Late Talkers: A Population-Based Study of Risk Factors and School Readiness Consequences
Late Talkers: A Population-Based Study of Risk Factors and School Readiness Consequences
Late Talkers: A Population-Based Study of Risk Factors and School Readiness Consequences
Research Article
Purpose: This study was designed to (a) identify socioeconomic status, being a nonsingleton, older
sociodemographic, pregnancy and birth, family health, maternal age at birth, moderately low birth weight,
and parenting and child care risk factors for being a late lower quality parenting, receipt of day care for less
talker at 24 months of age; (b) determine whether late than 10 hr/week, and attention problems. Being a
talkers continue to have low vocabulary at 48 months; late talker increased children’s risk of having low
and (c) investigate whether being a late talker plays a vocabulary at 48 months and low school readiness
unique role in children’s school readiness at 60 months. at 60 months. Family socioeconomic status had the
Method: We analyzed data from the Early Childhood largest and most profound effect on children’s school
Longitudinal Study, a population-based sample of readiness.
9,600 children. Data were gathered when the children Conclusions: Limited vocabulary knowledge at 24 and
were 9, 24, 48, and 60 months old. 48 months is uniquely predictive of later school readiness.
Results: The risk of being a late talker at 24 months Young children with low vocabularies require additional
was significantly associated with being a boy, lower supports prior to school entry.
I
t is well established that children’s language abilities mathematical abilities (Jordan, Levine, & Huttenlocher,
are critical to their academic success. Studies have 1995; Pappas, Ginsburg, & Jiang, 2003; Purpura, Hume,
consistently shown that language supports children’s Sims, & Lonigan, 2011). Larger vocabularies have been
reading abilities in early and later grades (Catts, Fey, associated with number naming abilities, knowledge of
Tomblin, & Zhang, 2002; National Institute of Child measurement and shapes, and geometry and the ability to
Health and Human Development [NICHD] Early Child manipulate symbolic representations (Hornung, Schiltz,
Care Research Network, 2005; Scarborough, 2001; Storch Brunner, & Martin, 2014; Vukovic & Lesaux, 2013). In ad-
& Whitehurst, 2002). Children with stronger language abil- dition, language abilities have been found to promote chil-
ities, and in particular larger vocabularies, have better dren’s behavioral functioning. For example, children with
reading comprehension and decoding skills than children stronger language are better able to communicate with
with weaker language abilities (Braze, Tabor, Shankweiler, teachers and peers and to regulate their behavior and emo-
& Mencl, 2007; Schatschneider, Fletcher, Francis, Carlson, tions (Carson, Klee, Lee, Williams, & Perry, 1998; Cole,
& Foorman, 2004; Verhoeven & Van Leeuwe, 2008). Al- Armstrong, & Pemberton, 2010; Menting, Van Lier, &
though less well studied, investigations have also demon- Koot, 2010; Qi & Kaiser, 2004).
strated significant relations between language and children’s Because of the importance of language, efforts have
been made to identify children who may be at risk for learn-
ing difficulties early in life. Much attention has been given
a
Teachers College, Columbia University, University Park, NY to late talkers or children who have limited expressive vocab-
b
The Pennsylvania State University, University Park ularies at 2 years of age. Interest in this group of children
c
University of California, Irvine began 15 to 20 years ago with the seminal works of Thal
d
Temple University, Philadelphia, PA and Bates (Thal, 1991; Thal & Bates, 1988), Paul (1991,
Dana Bitetti is now at La Salle University, Philadelphia, PA. 1993), and Rescorla and colleagues (Rescorla & Merrin,
Correspondence to Carol Scheffner Hammer: 1998; Rescorla, Roberts, & Dahlsgaard, 1997). Since work
carol.hammer@tc.columbia.edu in this area began, researchers have attempted to identify
Editor: Sean Redmond factors that place children at risk for being late talkers and
Associate Editor: Carolyn Mervis have investigated late talkers’ long-term outcomes.
