Documentation
Documentation
Documentation
Environmental Research
and Public Health
Article
Family Function and Child Adjustment Difficulties in the
COVID-19 Pandemic: An International Study
Sarah Foley 1, *, Farzaneh Badinlou 2,3 , Karin C. Brocki 2 , Matilda A. Frick 2 , Luca Ronchi 4 and Claire Hughes 5,†
1 Moray House School of Education and Sport, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH8 8AQ, UK
2 Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, 751 42 Uppsala, Sweden;
farzaneh.badinlou@psyk.uu.se (F.B.); Karin.Brocki@psyk.uu.se (K.C.B.); matilda.frick@psyk.uu.se (M.A.F.)
3 Centre for Psychiatry Research, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet,
117 63 Stockholm, Sweden
4 Department of Brain and Behavioural Sciences, University of Pavia, 27100 Pavia, Italy; luca.ronchi@unipv.it
5 Centre for Family Research, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3RQ, UK; ch288@cam.ac.uk
* Correspondence: sarah.foley@ed.ac.uk
† i-FAM COVID-19 Consortium.
Abstract: To estimate specific proximal and distal effects of COVID-19-related restrictions on families
on children’s adjustment problems, we conducted a six-site international study. In total, 2516 parents
from Australia, China, Italy, Sweden, the United Kingdom, and the United States of America living
with a young child (Mage = 5.77, SD = 1.10, range = 3 to 8 years, 47.9% female) completed an
online survey between April and July 2020. The survey included the Strengths and Difficulties
Questionnaire and family risk factors (parent distress, parent–child conflict, couple conflict, and
household chaos) as well as a scale to index COVID-19-related family disruption. Our analyses also
Citation: Foley, S.; Badinlou, F.; included public data on the stringency of national restrictions. Across the six sites, parental responses
Brocki, K.C.; Frick, M.A.; Ronchi, L.; indicated elevated levels of hyperactivity, conduct, and emotion problems in children from families
Hughes, C. Family Function and
characterized by heightened levels of parent distress, parent–child conflict, and household chaos. In
Child Adjustment Difficulties in the
contrast, increased peer problems were more strongly related to COVID-19-related social disruption
COVID-19 Pandemic: An
and stringency measures. Mediation models demonstrated that associations between COVID-19
International Study. Int. J. Environ.
social disruption and child difficulties could be explained by parental distress. Taken together, these
Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11136.
https://doi.org/10.3390/
results suggest that although the experience of the pandemic differed across countries, associations
ijerph182111136 between COVID-19-related family experiences and child adjustment difficulties were similar in their
nature and magnitude across six different contexts. Programs to support family resilience could help
Academic Editor: Paul B. Tchounwou buffer the impact of the pandemic for two generations.
Received: 20 September 2021 Keywords: COVID-19; child adjustment; family; risk; distress; international
Accepted: 20 October 2021
Published: 23 October 2021
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11136. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182111136 https://www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11136 2 of 14
understanding health risks may amplify the stress associated with changes and restrictions,
for example, “stay-at-home” rules and mass school closures.
or distinct family and COVID-19 experiences are associated with individual differences in
children’s adjustment difficulties across these six different countries.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants and Procedure
Between 1 April and 7 July 2020, we recruited 2516 parents with one or more children
aged 3 to 8 years old (Mage = 5.77, SD = 1.10) to participate in an online survey (via
Qualtrics) of family wellbeing and child adjustment in the COVID-19 pandemic. To be
eligible to participate, parents had to be aged 18 or above, report no major psychiatric
problems or learning difficulties, and have one or more child aged 4–6. Parents were asked
to choose one child in the relevant age group when completing the survey. Following
ethical review in each site (and translation from English), the survey was advertised widely
via social media and university/school mailing lists in Australia, China, Italy, Sweden, the
United Kingdom (UK), and the United States of America (USA).
In total, 6.4% of respondents were in Australia (n = 161), 13.4% were in China (n = 336),
9.7% were in Italy (n = 244), 31.6% were in Sweden (n = 795), 28.1% were in the UK
(n = 706), and 10.9% were in the USA (n = 274). As described in Table 1, respondents were
typically female (81.5% female and 8% male, with 10.5% preferring not to say), aged 21 to
65 years old (Mage = 37.34, SD = 5.39 years), and highly educated (63.1% undergraduate or
higher degree or equivalent vocational qualification). Only 50% reported their ethnicity,
of which 50% identified as white, 29% as Asian, and 21% as having mixed or multiple
ethnicities. Between-site contrasts for each of these demographic characteristics were weak
(see Table 1); however, these subsamples are not considered to be representative of all
parents and children in each site, in particular those from low socioeconomic backgrounds.
