Empathy Tesis
Empathy Tesis
Empathy Tesis
Helen Scott
Department of Psychology
March 2011
Declaration:
Signed................................................
Helen Scott
25/03/2011
1
Table of contents
Acknowledgements.................................................................................................. 9
Abstract ................................................................................................................. 10
2.3.3 Process model of clinical empathy (Larson & Yao, 2005) ................. 38
Chapter 4 – Individual differences and empathy: Personality and the IRI ............... 68
4.3.3 Analysis............................................................................................ 77
Appendix 5: Factor loadings for the Bar On EQ-i items ....................................... 225
Table 3.2: Reliability and validity information for existing empathy measures. 63
Table 4.2: Descriptive statistics for the NEO PI-R and Empathy (IRI) scales.... 79
Table 4.3: Correlations between NEO PI-R factors and Empathy (IRI) scales... 81
Table 4.4: Factor loadings for joint factor analysis of IRI and NEO PI-R.......... 85
Table 5.2: Factor names, descriptions, means, S.D.s and reliabilities for eight
Table 5.4: Correlations between EQ-i and Empathy (IRI) scales....................... 112
Table 5.5: Factor loadings for IRI and EQ-i subscales........................................ 114
Table 6.4: Descriptive statistics for UK/Ireland and Overseas trained doctors... 142
Table 6.5: Partial Correlations controlling for age and intelligence between
Table 6.6: Partial correlations controlling for age and intelligence between
List of figures
Figure 2.4: Process model of clinical empathy (from Larson & Yao, 2005).. 39
Figure 6.1: Process model of clinical empathy (from Larson & Yao, 2005).. 119
Figure 7.1: Process model of clinical empathy (from Larson & Yao, 2005).. 156
Figure 8.1: A process model of empathy in the healthcare setting from this
thesis............................................................................................................... 188
9
Acknowledgements
There are so many people to whom I am grateful for their support in the
Silvester. Jo, throughout this journey you have provided me with wisdom and
inspiration, as well as so much time and practical support and, in the end, the
much needed space to get it finished. Thank you so much for your never-ending
Thanks also to my colleagues who have given me support along the way. Anna,
Lara, Paul, Maddy and Sharon have all given me advice, motivation and great
opportunity to collect data in the GP assessment centre for my second and third
studies. I would also like to acknowledge the participants who were involved in
this research; in particular, those from the James Cook University Hospital. It
As anyone who completes a PhD knows, this process can take you away from
your family both physically and mentally. Andrew and Beatrice, I apologise for
my absence and thank you for your love and patience with me. I am grateful to
my sister Louise for her absolute certainty that I would get there as well as a
demon eye for detail in proof reading. To my parents, whose unfailing pride in
me is a constant source of motivation, I thank you both. Dad, at times you were
the only one who kept bringing me back on track, which I have appreciated so
Abstract
healthcare practitioners and their patients. In order to identify the best methods to
were cross sectional and quantitative in design. Studies one and two investigated
intelligence. Findings suggested that (1) perspective taking and empathic concern
were closely associated with agreeableness and extraversion, and also loaded on
to the single factor of emotional intelligence (2) fantasy was associated with
openness to experience but not emotional intelligence, and (3) personal distress
empathic behaviour, but not when doctors had qualified in a different country.
and work design. The specific behaviours associated with empathy as judged by
and work design interventions. Finally, areas for future research are identified.
12
Chapter 1 - Introduction
“Some patients, though conscious that their condition is perilous, recover their
health simply through their contentment with the goodness of the physician”.
Hippocrates 460-380 BC
Harkness, Ernst, Georgiou and Kleijnen (2001) found that those physicians who
adopt a warm, friendly and reassuring manner are perceived as more effective
than those who keep consultations formal. Empathy has been identified as
facilitating improved outcomes for both patients and doctors (e.g., Carmel &
Glick, 1996; Hardee, 2003; Hojat, Mangione, Nasca, Cohen, Gonnella, Erdmann,
Veloski & Magee, 2001), and an important trait for other healthcare
Hamilton & Issakainen, 2000; Reynolds, Scott & Jessiman, 1999). Yet despite
practitioners to develop, not least due to continuing debates about its definition
important by exploring potential outcomes and consequences for the patients and
healthcare professionals.
13
to have an impact on the patient experience, and patient satisfaction is one of the
(e.g. Kaplan, Ware & Greenfield, 1998; Mercer & Reynolds, 2002). In the US,
patient appraisals are used as part of performance related pay schemes, and
account for as much as 20% of a doctor’s pay (Kolata, 2005). In Scotland the
Reynolds, 2002; Mercer, Maxwell, Heaney & Watt, 2004) of patient satisfaction
has been accredited as a method for appraising General Practitioners (GPs), and
in the U.K. there has been a strategic move within the NHS to acknowledge the
Health, 1996). The rationale for such approaches derives from the large number
(Wasserman, Inui, Barriatua, Carter & Lippincott, 1984); stroke units (Pound,
Gompertz & Ebrahim, 1995); diabetic clinics (Hornsten, Lundman, Selstam &
satisfaction with treatment. For some time now, legal experts have argued that
professionals in the 1990s providing support for this assertion. For example,
between the doctor and patient in 32 of the 45 cases reviewed, with the most
studies per se, it was identified as a key issue, with the authors concluding that
“Doctors who can’t communicate are more likely to end up in court” (p.1365).
perhaps more important are those studies that find actual improvements in patient
health. As Kaplan, Greenfield and Ware (1989) point out, patients may be
satisfied but this may not necessarily lead to the best healthcare outcomes or the
behaviour change required for treatment success. One possible reason for
improvement in patient health (or the lack of this) is the patient adhering to the
with benefits resulting from greater sharing of concerns and motivation to get
better. In fact, several major reviews over the last three decades (e.g., Becker &
15
Maiman, 1975; Becker & Rosenstock, 1984; Garrity, 1981) have concluded that
relationship Frankel and Beckman (1989) identified that doctors who do not
are more likely to appear frustrated and judgmental, rather than empathic and
supportive. The study also showed that feedback to the doctor on this issue can
Winefield, Murrell and Clifford (1995). They analysed transcripts from 210 GP-
patient consultations and found that patient satisfaction after the consultation
non-compliance with treatment were also more likely to complain that the doctor
has not listened to their perspective or treated them as an equal during the
consultation.
the link between empathy and health outcomes. Empathy from a healthcare
behaviour and also change their patients’ perceptions of their health status
between 1983 and 1993, Stewart (1995) found that 16 reported a positive
functioning and physiological measures such as blood pressure and pain control.
In nursing, La Monica, Madea and Oberst (1987) found less anxiety, depression
and hostility among clients being cared for by highly empathic nurses. For
patients with a variety of illnesses, physician empathy has also been linked to
outcomes (Frasure Smith, Lesperance & Talajic, 1995; Rietveld & Prins, 1998).
and Ware (1989) concluded that increased patient control and greater expression
health outcomes. This was found to be the case whether the outcome measure
as blood pressure or blood sugar levels. Such physiological outcomes have also
have pointed out that professionals also need to recognise the benefits of
found that those nurses who reported having developed closer relationships with
Yet despite mounting evidence that empathy in healthcare professionals can have
empathy usually increases with maturity, it typically declines over the period of
medical education and early stages of the medical career (Bellini, Baime & Shea,
2005; Bellini & Shea, 2005; Woloschuk, Harasym & Temple, 2004). Hojat,
American Medical Colleges states that a key learning objective for medical
caring for patients”. Yet according to Carmel and Glick (1996), whilst
‘curing and caring’ are desired by patients they are rarely found in medical
settings. Again, Sparr, Gordon, Hickam and Girard (1988) found that medical
over the course of training, with bureaucratic pressures and experiences with
developing empathy have become more common and medical education has
Suchman, Markakis, Beckman & Frankel, 1997). Even with this renewed focus,
(2006) found that only seven out of 13 articles reporting evaluations of empathy
empathy, most articles still begin with a discussion of the meaning of the term.
worst they are conflicting. Interestingly, very few researchers in the medical field
relationships lies in the presence of empathy” but there has been “little attempt to
This chapter has considered why empathy is important in the helping professions.
Chapter two continues with a review of the existing literature concerned with
about the nature and definition of the concept (Hardee, 2003). Chapter two
the ability to perceive the subjective experience of another (1909). Since then,
developmental (e.g., Baron Cohen, 2003), social (e.g., Eisenberg, 1987), and
forensic (e.g., Blair, 2005), with research focusing on diverse topics such as:
interest in empathy relating to leadership and sales (e.g., Kellett, Humphrey &
Sleeth, 2002; Plank, Minton & Reid, 1996), and empathic processes in
relationships with clients and colleagues (e.g., Silvester, Patterson, Koczwara &
Ferguson, 2007). Studies of empathy in healthcare also span 50 years, with early
Bachrach (1976) argues that “we know what we mean” (p.35) when we think of
there has been much debate over its definition, with articles often beginning with
a discussion of the exact meaning of the term (e.g., Ohmdahl, 1995). Early
although more recently efforts towards a more integrated approach have resulted
Yao, 2005). In broad terms, however, four dimensions of empathy have been
1992; Stepien & Baernstein, 2006). Each of these is reviewed briefly below.
another’s emotion (Feshbach & Roe, 1968). Strayer (1987) adopted a definition
of empathy as “the self’s feeling into the affect of another person” (p.236), and
22
emotional experience, which in the medical context has been identified as similar
to sympathy, defined as “feeling with the patient or feeling similar emotions that
the patient feels” (Mehrabian & Epstein, 1972, p. 1563). However, it has been
suggests that merely to experience similar emotions to those of a target will not
Schoenrade, 1987; Zahn-Waxler & Robinson, 1995). Rather than the sharing of
congruent with, but not identical to, the emotion of another person (e.g., Batson
& Shaw, 1991). Vreeke and van der Mark (2003) note that genuine empathy that
emphasises the notion of congruent affect, as not all emotional reactions may be
Mendelsohn (1986) found that while there are situations in which both empathy
and sympathy are aroused, sympathy can be present without shared affect. They
found that emotional empathy was more consistently linked to personality traits
associated with affiliation and putting others’ needs first, while sympathy was
more situation-specific.
Although reviews by Morse et al. (1992) and Stepien and Baernstein (2006) both
practitioner and patient, the exact nature of the emotional response has not been
specified. Also, there are conflicting views such as those suggested by Hojat et
al. (2002) that an emotional response interferes with the objectivity of the
practitioner and reduces the effectiveness of diagnosis and care. However, no test
of these assertions has been made, and very few studies involve patients in their
methodologies. The emotional labour literature raises another issue with respect
the appearance of care and concern that is not necessarily experienced, deep
these emotions (Grandey, 2003). These are important issues to consider in efforts
to develop and sustain clinical empathy due to their potential impact on both the
practitioner and the patient. Whereas surface acting empathy might protect the
objectivity and emotional exhaustion (Larson & Yao, 2005). These issues may
clinical empathy over time (e.g. Hojat et al., 2004; Spencer, 2004).
movements and physical cues (Kohler, 1929). One of the most widely known
researchers to adopt this perspective was Hogan (1969) who defined empathy as
mind without actually experiencing that person’s feelings’ (p.308, italics added).
However, it has been suggested that healthcare professionals need more than the
educators (Halpern, 2001). Morse et al. (1992) and Stepien and Baernstein
helpful customer service. Axtell, Parker, Holman and Totterdell (2007) asked
347 agents from two UK call centres to complete self ratings of perspective
results of the study may be generalisable (Parker & Axtell, 2001). These two
in an applied, emotional labour role. But as Hynes, Baird and Grafton (2006)
argue cognitive processes can still have an emotional focus. They identified the
line with the studies previously discussed in which ratings of perspective taking
involved agents rating the extent to which they imagined their customers’
perspectives and thought about how they would feel in a customer’s situation
participants to read scenarios which required them to imagine what a target was
perspective taking activated specific areas of the medial orbital frontal lobes
identify and understand patients’ emotions and perspectives (Morse et al., 1992;
and presence” (Zderad, 1970, p.30) that drives someone to help their patient. The
Barrett-Lennard’s cyclical model of the phases of empathy (1993; see figure 2.1).
attend to the needs of the patient is present in the ‘pre-empathy condition’ will
the further stages of the model take place. By including this ‘moral’ motivation
27
as Morse et al. (1992) refer to it, empathy is seen as an altruistic rather than an
egoistic concept.
PA PB PA PB PA PB PA PB PA PB
Evidence for altruistic empathy has been provided by Batson and colleagues
(e.g., Batson & Coke, 1981), who argue that only if the motivation to help is
demonstrate this, Coke, Batson and McDavis (1978) played a radio broadcast of
participants who offered more help to the target in the broadcast were those who
reported greater empathic concern for others on a self report measure but not
higher personal distress. This led Coke et al. to conclude that empathy is an
were egoistic, those who reported greater personal distress would have also
offered help in order to reduce their own distress. However, there are concerns
with the ecological validity of this scenario as it is likely that real incidents that
28
would produce empathic concern may also produce personal distress. For
example, witnessing the distress of someone with whom one shares a close
relationship would produce not only concern for that person but also personal
distress due to the feeling of ‘oneness’ with the target (Cialdini et al., 1997).
Also, the study does not consider individual differences in motivation, such that
some people may be motivated to act by personal distress whereas others may be
that an altruistic motivation may lead to people being more likely engage in
perceptions of the target are given prominence in the model. Provided the
needs to convey that PA understands PB. The Phase II expressed empathy then
makes it possible for PB to receive this empathy in Phase III and the cycle repeats
following further expression from PB. If all conditions are satisfied ongoing and
29
meaningful communication between the two parties will result. This experience
of being both heard and understood is proposed to bring about feelings of relief,
initial interactions between nurse and patient when relationships were not
judgments of physician empathy from observers. Other than this, very little has
al. (1992) concluded that there is an urgent need for more empirical research to
encompass factors (patient and healthcare professional) that can lead to empathic
empathy in healthcare roles have been developed. The three that have had most
Davis’ (1983, 1996) multidimensional model of empathy, and the process model
of clinical empathy (Larson & Yao, 2005). Each of these is reviewed in more
detail below.
review of existing literature (see Figure 2.2 for an adapted version). The central
theme of the model is that the healthcare practitioner needs to engage in both
emotional empathy on the part of the healthcare professional is seen as the main
the patient’s motivation to get well. The model provides a useful link between
pathways have yet to be fully tested. Indeed, no research has been located which
has directly tested Squier’s model and sixteen years after the model was
31
Accurate Information
Perspective
provision
Taking
Shared
understanding of
health problems
Open communication
Practitioner Greater knowledge Stronger adherence to
Empathic
relationship Greater satisfaction treatment regimes and
Understanding
skills Reduced stress improved health
Sharing of Increased motivation
emotional
concerns about
Emotional illness
Emotional
concern and
reactivity
motivation
Figure 2.2: Model of empathic understanding and adherence to treatment regimens (adapted from Squier, 1990)
32
published, Stepien and Baernstein (2006) called for more empirical investigation
healthcare practitioners.
potential ways to develop empathy than Squier’s model, which does not specify
trait) empathy relatively stable across time (Davis, 1983; Gladstein, 1987;
develops during childhood as cognitive ability develops, and social and family
would depend on unusual events such as brain injury or illness which may affect
proposed in terms of both abilities and traits, as antecedents are said to include
33
both the intellectual ability to engage in perspective taking and the dispositional
between observer and target. Both of these would enable more accurate
1984) by which even young infants appear to experience shared affect. Simple
cognitive processes are said to occur via classical conditioning as a result of the
learning history of the observer. Certain emotions that one may have
experienced, and at the same time perceived in others, can be activated simply by
Poulin, Shea & Shell, 1991). This stage also includes the advanced cognitive
recognise the distinction between emotional and cognitive role taking (Hynes,
Baird & Grafton, 2006). Following these processes, a range of outcomes within
outcomes) may result. Affective outcomes within the observer are divided into
parallel and reactive emotions. Parallel emotions are Davis’ term for the shared
affective response. The observer feels emotions the same as those experienced by
the target, which may result directly from the individual characteristics of the
observer, from the primary circular reaction or from simple cognitive processes
34
NON-COGNITIVE
Primary Circular Reaction HELPING +
Motor Mimicry
SIMPLE COGNITIVE
Classical Conditioning SOCIAL BEHAVIOUR +
Direct Association
Labelling
other hand are those which are different from, but congruent with, those of the target,
such as compassion and sympathy. These are purported to result from more
empathy self ratings were found to partially mediate the relationship between self
ratings of perspective taking and manager ratings of helping behaviour. This study
helping behaviour.
Personal distress, defined as “the tendency to feel discomfort and anxiety in response
reproduction of the target’s affective state, but more of a response to it. It is also
congruent response which the target would perceive as helpful. Davis’ inclusion of
personal distress in the model supports the view of Cialdini, Baumann and Kenrick
(1981) who see helping behaviour in terms of egoistic motivation, resulting from the
desire to relieve one’s own negative state, which may include tension, stress or guilt.
However as discussed earlier, it has been argued that true empathic helping should
be altruistically motivated (e.g., Batson & Coke, 1981). Personal distress may
generate an egoistic motivation to reduce one’s own negative state but this would not
motivation to help is selfless and altruistic and thus can be considered as empathic
36
helping. Findings from studies such as that by Coke, Batson and McDavis (1978) in
which empathic concern and not personal distress predicted help for a target on a
radio broadcast have lead Batson, O’Quin, Fultz, Vanderplas and Isen (1983) to
conclude that personal distress is distinct from empathy, which ought to be purely
particularly relevant for healthcare practitioners who are seen as being defined by an
interpreting the other person’s thoughts and feelings. This relates to work by Ickes
Davis acknowledges the need for accuracy, his measure of empathy (the
The second non-affective outcome, attributions are judgements made by the observer
to explain the behaviour of the target. Several researchers have reported links
between empathy and attributions. Regan and Totten (1975) asked female students to
watch a conversation between two other female students and then rate one of them
thought each style resulted from the student’s personality or the situation.
37
(emotional perspective taking) with the student or simply observe them. They found
behaviour more to the situation rather than personal disposition. This finding has
been replicated several times (e.g., Archer, Foushee, Davis & Aderman, 1979;
Betancourt, 1990).
It has also been suggested that Weiner’s (1980; 1985; 1986) attributional theory may
events, rather than specific content, which determines the emotional and behavioural
behaviour, an observer will attempt to determine why help is needed before acting. If
the observer judges a negative outcome to be within a target’s control, this may lead
to negative emotions such as anger and annoyance, blaming the individual for the
outcome, and consequently, to help being withheld (Meyer & Mulherin, 1980;
Russell & McAuley, 1986). However, if the target is believed not to have control
over the situation, observers are more likely to experience sympathy, increasing the
interactions with patients were examined. Explanations were assessed via a modified
38
Metalsky & Seligman, 1982; Peterson & Villanova, 1988). Behaviours were
observers, with doctors who attributed positive patient outcomes to causes that were
more internal and controllable to him or herself being judged more empathic and
more motivated to engage in help-giving behaviour. This suggests the need for the
Finally, Davis’ model identifies the interpersonal outcomes associated with empathy
as helping, aggression and social relationships. Historically, empathy has most often
been linked with helping behaviour (Eisenberg, 1986). However, empathy has also
been found to have both a negative relationship with aggressive behaviours and a
empathy reviewed here and it could be argued that aspects within the model relating
to the development of empathy during childhood are less relevant for the study of
Davis’ model has been identified by Larson and Yao (2005; see Figure 2.4) as
their adaptation focusing on the more salient aspects for this context. Retaining the
Antecedents
• Physician characteristics
• Patient characteristics
• Situational
characteristics
Empathic Processes
• Non-cognitive processes
e.g. mimicry
Figure 2.4: Process model of clinical empathy (from Larson & Yao, 2005).
40
direct adaptation by Larson and Yao (2005) illustrates the potential applicability of
empathy and cynicism. Positive antecedents, if present, lead to the cognitive and
helping behaviours which include social behaviour and management of conflict. This
and sustain empathy, with the inclusion of both individual and situational
To summarise, chapter one concluded by arguing that to identify the best methods to
patients. Chapter two has reviewed recent models of empathy in healthcare that
emotional and behavioural components. Davis’ model (1983; 1996) is the most
comprehensive psychological model and the direct adaptation by Larson and Yao
(2005) highlights the potential application of Davis’ model to the healthcare context.
41
However, to date there has been very little research that has tested the utility of the
model for understanding empathy between patients and healthcare professionals and,
as such, very little is known about the individual characteristics that might have an
behaviours are. In order to answer these questions, this thesis now turns to a review
“In spite of the apparent difficulty involved in developing a valid and acceptable
measure of empathy, the theoretical import of the concept requires that continuing
efforts be made.”
multidimensional aspects of empathy, including measures designed for use with the
consistently (Rust & Golombok, 1999). Types of reliability include internal and test-
same group of respondents at two different time points and calculating the
correlation coefficient between the two resulting sets of scores (Kline, 2000).
reliability - the correlation between scores on one half of the scale items with the
43
other half (Rust & Golombok, 1999). Generally, a minimum Pearson’s r of 0.7 is
Validity on the other hand is concerned with the effectiveness of the test in
measuring what it claims to measure (Kline, 2000). Cronbach and Meehl’s (1955)
seminal paper on test validity identifies four types: content; construct; predictive, and
with subject matter experts, to determine if the scale samples the domain of interest
score (Messick, 1995) which can vary across populations. Content validity should
therefore be established for the population that will be sampled (Haynes, Richard &
empathy should therefore consider not only the complex nature of the concept but
The second type of validity identified by Cronbach and Meehl (1955) is construct
studies (Rust & Golombok, 1999). Construct validity is an umbrella term for types of
validity including convergent and discriminant validity (Domino & Domino, 2006).
validity is on the other hand established when no consistent relationships are found
empathy measures (e.g. Carey, Fox & Spraggins, 1988; Pulos, Elison & Lennon,
2004). The third and fourth types of validity, predictive and concurrent, are known as
independent criterion measure. When the scale score and criterion score are
measured at the same time, this is known as concurrent validity, whereas predictive
validity studies involve the criterion score being taken at a later date. For a scale to
be deemed effective, it requires both reliability and validity for the intended purpose,
review of existing measures designed for use with the general population identified
Hagen, 1985). These two measures were by Hogan (1969) and Mehrabian and
included, the George Washington Social Intelligence Test (Hunt, 1928; Moss, 1931;
Moss, Hunt, Omwake & Ronning, 1927; Moss, Hunt & Omwake, 1949); the Chapin
Social Insight Test (Chapin, 1942); the Dymond Rating Test of Insight and Empathy
(Dymond, 1949), and the Empathy Test (Kerr & Speroff, 1954). Since Chlopan et
al.’s review, however, the Interpersonal Reactivity Index developed by Davis (1983)
from his multidimensional model of empathy has also received support from
45
researchers (e.g., Pulos et al., 2004) and this and the questionnaires by Hogan (1969)
progress has been made in the development of empathy scales for use in healthcare
contexts. Within nursing, scales include the Empathy Construct Rating Scale (ECRS:
Relation Inventory (BLRI: 1964, 1978). More recently, the Jefferson Scale of
Physician Empathy (JSPE: Hojat et al., 2002) has been developed for use with
doctors although is now being used within other healthcare professions as well
(Chen, La Lopa & Dang, 2008). Publication of the Reynolds Empathy Scale
(Reynolds, 2000) and the Consultation and Relational Empathy measure (CARE:
Mercer, Maxwell, Heaney & Watt, 2004) have marked significant developments in
likely to be of most use in empirical research, this chapter reviews each of these
eight measures and information is also summarised in tables 3.1 and 3.2.
