Alan Kibbe Gaynor, Boston University: Development Toward School Readiness: A Holistic Model
Alan Kibbe Gaynor, Boston University: Development Toward School Readiness: A Holistic Model
27
standardized tests in language arts, math, and science), they were moderate-or high-income families on a range of particular readi-
not effective in closing the gap between these students and aver- ness measures—cognitive and behavioral.
age- and high-readiness students in the “good” school itself. This The connection between income and school readiness can be
was because in good schools the achievement of initially average- seen as even more compelling when placed in the context of the
and high-readiness students also improves, thus maintaining the rising proportion of poor children in school:
gap between these students and initially low-achieving students.
For the first time in at least 50 years, a majority of U.S. pub-
Although the computer simulation modeling effort that is
lic school students come from low-income families, accord-
reported in this article produced important insights into the
ing to a new analysis of 2013 federal data, a statistic that has
dynamics of schooling and their effects on the academic achieve-
profound implications for the nation. (Layton, 2015)
ment of students with different levels of school readiness, I asked
myself the same question many others have asked themselves: Why These data led me to build a computer simulation model
do 5-year-olds arrive in school with differential levels of readiness? informed by the relevant literature that would analyze the dynam-
We know that they do. ics of early childhood development. The goal was to determine
As noted earlier, poverty has been found by many researchers the implications for intervention policies that would help to
to be a significant critical causal factor. Isaacs (2012) compared lower the variance in the distribution of school readiness among
the difference between children from households with an annual 5-year-olds by increasing the school readiness of children raised
income below 100% of the poverty line ($18,000 for a family of in low-income families whose parents had attained relatively low
three in 2011 terms) and those from households with an income levels of education.
above 185% of the poverty line, a group that spans a wide spectrum
from just above 185% of poverty ($33,000 for a family of three in DEVELOPMENT OF THE MODEL
2011 terms) to much higher levels. Children who are described as
Although others have pursued this goal, it seemed that a computer
“near poor” (household income between 100% and 185% of pov-
simulation modeling approach would provide a more holistic view
erty) are also disadvantaged as they begin kindergarten, although
of early childhood development than the approaches taken by the
to a lesser extent than poor children. In this group, the 59% with
vast majority of other educational researchers. As described in
incomes just above the poverty line are ready for school at age 5.
the reports cited in this article, most of the research on the root
For children born into households with income above $100,000,
causes of low school readiness (and on its long-term effects) is cor-
school readiness increases to 86%. Recent research (Frey, 2015)
also reports that there is a significant correlation between family relational in design and focuses on individual factors. In contrast,
income and early childhood brain development, a likely further the work that is reported in this article and in my previous work
factor in explaining variances in school readiness. on school dynamics (Gaynor, 2011/2012a; 2011/2012b; 2012a;
The relationship between family income and school readiness 2012b) posits a set of causal interactions that seek to explain the cor-
is made even sharper in Figure 1. Students from poor families relations reported in the literature.
are seen to be highly more likely to score below students from The Importance of School Readiness
As noted by the authors of a themed issue of The Future of Children
Figure 1. Likelihood of Scoring Very Low (Failing to Be School- (Rouse, Brooks-Gunn, & McLanahan, 2005) that addressed the
Ready) on Measures of School Readiness by Poverty Status problem of school readiness about a decade ago, school readiness
is of great importance. The report notes that children who enter
Percentage Scoring Very Low (Failing to Be School Ready)
28 J O U R N A L O F E D U C AT I O N • V O L U M E 1 9 5 • N U M B E R 3 • 2 0 1 5
the “basic” level of reading and math achievement, indicat- defined as those with a poverty rate exceeding 20 percent)
ing that they have less than partial mastery of the knowledge provide more limited opportunities . . . in terms of social
and skills “fundamental for proficient work” at that grade interaction, positive role models, and other resources, such
level. Other manifestations of problems in school achieve- as quality child care, health facilities, parks, and playgrounds,
ment for disadvantaged children include higher rates of spe- that are important for healthy child development. (para. 4)
cial education placement, grade repetition, and dropping
Additional benefits of access to health care are child development
out of school. Ultimately, limited skills and low educational
screening and advice to parents regarding behaviors that promote
attainment increase the likelihood of undesirable outcomes
children’s social, emotional, cognitive, and physical development.
