This document discusses inaccuracies in how the Electra complex is portrayed in psychology textbooks. It was actually Carl Jung, not Sigmund Freud, who coined the term "Electra complex" to describe the female counterpart to Freud's Oedipus complex. Freud later rejected the term and did not believe female psychosexual development paralleled the male experience. However, many psychology textbooks incorrectly attribute the term to Freud or imply it was a Freudian concept. The document analyzes textbooks and finds most do not accurately represent the origins and rejection of the term by Freud and Jung. Teachers are encouraged to correct this common error for students.
This document discusses inaccuracies in how the Electra complex is portrayed in psychology textbooks. It was actually Carl Jung, not Sigmund Freud, who coined the term "Electra complex" to describe the female counterpart to Freud's Oedipus complex. Freud later rejected the term and did not believe female psychosexual development paralleled the male experience. However, many psychology textbooks incorrectly attribute the term to Freud or imply it was a Freudian concept. The document analyzes textbooks and finds most do not accurately represent the origins and rejection of the term by Freud and Jung. Teachers are encouraged to correct this common error for students.
This document discusses inaccuracies in how the Electra complex is portrayed in psychology textbooks. It was actually Carl Jung, not Sigmund Freud, who coined the term "Electra complex" to describe the female counterpart to Freud's Oedipus complex. Freud later rejected the term and did not believe female psychosexual development paralleled the male experience. However, many psychology textbooks incorrectly attribute the term to Freud or imply it was a Freudian concept. The document analyzes textbooks and finds most do not accurately represent the origins and rejection of the term by Freud and Jung. Teachers are encouraged to correct this common error for students.
This document discusses inaccuracies in how the Electra complex is portrayed in psychology textbooks. It was actually Carl Jung, not Sigmund Freud, who coined the term "Electra complex" to describe the female counterpart to Freud's Oedipus complex. Freud later rejected the term and did not believe female psychosexual development paralleled the male experience. However, many psychology textbooks incorrectly attribute the term to Freud or imply it was a Freudian concept. The document analyzes textbooks and finds most do not accurately represent the origins and rejection of the term by Freud and Jung. Teachers are encouraged to correct this common error for students.
Copyright:
Attribution Non-Commercial (BY-NC)
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online from Scribd
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 3
with parents under the single rubric of the Oedipus complex
Inaccurate Representation of the Electra (Freud, 1925/1961c).
Complex in Psychology Textbooks Jung wrote nothing about the Electra complex in his later development of the theory of archetypes, preferring instead to focus on anima and animus as the innate structures of sex and Christopher T. Kilmartin gender Qung, 1917/1953). The use of the term Electra com- Mary Washington College plex implies that sexual desire for the other-sex parent, the Daniel Dervin central issue in the phallic stage for boys, is also central to girls' Department of English, Linguistics, and Speech psychosexual development in this stage. Clearly, Freud did Mary Washington College not believe so, and therefore, the Electra complex and the Oedipus complex should not be represented as parallel in textbook discussions of his theory. The Electra complex is In their discussbn of Ereud's theory, authors of introductory actually not much more than a footnote in psychoanalytic psychobgy, devebpmental psychobgy, and persorwlity textbooks history, rejected by the father of the field (Freud) and later often refer to the female version of the Oedipus complex as the ignored by the very person who coined the term Oung). Electra complex. However, it mas actually Carl Jung who coined this term, which was later rejected by Ereud and even ignored by How, then, does the story of Electra find its way into Jung himself. A survey of a sample of recent psychobgy textbooks introductory psychology, developmental psychology, and per- reveals that a substantial number of authors attribute the Electra sonality textbooks, and bow is it represented there? Following complex term to Ereud. Teachers of psychobgy would do well to the form of similar content analyses such as Queresbi (1993), correct this common distortion for students. Linton (1992), and Leong and Pointer (1991), we surveyed the treatment of the Electra complex in recently published introductory psychology (n = 30), developmental psychology Introductory psychology, developmental psychology, and (n = 26) and personality textbooks (n = 19). We found personality textbooks usually include discussions of Sigmund substantial problems with the representation of this aspect of Freud's theories of personality and psychosexual develop- psychoanalytic theory, as an earlier article (Prytula, Oster, &. ment. Authors of these texts often devote significant space to Davis, 1977) did with textbook representations of the work of a discussion of one of the most controversial aspects of Freud's John Watson. theory, the Oedipus complex, in which Freud described a series Correct citations of the origins of the Electra complex were of intrapsychic events as taking place during the phallic stage more the exception than the rule. Almost one-third (23 of 77, of development. Frequently, the term Electra complex is used 30%) of the texts had no mention of the Electra complex. to describe the female counterpart of the male's Oedipus When the surveyed authors did discuss this term, however (n complex. However, the use of this term and its attribution to = 54), they mentioned Freud's rejection of it only 23% (n = Freud are historically and theoretically inaccurate. 12) of the time. Moreover, the Jungian origin of the term It was not Freud, but Carl Jung (1912/1961) who initiated Electra complex was not to be found in any of the texts. a clinical discourse on the Electra complex. Jealousy, rein- Overall, 72% (n = 39) of the texts that mentioned the Electra forced by a "budding eroticism," he wrote, gains in strength complex either directly attributed the term to Freud or else until the "Oedipus complex soon assumes its classical form" strongly implied that it was a Freudian term by the use of the (p. 154). passive voice, stating that the female Oedipus complex is sometimes referred to as the Electra complex. In these latter [A] daughter develops a specific liking for the father, cases, which comprise 26% (n = 14) of the texts that con- with a correspondingly jealous attitude toward the tained references to the Electra complex, the author does not mother. We could call this the Electra complex. As inform the reader of the source of this terminology. However, everyone knows, Electra took vengeance on her mother because this language appears in sections about Freud's the- Clytemnestra for murdering her husband Agamemnon ory, a reader would probably assume that Freud used the term. and thus robbing her [Electra] of her beloved father, (p. As a group, developmental psychology and introductory 134) psychology textbooks were the least accurate in their portrayal of the Electra complex, with 82% (n = 32) of books that Jung went on to discuss "the unconscious presence of numer- referred to the Electra complex (n = 39) citing the term ous fantasies which have their roots in the infantile past ... inaccurately. Although many developmental psychology which in men may be designated as the Oedipus complex, in books (n - 7, 41%) did so by implication, most introductory women as the Electra complex" (p. 154). psychology texts (n = 14, 64%) directly attributed the term Freud quite clearly rejected the term Electra complex to Freud. Personality textbooks fared better, with 46% (n — when, in 1920 he wrote, "I do not see any progress or advan- 6) describing Freud's rejection of the term accurately. How- tage in the introduction of the term 'Electra complex,' and do ever, none of the authors of these books attributed the Electra not advocate its use" (Freud, 1920/1961b, p. 155). Later, he complex to Jung, despite the fact that most personality texts wrote, "We are right in rejecting the term Electra complex contain extensive descriptions of Jung's theory. which seeks to insist that the situation of the two sexes is Historians of psychology may be interested in the path by analogous" (p. 229). Freud saw boys and girls as struggling in which the Electra complex migrated from Jung's writings into different ways with the reality of the penis—the girl with envy Freud's in contemporary textbooks, an issue obviously beyond at its lack and the boy with apprehension about its loss. Hence, the scope of this article. Teachers of psychology, however, Freud subsumed the conflicts over triangulated relationships would do well to correct this common distortion for students.
Vol. 24, No. 4,1997 269
References the therapist teaches the client to relax, creating a response that is incompatible with anxiety. For the second step, the therapist and client work together to create an anxiety hierar- Freud, S. (1961a). Female sexuality. In]. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), chy, ordering anxiety-provoking stimulifromlowest to highest The standard edition of the complete psychologicd works of Sigmund (related to a common fear dimension, such as proximity to a Freud (Vol. 21, pp. 223-255). London: Hogartb. (Original work publisbed 1931) snake). The therapist ensures that there is not a large difference Freud, S. (1961b). Tbe psycbogenesis of female homosexuality. I n ] . in anxiety ratings between any two adjacent hierarchy items. Stracbey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition of the complete For the third step, the therapist (starting at the bottom of the psychologicd works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 18, pp. 146-211). Lon- hierarchy) guides the progressive association of each hierarchy don: Hogartb. (Original work publisbed 1920) item with relaxation, until the client is able to remain relaxed Freud, S. (1961c). Some psycbological consequences of tbe anatomi- in response to each item. The therapist may accomplish this cal distinction between tbe sexes. In]. Stracbey (Ed. and Trans.), association by asking the client to imagine hierarchy items The standard edition of the complete psychologicd works of Sigmund (traditional SD) or by actually presenting the feared items (in Freud (Vol. 19, pp. 243-278). London: Hogarth. (Original work vivo SD). Therapists sometimes combine traditional SD and published 1925) in vivo SD, using the former technique first. ]ung, C. G. (1953). Anima and animus. InH. Read, M. Fordbam, &. G. Adler (Eds.), Collected works (Vol. 7, pp. 186-209). New York: Although Balch (1983) acknowledged the need for more Pantheon. (Original work published 1917) creative ways of teaching therapeutic techniques, few authors ]ung, C. G. (1961). Tbe tbeory of psychoanalysis: The Oedipus have discussed methods for teaching SD. Sprecher and complex. In H. Read, M. Fordbam, &. G. Adler (Eds.), Collected Worthington (1982) tried having general psychology students works (Vol.4, 2nd ed., pp. 151-156). New York: Pantheon. (Origi- experience SD for seven 30 min sessions. However, they nal work publisbed 1912) concluded tbat this teaching method was too lengthy and Leong, F. T. L., &. Pointer, M. A. (1991). Tbe representation of produced few therapeutic benefits. counseling versus clinical psycbology in introductory psycbology We present a technique for demonstrating in vivo SD in textbooks. Teaching o/Ps^ic/iologji, 18, 12-16. an engaging, humorous, and informative manner that uses Linton, S. (1992). Analysis of information about television in devel- opmental psycbology textbooks. Teaching of Psychohgy, 19,82-85. relatively little class time. Our technique involves brief role Prytula, R. E., Oster, G. D., & Davis, S. F. (1977). Tbe "rat rabbit" playing similar to that recommended by Balch (1983) for problem: What did ]obn B. Watson really do? Teaching of Psychol- teaching client-centered therapy (see also Low, 1996). ogy, 4, 44-46. Quereshi, M. Y. (1993). Tbe contents of introductory psychology Nature of the Demonstration books: A follow-up. Teaching of Psychology, 20, 218-222.
The second author, Michael, conducted the demonstra-
Note tion in three sections of general psychology. Prior to the demonstration, he explained the three steps of SD (see the Correspondence conceming tbis article should be sent to Christo- first paragraph for a similar explanation). He explained that pher Kilmartin, Department of Psycbology, Mary Washington Col- he would demonstrate the technique with a student who was lege, Fredericksburg, VA 22401; e-mail: ckilmart@mwc.edu. ostensibly currently in therapy with him because of a phobia of chalkboard erasers. Before the beginning ofthe class period, the course instructor had selected a student who always sat at the back of the room and quietly asked the student to play A Humorous Demonstration of along with the demonstration. In Vivo Systematic Desensitization: To start the demonstration, Michael explained that the The Case of Eraser Phobia student always sat at the back ofthe room because of a phobia of chalkboard erasers. He stated that he had already taught progressive relaxation to the student, worked with the student Timothy J. Lawson to develop an anxiety hierarchy, and desensitized the student Michael Reardon to a photograph of a chalkboard eraser. Michael then showed College of Mount St. Joseph the class the photograph and moved it toward the student to show that the student had indeed been desensitized to it. We present a technique for derrwrutrating in vivo systematic Then, Michael explained that he planned to desensitize the desensidzation in an engaging, humorous, and informative manner. student to items higher in the anxiety hierarchy. He took a It involves role playing the treatment of a student who ostensibly caged eraser out of his briefcase and showed it to the student. has a phobia of chalkboard erasers (hence the reason he or she The toy metal cage was slightly larger than the eraser and was always sits at the back of the room). Students enjoyed the demon- locked with a small toy padlock. As Michael moved closer to stratbn and believed it enhanced their understanding of systematic the student with the caged eraser, the student acted anxious desensitizatbn. and Michael asked the student to take some deep breaths and to invoke the relaxation training. Systematic desensitization (SD) is a common topic covered Once the student was relaxed, Michael removed the eraser in general, abnormal, and clinical or counseling psychology from the cage and showed it to the student until the student courses. Developed by Wolpe (1958), SD involves associating became relaxed once again. He moved the uncaged eraser a relaxed state with graduated exposure to anxiety-provoking closer and closer to the student until,finally,the student could stimuli. SD typically involves three steps. During the first step. touch the eraser without becoming overly anxious. At each