Finlay Jones
Finlay Jones
Finlay Jones
November 2014
ii
Declaration
To the best of my knowledge and belief this thesis contains no material previously
published by any other person except where due acknowledgment has been made.
This thesis contains no material which has been accepted for the award of any other
Signature:
A moment of self-compassion can change your entire day. A string of such moments
- Christopher K. Germer
iv
Table of Contents
Declaration ............................................................................................................. ii
Acknowledgements ................................................................................................. x
Abstract .................................................................................................................xii
Appendix C: Study 1 Participant Information Sheet and Consent Form ........ 230
List of Tables
List of Figures
Acknowledgements
participated in this research. Thank you for your time, and for sharing your
experiences; through you, I have gained a greater understanding of this field, and of
myself. I would also like to express heartfelt thanks to my supervisors. Clare, your
responsiveness to my ideas, and your wisdom and enthusiasm has been incredible.
Bob, thank you for helping me to design my research, and for your guidance in
analysing and interpreting the results. I am grateful for all that you have taught me.
for providing me with the facilities and support to undertake this project.
generously providing their wisdom, time, resources, and encouragement, all of which
I would also like to thank my teachers and colleagues and the Stanford Center for
Compassion and Altruism Research and Education. Thank you for inspiring me, and
for giving me deeper insight into this work. In particular, I would like to thank Silvia,
Maria Paula, Celedra, and Judy. You are my great friends and teachers.
Ash, Vic, Emiliano, Rebekah, Zoe, Pringle, Carly, David and Ali, for loving me
through this process. Thank you to Linda, for feeding me, laughing with me, and
keeping me company late at night. I would especially like to thank my mother for
her kindness and patience during this journey, and my father for his advice,
Abstract
conditions than many other professional groups, with serious implications for
themselves, their clients, and the discipline as a whole. The investigation of new
attesting to the relevance of this construct for health professionals; however, the
stress among psychologists are yet to be thoroughly explored. The aim of the current
The first study was a cross-sectional, correlational study that aimed to clarify
with emotion regulation were hypothesised to mediate the relationship between self-
compassion and psychological distress. After controlling for age and neuroticism,
relationship between self-compassion and stress. The final models accounted for
28.10% of variance in depression and 25.5% of variance in stress. When taken in the
depression and stress. In addition, this study provides novel insights into what
The overall aim of the second study was to develop and evaluate the
SCO) was developed, within the context of relevant theoretical models and findings
that are accessible, flexible, sustainable, and cost-effective, the program was then
(35%) completed three-month follow-up measures. A main effect for time was found
for each of the outcome measures, with significant increases in self-compassion and
stress, and emotion regulation difficulties reported between pre-test and post-test. All
from participants suggested that despite limitations such as time constraints, the
participants’ lives.
experiences participating in the SCO program. Using the data collected from
the SCO program were analyzed for common categories and themes. The data
collected provided further evidence that clinical training for psychologists is often a
stressful endeavor that entails a number of unique challenges. In addition, the data
intervention for trainee psychologists that has the potential to create lasting positive
predict depression, stress, and emotion regulation difficulties, and (b) its capacity to
intervention can be made more acceptable and to evaluate the full extent of its
psychologists.
1
CHAPTER 1
Psychologists across the career span are vulnerable to experiencing stress and
stress-related conditions (Orlinsky & Rønnestad, 2005), with potentially severe and
far-reaching implications for themselves, their clients, and the profession as a whole
APA ACCA, n.d.). Work-related stress among mental health professionals is linked
anxiety, depression, and burnout (Norcross, 2000; Pope & Vasquez, 2005). In
compromising the provision of care (Barnett, Baker, Elman, & Schoener, 2007;
who are younger or newer to the profession (Farber & Heifetz, 1981; Hellman,
(Cushway, 2005; Rønnestad & Skovholt, 2003; Skovholt & Trotter-Mathison, 2011).
As a result, it has been argued that the pursuit of psychologist well-functioning; “the
enduring quality in one’s professional functioning over time and in the face of
clinician as an essential part of the system” (Shapiro & Carlson, 2009, p. 107), and
2
the heart of a diverse, but interconnected range of research agendas. In line with
theories of stress that highlight the central role of appraisal and coping in
Jordan, 2004; Cox & Mackay, 1981; Goh, Sawang, & Oei, 2010), attention has been
given to individual differences in the way people appraise and deal with stressful
stimuli. Among human services workers there has been particular interest in
buffer against occupational stress (e.g., Baker, 2003; Gilbert, 2007; Grepmair et al.,
2007; Shapiro & Carlson, 2009). Such processes are thought to represent key
individual differences in the way people respond to stress, and have been found to
impact the types and frequencies of stressors people encounter, the way stressors are
Within this line of thinking, researchers have recently directed attention to the
question of how self-compassion may benefit clinicians as well as those with whom
they work. Self-compassion has been described as “being open to and moved by
one’s own suffering, experiencing feelings of caring and kindness towards oneself,
failures, and recognizing that one’s experience is part of the common human
occupational stress.
psychological distress among this occupational group. In addition, the potential for
insight into the mechanisms that underlie the relationship between self-compassion
compassion construct in terms of its ability to promote wellbeing and prevent stress
among psychologists and psychology trainees that elucidates the major working
its feasibility and acceptability, and to evaluate its potential utility in terms of
program to gain further insight into the major themes that characterise participants’
aetiology, and outcomes of occupational stress among psychologists across the career
span is provided, with particular consideration given to the experiences of the trainee
construct, including its links to adaptive functioning and its relevance for
may be developed through training, and the potential for a web-based program to
explored.
Literature Review
What is stress? The term stress is used inconsistently and ambiguously in the
scientific literature and has been used variously to denote a stimulus that induces a
stress response, the response itself, or the behavioural, cognitive, and emotional
response that includes stress appraisals and coping efforts. The stress response may
involve short-term physiological changes that are generally seen as adaptive: such
changes have evolved as a way of helping individuals cope with stressors effectively.
these more enduring clinical symptoms that are referred to in the current literature as
stress. Finally, in line with Kemeny (2003), the term psychological distress is used
essentially a complex and multidimensional process (Hart & Cooper, 2001). Over the
years, researchers have deliberated over whether occupational stress should be seen
individual), or both (e.g, Cooper, 1998; Cotton, 1995; Spector & Jex, 1998) and this
lack of clarity has led to a degree of fragmentation in the relevant literature. While
antecedents and consequences of occupational stress, there are several key concepts
that can be gleaned from a review of the research. The purpose of the following
stress research, and to consider the implications of the evidence for the current study.
This discussion is informed by the wider stress and coping literature, occupational
stress research in general, and occupational stress research specific to the health
professions.
6
were based in the stressors and strain approach which holds that stress is what
outcomes amongst its employees (Beehr, 1995; Hurrell, Nelson, & Simmons, 1998).
This “relatively simplistic” (Hart & Cooper, 2001, p. 94) approach underlies a vast
majority of recent research into occupational stress within the health professions
(e.g., Zapf, Seifert, Schmutte, Mertini, & Holz, 2001), as well as within the wider
occupational context (Hart & Cooper, 2001), and has led to a number of theoretical
identify environmental factors, such as work load and role ambiguity, that predict
strain-related outcomes such as distress, burnout, and job dissatisfaction (e.g., Heim,
1991; Lasalvia, 2011; Moore & Cooper, 1996). To a lesser extent this literature has
considered the way individual differences, such as locus of control and affectivity,
(e.g., Jain, Lall, McLaughlin, & Johnson, 1996; Jex & Spector, 1996).
Despite the influence of the stressors and strain model within the
which are elucidated by Hart and Cooper (2001). They highlight that this model is
erroneous assumptions. First, under the stressors and strain model, occupational
reciprocal relationship between stressors and strain, and cannot account for situations
are only capable of producing negative outcomes, and positive and negative
outcomes are considered to be inversely related. This cannot account for findings that
for some individuals, stressors are motivating and prompt engagement and growth
(e.g., Lazarus, Deese, & Osler, 1952); and that positive and negative outcomes of
simultaneously (e.g., Farber, 1990). Finally, it is argued that the stressors and strain
that occurs consistently within and between certain individuals in response to certain
stressors. Contradicting this assumption, it has been found that although certain
people, individuals and groups differ in terms of the type and severity of the response
that is elicited (Lazarus et al., 1952). Additionally, recent developments suggest that
process (Cooper, 1998) emerged in response to the consistent findings across stress
resilience tend to outweigh similarities (see, e.g. Crandall & Perrewé, 1995). The
stress, which emphasises the role of reciprocal interactions between the person and
their environment in determining stress outcomes (Hart & Cooper, 2001). Of the
many perspectives that have since been put forth under the interactional/transactional
umbrella (e.g., Bowers, 1973; Cox, 1978; Cox & Mackay, 1981), Lazarus and
theorists have successfully applied it to explain the role of the interactive relationship
stress outcomes (e.g., Ashkanasy et al., 2004; Cox, Kuk, & Leiter, 1993). In this
context, stress is specific to the cognitive, emotional and behavioural processes that
characterise peoples’ interactions with their work environment (Cox et al., 1993).
& Folkman, 1984). In their seminal work, Lazarus and Folkman (1984) proposed
that the impact of a certain event can be predicted according to whether an individual
appraises the demands of the environment as exceeding their capacity to cope with
them. According to this model, external sources of stress may have differential
effects between individuals, depending on the meaning of the event for the
individual, and the physical and psychological resources the individual has access to.
A key point here is that environmental “stressors” do not cause a stress response per
se; rather, they elicit stress in an individual only to the extent that (a) they are
appraised as threatening, and (b) the individual is under-resourced to deal with the
threat. The key aspects of this process – primary appraisal, secondary appraisal, and
relevance in terms of one’s wellbeing or the attainment of one’s goals. This process
environment, and the belief system and personal goals of the individual (Lazarus,
1993). Lazarus and Folkman (Folkman & Lazarus, 1986; Lazarus, 1966) distinguish
between primary appraisal – which determines whether the stimulus has the potential
to affect one’s wellbeing, and secondary appraisal, which evaluates whether one has
sufficient coping resources to deal with it. In the occupational context, these
processes are thought to work together to give meaning to the situations and events
that happen in the workplace (Ashkanasy et al., 2004). Within the primary appraisal
appraisals of harm/loss, appraisals of threat, and appraisals of challenge, each with its
own implications for the ensuing response. For example, harm/loss appraisals
involve evaluating damage that has already occurred, while threat appraisals are
and threat appraisals are thought to elicit emotions such as fear, anxiety, anger, and
sadness, and to lead to “fight or flight” responses or resistance coping (Selye, 1976).
appraisals also serve to mobilize an individual for adaptive change, they differ from
threat appraisals in that they focus on the potential for growth that may accompany
said change. Accordingly, challenge appraisals are more likely to be associated with
positive emotions linked to motivation, and to lead to increased effort and positive
performance outcomes.
poses a potential threat to one’s wellbeing, secondary appraisals evaluate whether the
individual is able to access and utilize the necessary resources to modulate the stress
10
experience (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). The magnitude of the discrepancy between
resources and demands is thought to be one of the primary influences on the nature
and intensity of the emotional response to the stimulus. In this context, the
stimulus have largely been researched under the rubric of coping (Lazarus, 1991).
According to Lazarus (1966), coping has three defining characteristics. First, coping
that precedes it and the resources available to deal with the stressful encounter.
Finally, coping operates independent of the outcome – that is, a particular strategy or
such not all coping strategies are adaptive: individuals may ‘cope’ with situations by
avoidance. Given the proliferation of coping strategies that have been identified, a
number of different taxonomies have been suggested for the classification of coping
avoidance coping (see Skinner, Edge, Altman, & Sherwood, 2003, for a review).
substantial body of research suggests that individuals tend to have relatively stable
styles of coping (Moos & Holahan, 2003). For example, Summerfeldt and Endler
(1996) note that: (1) dispositional traits play a role in the ways situations are
11
appraised, and therefore the goals of coping; (2) dispositional traits influence the
the coping process; and (4) past coping attempts influence the way people cope in the
present. While these arguments do not necessarily preclude the possibility of intra-
how the meaning of a stressor is constructed is offered by theorists who posit that
stress appraisals hinge on whether events represent potential loss, threat or challenge
to the self (e.g., Brown, Bifulco, & Harris, 1987; Hammen, Marks, deMayo, &
Mayol, 1985; Hammen, Marks, Mayol, & deMayo, 1985; Oatley & Bolton, 1985;
Thoits, 1991, 1995; Thoits, 2013). According to this perspective, events are
appraised as more threatening (and therefore stressful) the more they pose a sense of
wellbeing (Thoits, 2013). From this point of view, ideas about self and identity are
integral to the stress process and its outcomes, playing a part in stress appraisals,
There is evidence to suggest that beliefs about self are a key predictor in the
stress appraisal process, with individuals with a positive core self-worth consistently
1993; Judge, Van Vianen, & De Pater, 2004). Conversely, a substantial body of
evidence attests to the fact that deficits in self-worth are associated with a range of
12
schemas form one part of the “negative triad” of beliefs that is theorized to be a key
risk and maintaining factor in depression (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979; Evans,
After reviewing the literature related to the variables thought to influence stress
tend to be more resilient to stress than others. First, resilient individuals tend to
interpret their environment optimistically and with a level of trust and agreeableness
– expecting that things will go well, and that others do not pose a threat (as long as
there are not reasons to believe otherwise). Second, resilient individuals are
accepting of setbacks and failures, are able to put these experiences into perspective,
hostile world. Third, resilient people have a sense of self-efficacy and tendency to
emotional equanimity and that resilient people are likely to be lower in negative
Implications for the current research. The transactional model of stress has
implications for the way occupational stress interventions are developed and
from the literature reviewed above. First, it is assumed that while certain
characteristics of a given occupation tend to elicit stress more than others, individuals
respond to such stressors idiosyncratically. That is, what is stressful for one person
may not be stressful for someone else. Second, while intra-individual variation in
differences in appraising and coping with stress cannot be ignored. In fact, given that
the purpose of the current research is to investigate and promote resilience to stress
interventions that enhance personal resources in vulnerable domains. Third, the self-
has the potential to promote professional well-functioning amongst this group across
different working contexts. For the purposes of the current discussion, “stress-
related outcomes” and “stress symptoms” are considered to be the end-point of the
stress process; that is, the negative physical and psychological consequences that one
terms of the environmental, interpersonal, and intrapersonal factors that tend to elicit
number of unique challenges. It is a role that has been described as highly rewarding;
the opportunity to form close connections with people, and to assist, guide, and
participate in their healing processes is one that may facilitate one’s own emotional
growth and provide a sense of meaning, stimulation, and fulfilment (Norcross &
sometimes hostility (Rabin, Feldman, & Kaplan, 1999). As far back as 1937, Freud
wrote of the dangers of psychoanalysis for the practicing therapist (Thompson &
Calkins, 1996), however it was not until forty years later that researchers started
greater depth (e.g., Deutsch, 1984; Deutsch, 1985; Farber, 1983; Farber & Heifetz,
1981; Maslach, 1982; Maslach & Jackson, 1981, 1984). Despite these efforts, stress
prevention and management within this profession remains under-researched, and the
(Guy, 1987; Kilburg, Nathan, & Thoreson, 1986; Sussman, 1995), particularly for
conceptualisation, “emotional depletion” may refer to both mild stress symptoms and
perceived stress (e.g., Prosser et al., 1997), while distress has been defined as “an
experience of intense stress that is not readily resolved, affecting wellbeing and
15
functioning, or disruption of thinking, mood and other health problems that intrude
that while more minor symptoms of stress do not always progress to advanced
distress and impairment, they may be considered a “warning signal” (Baker, 2003, p.
wellbeing and professional well-functioning (e.g., Coster & Schwebel, 1997; Wise,
Hersh, & Gibson, 2012). These outcomes are not seen as merely the absence of
distress, but also encompass constructs such as growth, engagement, and personal
anxiety (Radeke & Mahoney, 2000; Tyssen, Vaglum, Grønvold, & Ekeberg, 2001),
feelings of isolation and loneliness (Lushington & Luscri, 2001; Penzer, 1984),
reductions in job satisfaction (Prosser et al., 1997), decreased self-esteem (Butler &
impede attention and concentration (Skosnik, Chatterton, Swisher, & Park, 2000),
interfere with decision-making skills (Klein, 1996), and negatively impact clinicians’
working relationships with their clients (Enochs & Etzbach, 2004; Renjilian, Baum,
& Landry, 1998). Further, stress can increase the likelihood of occupational burnout
and anxiety (Radeke & Mahoney, 2000), burnout (see Morse, Salyers, Rollins,
Monroe-DeVita, & Pfahler, 2012, for a review), and a number of conditions thought
& Saakvitne, 1995), compassion fatigue (Bride, Radey, & Figley, 2007; Craig &
Sprang, 2010; Figley, 2002a; Sprang, Clark, & Whitt-Woosley, 2007), and vicarious
traumatization (Harrison & Westwood, 2009; Pearlman & Saakvitne, 1995; Sabin-
Farrell & Turpin, 2003). Rothschild and Rand (2006) note that there is some
confusion in the field about the use of, and distinction between, these terms. While
there are some differences between these conditions, in terms of the circumstances
under which they arise, and the nature, severity, and duration of the symptoms
notions of self, and lack of a sense of accomplishment (Smith & Moss, 2009).
individuals who work with other people in some capacity” (Maslach, 1993, p. 20),
extending compassion and empathy to those who are suffering, with the result that
traumatization and secondary traumatic stress are considered to be conditions that are
have been used interchangeably in the research, some researchers have noted
17
and it is thought to involve changes in the way the therapist perceives self, others,
and the world (Baird & Kracen, 2006; Canfield, 2005; Sabin-Farrell & Turpin,
2003). Secondary traumatic stress, on the other hand, is used to describe a condition
(such as avoidance and numbing) as a result of working with individuals who suffer
Limited research has examined the nature and prevalence of specific stress-
related outcomes amongst trainee psychologists, but that which has suggests that
trainees are also at risk of suffering from psychological distress (Cushway, 2005),
vicarious trauma (Adams & Riggs, 2008), and secondary traumatic stress
(O'Halloran & O'Halloran, 2001). Other studies have identified relatively high rates
1995). More specific to the trainee experience of stress is acute performance anxiety,
therapeutic session (Morrissette, 1996; Skovholt & Rønnestad, 2003; Skovholt &
imposter phenomenon”(Henning, Ey, & Shaw, 1998) – the term coined by Clance
individuals, to the point that they persistently question their own abilities and fear
that others will realise that they are intellectual frauds. This syndrome is associated
(Chrisman, Pieper, Clance, Holland, & Glickauf-Hughes, 1995; McGregor, Gee, &
Posey, 2008) and health sciences students in particular (Henning et al., 1998).
This spectrum of conditions may have serious implications for trainee and
capacity to empathise with and relate to their clients (Enochs & Etzbach, 2004;
Lambert & Barley, 2001; Renjilian et al., 1998). Further, Canfield (2005) argues that
exposure to trauma, they may respond to clients in ways that are constrictive
In general, stress has also been found to impair attention and concentration
(Skosnik et al., 2000), and affect decision-making skills (Dias-Ferreira et al., 2009).
Clinical studies have suggested that individuals who suffer from severe burnout tend
professional functioning, because attention is focused on the self, rather than on the
occupational stress has focused on burnout, which has been found to correlate with
absenteeism and turnover (Kahill, 1988). In psychologists, burnout may also impair
interpersonal efficacy and the quality of care provided (McCarthy & Frieze, 1999).
Exhaustion and mental distancing – two of the key aspects of burnout (Schaufeli,
19
longer have the energy to maintain work performance; when they distance
themselves mentally, it may be that they are no longer willing to perform. Moreover,
it has been noted that “therapists’ dysfunctional emotional reactions and non-
constructive coping responses may not only negatively influence their interactions
with a particular client but also may transfer to their work with other clients”
(Orlinsky & Rønnestad, 2005, p. 184). In some cases, mental distancing may be
however when this strategy becomes habitual, impaired performance may result
(Schaufeli, 2003).
Despite limitations in the literature, it seems clear that when not well-
managed, occupational stress can have dire consequences for therapists and clients
work with other aspects of life, this constitutes an enormous loss of resources
different terms used to describe the various conditions described above (Smith &
Moss, 2009). Adding to this ambiguity, there is often a lack of consensus regarding
cut-off scores for the severity of stress-related outcomes, and the reliance on self-
20
report measures has led some researchers to suggest that rates of these conditions
suffers from the healthy worker effect – a systematic bias in the literature derived
from the fact that the majority of burnout studies include only those participants who
are still working, and thus comparably healthy (Schaufeli, 2003). Moreover, there is
conditions progress over time. What is presented here is thus intended to give only a
In a 2002 study, Gilroy, Carroll and Murra reported that 62% of counselling
clinician, with 42% also reporting suicidal ideation or behaviour. This echoes
findings reported by Deutsch (1985), that 57% of master’s and doctoral level
Similarly, Pope and Tabachnick (1994) found that depression was a primary concern
reported by psychologists. While Mahoney (1997) found that substance use was
have identified rates of problematic drug and alcohol use amongst therapists at 6-
11% (Deutsch, 1985; Thoreson, Miller, & Krauskopf, 1989). This can directly
and Keith-Spiegel’s (1987) study reporting that they had conducted therapy while
experiencing various symptoms of burnout. It has been suggested that of the three
exhaustion (Rupert & Morgan, 2005), with rates as high as 30-40% reported in the
literature (Ackerley, Burnell, Holder, & Kurdek, 1988; Mahoney, 1997). Ackerley et
Mahoney (1997) found that 42% of psychologists reported doubts about therapeutic
success, and 27% described feeling disillusioned about their work. Importantly,
however, other researchers have found burnout rates to be low to moderate (Rupert
& Kent, 2007; Rupert & Morgan, 2005; Vredenburgh, Carlozzi, & Stein, 1999), and
1988).
amongst trainee therapists is sparse (Kuyken, Peters, Power, & Lavender, 1998). A
287 clinical psychology trainees surveyed was significantly higher (59%) than that
found for trainees in other professions. Using the same criteria, Stafford-Brown and
Pakenham (2012) found that 73% of the 56 participants in their study of the
using the Employee Assistance Program Inventory (EAPI; Anton & Reed, 1994). In
1998), while in a later study of 364 trainees, 41% of participants reported significant
problems in at least one of these domains (Brooks, Holttum, & Lavender, 2002).
22
These rates may be loosely compared with estimates from studies of trainees across
the health professions, such as that conducted by Henning et al. (1998), who found
that of 477 medical, dental, nursing and pharmacy students surveyed, 27.5% were
Inventory.
acknowledgement that there are occupational hazards specific to the profession that
may increase the likelihood of stress-related outcomes amongst those who practise it
occupational stress amongst this group must recognize the interaction between
extrinsic (i.e. related to the nature of the profession) and intrinsic (i.e. related to
outcomes. However, while the relevant literature uses various taxonomies for
patient stressors; Farber & Heifetz, 1981), very little research considers the
interaction effects of these factors. Additionally, it should be noted that much of the
relationships is highly variable (Lee, Lim, Yang, & Lee, 2011). What is presented
here is thus intended to give a general sense of the various factors that have been
include the nature of therapeutic work, working conditions, and issues related to the
due to the fact that it requires constant empathic attention and “giving” to other
potentially isolating, and sometimes devalued (Barnett & Cooper, 2009). In a study
found that the stresses of therapeutic work clustered in three areas: managing "after
during the therapy hour, and problematic working conditions. For trainees in the
time constraints, an imbalance between work and personal life, peer competitiveness,
the pressure of frequent evaluation, and the added responsibilities related to patient
care (Vitaliano, Russo, Carr, & Heerwagen, 1984). These factors operate within the
and professional expectations, limited social and familial interactions, and identity-
difficulties involved in treating: (a) emotionally demanding clients (e.g. clients with
personality disorders and those who engage in high-risk behaviours); (b) clients who
have chronic difficulties, are resistant to treatment, are slow to improve, or who
relapse (c) clients who attempt or complete suicide, and those who are violent or
aggressive toward themselves or others (Barnett et al., 2007; Farber & Heifetz,
1981). It has been suggested that in general, professions that involve a caring
24
grief, and fear, as well as feelings of failure and the need to “rescue” (Meier, Back, &
psychologists, Lee et al. (2011) found that over-involvement (for example, feeling as
though one was working harder for change than the client), predicted both symptoms
contingent on the person of the psychologist (e.g., Guy, 1987). A number of studies
have examined the “type” of person who is drawn to a career in the helping
professions, and this research provides some interesting clues as to why some aspects
of psychotherapeutic work may be particularly distressing for those who practise it.
