Neuropsychology Notes 2
Neuropsychology Notes 2
Neuropsychology Notes 2
History
Neuropsychology is a relatively new discipline within the field of psychology. The first
textbook defining the field, Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology, was initially
published by Kolb and Whishaw in 1980.[3] However, the history of its development can
be traced back to the Third Dynasty in ancient Egypt, perhaps even earlier.[4] There is
much debate as to when societies started considering the functions of different organs.
For many centuries, the brain was thought useless and was often discarded during
burial processes and autopsies. As the field of medicine developed its understanding of
human anatomy and physiology, different theories were developed as to why the body
functioned the way it did. Many times, bodily functions were approached from a religious
point of view, and abnormalities were blamed on bad spirits and the gods. The brain has
not always been considered the center of the functioning body. It has taken hundreds of
years to develop our understanding of the brain and how it affects our behaviors.
Ancient Egypt[edit]
In ancient Egypt, writings on medicine date from the time of the priest Imhotep.[5] They
took a more scientific approach to medicine and disease, describing the brain, trauma,
abnormalities, and remedies for reference for future physicians. Despite this, Egyptians
saw the heart, not the brain, as the seat of the soul.[6]
Aristotle[edit]
Senses, percepti
on, memory, dreams, action in Aristotle's biology. Impressions are stored in the seat of
perception, linked by his Laws of Association (similarity, contrast, and contiguity).
[7]
Aristotle reinforced this focus on the heart which originated in Egypt. He believed the
heart to be in control of mental processes, and looked on the brain, due to its inert
nature, as a mechanism for cooling the heat generated by the heart.[8][9] He drew his
conclusions based on the empirical study of animals. He found that while their brains
were cold to the touch and that such contact did not trigger any movements, the heart
was warm and active, accelerating and slowing dependent on mood.[8][9] Such beliefs
were upheld by many for years to come, persisting through the Middle Ages and the
Renaissance period until they began to falter in the 17th century due to further research.
[9]
The influence of Aristotle in the development of neuropsychology is evident within
language used in modern day, since we "follow our hearts" and "learn by the heart."[9]
Hippocrates
Hippocrates viewed the brain as the seat of the soul. He drew a connection between the
brain and behaviors of the body, writing: "The brain exercises the greatest power in the
man."[10] Apart from moving the focus from the heart as the "seat of the soul" to the brain,
Hippocrates did not go into much detail about its actual functioning. However, by
switching the attention of the medical community to the brain, his theory led to more
scientific discovery of the organ responsible for our behaviors. For years to come,
scientists were inspired to explore the functions of the body and to find concrete
explanations for both normal and abnormal behaviors. Scientific discovery led them to
believe that there were natural and organically occurring reasons to explain various
functions of the body, and it could all be traced back to the brain. Hippocrates
introduced the concept of the mind – which was widely seen as a separate function
apart from the actual brain organ.
René Descartes
Philosopher René Descartes expanded upon this idea and is most widely known for his
work on the mind–body problem. Often Descartes's ideas were looked upon as overly
philosophical and lacking in sufficient scientific foundation. Descartes focused much of
his anatomical experimentation on the brain, paying special attention to the pineal gland
– which he argued was the actual "seat of the soul." Still deeply rooted in a spiritual
outlook towards the scientific world, the body was said to be mortal, and the soul
immortal. The pineal gland was then thought to be the very place at which the mind
would interact with the mortal and machine-like body. At the time, Descartes was
convinced the mind had control over the behaviors of the body (controlling the person) –
but also that the body could have influence over the mind, which is referred to
as dualism.[11] This idea that the mind essentially had control over the body, but the body
could resist or even influence other behaviors, was a major turning point in the way
many physiologists would look at the brain. The capabilities of the mind were observed
to do much more than simply react, but also to be rational and function in organized,
thoughtful ways – much more complex than he thought the animal world to be. These
ideas, although disregarded by many and cast aside for years led the medical
community to expand their own ideas of the brain and begin to understand in new ways
just how intricate the workings of the brain really were, and the complete effects it had
on daily life, as well as which treatments would be the most beneficial to helping those
people living with a dysfunctional mind. The mind–body problem, spurred by René
Descartes, continues to this day with many philosophical arguments both for and
against his ideas. However controversial they were and remain today, the fresh and
well-thought-out perspective Descartes presented has had long-lasting effects on the
various disciplines of medicine, psychology, and much more, especially in putting an
emphasis on separating the mind from the body in order to explain observable
behaviors.