in adulthood. Low educational attainment is associated with
Many of the model variables can also be found in the following
reduced rates of employment and with lower earnings for
graph published in a report by Isaacs (2012, p. 17):
those who are employed. Use of social welfare programs is
also higher among those with low educational attainment, as Figure 2. Overall School Readiness by Selected Child
are crime and incarceration rates. (p. 4) and Family Characteristics
Doyle, Finnegan, and McNamara (2010) also confirm and add Male
Female
Non-Hispanic White
to these findings in a review of studies that identified six lifelong Non-Hispanic Black
Hispanic
effects of school readiness across the following domains of devel- Other race
Normal birth weight
Low birth weight
opment: academic achievement (Heckman, 2006), peer relation- Maternal age: 15–19
Maternal age: 20–24
Maternal age: 25–29
ships (Ladd, 1999), psychological well-being (Alloy et al., 1999), Immigrant mother
Maternal age: 30+
100%–184% FPL
leagues (2005) summarize the factors that were identified as cru- HH income: ≤$25,000
185% FPL or more
family socioeconomic status, parenting, child health, maternal Percentage of Children Who Are School Ready (on All 5 Measures)
health and behaviors, and preschool attendance likely account “Starting school at a disadvantage: The school readiness of poor children,” by Julia B.
for most of the racial and ethnic gaps in school readiness. (p. 11) Isaacs, 2012, p. 4, Copyright 2012 by the Brookings Institution. Permission to reprint.
NEXT
GENERATION +
(+) + CHILD'S +
+ +
READINESS
FOR SCHOOL
+ Child's
Resilience
Single Parent
Family Status + + + +
+
+
Child's Self-
Child's Level of
(+) Reading
+
Language Child's Degree
+
Development of Positive
Behavior Child's Level of Social
+ + - + Development
+
Child's Level of
+ +
+
Conceptual
Development
(+)
Parent
Educational
+
Attainment
+
Family
Immigrant &
+ +
Minority Status Amount & Child's
Physical
+
Quality of
Health
+
Preschooling
(+)
Mother's Level of
+ - (+) +
Maternal Attachment
+ + Child's Level of
+ Emotional Security
+
-+
Family Availability of
Income Health Care
+ +
Family
+ - (+)
+
Nutrition
- + + Variety of
Food Available
+
Mean Income
+ of
Neighborhood Child's Level of
+
Maternal Attachment
Neighborhood
Unemployment,
Single Parent Crime & Drugs
Family Status
- Family Instability
& Stress
A report of the Social Genome Project, Center on Children Figure 4. Intergenerational Feedback Loop
and Families, Brookings Institution (Isaacs & Magnuson, 2011)
explains this finding when identifying a list of factors very similar +
Child’s School Experiences Child’s Life
those in the model as influencing school readiness. Controlling for and Development Achievements
parents’ level of education, marital status, and mother’s age at the
time of the child’s birth, as well as race/ethnicity, immigrant sta-
+ (+) +
tus, gender, and age in months, the report concludes: Child’s Readiness Child as Parent—
for School Effect on the
Next Generation
Parents’ education is a large factor explaining why chil- +
dren from moderate and high-income families enter school
Child’s Early Childhood
with higher reading and math skills—their parents are bet- Experiences and
Development
+
ter educated. Children’s early academic skills are higher,
on average, when parents have more years of schooling, +
and this association persists even after controlling for par- Original Parent’s
Education and
ents’ inherent abilities, according to evidence from wel- Income
fare reform evaluations and sophisticated statistical analysis
(Gennetian, Magnuson & Morris, 2008; Carneiro et al., The next step in the analysis was to transform the causal-loop
2007). In addition, the “education” effect also reflects diagram into a System Dynamics computer simulation model (a
underlying differences in parents’ skills and preferences, “stock-and-flow” diagram) in visual format (see Figure 5) and
which are often passed on to their children, by both inher- mathematized (see Appendix A for a list of the equations that com-
ited traits and upbringing. (p. 6) prise the computer simulation model).
