Mapping Mixed-Methods Research: Theories, Models, and Measures
Mapping Mixed-Methods Research: Theories, Models, and Measures
Mapping Mixed-Methods Research: Theories, Models, and Measures
Mapping Mixed-Methods
Research
Theories, Models, and Measures 8
CHAPTER OVERVIEW AND OBJECTIVES
113
311
114 VISUALIZING SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH
part of pre/post mixed-methods designs and will offer a new mixed-methods mea-
sure based on the use of mind maps. To understand these examples, it is important to
understand the theoretical basis for this sort of integration and to know how different
data-collection procedures can be used together. Finally, through the use of a research
example, readers will be encouraged to consider how the use of mixed methods offers
another means to address activities presented in Chapters 2, 3, and 4. By the end of
this chapter, readers should be able to do the following:
THEORETICAL JUSTIFICATION
As we have seen in previous chapters, the existing theoretical bases for quantitative
and qualitative research are rooted in postpositivism and constructivism. To understand
how mixed-methods research provides a different sort of theoretical understand-
ing of research, it may be useful to recall that earlier discussion. Postpositivists see
human knowledge as speculative and, therefore, not based on unchallengeable, rock-
solid foundations. They argue that the external world exists independently of an
individual’s experience of it, and thus knowledge is not hypothetical and foundation-
less. They acknowledge that all research will be incomplete in one way or another,
and they hold that approaches that can be tested and explored through the scientific
method should be favored. This often results in the application of deductive approaches
that rely on a series of steps to reach specific conclusions based on general premises.
In general, quantitative research seeks generalizability through controlled,
value-free (or value-neutral) processes that can test and validate theories through a
process of falsification. The emphasis on falsification often leads quantitative
researchers to focus on sample size and statistics to showcase broad generalizability.
At its most shortsighted, some quantitative research considers the role of setting
and context either irrelevant or unmanageable. A central critique is that some quan-
titative research models are statistics dependent, inflate the importance of mathe-
matical averages, and cannot capture the complexity associated with human
behavior (Goertzel & Fashing, 1981). By focusing solely on numeric information,
some approaches miss the depth and detail that are assigned to phenomena by
participants themselves.
Chapter 5 Mapping Mixed-Methods Research 115
tested by
used to build
that are accepted,
or refine
rejected, or modified
Identification
Social Science
Based on of themes
Reasoning
tests/experiments
that leads to
that lead to
In-depth data
More specifc can involve collection
conclusions
explored through
Observations of
Inductive begins
specific cases assumed
Reasoning with
to be RELEVANT
Chapter 5 Mapping Mixed-Methods Research 117
Abductive Reasoning
uses
both
Expertise/Intuition
of researchers
which both
For example, students are often overcome by the nature of quantitative information
collected within some data sets and the view that, to be valid, quantitative research
requires a large number of cases to analyze. As discussed in Chapter 3, this is because
of the assumptions required by certain statistical tests often used in the analysis of
numeric information. On the other hand, whereas qualitative research can require
smaller samples and thus may be easier for students to engage in, many are uncertain
about how to identify a good group from which to gather data or are unclear about
the interview process and how to prepare. Mixed methods may require more work,
multiple analyses, and nuanced thinking; however, they also can provide flexibility for
researchers. Miles and Huberman (2002) urge all researchers to entertain mixed mod-
els. By avoiding polarization, polemics, and life at the extremes, they suggested that
both quantitative and qualitative inquiry can support and inform each other in
important ways. Narratives and variable-driven analyses need to interpenetrate
and inform each other. Realists, idealists and critical theorists can do better by
incorporating other ideas than remaining pure. (Miles & Huberman, 2002, p. 396)
Mixed-methods research has been defined by Creswell and Plano Clark (2007, p. 5)
as a research design based on assumptions that guide the collection and analysis of
data and the mixture of qualitative and quantitative approaches. A central premise
is that the use of quantitative and qualitative approaches together can provide a
better understanding of research problems. Mixed methodologies can provide a
useful and novel way to communicate meaning and knowledge ( Johnson &
Onwuegbuzie, 2004) because they can combine the reliability of counts with the
validity of lived experience and perception. Mixed approaches to social science
research are increasingly popular. Tashakkori and Teddlie (1998) included 152 refer-
ences in their exploration of the growth of mixed methods in research areas such as
evaluation, health science and nursing, psychology, sociology, and education,
among others.
As mixed-methods research has grown during the past two decades, different
approaches to mixed-methods designs have been developed (Greene, Caracelli, &
Graham, 1989), revised (Creswell & Plano Clark, 2007), and reorganized (Teddlie &
Tashakkori, 2009). As discussed in Chapter 1, a variety of types and approaches of
mixed-methods research have been defined (Creswell & Plano Clark, 2007). One
approach is to use qualitative techniques to develop a theory that can then be tested
by establishing a conceptually connected hypothesis and quantitative means. Figure
5.3 provides an example.
Research Identification
Leads to Data collection that guides
question(s) of themes
used to
Developed Development
Test hypothesis used to
theory of measures
120 VISUALIZING SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH
allows
and development of new
New data collection or
Validation or reanalysis of existing data Hypothesis
skeptism of results testing
Another example of interest is a study by Myers and Oetzel (2003) that used
qualitative data to create and validate a quantitative instrument. This study was
also organized through two phases of data collection. Based on qualitative inter-
views, the authors first gathered data through field notes and transcripts. Later
they engaged in analysis using techniques drawn from qualitative data including
coding, theme identification, and connection to existing literature. Based on this
analysis, the authors developed an instrument that could provide quantitative mea-
sures based on the qualitative interviews. They then administered this instrument,
and the quantitative data were analyzed to test correlations from the qualitative
interviews.
However, data collection and data analysis may not always be so closely inter-
twined. There may be times that data collected simultaneously are analyzed sepa-
rately, in different ways, and at various times. Other studies might collect data
through multiple data-collection phases over longer time periods. Although collect-
ing data in multiple settings may be useful, there may be research designs in which
data can be usefully compiled and analyzed together and at the same time. Thus,
there is an important difference between descriptive and analytic timing/ordering
considerations (Creswell & Plano Clark, 2007). Descriptive considerations focus on
whether data were collected at the same time or over a longer period of time.
Analytic considerations focus on whether the data were analyzed together, at the
same time, or separately, one after another. Whereas both may require some justifi-
cation, they ought not be confused. Figure 5.5 provides a visual overview of some of
these considerations.
The second question is related to how you weight different methods in your
study, or the relative importance of each approach. This is often indicated using
capital letters for the dominant approach (QUAN or QUAL) and lowercase letters
for the secondary, less dominant methodological approach (qual or quan). Of
course, you may choose to give equal weight to both traditions, in which case both
would be capitalized (QUAL/QUAN). More often one tradition is selected as domi-
nant. Whether your approach is primarily quantitative or qualitative in nature
depends to a large degree on the type of research question you are interested in.
Both approaches have strengths and weaknesses, of course, but thinking about
how and why some methods might work together better than others is important.
Some researchers have gathered data through quantitative surveys and qualitative
interviews (Baumann, 1999; Way, Stauber, Nakkula, & London, 1994). This allows
researchers to define beforehand the kind of data they seek by utilizing specific
data-collection tools. In essence this question boils down to whether you will
assign equal or unequal weight to the different sorts of data you have collected
and whether your analysis emphasizes quantitative or qualitative assumptions
about meaning. Your decision about how to weight data may also be related to the
122 VISUALIZING SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH
Data Collection/Analysis
TIMING
How many
instruments/operations How many stages?
Over what period?
will you use?
1 for QUAN
2 for QUAL
1 with 2 or more Months Years
different unique
sections instruments
How will you justify
your choice?
Symbol Meaning
EXERCISE 5.1
Think You Get It?
These notations can help researchers present their approaches and think about
their designs. However, simply noting which design they have chosen, whether a
quantitative or qualitative approach will be dominant, or how their data will be mixed
is not enough. Central to any research, and perhaps especially to mixed-methods
research, is how researchers justify their approach. This is especially important with
regard to the question of mixing. There are at least three options available when
deciding how and why to mix your data. Data can be merged by transforming and/or
integrating two data types together, one data type can be embedded within another,
or they can be presented separately and then connected to answer different aspects
of the same or a similar research question. Creswell and Plano Clark (2007, p. 80) have
compiled a useful decision tree that provides an overview of a number of relevant
mixed-methods concerns. Building on their work, Figure 5.6 provides some examples
of how data might be mixed.
124 VISUALIZING SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH
Combine
during Merge and Embed Connect
data integrate the data the data
analysis the data
Focus
on QUAN
or QUAL? Must Must
Must
But what about mixed-methods approaches that seek to integrate data analysis in
a more interactive way? Teddlie and Tashakkori (2009, pp. 280–281) presented a study
by Jang, McDougall, Pollon, Herbert, and Russell (2008) that analyzed both QUAN and
QUAL data independently and then attempted more integrative analysis by presenting
both QUAN and QUAL to participants for feedback. By transforming QUAN factors into
QUAL themes, and vice versa ( for comparison), they consolidated the themes and fac-
tors that emerged through both analyses and used QUAL data to provide nuance to the
consolidated themes/factors. This is perhaps more complex than is practical to con-
sider at this point; however, that example points to one of the major strengths of
mixed-methods data. By providing multiple options, researchers can experiment with
different analysis strategies and, provided they justify their approach, can offer valu-
able new approaches, methods, and even measures. The mind map research example
in this chapter provides perhaps a more simplistic example of how different sorts of
data can be integrated and combined in a novel and potentially useful way.
Chapter 5 Mapping Mixed-Methods Research 125
Based on research by Wheeldon (2010b), this example shows how maps can offer a
unique way for research participants to represent their experiences while assisting
researchers to make better sense of gathered data. Maps can be used both in established
pre/post designs and in the construction of unique and novel mixed-methods measures
constructed by assigning weights to different data-collection stages. Do you agree with
the notion that data can be weighted in this way? On what assumptions is it based?
As discussed in Chapters 2 and 3, concept maps are most commonly used in quantita-
tive research. This may be because earlier versions of concept maps were used to explore
science education (Stewart, Van Kirk, & Rowell, 1979) and were often quantitatively
scored by an expert to assess how understanding was demonstrated through the struc-
ture of the map itself. A focus on structure remains an integral feature for many concept
map researchers (Novak & Cañas, 2008) because structured maps can be consistently
Chapter 5 Mapping Mixed-Methods Research 127
Data were collected during a 16-month interval from a student’s first preinternship
class to his or her final class following a criminal justice internship. The first stage of
data analysis was based on the qualitative data collected through the surveys and
focus groups. The open-ended survey and focus groups allowed students to provide
their views on the importance of ethics to their placements and the value of the dif-
ferent approaches, exercises, and scenarios used to teach ethical decision making
during the preinternship course (Wheeldon, 2008). This provided more nuance and
context to the quantified differences expressed in the maps. The survey questions of
interest are outlined in Table 5.3.
Number Question
1 How important are one’s ethics and values to a career in criminal
justice?
2 How well did ADJ 479 assist you to consider where your values
and ethics come from?
3 How useful were the exercises and discussions to assist you to
identify and address ethical dilemmas?
4 List any scenarios you recall from class that were useful in
exploring values, ethics, and criminal justice.
5 Anything you would like to add?
home where a youth was to be taken to a juvenile facility jail. For these students the
value of ethics instruction was very personal. They suggested the experience of
thinking through the ethical dilemmas prepared them because they said they “knew
themselves a bit better” as a result.
To test the extent to which the preinternship class assisted students to consider and
reflect on their values, the pre/post concept maps were quantitatively assessed. As
you may recall, students were asked to complete concept maps during the first prein-
ternship class based on the general instructions to identify both important values and
ethics and their origin(s). These maps demonstrated how, beginning with themselves,
participants could provide what they believed to be core values and connect them
with lines to where they believed these values originated. They were provided an
exemplar map for how their maps should be constructed as well as basic instructions
about which sorts of concepts might be included (e.g., honest, hardworking) and
where these concepts may have originated (e.g., parents, religion, school). Each stu-
dent was asked to complete another concept map using the same instructions and
exemplars near the end of the course.
If the qualitative data are to be believed, we ought to be able to see a change
in student concept maps before and after the course. To test this idea the premaps
and postmaps were quantitatively assessed, and values and ethics identified in
the maps and their perceived origins before and after the preinternship class were
compared. In this case, the null hypothesis is that there would be no difference
between the means of the premaps and postmaps. The research hypothesis was
that the maps completed after the course would contain more concepts and would
be constructed in more complex ways. To test this hypothesis, all relevant data for
each student were compiled into an Excel table. Based on this process, a descriptive
analysis was made possible that included the values in the maps and data about
from whom, or from where, students suggested they had originated. Values in the
premaps and postmaps were first compared in a table, as presented in Figures 5.7
and 5.8 below.
As you can see, truth and loyalty remained important for these students through-
out the course, but compassion was identified more often in the postmaps, with
open-mindedness identified for the first time in the postmaps. The use of traditional
tables is common, but another approach is based on a computer program called
Wordle (Feinberg, 2010). This online program is free for all, is easy to use, and pro-
vides another means to visualize which values were important. To create Figures 5.5
and 5.6, one can simply copy the text into the Create box at www.wordle.net. The more
Chapter 5 Mapping Mixed-Methods Research 131
Respect
Compassion
Loyalty
Truth
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35
Open-Mindedness
Compassion
Loyalty
Truth
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35
words you type, the more placement of the text changes, and the size of an individual
word depends on the number of times you enter the word into the Create box. The
resultant “wordle” is another way to visualize data. Figures 5.9 and 5.10 show the most
common values in the student pre- and postmaps.
132 VISUALIZING SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH
In addition, the student maps provided data about where these values origi-
nated. As Figure 5.11 presents, these changed pre- and postcourse.
As discussed above the value of using maps is that they can provide both narra-
tive and numeric data. Through a comparison of the pre- and postmaps, a number
of interesting narrative observations can be made. The values of honesty and loyalty
remained important for students both before and after the course; compassion as a
value of importance was identified more often postcourse, and open-mindedness
was identified for the first time postcourse. In terms of value origins, family, friends,
school, and religion all remain core sites of value origin. Postcourse, however, school
was identified more often. In addition to this descriptive information, the pre- and
postmaps also provided numeric data. The maps were scored based on the number
of concepts and the maps’ complexity, as outlined in Figures 5.12 and 5.13. In this
study, a complexity score was calculated based on one point for each unique con-
cept and five points for maps that included two or more connections between values
and origins.
To assess the significance of the changes in the pre- and postmaps, we can
return to our familiar friend: the dependent t test. As discussed in Chapter 3, this is
a very useful tool when we are comparing pre/post data from the same people. By
compiling the mean number of concepts in the premaps and the postmaps, and the
mean complexity of the pre- and postmaps, you might get something that looks like
Table 5.4.
School/Teachers
Religion
Family
−5 5 15 25 35 45
Pre Post
134 VISUALIZING SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH
Parents
Scoring
Unique Concepts—6
Hierarchies/Levels—0
Hard work
Complexity Score—6
Honesty ME
Responsibility Trust
Family Religion
Scoring
Religion Unique Concepts—11
Hierarchies/Levels—5
Complexity—16
Chapter 5 Mapping Mixed-Methods Research 135
In this example, of interest were the types of ethical instruction identified by students
based on the three approaches to this training provided during the preinternship
class. This involved a qualitative analysis of student surveys and focus groups that
suggested that approaches to ethical instruction should not be “too easy” and not shy
away from the “real-world complexity of ethics.” Some common themes were that
ethical instruction needed to provide (a) a means for students to understand their
own values and (b) opportunities to identify and address ethical dilemmas. Examples
drawn from a text that combined specific real-work situations with a step-by-step
approach to identifying the dilemmas and possible solutions were identified as useful
by students (Wheeldon, 2008). Yet not all students saw the preinternship course as
valuable, and as some suggested in the focus groups, ethics in the classroom and
ethics in the real world were two different things.
These qualitative findings led to the second, more general research question
designed to better understand the role of the preinternship class. The pre/post con-
cept maps were used to validate the hypothesis that exposure to ethical dilemmas
would influence how students represented their ethics and values and understood
their origins. Overall, the qualitative data suggested that students saw ethical deci-
sion making as very important in the justice system and that the instruction was most
136 VISUALIZING SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH
useful when it provided them with an opportunity to work in groups to identify ethi-
cal dilemmas and analyze different approaches to resolving them. Although the pre/
post concept maps could not be used to corroborate all the qualitative data, they did
validate the general notion that the course was useful in assisting students to reflect
on their values and ethics and provided some additional hypotheses that could be
tested in subsequent studies. This analysis strategy is represented in Figure 5.14.
Although this pilot study has since been built on and more data have been col-
lected and analyzed from the sample, it provides a useful example to consider how
maps can be used in mixed-methods designs and how to think about the timing,
weighting, and mixing of the data. Nevertheless, a number of limitations should be
noted. These include the size of the sample, the limited geographic location of the
students, and the failure to capture other kinds of demographic data such as ethnicity,
income level, and previous criminal justice employment. Another issue refers to how
the data from the maps and data drawn from surveys were combined and compiled.
In this example the qualitative findings were tested quantitatively. Yet the quantita-
tive analysis did not consider all of the qualitative data that emerged from the surveys
explored using
that may lead
to future
Survey responses
Qualitative analysis of
and focus groups
Value of classroom instruction
to influence student reflection
Hybrid which
philosophical/ including found
validated practical importance of
could not be approaches
tested through
Quantitative analysis of Value in
pre/post concept maps ethical instruction
and focus groups. Thus, we can say the pre-/postmaps suggested the course assisted
students to provide a more detailed account and understanding of their values; how-
ever, they did not (and could not) validate the survey data that suggested which types
of ethical instruction were best. The choice to focus principally on qualitative data
collection might be seen as a limitation.
Another approach might have tried to find new ways to combine the map data
and survey results by individual students. In addition, by having students complete yet
another concept map on how best to teach ethics, these data might have suggested
how changes in values orientation were specifically connected to the style of ethical
instruction favored by each student. Another concern in this example might be the
assumption that concept count/complexity measures are useful proxies for knowl-
edge transfer. This has not yet been fully demonstrated. Although there is research on the
value of concept maps in education, science, and nursing, their application and the
validity of different approaches in criminal justice is still emerging (Wheeldon, 2010b).
Like in the example above, the process of data collection and analysis here also
involved a number of steps and stages. In the first stage of data collection, partici-
pants were asked to complete mind maps about their experience of a legal technical
assistance project. Participants were provided with an exemplar map and encouraged
to make their own as reflective of their experiences during the project as possible.
One map adapted from the maps that were returned is presented in Figure 5.15.
In the second stage of data collection, participants were asked general interview
questions. Listed in Table 5.5, these general questions were open ended and probed
positive and negative experiences, perceived results and challenges, and previously
indentified concepts, gathered through a literature review.
In addition to the general questions, conclusionary and more reflective open-
ended questions followed the more directive data-collection stages. By providing
participants an opportunity to identify areas not previously addressed, the researcher
Chapter 5 Mapping Mixed-Methods Research 139
Became
Friends
Probation Pilot
Directors Study Tour to
Shared Canada
Expertise
Question
Number Question Text
3 What if anything did you learn through the mind map exercise?
4 How important was the role of the translator/translation within the training sessions?
(Continued)
140 VISUALIZING SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH
Table 5.5 (Continued)
Question
Number Question Text
6 What would you say was the biggest result of Latvian-Canadian cooperation?
7 What would you say was the biggest challenge of Latvian-Canadian cooperation?
8 Was working with Canadians different than working with other international experts?
Source: Wheeldon (2010b).
9 If you could change one thing about Canada’s involvement with Latvia, what would it be?
10 Anything else you’d like to add?
hoped they would reflect on their experience as whole, restating aspects of particular
significance, or provide additional clarifying commentary. By combining the maps
with the different stages of follow-up interviews, the frequency with which individual
variables were identified through the multiple data-collection stages was recorded.
To analyze the interview data in a more meaningful way, a mixed-methods mea-
sure called a “salience score” was developed (Wheeldon, 2010b). The construction of a
mixed-methods salience score may involve a number of separate yet rather simple
operations. In the first step, unique, individual concepts, elements, and activities iden-
tified by participants in different stages of data collection can be recorded as variables.
Individual variables might be identified in mind maps, through general or specific
interviews, or in summative and reflective statements. They also may be identified in
one, multiple, or all stages of data collection. These variables can then be quantified
through the use of a concept-counting technique that records the frequency or pres-
ence of individual variables throughout data collection. Table 5.6 lists some of the
variables identified through the study.
The number of times a variable was identified in total across the data-collection
stages and the number of times each participant identified a variable across multiple
data-collection stages were interesting, but these sorts of frequency measures can
provide only a sense of whether, and how often, these variables were identified. An
important assumption in this study was that the way in which the variables were
identified might more usefully demonstrate the relevance or legitimacy of a proposed
association (Cash et al., 2002).
For each variable identified in multiple stages of data collection, a salience score
or weighted measure was developed using a weighted count system (Stillwell,
Winterfeldt, & John, 1987). This strategy allows the researcher to assign participants
a score for each individual variable they identify depending on the stage(s) at which
these variables were recorded. For example, individual variables that emerge from
Chapter 5 Mapping Mixed-Methods Research 141
A final step involved validating the salience score by considering whether differ-
ences between groups within the sample had skewed the findings. Differences
between groups can mean that what you thought were generalizable findings are
instead the results of strongly held views within one or more groups. In this example
there were three groupings of interest. These included male and female, participants
from Riga and outside Riga, and headquarters staff and probation officers. There were
mean differences between the groups within the sample; through t tests (adjusted for
undertaking multiple tests), these differences were found to be statistically insignifi-
cant in all instances. This means that the findings that made up the salience score can
be attributed to the group as a whole.
As we saw above, the data were collected sequentially and weighted in such a way
as to privilege data collected through the mind maps and open-ended interview
Chapter 5 Mapping Mixed-Methods Research 143
This study developed an approach that allowed for the numeric salience score to help
present and organize qualitative findings about which elements of the training methods
and approaches were most useful. By mixing methods in this way, the research not only
presented a sense of what worked but provided some context and nuance about why
and how. The participants also noted the utility of the maps. Virtually all participants
identified the maps as a “useful way to see experience.” Some suggested this was
because making a map “helped them to remember events from years ago” and “organize
their thoughts about the experience systematically.” Others suggested that as visual
aids, maps helped put the experience in “context,” provided a “clearer view” by allowing
them to look at events again and realize how much had happened, and helped them to
“focus on the key experiences, concepts and connections.” For these participants, there
was value in visualizing their experiences and organizing their thoughts through maps.
Although the data collected in this study have been analyzed in a variety of ways
(Wheeldon, 2010a, 2011), they also provide a useful example to consider another way
maps can be used in terms of the timing, weighting, and mixing of data. Using mind
144 VISUALIZING SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH
maps in this way allows researchers to embrace quantitative measures that use qualitative
assumptions about which sorts of data are valuable and how they might be privileged.
The mixed measure should be built on and revised, but it represents a unique way to
combine quantitative and qualitative data as presented in Figure 5.16.
Some limitations with this study include the sample size and the choices made
within the method and analysis strategy. The development of a mixed-methods mea-
sure called the salience score usefully combines elements from both the quantitative
and the qualitative traditions; however, it remains untested and only a first draft of
sorts. By privileging more user-generated data-collection stages by assigning more
weight to the variables that emerge through these stages, the mixed-methods
measure combined the “clarity of counts, with the nuance qualitative reflection can
provide” (Wheeldon, 2010b, p. 87). Yet its novelty is an inherent limitation. There are
few studies that have attempted to weight data in this way, and more study is needed
to understand the value of a mixed-methods measure. One useful approach for others
testing this measure would be to develop an additional validation process in which
focus groups made up of a study’s participants could validate the main findings. In
this way, one could test whether the main findings that emerged through the score
Mixed-Methods Measure
can combine
by
Quantitative Qualitative weighting
indicators assumptions concepts that
emerge from
estimated as
Structured/ Free form/
justified as closed format open ended
data collection data collection
Reliable LOWER HIGHER
and Valid
must be
were seen as important by focus groups representative of the total sample. These
sorts of validation exercises can allow the findings to be reviewed by the participants
themselves through a more participatory approach toward the research process itself.
STUDENT ACTIVITY
Review the student activities in Chapters 2, 3, and 4. Consider how adding another
method to either of these activities can assist you to better understand the issue
under investigation. In Chapter 2, your class might have considered students’ ability
to recall key concepts and their relationships based on a lecture using concept maps.
In contrast, your class might have used mind maps to consider student perceptions of
the value of the material presented on that day. How might a mixed approach give you
more data from which to draw conclusions? Imagine each person in your class com-
pleted a mind map about the perceived value of that week’s lesson at the beginning of
class, based on that week’s readings. Now imagine that following the lesson, each
person completed a concept map in which he or she was to connect concepts and
propositions based on the lesson. Generate some hypotheses about what you might
see if you were to compare an individual’s prelesson interest level with his or her
postlesson understanding. What might this approach to student comparison miss?
How might you address this limitation?
Based on Chapter 2’s activity and the analysis presented in Chapter 3, how could
concept maps be used to explore how students learned concepts presented in a weekly
lesson? What additional information might be useful to gather? How could questions
to students about the most difficult concepts, propositions, or connections assist them
to reflect on their own learning and allow for teachers to better understand student
difficulties? How might you combine different sorts of data based on the timing,
weighting, and mixing considerations described above? Based on Chapter 4’s activity
and the analysis presented in Chapter 5, how could mind maps and interviews be
scored to assess their description of key people or events in their lives? How might the
different approaches to data gathering influence how you might score the data col-
lected in each? Are there common ideas that continually emerge? What additional
information might be useful to gather? How does this attempt to quantify qualitative
data assist your understanding, and to what extent do the numbers in your scoring
system connect to your experience interviewing your participant?
CONCLUSION
and qualitative approaches alongside their associated data analysis strategies, mixed
methods provide a means to gain a better understanding of phenomena under inves-
tigation. As visual records of understanding, concept maps and mind maps may be
important tools in this regard because the data that are represented through their
construction can be assessed both quantitatively and qualitatively.
This chapter has provided both a theoretical justification for the use of concept
maps and mind maps in mixed-methods research and some examples of how maps
might be used in this way. Pre/post concept maps offer one way to investigate how
views have quantitatively changed over time and suggest a means to explore in more
detail some of the reasons why using qualitative techniques makes sense. The mixed-
methods measure is a unique way to consider how data gathered through multiple
stages of data collection can be compiled. This single measure explicitly values data
collected through more unsolicited means while at the same time ensuring the
reliability of counts is respected.
REVIEW
1. Define mixed-methods research, and explain the assumptions about knowledge on which it is
based. How is it different from quantitative and qualitative research?
2. What are three ways mixed-methods studies have been undertaken in the past?
3. Why might concept maps and mind maps be useful for mixed-methods research?
4. How can pre/post concept maps be used with other kinds of methods?
5. What is a mixed-methods measure? How was it first constructed, and how might it be improved?
Creswell, J., & Plano Clark, V. (2007). Designing and conducting mixed methods research. London:
Sage.