Thomas Willis
It was in the mid-17th century that another major contributor to the field of
neuropsychology emerged. Thomas Willis studied at Oxford University and took a
physiological approach to the brain and behavior. It was Willis who coined the words
'hemisphere' and 'lobe' when referring to the brain.[12] He was one of the earliest to use
the words 'neurology' and 'psychology'. Rejecting the idea that humans were the only
beings capable of rational thought, Willis looked at specialized structures of the brain.
[9]
He theorized that higher structures accounted for complex functions, whereas lower
structures were responsible for functions similar to those seen in other animals,
consisting mostly of reactions and automatic responses.[13] He was particularly interested
in people with manic disorders and hysteria.[14][15] His research constituted some of the
first times that psychiatry and neurology came together to study individuals. Through his
in-depth study of the brain and behavior, Willis concluded that automated responses
such as breathing, heartbeats, and other various motor activities were carried out within
the lower region of the brain. Although much of his work has been made obsolete, his
ideas presented the brain as more complex than previously imagined, and led the way
for future pioneers to understand and build upon his theories, especially when it came to
looking at disorders and dysfunctions in the brain.[14]
Franz Joseph Gall
Neuroanatomist and physiologist Franz Joseph Gall made major progress in
understanding the brain. He theorized that personality was directly related to features
and structures within the brain. However, Gall's major contribution within the field of
neuroscience is his invention of phrenology. This new discipline looked at the brain as
an organ of the mind, where the shape of the skull could ultimately determine one's
intelligence and personality.[16] This theory was like many circulating at the time, as many
scientists were taking into account physical features of the face and body, head size,
anatomical structure, and levels of intelligence; only Gall looked primarily at the brain.
There was much debate over the validity of Gall's claims however, because he was
often found to be wrong in his predictions. He was once sent a cast of René Descartes'
skull, and through his method of phrenology claimed the subject must have had a
limited capacity for reasoning and higher cognition.[17] As controversial and false as many
of Gall's claims were, his contributions to understanding cortical regions of the brain and
localized activity continued to advance understanding of the brain, personality, and
behavior. His work is considered crucial to having laid a firm foundation in the field of
neuropsychology, which would flourish over the next few decades.
Jean-Baptiste Bouillaud
Towards the late 19th century, the belief that the size of ones skull could determine their
level of intelligence was discarded as science and medicine moved forward. A physician
by the name of Jean-Baptiste Bouillaud expanded upon the ideas of Gall and took a
closer look at the idea of distinct cortical regions of the brain each having their own
independent function. Bouillaud was specifically interested in speech and wrote many
publications on the anterior region of the brain being responsible for carrying out the act
of ones speech, a discovery that had stemmed from the research of Gall. He was also
one of the first to use larger samples for research although it took many years for that
method to be accepted. By looking at over a hundred different case studies, Bouillaud
came to discover that it was through different areas of the brain that speech is
completed and understood. By observing people with brain damage, his theory was
made more concrete. Bouillaud, along with many other pioneers of the time made great
advances within the field of neurology, especially when it came to localization of
function. There are many arguable debates as to who deserves the most credit for such
discoveries,[18] and often, people remain unmentioned, but Paul Broca is perhaps one of
the most famous and well known contributors to neuropsychology – often referred to as
"the father" of the discipline.
Paul Broca
Inspired by the advances being made in the area of localized function within the
brain, Paul Broca committed much of his study to the phenomena of how speech is
understood and produced. Through his study, it was discovered and expanded upon
that we articulate via the left hemisphere. Broca's observations and methods are widely
considered to be where neuropsychology really takes form as a recognizable and
respected discipline. Armed with the understanding that specific, independent areas of
the brain are responsible for articulation and understanding of speech, the brains
abilities were finally being acknowledged as the complex and highly intricate organ that
it is. Broca was essentially the first to fully break away from the ideas of phrenology and
delve deeper into a more scientific and psychological view of the brain.[19]
Carl Wernicke
Carl Wernicke was an influential nineteenth century neuropsychiatrist specifically
interested in understanding how abnormalities could be localized to specific brain
regions. Previously held theories attributed brain function as one singular process but
Wernicke was one of the first to attribute brain function to different regions of the brain
based on sensory and motor function.[20] In 1873, Wernicke observed a patient
presenting with poor language comprehension despite maintaining intact speech and
hearing following a severe stroke. Post-morbid analysis revealed a lesion near the
auditory region of the brain in the parietal-temporal region of the left hemisphere.