For example, there is some suggestion that psychologists tend to come from
motivation for entering the field is to fulfill unmet needs for closeness and intimacy
(Dryden & Spurling, 1989; Elliott & Guy, 1993; Guy, Tamura, & Poelstra, 1989).
Studies comparing mental health professionals with other health professionals have
found that the former report significantly higher rates of physical and sexual abuse,
family dysfunction, and parental psychiatric problems (Elliott & Guy, 1993;
O'Connor, 2001). It has been argued that these childhood experiences may increase
25
the risk of therapists experiencing stress and distress (O'Connor, 2001), particularly
focus on achievement amongst the academic culture, and an underlying fear of being
while Henning et al. (1998) found that perfectionism levels in their sample of
students in the health professions were no higher than those reported for other
student samples, the risk of psychological distress amongst those with high levels of
context of Skovholt and Trotter-Mathison’s (2011) suggestion that the ambiguity and
feelings of self-doubt and inadequacy. It is likely that for trainees who are high in
has been argued that “[s]tudents and interns who experience failure (e.g., clients
failing to show up for appointments or dropping out of therapy; clients not improving
therapists” (Orlinsky & Rønnestad, 2005, p. 184). This is consistent with the
findings of D'Souza, Egan, and Rees (2011), that increased perfectionism is linked to
therapist skill acquisition and refinement that illustrates the complexity of the
26
account for the development of a number of different types of therapist skills over
time. The first of these is the declarative system, which encompasses foundational
technical knowledge. Second, the procedural system accounts for the interpersonal
perceptual skills that are essential for therapists to put their procedural knowledge
into effective practice. Also accounted for by this system are the various skills and
The final system described by the DPR model is the reflective system, a
DPR model, it is the reflective system that is responsible for therapists’ capacity to
“make finer and finer differentiations between different situations and circumstances
plans, procedures and skills” (Bennett-Levy, 2006, p. 68). The evolving nature of this
process echoes the views of authors who have described the development of
Donovan & Ponce, 2009; Epstein & Hundert, 2002). Accordingly, while novice
therapists may face the greater burden of adjustment to new experiences and
27
skills, and attitudes is ongoing (Donovan & Ponce, 2009; Johnson et al., 2014). In the
context of the stressors described above, the DPR model provides some insight into
the multiple demands placed on the therapist who wishes to maintain and further
self-care in a positive ( i.e., promoting flourishing and wellbeing; Lawson & Myers,
2011; Wise et al., 2012), intentional, integrated, and sustainable way (Wise et al.,
(Grepmair et al., 2007; May & O'Donovan, 2007; Patsiopoulos & Buchanan, 2011;
Shapiro, Brown, & Biegel, 2007; Skovholt & Trotter-Mathison, 2011), and there is
effective for decreasing stress, negative affect, self-doubt and anxiety amongst
(Shapiro, Astin, Bishop, & Cordova, 2005; Shapiro et al., 2007; Stafford-Brown &
remains in its infancy. In 2007, Shapiro, and colleagues noted that “no research
and since this time there have been only a limited number of published studies that
Implications for the current research. The above discussion emphasises that
determining stress outcomes. Taken together, findings from the research suggest that
therapeutic work can be inherently challenging from both a professional and personal
perspective and that the nature and conditions of the work environment can present
amongst trainee therapists, who are faced with the dual demands of the academic and
target stress prevention and management programs at these individuals while they are
the career span (Henning et al., 1998; Shapiro et al., 2007). Given the nature of their
Along these lines, it has been argued that self-compassion is a relevant construct for
29
and resilience to stress (Mullenbach & Skovholt, 2011; Shapiro et al., 2007).
the similarities and differences between self-compassion and related constructs will
health will be reviewed, and consideration will be given to the mechanisms by which
evolutionary and attachment theory. The purpose of the current section is to (a)
psychological health.
Neff, 2003b; Salzberg, 1997). In Buddhist philosophy, compassion for self and
others are two inextricably linked processes; it is contended that any differentiation
30
between the two involves a separation between self and other that is inherently false
(Hofmann, Grossman, & Hinton, 2011; Neff, 2003b). Self-compassion or maitri and
related states such as metta (loving kindness) and karuna (compassion for others) are
thought to revolve around the desire for self and others to be free from suffering, and
Quite separate from any Buddhist underpinnings, Gilbert and Irons (2005)
mentality theory. This approach emphasises the role of attachment processes in the
development of self-compassion, and argues that evolved systems for caring and
social relating constitute the primary mechanism by which self-compassion exerts its
beneficial effects (e.g., Gilbert, 2000b; Gilbert, Allan, & Goss, 1996). Social
mentality theory proposes that self-referent information processing occurs along the
is argued that human systems for social relating involve responding to signal-
sensitive systems in others, for example: “a sexual display can activate processing
systems for sexual interest in another, an aggressive display can activate processing
systems for fear and submission in another, a distress call can activate processing
systems for seeking and help-giving in a parent, and parental affectionate care with
warmth can sooth and activate processing systems for attachment and safeness in an
fantasies (Gilbert, 2000b, 2005). Along this line of reasoning, Gilbert and Irons
(2005) argue that treating the self with hostility may generate a similar response to
that felt in reaction to hostility from external sources – i.e. it stimulates threat
contentment systems. A key element of social mentality theory is the notion that self-
role-focused. In essence, “social rank mentality (rooted in concerns with the power
of others, striving, social comparison, fear of rejection and attacks, and shame)
of the Self-Compassion Scale (Neff, 2003a, 2003b). Drawing largely from Buddhist
to Neff (2003b), self-compassion can exist as a trait or state process, and involves:
perceived flaws or inadequacies (e.g., Hannigan, Edwards, & Burnard, 2004). While
self-threat or self-attacking, isolation and reactivity) as being at the heart of the self-
these constructs, the interaction between them, and the relevant psychological
actively comforting and soothing oneself in times of difficulty, and being gentle and
failures (Neff, 2003a). The ability to be kind towards oneself is thought to be heavily
self-soothing during times of failure, or learning to direct their frustration and rage at
the self (Kohut, 1971, 1977). Deficits in self-soothing capacity are implicated in
2006; Kelly, Zuroff, & Shapira, 2009), borderline personality disorder (Linehan,
1993), bulimia nervosa (Esplen, Garfinkel, & Gallop, 2000) and the experience of
persecutory delusions (Hutton, Kelly, Lowens, Taylor, & Tai, 2013). Conversely, the
2007, p. 912) associated with being kind and reassuring towards oneself are thought
to promote happiness, optimism, contentedness, and adaptive coping (Neff, Hsieh, &
being actively self-critical and hostile towards oneself (Neff, 2003b). Self-judgment
psychological difficulties, including social phobia (Cox et al., 2000), mood disorder
(Blatt & Zuroff, 1992; Gilbert & Irons, 2005; Teasdale & Cox, 2001), and post-
difficulties (Zuroff, Moskowitz, & Cote, 1999), and the degree of self-criticism
Koestner, & Powers, 1994). Together, self-criticism and self-reassurance have been
found to mediate the link between students’ recall of parenting (e.g., rejecting or
warm) and self-reported depressive symptoms (Irons, Gilbert, Baldwin, Baccus, &
Palmer, 2006).
one’s experiences to the wider scope of human experience, particularly when one is
perspective engenders the understanding that the experience of pain and suffering is
reflects the Buddhist assertion that all beings are interconnected, and that the notion
likely to feel a sense of social connectedness and belonging (Neff, 2003a; Neff &
McGehee, 2010), ‘fundamental human needs’ that promote wellbeing (Steverink &
one’s suffering. Allen and Leary (2010, p. 108) suggest that “[w]hen people fail,
experience loss or rejection, are humiliated, or confront other negative events, they
often feel that their experience is personal and unique”. When people believe that
they alone are responsible for their mistakes, and that their negative experiences are
unique, they are more likely to experience feelings of shame and inadequacy
anxiety (Gilbert, 2000a), depression (Cheung, Gilbert, & Irons, 2004), and substance
and equanimity rather than overly focusing on, or attempting to avoid aspects of
one’s life or self that are disagreeable (Neff, 2003a). Traditionally, mindfulness has
present moment, and of cultivating a sense of acceptance and openness towards this
experiences, whilst mindfulness used more broadly is relevant across the scope of
2010; Gratz & Roemer, 2004), particularly through its association with increased
cognitive flexibility and decreased cognitive reactivity. Mindfulness has also been
found to be highly correlated with the use of adaptive emotion regulation strategies
patterns of thinking. Within this body of research, it has been found that mindfulness
practice is effective in treating depression (Ma & Teasdale, 2004; Segal, Williams, &
Teasdale, 2002; Teasdale et al., 2000), reducing negative affect and psychological
distress (Brown & Ryan, 2003), and treating anxiety in clinical (Evans et al., 2008)
has been found that mindfulness is linked with reductions in distress even in
situations that provoke negative self-evaluative cognitions (Jain et al., 2007; Kemeny
et al., 2012). Meta analyses have reported moderate-large average effect sizes for the
populations (Baer, 2003; Grossman, Niemann, Schmidt, & Walach, 2004; Hofmann,
failures (Neff et al., 2005; Shapiro et al., 2007) and acts as an obstacle to staying
interrelated processes, and while little has been written on the relationship between
them, Neff (2003b) provides some clarity. First, self-kindness may reduce self-
criticism, thus decreasing the feelings of shame associated with self-judgment and
diminishing the tendency to withdraw from others (Brown, 1999). When interactions
with others are fostered during times of suffering, it is more likely than an individual
will share their difficulties, relate their suffering, and in turn gain a deeper
it is hypothesized that individuals who are better able to soothe and reassure
experiences, and view them with equanimity. In addition, there is some support for
and higher perceived criticism from others (Dunkley, Zuroff, & Blankstein, 2003;
suffering and the inherent “imperfection” of humanity may be less likely to judge
themselves harshly for perceived weaknesses and failings. In this way, self-kindness
make the experience of suffering less aversive and threatening. As a result, one is
less likely to avoid or over-identify with difficult experiences, and is instead more
emotions and cognitions, thus reducing feelings of shame and separateness that may
likely that one will feel connected to others, and able to put one’s struggles into
perspective. Taken together, these factors illustrate the processes involved in the self-
constructs (Barnard & Curry, 2011b). Importantly, it is the interaction between these
three factors that are thought to confer psychological benefits beyond those
documented the link between self-compassion and self-reported life satisfaction and
Neff, 2003a, 2003b; Neff, Kirkpatrick, et al., 2007). In a pair of studies involving
college students, Neely and her colleagues (2009) investigated the relationship
self-mastery and life satisfaction, as well as low perceived stress and negative affect.
with life scales, and lower scores on perceived stress and intrusive thoughts
goal regulation, stress, and social support was controlled. They concluded that self-
stress, disappointment, or failure. These findings support the hypothesis that self-
optimism once the combined effect of self-esteem, age, and gender have been
accounted for (Neff & Vonk, 2009). Neff, et al. (2007a) suggest that the relationship
may stem from (and facilitate) the feelings of warmth, inter-relatedness, and
equilibrium that people experience when they are self-compassionate” (p. 912). It is
also suggested that the link between self-compassion and happiness may be partly
events, a tendency shared by happier people (Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2005;
controlled for (Neff & Vonk, 2009). It has been argued that these results suggests
suppression (see Neff, 2009, for a review). The findings regarding the relationships
between self-compassion and depression and anxiety are particularly robust, with
correlations of around -.60 consistently reported in the literature (Neff, 2003a; Neff
et al., 2005; Neff, Kirkpatrick, et al., 2007; Raes, 2010). One explanation for this is
the finding that self-compassionate people are less likely to be self-critical and
judgmental of their perceived shortcomings and failure (Leary, Tate, Adams, Allen,
& Hancock, 2007). Importantly, however, while self-criticism is a key factor in the
controlled for (Neff, 2003a). Additionally, it has been found that self-compassion
better predicts symptom severity and quality of life amongst individuals with mixed
anxiety and depression than mindfulness (Van Dam, Sheppard, Forsyth, &
Earleywine, 2011).
Raes (2010) found that decreases in rumination and worry partially explained
the relationship between self-compassion and anxiety, while the relationship between
worry and brooding. This finding has been replicated in a study involving a sample
40
from Thailand and Taiwan (Neff, Pisitsungkagarn, & Hseih, 2008). Interestingly,
Ying (2009) suggested that the relationship between self-compassion and depression
coherence) amongst social work students. In this study, it was concluded that self-
Greaves, 2003) amongst trainee social workers, who face similar professional
study of 69 Christian clergy (Barnard & Curry, 2011a). In their study, Barnard and
emotional exhaustion (r=- 0.60) and shame (r = -0.55), and significant positive
and negative psychological outcomes discussed above, and extend the research
providing evidence that self-compassion may buffer against stress during times of
students following a perceived failure (receiving a low examination grade) and found
Additionally, it was found that students who were higher in self-compassion were
following the failure. This is consistent with Neff’s (2003a) finding that self-
41
(2007) examined the link between self-compassion and emotional responses in the
context of both real and imagined negative events and found that self- compassion
was associated with reduced negative affect in both conditions. The authors also
examined the correlation between self-compassion and affect when students received
negative emotions in the face of neutral feedback. Next, they looked at the
embarrassing videotape of themselves: they found that students with higher self-
compassion were more likely to report experiencing positive emotions, and less
likely to report experiencing negative emotions after watching the tape. Finally, the
authors asked participants to recall a previous failure, and assigned them to one of
writing control condition; (d) a true control condition. They then asked students to
report their levels of negative affect, and found that students in the self-compassion
induction condition reported significantly lower levels of sadness, anger, and anxiety
system activity (Gilbert & Proctor, 2006). This proposition is supported by the
also appears to enhance immune function and promote adaptive stress responses.
Rein, Atkinson and McCraty (1995) found that self- and externally-induced feelings
and frustration had negative effects. The authors also found that self-induced feelings
of care and compassion were more effective at stimulating S-IgA than previously
used methods of external induction. It has been suggested that brief compassion
the brain, increase immune system function and feelings of wellbeing (Lutz,
connectedness (Hutcherson, Seppala, & Gross, 2008). Consistent with this, a study
by Arch et al. (2014) found that a group of female participants undergoing brief self-
threat that were consistent with lower stress, relative to attention-training and no-
training controls.
preoccupation of Western culture within the last 30 years, and it has generated a
large body of psychological research during this time (Crocker & Park, 2004). A
relevant to the current research given that within the wider occupational health
job satisfaction and performance (Judge, Locke, Durham, & Kluger, 1998) and
Kinnunen, & Feldt, 2004). It should be noted that self-efficacy is thought to play a
indistinguishable from global self-esteem (Semmer & Meier, 2009). There is some
suggestion that these constructs may buffer against distress and promote persistence
Additionally, self-esteem (along with locus of control and emotional stability), is part
Scott (2009) to inversely predict perceived stress, strain, and avoidance coping.
worth - has been the focus of self-esteem research (Tafarodi & Milne, 2006). Current
conceptualisations of global self-esteem are consistent with the early work of James
(1983) who suggested that self-concept was highly dependent on goal attainment,
and therefore threatened by real or perceived failure (Kernis, 2003). Additionally, the
the perceived evaluations of others (Neff & Vonk, 2009). For many years, this
Greenberg, Solomon, Ardnt, & Schimel, 2004), and the idea that global self-esteem
1995). More recently it has been suggested that self-esteem may take different forms,
Smart, & Boden, 1996; Crocker, Luhtanen, Cooper, & Bouvrette, 2003; Kernis,
2003) and true or optimal self-esteem (Deci & Ryan, 1995; Kernis, 2003), amongst
the degree to which one is perceived to meet expectations and goals in valued areas
of life (Baumeister et al., 1996; Deci & Ryan, 1995); that is, one’s self-worth rests on
the degree to which one is perceived as competent (Crocker & Park, 2004). True
of self-worth, that is not dependent on self-evaluation (Deci & Ryan, 1995). It is this
self-evaluation. On this point, Neff and Vonk (2009) found that self-compassion was
sample.
to (Barnard & Curry, 2011b). It is only recently that researchers have started to
indicate which form of self-esteem they are investigating (Barnard & Curry, 2011b),
and have often simply referred to the global self-esteem construct described above
(e.g., Baumeister et al., 1996). As the relationship between self-compassion and other
self-acceptance constructs has already been discussed, this section will focus on
outline a number of different findings from the research that indicate that self-
compassion and global self-esteem are distinct constructs, and which suggest that
0.56 (Leary et al., 2007) to r = 0.68 (Neff & Vonk, 2009), these correlations are not
sufficiently high to suggest that they are the same construct (Barnard & Curry,
45
2011b). Second, the two constructs have different patterns of association with other
compassion is not (Neff, 2003a; Neff & Vonk, 2009); self-compassion negatively
predicts anger (Neff & Vonk, 2009) and catastrophizing (Leary et al., 2007), while
both high and low self-esteem have been linked to distorted perceptions of self, only
controlled for, however the reverse is not true. For example, self-compassion
negatively predicts rumination, anger and negative affect, and positively correlates
with happiness, optimism and positive affect, even after self-esteem is controlled for
(Leary et al., 2007; Neff & Vonk, 2009). However, once self-compassion is
negative affect, rumination, and anger (Leary et al., 2007; Neff & Vonk, 2009).
Taken together, these findings suggest that self-compassion and global self-esteem
are quite distinct constructs (Barnard & Curry, 2011b). Despite this, Barnard and
literature may refer to constructs that are more similar to self-compassion than global
Fanning, 2000).
While this research provides some compelling support for the potential
46
occupational health research, there are a few final points to be considered. For
example, Crocker and Park (2004) argue that one of the dangers of self-esteem lies in
their abilities of qualities, and hence their self-worth. When people have the
succeed, yet react to threats or potential threats in ways that are destructive or
mean about the self; they view learning as a means to performance outcomes,
negative information about the self; they are preoccupied with themselves at
the expense of others; and when success is uncertain, they feel anxious and
do things that decrease the probability of success but create excuses for
This reiterates the view that maintaining self-esteem is an important goal and
threats to self-esteem are perceived as stressful (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Semmer,
threatening events, such as negative feedback and unfair treatment (Meier, Semmer,
(Crocker & Park, 2004), the pursuit of self-esteem has the potential to compromise
one’s capacity to learn from experience (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Dweck, 2000). As
Crocker and Park argue: “when people have self-validation goals, mistakes, failures,
criticism, and negative feedback are self-threats rather than opportunities to learn and
the potential for that self-worth to be threatened is high. This is reflected by the
findings discussed above - that psychologists often cite clients who are challenging,
with self-esteem are not shared by self-compassion, it has been suggested that self-
during times of challenge or failure (Leary et al., 2007; Neff & Vonk, 2009).
outcomes. The type of intervention used has ranged widely, and has included simple
meditation practice, and an integrated 8-week training program (Adams & Leary,
2007; Gilbert & Proctor, 2006; Kirkpatrick, 2005; Neff & Germer, 2013). Broadly,
target audiences. While the research has not yet produced conclusive evidence for the
48
Induction studies have provided initial evidence that (a) self-compassion can
wellbeing and reduced distress. For example, a study by Adams and Leary (2007)
and guilty eaters promoted adaptive affective and behavioural responses following a
break in their diet. The induction group were compared to a control group of
restrictive eaters who did not receive the induction, as well as a control group of
nondieters. Restrictive eaters who did not receive the induction ate more following
the break in their diet, and reported more negative affect and less positive affect. In
addition, a study by Leary et al. (2007) – discussed above – found that participants
event reported significantly less negative affect and more connectedness with others
than participants in any of the three control groups (a self-esteem induction, a writing
Along similar lines, Neff, Kirkpatrick, et al. (2007) used a Gestalt two-chair
Moreover, all of these correlations, apart from the correlation with depression,
Kirkpatrick, et al., 2007). While further research is needed to determine the specific
49
impact of this intervention and its utility in clinical settings, this study suggests that
From Paul Gilbert’s line of research, a number of studies have examined the
used to reduce psychological distress amongst people high in shame and self-
criticism (Gilbert & Proctor, 2006) and decrease the hostility of auditory
Compassion-focused therapy has also been used as a framework for the development
of treatments for eating disorders (Goss & Allan, 2010) and anxiety disorders
compassion is used to help develop feelings of compassion for self and others (Lee,
2005). This notion is supported by research that has found that engagement in
compassion for the self (Kristeller & Johnson, 2005). Meditative practices that focus
acceptance and awareness, and foster a curious, open, and non-judgmental approach
towards one’s experiences (Kabat-Zinn, 1990). Some theorists have suggested that a
compassion (Germer, 2009; Neff & Germer, 2013). Two studies (discussed above)
have looked at the effect of MBCT- and MBSR- based protocols amongst trainee and
experienced health professionals (Shapiro et al., 2005; Shapiro et al., 2007) and have
program for trainee clinical psychologists, scores on the self-kindness subscale did.
Most recently, the results of a randomized controlled trial of the Mindful Self-
compassion training program developed by Neff and Germer (2013) – indicated that
compassion, mindfulness, compassion for others, and life satisfaction, and significant
decreases in depression, anxiety, stress, and the impact of trauma after completing
the program. While the majority of the gains in wellbeing were explained by
variance in terms of happiness, stress, and the impact of trauma. In this study, the
authors found large effect sizes for increases in self-compassion and decreases in
effect size. They reported a small effect size for increases in social connectedness
51
and happiness.
outcomes amongst participants. In a 2010 study, Shapira and Mongrain found that
were instructed to write a compassionate letter to themselves every day for one week
instructed to write about early memories. Surprisingly, the researchers did not
levels, so it is not possible to say whether self-compassion accounted for the positive
changes reported.
that emotion regulation strategies may play a key role in mediating the self-
processes by which individuals influence which emotions they have, when they have
them, and how they experience and express these emotions” (Gross, 1998, p. 275).
The use of different emotion regulation strategies has been found to impact a range
(Aldao et al., 2010). As a result, emotion regulation has been highlighted as a key
52
hypothesis that self compassion may promote adaptive emotional responding and
reduce the use of problematic emotion regulation strategies. Neff (2003b) suggests
intelligence (Neff et al., 2005), which encompasses the ability to regulate and repair
Two.
relevant for psychologists across the career span, in terms of its ability to promote
wellbeing and resilience to occupational stress. First, the mindfulness aspect of self-
that therapists lacking self-awareness may resort to in the face of occupational stress
promote the self-care attitudes and behaviours that are a vital factor in maintaining
53
criticism and self-doubt that have been linked to increased stress and burnout
this professional group has not yet been directly tested. Given that self-compassion
has been shown to be a variable that is responsive to practice and training, the
investigation of these variables and the clarification of the ways in which they
Further, based on suggestions that emotion regulation theory may account for self-
relationship is warranted.
emotion regulation, and stress management throughout their careers, and particularly
during training (Baker, 2003; Kuyken, Peters, Power, & Lavender, 2003; Shapiro, et
al., 2005; Weiss, 2004). In particular, it has been suggested that the development and
providing services that are accessible, safe and capable of enhancing self-care
CHAPTER 2: STUDY 1
Introduction
among this occupational group, no research has sought to quantitatively examine the
professional psychologists, and there is little insight into the mechanisms that
latter issue is linked to the fact that across the broader field of self-compassion
research, there is no dominant explanatory model for the relationship between self-
order to address this gap, the current study sought to quantify the link between self-
trainees), and to test a theoretical model of self-compassion that aims to account for
The current study aimed to test the proposition that the primary way in which
1
An earlier version of this chapter is currently under revision for publication as Finlay-Jones, A., Rees,
C., & Kane, R. (under revision). Self-compassion, emotion regulation and distress among Australian
psychologists: Testing an Emotion Regulation Model of Self-Compassion using Structural Equation
Modeling. PLoS One.
56
stressful events. Emotion regulation refers to the ways in which individuals attend to
and appraise their emotions as well as the ways they modulate the intensity and
duration of emotional states (Gross & Muñoz, 1995; Thompson, 1994). Based on a
review of the extant emotion regulation literature, Gratz and Roemer (2004)
(a) emotional understanding, awareness, and acceptance; (b) the ability to reduce
emotions; (c) the ability to modulate the intensity or duration of emotions using
The authors suggested that difficulties in any of these areas may represent
Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004) to quantify these deficits.
difficult emotional encounters (Gratz & Roemer, 2004). In the absence of access to
2013) and avoidance (Gratz & Roemer, 2004) more frequently. These strategies are
over time (Campbell-Sills & Barlow, 2007; Werner & Gross, 2010).
57
There are a number of reasons to believe that self-compassion may exert its
difficult experiences and emotions is one of the three key components of the self-
is, individuals who are more self-compassionate are less likely to get caught up in or
encounters are likely to be less severe and more short-lived (Van Dam et al., 2011).
