Mindful Walking and Cognition in Older Adults - A Proof of Concept Study Using In-Lab and Ambulatory Cognitive Measures
Mindful Walking and Cognition in Older Adults - A Proof of Concept Study Using In-Lab and Ambulatory Cognitive Measures
Mindful Walking and Cognition in Older Adults - A Proof of Concept Study Using In-Lab and Ambulatory Cognitive Measures
A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T
Keywords: Mindfulness practice and walking have been linked individually to sustain cognition in older adults. This early-
Walking meditation phase study aimed to establish proof-of-concept by evaluating whether an intervention that integrates light-
Executive function intensity walking with mindfulness practices shows promising signs of improving cognition in older adults.
Lifestyle activity
Participants (N = 25, Mage = 72.4 ± 6.45) were community-dwelling older adults who engaged in a supervised
Ambulatory cognitive assessment
Healthy aging
mindful walking program over one month (8 sessions total, 2 sessions per week, 30-minute slow walking con
Light-intensity physical activity taining mindfulness skills). They completed performance-based and subjective ratings of cognitive measures in
field before and after two mindful walking bouts using a smartphone app. They also completed in-lab perfor
mance-based and self-report cognitive measures at baseline and after the entire program. Controlling for de
mographics, potential covariates, and time trends, short-term improvements in perceived cognition and
processing speed were observed from pre- to post-mindful walking sessions (i.e., 30 min) across multiple
ambulatory cognitive measures (Cohen’s ds range = 0.46–0.66). Longer-term improvements in processing speed
and executive function were observed between baseline and end of the program (i.e., one month) across various
performance-based cognitive measures (ds range = 0.43–1.28). No significant changes were observed for other
cognitive domains. This early-phase study (Phase IIa) provides preliminary support that mindful walking activity
is promising for sustaining cognition in older adults. Our promising findings form the building blocks of evidence
needed to advance this intervention to a fully powered randomized controlled trial that examines program ef
ficacy with a comparator. Favorable outcomes will inform the development of this lifestyle behavioral strategy
for promoting healthy brain aging in late adulthood.
1. Introduction adulthood. Two promising strategies for these purposes include physical
activity and mindfulness practices (Erickson et al., 2019; Gard et al.,
Human aging is associated with normative alterations in cognition 2014 Jan; Malinowski and Shalamanova, 2017; Sofi et al., 2011). It is
and increased risks for neurodegenerative disease in late life. These viable to integrate mindfulness practice with walking as an intervention
diseases are the most expensive US annual health expenditure and place strategy (i.e., mindful walking) (Kabat-Zinn, 2017). This “active form”
a tremendous economic burden on society and families (Alzheimer’s of mindfulness practice has been implemented as part of the standard
Association, 2020). Cognitive impairments caused by these diseases also mindfulness-based programs (i.e., Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction
exact a toll on the overall health, well-being, and quality of life among program) to enhance psychological well-being (Gotink et al., 2016; Teut
older adults. Preventive interventions are needed to help older adults et al., 2013). However, mindful walking has not been used as a major
reduce risks for these diseases and preserve functioning into late strategy to study cognitive outcomes.
* Corresponding author at: Department of Exercise Science, Technology Center to Promote Healthy Lifestyles, Arnold School of Public Health, University of South
Carolina, 915 Greene St, Columbia, SC 29201, USA.