[21]
Originally named sensory aphasia, this region later became known as Wernicke's
area.[21] Individuals with damage to this area present with fluent but receptive aphasia
characterized by the inability to comprehend or express written or spoken language
while maintaining intact speech and auditory processes.[22] Along with Paul Broca,
Wernicke's contributions greatly expanded the present knowledge of language
development and localization of left hemispheric function.
Karl Spencer Lashley
Lashley's works and theories that follow are summarized in his book Brain Mechanisms
and Intelligence.[23] Lashley's theory of the Engram was the driving force for much of his
research. An engram was believed to be a part of the brain where a specific memory
was stored. He continued to use the training/ablation method that Franz had taught him.
He would train a rat to learn a maze and then use systematic lesions and removed
sections of cortical tissue to see if the rat forgot what it had learned.
Through his research with the rats, he learned that forgetting was dependent on the
amount of tissue removed and not where it was removed from. He called this mass
action and he believed that it was a general rule that governed how brain tissue would
respond, independent of the type of learning. But we know now that mass action was a
misinterpretation of his empirical results, because in order to run a maze the rats
required multiple cortical areas. Cutting into small individual parts alone will not impair
the rats' brains much, but taking large sections removes multiple cortical areas at one
time, affecting various functions such as sight, motor coordination, and memory, making
the animal unable to run a maze properly.[24]
Lashley also proposed that a portion of a functional area could carry out the role of the
entire area, even when the rest of the area has been removed. He called this
phenomenon equipotentiality. We know now that he was seeing evidence of plasticity in
the brain: within certain constraints the brain has the ability for certain areas to take over
the functions of other areas if those areas should fail or be removed - although not to
the extent initially argued by Lashley.
Approaches
Experimental neuropsychology is an approach that uses methods from experimental
psychology to uncover the relationship between the nervous system and cognitive
function. The majority of work involves studying healthy humans in a laboratory setting,
although a minority of researchers may conduct animal experiments. Human work in
this area often takes advantage of specific features of our nervous system (for example
that visual information presented to a specific visual field is preferentially processed by
the cortical hemisphere on the opposite side) to make links between neuroanatomy and
psychological function.[25]
Clinical neuropsychology is the application of neuropsychological knowledge to the
assessment (see neuropsychological test and neuropsychological assessment),
management, and rehabilitation of people who have experienced illness or injury
(particularly to the brain) which has caused neurocognitive problems. In particular they
bring a psychological viewpoint to treatment, to understand how such illness and injury
may affect and be affected by psychological factors.[26] They also can offer an opinion as
to whether a person is demonstrating difficulties due to brain pathology or as a
consequence of an emotional or another (potentially) reversible cause or both. For
example, a test might show that both patients X and Y are unable to name items that
they have been previously exposed to within the past 20 minutes (indicating possible
dementia). If patient Y can name some of them with further prompting (e.g. given a
categorical clue such as being told that the item they could not name is a fruit), this
allows a more specific diagnosis than simply dementia (Y appears to have the vascular
type which is due to brain pathology but is usually at least somewhat reversible).