It is further proposed (Amato, 2005), although not pursued in this The great analytic value of a computer simulation model is
analysis, that the effects are intergenerational (see Figure 4). twofold. First, the model can be analyzed as a description of “the
30 J O U R N A L O F E D U C AT I O N • V O L U M E 1 9 5 • N U M B E R 3 • 2 0 1 5
Figure 5. The Computer Simulation Model
Emotional
Security Child's Academic
~ Language Development
Effect of Emotional Security
on Readiness for School
~
Effect of the Level of Aspir & Self
Expect on School Readiness
~ Child's Level or Readiness
for School
Effect of Maternal Attachment ~ Maternal
~ on Child Behavior Effect of Resilience on Attachment
Child's Level of
Effect on Child Behavior Readiness for School
Social Development
of Family Instab & Stress
Child's Physical ~
Rate of Increase Health Effects of Physical Healthon
or Decrease in Conceptual Development Child's Resilience
Social Development
Child's Level of
~ Conceptual Development
Effect of Parent Education on
the Child's Social Development ~
Effect of the Child's PreSchooling
on Social & Conceptual Develop Child's Aspiration and
Parent Rate of Increase in Self Expectation
Educational Attainment Conceptual Development
Increase in
Child's Age Child's Social Child's Level of ~
~ Development Divided Social Development
Rate of Increase in Academic by Age Effect of Parent Education on
Effect of Level of Standard English Child's Aspiration and Self Expect
Vocabulary and Language Development
& Acad Cult on Voc & Lang Dev
~ ~ ~
Effect of the Neighborhood Effect of Standard English & Effect of Maternal Attachment on
on Vocabulary and Language Level of Standard
~ English & Acad Cult Academic Culture in the Home Aspiration & Self Expectation
Development
Effect of Parent Education in the Home Parent
on PreSchooling Educational Attainment
~
Effect of Immigr & Min Status on ~
Effect of Parent Education on Child
Engl & Acad Culture in the Home Effect of Parent Education Child's Degree of
Vocab & Language Development
~ on Conceptual Development Positive Behavior ~
Effect of Parent Education on Effect of Maternal Attachment
English & Acad Cult in the Home on Child Behavior
Parent
Educational Attainment
~
Family Immigrant Effect of Child Behavior Goal of Maternal
~ and Minority Status ~ on Maternal Attachment Attachment
Effect of Family Income on Effect on Child Behavior Maternal
on Quality of PreSchooling of Family Instab & Stress Attachment
~
Effect of Family Instability &
Stress on Materna Attachment Flow 1
~
Mean Income of
Income Based Family Income Affordable Neighborhood
on Education ~ Natural Maternal
~ Neighborhood Gang Attachment
Neighborhood Rate Crime & DrugTrafficking
~
of Unemployment Effect of Crime & Drug
Effect of Single Parent Family Instability
Status on Family Income Trafficking on Emotional Security and Stress
~
~ Effect of Immigrant or Effect of Neighborhood Income Child's Physical Emotional
Effect of PEA on Minority Status on Income Level on Variety of Food Health Security
Use of Health Care Effect of Single Parent
Single Parent
Effect of Nutrition Status on Maternal Attachment
Family Status Family Nutrition
on Physical Health
~ ~
Family Immigrant Availability of Health Care Effect of Family Instability
and Minority Status & Stress on Emotional
Security
~
Effect of Family Instability &
Stress on Physical Health Single Parent
~ Family Status
Effect of Availability on Degree
of Utilization of Health Care Effect of Single Parent
Effect of Health Care Satus on Family Stress
Utilization on Physical Health Family Instability
and Stress
Degree of Utilization
of Health Care Effect of Income
on Family Stress Family Income
way things are,” and while based generally on understandings from As noted earlier, the numbers are only illustrative because prior
the literature, the model constitutes a “theory of the problem” and empirical research has not produced specific effect sizes among
allows one to generate or construct interventions, some of which the variables that collectively, and interactively, generate differ-
have been tried, and for which there are, therefore, some effects ent levels of school readiness.
data. Secondly, and very importantly, the structure of the model
can be altered “experimentally” and “run” to simulate different A THEORY OF EARLY CHILDHOOD DEVELOPMENT
interventions without the risks and costs of empirical experimen- THAT GENERATES SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES IN
tation. Both procedures were used, and the remainder of this arti- SCHOOL READINESS
cle is rooted in these two related but different kinds of analyses.