E-mail address: cy11@mailbox.sc.edu (C.-H. Yang).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pmedr.2021.101490
Received 6 March 2021; Received in revised form 5 July 2021; Accepted 10 July 2021
Available online 14 July 2021
2211-3355/© 2021 Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
C.-H. Yang et al. Preventive Medicine Reports 23 (2021) 101490
Walking and mindfulness programs may individually contribute to could read and spoke English fluently, and without allergy to plants and
short- (e.g., after brief practice) and longer-term (e.g., after completing flowers. A subset of older adults (N = 25, Mean age = 72.4 ± 6.45, age
the entire program) cognitive improvements in older adults, albeit range = 66–89, 84% female, 84% White) opted to participate in the
variability of intervention design exists among available studies cognitive assessments designed in this proof-of-concept study. A more
(Scherder et al., 2014; Venturelli et al., 2011; Berk et al., 2017; Chiesa detailed description of recruitment with a CONSORT diagram is re
et al., 2011). It is plausible that an integrated mindful walking program ported previously (Yang and Conroy, 2019). The primary goal of the
may likewise be associated with both short- and longer-term cognitive proof-of-concept study is to “determine if a treatment package can
benefits in older adults. This early phase proof-of-concept study (Phase achieve benefit on a clinically significant target in a small, select sam
IIa) evaluated whether a multi-session mindful walking program pro ple” (Czajkowski et al., 2015). In this context, “within-subjects designs
vided signals consistent with short- and longer-term cognitive im where subjects act as their own controls in a pre-post comparison are
provements in older adults (Czajkowski et al., 2015 Oct). ideal…[and] the sample can be selected from acceptable subjects, rather
Walking is the most prevalent type of physical activity among older than representative, because this initial test will determine only whether
adults and the most preferred physical activity among cognitively- the treatment merits more rigorous testing” (p. 977). The present sample
impaired older adults (Williams et al., 2008; Dai et al., 2015). Current size is comparable to proof-of-concept studies evaluating the potential
evidence indicates that accruing physical activity at a lower and more benefits of behavioral and technology-based health interventions
achievable intensity (i.e., walking) improves cognitive health in both (Conroy and Heartphone, 2020; Liu-Ambrose and Eng, 2015; Månsson
active and inactive older adults, as well as older adults with cognitive et al., 2013; Conroy et al., 2020).
impairments (Prohaska et al., 2009; Spartano et al., 2019; Wang et al., At baseline, participants were not sufficiently active (based on the
2012). Mindfulness practice trains individuals to elevate their attention Physical Activity Guidelines for Americans) and 16% (n = 4) of them
and awareness in every present moment, and engage their present were overweight/obese. They reported no formal mindfulness training
experience in a non-judgmental manner (Kabat-Zinn, 1994; Kabat-Zinn, experiences, cognitive/memory complaints, or diagnosis of neuropsy
2012). Practicing mindfulness is appealing to older adults, as evidenced chological diseases. The majority of the walking sessions were
by the high compliance rates, and initial evidence suggesting that daily completed on weekdays (n = 160, 80%) and before noon (n = 152, 76%)
mindfulness practice may improve their cognitive health (Gard et al., in October and December. Participants who completed all walking ses
2014 Jan; Wong et al., 2017). Previous work suggests that short bouts of sions and assessments were eligible to win one of nine $25 gift cards in a
mindful-walking sessions are feasible to implement in older adults living raffle. All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical
in the community (Yang and Conroy, 2019). No study to date has standards of the responsible committee on human subject research and
evaluated whether short bouts of mindful-walking practices produce with the Helsinki Declaration. Written informed consent was obtained
cognitive benefits in older adults. from all participants. The Institutional Review Board approved all study
This study applied both performance-based and subjective ratings of protocols.
cognition both in the lab and at the walking site to broadly assess
cognition in response to the mindful walking activity. To assess longer-
term outcomes, conventional methods including performance-based 2.2. Procedure
neuropsychological assessments, computerized experimental assess
ments, and questionnaires were applied at baseline and at post mindful Participants first completed an initial lab visit and completed base
walking program across one month. These lab-based measures are line cognitive assessments. These assessments included a computerized
relatively time-consuming, and they are not suitable for administration Stroop task, two sets of neuropsychological tests using paper–pencil
in the field (e.g., outdoor environments) to capture any acute changes format (see “Measures” below), and a survey of perceived cognition.
experienced following walking activity (Ladouce et al., 2017). To assess Participants then scheduled eight sessions of outdoor mindful walking
short-term outcomes, this study applied recently validated, ultra-brief, within the following month, with a maximum of scheduling two sessions
ambulatory cognitive assessments on smartphones to evaluate short- per week. After completing the mindful walking sessions, participants
term subjective and performance-based cognitive changes associated returned to the lab for post-program cognitive assessments that were
with brief 30-min mindful walking bouts (Sliwinski et al., 2018). identical to the formats used in the initial lab visit.
The purpose of this study was to establish proof-of-concept for using Each walking session consisted of a 30-minute individual slow
this mindful walking program to improve short- and longer-term walking along a flat, designated route in an arboretum. Participants
cognition among community-dwelling older adults. Proof-of-concept were instructed to walk at a slower pace of approximately one step per
study represents an early phase of intervention development in the second (i.e., light-intensity activity). Walking at a slower speed helped
Obesity-Related Behavioral Intervention Trials (ORBIT) framework. We participants elicit the state of mindfulness and elevate their awareness to
defined the meaningful change in cognitive outcomes as 0.20 standard the present moment experiences (Kabat-Zinn, 2017). The research staff
deviation, which is equivalent to 10 years of normative cognitive aging met with participants on the walking site to provide instructions on
documented in previous reviews (Salthouse, 1996 Jul; Salthouse, 2000). mindfulness skills and conducted pre-and post- walking assessments.
Available reviews on physical activity interventions on cognition also Three fundamental mindfulness skills were introduced and incorporated
reported small-to-moderate effect sizes (range = 0.20–0.48) among progressively in sequence starting from the second session to help par
cognitively normal older adults (Erickson et al., 2019; Mj, et al., 2016). ticipants build up mindful walking skills. These fundamental skills
Evidence from a proof-of-concept study is not sufficient to draw con involve being attentive to the rhythm of their breathing (i.e., each inhale
clusions about efficacy, but it is essential for determining whether this and exhale), being attentive to the movement of their every step, and
intervention warrants investment in a rigorous trial to evaluate effects in mentally scanning the body to identify and accept sensations/feelings
relation to a comparator (Czajkowski et al., 2015 Oct; Freedland, 2020). that arise in every present moment (Kabat-Zinn, 1994). In the last two
mindful walking sessions (7th and 8th), participants practiced all three
2. Methods mindfulness skills in sequence throughout their 30-minute walk.
Immediately before and after the 7th and 8th sessions, participants
2.1. Participants completed subjective ratings of cognition and a short battery of
smartphone-based ambulatory cognitive assessments (see “Measures”
Participants were community-dwelling older adults who participated below). Participants overall reported increased state mindfulness (p <
in an 8-session slow walking program at a local arboretum. Eligible older .001, d = 0.84) using items from the State Mindfulness Scale across all
adults were at least 65 years old, could walk without other’s assistance, mindful walking sessions (Tanay and Bernstein, 2013).
2
C.-H. Yang et al. Preventive Medicine Reports 23 (2021) 101490
3
C.-H. Yang et al. Preventive Medicine Reports 23 (2021) 101490
Table 1
Descriptives of in-lab cognitive assessments and the within-group differences between baseline and post mindful walking program.
Variable Baseline mean(SD) Post-program mean(SD) Mean difference(SD) 95%CI of mean difference t Pre-post Correlation
a
Everyday Cognition Scale
Memory 4.83 (0.39) 4.80 (0.56) 0.03 (0.57) [− 0.20 , 0.25] 0.21 0.31
Planning 5.54 (0.44) 5.61 (0.47) − 0.07 (0.51) [− 0.27 , 0.13] − 0.69 0.38
Organization 4.92 (0.85) 5.05 (0.76) − 0.13 (0.55) [− 0.34 , 0.09] − 1.17 0.77***
Divided attention 4.96 (0.79) 4.83 (0.80) 0.13 (0.72) [− 0.15 , 0.41] 0.94 0.60**
Trail Making Testa
Trail A completion time (sec) 26.62 (7.50) 24.50 (7.10) 2.12 (4.90) [0.14 , 4.10] 2.20* 0.78***