Finally, the ability to see difficult encounters and emotions as part of the
common human experience is thought to reduce feelings of isolation and shame that
58
commonly arise in response to challenging situations (Germer & Neff, 2013; Woods
& Proeve, 2014). This may be particularly relevant to the experience of negative self-
Leary et al., 2007). Self-compassionate individuals are more likely to see setbacks
and misfortunes as “part of life” and less likely to view them as indicative of
something wrong with the self (Siegel & Germer, 2012). This may make it less likely
might happen in the future). Hypothetically, feeling connected to others while facing
may make it more likely that such individuals are able to respond to such emotions
and the situations that elicit them in balanced and constructive ways.
The above discussion provides some insight into the ways that the three
often arise in response to failures and other stressful events (Sirois, Kitner, & Hirsch,
2014; Terry & Leary, 2011). As a result, stressors are appraised as less threatening,
individuals to cope effectively with the situation at hand (Sirois, Molnar, & Hirsch,
2015).
59
model of stress and coping, which highlight individuals’ cognitive and behavioural
responses to stress as a pivotal factor in determining the nature of the stress response.
Adaptive responding to stress involves changing the way that the nature of the
of the stressor. While a theoretical review conducted by Allen and Leary (2010)
proposed that the link between self-compassion and reduced stress may be mediated
by coping strategies, and some evidence suggests that self-compassion indeed helps
(e.g., Allen & Leary, 2010; Leary et al., 2007; Neff et al., 2005) the role of emotion
explored.
stressful encounters and stress responses. While emotion regulation and coping are
interlinked constructs. The current research examined stable difficulties with emotion
Emotion
regulation
difficulties
Self- Psychological
compassion Distress
reactivity to daily stressors (Bolger & Schilling, 1991), it was anticipated that this
variable may need to be controlled in the current study. As age and gender have
previously been linked with both self-compassion (Neff, 2003b) and stress (Deutsch,
1984), it was also expected that the effects of these variables would need to be
controlled. It was hypothesised that after controlling for neuroticism, age and gender:
difficulties.
psychological distress.
H4: SEM Comparison of the saturated structural model (Figure 2) and the
nested mediator model (Figure 3) will indicate that the relationship between
regulation.
61
Emotion
regulation
difficulties
Self- Psychological
compassion Distress
Emotion
regulation
difficulties
Self- Psychological
compassion Distress
Method
Ethics statement. Ethical approval for this study was granted by the Curtin
psychologists at various stages of the career span. Of these 105 (53%) were trainee
psychologists, and 93 (47%) were professional psychologists. This sample size was
had to be over 18 years old and currently engaged in clinical work as a trainee or
62
full member of the Australian Health Practitioner Regulation Agency (AHPRA), the
through social media platforms such as Facebook and Twitter. Emails and
advertisements outlined the purpose of the study and the eligibility criteria for
participants. In addition, participants were informed that they would be able to enter
a prize draw to win one of two AUD$150 vouchers upon completion of the study
questionnaire (see Appendix B for a copy of the email invitation). It was hoped that
this incentive would encourage interest from individuals others than those who had a
vested interest in the research question, increase response rates and reduce
The sample consisted of 27 males (13.6%) and 171 females (86.4%), ranging
the current study was skewed, it should be noted that estimates of the ratio of females
to males within the workforce of Australia psychologists are around 80:20 (Mental
occupational group and education level are outlined in Table 1. Participants’ degree
2
For the purposes of this study “professional psychologists” is the term used to denote psychologists
who (a) identify psychology as their primary profession and (b) are not currently enrolled in
postgraduate study. “Trainee” psychologists are those who are currently enrolled in a post-graduate
psychology degree.
63
Table 1
Mean SD
Frequency Percentage
Gender
Male 27 13.60
Current Occupation
Education Level
PhD/Doctorate 27 13.6
64
Table 2
Trainee Psychologists
Frequency Percentage
Current Degree
Doctorate 24 12.1
Current Stream
Professional Psychologists
Frequency Percentage
Primary Profession
carried out using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 17.0.
Structural equation modelling (SEM) with maximum likelihood estimation was used
carried out using LISREL version 9.10 (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 2013).
Measures.
distress (i.e. symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress) was measured using the
anxiety and stress over the past week using a 4-point Likert-type response format ( 0
= did not apply to me at all to 3 = applied to me very much, or most of the time). The
from this scale is “I couldn’t seem to experience any positive feeling at all”. The
anxiety, and subjective experience of anxious affect. An example item from this
scale is “I felt I was close to panic”. The Stress scale assesses levels of chronic non-
specific arousal, such as difficulty relaxing, nervous arousal, and being easily
have high internal consistency and may be utilized to measure current states of
depression, anxiety, and stress, as well as changes in these states over time. In the
66
current study, Cronbach’s alphas for the depression, anxiety, and stress subscales
Pommier, Neff, & Van Gucht, 2011) is a 12-item self-report measure used to
response format, ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). The SCS-SF is
a short version of the 26-item Self-Compassion Scale (Neff, 2003a), and measures
isolation, and over-identification subscales, with items from these scales reverse-
example item from the mindfulness subscale is “[w]hen something painful happens I
try to take a balanced view of the situation,” while an example item from the
condition”.
Factorial validation of the SCS-SF has indicated that it has a single higher-
the six dimensions of self-compassion (Raes et al., 2011). When examining total
scores, the SCS-SF correlates highly with the long scale, which has strong internal
al., 2011). The SCS-SF has demonstrated adequate internal consistency (Cronbach’s
alpha ≥ .86; Raes et al., 2011). In the current study, the reliability of this scale was α
= 0.89.
67
Difficulties with Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS). The DERS (Gratz &
clinically important problems with emotion regulation. The DERS uses a five-point
While the DERS was also designed to yield a total emotion regulation difficulties
score, it has been argued that these scores should be interpreted with caution given
controversy over the factor structure of the scale, with some authors finding that the
“Lack of Emotional Awareness” scale has only modest correlations with the other
difficulties domain, and does not consistently load onto an overarching higher-order
emotion regulation difficulties construct (e.g., Bardeen, Fergus, & Orcutt, 2012;
Neumann, van Lier, Gratz, & Koot, 2010; Tull, Barrett, McMillan, & Roemer, 2007).
The DERS has exhibited good validity, test-retest reliability, overall internal
consistency and subscale reliability, with Cronbach’s alphas > .80 for each subscale
(Gratz & Roemer, 2004; Ortega, 2009). In the current study, the reliability of the
Big Five Inventory (BFI). Neuroticism was measured using the Neuroticism
subscale of the BFI (John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991). The BFI is a 44-item
questionnaire that assesses the Big Five Personality domains. Each item is a short
descriptive statement, and respondents are asked to rate how much the characteristics
strongly to 5 = agree strongly). The BFI is freely available for research purposes and
68
has been found to have good reliability, a clear factor structure, convergent validity
with longer Big Five measures, and adequate self-peer agreement (Soto, John,
Table 3
Items Range
controlling my behaviours”
Awareness scored)
feelings”
was created to assess age, gender, current occupation (whether trainee or professional
experience, and average number of hours spent in clinical practice per week. In
addition, trainee psychologists were asked about type and stream of their current
69
degree, while professional psychologists were asked to specify their primary area of
Procedure.
of the Participant Information and Consent Form (Appendix C), which outlined the
nature of the study and the inclusion criteria. Participants were asked to read this
form and click through to the survey if they met the inclusion criteria and consented
asked to enter their email address if they wanted to be included in the prize draw, and
to select their choice of voucher (Amazon, iTunes, or ColesMyer) should they win.
Participants were informed that their email addresses would not be linked to the other
data provided. Participants were also given a brief description of the nature and aim
of Study Two, and were asked to provide an email address if they wished to be
contacted when Part Two of Study Two was open for registration. Qualtrics filters
were used to download and store participants’ email addresses separately to the rest
of the data. Winners of the prize draw were determined using an online random-
Results
data for each variable were screened for missing and out-of-range values
in the study, 88 submitted questionnaires that had more than 15% missing data on
one or more scales. Given the relatively large sample size, it was decided not to
include these questionnaires in this analysis. Following this, the data set contained
198 cases; for the two remaining cases with missing data, the results of Little’s
(1988) Missing Completely At Random (MCAR) test were obtained using the
missing value analysis feature in SPSS. The results of this test were non-significant,
χ2 = 115.786, df = 134, p > .05, supporting the use of maximum likelihood estimation
estimation provides unbiased parameter estimates (Enders, 2010) and improves the
accuracy and power of the analyses (Schafer & Graham, 2002). Estimation
Maximization methods were used to obtain maximum likelihood estimates for each
participants who completed the questionnaire and those who did not, a series of chi-
square tests were conducted on the demographic data provided. Across all
completers in terms of gender, χ2 (1, N = 261) = 2.36, p > .05, education level, χ2 (1,
N = 262) = 2.47, p > .05 or current occupation χ2 (1, N = 269) = 2.65, p > .05.
Among the trainee therapists, there was no significant difference between completers
and non-completers in terms of current degree χ2 (1, N = 160) = 0.93, p > .05. In this
terms of current stream, χ2 (1, N = 158) = 10.23, p < .05. Trainees studying clinical
other streams were more likely than not to be completers. Among the professional
71
the values for each item were within the expected range. Univariate normality was
assessed by examining the histograms with normality curve overlay for each
variable, as well as the skewness and kurtosis values and tests of normality. While
normality for a number of the variables, the skewness and kurtosis values did not
exceed the cut-offs suggested by West, Finch, and Curran (1995). As such, it was not
absence of multicollinearity (Kline, 2005). SPSS and LISREL procedures were used
to test these assumptions. Multivariate normality - the assumption that all variables
and all linear combinations of variables are normally distributed (Tabachnick &
Fidell, 2013) - was tested using the PRELIS program. Significant chi-square statistics
revealed that the data were not multivariate normal. As a result, all SEM analyses
were performed using Browne’s (1984) ADF chi-square, which does not assume
the scatterplots for the bivariate correlations was examined. No obvious curvilinear
trends were observed among the scatterplots inspected, indicating that this
assumption was not violated (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2013). To check for
multicollinearity, all of the variables were regressed onto the data identification
number (i.e., the number assigned to each case according to their place in the data
set) as the dependent variable, to produce a tolerance value. In line with Kleinbaum,
72
Kupper, Nizan, and Muller (2008), an absence of multicollinearity in the current data
was indicated by the fact that none of the tolerance values were less than .1.
variables revealed that neuroticism was significantly correlated with each of the
whether these variables would need to be controlled. Age and years of experience
were significantly correlated with each of the indicator variables; however, when age
correlations between years of experience and the indicator variables were non-
significant once age was controlled. As a result, age was retained as a control
the partial correlations between the variables after controlling for age) was conducted
to determine whether the partial correlations among the nine indicators varied as a
gender. As the relationship among the indicators did not vary as a function of
Descriptive statistics. The means, standard deviations, and ranges for the 11
observed variables are reported in Table 4, which also shows the correlations among
the variables. As the observed variables were not multivariate normal, Spearman’s
(Pugesek & Grace, 1998). Reliability analyses were performed for the scales and
subscales to be used in the analysis. All scales and subscales had an acceptable
coefficient alpha (Tavakol & Dennick, 2011); these statistics are also reported in
Table 4.
Table 5 shows the partial correlations among the observed variables after
controlling for age and neuroticism. After controlling for age and neuroticism, self-
stress (r = -.33, p < .001), but not anxiety (r = -.06, p = .370). As such, anxiety was
depression and stress revealed that the clarity and awareness subscales only shared
modest correlations with the other subscales, and did not significantly correlate with
either depression or stress. It was therefore decided to exclude these subscales from
the analysis. Given that different emotion regulation problems are theorised to
examine the partial correlations between each type of emotion regulation difficulty
and the depression and stress outcomes. In our sample, non-acceptance, goal-
significant positive correlation with stress. Given the differences between the types
of emotion regulation difficulties that were linked with each outcome, it was decided
to perform separate structural equation modelling analyses for depression and stress.
In each model only the emotion regulation difficulties that had significant partial
correlations with each outcome were used as indicators of a latent emotion regulation
difficulties variable.
74
Table 4
Means, Standard Deviations, Ranges, Internal Consistency Reliabilities, and Spearman’s Correlations for Observed Variables
Var. 1 2a 2b 2c 2d 2e 2f 3a 3b 3c M SD Min Max α
2f. -.29*** .29*** .22** .32*** .55*** .28*** - 8.80 2.51 5 19 .75
3a. -.46*** .38*** .37*** .34*** .10 .56*** .17* - 5.69 6.69 0 42 .89
3b. -.39*** .40*** .32*** .43*** -.02 .49*** .14* .44*** - 3.49 4.72 0 34 .77
3c. -.54*** .51*** .38*** .50*** .14* .60*** .20** .55*** .52*** - 11.96 8.00 0 42 .85
4. -.70*** .54*** .44*** .58*** .20** .63*** .25*** .37*** .43*** .49*** 2.70 .85 1 5 .86
1: Self-Compassion 2: Difficulties with Emotion Regulation: 2a: Non-Acceptance, 2b: Goal-Direction, 2c: Impulse Control, 2d: Awareness, 2e: Strategies, 2f: Clarity; 3.
Psychological distress: 3a: Depression, 3b: Anxiety, 3c: Stress; 4. Neuroticism *: p < .05, **: p < .01, ***: p < .001.
75
Table 5
Partial Correlations Among Observed Variables, Controlling for Neuroticism and Age
Variable 1 2a 2b 2c 2d 2e 2f 3a 3b 3c
1. -
2a. -.35*** -
1: Self-Compassion 2: Difficulties with Emotion Regulation: 2a: Non-Acceptance, 2b: Goal-Direction, 2c: Impulse Control, 2d: Awareness, 2e: Strategies, 2f: Clarity; 3.
Psychological distress: 3a: Depression, 3b: Anxiety, 3c: Stress; 4. Neuroticism *: p < .05, **: p < .01, ***: p < .001.
76
measured, mean scores for each group were calculated and compared. In addition,
mean scores on the DASS-21 were compared with severity cut-offs (Table 6). The
results indicated that both groups were in the normal range for stress symptoms, and
there was no statistically significant difference between the groups on this measure.
Despite this, examination of the frequency and range of scores on the DASS-21 found
range for depression, 8% of the sample was in this range for anxiety, and 20% of the
sample was in this range for stress. For professional psychologists, 4% of the sample
was in the moderate-extremely severe range for depression and 15% of the sample
was in this range for stress, with 2% of the sample in the moderate range for anxiety.
outcomes and higher for self-compassion when compared to trainees, however these
differences weren’t significant. Descriptive statistics for each group are reported in
Table 7.
Table 6
Table 7
Descriptive Statistics for Outcome Measures per Occupational Group
M SD Range M SD Range
ER Difficulties
Hypothesis testing. The hypotheses in the current study were based on the
premises of mediation outlined by Baron and Kenny (1986), namely: (1) the predictor
is related to the criterion; (2) the predictor is related to the mediator; (3) the mediator
is related to the criterion; and (4) when the effects of the mediator are controlled, the
relationship between the predictor and the criterion is weakened (indicating partial
current study, Hypotheses 1-5 relate to the first three premises of mediation, which
were tested by examining the relationships between the latent variables. Hypothesis 6
corresponds to the fourth premise of meditation, and was tested by comparing the fit
Analysis was conducted for each latent variable. Based on the results from Stage 1,
the measurement component of the structural equation model was generated, and this
model was tested at Stage 2. If the measurement model provided an adequate fit for
the data, the analysis progressed to Stage 3, where the fit of the structural model was
tested. In Stage 4, the fit of the saturated structural model was compared with the fit
of the nested mediation model to determine which model provided the better fit.
Hypotheses 1-5 were tested by examining the relationships among the latent variables,
while Hypothesis 6 was tested by comparing the fits of the partial and full mediation
models.
likelihood estimation using LISREL version 9.10 (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 2013) was
carried out. A number of indices were used to evaluate model fit; with good fit
indicated by the following cut-off values: less than 3 for χ2/df (Kline, 2005), above .90
for the comparative fit index (CFI) and the non-normed fit index (NNFI; Hu &
79
Bentler, 1999), below .10 for the standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR),
and below .05, or a confidence interval that encompasses this value, for the root-
mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA; Jaccard & Wan, 1996). All of the
SEM tests met Kline’s (2005) suggested minimum cases-to-parameter ratio of 5:1.
to determine the extent to which the observed variables measured the latent
constructs. The indices described above were used to determine model fit. The results
of the confirmatory factor analyses did not unequivocally support the factor structures
noted that there is some contention over using universal cut-offs to determine model
fit on this index, as sample size, model specifications, and degrees of freedom all
impact the appropriateness of a certain cut-off value (Chen, Curran, Bollen, Kirby, &
Paxton, 2008). As such, it was considered important to evaluate the fit of factor
structures across all of the goodness-of-fit indices described above, rather than
considering single indices as an absolute assessment of model fit (Barrett, 2007; Chen
Latent variables and indicators. The results of the CFA conducted on the
items of the DASS-21 supported the depression and stress scales as separate, single-
For each single-indicator latent variable, the error variance was computed by
subtracting the reliability of the scale used to measure the indicator from 1.
Two hierarchical CFAs were conducted for the latent emotion regulation
construct with 3 indicators (for the depression model) and 4 indicators (for the stress
on the non-acceptance, strategies, and goal direction subscales of the DERS was
supported by the data, as was a higher order, single-factor solution based on these
scales with the addition of impulse control items. The results of the CFA are reported
in Table 8.
Table 8
Confirmatory Factor Analysis Fit Statistics
Model χ2/df CFI NFI SRMR RMSEA
Emotion Regulation
Difficulties
Hierarchical 3F 339.61/149 = 2.28 0.97 0.95 0.06 0.08 (0.07, 0.10)
Hierarchical 4F 644.20/271 = 2.38 0.96 0.93 0.08 0.09 (0.08, 0.10)
Depression
1F 33.73/14 = 2.41 0.99 0.97 0.03 0.08 (0.05, 0.12)
Stress
1F 94.85/14 = 6.77 0.91 0.90 0.07 0.17 (0.14, 0.20)
Self-Compassion
1F 181.24/54 = 3.36 0.94 0.92 0.07 0.12 (0.09, 0.12)
CFI: Comparative Fit Index; NFI: Normed Fit Index; SRMR: Standardized Root Mean Square
Residual; RMSEA: Root Mean Square Error of Approximation
χ2/df: A value less than 3 indicates a good fit (Kline, 2005); CFI: A value greater than or equal to .90
indicates a good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999); NFI: A value greater than or equal to .90 indicates a good fit
(Hu & Bentler, 1999); SRMR: A value less than or equal to .08 indicates a good fit (Hu & Bentler,
1999); RMSEA: A value less than or equal to .05, or a CI that encompass this value, indicates a good
fit (Jaccard & Wan, 1996).
Model testing.
Hypothesis 2 predicted that after controlling for age and neuroticism, self-
in goal-directed behaviour when distressed. The other subscales of the DERS (lack of
behaviour when upset) did not significantly correlate with depression symptoms once
age and neuroticism were controlled for, and so were excluded from the model.
Hypothesis 3 predicted that after controlling for age and neuroticism, emotion
Based on the outcomes of testing Hypotheses 1-3, the first three requirements
of Baron and Kenny’s (1986) four step analytical procedure for testing mediation
models were satisfied. The correlations among the latent variables in these analyses
Table 9
Intercorrelations among Latent Variables for the Depression Model
Construct 1. 2. 3.
1. Self-Compassion -
Testing the measurement model. Examination of the fit statistics for the
measurement model suggested that this model fit the data well, and indicated that it
was appropriate to test the structural model: χ2 (4, N = 198) = 7.96, χ2/df = 1.99, CFI
= .98, NFI = .96, RMSEA = .07 (90% CI: .00, .14), SRMR = .05. These fit statistics
Testing the partial mediation model. The partial mediation model for
depression with its parameter estimates and standard errors is shown in Figure 4. In
emotion regulation difficulties, age and neuroticism are controlled. All other pathways
in this model were significant. The fit indices for this model suggested a good fit to
the data, χ2 (8, N = 198) = 18.05, χ2/df = 2.26, CFI = .97, NFI = .96, RMSEA = .08
(90% CI: .03, .13), SRMR = .05. These statistics are shown in Table 10.
Figure 4. The partial mediation model for depression with its parameter estimates.
83
Testing the full mediation model. In order to test the full mediation model, the
direct pathway from self-compassion to depression was removed: this model, with its
parameter estimates and standard errors is shown in Figure 5. The fit statistics for the
full mediation model suggested a good fit to the data, χ2 (5, N = 198) = 8.00, χ2/df =
1.60, CFI = .98, NFI = .96, RMSEA = .06 (90% CI: .00, .12), SRMR = .04. These
statistics are shown in Table 10. As models with more factors will always provide a
better fit to the data, a chi-square difference test was performed to determine whether
the partial mediation model (with one extra pathway) fit the data significantly better
than the full mediation model. The result of this test was non-significant χ2difference (1,
N = 198) = 0.04, p = .841. Therefore, it was determined that the data supported a
model in which the relationship between self-compassion and depression was fully
emotions, lack of access to emotion regulation strategies, and difficulties with goal-
directed behaviour when upset. This model accounted for 28.10% of variance in
Table 10
Fit statistics for Depression Models
Model χ2/df CFI NFI SRMR RMSEA
Measurement 7.96/4 = 1.99 0.98 0.96 0.04 0.07 (0.00, 0.13)
Model
Saturated 7.96/4 = 1.99 0.98 0.96 0.04 0.07 (0.00, 0.13)
Structural Model
Mediation Model 8.00/5 = 1.60 0.98 0.96 0.04 0.06 (0.00, 0.12)
CFI: Comparative Fit Index; NFI: Normed Fit Index; SRMR: Standardized Root Mean Square
Residual; RMSEA: Root Mean Square Error of Approximation.
χ2/df ≤ 3 indicates a good fit (Kline, 2005); CFI: ≥ .90 indicates a good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999); NFI:
≤ .90 indicates a good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999); SRMR: ≤ .08 indicates a good fit (Hu & Bentler,
1999); RMSEA: ≤ .05, or a CI that encompass this value, indicates a good fit (Jaccard & Wan, 1996).
84
Figure 5. The full mediation model for depression with its parameter estimates.
Test of the indirect effects. Standardised path estimates and standard errors for
were estimated with a bootstrapping procedure based on 1000 draws using Mplus
(Version 5.2). The statistical significance of the indirect effect was evaluated with a z-
Table 11
Standardised Path Estimates and Standardised Errors for Indirect Effects for
Depression Model
Stress. Hypothesis 1 predicted that after controlling for age and neuroticism,
Hypothesis 2 predicted that after controlling for age and neuroticism, self-
Hypothesis 3 predicted that after controlling for age and neuroticism, emotion
Based on the outcomes of testing Hypotheses 1-3, the first three requirements
of Baron and Kenny’s (1986) four step analytical procedure for testing mediation
models were satisfied. The correlations among the latent variables in these analyses
Table 12
Intercorrelations among Latent Variables for the Stress Model
Construct 1. 2. 3.
1. Self-Compassion -
Testing the measurement model. Examination of the fit statistics for the
measurement model suggested that the model fit the data well, and indicated that it
was appropriate to test the structural model: χ2 (8, N = 198) = 21.56, χ2/df = 2.69, CFI
= .96, NFI = .92, RMSEA = .09 (90% CI: .04, .14), SRMR = .05. These fit statistics
Testing the partial mediation model. The partial mediation model for stress
with its parameter estimates and standard errors is shown in Figure 6. In this model,
the pathway from self-compassion to stress was non-significant, indicating that there
difficulties, age and neuroticism are controlled. All other pathways in this model were
significant. The fit indices for this model suggested an adequate fit to the data, χ2 (8,
N = 198) = 21.56, χ2/df = 2.69, CFI = .96, NFI = .93, RMSEA = .09 (90% CI: .05,
Testing the full mediation model. In order to test the full mediation model, the
direct pathway from self-compassion to stress was removed: this model, with its
parameter estimates and standard errors is shown in Figure 7. The fit statistics for the
full mediation model suggested an adequate fit to the data, χ2 (9, N = 198) = 24.02,
χ2/df = 2.67, CFI = .95, NFI = .92, RMSEA = .09 (90% CI: .05, .14), SRMR = .04.
mediation model (with one extra pathway) fit the data significantly better than the full
mediation model. The result of this test was non-significant χ2difference (1, N = 198) =
2.46, p = .12. As a result, it was determined that the data supported a model in which
the relationship between self-compassion and stress was fully mediated by emotion
when upset. This model accounted for 25.5% of variance in stress symptoms, and all
Figure 6. The partial mediation model for stress with its parameter estimates.
88
Figure 7. The full mediation model for stress with its parameter estimates.