Between-Site
Australia China Italy Sweden UK USA
Overall Differences
n = 161 n = 336 n = 244 n = 795 n = 706 n = 274
Effect Size
Parent
Age M (SD) 37.15 (5.93) 38.89 (5.55) 34.98 (5.75) 39.32 (4.78) 36.63 (4.85) 37.43 (6.92) 37.70 (6.48) η2 = 0.04
Education % a
63.1% 73.9% 19% 59.4% 69.8% 67.4% 83.2% Cramer’s V = 0.11
degree
Child
Age
5.77 (1.10) 6.09 (1.08) 5.34 (0.99) 5.83 (1.13) 5.65 (1.11) 6.06 (1.05) 5.68 (1.08) η2 = 0.05
M (SD)
Gender
47.9% 50.3% 48.2% 51.2% 47.9% 46% 47.8% Cramer’s V = 0.04
% female
Family
No. of children
2.02 (1.01) 2.09 (0.78) 1.70 (1.73) 1.72 (0.74) 2.22 (0.84) 2.04 (0.85) 2.00 (0.81) η2 = 0.04
M (SD)
Note. degree = undergraduate degree or higher and/or equivalent vocational qualification. a = 75% missing.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11136 5 of 14
2.2. Measures
Child Adjustment
Participants completed the parents’ version of the Strengths and Difficulties Question-
naire (SDQ), which is used globally to screen for children’s social, emotional, and behavioral
problems [29]. The SDQ consists of 25 statements, each rated on a three-point scale (not
true, somewhat true, and certainly true), with five items yielding a prosociality subscale
and 20 items relating to child difficulties, namely hyperactivity and emotional, peer, and
conduct problems [29,30]. Multiple group confirmatory factor analysis confirmed that a
5-factor solution was the best fitting model and showed across-site scalar invariance [16].
For the purposes of this study, we focused on the four child difficulties subscales.
Household Chaos. Each of the 15 items in the Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale
(CHAOS) [32] had a 4-point scale enabling respondents to indicate how strongly they
agreed with statements about the current level of noise, crowding, and routines in their
home (1 = very much like our home, 2 = often like our home, 3 = a bit like our home, and
4 = not at all like our home). A mean score was created, with higher scores indicating a
more chaotic household (α = 0.75).
Demographics. Parents also provided information on their child’s age and gender as
well as indicators of their socioeconomic status (SES). A SES composite was developed
comprising measures of the parent(s) education, occupation, and housing. Specifically,
using a scale relevant to their own country, respondents reported on their own educational
attainment and, if applicable, the educational attainment of the target child’s other primary
caregiver. These scores were then recoded to reflect the closest equivalent in the British
system (i.e., high school/secondary, post-secondary, undergraduate, or higher degree).
Next, respondents reported on their own occupation, which was coded according to the
UK Standard Occupation Classification system [33]. Occupations were coded as low-level
(e.g., sales and factory work), mid-level (e.g., skilled-trade and services) and high-level
(e.g., managers and professionals). Finally, respondents were asked to report on their
household, specifically the number of bedrooms in their home and whether their home
was “small and cramped”, “small but adequate”, “quite spacious”, or “very spacious”. A
z-score was created for each item. The final aggregate reflected a mean of z-scores from
four to six items, whereby a high score reflected higher SES (α = 0.67).
Table 2. Robust maximum likelihood estimates for correlations between main study measures.
1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11.
1. Hyperactivity -
2. Conduct Problems 0.86 -
3. Emotion Problems 0.54 0.68 -
4. Peer Problems 0.50 0.63 0.51 -
5. Parent Distress 0.37 0.38 0.39 0.21 -
6. Parent–Child Conflict 0.31 0.38 0.25 0.18 0.24 -
7. Parent Conflict 0.07 0.16 0.14 0.09 0.09 0.05 -
8. Household Chaos 0.44 0.49 0.39 0.29 0.30 0.29 0.17 -
9. COVID-19 Disruption 0.18 0.23 0.20 0.22 0.27 0.15 0.11 0.16 -
10. Gov. Stringency 0.03 0.07 0.04 0.22 −0.05 0.01 0.10 −0.08 0.03 -
11. Socioeconomic Status −0.18 −0.16 −0.14 −0.08 −0.09 0.06 −0.10 −0.10 −0.06 0.08 -
model in which all paths were freely estimated across site and subsequently compared this
model to a model in which all paths were constrained to be equal across site. We assessed
differences in model fit using the Satorra–Bentler χ2 difference test [39]. Finally, we tested
whether parent distress mediated the association between COVID-19 disruption and child
adjustment problems using Mplus’ bootstrapping procedures (5000 bootstrap samples).
3. Results
3.1. Preliminary Analyses: Between-Site Differences in Parents’ COVID-19 Experience
and Distress
As illustrated in Figure 1, there were between-site contrasts in national government
stringency scores (F(5) = 755.84, p < 0.0001, η2 = 0.60) and in the experience of COVID-
19-related social disruption (F(5) = 38.34, p < 0.0001, η2 = 0.07). Due to the frequency of
planned post-hoc contrasts, we applied a more stringent alpha rate (p < 0.003). In terms
of stringency scores, there were significant contrasts between most countries, aside from
between China and Sweden. For COVID-19-related social disruption, families in the USA
experienced a greater level of disruption compared to families in all sites, aside from the
UK, while families in Sweden reported less disruption than all sites, except for Australia.
Families in the UK reported more disruption than those in Italy, Australia, and Sweden.
Prior to comparing between-site differences in parental distress, we tested for cross-
site measurement invariance of the GHQ using multiple-group confirmatory factor analysis.
A partially scalar invariant single-factor model with factor loadings and thresholds free
to vary across sites for only one out of 12 items (item 8 “Been able to face up to your
problems”) showed a good fit to the data (RMSEA = 0.082 [0.076, 0.087], CFI = 0.969,
TLI = 0.967). This suggests that across site, the organization of the construct is similar
(i.e., configural invariance), with each item contributing in a similar way to the construct
(i.e., metric invariance), and the cut-off underling the distribution of scores are consistent
(i.e., scalar invariance). Together, this suggests that the GHQ is appropriate for use across
different contexts during the pandemic.
Using this partially scalar invariant GHQ model, we then tested for mean differences
in the latent factor for parent distress across sites. Parents in the UK reported significantly
more distress than parents in Italy, China, and Sweden, whilst American parents also
reported higher levels of distress than Italian or Swedish parents.
Response
60
40
20
0
3
Disruption
0
UK Italy China Australia USA Sweden
8
6
4
2
0
UK Italy China Australia USA Sweden
Figure 1. Between-site
Figure comparisons
1. Between-site of (A)
comparisons of government stringency
(A) government in COVID-19
stringency response
in COVID-19 between
response between
April and July 2020, (B) COVID-19-related family disruption, and (C) parent distress.
April and July 2020, (B) COVID-19-related family disruption, and (C) parent distress.
Using this partially scalar invariant GHQ model, we then tested for mean differences
in the latent factor for parent distress across sites. Parents in the UK reported significantly
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11136 9 of 14
4. Discussion
Despite significant between-site contrasts in mean levels of parental distress and
experiences of COVID-19, two key findings highlighted the similarity of associations
between family risk factors, COVID-19 experiences, and children’s psychosocial adjustment
in the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic across the six sites. First, children from
families characterized by parental distress, increased levels of parent–child conflict, and
chaos showed elevated levels of hyperactivity, conduct, and emotion problems. In contrast,
increased peer problems were more strongly related to COVID-19-related experiences (both
stringency of national restrictions and family-level social disruption). The magnitude of
these associations was strongest for chaos, while couple conflict was not significantly related
to child adjustment problems. This differentiated pattern of associations supports domain-
specific models of social influences on child development and highlights the potential
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11136 10 of 14
4.1. Family Dysfunction and COVID-19 Experiences Show Direct Links with Child Difficulties
Over and above effects of child age and family SES, increased levels of problems across
each of the four problem subscales of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire [29,30]
were associated with both micro (parent distress, parent–child conflict, couple conflict, and
household chaos) and macro (national stringency) levels of influence. This multiplicity of
associations is consistent with developmental systems framework’s central premise that
children’s psychosocial development should always be considered in context and that chil-
dren are particularly responsive to principal relationships within their microsystem [1,12].
