Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

Monash 128321

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 309

DEVELOPMENT OF A NEW SCALE TO ASSESS HYPERACTIVITY,

IMPULSIVITY, AND INATTENTIVE BEHAVIOURS ACROSS

NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS

Nerelie Claire Freeman, B.A.(Hons), Grad.Dip.Ed, M.Ed.Psych

This thesis is submitted in fulfilment


of the requirements for the Degree of
Doctor of Philosophy

Centre for Developmental Psychiatry and Psychology


Southern Clinical School
and
School of Psychological Sciences
Faulty of Medicine, Nursing and Health Sciences
Monash University
Clayton, Victoria 3800

June, 2014
Notice 1
Under the Copyright Act 1968, this thesis must be used only under the normal conditions of
scholarly fair dealing. In particular no results or conclusions should be extracted from it, nor
should it be copied or closely paraphrased in whole or in part without the written consent of the
author. Proper written acknowledgement should be made for any assistance obtained from this
thesis.
TABLE OF CONTENTS

List of Figures .......................................................................................... vi


List of Tables .......................................................................................... vii

General Declaration .............................................................................. viii

List of Publications and Presentations during Candidature .............. ix

Acknowledgments ................................................................................... xi

Abstract ..................................................................................................... 1

CHAPTER 1 DEFINING TYPICAL AND ATYPICAL ATTENTION


AND ACTIVITY ...................................................................................... 3
1.1 What is attention? .................................................................................................................................. 4
1.1.1 Attention in everyday life ............................................................................................................... 4
1.1.2 Attention in a controlled environment ............................................................................................ 4
1.2 Typical development of attention and activity ....................................................................................... 6
1.3 Atypical development of attention and activity: The case of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder ... 9
1.3.1 Diagnostic criteria ......................................................................................................................... 10
1.3.2 Theories of ADHD........................................................................................................................ 14
1.3.3 Demographic, genetic and environmental correlates .................................................................... 16
1.3.4 Long-term outcomes ..................................................................................................................... 17
1.3.5 Comorbid psychopathology .......................................................................................................... 18

CHAPTER 2 ISSUES OF COMPLEXITY IN DIAGNOSING ADHD IN


INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY ......................................................... 20
CHAPTER 3 COGNITIVE AND BEHAVIOURAL PHENOTYPES OF
ATTENTION AND ACTIVITY IN CHILDREN WITH INTELLECTUAL
DISABILITY 26
3.1 Idiopathic intellectual disability .......................................................................................................... 27
3.2 Diagnoses with intellectual disability as a known cause ..................................................................... 31
3.2.1 Autism spectrum disorder ............................................................................................................. 32
3.2.2 Down Syndrome ........................................................................................................................... 35
3.2.3 Fragile X Syndrome ...................................................................................................................... 37
3.2.4 Williams Syndrome ...................................................................................................................... 40
3.2.5 Cross-syndrome studies ................................................................................................................ 43

CHAPTER 4 CURRENT CHECKLISTS AND RATING SCALES


MEASURING SYMPTOMS OF INATTENTION, HYPERACTIVITY,
AND IMPULSIVITY ............................................................................. 46
4.1 Measures with a focus on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and/or attentional difficulties
designed for use with typically developing children ........................................................................................ 48
4.1.1 ADD-H Comprehensive Teacher Rating Scale - Second edition (ACTeRS-2) ............................ 48
4.1.2 ADHD Rating Scale-IV (ADHD-RS-IV) ..................................................................................... 49
4.1.3 Conners Third edition (Conners 3) ............................................................................................... 56

ii
4.1.4 Swanson, Nolan & Pelham Checklist – Fourth edition (SNAP-IV) ............................................. 57
4.1.5 Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behaviour Scale (SWAN) ........... 58
4.1.6 Limitations of these measures ....................................................................................................... 59
4.1.7 Use with children and adolescents with ID ................................................................................... 60
4.2 Measures with a focus on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and/or attentional difficulties
designed for use with children with ID ............................................................................................................ 61
4.2.1 Attention-Distraction, Inhibition-Excitation Classroom Assessment Scale (ADIECAS) ............. 61
4.2.2 Attention Checklist (AC) .............................................................................................................. 62
4.2.3 Limitations of these measures ....................................................................................................... 62
4.3 Measures assessing a range of behavioural and emotional problems designed for typically
developing children.......................................................................................................................................... 65
4.3.1 Behavior Assessment System for Children – Second edition (BASC-2) ...................................... 65
4.3.2 Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) .............................................................................................. 71
4.3.3 Children’s Behaviour Questionnaire (CBQ) ................................................................................. 72
4.3.4 Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)........................................................................... 73
4.3.5 Limitations of measures ................................................................................................................ 74
4.3.6 Use with children and adolescents with ID ................................................................................... 75
4.4 Measures assessing a range of emotional functioning and behaviour disorders developed for children
with intellectual or developmental disabilities ................................................................................................ 77
4.4.1 Aberrant Behaviour Checklist – Community (ABC-C) ................................................................ 77
4.4.2 Developmental Behaviour Checklist (DBC)................................................................................. 78
4.4.3 Nisonger Child Behaviour Rating Form (NCBRF) ...................................................................... 85
4.4.4 Limitations of these measures ....................................................................................................... 87
4.5 Summary .............................................................................................................................................. 88

CHAPTER 5 THE FOCUS OF THE PRESENT STUDY ......... 91


5.1 Aims of the present study ..................................................................................................................... 92
5.2 Hypotheses ........................................................................................................................................... 93

CHAPTER 6 METHOD ................................................................ 94


6.1 Phase 1a: Item Development for Rating Scale .................................................................................... 95
6.2 Phase 1b: Focus Group Discussions ................................................................................................... 96
6.2.1 Participants ................................................................................................................................... 96
6.2.2 Procedure ...................................................................................................................................... 97
6.2.3 Analysis ........................................................................................................................................ 99
6.3 Phase 2: Community Survey .............................................................................................................. 100
6.3.1 Participants ................................................................................................................................. 100
6.3.2 Measures ..................................................................................................................................... 102
6.3.3 Procedure .................................................................................................................................... 105
6.3.4 Analysis ...................................................................................................................................... 108

CHAPTER 7 RESULTS FROM PHASE 1: QUESTIONNAIRE


DEVELOPMENT 111
7.1 Behaviours describing attention difficulties in children with ID ....................................................... 112
7.1.1 Talking ........................................................................................................................................ 112
7.1.2 Sitting still ................................................................................................................................... 112
7.1.3 Attention/concentration .............................................................................................................. 113
7.1.4 Impulsive .................................................................................................................................... 113
7.1.5 Executive functioning ................................................................................................................. 114
7.2 Other issues of interest raised ............................................................................................................ 115
7.2.1 Specificity of behaviours ............................................................................................................ 115
7.2.2 Medication .................................................................................................................................. 116

iii
7.3 Rating scale evaluation ...................................................................................................................... 116

CHAPTER 8 DEVELOPMENT OF A NEW ATTENTION RATING


SCALE FOR CHILDREN WITH INTELLECTUAL DISABILITIES:
THE SCALE OF ATTENTION IN INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY
(SAID) 120
8.1 Declaration ........................................................................................................................................ 121
8.2 Paper commentary ............................................................................................................................. 122

CHAPTER 9 THE SCALE OF ATTENTION IN INTELLECTUAL


DISABILITY (SAID): FACTOR ANALYSIS OF A NEW RATING
SCALE FOR USE WITH CHILDREN WITH AN INTELLECTUAL
DISABILITY 155
9.1 Declaration ........................................................................................................................................ 156
9.2 Paper commentary ............................................................................................................................. 157

CHAPTER 10 EVALUATION OF A NEW ATTENTION RATING


SCALE ACROSS NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS: THE
SCALE OF ATTENTION IN INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY.... 177
10.1 Declaration ........................................................................................................................................ 178
10.2 Paper commentary ............................................................................................................................. 179

CHAPTER 11 GENERAL DISCUSSION ................................. 215


11.1 Overview of findings .......................................................................................................................... 216
11.2 Points raised in Phase 1 of the study ................................................................................................. 218
11.3 Strengths of the present study ............................................................................................................ 220
11.3.1 Scale development ................................................................................................................. 220
11.3.2 Sample ................................................................................................................................... 221
11.3.3 Teachers as informants........................................................................................................... 221
11.4 Limitations of the present study ......................................................................................................... 222
11.4.1 Focus group data .................................................................................................................... 222
11.4.2 Respondents ........................................................................................................................... 223
11.4.3 Medication for ADHD ........................................................................................................... 224
11.4.4 Diagnoses of children............................................................................................................. 224
11.5 Directions for future research............................................................................................................ 225
11.5.1 Development of other versions .............................................................................................. 225
11.5.2 Independent validation of the psychometric properties.......................................................... 226
11.5.3 Examining attention profiles in other neurodevelopmental disorders .................................... 226
11.6 Concluding remarks ........................................................................................................................... 227

REFERENCES ..................................................................................... 228


APPENDIX A: FOCUS GROUP DISCUSSION OUTLINE ……………………………….……………...…A1

APPENDIX B: THE SCALE OF ATTENTION IN INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY……………………B1-B4

APPENDIX C: LETTER TO PRINCIPALS EXPLAINING STUDY…………………………….……….C1-C2

iv
APPENDIX D: PARENT/GUARDIAN CONSENT FORM…………………………………………………..D1

APPENDIX E: PARENT/GUARDIAN EXPLANATORY STATEMENT……………………….……….E1-E2

APPENDIX F: ITEM-TOTAL CORRELATIONS TABLE FOR T-SAID ITEMS………………………..F1-F2

APPENDIX G: INTER-ITEM CORRELATIONS TABLE FOR T-SAID ITEMS………...……………...G1-G8

v
List of Figures
Figure 1. Recruitment flowchart for the study. ...................................................................... 101

vi
List of Tables
Table 4.1 Psychometric Properties of Measures with a Focus on Attention-Deficit
Hyperactivity Disorder and/or Attentional Difficulties for Use with Typically Developing
Children.................................................................................................................................... 50
Table 4.2 Psychometric Properties of Measures with a Focus on Attention-Deficit
Hyperactivity Disorder and/or Attentional Difficulties for Use with Children with Intellectual
Disability .................................................................................................................................. 63
Table 4.3 Psychometric Properties of Measures Assessing a Range of Behavioural and
Emotional Problems for Use with Typically Developing Children ......................................... 67
Table 4.4 Psychometric Properties of Measures Assessing a Range of Behavioural and
Emotional Problems for Use with Children with Intellectual Disability ................................. 79

vii
General Declaration
In accordance with Monash University Doctorate Regulation 17 the following declarations
are made:

I hereby declare that this thesis contains no material which has been accepted for the award of
any other degree or diploma at any university or equivalent institution and that, to the best of
my knowledge and belief, this thesis contains no material previously published or written by
another person, except where due reference is made in the text of the thesis.

This thesis includes 3 papers submitted for publication. The core theme of the thesis is the
development of a new rating scale to measure attention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity in
children with neurodevelopmental disorders. The ideas, development and writing up of all the
papers in the thesis were the principal responsibility of myself, the candidate, working within
the Centre for Developmental Psychiatry and Psychology, Southern Clinical School and the
School of Psychological Sciences under the supervision of Associate Professor Kylie Gray
and Professor Kim Cornish. The inclusion of co-authors reflects the fact that the work came
from active collaboration between researchers and acknowledges input into team-based
research.

In the case of Chapters Eight, Nine and Ten my contribution to the work involved the
following:

Thesis Publication title Publication Nature and extent of


chapter status candidate’s
contribution
Eight Development of Submitted 70% contribution by
a new attention rating scale for children candidate. This included
with intellectual disabilities: The Scale formulation of project
of Attention in Intellectual Disability design, data collection,
(SAID) data analysis, and
writing manuscript.
Nine The Scale of Submitted 70% contribution by
Attention in Intellectual Disability candidate. This included
(SAID): Factor analysis of a new rating formulation of project
scale for children with intellectual design, data collection,
disabilities data analysis, and
writing manuscript.
Ten Evaluation a new attention rating scale Submitted 70% contribution by
across neurodevelopmental disorders: candidate. This included
The Scale of Attention in Intellectual formulation of project
Disability design, data collection,
data analysis, and
writing manuscript.

Signed: …….……………………

Date: …………………………….

viii
List of Publications and Presentations during Candidature

Publications

Freeman, N. C., Cornish, K. M., & Gray, K. M. (2012). Attention and activity

profiles in children with intellectual disability: Development of a teacher rating scale.

Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 56(7-8), 712.

Freeman, N. C., Gray, K. M., Taffe, J. R. & Cornish, K. M. (2013). Development of

a new attention rating scale for children with intellectual disabilities: The Scale of

Attention in Intellectual Disability (SAID). American Journal on Intellectual and

Developmental Disabilities.

Freeman, N. C., Cornish, K. M., Taffe, J. R., & Gray, K. M. (2013). The Scale of

Attention in Intellectual Disability (SAID): Factor analysis of a new rating scale for

children with intellectual disabilities. American Journal on Intellectual and

Developmental Disabilities.

Freeman, N.C., Gray, K. M., & Cornish, K. M. (2014). Evaluation of a new attention rating

scale across neurodevelopmental disorders: The Scale of Attention in Intellectual

Disability. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

Presentations

Freeman, N. C., Gray, K. & Cornish, K. (2012, September). Attention and activity

profiles in children with intellectual disability: Development of a teacher rating scale.

47th Australian Psychological Society Annual Conference, Perth, Western Australia

(Oral Presentation).

ix
Freeman, N. C., Gray, K. & Cornish, K. (2012, July). Development of a new

attention scale to assess hyperactivity, inattentive behaviours, and working memory

across neurodevelopmental disorders. International Association for the Scientific

Study of Intellectual Disabilities (IASSID) World Congress, Halifax, Canada (Oral

Presentation).

x
Acknowledgments

Firstly, I would like to extend a special thank you and acknowledgement to my two
supervisors, Professor Kim Cornish and Associate Professor Kylie Gray. The combination of
your skills, knowledge and experience has given me such a solid grounding for future life in
research and academia. Kim, your optimism and encouragement for me to “go for it” every
step of the way has helped inspire me, especially during those times when it felt like the end
was not in sight. Kylie, your common sense and advice has always kept me grounded and
reminded me that while it is important to shoot for the stars, you still need to keep your feet
on the ground. I am also so grateful for the support and encouragement you have given me
when I have presented my findings at conferences. Having you there in the audience was
enormously comforting. When other students said to me “I wish she was my supervisor” it
made me feel both proud and very fortunate to have the support of both of you.

I would also like to acknowledge the Apex Foundation for Research into Intellectual
Disability for helping to fund my study. The money provided by you enabled us to employ a
research assistant one day a week for six months to help me with data collection. Having a
research assistant was invaluable, especially having someone with the skill to help me
complete the cognitive assessments. I am certain that having a research assistant also
contributed to my excellent return rate of rating scales. Many studies that involve teachers are
plagued by low response rates, but ours was over 90% which I know in part was due to the
follow-up we were able to do by having that extra person to help out.

A special acknowledgement must go to three members of my family who have always been a
source of inspiration, comfort and support: my Dad, who has always supported my career
choices and has celebrated every small milestone that I have achieved throughout my Ph.D;
my sister, Selena, who has been an amazing support in every possible way (especially after
the birth of my son) and provided some much needed distraction and humour to my world
when I needed a break from the books; and my Auntie Pamela, who was the first person in
my family who I witnessed showing real discipline to study and higher learning. It helped set
a benchmark for me in my approach to my studies in future years.

I would like to acknowledge the research assistant we employed for the project, Ms Kristina
Clarke. You came with all the skills of a psychologist, having nearly completed your Masters.
It was so terrific to have someone to help out who I had complete confidence in. I knew that
anything I asked you to do would be done well, and I hope you benefitted from the
experience of completing cognitive assessments with children who are low functioning, as it
comes with challenges that are beyond the administrative and interpretive.

I would also like to acknowledge other staff within the Centre for Developmental Psychiatry
and Psychology. Dr John Taffe for his assistance with my statistical analyses, Dr Helen Jeges
for her insights into some unusual cognitive profiles, and Dr Caroline Mohr for her detailed
and insightful feedback on the first draft of my scale.

Thanks must also go to my fellow students located over at Monash Medical Centre. Caroline,
your “drop ins” several times a day were always welcomed and provided a break from the
isolation that I sometimes felt in the office, especially in the early days. Beth, your quiet and
friendly presence meant that I always looked forward to the office days when you were there.
As well as our chats about research and analysis, our shared love of Masterchef and cats were

xi
topics that provided a nice break from reading journal articles, writing, and making phone
calls. Anna, your encouragement and empathy at times when we were both struggling to get
more participants helped me stay optimistic.

Thank you to the Department of Education and Early Childhood Development and the
Catholic Education Office Melbourne who allowed me to approach principals when I was
recruiting participants for my study. I do not think I would have achieved a decent sample
size without being able to recruit through schools. I must also thank community support
groups Amaze and Down Syndrome Victoria who allowed me access to their registries so I
could contact and mail out information to families to inform them about our study.

A special thank you must be extended to all the classroom teachers who took the time to
complete the rating scales and return them to me. Completing one could take up to 20
minutes, so it was no mean feat that some teachers completed 3 or 4 of them, in their own
time, without any incentive or reward for doing so. Your generosity of time and your interest
in our study was so appreciated – my thesis could not have been completed without your
input. To those principals who gave their teachers time release to complete the rating scales –
I extend a special thank you.

Finally, my thanks go to the families and the children who participated in our research study.
I would particularly like to acknowledge those children who completed a cognitive
assessment as part of the data collection process. You taught me new and creative ways to
keep children engaged during an assessment, and gave me new insights into the array of
abilities and skills that you have; sometimes skills that cannot be measured using an
assessment tool.

xii
To my wonderful husband, Matthew,
for his unwavering love, support and constructive criticism;
to my son Michael, our wonderful little surprise who came near the end of my Ph.D;
and to my Nan and my late Pa. When I told them I was contemplating changing thesis topics,
they said I would be crazy if I didn’t take the opportunity to work with Kim.
They were right.

I love you all,


Nerelie

xiii
Abstract
Difficulties with attention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity are thought to be at least as

common, if not more so, among children with intellectual disability (ID) as they are in

typically developing children. Although rating scales exist that are able to measure ADHD

symptomatology, few if any can reliably measure the range and severity of behaviours within

the ID population. Limitations to existing measures include the inappropriateness of some

items for children operating within the intellectually disabled range, the lack of replicability

of the factor structure, and/or that they have not been used or validated in ID populations.

This study aimed to develop and evaluate a new teacher completed rating scale to measure

ADHD symptomatology in children with ID. Phase 1 involved the identification of

behaviours related to hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention that were specific to children

with ID, including a review of existing rating scales, diagnostic manuals, and observational

and descriptive data from existing research. These behaviours were organised into a rating

scale: the Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability – Teacher version (T-SAID). Focus

group discussions were held with nine health professionals (six psychologists and three

paediatricians) and nine teachers who worked in special schools. Comments and feedback

from these discussions were used the further refine the scale.

Phase 2 involved a community survey of 176 teachers who completed the T-SAID for

children aged 5 to 13 years from mild to severe/profound ID. Diagnoses of the children

included autism spectrum disorder (ASD), Down Syndrome (DS) and idiopathic ID. The

results indicated that the T-SAID is a reliable and valid measure for use with children with

ID. It had excellent internal consistency and strong test-retest reliability. It had strong

convergent validity with corresponding subscales on the Conners Third edition and the

1
Developmental Behaviour Checklist – Teacher version and moderate divergent validity with

the total score on the Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales – Second edition. The T-SAID

also had good content validity and good discriminant validity across children with a diagnosis

of ADHD and those who did not. An exploratory factor analysis of the T-SAID yielded a four

factor solution: Hyperactivity/Impulsivity, Inattention, Following Instructions, and Verbal

Communication.

When comparing the T-SAID total score across degrees of ID, regression analyses revealed

that children with severe/profound ID exhibited a greater breadth of behaviours compared

with those who had mild or moderate ID, and these behaviours were significantly more

severe. There was also a significant negative effect for age, suggesting that as children age

their ADHD symptomatology decreases, with fewer behaviours exhibited and these

behaviours being less severe. Cross-syndrome comparisons suggested that children with ASD

had a significantly greater breadth of hyperactive/impulsive behaviours than those with DS or

idiopathic ID. Children with ASD also had significantly greater difficulties with behaviours

that make up the Verbal Communication subscale than children with DS or idiopathic ID, and

that the intensity of these behaviours was also significantly greater.

This study has successfully developed a reliable and valid measure for identifying ADHD

symptomatology in children with ID. Further research would be needed to establish its utility

in clinical, school and research settings. Integrating this scale with neuropsychological and

clinical research holds exciting promise for enhancing our understanding of the nature of

difficulties with attention and hyperactivity/impulsivity within the ID population.

2
CHAPTER 1 DEFINING TYPICAL AND ATYPICAL
ATTENTION AND ACTIVITY

3
The ability to pay attention and to maintain appropriate levels of activity across

situational contexts are essential aspects of successful everyday functioning. Both attention

and activity are complex constructs that can be observed and measured at both the cognitive

and behavioural levels. Attention can be conceptualised by its various facets (e.g., sustained,

divided, selective) but activity is somewhat more difficult to define being a concept that is

generally defined by its excess (hyperactivity) or paucity (inactivity, or at its most extreme,

catatonia). This chapter examines attention and activity at both the cognitive and behavioural

levels, as well as summarising the research of both the typical development of attention

across childhood and adolescence, and atypical development at the behavioural and clinical

diagnostic levels, namely attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

1.1 What is attention?

1.1.1 Attention in everyday life

Attention refers to behaviours that may be manifest in academic, occupational or

social situations. It is characterised by a cluster of behaviours which can include difficulties

with paying attention to detail and making careless errors. In the classroom, it may also

manifest in failure to complete a task or not carrying out instructions or requests. It is also

related to organisational difficulties such as lack of time management, submitting work that is

disorganised, incomplete or out of sequence, and forgetting scheduled appointments or

activities (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

1.1.2 Attention in a controlled environment

At the cognitive level, attention has been defined by processes that were initially

thought to be integrated into a single functional entity, mediated by distinct neuroanatomical

regions (Mirsky, Anthony, Duncan, Ahearn, & Kellam, 1991). This concept has been revised,

however, and rather than being a unitary process it is now widely believed that attention

4
covers a multitude of processes including selective, sustained, and shifting attention (Cornish

& Wilding, 2010).

Selective attention is the ability to focus on a particular stimulus, attend to what is

relevant, and ignore other sources of distraction. While earlier models proposed that selective

attention occurred either through early selection (a limited amount of incoming information

being processed while other inputs are excluded by a filter; e.g., Broadbent, 1958) or late

selection (analysing as much input as possible and selecting the most important information

as late in the sequence as possible; e.g., Deutsch & Deutsch, 1963), more recent findings

offer a compromise between these two conflicting views. One such explanation is the load

theory which suggests that there are variations in the point that information is selected and

the degree of processing carried out on unattended information depending on the task (e.g.,

Lavie, Hirst, de Fockert, & Viding, 2004). This infers that selection is not all-or-nothing but

instead depends on other aspects of the task, such as the perceptual load required by the

incoming information and the current load already being held in the control processes

(Cornish & Wilding, 2010).

Sustained attention can be conceptualised in two parts: short-lived switching and

holding attention when a significant event or warning signal occurs (phasic arousal) and the

ability to maintain concentration over time (tonic arousal; Cornish & Wilding, 2010).

Signal detection theory is one popular explanation for the difficulty experienced in

sustaining attention over time. This theory assumes that signals must be detected against a

background of random disturbance called “noise” and that performance varies across

individuals and time depending upon factors such as physiological state (e.g., fatigue),

experience, and expectations (Tanner & Swets, 1954). Response in the presence or absence of

a signal results in four different categories: correct detection (stimulus present, response

present), missed signal (stimulus present, response absent), false alarm (stimulus absent,

5
response present), and correct rejection (stimulus absent, response absent). In low-input

situations, few targets appear in the presence of few other distractions. Individuals tend to

miss more targets over time not due to a decline in the efficiency of processes detecting the

input, but because they become more conservative in the amount of evidence required before

registering a signal detection (Stroh, 1971). In high-input situations, perceptual processes are

likely to become more inefficient over time. High-input situations involve tasks that are

demanding (e.g., when input is continuous and target detection is frequent) and have a high

perceptual load. Sustained exposure to such situations leads to an overload of processing

mechanisms and deterioration of efficiency (Cornish & Wilding, 2010).

Shifting attention is the ability to divide attention flexibly and adaptively between

different tasks. Many theories have been suggested to explain this process. The original filter

theory suggests that there is a central bottleneck in information processing which limits dual-

task performance, and that individuals are only able to process one stream of information at a

time (Broadbent, 1958). Multi-tasking, therefore, could only be achieved by rapidly

switching between tasks. Others have posited that attention is a flexible system which can

allocate resources to different tasks provided that the total load is not too high (e.g.,

Kahneman, 1973). A study conducted by Allport and colleagues (1972) extended this further,

suggesting that resources could be shared even when conducting complex tasks. No

consensus has been reached on explaining the process of shifting attention. More recent

evidence has suggested that individuals can demonstrate time sharing between tasks in some

conditions, but debate continues as to how this process takes place (Styles, 2006).

1.2 Typical development of attention and activity

The development of attention typically begins as early as infancy, with the infant’s

attention span and capacity to concentrate increasing as they develop into a toddler, child, and

adolescent. Toddlers begin the development of sustained attention through goal-directed

6
behaviours in their play such as stacking blocks (Ruff & Capozzoli, 2003). This can be

enhanced further by adults who may encourage sustained attention by observing a child

playing with an object and then encouraging further play with it by showing the child a

different function or use (e.g., observing a child handling a bell and then the adult rings it to

encourage further exploration by the child; Berk, 2011).

Although only limited research has examined the typical developmental of sustained

attention in children, the majority of researchers agree that it develops rapidly through

childhood up to the age of 10 years, with gradual improvements thereafter as they move into

adolescence (Betts, McKay, Maruff, & Anderson, 2006; Klenberg, Korkman, & Lahti-

Nuuttila, 2001; Manly et al., 2001; Rebok et al., 1997). A recent study supported this

developmental trajectory, but suggested that sustained and selective attention functions were

closely related in early childhood before subdividing in later childhood (Steele, Karmiloff-

Smith, Cornish, & Scerif, 2012). Researchers attempting to explain the underlying neural

basis of sustained attention have suggested that this occurs through gradually increasing

myelination of the central nervous system. They argue that it is not until early adolescence

that the reticular formation (the area of the brain responsible for attention regulation)

becomes fully myelinated (Shaffer, 2010).

Selective attention also increases with age as children become better at focusing on a

given task while ignoring distractions, and as they enhance their capacity to multi-task.

Development is believed to transition in the second and third years of life from attention

influenced by novelty of objects and events towards more cognitive factors such as planning

and goal-setting (Ruff & Capozzoli, 2003). It continues to develop at a steady rate up until

the age of approximately 10 years (Klenberg, et al., 2001; Manly, et al., 2001; Rebok, et al.,

1997; Steele, et al., 2012). As children move into adolescence, the rate of development

plateaus but their capacity continues to steadily increase (Klenberg, et al., 2001). The

7
importance of distinguishing a child’s attention to structured tasks as opposed to self-directed

play or physical activity has also been emphasised (Tandon, Si, Belden, & Luby, 2009). A

child’s capacity to attend to structured tasks is believed to be a more important predictor of

future attentional capacities than their ability to attend to activities of their own choice.

Response inhibition is the ability to inhibit the prepotent response to an event

(Barkley, 1997a; Logan, Schachar, & Tannock, 1997) with a deficiency in this area being

described as poor impulse control or impulsivity. The ability to inhibit responses has been

demonstrated to improve significantly between 24 and 36 months of age (Gerardi-Caulton,

2000). In one study of Finnish children aged 3 to 12 years, inhibition developed rapidly up to

the age of 7 years and then levelled off thereafter (Klenberg, et al., 2001). In another study,

steady development was reported throughout childhood and reached maturity at

approximately 12 years of age (Bunge, Dudukovic, Thomason, Vaidya, & Gabrieli, 2002).

Interpretation of inhibitory control development is difficult to compare across studies given

the different tasks used. The above findings do, however, suggest an increase in development

throughout childhood which reaches maturity by adolescence if not earlier. This skill

decreases later in life (B. R. Williams, Ponesse, Schachar, Logan, & Tannock, 1999), thus

suggesting inhibitory control has an inverted U shape of development.

Although excess in activity is one of the most widely researched behaviour problems

in childhood, the developmental precursors are still yet to be fully understood, thus making it

difficult to define “normal” levels of activity in early child development. One theory suggests

that normal activity levels relate to the capacity to self-regulate (Barkley, 1997a). Self-

regulation is a construct which captures a variety of different processes including affect

regulation and behaviour inhibition, and relates to the individual’s capacity to delay

responding to events that elicit emotional responses, especially those that are negative such as

anger. The greater the capacity for delaying response, the more likely it is that the individual

8
can gather the necessary information to understand the different facets of an event. This has

the potential to moderate their initial internal emotional response and modify their external

display of emotion to others. Between 3 and 4 years of age, children begin developing the

ability to use coping mechanisms such as self-generated strategies to regulate sadness and

anger (Cole, Dennis, Smith-Simon, & Cohen, 2009). Typically developing young children

become increasingly more capable of self-regulation as they move into preschool and early

primary school, and these skills continue to develop through adolescence (Eisenberg & Sulik,

2012; Rothbart & Bates, 2006). The ability to self-regulate behaviour has also been reported

to predict lower maladjustment, lower peer aggression, and greater social competence in

childhood and adolescence (Eisenberg & Sulik, 2012; Olson, Lopez-Duran, Lunkenheimer,

Chang, & Sameroff, 2011).

1.3 Atypical development of attention and activity: The case of attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) belongs to a group of

childhood-onset developmental disorders. It is recognised as a developmental disorder across

most cultures, although a few academics have questioned whether it is a social construct

limited to Western culture (e.g., Amaral, 2007; Anderson, 1996; Timimi & Taylor, 2004).

ADHD is characterised by a pattern of inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity that impairs

the individual’s functioning across different environments e.g., at home and at school.

Although diagnosis can be made at any age, the behaviours must be present before a child

turns twelve (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

ADHD is a condition which affects approximately 5-7% of children (American

Psychiatric Association, 2000; Polanczyk, de Lima, Horta, Biederman, & Rohde, 2007;

Willcutt, 2012) and attention difficulties – generally excluded from prevalence studies –

impact upon an even greater number of young people (Ramtekkar, Reiersen, Todorov, &

9
Todd, 2010; Tennant & Conaghan, 2007). This statistic represents a significant proportion of

individuals, and the difficulties associated with attention can have a significant, negative

impact throughout childhood, adolescence and into adulthood, particularly if they do not

receive optimal treatment and intervention (e.g., Barkley, Fischer, Smallish, & Fletcher,

2006; Hoza et al., 2005).

1.3.1 Diagnostic criteria

The conceptualisation and diagnostic criteria for ADHD have evolved with successive

editions of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM; American

Psychiatric Association, 1968, 1980, 1987, 1994, 2000, 2013) and the International

Classification of Diseases (ICD; World Health Organization, 1992) which can present

challenges when attempting to make direct comparisons across studies.

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder was labelled as hyperkinetic disorder in the

DSM-II (American Psychiatric Association, 1968), with a focus on the symptoms related to

motor disturbance. In the DSM-III (American Psychiatric Association, 1980), the condition

was labelled attention-deficit disorder (ADD) and was defined in two separate domains of

inattention and hyperactivity. Therefore, it was possible to be diagnosed with ADD while

only satisfying the criteria in one of these domains. The DSM-III-R reconceptualised ADD

into a single diagnostic category, but also added impulsivity to the criteria. The DSM-IV-TR

and DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2000, 2013) conceptualise ADHD as a two-

dimensional disorder consisting of clustered symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity and

inattention, allowing a diagnosis to be made based on the presence of behaviours in one or

both domains. This has resulted in three possible subtypes but impairment must be observed

in more than one setting. While previously an exclusionary criteria, the DSM-5 allows for the

comorbid diagnosis of ADHD in children with an autism spectrum disorder diagnosis.

10
The ICD-10 is the latest in a series of classifications endorsed by the World Health

Organisation that can be traced back to the 1850s. This system has two separate diagnoses

that ADHD can be classified under: disturbance of activity and attention and hyperkinetic

conduct disorder. Under this system, hyperkinetic conduct disorder is considered to be a

more severe form of ADHD. The ICD-10 (World Health Organization, 1992) criteria for both

disorders are more stringent than the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013),

requiring a minimum number of symptoms to be present across all three dimensions

(hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention) and all criteria must be met across at least two

situational contexts. Unlike the DSM-5, the ICD-10 lists mood and anxiety disorders as

exclusionary criteria.

1.3.1.1 Age and diagnostic criteria

The DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) describe the specific criteria

regarding age of onset, subtypes, and number of symptoms required to meet the threshold for

a diagnosis of ADHD. The DSM-5 has increased the age of onset from seven to 12 years,

responding to research calling for this change (e.g., Kieling et al., 2010; Polanczyk et al.,

2010). It is also supported by research suggesting that inattentive symptoms are identified at a

later age than hyperactive symptoms (Lahey et al., 1994) which had called into question the

validity of the age of onset criteria in diagnosing inattention in the DSM-IV-TR

(Waschbusch, King, & Gregus, 2007). The DSM-5 has decreased the number of symptoms

required for diagnosis in adolescents and adults from six to five, consistent with suggestions

made in prior research studies (e.g., Ramtekkar, et al., 2010).

Although these changes have been met with some concerns about increased

prevalence or false positives (Frances, 2010), recent longitudinal birth cohort studies have

attempted to address these issues. One study found that increasing the age of onset to 12

years had a negligible impact on existing prevalence rates, correlates, and risk factors

11
(Polanczyk, et al., 2010). Another study suggested that after a 5 year follow-up, the age when

symptoms first appeared was recalled as being significantly higher (i.e., 6 to 18 months later)

for both parent and self-report. In 46% of these cases, while still meeting symptom and

impairment criteria for ADHD, their increased reported age of onset would mean that they

would no longer have been diagnosed with ADHD (Todd, Huang, & Henderson, 2008) under

the DSM-IV-TR. This finding would appear to support the increased age of onset criteria, but

also questions whether it is appropriate in all cases. Adolescents and adults may have

difficulty recalling symptoms in the earlier stages of their life, or, as the above study

suggests, may have altered recall of the age when their symptoms first appeared when asked

at a later date. The implication is that this may actually exclude some people (who meet

symptom and impairing criteria) from diagnosis due to their inability to meet the age of onset

criteria, and therefore restrict their access to treatment.

The applicability of the DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000)

criteria for ADHD were questioned for very young children. One study suggested that

symptom persistence should be increased from 6 months, as specified in the DSM-IV-TR, to

9 months in preschool children (Kollins et al., 2006). It has also been suggested that

modifications may be needed to increase the sensitivity of these criteria in 3 and 4 year old

children (Tandon, et al., 2009). For example, descriptors such as often makes careless

mistakes may not be applicable as many young children are rarely placed in situations or

given tasks where such behaviours could be observed. Similarly, other descriptors such as

has difficulty organising tasks or activities may reflect behaviours that would be observed in

many young children, and therefore the discriminant validity between a child with attention

difficulties and one without would be low. The issues raised in these studies persist in the

DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) which has retained symptom persistence at

6 months, and contains similar behavioural descriptors to the DSM-IV-TR. At the present

12
time, a determination about which criteria would need to be modified to assist with

diagnosing ADHD in young children is yet to be conducted.

1.3.1.2 Gender and diagnostic criteria

It is well established in the literature that boys are diagnosed with ADHD more

frequently than girls. It has been suggested that this may be because the diagnostic criteria for

ADHD are more descriptive of boys rather than girls (Ohan & Johnston, 2005; Staller &

Faraone, 2006). Others have suggested that girls are more likely to present with inattentive

type which are less likely to be referred for treatment (J. Biederman et al., 2002) and that

children with these inattentive, internalising behaviours may be more difficult to identify.

In response to these observations, it has been suggested that the diagnostic criteria

may need to be changed so that separate classifications exist across genders (Rohde, 2008).

One study has gone so far as to outline possible “female sensitive” items that could be used

when diagnosing ADHD and other clinical diagnoses in females (Ohan & Johnston, 2005).

This view has not, however, met with universal agreement. Several studies have

suggested that ADHD symptoms do not differ across genders (Monuteaux, Mick, Faraone, &

Biederman, 2010), even when making comparisons across countries (Nøvik et al., 2006) and

when using a non-referred community sample (J. Biederman et al., 2005). Monuteaux and

colleagues (2010) stated that the differences in presentation are due to contrasts in comorbid

psychopathology, although an earlier study suggested no differences across genders (J.

Biederman, et al., 2005). Another study made gender comparisons across subtypes but while

few differences were noted, group assignment relied on parent report of symptoms rather than

a clinical diagnosis so these conclusions should be interpreted with caution (Graetz, Sawyer,

& Baghurst, 2005). Further research is needed to examine the possibility of gender

differences, and the interaction of comorbid psychopathology and ADHD subtypes, using

larger sample sizes.

13
1.3.2 Theories of ADHD

One of the most common theories of ADHD suggests that its symptoms arise from a

deficit in executive functioning. Executive functioning has been defined as a set of cognitive

processes that maintain a problem solving set in order to attain a goal (Welsh & Pennington,

1988). It represents a “top down” processing model whereby incoming information is held in

the working memory while simultaneously integrating knowledge about the current context.

These two processes aid in the individual’s decision-making process about the best strategy

or action to take in a given situation (Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone, & Pennington, 2005).

Under this theory, ADHD symptoms arise due to deficits relating to specific executive

functioning processes: namely response inhibition and working memory (Barkley, 1997b;

Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996). A meta-analytic

review of studies that examined the validity of this theory found that children and adolescents

with ADHD exhibited significant impairment in all areas of executive functioning when

compared with typically developing children. This was observed in both clinic and

community samples, even after controlling for variables such as diagnostic criteria used,

general intelligence, presence of a language disorder, and academic achievement (Willcutt, et

al., 2005). The authors suggested that the most impaired areas of executive functioning were

in the areas of response inhibition, working memory, planning, and vigilance which

supported the conclusions drawn in many of the studies included in their analysis. They also

emphasised, however, that the effect sizes were insufficient to suggest that weaknesses in

executive functioning explained the symptoms of ADHD in all individuals. Other researchers

have suggested that working memory impairments may be present in children with the

inattentive subtype, but reported little or no evidence of such deficits within the hyperactive

subtype (Gathercole & Alloway, 2006).

Delay aversion has been presented as another possible theory of ADHD. It

14
relates to the behavioural tendency to prefer small, immediate rewards as opposed to larger,

delayed rewards (Antrop et al., 2006; Sonuga-Barke, Taylor, Sembi, & Smith, 1992), but that

this behaviour may be reduced with the addition of stimulation during the delay interval

(Antrop, et al., 2006). In settings where children are unable to choose between immediate and

delayed rewards (such as during a classroom activity), they may systematically attempt to

reduce the perceived time spent in “delay” (that is, the time spent on the current activity

before moving on to another). This may be achieved by the child attending to other aspects of

their environment in an attempt to make it more interesting and absorbing, which may be

manifest through inattention or hyperactivity (Sonuga-Barke, 2005; Sonuga-Barke, et al.,

1992). A recent study found that delay aversion was related to inattentive symptoms rather

than hyperactivity symptoms (Paloyelis, Asherson, & Kuntsi, 2009), although others have

suggested that the opposite is true (Solanto et al., 2001). Despite these conflicting findings, it

does raise the possibility that delay aversion cannot be generalised to all children with

ADHD, and may be specific to severity or symptomatology.

Information processing theory relates to the way incoming information is

attended to, filtered, and processed in the brain through encoding, retention and retrieval (G.

A. Miller, 1956). In children with ADHD, it is proposed that information processing deficits

limit their ability to understand incoming information. Research has focused on specific areas

such as visual (Weiler, Bernstein, Bellinger, & Waber, 2002) and central auditory processing

disorders (CAPD; Jerome, 2000; Riccio & Hynd, 1996). Studies of visual processing

disorders among children with ADHD have primarily focused on performance in visual

search tasks. One study comparing children with ADHD inattentive type and/or dyslexia

found that the children with ADHD inattentive type (with or without dyslexia) had greater

difficulties with visual processing after controlling for inattention (Weiler, et al., 2002).

Studies examining CAPD have been inconsistent, with some suggesting that it is common in

15
children with ADHD (Riccio & Hynd, 1996; Riccio, Hynd, Cohen, & Hall, 1994), whereas

others have suggested it is more commonly associated with learning disabilities (Gomez &

Condon, 1999; Weiler, et al., 2002). The variation across studies may be partly related to a

lack of consensus among professionals regarding how CAPD is measured or assessed

(Cacace & McFarland, 1998; Riccio & Hynd, 1996).

While none of the theories discussed above – or any other theories that have been put

forward – provides the single explanation of the cause of ADHD, all of them enhance our

understanding of its symptomatology. Indeed, many researchers support the position that

ADHD is a heterogeneous disorder and is likely to be complex and multifactorial (e.g.,

Sergeant, Geurts, Huijbregts, Scheres, & Oosterlaan, 2003; Sonuga-Barke, 2005). This would

suggest that a single theory or cause is improbable, and that a combination of cognitive and

motivational models may enhance our understanding of ADHD and other developmental

disorders (Willcutt, et al., 2005).

1.3.3 Demographic, genetic and environmental correlates

One of the consistent findings of ADHD prevalence studies, irrespective of the

country that the study was conducted, is that a greater number of males are diagnosed

compared with females (Polanczyk, et al., 2007; Polanczyk & Jensen, 2008; Staller &

Faraone, 2006). It should be noted, however, that the ratio of males to females is thought to

be lower in children with predominantly inattentive ADHD (Lahey, et al., 1994) and those

with intellectual disability (Pearson, Yaffee, Loveland, & Lewis, 1996).

Research has suggested that the severity and frequency of some ADHD symptoms has

an inverse relationship with age. This has been reported irrespective of whether syndromatic

(i.e., still met full diagnostic criteria) or symptomatic persistence (i.e., continued to present

with impairing symptoms but failed to meet full diagnostic criteria) were examined (J.

Biederman, Mick, & Faraone, 2000; J. Biederman et al., 2006; Faraone, Biederman, & Mick,

16
2006; J. C. Hill & Schoener, 1996). These studies have suggested that as children get older,

impulse control improves and level of hyperactivity declines (J. Biederman, et al., 2000;

DuPaul, Power, Anastopoulos, & Reid, 1998; Fischer, Barkley, Smallish, & Fletcher, 2002),

although inattentive behaviours appear to persist over time (Barkley, 2006c; J. Biederman, et

al., 2000; DuPaul, et al., 1998).

Although the underlying cause of ADHD is not yet known, there are a number of

contributing factors that may exacerbate symptoms. Genetic factors such as disorders which

are known to present with attention difficulties as part of their behavioural phenotype (e.g.,

Fragile X Syndrome; R. J. Hagerman, 1999; Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2001), or a

family history of ADHD or attentional difficulties (Bennett, Levy, & Hay, 2007; Hay,

Bennett, Levy, Sergeant, & Swanson, 2007) can increase the likelihood of a child meeting the

criteria for this diagnosis. Neurological factors such as pre-natal exposure to illicit drugs

(Milberger, Biederman, Faraone, Guite, & Tsuang, 1997) or smoking (Milberger, et al., 1997;

Thapar et al., 2003), exposure to certain central nervous system infections (e.g., encephalitis;

Gau, Chang, et al., 2008), traumatic brain injury (McKinlay, Grace, Horwood, Fergusson, &

MacFarlane, 2010), and neurotoxin exposure (e.g., lead poisoning; Hussain, Woolf, Sandel,

& Shannon, 2007; Mendola, Selevan, Gutter, & Rice, 2002), can also result in a greater

likelihood of being diagnosed with ADHD. While family dysfunction is no longer believed to

cause ADHD, it has been suggested that it may contribute to the exacerbation or amelioration

of symptoms in an individual with this diagnosis (S. B. Campbell & Ewing, 1990).

1.3.4 Long-term outcomes

Research examining adults with ADHD has consistently reported that this diagnosis

can have a significant, negative impact on the individual throughout childhood, adolescence,

and into adulthood, particularly if they do not receive optimal treatment and intervention.

Children and adolescents with a diagnosis of ADHD can face significant difficulties in their

17
everyday lives, such as lower academic achievement (Barkley, et al., 2006; Polderman,

Boomsma, Bartels, Verhulst, & Huizink, 2010), less popularity among peers (Hoza, et al.,

2005; Waschbusch & Sparkes, 2003), friendship difficulties (Barkley, 2006b; Normand,

Schneider, & Robaey, 2007), and lower self-esteem (Barkley, 2006b; Graetz, Sawyer, Hazell,

Arney, & Baghurst, 2001). Negative outcomes that have been reported in adults with a

current and/or childhood diagnosis ADHD have included lower occupational status

(Mannuzza, Klein, Bessler, Malloy, & LaPadula, 1993), fewer close friendships (Barkley, et

al., 2006), greater frequency of alcohol consumption (Barkley, Fischer, Smallish, & Fletcher,

2004; Greenfield, Hechtman, & Weiss, 1988), increased likelihood of involvement in adverse

driving outcomes such as car accidents and traffic violations (Thompson, Molina, Pelham, &

Gnagy, 2007), and illicit substance dependence or abuse (J. Biederman, et al., 2006;

Mannuzza, et al., 1993; Nigg et al., 2005). One study by Greenfield and colleagues (1988),

however, reported that negative life outcomes were associated with only current moderate to

severe ADHD symptoms, whereas those with residual or no symptomatology as adults had

outcomes that were similar to the control group who had no history of an ADHD diagnosis.

In some of these studies negative long-term outcomes were independent of comorbid

conduct disorder or oppositional-defiant disorder (Barkley, et al., 2004, 2006; Thompson, et

al., 2007), with comorbid diagnoses additively contributing to the severity of negative life

outcomes. In other studies, however, these diagnoses were not controlled for in the analyses

(J. Biederman, et al., 2006; Mannuzza, et al., 1993) so it is possible that adults with comorbid

disruptive behaviour disorders may have different outcomes to those adults with ADHD

alone, or in combination with other comorbid disorders.

1.3.5 Comorbid psychopathology

Comorbid psychopathology is very common in people with a diagnosis of ADHD,

with reported conditions including conduct disorder in children and adolescents (August,

18
Realmuto, MacDonald, Nugent, & Crossby, 1996; Barkley, 2006b; Smalley et al., 2007),

antisocial personality disorder in adults (J. Biederman, et al., 2006; Fischer, et al., 2002) and

anxiety in children, adolescents and adults (Bloemsma et al., 2013; Bowen, Chavira, Bailey,

Stein, & Stein, 2008; CME Institute of Physicians, 2007). While oppositional defiant disorder

has also been reported in children and adolescents (August, Realmuto, Joyce, & Hektner,

1999; August, et al., 1996; Posner et al., 2007; Smalley, et al., 2007), one study suggested

that it is more common among those with hyperactive/impulsive and combined subtypes, and

less common in children with the inattentive subtype (Kadesjo, Hagglof, Kadesjo, &

Gillberg, 2003).

19
CHAPTER 2 ISSUES OF COMPLEXITY IN DIAGNOSING

ADHD IN INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY

20
This chapter will review some of the complexities encountered by clinicians when

diagnosing ADHD in children with intellectual disability. These include difficulties

encountered by clinicians in determining the child’s mental age, diagnostic overshadowing,

and conflicting guidelines across diagnostic manuals.

Intellectual disability (alternatively referred to as mental retardation or learning

disability in some countries; Department of Health, 2001; World Health Organization, 2007)

is defined by the American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities (2010)

as any individual with significant limitations in both adaptive behaviour and intellectual

functioning in terms of their conceptual, social and practical skills, with an age of onset

before 18 years. Some people with intellectual disability (ID) have a known etiology such as

a chromosomal disorder, neurofibromatosis, or tuberosclerosis (see section 3.2 below for an

exploration of some of these diagnoses and syndromes). For 30-50% of children, however,

there is no organic cause (Percy, 2007) and this is believed to account for many individuals

with mild ID (Volkmar, Dykens, & Hodapp, 2007). It has been suggested that a combination

of familial (i.e., a family history of lower intellectual ability; Iarocci & Burack, 1998) and

environmental factors (e.g., lack of oxygen at birth; Harris, 2006; Mendola, et al., 2002) may

increase the likelihood of being diagnosed with ID, but these are only possible contributing

factors and do not imply causation.

ADHD in children with ID has historically been reported as difficult to diagnose.

Some researchers have questioned the origin of inattentive symptoms among children with

ID, while others have suggested that attention difficulties may be related to their cognitive

deficits rather than being a comorbid diagnosis (Antshel, Phillips, Gordon, Barkley, &

Faraone, 2006; Guerin, Buckley, McEvoy, Hillery, & Dodd, 2009). The guidelines in the

DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) recognised that ADHD should be

diagnosed if the deficits exhibited were significantly disruptive and/or inappropriate for the

21
child’s developmental level, but provided very little guidance on how to make this

determination (Antshel, et al., 2006). This limitation has carried over in the DSM-5

(American Psychiatric Association, 2013) which also does not provide any further guidelines

related to this issue.

Difficulties may also arise in determining the child’s mental age when attempting to

ascertain whether the behaviours exhibited are significantly inappropriate (Barkley, 2006a;

Schaughency & Rothlind, 1991), particularly in children with severe or profound ID. Some

researchers have suggested that once correcting for mental age, there is a lack of strong

evidence for the presence of attention difficulties in children with ID (Burack, Evans,

Klaiman, & Iarocci, 2001). Other researchers, however, have suggested that there is no need

to correct for mental age when considering a child’s level of hyperactivity, and that the

interpretation of behaviours should be based on chronological age (Handen, Janosky, &

McAuliffe, 1997; Pearson & Aman, 1994). Handen and colleagues (1997) found no

difference in symptom severity in the majority of analyses when using either chronological or

mental age. It should be noted, however, that the study conducted by Pearson and Aman

(1994) used the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test to determine mental age, which may have

distorted the potential correlations between mental age and hyperactivity. Difficulties related

to identifying the mental age of some children, confusion about whether to use mental or

chronological age when considering behaviours, and difficulties in conceptualising the term

“excessive” in relation to attention difficulties, may all contribute to a lack of confidence

among clinicians in diagnosing ADHD in this group.

Diagnosing ADHD among children with ID can also be problematic due to the issue

of diagnostic overshadowing bias. This term refers to the tendency of clinicians, in the

presence of ID, to regard accompanying mental health issues as less salient and specific (Jopp

& Keys, 2001; Mason & Scior, 2004). It has been suggested that this bias also exists with

22
attention difficulties, with clinicians tending to perceive these behaviours as being part of

having ID rather than as a distinct comorbid problem (Deb, Dhaliwal, & Roy, 2008). A recent

and growing body of literature, however, suggests that children with comorbid ID and ADHD

form a distinct subgroup, and that some children who have ID display few or no difficulties

with attention (Hastings, Beck, Daley, & Hill, 2005).

Two recently published diagnostic manuals have attempted to address these issues:

the Diagnostic Criteria for Psychiatric Disorders for use with adults with Learning

Disabilities/Mental Retardation (DC-LD; Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2001) and the

Diagnostic Manual - Intellectual Disability (DM-ID; P. Lee & Friedlander, 2007). Both

recognise the limitations of the ICD-10 (World Health Organization, 1992) and DSM-IV-TR

(American Psychiatric Association, 2000) in making diagnostic decisions when a person has

ID, and appreciate that psychiatric conditions may present differently among adults within

this group. The two diagnostic manuals do contain some differences, however, in their

criteria for diagnosing ADHD in children and adults with ID.

The DC-LD (Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2001) conceptualises ADHD as a three-

dimensional disorder, namely hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention. It suggests that the

diagnosis of ADHD should be made based on the presence of behaviours in all three domains

but impairment must be observed in more than one setting and be persistent over time. The

DC-LD also contains two distinct diagnoses, depending upon whether the person has known

childhood onset (the behaviours were present before the age of seven) or unknown age of

onset. The unknown age of onset category acknowledges the possible lack of available

written developmental history, or an inability to obtain information regarding early

development from the individual or their family. It does emphasise, however, that the

presence of attention difficulties must be known to be long-standing based on the available

history. The DC-LD lists mood disorders and drug-induced psychoses as exclusionary

23
criteria, and similar to the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) it allows autism

spectrum disorder as a comorbid diagnosis.

One of the limitations of the DC-LD (Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2001) is that its

diagnostic criteria reflect symptomatology in adults, and therefore the behaviours may not be

readily applicable to children and adolescents. It is also unclear why behaviours in all three

domains (hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention) must be present for a diagnosis to be

made. Available research does not seem to suggest that individuals with ID are more likely to

present with symptoms in all three domains. It would seem more useful to diagnose ADHD

based on the presence of behaviours in one or more domains, resulting in diagnostic subtypes

(e.g., predominantly inattentive) similar to the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association,

2013).

The DM-ID (P. Lee & Friedlander, 2007) also recognises ADHD as a comorbid

disorder in individuals with ID. In developing their diagnostic criteria, the authors contend

that as the DSM-IV-TR criteria for ADHD were based on observed behaviour, few

modifications were needed for individuals with ID. Further to this, the editors of the DM-ID

contend that differential diagnostic criteria were not required to account for degree of ID

(Fletcher, Loschen, Stavrakaki, & First, 2007).The only major change suggested by the

authors is that when assessing developmentally inappropriate inattention, hyperactivity and/or

impulsivity, the informant should consider the child’s behaviour compared with peers of

comparable mental and chronological age, and not younger typically developing children of

comparable developmental age. They provide some common clinical examples of inattention

and hyperactivity to assist the clinician. Similar to the DC-LD (Royal College of

Psychiatrists, 2001), they have relaxed the age of onset criteria (i.e., before the age of 7 years)

and noted the acceptability of formulating a diagnosis even in the absence of written

developmental history.

24
The DM-ID (P. Lee & Friedlander, 2007) conceptualises ADHD in the same manner

as the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), namely as a two-dimensional

disorder consisting of clustered symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity. It

allows for a diagnosis to be made based on the presence of behaviours in one or both domains

resulting in three possible subtypes (predominantly inattentive, predominantly hyperactive or

combined) but impairment must be observed in more than one setting and be persistent over

time. They stated that caution must be exercised when making an ADHD diagnosis and

emphasised that the behaviours must result in clinically significant impairment in social,

academic or occupational functioning that specifically related to hyperactivity, impulsivity or

inattention and not just ID. The authors did not, however, go into further detail about how

this distinction should be made or identify the symptomatic threshold for clinically significant

impairment. Consistent with the ICD-10 (World Health Organization, 1992), the DM-ID

listed mood disorders, drug-induced psychoses and autism spectrum disorder as exclusionary

criteria.

Although these two manuals contain some differences in their diagnostic criteria,

namely whether all three behaviours (impulsivity, hyperactivity and inattention) need to be

present to make a diagnosis and their exclusionary criteria, both publications represent a

positive step in recognising the differences in presentation of comorbid disorders (including

ADHD) among people with ID. Future research in this area may result in greater consensus

across the DM-ID (P. Lee & Friedlander, 2007) and DC-LD (Royal College of Psychiatrists,

2001).

25
CHAPTER 3 COGNITIVE AND BEHAVIOURAL
PHENOTYPES OF ATTENTION AND ACTIVITY IN
CHILDREN WITH INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY

26
Initially thought to be a homogenous group, research in this area suggests that

differences in attention and hyperactivity may exist in children with intellectual disability

depending on their cognitive and behavioural phenotypes (see Cornish & Wilding, 2010 for a

comprehensive review) and that there are divergent trajectories in the development of these

behaviours (e.g., Cornish, Scerif, & Karmiloff-Smith, 2007; Scerif, Longhi, Cole, Karmiloff-

Smith, & Cornish, 2012). In this chapter, inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity are

examined among children with idiopathic (unknown cause) intellectual disability and across

four neurodevelopmental disorders where ADHD symptoms have commonly been studied:

autism spectrum disorder, Down Syndrome, Fragile X Syndrome, and Williams Syndrome.

While the majority of studies have used typically developing children as a comparison group

(either matched for chronological or mental age), a limited number of cross syndrome studies

are also described which provide a more meaningful and complete picture of these difficulties

within the area of intellectual disability.

3.1 Idiopathic intellectual disability

Although research examining the prevalence of ADHD in idiopathic intellectual

disability (hereafter referred to as ID) is scant, it is believed to be at least as common, if not

more so, as the prevalence among typically developing children (Neece, Baker, Blacher, &

Crnic, 2011; Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2001; Seager & O'Brien, 2003). Studies have

reported prevalence estimates from 4% to 70% of children with ID meeting the criteria for

ADHD (Dekker & Koot, 2003; Feinstein & Reiss, 1996; Hastings, et al., 2005; Lindblad,

Gillberg, & Fernell, 2011). Similar to sampling issues in research examining the prevalence

of ADHD in typically developing individuals (Polanczyk & Rohde, 2007), it has been

concluded that the variance in the estimates of ADHD in populations with ID is due to use of

samples that do not allow direct comparisons to be made such as: convenience samples;

27
clinical samples where comorbid conditions are more likely to be identified; and samples

including children with syndromes where attention difficulties are a common feature such as

Fragile X Syndrome. Inconsistencies in prevalence estimates may also have arisen as some

studies did not provide reliable information on how ID and/or ADHD were diagnosed, or

they included children who were in the borderline range of intellectual functioning. The

studies do however demonstrate that a substantial proportion of children with ID present with

symptoms that are within the clinical range for ADHD.

Several longitudinal studies have suggested that hyperactivity decreases from

childhood to adolescence. These included a sample of adolescents with severe ID (Chadwick,

Kusel, Cuddy, & Taylor, 2005), and another with individuals ranging from mild to profound

ID (Tonge & Einfeld, 2003). Some studies, however, have suggested the trend of decline may

differ in children with ID. A longitudinal study suggested that the decline in hyperactive

symptoms starts later in children with ID than in children who are typically developing

(Einfeld, Tonge, Gray, & Taffe, 2007) and a cross-sectional study suggested that children

with ID showed a larger decrease in attention problems from age 6 to 18 compared with

typically developing children (de Ruiter, Dekker, Verhulst, & Koot, 2007). The inclusion of

children with borderline intellectual functioning in this sample makes it difficult to be certain

that this trajectory would apply to children strictly within the ID range. Further research is

needed to support the finding that hyperactivity decreases with age and whether it mirrors the

trend observed in typically developing children with ADHD (J. Biederman, et al., 2000;

DuPaul, et al., 1998; Fischer, et al., 2002), or whether it follows a different trajectory.

In a study comparing ADHD symptoms across degrees of ID, O’Brien (2000) drew

upon a community sample of young adults (18 to 22 years). He reported a positive

association between the prevalence of ADHD and degree of intellectual disability, with the

proportion increasing dramatically in the groups with moderate (25%) and severe (29%) ID.

28
A comparative study conducted in children and adolescents also suggested that hyperactivity

symptoms increased with severity of ID (Rojahn et al., 2010), although given half the sample

had missing data for degree of ID, the generalisability of these findings is unclear. The

findings of these studies do, however, suggest that ADHD symptoms are common not only in

children with ID, but in adolescents and young adults as well. Further research would be

needed to confirm an association between severity of ADHD symptomatology and ID.

Several early studies used observational data to describe the behaviours associated

with ADHD among children with ID. These studies suggested that ADHD symptoms were

higher in children with ID compared with those who were typically developing matched by

chronological age (Epstein, Cullinan, & Gadow, 1986; Fee, Matson, & Benavidez, 1994).

Two studies conducted by Handen and colleagues (1994, 1998) observed children with ID in

classroom settings and attempted to be more specific about the behaviours related to attention

difficulties. Their first study found that those meeting the criteria for ADHD were more

fidgety and less likely to stay on task during individual (but not group) activities, less

interested during group activities, and more restless during either individual or group

activities (Handen, et al., 1994). In their second study the children who met the criteria for

ADHD were more likely to engage in vocalisations during play (such as humming or talking

to oneself) and played with a greater variety of toys for shorter periods of time. They were

also less likely to stay on task during an academic activity and more likely to impulsively

touch toys located on a nearby table when they had been explicitly instructed not to do so

(Handen, et al., 1998). The authors also included a group of children who met the criteria for

both ADHD and conduct disorder, but no significant differences were reported across the

ADHD and ADHD/conduct disorder groups. Given that neither of these studies included

typically developing children with ADHD, it is unclear whether these behaviours are unique

or more frequent in children with ID and ADHD.

29
All of these studies had shortcomings in their methodology. They all used a rating

scale that had not been developed for use with children or adolescents with ID (Conners

Rating Scales; Conners, 1989). Use of this rating scale could impact on the validity of the

results as it may have misrepresented children with ID as having more severe symptoms than

actually occurred within this group. For example, it contains items that are developmentally

inappropriate for children with ID such as Fails to complete assignments. This behaviour

could be endorsed by teachers for many children with ID, when assignments may rarely if

ever be given to children functioning at this level. Further, this behaviour may be observed

irrespective of the presence of comorbid ADHD. Two of the studies (Epstein, et al., 1986;

Fee, et al., 1994) identified ADHD in children by using a checklist completed by teachers and

teacher aides rather than a formal diagnosis. It should also be noted that these studies used

criteria from previous editions of the DSM (American Psychiatric Association, 1980, 1987).

Changes to the diagnostic criteria make it difficult to draw direct comparisons between

children diagnosed with ADHD in these studies and those being diagnosed in the present day.

A number of neuropsychological studies have also examined the attention profiles of

children with ID. Two studies reported that children with ID and ADHD had significantly

greater difficulties compared with those who had ID alone in selective attention but no group

differences were observed in sustained attention (Melnyk & Das, 1992; Pearson, et al., 1996).

A possible explanation for these differences in attentional processes is that selective attention

was considered a more cognitively demanding process that increased the information-

processing load to a greater degree (Melnyk & Das, 1992).

Studies comparing children with ID with typically developing children matched for

mental age have yielded inconsistent findings that are difficult to interpret. The use of

different age groups and a variety of tasks make direct comparisons across studies difficult.

The inclusion of children with borderline intellectual functioning in the ‘ID group’ may have

30
also diluted potential observed differences (Baker, Neece, Fenning, Crnic, & Blacher, 2010;

Henry & MacLean, 2002; van der Molen, van Luit, & Jongmans, 2007).

Lastly, emerging research from genetics has suggested that copy number variants

(CNVs, or large, chromosomal deletions or duplications) occur at a higher rate in children

and adolescents with ADHD and ID (N. M. Williams et al., 2010). This study drew upon

individuals aged 5 to 17 years from the United Kingdom and Iceland, and found that children

with ADHD and ID had 5.69 times the average number of CNVs compared with a typically

developing control group (although it should be noted that psychiatric data was not available

for this group, and therefore it is possible not all individuals in this group would be defined as

typically developing). Children with ADHD but without ID also had an elevated number of

CNVs (1.68 times), therefore suggesting that children with ADHD had a significant excess of

deletions or duplications. This represents the potential for new developments in

understanding genetic risk variants in ADHD if these results are replicated in future studies.

3.2 Diagnoses with intellectual disability as a known cause

Inattentive and hyperactive symptoms are commonly identified in children

with a number of known causes of ID including autism spectrum disorder, Cri Du Chat

Syndrome (also known as 5p-), Down Syndrome, Fragile X Syndrome, velocardiofacial (also

known as DiGeorge or 22q11 Deletion Syndrome), and Williams Syndrome (Cornish &

Wilding, 2010; Dykens, 2000). Recently it has been suggested that it may not be the disorders

themselves, but rather the gene deletions common across disorders, that may result in

attention difficulties (Scharf & Mathews, 2010). These authors suggested that this might

occur in combination with genetic and environmental factors, but their position still needs to

be verified by further research.

Four diagnoses with ID and ADHD symptoms as a common part of their presentation

are described below. While attempts have been made to describe the profile of each group,

31
these conclusions are only preliminary and must be interpreted with caution due to a number

of constraints: a) most studies recruited children and the findings may not necessarily

generalise to toddlers or adolescents as the developmental trajectory in each group cannot be

assumed to be static or linear; b) different aspects of inattention and hyperactivity were

measured using different instruments making direct comparisons difficult; c) the floor effects

reported in some studies mean that the abilities for some children could not be measured

(even when using simple measures developed for children who cannot yet read such as the

Day-Night Task; Diamond & Taylor, 1996; Hooper et al., 2008) and therefore differences

may not necessarily be representative of all children in that syndrome group; and d)

measurement of ID was often imprecise and used screening instruments rather than

standardised measures of intelligence, resulting in some studies estimating the degree of

intellectual impairment.

3.2.1 Autism spectrum disorder

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a childhood-onset developmental disorder

characterised by deficits in social communication and restricted, repetitive patterns of

behaviour (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Prior to the 1990s, the prevalence of

autism was estimated to be approximately 4.7 per 10,000 whereas recent research has

reported a median estimate of 62 per 10,000 for all pervasive developmental disorders (or 1

in 160 children; Elsabbagh et al., 2012). Approximately 70 to 80% of children with ASD also

have severe cognitive delays with many functioning in the moderate to severe range of

intellectual disability (Fombonne, 2005). In this subsample, the median prevalence estimate

increases to 17 per 10,000 (Elsabbagh, et al., 2012). Studies have consistently reported a

greater number of males diagnosed with ASD compared with females, although the disparity

is more pronounced in children with high-functioning autism, Asperger’s Syndrome or

pervasive developmental disorder – not otherwise specified, at approximately 6 to 8 males for

32
every female (Fombonne, 2005). The ratio of males to females is markedly lower among

children with ASD and ID, particularly at the severe to profound level where the ratio is 2

males to every female (Fombonne, 2005).

3.2.1.1 Studies examining the attention profile of children with ASD

The DSM-5 has changed its diagnostic criteria to allow ASD and ADHD to be made

as comorbid diagnoses. Previously ASD was one of the exclusionary criteria for ADHD in

both the DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) and ICD-10 (World Health

Organization, 1992). Despite this only recent change, children, adolescents, and adults

presenting with symptoms that satisfied the diagnostic criteria for both disorders have been

reported since the 1990s (e.g., Ghaziuddin, Tsai, & Alessi, 1992; Yoshida & Uchiyama,

2004). Academics and practitioners alike recognised the utility of a comorbid diagnosis if the

individual satisfied the criteria for both disorders (Frazier et al., 2001; Goldstein &

Schwebach, 2004; Holtmann, Bolte, & Poustka, 2005; Reiersen, Constantino, & Todd, 2008;

Rohde, 2008; Simonoff et al., 2008), and studies anecdotally reported that psychologists and

neurologists were making this dual diagnosis and disregarding the diagnostic guidelines as a

result of these convictions (Ghaziuddin, Welch, Mohiuddin, Lagrou, & Ghaziuddin, 2010;

Jensen, Larrieu, & Mack, 1997).

Irrespective of adherence to the diagnostic criteria, the presence of attention-deficit

hyperactivity symptoms have been commonly identified in individuals with ASD

(Ghaziuddin, et al., 2010; Rommelse, Franke, Geurts, Hartman, & Buitelaar, 2010). This has

been reported in both clinic-based (Frazier, et al., 2001; Gadow, DeVincent, & Pomeroy,

2006; Goldstein & Schwebach, 2004; Hartley, Sikora, & McCoy, 2008; Hattori et al., 2006;

D. O. Lee & Ousley, 2006; Leyfer, Folstein, et al., 2006; Sturm, Fernell, & Gillberg, 2004;

Witwer & Lecavalier, 2010) and population-based samples (Keen & Ward, 2004; Simonoff,

et al., 2008).

33
Research attempting to identify the behavioural phenotypes of inattention and

hyperactivity in children with ASD has been inconsistent. Several studies have suggested that

children with Asperger’s Syndrome or high-functioning autism have high levels of

inattention (Klin, Pauls, Schultz, & Volkmar, 2005; D. O. Lee & Ousley, 2006; Sinzig,

Walter, & Doepfner, 2009; Yoshida & Uchiyama, 2004) while comparative studies have

suggested that inattention is observed in children with ASD irrespective of their level of

cognitive functioning (Estes, Dawson, Sterling, & Munson, 2007; Konstantareas & Stewart,

2006; Mahan & Matson, 2011). Comparative studies have reported that hyperactivity is more

severe in children with ASD and ID (Estes, et al., 2007), while others have reported similar

severity across children with high functioning autism and those with ASD and ID (Kaat,

Lecavalier, & Aman, 2013; Lecavalier, 2006; Mahan & Matson, 2011). Recent studies have

also noted age and gender differences, with one study suggesting that males with high

functioning autism have greater levels of hyperactivity than females (May, Cornish, &

Rinehart, 2013) and another suggesting that increasing age is associated with lower levels of

hyperactivity in children with ASD (Kaat, et al., 2013).

It should be noted, however, that several of these studies had shortcomings in their

research designs. Several of the comparative studies had mixed samples of children with

ASD and ID and those with high functioning autism (D. O. Lee & Ousley, 2006; Mahan &

Matson, 2011; Sinzig, et al., 2009). This made the sample sizes of each group smaller and

thus reduced their ability to generalise to specific groups or to children with ASD in general.

The study by Estes and colleagues (2007) drew their conclusions from parent (primarily

mother) reports which were not confirmed by clinical evaluation or observations. These

findings would need to be replicated in larger samples and with information obtained from

various informants to determine their generalisability.

34
A limited number of neuropsychological studies have examined the cognitive

phenotype of children with ASD. They have reported that children with ASD had greater

attention difficulties compared with typically developing or intellectually disabled children

and adolescents after controlling for mental age and IQ (Burack, 1994). Sustained attention

appears to be an area of strength, even when compared with children who were typically

developing (Garretson, Fein, & Waterhouse, 1990; Johnson et al., 2007). Selective attention

has been identified as being comparable to (Iarocci & Burack, 2004) or better than (Jarrold,

Gilchrist, & Bender, 2005; Joseph, Keehn, Connolly, Wolfe, & Horowitz, 2009) their

typically developing peers. Christ and colleagues (2007; 2011) conducted two studies

examining inhibitory control among children with high-functioning autism. They found that

the children with autism experienced difficulties in some areas of inhibitory control when

compared with children who were typically developing.

3.2.2 Down Syndrome

Down Syndrome is one of the most common genetic syndromes causing ID. It is

caused by a third copy of chromosome 21 (trisomy 21) with three genetic subtypes: 95% are

non-familial, sporadic cases arising from non-disjunction; up to 5% are translocations of a

portion of chromosome 21 to other chromosomes, usually chromosome 14; and 1-2% are

mosaics, where both trisomy 21 and normal cell lines occur in the same individual

(McInerny, Adam, Campbell, Kamat, & Kelleher, 2009). The prevalence of Down Syndrome

births in Victoria, Australia has declined over the last 20 years, primarily due to an increase

in cases diagnosed prenatally which have resulted in termination of pregnancy. Each year,

between 45 and 60 babies are born with Down Syndrome in Victoria, with an overall natural

occurrence of approximately 1 in 650 live births (Collins, Muggli, Riley, Palma, & Halliday,

2008). This decline in prevalence is similar to rates reported in the United Kingdom (J. K.

Morris & Alberman, 2009) but contrasts with studies conducted in Europe and the United

35
States where the prevalence has either remained relatively stable (Loane et al., 2013) or has

increased (de Graaf et al., 2011; Shin et al., 2009) over the same period of time.

3.2.2.1 Studies examining the attention profile of children with Down

Syndrome

Studies examining the prevalence of ADHD in individuals with Down Syndrome

have yielded conflicting results. Earlier studies reported conservative estimates of between 4

and 8% (Dykens, 2007; McCarthy & Boyd, 2001), similar to that of typically developing

children (Willcutt, 2012) while a more recent study reported a prevalence rate of 43% with

children being diagnosed by a paediatric neurologist (Ekstein, Glick, Weill, Kay, & Berger,

2011). The small sample sizes, symptom identification (current versus retrospective), and

different diagnostic classifications all contribute to the difficulty in determining prevalence.

Studies examining the behaviour phenotype have reported that children with Down

Syndrome have greater levels of inattention compared with children who are typically

developing (Cornish, Steele, Monteiro, Karmiloff-Smith, & Scerif, 2012; Nygaard, Smith, &

Torgersen, 2002; van Gameren-Oosterom et al., 2011). Older studies have reported that

hyperactive symptoms were more frequently observed in boys with Down Syndrome

compared with girls, or compared with boys who were typically developing (Royal College

of Psychiatrists, 2001), but that these symptoms decreased in adolescence (Stores, Stores,

Fellows, & Buckley, 1998). Recent research has also suggested that children with Down

Syndrome (irrespective of gender) aged 4 to 9 years have greater severity of hyperactivity

compared with typically developing controls (Cornish, Steele, et al., 2012), but when

compared with children and adolescents with other neurodevelopmental disorders, their

severity was significantly lower (Einfeld, et al., 2007). Taken together, this research suggests

that children with Down Syndrome have difficulties with inattention and hyperactivity, but

36
they appear to decrease in adolescence and these symptoms are less severe than children with

other neurodevelopmental disorders.

Neuropsychological studies examining the cognitive phenotype have attempted to

identify a unique attention ‘signature’ or profile of individuals with Down Syndrome. Two

studies have identified sustained attention as a relative strength (Breckenridge, Braddick,

Anker, Woodhouse, & Atkinson, 2013), and reaching a level comparable to that of typically

developing children matched for mental age (Cornish, Scerif, et al., 2007). In the area of

selective attention, toddlers with Down Syndrome were reported to perform similarly to their

typically developing peers (matched for mental age) on a task requiring them to touch large

circles on a screen in the presence of smaller distractor circles. In childhood, however, they

performed significantly worse than typically developing children or children with Fragile X

Syndrome on a task requiring them to circle particular items on a map within a time limit.

Cornish and colleagues (2007) suggested a developmental trajectory in selective attention

abilities with toddlers performing similarly to their typically developing peers (matched for

mental age), but demonstrating a deterioration in selective attention skills in childhood before

improving again in adulthood. The conclusions drawn in these studies were however based

on small, cross-sectional samples; longitudinal studies with larger sample sizes are needed to

confirm these findings.

3.2.3 Fragile X Syndrome

Fragile X Syndrome is an X-linked genetic disorder which affects

approximately 1 in 2500 males and females worldwide (P. J. Hagerman, 2008), and

approximately 8665 people in Australia (L. Brown, 2010). It is the most common hereditary

cause of ID in males, but the level of cognitive impairment is more variable in females as

they possess one X chromosome with the gene mutation and one without (Cornish, Gray, &

Rinehart, 2010; R. J. Hagerman, 2002). Fragile X Syndrome is caused by a defect in the

37
Fragile X Mental Retardation-1 (FMR1) gene located near the end of the long arm of the X

chromosome (Cornish et al., 2008). This FMR1 gene is “turned off” in affected individuals

leading to a lack of production of a specific protein (FMRP) and results in a unique

constellation of strengths and weaknesses that can affect individuals across their lifespan.

Despite the increased likelihood of ID among children with Fragile X Syndrome,

particularly in males (Alanay et al., 2007; R. J. Hagerman, 2006), they do not exhibit the

typical global deficits characterised by those with ID. Their unique ‘signature’ of clinical and

cognitive strengths and difficulties differentiates them from other developmental disabilities

(Cornish, Turk, & Hagerman, 2008). In some areas, their reported deficits are similar to their

peers who have ID whereas on other tasks their performance is similar to children matched

for mental age (Cornish et al., 2004). Specifically, children with Fragile X Syndrome are

reported to have strengths in vocabulary (van der Molen et al., 2010), recognising visual

details in faces (Turk & Cornish, 1998), and recalling meaningful verbal information (Munir,

Cornish, & Wilding, 2000a). They may however exhibit deficits in the areas of recalling non-

meaningful information (Munir, et al., 2000a) and pragmatic language (Cornish, Sudhalter, &

Turk, 2004). They may also exhibit difficulties with social interaction and reciprocity similar

to those seen in children with ASD (Einfeld, Tonge, & Turner, 1999), although it has been

suggested that the functions of these behaviours may serve very different purposes across the

two diagnoses (Cornish, Turk, et al., 2008). Approximately one third of all children with

Fragile X Syndrome are thought to have ASD as a comorbid diagnosis (R. J. Hagerman,

2006).

3.2.3.1 Studies examining the attention profile of children with Fragile X

Syndrome

Many children with Fragile X Syndrome are reported to exhibit symptoms of

inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity that are consistent with ADHD (Hatton et al., 2002;

38
Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2001; K. Sullivan et al., 2006). A family survey examining

comorbid psychopathology in children with Fragile X Syndrome found that inattentive

behaviours were rated as a significant problem in 84% of males and 67% of females, and

hyperactivity was rated as a significant problem in 66% of males and 30% of females (Bailey,

Raspa, Olmsted, & Holiday, 2008).

Several studies have attempted to describe the behavioural phenotype of attention

difficulties in Fragile X Syndrome. An early study reported that boys with Fragile X

Syndrome displayed significantly higher levels of inattention and distractibility compared

with those with ID, although levels of hyperactivity were similar (Turk, 1998). Another early

study of females with Fragile X Syndrome suggested that they had significantly higher

hyperactive symptoms compared with a control group of girls with other neurodevelopmental

disorders. Further, girls with Fragile X Syndrome and ID had significantly higher levels of

hyperactivity than those with Fragile X and average intelligence (Lachiewicz & Dawson,

1994). There is some evidence to suggest that ADHD symptoms do not decrease with age in

children with Fragile X Syndrome (Cornish, Turk, et al., 2008), although a longitudinal study

of adolescents suggested that this decrease may happen later (i.e., between 16 and 19 years;

Einfeld, et al., 2007) than it does in typically developing children with ADHD (e.g., J.

Biederman, et al., 2006).

A large number of studies have examined the cognitive phenotype of children with

Fragile X Syndrome. Studies examining attention difficulties have reported that sustained

attention is a comparative strength (Cornish, Scerif, et al., 2007; Munir, Cornish, & Wilding,

2000b; K. Sullivan et al., 2007), although their performance is significantly lower than their

typically developing peers both in childhood (Cornish, Cole, Longhi, Karmiloff-Smith, &

Scerif, 2013; Scerif, et al., 2012), and adulthood (Cornish, Munir, & Cross, 2001). Selective

attention is an area of greater weakness compared with sustained attention, with moderate

39
difficulties observed in childhood (Munir, et al., 2000b) which are proposed to persist into

adulthood (Cornish, et al., 2001).

Difficulty with inhibitory control has also been identified among individuals with

Fragile X syndrome (Cornish & Wilding, 2010; Loesch et al., 2003). In a study by Sullivan

and colleagues (2007), boys with Fragile X Syndrome were compared with typically

developing children matched for mental age. Results suggested that while response inhibition

was similar across the two groups at the beginning of the task, they diverged significantly

over the 3 minute duration, and by the end of the task the boys with Fragile X Syndrome

were experiencing significantly greater difficulties with inhibitory control. They also

suggested that boys with Fragile X Syndrome who met diagnostic criteria for ADHD

hyperactive subtype had significantly more difficulties with response inhibition over time

compared with those who did not meet criteria, although this was based on teacher ratings

and not clinical diagnoses. The findings of this study support a proposed developmental

trajectory of inhibition difficulties that seem to appear in infancy (Scerif, Cornish, Wilding,

Driver, & Karmiloff-Smith, 2004), persist into childhood (Cornish, Scerif, et al., 2007;

Hooper, et al., 2008; Scerif, Cornish, Wilding, Driver, & Karmiloff-Smith, 2007), and later

into adulthood (Cornish, et al., 2001). Cornish and colleagues (2004) have suggested that this

inhibitory control deficit may contribute to some of the behaviours consistent with ADHD

such as impulsivity. They conceded however, that this is only the initial step in understanding

the difficulties with inattention and hyperactivity experienced by some children with Fragile

X Syndrome.

3.2.4 Williams Syndrome

Williams Syndrome is a genetic disorder characterised by a microdeletion of a

sequence of genes on the long arm of chromosome 7 (Kaplan, Wang, & Francke, 2001). It is

a relatively rare disorder with the prevalence estimated to be around 1 in 20,000 (C. A.

40
Morris & Mervis, 1999), although a more recent estimate has suggested it is more common at

a rate of 1 in 7,500 (Stromme, Bjornstad, & Ramstad, 2002). This syndrome is not usually

hereditary and occurs in equal rates across genders.

Cognitive impairment is a common feature of individuals with Williams Syndrome,

although not all have ID (Bellugi, Lichtenberger, Jones, Lai, & St George, 2000; Mervis &

John, 2010). Their personality is characterised as being hypersocial (Jones et al., 2000) with

an excessive display of empathy (Kaplan, et al., 2001) and use of verbose, florid language

(Kaplan, et al., 2001). Similar to children with Fragile X Syndrome, they do not exhibit the

typical global deficits characterised by those with ID, and have a distinctive behavioural and

cognitive profile. They have comparative strengths in the areas of processing eye gaze and

facial expressions (Riby, Doherty-Sneddon, & Bruce, 2008), but weaknesses in executive

functioning (Rhodes, Riby, Park, Fraser, & Campbell, 2010) and visuo-spatial construction

tasks (Pani, Mervis, & Robinson, 1999). Their verbal thinking and reasoning skills develop at

a faster rate than their nonverbal abilities. The discrepancy between these two areas appears

to get wider with age, although their verbal ability remains significantly below age

appropriate levels (Jarrold, Baddeley, & Hewes, 1998).

3.2.4.1 Studies examining the attention profile of children with Williams

Syndrome

ADHD is considered to be one of the most common comorbid disorders in children

with Williams Syndrome (Dodd & Porter, 2009), with one brain imaging study suggesting

that these difficulties were correlated with structural differences in grey/white matter

morphology (L. E. Campbell et al., 2009). Prevalence estimates are scant, but two studies

have suggested that ADHD symptoms are present in 65 to 100% of children and adolescents

(Leyfer, Woodruff-Borden, et al., 2006; Rhodes, Riby, Matthews, & Coghill, 2011).

41
An examination of the behavioural phenotype suggests that inattention is more

frequently reported than hyperactivity (Leyfer, Woodruff-Borden, et al., 2006), with some

suggestion that this may be an ‘intrinsic’ characteristic of Williams Syndrome (Gagliardi,

Martelli, Tavano, & Borgatti, 2011). The limited research available suggests no significant

gender differences in the diagnosis of ADHD among children with Williams Syndrome,

although a trend towards a higher proportion of males has been reported (Dodd & Porter,

2009; Leyfer, Woodruff-Borden, et al., 2006).

The limited number of studies makes it difficult to determine whether the behavioural

phenotype of inattention and hyperactivity described is representative of children with

Williams Syndrome. High rates of inattentive and hyperactive symptoms have been reported

by parents (Mervis & Klein-Tasman, 2000; Rhodes, et al., 2011; Rhodes, et al., 2010). A

cross-sectional study has suggested that hyperactivity prevalence decreases with age and

conversely, inattentive symptoms increase with age (Leyfer, Woodruff-Borden, et al., 2006),

while an Australian longitudinal study found that ADHD symptoms decreased with age more

markedly in adolescents with Williams Syndrome than those with other neurodevelopmental

disorders (Einfeld, et al., 2007). Earlier findings from this longitudinal study reported that

parents were significantly more likely to endorse the items overactive and short attention

span on the Developmental Behaviour Checklist, although whether these children actually

met the criteria for ADHD was not explored (Einfeld, Tonge, & Florio, 1997; Tonge &

Einfeld, 2003).

Several studies have also examined the cognitive phenotype but these have been

limited to studies of toddlers. One study suggested that sustained attention is an area of

strength, with toddlers with Williams Syndrome performing as well as typically developing

controls (J. H. Brown et al., 2003). Selective attention, however, has been identified as an

area of comparative weakness (Cornish, Scerif, et al., 2007; Scerif, et al., 2004). Brown and

42
colleagues (2003) further suggested that attention difficulties do not emerge until later in

development among individuals with Williams Syndrome. They speculated that as children

with Williams Syndrome develop, increasing demands are placed on their cognitive skills.

These interactions place a greater burden on attentional capacity which then manifest as

attention difficulties in later childhood and adulthood. Further studies are needed in order to

confirm this speculation, and to explore the developmental trajectory of attention and

hyperactivity into childhood and adolescence within this group.

3.2.5 Cross-syndrome studies

At the behavioural level, cross-syndrome and longitudinal comparisons have

suggested that children and adolescents with Down Syndrome generally have fewer ADHD

symptoms compared with those with ID (Einfeld, et al., 2007; Turk, 1998), Fragile X

Syndrome (Einfeld, et al., 2007; Turk, 1998), ASD (Einfeld, et al., 2007), or Williams

Syndrome (Cornish, Steele, et al., 2012; Einfeld, et al., 2007; Papaeliou et al., 2012). Further,

a longitudinal study suggested that ADHD symptoms decline slowly through adolescence

into early adulthood across neurodevelopmental disorders, and that this decline is greater in

males than females (Einfeld, et al., 2007). ADHD symptoms have also been compared in two

studies of children with ID, Down Syndrome and ASD. Both studies found that children with

ASD had more severe hyperactivity and impulsivity than the other groups (Bradley & Isaacs,

2006; Hastings, et al., 2005), but there were no differences for inattention (Bradley & Isaacs,

2006).

Neuropsychological studies comparing children with different causes of ID represent

a new direction in understanding children with attention difficulties, as they attempt to tease

out aspects of attention across diagnostic groups. These studies suggest that attention

difficulties in children with ID are not homogenous, and that differences may exist depending

on the child’s behavioural phenotype (Vicari & Carlesimo, 2006). Further, they suggest that

43
while some attention difficulties may appear similar at the behavioural level, this does not

infer that they operate in identical ways at the cognitive level (Cornish & Wilding, 2010).

Two related studies examined inhibitory control in boys with Fragile X and Down

Syndromes, and typically developing children matched on mental age classified as either

poor or good attenders (as measured by the ACTeRS; Ullman, Sleator, & Sprague, 1984).

Their findings suggested that boys with Fragile X Syndrome had significantly greater

difficulties with inhibitory control compared with boys with Down Syndrome or typically

developing boys, irrespective of whether they were identified as poor or good attenders

(Munir, et al., 2000b; Wilding, Cornish, & Munir, 2002).

Studies examining components of cognitive attention have also revealed differences

across syndromes. A study of toddlers suggested that the visual sustained attention of those

with Down Syndrome was significantly shorter than those with Williams Syndrome or

controls matched for mental age (J. H. Brown, et al., 2003), but a more recent study of

slightly older children (aged 3 to 6 years) found no differences across groups (Breckenridge,

et al., 2013). Auditory sustained attention was also identified to be stronger in children with

Down Syndrome than those with Williams Syndrome (Breckenridge, et al., 2013).

The conclusions drawn above are highly speculative given that cross-syndrome

studies are limited. Those studies that have been conducted generally compared either one or

two syndromes with a typically developing group, and most contained small sample sizes.

Studies examining sustained attention, selective attention, and inhibition across ages and

syndrome groups suggested that the development of attention is not linear and that it is

important to examine performance at different ages to gain further information about the

developmental trajectories across and within syndromes (Cornish & Wilding, 2010;

Karmiloff-Smith, 2009). Further research across a greater number of syndrome groups, both

longitudinal and cross-sectional, is needed to yield more conclusive evidence regarding the

44
development of attention and the differences across known and idiopathic causes of ID. More

cross-syndrome studies contrasting hyperactive symptoms are also needed.

45
CHAPTER 4 CURRENT CHECKLISTS AND RATING

SCALES MEASURING SYMPTOMS OF INATTENTION,

HYPERACTIVITY, AND IMPULSIVITY

46
The use of rating scales and checklists to identify behaviours characteristic of ADHD

can be used to assist with diagnosis (Chan, Hopkins, Perrin, Herrerias, & Homer, 2005). In

some studies this has been the only method of identifying and classifying ADHD (e.g.,

Gunter, Arndt, Riggins-Caspers, Wenman, & Cadoret, 2006) and therefore the diagnostic

accuracy and representativeness of these samples is likely to be questionable given the

potential for response bias by the respondent(s) and the diagnostic validity of the measures

used. The generally accepted diagnostic procedure advocates a multi-informant approach that

involves obtaining information from the parents, teachers, and the child if possible (Barkley,

2006c) as well as obtaining a developmental history, behavioural observations, and possibly

data from laboratory testing (Reid & Maag, 1994).

Issues of inter-observer agreement are often an issue, with variation across teacher

and parent observations reflected in the low inter-rater reliability of many rating scales and

checklists (Barkley, 2006c). Some have suggested that low inter-rater reliability should be

expected as different behaviours may be observed across different settings (van der Ende,

1999). Alternatively, reliability may differ due to the way behaviour is defined by the

observer. For example, an item such as fidgety may be rated by one individual as occurring

sometimes but by another as occurring often, as observers may have different perspectives,

tolerance levels, and thresholds for reporting behaviour (Reid & Maag, 1994; van der Ende,

1999). A third alternative is that a teacher may be more attuned to identifying atypical

behaviour due to their exposure to many children in their classroom and across their career,

whereas a parent may be accustomed to their child’s behaviour and may perceive problems as

being less severe, or being of no concern at all (K. Sullivan, et al., 2006).

Despite the many rating scales and checklists available to measure ADHD and

attention difficulties, comparatively few have been clinically validated in populations of

children and adolescents with ID, and even fewer have been specifically developed for this

47
population. This chapter will review the different rating scales that measure ADHD

symptomatology, their psychometric properties, and their reliability and validity for children

and adolescents with ID. With the exception of the Conners (whose recently released third

edition limits its inclusion in independent studies; Conners, 2008), only studies examining the

most recent edition of each rating scale will be included in this review.

The framework used to determine the inclusion and suitability of the scales in our

review was determined by selecting those that have been used extensively in studies to screen

for ADHD symptoms in both population-based samples and in atypical samples of children.

Further, we included some lesser known scales that have been developed for children with ID

given their relative scarcity. The most recent version of 14 scales were therefore reviewed. Of

these 14 scales, five specifically measure ADHD symptoms in typically developing children

and two in children with ID. A further seven scales measure ADHD symptoms within the

context of broader behavioural and emotional problems; four in typically developing children

and three in children with ID. Measures that contained subscales measuring aspects related

to ADHD symptomatology but did not specifically measure hyperactivity, impulsivity or

inattention (e.g., the inhibit and working memory subscales of the Behaviour Rating

Inventory of Executive Function; Gioia, et al., 2000) were excluded from the review.

4.1 Measures with a focus on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and/or attentional

difficulties designed for use with typically developing children

4.1.1 ADD-H Comprehensive Teacher Rating Scale - Second edition (ACTeRS-2)

The second edition of the ACTeRS (Ullman, Sleator, & Sprague, 2000) is a 24-item

rating scale completed by teachers and contains four subscales: attention, hyperactivity,

social skills and oppositional behaviour. Each item is ranked on a 5-point Likert scale from

almost never to almost always. The ACTeRS-2 has separate norms for boys and girls

48
reported in percentile ranks, and can be administered to children from preschool to Year

Eight.

The premise for developing this scale specifically for teachers is because the authors

believed that behaviours related to ADHD are more likely to manifest themselves in the

classroom, and therefore teachers are the best informants to observe and report upon them

(Ullman, et al., 2000). Despite this assertion, the ACTeRS-2 also has two other scales –

parent and self-report – to provide additional information for the clinician of the child’s

behaviour across different settings.

The psychometric properties of the ACTeRS-2 are described in Table 4.1. The authors

report sound psychometric properties for the teacher version (Ullman, et al., 2000), although

an independent study suggested that it had inadequate discriminant validity to distinguish

across ADHD subtypes (Forbes, 2001). For the parent version, only the internal consistency

was reported (Ullman, et al., 2000). No independent studies have evaluated the psychometric

properties of the parent version.

4.1.1 ADHD Rating Scale-IV (ADHD-RS-IV)

The ADHD Rating Scale-IV (DuPaul, et al., 1998) was developed as a revised version

of the ADHD Rating Scale (DuPaul, 1991) to reflect changes to diagnostic criteria in the

DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). It contains a home version completed by

the parent or caregiver and a school version completed by the teacher for children or

adolescents aged 5 to 18 years. A preschool version has also been released (McGoey,

DuPaul, Haley, & Shelton, 2007). Each version has 18 items with 9 items on the inattention

and hyperactivity-impulsivity subscales respectively. Each item is rated on a 4-point Likert

scale from never or rarely to very often. Raw scores are then converted to percentile scores

based on normative data for gender and age.

49
Table 4.1

Psychometric Properties of Measures with a Focus on Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and/or Attentional Difficulties for Use with

Typically Developing Children

Scale Psychometric properties reported by authors Independent studies with typically developing
children
ADD-H Comprehensive Teacher Ullman et al. (2000) None to date
Rating Scale – Second edition, Parent Fair to excellent internal consistency for the five
form (ACTeRS-2) subscales (α = .78 – .96)

ADD-H Comprehensive Teacher Ullman et al. (2000) Erford & Hase (2006)
Rating Scale – Second edition, Excellent internal consistency across the four factors (α Typically developing children between kindergarten and
Teacher form (ACTeRS-2) = .92 – .97) Grade 5
Strong test-retest reliability over 4 weeks (r = .78 – .82) Good to excellent internal consistency across the four
Moderate inter-rater reliability across teachers factors (α = .89 – .93)
(r = .51 - .73) Strong test-retest reliability over 30 days
Good discriminant validity between children with (r = .80 – .89)
ADHD and typically developing children Low to moderate convergent validity with the factors
Good discriminant validity between children with from the Conners Teacher Rating Scale -Revised (r = -
ADHD and children with a learning disability .42 – -.53)
Moderate specificity for inattentive and hyperactive
types (.81 and .88 respectively) but lower sensitivity for
both types (.77 and .81 respectively)

Forbes (2001)
Typically developing children between Grades 1 and 6
Strong convergent validity with the Conners Teacher
Rating Scale - Revised (r = -.54 – -.72)1
1
Correlations are negative as lower scores on the ACTeRS compared with higher scores on the Conners, indicate greater severity of symptoms

50
Low convergent validity with the Conners Parent Rating
Scale – Revised (r = -.01 – -.43)
Low discriminant validity - unable to distinguish
children with hyperactive/combined ADHD from those
with the inattentive subtype

ADHD Rating Scale – IV – Home DuPaul et al. (1998) The psychometric properties have been examined in
version (ADHD-RS-IV) Good to excellent internal consistency (α = .88 – .94) Korean (Kim et al., 2005) and Icelandic (Magnusson,
Strong test-retest reliability over four weeks (r = .78 – Smari, Gretarsdottir, & Prandardottir, 1999) language
.86) adaptations using typically developing children. It has
Moderate inter-rater reliability across parent and teacher also been examined in a number of studies using
ratings (r = .40 – .45) physician ratings (e.g., Dopfner et al., 2006; Zhang,
Considerable variability in convergent validity with the Faries, Vowles, & Michelson, 2005)
Conners Parent Rating Scale (r = .28 - .81)
Inadequate sensitivity (.57) but excellent specificity in
diagnosing ADHD – inattentive (.91). low sensitivity
(.76) but excellent specificity (.91) in diagnosing ADHD
– combined
Good discriminant validity between children with
ADHD and typically developing children in both clinic
and school samples
Good discriminant validity between children with
ADHD inattentive or combined subtypes in both clinic
and school samples

Power et al. (1998)


Moderate inter-rater reliability across parent and teacher
ratings (r = .30 – .41)
Inadequate sensitivity but excellent specificity in
diagnosing inattentive or combined ADHD (varied
depending on cutpoint used)
Good discriminant validity between children with

51
ADHD and typically developing children in a school
sample

ADHD Rating Scale - IV – School DuPaul et al. (1998) None to date


version (ADHD-RS-IV) Good to excellent internal consistency (α = .88 – .96)
Strong test-retest reliability over four weeks
(r = .88 – .90)
Considerable variability in convergent validity with the
Conners Teacher Rating Scale
(r = .29 - .88)

Conners - Third edition (Conners 3)2 Conners (2008) None to date


Parent form Good to excellent internal consistency across subscales
(α = .83 – .94)
Strong test-retest reliability over two to four weeks (r =
.67 – .91)
Moderate to strong inter-rater reliability across parent
and teacher ratings (r = .52 – .67)
Strong convergent validity between the Conners 3
subscales that measured attention difficulties with the
CBCL Attention problems subscale
(r = .70 – .92)
Strong convergent validity between the Conners 3
subscales that measured attention difficulties with the
BRIEF inhibit and working memory subscales (r = .60 –
.78)
Good discriminant validity between children with
ADHD and typically developing children
Good discriminant validity between children with
ADHD and children in other clinical groups (diagnosed

2
Psychometric properties relate to full-length Conners 3 forms

52
by a psychiatrist or psychologist)
Limited discriminant validity between children with
hyperactive/impulsive and inattentive subtypes of
ADHD

Conners - Third edition (Conners 3) Conners (2008) None to date


Teacher form Fair to excellent internal consistency across subscales (α
= .77 – .95)
Strong test-retest reliability over two to four weeks (r =
.72 – .83)
Strong convergent validity between the Conners 3
subscales that measured attention difficulties with the
CBCL Attention problems subscale (r = .72 – .76)
Strong convergent validity between the Conners 3
subscales that measured attention difficulties with the
BRIEF inhibit and working memory subscales (r = .61 –
.92)
Good discriminant validity between children with
ADHD and typically developing children
Good discriminant validity between children with
ADHD and children in other clinical groups (diagnosed
by a psychiatrist or psychologist)
Limited discriminant validity between children with
hyperactive/impulsive and inattentive subtypes of
ADHD

Swanson, Nolan & Pelham Checklist – Swanson (1992) Solanto & Alvir (2009)
Fourth edition (SNAP- IV) Psychometric data not reported. Parent and teacher ratings for typically developing
Validity implied as items are formulated from the DSM- children and children referred for attention difficulties
IV but no psychometric analyses to support this Fair to excellent internal consistency for parent ratings (α
conclusion = .71 – .92)
Excellent internal consistentency for teacher ratings (α =

53
.90 – .97)
Considerable variability in convergent validity with
corresponding subscale items on the Conners Rating
Scales – Revised for teacher ratings (k = .31 – .79) and
parent ratings (k = .37 – .72)

Stevens, Quittner, & Abikoff (1998)


Teachers rating videotapes of a typically developing
child and one with ADHD or ODD
Good to excellent internal consistency
(α = .84 – .95)

Used MTA-SNAP-IV
Bussing et al. (2008)
Parent and teacher ratings for typically developing
children
Fair to good internal consistency for parent ratings (α =
.79 – .90)
Excellent internal consistentency for teacher ratings (α =
.92 – .96)
Moderate inter-rater reliability across parent and teacher
ratings (r = .43 – .49)
Good discriminant validity between children who met
diagnostic criteria for ADHD and typically developing
children for parent ratings

The psychometric properties have also been examined in


Chinese (Gau et al., 2009; Gau, Shang, et al., 2008) and
Portugese (Mattos, Serra-Pinheiro, Rohde, & Pinto,
2006) language adaptations

54
Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD Swanson et al. (2005) Young, Levy, Martin & Hay (2009)
Symptoms and Normal Behaviour Psychometric data not reported. Excellent internal consistency (α = .94 – .96)
Scale (SWAN) Good discriminant validity between children with
ADHD (any subtype) and typically developing children

The psychometric properties have been examined in a


French language adaptation (Robaey, Amre, Schachar, &
Simard, 2007) using typically developing children with a
suspected diagnosis of ADHD

55
The psychometric properties of the ADHD-RS-IV are described in Table 4.1. The

convergent validity of the school version with the Conners Teacher Rating Scale (CTRS) are

variable, but all other properties are strong (DuPaul, et al., 1998). No independent studies,

however, have examined the psychometric properties of the school version. The home

version of this scale has variable psychometric properties. It has good to excellent internal

consistency, strong test-retest reliability, moderate inter-rater reliability, and good

discriminant validity between children with ADHD and those who are typically developing.

Similar to the school version, it has variable convergent validity with the Conners Parent

Rating Scale (CPRS). The authors also reported that while the ADHD-RS-IV home version

has inadequate to low sensitivity in diagnosing ADHD inattentive and combined subtypes, it

has excellent specificity (DuPaul, et al., 1998; Power, et al., 1998).

4.1.2 Conners Third edition (Conners 3)

The Conners Third edition (Conners, 2008) has separate forms completed by parents

and teachers for children and adolescents aged 6 to 18 years, as well as a self-report scale for

children and adolescents aged 8 to 18 years. It aims to evaluate symptoms of ADHD and

related disorders, and makes direct reference to the diagnostic criteria for ADHD set out in

the DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Short (39 to 43 items) and long

(105 to 110 items) forms are available which ask the respondent to respond on a 4-point scale

from not true at all (never, seldom) to very much true (very often, very frequently). The items

are grouped under the following subscales: inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, learning

problems, executive functioning, aggression, and peer relations. It also measures behaviours

consistent with oppositional defiant and conduct disorders which may be observed in children

with ADHD.

The teacher version of this scale has fair to excellent internal consistency and strong

test-test reliability (see Table 4.1; Conners, 2008). It also has strong convergent validity with

56
the relevant subscales measuring attention difficulties on the Child Behaviour Checklist

Teacher Report Form (CBCL TRF) and the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive

Function Teacher Form (BRIEF). The authors reported good discriminant validity between

children with ADHD, children with other clinical disorders and typically developing children,

but limited discriminant validity to distinguish across ADHD subtypes.

The parent version of this scale has good to excellent internal consistency, strong test-

test reliability and moderate to strong inter-rater reliability across parent and teacher ratings

(see Table 4.1; Conners, 2008). It also has strong convergent validity with the corresponding

subscales measuring attention difficulties on the Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) and

Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function Parent Form (BRIEF). The authors

reported good discriminant validity between children with ADHD, children with other

clinical disorders and typically developing children, but limited discriminant validity to

distinguish across ADHD subtypes. No independent studies to date have examined the

psychometric properties of the parent or teacher version.

4.1.3 Swanson, Nolan & Pelham Checklist – Fourth edition (SNAP-IV)

The SNAP-IV (Swanson, 1992) is a checklist completed by parents or teachers and

has 90 items. The initial 40 items relate to ADHD (with two subscales: hyperactivity and

inattention) and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD). The remaining 50 items relate to

behaviours taken from 15 DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) diagnoses

which the author proposed may overlap with or masquerade as ADHD symptoms. There is

also a shorter, 26 item version with 18 items relating to ADHD and 8 items relating to ODD.

This version is sometimes referred to as the MTA-SNAP-IV as it was used in the Multimodal

Treatment Study for ADHD (The MTA Cooperative Group, 1999). The SNAP-IV is freely

available from the author’s web site (Swanson, n.d.) but lacks age- and gender-based norms.

57
There is no psychometric data published by the author for the SNAP-IV (Swanson,

1992). Face validity could be implied given that the items were formulated from DSM-III-R

(American Psychiatric Association, 1987) diagnostic criteria but this is speculative.

Independent studies have suggested that the SNAP-IV has fair to excellent internal

consistency for parent ratings and good to excellent internal consistency for teacher ratings

(see Table 4.1; Solanto & Alvir, 2009; Stevens, et al., 1998). Its convergent validity with the

CPRS-R and CTRS-R had significant variation across subscales for both parent and teacher

ratings varying from weak to strong (Solanto & Alvir, 2009).

4.1.4 Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behaviour Scale

(SWAN)

The SWAN (Swanson, et al., 2005) is a revision of the SNAP-IV (reviewed above;

Swanson, 1992) with the items reworded in a positive (strength-based) manner e.g., Often

talks excessively became Modulate verbal activity (control excess talking). As with the MTA-

SNAP-IV (The MTA Cooperative Group, 1999), it consists of 18 items, with nine related to

inattention and the other nine related to hyperactivity/impulsivity. Like the SNAP-IV

(Swanson, 1992), it does not have separate scales for parents and teachers.

Unlike other rating scales that use diagnostic cut-points, the SWAN conceptualises

ADHD on a 7-point continuum ranging from far below average to far above average,

yielding a normal distribution of scores. The use of a strength-based scale is considered

preferable as it reduces the likelihood of over-inflating the proportion of children having

problems which can occur when using a truncated, problem-based scale (Hay, et al., 2007).

There is no psychometric data published by the authors of the SWAN (Swanson, et

al., 2005). Limited psychometric data from an independent study reported that the SWAN

had excellent reliability and good discriminant validity between children with ADHD (any

subtype) and typically developing children (see Table 4.1; Young, et al., 2009).

58
4.1.5 Limitations of these measures

A significant limitation of the SNAP-IV (Swanson, 1992) and SWAN (Swanson, et

al., 2005) is that the reliability and validity of these scales has not been established given the

lack of psychometric data. While the ADHD-RS-IV (DuPaul, et al., 1998) reports on its

psychometric properties, the reliability and validity of the home version is variable. The lack

of independent studies examining the psychometric properties of this scale also makes it

difficult to verify its reliability and validity. Along with the ACTeRS-2 (Ullman, et al., 2000),

the SNAP-IV (Swanson, 1992) and SWAN (Swanson, et al., 2005) also lack age-based norms

despite considerable research suggesting developmental differences in attention and

hyperactivity across different ages (Faraone, et al., 2006; J. C. Hill & Schoener, 1996).

The SNAP-IV (Swanson, 1992) and ADHD-RS-IV (DuPaul, et al., 1998) both

provide clinical cut-off points delineating typical functioning from clinical attentional

difficulties, although flaws in both designs suggest their discriminant validity may be limited.

In the case of the SNAP-IV, the assignment of children as ADHD and/or ODD was based on

teacher ratings and not formal diagnostic criteria. For the ADHD-RS-IV, the authors devised

different cut-off points depending upon: (a) whether the scale is being used as a screening or

diagnostic tool; (b) whether an ADHD – combined or ADHD – inattentive diagnosis is being

investigated; and (c) whether the user wishes to screen/diagnose ADHD or “rule out” this

diagnosis.

Another limitation inherent to the ACTeRS-2 (Ullman, et al., 2000), SNAP-IV

(Swanson, 1992) and SWAN (Swanson, et al., 2005) is that they lack details regarding the

demographic characteristics of their samples. The extent to which the samples used in their

development are representative of the wider population are therefore unclear. The ADHD-

RS-IV (DuPaul, et al., 1998) drew upon a diverse population of American children from a

range of cultural backgrounds but their normative data may need to be interpreted with

59
caution in cross-cultural studies. Students from an African-American background scored

consistently higher than students who were Caucasian (Reid et al., 1998). The majority of

children in this sample were also middle class, so the results may not generalise to children

from different socioeconomic backgrounds.

In scoring the ADHD-RS-IV, a total can be calculated with up to three missing items

on each subscale (or 33% of the total number of items; DuPaul, et al., 1998). This could

potentially result in lower scores that may misrepresent the child’s hyperactive/impulsive

and/or inattentive behaviours and may lead to incorrect conclusions being drawn about the

child’s difficulties in these areas (Demaray, Elting, & Schaefer, 2003).

One of the limitations of the Conners 3 (Conners, 2008) is the length of the long form,

with over 100 items being impractical for use as a clinical or screening tool. There is also

some repetition, thus calling into question the inclusion of some items. Other items are

designed to assess the validity of the ratings with generalised statements which some

respondents may choose to omit (e.g., Behaves like an angel), thus defeating the purpose of

their inclusion.

4.1.6 Use with children and adolescents with ID

Of the five measures reviewed above, only one has been included in studies

examining the psychometric properties in children or adolescents with ID, namely the

Conners Rating Scales (Conners, 1997, 2008). Given the relatively recent release of the

Conners 3 (Conners, 2008), independent studies that have used this instrument are not yet

available. For the purpose of this review, research on the properties of its predecessor, the

Conners’ Rating Scales – Revised (Conners, 1997) will be reported upon.

While the Conners Parent and Teacher Rating Scales – Revised (CPRS-R, CTRS-R;

Conners, 1997) has been used in many studies of children with ID, few have validated its use

within these populations (Guerin, et al., 2009). One study examined the utility of these scales

60
among children and adolescents with mild to severe ID (Deb, et al., 2008). As a screening

tool for ADHD, the CPRS-R was found to have excellent sensitivity (.90) but inadequate

specificity (.67). The CTRS-R was found to have inadequate sensitivity (.69) and specificity

(.67). The authors also found the inter-rater reliability across parents and teachers was

unacceptably low (r = .17). The findings suggested that while the CPRS-R may be able to

distinguish between children with ID with or without ADHD, the CTRS-R was unable to

make this distinction. The authors noted that 13 items (46.4%) on the CTRS-R were

dependent upon the child being verbal, thus invalidating the measure for children who do not

have meaningful communication skills. They also questioned the validity of many items on

the CTRS-R and suggested that it would not be a useful measure for rating children with

severe or profound ID.

4.2 Measures with a focus on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and/or attentional

difficulties designed for use with children with ID

4.2.1 Attention-Distraction, Inhibition-Excitation Classroom Assessment Scale

(ADIECAS)

Developed by Peter Evans (1975, in Evans & Hogg, 1984) as part of his

doctoral thesis, the ADIECAS has 16 items and was developed specifically to examine

inattention and hyperactivity in children with ID. The items are completed by the classroom

teacher and each item is ranked on a scale of 1 to 7, with higher scores indicating higher

severity. The authors reported that two dimensions can be extracted from the scale:

inattention/excitation (IE) and attentiveness/distractibility (AD). The IE subscale measures

behaviours such as how well a child can restrain their actions, inhibit their responses, and

coordinate their movements. The AD subscale measures behaviours such as the ability to

work well on a set task, resist disruptions and distractions, attend well to instructions, and

persevere.

61
The ADIECAS has moderate to strong test-test reliability but only weak to moderate

inter-rater reliability across teachers (see Table 4.2; Evans & Hogg, 1984). An independent

study of children with severe ID suggested that the ADIECAS has fair to excellent internal

consistency (Strand, Sturmey, & Newton, 1990). Its convergent validity with the CTRS-R

reported significant variation for teacher ratings from very weak to strong (Buckley, Hillery,

Guerin, McEvoy, & Dodd, 2008; Guerin, et al., 2009). A comparison of the factor structure

of the ADIECAS across studies provided by Guerin and colleagues (2009) suggested little

consistency to the composition of items within the subscales, with the only consistent finding

being that all studies extracted the IE and AD subscales (albeit with different items).

4.2.2 Attention Checklist (AC)

The Attention Checklist (AC) was developed specifically for children and adolescents

with ID (Das, 1986, in Das & Melnyk, 1989) with a focus on inattentive behaviours. It was

designed to be completed by teachers and contains 12 items that are rated on a 4-point scale

from not at all to pretty much. The authors reported that the AC has excellent internal

consistency and high convergent validity with the CRS (see Table 4.2; Das & Melnyk, 1989).

4.2.3 Limitations of these measures

While both the ADIECAS (Evans, 1975 in Evans & Hogg, 1984) and AC (Das, 1986

in Das & Melnyk, 1989) have the advantage of being developed specifically to measure

ADHD symptoms in children with ID, neither tool is a valid measure to use within this

population. The psychometric properties of the ADIECAS are variable, and its factor

structure has yielded inconsistent findings across studies (Evans & Hogg, 1984; Strand, et al.,

1990; Turner, Sloper, & Knussen, 1991). The psychometric properties of the AC are reported

to be strong by the authors, but there have been no independent studies that can confirm these

robust findings.

62
Table 4.2

Psychometric Properties of Measures with a Focus on Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and/or Attentional Difficulties for Use with

Children with Intellectual Disability

Scale Psychometric properties reported by authors Independent studies with children with ID

Attention-Distraction, Inhibition- Evans & Hogg (1984) Guerin et al. (2009)


Excitation Classroom Assessment Moderate to strong test-retest reliability over one month Children and adolescents with moderate to severe ID
Scale (ADIECAS) (r = .51 – .83) Good to excellent internal consistency
Weak to moderate inter-rater reliability across teachers (α = .83 – .92)
(r = .37 – .71) Considerable variation in convergent validity with the
Conners Teacher Rating Scale (r = .37 – .72)

Buckley, Hillery, Guerin, McEvoy & Dodd (2008)


Children and adolescents with ID (severity not defined)
Considerable variation in convergent validity with the
Conners Teacher Rating Scale – Revised
(r = .19 – .73)

Turner, Sloper, & Knussen (1991)


Children and adolescents with Down Syndrome
Suggested that the test items yielded a poor fit to the
original two-factor AD/IE model and suggested a four
factor alternative

Strand, Sturmey, & Newton (1990)


Children and adolescents with severe ID
Fair to excellent internal consistency using a four-factor
structure (α = .71 – .92)
Moderate concurrent validity with British Ability Scales
on attention-distraction factor (r = .41 - .60) but not the
inhibition-excitation factor (r not reported)

63
Attention Checklist (AC) Das & Melnyk (1989) None to date
Excellent internal consistency (α = .96)
High convergent validity with the Conners Rating Scale
(r = -.84).

64
There are several other limitations which may have contributed to their lack of uptake

by researchers and clinicians. Studies using the ADIECAS (Evans, 1975 in Evans & Hogg,

1984) have focused on children with moderate to severe ID, so its validity for use with

children who have mild ID has not been established. The AC (Das, 1986 in Das & Melnyk,

1989) only measures inattentive behaviours with the authors offering no explanation or

justification for excluding items measuring hyperactive/impulsive behaviours. Neither scale

has developed a parent version, precluding the ability to obtain and contrast behavioural data

from multiple informants. Both scales also lack commercial availability and lack age- or

gender-based normative data.

4.3 Measures assessing a range of behavioural and emotional problems designed for

typically developing children

The measures reviewed in this section assess a broad range of behavioural and

emotional problems in typically developing children. Given the focus of this thesis is on

behaviours relating to attention, hyperactivity and impulsivity these scales will be reviewed

in the context of those subscales that specifically measure these areas.

4.3.1 Behavior Assessment System for Children – Second edition (BASC-2)

The BASC-2 is a set of rating scales for children, adolescents, and young adults aged

2 to 21 years (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004) with separate forms for parents (Parent Rating

Scale; PRS) and teachers (Teacher Rating Scale; TRS). There is some variation in the number

and composition of items depending on the age of the individual being rated: preschool (2 to

5 years), child (6 to 11 years) and adolescent (12 to 21 years). The PRS forms have 134 to

160 items and the TRS forms have 100 to 139 items. Each item is rated on a 4-point scale

from never to almost always. The manual contains separate norms across genders, age ranges,

and for general as well as clinical (i.e., learning disability and ADHD) samples.

65
The BASC-2 contains two subscales relating to ADHD symptomatology: attention

problems and hyperactivity. Markers for ADHD are indicated by scores in the clinical range

(a T score ≥70) on either subscale. These scores are reported separately which allows for

different subtypes to be explored. The authors have emphasised that these scores can be used

to assist with diagnosis of ADHD but should not be used in isolation (Reynolds &

Kamphaus, 2004).

The authors reported that the TRS has good to excellent internal consistency in both

general and clinical samples (see Table 4.3; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004). They also

reported that the BASC-2 has strong test-retest reliability and moderate to strong inter-rater

reliability across teachers (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004). These properties have been

replicated in an independent study (Bergeron, Floyd, McCormack, & Farmer, 2008). Strong

convergent validity with the CTRS-R and CBCL TRF has been reported by the authors, but

no independent studies have confirmed these findings. The validity of the TRS to

discriminate children with ADHD from those who do not have this diagnosis, or to

discriminate between ADHD subtypes, has not been examined.

The PRS has good to excellent internal consistency in both general and clinical

samples (see Table 4.3; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004). The authors also reported that the

PRS has strong test-test reliability and strong inter-rater reliability across parents (Reynolds

& Kamphaus, 2004). The convergent validity of the attention problems and hyperactivity

subscales of the BASC-2 PRS with the attention problems subscale of the CBCL were found

to be strong in a clinically referred kindergarten sample (Myers, Bour, Sidebottom, Murphy,

& Hakman, 2010).

Reported advantages of the BASC-2 include its use of validity checks for excessively

negative or positive responses (Gladman & Lancaster, 2003), and its inclusion of adaptive

66
Table 4.3

Psychometric Properties of Measures Assessing a Range of Behavioural and Emotional Problems for Use with Typically Developing Children

Scale Psychometric properties reported by authors Independent studies with typically developing
children
3
Behavior Assessment System for Reynolds and Kamphaus (2004) Myers et al. (2010)
Children – Second edition – Parent Good to excellent internal consistency across composite Clinically referred preschool children
form (BASC-2 PRS) scales and the overall BSI for the general (α = .89 – .95) Moderate to strong convergent validity with
and clinical samples (α = .90 – .96) corresponding CBCL 1.5 -5 subscales (r = .44 - .86) and
Strong test-retest reliability over a period of 9 - 70 days4 composite scores (r = .63 - .90)
(r = .78 – .92)
Strong inter-rater reliability across mother and father
ratings (r = .65 – .86)
Strong convergent validity with the CBCL on the
externalising and internalising subscales, and the total
score (r = .67 – .84)
Strong convergent validity with the Conners PRS-R total
score (r = .65 – .79)
Moderate to strong convergent validity between the
attention problems and hyperactivity subscales and the
BRIEF working memory and inhibit subscales (r = .48 –
.79)

Behavior Assessment System for Reynolds and Kamphaus (2004) 5 Bergeron et al. (2008)
Children – Second edition – Teacher Good to excellent internal consistency across composite Typically developing children attending primary schools
form (BASC-2 TRS) scales and the overall BSI for the general (α = .87 – .97) Strong test-retest reliability over a period of 8-25 days (r

3
Child and adolescent forms were analysed separately by the authors, but are reported together here
4
General and clinical samples were combined
5
Child and adolescent forms were analysed separately by the authors, but are reported together here

67
and clinical samples (α = .87 – .97) = .83 – .93)
Strong test-retest reliability over a period of 8 - 65 days6 Strong inter-rater reliability across teachers (r = .72 –
(r = .81 – .93) .79)
Moderate to strong inter-rater reliability across teachers (r Strong convergent validity across the BASC-2
= .48 – .70) externalising problems subscale and the CBCL
Strong convergent validity with the CBCL TRF on the externalising behaviours composite (r = .89)7
externalising and internalising subscales, and the total
score (r = .64 – .80)
Strong convergent validity with the CTRS-R total score (r
= .69 – .84)

Child Behaviour Checklist - Parent Achenbach & Rescorla (2001) Hudziak, et al. (2004)
form (CBCL) Fair to excellent internal consistency Children with attention and/or aggression behaviour
(α = .72 – .97) problems compared with their siblings
Strong test-retest reliability over a mean period of 8 days Inadequate sensitivity (.34) but excellent specificity (.99)
(r = .80 – .94) in diagnosing ADHD
Strong convergent validity with the ADHD Index and
oppositional subscale on the Conners Parent Rating Scale
(r = .71 – .80)

Child Behaviour Checklist - Teacher Achenbach & Rescorla (2001) Bergeron et al. (2008)
report form (TRF) Fair to excellent internal consistency Typically developing children attending primary schools
(α = .72 – .95)8 Strong test-retest reliability over a period of 8-25 days (r
Very weak to moderate inter-rater reliability between = .83 – .90)
teachers and parents (r = .12 – .44) Strong inter-rater reliability across teachers (r = .62 –
Strong test-retest reliability over a mean period of 16 days .73)
(r = .62 – .96)
Strong convergent validity with the Conners Teacher

6
General and clinical samples were combined
7
Convergent validity for ADHD subscales were not calculated as only externalising behaviours were examined in this study, precluding a comparison of inattentive
behaviours
8
Lower alphas corresponded with the somatic complaints and thought problems subscales, which both comprised items that are seldom endorsed by teachers

68
Rating Scale (r = .77 – .89)

Children’s Behaviour Questionnaire Rothbart, Ahadi, Hersey, & Fisher (2001) Kochanska, DeVet, Goldman, Murray, & Putnam (1994)
(CBQ) Unacceptable to excellent internal consistency Mother ratings in 2-6 year old children
(α = .64 – .92) Unacceptable to excellent internal consistency
Considerable variability in inter-rater reliability across (α = .68 – .98)
parents (r = .28 – .79)
The psychometric properties have also been examined in
Chinese (Ahadi, Rothbart, & Ye, 1993) and Japanese
(Kusanagi, 1993, in Rothbart, et al., 2001) language
adaptations

Strengths and Difficulties Goodman (2007) Becker et al. (2006)


Questionnaire (SDQ) – Parent scale Weak to strong inter-rater reliability across parent and Children and adolescents with ADHD in 10 European
teacher ratings (r = .37 – .62) countries
Strong convergent validity with corresponding subscales Unacceptable to fair internal consistency
of the Rutter Scales (r = .78 – .88) (α = .58 – .72)
Weak to moderate convergent validity with
Goodman & Scott (1999) corresponding subscales on the ADHD-RS IV (r = .30 –
Moderate to strong convergent validity with .54)
corresponding subscalesof the CBCL (r = .59 – .87)
Bourdon, Goodman, Rae, Simpson, & Koretz (2005)
Typically developing children
Unacceptable to fair internal consistency
(α = .46 – .77)

Hawes & Dadds (2004)


Typically developing children – Australian study
Unacceptable to good internal consistency (α = .59 – .80)
Weak concurrent validity between SDQ hyperactivity
subscale and classroom observation for hyperactivity (r
= .20)

69
Strong test-retest reliability over 12 months
(r = .61 – .77)

Strengths and Difficulties Goodman (2007) Hill & Hughes (2007)


Questionnaire (SDQ) – Teacher Strong convergent validity with corresponding subscales Typically developing Grade 1 children with low literacy
scale of the Rutter Scales (r = .87 – .92) achievement scores
Unacceptable to good internal consistency (α = .64 – .89)
Weak to moderate inter-rater reliability across parents
and teachers (r = .26 – .47)

70
and maladaptive behaviour scales to provide a balanced perspective of each individual (Tan,

2007). It has been suggested that the use of similar scale and item structures on the parent and

teacher forms may increase inter-rater reliability (Gladman & Lancaster, 2003), although

others have reported that its reliability across teachers and parents is limited (Tan, 2007). The

division of hyperactive and inattentive behaviours into separate subscales is also perceived

by some as an advantage (Gladman & Lancaster, 2003) although this could also be

disadvantageous as it does not provide an overall score for ADHD.

4.3.2 Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL)

The CBCL has several parent, teacher and self-rating scales. Separate parent/caregiver

and teacher rating scales are available for children aged 1.5 to 5 years (Achenbach &

Rescorla, 2000) and 6 to 18 years (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). A self-report scale is also

available for adolescents aged 11 to 18 years. More recently, multicultural norms were also

developed by the author (Achenbach, 2007). The checklist uses a 3-point Likert rating scale

of not true (as far as you know), somewhat/sometimes true, and very/often true.

The parent and teacher rating scales have 118 items relating to specific behavioural

and emotional problems. The authors used a wide normative sample with children and

adolescents from different ethnic and socio-economic backgrounds, and from urban and

rural/remote areas. The CBCL contains syndrome and DSM-oriented subscales relating to

ADHD symptomatology: attention problems (syndrome) and attention/deficit hyperactivity

(DSM-oriented) subscales, with a T score above 70 considered to be within the clinical range.

The authors reported that the teacher version of this scale (TRF) had strong test-test

reliability but significant variation in internal consistency (fair to excellent) and very weak to

moderate inter-rater reliability across parent and teacher ratings. When focusing on the

attention problems and attention/deficit hyperactivity subscales, they have excellent internal

consistency (α = .95 and .94 respectively) and strong test-retest reliability (r = .95 for both

71
subscales). It also has strong convergent validity with corresponding subscales on the CTRS-

R (see Table 4.3; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). No studies to date have examined the

specificity and sensitivity of the TRF for clinical diagnoses, so its diagnostic utility for

ADHD is not known. The only independent study examining the psychometric properties of

the TRF reported strong test-retest and inter-rater reliability in a sample of typically

developing primary school students (Toplak, Bucciarelli, Jain, & Tannock, 2009).

The parent version of this scale (CBCL) is reported to have fair to excellent internal

consistency and strong test-test reliability. When focusing on the attention problems and

attention/deficit hyperactivity subscales, they have good internal consistency (α = .86 and .84

respectively), strong test-retest reliability (r = .92 and .93 respectively) and moderate to

strong convergent validity with corresponding subscales of the CPRS-R (see Table 4.3;

Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). One study reported that the sensitivity and specificity of

diagnosing ADHD improved significantly when using cut-off T scores of between 52 and 60

in both community and clinical samples (Hudziak, et al., 2004). Given the authors of this

measure would consider these T scores to be within the normal range (Achenbach &

Rescorla, 2001), it has been suggested that the CBCL may underdiagnose children meeting

the criteria for ADHD (Hudziak, et al., 2004). These findings suggested that while the CBCL

may be useful for ruling out a diagnosis of ADHD in children and adolescents, its diagnostic

utility for identification may be limited when using the author’s clinical cutpoints. No other

independent studies have examined the psychometric properties of this version of the CBCL.

4.3.3 Children’s Behaviour Questionnaire (CBQ)

The Children’s Behaviour Questionnaire, initially developed by Mary Rothbart

(1981), has 195 items and is completed by the parent/primary caregiver. Initially designed to

describe the temperaments of children aged 3 to 7 years, a version has been developed for

children aged 7 to 10 years (the Temperament in Middle Children Questionnaire) and a self-

72
report questionnaire for children and adolescents aged 9 to 15 years (Early Adolescent

Temperament Questionnaire). Each item is rated on a 7-point scale from extremely untrue to

extremely true. Short (94 items) and very short (36 items) forms have also been developed

(Putnam & Rothbart, 2006).

Although the CBQ was not designed to specifically measure ADHD symptomatology,

three of the subscales (attentional focusing, impulsivity and inhibitory control) can provide

information to measure attentional difficulties. The instrument has not yet, however, been

used among populations of children with ADHD.

The CBQ is reported to have unacceptable to excellent internal consistency (see Table

4.3; Rothbart, et al., 2001). The three subscales measuring attention difficulties all had fair

internal consistency (α = .67 – .78) a finding which was replicated in an independent study of

toddlers and young children (α = .70 – .72; Kochanska, et al., 1994). The authors also

reported considerable variation in the inter-rater reliability across several samples of parents,

ranging from weak for attentional focusing (r = .39 – .41) to moderate/strong for impulsivity

(r = .53 – .72) and inhibitory control (r = .40 - .72; Rothbart, et al., 2001). Only one

independent study has examined the psychometric properties, which reported variable

internal consistency in mother ratings of young children (Kochanska, et al., 1994).

4.3.4 Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)

The SDQ screens for behavioural difficulties and assesses the impact these

behaviours have on the child’s life (Goodman, 1997). It contains 25 items: 10 regarded as

strengths, 14 regarded as difficulties, and one neutral item. Each item is rated on a 3-point

Likert scale of not true, somewhat true and certainly true. Although there are separate forms

for parents and teachers across two different age groups (4 to 10 and 11 to 17 years), all

contain almost identically worded items. A self-report version is also available for

adolescents aged 11 to 17 years. The questionnaires are freely available for download from a

73
dedicated web site (Goodman, 2004) and normative data from six countries is also available

(Goodman, 2007). It is also available in over 30 languages (Goodman & Scott, 1999) making

it highly accessible for cross-cultural research. ADHD symptomatology is measured in the

hyperactivity subscale which contains three items relating to hyperactivity/impulsivity and

two items relating to inattention.

The author reported that the SDQ has moderate to strong convergent validity with

corresponding subscales of the CBCL and the Rutter Scales, The inter-rater reliability across

parents and teachers is variable across subscales (see Table 4.3; Goodman, 2007; Goodman

& Scott, 1999), but on the hyperactivity subscale it was moderate (r = .54). No other

psychometric analyses were conducted by the author. An independent study reported strong

test-retest reliability over 12 months (Hawes & Dadds, 2004) but other reports of the

psychometric properties have been less encouraging. Several studies reported that the SDQ

had unacceptable to fair internal consistency in typically developing children (Bourdon, et al.,

2005) and children with ADHD (Becker, et al., 2006). Weak to moderate convergent validity

has also been reported with corresponding subscales on the ADHD-RS-IV (Becker, et al.,

2006).

A strength of the SDQ is its inclusion of items rating the strengths of the child as well

as their limitations. While some authors have deemed that the inclusion of positive items

lengthens a rating scale unnecessarily (Aman, Singh, Stewart, & Field, 1985), this argument

does not hold for this particular scale given the SDQ has just 25 items.

4.3.5 Limitations of measures

A core limitation of both the SDQ (Goodman, 1997) and CBQ (Rothbart, et al., 2001)

are their weak psychometric properties. Similar properties have been reported in the limited

number of independent studies using these measures (e.g., Becker, et al., 2006; Bourdon, et

74
al., 2005). The CBQ has not been used in populations of children with ADHD, and therefore

its validity as a measure for this clinical group is not known.

A limitation of the BASC-2 (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004), CDQ (Rothbart, et al.,

2001) and SDQ (Rothbart, et al., 2001) relates to the forms completed by parents and

teachers. The CBQ only has a parent/caregiver form, and therefore it cannot provide

information or comparisons about the child’s behaviour across different environments. The

BASC-2 and SDQ, while offering separate forms, contain almost identical items, suggesting

that ADHD symptoms do not present differently across settings despite research suggesting

otherwise (Barkley, 2006c; Wolraich et al., 2004). Further, the authors of the BASC-2 did not

report inter-rater reliability across parents and teachers, which do little to support their choice

of developing similar rating scale items across scales.

Another limitation of the SDQ (Goodman, 1997) is that each subscale contains five

items, and therefore its ability to measure hyperactive and inattentive behaviours is restricted.

Compared with other measures, Goodman (1997) stated the SDQ provided a “better coverage

of inattention” (p. 581) but with only two items, it is unclear how he drew this conclusion.

Given the SDQ is intended for use as a screening tool, this clearly poses limitations on its

ability to screen for ADHD symptoms.

4.3.6 Use with children and adolescents with ID

The authors of the BASC-2 included children with mental retardation /developmental

delay as a clinical group (2.2%, n = 142) in their standardisation study and subscale norms

were calculated (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004). The manual does not state, however, how

this group was defined or how individuals were selected for inclusion in this group. No

independent studies to date have examined the psychometric properties of the BASC-2 in

children and adolescents with ID.

75
The psychometric properties of the CBQ and SDQ have been reported in several

studies of children and adolescents with Down Syndrome (Nygaard, et al., 2002) and

idiopathic ID (Emerson, 2005; Kaptein, Jansen, Vogels, & Reijneveld, 2008). The internal

consistency of the SDQ hyperactivity subscale was fair (α = .73) while the three subscales

measuring attention difficulties in the CBQ ranged from unacceptable to fair (α = .49 – .76).

The inter-rater reliability of the SDQ across parents and teachers was weak to moderate and

therefore low for clinical purposes. While none of these properties were dissimilar from those

reported for typically developing samples (e.g., Becker, et al., 2006; Kochanska, et al., 1994),

it would suggest that findings from these scales needed to be interpreted with caution when

used with children and adolescents with ID, and may not be an accurate reflection of their

behaviour profile.

Only two studies have examined the psychometric properties of the most recent

version of the CBCL, with both studies being conducted on children with ASD (Pandolfi,

Magyar, & Dill, 2009, 2012). The findings of the CBCL 6-18 reported fair to excellent

internal consistency (α = .76 – .94). Similar to Achenbach and Rescorla (2001), the attention

problems subscale had good internal consistency at .83. The internal consistency was

unacceptable on four of the six factors of the CBCL 1.5-5 including the attention problems

subscale (α = .68). Confirmatory factor analyses on both versions of the CBCL supported the

existing factor structure suggested by the authors (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000) and the

scale’s acceptability for use among children with ASD. Pandolfi and colleagues (2009, 2012)

recommended the CBCL be used in conjunction with other scales when examining comorbid

psychopathology.

When evaluating the suitability of the CBCL for children with ID, reviews have again

been restricted to earlier versions (Koskentausta, Iivanainen, & Almqvist, 2004; Pueschel,

Bernier, & Pezzullo, 1991; Turk, 1998). It is noteworthy, however, that qualitative feedback

76
based on the earlier edition remains relevant to the item set in the more recent release.

Koskentausta and colleagues (2004), for example, believed that the item Acts too young

referred to the child’s intellectual disability rather than being symptomatic of a behaviour

disorder. In another study, parents and teachers found some items irrelevant to the child being

rated, and omitted one or more items when completing it (Turk, 1998). Unfortunately the

author of this study did not provide any further detail of the items deemed irrelevant.

4.4 Measures assessing a range of emotional functioning and behaviour disorders

developed for children with intellectual or developmental disabilities

4.4.1 Aberrant Behaviour Checklist – Community (ABC-C)

The Aberrant Behaviour Checklist has 58 items and was originally developed to

measure treatment efficacy in adults with moderate to profound ID in residential settings

(ABC - R; Aman, et al., 1985). Since then it has been modified to measure challenging

behaviours in children (Marshburn & Aman, 1992). In 1994, the ABC was revised to reduce

language suggestive of an institutional environment thus creating two checklists: one for

residential settings (ABC-R) and one for the community (ABC-C; Aman & Singh, 1994).

The items are completed by a person well-known to the child (e.g., parent, teacher, carer)

with each item ranked on a 4-point scale from not at all a problem to the problem is severe in

degree. The ABC-C contains one subscale relating to ADHD symptomatology named

hyperactivity/noncompliance with normative data generated for children and adolescents in

the study conducted by Marshburn and Aman (1992).

While the ABC-C was developed to be a valid tool in measuring challenging

behaviours in individuals with ID, the original study drew on “medical judgement” to

estimate degrees of ID. By omitting to use findings from standardised instruments to classify

degree of ID in study participants, the author’s conclusions about behavioural differences

based on severity of ID could be called into question. Earlier studies also excluded

77
individuals with an estimated mild degree of ID (Aman, Richmond, Stewart, Bell, & Kissel,

1987; Aman, et al., 1985) although subsequent studies have since supported the reliability

and validity of this instrument across all degrees of ID (E. C. Brown, Aman, & Havercamp,

2002; Marshburn & Aman, 1992).

The authors of the ABC-C have reported the psychometric properties of this scale in

several studies. Good to excellent internal consistency was reported in studies of children and

adolescents using parent (see Table 4.4; E. C. Brown, et al., 2002; Kaat, et al., 2013) and

teacher ratings (Marshburn & Aman, 1992). The internal consistency of the

hyperactivity/noncompliance subscale was good (α = .89) for parent ratings of a sample of

toddlers and young children, although it ranged from unacceptable to excellent for the other

subscales (Karabekiroglu & Aman, 2009). Significant variation in convergent validity was

reported with corresponding subscales of the CBCL 1.5-5, although there was moderate to

strong correlation between the ABC-C hyperactivity/noncompliance subscale and the

internalising and externalising indices of the CBCL (r = .42 – .77; Karabekiroglu & Aman,

2009).

Several other studies have examined the subscale structure of the ABC-C among

children and adolescents with ASD (Brinkley et al., 2007; Kaat, et al., 2013; Karabekiroglu &

Aman, 2009), and a variety of other developmental disabilities and clinical disorders

(Karabekiroglu & Aman, 2009). The findings from these studies generally indicated a good

fit with the ABC-C subscales described by Marshburn and Aman (1992).

4.4.1 Developmental Behaviour Checklist (DBC)

The DBC is a 96-item checklist designed to assess a broad range of behavioural and

emotional problems in children and adolescents with ID. Two versions of the checklist have

been developed, with one completed by parents or caregivers (DBC-P) and the other by

teachers or teacher’s aides (DBC-T; Einfeld & Tonge, 2002). The DBC-P was derived from

78
Table 4.4

Psychometric Properties of Measures Assessing a Range of Behavioural and Emotional Problems for Use with Children with Intellectual

Disability

Scale Psychometric properties reported by authors Independent studies with children with ID

Aberrant Behaviour Checklist – Brown et al. (2002) Kaat, Lecavalier & Aman (2013)
Community (ABC-C) Parent ratings only Parent ratings of children with autistic disorder and high
Good to excellent internal consistency functioning autism
(α = .84 – .95) Good to excellent internal consistency (α = .85 – .94)
Strong concurrent validity between ABC hyperactivity
Marshburn & Aman (1992) subscale and CBCL attention problems subscale (r = .56)
Teacher ratings only. Included children with borderline
intellectual functioning (IQ = 70-80) Karabekiroglu & Aman (2009)
Excellent internal consistency (α = .90 – .96) Parent ratings of toddlers
Unacceptable to excellent internal consistency (α = .68 –
.90)
Moderate to strong concurrent validity between ABC
hyperactivity subscale and CBCL 1.5-5 internalising and
externalising indices (r = .42 – .77)

Miller, Fee & Netterville (2004)


Teacher and teacher assistant ratings in children and
adolescents with ID
Fair to excellent internal consistency
(α = .76 – .94)
Strong test-retest reliability over 2 weeks for teachers (r
= .68 – .85) and teaching assistants (r = .74 – 1.00)
Strong inter-rater reliability across teachers and teaching
assistants (r = .72 – .80)

79
Miller, Fee & Jones (2004)
Strong concurrent validity with corresponding subscales
of the ACTeRS (r = -.51 – .52), CBCL - Teacher Report
Form (r = .67), Conners Teacher Rating Scale (r = .63)
and SNAP-III (r = .81)
Weak concurrent validity between ABC hyperactivity
subscale and classroom observation for off-task
behaviour for teachers (r = .31) and teaching assistants (r
= -.07)

Paclawskyj et al. (1997)


Staff ratings of children, adolescents and adults with
severe to profound ID
Fair to excellent internal consistency
(α = .79 – .94)

Developmental Behaviour Checklist - Einfeld & Tonge (2002) Hastings et al. (2001)
Parent/caregiver scale (DBC-P) Unacceptable to excellent internal consistency Children and adolescents with mild to profound ID
(α = .66 – .91) Unacceptable to excellent internal consistency
Strong inter-rater reliability between parents (ICC = .80) (α = .66 – .91)9
and nurses (ICC = .83) of adolescents in residential
settings Dekker, et al. (2002)
Weak inter-rater reliability across parents and teachers Children with mild to profound ID as well as children
(ICC = .30) with borderline intelligence
Strong test-retest reliability over 2 weeks Unacceptable to excellent internal consistency
(ICC = .83) (α = .66 – .91)
Strong concurrent validity between parent ratings on Strong test-retest reliability over 17 days
DBC and psychiatrist/psychologist ratings on three (r = .76 – .89)
scales of behavioural/emotional disturbance (r = .81) Moderate to strong inter-rater reliability between
Strong convergent validity with the maladaptive mothers and fathers (r = .52 – .67)
behaviour subscale of the Adaptive Behavior Scale Moderate to strong convergent validity on
School edition (r = .86) and the problem behaviour disruptive/antisocial, anxiety and social relating
subscale on the Scales of Independent Behaviors (r = subscales compared with corresponding subscales on the

9
Parent and teacher rating scales were analysed together

80
.72) CBCL (r = .47 – .85)
Excellent sensitivity (.83) and specificity (.85) in
distinguishing children with severe psychopathology
from “non-cases”

Developmental Behaviour Checklist - Einfeld & Tonge (2002) Dekker et al. (2002)
Teacher scale (DBC-T) Unacceptable to excellent internal consistency Children with mild to profound ID as well as children
(α = .62 – .91) with borderline intelligence
Moderate inter-rater reliability between teachers and Unacceptable to excellent internal consistency
teacher’s aides (ICC = .60) (α = .67 – .91)
Strong test-retest reliability over 2 weeks Strong test-retest reliability over 19 days
(ICC = .73) (r = .69 – .91)
Strong concurrent validity between teacher ratings on Weak to moderate inter-rater reliability between parents
DBC and psychiatrist/psychologist ratings on three and teachers (r = .27 – .57)
scales of behavioural/emotional disturbance (r = .66) Moderate to strong convergent validity on
disruptive/antisocial, anxiety and social relating
subscales compared with corresponding subscales on the
CBCL (r = .43 – .87)

Nisonger Child Behaviour Rating Aman et al. (1996) None to date


Form (NCBRF) – Parent form Fair to excellent internal consistency
(α = .77 – .93)
Weak to moderate inter-rater reliability across parents
and teachers (r = .37 – .54)
Moderate to strong convergent validity with
corresponding subscales of the Aberrant Behaviour
Checklist (r = .49 – .80)

Lecavalier, Aman, Hammer, Stoica & Mathews (2004)


Children and adolescents with autism
Fair to excellent internal consistency (α = .71 – .92)
Construct validity of subscales supported in children
with autism

Lecavalier, Leone & Wiltz (2006)

81
Children and adolescents with autism
Weak to moderate inter-rater reliability across parents
and teachers (ICC = .16 – .57)

Rojahn et al. (2010)


Children and adolescents with mild to profound ID
Fair to excellent internal consistency
(α = .72 – .92)
Weak inter-rater reliability across teachers and parents
(ICC = .01 - .25)

Norris & Lecavalier (2011)


Children and adolescents with borderline functioning to
profound ID
Fair to excellent internal consistency
(α = .77 – .94)
Confirmatory factor analysis yielded a fair model fit
(RMSEA = .08) for a five-factor solution of the problem
behaviour subscales
Weak to very strong convergent validity with
corresponding subscales of the DBC – P (r = .37 –
.85)

The psychometric properties have also been examined in


French (Tasse, Morin & Girouard, 2000 in Tasse &
Lecavalier, 2000) and Romanian (Mircea, Rojahn, &
Esbensen, 2010) language adaptations

Nisonger Child Behaviour Rating Aman et al. (1996) None to date


Form (NCBRF) – Teacher form Good to excellent internal consistency
(α = .81 – .91)
Moderate to strong convergent validity with
corresponding subscales of the Aberrant Behaviour
Checklist (r = .55 – .85)

82
Lecavalier et al. (2004)
Children and adolescents with autism
Fair to excellent internal consistency for problem
behaviours (α = .77 – .92)
Construct validity of subscales supported in children
with autism

Rojahn et al. (2010)


Children and adolescents with mild to profound ID
Poor fit between factor analysis findings and factor
structure of the NCBRF
Strong inter-rater reliability across teachers
(ICC = .66 – .85)
Strong test-retest reliability for teachers
(ICC = .66 – .92)

The psychometric properties have also been examined in


a Romanian language adaptation (Mircea, et al., 2010)

83
an examination of the behavioural and emotional symptoms described in a large sample

(more than 7000) of case notes of children and adolescents with ID. The DBC-T contains

almost identical items, excluding the three items relating to sleep disturbance, and including

an additional item relating to popularity with peers. Each item is ranked on a 3-point Likert

scale from not true as far as you know to very true or often true. Although not specifically

developed to measure ADHD symptomatology, the DBC contains a subscale to measure

hyperactivity which was derived by pooling six items from the checklist that had face validity

for these behaviours. The authors conceptualised hyperactivity as a spectrum rather than a

categorical disorder (Hay, et al., 2007), and therefore did not provide a clinical cut-off score.

Instead, they suggested that higher scores related to greater severity of ADHD symptoms

(Einfeld & Tonge, 2002).

The authors have done an extensive examination of the psychometric properties of the

DBC-P. The internal consistency is variable, being good to excellent on the self-absorbed and

disruptive/antisocial subscales (α = .89 – .91), unacceptable on the anxiety subscale (α = .66)

and fair on the remaining two subscales (see Table 4.4; Einfeld & Tonge, 2002). Inter-rater

reliability is varied, being strong across parents and nurses of adolescents in residential

settings but weaker across parents and teachers. Other psychometric properties of the DBC-P

are strong, including test-retest reliability, concurrent validity and excellent sensitivity and

specificity in distinguishing children with severe psychopathology from “non-cases”. Two

independent studies reported similar psychometric properties in terms of internal consistency,

test-retest and inter-rater reliability (Dekker, et al., 2002; Hastings, et al., 2001).

The psychometric properties of the hyperactivity subscale were assessed with a group

of children aged 4 to 13 years (n = 57; Einfeld & Tonge, 2002). Using the DBC-P, the

subscale was found to have excellent internal consistency and convergent validity with the

hyperactivity subscale on the CPRS-R. The authors also reported that the DBC-P had

84
significant discriminant ability in detecting the presence or absence of hyperactivity using the

DSM-III-R criteria.

Similar to the DBC-P, the DBC-T has variable internal consistency, being excellent

on the self-absorbed and disruptive/antisocial subscales (α = .90 – .91), unacceptable on the

anxiety subscale (α = .62) and fair on the remaining two subscales. It has strong test-test

reliability, moderate inter-rater reliability across teacher and teacher’s aide ratings, and strong

concurrent validity with psychologist/psychiatrist ratings on three scales of

behavioural/emotional disturbance (see Table 2.4; Einfeld & Tonge, 2002). An independent

study has reported similar psychometric properties relating to the internal consistency and

test-retest reliability, although their sample included children with borderline intelligence

(Dekker, et al., 2002). The reliability and validity of the hyperactivity subscale on the DBC-T

have not yet been examined.

The reported advantages of the DBC include ease of administration and its ability to

assess a broad range of emotional and behavioural problems in children and adolescents with

ID (Dekker, et al., 2002; Hastings, et al., 2001). The robust psychometric properties have

been confirmed by independent studies using both the parent and teacher versions (Dekker, et

al., 2002; Hastings, et al., 2001). The face validity of the behaviours in the DBC are also

high, given they were drawn from case notes of children with ID presenting at clinics.

4.4.2 Nisonger Child Behaviour Rating Form (NCBRF)

The NCBRF was derived from an existing child psychopathology rating scale,

the Child Behavior Rating Form (Edelbrock, 1985), and was developed to assess behavioural

and emotional problems in children and adolescents with ID (Aman, et al., 1996; Tasse,

Aman, Hammer, & Rojahn, 1996). For the problem behaviour items, each is ranked on a 4-

point Likert scale from not a problem to a severe problem. It contains six subscales, with the

hyperactive subscale measuring ADHD symptomatology. The NCBRF has teacher/teacher’s

85
aide and parent versions (although the authors stipulate that it could also be completed by a

caregiver or mature sibling; Tasse, et al., 1996). The items and wording are identical on both

forms, although factor analyses extracted different items within the subscales across the two

versions (Aman, et al., 1996). The NCBRF forms are freely available to download (Research

Unit on Pediatric Psychopharmacology, 2010), although the authors have stipulated that only

qualified professionals should use and administer these forms.

The internal consistency of the teacher version is good to excellent among children

with ID (see Table 4.4; Aman, et al., 1996) and fair to excellent among children with ASD

(Lecavalier, et al., 2004). Moderate to strong convergent validity with corresponding

subscales of the ABC-C (Aman, et al., 1996), strong inter-rater reliability across teachers

(Rojahn, et al., 2010), and strong test-retest reliability (Rojahn, et al., 2010) were also

reported. No independent studies have examined the psychometric properties of the NCBRF

teacher form.

The parent version of this scale has fair to excellent internal consistency among

children with ID (Aman, et al., 1996; Rojahn, et al., 2010) or ASD (Lecavalier, et al., 2004).

Weak to moderate inter-rater reliability has been reported across parents and teachers (Aman,

et al., 1996; Lecavalier, et al., 2006; Rojahn, et al., 2010), but moderate to strong convergent

validity with corresponding subscales of the ABC-C (Aman, et al., 1996). No independent

studies have examined the psychometric properties of the NCBRF parent form.

One of the reported advantages of the NCBRF is its inclusion of positively-worded

items (Aman, et al., 1996; Hastings, et al., 2001), which others have suggested may improve

the response rates from parents and teachers when reporting on a child’s behaviours

(Goodman, 1997).

86
4.4.3 Limitations of these measures

A limitation common to all of these scales is the use of a rating system that places an

emphasis on deficits in attention. For example, symptom severity may be rated on a scale

from not at all to very often or always. The use of such rating systems may result in a skewed

representation of attention (Hay, et al., 2007), with all individuals who do not have ADHD

being assigned low or zero scores. This system implies that ADHD occurs as a categorisation

rather than recognising that it may exist on a continuum (Hay, et al., 2007; Levy, Hay,

McStephen, Wood, & Waldman, 1997; Waschbusch & Sparkes, 2003), with some individuals

performing better than average in their ability to pay attention or inhibit activity. While this

limitation has been addressed in the development of the SWAN (Swanson, et al., 2005), this

scale has been used in relatively few studies to date, none of which included children with ID.

Despite a favorable review of the ABC-C (Aman & Singh, 1994) by Miller and

colleagues (2004), their conclusions are limited by their lack of generalisability to the wider

population due to the small sample size (n = 48), exclusion of children with severe or

profound ID, lack of generalisability to the wider population due to the high proportion

(85.4%) of African Americans in their sample, and comparing the ABC-C with older versions

of ADHD rating scales that have since been revised. Another weakness of the ABC-C is that

it has not been revised since its development over 20 years ago. Our understanding of

inattention and hyperactivity has changed greatly in this time (Cornish & Wilding, 2010).

These developments suggest that this scale may be somewhat outdated in its behavioural

descriptors of ADHD symptoms.

The score on the hyperactivity/noncompliance subscale of the ABC-C (Aman &

Singh, 1994) may also be difficult to interpret at face value given the composition of the

items within it. The subscale contains items relating to hyperactivity, impulsivity,

noncompliance and inattention. It would be therefore possible that a child may be rated

87
highly only on the items relating to noncompliance and inattention, and yet without

examining the items individually one might draw the conclusion that the child had difficulties

with hyperactivity (based on the subscale name) even though few or none of those items were

endorsed. Even if examined at an item level, this subscale contains only two items relating to

inattention (Pays no attention when spoken to and Does not pay attention to instructions),

thus limiting both its ability to identify behaviours relating to inattentive symptoms and its

use as a screening tool.

The DBC (Einfeld & Tonge, 1995, 2002) was not specifically designed to assess

ADHD symptomatology but does contain a subset of six items that have face validity for

hyperactive and inattentive behaviours within the existing item set. This subset of items

forms part of a broader disruptive/antisocial subscale which describe behaviours that can be

disruptive to self or others. Although the hyperactivity subscale has demonstrated internal

consistency, further psychometric analyses have been limited to the parent version of the

rating scale (Einfeld & Tonge, 2002). Given the majority of children in the sample examining

the hyperactivity subscale had mild ID (86%), its validity for measuring hyperactivity in

children with moderate, severe, or profound ID is not clear at the present time. The small

number of hyperactive and inattentive behaviours within this scale, while being useful

markers, are not sufficiently broad enough to understand the range of behaviours related to

ADHD symptomatology that may exist across children with ID.

4.5 Summary

Rating scales are often used to assist with screening for and diagnosing ADHD

(Barkley & Edwards, 2006), and for identifying ADHD symptomatology in clinical research.

The utility of existing rating scales, however, reveal significant shortcomings when used

among populations with ID. These limitations can be seen both when considering scales

designed for typically developing children and those for children with ID.

88
When examining the rating scales designed for typically developing children, the

psychometric properties of four of the measures have not been examined in children or

adolescents with ID, namely the ADD-H Comprehensive Teacher Rating Scale - Second

edition (Ullman, et al., 2000); the ADHD Rating Scale IV (DuPaul, et al., 1998); the

Swanson, Nolan & Pelham Checklist – Fourth edition (Swanson, 1992); and the Strengths

and Weaknesses of ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behaviour Scale (SWAN; Swanson, et al.,

2005). The psychometric properties of the Behaviour Assessment System for Children –

Second edition (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004) have not been replicated in independent

studies. Studies using the Children’s Behaviour Questionnaire (Rothbart, 1981) and the

Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, 1997) have examined the psychometric

properties in children with ID, although the findings from both measures concluded that the

internal consistency was unacceptable to fair.

The Conners Rating Scales – Revised (Conners, 1997) have frequently been used in

studies of children with ID even though their validity for use within this population is

questionable (Guerin, et al., 2009). Two studies have reported limitations with its

psychometric properties, including its inter-rater reliability across parents and teachers (Deb,

et al., 2008; M. L. Miller, Fee, & Netterville, 2004). It was also noted that 13 items (46.4%)

on the teacher version were dependent upon the child being verbal, thus invalidating the

measure for a significant proportion of children with severe and profound intellectual

disabilities who are non-verbal (Deb, et al., 2008).

The Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) has also been

used in a number of studies of children with ID. An examination of the psychometric

properties is limited to two studies which reported the internal consistency for children with

autism was highly variable (Pandolfi, et al., 2009, 2012) and no further analyses of

psychometrics were conducted. Further examination of the psychometric properties would be

89
needed to establish whether the CBCL has reliability and validity within this population.

Several authors have questioned the validity of some of the rating scale items, citing that they

were developmentally inappropriate (Koskentausta, et al., 2004) or irrelevant (Turk, 1998) to

the child being rated, particularly for children with moderate, severe or profound ID.

Shortcomings also exist that are specific to those rating scales developed for children

with ID. A lack of independent studies has validated the psychometric properties of the

Nisonger Child Behaviour Rating Form (NCBRF; Aman, et al., 1996) and the Attention

Checklist (AC; Das, 1986 in Das & Melnyk, 1989). The NCBRF is restricted to only

measuring hyperactive behaviours and the AC only inattentive behaviours. The Attention-

Distraction, Inhibition-Excitation Classroom Assessment Scale (Evans, 1975 in Evans &

Hogg, 1984) lacks age- and gender-based normative data and a parent version of the form,

and independent studies report variable psychometric properties (Guerin, et al., 2009; Strand,

et al., 1990). It has not been validated for use in children with mild ID. Limitations of the

Aberrant Behaviour Checklist – Community (Aman & Singh, 1994) include the lack of

updated versions since its development 20 years ago, the difficult interpretability of the

hyperactivity subscale given items relating to noncompliance also loaded on this factor, and

the limited number of items relating to inattention. The items making up the hyperactivity

subscale of the Developmental Behaviour Checklist (Einfeld & Tonge, 2002) contain items

relating to both hyperactivity and inattention, although it is likely to be limited in its ability to

provide a complete picture of ADHD symptomatology given this subscale contains just six

items. Its ability to assess hyperactive symptoms in children with moderate to profound ID, or

older adolescents, is also yet to be evaluated.

90
CHAPTER 5 THE FOCUS OF THE PRESENT STUDY

91
The review of rating scales in the previous chapter suggests that most of the measures

are used infrequently among children and adolescents with ID, and few of the scales

developed for typically developing children demonstrated sound psychometric properties

when they are used with these populations. In the case of the Conners Third edition (Conners,

2008), the review drew upon results of studies using the previous edition (Conners, 1997)

which may not reflect their reliability or validity in the current version. The decrease in the

number of studies utilising these scales may also reflect the realisation that they are less

appropriate for use with children and adolescents who have ID, or the slow but growing

availability of clinically reliable and valid scales developed specifically for this population.

5.1 Aims of the present study

The need to develop a rating scale to measure attention and hyperactivity among

children with ID has been recognised (Deb, et al., 2008) but not yet adequately addressed.

This is particularly important given that the prevalence of ADHD is at least as common, if

not more so, in children with ID as it is among children who are typically developing (Neece,

et al., 2011). The administration of rating scales is useful when conducting a clinical

evaluation of ADHD in an individual (Barkley & Edwards, 2006), and is supported by a

recent study indicating that they are utilised by two-thirds of clinicians when using formal

criteria to make this diagnosis (Chan, et al., 2005).

Cross-syndrome studies have suggested that differences in inattention and

hyperactivity exist across known causes of ID (Cornish, Steele, et al., 2012; Hastings, et al.,

2005; Papaeliou, et al., 2012). In the absence of any alternatives, these studies have had to

utilise rating scales included in the review in Chapter 4 above, all of which have

shortcomings in their ability to measure ADHD symptomatology within this population. The

inclusion of a measure that has been validated for children with ID would assist researchers

92
in gaining a greater understanding of these differences. Therefore, it is clear that a reliable

and valid rating scale for children within this population is needed.

The aims of the present study are:

• to develop a reliable and valid rating scale that is more sensitive to exploring the range

and severity of ADHD symptoms in school-aged children with intellectual disability

• that the scores on the new rating scale will have good convergent validity with existing

measures of ADHD

• to describe and compare the profiles of ADHD symptoms in children with known causes

of ID

5.2 Hypotheses

It is hypothesised that:

1. there will be a positive relationship between hyperactivity/impulsivity and level of

intellectual disability.

2. there will be lower levels of inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive behaviours in the

Down Syndrome group.

3. there will be higher levels of hyperactive/impulsive behaviours in the ASD group.

93
CHAPTER 6 METHOD

94
6.1 Phase 1a: Item Development for Rating Scale

The guidelines as defined by DeVellis (2003) were used as a framework to inform all

phases of rating scale development. In the first phase, a variety of methods were drawn upon

to determine the behaviours related to attention difficulties in children and adolescents with

intellectual disability (ID) that were salient to teachers. In the first stage, all items from the

rating scales reviewed previously that were related to attention difficulties were listed in a

table under the headings of hyperactivity, impulsivity, inattention and working memory.

Behaviours were also recorded from observational and descriptive data provided in published

studies examining attention difficulties among children with ID.

These items and behaviours were categorised onto concept maps under these four

headings, with subcategories reflecting the criteria described in the DSM-IV-TR (American

Psychiatric Association, 2000), ICD-10 (World Health Organization, 1992), DC-LD (Royal

College of Psychiatrists, 2001) and DM-ID (P. Lee & Friedlander, 2007). The use of concept

maps allowed for consideration of many possible behaviours representing these four areas.

Two members of the research team experienced in the field of attention difficulties

among young people with known causes of ID (Professor Kim Cornish and Associate

Professor Kylie Gray), along with the author, evaluated the concept maps to determine

whether they reflected behaviours that would be observed in young people with an

intellectual disability. Many behaviours were discarded as they were not considered

representative of the behaviours of children with ID (e.g., Puts off projects until the last

minute). Some behaviours were modified to better reflect the abilities expected of a young

person with ID (e.g., Crosses the road independently was modified to Stops and waits when

they get to the road to take into account those children with greater degrees of ID for whom

asking them to independently cross the road would not be considered possible and/or

appropriate).

95
Those behaviours that were retained were converted into lay language that would be

readily understood by teachers and teacher’s aides. Examples followed some items to help

define or illustrate the behaviour they were being asked to rate. All items were worded

positively as it has been proposed that this may help to improve response rates of parents and

teachers when reporting on children's behaviours (Goodman, 1997). Prior to distributing the

questionnaire to participants in the study, the T-SAID was subjected to the Flesch-Kincaid

test (Bond & Fox, 2007; Pallant & Tennant, 2007). This test uses two formulae to calculate

reading ease and grade level based on sentence and word length and was calculated using

Flesh 2.0 software (Frink, 2007). The findings indicated that the rating scale has a readability

index of 56 and a reading grade level of 8.54, making it appropriate for distribution among

teachers.

Following these procedures, the list of attention difficulties encompassing the four

headings listed above were organised as a rating scale. The response set consisted of a 4-point

scale where each item is scored from never to often. Once the rating scale items and response

set had been developed, the rating scale was presented to groups of health professionals and

teachers via focus group discussions as described in Phase 1b below.

6.2 Phase 1b: Focus Group Discussions

6.2.1 Participants

Nine health professionals consisting of 6 psychologists (all females; mean years of

work experience = 10.4 years, range = 2.5 – 25 years) and 3 paediatricians (2 males and 1

female; mean years of work experience = 27.3 years, range = 15 – 37 years), and 9 teachers

(2 males and 7 females; mean years of teaching experience = 11.2 years, range = 6 months –

20 years) were recruited for the focus group discussions. Teachers and health professionals

were employed in the metropolitan area of Melbourne, Australia.

96
6.2.2 Procedure

The process of selecting participants for the focus groups was through two stages. For

the school teachers, approval to recruit participants for the focus group was first sought from

the school principals of their respective schools via a letter or phone call informing them

about the study and the questions to be used.

Subject recruitment at schools that consented to participate was through

announcements made at staff meetings by the school principal. The teachers were told that

the study was investigating the attention profiles of children with different causes of ID, and

that they would be asked to comment on items written that would potentially be included in a

new rating scale developed by the research team, the Scale of Attention in Intellectual

Disability (SAID). All teachers were invited to take an explanatory statement providing

details of the study and a consent form. Interested teachers returned the consent form by

reply-paid envelope. The letter informed teachers that they could withdraw from the study at

any time.

Health professionals were individuals known to the research team and were

approached individually or in small groups either via a face-to-face conversation or telephone

call. They were given the same information about the focus group discussion as the teacher

groups (outlined above).

Each focus group met for approximately 90 minutes in a quiet room at a convenient

location for participants (e.g., school staff room, meeting room at a workplace). The author

led each group, serving as the group facilitator. The participants were reminded that the

discussion would be recorded on Minidisk and that they could leave the focus group at any

time. Light refreshments were provided at each focus group discussion, but no other tangible

incentives were given for participation.

97
The focus group discussions began with introductions from the facilitator and the

participants. The facilitator proceeded with an overview of the study and the purpose of the

focus group discussion. The focus group discussion commenced with an activity that had the

participants divide into groups to promote engagement and facilitate communication between

members (see Appendix A for the procedure used). This activity used the freelist technique

(Borgatti, 1999) where each group was given a sheet of paper and were instructed to think

about the children they had seen in their practice or classroom who had ID and attention

difficulties. They were asked to list the behaviours they had observed in these children,

including difficulties with hyperactivity, impulsivity, inattention and working memory.

During this exercise, the facilitator observed the participants and monitored their progress but

did not participate in the discussions. After 10 minutes, the facilitator asked the groups to

share their ideas, which were listed on a whiteboard or poster paper. The facilitator then led a

brief discussion about the similarities and differences between the lists.

Following this activity, the facilitator proceeded with the focus group questions. Each

participant was given a draft copy of the SAID rating scale and asked to read the items

silently. Following this, the participants were invited to comment on the rating scale items.

They were also given some guiding issues to consider such as the clarity and expression of

the wording, whether they understood the behaviour they would be asked to rate for each

item, the usefulness of each item in the scale, and whether there was any redundancy in the

items. The participants were also asked if they felt there were any behaviours missing on the

rating scale that warranted inclusion, which included a consideration of the behaviours listed

in the brainstorming exercise.

After the discussion, a brief summary of the issues that had been raised was given by

the facilitator, and the participants were given the opportunity to ask any questions or to

98
discuss the experience of being in the focus group. Participants were thanked by the

facilitator for participating in the discussion.

6.2.3 Analysis

The focus group discussions were transcribed from the Minidisk into word processed

documents. These discussions were subjected to thematic analysis, using the method

described by Braun and Clarke (2006). The behaviours relating to attention difficulties from

the freelist activity were coded using a theory-driven (a priori) approach, in that only

particular behaviours discussed were analysed into codes. Codes are defined as a unit of

information extracted from a focus group discussion, and refer to “the most basic segment, or

element, of raw data or information that can be assessed in a meaningful way regarding the

phenomenon” (Boyatzis, 1998, pg. 63). Discussions that diverted away from the central

theme were excluded from the analysis.

Codes from the freelist were then organised into potential themes using mind maps.

These themes were then subjected to review and refinement. The homogeneity of themes was

assessed and resulted in some themes being collapsed, and others were broken down further

into separate themes. The importance of each code was also calculated across focus group

interviews. This was determined by the proportion of individuals to whom the code was

applied, rather than the absolute number of times a theme is expressed and coded (which

could be expressed many times by one participant emphasising his/her perceived importance

of this theme, but not at all by other participants; Guest, Bunce, & Johnson, 2006). Some

codes were discarded from the thematic analysis but were considered of interest and therefore

retained to be discussed separately.

Comments made about the rating scale were coded separately using a theory-driven

approach, and related to the central issues that participants had been asked to consider.

Namely, these issues pertained to: the response set used, clarity and expression of the

99
wording, whether they understood the behaviour described in each item, the usefulness of

each item in the scale, redundancy in the items, and any behaviours missing on the rating

scale that warranted inclusion. Coding for the rating scale review was organised into themes

consistent with these issues.

6.3 Phase 2: Community Survey

6.3.1 Participants

A total of 215 consent forms were returned by families of children attending a special,

special developmental, autism specialist, or mainstream school in the Melbourne

metropolitan area and across regional Victoria. A small number of children were on a split

placement (spending part of their school week in a mainstream school and the rest of their

time in a special school) or were located in a support centre for children with ID on a

mainstream school site. A flowchart illustrating recruitment in the study is illustrated in

Figure 1.

Children were eligible to participate in the study if: (i) they were aged between 5 and

13 years; and (ii) their most recent cognitive assessment placed their functioning in the

intellectually disabled range (i.e., their cognitive and/or adaptive living skills assessment total

score was less than 70). Children with a diagnosis of ASD were only included in the sample

if they scored above the recommended cutoff for autism (i.e., 15 or more) on the lifetime

version of the Social Communication Questionnaire (Rutter, Bailey, & Lord, 2003). A total

of 176 students were eligible to be included in the analyses (114 males and 62 females).

Socioeconomic status was determined via parental completion of the Hollingshead

(Hollingshead, 1975) Four Factor Index. This measure has been found to yield comparable

information to more recently developed SES measures (Cirino et al., 2002) but has the

advantages of being simple to complete and less time-consuming. The scale gives a rating for

each parent based on the highest level of education completed and their current occupation.

100
Consent form returned (n = 215)

Family contacted to obtain Excluded as family could not


background information (n = 209) be contacted (n = 6)

Child had IQ assessment in last 2 Excluded as FSIQ > 70 (n = 3)


years, or child was assessed by or assessed and profile
research team, and FSIQ < 70 suggested severe language
(n = 204) disorder rather than ID (n = 2)

Rating scale booklet mailed out to Family withdrew from study


teacher (n = 203) due to personal circumstances
(n = 1)

Rating scale booklet returned Excluded as SCQ < 15 (n =


(n = 195) 10) or SCQ not returned so
eligibility could not be
ascertained (n = 9)

Final sample entered into analyses


(n = 176)

Figure 1. Recruitment flowchart for the study.

Scores are averaged across ratings for each parent and an overall score is calculated. For

those families where there was only one parent or caregiver, only the ratings for that person

were used. For those families where one parent was unemployed or was a full-time carer, the

ratings were averaged for level of education but only the rating for the employed parent was

used for current occupation. Hollingshead SES scores range from 8 to 66, with a higher score

indicating a higher level of socioeconomic status.

101
6.3.1.1 Test-retest sample

The test-retest reliability of the new rating scale was assessed by asking a random

sample of classroom teachers to complete a second rating scale. The mean interval between

the first and second rating scales being completed was 28 days (SD = 10.41). Teachers who

took longer than 50 days to return the second rating scale (n = 12) were excluded from the

analyses.

6.3.2 Measures

6.3.2.1 Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence – Third

edition/Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth edition (WPPSI-III/WISC-IV;

Wechsler, 2002; Wechsler, 2003). The WPPSI-III and WISC-IV are standardised cognitive

assessments which measure the thinking and reasoning skills of children and adolescents. The

WPPSI-III is used with children aged 2 years 6 months to 7 years 3 months, and the WISC-

IV is used with children and adolescents aged 6 years 0 months to 16 years 11 months. The

WPPSI-III yields three index scores (verbal, performance and processing speed) as well as a

full scale score. The WISC-IV yields four index scores (verbal comprehension, perceptual

reasoning, working memory and processing speed) as well as a full scale score. The WPPSI-

III and WISC-IV index and full scale scores have moderate to excellent internal consistency

(α = .85 – .95) and the WISC-IV has very strong test-retest reliability over a mean interval of

27 days (r = .80 - .95)10. The WPPSI-III and WISC-IV were used in this study to determine

each child’s current cognitive functioning.

6.3.2.2 Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales, Second edition – Teacher Rating

Form (VABS-II; Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Balla, 2005). The VABS-II is a standardised

instrument that measures the adaptive living skills of children and adolescents. It consists of 9

subscales categorised into three composite scores: communication, socialisation and daily

10
Test-retest reliability not reported for the WPPSI-III.

102
living skills. In young children (5-6 years), there is also a fourth composite score (motor

skills) comprising fine and gross motor skill subscales. The scores are added together to yield

the adaptive behaviour composite. The VABS-II has low to excellent internal consistency for

children aged 5 to 18 years (α = .74 – .98). Test-retest reliability was measured over an

average period of 3 weeks and was found to be moderate to very strong (r = .43 – .97). The

inter-rater reliability (across two teachers or a teacher and a teacher’s aide) had significant

variation, from unacceptably low to strong (r = .04 – .79). The VABS-II-T was used in this

study to determine the current adaptive functioning of children whose cognitive abilities

could not be measured with the WPPSI-III or WISC-IV.

6.3.2.3 Conners Rating Scales – Third edition, Teacher Short form (Conners

3; Conners, 2008). The Conners 3 consists of 39 items and provides a means of screening for

symptoms of ADHD and related disorders. It has five subscales: inattention,

hyperactivity/impulsivity, learning problems/executive functioning, aggression and peer

relations. It has moderate to excellent internal consistency across subscales (α = .87 – .94),

strong test-retest reliability over two to four weeks (r = .70 – .81), and strong inter-rater

reliability across teachers (r = .72 – .83). The subscales of the Conners 3 have very strong

correlations with ratings on the full-length version for both general population and clinical

samples (r = .93 – .98), suggesting that scores on the short form are a sufficient proxy for

those obtained on the long form (Conners, 2008). The Conners 3 was completed by the

child’s current classroom teacher to determine convergent validity with the author’s new

attention scale.

6.3.2.4 Developmental Behaviour Checklist – Second edition, Teacher version

(DBC-T; Einfeld & Tonge, 2002). The DBC-T is a 93-item checklist designed to assess a

broad range of behavioural and emotional problems in children and adolescents with an ID.

The DBC-T has low to excellent internal consistency across the five subscales (α = .62 – .91),

103
strong test-retest reliability over 2 weeks (ICC = .73) and moderate inter-rater reliability

across teachers and teacher’s aides (ICC = .60). The DBC-T was completed by the child’s

current classroom teacher to determine convergent validity with the author’s new attention

scale.

6.3.2.5 Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; Rutter, Bailey, et al.,

2003). The SCQ, previously known as the Autism Screening Questionnaire (ASQ; Berument,

Rutter, Lord, Pickles, & Bailey, 1999), is a 40-item questionnaire completed by the

parent/primary caregiver that examines the areas of communication, socialisation and

restricted and repetitive behaviours and interests. It is used as a screener for autism spectrum

disorder and is based on the Autism Diagnostic Interview (ADI; le Couteur et al., 1989), an

earlier version of the Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised (ADI - R; Rutter, le Couteur, &

Lord, 2003). The authors reported moderate to excellent internal consistency for the SCQ

total score (α = .84 – .93) and strong convergent validity with the ADI - R (r = .71). The SCQ

has the ability to differentiate between individuals with pervasive developmental disorders

(including autism) and those without, with moderate sensitivity (.85) and specificity (.75).

The lifetime version of the SCQ was completed by families of children with a diagnosis of

autism and was used to determine eligibility to participate in the study.

6.3.2.6 Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability (SAID). This rating scale

consists of 46 items and was developed for the purpose of this study. It incorporates four

areas of attention difficulty: hyperactivity, impulsivity, inattention and aspects of working

memory. The teacher version (T-SAID) was examined for validation in this study (see

Appendix B) and was completed by the child’s current teacher (provided they have known

the child for a minimum of 6 months). A parent version (P-SAID) is under development.

Participants respond to each statement on a 4-point scale (never, rarely, sometimes, or often).

All items are worded positively as it has been proposed that this may help to improve

104
response rates when reporting on children's behaviours (Goodman, 1997). Higher scores

relate to fewer difficulties. The T-SAID was completed by the child’s teacher to determine

attention difficulties in the classroom.

6.3.3 Procedure

Participant selection was through a three-stage process. In the first stage, children

were recruited from several sources. Following ethics clearance from the Department of

Education and Early Childhood Development (DEECD) and the Catholic Education Office

Melbourne (CEOM), principals at special schools (n = 22), special developmental schools (n

= 8), and autism specialist schools (n = 2) were invited to assist with recruitment for the study

(see Appendix C). Nineteen schools agreed to assist in subject recruitment resulting in a

participation rate of 59.4%. Support groups and community organisations specific to the

diagnostic groups of interest were also approached (e.g., Autism Victoria, Williams

Syndrome Support Group of Victoria), asking them to advertise the study in their newsletter,

on their Internet forum, on their web site.

The majority of schools (n = 16; 84.2%) who agreed to assist with recruitment

consented to the research team sending home an envelope containing a poster, explanatory

statement and consent form (see Appendices D and E) to all eligible students at their school

aged between 5 and 12 years, either in the student’s diary, communication book or through

the mail. One school (5.3%) allowed the team to only send home information to children in

selected year levels. Two schools (10.5%) consented to a notice about the study being put in

the newsletter for two consecutive weeks. These families contacted the research team directly

either by phone or email, and information was mailed out to them as described above. The

explanatory statement described the study in detail and encouraged families to contact the

research team if they had any questions. Families were informed them that they or their child

could withdraw from the study within 6 weeks of the assessment phase of the study, as per

105
the ethical guidelines of Monash University. Families who consented to participate in the

study returned the consent form to the research team in a reply-paid envelope.

Support groups and community organisations who agreed to assist with advertising

the study displayed posters, included an article in their newsletter, and/or posted the

information on an Internet forum or web site. Interested families contacted the research team

directly by phone or email, and information was mailed out as described above. This

information was also available on a web site for families to download.

Two organisations (Down Syndrome Victoria and the Association of Genetic Support

of Australasia) allowed the research team to mail out the information to all member families

with a child aged between 5 and 12 years, following approval from their respective

Committees. Families who consented to participate in the study returned the consent form to

the research team in a reply-paid envelope.

In the second stage, a member of the research team telephoned each consenting family

to determine their child’s eligibility to participate in the study. Basic demographic (parent’s

occupation and highest level of schooling) and information about their child’s school (i.e.,

current school attended and classroom teacher) was obtained, as well as clinical information

including their child’s primary diagnosis, comorbid diagnoses, and any medication they were

currently prescribed. They were asked whether their child had received a cognitive and/or

adaptive living skills assessment in the past and, if this had taken place, were asked for the

date and results of these assessments (if available).

Children across all groups were administered a cognitive assessment, the WPPSI-III

or WISC-IV, depending on their chronological age. If the child had been administered a

cognitive assessment in the last 18 months (WPPSI-III) or 2 years (WISC-IV) then the

previous test results were used and another assessment was not administered. Assessments

were conducted either in a quiet room at the child’s school during school hours or at the

106
Centre for Developmental Psychiatry and Psychology, Monash Medical Centre. If a child was

deemed untestable using the WPPSI-III or WISC-IV, then the VABS-II total Adaptive

Behaviour Composite score (ABC) was used to determine the severity of impairment.

The families of all children who received an assessment were given a written report

and verbal feedback by the main investigator (a registered psychologist) explaining the

results. If the family consented, a copy of the report was also provided to the school. The

child needed a FSIQ of 70 or below to be eligible to participate in the study. Children who

received a FSIQ above 70 were excluded from the study.

In the third stage, the classroom teacher of each eligible child was mailed a booklet of

questionnaires explaining that the research team had informed consent to approach them and

explaining the nature of the study. Although teachers were encouraged to participate, they

were under no obligation to do so and were told that they could withdraw from the study at

any time. For families who consented to participate in the study via a community group or

support organisation, the principal of the school their child attended was contacted informing

them about the study and requesting permission to mail out a booklet to the child’s classroom

teacher. All principals (n = 36) contacted gave consent to assist with the study. It was a

requirement that each teacher who completed the questionnaires had known the child for a

minimum of 6 months. They were asked to complete the Conners 3 (Conners, 2008), the

DBC-T (Einfeld & Tonge, 2002) and the T-SAID.

The questionnaires were completed in the teacher’s own time and took approximately

30 minutes to complete. Questionnaires were returned to the research team in a reply-paid

envelope. Reminder letters or emails were sent to teachers if questionnaires had not been

returned within 4 weeks, and a second reminder was sent if they had not been returned within

6 weeks.

107
Parents of children with ASD were asked to complete the SCQ (Rutter, Bailey, et al.,

2003). This questionnaire was mailed to families to complete in their own time and was

returned via a reply-paid envelope. This questionnaire took approximately 10 minutes for

parents to complete. A reminder phone call was made to families if the SCQ had not been

returned within 4 weeks, and a second SCQ was mailed out if it had not been returned within

6 weeks.

6.3.4 Analysis

At the time of its development, the Hollingshead Four Factor Index (Hollingshead,

1975) did not account for single parents who were unemployed or families where both

parents were unemployed. Given that a significant number of families in this study fell into

these categories, the authors recoded the occupational status so that the range from 1 to 9

(service worker to senior manager) was altered and ranged from 1 to 10 (unemployed to

senior manager). Therefore, the total score could range from 8 to 71.

The grouping for level of ID was determined by using the child’s FSIQ from the

WPPSI-III or WISC-IV, or the ABC from the VABS-II for children who were untestable on

the cognitive assessment. Level of ID was defined using the criteria in Sattler (2001): mild ID

(55 – 70), moderate ID (40 – 54), and severe/profound (< 40). Eight children for whom

severity of ID could not be determined were excluded from the regression analyses.

As the response set of the T-SAID offered little distinction between the rarely and

never ratings, it was decided to collapse these two ratings into a single category. Scores on

the T-SAID were then reversed for analysis so that higher scores were indicative of greater

difficulties.

The total score was calculated by taking the mean of all the items (known as the Mean

Item Score, or MIS). This method has a number of advantages over calculating the sum of all

item scores (Taffe, Tonge, Gray, & Einfeld, 2008). One advantage is that the MIS may be

108
deconstructed to measure the breadth of behaviours an individual exhibits (the Proportion of

Items Checked, or PIC) and the intensity at which the items are checked for that person (the

Intensity Index, or II). PIC is the proportion of recoded items receiving codes of 1 or 2,

indicating that the corresponding items indicated problematic behaviours. The II is the

proportion of items scored 2 among the 1 or 2 coded items.

Regression analyses were conducted to examine the T-SAID total score across degree

of ID, controlling for age, gender, and SES, with idiopathic ID as the comparison group.

6.3.4.1 Reliability

Internal consistency of the T-SAID was assessed using Cronbach’s alpha (Cronbach,

1951). This assesses the degree to which each item on the T-SAID measures the same

construct based upon all possible correlations between two sets of items within the scale. The

range of the statistic is from 0 to 1. The accepted minimal standard to claim internal

consistency is .70 when evaluating the psychometrics of an instrument. The total score for the

T-SAID from all participants were used to compute Cronbach’s alpha.

Test-retest reliability was measured using inter-class correlations. This assesses the

extent to which a scale can reproduce the same score for the same individual at different

times.

6.3.4.2 Validity

Convergent validity measured the relationship between the T-SAID and other scales

thought to measure the same construct. In this study, convergent validity was assessed using

Pearson’s correlation coefficient across the following measures: the hyperactivity/impulsivity

and inattention subscale scores from the Conners 3 (Conners, 2008), the hyperactivity

subscale score from the DBC-T (Einfeld & Tonge, 2002), and the T-SAID total score.

Divergent validity measured the relationship between the T-SAID and other scales

thought to measure a different construct. In this study, divergent validity was assessed using

109
Pearson’s correlation coefficient between the adaptive behaviour composite on the VABS-II-

T (Sparrow, et al., 2005) and the total score on the T-SAID.

A number of factor analytic solutions were considered when examining the T-SAID

data. Oblique rotation was chosen given the assumption that there was a correlation across

factors (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). Given the sample size in this study, loadings at or above

.50 were selected for inclusion of an item in interpreting each factor (Hair, Anderson,

Tatham, & Black, 1998).

110
CHAPTER 7 RESULTS FROM PHASE 1: QUESTIONNAIRE
DEVELOPMENT

111
The focus groups were analysed in two parts: the freelist activity and the review of the

rating scale. A total of 52 behaviours related to attention difficulties experienced by children

and adolescents with ID were extracted from the transcript. Comments from the participants

relating to the composition of the rating scale items were also recorded.

7.1 Behaviours describing attention difficulties in children with ID

7.1.1 Talking

The majority of teachers who raised the issue of talking mainly focused on the speed

of speech: “On some days he talks so fast I can’t even understand him and I’ll just think

‘Boy, he’s got a bad case of the yaps today’” (F3, P7). Other teachers also spoke about

students with attention difficulties having poor topic maintenance and talking louder than

other students.

7.1.2 Sitting still

A number of teachers raised the issue of children with attention difficulties being

unable to sit still. They described the challenge and ongoing process of helping them to be

able to sit still, or to even stay in their seat:

At the start of the year he couldn’t even sit still on a chair. He would bang

the table...but we’ve got him to a point now where he can sit still in a chair for 30

seconds. He can sit longer than that, but for 30 seconds he can sit still (F3, P2).

Others said that a child’s inability to sit still serving a functional purpose as an

avoidance strategy: “like [he says] he needs paper or a pencil sharpened...he’ll just say he

needs something so as not to sit down and work” (F3, P6), while others said it depended on

the type of activity they were being asked to do:

112
Like they can be on the move all the time when you’re asking them to do some

arithmetic or a writing task but when you give them a hands on activity they are much

more engaged and focused (F3, P2).

7.1.3 Attention/concentration

Behaviours relating to inattention were discussed less frequently. When the issue was

raised, however, children with attention difficulties were reported to find it significantly more

difficult to maintain concentration during classroom activities:

He’ll have his head down as if he’s reading, but he does this head movement and his

eyes...I know that he’s not working, he’s looking at what others are doing. He needs

1:1 supervision to get anything done (F3, P2).

Some teachers and psychologists, however, elaborated on this observation and said that it

depended on the task a child was being asked to do, and that they had a greater capacity to

focus on tasks that were more practical and hands on, as opposed to academic tasks. Some

psychologists also discussed the difficulties that some children experienced in returning to a

task after they have been distracted, and that shifting attention back to a task can be as

challenging as getting a student to initiate the task in the first place.

7.1.4 Impulsive

A number of teachers within the focus groups spoke about the impulsivity of

children, and several related it specifically to the issue of their desire for instant gratification:

113
Kids are getting on the computers so much at home, they’ve become like ‘clickerati’

and kids just want things at the click of a button like what they get when they’re on

the computers (F3, P7).

Others agreed that they had observed this behaviour, but questioned whether this was specific

to children with ID: “I don’t think that’s specific to ID though. That’s any kid with ADHD”

(F3, P8).

Some psychologists discussed the variety of behaviours that could relate to taking

things that belong to other children. One psychologist suggested that the age of the child

needed to be taken into account when considering this behaviour: “A lot of kids in the junior

years of primary school...grab things without asking. I don’t think this is specific to ID” (F2,

P4). Another psychologist suggested that taking other people’s belongings was too specific,

as they engage in other, related behaviours as well:

Often kids don’t take, but they do muck around with....they touch, they play

with....They might not be taking things but they’re mucking up other people’s stuff.

Touching it, or pushing it over, or smudging it...[they’re] interfering. Getting in the

way (F4, P1).

7.1.5 Executive functioning

Several teachers in the focus groups talked about the difficulties of children with

ID organising their materials in class, or bringing things to and from school. There was some

difference of opinion regarding whether these behaviours were specific to ADHD, or whether

they reflected many children with ID more generally: “When you’re talking about planning

114
and organisation...these are really big issues for these kids. They need step by step; it has to

be broken down” (F3, P1).

Teachers also discussed how many children with attention difficulties have limited

working memory. Specifically, they detailed that these children have significant difficulties

with remembering instructions, or that they can only recall the first or last instruction given.

This was also observed out in the yard when asking these children about an incident:

...they can only remember the last event...like they’ll be teasing another kid,

teasing them and teasing them, but if that kid lashes out and hits them, then all they

can remember is “Such and such hit me”. Never mind that they were teasing the other

kid for half an hour. It’s like nothing happened before that. Nothing happened before

they got hit (F3, P9).

7.2 Other issues of interest raised

7.2.1 Specificity of behaviours

Teachers within the focus groups sometimes had difficulty distinguishing behaviours

that were specific to children with ID and attention difficulties, and those that were more

consistent presentations of children with ID generally:

F3, P1: I think you need [some items] on fine and gross motor skills as well. Cos

these kids have trouble in those areas.

F3, P7: [Name], that’s all kids with ID.

F3, P1: No, but I think these kids have even more difficulties. Cos their attention

difficulties make it more difficult for them to learn, and stuff like fine motor

skills...it takes them even longer.

115
7.2.2 Medication

Some teachers discussed the impact that medication could have on behaviour. They

made references to the positive impact that medication could have, such as the student

“seem[ing] like a different child” (F3, P5) or being able to understand the child’s speech

more easily when taking medication.

Other teachers discussed their beliefs about medication, such as its efficacy in the

long-term, dosage and medication compliance. Compliance related directly to parental

decision about ceasing medication; the issue of student refusal to take the medication was not

raised.

7.3 Rating scale evaluation

Comments or concerns that arose from any rating scale item were taken into

consideration, which resulted in some changes being made to the rating scale. In total, 58.5%

of the rating scale items were modified in some way. Twenty-three items were reworded,

generally to add clarity to the behaviour being rated. Eight items were deleted to eliminate

unnecessary duplication. Three of these eight items relating to a child’s ability to concentrate

and were deleted for several reasons. Firstly, these items had asked about the child’s ability to

concentrate over two different periods of time: “The rating scale should be able to capture

whether a child exhibits this behaviour...two different time limits do not add additional

information.” (F5, P2). Secondly, it was reported that the items might cause confusion as they

asked whether the child could concentrate independently or under supervision when no other

items made this distinction. One psychologist also felt that these items would raise a scoring

issue when summing the ratings:

116
Would items 14 and 15, which talk about 5 minutes, somehow get a higher or better

score than 12 and 13 which only talk about 1 minute? Do you see what I mean? It’s

sort of like a scale within a scale... I don’t quite know how you [would] deal with that

(F4, P1).

Concerns were raised in focus group discussions for a further three items but the

decision was made to retain the original wording. These related to small concerns such as

using a different example, or a rewording suggestion: “Avoids making a new mistake when

corrected once... I don’t know if this item is worded positively. The item is fine but maybe

you could reword it.” (F2, P2). Nineteen items had no concerns raised in any of the focus

groups, and also remained unchanged.

Concerns regarding the validity of behaviour as a symptom of attention difficulties

were raised for a single item. Item 2 on the rating scale, which asks whether the student

speaks at a reasonable volume, was not considered valid by one health professional: “I don’t

think volume of speech is necessarily a hyperactivity symptom” (F1 P1). This professional

noted that talking loudly may be distracting to other students, and may present a challenge to

teachers in terms of classroom management, but believed that this behaviour was not specific

to, or characteristic of, children with attention difficulties. The decision was made to retain

this item, however, due to the number of teachers who referred to this behaviour in their

freelist observations, and as only one professional raised this issue.

While several suggestions were made by focus group participants about possible

items that could be added to the scale, the majority of these suggestions were outside the

scope of the current scale being developed. These included suggestions for items relating to

117
tics, food fads, sleeping patterns11, social skills and academic achievement. One item was

added to the rating scale based on a suggestion made by a health professional and related to

shifting attention.

The majority of participants felt that the response set selected by the authors was

appropriate. One participant, however, felt that the wording of the response set needed to be

reviewed:

I’m not so much quibbling with the 5 points but I’ve got real trouble with that word

’always’. I mean, how many kids are going to walk from room to room when indoors

always?...Keeps legs and feet still while performing a classroom task or when

listening to a story. I don’t think they make a child who always does those things...I

just don’t think it’s happened in the life of the planet (F4, P1).

The author felt that this participant made a valid point about the response set and its

relationship with the rating scale items. Based on this observation it was decided to change

the response set from a 5-point to a 4-point scale, retaining the initial 4 responses (never,

rarely, sometimes, often) and removing the ‘always’ response option.

The general consensus from the focus groups were that the rating scale was easy to

understand and complete, and was not too time consuming. They also commented favourably

on the use of positive wording in the items, with teachers in particular appreciating this

feature. They reported that in psychological assessments and evaluations (particularly for

funding applications) they generally have to focus on the difficulties of the child. Several

teachers commented that this rating scale represented a significant, positive shift from the

11
Items on sleep were developed for the parent scale, but the authors believed that teachers would not be
appropriate informants for this behaviour. This is consistent with other rating scales that have been developed
for teachers and parents, such as the CBCL (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) and DBC (Einfeld & Tonge, 2002),
which also include items on sleep only in the parent scale.

118
deficit model: “I like the way the items are written in a positive way. It makes you feel less

judgemental” (F3 P7).

Following the scale revisions, the T-SAID was once again subjected to the Flesch-

Kincaid test using Flesh 2.0 software (Frink, 2007). The findings indicated a slight

improvement in readability with an index of 61 and a reading grade level of 8.16, confirming

that it was appropriate for distribution among teachers.

119
CHAPTER 8 DEVELOPMENT OF A NEW ATTENTION
RATING SCALE FOR CHILDREN WITH INTELLECTUAL
DISABILITIES: THE SCALE OF ATTENTION IN
INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY (SAID)

120
8.1 Declaration

In the case of Chapter 8, contributions to the work involved the following:

Name % contribution Nature of contribution


Nerelie Freeman 70% Formulation of project design, data
collection, data analysis, and writing
manuscript.
A/Prof Kylie Gray 12.5% Contribution to development of
project design, discussion of ideas
expressed in manuscript, and critical
review of manuscript.
Prof Kim Cornish 12.5% Contribution to development of
project design, discussion of ideas
expressed in manuscript, and critical
review of manuscript.
Dr John Taffe 5% Consultation in data analysis and
critical review of manuscript

Declaration by co-authors:

(1) they meet the criteria for authorship in that they have participated in the conception,
execution, or interpretation, of at least that part of the publication in their field of expertise;
(2) they take public responsibility for their part of the publication, except for the responsible
author who accepts overall responsibility for the publication;
(3) there are no other authors of the publication according to these criteria;
(4) potential conflicts of interest have been disclosed to (a) granting bodies, (b) the editor or
publisher of journals or other publications, and (c) the head of the responsible academic unit;
and
(5) the original data are stored at the following location(s) and will be held for at least five
years from the date indicated below:

Location: Centre for Developmental Psychiatry and Psychology, Department of


Psychiatry, Southern Clinical School, Monash University, Clayton Campus
Date

Signature 1

Signature 2

Signature 3

Signature 4

121
8.2 Paper commentary

Chapter 8 presents a paper that has been submitted for publication in the American

Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. This paper has been formatted to the

specific requirements of the journal. Pages have been re-numbered to provide consistency

throughout the thesis.

Paper 1 is a study examining the psychometric properties of a new teacher rating scale

developed to measure hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention in children with intellectual

disability (ID). Teachers of 176 children aged five to thirteen years with idiopathic ID, Down

Syndrome or autism spectrum disorder completed this rating scale. This study addresses an

important gap in the literature as there are currently no reliable or valid rating scales that

measure these behaviours among children with ID.

122
DEVELOPMENT OF A NEW ATTENTION RATING SCALE FOR CHILDREN

WITH INTELLECTUAL DISABILITIES: THE SCALE OF ATTENTION IN

INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY (SAID)

Abstract

Difficulties with attention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity are thought to be as common

among children with intellectual disability (ID) as they are in typically developing

populations. Despite this, there is a lack of assessment scales to specifically assess ADHD

symptomatology in children and adolescents with ID. This paper describes the development

and evaluation of a teacher completed measure; the Scale of Attention in Intellectual

Disability (T-SAID). A community survey of 176 teachers of children aged 5 to 13 years,

with mild-severe/profound ID, indicated that the T-SAID is a reliable and valid measure.

Integrating this scale with neuropsychological and clinical research holds exciting promise

for enhancing our understanding of the nature of attention difficulties within the ID

population.

Key words: intellectual disability, attention, rating scale, children

123
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a pervasive psychiatric condition

which affects approximately 11% of primary aged school children (Willcutt, 2012). ADHD is

characterised by a pattern of inattention, impulsivity and/or hyperactivity that impairs an

individual’s functioning across different environments, for example, at home and at school

(American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Inattentive behaviour, as observed in the

classroom, has an insidious impact on academic attainment (Spira & Fischel, 2005). For

example, inattentive behaviour in kindergarten children, but not hyperactive behaviour,

predicts poor reading outcomes in Grade 1 and also in Grade 5, independent of kindergarten

reading-related skills and concurrent levels of hyperactivity (Dally, 2006; Rabiner & Coie,

2000). This would suggest that if inattention is not treated in early years, deficits in academic

performance will become more pronounced with time. To date, the majority of published

research has focused on typically developing populations yet pervasive inattention,

impulsivity and hyperactivity characterise many children with developmental delay and

intellectual disability (Cornish & Wilding, 2010).

Historically, it was maintained that attention difficulties were part of the presentation

of intellectual disability (ID) (Antshel, et al., 2006; Guerin, et al., 2009) which may in part

have been due to diagnostic overshadowing bias. This term refers to the tendency of

clinicians, in the presence of ID, to regard accompanying mental health issues as less salient

and specific than they would if the child were typically developing (Jopp & Keys, 2001;

Mason & Scior, 2004). A recent and growing body of literature, however, suggests that

children with comorbid ID and ADHD form a distinct subgroup, and that some children who

have ID display few or no difficulties with attention (Hastings, et al., 2005). There is also

suggestion that inattention difficulties may vary depending upon factors such as the uneven

developmental trajectory in children with ID, and differences in presentation across

developmental disorders e.g., Fragile X Syndrome (Cornish, Turk, & Levitas, 2007; Turk,

124
2011), Down Syndrome (Cornish, et al., 2010; Cornish, Scerif, et al., 2007); Williams

Syndrome (Rhodes, et al., 2011; Rhodes, et al., 2010; Scerif, et al., 2004); and autism

(Ghaziuddin, et al., 2010; Witwer & Lecavalier, 2010).

Current Clinical Scales

Two diagnostic classificatory tools have been published to assist clinicians in making

diagnoses among individuals with ID: the Diagnostic Criteria for Psychiatric Disorders for

use with adults with Learning Disabilities/Mental Retardation (DC-LD; Royal College of

Psychiatrists, 2001) and the Diagnostic Manual – Intellectual Disability (DM-ID; P. Lee &

Friedlander, 2007). Both recognise the limitations of the ICD-10 (World Health Organization,

1992) and DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) in making diagnostic

decisions when a person has ID, and appreciate that psychiatric conditions may present

differently among adults within this group. These two tools contain differences in their

criteria for ADHD, namely whether all three behaviours (impulsivity, hyperactivity and

inattention) need to be present to make a diagnosis, and the exclusionary criteria used. For

example, the DC-LD allows comorbid diagnoses of autism and ADHD (consistent with the

DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) whereas the DM-ID does not (consistent

with DSM-IV-TR criteria, which the DM-ID was based upon). Despite these inconsistencies,

both publications represent a positive step in recognising differences in the clinical

presentation of people with ID.

While rating scales are often used to assist with making a diagnosis of ADHD in the

absence of ID (Barkley & Edwards, 2006), their usefulness in diagnosing ADHD symptoms

reveals significant shortcomings when used among populations with ID.

The Conners Rating Scales – Revised (Conners, 1997) have frequently been used in

studies of children with ID even though their validity for use within this population is

questionable (Guerin, et al., 2009). Two research groups have examined the psychometric

125
properties of the Conners Parent Rating Scales-Revised (CPRS-R) and the Conners Teacher

Rating Scales-Revised (CTRS-R) among children with either borderline intelligence or ID

ranging from mild to severe (Deb, et al., 2008; M. L. Miller, Fee, & Jones, 2004; M. L.

Miller, Fee, & Netterville, 2004). The findings suggested that the CPRS-R may be able to

indicate the presence of ADHD in children with borderline intelligence or ID, but the CTRS-

R was unable to do so (Deb, et al., 2008). Both studies reported limitations with its

psychometric properties, including its inter-rater reliability across parents and teachers (Deb,

et al., 2008; M. L. Miller, Fee, & Netterville, 2004). It was also noted that 13 items (46.4%)

on the CTRS-R were dependent upon the child being verbal, thus invalidating the measure

for a significant proportion of children with severe and profound intellectual disabilities who

are non-verbal (Deb, et al., 2008).

Shortcomings also exist in rating scales that have been developed for use within the

ID population. The Aberrant Behaviour Checklist – Community (ABC-C; Aman & Singh,

1994) measures a broad range of behavioural and emotional problems. In a review of seven

rating scales among children with ID, it was concluded that the ABC-C was the most reliable

and valid scale for measuring ADHD symptoms in children with ID (M. L. Miller, Fee, &

Jones, 2004; M. L. Miller, Fee, & Netterville, 2004). This was perhaps not surprising,

however, given that the ABC-C was the only measure included in the review that had been

developed for the ID population. Their conclusions are also limited by their lack of

generalisability to the wider population due to the small sample size, exclusion of children

with severe or profound ID, and the high proportion of African Americans in their sample.

A widely used rating scale that measures a broad range of behavioural and emotional

problems in children and adolescents with ID is the Developmental Behaviour Checklist

(DBC; Einfeld & Tonge, 1995, 2002). While the DBC was not specifically designed to assess

ADHD symptomatology, it does contain a subset of six items that have face validity for these

126
behaviours within the existing item set. Although this subset of items has demonstrated

internal consistency, further psychometric analyses have been limited to the parent version of

the rating scale and not the teacher version (Einfeld & Tonge, 2002).

Another, lesser-known scale is the Attention-Distraction, Inhibition-Excitation

Classroom Assessment Scale (ADIECAS; Evans, 1975, in Evans & Hogg, 1984) that

measures difficulties across two dimensions: inattention/excitation (IE) and

attentiveness/distractibility (AD). While it has been used in several independent studies

(Buckley, et al., 2008; Guerin, et al., 2009), it presents with a number of shortcomings.

Limited replication of the psychometric properties, inconsistent findings regarding the scale’s

factor structure (Evans & Hogg, 1984; Strand, et al., 1990; Turner, et al., 1991), lack of

commercial availability and no normative data may have contributed to its restricted use and

lack of uptake by researchers and clinicians.

Although the prevalence of ADHD symptomology is at least as common in children

with ID as without (Dekker & Koot, 2003; Feinstein & Reiss, 1996; Hastings, et al., 2005;

Lindblad, et al., 2011), the profile of attention difficulties across known causes of ID is only

starting to be identified and described. The need to develop a rating scale to measure attention

difficulties in these populations has been recognised (Deb, et al., 2008) but not yet adequately

addressed.

The limitations inherent in most commercially available attention scales fail to

capture subtle attention profiles in ID and yet attention difficulties represent core and

pervasive concerns in many children with ID. Therefore, the overarching aim of the present

study was to develop and evaluate the psychometric properties of a novel attention rating

scale to explore the range and severity of everyday inattention, hyperactivity, impulsivity,

and working memory difficulties in children with ID.

Method

127
Phase 1a: Development of the New Rating Scale

Behaviours related to attention difficulties in children with ID that are salient to

parents and teachers were identified through a review of the content of existing rating scales,

descriptive behaviours reported in published studies, and an examination of the diagnostic

criteria in the DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), ICD-10 (World Health

Organization, 1992), DC-LD (Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2001) and DM-ID (P. Lee &

Friedlander, 2007). These behaviours were categorised onto concept maps under four

headings of hyperactivity, impulsivity, inattention and working memory.

The research team evaluated the concept maps to determine the difficulties that would

be observed in young people with ID. Those behaviours retained were converted into

checklist items in lay language that would be readily understood by parents and teachers. All

items were worded positively as it has been proposed that this improves response rates when

reporting on children’s behaviours (Goodman, 1997).

Following these procedures, the list of attention difficulties encompassing the four

headings listed above were organised as a rating scale. The response set consisted of a 4-point

scale where each item is scored using ratings of never, rarely, sometimes and often, with

higher scores indicating fewer difficulties.

Phase 1b: Focus Group Discussions

The new rating scale, the Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability – Teacher

version (T-SAID), was presented to nine health professionals (three paediatricians and six

psychologists) and nine teachers from special schools via focus group discussions. An initial

activity using the freelist technique (Borgatti, 1999) required participants to think about the

children with ID they had seen in their practice or classroom and to list the behaviours they

had observed in these children that related to hyperactivity, impulsivity, inattention and

working memory. This enabled the research team to gather further behaviours for potential

128
inclusion in the rating scale that had not already been identified using the methods described

above. Participants were then asked to evaluate the items in the T-SAID, including the clarity

and expression of the wording, and any behaviours omitted that they believed warranted

inclusion.

Phase 2: Community Survey

Participants

Two hundred and fifteen consent forms were returned by families of children

attending schools in the Melbourne metropolitan area and across regional Victoria, Australia.

Children were eligible to participate in the study if: (1) they were aged between 5 and 13

years; and (2) their most recent cognitive assessment placed their functioning in the

intellectually disabled range (i.e., their cognitive and/or adaptive living skills assessment total

score was 70 or below).

Socioeconomic status (SES) was determined via parental completion of the

Hollingshead Four Factor Index (Hollingshead, 1975). This measure has been found to yield

comparable information to more recently developed SES measures (Cirino, et al., 2002) but

has the advantages of being simple to complete and less time-consuming. The scale gives a

rating for each parent based on the highest level of education completed and their current

occupation. Scores are averaged across ratings for both parents and an overall score is

calculated. A higher score indicates a higher level of socioeconomic status.

Measures

Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence – Third edition/Wechsler

Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth edition (WPPSI-III/WISC-IV; Wechsler, 2002;

Wechsler, 2003). The WPPSI-III and WISC-IV are established, reliable and valid measures

of intellectual ability with good to excellent internal consistency (α = .85 – .95) and strong

129
test-retest reliability (r = .80 – .95). They were used in this study to determine each child’s

current cognitive functioning.

Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales, Second edition – Teacher Rating Form

(VABS-II-T; Sparrow, et al., 2005). The VABS-II-T is a reliable and valid measure of

adaptive behaviour with acceptable to excellent internal consistency (α = .74 – .98) and

moderate to very strong test-retest reliability (r = .43 – .97). It was used in this study to

determine the current adaptive functioning of children whose intellectual ability could not be

measured with the WPPSI-III or WISC-IV.

Conners Rating Scales – Third edition, Teacher Short form (Conners 3; Conners,

2008). The Conners 3 has 39 items and screens for symptoms of ADHD and related

disorders. It has five subscales: inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, learning

problems/executive functioning, aggression and peer relations. It was developed for use with

typically developing children and has not been validated for children with ID (Conners,

2008). The Conners 3 was completed by each child’s current teacher to determine convergent

validity with the T-SAID.

Developmental Behaviour Checklist – Teacher version (DBC-T; Einfeld & Tonge,

2002). The DBC-T is a 93-item checklist designed to assess a broad range of behavioural and

emotional problems in children and adolescents with ID. It has five subscales:

disruptive/antisocial, communication disturbance, self-absorbed, social relating and anxiety.

A subset of items measure hyperactivity which was generated based on the face validity of

items that appeared consistent with hyperactive behaviour (Einfeld & Tonge, 2002). The

DBC-T was scored by calculating the mean of the six hyperactivity item ratings, with higher

scores indicating greater difficulties. It was completed by each child’s current teacher to

determine convergent validity with the T-SAID (M. L. Miller, Fee, & Netterville, 2004).

130
Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability (SAID). The proposed rating scale

developed in Phase 1 of the study consists of 46 items that taps hyperactivity/impulsivity,

inattention, and aspects of working memory. The teacher version (T-SAID) was completed

by the child’s current teacher (provided they had known the child for a minimum of 6

months). A parent version (P-SAID) is under development. Teachers responded to each

statement on a 4-point scale of never, rarely, sometimes and often, with lower scores on the

T-SAID relating to greater difficulties.

Procedure

Ethics approval was obtained from the Monash University Standing Committee on

Ethics in Research Involving Humans, the Victorian Department of Education and Early

Childhood Development, and the Catholic Education Office Melbourne, Australia.

School principals at special schools (n = 22), special developmental schools (n = 8),

and autism specialist schools (n = 2) were invited to assist with recruitment for the study. Of

the 32 schools, 19 agreed to assist in recruitment resulting in a school participation rate of

59.4%. Support groups and community organisations were also approached, asking them to

advertise the study on their web site and seeking permission to contact member families with

a child aged 5 –13 years. Families were sent home an envelope containing a poster,

explanatory statement and consent form. Those who consented to participate returned the

form in a reply-paid envelope. Principals of the schools attended by children recruited via a

support group or community organisation (n = 36) were also contacted, and all consented to

assist with the study. A member of the research team telephoned each consenting family to

determine their child’s eligibility to participate in the study, and to collect demographic and

clinical data.

If a child had been administered a cognitive and/or adaptive living skills assessment

in the past they were asked to provide these results to the research team. A cognitive

131
assessment was conducted if they had not been administered one in the last 18 months

(WPPSI-III) or two years (WISC-IV). If the child’s FSIQ could not be calculated, then the

VABS-II-T Adaptive Behaviour Composite (ABC) was used as a proxy for their IQ score.

Concurrent validity between the ABC and FSIQ has suggested a strong positive relationship

for children with severe and profound ID (r = .65; de Bildt, Kraijer, Sytema, & Minderaa,

2005).

The child’s classroom teacher was mailed a booklet of rating scales. It was a

requirement that each teacher who completed the questionnaires had known the child for a

minimum of six months. Teachers were asked to complete the DBC-T (Einfeld & Tonge,

2002), the Conners 3 (Conners, 2008) and the T-SAID. Questionnaires were returned to the

research team in a reply-paid envelope. Reminder letters or emails were sent to teachers if

questionnaires had not been returned within four weeks, and a second reminder was sent if

they had not been returned within six weeks.

Test-retest reliability was assessed by mailing a second copy of the T-SAID to a

random sample of classroom teachers 2 weeks after the first one was returned. The mean

interval between the first and second rating scales being completed was 28 days (SD =

10.41). Teachers who took longer than 50 days to return the second rating scale (n = 12) were

excluded from the analyses, leaving a total sample of 19 for analyses.

Analysis

As the response set offered little distinction between the rarely and never ratings, it

was decided to collapse these two ratings into a single category. Scores on the T-SAID were

then reversed for analysis so that higher scores were indicative of greater difficulties.

The total score was calculated by taking the mean of all the items (known as the Mean

Item Score, or MIS). This method has a number of advantages over calculating the sum of all

item scores (Taffe, et al., 2008). One advantage is that the MIS may be deconstructed to

132
measure the breadth of behaviours an individual exhibits (the Proportion of Items Checked,

or PIC) and the intensity at which the items are checked for that person (the Intensity Index,

or II). PIC is the proportion of recoded items receiving codes of 1 or 2, indicating that the

corresponding items indicated problematic behaviours. The II is the proportion of items

scored 2 among the 1 or 2 coded items.

Results

Phase 1: Development of the T-SAID Rating Scale

The focus groups were analysed in two parts: the freelist activity and the review of the

rating scale. A total of 52 behaviours related to attention difficulties experienced by children

with ID were extracted from the transcript. Five main themes were extracted: (1) talking

(speed; volume; amount; poor topic maintenance); (2) inability to sit still; (3) limited

attention span/concentration; (4) impulsivity; and (5) executive function difficulties (working

memory; organisation of materials).

A total of thirty-one items (58.5%) were modified following comments in the focus

group discussions. The reading ease and grade level of the T-SAID was analysed using the

Flesch-Kincaid test (Flesh 2.0 software; Frink, 2007). The T-SAID received a reading ease

score of 61 and a grade level score of 8.16, suggesting an appropriate level of readability.

Example items from the T-SAID are provided in Table 1.

[Insert Table 1 about here]

Phase 2: Community Survey

Study Sample

Of the 181 children deemed eligible to participate in the study, rating scales were

returned for 176; a return rate of 97.2%. The demographic characteristics of the sample are

133
described in Table 2. Almost half the sample (n = 76; 43.2%) had idiopathic ID, 33.5% (n =

59) autism spectrum disorder, and 23.3% (n = 41) Down Syndrome. Results from the

cognitive and adaptive living skills assessments, broken down by degree of ID, are described

in Table 3.

[Insert Table 2 about here]

[Insert Table 3 about here]

Comorbid Diagnoses

Across the sample, 31 parents (17.6%) reported their child as having at least one

comorbid diagnosis, the most common being ADHD (n = 20; 11.4%). Of the 20 children, 13

were currently taking medication e.g., Ritalin (65.0%), four had never been prescribed

medication (20.0%), and three had taken medication in the past but were not doing so at the

present time (15.0%). Other reported comorbid diagnoses included epilepsy (n = 18; 10.2%)

and anxiety (n = 6; 3.4%).

Reliability of the T-SAID

Item-total and inter-item correlations were calculated for all 46 items of the T-SAID.

Item-total correlations ranged from .16 – .78 and inter-item correlations ranged from .01 – 88.

Two items (item 38 Understands instructions presented non-verbally and item 39

Understands instructions better if they include non-verbal prompts) were found to have poor

item-total (.45 and .16 respectively) and inter-item correlations (.19 – .47 and .01 – .47). The

decision was made to remove these two items from the rating scale, leaving 44 items.

The Cronbach’s alpha for the T-SAID total score indicated it has excellent internal

consistency according to Cohen’s (1988) criteria, as shown in Table 4. The test-retest

reliability, assessed by intra-class correlations, was also strong.

134
Validity of the T-SAID

The content validity of the rating scale items was established via the method of item

derivation and use of focus group discussions to evaluate the scale as described above. The

general consensus from the focus group discussions suggested that the T-SAID was easy to

understand and complete, and was not too time consuming. Participants commented on the

usefulness of the examples that accompanied some of the items as it enhanced their

understanding of the behaviour they were being asked to rate. They also spoke favourably of

the use of positive wording in the items, with teachers in particular appreciating this feature.

Several teachers commented that this rating scale represented a significant, positive shift

from the deficit model: “I like the way the items are written in a positive way. It makes you

feel less judgemental”.

Convergent validity was measured by examining the raw scores for the hyperactivity

items on the DBC-T (n = 173) and the Conners 3 inattention and hyperactivity subscales. To

establish the appropriateness of using the Conners 3 as a measure to examine convergent

validity, the internal consistency of the hyperactivity and inattention subscales with the

current sample was calculated. It was found to be good to excellent (α = .88 – .90) and

therefore deemed to work reasonably well in an ID population. The T-SAID had strong

convergent validity with both the DBC-T and the Conners 3 suggesting that these different

scales measured the same construct, as shown in Table 4. The divergent validity of the T-

SAID was measured by comparing it with the total score on the VABS-II-T (n = 109). The T-

SAID had moderate divergent validity with the ABC, suggesting a lack of association

between these measures and that they measured different constructs. Higher adaptive

functioning was associated with fewer problems with attention, hyperactivity, impulsivity and

aspects of working memory.

135
Although it is beyond the scope of this study to evaluate the properties of the T-SAID

as a screening tool for ADHD, the discriminant validity was examined by comparing those

children with a comorbid ADHD diagnosis (n = 20) and those without (n = 156). A

significant difference in the total score was found across groups (t174 = 2.56, p < .01) with

children who had an ADHD diagnosis having significantly higher scores.

[Insert Table 4 about here]

ADHD Symptoms Across Degree of Intellectual Disability

Regression analyses were conducted to examine the T-SAID total score across degree

of ID, controlling for age, gender, and SES. As shown in Table 5, the mean item score (MIS)

on the T-SAID (possible range 0-2) was greater by .49 for those with severe or profound ID

than for those with mild ID, but only by .06 for those with moderate ID. A similar pattern

was evident in the regressions of the proportion of items checked (PIC) and intensity index

(II; both on a 0-1 scale), indicating that the two possible reasons for higher MIS (i.e., a

greater breadth of behaviours exhibited and these behaviours being noticeably more severe)

are both in evidence among those with severe or profound ID. On average, 25% (p < .001)

more items were marked sometimes or rarely/never for those with severe or profound ID than

for those with mild ID, and of these ‘checked’ items, 24% (p < .001) more were marked at

the more intense rarely/never level for those with severe or profound ID than for those with

mild ID. There was also a significant negative effect for age, with the MIS decreasing by .07

with every year of aging. This suggests that as children age their attention improves, with

fewer behaviours exhibited and these behaviours being less severe.

[Insert Table 5 about here]

136
Discussion

The findings of the present study suggest that the 44 item T-SAID is a reliable and

valid scale measuring attention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity in children with ID. The

advantages of this new measure over existing measures include: (1) no assumption of

academic competence in the scale items (e.g., items relating to literacy or numeracy) which is

appropriate given children with ID may not have the skills to do such tasks even with

assistance; (2) the use of positively worded items which are thought to improve response

rates when reporting on children’s behaviours (Goodman, 1997); and (3) the inclusion of

items related to aspects of working memory given its strong association with attention

(Scerif, 2010; Steele, et al., 2012). Previously, capturing attention profiles or ‘signatures’ in

children with ID was dependent either upon rating scales that were standardised on children

from non-ID populations but who had ADHD-like symptoms e.g., Conners Rating Scales, or

on more generalised rating scales of atypical behaviours but not especially focused on

inattentive, hyperactive or impulsive behaviours in children with ID.

Preliminary results suggest that the T-SAID may have the ability to discriminate

between children who have ADHD and those who do not, although its efficacy as a screening

tool is yet to be evaluated. It should be noted, however, that in this study the ADHD

diagnosis was determined via parent report only. As the majority of clinicians use rating

scales when considering a diagnosis of ADHD (Chan, et al., 2005), the availability of a

reliable and valid tool like the T-SAID may increase clinician confidence in making this

diagnosis when working with children who have ID. Its strong convergent validity with

existing rating scales i.e., the DBC-T and Conners 3, further suggests that the T-SAID

measures the intended constructs of hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention.

137
Regression analyses suggested that children with severe or profound ID had greater

difficulties with attention, hyperactivity and impulsivity, with a broader range of behaviours

and greater intensity being exhibited (accounting for 16.3 – 19.3% of the variance). This is

consistent with the findings of Rojahn and colleagues (2010), who reported that children and

adolescents with severe and profound ID had higher levels of hyperactivity. Given the

significant challenges already faced by educators of children with ID – particularly severe

and profound ID – in terms of maximising their learning potential and vocational options

(Einfeld et al., 2006), additional difficulties with attention would further compromise their

capacity to realise these long-term outcomes. This finding therefore has implications for

teachers as it may result in the need for more complex behaviour management plans to assist

children with severe or profound ID in the classroom. The analyses also suggested that as

children get older they have fewer difficulties with attention, hyperactivity and impulsivity, a

finding consistent with research into typically developing children (Faraone, et al., 2006).

When asking teachers and health professionals to identify behaviours relating to

attention difficulties in children with ID, more externalising behaviours were raised and

fewer behaviours relating to inattention. This is consistent with research which suggests that

teachers are less likely than parents to report and/or identify inattentive behaviours (Murray

et al., 2007). It highlights the importance of making teachers more aware of the problem of

inattention in the classroom – both through improved identification and greater understanding

of the impact it has on students. Even though these behaviours may not be as readily

observable or disruptive to others, they can still have a significant impact on individual

student outcomes and achievement.

It is important to consider the findings of this study in the context of methodological

limitations and directions for future research. Participants in the focus group discussions

consisted of teachers from schools for children with mild ID but not those working in schools

138
for children with moderate, severe or profound ID. Given that these teachers are likely to

have had experience working across settings, however, this is unlikely to have had an impact

on the representativeness of the items for children with moderate, severe or profound ID.

Convergent validity was measured by asking teachers to complete several rating scales that

measures ADHD symptomatology, but future studies could incorporate additional

information from a structured diagnostic interview or behavioural observations. While the

test-retest reliability was strong, it was conducted on a small sample, necessitating

replication. Finally, future research is needed to determine the psychometric properties of the

parent version (P-SAID) and the validity of the SAID in adolescent populations.

A strength of the current study is the high return rate of the T-SAID from teachers.

The return rates from teachers in the area of child psychopathology are often low (Bishop,

Laws, Adams, & Norbury, 2006; Lecavalier, et al., 2006; J. R. Sullivan & Riccio, 2007). The

high return rate suggests acceptability of the scale itself given the teachers were given no

incentive to return the booklet. A second strength is the rigorous development of the items in

the T-SAID. The integrated approach of reviewing the content of existing rating scales and

diagnostic manuals, gathering data from behaviour observations, reviewing relevant research

papers, and using focus group discussions during the drafting process all ensured sound

content validity for the items chosen. A third strength is that the T-SAID was developed

specifically for the population of interest. Researchers and clinicians may consider using this

measure as it focused on the particular attention difficulties experienced by children with ID,

as opposed to existing measures designed for typically developing children which may

contain items that are developmentally inappropriate.

The Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability – Teacher version (T-SAID) is the

first measure developed specifically for children with ID, facilitating reliable and valid

measurement of attention, impulsivity and hyperactivity for research and clinical purposes.

139
Potential applications of the T-SAID include use in research examining ADHD

symptomatology, and by clinicians to assist with assessment and diagnosis. It is anticipated

that the development of this disability specific measure will enhance identification, diagnosis

and subsequent access to treatment, along with improvements in the development and

evaluation of interventions for children with ID and ADHD.

Word count: 4793

140
References

Aman, M. G., & Singh, N. N. (1994). Aberrant Behaviour Checklist - Community

supplementary manual. East Aurora, New York: Slosson Educational Publications.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders - Fourth edition Text revision: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders - Fifth edition: DSM-5. Arlington, Virginia: Author.

Antshel, K. M., Phillips, M. H., Gordon, M., Barkley, R., & Faraone, S. V. (2006). Is ADHD

a valid disorder in children with intellectual delays? Clinical Psychology Review,

26(5), 555-572.

Barkley, R. A., & Edwards, G. (2006). Diagnostic interview, behavior rating scales, and the

medical examination. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder:

A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (3rd ed.) (pp. 337-368). New York: Guilford

Press.

Bishop, D. V., Laws, G., Adams, C., & Norbury, C. F. (2006). High heritability of speech and

language impairments in 6-year-old twins demonstrated using parent and teacher

report. Behavior Genetics, 36(2), 173-184.

Borgatti, S. P. (1999). Elicitation technqiues for cultural domain analysis. In J. J. Schensul,

M. D. LeCompte, B. K. Nastasi & S. P. Borgatti (Eds.), Enhanced ethnographic

methods III. Audiovisual techniques, focused group interviews, and elicitation

techniques (pp. 115-151). Walnut Creek, California: AltaMira Press.

Buckley, S., Hillery, J., Guerin, S., McEvoy, J., & Dodd, P. (2008). The prevalence of

features of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in a special school in Ireland.

Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 52(2), 156-162.

141
Chan, E., Hopkins, M. R., Perrin, J. M., Herrerias, C., & Homer, C. J. (2005). Diagnostic

practices for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A national survey of primary

care physicians. Ambulatory Pediatrics, 5(4), 201-208.

Cirino, P. T., Chin, C. E., Sevcik, R. A., Wolf, M., Lovett, M., & Morris, R. D. (2002).

Measuring socioeconomic status: Reliability and preliminary validity for different

approaches. Assessment, 9(2), 145-155.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,

New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Conners, C. K. (1997). Conners' Rating Scales - Revised Technical Manual. New York:

Multi-Health Systems Inc.

Conners, C. K. (2008). Conners 3 Manual. New York: Multi-Health Systems Inc.

Cornish, K., Gray, K. M., & Rinehart, N. J. (2010). Fragile X Syndrome and associated

disorders. In J. Holmes (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior, Volume

39 (pp. 211-235). London: Academic Press.

Cornish, K., Scerif, G., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2007). Tracing syndrome-specific trajectories

of attention across the lifespan. Cortex, 43(6), 672-685.

Cornish, K., Turk, J., & Levitas, A. (2007). Fragile X Syndrome and autism: Common

developmental pathways? Current Pediatric Reviews, 3, 61-68.

Cornish, K., & Wilding, J. (2010). Attention, genes and developmental disorders. New York:

Oxford University Press.

Dally, K. (2006). The influence of phonological processing and inattentive behavior on

reading acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(2), 420-437.

de Bildt, A., Kraijer, D., Sytema, S., & Minderaa, R. (2005). The psychometric properties of

the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales in children and adolescents with mental

retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(1), 53-62.

142
Deb, S., Dhaliwal, A. J., & Roy, M. (2008). The usefulness of Conners' Rating Scales -

Revised in screening for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children with

intellectual disabilities and borderline intelligence. Journal of Intellectual Disability

Research, 52(11), 950-965.

Dekker, M. C., & Koot, H. M. (2003). DSM-IV disorders in children with borderline to

moderate intellectual disability. I: Prevalence and impact. Journal of the American

Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(8), 915-922.

Einfeld, S. L., Piccinin, A. M., Mackinnon, A., Hofer, S. M., Taffe, J., Gray, K. M., . . .

Tonge, B. J. (2006). Psychopathology in young people with intellectual disability.

Journal of the American Medical Association, 296(16), 1981-1989.

Einfeld, S. L., & Tonge, B. J. (1995). The Developmental Behavior Checklist: The

development and validation of an instrument to assess behavioral and emotional

disturbance in children and adolescents with mental retardation. Journal of Autism

and Developmental Disorders, 25(2), 81-104.

Einfeld, S. L., & Tonge, B. J. (2002). Manual for the Developmental Behaviour Checklist

(2nd edition): Primary carer version (DBC- P) and teacher version (DBC - T).

Clayton, Victoria: Monash University Centre for Developmental Psychology &

Psychiatry.

Evans, P. L. C., & Hogg, J. (1984). A classroom rating scale for use with mentally retarded

children. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23(3), 187-194.

Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., & Mick, E. (2006). The age-dependent decline of attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis of follow-up studies. Psychological

Medicine, 36(2), 159-165.

143
Feinstein, C., & Reiss, A. L. (1996). Psychiatric disorder in mentally retarded children and

adolescents. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 5(4), 827-

852.

Frink, J. (2007). Flesh 2.0. Fairfax, Virginia: Sourceforge. Retrieved from

flesh.sourceforge.net

Ghaziuddin, M., Welch, K., Mohiuddin, S., Lagrou, R., & Ghaziuddin, N. (2010). Utility of

the Social and Communication Questionnaire in the differentiation of autism from

ADHD. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 22(4), 359-366.

Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal

of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 38(5), 581-586.

Guerin, S., Buckley, S., McEvoy, J., Hillery, J., & Dodd, P. (2009). The psychometric

properties of the Attention-Distraction, Inhibition-Excitation Classroom Assessment

Scale (ADIECAS) in a sample of children with moderate and severe intellectual

disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30(4), 727-734.

Hastings, R. P., Beck, A., Daley, D., & Hill, C. (2005). Symptoms of ADHD and their

correlates in children with intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental

Disabilities, 26(5), 456-468.

Hollingshead, B. A. (1975). Four factor index of socioeconomic status. New Haven, CT:

Yale University.

Jopp, D. A., & Keys, C. B. (2001). Diagnostic overshadowing reviewed and reconsidered.

American Journal on Mental Retardation, 106(5), 416-433.

Lecavalier, L., Leone, S., & Wiltz, J. (2006). The impact of behaviour problems on caregiver

stress in young people with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Intellectual

Disability Research, 50(3), 172-183.

144
Lee, P., & Friedlander, R. (2007). Attention-deficit and disruptive behavior disorders. In R.

Fletcher, E. Loschen, C. Stavrakaki & M. First (Eds.), Diagnostic Manual -

Intellectual Disability (DM-ID): A textbook of diagnosis of mental disorders in

persons with intellectual disability. New York: National Association for the Dually

Diagnosed Press.

Lindblad, I., Gillberg, C., & Fernell, E. (2011). ADHD and other associated developmental

problems in children with mild mental retardation. The use of the "Five-To-Fifteen"

questionnaire in a population-based sample. Research in Developmental Disabilities,

32(6), 2805-2809.

Mason, J., & Scior, K. (2004). 'Diagnostic overshadowing' amongst clinicians working with

people with intellectual disabilities in the UK. Journal of Applied Research in

Intellectual Disabilities, 17(2), 85-90.

Miller, M. L., Fee, V. E., & Jones, C. J. (2004). Psychometric properties of ADHD rating

scales among children with mental retardation II: Validity. Research in

Developmental Disabilities, 25(5), 477-492.

Miller, M. L., Fee, V. E., & Netterville, A. K. (2004). Psychometric properties of ADHD

rating scales among children with mental retardation I: Reliability. Research in

Developmental Disabilities, 25(5), 459-476.

Murray, D. W., Kollins, S. H., Hardy, K. K., Abikoff, H. B., Swanson, J. M., Cunningham,

C., . . . Chuang, S. Z. (2007). Parent versus teacher ratings of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in the Preschoolers with Attention-

Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Treatment Study (PATS). Journal of Child and

Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 17(5), 605-619.

145
Rabiner, D., & Coie, J. D. (2000). Early attention problems and children's reading

achievement: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of the American Academy of Child

& Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(7), 859-867.

Rhodes, S. M., Riby, D. M., Matthews, K., & Coghill, D. R. (2011). Attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder and Williams syndrome: Shared behavioral and

neuropsychological profiles. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,

33(1), 147-156.

Rhodes, S. M., Riby, D. M., Park, J., Fraser, E., & Campbell, L. E. (2010). Executive

neuropsychological functioning in individuals with Williams syndrome.

Neuropsychologia, 48(5), 1216-1226.

Rojahn, J., Rowe, E. W., Macken, J., Gray, A., Delitta, D., Booth, A., & Kimbrell, K. (2010).

Psychometric evaluation of the Behavior Problems Inventory-01 and the Nisonger

Child Behavior Rating Form with children and adolescents. Journal of Mental Health

Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 3(1), 28-50.

Royal College of Psychiatrists. (2001). DC-LD: Diagnostic criteria for psychiatric disorders

for use with adults with learning disabilities/mental retardation. London: Gaskell.

Scerif, G. (2010). Attention trajectories, mechanisms and outcomes: At the interface between

developing cognition and environment. Developmental Science, 13(6), 805-812.

Scerif, G., Cornish, K., Wilding, J., Driver, J., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2004). Visual search

in typically developing toddlers and toddlers with Fragile X or Williams syndrome.

Developmental Science, 7(1), 116-130.

Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales,

Second edition - Teacher rating form manual. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson.

146
Spira, E. G., & Fischel, J. E. (2005). The impact of preschool inattention, hyperactivity, and

impulsivity on social and academic development: A review. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(7), 755-773.

Steele, A., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Cornish, K., & Scerif, G. (2012). The multiple subfunctions

of attention: differential developmental gateways to literacy and numeracy. Child

Development, 83(6), 2028-2041.

Strand, S., Sturmey, P., & Newton, J. (1990). A classroom rating scale for use with mentally

handicapped children: A replication of the ADIECAS. British Journal of Clinical

Psychology, 29(1), 121-123.

Sullivan, J. R., & Riccio, C. A. (2007). Diagnostic group differences in parent and teacher

ratings on the BRIEF and Conners' Scales. Journal of Attention Disorders, 11(3), 398-

406.

Taffe, J. R., Tonge, B. J., Gray, K. M., & Einfeld, S. L. (2008). Extracting more information

from behaviour checklists by using components of mean based scores. International

Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 17(4), 232-240.

Turk, J. (2011). Fragile X syndrome: lifespan developmental implications for those without

as well as with intellectual disability. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 24(5), 387-397.

Turner, S., Sloper, P., & Knussen, C. (1991). The validity and applicability of the ADIECAS

classroom rating scale in a sample of children with Down's syndrome. Journal of

Mental Deficiency Research, 35(4), 384-391.

Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence - Third edition.

Sydney, NSW: Pearson.

Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Fourth edition. Sydney,

NSW: Pearson.

147
Willcutt, E. G. (2012). The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a

meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics, 9(3), 490-499.

Witwer, A. N., & Lecavalier, L. (2010). Validity of comorbid psychiatric disorders in

youngsters with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Developmental and Physical

Disabilities, 22(4), 367-380.

World Health Organization. (1992). International Classification of Diseases, Tenth edition.

Geneva, Switzerland: Author.

148
Table 1

Examples of Items from the Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability – Teacher Version

(T-SAID)

Problem behaviour Examples of items


Hyperactivity/impulsivity Item 6: Stays in own seat when expected to

Item 27: Takes turns when playing with others

Item 33: Waits until called on by the teacher

before giving an answer (avoids calling out)

Inattention Item 12: Persists with a task for 2 minutes

even if they find it difficult (avoids giving

up)

Item 15: Can easily give his/her attention to

start a new task

Item 21: Returns attention to task after being

distracted by another student

149
Table 2

Sample Demographics
Total sample Mild ID (n = Moderate ID Severe/profound

(n = 176) 62) (n = 79) ID (n = 27)

Gender n (% male) 114 (64.8) 43 (69.4) 51 (64.6) 15 (55.6)

Age

M (SD) 9.15 (2.13) 9.08 (2.13) 9.09 (1.92) 10.07 (2.34)

Range 5 – 13 5 – 12 5 – 12 5 – 13

SESa

M (SD) 35.44 (15.97) 33.42 (15.32) 35.62 (16.91) 38.65 (15.43)

Range 8 – 69 11 – 67 8 – 69 14 – 67

School attended n (%)

Mainstream 27 (15.3) 8 (12.9) 13 (16.5) 2 (7.4)

Special/Special 131 (74.4) 49 (79.1) 58 (73.4) 21 (77.7)

developmental

Split placementb 11 (6.3) 5 (8.1) 5 (6.3) 1 (3.7)

Autism specialist 2 (1.1) * * 1 (3.7)

school

Support centre in 5 (2.8) * 3 (3.8) 2 (7.4)

mainstream school

Note. Degree of ID could not be determined for eight students.


a
Hollingshead scores range from 8 to 71. bChildren who spend part of their week in a

mainstream school and part of their week in a special school.

150
Table 3

Scores from the Cognitive and Adaptive Living Skills Assessments by Degree of ID

Mild (n = Moderate Severe/

62) (n = 79) profound

(n = 27)

WISC-IV /WPPSI-III M (SD) 63.71 (4.71) 47.50 (4.02) 40.00 (0.00)

FSIQ

Range 56 – 70 40 – 54 40

WISC-IV

VCI M (SD) 64.64 (7.12) 53.18 (7.25) 45.44 (0.88)

Range 53 – 75 45 – 71 45 – 47

PRI M (SD) 72.43 (7.38) 55.44 (7.93) 47.83 (4.13)

Range 63 – 86 45 – 75 45 – 59

WMI M (SD) 66.72 (9.24) 56.53 (5.81) 50.22 (0.67)

Range 52 – 88 50 – 74 50 – 52

PSI M (SD) 75.25 59.09 (8.25) 50.75 (1.39)

(10.83)

Range 50 – 94 50 – 78 50 – 53

WPPSI-III

Verbal M (SD) 63.92 (8.87) 54.58 (3.85) *

Range 53 – 77 48 – 61 *

Performance M (SD) 68.23 (9.03) 51.50 (4.60) *

Range 53 – 81 47 – 61 *

VABS-II-T

151
ABC M (SD) 59.55 (6.71) 54.19 (9.18) 41.78

(13.17)

Range 42 – 70 26 – 74 20 – 64

Communication M (SD) 61.66 (5.11) 56.50 (8.11) 45.78

(10.15)

Range 54 – 74 42 – 74 25 – 60

Socialisation M (SD) 67.49 (8.26) 62.56 54.00

(10.17) (12.60)

Range 49 – 84 37 – 89 32 – 76

Daily Living Skills M (SD) 60.63 (8.65) 55.27 (9.62) 42.14

(11.23)

Range 38 – 78 36 – 80 21 – 62

Note. WISC-IV = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth edition; WPPSI-III =

Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence – Third edition; VABS-II-T =

Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales, Second edition – Teacher Rating Form.

152
Table 4

Reliability and Convergent Validity of the T-SAID

Reliability Validity

Convergent Divergent

Internal Test-retest Conners 3 Conners 3 DBC-T VABS-II-T

consistency ICC (95% CI) inattention (r) hyperactivity (r) hyperactivity (r) (r)

(α)

T-SAID total .98 . 96* (.90 – .99) .66* .69* .67* - .43*

Note. ICC = Intra-class correlation; CI = confidence interval.

* p < .001.

153
Table 5

Beta Coefficients of Regressions of the Mean Item Score (MIS), the Proportion of Items

Checked (PIC) and the Intensity Index (II) of the Teacher Version of the Scale of Attention in

Intellectual Disability (T-SAID) on Degree of ID

MIS PIC II

Age -0.07* -0.03* -0.04*

Gender -0.14 -0.08 -0.05

SES (ref: low)

Average 0.02 -0.02 0.02

High -0.05 -0.07 -0.01

Level of ID (ref: mild)

Moderate 0.06 0.01 0.04

Severe/profound 0.49* 0.25* 0.24*

Constant 1.56* 0.99* 0.69*

% variance 19.3 18.3 16.3

* p < .001.

154
CHAPTER 9 THE SCALE OF ATTENTION IN

INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY (SAID): FACTOR ANALYSIS

OF A NEW RATING SCALE FOR USE WITH CHILDREN

WITH AN INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY

155
9.1 Declaration
In the case of Chapter 9, contributions to the work involved the following:

Name % contribution Nature of contribution


Nerelie Freeman 70% Formulation of project design, data
collection, data analysis, and writing
manuscript.
A/Prof Kylie Gray 12.5% Contribution to development of
project design, discussion of ideas
expressed in manuscript, and critical
review of manuscript.
Prof Kim Cornish 12.5% Contribution to development of
project design, discussion of ideas
expressed in manuscript, and critical
review of manuscript.
Dr John Taffe 5% Consultation in data analysis and
critical review of manuscript

Declaration by co-authors:

(1) they meet the criteria for authorship in that they have participated in the conception,
execution, or interpretation, of at least that part of the publication in their field of expertise;
(2) they take public responsibility for their part of the publication, except for the responsible
author who accepts overall responsibility for the publication;
(3) there are no other authors of the publication according to these criteria;
(4) potential conflicts of interest have been disclosed to (a) granting bodies, (b) the editor or
publisher of journals or other publications, and (c) the head of the responsible academic unit;
and
(5) the original data are stored at the following location(s) and will be held for at least five
years from the date indicated below:

Location: Centre for Developmental Psychiatry and Psychology, Department of


Psychiatry, Southern Clinical School, Monash University, Clayton Campus
Date

Signature 1

Signature 2

Signature 3

Signature 4

156
9.2 Paper commentary

Chapter 9 presents a paper that has been submitted for publication in the American

Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. This paper has been formatted to the

specific requirements of the journal. Pages have been re-numbered to provide consistency

throughout the thesis.

Paper 2 is a companion to the previous paper and examines the factor structure of the

new teacher rating scale that has been developed: the Scale of Attention in Intellectual

Disability (SAID). Studies that have measured ADHD symptoms within children with ID

have often used measures that were designed for children who were typically developing in

the absence of any reliable or valid alternatives. Shortcomings of these rating scales when

used with children with ID include inconsistencies in the factor structure and the

inappropriateness of some of the rating scale items. A factor analysis of the SAID was needed

to determine whether the rating scale items grouped together into constructs that could

provide meaningful information about ADHD symptoms in children with ID.

157
THE SCALE OF ATTENTION IN INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY (SAID): FACTOR

ANALYSIS OF A NEW RATING SCALE FOR CHILDREN WITH INTELLECTUAL

DISABILITIES

Abstract

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a well-documented childhood

psychiatric condition but one that is inconsistently identified in the context of intellectual

disabilities (ID). Although ADHD ratings scales exist, few if any can reliably measure the

range and severity of behaviours within the ID population. Limitations of these scales include

the inappropriateness of some items and the lack of replicability of the factor structure. In an

attempt to remedy this problem, a novel scale measuring attention and hyperactivity specific

to ID – the Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability (SAID) – has been developed. An

exploratory factor analysis of the teacher version (T-SAID) yielded a four factor solution.

The results indicate that the T-SAID is a valid tool for use with children with ID.

Key words: intellectual disability, rating scale, factor analysis, children

158
The past 20 years has seen considerable progress in understanding difficulties with

attention and hyperactivity among children with intellectual disability (ID). Some earlier

studies suggested that ADHD symptoms (e.g., high levels of inattentive and

hyperactive/impulsive behaviours) were related to cognitive deficits rather than being a

comorbid psychiatric diagnosis (Burack, et al., 2001; Handen, et al., 1997). Conversely, other

studies argued that mental health issues were underdiagnosed due to the complexity of

diagnosing additional disorders in the presence of ID, a concept known as diagnostic

overshadowing (Jopp & Keys, 2001; Mason & Scior, 2004). These conflicting views

culminated in a comprehensive review by Antshel and colleagues (2006), who concluded that

ADHD was a valid disorder in ID but emphasised that more studies were needed to further

identify the complexities of these comorbid diagnoses across the areas of treatment,

assessment, behavioural and experimental studies.

Research from the neuropsychological field has led the way in demonstrating not only

that ADHD symptomatology is present in children with ID, but that at the cognitive level

there are disorder-specific profiles of attention functioning that differentiate one group from

another for example autism spectrum disorder (e.g., Christ, et al., 2011; Landry & Bryson,

2004), Fragile X Syndrome (e.g., Cornish, et al., 2013; Cornish, Scerif, et al., 2007),

Williams Syndrome (e.g., Breckenridge, et al., 2013; Rhodes, et al., 2011), and Down

Syndrome (e.g., Breckenridge, et al., 2013; Cornish, Steele, et al., 2012). Furthermore, recent

parent surveys suggest that ADHD symptomology in these groups is at least as common as

that reported in children with ADHD with no intellectual impairment (Neece, et al., 2011).

Indeed, in the case of autism spectrum disorder, Fragile X Syndrome and Williams

Syndrome, it would appear that ADHD symptoms are markedly greater with prevalence rates

as high as 59% (Goldstein & Schwebach, 2004), 62% (Bailey, et al., 2008) and 100%

159
(Rhodes, et al., 2011) respectively within these ID populations, compared with 6-7% of

typically developing children and adolescents (Willcutt, 2012).

Although there is now accruing consensus that ADHD behaviours have a significant

and long lasting impact in children with ID, there is currently no one measure that can

capture different severity and profiles of inattentive and hyperactive behaviours in children

specifically with ID. The Conners Rating Scales (CRS; Conners, 1989, 1997, 2008) and the

Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, 1991; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) are

amongst the most commonly used both clinically and in the research literature. The parent

version of these scales have been used to examine ADHD symptoms in studies of children

with idiopathic ID (Deb, et al., 2008; M. L. Miller, Fee, & Jones, 2004; M. L. Miller, Fee, &

Netterville, 2004), Fragile X Syndrome (Cornish, et al., 2013; Farzin et al., 2006), autism

spectrum disorder (Hartley, et al., 2008), and Williams Syndrome (Rhodes, et al., 2011). Few

studies have used the teacher version of these scales (Buckley, et al., 2008; M. L. Miller, Fee,

& Netterville, 2004) and all used samples of children with idiopathic ID.

One of the core disadvantages of both the CRS and CBCL scales is that they were

developed for children who function within normal range of IQ; they were not specifically

developed with the intention of being used to rate the behaviours of children with ID. One of

the key issues, therefore, is the appropriateness of the items for this population. Literacy and

numeracy skills cannot be assumed among children with ID, and yet both the CRS and the

CBCL contain several items on their attention subscales that relate to these skills such as Not

reading up to par or Has difficulty learning. Further, such items offer poor discriminant

validity as limited academic achievement can be observed in many children with ID,

irrespective of whether they have attention difficulties.

A second key issue to consider when evaluating a rating scale is the replicability of

the factor structure in independent studies. This is particularly important when using them in

160
a population for which they were not originally designed or validated. A study by Deb and

colleagues (2008) examined the factor structure of the CRS - R (Conners, 1997) among

children with either borderline intelligence (i.e., an IQ of 70 – 79) or mild to severe ID. While

the same factors were extracted for the parent version (i.e., inattention, hyperactivity and

conduct problems), the teacher version extracted less distinct factors with four items having

cross loadings, and five items with face validity for ADHD symptomatology loading on the

conduct problems factor. The authors concluded that the teacher version was not

recommended as a screening tool for ADHD symptomatology among children with ID. Two

studies by Pandolfi and colleagues (2009, 2012) examined the factor structure of the CBCL

among toddlers and children with autism spectrum disorder, although it should be noted that

over one third of the sample were high functioning and therefore the conclusions that can be

drawn on the validity of this structure within the ID population can only be viewed as

tentative. Further, these studies were restricted to the parent version of the CBCL and did not

examine teacher ratings.

Therefore, the inappropriateness of some rating scale items and limited replicability of

the factor structure suggest that both the CRS and CBCL have significant limitations in their

ability to reliably identify ADHD symptoms within the ID population. Despite these issues,

these scales continue to be used with children who have ID in the absence of any reliable or

valid alternatives. In an attempt to capture profiles of inattentive behaviours in children with

ID and across different syndrome aetiologies, we have developed a new rating scale – the

Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability (SAID) – which, for the first time, has items

related to ADHD symptomatology that specifically relate to the ID population. The reliability

and validity of this measure have been described in Paper 1 (this volume). In this paper we

focus on evaluating the factor structure of the Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability -

Teacher version (T-SAID).

161
Method

Participants

Two hundred and fifteen consent forms were returned by families of children aged 5

to 13 years attending schools in the Melbourne metropolitan area and across regional

Victoria, Australia. Further details about the sample and recruitment into the study are

described in Paper 1 (this volume).

Measures

Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability (SAID). This newly developed rating

scale consists of 44 items that tap three core domains: hyperactivity/impulsivity, inattention,

and aspects of working memory. The teacher version (T-SAID) was completed by the child’s

current teacher (provided they have known the child for a minimum of 6 months). A parent

version (P-SAID) is under development. Teachers responded to each statement on a 4-point

scale of never, rarely, sometimes and often, with lower scores on the T-SAID relating to

greater difficulties.

Procedure

Ethics approval was obtained from the Monash University Standing Committee on

Ethics in Research Involving Humans, the Victorian Department of Education and Early

Childhood Development, and the Catholic Education Office Melbourne, Australia.

The rating scale was developed firstly by identifying behaviours salient to children

with ID through a combination of reviewing items from existing rating scales, drawing on

descriptive behaviours reported in published studies, and an examination of the diagnostic

criteria in the DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), ICD-10 (World Health

Organization, 1992), DC-LD (Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2001) and DM-ID (P. Lee &

Friedlander, 2007). Items were then developed by the research team and presented to focus

groups of health professionals and teachers for their comment and feedback.

162
A community survey followed development of the new rating scale. Participant

selection was via a three-stage process described in greater detail in Paper 1 (this volume).

Briefly, families were approached to participate in the study via mail outs from schools

(following permission from the school principal). Support groups and community

organisations were also approached, asking them to advertise the study on their web site and

seeking permission to contact member families with a child aged between 5 and 13 years.

Families who consented to participate in the study returned a consent form to the research

team in a reply-paid envelope.

Eligibility to participate was determined via a telephone interview with the family

which included collection of demographic and clinical data, and assessment results. Children

were eligible to participate in the study if: (1) they were aged between 5 and 13 years; and (2)

their most recent cognitive assessment placed their functioning in the intellectually disabled

range (i.e., their cognitive and/or adaptive living skills assessment total score was 70 or

below).

For those children eligible to participate in the study, their classroom teacher was then

mailed a booklet of rating scales to complete which included the T-SAID. It was a

requirement that each teacher who completed the questionnaires had known the child for a

minimum of 6 months. Rating scales were mailed back to the research team in a reply-paid

envelope after completion.

Analysis

A number of factor analytic solutions were considered when examining the T-SAID

data. A principal components factor analysis was used, with oblique rotation chosen given the

assumption that there was a correlation across factors (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). The

number of factors to be extracted was determined by a number of considerations:

examination of the scree plot (Cattell, 1966), interpretability of the factors, the preference for

163
a simple structure, the generation of discrete factors with little overlap, and the rejection of

analyses that only had a few items loading onto a factor. Given the sample size in this study,

loadings at or above .50 were selected for inclusion of an item in interpreting each factor

(Hair, et al., 1998).

Results

Of the 181 children deemed eligible to participate in the study, rating scales were

returned for 176 yielding a return rate of 97.2%. Of these, 114 were male and 62 were female

with a mean age of 9.15 years. The children included in the sample had a diagnosis of autism

spectrum disorder (n = 59; 33.5%), Down Syndrome (n = 41; 23.3%) or idiopathic

intellectual disability (n = 76; 43.2%). The majority of children had either a mild (n = 62;

35.2%) or moderate intellectual disability (n = 79; 44.9%), with a smaller proportion being in

the severe/profound range (n = 27; 15.3%)12. Further details about the sample, including

demographic characteristics, cognitive ability and comorbid behavioural and emotional

problems are described in Paper 1 (this volume).

Communalities for the 33 items loaded on the four factors were all at or above .50.

The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was high at .95, and the Bartlett’s

test of sphericity was significant (χ2 (946) = 6541.26, p < .001). Taken together, these results

suggest that the data satisfied the assumptions for factor analysis. The results of this analysis

are shown in Table 1.

[Insert Table 1 about here]

12
Degree of ID based on cognitive ability was unable to be obtained for 8 children

164
Thirty-three of the items retained for analysis loaded onto one of the four factors, with

loadings from .50 to .87 accounting for 62.8% of the variance. There were no significant

cross loadings on any of the factors, suggesting a low shared variance across the variables.

Factor 1 accounted for 49.7% of the variance and consisted of items related to hyperactive

and impulsive behaviours. This subscale was therefore labelled Hyperactivity/Impulsivity.

Factor 2 accounted for 4.1% of the variance and consisted of behaviours related to sustained

and selective attention. This subscale was therefore labelled Inattention. Factor 3 accounted

for 5.6% of the variance and consisted of behaviours relating to language. This subscale was

therefore labelled Verbal Communication. Factor 4 accounted for 3.4% of the variance and

consisted of behaviours related to following instructions and compliance to rules. This

subscale was therefore labelled Following Instructions. The internal consistency of these

subscales was excellent for the hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention subscales (α = .91 –

.94), and good for the verbal communication and subscales (α = .79 – .82).

Discussion

The current study examined the factor structure of a new rating scale for use among

children with intellectual disability: the Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability – Teacher

version (T-SAID). Four factors were extracted using exploratory factor analysis in this study.

As expected, Hyperactivity/Impulsivity and Inattention were extracted, being the two core

dimensions of ADHD symptomatology (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). A third

subscale, labelled Verbal Communication, consisted of items relating to language such as

volume and pace of speech, making requests and retaining information. The fourth subscale,

labelled Following Instructions, contained items relating to instructions and compliance to

rules.

While the extracted Hyperactivity/Impulsivity and Inattention factors support the two

dimensional model of ADHD (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), there are few factor

165
analytic studies that we can compare these findings to, as most of the available rating scales

designed for or used with children with ID have only a hyperactivity subscale. It has

similarities with validation studies using the CBCL (Pandolfi, et al., 2009, 2012) and the CRS

– R (Deb, et al., 2008) which both have attention and hyperactivity subscales. The four factor

structure of the T-SAID, however, suggests that there are other elements which may also

need to be considered. Given these children may present with additional complexities, such

as limited or no language or a physical disability, the nature of ADHD symptoms may present

quite differently within children with ID.

The rating scale items generated for the T-SAID were based on diagnostic

classificatory tools, descriptive behaviours reported in published studies, and focus group

feedback suggesting that they were representative of hyperactive, impulsive and inattentive

behaviours observed in children with ID. An item such as Stays within the school grounds

during the day was therefore expected to load onto the Hyperactivity/Impulsivity factor,

consistent with the notion that a child who is impulsive might forget about school rules or

safety and abscond from the school grounds. In the current analysis, however, the item loaded

together with a group of items about understanding and carrying out instructions (Following

Instructions) therefore suggesting that among children with ID this behaviour might relate to

something else such as compliance around obeying the rules, and perhaps understanding the

rules. Qualitatively, some teachers from schools for children with moderate, severe or

profound ID commented that this item did not apply to the child they were rating as their

school had high fences and/or required a security code to exit. The child was therefore not

given the opportunity to demonstrate this behaviour as there was no way they could leave or

abscond. So it is also possible that this item could be interpreted differently depending on the

school setting attended by the child.

166
The extraction of an Inattention factor represents an exciting development in the study

of ADHD symptoms among children with ID. Recent research integrating attentional

processes in boys with Fragile X Syndrome (Cornish, Cole, Longhi, Karmiloff-Smith, &

Scerif, 2012; Cornish, et al., 2013) highlighted the importance of understanding both the

cognitive markers of inattention and the ways they map across into classroom behaviours.

Our understanding of this complex interplay, particularly when examining different

neurodevelopmental disorders, can only be enriched by using rating scales that are

standardised and validated for the population of interest. For the first time, the T-SAID will

enable researchers to supplement their observational laboratory data on inattention with

rating scale data that captures behaviours salient to the ID population.

Using the T-SAID to identify inattentive behaviours in young children with ID also

has important implications for academic achievement. While recent research has shed some

light on the different developmental trajectory in literacy acquisition for children with Down

and Williams Syndromes (Steele, Scerif, Cornish, & Karmiloff-Smith, 2013), research into

the mediating role of attention on academic achievement among children with ID is still

wanting. By identifying the specific behaviours impacting on the child’s ability to attend to

tasks, the T-SAID can help guide behaviour management plans and interventions that are

weaknesses for that child. In turn, this will enable the scaffolding of strategies that can be

used by the child to more rapidly acquire literacy and numeracy skills in the classroom.

The Verbal Communication factor contained items that a priori were believed to have

face validity across behaviours reflecting ADHD symptomatology and aspects of working

memory. It is logical that these items grouped together, however, as all the items inferred the

need for verbal ability in order to exhibit the behaviour. This recognises that verbal skills

cannot be assumed among children with ID, as children with certain neurodevelopmental

disorders (e.g., autism spectrum disorder) and/or severe/profound ID may be nonverbal. The

167
T-SAID therefore overcomes the scoring dilemma posed by rating scales such as the CRS

(Conners, 1989, 1997, 2008) and CBCL (Achenbach, 1991; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001)

where items related to verbal ADHD symptoms such as Talks too much or too fast cannot be

scored and thus subscale scores cannot be calculated. Children who are nonverbal would

simply receive a score of 0 as they would not have the ability to demonstrate any of the skills

in this subscale, an approach used by other disability specific measures such as the

Developmental Behaviour Checklist (Einfeld & Tonge, 1995).

A core strength of the current study is that analysis of the T-SAID extracted factors

that are consistent with ADHD symptomatology. Preliminary analyses in Paper 1 (this

volume) suggested that the T-SAID total score has discriminant validity across children who

have ADHD and those who do not, although its efficacy as a screening tool is yet to be

evaluated. Another strength is that the factor analysis extracted a Verbal Communication

factor, thus recognising the importance of considering children who are nonverbal; a

shortcoming identified in the CRS – R (Deb, et al., 2008) that was not addressed in the more

recently released Conners 3 (Conners, 2008).

It should be noted that the participants-to-items ratio for factor analysis is adequate in

the current study. While the 10:1 ratio of participants-to-items was recommended in the past

(Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001), there is agreement in the literature that this is no longer

required. This is supported by recent published studies in the field of developmental

psychopathology that have used similar ratios in their factor analyses. A study by Deb,

Dhaliwal and Roy (2008) which drew upon a sample of children with ID or borderline

intelligence conducted a factor analysis using the Conners and had a 5:1 ratio (151 children:

27 items). A study by Norris and Lecavalier (2011) which factor analysed the NCBRF using

a sample of children with autism also had a 5:1 ratio (399 children: 76 items). Another recent

168
study conducted by Pandolfi and colleagues (2012) drew upon a sample of children with

autism using the CBCL had a 1:1 ratio (122 children: 118 items).

Future studies need to look at the stability of this factor structure across different

neurodevelopmental disorders given that ADHD symptomatology can present differently

across diagnoses (Cornish, Scerif, et al., 2007). It may be, as with the Aberrant Behaviour

Checklist – Community (Aman & Singh, 1994), that different factor structures exist across

neurodevelopmental disorders (Brinkley, et al., 2007; Sansone et al., 2012), or the factor

structure may remain constant irrespective of diagnosis. The modest sample size of this study

also necessitates replication with a larger sample to further support the findings from this

factor analysis. An examination of the P-SAID is also needed to determine whether the factor

structure is similar in the parent version of this scale.

The findings of this study suggest that the T-SAID is a valid scale for measuring

hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention among children with ID. The findings lend support

to the notion that DSM-5 ADHD subtypes (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) are

valid among children with ID, although the clinical behaviours observed may be different

within this population. Some items that relate to ADHD symptomatology in typically

developing children loaded onto different factors in the present study. This reinforces the

value of developing rating scales specific to children with ID as the items give an accurate

representation of behaviours likely to be observed within this population. The development of

the T-SAID may assist clinicians and researchers to more reliably identify these behaviours,

which may aid with assessment and diagnosis within this population.

Word count: 3120

169
References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile.

Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and

profiles. Burlington, VT: Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment.

Aman, M. G., & Singh, N. N. (1994). Aberrant Behaviour Checklist - Community

supplementary manual. East Aurora, New York: Slosson Educational Publications.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders - Fourth edition Text revision: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders - Fifth edition: DSM-5. Arlington, Virginia: Author.

Antshel, K. M., Phillips, M. H., Gordon, M., Barkley, R., & Faraone, S. V. (2006). Is ADHD

a valid disorder in children with intellectual delays? Clinical Psychology Review,

26(5), 555-572.

Bailey, D. B., Jr., Raspa, M., Olmsted, M., & Holiday, D. B. (2008). Co-occurring conditions

associated with FMR1 gene variations: Findings from a national parent survey.

American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A, 146A(16), 2060-2069.

Breckenridge, K., Braddick, O., Anker, S., Woodhouse, M., & Atkinson, J. (2013). Attention

in Williams syndrome and Down's syndrome: Performance on the new early

childhood attention battery. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31(Pt 2),

257-269.

Brinkley, J., Nations, L., Abramson, R. K., Hall, A., Wright, H. H., Gabriels, R., . . . Cuccaro,

M. L. (2007). Factor analysis of the Aberrant Behavior Checklist in individuals with

autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(10),

1949-1959.

170
Buckley, S., Hillery, J., Guerin, S., McEvoy, J., & Dodd, P. (2008). The prevalence of

features of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in a special school in Ireland.

Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 52(2), 156-162.

Burack, J. A., Evans, D. W., Klaiman, C., & Iarocci, G. (2001). The mysterious myth of

attention deficits and other defect stories: Contemporary issues in the developmental

approach to mental retardation. International Review of Research in Mental

Retardation, 24, 299-320.

Christ, S. E., Kester, L. E., Bodner, K. E., & Miles, J. H. (2011). Evidence for selective

inhibitory impairment in individuals with autism spectrum disorder.

Neuropsychology, 25(6), 690-701.

Conners, C. K. (1989). Conners Rating Scales Manual. New York: Multi-Health Systems

Inc.

Conners, C. K. (1997). Conners' Rating Scales - Revised Technical Manual. New York:

Multi-Health Systems Inc.

Conners, C. K. (2008). Conners 3 Manual. New York: Multi-Health Systems Inc.

Cornish, K., Cole, V., Longhi, E., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Scerif, G. (2012). Does attention

constrain developmental trajectories in fragile X syndrome? A 3-year prospective

longitudinal study. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities,

117(2), 103-120.

Cornish, K., Cole, V., Longhi, E., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Scerif, G. (2013). Mapping

developmental trajectories of attention and working memory in fragile X syndrome:

Developmental freeze or developmental change? Development and Psychopathology,

25(2), 365-376.

Cornish, K., Scerif, G., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2007). Tracing syndrome-specific trajectories

of attention across the lifespan. Cortex, 43(6), 672-685.

171
Cornish, K., Steele, A., Monteiro, C. R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Scerif, G. (2012). Attention

deficits predict phenotypic outcomes in syndrome-specific and domain-specific ways.

Frontiers in Psychology, 11(3), 227.

Deb, S., Dhaliwal, A. J., & Roy, M. (2008). The usefulness of Conners' Rating Scales -

Revised in screening for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children with

intellectual disabilities and borderline intelligence. Journal of Intellectual Disability

Research, 52(11), 950-965.

Einfeld, S. L., & Tonge, B. J. (1995). The Developmental Behavior Checklist: The

development and validation of an instrument to assess behavioral and emotional

disturbance in children and adolescents with mental retardation. Journal of Autism

and Developmental Disorders, 25(2), 81-104.

Farzin, F., Perry, H., Hessl, D., Loesch, D., Cohen, J., Bacalman, S., . . . Hagerman, R.

(2006). Autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in boys

with the Fragile X premutation. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics,

27(Suppl 2), S137-S144.

Goldstein, S., & Schwebach, A. J. (2004). The comorbidity of pervasive developmental

disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Results of a retrospective chart

review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 329-339.

Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1998). Multivariate data

analysis (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.

Handen, B. L., Janosky, J., & McAuliffe, S. (1997). Long-term follow-up of children with

mental retardation/borderline intellectual functioning and ADHD. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 25(4), 287-295.

172
Hartley, S., Sikora, D., & McCoy, R. (2008). Prevalence and risk factors of maladaptive

behaviour in young children with autistic disorder. Journal of Intellectual Disability

Research, 52(10), 819-829.

Jopp, D. A., & Keys, C. B. (2001). Diagnostic overshadowing reviewed and reconsidered.

American Journal on Mental Retardation, 106(5), 416-433.

Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children

with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(6), 1115-1122.

Lee, P., & Friedlander, R. (2007). Attention-deficit and disruptive behavior disorders. In R.

Fletcher, E. Loschen, C. Stavrakaki & M. First (Eds.), Diagnostic Manual -

Intellectual Disability (DM-ID): A textbook of diagnosis of mental disorders in

persons with intellectual disability. New York: National Association for the Dually

Diagnosed Press.

Mason, J., & Scior, K. (2004). 'Diagnostic overshadowing' amongst clinicians working with

people with intellectual disabilities in the UK. Journal of Applied Research in

Intellectual Disabilities, 17(2), 85-90.

Miller, M. L., Fee, V. E., & Jones, C. J. (2004). Psychometric properties of ADHD rating

scales among children with mental retardation II: Validity. Research in

Developmental Disabilities, 25(5), 477-492.

Miller, M. L., Fee, V. E., & Netterville, A. K. (2004). Psychometric properties of ADHD

rating scales among children with mental retardation I: Reliability. Research in

Developmental Disabilities, 25(5), 459-476.

Neece, C., Baker, B., Blacher, J., & Crnic, K. (2011). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

among children with and without intellectual disability: An examination across time.

Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 55(7), 623-635.

173
Norris, M., & Lecavalier, L. (2011). Evaluating the validity of the Nisonger Child Behavior

Rating Form--Parent Version. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(6), 2894-

2900.

Pandolfi, V., Magyar, C. I., & Dill, C. A. (2009). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Child

Behavior Checklist 1.5-5 in a sample of children with autism spectrum disorders.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(7), 986-995.

Pandolfi, V., Magyar, C. I., & Dill, C. A. (2012). An initial psychometric evaluation of the

CBCL 6-18 in a sample of youth with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism

Spectrum Disorders, 6(1), 96-108.

Rhodes, S. M., Riby, D. M., Matthews, K., & Coghill, D. R. (2011). Attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder and Williams syndrome: Shared behavioral and

neuropsychological profiles. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,

33(1), 147-156.

Royal College of Psychiatrists. (2001). DC-LD: Diagnostic criteria for psychiatric disorders

for use with adults with learning disabilities/mental retardation. London: Gaskell.

Sansone, S. M., Widaman, K. F., Hall, S. S., Reiss, A. L., Lightbody, A., Kaufmann, W. E., .

. . Hessl, D. (2012). Psychometric study of the aberrant behavior checklist in fragile X

syndrome and implications for targeted treatment. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 42(7), 1377-1392.

Steele, A., Scerif, G., Cornish, K., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2013). Learning to read in

Williams syndrome and Down syndrome: Syndrome-specific precursors and

developmental trajectories. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(7), 754-

762.

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Needham

Heights, Minnesota: Allyn & Bacon.

174
Willcutt, E. G. (2012). The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a

meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics, 9(3), 490-499.

World Health Organization. (1992). International Classification of Diseases, Tenth edition.

Geneva, Switzerland: Author.

175
Table 1
Principal Components Factor Solution for T-SAID Items
Factor I Factor II Factor III Factor IV
Hyperactivity/impulsivi Loading Inattention Loading Verbal Loading Following Loading
ty communication instructions
1 Plays quietly inside .65 21 Returns to task .85 44 Verbally repeat 1 .87 41 Carry out one .70
after distracted part instruction part instruction
13 Keeps legs to self .62 19 Maintains attention .82 45 Verbally repeat 2 .82 40 Understands .65
without rewards part instruction instructions <10
words
4 Walks rather than .56 12 Persists for 2 .79 46 Recall main .76 37 Stays in school .61
runs minutes points of story grounds
33 Waits until called on .56 23 Timely completion .75 3 Speaks reasonable .73 42 Carry out two .59
of work pace part instruction
10 Keeps hands to self .55 11 Concentrates for 2 .73 16 Stays on topic .67
minutes
25 Waits in line .55 24 Does not get .71 2 Speaks reasonable .65
distracted volume
6 Stays in own seat .54 15 Gives attention .65 28 Asks before .60
joining in
26 Waits his/her turn .52 22 Completes work .64 29 Asking before .58
accurately taking
27 Takes turns playing .50 20 Maintains attention .59 34 Does not interrupt .54
when interested
18 Completes an .54
activity
8 Keeps legs still .50

176
CHAPTER 10 EVALUATION OF A NEW ATTENTION

RATING SCALE ACROSS NEURODEVELOPMENTAL

DISORDERS: THE SCALE OF ATTENTION IN

INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY

177
10.1 Declaration
In the case of Chapter 10, contributions to the work involved the following:

Name % contribution Nature of contribution


Nerelie Freeman 70% Formulation of project design, data
collection, data analysis, and writing
manuscript.
A/Prof Kylie Gray 15% Contribution to development of
project design, discussion of ideas
expressed in manuscript, and critical
review of manuscript.
Prof Kim Cornish 15% Contribution to development of
project design, discussion of ideas
expressed in manuscript, and critical
review of manuscript.

Declaration by co-authors:

(1) they meet the criteria for authorship in that they have participated in the conception,
execution, or interpretation, of at least that part of the publication in their field of expertise;
(2) they take public responsibility for their part of the publication, except for the responsible
author who accepts overall responsibility for the publication;
(3) there are no other authors of the publication according to these criteria;
(4) potential conflicts of interest have been disclosed to (a) granting bodies, (b) the editor or
publisher of journals or other publications, and (c) the head of the responsible academic unit;
and
(5) the original data are stored at the following location(s) and will be held for at least five
years from the date indicated below:

Location: Centre for Developmental Psychiatry and Psychology, Department of


Psychiatry, Southern Clinical School, Monash University, Clayton Campus

Date

Signature 1

Signature 2

Signature 3

178
10.2 Paper commentary

Chapter 10 presents a paper that has been submitted for publication in the Journal of

Autism and Developmental Disorders. This paper has been formatted to the specific

requirements of the journal. Pages have been re-numbered to provide consistency throughout

the thesis.

Paper 3 contrasted the hyperactive, impulsive and inattentive behaviours of 59

children with autism spectrum disorder, 41 children with Down Syndrome and 76 children

with idiopathic intellectual disability using data from the psychometric and factor analytic

studies (Papers 1 and 2). While neuropsychological research has helped enhance our

understanding of the cognitive phenotype of attention across neurodevelopmental disorders,

less is known about the behavioural phenotype due to limited research, inconsistent findings,

and the use of rating scales that were not appropriate for the population. More research was

needed to investigate the behavioural phenotype of ADHD symptoms across children with

different neurodevelopmental disorders using a reliable and valid rating scale.

179
EVALUATION OF A NEW ATTENTION RATING SCALE ACROSS

NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS: THE SCALE OF ATTENTION IN

INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY

Abstract

Whilst neuropsychological research has enhanced our understanding of inattentive and

hyperactive behaviours among children with neurodevelopmental disorders, the absence of

rating scales developed for those who have intellectual disability continues to be a gap in

knowledge. This study examined these behaviours in 176 children with autism spectrum

disorder (ASD), Down Syndrome, or idiopathic ID using a newly developed teacher rating

scale, the Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability. Findings suggested that children with

ASD had a significantly greater breadth of hyperactive/impulsive behaviours than those with

Down Syndrome or ID. These findings support existing research suggesting differing profiles

of attention and activity across neurodevelopmental disorders. Understanding disorder-

specific profiles has implications for developing strategies to support students with ID in the

classroom.

Key words: autism spectrum disorder, attention-deficit hyperactivity symptoms, rating scale,

teacher ratings, children, Down Syndrome, intellectual disability

180
The acquisition of skills such as the ability to sustain attention on a task, inhibit

impulsive actions and outbursts, and maintain attention in the presence of distractors are all

important developmental milestones and help predict academic outcomes (Metcalfe, Harvey,

& Laws, 2013). Children with significant deficits in these areas may be diagnosed with

attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) which is associated with adverse long term

outcomes such as lower academic achievement (Barkley, et al., 2006; Polderman, et al.,

2010), friendship difficulties (Normand, et al., 2007) and lower socioeconomic status (Joseph

Biederman et al., 2012). Considerable research has focused on identification, treatment, and

interventions for children with ADHD in an effort to ameliorate these outcomes, at least

among typically developing children.

Comparatively less research has examined the impacts of this disorder in children

with intellectual disability (ID). This may be due to the mistaken belief that attention

difficulties were part of the presentation of ID (Antshel, et al., 2006; Guerin, et al., 2009), a

phenomenon known as diagnostic overshadowing bias. This term refers to the tendency of

clinicians, in the presence of ID, to regard accompanying mental health issues as less salient

and specific than they would if the child were typically developing (Jopp & Keys, 2001;

Mason & Scior, 2004). Another contributing factor may be the exclusion criteria in the DSM-

IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) which did not allow clinicians to diagnose

ADHD in the presence of some neurodevelopmental disorders.

The beginning of this century saw momentum starting to gather in the field of

developmental psychology through greater recognition and identification of the impact of

ADHD symptoms among individuals with ID (e.g., Antshel, et al., 2006; Chadwick, et al.,

2005). Neuropsychological research has also made a significant contribution to our

understanding in this area by challenging the notion that children with ID and attention

181
difficulties formed a homogenous group, and have since suggested that there are divergent

trajectories in the development of these behaviours across neurodevelopmental disorders

(e.g., Cornish, Scerif, et al., 2007; Scerif, et al., 2012). Further, this research has revealed that

while rating scales and structured interviews may present behavioural symptoms of

inattention as being similar across disorders, these may not necessarily translate into identical

cognitive attention mechanisms (see Cornish & Wilding, 2010 for a comprehensive review).

An examination of the subcomponents of attention (including sustained, divided and selective

attention; Petersen & Posner, 2012) has provided us with an even richer understanding of

these differences to help us further understand the unique challenges faced by children across

neurodevelopmental disorders.

The current study focuses on children with two neurodevelopmental disorders that have

contrasting profiles of hyperactivity and inattention: Down Syndrome and autism spectrum

disorder. The selection of these two groups was guided by the high prevalence of ADHD

symptomatology in children with these disorders (e.g., Cornish, Steele, et al., 2012; Ekstein,

et al., 2011; Rommelse, et al., 2010; Witwer & Lecavalier, 2010). Down Syndrome (DS) is

the most common genetic syndrome causing ID due to a third copy of chromosome 21

(trisomy 21; McInerny, et al., 2009). Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a childhood-onset

developmental disability characterised by deficits in social communication and restricted,

repetitive patterns of behaviour (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Approximately

70-80% have severe cognitive delays, many functioning in the moderate to severe ID range

(Fombonne, 2005).

At the behavioural level, recent studies have suggested a high proportion of children

in both groups have ADHD symptoms (Ekstein, et al., 2011; Mahan & Matson, 2011; Witwer

& Lecavalier, 2010). Children with ASD and DS have both been identified as having

difficulties with inattention (Cornish, Steele, et al., 2012; Estes, et al., 2007; Papaeliou, et al.,

182
2012; Sinzig, et al., 2009; van Gameren-Oosterom, et al., 2011), but difficulties with

hyperactivity have only been reported in children with ASD (Estes, et al., 2007; D. O. Lee &

Ousley, 2006; Sinzig, et al., 2009). Within the ASD group, some studies have suggested

ADHD symptoms of similar severity across the spectrum (Kaat, et al., 2013; Mahan &

Matson, 2011; Witwer & Lecavalier, 2010). Others have suggested that these symptoms are

more severe in children with ASD and ID than those who are high functioning (Holtmann,

Bolte, & Poustka, 2007; Sinzig, et al., 2009).

At the cognitive level, sustained attention has been identified as an area of strength in

both groups (Breckenridge, et al., 2013; Cornish, Scerif, et al., 2007; Johnson, et al., 2007). In

the area of selective attention, children with ASD have been reported to perform comparable

to (Iarocci & Burack, 2004) or better than (Jarrold, et al., 2005; Joseph, et al., 2009) their

typically developing peers. In contrast, Cornish and colleagues (2007) suggested a

developmental trajectory for individuals with DS where toddlers perform similarly to their

typically developing peers (matched for mental age), but that this skill deteriorates in

childhood before improving again as they move into adulthood.

As demonstrated above, while research at the cognitive level has broadened our

understanding of this phenotype of attention and activity across neurodevelopmental

disorders, the behavioural phenotype has shown less evidence of progressing forward. This

has been compounded by limited and inconsistent findings across studies which may be due

to the lack of appropriate measures to examine ADHD symptoms. Neuropsychological

studies have often used measures developed specifically for children with ID (although see

Hooper, et al., 2008 who reported that even when using simple tasks the floor effects

precluded skills from being measured in some children with severe or profound ID) whereas

the majority of behavioural studies have used rating scales designed for typically developing

children such as the Conners Rating Scales (Conners, 1989, 1997, 2008) or the Child

183
Behaviour Checklist (Achenbach, 1991; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). Few of the studies

cited above (i.e., Estes, et al., 2007; Kaat, et al., 2013; Witwer & Lecavalier, 2010) utilised a

rating scale that has been developed for children with ID such as the Aberrant Behaviour

Checklist (Aman & Singh, 1994). Given that children with ID may present with ADHD

symptoms at the behavioural level that are quite different to those who are typically

developing (Freeman, Gray, Taffe, & Cornish, 2013a), the sensitivity of rating scales used to

capture ADHD symptoms in many of the studies cited above therefore need to be called into

question. This was also noted by Witwer and Lecavalier (2010), who reported that the

behaviours Interrupts others and Pushes their way into groups were frequently endorsed by

parents of children with ASD and ID but questioned whether these behaviours were a

function of the children’s deficits in social skills, rather than being ADHD symptoms per se.

This highlights the importance of using rating scales containing items appropriate to the ID

population.

Collectively, while recognition of ADHD symptoms among children with ID

represented a step forward, our understanding of these symptoms at the behavioural level has

been clouded by limited research, inconsistent findings and use of rating scales that are not

appropriate for the population. In the present study, we aimed to compare the range of ADHD

symptoms in children with ASD, DS and idiopathic ID ranging from mild to profound

impairment using a new attention rating scale, the Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability

(SAID; Freeman, et al., 2013a). It was hypothesized that different attention and activity

profiles could be identified across groups using this new scale. It was also hypothesized that

the SAID would be significantly better at identifying profiles of attention and activity across

groups compared with the Conners 3 (Conners, 2008).

Method

184
Participants

Two hundred and fifteen consent forms were returned by families of children

attending special (n = 156), mainstream (n = 32) or autism specific (n = 2) schools in the

Melbourne metropolitan area or across regional Victoria (Australia). A small number of

children were on a split placement (spending part of their school week in a mainstream

school and part of their time in a special school; n = 12) or were located in a support centre

for children with intellectual disabilities on a mainstream school site (n = 7). Children were

eligible to participate in the study if: (i) they were aged between 5 and 13 years; and (ii) their

most recent cognitive assessment placed their functioning in the intellectually disabled range

(i.e., their cognitive and/or adaptive living skills assessment total score was less than 70).

Children with a diagnosis of ASD were only included in the sample if they scored above the

recommended cutoff for autism (i.e., 15 or more) on the lifetime version of the Social

Communication Questionnaire (Rutter, Bailey, et al., 2003). A total of 181 students were

deemed eligible to participate in the study (117 males and 64 females).

Socioeconomic status was determined via parental completion of the Hollingshead

Four Factor Index (Hollingshead, 1975). This measure has been found to yield comparable

information to more recently developed SES measures (Cirino, et al., 2002) but has the

advantages of being simple to complete and less time-consuming. Scores are averaged across

ratings for both parents and an overall score is calculated. A higher score indicates a higher

level of socioeconomic status.

Measures

Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence – Third edition/Wechsler

Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth edition (WPPSI-III/WISC-IV; Wechsler, 2002;

Wechsler, 2003). The WPPSI-III and WISC-IV are established, reliable and valid measures

of intellectual ability with good to excellent internal consistency (α = .85 – .95) and strong

185
test-retest reliability (r = .80 – .95). They were used in this study to determine each child’s

current cognitive functioning.

Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales, Second edition – Teacher Rating Form

(VABS-II-T; Sparrow, et al., 2005). The VABS-II-T is a reliable and valid measure of

adaptive behaviour with acceptable to excellent internal consistency (α = .74 – .98) and

moderate to very strong test-retest reliability (r = .43 – .97). It was used in this study to

determine the current adaptive functioning of children who were unable to complete the

WPPSI-III or WISC-IV.

Social Communication Questionnaire – Lifetime version (SCQ; Rutter, Bailey, et

al., 2003). The SCQ, previously known as the Autism Screening Questionnaire (ASQ;

Berument, et al., 1999), is a 40-item questionnaire completed by the parent/primary caregiver

that examines the areas of communication, socialization, and restricted and repetitive

behaviour and interests. The SCQ was completed by families of children with a diagnosis of

ASD and was used to determine eligibility to participate in the study.

Conners Rating Scales – Third edition, Teacher Short form (Conners 3; Conners,

2008). The Conners 3 has 39 items and screens for symptoms of ADHD and related

disorders. It has five subscales: inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, learning

problems/executive functioning, aggression and peer relations. It was developed for use with

typically developing children and was not developed for use with children who have ID

(Conners, 2008). The Conners 3 was completed by each child’s current teacher. It was used

to examine its ability to detect differences in ADHD symptoms across groups compared with

the author’s new rating scale described below.

Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability (Teacher version) (T-SAID; Freeman,

et al., 2013a; Freeman, Gray, Taffe, & Cornish, 2013b). This newly developed rating scale

for teachers consists of 44 items with four subscales: hyperactivity/impulsivity, inattention,

186
verbal communication and following instructions. These items were developed with specific

consideration for behaviours that would be observed in children with ID. The items were

generated through behaviour observations, consultation with teachers and health

professionals, and a review of existing rating scales. Teachers respond to each statement on a

3-point scale of never/rarely, sometimes and often. All items are worded positively as it has

been proposed that this may help to improve response rates of parents and teachers when

reporting on children's behaviours (Goodman, 1997). Lower scores on the T-SAID relate to

greater difficulties. The subscales have good to excellent internal consistency (α = .79 – .94)

and strong test-test reliability over 30 days (ICC = .86 – .96; Freeman, et al., 2013a, 2013b).

Strong convergent validity with corresponding subscales on the Conners Rating Scales –

Third edition, Teacher Short form (r = .66 – .69) and the Developmental Behaviour Checklist

(r = .67) have also been reported (Freeman, et al., 2013a). The T-SAID was completed by

each child’s current teacher.

Procedure

Ethics approval was obtained from the Monash University Standing Committee on

Ethics in Research Involving Humans, the Victorian Department of Education and Early

Childhood Development, and the Catholic Education Office Melbourne.

Participants were selected through a three-stage process. In the first stage, children

were recruited from several sources. School principals were invited to assist with recruitment

for the study. Families at consenting schools were sent home an envelope containing a poster,

explanatory statement and consent form. Families who consented to participate returned the

consent form in a reply-paid envelope. Support groups and community organisations were

also approached to assist with recruitment and they advertised the study on their web site.

Interested families contacted the research team directly by phone or email, and information

was mailed out as described above.

187
In the second stage, a member of the research team telephoned each consenting family

to determine their child’s eligibility to participate in the study. Basic demographic

information was obtained, as well as clinical information including their child’s primary

diagnostic status, comorbid diagnoses, and any medication they were currently prescribed.

If their child had received a cognitive and/or adaptive behaviour assessment in the

past they were asked to provide these results to the research team. A cognitive assessment

was conducted if the child had not been administered one in the last 18 months (WPPSI-III)

or 2 years (WISC-IV). If a child was deemed untestable using the WPPSI-III or WISC-IV,

then the VABS-II total Adaptive Behaviour Composite score (ABC) was used to determine

the severity of impairment.

In the third stage, the classroom teacher was mailed a booklet of rating scales. It was a

requirement that each teacher who completed them had known the child for a minimum of 6

months. They were asked to complete a number of rating scales including the Conners 3

(Conners, 2008) and the T-SAID (Freeman, et al., 2013a, 2013b). Questionnaires were

returned to the research team in a reply-paid envelope. Reminder letters or emails were sent

to teachers if questionnaires had not been returned within four weeks, and a second reminder

was sent if they had not been returned within six weeks.

Parents of children with ASD were asked to complete the lifetime version of the SCQ

(Rutter, Bailey, et al., 2003). This questionnaire was mailed to families to complete in their

own time and was returned via a reply-paid envelope.

Analysis

The grouping for level of ID was determined by using the child’s FSIQ from the

WPPSI-III or WISC-IV, or the ABC from the VABS-II for children who were untestable on

the cognitive assessment. Severity of ID was defined using the criteria in Sattler (2001): mild

188
ID (55 – 70), moderate ID (40 – 54), and severe/profound (< 40). Eight children for whom

severity of ID could not be determined were excluded from the regression analyses.

Scores on the T-SAID were reversed for analysis so that higher scores were indicative

of greater difficulties. The total score was calculated by taking the mean of all the items

(known as the Mean Item Score, or MIS). This method has a number of advantages over

calculating the sum of all item scores (Taffe, et al., 2008). One advantage is that the MIS may

be deconstructed to measure the breadth of behaviours an individual exhibits (the Proportion

of Items Checked, or PIC) and the intensity at which the items are checked for that person

(the Intensity Index, or II). PIC is the proportion of recoded items receiving codes of 1 or 2,

indicating that the corresponding items indicated problematic behaviours. The II is the

proportion of items scored 2 among the 1 or 2 coded items.

Correlates of ADHD symptomatology on T-SAID scores were entered into a multiple

regression analysis including gender, chronological age, SES, level of ID and diagnostic

group. For level of ID, mild ID was used as the comparison group and for diagnostic group,

idiopathic ID was used as the comparison group.

Results

Of the 79 students in the idiopathic ID group (hereafter referred to as the ID group),

76 classroom teachers returned the rating scale booklet yielding a return rate of 96.2%. Of the

61 students with ASD, rating scales were returned for 59 students (96.7%). Rating scales

were returned for all 41 students in the DS group. This yielded a total sample of 176 students

(114 males, 62 females) with a mean age of 9.15 years. The demographic characteristics of

the sample are described in Table 1. There were no significant differences in age (p = .71) or

SES (p = .46) across diagnostic groups. There was a higher proportion of males in the ASD

group compared with the other two groups (χ2 = 10.91, p < .01). A higher proportion of

189
children in the ASD group were nonverbal compared with the other two groups (χ2 = 24.12, p

< .001).

[Insert Table 1 about here]

Across groups, there was no significant difference in gender (p = .82), age (p = .77),

or SES (p = .42) for the students whose teachers returned the rating scales and those who did

not.

Comorbid psychopathology

Across the total sample, 31 parents (17.1%) reported their child as having at least one

comorbid diagnosis. The most common comorbid diagnosis was ADHD, with 20 children

having this diagnosis (12 from the ASD group, 8 from the ID group; 11.4%). Thirteen

children were currently taking medication for ADHD (65.0%), of which nine were taking

methylphenidate, two were taking dexamphetamine, one was taking clonidine, and one was

taking atomoxetine. Three had never been prescribed medication (16.7%), and 3 had taken

medication in the past but were not doing so at the present time (16.7%). Other commonly

reported comorbid diagnoses included epilepsy (n = 17; 9.4%) and anxiety (n = 6; 3.3%).

Cognitive ability

One hundred and twenty-three children (69.9%) were able to complete at least part of

a cognitive assessment. The means, standard deviations and ranges for the assessment broken

down by group are described in Table 2. For those children for whom a Full Scale Score

(FSIQ) could be derived (n = 107), their mean FSIQ was 53.37, placing them in the

moderately intellectually disabled range. Across groups, the children with DS had a

significantly lower FSIQ than the other 2 groups (F2,104 = 19.05, p < .001). For those children

190
administered the WISC-IV (n = 95), the children with DS had significantly lower scores on

all four Indices: Verbal Comprehension (F2,83 = 5.89, p < .01), Perceptual Reasoning (F2,89 =

18.15, p < .001), Working Memory (F2,84 = 6.10, p < .01) and Processing Speed (F2,79 =

12.24, p < .001). In the younger students administered the WPPSI-III (n = 28), there was no

difference across groups for the Performance Scale (p = .12), but children with DS had a

significantly lower Verbal Score (F2,21 = 3.65, p < .05).

One hundred and nine students had the Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales

completed by their teacher. The means, standard deviations and ranges for the assessment

broken down by group are described in Table 2. Their mean Adaptive Behaviour Composite

(ABC) was 54.66 placing them in the moderately intellectually disabled range. Children in

the ID group had a significantly higher ABC than the other two groups (F2,106 = 3.98, p <

.05). There were no significant differences across diagnostic groups on the Communication

Scale (p = .47). Children in the DS group had a significantly lower Daily Living Skills Score

(F2,104 = 4.72, p < .01) than the other two groups. Children in the ASD group had a

significantly lower score on the Socialisation Scale than the other two groups (F2,104 = 8.32, p

< .001). The overall scores for both assessments broken down by diagnosis are described in

Table 2.

[Insert Table 2 about here]

Internal consistency

The internal consistency of the T-SAID was examined to determine whether it had

adequate reliability across neurodevelopmental disorders. As shown in Table 3 below, the T-

SAID had good to excellent internal consistency on the hyperactivity/impulsivity, inattention

and verbal communication subscales (α = .87 – .95). It had fair internal consistency on the

191
following instructions subscale (α = .74 – .81). Given that these results are similar to the

internal consistency reported for the combined sample (Freeman, et al., 2013b), the T-SAID

subscales were deemed appropriate as a method of comparison across neurodevelopmental

disorders.

[Insert Table 3 about here]

T-SAID scores across groups

Regression analyses were conducted to examine the T-SAID total score across

groups, controlling for age, gender, SES, and severity of ID. As shown in Table 4, the

proportion of items checked (PIC) on the hyperactivity/impulsivity subscale of the T-SAID

(possible range 0-2) was significantly greater for children with ASD. On average, 11% (p <

.05) more items were marked sometimes or rarely/never for those with ASD than for those

with ID. There were no differences across groups on this subscale for the mean item score

(MIS) or intensity index (II). This suggests that while the children with ASD exhibited a

greater breadth of hyperactive/impulsive behaviours, these were not noticeably more severe

than children in the other groups.

The MIS on the verbal communication subscale was greater by .21 for those with

ASD than for those with mild ID, but only by .09 for those with DS. A similar pattern was

evident in the regressions of the II (on a 0-1 scale), but there were no differences in the PIC.

This suggests that the higher MIS was due to verbal communication skills being noticeably

more limited among children with ASD. There were no differences across groups on the

inattention or following instructions subscales.

[Insert Table 4 about here]

192
The Conners 3 scores revealed no significant differences across groups for the

hyperactivity or inattention subscales. When examining the proportion of children within the

clinical range for ADHD symptomatology, again there were no differences across diagnostic

groups for the hyperactive, inattentive, or combined subtypes.

Discussion

The findings of this study suggest that children with ASD display a significantly

greater breadth of hyperactive/impulsive behaviours than children with ID or DS, which is

consistent with the findings of a previous study with adolescents (Bradley & Isaacs, 2006).

These difficulties were independent of gender, age and severity of ID. The findings also

suggest that hyperactive/impulsive symptoms significantly decrease with age in children with

ID, similar to the trend noted in children who are typically developing (Biederman, et al.,

2000; Biederman, et al., 2006). Children with severe/profound ID were also found to have a

significantly greater breadth and intensity of hyperactive/impulsive behaviours, a finding

which has been discussed previously by the authors (Freeman, et al., 2013a).

The suggestion that children with ASD have a significantly greater breadth of

hyperactive/impulsive behaviours compared with other neurodevelopmental disorders

provides support for the removal of the current exclusionary criteria in the ICD-10 that does

not permit dual diagnoses of ASD and ADHD (World Health Organization, 1992). Further, it

supports the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and the Diagnostic Criteria for

Psychiatric Disorders for use with adults with Learning Disabilities/Mental Retardation (DC-

LD; Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2001) which do allow for these comorbid diagnoses to be

made.

193
The case for removing such exclusionary criteria would appear even more convincing

when looking at our sample which included children with other types of ID who had lower

levels of hyperactivity than the ASD group, even though paradoxically no exclusionary

criteria exist that preclude a comorbid diagnosis of ADHD in these groups. With twelve

children in our sample being diagnosed with both ASD and ADHD, clearly some clinicians

have made this dual diagnosis, and two-thirds of these children are taking medication to

manage these symptoms. Given this exclusionary criteria has been removed in the DSM-5

(American Psychiatric Association, 2013), it is hoped that this will give other clinicians

“permission” to make a dual diagnosis if they feel that it is warranted. This may in turn

enable more children to access treatments to help manage these symptoms.

This study also found that children with ASD had significantly greater difficulties

with behaviours that make up the Verbal Communication subscale than children with DS or

ID, and that the intensity of these behaviours was also significantly greater. This subscale

includes behaviours such as Can verbally repeat back an instruction that has one step and

Asks before joining in a game. This is likely to be a reflection of language ability, given that

children with ASD often have impairments in verbal language skills (Luyster, Seery, Talbott,

& Tager-Flusberg, 2011; Seltzer, Shattuck, Abbeduto, & Greenberg, 2004). While children

with DS are also known to experience language difficulties (Luyster, et al., 2011) , the

findings from the present study would suggest their inattentive and impulsive behaviours as

they manifest in verbal communication may be comparatively stronger than those with ASD.

Further, the intensity of difficulty would naturally be greater in the ASD group given a higher

proportion of these children were non-verbal and would be unable to demonstrate their skills

on these items.

While differences across groups for hyperactivity/impulsivity were found using the T-

SAID (Freeman, Cornish, & Gray, 2012), the Conners 3 (Conners, 2008) did not detect such

194
differences. This finding is not surprising given that the Conners 3 (Conners, 2008)– or

indeed previous versions of the Conners (Conners, 1989, 1997) – was not developed for

children within the intellectually disabled range. Items on existing rating scales describe

behaviours related to hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention that are not often observed in

children with ID such as Dislikes it when phone is engaged when trying to call someone.

Other items relate to behaviours that have limited relevance such as Puts off

homework/projects to the last minute as such activities are rarely, if ever, asked of children

with ID. The assumption that a child is able to speak or verbalise also compromises the

validity of many rating scales, which often include items such as Talks too much or too fast.

Such items could not be rated in a significant proportion of people with severe and profound

ID who are non-verbal (Deb, et al., 2008). The T-SAID contains items that are more

developmentally appropriate and readily observable in children functioning within the ID

range, and avoids the use of vague terms such as Excitable, impulsive or Restless, overactive.

The inclusion of clarifiers and examples in the T-SAID further illustrate the behaviours of

interest and aim to minimise the degree of subjectivity when rating each item (Reid & Maag,

1994). Findings of this study emphasise the need for the development and use of measures

developed specifically for children with ID. Use of measures designed for typically

developing children may result in misleading or potentially inaccurate findings and

conclusions.

Many studies continue to use instruments such as the Child Behaviour Checklist

(CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) or the Conners (Conners, 1989, 1997, 2008) that have

limited or questionable validity when examining inattention and hyperactivity among the ID

population (Koskentausta, et al., 2004; Turk, 1998), with the shortcomings being reportedly

even more marked when using teacher ratings (Deb, et al., 2008). Therefore, the findings of

this study reinforce the importance and utility of using instruments that are clinically valid for

195
the population of interest. Using tools developed for typically developing children may mask

important differences in symptom presentation and behaviour that exist in children who have

ID.

It should be noted that while the results of this study suggest differences across

neurodevelopmental disorders in the area of hyperactivity/impulsivity, a rating scale alone is

insufficient to identify difficulties or to formulate a diagnosis, particularly when only

collected from one source. Irrespective of whether a rating scale can identify behavioural

phenotypes, it is best practice to use this information in conjunction with developmental

history, parent ratings, behavioural observations and results from neuropsychological tests

(e.g., Wilding Monster Card Sorting task; Wilding, Munir, & Cornish, 2001) which would

provide a more complete profile of these differences across disorders.

The findings of the present study suggest that the new rating scale developed by the

authors, the T-SAID (Freeman, et al., 2013a), has the ability to detect differences in ADHD

symptomatology in children with ID. Given the lack of reliable and valid rating scales that

have normative data to measure ADHD symptoms among children within the intellectually

disabled range (Deb, et al., 2008), the T-SAID has the potential to become a valuable tool

that can be used by clinicians and researchers not only to detect difficulties within this

population, but also to detect differences across neurodevelopmental disorders.

The value of using a teacher rating scale to measure inattention and hyperactivity is

that teachers, unlike parents, have the opportunity to observe large groups of children

engaging in goal-directed tasks. They can draw on behaviours they have observed both in the

present cohort of students being taught and past students, and can use this information to

make inferences about what behaviours deviate from the norm (Gadow, et al., 2006).

Teachers potentially have a wider exposure to children’s behaviour compared with parents

who may only know the behaviours of their own children and may consider unusual or

196
challenging behaviours to be “normal” because they have no basis of comparison (K.

Sullivan, et al., 2006). Children’s behaviour in school can also be markedly different from at

home due to the expectation of following instructions from a teacher and engaging in

prescribed activities where an outcome is expected, unlike at home where some children may

engage in fewer goal-directed activities and/or more free play where such difficulties are less

likely to be observed (Murray, et al., 2007; Tandon, et al., 2009).

One of the limitations of this study is that teachers of the children with a diagnosis of

ADHD were not naïve when completing the rating scales. This may have led them to rate the

student differently compared with those teachers whose students had similar difficulties but

had not undergone a professional evaluation. Another limitation is that medication use was

not formally collected or monitored among children with an ADHD diagnosis, and was

dependent upon parent report. For those children who were reported to be on medication, this

study did not control for dosage, length of time child had been on the medication, or whether

it was used continuously or sporadically. Assuming that the medication taken managed the

frequency and intensity of ADHD symptoms (Antshel, et al., 2006), the behaviours of these

children may have been qualitatively different from those in the study with hyperactivity or

inattention who did not take medication to manage these symptoms. A third limitation is that

while the T-SAID subscales were found to have adequate internal consistency across

neurodevelopmental disorders, it drew upon relatively small samples of children in each

group. Future studies need to look at the larger samples to confirm the stability of this factor

structure across neurodevelopmental disorders given that ADHD symptomatology can

present differently across diagnoses (Cornish, Scerif, et al., 2007).

Overall, the pattern of results reported in this paper supports research suggesting that

attention and activity levels are not homogenous in children with ID. Different trajectories

may exist across neurodevelopmental disorders and across development (Cornish & Wilding,

197
2010; Karmiloff-Smith, 2009). While this study suggests differences in hyperactive and

impulsive behaviours across children with ASD, DS and ID, further longitudinal and cross-

sectional studies are needed to yield more conclusive findings regarding inattention,

hyperactivity, and the differences across known and idiopathic causes of ID. An

understanding of these differences would also be beneficial for teachers and school settings

as it will enable them to develop strategies and implement interventions to improve learning

of students experiencing these difficulties in the classroom, which in turn will enhance long-

term outcomes.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the Apex Foundation for Research into

Intellectual Disability Ltd. (Victoria, Australia) for funding assistance to conduct this study,

and Ms Kristina Clarke for her assistance with data collection and entry.

198
References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile.

Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and

profiles. Burlington, VT: Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment.

Aman, M. G., & Singh, N. N. (1994). Aberrant Behaviour Checklist - Community

supplementary manual. East Aurora, New York: Slosson Educational Publications.

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders - Fourth edition Text revision: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders - Fifth edition: DSM-5. Arlington, Virginia: Author.

Antshel, K. M., Phillips, M. H., Gordon, M., Barkley, R., & Faraone, S. V. (2006). Is ADHD

a valid disorder in children with intellectual delays? Clinical Psychology Review,

26(5), 555-572.

Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2006). Young adult outcome of

hyperactive children: Adaptive functioning in major life activities. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(2), 192-202.

Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bailey, A. (1999). Autism screening

questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175(5), 444-451.

Biederman, J., Mick, E., & Faraone, S. V. (2000). Age-dependent decline of symptoms of

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Impact of remission definition and symptom

type. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(5), 816-818.

Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Mick, E., Spencer, T., Wilens, T. E., Silva, J. M., et al.

(2006). Young adult outcome of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A controlled

10-year follow-up study. Psychological Medicine, 36(2), 167-179.

199
Biederman, J., Petty, C. R., Woodworth, K., Lomedico, A., Hyder, L. L., & Faraone, S. V.

(2012). Adult outcome of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A controlled 16-

year follow-up study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 73(7), 941-950.

Bradley, E. A., & Isaacs, B. J. (2006). Inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity in teenagers

with intellectual disabilities, with and without autism. Canadian Journal of

Psychiatry, 51(9), 598-606.

Breckenridge, K., Braddick, O., Anker, S., Woodhouse, M., & Atkinson, J. (2013). Attention

in Williams syndrome and Down's syndrome: Performance on the new early

childhood attention battery. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31(Pt 2),

257-269.

Chadwick, O., Kusel, Y., Cuddy, M., & Taylor, E. (2005). Psychiatric diagnoses and

behaviour problems from childhood to early adolescence in young people with severe

intellectual disabilities. Psychological Medicine, 35(5), 751-760.

Cirino, P. T., Chin, C. E., Sevcik, R. A., Wolf, M., Lovett, M., & Morris, R. D. (2002).

Measuring socioeconomic status: Reliability and preliminary validity for different

approaches. Assessment, 9(2), 145-155.

Conners, C. K. (1989). Conners Rating Scales Manual. New York: Multi-Health Systems

Inc.

Conners, C. K. (1997). Conners' Rating Scales - Revised Technical Manual. New York:

Multi-Health Systems Inc.

Conners, C. K. (2008). Conners 3 Manual. New York: Multi-Health Systems Inc.

Cornish, K., Scerif, G., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2007). Tracing syndrome-specific trajectories

of attention across the lifespan. Cortex, 43(6), 672-685.

200
Cornish, K., Steele, A., Monteiro, C. R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Scerif, G. (2012). Attention

deficits predict phenotypic outcomes in syndrome-specific and domain-specific ways.

Frontiers in Psychology, 11(3), 227.

Cornish, K., & Wilding, J. (2010). Attention, genes and developmental disorders. New York:

Oxford University Press.

de Bildt, A., Kraijer, D., Sytema, S., & Minderaa, R. (2005). The psychometric properties of

the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales in children and adolescents with mental

retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(1), 53-62.

Deb, S., Dhaliwal, A. J., & Roy, M. (2008). The usefulness of Conners' Rating Scales -

Revised in screening for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children with

intellectual disabilities and borderline intelligence. Journal of Intellectual Disability

Research, 52(11), 950-965.

Ekstein, S., Glick, B., Weill, M., Kay, B., & Berger, I. (2011). Down syndrome and attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Child Neurology, 26(10), 1290-

1295.

Estes, A. M., Dawson, G., Sterling, L., & Munson, J. (2007). Level of intellectual functioning

predicts patterns of associated symptoms in school-age children with autism spectrum

disorder. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 112(6), 439-449.

Fombonne, E. (2005). Epidemiological studies of autism and pervasive developmental

disorders. In F. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism

and pervasive developmental disorders (3rd ed.) (pp. 42-69). New York: Wiley and

Sons.

Freeman, N. C., Cornish, K. M., & Gray, K. M. (2012). Attention and activity profiles in

children with intellectual disability: Development of a teacher rating scale. Journal of

Intellectual Disability Research, 56(7/8), 712.

201
Freeman, N. C., Gray, K. M., Taffe, J. R., & Cornish, K. (2013a). Development of a new

attention rating scale for children with intellectual disabilities: The Scale of Attention

in Intellectual Disability (SAID). American Journal on Intellectual and

Developmental Disabilities, Manuscript submitted for publication.

Freeman, N. C., Gray, K. M., Taffe, J. R., & Cornish, K. (2013b). The Scale of Attention in

Intellectual Disability (SAID): Factor analysis of a new rating scale for use with

children with intellectual disabilities. American Journal on Intellectual and

Developmental Disabilities, Manuscript submitted for publication.

Gadow, K. D., DeVincent, C. J., & Pomeroy, J. (2006). ADHD symptom subtypes in children

with pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders, 36(2), 271-283.

Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal

of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 38(5), 581-586.

Guerin, S., Buckley, S., McEvoy, J., Hillery, J., & Dodd, P. (2009). The psychometric

properties of the Attention-Distraction, Inhibition-Excitation Classroom Assessment

Scale (ADIECAS) in a sample of children with moderate and severe intellectual

disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30(4), 727-734.

Hollingshead, B. A. (1975). Four factor index of socioeconomic status. New Haven, CT:

Yale University.

Holtmann, M., Bolte, S., & Poustka, F. (2007). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

symptoms in Pervasive Developmental Disorders: Association with autistic behavior

domains and coexisting psychopathology. Psychopathology, 40(3), 172-177.

Hooper, S. R., Hatton, D., Sideris, J., Sullivan, K., Hammer, J., Schaaf, J., et al. (2008).

Executive functions in young males with fragile X syndrome in comparison to mental

202
age-matched controls: Baseline findings from a longitudinal study. Neuropsychology,

22(1), 36-47.

Iarocci, G., & Burack, J. A. (2004). Intact covert orienting to peripheral cues among children

with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 257-264.

Jarrold, C., Gilchrist, I. D., & Bender, A. (2005). Embedded figures detection in autism and

typical development: Preliminary evidence of a double dissociation in relationships

with visual search. Developmental Science, 8(4), 344-351.

Johnson, K. A., Robertson, I. H., Kelly, S. P., Silk, T. J., Barry, E., Daibhis, A., et al. (2007).

Dissociation in performance of children with ADHD and high-functioning autism on

a task of sustained attention. Neuropsychologia, 45(10), 2234-2245.

Jopp, D. A., & Keys, C. B. (2001). Diagnostic overshadowing reviewed and reconsidered.

American Journal on Mental Retardation, 106(5), 416-433.

Joseph, R. M., Keehn, B., Connolly, C., Wolfe, J. M., & Horowitz, T. S. (2009). Why is

visual search superior in autism spectrum disorder? Developmental Science, 12(6),

1083-1096.

Kaat, A. J., Lecavalier, L., & Aman, M. (2013). Validity of the Aberrant Behavior Checklist

in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders.

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Nativism versus neuroconstructivism: Rethinking the study of

developmental disorders. Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 56-63.

Koskentausta, T., Iivanainen, M., & Almqvist, F. (2004). CBCL in the assessment of

psychopathology in Finnish children with intellectual disability. Research in

Developmental Disabilities, 25(4), 341-354.

203
Lee, D. O., & Ousley, O. Y. (2006). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in a

clinic sample of children and adolescents with pervasive developmental disorders.

Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 16(6), 737-746.

Luyster, R. J., Seery, A., Talbott, M. R., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2011). Identifying early-risk

markers and developmental trajectories for language impairment in

neurodevelopmental disorders. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 17(2),

151-159.

Mahan, S., & Matson, J. L. (2011). Children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders

compared to typically developing controls on the Behavioral Assessment System for

Children, Second Edition (BASC-2). Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1),

119-125.

Mason, J., & Scior, K. (2004). 'Diagnostic overshadowing' amongst clinicians working with

people with intellectual disabilities in the UK. Journal of Applied Research in

Intellectual Disabilities, 17(2), 85-90.

McInerny, T. K., Adam, H. M., Campbell, D. E., Kamat, D. M., & Kelleher, K. J. (Eds.).

(2009). American Academy of Pediatrics Textbook of Pediatric Care. Elk Grove

Village, IL: American Academy of Pediatrics.

Metcalfe, L. A., Harvey, E. A., & Laws, H. B. (2013). The longitudinal relation between

academic/cognitive skills and externalizing behavior problems in preschool children.

Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(3), 881-894.

Murray, D. W., Kollins, S. H., Hardy, K. K., Abikoff, H. B., Swanson, J. M., Cunningham,

C., et al. (2007). Parent versus teacher ratings of attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder symptoms in the Preschoolers with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Treatment Study (PATS). Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology,

17(5), 605-619.

204
Normand, S., Schneider, B. H., & Robaey, P. (2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

and the challenges of close friendship. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry, 16(2), 67-73.

Papaeliou, C., Polemikos, N., Fryssira, E., Kodakos, A., Kaila, M., Yiota, X., et al. (2012).

Behavioural profile and maternal stress in Greek young children with Williams

syndrome. Child: Care, Health & Development, 38(6), 844-853.

Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years

after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73-89.

Polderman, T., Boomsma, D., Bartels, M., Verhulst, F., & Huizink, A. (2010). A systematic

review of prospective studies on attention problems and academic achievement. Acta

Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 122(4), 271-284.

Reid, R., & Maag, J. W. (1994). How many fidgets in a pretty much: A critique of behavior

rating scales for identifying students with ADHD. Journal of School Psychology,

32(4), 339-354.

Rommelse, N. N. J., Franke, B., Geurts, H. M., Hartman, C. A., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2010).

Shared heritability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum

disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 19(3), 281-295.

Royal College of Psychiatrists (2001). DC-LD: Diagnostic criteria for psychiatric disorders

for use with adults with learning disabilities/mental retardation (Occasional paper

48). London: Gaskell.

Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ). Los

Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Sattler, J. M. (2001). Assessment of children: Cognitive applications (4th ed.). San Diego:

Author.

205
Scerif, G., Longhi, E., Cole, V., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Cornish, K. (2012). Attention across

modalities as a longitudinal predictor of early outcomes: The case of fragile X

syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(6), 641-650.

Seltzer, M. M., Shattuck, P., Abbeduto, L., & Greenberg, J. S. (2004). Trajectory of

development in adolescents and adults with autism. Mental Retardation and

Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10(4), 234-247.

Sinzig, J., Walter, D., & Doepfner, M. (2009). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in

children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: Symptom or syndrome?

Journal of Attention Disorders, 13(2), 117-126.

Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales,

Second edition - Teacher rating form manual. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson.

Sullivan, K., Hatton, D., Hammer, J., Sideris, J., Hooper, S., Ornstein, P., et al. (2006).

ADHD symptoms in children with FXS. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part

A, 140(21), 2275-2288.

Taffe, J. R., Tonge, B. J., Gray, K. M., & Einfeld, S. L. (2008). Extracting more information

from behaviour checklists by using components of mean based scores. International

Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 17(4), 232-240.

Tandon, M., Si, X., Belden, A., & Luby, J. (2009). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in

preschool children: An investigation of validation based on visual attention

performance. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 19(2), 137-146.

Turk, J. (1998). Fragile X syndrome and attentional deficit. Journal of Applied Research in

Intellectual Disabilities, 11, 175-191.

van Gameren-Oosterom, H. B. M., Fekkes, M., Buitendijk, S. E., Mohangoo, A. D., Bruil, J.,

& Van Wouwe, J. P. (2011). Development, problem behavior, and quality of life in a

206
population based sample of eight-year-old children with Down syndrome. PLoS ONE,

6(7), e21879

Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence - Third edition.

Sydney, NSW: Pearson.

Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Fourth edition. Sydney,

NSW: Pearson.

Wilding, J., Munir, F., & Cornish, K. (2001). The nature of attentional differences between

groups of children differentiated by teacher ratings of attention and hyperactivity.

British Journal of Psychology, 92(2), 357-371.

Witwer, A. N., & Lecavalier, L. (2010). Validity of comorbid psychiatric disorders in

youngsters with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Developmental and Physical

Disabilities, 22(4), 367-380.

World Health Organization (1992). International Classification of Diseases, Tenth edition.

Geneva, Switzerland: Author.

207
Table 1

Sample Demographics

Autism (n = 59) ID (n = 76) Down Syndrome


(n = 41)

Gender n (% male) 48 (81.4) 44 (57.9) 22 (53.7)

Age

M (SD) 8.98 (2.22) 9.19 (1.94) 9.33 (2.36)

Range 5 – 12 5 – 12 5 – 13

Ethnicity n (% Australian) 40 (67.8) 60 (78.9) 31 (75.6)

Language skills n (%)

Nonverbal 11 (18.6) 7 (9.2) 6 (14.6)

Short sentences 42 (71.2) 45 (59.2) 29 (70.8)

Fluent 6 (10.2) 24 (31.6) 6 (14.6)

SESa

M (SD) 33.24 (15.32) 33.75 (15.28) 41.65 (16.85)

Range 11 – 69 8 – 69 14 – 69

School attended n (%)

Mainstream 2 (3.4) 13 (17.1) 12 (29.3)

Special/Special 50 (84.7) 55 (72.4) 26 (63.4)

developmental

208
Split placementb 1 (1.7) 7 (9.2) 3 (7.3)

Autism specialist school 2 (3.4) * *

Support centre in 4 (6.8) 1 (1.3) *

mainstream school
a
Hollingshead scores range from 8 to 71. bChildren who spend part of their week in a

mainstream school and part of their week in a special school.

209
Table 2

Scores From Cognitive and Adaptive Living Skills Assessments by Group

Autism ID Down

Syndrome

Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD)

Range Range Range

WISC-IV FSIQ 54.43 (8.69) 56.40 (9.05) 43.00 (3.14)†

/WPPSI-III 40 – 70 41 – 70 40 – 49

WISC-IV VCI 56.35 (10.01) 58.52 (9.16) 50.21 (5.99)**

45 – 75 45 – 75 45 – 63

PRI 64.59 (11.28) 62.26 (10.71) 47.95 (3.85)†

45 – 84 45 – 86 45 – 59

WMI 58.83 (8.94) 61.67 (9.04) 53.41 (4.74)**

50 – 77 50 – 88 50 – 65

PSI 68.17 (11.79) 66.22 (12.17) 52.67 (4.92)†

50 – 91 50 – 94 50 – 68

WPPSI-III Verbal 57.23 (7.05) 64.75 (8.39) 53.33 (5.51)*

49 – 75 53 – 77 48 – 59

Performance 59.46 (11.11) 64.78 (10.78) 49.67 (3.06)

47 – 79 49 – 81 47 - 53

VABS-II-T ABC 52.55 (11.22) 57.93 (9.71)** 51.89 (8.49)

20 – 70 30 – 74 31 – 63

Communication 56.27 (10.19) 58.34 (8.25) 56.00 (7.05)

25 – 74 40 – 74 46 – 72

210
Socialisation 58.68 (11.59)† 67.14 (9.13) 65.26 (7.49)

32 – 83 50 – 89 50 – 84

Daily Living 54.45 (11.04) 59.00 (11.01) 50.42 (8.81)**

Skills 21 – 72 30 – 80 36 – 69

Note. WISC-IV = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth edition; WPPSI-III =

Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence – Third edition; VABS-II-T =

Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales, Second edition – Teacher Rating Form.

* p < .05. ** p < .01. † p < .001.

211
Table 3.

Internal Consistency of T-SAID Subscales and Total Score Across Diagnostic Groups

Autism Down Syndrome Idiopathic ID

T-SAID .87 .89 .95

hyperactivity

T-SAID .93 .93 .95

inattention

T-SAID .95 .91 .93

verbal comm

T-SAID .74 .74 .81

follow inst

T-SAID .97 .97 .98

total

212
Table 4

Beta Coefficients of Regression Analyses for the T-SAID Hyperactivity/Impulsivity and

Verbal Communication Subscale Scores

MIS PIC II

Hyperactivity/impulsivity

Age -.07† -.04** -.03*

Female -.15 -.11* -.04

SES -.01 -.01 .01

Diagnosis (ref: ID)

Down Syndrome -.03 .03 -.09

Autism .16 .11* .05

Level of ID (ref: mild)

Moderate ID .02 -.01 .05

Severe ID .46† .24** .22**

Constant 1.37† .93† .45**

Verbal communication

Age -.08† -.03** -.06†

Female -.09 -.04 -.05

SES -.01 -.01 .01

Diagnosis (ref: ID)

Down Syndrome .09 .04 .12

Autism .21* .08 .14*

Level of ID (ref: mild)

Moderate ID .13 .04 .05

Severe ID .72† .27† .36†

213
Constant 1.60† .92† .83†

Note. MIS = Mean Item Score; PIC = Proportion of Items Checked; II = Intensity Index.

* p < .05. ** p < .01. † p < .001.

214
CHAPTER 11 GENERAL DISCUSSION

215
Although previous research exists to support the notion that attention difficulties exist

in children with ID, progress in this area has been hampered by issues such as diagnostic

overshadowing (Jopp & Keys, 2001; Mason & Scior, 2004); the belief that attention

difficulties relate to cognitive deficits rather than being a comorbid diagnosis (Antshel, et al.,

2006; Guerin, et al., 2009); and challenges associated with determining mental age thus

making it difficult to know whether the child’s behaviour is or is not developmentally

appropriate (Barkley, 2006a). While neuropsychological research has demonstrated support

for differing developmental trajectories across syndromes (Cornish, Scerif, et al., 2007;

Cornish & Wilding, 2010; Karmiloff-Smith, 2009), research into the development of rating

scales that can screen for and identify difficulties in attention, hyperactivity and impulsivity

has been limited, and often studies have had to resort to use of scales designed for typically

developing children.

With this background in mind, the present study had three aims: (1) to develop a

reliable and valid rating scale that was more sensitive to exploring the range and severity of

ADHD symptoms in school-aged children with intellectual disability; (2) that the new rating

scale would have good convergent validity with existing measures of ADHD; and (3) to

describe and compare the profiles of ADHD symptoms in children with known causes of ID.

11.1 Overview of findings

The findings of Chapter 8 suggested that the T-SAID is a reliable and valid measure

of attention and hyperactivity/impulsivity in children with ID. It had excellent internal

consistency and strong test-retest reliability. It had strong convergent validity with

corresponding subscales on the Conners Third edition (Conners, 2008) and the

Developmental Behaviour Checklist – Teacher version (Einfeld & Tonge, 1995, 2002)

suggesting that these different scales measured the same construct. It also had moderate

divergent validity with the total score on the Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales Second

216
edition - Teacher Rating Form (Sparrow, et al., 2005) suggesting a lack of association

between these measures and that they measured different constructs. Good content validity

was established via the method of item derivation and use of focus group discussions to

develop and evaluate the scale (DeVellis, 2003). Although it was beyond the scope of this

study to evaluate the properties of the T-SAID as a screening tool for ADHD, the T-SAID

demonstrated discriminant validity across children with a diagnosis of ADHD and those who

did not have this diagnosis. Regression analyses suggested that children with severe or

profound ID had greater difficulties with attention, hyperactivity and impulsivity, with a

broader range of behaviours and greater intensity being exhibited. The findings from this

chapter supported the first aim stating that the T-SAID would be a reliable and valid measure

of ADHD symptoms in school-aged children with ID. It supported the second aim stating that

the scores on the T-SAID would have good convergent validity with existing measures of

ADHD. It also supported the first hypothesis, stating that there would be a positive

relationship between hyperactivity/impulsivity and degree of ID.

The findings of Chapter 9 suggested that T-SAID had a four factor solution. As

expected, Hyperactivity/Impulsivity and Inattention were extracted, being the two core

dimensions of ADHD symptomatology (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). A third

subscale, labelled Verbal Communication, consisted of items relating to language such as

volume and pace of speech, making requests and retaining information. The fourth subscale,

labelled Following Instructions, contained items relating to instructions and compliance to

rules. The internal consistency of these subscales was good to excellent. The extraction of

four factors suggested that given children with ID may present with additional complexities,

such as limited or no language or a physical disability, the nature of ADHD symptoms may

present quite differently within this population as compared with typically developing

217
children. The findings from this chapter supported the first aim stating that the T-SAID

would be a reliable and valid measure of ADHD symptoms in school-aged children with ID.

The findings of Chapter 10 suggested that children with autism spectrum disorder

(ASD) displayed a significantly greater breadth of hyperactive/impulsive behaviours than

children with idiopathic ID or Down Syndrome. Children with ASD also had significantly

greater difficulties with behaviours that made up the Verbal Communication subscale than

children with Down Syndrome or idiopathic ID, and that the intensity of these behaviours

was also significantly greater. In the sample used in this study, the T-SAID was able to detect

differences in hyperactivity/impulsivity across groups but the Conners Third edition

(Conners, 2008) was unable to do so. These findings supported the third aim stating that the

T-SAID would be able to describe and compare differences in ADHD symptomatology in

children with ID. The findings from this chapter supported the third hypothesis. Higher levels

of hyperactivity/impulsivity were reported in the ASD group. The findings from this chapter

did not support the second hypothesis. While the Down Syndrome group had lower levels of

hyperactivity/impulsivity, there were no differences between the Down Syndrome and

idiopathic ID groups. Further, there were no group differences for inattention.

11.2 Points raised in Phase 1 of the study

The aim of the focus group discussions conducted in Phase 1 this study was twofold:

a) to collect a list of behaviours from teachers and health professionals exhibited by children

with attention difficulties and ID; and b) to obtain comments and feedback regarding the

development of a new rating scale. The majority of themes generated from these discussions

were consistent with those identified in previous observational studies, research and

diagnostic manuals regarding attention difficulties within this population. However, some of

the issues were specifically related to different professions which highlighted the value of

exploring these issues with both teachers and health professionals.

218
Teachers identified and discussed more externalising behaviours i.e., hyperactivity

and impulsivity, and fewer behaviours relating to inattention. This is consistent with research

which suggests that ADHD – predominantly inattentive subtype is underdiagnosed, and that

teachers are less likely than parents to report and/or identify inattentive behaviours consistent

with ADHD (Murray, et al., 2007). This could be due to a number of reasons. In a busy

classroom, it is likely that externalising behaviours will take up more of a classroom teacher’s

time and attention in trying to manage and/or minimise the impact these behaviours have on

other students and their teaching. Children who are displaying inattentive behaviours such as

staring off into space or taking a considerable amount of time to complete their work may be

less likely to draw the attention of their teacher, or depending on the composition of students

in the classroom may even escape their notice for long periods of time. Alternatively,

teachers may observe these problem behaviours but perceive them as less severe as they are

less disruptive to the learning of other students or the flow of the classroom in general. This

highlights the importance of making teachers more aware of the problem of inattention in the

classroom – both through improved identification and greater understanding of the impact it

has on students. Even though these behaviours may not be as readily observable or disruptive

to others compared with externalising behaviours, they can still have a significant impact on

individual student outcomes and achievement.

Even experienced teachers within the focus groups sometimes had difficulty

distinguishing behaviours that were specific to children with ID who had attention difficulties

and those that were characteristic of children with ID generally. This raises the issue of the

importance of educating teachers about children with ID who display ADHD symptoms so

that they can become better at identifying them (irrespective of whether diagnosis is

warranted) and tailoring their teaching to meet the needs of these students.

219
Issues of medication were also discussed briefly by both groups of teachers and one

group of health professionals. These issues reflected the same challenges faced by families of

typically developing children and adolescents with ADHD: parental concerns regarding

giving their child medication; compliance issues with children; and the impact of side-effects

(e.g., a child having to miss half of lunch time because the medication suppresses their

appetite and therefore it takes them a long time to eat their lunch). The fact that medication

was discussed illustrates that some children within their schools have received a diagnosis of

ADHD and are being treated for it. This is encouraging to note, given comorbid

psychopathology can be undiagnosed among individuals with ID due to diagnostic

overshadowing (Jopp & Keys, 2001; Mason & Scior, 2004).

11.3 Strengths of the present study

11.3.1 Scale development

One of the main strengths of this study was the rigour with which the rating scale was

developed, using the framework described by DeVellis (2003). Drawing upon behavioural

data from past research, a review of existing rating scales, an examination of diagnostic

manuals developed for individuals with ID and those who are typically developing, and

consultation with teachers and health professionals ensured that the T-SAID contains items

that are valid indicators of difficulties with attention, impulsivity and hyperactivity within

this population.

At the individual item level, the T-SAID describes behaviours that provide richer

information than vague, non-specific items such as Restless or Overactive which do little to

describe specific difficulties to a health professional who may not have the opportunity to

conduct observations of the child across settings. This information could then contribute to

the formulation of behaviour management plans, strategies and interventions that address

these specific difficulties.

220
11.3.2 Sample

Another strength of this study was in the breadth of the community sample. The

socioeconomic status of families ranged from very low (e.g., single parent families with the

parent being a full-time carer to one or more children with a disability) to high (e.g., dual

income families with both parents in professional jobs). Families also came from a range of

cultural backgrounds, with approximately one quarter identifying as being an ethnicity other

than Australian. Recruitment would have been dependent, however, upon families being able

to read the plain language statement sent home with their child. Families with English as a

first language would therefore be more likely to respond to requests for research

participation. Resources precluded the plain language statements being translated into other

languages, but this may be one strategy that could be used in future research to ensure the

sample is more representative of children in the community.

11.3.3 Teachers as informants

A third strength of the study is in the value of developing a teacher rating scale to

measure attention difficulties. Teachers, unlike parents, have the opportunity to observe large

groups of children working in the classroom. They can draw on behaviours they have

observed both in the present cohort of students being taught and past students, and can use

this information to make inferences about what behaviours deviate from the norm (Gadow, et

al., 2006). Teachers are also exposed to the behaviour of their students for long periods of

time (i.e., all day, 5 days a week) encompassing cognitively demanding tasks such as

following instructions, maintaining attention, and engaging in prescribed activities where an

outcome is expected, and activities encompassing creativity and play. This may contrast with

the home environment of some children who engage in less goal-directed activities or free

play where there may be fewer opportunities to observe such difficulties (Murray, et al.,

2007).

221
11.4 Limitations of the present study

11.4.1 Focus group data

One of the limitations of the present study was in the use of focus groups to facilitate

the identification of behaviours and to obtain their feedback on the new rating scale.

Although it has been recognised that focus groups can encourage an exploration of issues that

may not be presented in a one-on-one interview format (Vogt, King, & King, 2004),

participation in the discussions was uneven at times. The facilitator monitored participation

of the group members and elicited comments from those teachers and health professionals

who had made a lesser contribution to the discussion, but it is possible that the issues

presented and discussed were a product of the more articulate or dominant group members to

the neglect of the viewpoints of more inhibited or reflective members.

The possibility of selection biases in the composition of the focus group participants

is another potential limitation of this study. Teachers or health professionals who believed

that attention difficulties are present in all children with ID may have been less likely to

participate in a focus group discussion. Those who have had limited contact with such

children may have felt that they had little to offer to a discussion about these issues. The

transcript analysis revealed that the behaviours identified and opinions expressed regarding

the rating scale items were quite heterogeneous, however, and thus the extent of selection

bias or its impact on the findings is unclear.

While there are no straightforward tests for ensuring that qualitative research is

reliable and valid, guidelines exist (Pyett, 2003) and every effort was made to adhere to these

in conducting the focus groups and analysing the data. The focus group participants

comprised teachers, psychologists and paediatricians. The diversity of the experience which

these participants brought to the focus groups, and their knowledge of students with ID who

experience attention difficulties, was important for a number of reasons. The diversity of

222
professions ensured that interviewer bias did not occur which may have precluded the

consideration of important information or unique perspectives. The recruitment of teachers

ensured that behaviours typically observed in the classroom were reported, enhancing the

face validity of the T-SAID for the population for whom the scale was being designed. The

recruitment of psychologists and paediatricians ensured that the items had diagnostic

relevance and also enabled the collection of data on behaviours that may be highly relevant

but observed and/or reported less frequently by teachers.

11.4.2 Respondents

Even though the present study has reported some preliminary findings suggesting

differences in hyperactivity/impulsivity across neurodevelopmental disorders, it should be

noted that a rating scale alone is insufficient to draw conclusions about these difficulties, and

should be used in conjunction with developmental history, parent ratings, behavioural

observations and results from neuropsychological tests (e.g., Wilding Monster Card Sorting

task; Wilding, et al., 2001) in line with diagnostic guidelines (Barkley, 2006c). Future

research using the T-SAID may benefit from collecting multiple sources of data to draw

firmer conclusions about profiles and developmental trajectories of attention and activity.

While convergent validity of the T-SAID was measured by asking teachers to

complete several rating scales that measured ADHD symptoms, it would have been useful to

include one or more additional methods of assessing attention and hyperactivity/impulsivity

such as a structured diagnostic interview or behavioural observations. Future research could

compare additional methods of assessing attention profiles with the SAID to confirm whether

it appears to be assessing the intended constructs.

Further consideration must also be given to the validity of the T-SAID for children

with severe/profound ID who may have scored on some of the items due to their degree of

disability rather than presence of ADHD symptoms. This was addressed somewhat in the

223
factor structure presented as the items which inferred a need for verbal ability clustered

together onto the Verbal Communication factor. This recognised that verbal skills cannot be

assumed among children with ID, as children with certain neurodevelopmental disorders

(e.g., autism spectrum disorder) and/or severe/profound ID may be nonverbal. Another

alternative could be to consider separate factor structures for children who are verbal or

nonverbal, similar to the approach taken by Burbidge and colleagues (2010) for children with

severe/profound ID who had communication and/or mobility issues.

11.4.3 Medication for ADHD

For those children with a diagnosis of ADHD who were taking medication,

information on their dosage and compliance was not formally collected or monitored, and

was dependent upon parent report. The study was also unable to control for medication type

or length of time child had been on the medication. Medication may have modulated the

frequency and intensity of behaviours observed and reported by teachers, but given the aim of

this study was not to examine the effect of medication, this would not have impacted on the

psychometric properties reported in Papers 1 and 2. It is worth noting, however, that only 20

children had a diagnosis of ADHD and two thirds of these were currently taking medication.

Interactions between medication and observed behaviour may have potentially reduced the

rates of symptomatology in the groups compared in Paper 3, but given these children

represented such a small proportion of the groups or total sample they are likely to have had

only a minimal impact on the findings.

11.4.4 Diagnoses of children

The present study was unable to verify the accuracy of diagnosis for some children at

the time of recruitment to the study e.g., children recruited who were identified by their

parents as having idiopathic ID but may have had ASD. Resources precluded the use of a

gold standard screening instrument such as the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule

224
(ADOS; Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, 1999) which may have identified some of these

children as having ASD. Further, the screening instrument that was administered in the

present study (i.e., the Social Communication Questionnaire; Rutter, Bailey, et al., 2003) was

only administered to families of children with ASD, rather than being given to all families.

However, given the high rate of awareness of autism and autism symptoms, it is likely that

the rates of such undiagnosed cases of ASD were very low. Given that diagnostic accuracy

was not one of the aims of this study, this limitation would not have impacted on the analyses

of the psychometric properties of the T-SAID.

11.5 Directions for future research

11.5.1 Development of other versions

The most important direction for future research is the evaluation of the SAID using

other informants, such as parents. While inter-rater reliability of parents and teachers when

rating behaviour problems is noted across the literature as a significant challenge (Lavigne,

Dulcan, LeBailly, & Binns, 2012; Wolraich, et al., 2004), the use of rating scale data from

multiple informants is considered best practice when screening for ADHD symptoms

(Barkley, 2006c), so this would be a necessary next step.

The development of a version of the SAID to identify difficulties with attention,

hyperactivity and impulsivity in preschool-aged children is also needed. Research from the

field of neuropsychology has described attention difficulties in toddlers and young children

with neurodevelopmental disorders such as Williams Syndrome (Cornish, Scerif, et al., 2007;

Scerif, et al., 2004) and Down Syndrome (J. H. Brown, et al., 2003). It is therefore clear that

a reliable and valid, disability specific rating scale would be a useful and necessary tool for

research into this population. Early identification and intervention may help minimise the

impact these difficulties have on learning once children commence school.

225
11.5.2 Independent validation of the psychometric properties

Future research is needed to examine the psychometric properties of the T-SAID and

to confirm or modify the factor structure identified in the present study, particularly given the

modest sample size of this study. Independent studies are also needed to give further support

to the reliability and validity of the scale. Given the clinical importance of obtaining

information about attention and hyperactivity/impulsivity difficulties from multiple sources

when making diagnostic decisions (Barkley, 2006c), further development and examination of

the parent version of the scale (P-SAID) is also needed.

Preliminary results suggest that the T-SAID may have the ability to discriminate

between children who have ADHD and those who do not, although its efficacy as a screening

tool is yet to be evaluated. It should be noted, however, that in this study the ADHD

diagnosis was determined via parent report only. An urgent area for future research is

therefore to examine the validity of the T-SAID in a larger sample of children with a

confirmed clinical diagnosis of ADHD. As the majority of clinicians use rating scales when

considering this diagnosis (Chan, et al., 2005), further evidence supporting the efficacy of the

T-SAID would make it a useful screening tool for clinicians when working with children who

have ID.

11.5.3 Examining attention profiles in other neurodevelopmental disorders

Another direction for future research would be to examine the robustness of the T-

SAID to identify attention and activity profiles across a wider range of neurodevelopmental

disorders. Evidence suggests that different profiles and developmental trajectories may also

exist across other syndromes including Klinefelter (Lo-Castro, D'Agati, & Curatolo, 2011)

and DiGeorge Syndromes (Lo-Castro, et al., 2011). Understanding the differences (and

commonalities) across syndromes would be beneficial to clinicians in their development of

treatment, medication and intervention plans for these children. They could also be beneficial

226
to teachers of these children in their development of individual learning and behaviour

management plans. Exploration of the T-SAID across a wide range of neurodevelopmental

disorders would provide further evidence for the utility of this scale which has been

developed specifically for children with ID.

11.6 Concluding remarks

The findings of our study suggest that the Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability

– Teacher version (T-SAID) is a reliable and valid measure for children aged 5 to 13 years

with mild to profound ID. Further research is needed to ascertain its reliability and validity in

older children/adolescents and its use among children who are nonverbal. Factor analysis

extracted four subscales from the items developed: Inattention, Hyperactivity/Impulsivity,

Following Instructions and Verbal Communication. Examination of the subscales across

diagnoses suggests that the T-SAID may have the capacity to tease out differences across

neurodevelopmental disorders. This would need to be verified, however, by further research

combining this information with other sources such as behavioural observations, interviews

and neuropsychological measures of attention.

Potential applications of the T-SAID in future research include use in clinical or

neuropsychological studies examining ADHD symptomatology, and among clinicians to

assist with screening for ADHD. The T-SAID could also be used in school settings by

psychologists and teachers to inform the development of strategies and behaviour

management plans that can target specific areas of difficulty in any of these areas. The

intended outcome of its use in any of these settings is to improve the functioning and learning

outcomes of children experiencing these difficulties, which will in turn enhance their long-

term outcomes at home, in the classroom and in the general community.

227
REFERENCES

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile.

Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

Achenbach, T. M. (2007). Multicultural supplement for the School Age Forms and Profiles.

Burlington, VT: Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment.

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2000). Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms and

profiles. Burlington, VT: Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment.

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and

profiles. Burlington, VT: Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment.

Ahadi, S. A., Rothbart, M. K., & Ye, R. (1993). Children's temperament in the US and China:

Similarities and differences. European Journal of Personality, 7(5), 359-377.

Alanay, Y., Unal, F., Turanli, G., Alikasifoglu, M., Alehan, D., Akyol, U., . . . Tuncbilek, E.

(2007). A multidisciplinary approach to the management of individuals with fragile X

syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51(2), 151-161.

Allport, D. A., Antonis, B., & Reynolds, P. (1972). On the division of attention: A disproof

of the single channel hypothesis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,

24(2), 225-235.

Aman, M. G., Richmond, G., Stewart, A. W., Bell, J. C., & Kissel, R. C. (1987). The

Aberrant Behavior Checklist: Factor structure and the effect of subject variables in

American and New Zealand facilities. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 91(6),

570-578.

Aman, M. G., & Singh, N. N. (1994). Aberrant Behaviour Checklist - Community

supplementary manual. East Aurora, New York: Slosson Educational Publications.

228
Aman, M. G., Singh, N. N., Stewart, A. W., & Field, C. J. (1985). The Aberrant Behavior

Checklist: A behavior rating scale for the assessment of treatment effects. American

Journal of Mental Deficiency, 89(5), 485-491.

Aman, M. G., Tasse, M. J., Rojahn, J., & Hammer, D. (1996). The Nisonger CBRF: A child

behavior rating form for children with developmental disabilities. Research in

Developmental Disabilities, 17(1), 41-57.

Amaral, O. B. (2007). Psychiatric disorders as social constructs: ADHD as a case in point.

The American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(10), 1612.

American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. (2010). Intellectual

disability: Definition, classification and systems of support. Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (1968). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders - Second edition: DSM- II. Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders - Third edition: DSM - III. Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders - Third edition revised: DSM - III - R. Washington DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders - Fourth edition: DSM - IV. Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders - Fourth edition Text revision: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders - Fifth edition: DSM-5. Arlington, Virginia: Author.

Anderson, J. C. (1996). Is childhood hyperactivity the product of western culture? Lancet,

348(9020), 73-74.

229
Antrop, I., Stock, P., Verte, S., Wiersema, J. R., Baeyens, D., & Roeyers, H. (2006). ADHD

and delay aversion: the influence of non-temporal stimulation on choice for delayed

rewards. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 47(11), 1152-1158.

Antshel, K. M., Phillips, M. H., Gordon, M., Barkley, R., & Faraone, S. V. (2006). Is ADHD

a valid disorder in children with intellectual delays? Clinical Psychology Review,

26(5), 555-572.

August, G. J., Realmuto, G. M., Joyce, T., & Hektner, J. M. (1999). Persistence and

desistence of oppositional defiant disorder in a community sample of children with

ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(10),

1262-1270.

August, G. J., Realmuto, G. M., MacDonald, A. W., Nugent, S. M., & Crossby, R. (1996).

Prevalence of ADHD and comorbid disorders among elementary school children

screened for disruptive behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24(5), 571-

595.

Bailey, D. B., Jr., Raspa, M., Olmsted, M., & Holiday, D. B. (2008). Co-occurring conditions

associated with FMR1 gene variations: Findings from a national parent survey.

American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A, 146A(16), 2060-2069.

Baker, B. L., Neece, C. L., Fenning, R. M., Crnic, K. A., & Blacher, J. (2010). Mental

disorders in five-year-old children with or without developmental delay: Focus on

ADHD. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39(4), 492-505.

Barkley, R. A. (1997a). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New York: Guilford Press.

Barkley, R. A. (1997b). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions:

Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65-94.

230
Barkley, R. A. (2006a). Associated cognitive, developmental and health problems. In R. A.

Barkley (Ed.), Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and

treatment (3rd ed.) (pp. 122-183). New York: Guilford Press.

Barkley, R. A. (2006b). Comorbid disorders, social and family adjustment, and subtyping. In

R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for

diagnosis and treatment (3rd ed.) (pp. 184-218). New York: Guilford Press.

Barkley, R. A. (2006c). Primary symptoms, diagnostic criteria, prevalence and gender

differences. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A

handbook for diagnosis and treatment (3rd ed.) (pp. 76-121). New York: Guilford

Press.

Barkley, R. A., & Edwards, G. (2006). Diagnostic interview, behavior rating scales, and the

medical examination. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder:

A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (3rd ed.) (pp. 337-368). New York: Guilford

Press.

Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2004). Young adult follow-up of

hyperactive children: antisocial activities and drug use. Journal of Child Psychology

& Psychiatry, 45(2), 195-211.

Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2006). Young adult outcome of

hyperactive children: Adaptive functioning in major life activities. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(2), 192-202.

Becker, A., Steinhausen, H. C., Baldursson, G., Dalsgaard, S., Lorenzo, M. J., Ralston, S. J., .

. . Rothenberger, A. (2006). Psychopathological screening of children with ADHD:

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire in a pan-European study. European Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 15(Suppl 1), 56-62.

231
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W. P., Lai, Z., & St George, M. (2000). I. The

neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and

weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Suppl 1), 7-29.

Bennett, K. S., Levy, F., & Hay, D. A. (2007). Behaviour genetic approaches to the study of

ADHD. In M. Fitzgerald, M. Bellgrove & M. Gill (Eds.), Handbook of attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder (pp. 111-128). West Sussex, England: John Wiley

Bergeron, R., Floyd, R. G., McCormack, A. C., & Farmer, W. L. (2008). The generalizability

of externalizing behavior composites and subscale scores across time, rater, and

instrument. School Psychology Review, 37(1), 91-108.

Berk, L. E. (2011). Infancy and toddlerhood: The first two years. In L. E. Berk (Ed.), Infants,

children and adolescents (7th ed.) (pp. 202-245). Upper Saddle River, New Jersey:

Pearson.

Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bailey, A. (1999). Autism screening

questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175(5), 444-451.

Betts, J., McKay, J., Maruff, P., & Anderson, V. (2006). The development of sustained

attention in children: The effect of age and task load. Child Neuropsychology, 12(3),

205-221.

Biederman, J., Kwon, A., Aleardi, M., Chouinard, V., Marino, T., Cole, H., . . . Faraone, S.

V. (2005). Absence of gender effects on Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder:

Findings in nonreferred subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(6), 1083-1089.

Biederman, J., Mick, E., & Faraone, S. V. (2000). Age-dependent decline of symptoms of

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Impact of remission definition and symptom

type. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(5), 816-818.

232
Biederman, J., Mick, E., Faraone, S. V., Braaten, E., Doyle, A., Spencer, T., . . . Johnson, M.

A. (2002). Influence of gender on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children

referred to a psychiatric clinic. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(1), 36-42.

Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Mick, E., Spencer, T., Wilens, T. E., Silva, J. M., . . .

Faraone, S. V. (2006). Young adult outcome of attention deficit hyperactivity

disorder: A controlled 10-year follow-up study. Psychological Medicine, 36(2), 167-

179.

Biederman, J., Petty, C. R., Woodworth, K., Lomedico, A., Hyder, L. L., & Faraone, S. V.

(2012). Adult outcome of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A controlled 16-

year follow-up study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 73(7), 941-950.

Bishop, D. V., Laws, G., Adams, C., & Norbury, C. F. (2006). High heritability of speech and

language impairments in 6-year-old twins demonstrated using parent and teacher

report. Behavior Genetics, 36(2), 173-184.

Bloemsma, J., Boer, F., Arnold, R., Banaschewski, T., Faraone, S. V., Buitelaar, J. K., . . .

Oosterlaan, J. (2013). Comorbid anxiety and neurocognitive dysfunctions in children

with ADHD. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 22(4), 225-234.

Bond, T. G., & Fox, C. M. (2007). Applying the Rasch model: Fundamental measurement in

the human sciences (2nd ed.). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Borgatti, S. P. (1999). Elicitation technqiues for cultural domain analysis. In J. J. Schensul,

M. D. LeCompte, B. K. Nastasi & S. P. Borgatti (Eds.), Enhanced ethnographic

methods III. Audiovisual techniques, focused group interviews, and elicitation

techniques (pp. 115-151). Walnut Creek, California: AltaMira Press.

Bourdon, K. H., Goodman, R., Rae, D. S., Simpson, G., & Koretz, D. S. (2005). The

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: U.S. normative data and psychometric

233
properties. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,

44(6), 557-564.

Bowen, R., Chavira, D. A., Bailey, K., Stein, M. T., & Stein, M. B. (2008). Nature of anxiety

comorbid with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children from a pediatric

primary care setting. Psychiatry Research, 157(1-3), 201-209.

Boyatzis, R. E. (1998). Transforming qualitative information: Thematic analysis and code

development. Thousand Oaks, California: Sage.

Bradley, E. A., & Isaacs, B. J. (2006). Inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity in teenagers

with intellectual disabilities, with and without autism. Canadian Journal of

Psychiatry, 51(9), 598-606.

Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research

in Psychology, 3(2), 77-101.

Breckenridge, K., Braddick, O., Anker, S., Woodhouse, M., & Atkinson, J. (2013). Attention

in Williams syndrome and Down's syndrome: Performance on the new early

childhood attention battery. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31(Pt 2),

257-269.

Brinkley, J., Nations, L., Abramson, R. K., Hall, A., Wright, H. H., Gabriels, R., . . . Cuccaro,

M. L. (2007). Factor analysis of the Aberrant Behavior Checklist in individuals with

autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(10),

1949-1959.

Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. London: Pergamon Press.

Brown, E. C., Aman, M. G., & Havercamp, S. M. (2002). Factor analysis and norms for

parent ratings on the Aberrant Behavior Checklist-Community for young people in

special education. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 23(1), 45-60.

234
Brown, J. H., Johnson, M. H., Paterson, S. J., Gilmore, R., Longhi, E., & Karmiloff-Smith, A.

(2003). Spatial representation and attention in toddlers with Williams syndrome and

Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 41(8), 1037-1046.

Brown, L. (2010). The prevalence of Fragile X-associated disorders in Australia. Retrieved

from www.canberra.edu.au/centres/natsem

Buckley, S., Hillery, J., Guerin, S., McEvoy, J., & Dodd, P. (2008). The prevalence of

features of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in a special school in Ireland.

Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 52(2), 156-162.

Bunge, S. A., Dudukovic, N. M., Thomason, M. E., Vaidya, C. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002).

Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: evidence from

fMRI. Neuron, 33(2), 301-311.

Burack, J. A. (1994). Selective attention deficits in persons with autism: Preliminary evidence

of an inefficient attentional lens. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(3), 535-543.

Burack, J. A., Evans, D. W., Klaiman, C., & Iarocci, G. (2001). The mysterious myth of

attention deficits and other defect stories: Contemporary issues in the developmental

approach to mental retardation. International Review of Research in Mental

Retardation, 24, 299-320.

Burbidge, C., Oliver, C., Moss, J., Arron, K., Berg, K., Furniss, F., . . . Woodcock, K. (2010).

The association between repetitive behaviours, impulsivity and hyperactivity in

people with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54(12),

1078-1092.

Bussing, R., Fernandez, M., Harwood, M., Hou, W., Garvan, C. W., Eyberg, S. M., &

Swanson, J. M. (2008). Parent and teacher SNAP-IV ratings of attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder symptoms: Psychometric properties and normative ratings from

a school district sample. Assessment, 15(3), 317-328.

235
Cacace, A. T., & McFarland, D. J. (1998). Central auditory processing disorder in school-

aged children: A critical review. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing

Research, 41(2), 355-373.

Campbell, L. E., Daly, E., Toal, F., Stevens, A., Azuma, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., . . .

Murphy, K. C. (2009). Brain structural differences associated with the behavioural

phenotype in children with Williams syndrome. Brain Research, 1258, 96-107.

Campbell, S. B., & Ewing, L. J. (1990). Follow-up of hard-to-manage preschoolers:

Adjustment at age 9 and predictors of continuing symptoms. Journal of Child

Psychology & Psychiatry, 31(6), 871-889.

Chadwick, O., Kusel, Y., Cuddy, M., & Taylor, E. (2005). Psychiatric diagnoses and

behaviour problems from childhood to early adolescence in young people with severe

intellectual disabilities. Psychological Medicine, 35(5), 751-760.

Chan, E., Hopkins, M. R., Perrin, J. M., Herrerias, C., & Homer, C. J. (2005). Diagnostic

practices for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A national survey of primary

care physicians. Ambulatory Pediatrics, 5(4), 201-208.

Christ, S. E., Holt, D. D., White, D. A., & Green, L. (2007). Inhibitory control in children

with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,

37(6), 1155-1165.

Christ, S. E., Kester, L. E., Bodner, K. E., & Miles, J. H. (2011). Evidence for selective

inhibitory impairment in individuals with autism spectrum disorder.

Neuropsychology, 25(6), 690-701.

Cirino, P. T., Chin, C. E., Sevcik, R. A., Wolf, M., Lovett, M., & Morris, R. D. (2002).

Measuring socioeconomic status: Reliability and preliminary validity for different

approaches. Assessment, 9(2), 145-155.

236
CME Institute of Physicians. (2007). Managing ADHD in children, adolescents, and adults

with comorbid anxiety. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 68(3), 451-462.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,

New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Cole, P. M., Dennis, T. A., Smith-Simon, K. E., & Cohen, L. H. (2009). Preschoolers'

emotion regulation strategy understanding: Relations with emotion socialization and

child self-regulation. Social Development, 18(2), 324-352.

Collins, V. R., Muggli, E. E., Riley, M., Palma, S., & Halliday, J. L. (2008). Is Down

syndrome a disappearing birth defect? Journal of Pediatrics, 152(1), 20-24.

Conners, C. K. (1989). Conners Rating Scales Manual. New York: Multi-Health Systems

Inc.

Conners, C. K. (1997). Conners' Rating Scales - Revised Technical Manual. New York:

Multi-Health Systems Inc.

Conners, C. K. (2008). Conners 3 Manual. New York: Multi-Health Systems Inc.

Cornish, K., Cole, V., Longhi, E., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Scerif, G. (2012). Does attention

constrain developmental trajectories in fragile X syndrome? A 3-year prospective

longitudinal study. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities,

117(2), 103-120.

Cornish, K., Cole, V., Longhi, E., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Scerif, G. (2013). Mapping

developmental trajectories of attention and working memory in fragile X syndrome:

Developmental freeze or developmental change? Development and Psychopathology,

25(2), 365-376.

Cornish, K., Gray, K. M., & Rinehart, N. J. (2010). Fragile X Syndrome and associated

disorders. In J. Holmes (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior, Volume

39 (pp. 211-235). London: Academic Press.

237
Cornish, K., Li, L., Kogan, C. S., Jacquemont, S., Turk, J., Dalton, A., . . . Hagerman, P. J.

(2008). Age-dependent cognitive changes in carriers of the fragile X syndrome.

Cortex, 44(6), 628-636.

Cornish, K., Munir, F., & Cross, G. (2001). Differential impact of the FMR-1 full mutation

on memory and attention functioning: A neuropsychological perspective. Journal of

Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(1), 144-150.

Cornish, K., Scerif, G., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2007). Tracing syndrome-specific trajectories

of attention across the lifespan. Cortex, 43(6), 672-685.

Cornish, K., Steele, A., Monteiro, C. R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Scerif, G. (2012). Attention

deficits predict phenotypic outcomes in syndrome-specific and domain-specific ways.

Frontiers in Psychology, 11(3), 227.

Cornish, K., Sudhalter, V., & Turk, J. (2004). Attention and language in Fragile X. Mental

Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10(1), 11-16.

Cornish, K., Turk, J., & Hagerman, R. J. (2008). The fragile X continuum: New advances and

perspectives. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 52(6), 469-482.

Cornish, K., Turk, J., & Levitas, A. (2007). Fragile X Syndrome and autism: Common

developmental pathways? Current Pediatric Reviews, 3, 61-68.

Cornish, K., Turk, J., Wilding, J., Sudhalter, V., Munir, F., Kooy, F., & Hagerman, R. (2004).

Annotation: Deconstructing the attention deficit in fragile X syndrome: A

developmental neuropsychological approach. Journal of Child Psychology &

Psychiatry, 45(6), 1042-1053.

Cornish, K., & Wilding, J. (2010). Attention, genes and developmental disorders. New York:

Oxford University Press.

Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika,

16, 297-334.

238
Dally, K. (2006). The influence of phonological processing and inattentive behavior on

reading acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(2), 420-437.

Das, J., & Melnyk, L. (1989). Attention Checklist: A rating scale for mildly mentally

handicapped adolescents. Psychological Reports, 64(3, Pt 2), 1267-1274.

de Bildt, A., Kraijer, D., Sytema, S., & Minderaa, R. (2005). The psychometric properties of

the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales in children and adolescents with mental

retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(1), 53-62.

de Graaf, G., Haveman, M., Hochstenbach, R., Engelen, J., Gerssen-Schoorl, K., Poddighe,

P., . . . van Hove, G. (2011). Changes in yearly birth prevalence rates of children with

Down syndrome in the period 1986-2007 in The Netherlands. Journal of Intellectual

Disability Research, 55(5), 462-473.

de Ruiter, K. P., Dekker, M. C., Verhulst, F. C., & Koot, H. M. (2007). Developmental

course of psychopathology in youths with and without intellectual disabilities.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(5), 498-507.

Deb, S., Dhaliwal, A. J., & Roy, M. (2008). The usefulness of Conners' Rating Scales -

Revised in screening for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children with

intellectual disabilities and borderline intelligence. Journal of Intellectual Disability

Research, 52(11), 950-965.

Dekker, M. C., & Koot, H. M. (2003). DSM-IV disorders in children with borderline to

moderate intellectual disability. I: Prevalence and impact. Journal of the American

Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(8), 915-922.

Dekker, M. C., Nunn, R., & Koot, H. (2002). Psychometric properties of the revised

Developmental Behaviour Checklist scales in Dutch children with intellectual

disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 46(1), 61-75.

239
Demaray, M. K., Elting, J., & Schaefer, K. (2003). Assessment of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A comparative evaluation of five, commonly

used, published rating scales. Psychology in the Schools, 40(4), 341-361.

Department of Health. (2001). Valuing people: A new strategy for learning disability for the

21st century. Retrieved from www.archive.official-

documents.co.uk/document/cm50/5086/5086.htm

Deutsch, J., & Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: Some theoretical considerations. Psychological

Review, 70(1), 80-90.

DeVellis, R. F. (2003). Scale development: Theory and applications (2nd ed.). Thousand

Oaks, California: Sage.

Diamond, A., & Taylor, C. (1996). Development of an aspect of executive control:

Development of the abilities to remember what I said and to "Do as I say, not as I do".

Developmental Psychobiology, 29(4), 315-334.

Dodd, H. F., & Porter, M. A. (2009). Psychopathology in Williams syndrome: The effect of

individual differences across the life span. Journal of Mental Health Research in

Intellectual Disabilities, 2(2), 89-109.

Dopfner, M., Steinhausen, H. C., Coghill, D., Dalsgaard, S., Poole, L., Ralston, S. J., &

Rothenberger, A. (2006). Cross-cultural reliability and validity of ADHD assessed by

the ADHD Rating Scale in a pan-European study. European Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 15(Suppl 1), 46-55.

DuPaul, G. J. (1991). Parent and teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms: Psychometric

properties in a community-based sample. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 20(3),

245-253.

DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., & Reid, R. (1998). ADHD Rating Scale -

IV: Checklists, norms, and clinical interpretation New York: Guilford Press.

240
Dykens, E. M. (2000). Psychopathology in children with intellectual disability. Journal of

Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 41(4), 407-417.

Dykens, E. M. (2007). Psychiatric and behavioral disorders in persons with Down syndrome.

Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 13(3), 272-

278.

Edelbrock, C. S. (1985). Child Behavior Rating Form. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 21,

835-837.

Einfeld, S. L., Piccinin, A. M., Mackinnon, A., Hofer, S. M., Taffe, J., Gray, K. M., . . .

Tonge, B. J. (2006). Psychopathology in young people with intellectual disability.

Journal of the American Medical Association, 296(16), 1981-1989.

Einfeld, S. L., & Tonge, B. J. (1995). The Developmental Behavior Checklist: The

development and validation of an instrument to assess behavioral and emotional

disturbance in children and adolescents with mental retardation. Journal of Autism

and Developmental Disorders, 25(2), 81-104.

Einfeld, S. L., & Tonge, B. J. (2002). Manual for the Developmental Behaviour Checklist

(2nd edition): Primary carer version (DBC- P) and teacher version (DBC - T).

Clayton, Victoria: Monash University Centre for Developmental Psychology &

Psychiatry.

Einfeld, S. L., Tonge, B. J., & Florio, T. (1997). Behavioral and emotional disturbance in

individuals with Williams syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation,

102(1), 45-53.

Einfeld, S. L., Tonge, B. J., Gray, K., & Taffe, J. (2007). Evolution of symptoms and

syndromes of psychopathology in young people with mental retardation. International

Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 33, 247-265.

241
Einfeld, S. L., Tonge, B. J., & Turner, G. (1999). Longitudinal course of behavioral and

emotional problems in Fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics,

87(5), 436-439.

Eisenberg, N., & Sulik, M. J. (2012). Emotion-related self-regulation in children. Teaching of

Psychology, 39(1), 77-83.

Ekstein, S., Glick, B., Weill, M., Kay, B., & Berger, I. (2011). Down syndrome and attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Child Neurology, 26(10), 1290-

1295.

Elsabbagh, M., Divan, G., Koh, Y., Kim, Y. S., Kauchali, S., Marcin, C., . . . Fombonne, E.

(2012). Global prevalence of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders.

Autism Research, 5(3), 160-179.

Emerson, E. (2005). Use of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire to assess the mental

health needs of children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities. Journal of

Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 30(1), 14-23.

Epstein, M. H., Cullinan, D., & Gadow, K. D. (1986). Teacher ratings of hyperactivity in

learning-disabled, emotionally disturbed, and mentally retarded children. Journal of

Special Education, 20(2), 219-229.

Erford, B. T., & Hase, K. (2006). Reliability and validity of scores on the ACTeRS-2.

Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 39(2), 97-106.

Estes, A. M., Dawson, G., Sterling, L., & Munson, J. (2007). Level of intellectual functioning

predicts patterns of associated symptoms in school-age children with autism spectrum

disorder. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 112(6), 439-449.

Evans, P. L. C., & Hogg, J. (1984). A classroom rating scale for use with mentally retarded

children. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23(3), 187-194.

242
Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., & Mick, E. (2006). The age-dependent decline of attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis of follow-up studies. Psychological

Medicine, 36(2), 159-165.

Farzin, F., Perry, H., Hessl, D., Loesch, D., Cohen, J., Bacalman, S., . . . Hagerman, R.

(2006). Autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in boys

with the Fragile X premutation. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics,

27(Suppl 2), S137-S144.

Fee, V. E., Matson, J. L., & Benavidez, D. A. (1994). Attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder

among mentally retarded children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 15(1), 67-

79.

Feinstein, C., & Reiss, A. L. (1996). Psychiatric disorder in mentally retarded children and

adolescents. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 5(4), 827-

852.

Fischer, M., Barkley, R. A., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2002). Young adult follow-up of

hyperactive children: Self-reported psychiatric disorders, comorbidity, and the role of

childhood conduct problems and teen CD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,

30(5), 463-475.

Fletcher, R., Loschen, E., Stavrakaki, C., & First, M. (Eds.). (2007). Diagnostic Manual -

Intellectual Disability (DM-ID): A textbook of diagnosis of mental disorders in

persons with intellectual disability. New York: National Association for the Dually

Diagnosed Press.

Fombonne, E. (2005). Epidemiological studies of autism and pervasive developmental

disorders. In F. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism

and pervasive developmental disorders (3rd ed.) (pp. 42-69). New York: Wiley and

Sons.

243
Forbes, G. B. (2001). A comparison of the Conners' Parent & Teacher Rating Scales, the

ADD-H Comprehensive Teacher's Rating Scale, and the Child Behavior Checklist in

the clinical diagnosis of ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 5(1), 25-40.

Frances, A. (2010). Increasing the age at onset for ADHD? The American Journal of

Psychiatry, 167(6), 718.

Frazier, J., Biederman, J., Bellordre, C., Garfield, S., Geller, D., Coffey, B., & Faraone, S.

(2001). Should the diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder be considered

in children with pervasive developmental disorder? Journal of Attention Disorders,

4(4), 203-211.

Freeman, N. C., Cornish, K. M., & Gray, K. M. (2012). Attention and activity profiles in

children with intellectual disability: Development of a teacher rating scale. Journal of

Intellectual Disability Research, 56(7/8), 712.

Freeman, N. C., Gray, K. M., Taffe, J. R., & Cornish, K. (2013a). Development of a new

attention rating scale for children with intellectual disabilities: The Scale of Attention

in Intellectual Disability (SAID). American Journal on Intellectual and

Developmental Disabilities, Manuscript submitted for publication.

Freeman, N. C., Gray, K. M., Taffe, J. R., & Cornish, K. (2013b). The Scale of Attention in

Intellectual Disability (SAID): Factor analysis of a new rating scale for use with

children with intellectual disabilities. American Journal on Intellectual and

Developmental Disabilities, Manuscript submitted for publication.

Frink, J. (2007). Flesh 2.0. Fairfax, Virginia: Sourceforge. Retrieved from

flesh.sourceforge.net

Gadow, K. D., DeVincent, C. J., & Pomeroy, J. (2006). ADHD symptom subtypes in children

with pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders, 36(2), 271-283.

244
Gagliardi, C., Martelli, S., Tavano, A., & Borgatti, R. (2011). Behavioural features of Italian

infants and young adults with Williams-Beuren syndrome. Journal of Intellectual

Disability Research, 55(2), 121-131.

Garretson, H. B., Fein, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1990). Sustained attention in children with

autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 101-114.

Gathercole, S. E., & Alloway, T. P. (2006). Practitioner review: Short-term and working

memory impairments in neurodevelopmental disorders: Diagnosis and remedial

support. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 47(1), 4-15.

Gau, S. S., Chang, L. Y., Huang, L. M., Fan, T. Y., Wu, Y. Y., & Lin, T. Y. (2008).

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity-related symptoms among children with enterovirus 71

infection of the central nervous system. Pediatrics, 122(2), e452-458.

Gau, S. S., Lin, C., Hu, F., Shang, C., Swanson, J. M., Liu, Y., & Liu, S. (2009).

Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham,

Version IV Scale - Teacher Form. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34(8), 850-861.

Gau, S. S., Shang, C., Liu, S., Lin, C., Swanson, J. M., Liu, Y., & Tu, C. (2008).

Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham,

Version IV Scale - Parent Form. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric

Research, 17(1), 35-44.

Gerardi-Caulton, G. (2000). Sensitivity to spatial conflict and the development of self-

regulation in children 24-36 months of age. Developmental Science, 3(4), 397-404.

Ghaziuddin, M., Tsai, L. Y., & Alessi, N. (1992). ADHD and PDD. Journal of the American

Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 31(3), 567.

Ghaziuddin, M., Welch, K., Mohiuddin, S., Lagrou, R., & Ghaziuddin, N. (2010). Utility of

the Social and Communication Questionnaire in the differentiation of autism from

ADHD. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 22(4), 359-366.

245
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior Rating Inventory

of Executive Function (BRIEF). Odessa, Florida: Psychological Assessment

Resources.

Gladman, M., & Lancaster, S. (2003). A review of the Behaviour Assessment System for

Children. School Psychology International, 24(3), 276-291.

Goldstein, S., & Schwebach, A. J. (2004). The comorbidity of pervasive developmental

disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Results of a retrospective chart

review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 329-339.

Gomez, R., & Condon, M. (1999). Central auditory processing ability in children with

ADHD with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(2),

150-158.

Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal

of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 38(5), 581-586.

Goodman, R. (2004, June 30). SDQ: Information for researchers and professionals about the

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaires Retrieved April 27, 2010, from

http://www.sdqinfo.com/ba3.html

Goodman, R. (2007, October 19). SDQ: Normative data Retrieved April 27, 2010, from

http://www.sdqinfo.com/b8.html

Goodman, R., & Scott, S. (1999). Comparing the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire

and the Child Behavior Checklist: Is small beautiful? Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology, 27(1), 17-24.

Graetz, B. W., Sawyer, M. G., & Baghurst, P. (2005). Gender differences among children

with DSM-IV ADHD in Australia. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(2), 159-168.

246
Graetz, B. W., Sawyer, M. G., Hazell, P. L., Arney, F., & Baghurst, P. (2001). Validity of

DSM-IV ADHD subtypes in a nationally representative sample of Australian children

and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,

40(12), 1410-1417.

Greenfield, B., Hechtman, L., & Weiss, G. (1988). Two subgroups of hyperactives as adults:

Correlations of outcome. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 33(6), 505-508.

Guerin, S., Buckley, S., McEvoy, J., Hillery, J., & Dodd, P. (2009). The psychometric

properties of the Attention-Distraction, Inhibition-Excitation Classroom Assessment

Scale (ADIECAS) in a sample of children with moderate and severe intellectual

disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30(4), 727-734.

Guest, G., Bunce, A., & Johnson, L. (2006). How many interviews are enough? An

experiment with data saturation and variability. Field Methods, 18(1), 59-82.

Gunter, T. D., Arndt, S., Riggins-Caspers, K., Wenman, G., & Cadoret, R. J. (2006). Adult

outcomes of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and conduct disorder: Are the

risks independent or additive? Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 18(4), 233-237.

Hagerman, P. J. (2008). The Fragile X prevalence paradox. Journal of Medical Genetics,

45(8), 498-499.

Hagerman, R. J. (1999). Psychopharmacological interventions in Fragile X syndrome, fetal

alcohol syndrome, Prader-Willi syndrome, Angelman syndrome, Smith-Magenis

syndrome, and velocardiofacial syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental

Disabilities Research Reviews, 5(4), 305-313.

Hagerman, R. J. (2002). The physical and behavioral phenotype. In R. J. Hagerman & P. J.

Hagerman (Eds.), Fragile X Syndrome: Diagnosis, treatment and research (3rd ed.)

(pp. 3-109). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

247
Hagerman, R. J. (2006). Lessons from fragile X regarding neurobiology, autism, and

neurodegeneration. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(1), 63-74.

Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1998). Multivariate data

analysis (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.

Handen, B. L., Janosky, J., & McAuliffe, S. (1997). Long-term follow-up of children with

mental retardation/borderline intellectual functioning and ADHD. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 25(4), 287-295.

Handen, B. L., McAuliffe, S., Janosky, J., Feldman, H., & Breaux, A. M. (1994). Classroom

behavior and children with mental retardation: Comparison of children with and

without ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22(3), 267-280.

Handen, B. L., McAuliffe, S., Janosky, J., Feldman, H., & Breaux, A. M. (1998). A playroom

observation procedure to assess children with mental retardation and ADHD. Journal

of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26(4), 269-277.

Harris, J. C. (2006). Intellectual disability: Understanding its development, causes,

classification, evaluation and treatment. New York: Oxford University Press.

Hartley, S., Sikora, D., & McCoy, R. (2008). Prevalence and risk factors of maladaptive

behaviour in young children with autistic disorder. Journal of Intellectual Disability

Research, 52(10), 819-829.

Hastings, R. P., Beck, A., Daley, D., & Hill, C. (2005). Symptoms of ADHD and their

correlates in children with intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental

Disabilities, 26(5), 456-468.

Hastings, R. P., Brown, T., Mount, R. H., & Cormack, K. (2001). Exploration of

psychometric properties of the Developmental Behavior Checklist. Journal of Autism

and Developmental Disorders, 31(4), 423-431.

248
Hatton, D. D., Hooper, S. R., Bailey, D. B., Skinner, M. L., Sullivan, K. M., & Wheeler, A.

(2002). Problem behavior in boys with fragile X syndrome. American Journal of

Medical Genetics, 108(2), 105-116.

Hattori, J., Ogino, T., Abiru, K., Nakano, K., Oka, M., & Ohtsuka, Y. (2006). Are pervasive

developmental disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder distinct

disorders? Brain & Development, 28(6), 371-374.

Hawes, D. J., & Dadds, M. R. (2004). Australian data and psychometric properties of the

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. Australian and New Zealand Journal of

Psychiatry, 38(8), 644-651.

Hay, D. A., Bennett, K. S., Levy, F., Sergeant, J., & Swanson, J. (2007). A twin study of

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder dimensions rated by the Strengths and

Weaknesses of ADHD-Symptoms and Normal-Behavior (SWAN) Scale. Biological

Psychiatry, 61(5), 700-705.

Henry, L. A., & MacLean, M. (2002). Working memory performance in children with and

without intellectual disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 107(6),

421-432.

Hill, C. R., & Hughes, J. N. (2007). An examination of the convergent and discriminant

validity of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. School Psychology Quarterly,

22(3), 380-406.

Hill, J. C., & Schoener, E. P. (1996). Age-dependent decline of attention deficit hyperactivity

disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153(9), 1143-1146.

Hollingshead, B. A. (1975). Four factor index of socioeconomic status. New Haven, CT:

Yale University.

249
Holtmann, M., Bolte, S., & Poustka, F. (2005). Letters to the editor: ADHD, Aspergers

syndrome, and high-functioning autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(11), 1101.

Holtmann, M., Bolte, S., & Poustka, F. (2007). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

symptoms in Pervasive Developmental Disorders: Association with autistic behavior

domains and coexisting psychopathology. Psychopathology, 40(3), 172-177.

Hooper, S. R., Hatton, D., Sideris, J., Sullivan, K., Hammer, J., Schaaf, J., . . . Bailey, D. B.,

Jr. (2008). Executive functions in young males with fragile X syndrome in

comparison to mental age-matched controls: Baseline findings from a longitudinal

study. Neuropsychology, 22(1), 36-47.

Hoza, B., Mrug, S., Gerdes, A. C., Hinshaw, S. P., Bukowski, W. M., Gold, J. A., . . . Arnold,

L. (2005). What aspects of peer relationships are impaired in children with attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(3),

411-423.

Hudziak, J. J., Copeland, W., Stanger, C., & Wadsworth, M. (2004). Screening for DSM-IV

externalizing disorders with the Child Behavior Checklist: A receiver-operating

characteristic analysis. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 45(7), 1299-1307.

Hussain, J., Woolf, A. D., Sandel, M., & Shannon, M. W. (2007). Environmental evaluation

of a child with developmental disability. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 54(1),

47-62.

Iarocci, G., & Burack, J. A. (1998). Understanding the development of attention in persons

with mental retardation: Challenging the myths. In J. A. Burack, R. M. Hodapp & E.

Zigler (Eds.), Handbook of mental retardation and development (pp. 349-381). New

York: Cambridge University Press.

250
Iarocci, G., & Burack, J. A. (2004). Intact covert orienting to peripheral cues among children

with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 257-264.

Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., & Hewes, A. K. (1998). Verbal and nonverbal abilities in the

Williams syndrome phenotype: Evidence for diverging developmental trajectories.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(4), 511-523.

Jarrold, C., Gilchrist, I. D., & Bender, A. (2005). Embedded figures detection in autism and

typical development: Preliminary evidence of a double dissociation in relationships

with visual search. Developmental Science, 8(4), 344-351.

Jensen, V. K., Larrieu, J. A., & Mack, K. K. (1997). Differential diagnosis between attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder and pervasive developmental disorder - not otherwise

specified. Clinical Pediatrics, 36(10), 555-561.

Jerome, L. (2000). Central auditory processing disorder and ADHD. Journal of the American

Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(4), 399-400.

Johnson, K. A., Robertson, I. H., Kelly, S. P., Silk, T. J., Barry, E., Daibhis, A., . . .

Bellgrove, M. A. (2007). Dissociation in performance of children with ADHD and

high-functioning autism on a task of sustained attention. Neuropsychologia, 45(10),

2234-2245.

Jones, W. P., Bellugi, U., Lai, Z., Chiles, M., Reilly, J., Lincoln, A., & Adolphs, R. (2000).

II. Hypersociability in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,

12(Suppl 1), 30-46.

Jopp, D. A., & Keys, C. B. (2001). Diagnostic overshadowing reviewed and reconsidered.

American Journal on Mental Retardation, 106(5), 416-433.

Joseph, R. M., Keehn, B., Connolly, C., Wolfe, J. M., & Horowitz, T. S. (2009). Why is

visual search superior in autism spectrum disorder? Developmental Science, 12(6),

1083-1096.

251
Kaat, A. J., Lecavalier, L., & Aman, M. (2013). Validity of the Aberrant Behavior Checklist

in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders.

Kadesjo, C., Hagglof, B., Kadesjo, B., & Gillberg, C. (2003). Attention-deficit-hyperactivity

disorder with and without oppositional defiant disorder in 3- to 7- year-old children.

Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 45(10), 693-699.

Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice Hall.

Kaplan, P., Wang, P. P., & Francke, U. (2001). Williams (Williams Beuren) syndrome: A

distinct neurobehavioral disorder. Journal of Child Neurology, 16(3), 177-190.

Kaptein, S., Jansen, D., Vogels, A., & Reijneveld, S. (2008). Mental health problems in

children with intellectual disability: Use of the Strengths and Difficulties

Questionnaire. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 52(2), 125-131.

Karabekiroglu, K., & Aman, M. G. (2009). Validity of the Aberrant Behavior Checklist in a

clinical sample of toddlers. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 40(1), 99-110.

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Nativism versus neuroconstructivism: Rethinking the study of

developmental disorders. Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 56-63.

Keen, D., & Ward, S. L. (2004). Autistic spectrum disorder: A child population profile.

Autism, 8(1), 39-48.

Kieling, C., Kieling, R. R., Rohde, L. A., Frick, P. J., Moffitt, T., Nigg, J. T., . . . Castellanos,

F. X. (2010). The age at onset of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American

Journal of Psychiatry, 167(1), 14-16.

Kim, J., Park, K., Cheon, K., Kim, B., Cho, S., & Hong, K. E. (2005). The child behavior

checklist together with the ADHD rating scale can diagnose ADHD in Korean

community-based samples. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 50(12), 802-805.

252
Klenberg, L., Korkman, M., & Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (2001). Differential development of

attention and executive functions in 3- to 12-year-old Finnish children.

Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 407-428.

Klin, A., Pauls, D., Schultz, R. T., & Volkmar, F. (2005). Three diagnostic approaches to

Asperger Syndrome: Implications for research. Journal of Autism and Developmental

Disorders, 35(2), 221-234.

Kochanska, G., DeVet, K., Goldman, M., Murray, K., & Putnam, S. (1994). Maternal reports

of conscience development and temperament in young children. Child Development,

65(3), 852-868.

Kollins, S., Greenhill, L., Swanson, J., Wigal, S., Abikoff, H., McCracken, J., . . . Bauzo, A.

(2006). Rationale, design, and methods of the Preschool ADHD Treatment Study

(PATS). Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(11),

1275-1283.

Konstantareas, M., & Stewart, K. (2006). Affect regulation and temperament in children with

autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(2),

143-154.

Koskentausta, T., Iivanainen, M., & Almqvist, F. (2004). CBCL in the assessment of

psychopathology in Finnish children with intellectual disability. Research in

Developmental Disabilities, 25(4), 341-354.

Lachiewicz, A. M., & Dawson, D. V. (1994). Behavior problems of young girls with fragile

X syndrome: Factor scores on the Conners' Parent's Questionnaire. American Journal

of Medical Genetics, 51(4), 364-369.

Lahey, B. B., Applegate, B., McBurnett, K., Biederman, J., Greenhill, L., Hynd, G. W., . . .

Shaffer, D. (1994). DSM-IV field trials for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in

children and adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151(11), 1673-1685.

253
Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children

with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(6), 1115-1122.

Lavie, N., Hirst, A., de Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention

and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(3), 339-

354.

Lavigne, J. V., Dulcan, M. K., LeBailly, S. A., & Binns, H. J. (2012). Can parent reports

serve as a proxy for teacher ratings in medication management of attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder? Journal of Developmental And Behavioral Pediatrics, 33(4),

336-342.

le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P., Robertson, S., Holdgrafer, M., & McLennan, J.

(1989). Autism Diagnostic Interview: A standardized investigator-based instrument.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(3), 363-387.

Lecavalier, L. (2006). Behavioral and emotional problems in young people with pervasive

developmental disorders: Relative prevalence, effects of subject characteristics, and

empirical classification. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(8),

1101-1114.

Lecavalier, L., Aman, M. G., Hammer, D., Stoica, W., & Mathews, G. L. (2004). Factor

analysis of the Nisonger Child Behavior Rating Form in children with autism

spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(6), 709-721.

Lecavalier, L., Leone, S., & Wiltz, J. (2006). The impact of behaviour problems on caregiver

stress in young people with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Intellectual

Disability Research, 50(3), 172-183.

Lee, D. O., & Ousley, O. Y. (2006). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in a

clinic sample of children and adolescents with pervasive developmental disorders.

Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 16(6), 737-746.

254
Lee, P., & Friedlander, R. (2007). Attention-deficit and disruptive behavior disorders. In R.

Fletcher, E. Loschen, C. Stavrakaki & M. First (Eds.), Diagnostic Manual -

Intellectual Disability (DM-ID): A textbook of diagnosis of mental disorders in

persons with intellectual disability. New York: National Association for the Dually

Diagnosed Press.

Levy, F., Hay, D. A., McStephen, M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I. (1997). Attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder: A category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale

twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,

36(6), 737-744.

Leyfer, O. T., Folstein, S. E., Bacalman, S., Davis, N. O., Dinh, E., Morgan, J., . . . Lainhart,

J. E. (2006). Comorbid psychiatric disorders in children with autism: Interview

development and rates of disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,

36(7), 849-861.

Leyfer, O. T., Woodruff-Borden, J., Klein-Tasman, B. P., Fricke, J. S., Mervis, C. B., Leyfer,

O. T., . . . Mervis, C. B. (2006). Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in 4 to 16-year-

olds with Williams syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B,

141B(6), 615-622.

Lindblad, I., Gillberg, C., & Fernell, E. (2011). ADHD and other associated developmental

problems in children with mild mental retardation. The use of the "Five-To-Fifteen"

questionnaire in a population-based sample. Research in Developmental Disabilities,

32(6), 2805-2809.

Lo-Castro, A., D'Agati, E., & Curatolo, P. (2011). ADHD and genetic syndromes. Brain &

Development, 33(6), 456-461.

Loane, M., Morris, J. K., Addor, M., Arriola, L., Budd, J., Doray, B., . . . Dolk, H. (2013).

Twenty-year trends in the prevalence of Down syndrome and other trisomies in

255
Europe: Impact of maternal age and prenatal screening. European Journal of Human

Genetics, 21(1), 27-33.

Loesch, D. Z., Bui, Q. M., Grigsby, J., Butler, E., Epstein, J. N., Huggins, R. M., . . .

Hagerman, R. J. (2003). Effect of the Fragile X status categories and the Fragile X

mental retardation protein levels on executive functioning in males and females with

Fragile X. Neuropsychology, 17(4), 646-657.

Logan, G. D., Schachar, R., & Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and impulse control.

Psychological Science, 8(1), 60-64.

Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism Diagnostic Observation

Schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Luyster, R. J., Seery, A., Talbott, M. R., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2011). Identifying early-risk

markers and developmental trajectories for language impairment in

neurodevelopmental disorders. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 17(2),

151-159.

Magnusson, P., Smari, J., Gretarsdottir, H., & Prandardottir, H. (1999). Attention-

deficit/hyperactivity symptoms in Icelandic schoolchildren: Assessment with the

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Rating Scale-IV. Scandinavian Journal of

Psychology, 40(4), 301-306.

Mahan, S., & Matson, J. L. (2011). Children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders

compared to typically developing controls on the Behavioral Assessment System for

Children, Second Edition (BASC-2). Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1),

119-125.

Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P., & Robertson, I. H.

(2001). The differential assessment of children's attention: The Test of Everyday

256
Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(8), 1065-1081.

Mannuzza, S., Klein, R. G., Bessler, A., Malloy, P., & LaPadula, M. (1993). Adult outcome

of hyperactive boys: Educational achievement, occupational rank, and psychiatric

status. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50(7), 565-576.

Marshburn, E. C., & Aman, M. G. (1992). Factor validity and norms for the Aberrant

Behavior Checklist in a community sample of children with mental retardation.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22(3), 357-373.

Mason, J., & Scior, K. (2004). 'Diagnostic overshadowing' amongst clinicians working with

people with intellectual disabilities in the UK. Journal of Applied Research in

Intellectual Disabilities, 17(2), 85-90.

Mattos, P., Serra-Pinheiro, M. A., Rohde, L. A., & Pinto, D. (2006). A Brazilian version of

the MTA-SNAP-IV for evaluation of symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder and oppositional-defiant disorder. Revista de Psiquatria do Rio Grande do

Sul, 28(3), 1-19.

May, T., Cornish, K., & Rinehart, N. (2013). Does gender matter? A one year follow-up of

autistic, attention and anxiety symptoms in high-functioning children with autism

spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

McCarthy, J., & Boyd, J. (2001). Psychopathology and young people with Down's syndrome:

Childhood predictors and adult outcome of disorder. Journal of Intellectual Disability

Research, 45(2), 99-105.

McGoey, K. E., DuPaul, G. J., Haley, E., & Shelton, T. L. (2007). Parent and teacher ratings

of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in preschool: The ADHD rating scale-IV

preschool version. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 29(4),

269-276.

257
McInerny, T. K., Adam, H. M., Campbell, D. E., Kamat, D. M., & Kelleher, K. J. (Eds.).

(2009). American Academy of Pediatrics Textbook of Pediatric Care. Elk Grove

Village, IL: American Academy of Pediatrics.

McKinlay, A., Grace, R. C., Horwood, L. J., Fergusson, D. M., & MacFarlane, M. R. (2010).

Long-term behavioural outcomes of pre-school mild traumatic brain injury. Child:

Care, Health & Development, 36(1), 22-30.

Melnyk, L., & Das, J. (1992). Measurement of attention deficit: Correspondence between

rating scales and tests of sustained and selective attention. American Journal on

Mental Retardation, 96(6), 599-606.

Mendola, P., Selevan, S. G., Gutter, S., & Rice, D. (2002). Environmental factors associated

with a spectrum of neurodevelopmental deficits. Mental Retardation and

Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 8(3), 188-197.

Mervis, C. B., & John, A. E. (2010). Cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with

Williams syndrome: Implications for intervention approaches. American Journal of

Medical Genetics. Part C, Seminars in Medical Genetics, 154C(2), 229-248.

Mervis, C. B., & Klein-Tasman, B. P. (2000). Williams syndrome: Cognition, personality,

and adaptive behavior. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research

Reviews, 6(2), 148-158.

Metcalfe, L. A., Harvey, E. A., & Laws, H. B. (2013). The longitudinal relation between

academic/cognitive skills and externalizing behavior problems in preschool children.

Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(3), 881-894.

Milberger, S., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Guite, J., & Tsuang, M. T. (1997). Pregnancy,

delivery and infancy complications and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: issues

of gene-environment interaction. Biological Psychiatry, 41(1), 65-75.

258
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our

capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81-97.

Miller, M. L., Fee, V. E., & Jones, C. J. (2004). Psychometric properties of ADHD rating

scales among children with mental retardation II: Validity. Research in

Developmental Disabilities, 25(5), 477-492.

Miller, M. L., Fee, V. E., & Netterville, A. K. (2004). Psychometric properties of ADHD

rating scales among children with mental retardation I: Reliability. Research in

Developmental Disabilities, 25(5), 459-476.

Mircea, C. E., Rojahn, J., & Esbensen, A. J. (2010). Psychometric evaluation of Romanian

translations of the Behavior Problems Inventory-01 and the Nisonger Child Behavior

Rating Form. Journal of Mental Health Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 3(1), 51-

65.

Mirsky, A. F., Anthony, B. J., Duncan, C. C., Ahearn, M. B., & Kellam, S. G. (1991).

Analysis of the elements of attention: A neuropsychological approach.

Neuropsychology Review, 2(2), 109-145.

Monuteaux, M. C., Mick, E., Faraone, S. V., & Biederman, J. (2010). The influence of sex on

the course and psychiatric correlates of ADHD from childhood to adolescence: A

longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(3), 233-241.

Morris, C. A., & Mervis, C. B. (1999). Williams syndrome. In C. R. Reynolds & S. Goldstein

(Eds.), Handbook of neurodevelopmental and genetic disorders in children (pp. 555-

590). New York: Guilford Press.

Morris, J. K., & Alberman, E. (2009). Trends in Down's syndrome live births and antenatal

diagnoses in England and Wales from 1989 to 2008: Analysis of data from the

National Down Syndrome Cytogenetic Register. British Medical Journal, 339, b3794.

259
Munir, F., Cornish, K. M., & Wilding, J. (2000a). Nature of the working memory deficit in

Fragile-X syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 44(3), 387-401.

Munir, F., Cornish, K. M., & Wilding, J. (2000b). A neuropsychological profile of attention

deficits in young males with fragile X syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 38(9), 1261-

1270.

Murray, D. W., Kollins, S. H., Hardy, K. K., Abikoff, H. B., Swanson, J. M., Cunningham,

C., . . . Chuang, S. Z. (2007). Parent versus teacher ratings of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in the Preschoolers with Attention-

Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Treatment Study (PATS). Journal of Child and

Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 17(5), 605-619.

Myers, C. L., Bour, J. L., Sidebottom, K. J., Murphy, S. B., & Hakman, M. (2010). Same

constructs, different results: Examining the consistency of two behavior-rating scales

with referred preschoolers. Psychology in the Schools, 47(3), 205-216.

Neece, C., Baker, B., Blacher, J., & Crnic, K. (2011). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

among children with and without intellectual disability: An examination across time.

Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 55(7), 623-635.

Nigg, J. T., Stavro, G., Ettenhofer, M., Hambrick, D. Z., Miller, T., & Henderson, J. M.

(2005). Executive functions and ADHD in adults: Evidence for selective effects on

ADHD symptom domains. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 706-717.

Normand, S., Schneider, B. H., & Robaey, P. (2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

and the challenges of close friendship. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry, 16(2), 67-73.

Norris, M., & Lecavalier, L. (2011). Evaluating the validity of the Nisonger Child Behavior

Rating Form--Parent Version. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(6), 2894-

2900.

260
Nøvik, T. S., Hervas, A., Ralston, S. J., Dalsgaard, S., Pereira, R. R., & Lorenzo, M. J.

(2006). Influence of gender on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Europe -

ADORE. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 15(Suppl 1), 5-24.

Nygaard, E., Smith, L., & Torgersen, A. M. (2002). Temperament in children with Down

syndrome and in prematurely born children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology,

43(1), 61-71.

O'Brien, G. (2000). Learning disability. In C. Gillberg & G. O'Brien (Eds.), Developmental

disability and behaviour (pp. 12-26). London: MacKeith Press.

Ohan, J. L., & Johnston, C. (2005). Gender appropriateness of symptom criteria for

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Oppositional-Defiant Disorder, and

Conduct Disorder. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 35(4), 359-381.

Olson, S., Lopez-Duran, N., Lunkenheimer, E. S., Chang, H., & Sameroff, A. J. (2011).

Individual differences in the development of early peer aggression: Integrating

contributions of self-regulation, theory of mind, and parenting. Development and

Psychopathology, 23(1), 253-266.

Paclawskyj, T. R., Matson, J. L., Bamburg, J. W., & Baglio, C. S. (1997). A comparison of

the Diagnostic Assessment for the Severely Handicapped-II (DASH-II) and the

Aberrant Behavior Checklist (ABC). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 18(4),

289-298.

Pallant, J. F., & Tennant, A. (2007). An introduction to the Rasch measurement model: An

example using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS). British Journal of

Clinical Psychology, 46(1), 1-18.

Paloyelis, Y., Asherson, P., & Kuntsi, J. (2009). Are ADHD symptoms associated with delay

aversion or choice impulsivity? A general population study. Journal of the American

Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(8), 837-846.

261
Pandolfi, V., Magyar, C. I., & Dill, C. A. (2009). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Child

Behavior Checklist 1.5-5 in a sample of children with autism spectrum disorders.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(7), 986-995.

Pandolfi, V., Magyar, C. I., & Dill, C. A. (2012). An initial psychometric evaluation of the

CBCL 6-18 in a sample of youth with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism

Spectrum Disorders, 6(1), 96-108.

Pani, J. R., Mervis, C. B., & Robinson, B. F. (1999). Global spatial organization by

individuals with Williams syndrome. Psychological Science, 10(5), 453-458.

Papaeliou, C., Polemikos, N., Fryssira, E., Kodakos, A., Kaila, M., Yiota, X., . . .

Vrettopoulou, M. (2012). Behavioural profile and maternal stress in Greek young

children with Williams syndrome. Child: Care, Health & Development, 38(6), 844-

853.

Pearson, D. A., & Aman, M. G. (1994). Ratings of hyperactivity and developmental indices:

Should clinicians correct for developmental level? Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 24(4), 395-411.

Pearson, D. A., Yaffee, L. S., Loveland, K. A., & Lewis, K. R. (1996). Comparison of

sustained and selective attention in children who have mental retardation with and

without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal on Mental

Retardation, 100(6), 592-607.

Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental

psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 37(1), 51-87.

Percy, M. (2007). Factors that cause or contribute to intellectual and developmental

disabilities. In I. Brown & M. Percy (Eds.), A comprehensive guide to intellectual and

developmental disabilities. Baltimore: Paul H Brookes Publishing Co.

262
Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years

after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73-89.

Polanczyk, G., Caspi, A., Houts, R., Kollins, S. H., Rohde, L. A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2010).

Implications of extending the ADHD age-of-onset criterion to age 12: Results from a

prospectively studied birth cohort. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(3), 210-216.

Polanczyk, G., de Lima, M. S., Horta, B. L., Biederman, J., & Rohde, L. A. (2007). The

worldwide prevalence of ADHD: A systematic review and metaregression analysis.

American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(6), 942-948.

Polanczyk, G., & Jensen, P. (2008). Epidemiologic considerations in attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder: A review and update. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric

Clinics of North America, 17(2), 245-260.

Polanczyk, G., & Rohde, L. A. (2007). Epidemiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder across the lifespan. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 20(4), 386-392.

Polderman, T., Boomsma, D., Bartels, M., Verhulst, F., & Huizink, A. (2010). A systematic

review of prospective studies on attention problems and academic achievement. Acta

Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 122(4), 271-284.

Posner, K., Melvin, G. A., Murray, D. W., Gugga, S., Fisher, P., Skrobala, A., . . . Greenhill,

L. L. (2007). Clinical presentation of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in

preschool children: The Preschoolers with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Treatment

Study (PATS). Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 17(5), 547-

562.

Power, T. J., Andrews, T. J., Eiraldi, R. B., Doherty, B. J., Ikeda, M. J., DuPaul, G. J., &

Landau, S. (1998). Evaluating attention deficit hyperactivity disorder using multiple

263
informants: The incremental utility of combining teacher with parent reports.

Psychological Assessment, 10(3), 250-260.

Pueschel, S. M., Bernier, J., & Pezzullo, J. (1991). Behavioural observations in children with

Down's syndrome. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 35(6), 502-511.

Putnam, S. P., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Development of short and very short forms of the

Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87(1), 102-

112.

Pyett, P. M. (2003). Validation of qualitative research in the 'real world'. Qualitative Health

Research, 13(8), 1170-1179.

Rabiner, D., & Coie, J. D. (2000). Early attention problems and children's reading

achievement: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of the American Academy of Child

& Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(7), 859-867.

Ramtekkar, U. P., Reiersen, A. M., Todorov, A. A., & Todd, R. D. (2010). Sex and age

differences in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and diagnoses:

Implications for DSM-V and ICD-11. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(3), 217-228.

Rebok, G. W., Smith, C. B., Pascualvaca, D. M., Mirsky, A. F., Anthony, B. J., & Kellam, S.

G. (1997). Developmental changes in attentional performance in urban children from

eight to thirteen years. Child Neuropsychology, 3(1), 28-46.

Reid, R., DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., Rogers-Adkinson, D., Noll, M., &

Riccio, C. (1998). Assessing culturally different students for attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder using behavior rating scales. Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology, 26(3), 187-198.

264
Reid, R., & Maag, J. W. (1994). How many fidgets in a pretty much: A critique of behavior

rating scales for identifying students with ADHD. Journal of School Psychology,

32(4), 339-354.

Reiersen, A. M., Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2008). Co-occurrence of motor problems

and autistic symptoms in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(6), 662-672.

Research Unit on Pediatric Psychopharmacology. (2010). The Nisonger Child Behavior

Rating Form Retrieved April, 27, 2010, from psychmed.osu.edu/ncbrf.htm

Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2004). Behavior Assessment System for Children -

Second edition. Circle Pines, Minnesota: American Guidance Service, Inc.

Rhodes, S. M., Riby, D. M., Matthews, K., & Coghill, D. R. (2011). Attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder and Williams syndrome: Shared behavioral and

neuropsychological profiles. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,

33(1), 147-156.

Rhodes, S. M., Riby, D. M., Park, J., Fraser, E., & Campbell, L. E. (2010). Executive

neuropsychological functioning in individuals with Williams syndrome.

Neuropsychologia, 48(5), 1216-1226.

Riby, D. M., Doherty-Sneddon, G., & Bruce, V. (2008). Exploring face perception in

disorders of development: Evidence from Williams syndrome and autism. Journal of

Neuropsychology, 2(1), 47-64.

Riccio, C. A., & Hynd, G. W. (1996). Relationship between ADHD and central auditory

processing disorder. School Psychology International, 17(3), 235-252.

Riccio, C. A., Hynd, G. W., Cohen, M. J., & Hall, J. R. (1994). Comorbidity of central

auditory processing disorder and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of

the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 33(6), 849-857.

265
Robaey, P., Amre, D., Schachar, R., & Simard, L. (2007). French version of the Strengths

and Weaknesses of ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behaviors (SWAN-F)

questionnaire. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,

16(2), 80-89.

Rohde, L. A. (2008). Is there a need to reformulate attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

criteria in future nosologic classifications? Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics

of North America, 17(2), 405-420.

Rojahn, J., Rowe, E. W., Macken, J., Gray, A., Delitta, D., Booth, A., & Kimbrell, K. (2010).

Psychometric evaluation of the Behavior Problems Inventory-01 and the Nisonger

Child Behavior Rating Form with children and adolescents. Journal of Mental Health

Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 3(1), 28-50.

Rommelse, N. N. J., Franke, B., Geurts, H. M., Hartman, C. A., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2010).

Shared heritability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum

disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 19(3), 281-295.

Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52(2),

569-578.

Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hersey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of

temperament at three to seven years: The Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Child

Development, 72(5), 1394-1408.

Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In W. Damon, R. M. Lerner & N.

Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Volume 3: Social, emotional and

personality development (pp. 99-166). New York: Wiley.

Royal College of Psychiatrists. (2001). DC-LD: Diagnostic criteria for psychiatric disorders

for use with adults with learning disabilities/mental retardation. London: Gaskell.

266
Ruff, H. A., & Capozzoli, M. C. (2003). Development of attention and distractibility in the

first 4 years of life. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 877-890.

Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ). Los

Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Rutter, M., le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Autism Diagnostic Interview - Revised Manual.

Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Sansone, S. M., Widaman, K. F., Hall, S. S., Reiss, A. L., Lightbody, A., Kaufmann, W. E., .

. . Hessl, D. (2012). Psychometric study of the aberrant behavior checklist in fragile X

syndrome and implications for targeted treatment. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 42(7), 1377-1392.

Sattler, J. M. (2001). Assessment of children: Cognitive applications (4th ed.). San Diego:

Author.

Scerif, G. (2010). Attention trajectories, mechanisms and outcomes: At the interface between

developing cognition and environment. Developmental Science, 13(6), 805-812.

Scerif, G., Cornish, K., Wilding, J., Driver, J., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2004). Visual search

in typically developing toddlers and toddlers with Fragile X or Williams syndrome.

Developmental Science, 7(1), 116-130.

Scerif, G., Cornish, K., Wilding, J., Driver, J., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2007). Delineation of

early attentional control difficulties in fragile X syndrome: Focus on

neurocomputational changes. Neuropsychologia, 45(8), 1889-1898.

Scerif, G., Longhi, E., Cole, V., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Cornish, K. (2012). Attention across

modalities as a longitudinal predictor of early outcomes: The case of fragile X

syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(6), 641-650.

Scharf, J. M., & Mathews, C. A. (2010). Copy number variation in Tourette syndrome:

Another case of neurodevelopmental generalist genes? Neurology, 74(20), 1564-1565.

267
Schaughency, E. A., & Rothlind, J. (1991). Assessment and classification of Attention Deficit

Hyperactive Disorders. School Psychology Review, 20(2), 187-202.

Seager, M. C., & O'Brien, O. (2003). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Review of

ADHD in learning disability: The diagnostic criteria for psychiatric disorders for use

with adults with learning disabilities/mental retardation [DC-LD] criteria for

diagnosis. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47(Suppl. 1), 26-31.

Seltzer, M. M., Shattuck, P., Abbeduto, L., & Greenberg, J. S. (2004). Trajectory of

development in adolescents and adults with autism. Mental Retardation and

Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10(4), 234-247.

Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H., Huijbregts, S., Scheres, A., & Oosterlaan, J. (2003). The top and

the bottom of ADHD: A neuropsychological perspective. Neuroscience and

Biobehavioral Reviews, 27(7), 583-592.

Shaffer, D. R. (2010). Cognitive development: Information-processing perspectives. In D. R.

Shaffer & K. Kipp (Eds.), Developmental psychology: Childhood and adolescence

(8th ed.) (pp. 299-342). Belmont, Canada: Wadsworth.

Shin, M., Besser, L. M., Kucik, J. E., Lu, C., Siffel, C., Correa, A., & Collaborative., C. A.

M. P. a. S. (2009). Prevalence of Down syndrome among children and adolescents in

10 regions of the United States. Pediatrics, 124(6), 1565-1571.

Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., & Baird, G. (2008).

Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: Prevalence,

comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(8), 921-929.

Sinzig, J., Walter, D., & Doepfner, M. (2009). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in

children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: Symptom or syndrome?

Journal of Attention Disorders, 13(2), 117-126.

268
Smalley, S. L., McGough, J. J., Moilanen, I. K., Loo, S. K., Taanila, A., Ebeling, H., . . .

Jarvelin, M. (2007). Prevalence and psychiatric comorbidity of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder in an adolescent Finnish population. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(12), 1575-1583.

Solanto, M. V., Abikoff, H., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Schachar, R., Logan, G. D., Wigal, T., . . .

Turkel, E. (2001). The ecological validity of delay aversion and response inhibition as

measures of impulsivity in AD/HD: A supplement to the NIMH multimodal treatment

study of AD/HD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(3), 215-228.

Solanto, M. V., & Alvir, J. (2009). Reliability of DSM-IV symptom ratings of ADHD:

Implications for DSM-V. Journal of Attention Disorders, 13(2), 107-116.

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2005). Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: From

common simple deficits to multiple developmental pathways. Biological Psychiatry,

57(11), 1231-1238.

Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Taylor, E., Sembi, S., & Smith, J. (1992). Hyperactivity and delay

aversion: I. The effect of delay on choice. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry,

33(2), 387-398.

Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales,

Second edition - Teacher rating form manual. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson.

Spira, E. G., & Fischel, J. E. (2005). The impact of preschool inattention, hyperactivity, and

impulsivity on social and academic development: A review. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(7), 755-773.

Staller, J., & Faraone, S. V. (2006). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in girls:

Epidemiology and management. CNS Drugs, 20(2), 107-123.

269
Steele, A., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Cornish, K., & Scerif, G. (2012). The multiple subfunctions

of attention: Differential developmental gateways to literacy and numeracy. Child

Development, 83(6), 2028-2041.

Steele, A., Scerif, G., Cornish, K., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2013). Learning to read in

Williams syndrome and Down syndrome: Syndrome-specific precursors and

developmental trajectories. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(7), 754-

762.

Stevens, J., Quittner, A. L., & Abikoff, H. (1998). Factors influencing elementary school

teachers' ratings of ADHD and ODD behaviors. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,

27(4), 406-414.

Stores, R., Stores, G., Fellows, B., & Buckley, S. (1998). Daytime behaviour problems and

maternal stress in children with Down's syndrome, their siblings, and non-

intellectually disabled and other intellectually disabled peers. Journal of Intellectual

Disability Research, 42(3), 228-237.

Strand, S., Sturmey, P., & Newton, J. (1990). A classroom rating scale for use with mentally

handicapped children: A replication of the ADIECAS. British Journal of Clinical

Psychology, 29(1), 121-123.

Stroh, C. M. (1971). Vigilance: The problem of sustained attention. Oxford: Pergamon Press.

Stromme, P., Bjornstad, P. G., & Ramstad, K. (2002). Prevalence estimation of Williams

syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, 17(4), 269-271.

Sturm, H., Fernell, E., & Gillberg, C. (2004). Autism spectrum disorders in children with

normal intellectual levels: Associated impairments and subgroups. Developmental

Medicine & Child Neurology, 46(7), 444-447.

Styles, E. A. (2006). The psychology of attention. New York: Psychology Press.

270
Sullivan, J. R., & Riccio, C. A. (2007). Diagnostic group differences in parent and teacher

ratings on the BRIEF and Conners' Scales. Journal of Attention Disorders, 11(3), 398-

406.

Sullivan, K., Hatton, D., Hammer, J., Sideris, J., Hooper, S., Ornstein, P., & Bailey, D., Jr.

(2006). ADHD symptoms in children with FXS. American Journal of Medical

Genetics Part A, 140(21), 2275-2288.

Sullivan, K., Hatton, D. D., Hammer, J., Sideris, J., Hooper, S., Ornstein, P. A., & Bailey, D.

B., Jr. (2007). Sustained attention and response inhibition in boys with fragile X

syndrome: measures of continuous performance. American Journal of Medical

Genetics Part B, 4, 517-532.

Swanson, J. M. (1992). School-based assessments and interventions for ADD students.

Irvine, California: KC Publishing.

Swanson, J. M. (n.d.). ADHD.net: The SNAP-IV Teacher and Parent Rating Scale Retrieved

March 17, 2010, from http://www.adhd.net/snap-iv-form.pdf

Swanson, J. M., Schuck, S., Mann, M., Carlson, C., Hartman, K., Sergeant, J., . . . McCleary,

R. (2005). Categorical and dimensional definitions and evaluations of symptoms of

ADHD: The SNAP and the SWAN Ratings Scales (Draft) Retrieved 17/03/2010,

from www.adhd.net/SNAP_SWAN.pdf

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Needham

Heights, Minnesota: Allyn & Bacon.

Taffe, J. R., Tonge, B. J., Gray, K. M., & Einfeld, S. L. (2008). Extracting more information

from behaviour checklists by using components of mean based scores. International

Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 17(4), 232-240.

Tan, C. S. (2007). Test Review: Behavior Assessment System for Children (2nd ed.).

Assessment for Effective Intervention, 32(2), 121-124.

271
Tandon, M., Si, X., Belden, A., & Luby, J. (2009). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in

preschool children: An investigation of validation based on visual attention

performance. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 19(2), 137-146.

Tanner, W. P., Jr., & Swets, J. A. (1954). A decision-making theory of visual detection.

Psychological Review, 61(6), 401-409.

Tasse, M. J., Aman, M. G., Hammer, D., & Rojahn, J. (1996). The Nisonger Child Behavior

Rating Form: Age and gender effects and norms. Research in Developmental

Disabilities, 17(1), 59-75.

Tasse, M. J., & Lecavalier, L. (2000). Comparing parent and teacher ratings of social

competence and problem behaviors. American Journal on Mental Retardation,

105(4), 252-259.

Tennant, A., & Conaghan, P. G. (2007). The Rasch measurement model in rheumatology:

What is it and why use it? When should it be applied, and what should one look for in

a Rasch paper? Arthritis & Rheumatism, 57(8), 1358-1362.

Thapar, A., Fowler, T., Rice, F., Scourfield, J., van den Bree, M., Thomas, H., . . . Hay, D.

(2003). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity

Disorder symptoms in offspring. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(11), 1985-

1989.

The MTA Cooperative Group. (1999). A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment

strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: The multimodal treatment study

of children with ADHD. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56(12), 1073-1086.

Thompson, A. L., Molina, B. S., Pelham, W., Jr., & Gnagy, E. M. (2007). Risky driving in

adolescents and young adults with childhood ADHD. Journal of Pediatric

Psychology, 32(7), 745-759.

272
Timimi, S., & Taylor, E. (2004). ADHD is best understood as a cultural construct. British

Journal of Psychiatry, 184(1), 8-9.

Todd, R. D., Huang, H., & Henderson, C. A. (2008). Poor utility of the age of onset criterion

for DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Recommendations for DSM-V

and ICD-11. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(9), 942-949.

Tonge, B. J., & Einfeld, S. L. (2003). Psychopathology and intellectual disability: The

Australian child to adult longitudinal study. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), International

review of research in mental retardation (pp. 61-91). San Diego, California:

Academic Press.

Toplak, M. E., Bucciarelli, S. M., Jain, U., & Tannock, R. (2009). Executive functions:

Performance-based measures and the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive

Function (BRIEF) in adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder

(ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, 15(1), 53-72.

Turk, J. (1998). Fragile X syndrome and attentional deficit. Journal of Applied Research in

Intellectual Disabilities, 11, 175-191.

Turk, J. (2011). Fragile X syndrome: lifespan developmental implications for those without

as well as with intellectual disability. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 24(5), 387-397.

Turk, J., & Cornish, K. (1998). Face recognition and emotion perception in boys with fragile-

X syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 42(6), 490-499.

Turner, S., Sloper, P., & Knussen, C. (1991). The validity and applicability of the ADIECAS

classroom rating scale in a sample of children with Down's syndrome. Journal of

Mental Deficiency Research, 35(4), 384-391.

Ullman, R. K., Sleator, E. K., & Sprague, R. (1984). A new rating scale for diagnosis and

monitoring of ADD children. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 20, 160-164.

273
Ullman, R. K., Sleator, E. K., & Sprague, R. (2000). The ADD-H Comprehensive Teacher

Rating Scale (2nd edition). Champaign, Illinois: Metritech.

van der Ende, J. (1999). Multiple informants: multiple views. In H. Koot, A. Crijnen & R.

Ferdinand (Eds.), Child psychiatric epidemiology: Accomplishments and future

directions (pp. 39-52). Assen, The Netherlands: Van Gorcum.

van der Molen, M., Huizinga, M., Huizenga, H., Ridderinkhof, K., Hamel, B., Curfs, L., &

Ramakers, G. (2010). Profiling Fragile X Syndrome in males: Strengths and

weaknesses in cognitive abilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31(2), 426-

439.

van der Molen, M., van Luit, J., & Jongmans, M. (2007). Verbal working memory in children

with mild intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51(2),

162-169.

van Gameren-Oosterom, H. B. M., Fekkes, M., Buitendijk, S. E., Mohangoo, A. D., Bruil, J.,

& Van Wouwe, J. P. (2011). Development, problem behavior, and quality of life in a

population based sample of eight-year-old children with Down syndrome. PLoS ONE,

6(7), e21879. Retrieved from

Vicari, S., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2006). Short-term memory deficits are not uniform in Down

and Williams Syndromes. Neuropsychology Review, 16(2), 87-94.

Vogt, D. S., King, D. W., & King, L. A. (2004). Focus groups in psychological assessment:

Enhancing content validity by consulting members of the target population.

Psychological Assessment, 16(3), 231-243.

Volkmar, F., Dykens, E., & Hodapp, R. M. (2007). Mental retardation. In M. Andres & F.

Volkmar (Eds.), Lewis's child and adolescent psychiatry: A comprehensive textbook

(4th ed.) (pp. 401-409). Philadelphia: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins.

274
Waschbusch, D. A., King, S., & Gregus, A. (2007). Age of onset of ADHD in a sample of

elementary school students. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,

29(1), 9-16.

Waschbusch, D. A., & Sparkes, S. J. (2003). Rating scale assessment of Attention-

Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD): Is

there a normal distribution and does it matter? Journal of Psychoeducational

Assessment, 21(3), 261-281.

Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence - Third edition.

Sydney, NSW: Pearson.

Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Fourth edition. Sydney,

NSW: Pearson.

Weiler, M. D., Bernstein, J. H., Bellinger, D., & Waber, D. P. (2002). Information processing

deficits in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, inattentive type, and

children with reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35(5), 448-461.

Welsh, M. C., & Pennington, B. F. (1988). Assessing frontal lobe functioning in children:

Views from developmental psychology. Developmental Neuropsychology, 4(3), 199-

230.

Wilding, J., Cornish, K., & Munir, F. (2002). Further delineation of the executive deficit in

males with fragile-X syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 40(8), 1343-1349.

Wilding, J., Munir, F., & Cornish, K. (2001). The nature of attentional differences between

groups of children differentiated by teacher ratings of attention and hyperactivity.

British Journal of Psychology, 92(2), 357-371.

Willcutt, E. G. (2012). The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a

meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics, 9(3), 490-499.

275
Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005).

Validity of the executive function theory of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder:

A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1336-1346.

Williams, B. R., Ponesse, J. S., Schachar, R. J., Logan, G. D., & Tannock, R. (1999).

Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Developmental Psychology,

35(1), 205-213.

Williams, N. M., Zaharieva, I., Martin, A., Langley, K., Mantripragada, K., Fossdal, R., . . .

Thapar, A. (2010). Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications in attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet, 376(9750), 1401-1408.

Witwer, A. N., & Lecavalier, L. (2010). Validity of comorbid psychiatric disorders in

youngsters with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Developmental and Physical

Disabilities, 22(4), 367-380.

Wolraich, M. L., Lambert, E., Bickman, L., Simmons, T., Doffing, M. A., & Worley, K. A.

(2004). Assessing the impact of parent and teacher agreement on diagnosing

Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Developmental And Behavioral

Pediatrics, 25(1), 41-47.

World Health Organization. (1992). International Classification of Diseases, Tenth edition.

Geneva, Switzerland: Author.

World Health Organization. (2007). Atlas: Global resources for people with intellectual

disabilities. Retrieved from www.who.int/mental_health/evidence/atlas_id_2007.pdf

Yoshida, Y., & Uchiyama, T. (2004). The clinical necessity for assessing Attention

Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (AD/HD) symptoms in children with high-functioning

Pervasive Developmental Disorder (PDD). European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,

13(5), 307-314.

276
Young, D. J., Levy, F., Martin, N. C., & Hay, D. A. (2009). Attention deficit hyperactivity

disorder: A rasch analysis of the SWAN rating scale. Child Psychiatry and Human

Development, 40(4), 543-559.

Zhang, S., Faries, D., Vowles, M., & Michelson, D. (2005). ADHD Rating Scale IV:

Psychometric properties from a multinational study as a clinician-administered

instrument. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 14(4), 186-

201.

277
APPENDIX A: Focus group discussion outline

• Small group brainstorming exercise (4-5 students in each group)

Think about students you have worked with, both currently and in the past, who have had
difficulties with hyperactivity, impulsivity, and paying attention in the classroom. What sort
of behaviours come to mind? What behaviours stand out that make them different from other
students with an intellectual disability?

Allow small groups to brainstorm their ideas and write them out on sheets of butcher’s paper.
Come back as a large group with a spokesperson from each group sharing their ideas. One of
the moderators guides the discussion while the other one lists the behaviours named on the
whiteboard. What are the similarities between groups? Differences?

• Focus group questions

We are developing the attached rating scale as part of our study. We have called it the Scale
of Attention in Intellectual Disability, or SAID. We would like you to read the items that
make up this scale and consider the following questions.

1. When reading each item:


a. Is it clearly written?
b. Do you understand the behaviour that you are being asked to rate?
c. Can you think of a way that this behaviour could be expressed better?
d. Do you think this item is needed on the rating scale?
e. Is the item redundant? Are there other items that appear to be asking the same
thing?

2. Are there any behaviours that we have omitted that you think should be included as an
item in this rating scale?

3. Given that this scale has been developed to be completed by teachers, do you think that
they will be able to complete this rating scale?

Please feel free to write on the rating scale or make comments on a separate piece of paper.
Any feedback you can give will be helpful.

A1
APPENDIX B: The Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability
Some students with developmental disabilities have problems with their attention, impulsivity, and
hyperactivity. These issues can interfere with learning.

By completing this checklist, you will help us learn more about these problems. This will assist us to
know how the student might respond to help.

Please rate each item based on your general impression of this student over the past month, and
whether the student exhibits this behaviour independently, without assistance from a teacher or
teacher’s aide. Please circle the 2 if you have observed the behaviour often in this student. Circle 1 if you
have sometimes observed the behaviour. If you have never or rarely observed the behaviour in this
student circle the 0.

0 = never or rarely 1 = sometimes 2 = often

If the student is unable to perform an item, circle the 0. For example, if the student is unable to talk,
then for the item “Speaks at a reasonable pace” circle the 0.

Underline any behaviour you are particularly concerned about


Office Please Circle
Use Only
1.  0 1 2 Asks before joining in a game (avoids butting in or trying to take over the game).
2.  0 1 2 Asks before taking or touching a pencil, toy or something belonging to another person (avoids
grabbing things without asking).

3. 0 1 2 Avoids making the same mistake when corrected once.


4.  0 1 2 Can concentrate for 2 minutes when given an activity to do (does not become fatigued; avoids
getting distracted or disengaged).

5.  0 1 2 Can easily give his/her attention to start a new task.


6.  0 1 2 Can finish work within a reasonable time limit.

7.  0 1 2 Can maintain attention without requiring rewards or praise.


8.  0 1 2 Can recall the main points in a short story that has been read aloud.

9.  0 1 2 Can understand and carry out a simple, two-part instruction (e.g., Take off your shoes and sit
on the floor).
10.  0 1 2 Can verbally repeat back an instruction that has one step and does not require instruction
repeated (e.g., Get your hat).

11.  0 1 2 Can verbally repeat back an instruction that has two steps and does not require instruction
repeated (e.g., Get your bag and sit down).
12.  0 1 2 Completes one activity or task before moving on to another (avoids moving between tasks
without completing any of them).

13.  0 1 2 Completes work with reasonable accuracy if work given is within his/her ability.
14. 0 1 2 Keeps hands still while participating in a short classroom task or listening to a story (does not
excessively fidget, tap, fiddle or pick).

B1
0 = never or rarely 1 = sometimes 2 = often
Underline any behaviour you are particularly concerned about

15.  0 1 2 Keeps hands to self when working in a group activity (does not poke or touch others).
16.  0 1 2 Keeps legs and feet still while participating in a short classroom task or listening to a story (does
not excessively swing legs or rock on chair).

17.  0 1 2 Keeps legs and feet to self when working in a group activity (does not kick others).
18. 0 1 2 Keeps track of personal possessions (e.g., coat, lunch box, pencil case).

19.  0 1 2 Maintains attention when a topic is perceived as very interesting.


20. 0 1 2 Pays attention when spoken to directly (looking in teacher’s direction, body turned towards
teacher).
21.  0 1 2 Persists with a task for 2 minutes even if they find it difficult (avoids giving up).
22.  0 1 2 Plays quietly when they have free time inside (keeps noise down to a minimum).

23. 0 1 2 Remains calm (avoids getting agitated) when changing from one activity or place to another.
24.  0 1 2 Returns attention to task after being distracted by another student.

25. 0 1 2 Shows responsibility for own safety when indoors (e.g., avoids jumping off furniture).
26. 0 1 2 Shows responsibility for own safety when outdoors (e.g., avoids climbing trees or walls when
they might not be able to get down again).

27. 0 1 2 Slows down to a walking pace if asked to do so.

28.  0 1 2 Speaks at a reasonable pace (can be understood, words do not run together).

29.  0 1 2 Speaks at a reasonable volume (not too loud or so soft that cannot be heard).
30.  0 1 2 Stays in own seat when expected to.

31.  0 1 2 Stays on topic when talking.


32. 0 1 2 Stays with group (e.g., avoids running or wandering off when on an excursion).

33.  0 1 2 Stays within school grounds during the day (avoids climbing the fence, running out the school
gate, or attempting to leave the grounds).
34. 0 1 2 Takes his/her turn when doing group work in the classroom.

35.  0 1 2 Takes turns when playing with others.


36.  0 1 2 Understands instructions that have 10 words or less.

37.  0 1 2 Waits for his/her turn to talk during a conversation (avoids interrupting).
38.  0 1 2 Waits patiently in a line (e.g., when queuing for the toilet, when waiting to get on the bus).

39.  0 1 2 Waits until a question is finished before giving an answer.


40.  0 1 2 Waits until called on by the teacher before giving an answer (avoids calling out).

41.  0 1 2 Walks rather than runs from room to room when indoors.
42.  0 1 2 When given one simple instruction, can understand it and carry it out (e.g., Get your bag).

43.  0 1 2 When participating in a class activity, does not get easily distracted (e.g., outside noises,
sudden noises).
44. 0 1 2 Works quietly during specific class activities (avoids humming, singing, talking to self or other
throat noises).

B2
APPENDIX C: Letter to principals explaining study

Dear Principal,

We are supervising a student research project on attention and activity profiles of children with
different developmental disabilities. The student researcher is Nerelie Freeman who is currently
undertaking a Doctor of Philosophy (Ph.D) degree. We are hoping to gain a better understanding of
the attention strengths and weaknesses of children with an intellectual disability, autism, Down
Syndrome, Fragile X Syndrome and Williams Syndrome. There are currently no measures to assist
with assessing the attention difficulties of children with an intellectual disability. The development of
parent and teacher questionnaires will assist in evaluating the attention difficulties that some children
may experience.

We wish to involve children from your school. Participation in the study comprises first seeking
permission from yourself. If you give permission, the student researcher will then send information in
the mail advertising the study in the form of posters and a brief notice you might include in your
school newsletter. Consent forms and explanatory statements will also be provided.

Children with parental consent will be administered a cognitive (IQ) assessment. If they have already
had this assessment in the last two years, another one will not be conducted and the previous
assessment scores will be used. If a child is given an assessment, the family will be provided with a
report on the results. The results from this assessment could be used for the child’s next Program for
Students with Disabilities (PSD) review if it is coming up in the next two years. We hope that you
will allow us to assess these children during school hours. We will provide all letters and materials
needed for the study.

The assessment will take approximately 60-75 minutes. The student researcher or a research assistant
will administer the assessment tasks. The student researcher is a registered psychologist and has had 5
years’ experience working as a school psychologist, so she is experienced in administering and
interpreting assessments.

If the assessment(s) indicate that the child is eligible to participate, their classroom teacher will be
asked to fill out questionnaires which ask about the child’s behaviour, attentional difficulties and their
daily living skills. This will take approximately 75 minutes and will be completed individually in the
teacher’s own time, or they can be completed with the student researcher on request.

The Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Skills – Second edition, Teacher Rating Form (VABS-II) is a
general assessment of adaptive behaviour examining the child’s socialisation, communication and
daily living skills. If the child has had a VABS completed in the last 2 years, the teacher will not be
required to fill this out and the previous assessment scores will be used.

The Scale of Attention in Intellectual Disability – Teacher Form is a rating scale developed by the
research team for this study. It comprises 46 items relating to inattention, hyperactivity, impulsivity
and working memory.

The Conners Rating Scales – Third edition is a rating scale comprising 39 items which measures
ADHD symptoms, conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder.
The Developmental Behaviour Checklist – Teacher is a questionnaire comprising 96 items which
measures behavioural and emotional problems in children with a developmental delay.

C1
A copy of the rating scales listed above can be sent to you on request.

All information provided by the student and teacher, and the scores obtained in the study, will be
strictly confidential. Participating families may withdraw within 8 weeks of participating in the
assessment phase of the study. We will not be analysing individual responses, rather the group as a
whole. When the study is complete, a report will be made available to you. We wish to point out that
this project has received ethics clearance from the Human Research Committee of Monash University
and the Department of Education and Early Childhood Development (see attached approval letters).

If you have any queries whatsoever regarding this project, please feel free to contact Ms Nerelie
Freeman or Dr Kylie Gray on 9594-1301 or by email at The student
researcher will follow up this letter with a phone call in a couple of weeks, and would be happy to
come to your school if you would like to discuss this project in more detail.

Yours sincerely,

Professor Kim Cornish Dr Kylie Gray Ms Nerelie Freeman


Head of School Senior lecturer Ph.D candidate

School of Psychology and Psychiatry


Monash University

C2
APPENDIX D: Parent/Guardian Consent Form

Centre for Developmental Psychiatry & Psychology, Monash University

Title: Attention and activity profiles in children with different developmental disabilities
Conducted by: Dr Kylie Gray, Professor Kim Cornish, and Nerelie Freeman

PARENT / GUARDIAN CONSENT FORM

I Print name

Address
of

Contact number

has been asked to participate in the research project entitled ‘Attention and activity profiles in children
with different developmental disabilities’ being conducted by Dr Kylie Gray, Professor Kim Cornish,
and Nerelie Freeman and involving myself, my child’s classroom teacher, and my child:

Name of child Date of birth

I give voluntary consent for my son/daughter for whom I am the guardian to participate in the above
Monash University project. I have had the project explained to me, and I have read the Explanatory
Statement, which I will keep for my records. I understand that the research study will be carried out
in a manner conforming with the principles set out by the National Statement on Ethical Conduct in
Research Involving Humans, and further that:
1. I understand the general purposes, methods, demands and benefits and possible risks,
inconveniences and discomforts of the study as outlined in the 'Parent/Guardian Information
Sheet' that has been given to me.
2. Although I understand that the purpose of this research project is to improve the quality of care,
it has also been explained that my involvement may not be of any direct personal benefit to me
or my son/daughter/person for whom I am the guardian.
3. My participation in the research study is voluntary, and I am free to withdraw at any time, and to
continue receiving appropriate treatment for my son/daughter/person for whom I am the
guardian, as will be the case if I do not volunteer to enter the study.
4. I have been given the opportunity to ask questions in relation to the research study, and I have
received all the information and explanations I have requested.
5. I understand that any information I provide is confidential, and that no information that could
lead to the identification of any individual will be disclosed in any reports on the project, or to
any other party.

Parent / guardian signature


Signature Date

D1
APPENDIX E: Parent/Guardian Explanatory Statement
Title: Attention and activity profiles in children with different developmental disabilities

This information sheet is for you to keep.

My name is Nerelie Freeman and I am conducting a research project with Professor Kim Cornish,
Head of Discipline, and Dr Kylie Gray, Senior Lecturer, in the Department of Psychology and
Psychiatry towards a PhD at Monash University. This means that I will be writing a thesis which is
the equivalent of a 300 page book.

I am looking for male and female students aged between 5 and 12 years who are attending school and
have a diagnosis of: an intellectual disability, autism spectrum disorder (not Asperger Syndrome or
high-functioning autism), Fragile X Syndrome, Down Syndrome or Williams Syndrome.

Aim/purpose of the research


The aim of this study is to develop a new rating scale that will examine the behaviours associated with
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) specific to students with an intellectual disability. It
also aims to describe any differences in ADHD that may exist across children with different known
causes of intellectual disability.

Possible benefits
There are currently no well-validated measures to assist with assessing the attention and activity
profiles of children with an intellectual disability. The development of a teacher questionnaire will
assist in evaluating the attention strengths and difficulties that some children may experience. The
findings of this research will assist educators and health professionals in their understanding of the
attention profiles of children with an intellectual disability, and this may enable them to tailor learning
programs, treatments and interventions that are more specific to the needs of these children.

What does the research involve?


A cognitive (IQ) assessment will be completed if your child has not had one in the last two years. If it
is not possible to conduct an assessment with your child, alternative results will be used such as those
from an adaptive living skills assessment. The assessment will be done at school at a time negotiated
with staff and will take approximately 60-75 minutes. If your child has had an assessment in the last
two years, their previous assessment results will be used. If your child is given an assessment, you
will be provided with a report on the results. These results may be of use to professionals involved in
the care of your child. Copies of reports for other professionals will only be provided with your
consent.

If your child has autism, you will be asked to fill out a questionnaire which asks about your child’s
communication and socialisation skills. This will take approximately 10 minutes. If your child has
another diagnosis, you will not be required to fill out any questionnaires. Your child’s classroom
teacher will also be asked to fill out questionnaires which ask about your child’s behaviour,
attentional difficulties and their daily living skills. This will take approximately 45 minutes.

If any specific difficulties or problems are identified, a referral to appropriate services will be
arranged. If you express any concerns or need any help, this will also be arranged.

If you agree to be a part of this project, please complete the attached consent form. A member of our
research team will then contact you about the project. You can return the form to us in the reply paid
envelope which is attached to the form. No stamp is necessary.

E1
Inconvenience/discomfort
There is no risk of physical or psychological harm in the study. If any specific difficulties or problems
are identified, a referral to appropriate services will be arranged. If you have any concerns, questions,
or need any help please feel free to directly contact Ms Nerelie Freeman or Dr Kylie Gray (contact
details below).

Voluntary participation
Being in this study is voluntary and you are under no obligation to consent to participation. However,
if you do consent to participate, you may only withdraw within 8 weeks of participating in the
assessment phase of the study. Whether you take part or not, it will not make any difference to the
funding or services which your child or your family currently receives.

Confidentiality
Data on computers is securely stored and deidentified (names are not used). Only the research team
will have access to the data. There will be nothing in any reports of the study that could identify
individual children or families. Reports on the study will be submitted for publication, but individual
participants will not be identifiable in such reports. Participation in this project is voluntary. You are
free to withdraw from the project within 8 weeks of participating in the assessment phase of the study.

Storage of data
Storage of the data collected will adhere to the University regulations and kept on University premises
in a locked cupboard/filing cabinet for 5 years. Only the research team will have access to the data. A
report of the study may be submitted for publication, but individual participants will not be
identifiable in such a report.

Results
If you would like to be informed of the aggregate research finding, please contact Dr Kylie Gray (see
details below). The findings will be available from December 2012.

If you would like to contact the researchers If you have a complaint concerning the manner
about any aspect of this study, please contact: in which this research is being conducted, please
contact:

Ms Nerelie Freeman or Dr Kylie Gray Executive Officer, Human Research Ethics


Centre for Developmental Psychiatry & Monash University Human Research Ethics
Psychology Committee (MUHREC)
Monash Medical Centre Building 3e Room 111
246 Clayton Rd Research Office
Clayton VIC 3168 Monash University VIC 3800

Thank you for taking the time to assist us with our research project.

Dr Kylie Gray Prof Kim Cornish Nerelie Freeman

E2
APPENDIX F: Item-Total Correlations Table for T-SAID Items

Item Item-Total

number correlation

1 .59

2 .63

3 .65

4 .65

5 .68

6 .72

7 .70

8 .68

9 .72

10 .62

11 .74

12 .70

13 .70

14 .63

15 .78

16 .76

17 .66

18 .72

19 .75

20 .71

21 .70

22 .69

F1
23 .65

24 .61

25 .66

26 .74

27 .78

28 .75

29 .77

30 .77

31 .72

32 .69

33 .72

34 .73

35 .76

36 .68

37 .48

38 .45

39 .16

40 .51

41 56

42 .67

43 .67

44 .71

45 .69

46 .65

F2
APPENDIX G: Inter-Item Correlations Table for T-SAID Items

Item
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28
number

1 – .47 .32 .53 .47 .47 .41 .43 .51 .52 .50 .39 .49 .40 .46 .29 .30 .37 .41 .47 .44 .40 .36 .40 .52 .49 .47 .40

2 .47 – .69 .41 .42 .46 .38 .38 .46 .35 .45 .42 .46 .37 .44 .59 .45 .43 .43 .47 .33 .37 .39 .34 .43 .53 .50 .48

3 .32 .69 – .41 .44 .44 .44 .40 .40 .42 .41 .40 .41 .38 .50 .71 .42 .48 .41 .44 .35 .37 .37 .38 .39 .48 .51 .50

4 .53 .41 .41 – .64 .54 .47 .50 .44 .54 .51 .38 .53 .40 .49 .42 .37 .44 .50 .39 .45 .45 .37 .39 .63 .53 .57 .50

5 .47 .42 .44 .64 – .59 .47 .42 .41 .48 .49 .39 .55 .36 .49 .48 .46 .47 .49 .48 .38 .48 .41 .30 .52 .52 .57 .48

6 .47 .46 .44 .54 .59 – .63 .66 .60 .44 .58 .46 .54 .49 .54 .49 .50 .58 .56 .45 .55 .45 .42 .43 .52 .56 .61 .43

7 .41 .38 .44 .47 .47 .63 – .77 .63 .50 .61 .49 .55 .55 .56 .51 .42 .50 .64 .47 .59 .45 .39 .54 .48 .54 .60 .49

8 .43 .38 .40 .50 .42 .66 .77 – .67 .50 .60 .55 .60 .56 .55 .46 .40 .50 .63 .48 .58 .48 .43 .43 .46 .56 .55 .47

9 .51 .46 .40 .44 .41 .60 .63 .67 – .54 .59 .59 .66 .51 .59 .48 .48 .54 .66 .55 .59 .45 .43 .50 .45 .56 .57 .49

10 .52 .35 .42 .54 .48 .44 .50 .50 .54 – .46 .40 .73 .39 .42 .45 .38 .38 .46 .40 .47 .39 .37 .38 .51 .47 .52 .47

11 .50 .45 .41 .51 .49 .58 .61 .60 .59 .46 – .72 .53 .52 .67 .50 .52 .63 .70 .64 .66 .59 .58 .55 .53 .53 .56 .50

12 .39 .42 .40 .38 .39 .46 .49 .55 .59 .40 .72 – .51 .46 .64 .49 .47 .58 .67 .53 .60 .62 .60 .54 .48 .54 .51 .55

G1
13 .49 .46 .41 .53 .55 .54 .55 .60 .66 .73 .53 .51 – .48 .51 .45 .48 .42 .55 .46 .52 .45 .45 .37 .53 .52 .62 .47

14 .40 .37 .38 .40 .36 .49 .55 .56 .51 .39 .52 .46 .48 – .62 .51 .34 .43 .53 .43 .50 .32 .36 .42 .48 .49 .55 .42

15 .46 .44 .50 .49 .49 .54 .56 .55 .59 .42 .67 .64 .51 .62 – .65 .51 .64 .67 .61 .64 .63 .62 .50 .49 .52 .60 .54

16 .29 .59 .71 .42 .48 .49 .51 .46 .48 .45 .50 .49 .45 .51 .65 – .59 .57 .55 .55 .48 .53 .51 .43 .41 .60 .60 .63

17 .30 .45 .42 .37 .46 .50 .42 .40 .48 .38 .52 .47 .48 .34 .51 .59 – .60 .52 .53 .49 .48 .46 .31 .33 .40 .43 .59

18 .37 .43 .48 .44 .47 .58 .50 .50 .54 .38 .63 .58 .42 .43 .64 .57 .60 – .58 .61 .54 .56 .57 .46 .40 .50 .51 .54

19 .41 .43 .41 .50 .49 .56 .64 .63 .66 .46 .70 .67 .55 .53 .67 .55 .52 .58 – .66 .75 .59 .57 .63 .51 .54 .58 .57

20 .47 .47 .44 .39 .48 .45 .47 .48 .55 .40 .64 .53 .46 .43 .61 .55 .53 .61 .66 – .60 .56 .58 .46 .39 .45 .52 .54

21 .44 .33 .35 .45 .38 .55 .59 .58 .59 .47 .66 .60 .52 .50 .64 .48 .49 .54 .75 .60 – .52 .58 .65 .47 .48 .55 .52

22 .40 .37 .37 .45 .48 .45 .45 .48 .45 .39 .59 .62 .45 .32 .63 .53 .48 .56 .59 .56 .52 – .74 .51 .44 .50 .44 .50

23 .36 .39 .37 .37 .41 .42 .39 .43 .43 .37 .58 .60 .45 .36 .62 .51 .46 .57 .57 .58 .58 .74 – .53 .43 .42 .40 .43

24 .40 .34 .38 .39 .30 .43 .54 .43 .50 .38 .55 .54 .37 .42 .50 .43 .31 .46 .63 .46 .65 .51 .53 – .51 .48 .55 .44

25 .52 .43 .39 .63 .52 .52 .48 .46 .45 .51 .53 .48 .53 .48 .49 .41 .33 .40 .51 .39 .47 .44 .43 .51 – .58 .58 .53

26 .49 .53 .48 .53 .52 .56 .54 .56 .56 .47 .53 .54 .52 .49 .52 .60 .40 .50 .54 .45 .48 .50 .42 .48 .58 – .68 .61

G2
27 .47 .50 .51 .57 .57 .61 .60 .55 .57 .52 .56 .51 .62 .55 .60 .60 .43 .51 .58 .52 .55 .44 .40 .55 .58 .68 – .65

28 .40 .48 .50 .50 .48 .43 .49 .47 .49 .47 .50 .55 .47 .42 .54 .63 .59 .54 .57 .54 .52 .50 .43 .44 .53 .61 .65 –

29 .42 .55 .54 .51 .52 .53 .57 .48 .53 .57 .53 .51 .51 .44 .56 .61 .58 .58 .58 .56 .56 .49 .44 .50 .55 .63 .67 .80

30 .48 .43 .44 .41 .54 .63 .51 .55 .60 .47 .57 .54 .52 .58 .64 .58 .52 .57 .53 .57 .57 .48 .49 .36 .51 .60 .67 .61

31 .46 .41 .40 .37 .54 .57 .45 .52 .55 .40 .51 .48 .54 .52 .57 .53 .52 .57 .50 .56 .49 .45 .47 .33 .43 .53 .63 .55

32 .45 .45 .43 .42 .45 .53 .48 .51 .61 .46 .54 .54 .58 .53 .60 .49 .40 .49 .51 .50 .50 .43 .39 .42 .50 .52 .58 .48

33 .51 .63 .51 .49 .51 .57 .48 .48 .55 .49 .46 .47 .57 .43 .44 .60 .52 .50 .48 .43 .41 .46 .42 .43 .53 .76 .58 .55

34 .50 .58 .52 .49 .52 .48 .45 .43 .56 .46 .48 .48 .53 .40 .48 .61 .48 .51 .48 .51 .43 .48 .37 .43 .50 .72 .58 .61

35 .46 .56 .53 .51 .48 .58 .58 .53 .60 .43 .54 .51 .54 .56 .59 .60 .42 .51 .58 .59 .56 .47 .49 .49 .50 .66 .71 .58

36 .49 .46 .48 .52 .49 .59 .49 .47 .45 .35 .46 .36 .41 .48 .47 .49 .48 .49 .49 .52 .50 .43 .42 .44 .50 .48 .55 .53

37 .29 .20 .33 .25 .40 .40 .29 .28 .37 .28 .30 .27 .31 .33 .36 .32 .26 .39 .26 .37 .28 .35 .32 .32 .28 .31 .37 .29

38 .21 .19 .21 .25 .36 .29 .27 .25 .27 .27 .31 .35 .28 .35 .44 .38 .33 .31 .35 .31 .40 .44 .45 .33 .30 .29 .33 .31

39 .12 -.01 -.02 .18 .18 .10 .11 .14 .06 .14 .11 .18 .19 .06 .10 .07 .11 .07 .10 .11 .13 .25 .16 .17 .10 .08 .15 .10

40 .24 .27 .37 .35 .47 .27 .30 .19 .23 .29 .28 .30 .31 .25 .47 .49 .33 .35 .31 .40 .31 .46 .47 .28 .32 .28 .36 .37

G3
41 .41 .34 .32 .33 .51 .49 .35 .38 .41 .31 .41 .41 .42 .36 .38 .39 .42 .42 .39 .44 .32 .46 .40 .29 .36 .33 .41 .31

42 .32 .41 .51 .44 .54 .49 .40 .41 .39 .38 .49 .44 .43 .42 .54 .62 .53 .52 .45 .47 .37 .51 .49 .30 .40 .42 .43 .47

43 .34 .35 .43 .41 .47 .44 .49 .46 .42 .37 .46 .47 .46 .42 .53 .58 .48 .53 .44 .44 .39 .53 .52 .39 .44 .55 .48 .48

44 .32 .59 .67 .45 .51 .43 .38 .34 .38 .40 .46 .40 .40 .37 .55 .67 .54 .53 .41 .47 .34 .46 .40 .34 .40 .53 .53 .63

45 .30 .53 .64 .44 .46 .39 .40 .37 .36 .40 .46 .37 .39 .42 .56 .66 .52 .51 .41 .46 .36 .49 .38 .34 .39 .47 .52 .62

46 .27 .50 .57 .35 .47 .37 .39 .30 .34 .39 .42 .39 .34 .31 .51 .62 .51 .47 .42 .46 .42 .46 .41 .33 .35 .42 .51 .62

G4
Item
29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46
number

1 .42 .48 .46 .45 .51 .50 .46 .49 .29 .21 .12 .24 .41 .32 .34 .32 .30 .27

2 .55 .43 .41 .45 .63 .58 .56 .46 .20 .19 -.01 .27 .34 .41 .35 .59 .53 .50

3 .54 .44 .40 .43 .51 .52 .53 .48 .33 .21 -.02 .37 .32 .51 .43 .67 .64 .57

4 .51 .41 .37 .42 .49 .49 .51 .52 .25 .25 .18 .35 .33 .44 .41 .45 .44 .35

5 .52 .54 .54 .45 .51 .52 .48 .49 .40 .36 .18 .47 .51 .54 .47 .51 .46 .47

6 .53 .63 .57 .53 .57 .48 .58 .59 .40 .29 .10 .27 .49 .49 .44 .43 .39 .37

7 .57 .51 .45 .48 .48 .45 .58 .49 .29 .27 .11 .30 .35 .40 .49 .38 .40 .39

8 .48 .55 .52 .51 .48 .43 .53 .47 .28 .25 .14 .19 .38 .41 .46 .34 .37 .30

9 .53 .60 .55 .61 .55 .56 .60 .45 .37 .27 .06 .23 .41 .39 .42 .38 .36 .34

10 .57 .47 .40 .46 .49 .46 .43 .35 .28 .27 .14 .29 .31 .38 .37 .40 .40 .39

11 .53 .57 .51 .54 .46 .48 .54 .46 .30 .31 .12 .28 .41 .49 .46 .46 .46 .42

12 .51 .54 .48 .54 .47 .48 .51 .36 .27 .35 .18 .30 .41 .44 .47 .40 .37 .39

13 .51 .52 .54 .58 .57 .53 .54 .41 .31 .28 .19 .31 .42 .43 .46 .40 .39 .34

G5
14 .44 .58 .52 .53 .43 .40 .56 .48 .33 .35 .06 .25 .36 .42 .42 .37 .42 .31

15 .56 .64 .57 .60 .44 .48 .59 .47 .36 .44 .10 .47 .38 .54 .53 .55 .56 .51

16 .61 .58 .53 .49 .60 .61 .60 .49 .32 .38 .07 .49 .39 .62 .58 .67 .66 .62

17 .58 .52 .52 .40 .52 .48 .42 .48 .26 .33 .11 .33 .42 .53 .48 .54 .52 .51

18 .58 .57 .57 .49 .50 .51 .51 .49 .39 .31 .07 .35 .42 .52 .53 .53 .51 .47

19 .58 .53 .50 .51 .48 .48 .58 .49 .26 .35 .10 .31 .39 .45 .44 .41 .41 .42

20 .56 .57 .56 .50 .43 .51 .59 .52 .37 .31 .11 .40 .44 .47 .44 .47 .46 .46

21 .56 .57 .49 .50 .41 .43 .56 .50 .28 .40 .13 .31 .32 .37 .39 .34 .36 .42

22 .49 .48 .45 .43 .46 .48 .47 .43 .35 .44 .25 .46 .46 .51 .53 .46 .49 .46

23 .44 .49 .47 .39 .42 .37 .49 .42 .32 .45 .16 .47 .40 .49 .52 .40 .38 .41

24 .50 .36 .33 .42 .43 .43 .49 .44 .32 .33 .12 .28 .29 .30 .39 .34 .34 .33

25 .55 .51 .43 .50 .53 .50 .50 .50 .28 .30 .10 .32 .36 .40 .44 .40 .39 .35

26 .63 .60 .53 .52 .76 .72 .66 .48 .31 .29 .08 .28 .33 .42 .55 .53 .47 .42

27 .67 .67 .63 .58 .58 .58 .71 .55 .37 .33 .15 .36 .41 .43 .48 .53 .52 .51

G6
28 .80 .61 .55 .48 .55 .61 .58 .53 .29 .31 .10 .37 .31 .47 .48 .63 .62 .62

29 – .64 .58 .53 .58 .61 .58 .56 .29 .28 .00 .33 .32 .48 .47 .64 .60 .62

30 .64 – .86 .60 .50 .54 .59 .60 .44 .38 .09 .36 .46 .50 .52 .54 .53 .53

31 .58 .86 – .58 .50 .53 .56 .58 .44 .34 .11 .35 .47 .46 .50 .48 .47 .47

32 .53 60 .58 – .58 .67 .59 .49 .39 .29 .10 .31 .38 .41 .37 .46 .43 .38

33 .58 .50 .50 .58 – .85 .59 .48 .28 .23 .07 .30 .42 .46 .49 .56 .49 .43

34 .61 .54 .53 .67 .85 – .63 .49 .31 .22 .08 .35 .40 .45 .50 .61 .53 .48

35 .58 .59 .56 .59 .59 .63 – .57 .45 .33 .13 .40 .41 .45 .53 .48 .43 .46

36 .56 .60 .58 .49 .48 .49 .57 – .49 .26 .05 .31 .43 .48 .47 .49 .50 .40

37 .29 .44 .44 .39 .28 .31 .45 .49 – .27 .15 .43 .38 .35 .39 .37 .33 .36

38 .28 .38 .34 .29 .23 .22 .33 .26 .27 – .47 .43 .30 .39 .43 .24 .30 .31

39 .00 .09 .11 .10 .07 .08 .13 .05 .15 .47 – .24 .24 .15 .23 .03 .02 -.02

40 .33 .36 .35 .31 .30 .35 .40 .31 .43 .43 .24 – .44 .58 .50 .43 .45 .51

41 .32 .46 .47 .38 .42 .40 .41 .43 .38 .30 .24 .44 – .68 .43 .37 .37 .36

G7
42 .48 .50 .46 .41 .46 .45 .45 .48 .35 .39 .15 .58 .68 – .64 .64 .64 .53

43 .47 .52 .50 .37 .49 .50 .53 .47 .39 .43 .23 .50 .43 .64 – .55 .56 .46

44 .64 .54 .48 .46 .56 .61 .48 .49 .37 .24 .03 .43 .37 .64 .55 – .88 .74

45 .60 .53 .47 .43 .49 .53 .43 .50 .33 .30 .02 .45 .37 .64 .56 .88 – .75

46 .62 .53 .47 .38 .43 .48 .46 .40 .36 .31 -.02 .51 .36 .53 .46 .74 .75 –

G8

You might also like