Figure 5 presents the model. The report of the Center on Children and Families (Figure 1)
provides empirical data to indicate a 27% gap in school readiness
The Basic Model: Illustrative Simulation Results between children of poor and moderate- or high-income families
Having formulated a theoretical computer simulation model of (48% vs. 75%).The theory presented in this analysis is that this dis-
the current “system” of early childhood development that pro- crepancy is the result of interactions among a set of variables that
duces a range of students’ readiness for school, it was then pos- are caused by the effects of exogenous differences in parental edu-
sible to run the model to see the extent to which it generated cation, ethnic and racial identity, and being a child of a stable mar-
results consistent with what is known about the distribution of riage vs. being a child of a single parent (unmarried or divorced
school readiness among students with different backgrounds. It is mother) or of a second marriage. These interactions are shown in
important to note, however, that the numbers from the computer a causal-loop diagram (Figure 4), a stock-and-flow diagram (Fig-
simulation are illustrative only. That is, they show the effects, ure 5), and a set of equations (see Appendix A) that facilitate the
when run through the theoretical system of variables which com- simulation of the basic theoretical model. An appropriate subset of
prises the model, that reflect the idea that different backgrounds these equations can later be modified to test structural changes in
tend, in general, to develop different levels of school readiness. the model that represent various policy initiatives.
7.5
no degree
High school diploma 651
Linearity means that there is a straight-line relationship
11.0 Less than a
high school diploma 472
between the IVs and the DV. This assumption is important
All workers: 6.1% All workers: $827
because regression analysis only tests for a linear relation-
Note: Data are for persons age 25 and over. Earnings are for full-time wage and salary workers.
Source: Current Population Survey, U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, U.S. Department of Labor
ship between the IVs and the DV. Any nonlinear relation-
Data Table
These education categories reflect only the highest level of education attained. They do not take into account completion of training
ship between the IV and DV is ignored. You can test for
programs in the form of apprenticeships and other on-the-job training, which may also influence earnings and unemployment rates. For more
information on training, see http://www.bls.gov/emp/ep_table_education_summary.htm and http://www.bls.gov/emp/ep_table_education_by_train.htm. linearity between an IV and the DV by looking at a bivariate
BLS has some data on the employment status of the civilian noninstitutional population 25 years and over by educational attainment, sex, race, and Hispanic origin online.
The Census Bureau also has some data on education attainment online. scatterplot (i.e., a graph with the IV on one axis and the
“America’s youth at 25: School enrollment, number of jobs held and labor market DV on the other). If the two variables are linearly related,
activity: Results from a longitudinal study,” a report of the United States Depart- the scatterplot will be oval. (Princeton University Library,
ment of Labor, Bureau of Labor Statistics (2013).
Data and Statistical Services, 2007, para. 13)
32 J O U R N A L O F E D U C AT I O N • V O L U M E 1 9 5 • N U M B E R 3 • 2 0 1 5
Thus, correlational analysis, on which most educational research 6 (parent doctorate or equivalent). When parental education is
related to the factors that influence school readiness is based, held constant, racial and ethnic effects are not as strong. Given the
stands in contrast to the systemic analysis that is represented in this data that show that children of a White or Asian stable marriage
article. The theory described here explains the interactions among have attained at age 5, the school readiness that is known to affect
a set of individual causal relationships that collectively account for their subsequent achievement, for the purposes of this analysis,
the variability in school readiness among a representative sample White or Asian stable marriage status was considered to be the
of 5-year-olds. “standard.” Then the gain or loss in readiness (shown as the child’s
Unfortunately, as noted earlier, inter-variable causal effect sizes relative “readiness age”) varies under different family conditions.
are generally unknown. Thus, in this model, effect sizes are pos- Children of non-White or non-Asian parents with minimal educa-
ited only in highly generalized forms as high, medium, or low or tion in stable marriages had a readiness age just 1.04% below their
as hypothesized fractional values between 0 and 1. Thus, the vast White or Asian counterparts while children of single or remar-
preponderance of the effect sizes in my models are—of necessity— ried non-White or non-Asian parents who had graduated from
estimates, not known experimental effect sizes, estimated effect high school had a readiness age 24% below their White or Asian
sizes that are shown either as fractions of one or, more frequently, counterparts from stable marriages. In fact, it appears that given
as non-linear table values. the relationships posited in this model, other than parental educa-
This kind of theoretical imprecision has been a common obser- tion, the largest negative exogenous factor is single parenthood or
vation throughout the history of science. Siddhartha Mukherjee, divorce and remarriage.