While family risk factors were weakly associated with peer problems, they explained
around a third of the variance in ratings for hyperactivity, emotion problems, and conduct
problems, strengthening the view that coregulation (reflecting parent–child reciprocity,
responsiveness, and cooperation) provides a key foundation for behavioral adjustment
(e.g., [42]), that is, children’s acquisition of useful skills and habits for regulating their emo-
tions and behavior hinges upon parental warmth and structure, which can be constrained
by mental health problems and household chaos [43].
Our findings also mirror reports of a protective effect of family routines on children’s
internalizing and externalizing symptoms during the pandemic in the USA [24]. As
economic insecurity, irregular employment, and limited access to childcare all predict
household chaos (e.g., [44]), elevated levels of family chaos are likely to persist. Given that
initial levels and increases in the quality of the home environment predict gains in school
readiness [45], a clear practical implication of our results is the need to support parents in
their efforts to provide a stable home environment.
Family risk factors exerted a weaker effect on peer problems, which is unsurprising
given mass school closures. Instead, highlighting the impact of depriving children of
the stimulation and enrichment associated with interactions with other children, peer
problems were more clearly associated with the country-wide severity of containment and
family-level disruption. On this latter point, it follows that in experiencing unemployment,
financial strain, work–family conflict, and worry about loved ones, parents themselves are
(or feel) unable to participate in typical group practices, thus limiting children’s opportuni-
ties to observe and practice their social skills [46]. Importantly, positive peer relationships
are associated with a host of other child outcomes, including the development of chil-
dren’s identity, socio-cognitive skills, and understanding of social norms [47]. As a result,
“catch-up” interventions need to extend beyond the academic domain to include emotional
support and enriched opportunities for peer play. Notably, these suggestions are possible
due to our inclusion of peer problems as a specific outcome of interest, a domain largely
absent from earlier research that demonstrated large-scale economic crashes were linked to
decline in children’s mental health [48].
4.2. COVID-19-Related Disruption, Family Risk Factors, and Child Difficulties: Indirect Effects
The parents who took part in our online survey reported poor mental health during
the early stages of the pandemic, with just over half of parents scoring 3 or more on the
GHQ, a cut-off with high specificity and sensitivity commonly used to indicate the presence
of a clinically relevant symptoms across diverse samples [49]. Our results highlighted
the impact of parental distress as a mediator of the negative impact of the COVID-19
pandemic on child adjustment. This is consistent with a recent American study that
showed experiencing the disease in the family as well as the succeeding economic effects
both impact family wellbeing [8]. Social learning accounts would suggest the onset and
maintenance of child difficulties result from children modeling their parents’ behaviors or
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11136 11 of 14
ineffective coping strategies. Alternatively, this indirect effect may be carried by changes
in parent–child talk, such as inappropriate health risk communication, or compromised
parenting practices, such as reduced sensitivity (e.g., [50]). Interestingly, an American
study found parents’ worries about COVID-19 were not associated with harsh discipline
or reduced warmth [22]. However, this null association may reflect the focus on worrying
about contracting and passing on the virus, which may have instead manifested in other
behaviors, such as increased handwashing. Future longitudinal research will help tease
apart these competing explanations as well as test whether changes in parents’ wellbeing
across the pandemic contribute to individual differences in the trajectories of children’s
psychosocial problems. That said, it seems evident that family-based interventions may
prove most effective in mitigating the negative impact of the pandemic on child adjustment.
Furthermore, the advent of innovative work evaluating the success of online family therapy
suggests that these interventions do not have to wait until families can meet their therapist
in person [51].
Our findings draw on data collected from families across six study sites with different
virus infection rates and government approaches, leading to contrasting social, economic,
and cultural effects. Reflecting these contrasts, our findings showed site contrasts in levels
of COVID-19-related social disruption and stringency. Specifically, families in the USA
and UK experienced more disruption than families in all other sites, and Swedish families
experienced the least disruption. Nevertheless, with regard to mechanisms of influence on
child adjustment, the findings from the different sites indicate striking similarities. This
generality of results strengthens the evidence suggesting the cascading impact of economic
pressure on child developmental outcomes via parent wellbeing across a variety of family
structures, ethnic backgrounds, and geographic locations [52].
5. Conclusions
Our study drew on an international sample of 2516 parents and applied tests of mea-
surement invariance to both our outcome and mediating variables to provide meaningful
cross-cultural comparisons. That said, our findings should be interpreted considering a
number of methodological limitations.
