. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 118–121 (2006).
In the version of the article originally published, the boat in the upper right of the figure was blue. It should be red. The error
has been corrected in the HTML and PDF versions of the article.
© 2006 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureprotocols