Hughes - A Model of Musical Memory
Hughes - A Model of Musical Memory
Hughes - A Model of Musical Memory
Jennifer Mishra,
University of Northern Iowa
Processing Strategies Additive and Serial Processing Strategies have received little
research attention. These two strategies are similar in that the music
The way in which a performer approaches a piece of music is is initially sectionalized. The difference lies in how the sections are
determined by training, experience and task difficulty. Four combined. Sections are systematically lengthened when the
processing strategies have been identified to-date (Mishra, 2003): Additive Processing Strategy is utilized, but in the Segmented
Segmented, Holistic, Serial, and Additive. Isolating portions of the Processing Strategy, sections are discrete. It is possible that the
piece for practice is the primary characteristic of both the Additive Processing Strategy may be a sub-set or variation on the
Segmented and Additive Processing Strategies. Where these Segmented strategy, but preliminary evidence suggests disparate
procedures differ is in how new material is added to a fragment. outcomes for the two strategies (Mishra, 2003). Sequential
Practicing discrete fragments separately, then as a unit processing was found to interact with phrase structures as memory
demonstrates the use of a Segmented Processing Strategy; while was highest at the start of each phrase and declined towards the end.
continually adding material, thus lengthening a segment constitutes
the Additive Procedure. When utilizing a Holistic Processing Each of the four processing strategies can be successfully utilized
Strategy, the piece is repeatedly performed in its entirety, allowing in the memorization of music (Mishra, 2003). It remains to be seen
for minor regressions if errors or memory lapses occur. However, whether the use of one strategy, or combination of strategies,
when errors or memory lapses result in a repeated return to the results in a more efficient, effective, or secure memory. Regardless
beginning of the piece rather than a concerted push through to the of the approach to memorization, various sensory and analytical
end, the procedure is termed Serial. learning styles are also available to the performer. For the purposes
232
ICMPC8, Evanston, IL, USA August 3-7, 2004
of this model, the four most commonly discussed memorization independently of a structural representation of the piece. For
strategies (Aural, Visual, Kinesthetic, and Analytical) have been example, segments may be based on the physical features of the
divided and grouped into Sensory Learning Styles (consisting of printed page. Even experienced performers may organize practice
Aural, Visual, and Kinesthetic Memories) and an Analytical segments based on page breaks rather than musical structure
Learning Style and will be discussed separately. While these four (Chaffin, Imreh, & Crawford, 2002).
types of memory are generally discussed together, the Analytical
Strategy appears to function discretely; influenced by the training Various techniques are commonly used when memorizing music:
and ability of the performer. Further, preliminary evidence suggests playing hands separately and blocking chords (on a keyboard
that the use of Aural, Visual, and Kinesthetic Strategies may be instrument), rehearsing under-tempo, and rehearsing segments of
related to personal learning styles (Svard & Mack, 2002). the piece in reverse order. These techniques may emphasize or
promote the use of the Analytical Learning Style, generally in
Sensory Learning Styles conjunction with one or more of the Sensory Learning Styles;
allowing the perception of patterns and associations, which would
Aural Memory is the ability to hear the notes of a piece of music in otherwise be obscured. Performing at tempos slower than the
the proper order without relying on a sound source or notational performance tempo, especially for difficult pieces or sections of
cues. The ability to recognize a performed passage as correct or pieces, may serve to focus cognitive attention and increase
incorrect may also rely upon Aural Memory. Visual Memory is the concentration (Lehmann & Ericsson, 1998). As kinesthetic
ability to recall a mental picture of musical notation, as a whole or memory may develop more rapidly and without conscious thought
in parts, or visualize finger patterns or hand positions on an during Notation-Based Practice, it is possible that reducing
instrument. Retention of the muscular movements involved in rehearsal tempi reduces the automaticity of the physical motions
performing a piece of music is termed Kinesthetic Memory. and allows slower cognitive associations to develop. Performing
Though much discussion of these strategies exists in the hands separately on a keyboard instrument may serve a similar
pedagogical literature, it is unclear whether one or another is function. Attention is divided when both left and right hands are
superior. It is generally accepted that these strategies interact to simultaneously performing. By rehearsing hands separately,
various degrees and a combination of strategies will result in a more patterns may be identified that would otherwise be over-looked.
secure memory. Blocking chords, allows for dynamic kinesthetic movements to be
observed concurrently. Linking segments in reverse order, or in fact
Individual learning preferences or the type of musical material may any order other than starting at the first segment of the piece, forces
influence the choice of learning style. Preliminary evidence the performer to create a new beginning to the piece; negating serial
suggests that sensory strategies may be based on an individual’s effects. The value of these techniques appears to be in focusing
learning style. Preferred learning styles were determined by a attention and encouraging cognitive analysis.
published measurement and correlated with memorization
techniques determined through interviews. Musicians utilizing The above Processing Strategies and Learning Styles can all be
visual strategies to memorize music (e.g., visualizing the page, used in various combinations and possibly to a differing degree of
recognizing visual patterns, watching hands) were generally efficiency, to achieve a memorized performance. A complete
classified as visual learners. Aural learners generally reported memorized performance, which defines the end of the Conscious
listening to the piece to recognize auditory patterns, hearing Memorization Stage, is defined as performing the piece, near or at
harmonic progressions, and awareness of variations in the melody. concert tempo, without reference to notational cues. Imreh was able
Kinesthetic learners relied on muscle memory and physical to achieve her first, halting memorization attempts during her
sensations (Svard & Mack, 2002). eighth practice session, but it was not until her seventeenth session
that she was able to fluently perform the piece (Chaffin, Imreh, &
Analytical Learning Style Crawford, 2002). The result of Conscious Memorization Practice is
a complete memorized performance, but rarely is one successful
Analytical Memory requires the cognitive interpretation of patterns memorized performance sufficient for a musician outside
and repetitions in a musical work. The focus is on forming experimental research studies.
relationships and finding familiar patterns. An Analytical Learning
Style may be utilized in practice as early as the Preview Stage, OVER-LEARNING STAGE
forming a mental representation of the form, harmony, and texture
of the piece. While analysis of the piece may be informal, not Performing musicians generally continue to rehearse even after a
requiring theoretical terminology, training and education in formal piece can be performed from memory. This final stage of
and harmonic analyses may inform the use of Analytical Strategies. Over-learning has received very little research attention primarily
Experts and advanced performers appear to utilize Analytical due to research protocols in which the ultimate goal is not a concert
Strategies frequently while inexperienced performers do not performance, but memorization itself. Naturalistic studies such as
(Hallam, 1997; Nuki, 1984; Williamon & Valentine, 2002). that conducted by Chaffin and Imreh highlight the importance of
practicing beyond the point when the piece has been memorized.
Analytical Strategies are most frequently discussed in reference to Imreh was able to perform the piece from memory during her
a Segmented Processing Strategy, utilizing structural features of the seventeenth practice session; however, she did not feel the piece
piece to define practice segments (Chaffin, Imreh, & Crawford, was sufficiently learned at that point and continued practicing for
2002; Gruson, 1988; Hallam, 1997; Miklaszewski & Sawicki, 1992; an additional 27 sessions before performing the piece sans notation
Miklaszewski 1983, 1989 & 1995; Williamon & Valentine, 2002). (Chaffin, Imreh, & Crawford, 2002).
However, it is possible to utilize a Segmented Processing Strategy
233
ICMPC8, Evanston, IL, USA August 3-7, 2004
While this stage may be very important in preparing a piece for highlight a difference between over-learning and over-playing. It
performance, the amount of over-learning necessary to prepare a remains to be seen whether thoughtless repetition beyond the point
piece for a concert performance has not been systematically of memorization is beneficial to retention or if conscious
investigated, nor have the motivations underpinning this stage of re-organization of the musical material is necessary.
rehearsal. Further, it is unclear to what extent the procedures
adopted by the performer during this stage are efficient and/or CONCLUSION
necessary. Three prospective subdivisions of the Over-Learning
Stage will be discussed: Re-Learning, Automatization, and The purpose of this article was to clarify the complex process of
Maintenance Rehearsal. memorization by creating a structure on which to connect disparate
research findings. Based on the literature to-date, the memorization
The musical information is cognitively reorganized during the of music appears to comprise three stages: Preview, Practice, and
Re-Learning stage. Alternate learning strategies and procedures Over-Learning. Previous experience and enculturation are also
may be utilized to strengthen memory and provide additional or very important in informing the memorization process. Allowing
alternative retrieval cues. Retrieval cues, as discussed by Chaffin for individual differences and preferences has been the greatest
and Imreh (2002) and Williamon and Valentine (2002), serve to challenge in constructing this model. Further, the process of
activate musical material, bringing the information from long-term memorization appears to be dynamic; changing as practice
into working memory. Retrieval cues may be refined during this progresses towards the ultimate goal of a concert performance.
stage and practice may shift from detailed, phrase-by-phrase Though the model has a great deal of flexibility built-in, it is
memory cues to more general, formal cues (see Williamon & anticipated that as the research into memorization continues to
Valentine, 2002 for a discussion of hierarchical memory cues). grow, the model will adapt and change to allow for new answers as
Retrieval cues developed Conscious Memorization may be tested well as new questions about how musicians memorize and what
to determine whether they are efficient and effective. Superfluous constitute the most efficient and effective memorization strategies.
or unstable cues may be replaced or eliminated; reducing the
number of retrieval cues to only those that are absolutely necessary. REFERENCES
Automatization is the extended and consistent repetition of a 1. Chaffin, R., Imreh, G., & Crawford, M. Practicing perfection:
behavioral sequence, which results in a routinized, stereotyped, Piano performance as expert memory, Lawrence Erlbaum
inflexible performance; completed automatically with no conscious Associates, Mahwah, NJ, 2002.
control. This theory of automatization as described by Shiffrin and
Schneider (Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977) may or may not accurately 2. Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. “Expert and exceptional
reflect what musicians seek to accomplish by continuing to performance: Evidence of maximal adaptation to task
rehearse past the point of initial memorization. While it is by no constraints”, Annual Rev. Psy., Vol. 47, 1996, pp 273-305.
means clear whether musicians are attempting to standardize the
3. Gruson, L. M. Rehearsal skill and musical competence: Does
performance in the Over-learning Stage, it is possible that
practice make perfect? In J. A. Sloboda (Ed.)., Generative
over-learning serves to automate the physical motions and/or the
Processes in Music: The Psychology of Performance,
retrieval cues. This would allow conscious thought to be redirected
Improvisation and Composition. Oxford University Press,
during a performance to musical or interpretative, rather than
London, 1988.
physical, concerns.
4. Hallam, S. “The development of memorization strategies in
Maintenance Rehearsal is the periodic performance of a piece to
musicians: Implications for education”, Br. J. of Mus. Ed., Vol.
keep the information active in memory over a long period of time.
14(1), 1997, pp 87-97.
This stage does not necessarily require additional learning, but is
the practice of retrieving the information. Maintenance Rehearsal is 5. Halpern, A. R., & Bower, G. H. “Musical expertise and
only necessary when a piece must be retained over a long period of melodic structure in memory for musical notation”, Am. J. Psy.,
time. Vol. 95(1), 1982, pp 31-50.
The Over-learning Stage may provide the opportunity to practice 6. Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. “Performance without
the performance, anticipating memory lapses that might occur. preparation: Structure and acquisition of expert sight-reading
Experienced musicians may purposely practice for memory lapses and accompanying performance”, Psychomusicology, Vol. 15,
in performance. Imreh reported purposefully trying to deliberately 1996, pp 1-29.
disrupt performance and add discomfort to check her memory
back-up systems (Chaffin, Imreh, & Crawford, 2002). Distributing 7. Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. “Preparation of a public
practice and practicing in alternate internal and external contexts piano performance: The relation between practice and
may strengthen memory and reduce the risks of memory lapses in a performance”, Musicae Scientiae, Vol. 2, 1998, pp 67-94.
performance. 8. Miklaszewski, K. “A case study of a pianist preparing a musical
The only experimental research study specifically concerned with performance”, Psy. of Mus., Vol. 17, 1989, pp 95-109.
the over-learning process found that playing beyond the 9. Miklaszewski, K. Individual differences in preparing a musical
memorization point did not result in superior retention of the piece composition for public performance. In M. Manturzewska, K.
(Rubin-Rabson, 1941d). While this may be a by-product of Miklaszewski, & A. Bialkowski (Eds.). Psychology of Music
experimental research using very short excerpts, it may also Today. Warsaw: Fryderyk Chopin Academy of Music, 1995.
234
ICMPC8, Evanston, IL, USA August 3-7, 2004
10. Miklaszewski, K., & Sawicki, L. Segmentation of music 26. Svard, L., & Mack, A. (2002, September). Playing by ear or is
introduced by practicing pianists preparing compositions for it by sight or feel: Learning styles and musicians. Paper
public performance. In R. Dalmonte and M. Baroni (Eds.), presented at the 45th Annual Conference of the College Music
Secondo Conegno Europedi Analisi Musicale (pp. 113-121). Society, Kansas City, MO.
Trento, Italy: Dipartimento di storia della civiltá europea,
Universitá degli studi di Trento, 1992. 27. Williamon, A. “The value of performing by memory”, Psy. of
Mus., Vol. 27, 1999, pp 84-95.
11. Mishra, J. “A qualitative analysis of strategies employed in
efficient and inefficient memorization”, Bul. Council Res. in 28. Williamon, A., & Valentine, E. “The role of retrieval structures
Mus. Ed., Vol. 152, 2003, pp 74-86. in memorizing music”, Cog. Psy., Vol. 44, 2002, pp 1-32.
15. Oura, Y., & Hatano, G. “Memory for melodies among subjects
differing in age and experience in music”, Psy. of Mus., Vol. 16,
1988, pp 91-109.
235
ICMPC8, Evanston, IL, USA August 3-7, 2004
Enculturation/Experience
Preview
Overlearning
236