Table 13
Fit Statistics for Stress Models
Model χ2/df CFI NFI SRMR RMSEA
Measurement 21.56/8 = 2.68 0.96 0.92 0.05 0.09 (0.05, 0.14)
Model
Saturated 21.56/8 = 2.69 0.96 0.93 0.04 0.09 (0.05, 0.14)
Structural Model
Mediation Model 24.02/9 = 2.67 0.95 0.92 0.04 0.09 (0.05, 0.14)
CFI: Comparative Fit Index; NFI: Normed Fit Index; SRMR: Standardized Root Mean Square
Residual; RMSEA: Root Mean Square Error of Approximation.
χ2/df ≤ 3 indicates a good fit (Kline, 2005); CFI: ≥ .90 indicates a good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999); NFI:
≤ .90 indicates a good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999); SRMR: ≤ .08 indicates a good fit (Hu & Bentler,
1999); RMSEA: ≤ .05, or a CI that encompass this value, indicates a good fit (Jaccard & Wan, 1996).
Test of the indirect effects. Standardised path estimates and standard errors for
both of the indirect effects were estimated with a bootstrapping procedure based on
each indirect effect was evaluated with a z-test. The indirect effect of self-compassion
on stress symptoms via emotion regulation difficulties was significant. These results
Table 14
Standardised Path Estimates and Standardised Errors for Indirect Effects for Stress
Model
Discussion
and that this relationship is fully mediated by difficulties in emotion regulation. After
predicted depression (r = -.22, p = .002) and stress (r = -.33, p < .001), although the
depression, while a latent emotion regulation difficulties variable based on the these
indicators with the addition of impulse control difficulties mediated the relationship
between self-compassion and stress. In SEM terms, self-compassion did not directly
90
regulation difficulties.
compassion and anxiety were significant (-.34, p < .001), this relationship was no
longer significant once age and neuroticism were controlled for. This suggests that the
current data do not fully support the theoretical model as specified in the current
outcomes (such as depression, anxiety, and stress) individually when examining their
relationship with self-compassion. While previous research has found that self-
compassion is a strong predictor of anxiety (Neff, 2003a; Raes, 2010; Werner et al.,
2012), these studies failed to control for neuroticism. The current results suggest that
the relationship between self-compassion and anxiety may be a spurious one that is
better explained by trait neuroticism (e.g., Bolger & Eckenrode, 1991; Cohen &
depression and stress (e.g., Clark, Watson, & Mineka, 1994; Kendler, Gardner, &
burnout (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). In the context of these findings, the finding in
the current study - that self-compassion significantly predicts depression and stress
once age and neuroticism are controlled - is particularly noteworthy. These results add
stress (for a review, see Barnard & Curry, 2011b), and extend the literature
(Keng, Smoski, Robins, Ekblad, & Brantley, 2012; Vettese, Dyer, Li, & Wekerle,
2011),
In addition, these results also support research documenting the link between
depression and stress (for a review, see Aldao et al., 2010). An important addition to
the literature derived from the current study is the finding that the relationship
between self-compassion and both depression and stress is fully mediated by emotion
has been conducted has found that specific maladaptive emotion regulation strategies
such as rumination and worry are key mechanisms in the relationship between self-
compassion and symptoms of depression and anxiety (Raes, 2010). In the context of
the previous research, the results of the current study may be extrapolated to suggest
that emotion regulation is a key explanatory mechanism underlying the link between
self-compassion and psychological health more broadly. This finding is in line with a
including stress appraisals and coping efforts (Cox & Mackay, 1981; Folkman, 2008;
used in the current study is only one of a number of ways of defining and measuring
this construct (Aldao et al., 2010). The emotion regulation difficulties measure used in
the current study is based on Gratz and Roemer’s (2004) model of adaptive emotion
regulation that incorporates four key capacities: (1) emotional awareness and
behaviours and behave in accordance with desired goals in the face of unpleasant
emotions; and (4) the ability to use appropriate emotion regulation strategies. This
emotion regulation domain, however support for its factor structure is not
unequivocal. In particular, there has been some doubt over whether the lack of
the other subscales (Bardeen et al., 2012). In the current study, the items from the lack
of awareness and lack of clarity subscales had low correlations with items from the
other scales, and failed to significantly predict psychological distress outcomes. This
finding suggests that further investigation into the factor structure of the DERS is
warranted. It is possible that the types of emotion regulation difficulties that cluster
together vary across populations, and this hypothesis is also worth exploring.
compassion and stress. While emotion regulation is a broad construct that plays a
central role in psychopathology, the current findings suggest that different aspects of
depression or stress. The current results suggest that limited access to emotion
maintaining goal directed behaviour are related to depression, while difficulties with
impulse control are also implicated in the experience of stress. Broadly, these findings
are in line with previous research that examined the role of particular subscales of the
Bornovalova, Delany-Brumsey, Nick, & Lejuez, 2007). Further research into the role
93
outcomes is recommended.
The findings of the current study have important implications for the
such as emotional clarity and mood regulation (Heffernan, Quinn Griffin, McNulty, &
Fitzpatrick, 2010; Neff, 2003a), the current results support the prospect that therapists
who are more self-compassionate are likely to be better equipped to deal with the
However, due to the cross-sectional research design, the results of the current research
outcomes. While increased self-compassion may buffer against distress (Brown &
Ryan, 2003), it is also viable there is a reciprocal relationship between the constructs
(Macbeth & Gumley, 2012). It is also possible that self-compassion mediates the
This competing model was not investigated in the current study, as the broader
research question was concerned with mechanisms of action underlying the self-
compassion construct. In part, this is due to the fact that self-compassion was
on the other hand, is a broad construct encompassing a range of different skills and
step in future research investigating the theoretical model described in the current
study. In addition, in order to address the question of causation, future research would
benefit from the use of a longitudinal design to investigate the impact of changing
stress for psychologists were explored. Concerns about the levels of stress faced by
this professional group have lead a number of authors to argue that stress management
2007; Shapiro & Carlson, 2009), with self-care programs for trainees seen as “an
psychological problems” (Shapiro & Carlson, 2009, p. 110). However, while clinical
training programs often highlight the importance of self-care over the course of the
career span, teaching self-care skills to trainees is often a neglected part of the
curricula (Christopher et al., 2006). Shapiro and Carlson (2009) argue that this is a
notable drawback of current health care training programs, that has important
Study 1 found evidence that, after controlling for age and neuroticism, self-
programs may be of benefit in helping therapists to cope with the inevitable stressors
that occur in the process of therapeutic work, by helping them to regulate their
(see, e.g., Germer & Neff, 2013; Hofmann et al., 2011); although research in this area
is in its infancy. In order to explore the potential effectiveness and feasibility of self-
compassion training for psychologists, the present study sought to develop a self-
focus on trainees as the majority of the literature indicates that this subgroup tends to
range for depression, anxiety, and stress. In addition, there is a recognized need for
preventative efforts during clinical training that equip trainees with the skills
resilience over the career span. In this chapter, the rationale for the development of
development are outlined, and the protocol for the program is detailed.
writing, only 3 published studies were found that investigated the impact of
interventions designed to reduce or prevent stress among this group, and none of these
studies reported effect size. Consistent with the suggestion that constructs such as
Shapiro et al., 2007; Skovholt & Trotter-Mathison, 2011), the studies that were
for reducing stress and enhancing wellbeing amongst master’s level trainee
affect, rumination, and anxiety, and significant increases in positive affect, empathy,
were not investigated, a previous study by Shapiro et al. (2005) found that changes in
In a more recent study, Rimes and Wingrove (2011), evaluated the impact of
an 8-week Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy program (modified for stress rather
psychologists in various stages of training (first, second, and third year). The authors
based their rationale for this choice of intervention around three primary
considerations: (1) experiential learning was considered an important way for students
instructing others in the practice (e.g., Segal et al., 2002); (2) the practice of
mindfulness may enhance therapist skill development, as has been found to promote
for clinical psychologists, who often report elevated levels of stress (e.g., Hannigan et
only the first year students displayed a significant reduction in stress (p = .028).
Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999). Stafford-Brown and Pakenham (2012), found that
interventions amongst this group, they also found that levels of distress and self-doubt
Carlson, 2009); however, there are a number of reasons to believe that self-
the potential to increase trainees’ awareness of their current stress levels and to
enhance understanding of the internal processes that are implicated in stress appraisals
and responses (Shapiro et al., 2005; Shapiro et al., 2007; Shapiro et al., 1998). This
provides a starting point for trainees to better address stress as it arises, as well as
99
giving them scope to change their patterns of emotional responding over time
(Shapiro & Carlson, 2009). Therapists who are higher in mindfulness report feeling
more life satisfaction, more frequent positive emotions, less frequent negative
emotions, more job satisfaction, and less burnout (May & O'Donovan, 2007).
beyond those conferred by mindfulness training alone. A second major benefit of self-
compassion training is that it aims to help trainees treat themselves with kindness
psychopathology” (Van Dam et al., 2011, p. 2). Thus, in addition to enhancing basic
training may help to directly ameliorate negative or critical self-talk during times of
difficulty. This is significant given that negative self-talk can increase anxiety and
(Friedlander, Keller, Peca-Baker, & Olk, 1986; Hiebert, Uhlemann, Marshall, & Lee,
1998; Kurpius, Benjamin, & Morran, 1985; Nutt-Williams, Hayes, & Fauth, 1992;
Nutt-Williams & Hill, 1996). Encouraging self-kindness may also help to counter the
outcomes in the general population and which have been found to be predictive of
stress, burnout and reduced work satisfaction in psychologists (D'Souza et al., 2011;
that it may help to enhance trainees’ sense of interconnectedness, thereby reducing the
100
feeling that they are unique or alone in their stresses and difficulties. Trainee
therapists often report high levels of self-evaluative anxiety when starting out in
therapists from clients, as well as retrospective feedback from supervisors and peers
(Rønnestad & Skovholt, 1993; Skovholt & Rønnestad, 2003). High levels of anxiety
can interfere with therapists’ learning (Ho, Hosford, & Johnson, 1985) and
performance (Friedlander et al., 1986; Kelly, Hall, & Miller, 1989), while feelings of
performance and limit their disclosure with supervisors (Yourman, 2003). It is hoped
by helping trainees to see they are not alone in the challenges they face, their feelings
others is also important given that the professional role of the psychologist is one that
is potentially quite isolating (O'Connor, 2001), and feelings of loneliness are linked to
Aims of Study 2, Stage 1. The primary aim of the current study was to
currently undertaking clinical work in Australia. At the time of writing, there were
literature (e.g., Kirkpatrick, 2005; Shapira & Mongrain, 2010), and no extant protocol
available for any longer-term intervention with the primary aim of increasing self-
compassion among participants. Thus, the aim was to develop a novel intervention
that was grounded in relevant theory and findings from the empirical literature. An
additional aim for the current study was to develop an intervention that would be
accessible to individuals across different universities and organizations, and that could
access at their own discretion. The rationale for using a web-based intervention is
described in further detail below. Following this, the content of the Self-Compassion
Online (SCO) program is summarized, and the development process for the SCO
protocol is explained.
(Christensen & Hickie, 2010; Kazdin & Blase, 2011). Additionally, the Australian
Fourth National Mental Health Plan Working Group (2009) has recently called for the
compassion training in the current study. The term web-based intervention refers to “a
online program operated through a website and used by consumers seeking health-
and mental-health related assistance” (Barak, Klein, & Proudfoot, 2009, p. 5). Web-
based interventions are part of a wider range of e-mental health services which offer
mental health promotion and prevention, early intervention, support provision, and
clinical treatment services via telephone or the Internet (Christensen & Hickie, 2010).
service delivery within the helping professions, as they are constantly and remotely
accessible, support standardized but personalized service provision, and allow for
flexibility, by allowing them to manage their health care, determine the pace of
treatment, and engage with content as often as they like (Proudfoot et al., 2011). Web-
promoting mental health among young people and student populations, who are more
likely to have experience with using the Internet for information seeking, support, and
education (Burns, Davenport, Durkin, Luscombe, & Hickie, 2010; Monshat, Vella-
interventions via the Internet reduces some Beyond the potential advantages to
which decreases staff time, costs, and the risk of experimenter bias and error
Given that the target population in the current study were (a) tertiary students
likely to have familiarity with using the Internet for educational purposes; (b) located
around Australia; and (c) likely facing elevated time constraints (Shapiro & Carlson,
delivery. The evidence supporting the efficacy of e-health services is outlined briefly
reviewed.
evidence that e-health services are effective in the prevention and treatment of a range
of mental and physical health conditions amongst general, at risk, and clinical
populations (Barak, Hen, Boniel-Nissim, & Shapira, 2008; Cuijpers, van Straten,
Smit, Mihalopoulos, & Beekman, 2008; Griffiths, Farrer, & Christensen, 2010).
103
Christensen and Hickie (2010) argue that web-based technologies are “uniquely
placed” to deliver prevention and early intervention services as they allow cost-
the current research are findings that web-based programs can be used to deliver
stress management training (Zetterqvist, Maanmies, Strom, & Andersson, 2003), and
happiness (Seligman, Steen, Park, & Peterson, 2005), cognitive wellbeing (Mitchell et
al., 2009), and hope, efficacy, optimism, and resilience (Luthans et al., 2008).
or audio tape) in the treatment of a range of mental health problems (e.g., Anderson et
al., 2005; Apodaca & Miller, 2003; Cuijpers, 1997; Cuijpers, Donker, van Straten, Li,
& Andersson, 2010; den Boer, Wiersma, & Van de Bosch, 2004), and recent research
(Griffiths et al., 2010; Spek et al., 2007). For example, van Straten, Cuijpers, and
trialed its effectiveness for reducing symptoms of depression, anxiety, and burnout.
Using a randomized controlled trial with (n = 213) a wait-list control group, they
found that participants in the intervention group reported statistically and clinically
104
randomized controlled trial (n = 3070) with a wait-list control, they obtained post-test
and 12-week follow-up data for 1529 (49.80%) participants, with significant increases
control group.
Method
drafting content and structure for the program based on a review of the literature
and the promotion of wellbeing; (2) adapting the content and structure of the program
and designing the website in line with recommendations from the literature on the
program, and revising the content and structure in line with the feedback provided.
development of the SCO program was informed at the broadest level by theory and
intervention research relating to the promotion of resilience and the prevention and
(1984) transactional model of stress (outlined in Chapter One) as its theoretical basis.
This model suggests that the ways in which individuals appraise and cope with stress
impact their psychological wellbeing more than the experience of particular types of
stressors (see Aldwin & Reveson, 1987 for a review of research supporting this
proposition).
105
Within this framework, the content and structure of the intervention was
Cohn & Fredrickson, 2011; Hayes, Follette, & Linehan, 2004; Roemer & Orsillo,
2009; Seligman et al., 2005; Wilson, 2009), given that self-compassion spans these
distinct but interrelated fields. The content for the current intervention aimed to
include common themes and exercises from extant books that provided “self-help”
self-compassion training, such as Germer (2009), Neff (2011) and Gilbert (2010a,
2010c). The findings from Study 1 also guided the development of the content of the
evidence base for the structure and content of the SCO program is described in further
detail below.
Key components of the SCO program. Content for each module of the SCO
and exercises with reflective and experiential components. The rationale for the
therapies (Roemer & Orsillo, 2009) and is thought to provide participants with the
motivation to make such change (Roemer & Orsillo, 2009). In addition, an intellectual
participants with support and encouragement during times when change is difficult,
learning to new contexts (Roemer & Orsillo, 2009). Throughout the program, the
minute) videos; for more complex concepts, some supportive text and illustrations
(Kristeller & Johnson, 2005), and was therefore a key component of the SCO
protocol. While Goyal et al.’s (2014) recent review and meta-analysis did not find
evidence that meditation training has an effect on positive affect or wellbeing, it was
context out of which self-compassion arises; thus, mindfulness skills are seen as an
compassion, and warmth (Hofmann et al., 2011), and has been found to increase
107
feelings of compassion for self and others (for a review, see Boellinghaus et al.,
practice among trainee therapists Boellinghaus, Jones, and Hutton (2013) reported
compassion after engaging in this practice, and that it positively impacted their
meditative practice, provided permission for his audio (mp3) series of mindfulness
and self-compassion meditations (Germer, n.d.) to be used in the research. In line with
training was graduated; short (5 minute) meditations were offered in the first module,
the duration of the program. This structure was designed to introduce participants to
online, or to download them and listen to them on a computer or portable mp3 player.
as compassionate letter writing and self-care practice) have been used previously in
helping participants apply the skills and knowledge they have learned to their own
lives and individual circumstances (e.g., Neff, 2011; Shapira & Mongrain, 2010). In
the variability in their experiences, and their relationships with certain contexts and
behaviours. All exercises were adapted from existing MAB protocols or designed
comprised six modules, administered over six weeks, with weekly exercises and
contact from the program administrator via email. It was decided to have participants
complete exercises on a weekly basis, rather than more frequently, to make the
intervention more feasible for participants with time constraints. In addition, there is
some evidence that positive psychology interventions are more effective when
exercises are administered weekly rather than daily (Emmons & McCullogh, 2003;
Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, & Schkade, 2005). At the end of each module, a summary of
the module was provided in Portable Document Format (PDF) form, for participants
homework each week. These exercises were designed to reinforce the concepts
Intervention Protocol.
Module one: introduction. The primary aim of the first module was to
video format provided an introduction to the module, and Neff’s (2003b) tripartite
that asks listeners to simply tune in to their current internal state with a sense of
Following this was a “reflection space” in which participants were invited to reflect
In the next steps of this module, participants were invited to reflect on their
current self-care practices, and watch a short video outlining the rationale for self-care
followed this exercise. Using an exercise based on Germer (2009), participants are
invited to contemplate they habitual ways in which they respond to difficulties in their
lives. Participants were then asked to listen to an audio recording that discusses the
humanity) in greater depth. Following this was a video that explained these
along with a quiz that allowed participants to reflect on which skills they may already
In the final steps of Module One, participants were asked to reflect on how
they think self-compassion might be useful for them, and to set some goals for their
participation in the program. The module concludes with a list of frequently asked
questions about self-compassion, and their corresponding answers. Given the self-
guided nature of the program, this information was included as a way of addressing
any potential conceptual or practical issues that participants may have with self-
compassion training.
participants were asked to practice the “Waiting on Yourself” meditation daily for a
110
week. They were also asked to complete a reflective exercise (adapted from Germer,
2009), that asked them to reflect on the ways in which they take care of themselves in
and to brainstorm new ideas for self-care in each domain. Based on these ideas,
Module two: mindfulness. The aim of the second module was to focus on the
their attention to mindfulness of difficult experiences, which is the core focus in self-
phenomena (Bishop et al., 2004). These concepts were also explained using a
developed by the researcher, and a “Three Breaths” practice based on the “Three
In the second steps of this module, participants were invited to watch a video
describing the ways in which people commonly act “on autopilot” (i.e., non-
mindfully). Participants were then invited to reflect on the ways that they may act on
autopilot, as a way of drawing their attention to the areas of their lives in which they
may benefit from acting more mindfully. Following this, participants were invited to
watch a video explaining two different ways of relating to difficult experiences (for
the difference between these two modes of relating to experience, they were provided
with instructions for a “Mindfully working with Pain” exercise based on an activity
outlined in Neff (2011). As the final part of this step, participants were asked to
bringing their attention to the ways emotion can physically manifest in the body.
explained the difference between formal and informal mindfulness practice, and an
experiential exercise that provided participants with instructions for carrying out daily
mindfulness can inform and benefit psychotherapeutic practice was provided, and
participants were asked to reflect on some of the ways that mindfulness practice might
be most relevant for them. Frequently asked questions and corresponding responses
participants were asked to engage in their choice of one short (5-10 minute) formal
and one informal meditation practice each day for a week. As preparation for the third
module, participants were also asked to start self-monitoring the nature of their self-
112
talk, by completing the “Self-Talk Scale” (STS; Brinthaupt, Hein, & Kramer, 2009).
The STS is a 16-item questionnaire designed to evaluate the frequency with which
respondents engage in self-talk under certain situations. Sample items include “I talk
to myself when I want to reinforce myself for doing well” and “I talk to myself when
I feel ashamed of something I’ve done” (Brinthaupt et al., 2009). Responses are given
Module three: self-kindness. The focus of the third module was the
compassion construct, and “entails being warm and understanding toward ourselves
when we suffer, fail, or feel inadequate, rather than flagellating ourselves with self-
criticism” (Germer & Neff, 2013, p. 1). Key themes explored in this module were
“Soften, Soothe, Allow” practice (Germer, n.d.) presented using an audio recording,
“Self-Talk Scale” to describe the nature of their self-talk and the emotional and
physical impact that this had on them. Drawing attention to self-talk in this way was
criticism (e.g., Gilbert et al., 2006; Longe et al., 2010; Zuroff et al., 1999), and given
113
that may be linked to their perceptions of self-worth, and the distinction between
Beginners” audio meditation (Germer, n.d.), which aims to help participants develop
participants were also introduced to a practice that aims to help individuals self-soothe
during times of difficulty that may allow them to self-soothe and focus on having their
needs met, rather than exacerbating the negative emotional experience (Leahy, Tirch,
daily practice of mindfulness meditations (from Module Two) alternated with the
they were feeling stressed during the week, and to monitor how they were feeling
before and after this practice. Finally, participants were encouraged to practice
putting them somewhere where they were likely to see them frequently.
114
was designed to focus on the theme of common humanity – which, contrasted with
one’s flaws and difficulties to the wider scope of human experience, and represents an
alternative to feeling alienated by one’s own suffering. A primary goal of this module
was to facilitate participants’ contemplation of the ways in which they feel connected
or separate from others; moreover, given that trainee psychologists commonly report
performance concerns are a common experience amongst trainees and a normal part
of therapist development. Key themes of this module were belongingness and social
their current emotional experiences in social situations using the Social Safeness and
Pleasure Scale (SSPS; Gilbert et al., 2008). The SSPS is an 11-item measure that asks
participants to rate the extent to which they experience warmth, reassurance, and
with people”. Following this, participants were asked to watch a video that explored
the themes of connection with others, and feeling a sense of “belonging” as opposed
to “fitting in”. Participants were asked to further explore ideas of belongingness and
them and were provided with psychoeducation around the link between the
115
construction and maintenance of social identity and feedback about self-worth (see,
Following this, participants were provided with an overview of how low self-
compassion can lead to feeling isolated and disconnected from others during times of
difficulty. They were invited to reflect on their social comparison processes – that is,
the way they see themselves in relation to others, using the Social Comparison Scale
(Allan & Gilbert, 1995). This measure uses a semantic differential scale to measure
reflect on who they were comparing themselves with when responding to this scale,
and provided with psychoeducation around how reference groups for social
connection with others, and how these might be overcome. They were also asked to
contemplate how changing the way they perceive themselves in relation to others may
into their daily practice. This meditation is 20 minutes long and builds on the loving-
kindness meditation introduced in Module Three; participants were given the option
of using a shorter recording if they were facing time restrictions. Participants were
particularly those who they felt distant or disconnected from. The aim of this exercise
was to counter the tendency to distinguish oneself from others using social
116
comparison, and instead draw attention to features of self and others that underscore
Module five: dealing with difficult emotions. The aim of the fifth module was
Modules 2-4 to help them cope with difficult emotional experiences. The decision to
informed by the findings from the first study, that self-compassion impacted
depression and stress via reducing difficulties with emotion regulation. This is also in
line with the general design of MAB protocols which seek to facilitate change by
were given a brief summary of the emotion regulation construct. They were also given
difficulties, each of which was illustrated with an example. Participants were asked to
reflect on whether any of these difficulties resonated with them and were encouraged
difficulties had influenced the way they responded. In the second step of this module,
participants were asked to watch a video explaining how the various aspects of self-
deal with. This concept was reinforced with psychoeducational content that contrasted
ruminative, or self-critical way. Participants were also asked to watch a video that
In the third step of this module, participants were asked to integrate their self-
the Self-Compassion Letter-Writing exercise. This exercise, drawn from Neff (2011),
asks participants to recall a difficult situation they have encountered, and then write a
framework that (a) brings mindful awareness to the underlying needs and emotional
processes implicated in the situation; (b) offers words of reassurance and support in
recognition of the difficulty of the experience; and (c) seeks to emphasize connection
with others during this difficult moment by considering how such situations are a
Module six: integrating self-compassion into clinical training. The focus of the
against occupational stress and burnout. This module also aimed to summarise key
concepts introduced in the program and provide a rationale for ongoing self-compassion
practice.
Waiting on Yourself audio meditation that was first introduced in the first module.
Participants were asked to reflect on how their experience with this practice may have
changed over the duration of the program. Following this, participants were asked to
psychologist, particular those related to starting clinical work. Participants were also
asked to reflect on some of the concerns that were salient for them in relation to their
work as psychologists.