Trail B completion time (sec) 59.11 (20.88) 51.08 (19.14) 8.03 (18.75) [0.46 , 15.60] 2.18* 0.56**
Trail B-A time difference (sec) 32.49 (18.45) 26.58 (16.31) 5.91 (19.80) [− 2.08 , 13.91] 1.52 0.36
Trail A Errors 0.26 (0.66) 0.37 (0.63) − 0.11 (0.80) [− 0.43 , 0.21] − 0.72 0.22
Trail B Errors 1.19 (1.62) 0.59 (0.89) 0.59 (1.80) [− 0.12 , 1.31] 1.71 0.06
Porteus Maze Testa
Maze I completion time (sec) 60.81 (34.96) 44.45 (28.72) 16.35 (33.65) [2.76 , 29.94] 2.48* 0.46*
Maze II completion time (sec) 103.72 (72.02) 72.49 (41.17) 31.23 (69.19) [2.67 , 59.79] 2.26* 0.35
Maze I Errors 2.85 (1.98) 2.04 (2.26) 0.82 (2.42) [− 0.14 , 1.78] 1.75 0.35
Maze II Errors 2.70 (1.88) 2.30 (1.44) 0.41 (2.12) [− 0.43 , 1.25] 1.00 0.21
Stroop Testb
Congruence reaction time (ms) 1120.26 (141.83) 1047.46 (125.23) − 72.80 (132.47) [− 127.48 , − 18.11] − 2.75* 0.51**
Incongruence reaction time (ms) 1282.78 (136.85) 1205.85 (139.95) − 76.93 (141.35) [− 135.27 , − 18.58] − 2.72* 0.48*
Congruence accuracy rate (%) 96.30 (7.94) 98.80 (3.16) 2.50 (7.97) [− 0.79 , 5.79] 1.57 0.19
Incongruence accuracy rate (%) 80.62 (17.10) 91.56 (6.79) 10.94 (13.29) [5.33 , 16.55] 4.03** 0.70***
MSE = 0.32, p < .001). Post-hoc analyses revealed that overall accuracy 1994; Salthouse, 1996 Jul; Salthouse, 2000). Previous studies of mindful
rate was higher post-program compared with baseline (d = 1.37, p < walking have focused on mental health (Mj, et al., 2016; Peavy et al.,
.01) and during congruent trials compared with incongruent trials (d = 2012). This study extended the literature by modifying key domains of
2.78, p < .001). Additionally, there was a condition × occasion inter cognition in response to a multi-session mindful walking program for
action in predicting overall accuracy rate (F(1,23) = 22.57, MSE = 0.01, p older adults.
< .001). During incongruent trials, participants significantly increased The current study identified longer-term within-person improve
overall accuracy rate post-program compared with baseline (d = 1.96, p ments in processing speed and executive function across paper–pencil
< .001). There was no difference in accuracy rate during congruent trials and computerized assessments. Performance improvements on the
between baseline and post-program measures. Stroop task appeared to be specifically associated with incongruent
condition accuracy. This finding may imply improvements in inhibitory
control, selective attention, and overall executive function (Scarpina,
3.2. Short-term cognitive change (in field) 2017). However, we caution that accuracy during the congruent con
dition was overall very high at baseline, and thus, the observed inter
Table 2 summarizes the mixed-effects model results for the perceived action effect may be driven by either the changes in executive function
and objective ambulatory cognitive assessments from the mobile (incongruent condition-only) or general task performance improve
cognitive assessment protocol. Four participants had missing records in ments (in both conditions) that were masked by baseline ceiling per
their 7th walking session due to malfunction identified in one of the formance in the congruent condition. A potential ceiling effect may also
study smartphones, resulting in a total of 92 measurement occasions. explain the no difference in subjective measures of everyday cognition.
Controlling for contextual and time-based factors (main sources of Participants in this study were not cognitively impaired; their ability to
practice effect), subjective ratings of cognition were better at post- carry out daily cognitive tasks should be similar before and after the
walking session compared to pre-walking session (d = 1.15, p < .001). program.