the author of The Emperor of All Maladies: A Biography of Cancer With regard to parent education, it is important to keep in
(2010), writes that, on the one hand, “Science begins with count- mind that this variable is laden with the effects of race and ethnic-
ing. To understand a phenomenon, a scientist must first describe it; ity. Therefore, effects that are attributed in the model (in which
to describe it objectively, he must first measure it” (p. 19). But a educational attainment is an independent exogenous variable)
later quote, this one attributed to Howard Skipper, a noted cancer incorporate some of the effects of race and ethnicity. In a graph
researcher, presents the view that, “A model is a lie that helps you published by the United States Department of Labor, Bureau of
see the truth” (p. 491). Labor Statistics (2013), the percentage of 25–29 year olds who
My sense is that holistic, interactive, computer simulation mod- completed a bachelor’s degree or higher between 1990 and 2013
els, involving many “soft variables” and “soft relationships,” with all varied between 43% and 60% for Asians/Pacific Islanders, between
their problems of imprecision, help to provide a holistic sense of 25% and 40% for Whites, between 13% and 22% for Blacks, and
interaction, to identify areas where further research is needed, and between 9% and 15% for Hispanics (p. 3). Clearly, this makes the
to inform policymakers about the complexity of the problems they numbers in Table 1 more consistent with these data.
are attempting to resolve. With regard to marital status, data were gathered on a nationally
representative sample of men and women who were ages 14 to 22
Effects of Initial Conditions on Children’s Readiness when they were first interviewed in 1979 (United States Depart-
for School ment of Labor, Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2013). Respondents
The following table shows, in general, the ultimate effects in the were interviewed annually until 1994 and since then have contin-
model of parental education, minority identity and immigrant status, ued to be interviewed on a biennial basis. The detailed information
and stable or non-stable marriage on a child’s readiness for school: on fertility, marital transitions, and employment is collected in a
format that allows the dating of the specific events.
Table 1. Effects of Minority Identity and Immigrant Status on
School Readiness Marriage and divorce patterns differ according to race and
ethnicity. Whites are about twice as likely as Blacks and His-
Remarriage
Remarriage
Other and
Other and
Education
Parent or
Marriage
Marriage
White or
White or
Parental
Asian &
Asian &
Stable
Stable
Single
Single
34 J O U R N A L O F E D U C AT I O N • V O L U M E 1 9 5 • N U M B E R 3 • 2 0 1 5
CONCLUSIONS we’re serious, we have to do things that overcome the dam-
ages of poverty. We have to meet their health needs, their
What has been reported in this article has been an exercise in sys-
mental health needs, after-school programs, summer pro-
tems analysis. The problem addressed, which has an extensive lit-
grams, parent engagement, early-childhood services. These
erature, is the variability in the readiness of 5-year-olds for school.
are the so-called wraparound services. Some people think of
While the analysis is theoretical in nature, a kind of thought exper-
them as add-ons. They’re not. They’re imperative.”
iment, it is generally consistent with the literature on school read-
iness and early childhood development and yet, at the same time, No, it’s not easy and there’s more to the problem than closing the
emphasizes the limitations of the current knowledge base. Most of school readiness gap and the academic achievement gap, per se.
the research available on the development of readiness for school There is also the larger task of closing the “life gap,” the dramatic
is correlational in nature. What is needed, although it is difficult to difference in standards of living that characterize families with dif-
do, is bivariate experimental research that would provide the effect ferent educational backgrounds and incomes—which goes well
sizes that are needed for more precise systemic analysis. beyond scores on a limited range of high-stakes tests.
The overarching conclusion from studying the “achievement gap”
over the past several years, and looking systematically at the dynam- References
ics of both schooling and early childhood development, is not opti- Amato, P. R. (2005). The impact of family formation change on the
mistic.The policies that work in schools to improve the achievement cognitive, social, and emotional well-being of the next generation. The
of initially low-readiness children work also to improve the achieve- Future of Children, 15(2), 75–86.
ment of initially average- and high-readiness students. Doyle, O., Finnegan, S., & McNamara, K. A. (2010). Differential teacher
This is not to say that we should not invest in “good schools”: and parent ratings of school readiness in a disadvantaged community. Dublin,
strong school leadership, high-quality teachers, rigorous curricula, IE: Geary Institute, University College.
and all the other factors that characterize “good schools.” Bring- Frey, S. (2015, August). Study of brain scan shows impact of poverty on academic
ing initially low-readiness students closer to national academic achievement. Oakland, CA: EdSource.
achievement norms even if other students do better is a virtuous Gaynor, A. K. (2011/2012a). Different students: How typical schools
and worthy effort. are built to fail and need to change: A structural analysis. Journal of
At the level of early childhood development, as the analytic Education, 192(2/3), 13–27.