Received December 3, 2015
Revision received July 30, 2016
Accepted August 8, 2016 Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the time
https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0417 of publication.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 60 • 607–626 • March 2017 • Copyright © 2017 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 607
Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Lund Univ User on 01/17/2018
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Despite this work’s many contributions, it has been significant factor in three studies that included this variable
difficult to generalize their findings to the larger population in the analyses. Complementing this finding, TEDS found a
of U.S. children. This is because the majority of studies in- genetic component to late talking. TEDS and Generation R
volve clinical samples with relatively small numbers of par- also found a relationship between late talking and exter-
ticipants, limited diversity in terms of socioeconomic status nalizing problems. One or two studies tested the following
(SES), and unspecified racial or ethnic identities (Bavin & possible risk factors, which were not found to be signifi-
Bretherton, 2013). For example, Thal and Bates (Thal, 1991; cantly related to being a late talker at 18 to 24 months:
Thal & Bates, 1988) studied the development of 10 late maternal mental health, maternal behavioral risk factors
talkers and 10 language-matched peers (SES not provided). (e.g., cigarette smoking), quality of parenting, and attendance
Paul (Paul & Fountain, 1999; Paul & Shiffer, 1991) investi- in day care. The studies that followed children into their
gated 36 late talkers from primarily middle-SES homes. school years found that late talkers display later language
Rescorla and colleagues (Rescorla & Merrin, 1998; Rescorla, abilities at the lower end of the typical range, with a subset
Roberts, & Dahlsgaard, 1997) followed the language devel- of these children showing specific language impairment
opment of 34 late talkers and 21 typically developing children (SLI; Dale & Hayiou-Thomas, 2013; Rice et al., 2008).
of middle and upper-middle SES. Population-based studies Although these studies have made valuable contribu-
are needed to better understand the risk factors for being a tions, additional research is needed for several reasons. First,
late talker as well as whether and to what extent late taking most of these studies took place outside of the United States.
may interfere with children’s school readiness. This, of course, is not by itself a limitation; however, the
To date, several community-based and population- United States differs from European countries and Australia
based studies have been conducted. Two took place in in many ways, including its racial and ethnic diversity and
Australia: (a) the Randomly Ascertained Sample of Children its educational, medical, and political contexts that can af-
born in Australia’s Largest State (RASCL), a longitudinal fect children’s development. Thus, a study conducted on a
study of 2,224 children from birth through 8 years of age sample from the United States would broaden the field’s
(cf. Rice, Taylor, & Zubrick, 2008; Zubrick, Taylor, Rice, & understanding of the effect of being a late talker in a more
Slegers, 2007), and (b) the Early Language in Victoria Study, diverse context. Second, three of the studies—the RASCL,
a longitudinal study of 1,911 children from birth through Early Language in Victoria, and Denmark studies—focused
age 4 years, which also included children learning English on children’s later development of their language abilities
as a second language (cf. Reilly et al., 2006, 2010). Others but not academic achievement or school readiness. It is
were conducted in the Netherlands, Sweden, Demark, and important to understand whether being a late talker is a
England and Wales. The study conducted in the Netherlands factor that affects children’s school readiness and academic
was titled “Generation R,” which was a longitudinal study outcomes given that academic success is critical for children’s
of more than 3,700 children who were assessed at 18 months future well-being. Third, the Early Language in Victoria,
and during the preschool years (Henrichs et al., 2011, 2013). TEDS, and Denmark studies investigated a relatively limited
The Swedish study involved more than 1,500 children who set of characteristics when identifying risk factors for low
were recruited at birth (Westerlund & Lagerberg, 2008). vocabulary at 24 months. An increased understanding is
The study in Denmark was a cross-sectional study of chil- needed of the role that various demographic, neurodevelop-
dren at 2 and 3 years of age (Bleses & Vach, 2013; note mental, maternal, and environmental factors and parenting
that middle-SES families were overrepresented in this study). and child care experiences may have in placing children at
The Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), which was risk for being a late talker and the effect these may have
carried out in England and Wales, was a longitudinal study on children’s later development (Bavin & Bretherton, 2013;
of twins who were followed from birth through age 12 years Bleses & Vach, 2013). This study was designed to address
(cf. Dale, Harlaar, Hayiou-Thomas, & Plomin, 2010; Dale these needs. The study (a) involved a population-based sample
et al., 1998). In addition, data from the NICHD Early Child from the United States, (b) included a large number of poten-
Care Study, a birth cohort study conducted in the United tial risk factors associated with late talking and later school
States, have been used to investigate children’s vocabulary readiness skills that have been implicated in the literature,
delays at 3 years of age and beyond (La Paro, Justice, Skibbe, and (c) focused on the unique role of late talking in school
& Pianta, 2004). readiness by targeting children’s language, literacy, math,
In general, these studies identified potential risk fac- and socioemotional outcomes at 48 and 60 months of age.
tors for being a late talker, although the specific risk fac-
tors considered and the number of factors included in the
analyses varied across the studies. Being a boy and of low Risk Factors Affecting Development
SES were identified as risk factors for late talking in most A number of factors may place children at risk for
of the studies. Low birth weight was found to be a factor being a late talker at age 2 years and may affect children’s
in one of the three studies that considered it. Preterm birth vocabulary, reading, math, and behavioral abilities during
was identified in one of two studies that investigated this the preschool years. These include sociodemographic fac-
factor. Low maternal age was found to be factor in being tors, pregnancy and birth characteristics, family health and
a late talker in the Swedish study but not in the RASCL family history of learning problems, parenting and child
study. Also, family history of language difficulties was a care, and early behavioral functioning. Many of these factors
608 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 60 • 607–626 • March 2017
610 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 60 • 607–626 • March 2017
Parenting
Parenting quality was indexed by the average of scores Outcome Variables
on two parenting assessments administered at 24 months. Late Talker at 24 Months
The first was a modified version of the Home Observation Children’s vocabulary was assessed during interviews
for Measurement of the Environment (Caldwell & Bradley, with the children’s mothers at 24 months of age using a
1984). This assessment measured activities done with modified version of the M-CDI that was developed by
612 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 60 • 607–626 • March 2017
614 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 60 • 607–626 • March 2017
Note. Child age and parenting score are standardized with a mean of 0 and an SD of 1. SES = socioeconomic status.
a
Sample size is rounded to the nearest 50 per Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort confidentiality requirements.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Weighted regressions.
as late talker, so the direction of causality cannot be deter- late talkers were found to have a low receptive vocabulary
mined. However, it is still useful to see that these variables 2 years later. Eighty-three percent of the sample did not
were significantly related even with many other variables have vocabulary difficulties at 24 or 48 months; however,
controlled. 8% of these children had a vocabulary problem at 48 months.
The final three columns of Table 2 show the results Of those who were late talkers at 24 months, one fourth
using a sample-wide (rather than gender-specific) cutoff continued to have a vocabulary problem at 48 months. This
for defining being a late talker at 24 months. As expected, accounted for 2.6% of the total sample.
using a sample-wide cutoff showed boys as being much
more likely than girls (an odds ratio of about 3 to 1) to be
late talkers. The other findings using the gender-specific Role of Being a Late Talker at 24 Months on
cutoffs were generally robust to the use of a single sample- Vocabulary at 48 Months
wide cutoff. Once again, we found strong SES effects Table 3 shows the results of the logistic regressions
that were largely mediated by control variables such as predicting low vocabulary at 48 months. The first three
parenting quality, child care, and the child’s approaches to models used gender-specific cutoffs for 24-month late talker,
learning (attention). 48-month low vocabulary, and 24-month child behavior
problems, whereas the final three models used sample-wide
cutoffs. Model 1 included the following as predictors: being
Persistence of Vocabulary Problems at 48 Months a late talker, race/ethnicity, the child’s age at assessment,
Using sample-wide cutoffs, we also investigated the gender, a Gender × Age interaction, and the SES quintiles.