Further, participants’ response time was generally faster post- compared Mirroring the longer-term cognitive improvements, short-term im
to pre-walking sessions across objective ambulatory cognitive assess provements in processing speed were observed during performance of a
ments. Significant faster post-walking response time was observed task with instructions that stressed speeded performance (Symbol
during two of the three ambulatory cognitive tasks, including Symbol Match) and another that stressed accuracy (N-Back), indicated that a
Search (d = 0.46, p < .05) and the N-Back task (d = 0.66, p < .01). The general impact on cognition may exist from practicing mindful walking.
mean reduction in response time observed in the Dot Memory task was It is possible that these short-term changes of mindful walking on pro
not significant (p = .61). No significant changes in mean accuracy were cessing speed are the mechanisms by which longer-term advantages are
observed from pre- to post-walking sessions among cognitive tasks. conferred (e.g., improvements are immediate and incremental). Pro
cessing speed is a central marker of neurocognitive function that
4. Discussion changes with age, and is altered significantly by the presence of
neurodegenerative disease (Salthouse, 2000; Finkel et al., 2007). Slower
Overall, the observed within-person changes of cognitive outcomes processing speed can have a widespread influence on other higher-order
in both short- and longer-term exceeded the meaningful benchmarks cognitive processes that unfold over time and require coordination of
that were given (i.e., d ≥ 0.20) for concluding that there was a favorable lower-level processes (e.g., working memory) (Kail, 2000). This proof-
signal on sustaining cognition from mindful walking. These results of-concept study controlled for potential practice effect, but an effi
indicate that mindful walking warrants progression in the intervention cacy trial is needed to evaluate if mindful walking practice contributes to
development pipeline (Phase IIb/III) described in the ORBIT model. The improvements of processing speed (Duff et al., 2007). Further, a short-
benefits of both acute (from 30-min bout) and accumulated practice term improvement in subjective cognition from pre-to post-walking
(from multiple sessions) of mindful walking appear to be conferred to session was observed using a single self-report item. This single item is a
information processing speed, which holds implications for a wide range global measure of cognition that does not represent a specific cognitive
of cognitive processes affected by cognitive aging (Kail and Salthouse,
4
C.-H. Yang et al. Preventive Medicine Reports 23 (2021) 101490
domain. Thus, finding from this self-report may be different from those
Perceivedcognitivefunction
measured by Everyday Cognition Scale in which daily cognitive aspects
were targeted.
The mindfulness skills practiced in this study emphasized cultivating
heightened awareness and attention to every present moment and
movement. These basic skills may enhance older adults’ ability to focus
5.05(1.12)
− 1.52***
5.92*** on timed or speeded tasks that lead to the observed improvement in
0.74***
− 0.65
− 0.02
− 0.10
processing speed. A review on mindfulness trainings suggested that the
0.02
0.02
0.07
0.85
0.79
development of focused attention is linked to improved executive
function and selective attention (Chiesa et al., 2011). The simultaneous
rhythmic walking activity carried out in this program may also evoke
Dotmemorymean error dist.
2015; Füzéki et al., 2017). Future trials can collect blood drops to un
− 0.38
− 0.08
− 0.01
0.01
0.09
0.89
1.18
8138.66(4704.54)
(Erickson et al., 2019). One possible explanation may be that brain re
gion governing memory capacities (i.e., amygdala, hippocampus, cere
bellum, prefrontal cortex) are less likely to be engaged by light-intensity
394.02**
− 321.21
3671.83
210.63
114.22
375.86
130.65
14.46
2788
3581
3. Perceived cognitive function was measured using self-report; all other cognitive outcomes were measured objectively.
<-0.0.01
<-0.01
<-0.01
− 0.01
− 0.02
− 0.02
− 0.01
Results of the within-person changes in mobile-based cognitive outcomes from pre to post mindful walking session.
0.02
0.08
0.10
− 10.36
187.85
180.61
50.39*
23.72
63.03
35.32
444.6
496.7
<-0.01
<-0.01
− 0.04
0.01
0.01
0.01
0.05
0.06
157.11*
− 61.07
− 17.06
280.00
80.64*
13.04
765.7
695.3
Intercept (SD)
Residual (SD)
Fixed Effect
Model
5
C.-H. Yang et al. Preventive Medicine Reports 23 (2021) 101490
Declaration of Competing Interest Ladouce, S., Donaldson, D.I., Dudchenko, P.A., Ietswaart, M., 2017. Understanding
minds in real-world environments: toward a mobile cognition approach. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 10, 694.