Gaynor, A. K. (2011/2012b). A reflection on the structural analysis and
work reported in this article suggests, implementing policies to
the case study. Journal of Education, 192(2/3), 31–32.
close the “readiness gap” is costly, imperfect, and probably politi-
Gaynor, A. K. (2012a). The racial, ethnic, and social class achievement
cally difficult. However, the reports of a teacher and an advocate gaps: A systems analysis. International Education Studies, 5(1), 29–49.
offer evidence of the critical need to address this gap that, as has Gaynor, A. K. (2012b, Summer).Typical schools need change. The Connector,
been noted, is a matter of social justice. The teacher is James Bou- isee Systems. Retrieved from http://www.iseesystems.com/community/
tin, who told the story of one of his former students in a blog that connector/Zine/2012_Summer/TypicalSchoolsNeedChange.aspx
was quoted by Valerie Strauss in The Washington Post on December Isaacs, J. B. (2012, March). The Social Genome Project: Starting school at
24, 2014: a disadvantage: The school readiness of poor children. Washington, D.C.:
Center on Children and Families, Brookings Institution.
Getting to and from school was not the only challenge Guill- Isaacs, J., & Magnuson, K. (2011). The Social Genome Project: Income and
ermo faced, though. His father abandoned his mother and education as predictors of children’s school readiness. Washington, D.C.:
siblings when he was 4 years old after some years of ver- Center on Children and Families, Brookings Institution.
bal and physical abuse, and his mom could not get a regular Karoly, L. A., Kilburn, M. R., & Cannon, J. S. (2005). Research brief:
housing situation on her own. Although I didn’t learn about Children at risk: Consequences for school readiness and beyond. Santa Monica,
these facts until after he’d left my classroom, it made a lot of CA: The RAND Corporation.
sense. Guillermo was a student who had suffered the loss and Layton, L. (2015, January 16). Majority of U.S. public school students are
abuse of his father, and the financial instability of his mother. in poverty. The Washington Post. Retrieved from http://wbhsenglish
On top of that, he struggled with the same challenges that .net/nic hols11/hhar r is11/tag/httpwww-washingtonpost
teenagers who don’t face such tremendous trauma deal with -comlocaleducationmajority-of-us-public-school-students-are-in
on a daily basis: hormonal changes, fitting in at school, and -poverty20150115df7171d0-9ce9-11e4-a7ee-526210d665b4_story
-html/
finding an identity.
Mukherjee, S. (2010). The emperor of all maladies: A biography of cancer. New
The advocate is Michael Rebell, Campaign for Educational Equity, York, NY: Scribner.
Teachers College, Columbia University, quoted by Linda Layton in Princeton University Library, Data and Statistical Services. (2007).
an article in TheWashington Post on January 16, 2015. Introduction to regression. Retrieved from http://www.princeton.edu
Rouse, C., Brooks-Gunn, J., & McLanahan, S. (Eds.). (2005). School
“We have to think about how to give these kids a meaningful readiness: Closing racial and ethnic gaps. The Future of Children, 15(1).
education,” he said. “We have to give them quality teachers, Strauss, V. (2014, December 24). We are trying to close the achievement
small class sizes, up-to-date equipment. But in addition, if gap all wrong. The Washington Post. Retrieved from http://www
36 J O U R N A L O F E D U C AT I O N • V O L U M E 1 9 5 • N U M B E R 3 • 2 0 1 5
Appendix A. Model Equations
INFLOWS:
Rate_of_Increase_in_Academic__Vocabulary_and_Language_Development = 1*((Quality_and_Intensity_of_Child’s_Formal_
and_Informal_PreSchooling+Effect_of_the_Neighborhood_on_Vocabulary_and_Language_Development+2*Effect_of_Parent_
Education_on_Child_Vocab_&_Language_Development+2*Effect_of_Level_of_Standard_English_&_Acad_Cult_on_Voc_&_Lang_
Dev+Effect_of_Self_Reading_on_Lang_&_Voc_Development)/7)
Child’s_Age(t) = Child’s_Age(t - dt) + (Increase_in_Child’s_Age) * dt
INIT Child’s_Age = 0.01
INFLOWS:
Increase_in_Child’s_Age = 1
Child’s_Level_of_Conceptual_Development(t) = Child’s_Level_of_Conceptual_Development(t - dt) + (Rate_of_Increase_in_
Conceptual_Development) * dt
INIT Child’s_Level_of_Conceptual_Development = 0
INFLOWS:
Rate_of_Increase_in_Conceptual_Development = (1*((3*Effect_of_Parent_Education_on_Conceptual_Development+2*Effect_
of_the_Child’s_PreSchooling_on_Social_&_Conceptual_Develop+2*Effect_of_Standard_English_&_Academic_Culture_in_the_
Home+Effects_of_Physical_Healthon_Conceptual_Development)/8))
Child’s_Level_of_Social_Development(t) = Child’s_Level_of_Social_Development(t - dt) + (Rate_of_Increase_or_Decrease_in_
Social_Development) * dt
INIT Child’s_Level_of_Social_Development = 0
INFLOWS:
Rate_of_Increase_or_Decrease_in_Social_Development = ((Effect_of_Maternal_Attachment_on__Child_Behavior+Effect_on_
Child_Behavior_of_Family_Instab_&_Stress+Effect_of_the_Child’s_PreSchooling_on_Social_&_Conceptual_Develop+Effect_of_
Parent_Education_on_the_Child’s_Social_Development)/4)
Maternal_Attachment(t) = Maternal_Attachment(t - dt) + (Flow_1) * dt
INIT Maternal_Attachment = 2
INFLOWS:
Flow_1 = Goal_of_Maternal_Attachment-Maternal_Attachment
Availability_of_Health_Care = GRAPH(Mean_Income_of__Affordable_Neighborhood)
(20000, 1.00), (27000, 1.00), (34000, 1.20), (41000, 1.75), (48000, 2.00), (55000, 2.20), (62000, 2.50), (69000, 2.50), (76000,
2.80), (83000, 3.00), (90000, 3.00)
Child’s_Aspiration_and_Self_Expectation = 2*((Effect_of_Parent_Education_on_Child’s_Aspiration_and_Self_Expect+Effect_of_
Maternal_Attachment_on_Aspiration__&_Self_Expectation)/2)
Child’s_Degree_of_Positive_Behavior = IF (2*(Child’s_Social_Development__Divided_by_Age_Age*((Effect_of_Maternal_
Attachment_on__Child_Behavior+Effect_on_Child_Behavior_of_Family_Instab_&_Stress))))<3 THEN((Child’s_Social_
Development__Divided_by_Age_Age*((Effect_of_Maternal_Attachment_on__Child_Behavior+Effect_on_Child_Behavior_of_
Family_Instab_&_Stress)/2))) ELSE (3)
38 J O U R N A L O F E D U C AT I O N • V O L U M E 1 9 5 • N U M B E R 3 • 2 0 1 5
Effect_of_Immigrant_or__Minority_Status_on_Income = IF (Family_Immigrant__and_Minority_Status = 1) THEN (.8) ELSE (1)
Effect_of_Immigr_&_Min_Status_on_Engl_&_Acad_Culture_in_the_Home = IF (Family_Immigrant__and_Minority_Status=1)
THEN (.8) ELSE (1)
Effect_of_Income_on_Family_Stress = IF (Family_Income<50000) THEN (3) ELSE IF (Family_Income<90000) THEN (2) ELSE (1)
Effect_of_Level_of_Standard_English_&_Acad_Cult_on_Voc_&_Lang_Dev =
GRAPH(Level_of_Standard_English_&_Acad_Cult_in_the_Home)
(1.00, 0.5), (1.20, 0.6), (1.40, 0.7), (1.60, 0.8), (1.80, 0.9), (2.00, 1.00), (2.20, 1.00), (2.40, 1.00), (2.60, 1.00), (2.80, 1.10), (3.00, 1.20)
Effect_of_Maternal_Attachment_on_Aspiration__&_Self_Expectation =
GRAPH(Maternal_Attachment)
(1.00, 0.5), (1.20, 0.5), (1.40, 0.5), (1.60, 0.6), (1.80, 0.7), (2.00, 0.8), (2.20, 0.85), (2.40, 0.9), (2.60, 1.00), (2.80, 1.00), (3.00, 1.20)
Effect_of_Maternal_Attachment_on__Child_Behavior = GRAPH(Maternal_Attachment)
(1.00, 0.7), (1.20, 0.75), (1.40, 0.8), (1.60, 0.85), (1.80, 0.9), (2.00, 1.00), (2.20, 1.10), (2.40, 1.20), (2.60, 1.25), (2.80, 1.30),
(3.00, 1.35)
Effect_of_Neighborhood_Income_Level_on_Variety_of_Food = GRAPH(Mean_Income_of__Affordable_Neighborhood)
(20000, 1.00), (33000, 1.00), (46000, 1.20), (59000, 1.75), (72000, 2.00), (85000, 2.20), (98000, 2.50), (111000, 2.50), (124000,
2.80), (137000, 3.00), (150000, 3.00)
Effect_of_Nutrition_on_Physical_Health = Family__Nutrition
Effect_of_Parent_Education_on_Child’s_Aspiration_and_Self_Expect = GRAPH(Parent___Educational_Attainment)
(10.0, 1.00), (11.0, 1.30), (12.0, 1.50), (13.0, 1.75), (14.0, 2.00), (15.0, 2.10), (16.0, 2.70), (17.0, 2.80), (18.0, 3.00), (19.0, 3.00),
(20.0, 3.00)
Effect_of_Parent_Education_on_Child_Vocab_&_Language_Development = GRAPH(Parent___Educational_Attainment) (10.0, 0.7),
(11.0, 0.8), (12.0, 0.9), (13.0, 0.95), (14.0, 1.00), (15.0, 1.00), (16.0, 1.20), (17.0, 1.30), (18.0, 1.35), (19.0, 1.40), (20.0, 1.50)
Effect_of_Parent_Education_on_Conceptual_Development =
GRAPH(Parent___Educational_Attainment) (10.0, 0.7), (11.0, 0.75), (12.0, 0.8), (13.0, 0.85), (14.0, 0.9), (15.0, 1.00), (16.0,
1.20), (17.0, 1.30), (18.0, 1.35), (19.0, 1.40), (20.0, 1.50)
Effect_of_Parent_Education_on_English_&_Acad_Cult_in_the_Home = GRAPH(Parent___Educational_Attainment) (10.0, 0.5),
(11.0, 0.55), (12.0, 0.6), (13.0, 0.7), (14.0, 0.9), (15.0, 1.00), (16.0, 1.20), (17.0, 1.30), (18.0, 1.35), (19.0, 1.40), (20.0, 1.50)
Effect_of_Parent_Education_on_PreSchooling = GRAPH(Parent___Educational_Attainment) (10.0, 0.7), (11.0, 0.8), (12.0, 0.9),
(13.0, 0.95), (14.0, 1.00), (15.0, 1.00), (16.0, 1.20), (17.0, 1.30), (18.0, 1.35), (19.0, 1.40), (20.0, 1.50)
Effect_of_Parent_Education_on_the_Child’s_Social_Development = GRAPH(Parent___Educational_Attainment) (10.0, 0.7), (11.0,
0.8), (12.0, 0.9), (13.0, 0.95), (14.0, 1.00), (15.0, 1.00), (16.0, 1.20), (17.0, 1.30), (18.0, 1.35), (19.0, 1.40), (20.0, 1.50)
Effect_of_PEA_on_Use_of_Health_Care = GRAPH(Parent___Educational_Attainment) (10.0, 1.00), (11.0, 1.00), (12.0, 1.00),
(13.0, 2.00), (14.0, 2.00), (15.0, 2.00), (16.0, 3.00), (17.0, 3.00), (18.0, 3.00), (19.0, 3.00), (20.0, 3.00)
Effect_of_Resilience_on_Readiness_for_School = GRAPH(Child’s_Resilience) (1.00, 0.5), (1.20, 0.6), (1.40, 0.7), (1.60, 0.8),
(1.80, 0.9), (2.00, 1.00), (2.20, 1.00), (2.40, 1.00), (2.60, 1.00), (2.80, 1.10), (3.00, 1.20)
Effect_of_Self_Reading_on_Lang_&_Voc_Development = GRAPH(Child’s_Self_Reading) (1.00, 0.5), (1.20, 0.6), (1.40, 0.7), (1.60,
0.8), (1.80, 0.9), (2.00, 1.00), (2.20, 1.00), (2.40, 1.00), (2.60, 1.00), (2.80, 1.10), (3.00, 1.20)
Effect_of_Single_Parent_Satus_on_Family_Stress = IF (Single_Parent_Family_Status=1) THEN (1.2) ELSE (1)
Effect_of_Single_Parent_Status_on_Family_Income = IF (Single_Parent_Family_Status=1) THEN (.75) ELSE (1)
Effect_of_Single_Parent_Status_on_Maternal_Attachment = IF (Single_Parent_Family_Status=1) THEN (.9) ELSE (1)
40 J O U R N A L O F E D U C AT I O N • V O L U M E 1 9 5 • N U M B E R 3 • 2 0 1 5