One of the first specific measures of healthcare empathy was La Monica’s (1981)
Empathy Construct Rating Scale (ECRS) for nurses, which was originally developed
to evaluate an empathy training program (La Monica, Carew, Winder, Haase &
Blanchard, 1976). Initially, 259 items were generated by 25 female graduate nursing
46
and nursing respectively. These items were reduced to the final 100 via an item
facility analysis using three expert and 10 student ratings. To calculate reliability, La
Monica (1981) asked 103 nursing students to rate two colleagues, one high (Form A)
and one low (Form B) in empathy. Both forms were found to have high Cronbach’s
α coefficients and split-half reliabilities. However, no reliability statistics for the self
rating version were reported. Sample sizes are small in that experts recommend at
least 200 people for an item and reliability analysis (Rusk & Golombok, 1999).
Three hundred female nurses and nursing students then completed a battery of scales
in order to assess convergent and construct validity. The five subscales hypothesised
to be present within the ECRS included: non verbal behaviour; personality traits such
encouraging and supporting, and finally respect for self and others. However these
five subscales were not supported by a factor analysis. In analysis of the self ratings
of the 300 nurses and students, all five subscales loaded on to a single factor.
Analysis of self, patient and peer ratings concluded that the 100 items loaded on to
seven factors, although 84 of the items loaded significantly on to the first two
factors, one being positive and one negative indicators of empathy. Subsequent
studies using the ECRS have used these 84 items rather than the original 100 (e.g.,
With respect to construct validity, self and peer ratings showed a correlation of only
.20 (p<.001) while self and patient ratings were found to have an even lower
between self ratings and the Chapin Social Insight Test (Chapin, 1942) was found,
suggesting concerns over construct and convergent validity of the test. Criterion
related validity was also called into question when La Monica (1987) and Reynolds
related validity. Theoretically, the content of the scale does not appear to relate
clearly to any models of empathy discussed in chapter two, suggesting that there is
Although an older measure than La Monica’s ECRS, some researchers have argued
Inventory (BLRI: Barrett- Lennard, 1964, 1978) makes it suitable for use in applied
settings beyond the counselling relationship for which it was originally developed
(Layton & Wykle, 1990). The most widely used form of the BLRI relies on the
patient describing the healthcare practitioner (the ‘other towards self’ or OS version).
There is also a ‘myself towards other’ (MO) version which is effectively a self rating
empathy in its own right (e.g., Layton & Wykle, 1990) and the subscale has also
been adapted for use within medicine to assess the physician-patient relationship
described in chapter two. According to this model the empathy cycle starts with the
empathiser in an attentional set which allows them to pick up cues from the target
person. The cues are then perceived and empathy is expressed. The target person
receives this expression and responds in turn, thus the cycle continues. The OS
empathic understanding subscale taps into the empathy cycle at phase three, where
the patient receives empathy and judges the motivation and understanding of the
empathiser.
From development, the rationale of the BLRI has been "the logical presumption that
it is what the patient… himself [sic] experiences that affects him most directly"
(Barrett-Lennard, 1962, p. 2). In turn, this patient experience should be most closely
presented in Table 3.2. Numerous studies have reported results in which the OS
predicted positive outcomes. Gurman and Razin (1977) for example reviewed 20
studies of actual help-seeking patients. In only one of these studies did the patient’s
outcomes.
49
As noted by Reynolds (2000), however, the scale may have limited success when
applied to the less formal nurse-patient relationship. It has been found that when
used in conditions other than the counselling context for which it was designed, the
reliability of the BLRI is reduced (e.g. Polit & Hungler, 1983). In addition, content
1977) rather than more general healthcare professions. According to Bennett (1995),
the range of issues discussed by nurses and patients is more diverse than the
therapist-patient interaction, and the content validity of the measure for the role of a
non-psychiatric nurse or doctor may therefore be limited. Whilst the BLRI appears to
The most commonly used measure for assessing practitioner empathy reported in the
Hojat, Gonnella, Nasca, Mangione, Vergare & Magee, 2002). The authors began the
empathy, with the authors concluding that empathy in medicine should be viewed as
a cognitive process. Qualitative research with subject matter experts (100 physicians)
experts (e.g., Rust & Golombok, 1999). Items for the scale were generated by these
patient’s experiences, emotions, and feelings” (Hojat et al., 2002, p. 1563). This
50
definition was explicitly contrasted with sympathy, which was defined as “feeling
with the patient or feeling similar emotions that the patient feels” (p. 1563). The
items were therefore immediately focused towards a cognitive view of empathy, and
diagnosis and treatment (Hojat et al., 2002). The JSPE scale is widely used across
various healthcare roles apart from medicine, including nursing and pharmacy
(Chen, La Lopa & Dang, 2008; Fields, Hojat, Gonnella, Mangione, Kane & Magee,
2004). It has been found to have satisfactory psychometric properties, with high
reliability for samples of 56 nurses and 42 physicians (Fields et al., 2004). Tables 3.1
and 3.2 provide more information on the JSPE and its properties.
Despite the psychometric robustness of the JSPE it has theoretical limitations in that
empathy is more stable over time (Feighny Arnold, Monaco, Munro & Earl, 1998),
Of particular interest is the fact that although models of empathy emphasise the need
Kane, Gotto, Mangione, West and Hojat (2007) developed a five item patient version
empathy, and asks the patient to rate the physician’s empathic concern, perspective
51
taking and behaviour. Analysis of data from 225 patients resulted in a single factor
with a Cronbach’s α of .58, which was deemed to be satisfactory given the small
number of items in the measure. However, the patient scale appears to suffer from a
ceiling effect as the mean rating was 23.8 while the maximum possible score is 25.
The patient scale may therefore not be useful in discriminating between physicians,
perhaps due to social desirability or that patients are unable to judge the internal
In summary, although the JSPE is a popular measure of empathy, the definition used
to develop the scale is not multidimensional and neither version of the scale included
most effectively. There has been some progress in addressing these limitations in
This scale was developed to ‘demystify’ the process of empathy between nurse and
patient, and allow nurses to better understand how to apply skills and appear
effectiveness of existing measures. First, he argued that a measure was needed that
the user could be confident of in terms of reliability and validity. Secondly, that an
something which other measures had typically failed to take into account and was
52
Office, 1997).
In order to develop the scale, 30 patients were asked for their perceptions of effective
(Reynolds, 1986). Rather than use a method of qualitative inquiry purely focusing on
influenced by the content of the ECRS. Comments were categorised into helpful or
possible to become aware of the patient’s emotions; listening to the concerns of the
patient; using a range of strategies to help the patient, and awareness of how the
To be included in the scale, the item must have been mentioned by at least twenty of
the thirty patients and also be relevant to empathy as judged by reports in previous
literature. Twelve items resulted on the scale, six positive and six negative. Further
empathy is sound (particularly for the behavioural dimension where patients are in
approach taken by Reynolds. First, the qualitative data gathered from patients was
limited due to a focus on a previously constructed scale rather than the patients’ own
conceptualisations of empathy. Secondly, the actual words used by patients were not
53
example is given to illustrate this point. Item 1 on the scale is ‘Attempts to explore
about something she seems to recognise this mood and asks me about it. She won’t
persist if I am reluctant” (Reynolds, 2000, p. 56). However one could argue that this
statement reflects both items 1 and 5. Potentially, in moving from patient statements
to item generation, the researcher’s own interpretation may have altered the items
thus negating the argument to put the patient perceptions at the centre of the scale.
Indeed, content validation was checked in consultation with five experts from
nursing and clinical psychology rather than referring back to patients. Finally, in
and affective processes involved with empathy, the scale again fails to incorporate a
and as yet little evidence is available of its usefulness, however, his central theme of
including patient perceptions is one which is growing in popularity and has also led
The CARE measure was developed on the basis of arguments that patients’ views
are central to the effectiveness of empathy in the clinical encounter (Mercer et al.,
2004). The authors also intended to build on the work of Reynolds by creating a
measure of empathy which could be relevant for clinical encounters other than
nursing. Their initial measure was piloted using a sample of general practitioners and
54
appropriate revision of the CARE measure until the third version of the scale was
Although sample sizes were small in testing the two pilot versions of the scale (20
patients in first pilot followed by 13 in the second), a key stage in ensuring that the
final scale represented patients’ perceptions was to validate the scale with patient
consultation. To further validate the CARE measure, 3044 patients then completed
the final version of the questionnaire for 26 GPs from different practices (Mercer,
McConnachie, Maxwell, Heaney & Watt, 2005). In total 76% of patients rated the
items within the measure as being very important to their consultation with the
patients rated the items as more important. This suggests that the measure is a good
as it might reasonably be assumed that these patients would be even more in need of
empathy from their physician. Further analysis revealed that acceptable reliability of
(Mercer et al., 2005), and norms were established such that within the scoring range
of 10-50, a score of less than 39 represented a below average score while above 42
represented above average. This final stage of analysis has resulted in the measure
being adopted for use in appraisal of GPs practicing in Scotland. The measure
patient consultation, but it again focuses on the behavioural dimension (perhaps not
The final three measures of empathy reviewed here were developed for use with
condition that does not imply (or therefore require) accuracy, consequently a self
to nine psychologists and 14 other people who were asked independently to use
California Q-sort items (Block, 1961) to describe a highly empathic person. A high
psychology. Hogan therefore used it to develop a scale comprised of 100 items that
completed this new scale alongside other personality scales. Reliability and validity
analyses resulted in 64 items being retained in the final scale. The choice of sample
for scale construction is an interesting one, as it could be argued that military officers
and those in scientific careers might not be the most representative on which to base
56
surprisingly, Chlopan et al. (1985) note there is evidence to suggest that this scale is
more valid for males than females, possibly resulting from the participants being
internal consistency was found to be only .57 while test-retest reliability was only
.41 over 12 months (Froman & Peloquin, 2001). Using a restricted definition may
have limited the success of the Hogan empathy scale as a measure as reliability has
males, concerns about using a purely cognitive definition remain and inclusion of an
Mehrabian and Epstein (1972) addressed this need in the development of the
also contains an emotional response for which there was no adequate instrument, but
whilst Hogan’s scale can be criticised for being purely cognitive in nature, the
have problems. Very little information is published about how items were
constructed, or the psychometric properties of the scale and the sample of individuals
used to develop it. However, the 33 final items were apparently selected as a result
of item analysis, validity analysis and a check of socially desirable responding. Each
from the data and showed no significant correlation with a measure of social
Chlopan et al. (1985) concluded that of six measures reviewed, only the QMEE and
Hogan’s Empathy Scale had sufficient research to support their use. This is despite
the fact that theoretically the two purport to measure different components of
empathy, illustrated by low correlations between the two measures (Davis, 1983).
model of the process of empathy. This has four distinct subscales and is based on the
rationale that “empathy can best be considered as a set of constructs, related in that
they all concern responsivity to others but are also clearly discriminable from each
other”(Davis, 1983, p.113). The four constructs measured by the scale are
perspective taking, empathic concern, fantasy and personal distress. Table 3.1
contains basic information regarding each of these subscales. Briefly, the Perspective
oneself in the role of characters in books, films or plays. The Empathic Concern
subscale asks about the individual’s own feelings of concern in response to another
person. The Personal Distress subscale is also emotional, but focuses on how much
one feels distress in response to another. According to Davis, while these four
subscales do not exhaust the possible range of reactions to others, previous theory
and research suggests that they reflect the variety of reactions to another that have at
To establish validity of the IRI and four subscales, Davis investigated relationships
between these and other psychological constructs. Findings from construct and
convergent validity studies provide support for the theoretical and psychometric
properties of the scale, with details of these investigations provided in Table 3.2.
Furthermore, given the theoretical accounts that the four subscales are related (Coke
et al., 1978; Hoffman, 1977), Davis argued that correlations between the subscales
would also provide evidence for the validity of the scale as a whole.
Although the IRI has proved a popular, reliable and valid instrument, based on a
was not designed specifically for the healthcare context. However, Yarnold, Bryant,
Nightingale and Martin (1996), in a study of 114 physicians and 95 medical students,
found the IRI to have good structural integrity and convergent validity with a
measure of problem solving in the context of concern for the well being of others.
Evans, Stanley and Burrows (1993) also undertook a study using the IRI to assess
five items from the 16-item History-taking Interview Rating Scale (Verby et al.,
1979) that assess behaviours relevant to empathy such as: eye contact; use of jargon;
and ability to detect leads in what the patient is saying. Scores on the IRI and the five
items measuring empathic behaviours were positively correlated, suggesting that the
measure has the potential for use specifically with healthcare professionals.
Eight measures of empathy have been reviewed in this chapter; five designed
specifically for healthcare professionals and three for the general population. The
five healthcare measures were: the ECRS, the empathy subscale of the BLRI, the
JSPE, the Reynolds Empathy Scale, and the CARE measure. Questions arose over
the reliability of the ECRS and BLRI, particularly as the BLRI was designed in a
counselling context and use outside this setting led to reduced reliability (Polit &
Hungler, 1983). The JSPE is limited by its basis in a pure cognitive definition of
communicated to patients. Whilst the more recent scales by Reynolds (2000) and
Mercer et al. (2004) have attempted to include a patient perspective, both suffer from
Target Model and definition Number of items and Response format and scoring
Population of empathy sample items
Nurses Cognitive/Behavioural: ‘Signifies a central focus and 100 item (e.g., ‘Listens carefully’, Self, peer and patient versions
feeling with and in the patient’s world. It involves ‘Checks to see if understanding of 6 point scale ‘extremely like’ to
accurate perception of the patient’s world by the helper, another’s experience is valid’) 20 ‘extremely unlike’
communication of this understanding to the patient, and item short version (La Monica, 1996) 46 negatively and 54 positively
the patient’s perception of the helper’s understanding’. worded items
Empathic understanding sub-test of the Barrett-Lennard Relation Inventory (BLRI: Barrett-Lennard, 1964)
Counselors Multidimensional: ‘To perceive the internal frame of 16 items (e.g., Usually senses and Self and patient versions
reference of another with accuracy, and with the realise what I am feeling - patient) 7 point scale ‘strong agreement’ to
emotional components...as if one were the other person ‘strong disagreement’
but without ever losing the ‘as if’ condition’ (Rogers, Half items negatively worded
1957). Extended to communicative aspects of empathy.
Medics Cognitive: ‘An uncritical understanding of the patient’s 20 items (e.g., I try to understand Self and patient versions
/general experiences, emotions, and feelings’. what is going on in my patients’ 7 point scale ‘strongly agree’ to
healthcare minds by paying attention to their ‘strongly disagree’
roles non-verbal cues and body language) Half items negatively worded
61
Nursing Cognitive /Behavioural: ‘Signifies a central focus and 12 items (e.g., ‘Attempts to explore Self, peer and patient versions
feeling with and in the patient’s world...’ from La Monica and clarify feelings’) 7 point scale. from ‘never like’ to
(1981). ‘always like’. Half items negatively
worded, reverse scored.
CARE Measure (CARE: Mercer et al., 2004)
General Cognitive / Behavioural: ‘Ability to: understand the 10 items (e.g., How was the doctor Patient ratings 5 point scale
healthcarepatient’s situation, perspective and feelings; to at… Making you feel at ease?) ‘poor’ to excellent’ plus ‘Does not
roles communicate that understanding and check its accuracy, apply’ No negatively worded items
and to act on that understanding with the patient in a
helpful way’.
Hogan Empathy Scale (HES: Hogan, 1969)
General Emotional: An involuntary vicarious 33 items (e.g., The people around me 9 point scale +4 (very strong
Population experience of another’s emotional state have a great influence on my moods) agreement) to -4 (very strong
disagreement). Total summed score
Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI: Davis, 1983)
General Multidimensional. 4 subscales: Perspective Taking, 28 items , 7 per scale (e.g., I 5 point scale from 0 (does not
Population Fantasy, Empathic Concern, Personal Distress sometimes try to understand my describe me well) to 4 (describes me
friends better by imagining how very well)
things look from their perspective)
62
Table 3.2: Reliability and validity information for existing empathy measures
Reliability Validity
ECRS (La Monica, 1981)
Cronbach’s α .97 for Form A, .98 for Form B. Construct: expected subscales not confirmed by factor analysis: single factor for self ratings (La
Split half reliability .89 for Form A, .96 for Monica, 1981). Convergent: large correlation with BLRI empathic understanding (r = .78, p<.001,
Form B. Peer ratings only, reliability of self Layton & Wykle, 1990); only small to moderate relationships between self, peer and patient ratings,
ratings not reported (La Monica, 1981). or self ratings and Chapin Social Insight Test (La Monica, 1981). Criterion: no significant changes
reported following empathy training (La Monica, 1981).
Broadly therefore, none of the specific healthcare measures possess both satisfactory
Similarly, among the general population measures the Hogan Empathy Scale and
psychometric properties for the IRI and Evans et al. (1993) used the IRI measure
positively with observed behaviour. Therefore the measure that appears to escape
Reactivity Index (IRI: Davis, 1983). More generally, the IRI questionnaire also has
Golombok, 1999) in beings useful for theory and hypothesis testing, and convenient
to use due to its brevity and usability for the participant (Coolican, 2009). Based on
this review of available measures, the IRI is therefore chosen as the main focus for
To summarise thus far, chapter one concluded by arguing that in order to identify the
with Larson and Yao’s (2005) adaptation of this model emphasising its potential
65
utility for the healthcare context. To date very little research has tested either model
very little is known about the individual characteristics that might impact on how
address these questions, this chapter reviewed a range of existing measures and
This thesis aims to further understanding of the antecedents and behaviours involved
empirical studies utilise the IRI in an attempt to answer these questions. Studies one
and two begin by investigating the individual differences that are the antecedents of
general population sample (n = 105). It utilises the IRI and the NEO Personality
the framework of the five factor model of personality (Costa & McCrae, 1992).
medical professionals and students (n = 297). The study examines the link between
individual differences in emotional intelligence and empathy, using the IRI and Bar-
66
propensity to empathise and observed empathic behaviour. The study uses a cross
sectional, quantitative methodology to test the relationships between the IRI, EQ-i
and ratings of behaviour by assessors and simulated patients. Finally, study four
examines the antecedents of empathy in the healthcare context and identifies the
qualitative research design, using semi structured interviews with twenty patients.
Study One
Individual differences in personality associated with a propensity to empathise
IRI and NEO PI-R, n=105, general population sample
Study Two
Individual differences in EI associated with a propensity to empathise
IRI and Bar-On EQ-in=297, general population and medical sample
Intrapersonal empathy
Empathic behaviour
Antecedents of empathy Reactive emotions
Helping behaviour
Practitioner characteristics (compassion and sympathy,
Social behaviour
Situational characteristics personal distress)
Conflict management
Perspective Taking
Study Three
Relationships between individual differences in propensity to empathise end empathic behaviour
IRI, EQ-i and ratings of behaviour (n=192, medical sample)
Fig 3.1: Framework for investigating empathy addressed by the studies within this thesis
68
Reactivity Index (IRI: 1983), with its strong theoretical background and good
Reactivity Index is now almost thirty years old and evidence of construct validity
that could support its use in the healthcare setting is limited, particularly in light of
including the five factor model (FFM) of personality. The aim of study one was
antecedents of empathy and, in doing so, to further investigate construct validity for
the IRI in terms of a broad spectrum five factor measure of personality. Before
reviewing previous evidence linking empathy to the FFM, the concept itself is
introduced.
thought, emotion and behaviour” (Funder, 1997, pp1-2) that are relatively stable
across time and situations. Traits can be quantitatively assessed, using measures of
69
personality that typically rely on self perceptions (McCrae & Costa, 1999). A
significant body of evidence now exists that supports claims that personality
descriptors can be accounted for almost completely by five robust factors (Digman,
1990). The five factors are: extraversion (or surgency); neuroticism (or emotional
empirical findings, suggests that traits are organised hierarchically with these five
factors representing the highest level of the hierarchy (1999). Despite the recent
dominance of the five factor model (McCrae & Costa, 1999), however, most
extraversion, neuroticism and psychoticism (e.g. Mehrabian, Young & Sato, 1988).
model of empathy. All three sources have used correlational analyses to investigate
and agreeableness.
anger (Costa & McCrae, 1999). As such, studies examining links between empathy
This research has found that emotional empathy is positively related to trait anxiety.
70
For example, in a study of 250 adult participants using the Questionnaire Measure
was found to have moderate positive correlations with neuroticism (r = .42, p<.05;
Rim, 1994). Similar findings were later reported by Eysenck and Eysenck (1978; r
= .35, p<.05) in a sample of adults and by Eysenck & McGurk (1980; r = .40,
In the IRI there are two subscales associated with emotional empathy: empathic
and concern for other people while personal distress refers to a tendency to
experience anxiety and unease in response to others’ distress. In Davis’ own initial
validation of the IRI (1983), several measures were used that relate to neuroticism,
including the public self consciousness and social anxiety subscales of the Self
Consciousness Scale (Fenigstein et al., 1975) and the Fearfulness scale of the
(Buss & Plomin, 1975). For both males and females, the two emotional subscales of
the IRI, personal distress and empathic concern, were found to be positively related
to these measures. This was explained by suggesting that emotional empathy and
trait anxiety both result from a higher level of emotional arousability (Eysenck,
1990). These results suggest that neuroticism will be positively correlated with the
two emotional subscales of the IRI, personal distress (Hypothesis 1a) and empathic
Habashi, Sheese & Tobin (2007) descriptors for the domain of agreeableness most
71
closely resemble those associated with empathy, because they include words such
as sympathetic, forgiving and helpful. Graziano and Eisenberg (1997) have also
validation of the IRI (Davis, 1983). The four subscales of the IRI were examined in
feelings. Davis also used the Masculinity Scale of the EPAQ, which contains
descriptors such as arrogant and boastful that are negatively associated with
and Empathic Concern subscales of the IRI. Based on these initial findings it was
positively associated with agreeableness, such that individuals who rate themselves
The third personality factor that has been found to relate to interpersonal
concerned with the degree to which an individual seeks out social contact rather
than the nature of specific interactions (Costa & McCrae, 1999), individuals who
seek out social contact may also show a greater tendency to empathise with others.
72
and extraversion have been mixed, however. For example, in a study using 250
Eysenck’s EPQ, while Klis (1997) used the same measures with a sample of
teachers and found no significant correlation. However, the sample size of this
undergraduates. Davis’ validation of the IRI also used several scales relating to the
domain of extraversion. These included shyness (Cheek and Buss, 1981), the
Masculinity Scale of the EPAQ (Spence et al., 1979) which contains descriptors
such as arrogant and boastful, and a scale of Extraversion composed of six items
from the Self Monitoring scale (Briggs, Cheek and Buss, 1980). The emotional
subscale of personal distress from the IRI was found to relate positively to shyness
findings relating to these scales and any of the other subscales of the IRI were
found for both males and females. Although less extensively studied, the picture is
similar for cognitive empathy and extraversion. Even with a small sample size of
79, Klis’s study (1997) found a significant positive correlation with extraversion (r
personality, which they defined as a level of analysis between the factor and facet
73
levels, arguing that extraversion could be meaningfully split into two such aspects:
regarding emotional empathy and extraversion are due to sample size, existing
studies have explored relationships between empathy and the five higher factors,
therefore that these mixed findings can be accounted for by the fact that that
emotional empathy is related to certain facets of extraversion rather than the entire
related to the prosocial concepts of empathy, such as warmth and positive emotions.
For the purposes of this study it was hypothesised that extraversion would be
positively associated with perspective taking (hypothesis 3a) and empathic concern
(hypothesis 3b) as these are the two components of empathy associated with social
relationships between the empathic dimensions identified by Davis and the more
fine grained facets of the FFM of personality and addresses the construct validity of
the IRI.
define the quality to be measured” (p.281). Chapter two presented the ongoing
debates regarding definitions and models of empathy were presented, and as such
further evidence of construct validity for the IRI would be useful. Evidence of
studies of the IRI is confirmatory factor analysis (e.g. Carey, 1988; Pulos, Ellison &
Lennon, 2004), which has provided evidence to support a four factor model in
general adult populations and healthcare workers (Carey, 1988; Pulos, Elison &
test the multidimensional model of empathy in relation to the five factor model of
4.3. Method
A total of 105 volunteers took part in the study, 86 of whom were psychology
Mean age of the 105 participants was 22.83 years (S.D 8.41 years) while 75.7%
were female. Two pencil and paper questionnaires were administered in person to
participants for this part of the study. These were the 240 item NEO Personality
Inventory – Revised (NEO PI-R: Cost & McCrae, 1992) and the 28 item IRI
4.3.2 Measures
Personality: NEO PI-R. The NEO PI-R (Cost & McCrae, 1992) is a measure of
each factor, a person’s traits are reflected in the fine grain ‘facets’. Each of the five
factors is made up of six facets, with eight items per facet. The study utilises a facet
level measure of the FFM to address the mixed findings from previous research
with respect to empathy and extraversion. The items within the questionnaire ask
the person to consider statements which are general rather than situation specific,
used for multiple purposes. For each of the 240 items, participants respond to a
brief description of the five factors is provided in Table 4.1, together with the facets
associated with each factor. According to the manual, each factor has acceptable
internal reliability, with αs ranging from .86 to .95. Facets have slightly lower
reliability statistics (αs from .56 to .81) which are deemed acceptable for scales
(IRI; Davis, 1983) has been described extensively in Chapter Three. the Perspective
psychological point of view of another with a sample item being “I sometimes try
imagining oneself in the role of characters in books, films or plays, with a sample
item being “I really get involved with the feelings of the characters in a novel”. The
Empathic Concern subscale asks about the individual’s own feelings of concern in
misfortunes do not usually disturb me a great deal”. The Personal Distress subscale
is also emotional, but is more self-oriented, focusing on how much one feels
distress in response to another. A sample item from this subscale is “I tend to lose
control during emergencies”. The four subscales are each composed of seven items,
(‘does not describe me well’ to ‘describes me very well’). All four sub-scales of the
IRI have been shown to have satisfactory internal reliability (α = 0.71 to 0.77) and
test – retest reliability (α = 0.62 to 0.71, Davis, 1983). A 0-4 scale is used for each
item, so the minimum possible score for each subscale is zero, with a maximum of
28 for each subscale. The scores are not intended to be combined, as each
4.3.3 Analysis
The hypotheses were tested by calculating subscale scores for each subscale of the
IRI and domains and facets of the NEO PI-R. Correlations were calculated using
the domain scores of the NEO PI-R and the IRI subscales. A joint factor analysis
empathy and the facets scores of personality measure. A joint factor analysis was
deemed appropriate for this because it avoids the risk of type I errors that are
4.4. Results
Descriptive statistics for the NEO PI-R facets and the four IRI subscales are
provided in Table 4.2. For this sample of participants, reliabilities were comparable
for the NEO PI-R facet subscales and .69 to .80 for the IRI subscales. As
Cronbach’s α is sensitive to the number of items in the scale, these were deemed
acceptable reliabilities for the numbers of items within each scale. For shorter
78
Table 4.2: Descriptive statistics for the NEO PI-R and Empathy (IRI) scales.
Note: N=105 for all subscales. NEO PI-R facet scales all have 8 items each with a minimum score
of 5 and a maximum of 25, IRI scales have 7 items each with a minimum score of 0 and a maximum
of 28.
79
reliability (Briggs & Cheek, 1986). For all scales, mics fell within the range .18 to
4.4.1 Correlations
To test hypotheses 1-3, first the correlations between the four empathy subscales
and five personality factors were calculated (see Table 4.3). A multi-stage adjusted
level of significance was adopted to control the family-wise error rate (Howell,
Hypothesis one: Neuroticism and empathy. Support was not found for hypothesis
1b, which predicted that a significant positive relationship would be found between
empathic concern, and neuroticism (r = .07, ns). However support was found for
positive relationships with agreeableness. This was found to be the case (r = .38,
Table 4.3: Correlations between NEO PI-R factors and Empathy (IRI) scales.
Empathy
Perspective Empathic Personal
NEO PI-R Fantasy
Taking Concern Distress
Neuroticism (N) .13 -.33** .07 .47**
Hypothesis three: Extraversion and empathy. Support was found for hypotheses 3a
extraversion and both perspective taking and empathic concern (r = .35, p<.01 and r
between the IRI and openness to experience, this domain score showed a
significant, large positive correlation with the IRI Fantasy subscale (r = .54, p<.01).
(r = -.30, p<.05).
Correlations were only calculated between the four IRI subscales and the five
domain scores for the NEO PI-R. To produce a correlation matrix at a facet level
81
would involve a very large number of tests and applying a correction for this would
result in very few interpretable results. However, Ferguson (2001) notes that joint
factor analysis (i.e. including subscale scores from the IRI and facet scores from the
the simultaneous analysis allows for understanding of the IRI subscales within the
framework of the five factor model. This approach was followed to undertake a
facet level analysis of relationships between empathy and personality. Several pre-
Pre-analysis checks: To ensure that the IRI subscale scores and NEO PI-R facet
scores were suitable for an exploratory factor analysis, the data were checked
following Ferguson and Cox (1993). These checks include: examining that a stable
factor structure can be produced; that the variables are appropriately scaled and
distributed, and that there is systematic covariation within the data. In order to
determine the sample size required to produce a stable factor structure, statisticians
suggest a range of heuristics. Guadagnoli and Velicer (1988) reviewed these and
suggested that absolute sample size was the most important, as well as the
component saturation. The absolute sample size here of 105 participants is more
than the minimum of 100 suggested by Kline (1994). If the component saturation
(mean factor loading for a factor) is greater than 0.6, then according to Guadagnoli
and Velicer, increasing sample size will be of little value. This can only be
evaluated post-analysis and so this point will be returned to following the factor
analysis. Finally, skew and kurtosis of the variables were then explored. Muthen
and Kaplan (1985) identified three parameters as important when checking skew
and kurtosis. These were: the absolute magnitude of skew; the number of variables
82
affected by skew, and the proportion of the initial correlations between variables
less than 0.2. None of the variables had skew of a magnitude of 2.0 (the modesty
subscale of the NEO PI-R demonstrated the greatest skew of -1.17 and was the only
variable to have a kurtosis value of more than 2.0: actual value 2.66).
Field (2005) also recommends calculating z scores for skew and kurtosis, with a
value of more than 1.96 marking significant skew or kurtosis. Using this method,
only four of the 34 variables were significantly skewed. These were from the IRI,
fantasy (z = -2.02) and empathic concern (z = -2.07) and from the NEO, excitement
seeking (z = 3.09) and modesty (z = 4.98). Only one variable showed significant
skew and/or kurtosis. According to Ferguson and Cox (1993), the cut off point for
acceptability is 25%. As only four out of 34 (12%) of the variables here were
affected, this should not adversely affect the solution. In addition, upon inspection
of the correlation matrix, 340 of a possible 561 (60.61%) correlations between all
34 variables were lower than 0.2. If the majority (more than 60%) of variables are
correlated less than 0.2, all variables can remain in the analysis regardless of skew
or kurtosis (Ferguson & Cox, 1993; Muthen & Kaplan, 1985). Therefore despite
there being some skew and kurtosis in four of the 34 variables, all were kept in this
analysis.
The final statistics recommended for inspection by Ferguson and Cox (1993) before
test of sampling adequacy and Bartlett’s test of sphericity. The Bartlett’s test of
sphericity should be significant to indicate that relationships are present within the
data. A KMO statistic of at least 0.5 is required to indicate that the relationships
83
(Dzubian & Shirkey, 1974). For this data, the KMO was found to be 0.73 and
Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (2176.38, p<.0001). The data were
Factor extraction. The next stage is to choose a method for extracting factors
(Ferguson & Cox, 1993). The most widely used method is the Kaiser 1 (K1)
method, where all factors with eigenvalues greater than one are retained. A
principal components analysis using this method found seven factors with
factor extraction, Zwick and Velicer (1986) concluded that this method leads to the
retention of too many factors. Instead the method of parallel analysis (Horn, 1965)
of eigenvalues, based on the same sample size, with those produced in the observed
data. A number of such randomly generated values are run and the average
eigenvalues are calculated. These average values and the observed values are then
plotted against the number of variables. The number of factors to retain is identified
immediately prior to the point where the two lines cross (Zwick & Velicer, 1986).
This method has been shown to be the most accurate method of factor extraction
and was therefore used here. Using syntax from O’Connor (2000), four analyses
were conducted at the 50th and 95th percentiles using 40 and 100 sets of randomly
generated data. The parallel analysis indicated that five factors should be retained.
Five factors were extracted using principal components analysis (Tabachnick &
Table 4.4. Factor loadings for joint factor analysis of IRI subscales and NEO PI-R
facets
Component
Factor
1 2 3 4 5
1 Anxiety (N) 0.83 -0.01 -0.01 0.07 -0.01
Depression (N) 0.80 0.06 -0.21 -0.27 0.04
Vulnerability (N) 0.75 0.37 -0.10 -0.27 -0.11
Angry-Hostility (N) 0.72 -0.46 -0.17 0.00 -0.04
Self-Consciousness (N) 0.70 0.17 -0.27 -0.10 0.06
Personal Distress 0.54 0.31 0.05 -0.20 -0.29
Impulsivity (N) 0.52 -0.41 0.05 -0.32 0.17
Openness to Actions (O) -0.42 -0.19 0.18 -0.22 0.40
2 Compliance (A) -0.31 0.77 0.13 -0.06 0.06
Straightforwardness (A) 0.00 0.73 0.21 0.02 -0.05
Assertiveness (E) -0.29 -0.69 0.22 0.09 0.14
Modesty (A) 0.28 0.59 0.22 0.07 -0.02
Activity (E) -0.05 -0.49 0.22 0.07 0.14
Tender-mindedness (A) 0.13 0.47 0.34 -0.04 0.18
3 Warmth (E) -0.31 0.06 0.79 -0.02 0.19
Positive Emotions (E) -0.36 -0.01 0.73 0.01 0.17
Empathic Concern 0.24 0.06 0.67 0.04 0.17
Altruism (A) -0.01 0.37 0.66 0.17 0.13
Gregariousness (E) -0.23 -0.17 0.64 -0.33 -0.12
Perspective Taking -0.29 0.15 0.53 0.11 0.11
Trust (A) -0.46 0.43 0.47 0.05 0.07
4 Self-Discipline (C) -0.24 -0.23 0.20 0.78 -0.12
Dutifulness (C) -0.07 0.30 0.07 0.74 0.04
Achievement Striving (C) -0.13 -0.34 0.03 0.73 0.04
Competence (C) -0.36 -0.24 0.06 0.67 0.10
Order (C) 0.20 -0.08 -0.03 0.67 -0.17
Deliberation (C) -0.18 0.27 -0.05 0.65 -0.10
Excitement-Seeking (E) -0.08 -0.38 0.38 -0.52 0.11
5 Openness to Ideas (O) -0.15 -0.29 0.07 0.11 0.73
Openness to Aesthetics (O) -0.15 -0.10 0.20 0.00 0.67
Fantasy 0.19 0.02 0.22 -0.07 0.66
Openness to Fantasy (O) 0.06 0.05 0.11 -0.28 0.65
Openness to Feelings (O) 0.32 -0.04 0.45 0.14 0.60
Openness to Values (O) -0.18 0.20 -0.14 -0.06 0.57
Note: IRI subscales and factor loadings of 0.30 and greater are in boldface.
85
Factor extraction using an oblique rotation was also performed, with five factors
being extracted. Factors two and three found to have a correlation coefficient of
0.24. Other than this, no factors showed substantial correlations and the pattern
matrix showed a highly similar solution to that of the varimax rotation. For
The five factors extracted (eigenvalues = 6.72, 4.51, 4.28, 2.89, 1.94) accounted for
59.81% of the variance. Rotated factor loadings are presented in Table 4.4. The
acceptable magnitude of a factor loading for a variable to define a factor varies, but
the most commonly accepted level is 0.30 (e.g., Field, 2005). As mentioned
previously, factor saturation is defined as the mean factor loading for a factor: these
ranged from .62 to .73 for the five factors extracted in this analysis. As all were
found to be greater than 0.6, then according to Guadagnoli and Velicer (1988),
increasing sample size would be of little value. Therefore although the sample size
was relatively small (105), this was deemed to be sufficient for this analysis.
Interpretation of factors. Factor one contained all of the neuroticism facets of the
NEO PI-R. In addition, one of the openness facets (to actions) had a negative
loading on this factor but also cross-loaded positively on to Factor five with a
similar magnitude. This factor also included the Personal Distress subscale of the
IRI, with a factor loading of .54, suggesting that the tendency to become distressed
Factors two and three both contained a mix of facets from the extraversion and
perspective taking, both loaded positively on to Factor three with factor loadings of
.67 and .53 respectively. Factor Three also contains the Warmth, Positive Emotions,
Altruism, Gregariousness and Trust facets of the NEO PI-R. The Openness to
Feelings facet of openness also cross-loaded positively with this factor. This
Factor Four contained all of the conscientiousness facets of the NEO PI-R along
with one extraversion facet: excitement seeking negatively loaded on to this factor.
None of the IRI subscales loaded significantly on to this factor. Finally, Factor five
was made up of the remaining five openness facets. The Fantasy subscale of the IRI
4.5. Discussion
regarding neuroticism and empathy. Support was not found for hypothesis 1b,
empathic concern, and neuroticism. However support was found for hypothesis 1a
as there was a significant large positive correlation between personal distress and
empathy. Hypotheses 2a and 2b were both supported, with perspective taking and
positive relationships were found between extraversion and both perspective taking
between the IRI and openness to experience, this domain was found to be positively
correlated with fantasy and negatively correlated with personal distress. The joint
factor analysis provided a deeper, facet level understanding of the results for
perspective taking and empathic concern. Clear links were established between
these subscales and a single factor comprising a range of facets of Extraversion and
cross loaded on to this factor. Personal distress was found to load positively on to
experience.
differences and more specifically the five factor model of personality. In order to do
this, the relationships between each subscale of the IRI and the facets of the NEO
PI-R have been investigated. Similar patterns of results were found for two of the
the five factor model. These two subscales are therefore discussed together,
followed by findings for Personal Distress and then the Fantasy subscale which
The Perspective Taking and Empathic Concern subscales were both found to
factor analysis at the facet level has provided a greater understanding of these
relationships. It should be noted that it was not entirely unexpected to find that the
facets from extraversion and agreeableness appeared to form two new composite
factors in this analysis (Factors two and three). Other analyses have repeatedly
found these two factors to be related in their impact upon relationships and social
functioning (e.g., Noftle & Shaver, 2006; White, Hendrick & Hendrick, 2004).
Further evidence from Trapnell and Wiggins (1990) and McCrae and Costa (1989)
explains that these two factors comprise interpersonal aspects of personality and
that a circumplex approach combining the two factors complements the five factor
model. The two composite factors which emerged are in line with Mehrabian’s
dominance. The two empathy subscales did not load significantly on to this factor.
dimension. This was the factor onto which the subscales of Perspective Taking and
Young, Quilty and Peterson (2007) identified ‘aspects’ of personality which they
defined as a level of analysis between the factor and facet levels. They found that
both the extraversion and agreeableness factors could be meaningfully split into
two such aspects. The extraversion aspects were named ‘enthusiasm’ and
‘politeness’. In line with their analysis, the two subscales of empathy were related
to the aspects of enthusiasm and compassion from De Young et al. (2007). These
findings suggest that an empathic person with a tendency for perspective taking and
empathic concern also reports being warm, caring, outgoing, compassionate and
trusting.
relationships with the other trait-based measure. Those relationships were in line
support for the construct validity of the two subscales as part of a measure of
empathy. Furthermore, the findings also provide evidence to include both cognitive
Vreeke & van der Mark, 2003). Researchers arguing for one or the other may well
be making artificial distinctions (Preston & de Waal, 2002). However, the subscale
Shared affect, the process which is the usual topic of focus within the empathy
literature, is not captured by this subscale. The results of the analysis involving the
Personal Distress subscale are therefore relevant at this point. This subscale showed
very different relationships with the traits measured in this study, which are now
discussed.
90
The Personal Distress subscale of the IRI showed a significant and moderate
positive correlation with the Neuroticism scale of the NEO PI-R. In addition, a
moderate significant negative correlation was found with Openness. Two moderate
significance once a correction had been applied to reduce the risk of Type I error.
Personal Distress was found to load positively on to the factor composed of all the
facets of Neuroticism. This factor also contained one facet of Openness, namely
Openness to Actions. The findings are in line with previous research that suggests
underpins the emotional empathic response (Jabbi, Swart & Keysers, 2007). The
pattern of relationships found in this study suggest that people who become
related validity of the IRI within the healthcare context. Before considering this
further, the results of the final section of the analysis require discussion.
4.5.3 Fantasy
The final subscale of the IRI, Fantasy, showed a different pattern of relationships to
any of the other subscales. In terms of its relationship to the five factor model,
positively on to the fifth factor of the joint factor analysis, which all of the
Openness facets also loaded positively on to. This suggests that those people who
score highly on items in the Fantasy subscale concerning imagination and fantasy
of thought are more open to new experiences in general. There is little previous
research to explain this finding as the relationship between empathy and openness
had not previously been explored. The Fantasy subscale was originally included in
a link between fantasy and greater emotional responding to the emotions of others
was provided by Stotland et al., (1978), but in this study there was no significant
correlation between this subscale and Neuroticism. The factor loading of the
magnitude of .19. This suggests that for this sample of participants, self reported
negative emotionality did not clearly relate to Fantasy. However, the Openness to
Experience factor did positively correlate with Fantasy (r = .54, p<.01), perspective
taking and empathic concern (although non significantly with Pearson’s r being .24
and .25 respectively). It may be that being open to new ideas, feelings and more
imaginative generally helps one to imagine the experience of another person, thus
particularly relevant when encountering new people with whom one has not yet
therefore be useful in this context. However, there are those who have used the IRI
without the Fantasy subscale, arguing for its lack of relevance to the topic of
research (e.g., Christopher, Owens & Stecker, 1993). It will therefore be important
The study makes a useful contribution in that it constitutes the first known
comparison of the IRI with a broad five factor measure of personality, however,
whilst the factor saturations suggested that sample size would not have altered the
findings, the sample was relatively small size for this type of research (Field, 2005).
More importantly, given that the aim of this thesis is to investigate processes of
from the healthcare professions and demonstrate the utility of the IRI in this
since the construction of the IRI. The last chapter focused on empathy within the
Salovey & Mayer, 1990). Empathy and EI share common theoretical and research
roots in social intelligence (Chlopan et al., 1985; Landy, 2005), but despite
rather than investigate relationships between the two empirically, and the terms
with the concept of EI. In addition, as the previous study used a sample of
participants from the general population and this thesis aims to further
investigate self-assessed empathy using the IRI among doctors currently employed
in the NHS. Before considering the relationships between empathy and EI, the
Emotional intelligence [EI] has received considerable attention in both popular and
academic literatures in recent years (e.g., Petrides & Furnham, 2000). Although
many different definitions have been proposed, a review by Ciarrochi, Chan and
Caputi (2000) identified four aspects that are included by most definitions of EI:
utilisation. These areas relate to emotions in both the self and others, for example
regulation of one’s own emotions in coping with stress and understanding of others
The four aspects are similar across the definitions of many researchers such as
Davies, Stankov and Roberts (1998), Mayer, Caruso and Salovey (2000), and Law,
Wong and Song (2004). However, as in the empathy literature, debates continue
over definitions of EI. In fact, Pérez, Petrides and Furnham (2005) have argued that
whilst most studies of EI are carried out in a “theoretical vacuum” (p.182), this
does not appear to have hampered the development of measures of EI that are now
conceptualised as a trait, an ability, or a mix of the two (Pérez, Petrides & Furnham,
“the subset of intelligence that involves the ability to monitor one’s own and
others’ feelings and emotions, to discriminate among them and to use this
information to guide one’s thinking and actions” (p.189). In line with this, a ‘test’
of EI has been developed, the Mayer, Salovey & Caruso Emotional Intelligence
95
Test (MSCEIT; Mayer Salovey & Caruso, 2002). However, in a review of evidence
pertaining to trait or ability EI, Pérez, Petrides and Furnham (2005) argue that such
correct response has either been determined through consensus or expert scoring,
trait.
Petrides and Furnham (2001) emphasise the importance of the distinction between
performance for traits. Petrides, Perez-Gonzalez & Furnham, (2007) argue that EI is
which accord with the subjective nature of emotions” (p, 274). One of the most
commonly used self-report measures, which has received support from empirical
studies, is the Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i: Bar-On, 1997). Bar-On
pressures” (p.14). Although it has been claimed that the Bar-On EQ-i is a ‘mixed’
model of EI, assessing both abilities and preferences (e.g. Bar-On, 1997; Brackett
& Mayer, 2003), the self-report nature of the assessment aligns itself more closely
with a trait approach. Indeed, construct validation studies have been successful in
finding relationships between this measure and the five factor model of personality,
indicating that it can be viewed as a trait-based measure (e.g O’Connor & Little,
2003; Petrides & Furnham, 2001). The Bar-On EQ-i comprises fifteen different
subscales, scores from which are then combined into five composite factors:
96
mood. The exact mapping of the fifteen subscales to the five composite factors is
Regarding construct validity of the EQ-i, the factor structure of the EQ-i has been
questioned. For example, the Bar-On EQ-i manual does not find support for the five
factors, instead reporting thirteen. However the method used for extracting factors
was the K1 method retaining all factors with eigenvalues greater than one. This
method has been found to lead to the retention of too many factors (Zwick &
Velicer, 1986). Using confirmatory factor analysis, Petrides and Furnham (2001)
found a single factor to be a better fit to data for a sample of 227 working adults.
Despite this, other evidence regarding the reliability and criterion-related validity of
the measure is positive (e.g. Dawda & Hart, 2000; O’Connor & Little, 2003). It is
therefore the measure used within this study. Having introduced the concept of EI,
1985; Landy, 2005), the two terms are often referred to interchangeably. Salovey
and Mayer argue that empathy is a component of EI (1990; Mayer & Salovey,
1997). Empathy is also named as a subscale of the EQ-i (Bar-On, 1997) and the
Table 5.1: Description of Bar-On EQ-i five factors and fifteen subscales
measures they used were in fact empathy measures – the QMEE and IRI, not EI
measures – a fact that was not acknowledged. The ability based measure of EI
and not the trait measures (of empathy) were found to predict performance on the
video-based task, and the authors interpret this finding as evidence that EI is
QMEE or the IRI do not assess emotion detection per se, but encompass a
provide useful evidence of construct validity of the different IRI subscales. The
two concepts show overlap in definitions and measures and should therefore be
related. There are a small number of studies that have conducted correlational
analyses between measures of empathy as measured by the IRI and EI and shown
Perspective Taking and EI. The empathy subscale of the TEIQue (Petrides,
taking, implying that this is the key dimension of empathy relating to EI.
with a mean age of 14 years. Perspective taking was found to have a moderate
Schutte et al., 1998). Schutte et al., (2001) also replicated this finding with two
samples of adults using the same measures. This was true for both a sample of 24
students (r = .35, p<.05) and 37 teaching interns (r = .59, p<.001). However, the
teaching interns were not given other subscales of the IRI and the sample size is
small, therefore generalisations from the findings of these studies are not
possible. Stratton, Elam, Murphy-Spencer and Quinlivan (2005) used the Trait
Meta Mood Scale of EI (TMMS: Salovey et al., 1995) with 165 medical students.
clarity of feelings (CF), and mood repair (MR). Perspective taking was found to
all p<.05). Therefore hypothesis 1 states that perspective taking will be positively
Empathic concern and EI. Barchard (2003) attempted to assess the predictive
measures of EI. One of the measures chosen was the empathic concern subscale
of the IRI. All of the measures were used in a regression as separate predictors of
concern and the other EI measures. Rather, it was assumed that empathic concern
represents EI. In their study of medical students, Stratton et al. (2005) found
positive relationships between the three TMMS subscales and empathic concern
(AF r = .71; CF r = .30; MR r = .49, all p<.05). Charbonneau and Nicol (2002)
also found moderate positive correlations between empathic concern and EI for a
100
Fantasy and EI. The relationship between fantasy and EI is perhaps less clear on
Stratton et al. (2005) did not administer this subscale of the IRI to the students,
although no reason was given for this. Charbonneau and Nicol (2002) did find
moderate positive correlations between fantasy and EI with the sample of 191
adolescents. (TMEI: Schutte et al., 1998). Therefore hypothesis three states that
social functioning, and trait-based measures such as the EQ-i have been
Petrides & Furnham, 2001). Evidence has been presented in the previous chapter
reported significant negative correlations between personal distress and the three
all p<.05). Hypothesis four is therefore that personal distress will be negatively
intelligence, this study will aim to provide further construct validity evidence for
101
intelligence. As the few other studies reported in this section have only used
5.3 Method
5.3.1 Participants
Data were collected from two sources for the study. Two hundred and fifty six
attend assessment centres conducted over a one week period. On arrival at the
purpose of the research study and applicants were assured that any information
from the psychometric questionnaires used for the research would not be made
available to those making selection decisions and as such the research did not
form part of the selection process. Consent forms were signed by all participants
and 3 for the information sheet and consent form). Out of 256 applicants
attending the assessment centres, 192 doctors agreed to take part in the study and
completed the measures detailed below. Data were also collected from 105
students, to increase the sample size to an adequate number. The larger sample
size was required as a decision was taken to conduct an item level factor analysis
102
of the emotional intelligence measure. The factor structure of the Bar-On EQ-i
has been a subject of debate (e.g. Petrides & Furnham, 2001) and currently there
is no clear agreement, therefore a factor structure for this sample was established.
Due to the high number of items (133), a large sample size was required. The
total sample for this study therefore comprised 297 participants. 59.1% of the
sample was female, with a mean age of 22.83 years (S.D 8.41 years).
Participants were administered two pencil and paper questionnaires for this part
of the study. These were the 133 item Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory
(EQ-i: Bar-On, 1997) and the 28 item IRI (Davis, 1983). No time limit was set.
(IRI; Davis, 1983) has been described extensively in Chapter Three. the
tendency to adopt the psychological point of view of another with a sample item
look from their perspective”. The Fantasy subscale is similar to this, although is
based on imagining oneself in the role of characters in books, films or plays, with
a sample item being “I really get involved with the feelings of the characters in a
novel”. The Empathic Concern subscale asks about the individual’s own feelings
on how much one feels distress in response to another. A sample item from this
subscale is “I tend to lose control during emergencies”. The four subscales are
each composed of seven items, to which participants are asked to respond using a
well’). All four sub-scales of the IRI have been shown to have satisfactory
internal reliability (α = 0.71 to 0.77) and test – retest reliability (α = 0.62 to 0.71,
Davis, 1983). A 0-4 scale is used for each item, so the minimum possible score
for each subscale is zero, with a maximum of 28 for each subscale. The scores
is included in Appendix 1.
Emotional Intelligence: the EQ-i (Bar-On, 1997). The Bar-On EQ-i (1997) is
factors: (a) intrapersonal EQ, (b) interpersonal EQ, (c) adaptability (d) stress
factor. Respondents are asked to rate 133 items using a five-point likert scale (1
= ‘Very seldom or not true of me’ to 5 = ‘Very often true or true of me’). Only
117 of the 133 items relate to the five composite factors, the remaining 16 items
acting as ‘validity indicators’. Reliability coefficients for the five subscales range
104
from α = 0.69 to 0.86 (Bar-On, 1997). Independent studies have also provided
support for the validity and reliability of the measure (e.g. Dawda & Hart, 2000).
EQ-i item level factor analysis. Although the manual for the EQ-i provides
information regarding the five composite factors, the decision was taken here to
undertake an exploratory factor analysis of the EQ-i. Two reasons are given for
this decision. First, 12 of the 117 items are used to calculate scores for more than
one of the subscales. This is not uncommon within psychology (e.g., the
Ward, 2006) but suggests that a more parsimonious model might be in order.
Secondly, the factor structure of the EQ-i is a subject of debate. For example, the
manual for the measure produces a 13 factor solution rather than five factors
(Bar-On, 1997), while Petrides and Furnham (2001) found a unifactorial solution
to be a better fit to the data. Therefore an item level factor analysis of the EQ-i
was conducted to determine the factors to use in the joint factor analysis with the
IRI. This process is described before reporting the main analysis to test the
hypotheses.
Before conducting any analysis on the data from the EQ-i, 16 of the 133 items
were removed. These items are known as ‘validity indicators’, designed to detect
themselves. While they may be a result of a lack of self awareness or issues with
self esteem, which may be related to one’s emotional intelligence, these scales do
105
not form part of the emotional intelligence score (Bar-On, 1997) and so have
been removed from this analysis. A reliability analysis following guidance from
The reliability analysis started with a full item analysis of the remaining 117
items. Item facilities were inspected to check that the items were able to
differentiate between respondents. Any items with a mean of less than 0.50 or
more than 4.50, or with an acceptable mean but a standard deviation of less than
0.75 were deemed to have insufficient facility indices and so were removed. Four
such items were removed. Next, item discrimination was checked to see if the
item-total correlation of less than 0.2 were removed as these would not appear to
an item-total correlation of more than 0.8, suggesting that there was no issue of
multicollinearity.
Having removed the 16 ‘validity indicators’ and ten items which did not meet the
criteria of the item analysis, 107 items remained in the scale. Exploratory factor
analysis was then conducted to determine how these items would form the
subscales. A similar procedure to that of study one was followed. Again this
scale and distribution and also that there was systematic covariation within the
The absolute sample size of 297 participants is more than the minimum of 100
suggested by Kline (1994). In terms of skew and kurtosis, no items had a skew of
more than 2.0 and only 4 items had a kurtosis value greater than 2.0. This was
within the acceptable limit of 25% of items suggested by Ferguson and Cox
(1993), in line with Muthen and Kaplan’s parameters (1985). The solution should
suggested that there were discoverable relationships within the data and so
Factors were then extracted, with parallel analysis determining the number of
factors to extract (Horn, 1965; Zwick & Velicer, 1986). Analyses were
conducted at the 50th and 95th percentiles using 40 and 100 sets of randomly
generated data. The parallel analysis indicated that eight factors should be
(Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007) using an oblimin rotation with delta set at 01. This
rotation was used as the factors were anticipated to be intercorrelated, which was
found to be the case (Field, 2005). The eight factors extracted (eigenvalues
ranged from 25.34 to 2.09) accounted for 45.44% of the variance. Eight items of
1
Rotations with delta set at 1 and 2 were found to produce similar solutions.
107
Table 5.2: Factor names, descriptions, means, S.D.s and reliabilities for eight subscales resulting from EQ-i factor analysis
Original EQ-i subscale Description and sample items N.
New Factor Name M S.D α
Items
Self esteem Self contentment, Having a positive self regard e.g. I have good self respect; I don't 21 3.47 .68 .94
enjoyment feel good about myself (-)
Self control Reality testing, impulse Having some control over one’s thoughts and behaviour e.g. I 11 3.37 .63 .78
control have strong impulses that are hard to control (-); I think its
important to be a law abiding citizen
Flexibility Flexibility Openness to change and adjustment e.g. its hard for me to change 10 3.30 .64 .80
my ways (-); I'm able to change old habits
Rationality Problem solving, stress Taking a logical and reasoned approach e.g. when trying to solve a 15 3.41 .56 .82
tolerance problem I look at each possibility and then decide on the best
way; I try to see things as they really are, without fantasizing or
daydreaming about them
Emotional Stress tolerance, anger Keeping control over one’s emotions e.g. I feel that its hard for 11 3.62 .75 .83
Regulation control me to control my anxiety (-); I can handle stress without getting
too nervous
Interpersonal Interpersonal Showing interpersonal respect and sensitivity e.g. I'm sensitive to 12 3.44 .59 .81
Sensitivity relationships, empathy, the feelings of others; I avoid hurting other people's feelings
social responsibility
Emotional Emotional self awareness Expression of one’s feelings e.g. it's fairly easy for me to express 10 3.47 .73 .84
Expression feelings; I'm unable to show affection (-)
Assertiveness Assertiveness/ Demonstrating resolve and decisiveness e.g. when I disagree with 17 4.08 .45 .86
Independence someone, I'm able to say so; Others think that I lack assertiveness
(-)
108
These items have been kept in the analysis as their removal has the effect of
Appendix five. Mean factor loadings for this solution ranged from 0.32 to 0.54,
suggesting that component saturation is an issue and increasing the sample size
may be of benefit in future (Guadagnoli & Velicer, 1988). This was the primary
reason for using all of the data available in a single analysis. However, as the
For copyright reasons, the lists of items loading on to each of the eight
interpretation and naming. Reliabilities and scores for the eight subscales were
then created in accordance with this factor analysis, details of which are provided
in Table 5.2. This table also shows the original EQ-i subscales that go into each
factor. The only subscale which was split over two of the new factors was stress
tolerance. Some of the items from this subscale loaded on to the rationality
factor, while others loaded on to emotional regulation. Apart from this, the new
factors were broadly represented by items from one or more of the original
subscales.
5.4 Results
To increase sample size for analysis, extra data were gathered from a sample of
To check that combination of the data was appropriate, the two groups were
compared on the subscales of both the EQ-i and the IRI. A MANOVA revealed
109
(see Table 5.3) did reveal a significant main effect of group for two of the
subscales, with doctors reporting higher scores for emotional regulation and
empathic concern. However, these effect sizes were small (partial eta squared =
.01 in both cases). Therefore the two groups were deemed suitable to combine.
5.4.1 Correlations
Using the newly created subscale scores for the EQ-i, and the four subscale
scores from the IRI, correlations and joint factor analysis were conducted to test
Empathy
EQ-i F EC PT PD
Self esteem -.11 .22** .37** -.38**
Note: Larzalere and Mulaik (1977) adjusted *significant p<.05, ** p<.01. Empathy (IRI) scales:
F = Fantasy, EC = Empathic Concern, PT = Perspective Taking, PD = Personal Distress.
Hypothesis one: Perspective Taking and EI. Support was found for hypothesis
and all eight EQ-i subscales. Pearson’s r ranged from .29 to .48 (all p<.01).
Hypothesis two: Empathic Concern and EI. Partial support was found for
empathic concern and six of the eight EQ-i subscales. Pearson’s r ranged from
.20 (p<.05) to .45 ( p<.01). The larger correlations were reported for
Hypothesis three: Fantasy and EI. Support was not found for hypothesis three as
no significant positive relationships were reported between fantasy and the EQ-i
subscales. In fact, one significant moderate negative correlation was reported for
the subscale self control (r = -.34, p<.01). Self control was described in Table 5.2
111
as ‘having some control over one’s thoughts and behaviour’ as opposed to the
Hypothesis four: Personal Distress and EI. Support was found for hypothesis four
all eight EQ-i subscales. P Pearson’s r ranged from -.21 (p<.05) with
To further understand the relationships between the IRI and EQ-i, a second joint
factor analysis of the two was conducted using the four IRI subscale scores and
eight EQ-i subscale scores. Using exactly the same procedure as for the joint
factor analysis in the previous study, pre-analysis checks were carried out to
check that a stable factor structure could be produced; that the variables are
within the data (Ferguson & Cox, 1993). The absolute sample size here of 297
participants is again suitable (Kline, 1994). Skew and kurtosis of the variables
adequacy was found to be 0.89 and Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant
(1556.10, p<.0001). The data were therefore deemed suitable for exploratory
factor analysis.
112
Using parallel analysis, four analyses were conducted at the 50th and 95th
percentiles using 40 and 100 sets of randomly generated data. The parallel
analysis indicated that two factors should be retained. However the two factor
solution produced cross loadings for six of the 12 variables in the analysis, with
only two of the variables loading primarily on to Factor 2. These two variables
were from the IRI (EC and F). Instead, a single factor solution was inspected and
accepted as making more theoretical sense. The single factor was extracted using
of 5.57, the factor accounted for 46.4% of the variance. Component saturation of
this single factor was 0.59. Factor loading are presented in Table 5.5.
Factor Loadings
Rationality .84
Self Esteem .84
Flexibility .78
Emotion regulation .78
Interpersonal sensitivity .75
Assertiveness .74
Emotional Expression .74
Self control .69
Perspective Taking [IRI] .58
Personal Distress [IRI] -.56
Empathic concern [IRI] .35
Fantasy [IRI] -.17
113
In support of hypotheses one, two and four, this analysis suggests that the eight
associated with Personal Distress. Not in support of hypothesis three, the fantasy
5.5 Discussion
This study has investigated the relationship between each of subscale of the IRI
and emotional intelligence. Perspective taking and empathic concern were found
fantasy broadly unrelated to EI. Similar patterns of results were found for two of
relationships with EI. These are discussed together, followed by findings for
Personal Distress and then the Fantasy subscale which, as in Chapter four,
The joint factor analysis with the emotional intelligence measure found that
single factor of emotional intelligence, suggesting that people who report high
Perspective Taking and Empathic Concern are also more socially oriented with
most highly correlated with all of the EQ-i scales and had a stronger factor
loading of .58. Empathic Concern only correlated with significantly with 5 of the
114
8 EI subscales and had a lower factor loading of .35. Perspective taking therefore
appears more strongly related to EI. This is in line with previous research which
relates perspective taking more closely to EI (e.g. Charbonnueau & Nicol, 2002;
descriptions for empathic individuals, providing support for the construct validity
both cognitive and congruent affective processes (Davis, 1996; Vreeke & van der
Mark, 2003). This study also found that the personal distress subscale showed
concern subscales.
In fact, the personal distress subscale was found to correlate negatively with all
of the subscales of the EQ-i and loaded negatively on to the EI factor in the joint
and EI. The distinction between the different dimensions of empathy is therefore
found to be negatively related to EI, which requires more control over one’s
emotional reactions (e.g. Bar-On, 1997). This finding provides new evidence that
115
the two concepts, while showing some areas of overlap, also show a key
this emotional response within the healthcare context. Before considering this
further, the results of the final section of the analysis require discussion.
5.5.3 Fantasy
The imaginative aspect of the IRI does not appear to be related to self reported
emotional intelligence as the fantasy subscale was neither correlated with, nor
Davis’ original rationale for the subscale in the first place (1983). Both the
fantasy and personal distress subscales were defined as being ‘self oriented’ as
Concern. Indeed, the ‘other oriented’ subscales are the only ones to show
The fantasy subscale was originally included in the IRI as it supposedly related
the IRI, also generated a one factor model of empathy which significantly
also recognised the need to extend her findings as each subscale consists of
not clear. Indeed, there are those who have used the IRI without the Fantasy
subscale, arguing for its lack of relevance to the topic of research (e.g.,
116
Christopher, Owens & Stecker, 1993; Stratton et al., 2005). The mixed research
5.5.4 Limitations
It should be noted that the original research design intended to gather data on the
IRI and the NEO PI-R for a sample of doctors, in order to confirm the results of
the factor analysis in the previous study. However this was not possible on this
occasion as participants were in an assessment centre context and the time and
effort required from participants gather this data was not realistic. This is one of
the limitations of attempting to conduct field research and so attempts were made
that of EI. In itself, this was a useful theoretical comparison to make. The further
point to note is that the choice of EI measure was limited somewhat within the
marketed and for financial reasons were not practical to use within this thesis. Of
those that were available, as EI research is still very much in its infancy, limited
evidence was available to guide the choice of a suitable measure. While the Bar-
replicate findings with a further trait measure of EI such as the TEIQue (Petrides,
empathy as measured by the IRI. Taken as a whole, it has been found that Davis’
117
with all hypotheses being supported for these subscales. Clear links were
distress was found to load negatively with EI while fantasy was found to be
unrelated to EI. Chapter three ended by identifying two key aims of this thesis as
follows:
practitioner?
The first two studies have therefore addressed the first aim only, by investigating
the individual differences that are associated with empathy. While empathy
over those reactions (Bar-On, 1997; Hodges & Wegner, 1997). It will be
important to investigate which concept, empathy or EI, has the greater utility in
terms of the healthcare context by examining the links between empathy, EI and
behaviour. This will begin to address the second aim of the thesis and is the focus
empathic behaviour
taking and empathic concern are closely associated with agreeableness and
extraversion, and also load positively onto the single factor of emotional
this, the thesis now turns to examine the relationship between individual
differences and empathic behaviour in the healthcare context, and explore the
processes’ and the final stage of empathy as positive outcomes for patients and
Antecedents
• Physician characteristics
• Patient characteristics
• Situational characteristics
Empathic Processes
• Non-affective reactions
Attributions • Social behaviour
Figure 6.1: Process model of clinical empathy (from Larson & Yao, 2005).
120
understands the perspective of patients and their emotional response, but also the
behaviour towards the patient. Therefore, whilst a doctor may rate themselves as
treatment, quality of care relationships and satisfaction with medical services will
depend on patients’ judgements that the doctor is empathic, which in turn will
depend on the doctor’s behaviour (Barnett, Howard, King & Dino, 1981; Becker
daily practice for the benefit of the individual and the community being served”
(p.226). In addition, the CARE measure, reviewed in Chapter three, now forms
part of the method for appraising General Practitioners in Scotland. Further work
physicians in order to deliver good medical care, and among the competencies
Martlew & Wells (2000) is ‘empathy and sensitivity’ which appears to relate
‘professional integrity’ also reflect helping and social behaviours and may
The third and fourth types of validity identified by Cronbach and Meehl (1955)
and an independent criterion measure. When test scores and criterion scores are
predictive validity studies involve the criterion score being taken at a later date.
Landy and Conte (2007) note that the usual limitation of concurrent compared to
used, then range is restricted as only those with higher test scores are sampled.
sample of applicants for a role. This limitation was therefore not an issue and this
that comparatively few studies have investigated whether any of the self-report
observers. Even less research has tested these relationships among practitioners
in the healthcare context. Of the few studies that have been conducted in the
example, Hojat et al. (2005) found that a measure of attitudes towards cognitive
Only one study has been located which aimed to compare self report with others’
judgements of empathy using the IRI. Carmel and Glick (1996) asked 324
is often put, curing and caring” (p.1253). Those most frequently identified were
put into the high CEP category, while those least frequently identified were put
into the low CEP category. Results indicated that the high CEP group scored
distress subscale of the IRI. This supports research from other organisational
contexts which has also found a positive link between perspective taking and
significant differences were found for the empathic concern or fantasy subscales
by Carmel and Glick. However this study did not use actual measures of
Furthermore, the perspective taking and empathic concern subscales for the IRI
have been found to be closely related in the first two studies of this thesis, in line
with previous research. For example, Axtell, Parker, Holman and Totterdell
(2007) asked 347 agents from two UK call centres to complete self ratings of
results of the study may be generalisable (Parker & Axtell, 2001). In study three
judged by others.
With respect to the fantasy scale of the IRI, Davis neither expected nor found any
commenting that ‘‘it is not apparent that a tendency to become deeply involved
in the fictitious world of books, movies, and plays will systematically affect one’s
social relationships’’ (Davis, 1983, p.123). However, Stinson and Ickes (1992)
found that those scoring higher on the fantasy scale also performed better on a
practitioners are often interacting with patients with whom they are not well
stranger, as with a fictional character, could aid perspective taking and therefore
enhance the empathic process. The following hypothesis was therefore made:
empathic behaviour.
124
Turning to the fourth IRI subscale, Davis’ suggests that emotional arousability,
another, motivates the observer to help that person, and therefore acts as a
Salgado, 1997; Tett et al., 1991) and Carmel and Glick found that physicians
treatment decisions, Hojat et al. (2001) argue that personal distress is the kind of
has been argued that patients will not perceive this type of emotional response as
helpful at a time when they are seeking reassurance (e.g. Morse et al., 2006).
context.
As a key difference between empathy and emotional intelligence was found with
years (e.g., Salovey & Mayer, 1990; Goleman, 1995), with EI found to play a
(Mavroveli, Petrides, Rieffe & Bakker, 2007). EI has also been suggested as an
important skill in medicine (Elam, Stratton & Andrykowski, 2001; Stratton et al.,
2005). However no study has been located to date that tests the relationship
healthcare context. This study therefore investigated whether physicians who rate
6.3 Method
The data for study three were collected during an assessment centre to select
hundred and fifty six applicants applying for GP specialty training in one UK
deanery were invited to attend assessment centres conducted over a one week
explain the purpose of the research study and applicants were assured that any
information from the psychometric questionnaires used for the research would
not be made available to those making selection decisions and as such the
research did not form part of the selection process. Consent forms were signed by
all participants to indicate their understanding and agreement to take part. (See
Appendices 2 and 3 for the information sheet and consent form). Out of 256
applicants attending the assessment centres, 192 doctors agreed to take part in the
study and completed the measures detailed below. Half of the participants
completed the self-report measures before the assessment centre exercises, with
the other half completing the measures afterwards. This was primarily for
logistical reasons but also helpful in controlling for potential order effects. Of the
192 doctors taking part, 109 (56.77%) were male and 83 (43.23%) female. 32.8%
30.55 years (SD 5.34 years). Sixty seven doctors (36.5%) had completed their
medical training within the UK and Ireland, while the other 125 were trained
predominantly India (35.4%) and Pakistan (10.3%) with the final 17.8%
qualifying in a wide range of countries including Sri Lanka, Nigeria, Iraq, Libya
and Myanmar.
doctors all currently resident and working in the UK. Many were however
prevent overseas doctors from registering to work within the NHS, in an attempt
to preserve jobs for UK graduates. By this time however, some 277,000 overseas
doctors were already registered with the General Medical Council, with almost
therefore be interesting to see if there are cross cultural challenges in empathy for
6.3.2 Measures
1. Empathy: The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis, 1983): This is the
same measure used in Chapters four and five, chosen because of its positive
oneself in the role of characters in books, films or plays. The Empathic Concern
another person. The Personal Distress subscale is also emotional, but focuses on
128
how much one feels distress in response to another. The four subscales
(Perspective Taking, Empathic Concern, Fantasy and Personal Distress) are each
composed of seven items, to which participants are asked to respond using a five-
point likert scale (‘does not describe me well’ to ‘describes me very well’). All
four sub-scales of the IRI have been shown to have satisfactory internal
reliability (α = 0.71 to 0.77) and test – retest reliability (α = 0.62 to 0.71, Davis,
1983). A 0-4 scale is used for each item, so the minimum possible score for each
Appendix 1.
2. Emotional Intelligence: The Bar-On EQ-i (1997): Again, this measure was
used in Chapter five to investigate the construct validity of the IRI. It measures
(Bar-On, 1997, p. 14). Respondents are asked to rate items using a 5 point Likert-
type scale (where 1 = ‘Very seldom or not true of me’ and 5 = ‘Very often true or
true of me’). Chapter five produced an eight factor solution using 107 items of
the measure. Given that the participants in this study formed part of the sample
for Chapter five, it was deemed appropriate to use this factor structure for the test
of EI in this study. Eight subscale scores were therefore calculated: self esteem
(21 items); self control (11 items); assertiveness (17 items); rationality (15
regulation (11 items), and adaptability (10 items). Subscale scores were
calculated by reverse scoring negatively worded items and calculating the mean
score from the items for each scale. Mean scores were used to account for the
129
differing numbers of items in each subscale. Reliabilities for all of the subscales
0.78 to 0.94. An EQ-i total was calculated by summing these means. The
minimum possible score for each subscale is therefore one and the maximum is
five, while the minimum possible EQ-i total score is eight and the maximum is
40.
& Raven, 1994): This was administered according to the standard instructions.
missing. The task is to identify the correct missing part from a range of eight
0.90 (Raven et al., 1994). Each correctly answered item receives one point, such
that the minimum possible total score is zero while the maximum is 36.
behaviour were obtained from two sources, trained assessors and medical actors
House Officers in a city hospital who must prioritise which patients should
medical actor, who has just been diagnosed with cancer. ‘Patients’ followed a
emotions including anger, fear and confusion, while responding naturally to the
interaction with the participant. In each of the exercises, the participant was
observed by a trained assessor who then rated them using behavioural indicators
sample indicators are provided in Table 6.1. After each exercise, assessors rated
competency, scores from the two exercises were combined. The minimum
possible score for each competency is therefore two, while the maximum is eight.
After the simulated consultation, the ‘patients’ also rated the participants on two
skills’. For each competency, patients were given 5 statements and asked to rate
Table 6.1 Definitions and sample behavioural indicators of competencies related to empathy (from Patterson et al 2000).
Empathy and Desire and ability to take in Demonstrated a caring manner towards Showed very little visible
sensitivity [ES] the perspective of others, and others interest/understanding.
sense associated feelings, Was clearly intent on establishing exactly Was quick to judge, make assumptions.
generating a safe, reassuring what others were thinking or feeling Appeared isolated or authoritarian.
atmosphere Was perceptive, responding to the Lacked warmth in voice/manner and
concerns of others with understanding failed to encourage patient
Clearly reassured others with appropriate Created uncomfortable atmosphere
words and actions
Communication Ability to engage others, Actively encouraged others through use of Failed to use supportive words or
Skills [CS] clearly and actively, in supportive words or comments comments to encourage others
constructive dialogue, Used open exploratory questions inviting Asked closed questions, restricting
adjusting language and non- others to become actively involved opportunities for others to become
verbal behaviour according to Adjusted language as appropriate to suit involved
the needs of differing particular needs of the situation Unable to adapt language to suit particular
situations needs of the situation
Professional Professional commitment (i) to Showed clear respect for others (whether Appeared to lack sufficient respect for
integrity [PI] provide equality of care for all, through words or actions) others (whether through words or actions)
(ii) to take responsibility for Was positive/enthusiastic during the Approached the exercise defensively,
own actions – while at the exercise, however challenging it seemed more as a problem than a challenge
same time recognising the When appropriate, was open and Appeared judgmental, not prepared to
parameters of one’s role and accepting of the particular situation of consider each situation on its merits
expertise, (iii) to act others
confidently but safely
132
concerns’. For Empathy and Sensitivity, statements included ‘This doctor was
situation/concerns’ and ‘I felt at ease with this doctor’. A mean score was then
taken of these five statements, thus the patient ratings for each competency
ranged from 1-5. Unfortunately the assessment centre administrators did not
provide raw scores for every statement so a reliability analysis of these subscales
6.4 Results
Table 6.2 presents the means and standard deviations of the variables. Before
However, these tests are often significant with a large sample size and so
large sample sizes give rise to small standard errors, z scores are large and so
significant z scores are found from small deviations from normality. In this case,
the criterion of 2.58 was used to represent a significant deviation from normality
(Field, 2005). All of the z scores for both skewness and kurtosis were less than
2.58, taken as support for the assumption of normality, for the subscales of the
IRI, EQ-i, RAPM scores and assessor ratings of behaviour from the three
acceptable for using parametric tests. However, the z scores were greater than
2.58 for age and the two patient competency ratings. The patient ratings for both
133
Age was also significantly positively skewed as most of the doctors were at the
beginning of their medical careers at the stage of choosing a specialty with the
minority coming to this point later in their careers. Transformations were not
Variable Mean SD
6.4.1 Correlations
Table 6.3 then presents correlations between the variables for all 192 doctors.
Correlations for age and the patient ratings are Spearman’s rho non-parametric
ratings of all three competency scores (Empathy and Sensitivity [ES]: rho = -.36;
Communication Skills [CS]: rho = - .39; Professional Integrity [PI]: rho = -.40,
all p < .01). There were also small negative correlations between age and patient
ratings of ES (rho = -.14, p<.05) and CS (rho = -.21, p<.01). Age also showed a
small significant negative correlation with the Fantasy subscale of the IRI (r = -
.17, p<.05). The RAPM scores were significantly positively correlated with the
.43, all p < .01). These are all moderate positive correlations (Cohen, 1988). As
anticipated, the RAPM scores were not significantly correlated with any of the
subscales of either IRI. There was only one small negative correlations between
RAPM and one subscale of the EQ-i, self control (r = .19, p<.05). The positive
the IRI with the various subscales of the EQ-i, again illustrate the overlap
1. Age
2. RAPM -.37**
9. IRI-Empathic Concern .10 -.05 -.03 -.07 .01 -.05 .02 .08
10. IRI-Perspective Taking .14 -.03 -.01 -.07 .00 .09 .03 -.01 .39**
11. IRI-Personal Distress -.10 -.13 -.24** -.15 -.12 -.18* -.25** .10 .01 -.22**
12. EQ-i Self Esteem .09 -.05 -.04 -.05 -.02 -.04 -.03 -.03 .25** .35** -.31**
13. EQ-i Self Control .08 -.19* .01 .00 .02 -.02 -.07 -.22** .27** .30** -.29** .56**
14. EQ-Assertiveness -.11 -.03 .17* .16* .09 .16* .21** -.14 .05 .20** -.52** .60** .40**
15. EQ-i Rationality .32** -.12 -.02 .00 -.02 .01 -.05 -.06 .16* .41** -.36** .66** .36** .53**
EQ-i Interpersonal
16. -.09 .14 .14 .17* .13 .11 .16* .05 .47** .45** -.18* .69** .52** .45** .57**
Sensitivity
17. EQ-i Emotional Expression .08 -.11 .16* .13 .16* .05 .08 .06 .24** .27** -.31** .58** .46** .55** .48** .51**
18. EQ-i Emotion Regulation -.06 -.05 .14 .12 .10 -.11 -.13 -.02 .13 .42** -.46** .65** .57** .59** .54** .59** .52**
19. EQ-i Flexibility .07 .02 .03 .02 .01 .06 .05 -.15* .10 .29** -.33** .50** .42** .44** .47** .43** .31** .54**
Note: RAPM = Ravens Advance Progressive Matrices.For competency ratings, A indicates an assessor rating, P a ‘patient’ rating
136
correlations between the IRI, EQ-i and assessor ratings of the interpersonal
the IRI, only the Fantasy subscale showed significant positive correlations with the
three competency scores (ES: r = .23, p < .01; CS: r = .19, p < .05; PI: r = .25, p <
.01). All of these correlations are small (Cohen, 1988). The Personal Distress
subscale showed a small negative correlation with one of the competency scores,
empathy and sensitivity (r = -.24, p<.01). Thus, hypotheses one and two
(perspective taking and empathic concern) were not supported while hypothesis
three regarding fantasy was supported and hypothesis 4 (personal distress) was
partially supported.
Some of the subscales of the EQ-i were correlated with assessor ratings of
behavior: Assertiveness showed small significant positive correlations with the two
of the three competency scores (ES: r = .17, CS: r = .16, both p < .05), and
Interpersonal Sensitivity was also positively correlated with the assessor rating of
positively correlated with two of the three competency scores (ES: r = .16, PI: r =
To assess the relationships between individual differences and behaviour from the
patient perspective, the non-parametric correlations between the IRI, EQ-i and
137
patient ratings of the interpersonal competencies were inspected. From the IRI, the
Personal Distress subscale was significantly negatively correlated with the patient
ratings of empathy and sensitivity (rho = -.18, p<.05) and communication skills
(rho = -.25, p<.01), providing support for hypothesis four. With regard to the EQ-i,
a similar pattern of results was found as with the assessor ratings. The
Assertiveness factor showed small significant positive correlations with the two
competency scores (ES: rho = .16, CS: r = .21, both p < .05). The Interpersonal
Sensitivity subscale was also positively correlated with the patient rating of
Communication Skills (rho = .16, p < .05). Partial support was therefore found for
One of the potential reasons for the lack of a clear relationship between individual
previously, sixty seven doctors (36.5%) had completed their medical training within
the UK and Ireland, while the other 125 were trained overseas. The sample
the study, but as the opportunity arose, the decision was taken to investigate the
study hypotheses again, for both the sample of UK and Ireland trained doctors (n =
67) and those trained overseas (n = 125). The group of doctors trained overseas
Although not a homogenous group, they differ from the UK doctors in that they did
138
not receive their initial medical training within the healthcare system in which they
are currently applying for GP training. This is the key difference being explored in
Before doing this, tests for differences between the two groups on the key variables
were conducted. As age and RAPM scores were identified in Table 6.3 as
normality was not met. This found that applicants trained in the UK and Ireland
(Mdn = 26.00 years) were significantly younger than applicants trained overseas
an independent t-test was conducted which revealed that applicants trained in the
UK and Ireland (M = 24.57, SE = .53) scored significantly higher than those trained
covariates, a randomised matched pairs design was used (Pedhazur & Schmelkin,
1991) to create two groups of doctors who had either trained in the UK and Ireland
(Group 1) or Overseas (Group 2). The groups were matched for age and RAPM
scores. In creating the two groups it was not possible to match all of the participants
therefore sample size for this set of analyses was 63 applicants in each group. To
confirm they were matched for age, a non-parametric Mann Whitney U test was
used because the assumption of normality was not met. This found that Group 1
139
(Mdn = 27.00 years) and Group 2 (Mdn = 28.00 years) did not differ significantly in
t(124) = 1.52, ns). To assess homogeneity of variance between the two groups, the
variance ratios were used rather than Levene’s test, which is sensitive to sample
size. All variance ratios were less than two indicating homogeneity of variance
(Field, 2005).
as Multiracial/Other.
To investigate differences between the two groups on the IRI, EQ-i and
competency scores, a MANOVA was conducted with one fixed factor (country of
qualification) and eight dependent variables (FS, EC, PT, PD, EQ-i total score,
assessor ratings of ES, CS and PI). All of the dependent variables were included in
which were partially supported in Table 6.3, therefore the variables were not
ANOVAs was conducted. Table 6.4 presents the means and standard deviations for
all variables for each group as well as F values from the ANOVAs.
Table 6.4 Descriptive statistics for UK and Ireland and Overseas trained doctors.
Large group effects were found for all three competency scores. Applicants who
trained in the UK and Ireland (Group 1) were rated significantly higher by assessors
= 1, p<.01, η2 = .29) than doctors who trained overseas (Group 2). Despite the large
differences on the competency scores, the two groups did not differ significantly on
the three of the IRI subscales or the EQ-i. The only exception to this was the
11.96, df = 1, p<.01, η2 = .09). This was a medium effect size. Additionally, non-
parametric tests were conducted to assess differences between the two groups on
the patient ratings of behaviour. For both competencies, applicants trained in the
141
applicants trained overseas (Mdn = 2). Non parametric tests confirmed that these
In order to explore the potential moderating role of the grouping variable on the
relationships between the self report questionnaires and the ratings of the
competency scores for the two groups, one method is to conduct hierarchical
with the grouping variable. However, due to the high number of independent
variables resulting and the relatively low sample size, there was insufficient power
to conduct this analysis (Field, 2005; Kline, 2000). Instead, partial correlations
were calculated controlling for age and RAPM scores. Age and RAPM scores were
controlled for because of the moderate correlations found between these variables
and some of the competency ratings (detailed in Table 6.3). The partial correlations
between IRI and EQ-i subscales and assessor ratings are reported in Table 6.5, and
patient ratings in Table 6.6. Fisher’s z transformation was then used to compare the
significant difference at the .01 level. For this sample size, correlations needed to
differ by at least .35 to be deemed significantly different. Table 6.5 shows the z
For the Perspective Taking subscale, the correlation for Group 1 was significantly
positive with Professional Integrity, (r = .29, p<.05) whereas for Group 2 the r
although the correlation between perspective taking and empathy and sensitivity
approached significance (r = .20, p = .07). Only partial support was therefore found
for hypothesis one with group one and no support at all with group 2.
For Group 1, all correlations between the empathic concern subscale of the IRI and
the competencies were significantly positive (ES: r = .27, p<.05, CS: r = .28, p<.05,
PI r = .41, p<.01). Again for Group 2, none were significant. All pairs of
2.35, p<.05, PI: z = 2.94, p<.01). Thus hypothesis two was fully supported for
group 1 only.
For Group 1, applicants trained in the UK & Ireland, the Fantasy subscale of the
p<.01). For Group 2, this subscale did not correlate significantly with any of the
competency scores.
143
Table 6.5 Partial Correlations controlling for age and intelligence between IRI, EQ-I and assessor-rated competency scores
EQ-i Total
Self Esteem .21 -.03 .03 .01 .14 -.02
Self Control .13 -.07 .00 -.02 .07 -.07
Assertiveness .27* .14 .06 .15 .14 .05
†
Rationality .24 .01 .19 .10 .29* .10
Interpersonal Sensitivity .45** .06 2.33* .37** .16 .39** .16
Emotional Expression .29* .10 .18 .10 .32* .10
Emotional Regulation .23† .08 .04 .10 .13 .10
Flexibility .17 .11 -.01 .19 .01 .09
Note: For each group, n = 63; Larzalere and Mulaik (1977) adjusted *significant p<.05, ** p<.01, †p = .07
144
For both Communication Skills and Professional Integrity, the correlations for Group
1 were significantly different from Group 2 (CS: z = 2.23, p<.05, PS: z = 2.40,
p<.05). Thus hypothesis three was fully supported for group 1 but not for group 2.
The final subscale of the IRI, Personal Distress, was not significantly positively
correlated with any of the competency scores for either group. Therefore hypothesis
There were some significant findings when looking at the subscales of the EQ-i and
the assessor ratings of competency scores, but again only for Group 1. Assessor
EQ-i was the only one for which this correlation differed significantly for Group 2 (z
between EQ-i Interpersonal Sensitivity and the Empathy and Sensitivity competency
score, none of the Fisher’s zs were significant looking at the pairs of correlations.
Thus partial support was found for hypothesis five with group 1 only.
.
145
Table 6.6 Partial correlations controlling for age and intelligence between IRI, EQ-i and ‘patient’-rated competency scores
EQ-i
Self Esteem .15 -.08 .22 -.00
Self Control -.07 -.01 .03 -.07
Assertiveness .16 -.12 .04 -.01
Rationality .19 .11 .22 .08
Sensitivity .07 .11 .14 .19
Emotional Expression .08 .04 .31** .12
Emotional Regulation .14 .23 .07 .22
Adaptability .19 .12 .25* .15
Note: For each group, n = 63; Larzalere and Mulaik (1977) adjusted *significant p<.05, ** p<.01.
146
Partial correlations were calculated controlling for age and RAPM using rank scores
for the patient ratings (Pallant, 2007). The UK & Ireland group correlations did not
differ significantly from those in the overseas group, with all zs being less than 1.96.
It was found that personal distress was significantly negatively related to the two
competency ratings from patients for the UK and Ireland group only. For empathy
and sensitivity, r = -.25, p<.05 and for communication skills r = -.26, p<.05. Thus
partial support was found for hypothesis four but only for group 1. Also, in terms of
the EQ-i, emotional expression (r = .31, p<.01) and flexibility (r = .25, p<.05) were
positively related to communication skills for the UK and Ireland group only. Thus
hypothesis five was partially supported for group 1 but not for group 2.
6.5 Discussion
This study has built upon the first two studies to test the relationships between
rated by assessors and patients. Specifically, the study examined the relationship
between empathy as assessed using the Interpersonal Reactivity Index and the Bar-
summarised as follows:
behaviour and personal distress was negatively associated with patient ratings of
empathic behaviour. Few other relationships were apparent between self report
147
2. Comparing doctors who had trained in the UK & Ireland with those trained
overseas revealed very little difference in terms of the self report measures of
individual differences. However, doctors who qualified in the UK and Ireland were
empathic behaviour for UK and Ireland trained doctors, but not for overseas trained
taking were all positively related to assessor ratings of behaviours for the UK and
behaviour for UK and Ireland trained doctors, but not for overseas trained doctors.
5. There were more clear relationships between the self report measures of
empathy and EI and assessor ratings of behaviour than between the self report
There are various potential explanations for this set of findings, related to both
method and theory. Theoretical considerations relate to situational factors such as the
first.
The first finding was that fantasy was associated with assessor ratings of empathic
behaviour. This may be surprising to some in the medical community who have
chosen to ignore this aspect of empathy (e.g. Elam, Stratton & Andrykovski, 2001).
However the relationship was predicted as there is some evidence to suggest that
fantasy is associated with empathy for a stranger (Stinson & Ickes, 1992), as is the
case in the assessment centre exercises where applicants consult with an unknown
simulated patient and interact in a group with other applicants generally unknown to
them. It may be that as relationships become well established, fantasy becomes less
important although this has yet to be explored. The weak findings for perspective
taking are perhaps surprising although it may be that this scale becomes more
relevant as relationships are established. Additionally, this subscale has been shown
The negative relationship between patient ratings of behaviour and personal distress
is consistent with the argument that automatic emotional reactions, while they may
patients in this context. Indeed, the more controlled responses of assertiveness and
interpersonal sensitivity from the EQ-i were positively related to patient ratings of
149
empathy by comparing doctors trained in the UK and Ireland with those trained
need to investigate EI cross culturally as most of the research to date has been
competence (Betancourt, 2003), in the US and Canada it has been claimed that
ensure culturally competent care” (Zabar et al., 2006, p.510). Whilst this is a
potential issue needing investigation, surprisingly little research has taken place. The
term ‘ethnocultural empathy’ was proposed by Wang et al. (2003) to describe the
process of empathising with people from racial, ethnic or cultural groups different
from one’s own. Wang et al. argue that in a multicultural environment, empathy is
awareness and understanding of how care may need to adapt for diverse groups.
Thus far, studies have established that those with more open attitudes towards
diversity training are more likely to report intentions to empathise with people from
diverse backgrounds (e.g. Brouwer & Boros, 2010; Cundiff, Nadler & Swann, 2009).
150
Appreciating the thoughts and concerns of a patient from another culture may require
the doctor to elicit extra information from that person, as comparisons from one’s
own experiences will become less appropriate. Also, from a patient perspective, the
behavioural cues that patients use to determine whether the doctor is empathising
with them may be different. If this is the case, there could be important implications
for doctors trained in one country operating successfully in another. Although there
some evidence from the general population and other healthcare settings such as
interactions outside the formal helping relationship, Webster Nelson and Baumgarte
(2004) found that American college students were less able to take the perspective of
targets who were dissimilar from U.S. cultural norms. In addition, less compassion
and sympathy were reported for targets from an unfamiliar cultural perspective.
cultures, Sue and Sundberg (1996) found that counsellors who demonstrated an
them to be different from their own were evaluated more positively by those patients.
The findings of the present study, by comparing partial correlations from the
randomised matched groups, support the possibility that there is a moderating effect
The different patterns of associations between self report empathy and EI measures
and ratings of behaviours are consistent with previous research which suggests that,
151
from a different culture is reduced as a result of less familiarity with that person’s
experiences and background. It was noted during the assessment centre that all of the
medical actors were of White British origin. It would be interesting to see if the same
findings were apparent if there were more of a mix in ethnicity among the medical
6.5.3 Limitations
It is not possible to definitively explain the large group differences found between
the UK and Ireland doctors and the overseas trained doctors. An alternative
explanation for these findings could be that demonstrated behaviours are not in fact
different from those operating cross-culturally, but that they are being evaluated
differently when the assessor and candidate are from different cultural backgrounds.
assessment centre, race of assessor was not found to have a significant impact on
scores (Lowry, 1993). Furthermore, the assessment centre in this study used only
trained assessors with very specific behavioural criteria, which has been found to
reduce any reliance on stereotypes or hence biases from occurring (London, 2001).
Future studies are clearly needed to provide a greater understanding of the impact of
cross-culturally.
152
There are also some limitations to note with regard to the method of assessing
behaviours. First, within the assessment centre exercises, participants are required to
demonstrate maximal performance whereas the self report measures are assessments
would not be large correlations between what participants can do and what they
typical performance is well established (e.g. Sackett, Zedeck & Fogli, 1988). The
second issue of note is that each assessment centre exercise lasted for no more than
thirty minutes and was a one off ‘snapshot’ of behaviour regarding interactions with
more reliable (Rust & Golombok, 1999). In addition, due to the context, sources of
error in all measures may have included test anxiety (Fletcher & Kerslake, 1993). It
The final point to note is that few relationships were apparent between the self report
measures and the patient ratings of behaviour. This may have been because of the
skewed data on these variables, or because the behaviours being assessed aren’t
those that actually predict judgments of empathy in patients. There is some evidence
to suggest that patients use different cues from assessors in judging empathy
(Silvester et al., 2007). Ultimately, it is the patients who are the consumers of
practitioner empathy in this context and as such, this question requires further
research. Although patients did play a role in the creation of the competency model
153
(Patterson et al., 2000), their role was relatively minor and not focused specifically
behaviour were provided by trained assessors and patients and compared to self
report empathic disposition. For those doctors trained in the UK and Ireland, fantasy,
interpersonal behaviour. However this relationship did not hold for doctors trained
empathic process in this context. However this study also showed that patient ratings
the behaviours that patients associate with empathy. Therefore the final study of the
thesis will explore understanding of the specific behaviours associated with empathy
Studies 1-3 utilised the IRI in understanding the individual differences associated
with the antecedents of empathy and aimed to explore the specific behaviours
connected with those individual differences in the healthcare context. While a clear
associated with these differences remain unclear. There are several reasons for this,
both methodological and theoretical, which were explored in detail in the previous
behaviours from the perspective of the ultimate judge in this context, the patient.
previous study. The purpose of this final study is therefore twofold. First, it will aim
in this context, as judged by patients. Secondly, it will build upon the findings of
considering the theoretical basis of the present study, before moving on to justify an
alternative methodology.
155
multidimensional model of empathy, with the adapted model from Larson and Yao
(2005: see Figure 7.1) arguing that these ‘interpersonal processes’ are the indicators
will depend not only on whether the practitioner understands the perspective of
patients and their emotional response, but also the extent to which the practitioner
Outcomes such as patient trust, compliance with medical treatment, quality of care
judgements that the doctor is empathic, which in turn will depend on the doctor’s
behaviour (Barnett, Howard, King & Dino, 1981; Becker & Maiman, 1975).
However, there is some evidence to suggest that the behaviours that patients use to
make these judgments are different to the ones identified as important by healthcare
cues (Silvester et al., 2007). It is therefore important that research should focus on
the patient perspective, particularly in light of the fact that within the NHS there is an
Antecedents
• Physician characteristics
e.g. compassion (+),
cynicism (-)
• Patient characteristics
• Situational characteristics
e.g. patient-physician
similarity
Empathic Processes
Figure 7.1. Process model of clinical empathy (from Larson & Yao, 2005).
157
specification of the behaviours remains vague (Stepien & Baernstein, 2006). There is
medical competence alongside clinical knowledge (e.g Epstein & Hundert, 2002).
strong theoretical focus on empathy (e.g. Mercer & Reynolds, 2002). Although work
in order to deliver good medical care (e.g. Patterson et al., 2000), these investigations
have also been broad in focus, looking at the full range of physician performance.
behaviour more specifically, examples are few. A study by Forchuk and Reynolds
behaviours included exploration and clarification of feelings and their meaning to the
patient as well as helping clients to focus on future solutions rather than past
useful starting point, it is limited to one context (psychiatric patients) and therefore
In their adapted model, Larson and Yao (2005) also highlight the need to consider
not only the characteristics of the healthcare practitioner but also those of the patient
and the situation in discussing antecedents to empathic processes. For example, they
factor. However, the majority of empathy research within the healthcare literature
focuses on the individual characteristics of the practitioner (e.g. Morse et al., 2006;
Stepien & Baernstein, 2006) and so very little is known about the situational factors
that may influence the relationship between individual differences and behaviour.
This final study will therefore pose two central research questions:
1. What are the specific behaviours identified by patients when making empathy
2. What are the situational antecedents of empathy that might impact on the
used for this study, namely a qualitative one. It is therefore necessary to first justify
this decision.
psychometric questionnaires used in this thesis are useful for testing hypotheses via
measurement and control of a structured sample of variables (De Vaus, 2002). They
159
are underpinned by a positivist assumption that ‘the truth is out there’, in that there
are consistent relationships which can be measured and observed. Indeed, the very
word ‘quantitative’ implies that measurement can be made on some numerical basis
objective data. It is this kind of evidence which has been presented thus far.
However, this psychometric approach can have its limitations. For example, analysis
focuses on areas defined by the researcher, rather than exploring what is meaningful
that of a self report pencil and paper questionnaire, could be seen as incongruent as a
method for examining a dynamic interpersonal concept such as empathy (Cassell &
Symon, 2004).
methodology. For many years within psychology, a debate has existed with regard to
reliability and validity, being uncontrolled, subjective and biased (Coolican, 2009).
Qualitative researchers on the other hand would reply that a reductionist, quantitative
approach removes understanding of humans who are ‘laden with values and must be
understood in the context of their time and cultural setting’ (Bem & Looren de Jong,
1997, p.23).
160
observable objective truth, the assumption is that reality is multiple and constructed
are not simply representations of the world. Rather knowledge is actively created
between researcher and participant (Miles & Huberman, 1994). Importantly, rather
themes or patterns. Raw data consists of what people have said in interviews or
Within the context of empathy research, this would involve allowing patients to talk
about their descriptions and experiences of empathy instead of guiding them with
reference to previous scales and definitions. It would seem then that empathy is a
suitable topic for qualitative research. It is complex concept with many dimensions
therefore qualitative investigation will allow the participants to focus on those areas
exchange rather than a pencil and paper questionnaire could be seen as more
It should further be noted that qualitative and quantitative methods are not
(Coolican, 2009). According to Bartunek & Seo (2002), qualitative research can
data using an authentic research method for the topic of interest can allow for a
161
terms of depth and meaning from a smaller sample of participants (Lincoln & Guba,
1985). This is the therefore the approach taken in this study. Empathy from the
7.5 Method
7.5.1 Participants
The study was located within three wards of the medical division of a large teaching
hospital in the north east of England. Approval was sought and finally gained from
the local ethics and research & development committees. Twenty patients were
interviewed over a two week period within private rooms on the wards. Of the 20
patients interviewed, 14 were female and six were male. Nineteen of the patients
were from the local area and of White (British) ethnic origin. One patient was from
Pakistan and of Pakistani ethnic origin. Age ranged from 28 to 78 years (Mean age
55 years, S.D. 15.62 years). Length of stay in the hospital ranged from one week to a
period of several months. In accordance with requests from the ethics committee, no
additional information was gathered as this was not deemed relevant to the study.
7.5.2 Procedure
Participation in the study was voluntary. Ward sisters on each of the three wards
involved were asked to identify current patients whom they felt were well enough to
take part in a 30 minute interview in a side room. Before starting the interview,
162
information sheets were provided and the purpose of the study and format of the
interview were explained fully. Patients were asked to sign a consent form to
indicate that they understood this information and agreed to take part (see
Appendices 5 and 6 for the information sheet and consent form). Twenty two
patients were approached to take part in the study; only two patients declined to be
interviewed. All interviews were conducted face to face and audio-recordings were
A semi structured interview incorporating a critical incident method was adopted for
all patient interviews (Flanagan, 1954). Patients were asked to describe incidents of
when they felt a nurse had empathised with them and also when a nurse had not
empathised with them. Where patients spoke in general terms about a nurse, they
were then prompted to give a specific example of an incident to illustrate their point.
Within each critical incident, patients were asked to describe briefly what had
happened during a specific example and also the outcome for them (see Appendix 7
for the interview schedule). Confidentiality was assured by explaining that any
information provided in the interviews would only be fed back to hospital staff in
general terms so that individuals could not be identified. Interviews lasted from 15 to
7.5.3 Analysis
Template analysis was used to code the data from the patient interview transcripts,
using the approach specified by King (2004). In template analysis (King, 2004), a
163
lower order categories being grouped together to produce a more general, higher
order code. Production of the initial template was directly influenced by the model of
Larson and Yao (2005; Fig 7.1) in their adaptation of Davis’ (1996)
multidimensional model for the clinical encounter, but deliberately kept to only two
levels of code to allow for emergent themes from that data rather than taking a very
According to the Larson and Yao model, six higher order (or level one) codes were
outcomes. Within these six higher order codes, one further lower level of code was
present. For example within ‘antecedents’, a level two code was ‘situation’. The
model could have been used to identify further levels of code at the initial stage, but
this was avoided so that the final template could be guided by the data rather than
being constrained by the initial template. The initial template is shown in Figure 7.2.
164
1. Antecedents of Empathy
1 Nurse
2 Patient
3 Situation
2. Intrapersonal processes
3. Empathic Processes
4. Interpersonal Behaviour
1 Helping Behaviour
2 Social Behaviour
3 Conflict management
5. Nurse Outcomes
6. Patient Outcomes
The initial template was used to code all 20 patient interview transcripts. It is
important to note that template analysis allows for parallel coding, in which the same
segment of text might be classified under two or more categories. A brief illustration
of the coding process is given for the following extract from the first patient who
was talking about the fact that a nurse had accompanied her for some tests because
considerate social style] because she said she’d learnt a lot [5 - nurse
outcome: thanking the patient] and we’d had a nice chat on the way [3 –
template and make changes during this initial coding process, in order to develop a
between higher (broader) and lower (narrower) coding themes. It is also possible to
theories or previous research may suggest certain themes (and can include these to
formulate initial templates) the modification process allows for a degree of open
coding such that the final template fully represents emergent themes from the data.
First, two higher order themes were combined. Specifically, there was little use of
the themes ‘intrapersonal processes’ and ‘empathic processes’ during coding, but
where they were, they appeared to fit together. This is unsurprising given that the
methodology focused on the patient perspective. Patients spoke relatively less about
the process of empathising within the nurse and more about the context and
behaviour.
Secondly, there was a need to differentiate several codes with the addition of a
number of lower level codes. It became apparent during initial coding that many of
second level codes needed to be redefined with the use of third and fourth level
codes in order to produce a useful final template. For example, the higher order
theme ‘interpersonal processes’ was originally split into three second level codes,
management’ was not used at all in the coding process and this was deleted. The
other two codes ‘helping behaviour’ and social behaviour’ were used extensively.
Helping behaviour was split into four third level codes: responding to requests; being
quick to help; problem solving, and acting as a patient advocate. Social behaviour
was split into two third level codes, communication and considerate social style.
These were then split into a further five level four codes each, representing the fine
grained nature of the coding process for this area of the template.
Analysis was conducted fully by the researcher with modifications made throughout
the coding process. The final template was reached after four rounds of coding of the
rigorous coding process (King 2004). Once the final template had been developed,
transcripts were reviewed and recoded by the researcher to check that the coding
In terms of quality checks on the qualitative analysis process, there are several
the analysis (King, 2004). In order to do this, those who participated in the research
area asked to comment on the analysis and interpretation. Unfortunately the hospital
participating in the research was not willing to allow this final stage of checking.
There are some researchers who recommend that it is necessary to consider inter-
rater reliability of identifying codes (Coolican, 2009). However this is not advocated
by King (2004) and, as noted by Coolican (2009), this may be more relevant where
teams of researchers are involved in coding the data from a single study, which was
167
not the case here. Despite this, there remains some need to demonstrate the vigour
and transparency of the data analysis process. King (2004) suggests that a final
template may be satisfactory when: (a) no sections of the transcripts that are relevant
to the research question remain uncoded; (b) all data have been read through and the
coding checked at least twice, perhaps three or four times, and (c) collaborating
researchers (or in the absence of a collaborator, an outside expert) agree that the
template is sufficiently clear and complete. The final template was reached by the
researcher after four rounds of coding. At this point, independent scrutiny of the final
version of the template was conducted by two psychologists who were experienced
in the use of coding approaches to qualitative data. These psychologists used four of
the transcripts to consider whether there were: any themes that they found difficult to
employ; any aspects of text not covered by the template, and any other issues of note
when reading the text. No issues of concern were raised by either, therefore the
template was considered to be sufficient as a final version. The final template for all
transcripts from both positive and negative incidents of empathy is given in Figure
7.3.
168
the concept. As a result, it is necessary to justify the nature of the presentation of the
findings. To go through every code within the final template in equal depth would
discussion of the findings focuses first on the two central research questions for the
study before moving on to briefly consider other emergent findings. This selective,
template analysis (e.g. King, Carroll, Newton & Dornan, 2002). Any participant
1. What are the specific behaviours identified by patients when making empathy
2. What are the situational antecedents of empathy that might impact on the
The findings from the template analysis with respect to ‘Interpersonal Processes’
most frequently identified codes fell within the higher order category of
interpersonal processes. These are of course much easier for the patient to observe
with respect to interpersonal processes, namely helping and social behaviour. Each
of these second level codes was then split into several level three codes. These
responsive to requests: “No matter what I’m asking about, she doesn’t give me the
brush off, she answers me properly. And she responds at the time that I’ve asked her
to. And she sits and listens to you. And I find that very helpful”. Many patients
identified other factors within helping behaviour as well as being responsive which
they saw as demonstration of the nurses’ understanding and caring. These included
acting as a patient advocate, helping quickly and resolving issues fully. All of these
themes are reflected in the quote from Ann, a 62 year old stroke victim talking about
“If you have a problem, she sorts it out. I was supposed to have an
appointment with the physio, the doctor said. I waited for days...but it didn’t
happen. I just spoke to Beth and she made it happen for me right away. She
gives me privacy when I want it, she’s the one that thinks about that sort of
171
thing. She’s got more go. She sorts you out very quickly, even though she’s
Whereas helping behaviour referred largely to what the nurse did in response to the
patient, prosocial behaviour was seen as the style of responding. There were two
nurse empathy, namely communication and considerate social style. Both of these
themes were seen as resulting from the nurses’ personality and situational
necessarily through spending more time with them, although this was sometimes the
case. The patients on these wards frequently spoke of empathic nurses as busy but
able to make the most of their time by chatting about social topics in addition to
stroke victim:
“I was scared at first that I wasn’t going to be able to talk but she kept
talking to me and ...I was a lot better after a week. She would chat about
understood. But also just talking to me about the weather and my family, you
In addition, empathic nurses were described as those who initiated and opened up
lonely patient or asking open questions to find out more about a situation:
“When I was in the side room, I didn’t see many people coming and going.
She would try to have a chat with you and buck you up a bit you know
investigation:
“She listens to you, listens to the problems you’ve got. When she’s got time
then she’ll listen to you... To her, it’s more than my medical condition, I’m a
person”.
Finally within this theme, over half of the patients identified the nurses’
“My Mum and Dad are able to ask her things, that’s important. It’s difficult
for me because I’ve been due to go home a few times now and then at the last
minute there’s been a problem with my tests. They know how I desperate I am
to get home... she was disappointed for me as well. But at least I know Mum
This theme was closely linked to the patient withdrawal as an antecedent to empathy;
where patients became less communicative, they judged communication with the
In addition to communication, the second theme within prosocial behaviour was the
“Things were explained fully, whether they could be treated or not. I wasn’t
and patience:
“she’s very patient and very kind. Like I had a stroke, and she was very
patient with me. Whatever you ask for, she helps you so nicely and you don’t
feel like you’re any trouble and you feel very good”.
In contrast, nurses who did not empathise were seen as being too quick and harsh in
their communication style: “I asked her something and she really turned round and
The provision of reassurance alongside other actions was also seen as important by
more than half of the patients, in particular those who reported feeling anxious,
frightened or worried. This is an example from a patient who was anxious about
“And she reassured me, she said “This is what we mean, we can’t see it on
you, no one knows you’ve got it on” I agreed and I was pleased, so I said yes,
For those patients who reported feeling sad or down, rather than reassurance, they
frequently mentioned the use of humour as an effective social style in helping them
“I felt completely suicidal last year when I was in for so long. She would
come and sit with me and let me talk, she was here when I was upset. She let
me talk and she tried to cheer me up, you know making jokes and that. It
This use of humour and a general display of positive emotions is interesting because
it does not necessarily require the experience of a particular emotion from the nurse.
Larson and Yao (2005) identify clinical empathy as a form of emotional labour.
Whereas surface acting emotional empathy can refer to the appearance of emotions
that are not necessarily experienced, deep acting empathy involves the actual
experience and subsequent expression of these emotions (Grandey, 2003). These are
within the emotional labour literature, researchers have much to say about the
potential impact of care work on the practitioner and the patient. For example, it has
been argued that surface acting empathy might protect the practitioner from
becoming over-involved and emotionally exhausted (Maslach, 1978) but might not
be perceived by patients as genuine. However this did not seem to be the case for
these patients. Conversely, while deep acting emotional empathy provide the
(e.g. Hojat et al., 2004; Spencer, 2004). The discussion now turns to the antecedents
The patients interviewed in this study discussed several themes regarding the
antecedents of empathy. The situational factors that were the subject of the second
Most frequently, when patients were asked to describe an incident when a nurse had
been unable to empathise with them, they did not feel that the nurse was particularly
responsible for this situation occurring. Rather they saw it as a result of staffing
levels in the ward: “I wouldn’t say anyone doesn’t understand, they’re just busy”
and “They can’t spend too much time with each patient because there’s so many
patients and so few nurses”. Although workload might not prevent all nurses being
according to many of the patients. For example, Frank compared wards where nurses
“I don’t know, I suppose it’s just that they don’t have the time down there
[different ward]. Mind you, they don’t really have the time up here but they
always try to make time for you, just to pop in and see if you’re OK, if there’s
anything you need. You know you’re not going to get lonely up here”.
176
Lindsey, a 33 year old female patient agreed with this, by describing incidents of
presence and absence of empathy within the same nurse whom she had previously
not sufficient to ensure effective empathy in the nursing role: “If they’ve got time
then she’ll listen to you. Time is the main problem, because they have to look after so
many patients”.
Although the discussion of positive incidents of empathy did not identify this theme
within the data, three patients also mentioned an absence of empathy where there had
been poor communication between staff. “When you come to a shift change, the
nurse who comes on doesn’t usually understand what has happened to you during
the day - that is annoying”. Handover of information is not the only potential barrier
within this theme. Potential conflicts or poor team relationships create issues which
divert nurses’ attention away from patients. David, a 58 year old male patient in the
“It’s very frustrating not knowing what’s happening or why...I sometimes feel
like the nurses don’t know what the doctors are doing and the doctors don’t
trust the nurses. The doctor put me on antidepressants and the nurse said I
wasn’t depressed. I was just worried because I didn’t know what was going
on. The communication isn’t there between the doctors and the nurses...”.
nurses on these wards are not entirely new findings. For example McCormack &
nursing leadership. However, it is an aspect of the process model of empathy that has
thus far been largely ignored, the focus instead placed centrally on the individual
So far, this discussion has focused on the areas of the template most relevant to the
two research questions. It is important to briefly describe the findings from the rest
healthcare settings.
Most of the patients began describing the incidents by discussing their own feelings
and behaviours which created the need for empathy from the nurse. This was to be
negative feelings which ranged in both nature and intensity. These negative emotions
over their situation. For other patients, there were feelings of sadness and upset. This
went as far as clinically diagnosed depression for two patients, associated to longer
and repeated stays in hospital and serious diagnoses (not recorded for confidentiality
reasons). Others reported feelings including frustration and shock. The large range
with the medical conditions suffered by patients or indirectly with the experiences of
treatment and being in hospital all seemed to act as the trigger for the subsequent
Sometimes the patients also identified their own behaviours which resulted from
these feelings. Most frequently, patients reported that their feelings resulting in
questioning of nurses and other staff. The questions tended to be medical ones
stemming from uncertainty over the condition or treatment. Some patients on the
other hand reported the opposite of asking for help, in that they would withdraw
from interaction with others. This highlights the difficult task faced by nurses in
Words such as kind, gentle, caring and approachable were frequently used
throughout all twenty of the interviews. Some of the patients said more empathic
nurses were more tolerant and patient of their particular circumstances which they
appreciated. The patients who mentioned it definitely felt it was part of the nurse’s
In addition to personality, patients also identified nurses who were more motivated
and engaged as ones who were more approachable to share their feelings with:
“She’s always there for your needs. If you need anything, she’s prepared to go and
179
do it for you”. This was highlighted particularly well by Andrew, a 55 year old male
“She’s got more go. She sorts you out very quickly, even though she’s very
busy, she pays attention to everybody... If you need anything, she’s prepared
to go and do it for you. Even down to one someone getting the wrong tea,
she’ll go and get them the right meal. It’s not part of her job but she does
it...She does her job as a nurse, the obs and everything, but it’s more than
that”.
currently a topic for much investigation, with drivers of engagement being identified
2009). This links back to the situational characteristics identified earlier and again
responsiveness are well documented and supported by the results of this analysis.
This is also the case for outcomes of empathy for both nurses and patients. As such,
these findings are not be explored in depth but are briefly described.
Most patients interviewed perceived that nurses who were more kind and caring
were more likely to engage in cognitive role taking such that they appeared to be
180
thinking about what the individual patient might be feeling or needing. Indeed,
absence of this perspective taking was identified many times as being a factor in
negative incidents of empathy. Patients only talked about the affective reactions of
own negative feelings. Words such as sympathy and concern were used frequently
“They did explain but they didn’t show much sympathy”. Although patients did not
identify parallel emotions or shared affect, this could have been because they are not
able to judge these kinds of processes within the nurse accurately. However, with
regard to the compassion and sympathy, there was a perception that this was
empathic because it was genuinely felt by the nurses. This was perceived to be a
genuine felt emotional reaction rather than merely the display of it, as described by
John, a 47 year old patient who had been in hospital for several months: “It bucks
you up, especially if you’re feeling low. It makes you think at least somebody cares,
you feel stronger you know and you don’t feel like you’re going to be just stuck in
here. She really does care... she was so good to me and my wife”.
processes described above included both satisfaction with the care they had received
and perceived improvement in health outcomes. Satisfaction with care fell into two
main themes: trust and confidence in the nurses, and the alleviation of negative
feelings that had acted as the trigger to the incidents described within the interviews.
Feelings of trust and confidence in the nurses were strongly reported by patients,
particularly resulting from the open communication and explanation of the current
181
and future situation for them. The alleviation of negative feelings, crossing into the
generation of positive feelings for some patients, was the most frequently identified
outcome of nurse empathy: “It bucks you up, especially if you’re feeling low. It
makes you think at least somebody care”. As well as reporting satisfaction with
care, patients also reported perceived benefits in terms of health outcomes of nurse
recover or improve, particularly resulting from the encouraging style of nurses: “You
always feel nervous in hospital but when the nurses are good to you like that, you
feel like you’re being looked after and you’re going to get out”. The participative
style was also used to great effect in gaining compliance with treatment in those
occasions where the patient’s negative feelings were around a proposed treatment or
course of action.
Whilst literature suggests that the long term outcomes of empathising for nurses may
involve greater job satisfaction and burnout (e.g. Larson & Yao, 2005), as mentioned
previously patients were unable to comment on these broader issues. Although the
patients were able to identify positive experiences for the nurses, they were unable to
say whether or not this resulted in overall job satisfaction which can of course be
influenced by a large range of other factors such as pay or leadership (Larrabee et al.,
2003). Instead, the nurse outcomes identified revolved around the impact of patients’
own behaviour towards the nurses following a demonstration of empathy (or lack of
it). First, patients reported that once they had experienced nurse empathy, they were
more likely to communicate openly with that nurse. Secondly, many patients
perceived that the enhanced communication between nurse and patient did provide
There were several limitations within this study. First, it is important to note that the
patients interviewed as part of this study were identified by the Ward Sisters and as
such the sample was not random. In addition, interviewing current patients may in
itself be problematic as there were perhaps times when patients were reluctant to
speak openly about people upon whom they are currently relying for care. One way
patients who had recently experienced a hospital stay. However, the critical incident
technique and use of a private room were both effective in encouraging patients to
identify example of empathy and speak at some length. The interview skills required
for this kind of research are also important to note. It is unlikely that a patient would
interviewing was not taken. In this case, having been a relative of a patient on one of
the wards involved in this study, it was possible for the researcher to understand and
A further limitation was that the findings of the template analysis could not be shown
to the participants for feedback (King, 2004). However, the process of gaining
ethical approval was a long and drawn out one and in the end this was a part of the
research design which was not possible. It took eleven months, two committee
ethics committees are possibly more used to medical research and so this kind of
proposal seemed to pose problems for them. Organisational access to collect data can
183
be fraught with difficulties at the best of times (Robson, 2002). Perhaps this is one of
the reasons why the concrete experiences of patients with regard to this topic
7.6.5 Summary
context. Importantly, the study aimed to gain the perspective of the receivers of
interpersonal nature of the topic. The themes resulting from the template analysis
supported the findings of the first two studies in terms of individual differences,
A basic premise of this thesis has been that greater understanding is required of how
foster the development of more effective training and development. Despite evidence
care and professional satisfaction, surprisingly little progress has been made in
in the medical field have sought to develop an integrated theory of empathy based on
model of empathy for this context. This was the process model of clinical empathy.