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial Liu-Ambrose, Teresa, Eng, Janice J., 2015. Exercise training and recreational activities to
interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence promote executive functions in chronic stroke: A proof-of-concept study. J. Stroke
the work reported in this paper. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 24 (1), 130–137.
Malinowski, Peter, Shalamanova, Liliana, 2017. Meditation and cognitive ageing: The
role of mindfulness meditation in building cognitive reserve. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 1 (2),
References 96–106.
Månsson, Kristoffer NT, Skagius Ruiz, Erica, Gervind, Elisabet, Dahlin, Mats,
Alzheimer’s Association. 2020. Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement Andersson, Gerhard, 2013. D Development and initial evaluation of an internet-
J. Alzheimers Assoc. based support system for face-to-face cognitive behavior therapy: A proof of concept
Berk, Lotte, van Boxtel, Martin, van Os, Jim, 2017. Can mindfulness-based interventions study. J. Med. Internet Res. 15 (12), e280. https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.3031.
influence cognitive functioning in older adults? A review and considerations for Marshall, Gad A., Zoller, Amy S., Kelly, Kathleen E., Amariglio, Rebecca E.,
future research. Aging Ment Health. 21 (11), 1113–1120. Locascio, Joseph J., Johnson, Keith A., Sperling, Reisa A., Rentz, Dorene M., 2014.
Berman, Marc G., Jonides, John, Kaplan, Stephen, 2008. The cognitive benefits of Everyday cognition scale items that best discriminate between and predict
interacting with nature. Psychol. Sci. 19 (12), 1207–1212. progression from clinically normal to mild cognitive impairment. Curr. Alzheimer
Bratman, G.N., Daily, G.C., Levy, B.J., Gross, J.J., 2015 Jun. The benefits of nature Res. 11 (9), 853–861.
experience: Improved affect and cognition. Landsc. Urban Plan. 1 (138), 41–50. Mj, K., Ca, D., C W, Mj, S, A E, Me, Z., et al. 2016. Influence of perceived stress on
Bugg, J.M., Jacoby, L.L., Toth, J.P., 2008. Multiple levels of control in the Stroop task. incident amnestic mild cognitive impairment: Results from the Einstein Aging Study.
Mem. Cognit. 36 (8), 1484–1494. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 1;30(2):93–98.
Chiesa, Alberto, Calati, Raffaella, Serretti, Alessandro, 2011. Does mindfulness training Nebes, R.D., Buysse, D.J., Halligan, E.M., Houck, P.R., Monk, T.H., 2009. Self-reported
improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings. sleep quality predicts poor cognitive performance in healthy older adults.
Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31 (3), 449–464. J. Gerontol. Ser. B. 64B (2), 180–187.
Christie, G.J., Hamilton, T., Manor, B.D., Farb, N.A.S., Farzan, F., Sixsmith, A., Peavy, G.M., Jacobson, M.W., Salmon, D.P., Gamst, A.C., Patterson, T.L., Goldman, S.,
Temprado, J.-J., Moreno, S., 2017. Do lifestyle activities protect against cognitive et al., 2012. The influence of chronic stress on dementia-related diagnostic change in
decline in aging? A review. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9, 381. https://doi.org/10.3389/ older adults. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 26 (3), 260–266.
fnagi.2017.00381. Porteus, S.D., Peters, H.N. 1947. Maze test validation and psychosurgery. Genet Psychol
Conroy, D.E., Heartphone, Kim I., 2020. Mobile evaluative conditioning to enhance Monogr. 36:86–86.
affective processes and promote physical activity. Health Psychol. Off. J. Div. Health. Prohaska, T.R., Eisenstein, A.R., Satariano, W.A., Hunter, R., Bayles, C.M., Kurtovich, E.,
Psychol. Am. Psychol. Assoc. Jun 11. et al., 2009. Walking and the preservation of cognitive function in older populations.
Conroy, D.E., West, A.B., Brunke-Reese, D., Thomaz, E., Streeper, N.M., 2020. Just-in- Gerontologist 49 (S1), S86–93.
time adaptive intervention to promote fluid consumption in patients with kidney Reitan, R.M. 1986. Trail Making Test: Manual for Administration and Scoring. Reitan
stones. Health Psychol. 39 (12), 1062–1069. Neuropsychology Laboratory. book.
Czajkowski, Susan M., Powell, Lynda H., Adler, Nancy, Naar-King, Sylvie, Reynolds, Kim Salthouse, T.A., 1996 Jul. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in
D., Hunter, Christine M., Laraia, Barbara, Olster, Deborah H., Perna, Frank M., cognition. Psychol. Rev. 103 (3), 403–428.
Peterson, Janey C., Epel, Elissa, Boyington, Josephine E., Charlson, Mary E., 2015 Salthouse, Timothy A., 2000. Aging and measures of processing speed. Biol. Psychol. 54
Oct. From ideas to efficacy: The ORBIT Model for developing behavioral treatments (1–3), 35–54.
for chronic diseases. Health Psychol. Off. J. Div. Health Psychol. Am. Psychol. Assoc. Sato, N., Morishita, R., 2015. The roles of lipid and glucose metabolism in modulation of
34 (10), 971–982. β-amyloid, tau, and neurodegeneration in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer disease.
Dai, S., Carroll, D.D., Watson, K.B., Paul, P., Carlson, S.A., Fulton, J.E., 2015. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7, 199.
Participation in types of physical activities among US adults—National health and Scarpina, F., Tagini, S. 2017. The Stroop Color and Word Test. Front Psychol [Internet].
nutrition examination survey 1999–2006. J Phys. Act Health 12 (s1), S128–S140. [cited 2020 Apr 26];8. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/artic
Duff, K., Beglinger, L.J., Schultz, S.K., Moser, D.J., McCaffrey, R.J., Haase, R.F., et al. les/PMC5388755/.
2007. Practice effects in the prediction of long-term cognitive outcome in three Scherder, Erik, Scherder, Rogier, Verburgh, Lot, Königs, Marsh, Blom, Marco,
patient samples: A novel prognostic index. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. Off. J. Natl. Kramer, Arthur F., Eggermont, Laura, 2014. Executive functions of sedentary elderly
Acad. Neuropsychol. 2007;22(1):15–24. may benefit from walking: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Am. J. Geriatr.
Erickson, K.I., Hillman, C., Stillman, C.M., Ballard, R.M., Bloodgood, B., Conroy, D.E., Psychiatry. 22 (8), 782–791.
et al. 2019. Physical activity, cognition, and brain outcomes: A review of the 2018 Sliwinski, Martin J., Mogle, Jacqueline A., Hyun, Jinshil, Munoz, Elizabeth,
physical activity guidelines. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 51(6):1242–1251. Smyth, Joshua M., Lipton, Richard B., 2018. Reliability and validity of ambulatory
Farias, Sarah Tomaszewski, Mungas, Dan, Reed, Bruce R., Cahn-Weiner, Deborah, cognitive assessments. Assessment 25 (1), 14–30.
Jagust, William, Baynes, Kathleen, DeCarli, Charles, 2008. The measurement of Sofi, F., Valecchi, D., Bacci, D., Abbate, R., Gensini, G.F., Casini, A., et al., 2011. Physical
everyday cognition (ECog): Scale development and psychometric properties. activity and risk of cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies.
Neuropsychology 22 (4), 531–544. J. Intern. Med. 269 (1), 107–117.
Finkel, D., Reynolds, C.A., McArdle, J.J., Pedersen, N.L. 2007. Age changes in processing Spartano, Nicole L., Davis-Plourde, Kendra L., Himali, Jayandra J., Andersson, Charlotte,
speed as a leading indicator of cognitive aging. Psychol. Aging. 22(3):558–568. Pase, Matthew P., Maillard, Pauline, DeCarli, Charles, Murabito, Joanne M.,
Freedland, Kenneth E., 2020. Pilot trials in health-related behavioral intervention Beiser, Alexa S., Vasan, Ramachandran S., Seshadri, Sudha, 2019. Association of
research: Problems, solutions, and recommendations. Health Psychol. 39 (10), accelerometer-measured light-intensity physical activity with brain volume: The
851–862. Framingham Heart Study. JAMA Netw. Open 2 (4), e192745. https://doi.org/
Fries, J.F., Bruce, B., Cella, D., 2005 Oct. The promise of PROMIS: using item response 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.2745.
theory to improve assessment of patient-reported outcomes. Clin. Exp. Rheumatol. Tanay, G., Bernstein, A., 2013. State Mindfulness Scale (SMS): Development and initial
23 (5 Suppl 39), S53–S57. validation. Psychol. Assess. 25 (4), 1286–1299.
Füzéki, Eszter, Engeroff, Tobias, Banzer, Winfried, 2017. Health benefits of light- Teut, M., Roesner, E.J., Ortiz, M., Reese, F., Binting, S., Roll, S., Fischer, H.F.,
intensity physical activity: A systematic review of accelerometer data of the National Michalsen, A., Willich, S.N., Brinkhaus, B., 2013. Mindful walking in psychologically
Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES). Sports Med. 47 (9), distressed individuals: A randomized controlled trial. Evid. Based Complement
1769–1793. Alternat. Med. 2013, 1–7.
Gard, T., Hölzel, B.K., Lazar, S.W., 2014 Jan. The potential effects of meditation on age- Tulving, Endel, Markowitsch, Hans J, 1997. Memory beyond the hippocampus. Curr.
related cognitive decline: A systematic review. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1307, 89–103. Opin. Neurobiol. 7 (2), 209–216.
Gotink, Rinske A., Hermans, Karlijn S.F.M., Geschwind, Nicole, De Nooij, Reinier, De Valliant, R., Rust, K.F., 2010. Degrees of freedom approximations and rules-of-thumb.
Groot, Wouter T., Speckens, Anne E.M., 2016. Mindfulness and mood stimulate each J. Off. Stat. 26 (4), 585–602.
other in an upward spiral: A mindful walking intervention using experience Venturelli, Massimo, Scarsini, Renato, Schena, Federico, 2011. Six-month walking
sampling. Mindfulness 7 (5), 1114–1122. program changes cognitive and ADL performance in patients with Alzheimer. Am. J.
Kabat-Zinn, J., 1994. Wherever You Go, There You Are: Mindfulness Meditation in Alzheimers Dis. Dementiasr. 26 (5), 381–388.
Everyday Life. Hyperion, New York. Wang, Hui-Xin, Xu, Weili, Pei, Jin-Jing, 2012. Leisure activities, cognition and dementia.
Kabat-Zinn, Jon, 2017. Walking meditations. Mindfulness 8 (1), 249–250. Biochim. Biophys. Acta. BBA Mol. Basis Dis. 1822 (3), 482–491.
Kabat-Zinn, J. 2012. Mindfulness for Beginners: Reclaiming the Present Moment—and Williams, D.M., Matthews, C., Rutt, C., Napolitano, M.A., Marcus, B.H., 2008.
Your Life. Sounds True. Interventions to increase walking behavior. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 40 (7 Suppl),
Kail, Robert, 2000. Speed of information processing: Developmental change and links to S567–S573.
intelligence. J. Sch. Psychol. 38 (1), 51–61. Wong, W.P., Coles, J., Chambers, R., Wu, D.B.-C., Hassed, C., 2017. The effects of
Kail, Robert, Salthouse, Timothy A., 1994. Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta mindfulness on older adults with mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. Rep.
Psychol. Amst. 86 (2–3), 199–225. 1 (1), 181–193.
Kim, C., Johnson, N.F., Gold, B.T., 2014. Conflict adaptation in prefrontal cortex: Now Yang, Chih-Hsiang, Conroy, David E., 2019. Feasibility of an outdoor mindful walking
you see it, now you don’t. Cortex 1 (50), 76–85. program for reducing negative affect in older adults. J. Aging Phys. Act. 27 (1),
18–27.