extent to which late talkers continued to have vocabulary Being a late talker at 24 months was a strong predictor of
problems and the extent to which children who were not low vocabulary at 48 months, increasing the odds of this
Note. Child age and parenting scores are standardized with a mean of 0 and an SD of 1. SES = socioeconomic status.
a
Sample size is rounded to the nearest 50 per Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort confidentiality requirements.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Weighted regressions.
outcome by a factor of 4.3 in Model 1. Being Hispanic behavior problems at 60 months. These models included be-
(1.67:1) and low or middle SES compared with high SES ing a late talker at 24 months, low vocabulary at 48 months,
also significantly increased these odds (i.e., the lower three the interaction between these variables as predictors, and
quintiles had odds ratios of 5.02:1, 2.99:1, and 1.85:1). behavioral functioning at 24 months and were estimated
Additional demographics were included in Model 2, using both gender-specific and sample-wide cutoffs for late
but none were significant. Model 3 added gestational and talker and low vocabulary.
birth conditions, family, child care, and child behavior
problems. Significant predictors were parenting, child care, Low Reading Performance
and approaches to learning problems, and these partially Table 4 displays the results for low reading perfor-
explained the SES effects. The final three models used sam- mance at 60 months. As before, similar patterns were ob-
ple-wide rather than gender-specific cutoffs. These results served for both gender-specific and sample-wide cutoffs for
showed the same patterns as were found with gender- late talker and 48-month vocabulary. It is not surprising
specific cutoffs. In particular, the very strong effect of SES that low vocabulary at 48 months strongly increased the
on low vocabulary continued to be observed. odds of low reading performance at 60 months. However,
even with this variable controlled, being a late talker also
increased the odds of low reading at 60 months. This was
Role of Being a Late Talker at 24 Months on
significant at the .05 level using the overall cutoffs and at
School Readiness the .10 level using gender-specific cutoffs. In addition, even
Tables 4–7 present the results of logistic regressions with these controls, low SES had exceptionally powerful
predicting low reading and math performance and high effects on low reading, with odds ratios of low vocabulary
616 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 60 • 607–626 • March 2017
Note. Child age and parenting scores are standardized with a mean of 0 and an SD of 1. SES = socioeconomic status.
a
Sample size is rounded to the nearest 50 per Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort confidentiality requirements.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Weighted regressions.
in the range of 7:1 when comparing the lowest and highest lowest SES quintile and almost 8 to 1 for the second-lowest
SES quintiles. Other noteworthy findings included the posi- quintile. In Model 3, quality parenting and 48-month
tive effect on low reading of having a family member with center-based child care or Head Start for more than 10 hr/
a learning disability as well as the strong effects of child week resulted in decreased odds of low math performance.
care more than 10 hr/week in decreasing the odds of low Having a family member with learning disabilities increased
reading at kindergarten entry. the odds by 1.65:1. These variables also partially accounted
for the negative effect of low SES on the outcomes. The re-
Low Math Performance sults using sample-wide cutoffs showed very similar patterns.
Table 5 includes the calculations for low math perfor-
mance at 60 months; results generally are similar to those Problem Behavior
for low reading. The effect of being a late talker (2.20:1) Table 6 shows calculations for the odds of teacher-
was significantly and positively related to low math scores, reported behavior problems—approaches to learning and
as was low receptive vocabulary (3.51:1). The interaction externalizing and internalizing problems—at 60 months.
between these variables was not significant. African Model 2 for approaches to learning problems showed that,
American children were at increased odds for low math even after extensive controls, being a late talker signifi-
scores (1.52:1). cantly increased a child’s odds (2.19:1) for difficulties in
The effect of the lowest SES quintile was even larger this area. In addition, in Model 1, low SES increased a
than for low reading, with odds of almost 15 to 1 for the child’s odds of having approaches to learning problems,
Note. Child age and parenting scores are standardized with a mean of 0 and an SD of 1. SES = socioeconomic status.
a
Sample size is rounded to the nearest 50 per Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort confidentiality requirements.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Weighted regressions.