First, our convenience sample of parents from middle to high socioeconomic back-
grounds impacts the external validity and generalizability of our findings. The subsamples
were restricted in terms of geographic (e.g., American parents in Pennsylvania and Ohio
and Chinese parents in Beijing) and socioeconomic (e.g., almost 60% of Italian parents in
our study had a tertiary education qualification compared to the national average level
of 20% [53]) position and were ethnically homogenous. Here, it is worth noting that, un-
fortunately, a large proportion of parents in China did not report on their ethnicity, which
further limits our understanding of the representativeness of the Chinese subsample. Fur-
thermore, the necessary reliance on online research designs during the pandemic increases
the likelihood of selection bias and collider bias [54]. For example, participation is likely
associated with increased engagement with social media campaigns, scientific interest, and
higher education levels [54]. As a result, we cannot rule out that our models replicate across
different countries because those who took part were largely similar (i.e., middle to high
socioeconomic backgrounds). However, given we found associations between COVID-19
disruption, family risk factors, and child adjustment within a restricted range of SES, it
is likely our estimates are conservative. Further research is required with more diverse
samples as well as with families living in low or middle income countries to test whether
our results replicate beyond this specific demographic.
Second, the cross-sectional design limits our appreciation of the nature of the me-
diation findings. The findings may not be pandemic specific given we cannot rule out
the impact of pre-existing levels of distress on parents’ current mental health. That said,
our sample did not have a history of psychopathology, and our theoretically informed
analyses focused on disruption to life due to COVID-19-specific changes in association
with current levels of distress [13]. Future longitudinal research is required to rule out
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11136 12 of 14
alternative explanations regarding the directionality of our results (i.e., child difficulties
eliciting parental stress).
Third, like other colleagues, we relied upon a single informant to report on both
parents and children, which may inflate associations between constructs. However, as
illustrated in Table 2, we found modest associations between family processes and child
outcomes. Our results are strengthened by including an objective assessment of COVID-
19 experiences. Specifically, a publicly available data set that assessed the stringency of
national restrictions enabled us to test the impact of this distal influence on child adjustment.
As this measure is strongly positively correlated with national rates of infection [55], it also
provides a proxy for national infection rates and a time-specific index of the severity of the
disease’s impact. As we move towards more open science, future collaborative efforts that
pool individual participant data measuring similar constructs will be helpful in developing
an understanding of the varied impact of the pandemic on parent and child adjustment
across diverse contexts.
With these caveats in mind, the findings from this international study provide further
evidence that the disruptive impact of the COVID-19 pandemic is multigenerational,
impacting parents’ wellbeing and children’s social skills and adjustment difficulties. As
a result, family-based interventions may prove most effective in mitigating the negative
impact of the pandemic on child adjustment.
Author Contributions: Conceptualization, C.H. and S.F.; data curation, S.F.; formal analysis, S.F.
and L.R.; funding acquisition, C.H. and S.F.; investigation, S.F., F.B., K.C.B., M.A.F., L.R. and C.H.;
methodology, C.H.; supervision, C.H.; writing—original draft, S.F. and C.H.; writing—review and
editing, F.B., K.C.B., M.A.F. and L.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the
manuscript.
Funding: The first author was funded by an Economic and Social Research Council Post-Doctoral
Fellowship (ES/T008989/1). The Principal Investigator (C.H.) received funding for the UK arm of
the study from Newnham College Cambridge. Data collection in the USA was supported by the
National Science Foundation under grant DUE1534830, the James S. McDonnell Foundation Scholar
Award, and the National Institutes of Health under grant NICHD F32 HD102106-01.
Institutional Review Board Statement: This study was conducted according to the guidelines
of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Psychology Research Ethics Committee at
the University of Cambridge, Uppsala University, University of Pavia, University of Pittsburgh,
University of Queensland, and Peking University.
Informed Consent Statement: Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in
the study.
Data Availability Statement: The data presented in this study are available on request from the
corresponding author.
Acknowledgments: We would like to thank the families involved in the study for completing data
collection in extraordinary circumstances. The i-FAM-Covid Consortium consists of Claire Hughes,
Sarah Foley, Elian Fink, Samantha Friedman, Rory T. Devine, Amy Selby, Karin Brocki, Matilda Frick,
Farzaneh Badinlou, Xin Feng, Meingold Chan, Virginia Slaughter, Sally Clark, Yanjie Su, Shan Wan,
Serena Lecce, Luca Ronchi, Chiara Basile, Paola del Sette, Andrew Ribner, Melissa Libertus, Leanne
Elliott, and Alex Silver.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors report no conflict of interest.
References
1. Overton, W.F. Process and relational-develop- mental systems. In Theory and Method. Vol. 1: Handbook of Child Psychology and
Developmental Science; Lerner, R.M., Liben, L.S., Mueller, U., Eds.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2015; pp. 323–331.
2. Beazidou, E.; Botsoglou, K. Peer acceptance and friendship in early childhood: The conceptual distinctions between them. Early
Child Dev. Care 2016, 186, 1615–1631. [CrossRef]
3. Eltanamly, H.; Leijten, P.; Jak, S.; Overbeek, G. Parenting in Times of War: A Meta-Analysis and Qualitative Synthesis of War
Exposure, Parenting, and Child Adjustment. Trauma Violence Abus. 2021, 22, 147–160. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11136 13 of 14
4. Moore, S.A.; Faulkner, G.; Rhodes, R.E.; Brussoni, M.; Chulak-Bozzer, T.; Ferguson, L.J.; Mitra, R.; O’Reilly, N.; Spence, J.C.;
Vanderloo, L.M.; et al. Impact of the COVID-19 virus outbreak on movement and play behaviours of Canadian children and
youth: A national survey. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2020, 17, 1–11. [CrossRef]
5. World Health Organization. Mental Health and Psychosocial Considerations during the COVID-19 Outbreak; WHO/2019-
nCoV/MentalHealth/2020.1; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2020.
6. Jiao, W.Y.; Wang, L.N.; Liu, J.; Fang, S.F.; Jiao, F.Y.; Pettoello-Mantovani, M.; Somekh, E. Behavioral and Emotional Disorders in
Children during the COVID-19 Epidemic. J. Pediatr. 2020, 221, 264–266.e1. [CrossRef]
7. Bignardi, G.; Dalmaijer, E.S.; Anwyl-Irvine, A.L.; Smith, T.A.; Siugzdaite, R.; Uh, S.; Astle, D.E. Longitudinal increases in
childhood depression symptoms during the COVID-19 lockdown. Arch. Dis. Child. 2021, 106, 791–797. [CrossRef]
8. Gassman-Pines, A.; Ananat, E.O.; Fitz-Henley, J. COVID-19 and Parent-Child Psychological Well-being. Pediatrics 2020,
146, e2020007294. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
9. Adams, E.L.; Smith, D.; Caccavale, L.J.; Bean, M.K. Parents Are Stressed! Patterns of Parent Stress Across COVID-19. Front.
Psychiatry 2021, 12, 626456. [CrossRef]
10. Prime, H.; Wade, M.; Browne, D.T. Risk and resilience in family well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. Am. Psychol. 2020,
75, 631–643. [CrossRef]
11. Brown, S.M.; Doom, J.; Lechuga-Peña, S.; Watamura, S.E.; Koppels, T. Stress and parenting during the global COVID-19 pandemic.
Child Abus. Negl. 2020, 110, 104699. [CrossRef]
12. Osher, D.; Cantor, P.; Berg, J.; Steyer, L.; Rose, T. Drivers of human development: How relationships and context shape learning
and development1. Appl. Dev. Sci. 2020, 24, 6–36. [CrossRef]
13. Hale, T.; Angrist, N.; Goldszmidt, R.; Kira, B.; Petherick, A.; Phillips, T.; Webster, S.; Cameron-Blake, E.; Hallas, L.; Majumdar, S.;
et al. A global panel database of pandemic policies (Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker). Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021, 5,
529–538. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
14. Gordon, M.; Burgess, M. The Hidden Impact of COVID-19 on Children’s Education and Learning; Save the Children International:
London, UK, 2020.
15. Putnick, D.L.; Bornstein, M.H. Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: The state of the art and future directions for
psychological research. Dev. Rev. 2016, 41, 71–90. [CrossRef]
16. Ronchi, L.; Foley, S.; Lecce, S.; Hughes, C. Cross-cultural Equivalence of Parental Ratings of Child Difficulties during the Pandemic:
Findings from a Six-Site Study. Manuscript Submitted for Publication, 2021.
17. Sonuga-Barke, E.J. “School of hard knocks”—what can mental health researchers learn from the COVID-19 crisis? J. Child Psychol.
Psychiatry 2021, 62, 1–4. [CrossRef]
18. Hughes, C.; Devine, R.T. For Better or for Worse? Positive and Negative Parental Influences on Young Children’s Executive
Function. Child Dev. 2017, 90, 593–609. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
19. Goodman, S.H.; Rouse, M.H.; Connell, A.M.; Broth, M.R.; Hall, C.M.; Heyward, D. Maternal Depression and Child Psychopathol-
ogy: A Meta-Analytic Review. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2011, 14, 1–27. [CrossRef]
20. Sweeney, S.; MacBeth, A. The effects of paternal depression on child and adolescent outcomes: A systematic review. J. Affect.
Disord. 2016, 205, 44–59. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
21. Romero, E.; López-Romero, L.; Domínguez-Álvarez, B.; Villar, P.; Gómez-Fraguela, J.A. Testing the Effects of COVID-19
Confinement in Spanish Children: The Role of Parents’ Distress, Emotional Problems and Specific Parenting. Int. J. Environ. Res.
Public Health 2020, 17, 6975. [CrossRef]
22. Waller, R.; Powell, T.; Rodriguez, Y.; Corbett, N.; Perlstein, S.; White, L.K.; Barzilay, R.; Wagner, N.J. The Impact of the COVID-19
Pandemic on Children’s Conduct Problems and Callous-Unemotional Traits. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2021, 1–12. [CrossRef]
23. Smith, J.D.; Dishion, T.J.; Shaw, D.S.; Wilson, M.N.; Winter, C.C.; Patterson, G.R. Coercive family process and early-onset conduct
problems from age 2 to school entry. Dev. Psychopathol. 2014, 26, 917–932. [CrossRef]
24. Glynn, L.M.; Davis, E.P.; Luby, J.L.; Baram, T.Z.; Sandman, C.A. A predictable home environment may protect child mental health
during the COVID-19 pandemic. Neurobiol. Stress 2021, 14, 100291. [CrossRef]
25. Deater-Deckard, K.; Godwin, J.; Lansford, J.E.; Tirado, L.M.U.; Yotanyamaneewong, S.; Alampay, L.P.; Al-Hassan, S.M.; Bacchini,
D.; Bornstein, M.H.; Chang, L.; et al. Chaos, danger, and maternal parenting in families: Links with adolescent adjustment in low-
and middle-income countries. Dev. Sci. 2019, 22, e12855. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
26. Pietromonaco, P.R.; Overall, N.C. Applying relationship science to evaluate how the COVID-19 pandemic may impact couples’
relationships. Am. Psychol. 2021, 76, 438–450. [CrossRef]
27. Cummings, E.M.; Koss, K.J.; Davies, P. Prospective Relations between Family Conflict and Adolescent Maladjustment: Security in
the Family System as a Mediating Process. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2015, 43, 503–515. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
28. Harold, G.T.; Sellers, R. Annual Research Review: Interparental conflict and youth psychopathology: An evidence review and
practice focused update. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2018, 59, 374–402. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
29. Goodman, R. Psychometric Properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2001,
40, 1337–1345. [CrossRef]
30. Goodman, A.; Goodman, R. Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire as a Dimensional Measure of Child Mental Health. J. Am.
Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2009, 48, 400–403. [CrossRef]
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11136 14 of 14
31. Goldberg, D.P.; Gater, R.; Sartorius, N.; Ustun, T.B.; Piccinelli, M.; Gureje, O.; Rutter, C. The validity of two versions of the GHQ
in the WHO study of mental illness in general health care. Psychol. Med. 1997, 27, 191–197. [CrossRef]
32. Matheny, A.P.; Wachs, T.D.; Ludwig, J.L.; Phillips, K. Bringing order out of chaos: Psychometric characteristics of the confusion,
hubbub, and order scale. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 1995, 16, 429–444. [CrossRef]
33. ONS. The National Statistics Socio-Economic Classification (NS-SEC)–Office for National Statistics. 2021. Available on-
line: https://www.ons.gov.uk/methodology/classificationsandstandards/otherclassifications/thenationalstatisticssocioecono
micclassificationnssecrebasedonsoc2010 (accessed on 14 October 2021).
34. Muthén, L.K.; Muthén, B.O. Mplus User’s Guide, 8th ed.; Muthén & Muthén: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2017.
35. Brown, T. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research, 2nd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015.
36. Byrne, B. Structural Equation Modeling with Mplus: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming (Multivariate Applications Series);
Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2012.
37. Svetina, D.; Rutkowski, L.; Rutkowski, D. Multiple-Group Invariance with Categorical Outcomes Using Updated Guidelines: An
Illustration Using Mplus and the lavaan/semTools Packages. Struct. Equ. Model. A Multidiscip. J. 2020, 27, 111–130. [CrossRef]
38. Enders, C.K. A Primer on Maximum Likelihood Algorithms Available for Use with Missing Data. Struct. Equ. Model. A Multidiscip.
J. 2001, 8, 128–141. [CrossRef]
39. Satorra, A.; Bentler, P.M. Ensuring Positiveness of the Scaled Difference Chi-square Test Statistic. Psychometrika 2010, 75, 243–248.
[CrossRef]
40. Hayes, A.F. Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical Mediation Analysis in the New Millennium. Commun. Monogr. 2009, 76, 408–420.
[CrossRef]
41. Spinelli, M.; Lionetti, F.; Pastore, M.; Fasolo, M. Parents’ Stress and Children’s Psychological Problems in Families Facing the
COVID-19 Outbreak in Italy. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1713. [CrossRef]
42. Deater-Deckard, K.; Petrill, S.A. Parent-child dyadic mutuality and child behavior problems: An investigation of gene-
environment processes. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2004, 45, 1171–1179. [CrossRef]
43. Coldwell, J.; Pike, A.; Dunn, J. Household chaos ? links with parenting and child behaviour. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2006, 47,
1116–1122. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
44. Vernon-Feagans, L.; Garrett-Peters, P.; Willoughby, M.; Mills-Koonce, R. Chaos, poverty, and parenting: Predictors of early
language development. Early Child. Res. Q. 2012, 27, 339–351. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
45. Korucu, I.; Schmitt, S.A. Continuity and change in the home environment: Associations with school readiness. Early Child. Res. Q.
2020, 53, 97–107. [CrossRef]
46. Grusec, J.E.; Davidov, M. Socialization in the family: The role of parents. In Handbook of Socialization: Theory and Research, 2nd ed.;
Grusec, J.E., Hastings, P.D., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 284–308.
47. Gifford-Smith, M.E.; Brownell, C.A. Childhood peer relationships: Social acceptance, friendships, and peer networks. J. Sch.
Psychol. 2003, 41, 235–284. [CrossRef]
48. Golberstein, E.; Gonzales, G.; Meara, E. How do economic downturns affect the mental health of children? Evidence from the
National Health Interview Survey. Health Econ. 2019, 28, 955–970. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
49. Goldberg, D.P.; Oldehinkel, T.; Ormel, J. Why GHQ threshold varies from one place to another. Psychol. Med. 1998, 28, 915–921.
[CrossRef]
50. Foley, S.; Álvarez, C.; McCarthy, J.; Hughes, C. Two’s Company, Three’s a Crowd? Maternal and Paternal Talk About Their
Infant Differs in Associations with Wellbeing, Couple Relationship Quality, and Caregiving Sensitivity. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
51. Burgoyne, N.; Cohn, A.S. Lessons from the Transition to Relational Teletherapy during COVID-19. Fam. Process. 2020, 59, 974–988.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
52. Masarik, A.S.; Conger, R.D. Stress and child development: A review of the Family Stress Model. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2017, 13,
85–90. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
53. OECD. Education Attainment–Population with Tertiary Education–OECD DATA. 2021. Available online: https://data.oecd.org/
eduatt/population-with-tertiary-education.htm (accessed on 14 October 2021).
54. Griffith, G.J.; Morris, T.T.; Tudball, M.J.; Herbert, A.; Mancano, G.; Pike, L.; Sharp, G.C.; Sterne, J.; Palmer, T.M.; Smith, G.D.; et al.
Collider bias undermines our understanding of COVID-19 disease risk and severity. Nat. Commun. 2020, 11, 1–12. [CrossRef]
55. Hale, T.; Webster, S.; Petherick, A.; Phillips, T.; Kira, B. Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker, Blavatnik School of
Government. Data Use Policy: Creative Commons Attribution CC BY Standard. 2020. Available online: https://covidtracker.bsg
.ox.ac.uk/stringency-scatter (accessed on 14 October 2021).