118
In the next steps of this module, participants were invited to watch a video that
outlined some of the ongoing stressors involved in psychotherapeutic work (i.e. those
that continue across the career span and are not just limited to the trainee experience).
They were then asked to contemplate which aspects of self-compassion practice may be
occupational stress. Following this, participants were invited to read a list of ways in
In the final steps of this module, participants were provided with psychoeducational
content that explored the consequences of occupational stress for psychologists, and
stress and prevent burnout. Participants were also given a list of symptoms of stress and
burnout, and asked to reflect on which symptoms they were currently experiencing, and
participants were asked to continue their meditation practice, with the option of
were asked to experiment with the “Waiting on Yourself” practice in an informal way
sensations) at some point during their working day. Finally, participants were asked to
complete the Professional Resilience and Self-Care Inventory (Skovholt & Trotter-
Mathison, 2011).
and high attrition rates often associated with the delivery of such services (Kelders, Kok,
Ossebaard, & Van Gemert-Pijnen, 2012; Powell et al., 2013). As such, creating a rich
119
compliance and minimise program drop-out. The SCO program was administered via a
website, which was designed in line with the recommendations outlined in Ritterband,
nature of behavioural instructions and prompts, ease of use, accuracy and clarity of
content, mode and message of content delivery (including form, style, likeability, and
the online structure of the program utilised features of persuasive system design
administration was the fact that mindfulness- and acceptance- based programs are
with facilitators and fellow participants as an important part of learning (Segal et al.,
2002). The presence of other participants is thought to enhance social support and
Hollon, 1998; Hollon & Ponniah, 2010). As such, an online environment in which
presents unique challenges for teaching the core components of a mindfulness- and
addition, weekly emails were sent to provide participants with an outline of the
module they would be completing that week, and participants were given the option
Protocol review and website testing. Once the protocol for the program had
been developed, its content was reviewed by one expert reviewer and two users who
were drawn from the target population but unable to participate in Studies 3 and 4. All
three reviewers were asked to provide feedback on the language used, the clarity and
coherence of the information presented, the ease of use of the website and associated
audiovisual elements (such as audio and video recordings), module length, and
simplifying the language used, changing the layout so that text was easier to read,
presenting some of the text content in video format, and including more explicit
implemented as per these suggestions. A screenshot of a page from the final program
is included in Appendix D.
Discussion
interventions, the aim of the current study was to develop a protocol for a
to occupational stress among psychologists, the aim of the current study tailored the
subsequently refined based on recommendations from one expert and two user
reviewers. Preliminary research into the effectiveness and feasibility of the SCO
Introduction
Study 1 found evidence that, after controlling for age and neuroticism, self-
these findings and the rationale for the development of stress prevention programs for
developed in Stage 1 of Study 2. The present study sought to examine the effects of
The primary aim of the present study was to examine the impact of the SCO
These outcomes were identified as relevant to the current study based on the results of
A second aim of the current study was to examine the impact of the program
Authentic happiness has been defined as the degree to which one experiences a life
which is pleasant, engaging, and meaningful (Seligman, 2002), and is a construct that
aligns closely with the concept of eudemonic happiness. This outcome was chosen
123
considered to be more aligned with eudemonic than hedonic happiness, a view that
has since been supported by Neff and Costigan (2014). It was considered important to
The final aim of the current study was to evaluate the acceptability and
satisfaction. In order to address these latter questions, feedback data was gathered
from participants at the end of each module, as well as at post-test and 12-week
follow-up.
Hypotheses.
scores between pre- and post-test, and these changes would be maintained at
follow-up.
pre- and post-test, and these changes would be maintained at follow up.
124
(depression, anxiety, and stress) scores between pre- and post-test, and these
Method
Ethics statement. Ethical approval for this study was granted by the Curtin
Participants. Thirty seven (89% female, mean age 32.61 years) postgraduate
psychology trainees from around Australia participated in the study. This sample size
was deemed adequate, given power calculations using the G*Power program (Faul,
Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007) indicated that a sample size of 28 would be
Psychology Doctorate program. The average number of hours of clinical work per
week reported across participants was 19. The demographic characteristics of the
university email channels and advertising on social media. Participants were invited
program. The screening questionnaire was designed to screen out participants who did
125
not meet the inclusion criteria. Following screening, eligible participants were
emailed a link to the pre-test measures one week prior to the commencement of the
and password, which they used to log in to the program once it was open.
Table 15
Demographic Characteristics of Sample (n = 37)
Variable M SD n %
Age 29 3.45
Gender
Female 34 91.89
Male 3 8.11
beginning of each week, they received an email giving them an overview of the
current module and encouraging them to log in to the program. At the end of each
week, they were emailed with a link to the weekly feedback questionnaire. At the end
of the week of the sixth module, participants were emailed a link to the post-test
measures, which were available for one week. Twelve weeks after the end of the sixth
126
module, participants were emailed a link to the follow-up measures, which were also
available for one week. Each time a participant completed a weekly measure or the
post-test or follow-up questionnaire, they became eligible to enter a prize draw for an
weekly program feedback survey, and further feedback questions were asked at post-
focused on this total scale score. However, this version of the SCS also provides
reliable information about subscale scores, and is recommended for use where
reliability, convergent validity and discriminant validity for the scale are all strong
(Neff, 2009).
Authentic Happiness Inventory (AHI; Peterson & Park, 2008). The AHI is a
24-item updated version of the Steen Happiness Index (SHI) developed by Seligman
et al. (2005) to measure authentic happiness. Each item is a set of five statements that
range from negative (e.g., My life does not have any purpose or meaning) to
extremely positive (e.g., I have a very clear idea about the purpose or meaning of my
life). Preliminary data support the reliability of this scale (e.g., Schiffrin & Nelson,
2010).
the current study, the DERS was used to measure difficulties with emotion regulation.
Perceived Stress Scale (PSS; Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983). In the
current study, the PSS (Cohen et al., 1983) was used to measure perceived stress. This
scale is a 10-item self-report measure which asks respondents to rate the frequency of
scale, ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (very often). The PSS has reported adequate
construct and discriminant validity, and good reliability, with reported Cronbach’s
Lovibond, 1995). In the current study, the DASS-21 was used to measure symptoms
of depression, anxiety, and stress. Please refer to Study 1 for details of this measure.
Research design and data analysis. The aims of the current study were tested
using a single-group, open trial design, with measures administered at pre-test, post-
test, and 12-week follow-up. An open trial research design was considered an
128
the context of a controlled trial) may not be justifiable, and the use of a single-group
design may thus be supported (Ip et al., 2013). In line with Chambless and Hollon
and feedback questionnaires to further understand the clinical utility of the program.
Generalised Linear Mixed Model (GLMM) that tested for the effect of time within the
context of a hierarchical design in which time (pre-test, post-test, and follow-up) was
nested within participants, with time treated as a fixed effect, and participants treated
(such as repeated measures ANOVA), as it uses all the data available at each
assessment point, rather than relying on all participants to provide data at each point,
thereby optimising statistical power and lessening the impact of sampling bias due to
subject attrition (Elobeid et al., 2009; Kwok et al., 2008). This analytical technique
may thus be particularly suited to research on web-based interventions, for which high
dropout rates are often reported (Melville, Casey, & Kavanagh, 2010). In addition,
while alternative methods of analysing behavioural change over time are based on
129
(Kwok et al., 2008). GLMM is also more able to estimate group means when group
size is small compared to other statistical techniques (Holden, Kelley, & Agarwal,
2008).
conducted for each of the nineteen outcome variables. As analysing each outcome
independently inflates the familywise error rate, per-test alpha levels needed to be
corrected to control the inflation. To optimise statistical power, alpha correction were
applied within groups of conceptually related outcomes, across the entire set of
outcomes (Klockars, Hancock, & McAweeney, 1995). For the psychological distress
outcomes, the Bonferroni-corrected alpha level was .017. For the subscales of the
corrected alpha level was .008. All other tests were performed at the conventional
refers to whether the impact of treatment has practical significance for a client; i.e.,
whether change on outcome measures translates into meaningful changes in daily life
change, and each method has its own strengths and weaknesses (Kazdin, 2003;
individual has demonstrated reliable change (i.e. a change that is more than expected
given the measurement error of the scale) and then determining whether their scores
130
reflect a shift away from the norms of the clinical population and towards the norms
of the functional population (Kazdin, 2001). Based on this method, Jacobson and
Truax (1991) have proposed four categories for classifying treatment outcomes,
according to whether clients have achieved reliable and clinically significant change.
Recovered individuals are those whose scores on the outcome measure reflect reliable
change and a shift into the functional population; improved individuals are those
whose scores on the outcome measure reflect reliable change but remain in the
clinical range but have shifted in a positive direction; unchanged individuals are those
whose scores do not indicate any reliable change, while individuals whose scores
While Jacobson and Truax (1991) offer three different cut-off criteria to
only: the norms for the “functional” population and the norms for the “dysfunctional”
population. Ronk, Korman, Hooke, and Page (2013) argue that this approach “ignores
hetereogeneity within the “dysfunctional” population” (p. 1103) and may not provide
Burlingame, and Hansen (1996), Ronk et al. (2013) proposed cut-offs for determining
(DASS-21; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995) based on the norms for “inpatient,”
within or between each of these three categories. While this approach is relatively
new, it was considered an appropriate method for calculating reliable and clinically
131
significant change on the DASS-21 for each individual in the current study because
the target group was not a clinical sample per se and there was marked heterogeneity
in symptom severity.
five categories according to (a) whether the magnitude of their score change is above
a certain threshold (i.e. whether it is reliable) and (b) which of the three distributions
whose scores demonstrate positive reliable change and have shifted into the
nonclinical range are classified as recovered; those who have made a positive reliable
change from the inpatient to outpatient range are classified as recovering; individuals
whose scores represent a positive reliable change within any range are considered
improved; those who have not made a reliable change in either direction are classified
as unchanged, while those whose scores reflect negative reliable change are said to
have deteriorated.
Effect Sizes.Effect sizes for changes on outcome variables between pre- and
post-test and pre-test and pre-test and follow-up were determined by calculating
Table 16 provides Cohen’s (1992) conventions for determining the effect size
indicated by Cohen’s d.
Table 16
Effect Size Conventions for Cohen’s d
0.2 Small
0.5 Moderate
≥ 0.8 Large
132
Results
Data screening. In line with Tabachnick and Fidell (2013), data for each
variable were screened to check for missing and out-of-range values prior to analysis.
one-way, repeated-measures ANOVA for each of the outcomes using SPSS indicated
uses all the data present at each assessment point. This reduces sampling bias and the
need to replace missing data. GLMM is able to use the data present at each
assessment point because time (T1, T2, T3) is interpreted as a Level 1 variable that is
In order to assess whether there were differences between completers and non-
completers (i.e. those who provided data at post-test and those who did not), Mann-
Whitney U tests were used to compare these groups on pre-test scores for each of the
outcome variables. The results of these tests were non-significant for all variables
apart from perceived stress, with pre-test levels of perceived stress for non-completers
significantly higher (Mean Rank = 22.00, n = 17) than for completers (Mean Rank =
difference in terms of age and gender between participants who completed the
program and those who did not, chi-square tests were conducted on the demographic
data provided. Across all participants, there was no significant difference between
completers and non-completers in terms of gender, χ2 (1, N = 37) = .04, p > .05 or age
Descriptive statistics. The mean scores for each outcome variable at pre-test,
post-test and follow-up are reported in Table 17. While the DASS-21 is not intended
severity labels (normal, mild, moderate, severe, extremely severe) are provided
(Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995, see Study 1). In the current study, mean depression and
anxiety scores were in the normal range at pre-test, post-test and follow-up, while
mean stress scores were in the mild range at pre-test, but in the normal range at post-
Hypothesis testing.
Main effects of time and pairwise contrasts. A series of GLMMs was used to
evaluate the relationship between the fixed effect of time and each of the outcome
variables (SCS, DERS, AHI, PSS, and DASS-21). The main effects for time for each
of the outcome variables are summarized in Table 18. In addition, post hoc Least
Significant Difference (LSD) tests were conducted to test for significant differences
between pre-test and post-test, post-test and follow-up, and pre-test and follow-up.
was supported, F[2,65] = 28.51, p <.001, with significant, positive changes in self-
compassion scores observed between pre- and post-test (p < .001) . Additionally,
between post-test and follow-up (p <.001). Effect size calculations indicated a large
effect for the changes in self-compassion between pre- and post- test (d = .86) and
Table 17
Means and Standard Deviations for Outcomes at Pre-test Post-test and Follow-up.
Post-test Follow-up
Pre-test (n = 37)
(n = 20) (n = 13)
M SD M SD M SD
SCS
Total 2.76 .01 3.43 .14 3.49 .11
Self-Kindness 2.79 .11 3.72 .14 3.45 .12
Self-Judgment 3.37 .13 2.65 .14 2.57 .15
Common Humanity 3.08 .13 3.86 .16 3.61 .15
Isolation 3.30 .15 2.74 .19 2.88 .22
Mindfulness 3.11 .12 3.82 .13 3.78 .16
Overidentification 3.45 .14 2.91 .26 2.73 .93
DERS
Total 76.78 2.80 67.35 3.04 67.23 4.16
Non-Acceptance 12.81 .93 10.06 .61 9.44 .72
Goal-Direction 15.73 .74 13.02 .73 12.84 .63
Impulse Control 10.24 .45 9.02 .51 9.14 .76
Awareness 13.46 .68 12.53 .81 12.94 1.00
Strategies 15.35 1.03 13.40 .95 12.61 1.03
Clarity 9.19 .50 8.53 .50 8.74 .47
AHI 3.12 .07 3.35 .074 3.28 .10
PSS 19.57 6.25 14.65 5.18 13.92 6.07
DASS-21
Depression 7.40 .98 4.00 .88 4.00 1.18
Anxiety 5.24 1.03 3.50 1.08 1.64 .85
Stress 15.13 1.36 9.10 1.16 8.92 1.98
SCS: Self-compassion Scale; DERS: Difficulties with Emotion Regulation Scale; AHI: Authentic
Happiness Inventory; PSS: Perceived Stress Scale; DASS-21: 21-Item Depression, Anxiety, Stress
Scales.
135
Table 18
Results of the Fixed Effects of Time for Each Outcome.
Outcome Numerator df Denominator df F-value Sig.
SCS
Total 2 65 28.51 <.001
Self-Kindness 2 65 18.58 <.001
Self-Judgment 2 67 11.64 <.001
Common Humanity 2 67 14.39 <.001
Isolation 2 67 3.50 .036
Mindfulness 2 67 13.25 <.001
Overidentification 2 60 10.63 <.001
DERS
Total 2 67 17.01 <.001
Non-Acceptance 2 67 6.52 .003
Goal-Direction 2 67 19.27 <.001
Impulse Control 2 67 6.57 .002
Lack of Awareness 2 66 .67 .515
Strategies 2 67 4.97 .010
Clarity 2 65 1.56 .218
AHI 2 67 6.75 .002
PSS 2 67 6.73 .002
DASS
Depression 2 67 5.37 .007
Anxiety 2 65 7.92 .001
Stress 2 67 14.60 <.001
SCS: Self-compassion Scale; DERS: Difficulties with Emotion Regulation Scale; AHI: Authentic
Happiness Inventory; PSS: Perceived Stress Scale; DASS-21: 21-Item Depression, Anxiety, Stress
Scales.
136
Significant main effects for time were also observed for the six self-
directions (i.e. positive), and were maintained at follow-up. Data for the pairwise
contrasts between pre-test and post-test, post-test and follow-up, and pre-test and
Table 19
Least Significant Difference (LSD) Tests of the Simple Main Effects of Time with Pairwise
Contrasts of Pre-Test and Post-Test (T1-T2), Post-Test and Follow-Up (T2-T3) and Pre-Test
and Follow-Up (T1-T3) for Self-Compassion Outcomes.
<.001, with significant negative changes in total emotion regulation difficulties scores
observed between pre- and post-test (p < .001). These scores continued to decrease
between post-test and follow-up; however these changes were non-significant. Effect
size calculations indicated a moderate-large effect size for the changes in total
emotion regulation difficulties between pre- and post-test, and a moderate effect size
The significance of pre- to post-test changes for the DERS subscales was
for time were observed for three of the six DERS subscales: non-acceptance (F[2,67]
.001), and impulse control difficulties (F[2,67] = 6.57, p = .002), with limited access
.010). Changes for impulse control were maintained at follow-up, while mean scores
up; however these changes were non-significant. In addition, scores on the lack of
emotional awareness and lack of emotional clarity subscales decreased between pre-
and post-test; however these changes were not significant. Data for the pairwise
contrasts between pre-test and post-test, post-test and follow-up, and pre-test and
follow-up for the DERS total score, and for each of the DERS subscales are reported
in Table 20.
138
Table 20
Least Significant Difference (LSD) Tests of the Simple Main Effects of Time with Pairwise
Significance; d: Cohen’s d.
observed between pre- and post-test (p = .001). These changes were maintained at
follow-up. A moderate effect size was observed for the pre-post test changes in
happiness, while the effect size for changes in happiness between pre-test and follow-
up was small. Data for the pairwise contrasts between pre-test and post-test, post-test
and follow-up, and pre-test and follow-up for happiness are reported in Table 21.
139
Table 21
Least Significant Difference (LSD) Tests of the Simple Main Effects of Time with Pairwise
Significance; d: Cohen’s d.
This hypothesis was supported (F[2,66] = 4.97, p =.002), with significant decreases in
perceived stress observed between pre- and post-test (p = .002). These changes were
maintained at follow-up. A moderate effect size was observed for the pre-post
changes in perceived stress (d = .52), while a small-moderate effect size was observed
for the changes in perceived stress between pre-test and follow up (d = .48). Data for
the pairwise contrasts between pre-test and post-test, post-test and follow-up, and pre-
test and follow-up for stress appraisals are reported in Table 22.
Table 22
Least Significant Difference (LSD) Tests of the Simple Main Effects of Time with Pairwise
Significance; d: Cohen’s d.
Finally, Hypothesis 5 predicted significant main effects for time on each of the
hypothesis was supported for each of the three psychological distress outcomes:
depression (F[2,67] = 5.37, p =.007), anxiety (F[2,65] = 7.92, p =.001), and stress
(F[2,67] = 14.60, p < .001). Mean depression scores significantly decreased between
pre- and post-test (p - .002), with changes maintained at follow-up. For anxiety
.165); however there were significant reductions between pre-test and follow-up (p =
.003). Mean stress scores significantly decreased between pre-and post-test (p < .001),
and continued to decrease between post-test and follow-up; however these changes
were non-significant. A moderate effect size was observed for changes in depression
between pre- and post-test (d = .54), while a small-moderate effect size was observed
for pre-test to follow-up changes (d = .31). For changes in anxiety, a small effect size
was observed between pre- and post-test (d = .23), while the effect size for pre-test to
follow-up changes was moderate (d = .52). For changes in stress, a large effect size
was observed for pre-posttest changes, while pre-test to follow-up changes showed a
small-moderate effect size (d = .46). Data for the pairwise contrasts between pre-test
and post-test, post-test and follow-up, and pre-test and follow-up for depression,
Table 23
Least Significant Difference (LSD) Tests of the Simple Main Effects of Time with
Pairwise Contrasts for Psychological Distress Outcomes.
Significance; d: Cohen’s d.
Reliable and clinically significant change. In line with Ronk et al. (2013)
reliable change calculations for each participant’s pre-post-test and pre-test – follow-
up scores on the DASS-21 were carried out using the cut-offs and minimum change
thresholds provided in Table 24. For participants who had achieved reliable change,
change on depression scores. Of these, two were classified as improved, and three
anxiety scores. Of these, two were classified as deteriorated, two were classified as
improved, and three were classified as recovered. Six participants (30%) achieved
reliable change on stress scores. Of these, three were classified as improved, and three
change on depression scores, with all three classified as recovered. Four participants
(31%) achieved reliable change on anxiety scores; of these, three were classified as
recovered, and one was classified as improved. Four participants achieved reliable
change on stress scores, with two classified as recovered and two classified as
improved.
follow-up change on the DASS-21, the severity of their pre-test, post-test, and follow-
up scores according to the cut-offs provided by Lovibond and Lovibond (1995), and
whether they achieved reliable change according to Ronk et al. (2013). For
Table 24
Cutoff Scores and Reliable Change Thresholds for Determining Clinically Significant
DASS-21 scale
Table 25
Direction of Change, Severity Indices, and Reliable and Clinically Significant Change Data for Participant’s Pre- and Post-Intervention
Scores on the DASS-21.
1 NC Norm. Norm. - Dec. Norm. Norm. Yes Imp. Dec. Norm. Norm. No -
2 NC Norm. Norm. - NC Norm. Norm. - Dec. Norm. Norm. Yes Imp.
3 Dec. Mild. Norm. Yes Imp. Dec. Mild Norm. Yes Rec. Dec. Sev. Norm. Yes Rec.
4 NC Norm. Norm. - NC Norm. Norm. - Dec. Norm. Norm. No -
5 Dec. Mod. Norm. Yes Rec. Dec. Norm. Norm. Yes Imp. Dec. Mod. Norm. Yes Rec.
6 Dec. Norm. Norm. No - Dec. Norm. Norm. N/A NC Norm. Norm. -
7 Dec. Norm. Norm. N/A Dec. Norm. Norm. N/A Dec. Norm. Norm. No -
8 Dec. Sev. Norm. Yes Rec. Dec. Mild Norm. Yes Rec. Dec. Mod. Norm. No -
9 Inc. Norm. Norm. No - Dec. Mod Norm. Yes Rec. Dec. Norm. Norm. No -
10 Dec. Norm. Norm. No - Inc. Mod Sev No - Dec. Mild. Norm. No -
(table continues)
145
No DC Pre Sev. Post Sev. RC CSC DC Pre Sev. Post Sev. RC CSC DC Pre Sev. Post Sev. RC CSC
11 Dec Mod. Norm. Yes Rec. Inc. Norm. Norm. No Dec. Mild. Norm. Yes Rec.
14 NC Mild. Norm. - Dec. Mod Mild No - Dec. Mod. Norm. Yes Rec.
16 NC Norm. Norm. - Inc. Norm. Mild Yes Det. Dec. Mod. Norm. No -
17 Dec. Norm. Norm. Yes Imp. NC Norm. Norm. - Dec. Norm. Norm. Yes Imp.
DC: Direction of Change; Dec: Decrease; Inc: Increase; NC: No Change; Pre Sev.: Pre-Test Severity; Post-Sev: Post-Test Severity; Norm: Normal; Mod.: Moderate;
Sev.: Severe; RC: Reliable Change; CSC: Clinically Significant Change; Imp.: Improved; Rec.: Recovered; Det: Deteriorated
N.B. For participants whose pre-test score was below the minimum score change required for reliable change, reliable and clinically significant change columns are
marked “N/A”.
146
Table 26
Direction of Change, Severity Indices, and Reliable and Clinically Significant Change Data for Participant’s Pre-Intervention and
Follow-Up Scores on the DASS-21.
1 Dec. Norm. Norm. No - Dec. Norm. Norm. Yes Imp. Dec. Mild Norm. No -
2 Dec. Mild Norm. No - Dec. Norm. Norm. N/A Dec. Norm. Norm. No -
3 Dec. Mod. Norm. Yes Rec. NC Norm. Norm. - Inc. Mild Mild No -
4 Dec. Mild Norm. No - Dec. Mod. Norm. Yes Rec. Dec. Mod. Mod. No -
5 NC Norm. Norm. - NC Norm. Norm. - Dec. Norm. Norm. No -
6 Dec. Norm. Norm. No - Dec. Mod. Mod. No - Inc. Mild. Norm. No -
7 NC Norm. Norm. - NC Norm. Norm. N/A NC Norm. Norm. -
8 Dec. Sev. Norm. Yes Rec. Dec. Mild Norm. Yes Rec. Dec. Mod. Norm. Yes Rec
9 Dec. Norm. Norm. N/A Dec. Norm. Norm. N/A Dec. Norm. Norm. Yes Imp.
10 Dec. Norm. Norm. No - NC Norm. Norm. - Dec. Norm. Norm. Yes Imp.
(table continues)
147
No DC Pre Sev. Post Sev. RC CSC DC Pre Sev. Post Sev. RC CSC DC Pre Sev. Post Sev. RC CSC
11 Inc. Norm. Mod. Yes Rec. NC Norm. Norm. - Dec. Norm. Norm. Yes Rec.
12 Dec. Norm. Norm. No - Dec. Mod Norm. Yes Rec. NC Norm. Norm. -
a number of feedback measures during and after the intervention that provide some
indication of the degree to which they complied and were satisfied with the various
Feedback response rates varied per module; however for each module, over
entirely. Completion rates per module are shown in Table 27. In addition, all
respondents for each module reported understanding the module, while 75% or more
reported being able to do the module well. In a similar vein, responses collected for
each module indicated that 75% or more of respondents found the module enjoyable,
while 25% or less found it difficult. Over 75% of respondents for each module
reported learning something from the module, while over 85% reported finding the
to a question asking them what benefits (if any) they felt they had gained from
that they had a better understanding of self-compassion and how to apply within their
personal lives, including being more mindful and kinder to themselves, not being so
self-critical, having increased self-acceptance, and realising that they are not alone in
practices and an increased capacity to handle stress and to recover from difficult
149
experiences. They reported that the SCO program provided them with a structure for
taking time for self, as well as a platform for self-reflection. Finally, participants
reported benefits in terms of the impact on their therapeutic work, including using
self-reflection as part of the therapeutic process and being able to “practise what you
any) they experienced with the SCO program. The primary difficulty that participants
reported experiencing was time constraints, although there were also difficulties with
suggested that making the program less dense may increase the likelihood of them
Table 27
Participants’ Program Completion Rates Per Module
Table 28
“I found this module relevant to me” 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 2 (12.50%) 10 (62.50%) 4 (25%)
“I learned something from this module” 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 2 (12.50%) 9 (56.25%) 5 (31.25%)
Module 2 (n = 8)
“I found this module relevant to me” 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 5 (62.50%) 3 (37.50%)
“I learned something from this module” 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 1 (12.50%) 4 (50.00%) 3 (37.50%)
(table continues)
151
Level of Agreement
Module 3 (n = 8) SD D NAND A SA
“I found this module relevant to me” 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 6 (75.00%) 2 (25.00%)
“I learned something from this module” 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 6 (75.00%) 2 (25.00%)
Module 4 (n = 9)
“I found this module relevant to me” 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 4 (44.44%) 5 (55.56%)
“I learned something from this module” 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 3 (33.33%) 6 (66.67%)
(table continues)
152
Level of Agreement
Module 5 (n = 9) SD D NAND A SA
“I found this module relevant to me” 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 3 (33.33%) 2 (22.22%) 4 (44.44%)
“I learned something from this module” 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 5 (55.56%) 4 (44.44%)
Module 6 (n = 4)
“I found this module relevant to me” 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 1 (25.00%) 0 (0%) 3 (75.00%)
“I learned something from this module” 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 1 (25.00%) 1 (25.00%) 2 (50.00%)
SD: Strongly disagree; D: Disagree; NAND: Neither Disagree nor Disagree; A: Agree; SA: Strongly Agree
153
experience of the Self-Compassion Online program (“If there is any further feedback
you would like to provide about the program please enter it here”). Nineteen
experiences with the program, with only two participants reporting negative or neutral
feedback (“[n]ot too bad. Would prefer if the surveys were shorter” and “[i]t was ok,
but I didn't feel like I was getting anything from it.”). The primary issues with the
program involved the length of the meditations and surveys, and finding the content
their academic, professional, and personal lives (e.g., “I really enjoyed the program. It
was something to look forward to each week and I found it to be a source of support. I
found the topics covered to be extremely relevant not only to the experiences
associated with masters but other areas of my life like interpersonal issues, etc.”). In
particular, participants reported that the program provided them with a source of
support, and with techniques to reduce stress or distress during clinical training. In
Discussion
while the focus of the current intervention was on raising self-compassion levels, it
mediation analyses and the inclusion of a control group, these outcomes may be most
The increases in self-compassion observed in the current study are in line with
cultivation through training (see, e.g., Barnard & Curry, 2011b; Neff & Germer,
2013). Importantly, these results also support the proposition that changes in self-
compassion can remain robust over time. The increases in happiness observed
between pre- and post- test are in line with previous findings that self-compassion is
positively correlated with happiness (Hollis-Walker & Colosimo, 2011; Neff, Rude, et
al., 2007; Shapira & Mongrain, 2010; Smeets, Neff, Alberts, & Peters, 2014),
optimism and gratitude (Breen, Kashdan, Lenser, & Fincham, 2010). Finally, the
decreases on psychological distress outcomes support the evidence that increased self-
compassion is associated with lower levels of depression, anxiety, and stress (Barnard
test and 0.73 points at follow-up, with a large effect size indicated) in the current
155
study provide a point of comparison with other intervention studies that have also
with a large effect size indicated. These results can also be compared with
three studies on the effectiveness of MBSR (Birnie, Speca, & Carlson, 2010; Shapiro
et al., 2005; Shapiro et al., 2007) reported an average .54 point increase in self-
compassion, while three studies utilising MBCT as the intervention (Kuyken et al.,
2012; Lee & Bang, 2010; Rimes & Wingrove, 2011) reported an average .30 point
increase in self-compassion. Thus, while the current intervention was not as effective
effective. It should be noted that Neff and Germer’s (2013) MSC program was a face-
to-face intervention, whereas the current study is the first to consider whether a web-
with the 8-week format used by Neff and Germer (Neff & Germer, 2013).
Effect sizes for changes in other outcomes in the current study were
comparable with effect sizes reported by Neff and Germer (2013). For example,
following the SCO program effect sizes for changes in depression, anxiety, stress, and
perceived stress were moderate, small, large, and moderate, respectively. For the
MSC program, effect sizes for changes in depression, anxiety, and perceived stress
were large, moderate-large, and small, respectively. In addition, a moderate effect size
was observed for changes in happiness following the SCO program, while a small
156
effect size was observed for changes in happiness following the MSC program.
Finally, moderate-large effect sizes were observed in the current study for changes in
for the majority of the participants, which is a crucial consideration when designing
nonclinical population. The majority of respondents reported that they were able to
understand the modules, and that they found them relevant and enjoyable. In addition,
the majority of participants reported that they learned something from each module.
Participants’ responses indicated that overall, they were engaged with the program,
internet intervention research suggest that these attrition rates are well within
expected levels: treatment completion rates as low as 0.5% have been reported for
(Christensen, Griffiths, Korten, Brittliffe, & Groves, 2004) while completion rates of
1% have been reported for users of a web-based CBT intervention for panic delivered
in a scientific trial environment (e.g., Farvolden, Denisoff, Selby, Bagby, & Rudy,
2005). This dropout rate may also be compared with dropout from studies of face-to-
face stress prevention initiatives for health care professionals; a 44% attrition rate was
157
(2005), with participants citing time constraints as their primary reason for dropping
out.
current study indicated that the only difference between these groups was the level of
of perceived stress prior to starting the intervention. Although there was no follow-up
assessment conducted with participants who dropped out of the program, given the
difficulties reported with time constraints reported by participants who did remain in
the program, it may be surmised that the time commitment required by the program
was not feasible for those who dropped out. It is possible that for participants who are
prevention interventions may actually add to stress levels by placing further pressures
postgraduate training (for example, prior to commencing clinical work), when time
demands are less, and stress-prevention strategies can be more easily incorporated
participants who remained in the program reported a high degree of engagement and
compliance with the intervention. Given the difficulties with time constraints
described above, it may be argued that web-based intervention delivery may be more
remotely at any time (see, e.g., Wolever et al., 2012). Further research is required to
state whether the changes in psychological distress observed in the current study were
evidence from Study 1 it may be hypothesised that this represents at least one
mechanism underlying the observed changes. This finding can be placed in the
& Leary, 2010; Leary et al., 2007; Neff et al., 2005). Future research is required to
has been argued that positive emotions can mediate and moderate the stress response
at various points in the stress process (Folkman, 2008; Folkman & Moskowitz, 2000;
that experiencing positive emotions can lead to more rapid recovery from the
psychological resilience. While increases in happiness were observed over the course
of the intervention in the current study, again it is not possible to say whether these
and decreased psychological distress is the possibility that as trainees become more
self-compassionate they are less likely to be exposed to stressful events. For example,
it is possible that trainees with higher self-compassion are more inclined to protect
themselves from certain stressors as an act of self-care. Alternatively, trainees who are
compassionate mindset makes it less likely that stressors will pose a threat to one’s
psychologists who are high in self-compassion have a higher sense of coping self-
efficacy, and are therefore less likely to interpret daily events as stressful. To
elucidate these possibilities, further research into the qualitative distinctions in stress
responses and coping processes between high- and low- self-compassion individuals
is recommended.
The major implication of this research for individuals and training institutions
psychological distress and promoting wellbeing. Importantly, this study suggests that
and sustainable form of stress prevention and wellbeing promotion among the target
group.
There are a number of limitations in the current research that should be noted.
As this was an open trial with no control group, it cannot be concluded that changes in
160
the outcome measures were not due to uncontrolled variables such as the passage of
the possibility that there was some degree of specificity of the intervention; in
addition, participants’ reflections on the benefits they derive from the program
indicate that it was a meaningful intervention for them in terms of creating change in
their lives. It is also important to note that given that the intervention was self-guided
to-face studies (Hollon & Ponniah, 2010) – did not impact the results in the current
study.
A second limitation of the current study was the small sample size and self-
selection bias, which limits the generalizability of the findings. As participants self-
selected into the intervention, they may have been more motivated to complete it and
comply with it than the general population of psychology trainees. A larger sample
recommended to investigate how well psychology trainees typically engage with and
A final limitation in the current study concerns the rate of drop-out from the
program. While web-based self-help interventions tend to have higher attrition rates
information regarding which stage of the program the participants dropped out at, and
what factors lead to them dropping out of the program. Given that a number of
hypothesised that lack of time may be one of the primary reasons behind participants’
161
attrition. It is suggested that future revisions to the program aim to reduce the density
that future evaluations of the program include follow-up assessment with participants
who drop out, so that the reasons for attrition can be examined in greater detail.
completion of the post-test measures) and treatment adherence (i.e. completion of the
2011), it is recommended that future research into the effectiveness of the SCO
program include metrics such as number of logins, number of modules accessed, and
This study provides the first evidence that an online self-compassion training
psychologists. The results also provide evidence that changes in these outcomes can
Introduction
was developed. The second stage of Study 2 found evidence that the SCO program
was an effective and feasible way of increasing self-compassion and happiness and
of the current study was to gain further insight into the experience of trainee
psychologists and the relevance of the SCO program for this group. To identify
and their experiences undertaking the SCO program, data provided by participants in
Method
forms administered via the SCO website, data were collected from participants’ online
numbers of participants provided online responses to the selected exercises, with all
data provided for a particular exercise used in the analyses. In order to protect
participants’ privacy and anonymity during data collection, responses across exercises
Data analysis. Data in the current study were analysed using qualitative
content analysis techniques. It was hoped that this approach would provide a starting
point for the development of inductive hypotheses with which to guide future
163
research, as well as providing insight into appropriate revisions to the program. In the
current study, the units of analysis were participants’ written responses to exercises
delivered as part of the SCO program. These exercises involved open-ended questions
(e.g., “What was your experience with the Just Being exercise”), and a mixture of
open-ended and closed questions (e.g. “How did you find the Waiting on Yourself
Responses to each exercise were read through several times to obtain a sense
of the overall text, and they were then collated into a single text, which formed the
unit of analysis. Each text was then broken down into units of meaning that were
coded into categories and subcategories. Given the aims of the current study, the
generally low level of complexity of the units of meaning, and the limitations
presented by online data collection, the primary focus of the current analysis was on
the manifest content: these are the expressed, observable components of the text that
Amundson, 2002). The categories comprised content that was identified based on
some shared commonality (Krippendorff, 1980). While it has been argued that content
heterogenous (Patton, 1987), given the intertwined nature of human experiences, the
creation of completely distinct and uniform categories may not always be possible
(Graneheim & Lundman, 2004). Once the categories were formed, the original texts
were re-read to ensure that the categories adequately covered the data before themes
were generated.
Themes are recurring concepts across categories that can be linked together to
create meaning (Baxter, 1991; Polit & Hungler, 1999), and a theme can be seen as an
expression of the latent content of the text (Graneheim & Lundman, 2004). Following
164
themes were identified that were thought to reflect the underlying meaning of each
text. The categories and themes that relate to the major aspects of the program
Results
and thoughts of incompetence and lack of coping self-efficacy (e.g., “It's my fault, if
only I were more organised, more motivated, more competent. If I'm like this now,
how am I going to cope when things are tougher? (not well!)”. Participants reported a
sense of not fulfilling expectations of how they “should” be coping and the
detrimental impact this had on their sense of self-worth (e.g., “I should be able to do
On the other hand, a few participants reported actively coping with stress by
focusing on the task at hand, reminding themselves of times that they had coped with
difficult situations in the past, and practising mental toughness (e.g. “Suck it up, I just
withdrawn, physically ill, alone, sad, anxious, angry”. One participant noted that
feelings of stress didn’t necessarily preclude them from taking constructive action,
reporting that they felt “stressed and aroused but proactive enough to do something
about it”.
“time out to regroup,” while others reported neglecting their diet, exercise and sleep
avoiding people and withdrawing. One participant noted the difference in behaviour
between public and private places, reporting that they behaved “as professionally as I
can in public and then collapse into self-pity in private”. While some participants
reported a tendency to focus on the stress or task at hand, others described avoiding
people, feelings, sensations. By eating. By tuning out through reading self-help books,
looking online, watching tv. By ignoring the fact that there are people who care about
me and withdrawing socially and isolating myself. By completely stepping away from
the problem, telling myself it’s too hard or that there are no solutions.”). Many
participants’ responses indicated that they struggled to maintain balance in their lives
during times of stress, and that the need to “get things done” often eclipsed everything
166
else (e.g. “do what it takes to get the job done and do it well. Less sleep, poorer
balance in life. I have no time for anything/anybody that is not directly related to what
needs to be achieved”).
while others noted that their self-relation alternated from kind and caring to angry and
critical (e.g. “Sometimes with kindness and understanding – balancing the work with
however, reported that they treated themselves harshly, critically, and poorly in the
face of stress (e.g. “Terribly. I berate myself, I blame myself, I’m judgmental and
critical. I call myself names, I don’t give myself a break, I focus on all the things I
have done wrong or that are problematic and ignore the good things”). Participants
described relating to themselves aggressively, coldly and firmly, and one participant
noted that there was the assumption that “beating myself up will make me work harder
or not make mistakes”. Some participants responses indicated that while they were
enact this at times (e.g. “With some degree of compassion. Doesn’t seem to work most
of the time as I feel guilty for that; felt like I don’t deserve any compassion because I
responded to an exercise asking them to reflect on the concerns that they have about
their work as a therapist, and the challenges and stressors they face as part of their
a therapist, feeling fraudulent (e.g. “What if I’m not effective? What if I’m just not cut
out for this type of work?”, and feeling as though they “should” know what to do or
say but didn’t (e.g. “I sometimes feel lost in sessions but feel like I should know where
167
to others (e.g. “everyone else is a better therapist than me and always seems to know
for therapeutic outcomes and reflected the idea that they would be to blame if therapy
was not going “as planned” (e.g. “If this isn’t working there must be something I’m
also reflected general doubts about being able to help clients and of not knowing what
responses reflected difficulties working with particular “types” of clients and client
issues, as well as struggles with certain aspects of therapeutic process. For example,
participants reported finding working with chronic pain clients, clients with
personality disorders, children who have survived trauma, and clients with suicide or
about being liked by clients, and having difficulties with boundary-setting, refraining
from projecting onto or over-identifying with clients, and being assertive or directive
uncertainty and ambiguity, in terms of not always being able to predict the issues a
client might bring to a session and plan the session accordingly (e.g. “realising that I
can’t plan exactly what will happen in session and everything won’t always go to
plan”). Participants also reported difficulties working with other professionals, and
finding cases with complex legal issues stressful. Finally, one participant highlighted
168
the challenge of balancing time demands in the face of assignments, paperwork, and
clinical work.
participants responded to an exercise asking them about the nature of their self-talk
and their responses to it. Participants’ reflections on the nature of their self-talk raised
four major themes. The first theme related to awareness of self-talk: it appeared that
self-talk was generally something that participants were aware of, although the
self-talk as it was happening, while others reported becoming more aware of their
talk. Some participants described their awareness of self-talk being linked to their
stress levels, for example, “[w]hen under stress I am only aware of it after, but during
reported experiencing self-talk that was negative and critical, as well as self-talk that
was either neutral or positive. For some participants, self-talk was primarily negative:
incompetent, worthless, lazy, stupid, and annoying. Negative self-talk also appeared
to reflect exacting personal standards and involve global and stable negative
judgements of self. For example, one participant described their self-talk as follows:
“Generally critical - not good enough, haven't done enough, hopeless, idiot,
disorganised, never get anything done. Also very generalising, focusing on negatives
and ignoring positives, and setting up perfect standards for myself and not holding
169
others to those. Words that crop up subconsciously are: bad daughter, failure in job,
contributed to stress levels and led to them feeling depressed, sad, anxious, afraid and
For other participants, positive self-talk was more dominant: within the
positive self-talk described were ideas of feeling compassion and kindness for oneself,
and reassuring. For example, one participant described their self-talk as “caring and
encouraging of rest and kindness to self”. The nature of self-talk was linked to mood
and stress levels: for example, one participant identified “when I'm feeling stressed or
get something wrong then my self-talk is quite negative-I'm never going to be able to
do it, I'm no good at this, I'm going to be a terrible psychologist, I don't know what to
do. When I'm in a good mood however I am quite encouraging and compassionate
reassuring, although one participant noted that they had “trouble accepting my
soothing self-talk”.
self-soothing or reassurance, and was used to relieve anxiety or deal with feelings of
being overwhelmed. For example, one participant described giving themselves “"I
can do it" messages in the face of feeling stressed and overwhelmed”. On the other
hand, negative self-talk was described as being motivating in a harsh and critical
170
manner, for example: “I talk down to myself regularly, I tell myself when I'm "being a
doofus" and use a firm voice when trying to encourage or motivate myself. The
positive self-talk that I have is usually more situational-based whereas the more
critical self-talk is personal and global. The criticism I use is not too harsh though.”
The final major theme related to participants’ capacity to change the nature of
their self-talk. Participants reported a difference between automatic self-talk and self-
talk that was constructed or deliberate. Furthermore, participants noted that paying
attention to self-talk (such as in the self-talk exercise) helped them to change the way
that they were talking to themselves so that they were less critical and more
reassuring. For example “[m]y self-talk varied a lot from quite critical to more
supportive, probably because starting this course made me more aware” and “[s]elf-
talk was primarily encouraging and re-assuring... although it used to be quite critical
I think.... noticing the self-talk has helped me to change the way I talk to myself”.
Seventeen participants provided responses to the “Just Being” exercise, which asks
participants what it would be like for them to reduce goal-oriented activity or striving
in their lives and cultivate present-moment awareness. One of the primary themes that
emerged in participants’ responses to this question was that that “just being” was
theoretically a nice idea, but difficult to put into practice. Participants described a
represented “happiness and freedom”. At the same time, “just being” was seen to
represent laziness or lack of achievement and was associated with negative feelings
such as guilt and anxiety. For example, one participant described “just being” as a
state that “[i]ndicates laziness. Brings up feelings of guilt, anxiety at the thought of
not achieving something from a list, wasting time, not "improving myself" or getting
171
wholeness. But also in some ways a sense of being unproductive and useless.”
achieving, and that there was a quite a high degree of pressure to be “doing” things.
As a result, this exercise elicited stress around “not getting things done”. Participants
identified that the pressure to achieve was linked to others expectations, as well as
their own “perfectionistic voice”. They reported that the “just being” exercise brought
up thoughts of not being good enough and of not trusting themselves to do well unless
they push themselves hard. One participant also reported that they felt critical of
training could be effectively translated into an online environment, and what (if any)
completely self-guided manner. In order to gain insight into these issues, participants’
meditation, reporting that it gave them a sense of calm, clarity, and relaxation.
object (i.e. thinking of someone who evokes feelings of love and happiness),
172
extending loving-kindness to oneself, staying awake, and accepting the time-frame for
meditation as it progressed: “Initially I was irritated that it would take 20 minutes and
looked for other tasks that I could "do" simultaneously. As I began to focus upon the
meditation I found a glow emerging from within me that radiated out beyond me”.
an exercise asking them to reflect on the key themes of Module Four, which focused
on cultivating awareness of common humanity. One of the major themes that arose
from this reflection was the degree to which participants struggle with being
developing a greater degree of trust with others. It was noted that processes of social
comparison can restrict the degree to which one feels comfortable being vulnerable
with others, and that vulnerability entails being open about one’s joys and successes
become more self-compassionate could become more about being “good at” self-
compassion, rather than true self-acceptance: “As I go through these exercises I like to
think that "I'm fine" - I'm happy with myself, compassionate towards myself,
connected with other people but I think that is my barrier. I want myself to be these
Another key theme here was the development of authenticity, and the
importance of acting in a way that is true to oneself, rather than acting to please others
and seek approval. Participants’ responses reflected the idea that having an inherent
sense of self-worth or of “being enough” was crucial to being able to behave more
authentically: “I need to remind myself that "I am enough" and just be me! The
173
benefits of this would be that I would be more authentic and true to myself and stop
trying to work towards being what I think everyone wants me to be and I should be”.
This sense of self-worth was described as something that was quite difficult to
cultivate, despite recognition of its value: “I love the idea of believing I'm worthy, but
it seems as realistic as waiting for water to flow uphill”. Participants noted the
relevance of being authentic in their work as therapists, reporting that it would allow
them to connect more with others, enhance their empathy, and help them model self-
responded to an exercise that asked them to reflect on what relevance (if any) self-
compassion had for their professional lives. Content analysis of these responses
be challenging and their expectations of their own performance should reflect this:
“there is so much to learn in the helping field and as a student I am just starting out
so I should just expect myself to be doing my best and therefore not need to do
heightened self-acceptance: “to learn to accept myself for who I am even though I feel
like a failure sometimes...”. Participants identified that recognising their own limits
and not seeing this as a weakness, and recognising the common humanity in their
internal processes were important to being able to limit self-criticism and reduce
professional lives in terms of reducing stress and enhancing wellbeing: “I think that
self-criticism and shame only add secondary stress to original or “pure” stress or
pain. I could practice identifying self-critical and other unhelpful thoughts when they
arise (attending mindfully to the body as an indicator of this stress), then practising
with self-care practices such as seeking supervision, taking time out, and practising
Finally, participants conveyed the importance of trusting in their own coping efficacy:
will say and do and trust in the learning I have done over the previous 6 years that I
Discussion
The aim of the current study was to gain further insight into the experience of
trainee psychologists, in terms of the ways in which they appraise and cope with
stress, and the ways in which they respond to self-compassion training. Text
Online program open trial (Study 3) were subject to content analysis. The results
provide support for the proposition that self-compassion training is a relevant and
engaging intervention for psychology trainees, and suggest some interesting directions
for future research, as well as some pertinent revisions to the SCO program.
described in the research on stressors associated with training in the health professions
(e.g., Orlinsky & Rønnestad, 2005; Skovholt & Rønnestad, 2003; Skovholt & Trotter-
Mathison, 2011). For example, participants reported concerns about their professional
175
knowledge and competence, worries about whether they would know what to say or
do with clients, and fears of being inferior to other trainees, of being found out as
fraudulent, and of not being “cut out” for work as a psychologist. Additionally,
the literature, namely: dealing with ambiguity, working with clients with chronic
difficulties and personality disorders, negotiating boundaries, and dealing with cases
with complex legal and ethical issues (Barnett et al., 2007; Farber, 1983; Farber &
Heifetz, 1981; Kottler, 2003). These findings support the proposition that there are a
occurring concerns among trainees that may exacerbate the experience of work-
related stress.
The findings regarding participants’ responses to their self-talk are in line with
cognitive theorists who have proposed that the tone and content of internal dialogue
Participants reported the capacity to engage in both positive and negative self-talk,
described by Meichenbaum and Butler (1980). Given that this form of self-talk has
the capacity to negatively affect performance (Hiebert et al., 1998; Meichenbaum &
Butler, 1980), it has been argued that teaching trainee therapists to manage negative
1996). Indeed, one of the goals of the SCO program is to help participants become
more aware of their self-talk and reframe negative and critical self-statements in a
research would be to look in greater detail at the impact that the SCO program has on
176
performance.
reported difficulty with enacting processes such as being in the moment (without
the potential benefits of these processes. This finding is in line with research that
McEwan, Matos, & Rivis, 2011). This suggests that a useful revision to the SCO
program may be to directly address these fears, either through psychoeducation or the
use of behavioural experiments. Given that the SCO program is completely self-
experiences so that participants are less likely to criticize themselves for struggling to
program for their professional lives supported the findings of Study 3 that the SCO
among this group. Participants reported that increased awareness and acceptance of
their own internal processes, as well as of the nature of their profession and the
challenges involved in clinical training were of benefit in helping them to be less self-
critical and feel less isolated and stressed. In addition, participants reported increased
sustaining resilience and wellbeing over time. This is important as it suggests that
there is some degree of specificity for the effects of the SCO program, which cannot
While the current results provide some interesting insights into the experiences
of the participants in the SCO program, and highlight some valuable avenues for
future research, there were a number of factors that may impact the credibility of the
current findings. In qualitative terms, credibility refers to how well the selected data
and the analytic processes address the research question (Polit & Hungler, 1999). It
should be emphasized that in the current study, participants were those who (a) self-
elected to participate in the SCO program, and (b) responded to the various exercises
contained within the program. These participants likely had experiences that gave
them a vested interest in the topics of inquiry, and therefore should not be seen as
into some of the issues raised by the current research may wish to select participants
with a range of experiences in relation to the area of interest, thereby illuminating the
research question from a variety of angles (Adler & Adler, 1988; Patton, 1987).
It should also be noted that responses were provided online, meaning that it
was not possible to clarify or expand on responses, or to interpret them in the light of
contextual clues. In addition, the data were interpreted by a single researcher, and
therefore the findings may reflect a degree of subjectivity. Future research into the
there may be many meanings implicated by a single text, and some degree of
Despite these limitations, this study provides further evidence that self-
psychologists that has the potential to impact the ways in which they relate to
stressors. Revisions to the SCO program in line with the recommendations here
effectiveness.
179
CHAPTER 6
General Discussion
Introduction
The overall aim of the current research was to investigate the relevance of the
among trainee and professional psychologists. Specific goals of the research were to
(a) clarify the nature of the relationship between self-compassion and psychological
compassion training program; and (d) evaluate the relevance, feasibility and
wellbeing among postgraduate psychology trainees. This chapter will summarise the
main findings of the two studies that comprise this thesis and discuss the implications
Major Findings
The results of Study 1 indicated that after controlling for age and neuroticism, self-
compassion has a clear link to depression and stress in psychologists across the career
Using a preliminary, open-trial design, this study found evidence that, among
those who completed it, the intervention was effective in increasing self-compassion
and happiness, and reducing emotion regulation difficulties, perceived stress, and
symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress. In addition, this study found evidence
that these changes could be maintained over time. Attrition rates between pre- and
post-test were within expected ranges for an online intervention, and were comparable
professionals. Further, the majority of the participants in this study reported finding
the program relevant and enjoyable, and stated that they could understand and
complete the program content, and that they learned something from the program.
Overall, participants described the benefits of the program in terms of increased self-
acceptance and self-kindness, higher levels of efficacy in managing stress, and greater
own limitations as trainees, who are still learning how to negotiate professional
challenges. The main difficulty with the program reported by participants was finding
181
written responses to the SCO program exercises. The findings support the proposition
that while there are a number of stressors that commonly arise in the course of one’s
and fears of incompetence and inadequacy play an integral role in heightening the
value of these processes for their personal and professional lives. In addition,
participants reported that changing the nature and tone of their self-relation was
acceptable and relevant intervention for trainee psychologists that has the potential to
stress, and the relevance of self-compassion for psychologists across the career span.
While this model needs to be replicated in different populations and with a wider
array of outcomes, in the context of previous findings, Study 1 provides evidence that
one of the primary pathways by which self-compassion impacts depression and stress
is by reducing difficulties with emotion regulation. Given that the evidence regarding
potentially represents a robust and parsimonious explanatory model for the role of
Study 2 also extends the extant literature in a number of ways. At the time of
that aims to increase self-compassion among its participants. Importantly, this study
also presents a protocol for a web-based self-compassion intervention that takes into
account relevant findings from the internet intervention literature. This provides
compassion training program for increasing wellbeing and reducing distress among
interventions are thought to be uniquely suited. Study 3 provides evidence that self-
symptom severity within this group at pre-test, this study also provides some
populations.
this is one of the first studies to have examined the acceptability and utility of using a
web-based program to deliver a MAB intervention. The current results suggest that
web-based programs are a feasible and effective mode of intervention delivery for
interventions, given that they offer a greater degree of flexibility and accessibility to a
group who commonly report significant limitations on their time. In addition, trainee
that they may explore their anxieties and concerns without fear of being judged and
ashamed.
psychological wellbeing and reducing psychological distress. Given that research into
training. Due to insufficient sample size, this study did not examine whether these
feelings and thoughts, rather than avoiding them or becoming entrenched in them
(Neff, 2003b). In addition, there is some evidence that the relationship between self-
psychological distress and promote wellbeing is by changing the way that people
185
appraise stressors. There is evidence that self-compassionate people are more likely to
think about negative events in ways that are adaptive: they are more objective, less
likely to catastrophize and judge themselves harshly, and more able to see difficult
experiences as a normal part of life (Allen & Leary, 2010; Leary et al., 2007).
relevant events (Leary et al., 2007), as it promotes a sense of positive self-worth that
psychosocial and physical resources to cope with the stressor. It is plausible that self-
compassion impacts both primary and secondary appraisal processes. In line with
theorists who highlight how stress outcomes are influenced by how one construes
oneself, it may be inferred that due to the enhanced sense of self-acceptance and more
likely to be viewed as threatening to worth and identity. On the other hand, it is likely
that individuals who are low in self-compassion interpret negative events in ways that
are personal, global, and enduring, thereby making them more likely to make primary
provides a framework for reinterpreting and coping with events in ways that are self-
an effective strategy for coping with depressed mood (Diedrich, Grant, Hofmann,
Hiller, & Berking, 2014), and that self-compassion is linked with the ability to re-
186
Neely et al., 2009; Shepherd & Cardon, 2009). Second, there is preliminary evidence
optimism (Neff, Rude, et al., 2007): as such, it may be hypothesised that self-
compassionate individuals are more likely to feel able to cope with certain stressors
because they appraise them as temporary, specific, and external (Chang, 1998).
understanding that difficulties and setbacks were a normal part of clinical training,
for stressful events (e.g. being mindful of their responses, reducing self-criticism and
being kinder to themselves, and reminding themselves that others may also feel this
way). These findings suggest a number of avenues for future research in terms of
investigating the way trait self-compassion impacts the nature and outcome of stress
appraisals, and also in terms of further examination of how the SCO program might
build theory (Fredrickson, 1998, 2003), which posits that increasing one’s positive
positive emotions, which, in turn, are thought to buffer against and undo the negative
range of cognitive processes and enhancing behavioural flexibility, which over time
Colosimo, 2011; Neff, Rude, et al., 2007) and experimental manipulations (Leary et
al., 2007) and interventions (e.g., Neff & Germer, 2013; Shapira & Mongrain, 2010).
Authentic happiness reflects not only the experience of positive emotions, but also
the ability to make meaning out of one’s stressful experiences is argued to enhance
resilience in the face of different stressors (Park & Folkman, 1997). In this way, these
broaden-and-build theory.
buffering role of positive emotions across the stress process (Folkman, 2008;
Folkman & Moskowitz, 2000), it may be surmised that the sense of eudemonic
different levels. For example, the positive emotions that accompany an increased
sense of self-compassion may impact the way that individuals appraise stress; there is
evidence that level of positive affect impacts the way that individuals interpret
188
particular events (Ashkanasy et al., 2004; Lazarus et al., 1980). In addition, the
copes with the event: the valence of affect during a stressful encounter has been found
meaning-focused coping (see, e.g., Park & Folkman, 1997) via this mechanism,
outcomes.
Chapter 1, there is a notable need for prevention initiatives that protect psychologists
and other health care professionals from stress, distress, and burnout. Such initiatives
may be particularly relevant for clinicians in training (Shapiro & Carlson, 2009), and
self-care (Barnett et al., 2007; Grepmair et al., 2007; Shapiro & Carlson, 2009).
Extending these findings and suggestions, the current research suggests that self-
distress and wellbeing among psychology trainees. In addition, the current findings
provide some evidence that self-compassion training is acceptable and effective when
These findings are important, given that researchers in the field emphasise the
development of stress prevention programs that are positive - i.e., they promote
can be maintained over time (Lawson & Myers, 2011; Wise et al., 2012). In addition,
the current results suggest that the SCO program is an accessible and cost-effective
intervention, which is relevant given that trainee psychologists tend to report limited
time and resources as barriers to accessing stress prevention and treatment solutions
(Shapiro & Carlson, 2009). Overall, the findings of Study 2 support revisions to the
SCO program in terms of reducing the length and complexity of the program,
normalising common experiences within the program, and making the program more
modules. Given the attrition rate in Study 2, it may be useful to conduct further
research into the types of intervention delivery that psychology trainees find most
acceptable, and to use this feedback to inform decisions about future revisions of the
“clinical training as usual” condition with a “clinical training plus SCO participation”
would provide insight into the influence of clinical experience and learning gained in
postgraduate training, and allow these variables to be controlled when examining the
impact of the SCO program. A further step would be to test the effectiveness of the
SCO program relative to other interventions that are supported for reducing stress
(MBSR; e.g., Newsome, Waldo, & Gruszka, 2012; Shapiro et al., 2007) and
With further research, there is a rationale for including the SCO program as
avenue of inquiry is supported by the evidence presented in the current research that
addition, given that emotion regulation plays an integral role in professional stress and
job satisfaction (Rosen, Halbesleben, & Perrewé, 2013), it may be hypothesised that
and robust negative predictor of psychological distress outcomes, and support the
distress over time. While the target population in the current research was a non-
clinical one, clinical significance analyses in Stage 2 of Study 2 revealed that some
and stress at baseline. After participating in the SCO program, a number of these
outcomes. This suggests that there is some potential for the SCO program as a
should be noted that this hypothesis is extremely tentative, given that these results are
from the first trial of the SCO program, which was uncontrolled, therefore limiting the
191
extent to which changes can be attributed to the impact of the program and not some
2 reported worsening symptoms of anxiety between pre- and post- test. It is possible
populations. Preliminary research indicates that some individuals may harbour fears
of self-compassion (Gilbert et al., 2011), which is a factor that should be taken into
for.
With this caveat in place, it is recognised that given the key predictive role of
recommended. Such research would provide further insights into the relevance of the
SCO program for clinical populations. As the use of different emotional regulation
psychopathology (Aldao et al., 2010), specific investigation into the way self-
research would provide insight into the question of whether self-compassion is always
For promoting wellbeing. It has been acknowledged that there is a high level
192
of demand for psychological interventions for individuals who are not clinically
unwell but wish to increase their wellbeing (Wood & Tarrier, 2010). The current
outcomes such as happiness, optimism and gratitude (Neff, Rude, et al., 2007). On
this point, it is important to note the distinction between hedonic wellbeing (also
known as subjective wellbeing) and eudemonic wellbeing, which has also been
referred to as psychological wellbeing (Ryan & Deci, 2001). Hedonic wellbeing refers
life, and reflects a combination of high positive affect, low negative affect, and high
satisfaction with life (Diener, 1984). On the other hand, eudemonic wellbeing focuses
on how individuals interact with the world in meaningful ways, and reflects constructs
environmental mastery, and personal growth (Ryff, 1989; Ryff & Keyes, 1995).
in enduring ways (Neff & Germer, 2013; Shapira & Mongrain, 2010). This is
important not only for its potential to promote desired positive outcomes in non-
clinical populations, but also because positive functioning plays a key role in
Fredrickson et al., 2000; Wood & Tarrier, 2010). Given that deficits in eudemonic
wellbeing represent a robust risk factor for distress (Wood & Joseph, 2010), these
results provide further support for the utility of the SCO program in promoting
that the “person” of the therapist – in terms of their personal characteristics and
for both therapist and client (e.g., Andrews, 2000; Baldwin, 2000; Crits-Christoph &
Mintz, 1991; Lambert & Barley, 2001; Luborsky, McLellan, Digner, Woody, &
Seligman, 1997; Wampold & Brown, 2005).In particular, it has be found that
common factors such as therapeutic alliance and therapist empathy reliably influence
therapeutic outcomes (e.g., Norcross & Wampold, 2011). Given that self-compassion
is associated with compassion for others (Neff & Pommier, 2012), and techniques
health professionals for their capacity to promote empathy (e.g., Boellinghaus et al.,
2014; Kristeller & Johnson, 2005; Neff & Germer, 2013), it follows that increasing
(Pieterse, Lee, Ritmeester, & Collins, 2013) and ethical decision-making (Mattison,
2000), and meta-cognitive skills such as mindfulness have been identified as a vital
target in counselor training (Fauth, Gates, Vinca, Boles, & Hayes, 2007). In a study of
4,000 therapists, Orlinsky, Botermans, and Rønnestad (2001) found that therapists
rated “getting personal therapy” as the second most salient positive influence on their
professional development. The authors argued that getting personal therapy should be
enhancing awareness of one’s impact on others, and recognizing and accepting one’s
The results of Stage 3 of Study 2 provide some support for the notion that
undertaking the SCO program provided therapeutic benefits for participants along the
dimensions described by Orlinsky et al. (2001), and positively impacted on the way
they worked with clients. Participants reported increased acceptance of the inherent
reactive to certain clients and issues that may have previously represented acute
communicating more openly and authentically, and described the relevance of this for
their interactions with clients. These descriptions of the relevance of the SCO
determine how such changes may affect therapeutic processes and outcomes.
SCO program increases levels of empathy and compassion for others. Increasingly,
these outcomes are being recognised as a key target in training programs for health
195
al., 2014; Ely, 2012; Krasner et al., 2009). In addition to discerning whether this
and the lack of a control group in Stage 2 of Study 2, the results of the current
distress outcomes, nor can the changes on the outcomes observed in Stage 2 of Study
2 be reliably linked to the impact of the SCO program. In order to address the
question of causation, future research would benefit from the use of a controlled
time. Comparing the SCO intervention to a no-treatment control is relevant for seeing
the utility of this program would be to compare its effectiveness to more established
aims of the current research was to consider self-compassion in the context of the
2011; Kopp et al., 2010; Rosenberg & Pace, 2006), they should not be seen as
measures was deemed appropriate given the heterogeneity of our samples (in terms of
the types and duration of stressors experienced), future research in this area may wish
such as burnout.
provides some of the first insights into emotion regulation as a fundamental working
to address in Study 3 was the failure to evaluate the role of changes in emotion
change process in acceptance- and mindfulness-based therapies (e.g., Gratz & Tull,
throughout the course of the intervention to determine whether these changes precede
evidence supporting the buffering role of positive emotions in the stress process
Finally, in the light of evidence that some individuals experience fears of self-
197
In the past two decades there has been increasing recognition that occupational
stress for psychologists presents unique challenges, and can have serious and far-
reaching consequences for psychologists themselves, their clients, and the profession
as a whole. Research into constructs and interventions that have the potential to
health care paradigm that sees the wellness of clinicians as an essential part of care
provision. Overall, the results of the current research suggest that self-compassion
mechanisms of change.
198
References
Ackerley, G. D., Burnell, J., Holder, D. C., & Kurdek, L. A. (1988). Burnout among
licensed psychologists. Professional Psychology, 19, 624-631. doi:
10.1037/0735-7028.19.6.624
Adams, C. E., & Leary, M. R. (2007). Promoting self-compassionate attitudes toward
eating among restrictive and guilty eaters. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 26, 1120-1144. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2007.26.10.1120
Adams, S. A., & Riggs, S. A. (2008). An exploratory study of vicarious trauma
among therapist trainees. Training and Education in Professional Psychology,
2, 26-34. doi: 10.1037/1931-3918.2.1.26
Adamson, C., Beddoe, L., & Davys, A. (2014). Building resilient practitioners:
Definitions and practitioner understandings. British Journal of Social Work,
44, 522-541. doi: 10.1093/bjsw/bcs142
Adler, P. A., & Adler, P. (1988). Observational techniques. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S.
Lincoln (Eds.), Collecting and interpreting qualitative materials (pp. 79-109).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Inc.
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation
strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical
Psychology Review, 30, 217-237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004
Aldwin, C. M., & Reveson, T. A. (1987). Does coping help? A reexamination of the
relation between coping and mental health. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 53, 337-348.
Allan, S., & Gilbert, P. (1995). A social comparison scale: Psychometric properties
and relationship to psychopathology. Personality and Individual Differences,
19(3), 293-299. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(95)00086-L
Allen, A. B., & Leary, M. R. (2010). Self-compassion, stress, and coping. Social and
Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 107-118. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-
9004.2009.00246.x
Allen, T. W. (1967). Effectiveness of counselor trainees as a function of
psychological openness. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 14, 35-40. doi:
10.1037/h0024221
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (5th Ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Association.
American Psychological Association Advisory Committee on Colleague Assistance.
(n.d.). The stress-distress-impairment continuum for psychologists. from
http://www.apapracticecentral.org/ce/self-care/colleague-assist.aspx
Amirkhan, J. H., & Greaves, H. (2003). Sense of coherence and stress: The mechanics
of a healthy disposition. Psychology and Health, 18, 31-62. doi:
10.1080/0887044021000044233
Anderson, L., Lewis, G., Araya, R., Rodney, E., Harrison, G., Proudfoot, J. G., . . .
Williams, C. (2005). Self-help books for depression: how can practitioners and
patients make the right choice? British Journal of General Practice, 55(514),
387-392.
Andrews, H. (2000). The myth of the scientist-practitioner: A reply to R King (1998)
and N. King and Ollendick (1998). Australian Psychologist, 35, 60-63.
Anton, W. D., & Reed, J. R. (1994). Employee assistance programme inventory:
professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
199
Barnett, J. E., Baker, E. K., Elman, N. S., & Schoener, G. R. (2007). In pursuit of
wellness: The self-care imperative. Professional Psychology: Research and
Practice, 38, 603-612. doi: 10.1037/0735-7028.38.6.603
Barnett, J. E., & Cooper, N. (2009). Creating a culture of self-care. Clinical
Psychology: Science and Practice, 16, 16-20. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-
2850.2009.01138.x
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction is
social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical
considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173
Barrett, P. (2007). Structural equation modelling: Adjudging model fit. Personality
and Individual Differences, 42, 815-824. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.018
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism
to violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological
Review, 103, 5-33. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.103.1.5
Baxter, L. A. (1991). Content analysis. In B. M. Montgomery & S. Duck (Eds.), Study
interpersonal interaction (pp. 239-254). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of
depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Beddoe, A. E., & Murphy, S. O. (2004). Does mindfulness decrease stress and foster
empathy among nursing students? Journal of Nursing Education, 43, 305-312.
Beehr, T. A. (1995). Psychological stress in the workplace. London: Routledge.
Bennett-Goleman, T. (2001). Emotional alchemy: How the mind can heal the heart.
New York: Three Rivers Press.
Bennett-Levy, J. (2006). Therapist skills: a cognitive model of their acquisition and
refinement. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 34, 57-78. doi:
10.1017/S1352465805002420
Berscheid, E., & Reis, H. T. (1998). Attraction and close relationships. In D. T.
Gilbert, S. T. Fiske & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology
(4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 193-281). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Birnie, K., Speca, M., & Carlson, L. E. (2010). Exploring self-compassion and
empathy in the context of Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR).
Stress and Health, 26, 359-371. doi: 10.1002/smi.1305
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., .
. . Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical
Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 230-241. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bph077
Blatt, S. J., & Zuroff, D. C. (1992). Interpersonal relatedness and self-definition: Two
prototypes for depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 527-562. doi:
10.1016/0272-7358(92)90070-O
Boellinghaus, I., Jones, F. W., & Hutton, J. (2013). Cultivating self-care and
compassion in psychological therapists in training: The experience of
practicing loving-kindness meditation. Training and Education in
Professional Psychology, 7, 267-277. doi: 10.1037/a0033092
Boellinghaus, I., Jones, F. W., & Hutton, J. (2014). The role of mindfulness and
loving-kindness meditation in cultivating self-compassion and other-focused
concern in health care professionals. Mindfulness, 5, 129-138. doi:
10.1007/s12671-012-0158-6
Bolger, N., & Eckenrode, J. (1991). Social relationships, personality, and anxiety
during a major stressful event. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
61, 440-449. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.61.3.440
201
Bolger, N., & Schilling, E. A. (1991). Personality and the problems of everyday life:
The role of neuroticism in exposure and reactivity to daily stressors. Journal
of Personality, 59, 355-386. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1991.tb00253.x
Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. (2003). Relationship of core self-evaluations traits - self-
esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability -
with job satisfaction and job performance: A meta-analysis. European Journal
of Personality, 17, s5-s18. doi: 10.1002/per.481
Bowers, K. S. (1973). Situationism in psychology: An analysis and a critique.
Psychological Review, 80, 307-336. doi: 10.1037/h0035592
Breen, W. E., Kashdan, T. B., Lenser, M. L., & Fincham, F. D. (2010). Gratitude and
forgiveness: Convergence and divergence on self-report and informant ratings.
Personality and Individual Differences, 49(8), 932-937. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.033
Breines, J. G., Thoma, M. V., Gianferante, D., Hanlin, L., & Chen, X. (2014). Self-
compassion as a predictor of interleukin-6 response to acute psychosocial
stress. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 37, 109-114. doi:
10.1016/j.bbi.2013.11.006
Brewin, C. R. (2003). Post-traumatic Stress Disorder: Malady or Myth? New Haven,
NY: Yale University Press.
Bride, B. E., Radey, M., & Figley, C. R. (2007). Measuring compassion fatigue.
Clinical Social Work Journal, 35, 155-163. doi: 10.1007/s10615-007-0091-7
Brinthaupt, T. M., Hein, M. B., & Kramer, T. E. (2009). The Self-Talk Scale:
Development, factor analysis, and validation. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 91, 82-92. doi: 10.1080/00223890802484498
Brockner, J. (1988). Self-esteem at work. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Brooks, J., Holttum, S., & Lavender, T. (2002). Personality style, psychological
adaptation and expectations of trainee clinical psychologists. Clinical
Psychology & Psychotherapy, 9, 253-270. doi: 10.1002/cpp.318
Brown, B. (1999). Soul without shame: A guide to liberating yourself from the judge
within. Boston, MA: Shambhala Publications.
Brown, G. W., Bifulco, A., & Harris, C. R. (1987). Life events, vulnerability and
onset of depression: Some refinements. The British Journal of Psychiatry,
150, 30-42. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.1.30
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and
its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84, 822-848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
Browne, M. W. (1984). Asymptotically distribution-free methods for the analysis of
covariance structures. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical
Psychology, 37, 62-83. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8317.1984.tb00789.x
Burns, J. M., Davenport, T. A., Durkin, L. A., Luscombe, G. M., & Hickie, I. B.
(2010). The internet as a setting for mental health service utilisation by young
people. Medical Journal of Australia, 192, s22-s26.
Butler, S. K., & Constantine, M. G. (2005). Collective self-esteem and burnout in
professional school counselors. Professional School Counseling, 9, 55-62.
Campbell-Sills, L., & Barlow, D. H. (2007). Incorporating emotion regulation into
conceptualizations and treatments of anxiety and mood disorders. In J. J.
Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 542-559). New York:
Guilford Press.
202
Evans, J., Heron, J., Lewis, G., Araya, R., & Wolke, D. (2005). Negative self-
schemas and the onset of depression in women: Longitudinal study. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 302-307. doi: 10.1192/bjp.186.4.302
Evans, S., Ferrando, S., Findler, M., Stowell, C., Smart, C., & Haglin, D. (2008).
Mindfulness-based congitive therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. Journal
of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 716-721. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.07.005
Eysenbach, G. (2011). CONSORT-EHEALTH: Improving and standardizing
evaluation reports of web-based and mobile health interventions. Journal of
Medical Internet Research, 13, e126. doi: 10.2196/jmir.1923
Farber, B. A. (1983). Dysfunctional aspects of the psychotherapeutic role. In B. A.
Farber (Ed.), Stress and burnout in the human services professions (pp. 97-
118). New York, NY: Pergamon.
Farber, B. A. (1990). Burnout in psychotherapists: Incidence, types, and trends.
Psychotherapy in Private Practice, 28, 5-13. doi: 10.1300/J294v08n01_07
Farber, B. A., & Heifetz, L. J. (1981). The satisfactions and stress of
psychotherapeutic work: A factor analytic study. Professional Psychology, 12,
621-630. doi: 10.1037//0735-7028.12.5.621
Farvolden, P., Denisoff, E., Selby, P., Bagby, R. M., & Rudy, L. (2005). Usage and
longitudinal effectiveness of a Web-based self-help cognitive behavioral
therapy program for panic disorder. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 26,
e7. doi: 10.2196/jmir.7.1.e7
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible
statistical power analyis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical
sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175-191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146
Fauth, J., Gates, S., Vinca, M., Boles, S., & Hayes, J. A. (2007). Big ideas for
psychotherapy training. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training,
44, 384-391. doi: 10.1037/0033-3204.44.4.384
Feldman, G., Hayes, A. M., Kumar, S. M., Greeson, J., & Laurenceau, J.-P. (2007).