Morgan, D. L. (2007). Paradigms lost and pragmatism regained: Methodological implications of
combining qualitative and quantitative methods. Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 1(1),
48–76.
Teddlie, C. B., & Tashakkori, A. (2009). Foundations of mixed methods research: Integrating quan-
titative and qualitative approaches in the social and behavioral sciences. Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Wheeldon, J. P. (2010). Mapping mixed methods research: Methods, measures, and meaning.
Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 4(2), 87–102.
Chapter 5 Mapping Mixed-Methods Research 147
REFERENCES
Aldridge, J. M., Fraser, B. J., & Huang, T. (1999). Investigating classroom environments in Taiwan
and Australia with multiple research methods. Journal of Educational Research, 93, 48–57.
Baumann, C. (1999). Adoptive fathers and birthfathers: A study of attitudes. Child and
Adolescent Social Work Journal, 5(16), 373–391.
Cash, D., Clark, W. C., Alcock, F., Dickson, N., Eckley, N., & Jäger, J. (2002). Credibility, legitimacy
and boundaries: Linking, assessment and decision making. Cambridge, MA: Kennedy
School of Government.
Cederblom, J., & Spohn, C. (1991). A model for teaching criminal justice ethics. Journal of
Criminal Justice Education, 2, 201–218.
Creswell, J., & Plano Clark, V. (2007). Designing and conducting mixed methods research. London:
Sage.
Feinberg, J. (2010). Wordle. Retrieved July 28, 2010, from http://www.wordle.net/
Goertzel, T., & Fashing, J. (1981). The myth of the normal curve: A theoretical critique and
examination of its role in teaching and research. Humanity and Society, 5, 14–31.
Gogolin, L., & Swartz, F. (1992). A quantitative and qualitative inquiry into the attitudes toward
science of nonscience college students. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 29, 487–504.
Greene, J. C., & Caracelli, V. J. (1997). Defining and describing the paradigm issue in mixed-
method evaluation. In J. C. Greene & V. J. Caracelli (Eds.), Advances in mixed-method
evaluation: The challenges and benefits of integrating diverse paradigms (New Directions
for Program Evaluation, No. 74, pp. 5–17). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Greene, J. C., Caracelli, V. J., & Graham, W. F. (1989). Toward a conceptual framework for mixed-
method evaluation design. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 11(3), 255–274.
Guba, E., & Lincoln, Y. (1989). Fourth generation evaluation. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Jang, E. E., McDougall, D. E., Pollon, D., Herbert, M., & Russell, P. (2008). Integrative mixed meth-
ods data analysis strategies in research on school success in challenging circumstances.
Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 2(3), 221–247.
Jenkins, J. E. (2001). Rural adolescent perceptions of alcohol and other drug resistance. Child
Study Journal, 31(4), 211–224.
Jick, T. D. (1979). Mixing qualitative and quantitative methods: Triangulation in action.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 24, 602–611.
Johnson, R. B., & Onwuegbuzie, A. (2004). Mixed methods research: A research paradigm whose
time has come. Educational Researcher, 33(7), 14–26.
Kaplan, A. (1964). The conduct of inquiry: Methodology for behavioral science. San Francisco:
Chandler Press.
Kilic, Z., Kaya, O. N., & Dogan, A. (2004, August 3–8). Effects of students’ pre- and post-laboratory
concept maps on students’ attitudes toward chemistry laboratory in university general chem-
istry. Paper presented at the International Conference on Chemical Education, Istanbul,
Turkey.
Miles, M., & Huberman, M. (2002). Reflections and advice. In M. Huberman & M. Miles (Eds.),
The qualitative researcher’s companion (pp. 393–397). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Morgan, D. L. (2007). Paradigms lost and pragmatism regained: Methodological implications
of combining qualitative and quantitative methods. Journal of Mixed Methods Research,
1(1), 48–76.
148 VISUALIZING SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH