Part One
Part One
Part One
McNally, R. M. (2012). Are we winning the war against posttraumatic stress dis-
order? Science, 336, 872–874.
Merckelbach, H., Hogervorst, E., Kampman, M., & deJong, A. (1994). Effects of
“eye movement desensitization” on emotional processing in normal subjects.
Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 22, 331–335.
Mitchell, J. T. (1983). When disaster strikes . . . the critical incident stress debrief-
ing process. Journal of Emergency Medical Services, 8, 36–39.
Mitchell, J. T. (1988a). Development and functions of a critical incident stress
debriefing team. Journal of Emergency Medical Services, 13, 42–46.
Mitchell, J. T. (1988b). The history, status, and future of critical incident stress
debriefing. Journal of Emergency Medical Services, 13, 49–52.
Mitchell, J. T., & Everly, G. S., Jr. (1993). Critical incident stress debriefing: An
operations manual. The prevention of traumatic stress among emergency ser-
vices and disaster workers. Ellicott City, MD: Chevron.
Mitchell, J. T., & Everly, G. S., Jr. (1995). Critical incident stress debriefing and
the prevention of work related traumatic stress among high risk occupational
groups. In G. S. Everly Jr. & J. M. Lating (Eds.), Psychotraumatology: Key
papers and core concepts in post traumatic stress (pp. 267–280). New York:
Plenum Press.
Mitchell, J. T., & Everly, G. S., Jr. (1998). Critical incident stress management: A
new era in crisis intervention. Traumatic Stress Points, 12, 6–11.
Nathan, P. E. (2000). The Boulder Model: A dream deferred or lost. American
Psychologist, 55, 250–252.
National Fire Prevention Association. (2012). Standard on fire department occu-
pational safety and health program. Accessed at www.nfpa.org.
National Institute for Clinical Excellence. (2004). Reviewing and grading the evi-
dence (Chapter 7). Guideline development methods. London: Author.
National Institute for Clinical Excellence. (2005). Posttraumatic stress disorder:
The management of PTSD in adults and children in primary and secondary
care (Vol. 26). Trowbridge, Wiltshire, UK: Cromwell Press.
Newbold, K. M., Lohr, J. M., & Gist, R. (2008). Apprehended without warrant:
Issues of evidentiary warrant for critical incident services and related trauma
interventions in a federal law enforcement agency. Criminal Justice and
Behavior, 35, 1337–1353.
Nezu, A. (1986). Efficacy of a social problem-solving therapy approach to unipolar
depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 196–202.
Nezu, A., & Perri, M. G. (1989). Social problem-solving therapy for unipolar
depression: An initial dismantling investigation. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 57, 408–413.
Nishith, P., Nixon, R. D. V., & Resick, P. A. (2005). Resolution of trauma-related
guilt following treatment of PTSD in female rape victims: A result of cogni-
tive processing therapy targeting comorbid depression? Journal of Affective
Disorders, 86, 259–265.
Nishith, R., Resick, P. A., & Griffin, M. G. (2002). Pattern of change in prolonged
exposure and cognitive-processing therapy of female rape victims with post-
traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70,
880–886.
Ohayon, M. M., & Shapiro, C. M. (2000). Sleep disturbances and psychiatric
318 CONTROVERSIES IN THE TREATMENT OF ADULT DISORDERS
Basic principles, protocols, and procedures (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford
Press.
Shapiro, F. (2002). EMDR 12 years after its introduction: Past and future research.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 1–22.
Shapiro, R. (2005). EMDR solutions: Pathways to healing. New York: Norton.
Sharpley, J. G., Fear, N. T., Greenberg, M., Jones, M., & Wessely, S. (2008). Pre-
deployment stress briefing: Does it have an effect? Occupational Medicine,
58, 30–34.
Sinclair, C., Simon, N. P., & Pettifor, J. L. (1996). The history of ethical codes
and licensure. In L. J. Bass, S. T. DeMers, J. R. P. Ogloff, C. Peterson, J. L.
Pettifor, R. P. Reaves, et al. (Eds.), Professional conduct and discipline in psy-
chology (pp. 1–15). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Singer, M., & Lalich, J. (1996). Crazy therapies: What are they? Do they work?