Mindfulness and emotion regulation: the development and initial validation of
the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale - Revised (CAMS-R). Journal
of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 29, 177-190. doi:
10.1007/s10862-006-9035-8
Figley, C. R. (2002a). Compassion fatigue: Psychotherapists' chronic lack of self care.
Psychotherapy in Practice, 58, 1433-1441. doi: 10.1002/jclp.10090
Figley, C. R. (2002b). Treating compassion fatigue. New York, NY: Brunner-
Routledge.
Folkman, S. (2008). The case for positive emotions in the stress process. Anxiety,
Stress & Coping, 21, 3-14. doi: 10.1080/10615800701740457
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1986). Stress process and depressive symptomatology.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 107-113. doi: 10.1037//0021-
843X.95.2.107
Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Stress, positive emotion, and coping.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 115-118. doi: 10.1111/1467-
8721.00073
Fourth National Mental Health Care Plan Working Group. (2009). Fourth National
Mental Health Plan: an agenda for collaborative government action in mental
health 2009-2014. from
http://www.health.gov.au/internet/main/publishing.nsf/Content/mental-pubs-f-
plan09
207
Harrison, E. (2006). How to meditate. Part 1: The Basic Kit. Subiaco, Western
Australia: Perth Meditation Centre.
Harrison, R. L., & Westwood, M. J. (2009). Preventing vicarious traumatization of
mental health therapists: Identifying protective practices. Psychotherapy
Research, Theory, Practice, Training, 46, 203-219. doi: 10.1037/a0016081
Hart, P. M., & Cooper, C. L. (2001). Occupational stress: Toward a more integrated
framework. In N. D. Anderson, D. S. Ones, H. K. Sinangil & C. Viswesvaran
(Eds.), Handbook of industrial, work and organizational psychology (Vol. 2:
Personnel Psychology). London, UK: Sage.
Hayes, A. M., & Feldman, G. (2004). Clarifying the construct of mindfulness in the
context of emotion regulation and the process of change in therapy. Clinical
Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 255-262. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bph080
Hayes, S. C., Follette, V. M., & Linehan, M. N. (2004). Mindfulness and acceptance:
Expanding the cognitive behavioral tradition. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and commitment
therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York, NY:
Guilford.
Heffernan, M., Quinn Griffin, M. T., McNulty, R., & Fitzpatrick, J. J. (2010). Self-
compassion and emotional intelligence in nurses. International Journal of
Nursing Practice, 16, 366-373. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-172X.2010.01853.x
Heim, E. (1991). Job stressors and coping in health professions. Psychotherapy and
Psychosomatics, 55, 2-4. doi: 10.1159/000288414
Hellman, I. D., Morrison, T. L., & Abramowitz, S. I. (1987). Therapist experience and
the stresses of psychotherapeutic work. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research,
Practice, Training, 24, 171-177. doi: 10.1037/h0085701
Henning, K., Ey, S., & Shaw, D. (1998). Perfectionism, the imposter phenomenon
and psychological adjustment in medical, dental, nursing and pharmacy
students. Medical Education, 32, 456-464. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-
2923.1998.00234.x
Hiebert, B., Uhlemann, M. R., Marshall, A., & Lee, D. Y. (1998). The relationship
between self-talk, anxiety, and counselling skill. Canadian Journal of
Counselling, 32, 163-171.
Ho, P., Hosford, R., & Johnson, M. (1985). The effects of anxiety on recall in self
versus other-model observation. Counselor Education and Supervision, 25,
48-55. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6978.1985.tb00511.x
Hofmann, S. G., Grossman, P., & Hinton, D. E. (2011). Loving-kindness and
compassion meditation: Potential for psychological interventions. Clinical
Psychology Review, 31, 1126-1132. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.003
Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., & Oh, S. (2010). The effect of
mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 169-183. doi:
10.1037/a0018555
Holden, J. E., Kelley, K., & Agarwal, R. (2008). Analyzing change: a primer on
multilevel models with applications to nephrology. American Journal of
Nephrology, 28, 792-801. doi: 10.1159/000131102
Hollis-Walker, L., & Colosimo, K. (2011). Mindfulness, self-compassion, and
happiness in non-meditators: A theoretical and empirical examination.
Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 222-227. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.033
211
John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The Big Five Inventory -
Versions 4a and 54. Berkeley, CA.
Johnson, W. B., Barnett, J. E., Elman, N. S., Forrest, L., Schwartz-Mette, R. A., &
Kaslow, N. J. (2014). Preparing trainees for lifelong competence: Creating a
communitarian training culture. Training and Education in Professional
Psychology, 8, 211-220. doi: 10.1037/tep0000048
Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (2013). LISREL 9.10. Chicago: Scientific Software
International, Inc.
Judge, T. A., Locke, E. A., Durham, C. C., & Kluger, A. N. (1998). Dispositional
effects on job and life satisfaction: The role of core evaluations. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 83, 17-34. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.83.1.17
Judge, T. A., Van Vianen, A. E. M., & De Pater, I. E. (2004). Emotional stability,
core self-evaluations, and job outcomes: A review of the evidence and an
agenda for future research. Human Performance, 17, 325-346. doi:
10.1207/s15327043hup1703_4
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living. New York, NY: Delacorte.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and
future. Clinical Psychology, 10, 144-156. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpg016
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2005). Coming to our senses. New York, NY: Hyperion.
Kahill, S. (1988). Symptoms of professional burnout: A review of the empirical
evidence. Canadian Psychology, 29, 284-297. doi: 10.1037/h0079772
Kammeyer-Mueller, J. D., Judge, T. A., & Scott, B. A. (2009). The role of core self-
evaluations in the coping process. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 177-
195. doi: 10.1037/a0013214
Kazdin, A. E. (2001). Almost clinically signficant (p < .10): Current measures may
only approach clinical significance. Clinical Psychology: Science and
Practice, 8, 455-462. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.8.4.455
Kazdin, A. E. (2003). Research design in clinical psychology (4th ed.). Needham
Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Kazdin, A. E., & Blase, S. L. (2011). Rebooting psychotherapy research and practice
to reduce the burden of mental illness. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
6, 21-37. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393527
Kazdin, A. E., & Kendall, P. C. (1999). The meanings and measurement of clinical
significance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 332-339. doi:
10.1037/0022-006X.67.3.332
Kelders, S. M., Kok, R. N., Ossebaard, H. C., & Van Gemert-Pijnen, J. E. W. C.
(2012). Persuasive system design does matter: A systematic review of
adherence to web-based interventions. Journal of Medical Internet Research,
14, e152. doi: 10.2196/jmir.2104
Kelly, A. C., Zuroff, D. C., & Shapira, L. B. (2009). Soothing oneself and resisting
self-attacks: The treatment of two intrapersonal deficits in depression
vulnerability. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 33, 301-313. doi:
10.1007/s10608-008-9202-1
Kelly, K. R., Hall, A. S., & Miller, K. L. (1989). Relation of counselor intention and
anxiety to brief counselling outcome. Journal of Counselling Psychology, 36,
158-162. doi: 10.1037//0022-0167.36.2.158
Kemeny, M. E. (2003). The psychobiology of stress. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 12, 124-129. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01246
Kemeny, M. E., Foltz, C., Cavanagh, J. F., Cullen, M., Giese-Davis, J., Jennings, P., .
. . Ekman, P. (2012). Contemplative/emotion training reduces negative
213
Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (2005). Pursuing happiness: The
architecture of sustainable change. Review of General Psychology, 9, 111-131.
doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.111
Ma, S. H., & Teasdale, J. (2004). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression:
Replication and exploration of differential relapse prevention effects. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 31-40. doi: 10.1037/0022-
006X.72.1.31
Macbeth, A., & Gumley, A. (2012). Exploring compassion: A meta-analysis of the
association between self-compassion and psychopathology. Clinical
Psychology Review, 32, 545-552. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.06.003
Mahoney, M. J. (1993). Theoretical development in the cognitive psychotherapies.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 187-193. doi:
10.1037//0022-006X.61.2.187
Mahoney, M. J. (1997). Psychotherapists' personal problems and self-care patterns.
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 28, 14-16. doi:
10.1037/0735-7028.28.1.14
Mäkikangas, A., & Kinnunen, U. (2003). Psychosocial work stressors and well-being:
self-esteem and optimism as moderators in a one-year longitudinal sample.
Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 537-557. doi: 10.1016/S0191-
8869(02)00217-9
Mäkikangas, A., Kinnunen, U., & Feldt, T. (2004). Self-esteem, dispositional
optimism, and health: Evidence from cross-lagged data on employees. Journal
of Research in Personality, 38, 556-575. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2004.02.001
Mann, M., Hosman, C. M. H., Schaalma, H. P., & de Vries, N. K. (2004). Self-esteem
in a broad-spectrum approach for mental health promotion. Health Education
Research: Theory & Practice, 19, 357-372. doi: 10.1093/her/cyg041
Maslach, C. (1982). Burnout: The cost of caring. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-
Hall.
Maslach, C. (1993). Burnout: A multidimensional perspective. In W. B. Schaufeli, C.
Maslach & T. Marek (Eds.), Professional burnout: Recent developments in
theory and research (pp. 19-32). New York, NY: Hampshire.
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1981). The measurement of experienced burnout.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 2, 99-113. doi: 10.1002/job.4030020205
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1984). Burnout in organizational settings. In S.
Oskamp (Ed.), Applied social psychology annual (Vol. 5, pp. 133-153).
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Mattison, M. (2000). Ethical decision making: The person in the process. Social
Work, 45, 201-212. doi: 10.1093/sw/45.3.201
May, S., & O'Donovan, A. (2007). The advantages of the mindful therapist.
Psychotherapy in Australia, 13(4), 46-53.
Mayhew, S. L., & Gilbert, P. (2008). Compassionate mind training with people who
hear malevolent voices: A case series report. Clinical Psychology and
Psychotherapy, 15, 113-138. doi: 10.1002/cpp.566
McCarthy, W. C., & Frieze, I. H. (1999). Negative aspects of therapy: Client
perceptions of therapists' social influence, burnout, and quality of care.
Journal of Social Issues, 55, 33-50. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.0010
McEwen, B. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 840, 33-44. doi:
10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.
217
McGregor, L. N., Gee, D. E., & Posey, K. E. (2008). I feel like a fraud and it
depresses me: The relation between the imposter phenomenon and depression.
Social Behavior and Personality, 36, 43-48. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2008.36.1.43
McKay, M., & Fanning, P. (2000). Self-esteem: A proven program of cognitive
techniques for assessing, improving, and maintaining your self-esteem.
Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications.
Meichenbaum, D. (1977). Cognitive-behavior modification: An integrative approach.
New York, NY: Plenum.
Meichenbaum, D., & Butler, L. (1980). Cognitive ethology: Assessing the streams of
cognition and emotion. In K. R. Blankstein, P. Priner & J. Polivy (Eds.),
Advances in the study of communication and affect. New York, NY: Plenum
Press.
Meier, D. E., Back, A. L., & Morrison, R. S. (2001). The inner life of physicians and
care of the seriously ill. Journal of the American Medical Association, 286,
3007-3114. doi: 10.1001/jama.286.23.3007
Meier, L. L., Semmer, N. K., & Hupfeld, J. (2009). The impact of unfair treatment on
depressive mood: The moderating role of self-esteem level and self-esteem
instability. Personal and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 643-655. doi:
10.1177/0146167208331337
Melville, K. M., Casey, L. M., & Kavanagh, D. J. (2010). Dropout from internet-
based treatment for psychological disorders. British Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 49, 455-471. doi: 10.1348/014466509X472138
Mental Health Workforce Advisory Committee. (2008). Mental health workforce:
Supply of psychologists. Retrieved January 27, 2012, from
http://www.ahwo.gov.au
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure,
dynamics, and change. New York, NY: Guilford.
Miranda, R., Tsypes, A., Gallagher, M., & Rajappa, K. (2013). Rumination and
hopelessness as mediators of the relation between perceived emotion
dysregulation and suicidal ideation. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37, 786-
795. doi: 10.1007/s10608-013-9524-5
Mitchell, J., Stanimirovic, R., Klein, B., & Vella-Broderick, D. (2009). A randomised
controlled trial of a self-guided internet intervention promoting well-being.
Computers in Human Behavior, 25, 749-760. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2009.02.003
Monshat, K., Vella-Broderick, D., Burns, J., & Herrman, H. (2012). Mental health
promotion in the Internet age: a consultation with Australian young people to
inform the design of an online mindfulness training programme. Health
Promotion International, 27, 177-186. doi: 10.1093/heapro/dar01
Moore, K. A., & Cooper, C. L. (1996). Stress in mental health professionals: A
theoretical overview. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 42, 82-89.
doi: 10.1177/002076409604200202
Moore, P. (2008). Introducing mindfulness to clinical psychologists in training: An
experiential course of brief exercises. Journal of Clinical Psychology in
Medical Settings, 15, 331-337. doi: 10.1007/s10880-008-9134-7
Moos, R. H., & Holahan, C. J. (2003). Dispositional and contextual perspectives on
coping: Towards an integrative framework. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
59, 1387-1403. doi: 10.1002/jclp.10229
Morrissette, P. J. (1996). Recurring critical issues of student counsellors. Canadian
Journal of Counselling, 30, 31-41.
218
Morse, G., Salyers, M. P., Rollins, A. L., Monroe-DeVita, M., & Pfahler, C. (2012).
Burnout in mental health services: A review of the problem and its
remediation. Administration and Policy in Mental Health, 39, 341-352. doi:
10.1007/s10488-011-0352-1
Mullenbach, M., & Skovholt, T. M. (2011). Burnout prevention and self-care
strategies of expert practitioners. In T. M. Skovholt & M. Trotter-Mathison
(Eds.), The resilient practitioner: Burnout prevention and self-care strategies
for counselors, therapists, teachers, and health professionals (pp. 219-243).
New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.
Munsey, C. (2006). Helping colleagues to help themselves. Monitor on Psychology,
37, 35.
Myers, M. F. (1994). Doctors' marriages: A look at the problems and their solutions
(2nd ed.). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Neely, M. E., Schallert, D. L., Mohammed, S. S., Roberts, R. M., & Chen, Y. (2009).
Self-kindness when facing stress: The role of self-compassion, goal regulation
and support in college students' well-being. Motivation and Emotion, 33, 88-
97. doi: 10.1007/s11031-008-9119-8
Neff, K. D. (2003a). The development and validation of a scale to measure self-
compassion. Self and Identity, 2, 223-250. doi: 10.1080/15298860309027
Neff, K. D. (2003b). Self-compassion: An alternative conceptualization of a healthy
attitude toward oneself. Self and Identity, 2, 85-101. doi:
10.1080/15298860309032
Neff, K. D. (2009). Self-compassion. In M. R. Leary (Ed.), Handbook of individual
differences in social behavior (pp. 561-573). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Neff, K. D. (2011). Self-compassion: Stop beating yourself up and leave insecurity
behind. New York, NY: HarperCollins Publishers.
Neff, K. D., & Costigan, A. P. (2014). Self-compassion, wellbeing, and happiness.
Psychologie in Österreich, 2, 114-119.
Neff, K. D., & Germer, C. K. (2013). A pilot study and randomized controlled trial of
the mindful self-compassion program. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 69, 28-
44. doi: 10.1002/jclp.21923
Neff, K. D., Hsieh, Y.-P., & Dejitterat, K. (2005). Self-compassion, achievement
goals, and coping with academic failure. Self and Identity, 4, 263-287. doi:
10.1080/13576500444000317
Neff, K. D., Kirkpatrick, K. L., & Rude, S. S. (2007). Self-compassion and adaptive
psychological functioning. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 139-154.
doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.03.004
Neff, K. D., & McGehee, P. (2010). Self-compassion and psychological resilience
among adolescents and young adults. Self and Identity, 9, 225-240. doi:
10.1080/15298860902979307
Neff, K. D., Pisitsungkagarn, K., & Hseih, Y. (2008). Self-compassion and self-
construal in the United States, Thailand, and Taiwan. Journal of Cross-
Cultural Psychology, 39, 267-285. doi: 10.1177/0022022108314544
Neff, K. D., & Pommier, E. (2012). The relationship between self-compassion and
other-focused concern among college undergraduates, community adults, and
practicing meditators. Self and Identity, 12, 160-176. doi:
10.1080/15298868.2011.649546
Neff, K. D., Rude, S. S., & Kirkpatrick, K. L. (2007). An examination of self-
compassion in relation to positive psychological functioning and personality
219
Proudfoot, J. G., Klein, B., Barak, A., Carlbring, P., Cuijpers, P., Lange, A., . . .
Andersson, G. (2011). Establishing guidelines for executing and reporting
internet intervention research. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 40, 82-97.
Pugesek, B. H., & Grace, J. B. (1998). On the utility of path modelling for ecological
and evolutionary studies. Fuctional Ecology, 12, 853-856. doi:
10.1046/j.1365-2435.1998.00253.x
Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Ardnt, J., & Schimel, J. (2004). Why do
people need self-esteem? A theoretical and empirical review. Psychological
Bulletin, 130, 435-468. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.435
Rabin, S., Feldman, D., & Kaplan, Z. (1999). Stress and intervention strategies in
mental health professionals. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 72, 159-
169. doi: 10.1348/000711299159916
Radeke, J. T., & Mahoney, M. J. (2000). Comparing the personal lives of
psychotherapists and research psychologists. Professional Psychology:
Research and Practice, 31, 82-84. doi: 10.1037/0735-7028.31.1.82
Raes, F. (2010). Rumination and worry as mediators of the relationship between self-
compassion and depression and anxiety. Personality and Individual
Differences, 48, 757-761. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.01.023
Raes, F., Pommier, E., Neff, K. D., & Van Gucht, D. (2011). Construction and
factorial validation of a short form of the Self-Compassion Scale. Clinical
Psychology and Psychotherapy, 18, 250-255. doi: 10.1002/cpp.702
Rein, G., Atkinson, M., & McCraty, R. (1995). The physiological and psychological
effects of compassion and anger. Journal of Advancement in Medicine, 8, 87-
105.
Renjilian, D. A., Baum, R. E., & Landry, S. L. (1998). Psychotherapist burnout: Can
college students see the signs? Journal of College Student Psychotherapy, 13,
39-48. doi: 10.1300/J035v13n01_04
Rimes, K. A., & Wingrove, J. (2011). Pilot study of mindfulness-based cognitive
therapy for trainee clinical psychologists. Behavioural and Cognitive
Psychotherapy, 39, 235-241. doi: 10.1017/S1352465810000731
Ritterband, L. M., Thorndike, F. P., Cox, D. J., Kovatchev, B., & Gonder-Frederick,
L. A. (2009). A behavior change model for internet interventions. Annals of
Behavioral Medicine, 38, 18-27. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9133-4
Roemer, L., & Orsillo, S. M. (2009). Mindfulness & acceptance-based behavioral
therapies in practice. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Ronk, F. R., Korman, J. R., Hooke, G. R., & Page, A. C. (2013). Assessing clinical
significance of treatment outcomes using the DASS-21. Psychological
Assessment, 25, 1103-1110. doi: 10.1037/a0033100
Rønnestad, M. H., & Skovholt, T. M. (1993). Supervision of beginning and advanced
graduate students of counselling and psychotherapy. Journal of Counselling
and Development, 71, 396-405. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.1993.tb02655.x
Rønnestad, M. H., & Skovholt, T. M. (2003). The journey of the counselor and
therapist: Research findings and perspectives on professional development.
Journal of Career Development, 30, 5-44. doi: 10.1023/A:1025173508081
Rosen, C. C., Halbesleben, J. R. B., & Perrewé, P. L. (Eds.). (2013). The role of
emotion and emotion regulation in job stress and well being (Vol. 11).
Bingley, UK: Emerald Group Publishing Limited.
Rosenberg, T., & Pace, M. (2006). Burnout among mental health professionals:
Special considerations for the marriage and family therapist. Journal of
222
Sirois, F. M., Molnar, D., & Hirsch, J. K. (2015). Self-Compassion, stress and coping
in the context of chronic illness. Self and Identity, 1-15. doi:
10.1080/15298868.2014.996249
Skinner, E. A., Edge, K., Altman, J., & Sherwood, H. (2003). Searching for the
structure of coping: a review and critique of category systems for classifying
ways of coping. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 216-269. doi: 10.1037/0033-
2909.129.2.216
Skosnik, P. D., Chatterton, R. T., Swisher, T., & Park, S. (2000). Modulation of
attentional inhibition by norepinephrine and cortisol after psychological stress.
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 36, 59-68. doi: 10.1016/S0167-
8760(99)00100-2
Skovholt, T. M., & Rønnestad, M. H. (2003). Struggles of the novice counselor and
therapist. Journal of Career Development, 30, 45-58. doi:
10.1177/089484530303000103
Skovholt, T. M., & Trotter-Mathison, M. (2011). The resilient practitioner: Burnout
prevention and self-care strategies for counselors, therapists, teachers, and
health professionals (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.
Smeets, E., Neff, K. D., Alberts, H., & Peters, M. (2014). Meeting suffering with
kindness: Effects of a brief self-compassion intervention for female college
students. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 70, 794-807. doi:
10.1002/jclp.22076
Smith, P. L., & Moss, S. B. (2009). Psychologist impairment: What is it, how can it
be prevented, and what can be done to address it? Clinical Psychology:
Science and Practice, 16, 1-15. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2009.01137.x
Soto, C. J., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). The developmental
psychometrics of big five self-reports: acquiescence, factor structure,
coherence, and differentiation from ages 10 to 20. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 94, 718-737. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.4.718
Spector, P. E., & Jex, S. M. (1998). Development of four self-report measures of job
stressors and strain: Interpersonal Conflict at Work Scale, Organizational
Constraints Scale, Quantitative Workload Inventory, and Physical Symptoms
Inventory. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 3, 356-367. doi:
10.1037/1076-8998.3.4.356
Spek, V., Cuijpers, P., Nyklíček, I., Riper, H., Keyzer, J., & Pop, V. (2007). Internet-
based cognitive behaviour therapy for symptoms of depression and anxiety: a
meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 37, 319-328. doi:
10.1017/S0033291706008944
Sprang, G., Clark, J. J., & Whitt-Woosley, A. (2007). Compassion fatigue,
compassion satisfaction, and burnout: Factors impacting a professional's
quality of life. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 12, 259-280. doi:
10.1080/15325020701238093
Stafford-Brown, J., & Pakenham, K. I. (2012). The effectiveness of an ACT informed
intervention for managing stress and improving therapist qualities in clinical
psychology trainees. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68, 592-613. doi:
10.1002/jclp.21844
Steverink, N., & Lindenberg, S. (2006). Which social needs are important for
subjective well-being? What happens to them with aging? Psychology and
Aging, 21, 281-290. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.281
225
Appendix A
Demographic Questionnaire
1. Age (years):
2. Gender:
3. Profession:
Counsellor
Psychologist
Counselling Psychologist
Clinical Psychologist
Educational Psychologist
Psychoanalyst
Health Psychologist
Others (please specify)
4. Academic Qualifications:
Diploma Master’s Degree
Bachelor PhD/Doctorate
Grad Diploma Others (please specify)
Post Grad Diploma
Appendix B
Study 1 Email Invitation
Dear ___________
The primary aim of this research is to examine the relationship between self-
regulation plays a role in this relationship. Responses are anonymous, and will inform
future interventions for stress prevention and wellbeing in psychologists. The survey
to participate. You are also welcome to pass this information on to any colleagues you
feel would be interested in participating. Participants will be rewarded for their time
by having the opportunity to enter a draw to win one of two $150 vouchers.
https://www.qualtrics.com/______
(c.rees@curtin.edu.au).
230
Appendix C
Appendix D
Appendix E
ELIGIBILITY
how much of the module you complete. You will be able to work through the
modules at your own pace, at a time that is suitable to you, and to revisit the content
of previous modules whenever you wish. Participants will be asked to complete a
short survey at the end of each week to monitor their progress. In addition,
participants will be asked to complete a series of surveys at completion of the
program, and again 4 weeks after the program has ended.
REGISTRATION
If you wish to register for this study, please read the following participation
information. If, after reading the information, you consent to participate in the study,
please indicate your consent on the following page. The registration process will then
proceed as follows:
You will be asked to fill out a survey that will assess your eligibility and
suitability for the program.
Eligible applicants will receive an email regarding their participation in the
program.
You will be asked to complete additional online surveys before, immediately
after, and 12 weeks after completing the program.
VOLUNTARY PARTICIPATION
Participation in this research is voluntary. If you do not wish to participate, you do not
have to. If you choose to participate and then change your mind, you can withdraw
from the research at any time without any negative consequences.
CONFIDENTIALITY
All identifying information collected will remain confidential to those outside the
study. The information will be stored for five years in a locked cupboard at the Curtin
University Psychology and Speech Clinic. The data from each person will be
identified only by the six-digit code you will be assigned at registration. If the study is
published, you will not be identified.