The key aspects of this model which were identified for investigation were:
Chapters four to seven outlined four studies which examined these aspects. This
chapter will begin by summarising their findings before moving on to consider the
The thesis began with a psychometric investigation of empathy and the five factor
examined the relationship between empathy and behaviour in the same sample of
differences that are the antecedents of empathising. The first study was conducted
using a general population sample and found a clear pattern of traits associated with
taking and empathic concern, while personal distress was characterised by greater
agreeableness related to those same scales included altruism, trust and tender-
mindedness.
The second study then moved on to look at the individual differences associated with
to data from a general population sample. A preference for perspective taking and
intelligence. Of interest, those who reported higher personal distress also reported
186
required.
behaviour were provided by trained assessors and compared to self report empathic
disposition. For those doctors trained in the UK and Ireland, fantasy, perspective
behaviour. However this relationship did not hold for doctors trained overseas.
response in this context. However study three also showed that patient ratings of
behaviour were highly skewed suggesting that there is no clear understanding of the
Therefore the final study adopted a different approach to extend understanding of the
healthcare context. Importantly, the research aimed to gain the perspective of the
more congruent with the interpersonal nature of the topic. Twenty semi-structured
interviews with patients within three medical wards were analysed. Themes resulting
from the template analysis supported the findings of the first two studies in terms of
also identified in considerable detail, including the provision of practical help as well
approach.
The thesis used a multidimensional model of empathy (Davis, 1983; 1996) and an
empathy in the healthcare setting. Figure 8.1 summarises the implication of the
findings by incorporating them into a model extended from Larson & Yao (2005).
Developments of this model in comparison to the Larson & Yao model can be seen
findings, as are those of the situational characteristics around the work environment
perspective taking and empathic concern were both found to fit with the model.
Personal distress, a kind of automatic emotional response to those in need, was not
intelligence therefore appears to fit well with the emotional labour context. Finally,
the interpersonal processes that are likely to be judged as empathic have been
188
Antecedents
• Practitioner characteristics
o Personality (emotional intelligence, personality)
o Role engagement
• Patient characteristics
o Negative emotions (sadness, frustration, anger, shock)
o Behaviours (asking questions, withdrawal)
• Situational characteristics
o Work environment
o Team communication
o Cultural similarity
Intrapersonal Processes
• Congruent reactive emotions (compassion and sympathy)
• Perspective Taking
Interpersonal Processes
• Helping behaviour
o Responding,
o Acting as an advocate
o Quick help
o Problem solving
• Prosocial behaviour
o Communication (initiating, listening, explaining, family)
o Considerate social style (participative, kind, reassuring, positive)
specified with much greater detail than previous models. The model as it now stands
As stated throughout this thesis, a basic premise of this research has been that greater
useful to guide the development of more effective training and development. The
development of the model in Figure 8.2 has clear implications for the strategies
These can be broadly aimed at three areas: training interventions; longer term
mixed at best (Stepien & Baernstein, 2006). Of particular note was the range of
theoretical definitions adopted within Chapter two, which have guided the design of
training content. Findings from this research clearly indicate that the concept of
courses could form part of longer development interventions; the experiences shared
by patients in the final study of the thesis could be put to very good use in the design
of training materials.
As empathy is often seen as part of personality, support for which has been found
from this research, there are those who believe that training may only have a limited
190
effect on practice as personality remains stable over time (e.g. Evans et al., 1993).
However, the focus on behaviour will be useful, as trait theory tells us that while our
natural preferences may not change over time, one can learn characteristics
adaptations to ensure a better fit to the requirements of the environment (McCrae &
prove useful for this purpose, providing a method of assessing empathy but also
interventions that could create a work environment where empathy is more likely to
occur should also be considered. This is building on the findings from the research of
similarity between patients and practitioners. Where employees are required to work
2003). Given that more engaged nurses were identified by patients as the ones who
provide more empathic interactions, it will be important to assess the link between
psychology. Issues of leadership and teamwork are likely to be key to this kind of
This thesis has raised many questions for future research. A limitation of all four
studies was the cross-sectional nature of data collection. Future research should aim
labour roles are known to have impact on well-being over time (Maslach, 1978). It is
setting. Although this thesis has focused entirely on the healthcare setting, it would
in other roles. Any roles in which interactions with people are part of the job are
likely to be relevant. Finally, the cross cultural challenge for empathy in healthcare
Summarising the thesis, key messages arising from this research are as follows:
192
can be used in personal development work. However, a wider focus than the
She [nurse] sometimes doesn’t get what they [patients] are on about. She’s
not from here, she doesn’t understand some of our little expressions, so
maybe she doesn’t know when they’re upset
London
193
References
Archer, R. L., Foushee, H. C., Davis, M. H., & Aderman, D. (1979). Emotional
empathy in a courtroom simulation: A person-situation interaction. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 9(3), 275-291.
Avery, J. (1985). Lawyers Tell What Turns Some Patients Litigious. Medical
Malpractice Review, 2, 35-37.
Axtell, C. M., Parker, S. K., Holman, D., & Totterdell, P. (2007). Enhancing
customer service: Perspective taking in a call centre. European Journal of Work
and Organizational Psychology, 16(2), 141-168.
Banister, P., Burman, E., Parker, I., Taylor, M., & Tindall, C. (1994). Qualitative
methods in psychology: A research guide. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Barnett, M., Howard, J., King, L., & Dino, G. (1981). Helping Behavior and the
Transfer of Empathy. The Journal of Social Psychology, 115(1), 125-132.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2003). The Essential Difference: Men, Women and the Extreme
Male Brain. London: Penguin Books.
194
Barrett-Lennard, G. T. (1993). The phases and focus of empathy. The British Journal
of Medical Psychology, 66(1), 3-14.
Bartram, A., Brown, A., Fleck, S., & Inceoglu, I. (2006). OPQ32 Technical Manual.
Thames Ditton: SHL.
Bartunek, J. M., & Seo, M. G. (2002). Qualitative research can add new meanings to
quantitative research, 23(2), 237-242.
Batson, C. D., & Coke, J. S. (1981). Empathy: A source of altruistic motivation for
helping? In J. P. Rushton & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Altruism and helping
behavior: Social, personality, and developmental perspectives (pp. 167-187).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Batson, C. D., Fultz, J., & Schoenrade, P. A. (1987). Distress and empathy: two
qualitatively distinct vicarious emotions with different motivational
consequences. Journal of Personality, 55(1), 19-39.
Batson, C. D., O’Quin, K., Fultz, J., Vanderplas, M., & Isen, A. M. (1983). Influence
of self-reported distress and empathy on egoistic versus altruistic motivation to
help. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 706-718.
Batson, C. D., & Shaw, L. L. (1991). Evidence for altruism: Toward a pluralism of
prosocial motives. Psychological Inquiry, 2(2), 107-122.
195
Beckman, H. B., Markakis, K. M., Suchman, A. L., & Frankel, R. M. (1994). The
Doctor-Patient Relationship and Malpractice: Lessons From Plaintiff
Depositions. Archives of Internal Medicine, 154(12), 1365-1370.
Bellini, L. M., Baime, M., & Shea, J. A. (2002). Variation of mood and empathy
during internship. Journal of The American Medical Association, 287(23),
3143-3146.
Bellini, L. M., & Shea, J. A. (2005). Mood change and empathy decline persist
during three years of internal medicine training. Academic Medicine, 80(2),
164-167.
Briggs, S. R., & Cheek, J. M. (1986). The role of factor analysis in the development
and evaluation of personality scales. Journal of Personality, 54(1), 106-148.
Briggs, S. R., Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1980). An analysis of the Self-
Monitoring Scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 679-686.
Brouwer, M. A. R., & Boros, S. (2010). The influence of intergroup contact and
ethnocultural empathy on employees’ attitudes toward diversity. Cognition,
Brain and Behavior, 14(3), 243-260.
Carey, J. C., Fox, E. A., & Spraggins, E. F. (1988). Replication of structure findings
regarding the interpersonal reactivity index. Measurement and Evaluation in
Counseling and Development, 21, 102-105.
Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Shyness and sociability. Journal of personality
and social psychology, 41(2), 330-339.
Chen, J. T., La Lopa, J. M., & Dang, D. K. (2008). Innovations in teaching: Impact
of patient empathy modeling on pharmacy students caring for the underserved.
American Journal of Pharmacy Education, 72(2), 40.
Chi-Ying Chung, R., & Bemak, F. (2002). The relationship of culture and empathy
in cross-cultural counseling. Journal of Counseling and Development, 80(2),
154-159.
Chlopan, B. E., McCain, M. L., Carbonell, J. L., & Hagen, R. L. (1985). Empathy:
Review of available measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
48(3), 635-653.
Christopher, F. S., Owens, L. A., & Stecker, L, S. H. (1993). Exploring the darkside
of courtship: A test of a model of male premarital sexual aggressiveness.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 55, 749-779.
Cialdini, R. B., Baumann, D. J., & Kenrick, D. T. (1981). Insights from sadness: A
three-step model of the development of altruism as hedonism. Developmental
Review, 1(3), 207-223.
Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S. L., Lewis, B. P., Luce, C., & Neuberg, S. L. (1997).
Reinterpreting the empathy-altruism relationship: when one into one equals
oneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(3), 481-494.
Ciarrochi, J., Chan, A., & Caputi, P. (2000). A critical evaluation of the emotional
intelligence construct. Personality and Individual Differences, 28(3), 539-561.
198
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Coke, J. S., Batson, C. D., & McDavis, K. (1978). Empathic mediation of helping: A
two-stage model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(7), 752-
766.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO PI-R professional manual. Odessa, FL:
Psychological Assessment Resources Inc.
Cross, D. G., & Sharpley, C. (1982). Measurement of empathy with the Hogan
Empathy Scale. Psychological Reports, 50, 62.
Cundiff, N. L., Nadler, J. T., & Swan, A. (2009). The Influence of Cultural Empathy
and Gender on Perceptions of Diversity Programs. Journal of Leadership &
Organizational Studies, 16(1), 97-110.
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion; reason and the human brain.
New York: Grosset.
Davies, M., Stankov, L., & Roberts, R. D. (1998). Emotional intelligence: in search
of an elusive construct. Journal of personality and social psychology, 75(4),
989-1015.
199
Dawda, D., & Hart, S. D. (2000). Assessing emotional intelligence: reliability and
validity of the Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i) in university
students. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 37-48.
Department of Health. (1999). Patient and public involvement in the new NHS.
London: NHS Executive.
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and
domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of personality and social
psychology, 93(5), 880-96.
Di Blasi, Z., Harkness, E., Ernst, E., Georgiou, A., & Kleijnen, J. (2001). Influence
of context effects on health outcomes: a systematic review. Lancet, 357(9258),
757-762.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Schaller, M., Miller, P., Carlo, G., Poulin, R., et al.
(1991). Personality and socialization correlates of vicarious emotional
responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(3), 459-470.
Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and
related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101(1), 91-119.
Elam, C., Stratton, T. D., & Andrykowski, M. A. (2001). Measuring the emotional
intelligence of medical school matriculants. Academic Medicine, 76(5), 507-8.
Evans, B. J., Stanley, R. O., & Burrows, G. D. (1993). Measuring medical students’
empathy skills. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 66(2), 121–33.
Feighny, K. M., Arnold, L., Monaco, M., Munro, S., & Earl, B. (1998). In pursuit of
empathy and its relationship to physician communication skills:
multidimensional empathy training for medical students. Annual Behavioural
Science Medical Education, 5, 13-21.
Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self
consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 43, 522-527.
Ferguson, E. (2001). Personality and coping traits: A joint factor analysis. British
journal of health psychology, 6(4), 311-325.
Fields, S. K., Hojat, M., Gonnella, J. S., Mangione, S., Kane, G., & Magee, M.
(2004). Comparisons of nurses and physicians on an operational measure of
empathy. Evaluation & the health professions, 27(1), 80-94.
Fletcher, C., & Kerslake, C. (1993). Candidate Anxiety Level and Assessment
Centre Performance. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 8(5), 19 – 23.
Frasure-Smith, N., Lespérance, F., & Talajic, M. (1995). The impact of negative
emotions on prognosis following myocardial infarction: is it more than
depression? Health Psychology, 14(5), 388-398.
Froman, R. D., & Peloquin, S. M.(2001). Rethinking the use of the Hogan Empathy
Scale: a critical psychometric analysis. American Journal of Occupational
Therapy, 55(5), 566-72.
Geher, G., Warner, R. M., & Brown, A. S. (2001). Predictive validity of the
emotional accuracy research scale. Intelligence, 29(5), 373-388.
Grandey, A. A. (2003). When "The Show Must Go on": Surface Acting and Deep
Acting as Determinants of Emotional Exhaustion and Peer-Rated Service
Delivery. Academy of Management Journal, 46(1), 86-96.
Guadagnoli, E., & Velicer, F. W. (1988). Relation of sample size to the stability of
component patterns. Psychological bulletin, 103(2), 265-75.
Gurman, A., & Razin, A. (1977). The therapist’s handbook for effective
psychotherapy. New York: Pergamon Press.
Hojat, M., Gonnella, J. S., Nasca, T. J., Mangione, S., Veloksi, J., & Magee, M.
(2002). The Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy: further psychometric data
and differences by gender and specialty at item level. Academic Medicine, 77,
58-60.
Hojat, M., Mangione, S., Nasca, T. J., Cohen, M. M., Gonnella, J. S., Erdmann, J. B.,
et al. (2001). The Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy: Development and
Preliminary Psychometric Data. Educational and Psychological Measurement,
61(2), 349-365.
Hojat, M., Mangione, S., Nasca, T. J., Gonnella, J. S., & Magee, M. (2005).
Empathy scores in medical school and ratings of empathic behavior in
residency training 3 years later. The Journal of Social Psychology, 145(6), 663-
72.
Hojat, M., Mangione, S., Nasca, T. J., Rattner, S., Erdmann, J. B., Gonnella, J. S., et
al. (2004). An empirical study of decline in empathy in medical school. Medical
Education, 38(9), 934-941.
Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis.
Psychometrica, 30, 179-185.
Hornsten, A., Lundman, B., Selstam, E. K., & Sandstrom, H. (2005). Patient
satisfaction with diabetes care. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 51(6), 609-617.
Hynes, C. A., Baird, A. A., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Differential role of the orbital
frontal lobe in emotional versus cognitive perspective-taking.
Neuropsychologia, 44(3), 374-383.
Jabbi, M., Swart, M., & Keysers, C. (2007). Empathy for positive and negative
emotions in the gustatory cortex. NeuroImage, 34(4), 1744-53.
Kane, G. C., Gotto, J. L., Mangione, S., West, S., & Hojat, M. (2007). Jefferson
Scale of Patient’s Perceptions of Physician Empathy: preliminary psychometric
data. Croatian Medical Journal, 48(1), 81-6.
Kaplan, S. H., Greenfield, S., & Ware, J. E. (1989). Assessing the effects of
physician-patient interactions on the outcomes of chronic disease. Medical
Care, 27(3), 110-127.
Kellett, J. B., Humphrey, R. H., & Sleeth, R. G. (2002). Empathy and complex task
performance: two routes to leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 13(5), 523-
544.
Kerr, W. A., & Speroff, B. J. (1954). Validation and evaluation of the empathy test.
Journal of General Psychology, 50, 269-276.
King, N. (2004). Using templates in the thematic analysis of texts. Essential guide to
qualitative methods in organizational research, 256–270.
King, N., Carroll, C., Newton, P., & Dornan, T. (2002). "You can’t cure it so you
have to endure it": The Experience of Adaptation to Diabetic Renal Disease.
Qualitative Health Research, 12(3), 329-346.
Kolata, G. (2005). When the doctor is in, but you wish he wasn’t. The New York
Times. Retrieved March 19, 2011, from
http://www.nytimes.com/2005/11/30/health/30patient.html.
La Monica, E. L. (1996). Empathy construct rating scale (pp. 1-8). Santa Clara, CA:
Xicom.
La Monica, E. L., Carew, D. K., Windsor, A. E., Haase, A. M., & Blanchard, K. H.
(1976). Empathy training as the major thrust of a staff development program.
Nursing Research, 25(6), 447-451.
La Monica, E. L., Wolf, R. M., Madea, A. R., & Oberst, M. T. (1987). Empathy and
Nursing Care Outcomes. Scholarly Inquiry in Nursing Practice, 1, 197-213.
Landy, F. J., & Conte, J. M. (2007). Work in the 21st century: An introduction to
industrial and organizational psychology. Oxford: Blackwell.
Larson, E. B., & Yao, X. (2005). Clinical empathy as emotional labor in the patient-
physician relationship. Journal Of The American Medical Association, 293(9),
1100-1106.
207
Larzelere, R. E., & Mulaik, S. A. (1977). Single-sample tests for many correlations.
Psychological Bulletin, 84(3), 557-569.
Law, K. S., Wong, C.-S., & Song, L. J. (2004). The construct and criterion validity
of emotional intelligence and its potential utility for management studies. The
Journal of applied psychology, 89(3), 483-96.
Layton, J. M., & Wykle, M. H. (1990). A validity study of four empathy instruments.
Research in Nursing & Health, 13(5), 319-325.
Lilja, J., Larsson, S., Hamilton, D., & Issakainen, J. (2000). Empathy as a
communication strategy in the pharmacy - a study based on cognitive and
behavioural analysis. International Journal of Pharmacy Practice, 8(3), 176-
187.
Lincoln, Y., & Guba, E. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverly Hills: Sage.
Mackay, R. C., Hughes, J. R., & Carver, J. (1990). Empathy in the Helping
Relationship. New York: Springer.
Maslach, C. (1978). Job burnout: How people cope. Public Welfare, 36, 56-58.
208
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. (2000). Models of Emotional Intelligence.
London: Cambridge University Press.
Mehrabian, A., Young, A. L., & Sato, S. (1988). Emotional empathy and associated
individual differences. Current Psychology, 7(3), 221-240.
Mercer, S. W., & Reynolds, W. J. (2002). Empathy and quality of care. The British
Journal of General Practice, 52, 9-12.
209
Mercer, S. W., Maxwell, M., Heaney, D, & Watt, G. (2004). The consultation and
relational empathy (CARE) measure: development and preliminary validation
and reliability of an empathy-based consultation process measure. Family
practice, 21(6), 699-705.
Mercer, S. W., McConnachie, A., Maxwell, M., Heaney, D, & Watt, G. (2005).
Relevance and practical use of the Consultation and Relational Empathy
(CARE) Measure in general practice. Family practice, 22(3), 328-334.
Meyer, J. P., & Mulherin, A. (1980). From attribution to helping: An analysis of the
mediating effects of affect and expectancy. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 39(2), 201-210.
Moore, P. J., Adler, N. E., & Robertson, P. A. (2000). Medical malpractice: the
effect of doctor-patient relations on medical patient perceptions and malpractice
intentions. The Western Journal of Medicine, 173(4), 244-50.
Morse, J. M., Anderson, G., Bottorff, J. L., Yonge, O., O’Brien, B., Solberg, S. M.,
et al. (1992). Exploring empathy: a conceptual fit for nursing practice? Journal
of Nursing Scholarship, 24(4), 273-280.
Morse, J. M., Bottorff, J., Anderson, G., O'Brien, B., & Solberg, S. (2006). Beyond
empathy: expanding expressions of caring. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 53,
75–87.
Moss, F. A., Hunt, T., & Omwake, K. T. (1949). Manual for the social intelligence
test, revised form. Washington, DC: Center for Psychological Service.
Moss, F. A., Hunt, T., Omwake, K. T., & Ronning, M. M. (1927). Social Intelligence
Test. Washington, DC: Center for Psychological Service.
Muthen, B., & Kaplan, D. (1985). A comparison of some methodologies for the
factor analysis of non-normal Likert variables. Journal of Mathematical and
Statistical Psychology, 38(2), 171-189.
O’Connor, B. P. (2000). SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of
components using parallel analysis and Velicer’s MAP test. Behavior Research
Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 32(3), 396-402.
Pallant, J. (2007). SPSS Survival Manual (3rd ed). Maidenhead: Open University
Press.
Parker, S. K., & Axtell, C. M. (2001). Seeing Another Viewpoint: Antecedents and
Outcomes of Employee Perspective Taking. Academy of Management Journal,
44(6), 1085-1100.
211
Patterson, F, Ferguson, E., Lane, P., Farrell, K., Martlew, J., & Wells, A. (2000). A
competency model for General Practice: Implications for selection and
development. British Journal of General Practice, 50, 188-193.
Patterson, F, Ferguson, E., Norfolk, T., & Lane, P. (2005). A new selection system to
recruit general practice registrars: preliminary findings from a validation study.
British Medical Journal, 330(4), 711-714.
Peplau, H. E. (1952). Interpersonal Relations in Nursing. New York: G.P. Putnam &
Sons.
Perez, J., Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2005). Measuring Trait Emotional
Intelligence. In R. Schultz & R. Roberts (Eds.), Emotional Intelligence: An
International Handbook (pp. 181-201). Gottingen: Hogrefe.
Peterson, C., Semmel, A., Baeyer, C., Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., &
Seligman, M. E. P. (1982). The attributional Style questionnaire. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 6(3), 287-299.
Petrides, K. V., Pérez-González, J. C., & Furnham, A. (2007). On the criterion and
incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence. Cognition and Emotion, 21,
26-55.
212
Plank, R. E., Minton, A. P., & Reid, D. A. (1996). A short measure of perceived
empathy. Psychological Reports, 79(3), 1219-1226.
Polit, D., & Hunglar, B. (1983). Nursing research. Principles and methods.
Philadelphia: Sage.
Pound, P., Gompertz, P., & Ebrahim, S. (1998). A patient-centred study of the
consequences of stroke. Clinical Rehabilitation, 12(4), 338-347.
Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate
bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(01), 1-20.
Pulos, S., Elison, J., & Lennon, R. (2004). The hierarchical structure of the
interpersonal reactivity index. Social Behavior and Personality: an
international journal, 32(4), 355-359.
Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1994). Raven’s Advanced Progressive
Matrices. Pearson Assessment.
Regan, D. T., & Totten, J. (1975). Empathy and attribution: turning observers into
actors. Journal of personality and social psychology, 32(5), 850-6.
Reynolds, W. J., Scott, B., & Jessiman, W. C. (1999). Empathy has not been
measured in clients’ terms or effectively taught: a review of the literature.
Journal of Advanced Nursing, 30(5), 1177-1185.
Reynolds, W. J., & Scott, B. (2000). Do nurses and other professional helpers
normally display much empathy? Journal of Advanced Nursing, 31(1), 226-234.
213
Rietveld, S., & Prins, P. J. (1998). The relationship between negative emotions and
acute subjective and objective symptoms of childhood asthma. Psychological
Medicine, 28(2), 407-415.
Robinson, D., Perryman, S., & Hayday, S. (2004). The Drivers of Employee
Engagement. Brighton: Institute for Employment Studies.
Robson, C. (2002). Real world research: A resource for social scientists and
practitioner –researchers. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
Russell, D., & McAuley, E. (1986). Causal attributions, causal dimensions, and
affective reactions to success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 50(6), 1174-1185. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.6.1174.
Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, C., & Palfai, T. P. (1995)
Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: Exploring emotional intelligence using
the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. In J. W. Pennebaker (Ed .), Emotion, Disclosure, &
Health. Washington: American Psychological Association.
Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Bobik, C., Coston, T. D., Greeson, C., Jedlicka, C.,
Rhodes, E., & Wendorf, G. (2001). Emotional intelligence and interpersonal
relations. The Journal of Social Psychology, 141(4), 523-536.
Schutte, N.S., Malouff, J.M., Hall, L.E., Haggerty, D.J., Cooper, J.T., Golden, C.J.,
et al. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional
intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 167-177.
Scotland, E., Mathews, K. E., & Sherman, S. (1978). Empathy, fantasy, and helping.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Silvester, J., Patterson, Fiona, Koczwara, A., & Ferguson, E. (2007). "Trust me...":
psychological and behavioral predictors of perceived physician empathy.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(2), 519-527.
Simmons, J., Roberge, L., Kendrick, S. B., & Richards, B. (1995). The Interpersonal
Relationship in Clinical Practice: The Barrett-Lennard Relationship Inventory
as an Assessment Instrument. Evaluation & the Health Professions, 18(1), 103-
112.
Sparr, L. F., Gordon, G. H., Hickam, D. H., & Girard, D. E. (1988). The doctor-
patient relationship during medical internship: the evolution of dissatisfaction.
Social Science & Medicine, 26(11), 1095-1101.
Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Holohan, C. K. (1979). Negative and positive
components of psychological masculinity and femininity and their relationships
to self reports of neurotic and acting out behaviors. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 37, 1673-1682.
215
Spencer, J. (2004). Decline in empathy in medical education: how can we stop the
rot? Medical Education, 38(9), 916-918.
Stepien, K. A., & Baernstein, A. (2006). Educating for Empathy. Journal of General
Internal Medicine, 21(5), 524-530.
Stinson, L., & Ickes, W. (1992). Empathic accuracy in the interactions of male
friends versus male strangers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
62, 787-797.
Strayer, J., & Eisenberg, N. (1987). Empathy viewed in context. In N. Eisenberg & J.
Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 389-398). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Sue, D., & Sundbery, N. D. (1996). Research and research hypotheses about
effectiveness in intercultural counseling. In P. B. Pedersen, J. Draguns, W.
Lonner, & J. Trimble (Eds.), Counseling across cultures (pp. 323-352).
California: Sage.
Tarrant, C., Windridge, K., Boulton, M., Baker, R., & Freeman, G. K. (2003).
Qualitative study of the meaning of personal care in general practice. British
Medical Journal, 326, 1310-1316.
The Scottish Office. (1997). Designed to care: Renewing the National Health
Service in Scotland. London: The Stationary Office.
Verby, J. E., Holden, P., & Davis, R. H. (1979). Peer review of consultations in
primary care: The use of audio-visual recordings. British Medical Journal, 1,
1686-1688.
Vreeke, G., & van der Mark, I. L. (2003). Empathy, an integrative model. New Ideas
in Psychology, 21(3), 177-207.
Walter, F. H., Cole, M. S., & Humphrey, R. H. (2011). Emotional Intelligence: Sine
Qua Non of Leadership or Folderol? Academy of Management Perspectives,
25(1), 45-59.
Wang, Y.-W., Bleier, J., Davidson, M., Savoy, H., Tan, J., & Yakushko, O. (2003).
The scale of ethnocultural empathy. Development, validation, and reliability.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 2, 221-234.
217
Wasserman, R. C., Inui, T. S., Barriatua, R. D., Carter, W. B., & Lippincott, P.
(1984). Pediatric clinicians’ support for parents makes a difference: an
outcome-based analysis of clinician-parent interaction. Pediatrics, 74(6), 1047-
1053.
Winefield, H. R., Murrell, T. G., & Clifford, J. (1995). Process and outcomes in
general practice consultations: problems in defining high quality care. Social
science medicine, 41(7), 969-975.
Woloschuk, W., Harasym, P. H., & Temple, W. (2004). Attitude change during
medical school: a cohort study. Medical Education, 38(5), 522-534.
Yarnold, P. R., Bryant, F. B., Nightingale, S. D., & Martin, G. J. (1996). Assessing
physician empathy using the Interpersonal Reactivity Index: A measurement
model and cross-sectional analysis. Psychology, Health and Medicine, 1, 207-
221.
218
Zabar, S., Hanley, K., Kachur, E., Stevens, D., Schwartz, M. D., Pearlman, E.,
Adams, J., Felix, K., Lipkin, M., & Kalet, A. (2006), “Oh! She Doesn't Speak
English!” Assessing Resident Competence in Managing Linguistic and Cultural
Barriers. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 21, 510–513.
Zahn-Waxler, C., & Robinson, J. (1995). Empathy and guilt: early origins of feelings
of responsibility. In K. Fischer & J. Tangney (Eds.), Self conscious emotions:
Shame, guilt, embarrassment and pride (pp. 143-173). New York: Guilford
Press.
Zwick, W. R., & Velicer, F. W. (1986). Comparison of five rules for determining the
number of components to retain. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 432-442.
219
This thesis began with the aim of designing an intervention to increase empathy in
doctors. For several reasons, this aim altered. The first was that, upon reading
evaluations of empathy training interventions, very little was found by way of detail
or consensus on what empathy training or development interventions should look
like in this context. The heart of this problem was a lack of consensus on a definition
of empathy in healthcare settings. At times, literature took a very broad approach,
but at times it was conflicting, with disagreement over the role of an emotional
response to patients. Some (e.g. Hardee, 2003) would view detached concern for
patients as insufficient, with a genuine emotional response required to demonstrate
empathy. Others, including researchers at the Jefferson Medical School in the US
(Hojat et al., 2004), view an emotional response as an interference in the process of
accurate diagnosis.
After reviewing the literature, the decision was taken that this thesis should therefore
take a step backwards. The research therefore seeks to build a model of empathy in
medicine that came from empirical research as opposed to opinion or literature
review. This model would then provide a solid foundation for designing
interventions in the future. In deciding on a model and measure to test as the central
theme of the thesis, Davis’ multidimensional model and corresponding measure of
empathy were chosen as an appropriate starting point.
The first data obtained were in fact the self report ratings of empathy and emotional
intelligence (study two) alongside the behavioural ratings within an assessment
centre context (study three). The aim of this study was to compare empathy with
emotional intelligence, then comparing their relative criterion-related validities by
examining the relationships with behaviour.
On completion of this study, it was noted that the measure being used would benefit
from greater evidence of construct validity in terms of recent personality
frameworks, namely the five factor model. At this stage then, the self report empathy
and personality questionnaires were administered (study one). It was a shame not to
220
get access to detailed personality data for doctors; instead study one used a general
population study. However, this was an issue of access at the time, in taking the
decision to conduct a facet level analysis. It is certainly something for future
research.
Study four represented a break in the research in several ways. Primarily, the
methodology switched from quantitative to qualitative. This stemmed from a desire
to employ a more authentic method for the topic in question: the research conducted
using psychometric questionnaires was felt insufficient in fully capturing such an
interpersonal dynamic concept. In addition, the study focused on patient perceptions
of nurses’ empathy. Again this was not ideal as the other research was more focused
on empathy in doctors. However the hospital that agreed to take part in the project
and granted me ethical approval were more interested in the question of empathy in
nurses as they felt this was a more important issue for their practice. This is the
reality of conducting applied research – designs must be agreed in partnership with
organisations and as such need to be adapted to take their needs into consideration.
In the end, the study was valuable and there is much research to suggest that it is not
necessarily the role of the healthcare practitioner but the needs of the patient that
dictate the characteristics of an empathic interaction.
Overall, while the studies did not progress as originally intended, they do show clear
progression in the exploration of a model of empathy in healthcare roles. They have
hopefully built a solid foundation for the design and evaluation of empathy
development interventions, as well as identifying many further opportunities for
worthwhile and interesting research.
221
The following statements inquire about your thoughts and feelings in a variety of situations.
For each item, indicate how well it describes you by choosing the appropriate letter on the
scale: A, B, C, D, or E. When you have decided on your answer, circle the appropriate letter.
Read each item carefully before responding, answering as honestly as you can.
Within the Wales Deanery psychometric tests are currently being piloted as part of the
selection centre process. This pilot work is entirely separate from the actual selection and
recruitment process. None of the results from the psychometric tests will be used in the
decision-making process.
Your Participation
I would be very grateful if you could spare around two hours to be involved in this piloting
of psychometric materials as part of the selection centre. Your participation is entirely
voluntary. Your decision about whether or not to participate will have no bearing whatsoever
on the selection and appointments process. If you choose not to participate, you will not be
adversely affecting your chances of being selected.
For the project, I will be asking you to complete 2 questionnaires, the Interpersonal
Reactivity Index which is a measure of empathy and the Bar-On EQ-I measure of emotional
intelligence. I would like to look at how your performance on the psychometric tests relates
to your performance in the selection centre exercises.
It should be stressed that all questionnaires will be anonymous. Findings will be discussed in
general terms only. Participation is voluntary and all information will be kept strictly
confidential.
If at any stage you wish to withdraw from the project, you are entirely free to do so. This
will in no way have any bearing on the outcome of this assessment centre. If you wish to
receive further information about the project following your participation, please provide
details on the following page so that I can send this to you.
Yours faithfully,
Helen Wilkin
224
Record of Consent
Signed
I confirm that I have volunteered to
participate in the piloting of psychometric
tests.
You may withdraw your consent at any time during or after completion of the
psychometric tests.
If you would like to receive a summary of the findings of this project, please leave an
address where this can be sent (this will not be used for research purposes and will be kept
strictly confidential).
Factor Loadings
Items 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Factor 1
Item 56 0.82 0.08 0.03 0.07 -0.09 -0.10 -0.01 -0.01
Item 100 0.70 0.00 -0.01 -0.08 0.06 0.14 0.12 -0.25
Item 114 0.68 0.05 -0.05 -0.09 0.05 0.05 0.02 -0.21
Item 70 0.68 0.19 0.08 0.19 -0.04 -0.07 -0.05 -0.03
Item 85 0.67 -0.01 0.09 0.08 0.14 -0.07 -0.11 -0.08
Item 129 0.65 0.13 0.01 -0.15 0.09 -0.08 0.11 -0.05
Item 47 0.65 0.13 0.02 0.09 0.05 -0.20 -0.07 0.08
Item 40 0.63 -0.02 0.09 -0.15 -0.01 -0.16 0.04 0.08
Item 106 0.59 -0.03 -0.08 -0.05 0.26 0.05 -0.10 0.12
Item 91 0.57 -0.01 0.11 0.18 0.06 -0.22 -0.14 -0.03
Item 26 0.51 -0.10 -0.10 -0.17 0.02 -0.11 -0.17 -0.21
Item 77 0.49 0.22 0.09 0.01 -0.12 -0.02 -0.17 0.11
Item 02 0.49 -0.02 0.02 0.21 0.01 -0.28 -0.27 0.09
Item 24 0.46 0.00 0.16 -0.15 -0.26 -0.21 0.01 -0.19
Item 54 0.45 0.02 -0.09 -0.07 0.36 0.09 -0.04 0.02
Item 11 0.44 -0.10 0.07 -0.28 -0.09 0.05 -0.11 -0.09
Item 51 0.39 0.21 0.16 0.08 0.17 -0.15 0.04 -0.14
Item 80 0.38 -0.13 0.09 -0.18 0.17 -0.04 0.08 -0.13
Item 81 0.36 -0.13 0.16 -0.20 0.33 -0.02 0.00 -0.09
Item 21 0.33 0.08 0.11 0.02 -0.08 -0.16 -0.04 -0.11
Item 127 0.30 0.17 0.08 -0.10 0.10 0.05 -0.08 -0.14
Factor 2
Item 86 0.06 0.64 0.04 -0.09 -0.04 -0.11 -0.15 0.08
Item 102 0.06 0.61 -0.14 -0.09 0.03 0.02 -0.03 -0.11
Item 83 0.13 0.55 0.19 -0.20 -0.22 -0.10 0.02 0.06
Item 58 -0.06 0.51 0.02 0.03 0.16 0.13 -0.13 -0.12
Item 42 0.10 0.50 -0.08 -0.04 0.11 -0.17 -0.17 0.07
Item 76 0.15 0.50 -0.16 -0.03 0.09 -0.07 0.17 -0.14
Item 97 0.04 0.45 0.15 -0.04 0.07 0.26 -0.11 -0.23
Item 53 0.02 0.39 0.14 0.23 0.07 -0.37 0.00 -0.07
Item 104 0.16 0.36 -0.11 -0.24 0.10 0.00 0.14 -0.18
Item 38 -0.09 0.35 0.29 0.09 0.03 -0.20 -0.12 0.13
Item 39 0.26 -0.33 0.01 -0.11 0.08 -0.24 -0.30 -0.04
Factor 3
Item 107 0.10 0.07 0.58 0.09 -0.06 0.12 -0.05 -0.15
Item 48 0.14 0.12 0.56 -0.17 -0.05 0.09 0.07 -0.04
Item 92 -0.04 -0.31 0.55 -0.04 -0.14 -0.12 -0.10 -0.09
Item 19 0.04 0.00 0.55 -0.07 0.14 -0.01 0.16 0.07
Item 111 -0.03 0.00 0.55 0.00 0.03 -0.17 0.06 -0.16
Item 118 0.06 0.10 0.54 -0.14 0.05 0.09 -0.16 -0.09
Item 126 0.13 -0.07 0.54 -0.07 0.11 -0.24 0.02 0.00
Item 03 0.00 -0.12 0.52 0.07 0.05 0.07 -0.05 0.02
Item 32 0.00 -0.01 0.51 -0.20 -0.15 -0.07 0.06 -0.20
Item 67 0.15 -0.24 0.42 -0.06 -0.01 -0.14 -0.05 -0.08
Item 121 0.31 0.12 0.38 -0.06 0.03 0.11 0.03 -0.03
Item 82 -0.07 0.12 0.36 0.05 -0.27 -0.29 -0.13 -0.14
Item 93 -0.09 -0.01 0.36 -0.08 -0.14 -0.10 -0.21 -0.31
Item 75 -0.02 0.11 0.35 -0.16 -0.11 -0.07 -0.19 -0.22
Item 46 -0.01 0.12 0.34 -0.03 0.19 -0.17 0.03 0.07
Item 68 0.04 0.22 0.32 0.08 0.15 -0.23 -0.07 0.02
Item 66 0.09 0.00 0.25 0.19 0.23 -0.12 -0.19 -0.11
226
Factor Loadings
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Factor 4
Item 45 0.04 0.16 -0.03 -0.64 -0.04 -0.07 -0.01 -0.14
Item 15 -0.20 0.21 0.10 -0.55 0.13 -0.08 -0.04 -0.06
Item 29 0.00 0.20 0.04 -0.51 0.01 -0.14 0.05 -0.16
Item 60 0.06 -0.03 0.13 -0.47 0.27 -0.15 0.19 -0.26
Item 89 -0.07 0.03 0.13 -0.45 0.24 -0.05 0.15 -0.25
Item 20 0.20 -0.09 0.27 -0.41 0.04 0.14 -0.18 0.05
Item 04 0.00 -0.05 0.18 -0.41 0.01 -0.03 -0.20 -0.04
Item 108 0.25 -0.09 0.18 -0.40 0.13 -0.05 -0.10 -0.11
Item 78 0.15 0.07 0.21 -0.39 0.16 0.18 -0.23 -0.01
Item 08 0.25 0.26 0.02 -0.39 -0.21 -0.07 0.12 0.10
Item 30 -0.04 0.16 0.10 0.36 0.33 0.03 -0.06 -0.15
Item 88 0.13 -0.07 -0.09 -0.31 0.27 -0.04 -0.22 0.00
Item 06 0.25 -0.11 -0.05 -0.29 0.18 -0.06 0.12 -0.28
Item 63 0.02 -0.12 0.07 -0.29 0.29 -0.15 -0.28 0.15
Item 36 0.23 0.10 0.19 0.26 0.08 -0.21 -0.13 -0.22
Factor 5
Item 98 0.05 0.06 -0.02 0.01 0.65 -0.05 0.08 -0.10
Item 72 0.06 -0.04 -0.12 0.05 0.58 -0.08 -0.05 -0.16
Item 124 0.05 0.19 -0.16 0.01 0.55 0.09 -0.01 -0.21
Item 84 -0.06 0.01 0.11 0.10 0.49 -0.17 -0.19 0.08
Item 55 0.05 0.03 0.01 -0.14 0.49 -0.19 0.16 0.15
Item 90 0.06 0.09 0.11 -0.03 0.45 0.05 -0.07 -0.07
Item 110 0.31 -0.07 0.18 -0.10 0.43 0.03 -0.09 -0.07
Item 95 0.34 -0.08 0.18 -0.06 0.40 -0.01 -0.09 0.11
Item 44 -0.03 0.01 -0.05 -0.27 0.35 -0.05 -0.20 0.01
Item 128 0.14 0.00 0.24 0.08 0.28 -0.24 0.10 0.01
Item 112 0.08 0.06 0.18 -0.23 0.27 0.03 -0.10 0.02
Item 69 0.18 0.02 0.17 0.11 0.26 -0.20 -0.23 -0.06
Factor 6
Item 23 0.02 0.13 -0.05 0.02 0.05 -0.68 0.05 -0.18
Item 07 -0.05 -0.16 -0.10 -0.25 0.02 -0.68 -0.08 0.01
Item 52 0.02 0.07 0.11 0.11 0.01 -0.61 -0.03 -0.14
Item 116 0.12 0.19 -0.13 -0.07 -0.07 -0.61 -0.04 -0.18
Item 10 0.04 0.05 0.01 0.07 0.30 -0.56 0.11 0.08
Item 37 0.09 -0.11 0.11 -0.22 -0.13 -0.48 0.10 0.09
Item 96 0.17 -0.14 0.14 -0.18 0.08 -0.45 -0.06 -0.14
Item 35 0.23 0.26 0.01 -0.04 -0.01 -0.40 -0.14 -0.08
Item 31 0.30 -0.08 -0.20 -0.08 0.05 -0.39 -0.35 -0.06
Item 113 0.18 -0.21 0.12 -0.10 0.30 -0.31 -0.15 0.02
Factor 7
Item 13 0.03 0.21 -0.26 0.06 0.10 0.00 -0.60 -0.14
Item 64 0.28 0.06 0.14 0.04 -0.13 -0.01 -0.53 -0.13
Item 130 0.03 0.27 -0.18 -0.01 0.25 0.06 -0.51 -0.14
Item 117 0.08 0.35 -0.21 -0.02 0.14 0.05 -0.50 -0.22
Item 33 0.21 0.00 0.12 -0.44 -0.15 0.09 -0.45 0.02
Item 14 -0.02 -0.18 0.18 0.05 -0.08 -0.15 -0.44 -0.27
Item 122 0.37 0.01 0.10 0.04 -0.14 0.06 -0.43 -0.18
Item 49 0.15 0.12 0.26 -0.25 -0.12 0.10 -0.40 -0.08
Item 17 0.05 0.05 -0.03 0.12 0.17 -0.25 -0.39 0.00
Item 18 -0.08 0.18 0.14 -0.06 0.18 -0.03 -0.34 -0.01
Item 62 0.07 -0.29 0.27 -0.09 0.22 -0.20 -0.30 0.12
227
Factor Loadings
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Factor 8
Item 28 0.03 -0.03 -0.07 -0.02 -0.05 -0.19 -0.15 -0.61
Item 103 0.04 0.07 0.06 -0.03 0.16 -0.12 -0.08 -0.55
Item 87 0.07 0.14 0.08 -0.04 0.10 -0.20 -0.02 -0.52
Item 131 -0.01 -0.11 0.11 -0.03 -0.07 0.07 -0.06 -0.48
Item 74 0.16 -0.01 0.09 -0.06 0.12 -0.05 0.12 -0.46
Item 73 0.05 0.15 -0.13 -0.04 -0.03 0.04 -0.17 -0.45
Item 43 -0.02 0.20 0.12 0.27 0.13 0.07 -0.18 -0.42
Item 59 0.28 -0.27 0.18 -0.05 0.03 0.00 -0.01 -0.38
Item 01 -0.04 0.13 0.03 -0.29 0.11 -0.16 0.09 -0.32
Item 125 0.25 0.09 0.07 -0.04 0.09 -0.19 0.10 -0.28
What is involved
I am talking to a range of patients, to find out how and when nurses are able to show
empathy to them. Participation is voluntary. It is up to you to decide whether or not to take
part. If you do decide to take part you will be given this information sheet to keep and be
asked to sign a consent form. If you decide to take part you are still free to withdraw at any
time and without giving a reason. A decision to withdraw at any time, or a decision not to
take part, will not affect the standard of care you receive.
For the project, I would like to interview you for 30 minutes to talk about nurses who are
good at empathising with patients as well as those who find it more difficult. I will ask for
examples of how these people differ from each other. You will not need to mention names
and all interviews will be confidential. Following the interviews, if you have any questions
or concerns, you will able to contact me to discuss them. I will be tape recording the 30
minute interview to make sure that I record all of the information accurately. These tapes
will be kept securely until I have written up the information which I need, all of which will
be anonymous. I will then destroy the tapes so that they are unusable.
If you have had a bad experience or find it upsetting to talk about this, please do not feel you
need to take part. If you wish to complain, or have any concerns about any aspect of the way
you have been approached or treated during the course of the study, the normal National
Health Service complaints mechanism should be available to you.
If you consent to take part in this study I will not have access to your medical records. Your
name will not be disclosed, you will not be recognised from the written information and all
information will be kept strictly confidential. If you take part, a copy of the consent form
will be kept on your hospital notes.
229
The results of the research will be written up as part of my PhD. They may also be published
in a journal for other psychologists. You will not be identified in any written or published
reports.
The research is being funded by Goldsmiths College, University of London. The James
Cook University Hospital will not be paid for the study, nor will I.
The Research Ethics Committee here at the James Cook University hospital has reviewed
this study to make sure it is ethical.
You can keep this copy of the information sheet and also a copy of your signed consent form
if you agree to take part.
I would very much appreciate your participation in this project. If you are interested in
taking part, please contact me by either telephone or email (details above) and we will
arrange a time to meet. I look forward to hearing from you,
Yours faithfully,
Helen Wilkin
230
3. Please can you tell me about an incident you have experienced here when one of
the nurses was able to empathise with you?
Prompts: Can you give a specific example?
What was the situation?
What did the nurse do?
Why did that happen?
What was the outcome?
3. Please can you tell me about an incident you have experienced here when one of
the nurses was NOT able to empathise with you?
Prompts: Can you give a specific example?
What was the situation?
What did the nurse do?
Why did that happen?
What was the outcome?
4. Finally, I will be sending summary of the research to anyone who wishes to see it.
Would you like to receive this? If so, please leave contact details which will be kept
strictly confidential.