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Solomon, R. M., & Shapiro, F. (2008). EMDR and the adaptive information pro-
cessing model: Potential mechanisms of change. Journal of EMDR Practice
and Research, 2, 315–325.
Spates, C. R., Koch, E., Cusack, K., Pagoto, S., & Waller, S. (2009). Eye movement
desensitization and reprocessing. In E. B. Foa, T. M. Keane, M. J. Friedman, &
J. A. Cohen (Eds.), Effective treatments for PTSD: Practice guidelines from the
International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies. New York: Guilford Press.
Stanovich, K. E. (2012). On the distinction between rationality and intelligence:
Implications for understanding individual differences in reasoning. In K. J.
Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of thinking and
reasoning (pp. 433–455). New York: Oxford University Press.
Steenkamp, M. M., Nash, W. P., & Litz, B. T. (2013). Post-traumatic stress disor-
der: Review of the comprehensive soldier fitness program. American Journal
of Preventive Medicine, 44, 507–512.
Stuart, R. B., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). The evidence missing from evidence-based
practice. American Psychologist, 62, 615–616.
Stuhlmiller, C., & Dunning, C. (2000). Challenging the mainstream: From patho-
genic to salutogenic models of posttrauma intervention. In J. M. Violanti, D.
Paton, & C. Dunning (Eds.), Posttraumatic stress intervention: Challenges,
issues, and perspectives (pp. 10–42). Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
Tallis, F., & Smith, E. (1994). Does rapid eye movement desensitization facilitate
emotional processing? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 459–461.
Tarrier, N., Pilgrim, H., Sommerfield, C., Faragher, B., Reynolds, M., Graham, E.,
et al. (1999a). A randomized trial of cognitive therapy and imaginal exposure
in the treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consult-
ing and Clinical Psychology, 67, 13–18.
Tarrier, N., & Sommerfield, C. (2004). Treatment of chronic PTSD by cognitive
therapy and imaginal exposure: 5-year follow-up. Behavior Therapy, 35,
231–246.
Tarrier, N., Sommerfield, C., Pilgrim, H., & Humphreys, L. (1999b). Cognitive
therapy or imaginal exposure in the treatment of posttraumatic stress disor-
der: Twelve-month follow-up. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 571–575.
Task Force on Promotion and Dissemination Psychological Procedures. (1995).
Training and dissemination of empirically validated treatments. Clinical Psy-
chologist, 48, 3–23.
Trauma-Related Stress Disorders 321
Taylor, S., Thordarson, D. S., Maxfield, L., Fedoroff, I. C., Lovell, K., & Ogrod-
niczuk, J. (2003). Comparative efficacy, speed, and adverse effects of three
PTSD treatments: Exposure therapy, EMDR, and relaxation training. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 330–338.
Tedeschi, R. G., & McNally, R. J. (2011). Can we facilitate posttraumatic growth
in combat veterans. American Psychologist, 66, 19–24.
Tolin, D. F. (2010). Is cognitive-behavioral therapy more effective than other thera-
pies?: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 710–720.
Tryon, W. W. (2005). Possible mechanisms for why desensitization and exposure
therapy work. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 67–95.
van den Hout, M. A., & Englehard, I. M. (2012). How does EMDR work? Journal
of Experimental Psychopathology, 3, 724–728.
van Emmerik, A. A. P., Kamphuis, J. H., Hulsbosch, A. M., & Emmelkamp, P.
M. G. (2002). Single session debriefing after psychological trauma: A meta-
analysis. The Lancet, 360, 776–771.
Varker, T., & Devilly, G. J. (2012). An analogue trail of inoculation/resilience
training for emergency services personnel: Proof of concept. Journal of Anxi-
ety Disorders, 26, 696–701.
Vernberg, E. M., Steinberg, A. M., Jacobs, A. K., Brymer, M. J., Watson, P, Osof-
sky, J. D., et al. (2008). Innovations in disaster mental health. Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice, 39, 381–388.
Wampold, B. E. (2001). The great psychotherapy debate: Models, methods, and
findings. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wilson, D. L., Silver, S. M., Covi, W. G., & Foster, S. (1996). Eye movement desen-
sitization and reprocessing: Effectiveness and autonomic correlates. Journal
of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 27, 219–229.
Wilson, N. (2003). Commercializing mental health issues: Entertainment, advertis-
ing, and psychological advice. In S. O. Lilienfeld, S. J. Lynn, & J. M. Lohr
(Eds.), Science and pseudoscience in clinical psychology (pp. 425–460). New
York: Guilford Press.
World Health Organization. (2006). Single session psychological debriefing: Not
recommended. Retrieved August 1, 2007, from www.who/hac/techguidance/
pht/13643.pdf.
World Health Organization. (2011). Psychological first aid: Guide for field work-
ers. Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
Chapter Eleven
Controversial Treatments
for Alcohol Use Disorders
Introduction
The modern array of treatment options and facilities for alcohol use disor-
ders (AUDs) belies the fact that formal treatment for alcohol problems is
a relatively recent endeavor. Historically, excessive drinking was typically
viewed through a moral lens as a character defect, reflecting weakened
mental capacity and a lack of self-control (Levine, 1978). As a result, soci-
etal responses were primarily in the form of legal sanctions, such as fines or
jail sentences, and intercession by religious leaders for spiritual and moral
restoration. During the 19th and early 20th centuries, grass-roots move-
ments, such as the Washingtonians and, later, Alcoholics Anonymous,
also arose to aid individuals with drinking problems (for a full review, see
White, 1998).
Over the course of the 20th century, treatment for alcohol problems
shifted into the purview of medicine, clinical psychology, and other clinical
disciplines for a number of reasons. Psychology, physiology, pharmacology,
and neuroscience burgeoned as scientific disciplines and offered new theo-
ries for the causes of alcoholism (see Miller & Hester, 1995, for a review
of 13 theoretical perspectives) and novel ways to parse the condition (e.g.,
Jellinek, 1946, 1960). Experimental methods were developed that permit-
ted the systematic study of alcohol consumption under controlled condi-
tions (Lisman, 1974; Mello & Mendelson, 1965), further strengthening the
scientific enterprise. Perhaps the single most important event in advancing
a scientific approach to AUD treatment in the United States, however, was
passage of the Comprehensive Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Prevention,
322
Alcohol Use Disorders 323
Treatment, and Rehabilitation Act in 1970. This act in turn created the
National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) in 1971,
which was in large part responsible for the substantial expansion of AUD
clinical research. Furthermore, it marked the inception of the professional
treatment establishment from which inpatient and outpatient clinics subse-
quently proliferated.
The professional treatment establishment that now exists, however, is
highly heterogeneous and fractious in terms of perspectives on best-prac-
tices. This friction in part stems from the fact that the alcohol treatment
enterprise includes two broad constituencies: treatment providers who have
limited formal training but have personally experienced AUDs (or other
addictive disorders) and are now stably recovered; and treatment provid-
ers who are formally trained but have not personally suffered from these
conditions. For example, in two recent studies of treatment professionals,
approximately half of the treatment providers were successfully recovered
individuals (Curtis & Eby, 2010; Davis & Rosenberg, 2012). A heuristic
distinction that has been made between these groups is that of “craftsmen,”
the informally trained recovered individuals, and “scientists,” the formally
trained clinicians (Kalb & Propper, 1976). The craftsmen are often char-
acterized (or perhaps caricatured) as having strong and uncritical adher-
ence and advocacy of specific approaches, with relatively little interest in
systematic research, whereas the scientists are characterized as being overly
dogmatic and unwilling to consider the merits of treatments that are not
directly supported by empirical evidence. Although recent data suggest that
this distinction is increasingly less black-and-white (Davis & Rosenberg,
2012), the fact remains that professional alcohol treatment continues to
comprise two constituencies with quite different perspectives. Based on the
varying emphasis on research, a wide assortment of treatment approaches
are currently employed, with considerable controversy and highly variable
levels of empirical support.
Reviewing the controversial treatments for AUDs is the first goal of
this chapter. We define treatments as “controversial” if they have become
widely adopted or have generated popular appeal, but have been demon-
strated by controlled research to be of questionable or even nonexistent
efficacy. Equally, other controversial treatments are defined by the inverse:
approaches that have been shown to be effective but remain largely unused
due to practitioner taboos. In other words, controversial treatments are
those that are in use despite the lack of evidence for their efficacy or are
not in use despite evidence for their efficacy. Specifically, the controver-
sial approaches we discuss are confrontational Johnson Institute “inter-
ventions;” Alcoholics Anonymous, and other mutual-help groups; the
prevention program Drug Abuse Resistance Education; and interventions
designed to promote moderate drinking. The second goal of the chapter is
to review evidence-based treatments for AUDs. Increasing the use of treat-
ments that have foundations in behavioral science and are supported by
324 CONTROVERSIES IN THE TREATMENT OF ADULT DISORDERS
Controversial Treatments
The Johnson Intervention
If asked, most members of the general public would almost certainly report
“an intervention” as the most effective strategy for motivating an individ-
ual to seek treatment for an AUD. This refers to a structured confronta-
tion of the individual by family and friends to convince the person to stop
drinking and seek treatment immediately. It was initially developed in the
1970s by Vernon Johnson, an Episcopal priest and founder of an alcohol
treatment facility, the Johnson Institute, giving rise to the strategy’s formal
name, the Johnson Intervention. The format of the Johnson Intervention is
relatively well known. Responding to a false pretext, targeted individuals
are lured to a meeting with the important people in their lives. There, they
are systematically confronted with the negative effects of their drinking (or
drug use) and the consequences that will follow if they elect not to imme-
diately accept the offer of treatment. The putative strategy is to overwhelm
the individuals’ denial, thereby motivating them for treatment, and then
initiate the process (Johnson, 1986; Twerski, 1983).
The Johnson Intervention is not the only form of confrontational inter-
vention used in AUD treatment (White & Miller, 2007), but it has become
the most widespread for a number of reasons. A Johnson Intervention was
the catalyst for former U.S. First Lady Betty Ford (wife of President Gerald
Ford) to seek treatment, bringing it into the popular consciousness. More-
over, because Johnson Interventions have the potential for high drama,
they have increasingly become plot lines on a wide variety of popular tele-
vision programs (e.g., The Sopranos, Party of Five, Beverly Hills 90210).
Indeed, since 2005, an eponymous reality television program, Intervention
(A&E, 2012), has followed over 200 individuals over the course of a John-
son Intervention, winning an Emmy award in 2009.
The proponents of Johnson Interventions make undocumented claims
of very high rates of successful long-term recovery (e.g., 80%; Assistance
in Recovery, 2012). From the perspective of controlled clinical research,
however, the Johnson Intervention is not robustly supported. Liepman,
Nirenberg, and Begin (1989) examined the effects of Johnson Interven-
tion on treatment initiation and reported high rates of entry, but acknowl-
edged several limitations, including very high rates of families who did
Alcohol Use Disorders 325
Alcoholics Anonymous
The gold standard for empirical support for the efficacy of clinical
interventions is the randomized controlled trial (RCT), but relatively few
studies have examined AA in RCTs. Of those that have, the data are not
especially supportive. For example, three randomized trials of AA have
been undertaken involving populations mandated to attend AA and were
not supportive (Alford, 1980; Ditman, 1967; Walsh et al., 1991). Meta-
analyses and systematic reviews of controlled studies have also concluded
that the balance of evidence does not support the efficacy of AA (Ferri,
Amato, & Davoli, 2006; Kownacki & Shadish, 1999).
Superficially, these findings suggest that AA is indeed a pseudoscien-
tific treatment, persisting by virtue of intuitive appeal and strident adherents
despite weak empirical support. However, there are a number of reasons
that this conclusion may not be accurate, and there remains a need to con-
sider additional research evidence. First, as a consumer-led nonprofessional
mutual-help organization, a number of AA’s characteristics distinguish it
from formal psychological or medical interventions. To start, AA does not
purport to be a formal treatment per se (Kelly, 2013) and, although the
AA format is well known, the nature of AA groups and the experiences
an individual has in AA may be highly variable. All that is required for an
AA group is that two or more alcoholics meet together for the purpose of
achieving sobriety (McCrady, Horvath, & Delaney, 2003). As such, none
of the essential features of formal treatment outcome research, like treat-
ment manuals and fidelity checks, can be applied. Random assignment of
individuals to AA does not replicate the typical pathway that leads to par-
ticipation and has the potential to create a negatively predisposed group
of subjects (McCrady et al., 2003). Furthermore, random assignment to
conditions other than AA may be ethically questionable, as it involves pro-
hibiting individuals from participating in a free community resource (Kelly,
2013).
By virtue of these innate characteristics, quasi-experimental and nat-
uralistic studies may be more appropriate for understanding the clinical
utility of AA. Here, the data are persuasive that AA participation is associ-
ated with improved drinking outcomes. For example, in a large naturalis-
tic study on individuals in the Veterans Administration, Ouimette, Moos,
and Finney (1998) found that, relative to individuals who only received
outpatient treatment, patients who attended only AA meetings were more
likely to be abstinent at 1-year follow-up, although, not surprisingly, the
best outcomes were for individuals attending both treatment modalities.
Timko, Moos, Finney, and Lesar (2000) studied the outcome of 248 prob-
lem drinkers who self-selected AA, formal treatment, formal treatment plus
AA, or no treatment at all at 1-year, 3-year, and 8-year follow-up. Those
who chose no treatment fared the worst, and the AA-only and combined
AA/formal treatment groups fared better than the formal treatment group
alone at 1 and 3 years, although not at the 8-year follow-up. A follow-
up at 16 years found that the duration of AA attendance in years 1 to 3
328 CONTROVERSIES IN THE TREATMENT OF ADULT DISORDERS
(Kelly, Hoeppner, Stout, & Pagano, 2012), there was consistent evidence
that adaptive social network changes partially mediated the link between
AA attendance and positive drinking outcomes.
These selection and mechanism findings are important for both obvi-
ous and more subtle reasons. Clearly, knowing client characteristics asso-
ciated with positive AA outcomes can inform clinicians with regard to
which patients may be most appropriate for AA. Similarly, understanding
AA’s mechanisms is important for clinicians to anticipate probable positive
consequences from AA and to orient recommendations toward those pro-
cesses. Furthermore, understanding AA’s mechanisms of behavior change,
and especially the fact that these mechanisms do not appear to be content
specific, may also inform more formal treatments, such as the develop-
ment of interventions to foster more adaptive social networks. Although
the largest by far, it is notable that AA is not the only mutual-help group
for AUDs. Others include Secular Organizations for Sobriety, which shares
similar tenets to AA but has no spiritual dimension; SMART recovery,
which is a support group that includes elements of motivational interview-
ing, cognitive-behavioral therapy, and rational emotive behavior therapy;
and Moderation Management, which focuses on helping people achieve
nonabstinent outcomes. These mechanistic findings suggest that the indi-
viduals with AUDs may benefit from diverse types of mutual-help groups
that commonly enhance self-efficacy, increase coping skills, and nurture
the development of a nondrinking social network.
A final consideration with regard to AA in AUD treatment is logisti-
cal. In a mental health treatment environment that is highly focused on the
costs of services, AA is unarguably a highly cost-effective resource, for it is
free and self-supporting. Moreover, there is increasing evidence that par-
ticipation in AA reduces general health care costs (Humphreys & Moos,
2001, 2007; Mundt, Parthasarathy, Chi, Sterling, & Campbell, 2012). For
example, in a recent longitudinal study of 403 adolescents with AUDs,
mutual-help group attendance following formal treatment was associated
with a 4.7% health care cost reduction during a 7-year follow-up (Mundt
et al., 2012). That translated into a $145 reduction in health care costs for
each meeting attended, primarily from additional treatment costs, hospi-
talizations, and psychiatric follow-up. In addition to being free, a further
advantage is that AA and other mutual-help groups are highly accessible to
the public (e.g., no formal initiation, daily meetings).
To conclude, the role of AA in the science-based AUD treatment enter-
prise has been highly controversial, but it has been increasingly informed
by data. There is weak evidence for the clinical efficacy of AA in terms of
RCTs, but the RCT may mischaracterize the nature of AA and oversimplify
the question at hand. There is consistent evidence that AA involvement
and AA facilitation are associated with positive AUD outcomes. Moreover,
research clarifying the active ingredients of AA offers novel insights that
suggest common process factors are operative and may be applicable to
330 CONTROVERSIES IN THE TREATMENT OF ADULT DISORDERS