618 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 60 • 607–626 • March 2017
Note. Child age and parenting scores are standardized with a mean of 0 and an SD of 1. SES = socioeconomic status.
a
Sample size is rounded to the nearest 50 per Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort confidentiality requirements.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Weighted regressions.
enable us to control for possible confounding factors to population-based investigations that addressed this issue
determine whether late talking has a unique effect on chil- (Bleses & Vach, 2013; Dale et al., 2010; Reilly et al., 2007;
dren’s school readiness in the U.S. population as a whole. Westerlund & Lagerberg, 2008) as well as other studies of
early language development (e.g., Harrison & McLeod,
2010; Maatta et al., 2012).
Risk Factors for Being a Late Talker Also, difficulties with approaches to learning at
We found that a number of sociodemographic, birth, 24 months (i.e., having difficulties paying attention, work-
and family variables were significantly related to being a ing independently, concentrating, and completing tasks)
late talker at 24 months of age. As expected, boys were sig- were associated with late talker status. This result is consis-
nificantly more likely than girls to be a late talker. Remov- tent with that of Henrichs et al. (2013). Because ratings of
ing this effect by using gender-specific norms, we found children’s behavior and the M-CDI were both completed
that low SES strongly increased the odds of being a late when the children were 24 months of age, the direction of
talker—an effect largely explained by low birth weight, the the relationship is unclear. It could be that having prob-
quality of parenting, time in day care, and the child’s ap- lems with approaches to learning is a risk factor for being
proaches to learning (attention) problems. The finding that a late talker, or it could be that this difficulty is the result
boys are at greater risk than girls of being a late talker and of low language abilities at an early age. Whichever the di-
in early language development is consistent with several rection of causality, and other things being equal, problems
Note. Child age and parenting scores are standardized with a mean of 0 and an SD of 1. SES = socioeconomic status.
a
Sample size is rounded to the nearest 50 per Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort confidentiality requirements.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Weighted regressions.
with approaches to learning are positively associated with shared environment of parents and children influences
being a late talker. As noted earlier, co-occurrence of atten- children’s language development (Dale et al., 2015). Warm
tional difficulties and SLI have been observed, but the na- and nurturing parents create stimulating environments and
ture of the relationship remains unclear (Redmond, 2016). provide supports by establishing routines. This, in turn, re-
Of note is that the strong relation between SES and sults in higher cognitive and language abilities (cf. Conger &
being a late talker was fully explained by the mediating Donnellan, 2007; Guo & Harris, 2000; Yeung et al., 2002).
variables. The effect of SES on late talking was inconsis- Also, these results are supported by past studies that have
tent in the four prior population-based studies that were shown that center-based child care promotes children’s out-
reviewed. Our study helps clarify the relationship because comes in many areas and that child care appears to minimize
the investigation targeted a large number of possible ex- the effects of stress experienced by low-income parents (Hall
planatory variables in the statistical analyses. We found et al., 2009; NICHD Early Child Care Research Network,
that the effects of SES were accounted for by a number of 2000, 2002, 2003; Melhuish et al., 2008; Vallotton et al.,
variables, including quality parenting and children’s atten- 2012; Vandell et al., 2010). However, attendance in child
dance in a day care center. These findings are consistent care has also been found to increase children’s risk for be-
with a large number of studies that have found that quality havior problems (NICHD Early Child Care Research Net-
parenting supports children’s development and that the work, 2003).
620 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 60 • 607–626 • March 2017
622 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 60 • 607–626 • March 2017
624 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 60 • 607–626 • March 2017
626 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 60 • 607–626 • March 2017