Clinical neuropsychologists often work in hospital settings in an interdisciplinary medical
team; others work in private practice and may provide expert input into medico-legal
proceedings.[27] Current research into biological science of memory bridges multiple
scales, from the molecular to the neuropsychological (Moscovitch et al.,2016). Memory
needs specific details on the specifics of synaptic dynamism and also requires an
explanation of the comprehension procedures and memory structures having
neurobiological capabilities
Cognitive neuropsychology is a relatively new development and has emerged as a
distillation of the complementary approaches of both experimental and clinical
neuropsychology. It seeks to understand the mind and brain by studying people with
brain injuries or neurological illnesses. One model of neuropsychological functioning is
known as functional localization.[28] This is based on the principle that if a specific
cognitive problem can be found after an injury to a specific area of the brain, it is
possible that this part of the brain is in some way involved. However, there may be
reason to believe that the link between mental functions and neural regions is not so
simple. An alternative model of the link between mind and brain, such as parallel
processing, may have more explanatory power for the workings and dysfunction of the
human brain. Yet another approach investigates how the pattern of errors produced by
brain-damaged individuals can constrain our understanding of mental representations
and processes without reference to the underlying neural structure. A more recent but
related approach is cognitive neuropsychiatry which seeks to understand the normal
function of mind and brain by studying psychiatric or mental illness.[29]
Connectionism is the use of artificial neural networks to model specific cognitive
processes using what are considered to be simplified but plausible models of how
neurons operate. Once trained to perform a specific cognitive task these networks are
often damaged or 'lesioned' to simulate brain injury or impairment in an attempt to
understand and compare the results to the effects of brain injury in humans.[30]
Functional neuroimaging uses specific neuroimaging technologies to take readings from
the brain, usually when a person is doing a particular task, in an attempt to understand
how the activation of particular brain areas is related to the task. In particular, the
growth of methodologies to employ cognitive testing within established functional
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) techniques to study brain-behavior relations is
having a notable influence on neuropsychological research.[31]
In practice these approaches are not mutually exclusive and most neuropsychologists
select the best approach or approaches for the task to be completed.
References
1. ^ Gluck MA, Mercado E, Myers CE (2016). Learning and Memory: From Brain
to Behavior. New York/NY, US: Worth Publishers. p. 57. ISBN 978-1-319-
15405-9.
2. ^ Posner MI, DiGirolamo GJ (November 2000). "Cognitive neuroscience:
origins and promise". Psychological Bulletin. 126 (6): 873–
889. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.6.873. PMID 11107880.
3. ^ "The Great Canadian Psychology Website - Researchers". University of
Calgary. Retrieved 14 August 2017.
4. ^ Finger S (2000). Minds Behind the Brain: A History of the Pioneers and their
discoveries. New York: Oxford. pp. 22. ISBN 978-0-19-518182-1.
5. ^ Highfield R. "How Imhotep gave us medicine". The Daily
Telegraph. Archived from the original on 2022-01-12. Retrieved 24
March 2018.
6. ^ Carus P (1905). "The Conception of the Soul and the Belief in Resurrection
Among the Egyptians". The Monist. 15 (3): 409–
428. doi:10.5840/monist190515326. JSTOR 27899609.
7. ^ Warren H (1921). "A History of the Association
Psychology". Nature. 110 (2750): 19–30, 259,
296. Bibcode:1922Natur.110S..75.. doi:10.1038/110075d0. hdl:2027/chi.6541
3836. S2CID 4115902.
8. ^ Jump up to:a b "History of Neuropsychology | BRAIN". brainaacn.org. 31
January 2016. Retrieved 2018-09-25.
9. ^ Jump up to:a b c d e Benton AL, Sivan AB (March 2007). "Clinical
neuropsychology: a brief history". Disease-a-Month. 53 (3): 142–
147. doi:10.1016/j.disamonth.2007.04.003. PMID 17544643.
10. ^ Finger 2000, p. 44
11. ^ Finger 2000, p. 92
12. ^ Finger S (1994). "History of Neuropsychology". Neuropsychology. Elsevier.
pp. 1–28. doi:10.1016/b978-0-08-092668-1.50007-7. ISBN 978-0-08-092668-
1. S2CID 140943975.
13. ^ Finger S (2005-03-03). "Thomas Willis: The Functional Organization of the
Brain". Minds Behind the Brain. Oxford University Press. pp. 85–
100. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195181821.003.0007. ISBN 978-0-08-
092668-1.
14. ^ Jump up to:a b Arráez-Aybar LA, Navia-Álvarez P, Fuentes-Redondo T,
Bueno-López JL (March 2015). "Thomas Willis, a pioneer in translational
research in anatomy (on the 350th anniversary of Cerebri anatome)". Journal
of Anatomy. 226 (3): 289–
300. doi:10.1111/joa.12273. PMC 4337668. PMID 25688933.
15. ^ Eadie MJ (March 2003). "A pathology of the animal spirits -- the clinical
neurology of Thomas Willis (1621-1675). Part II -- disorders of intrinsically
abnormal animal spirits". Journal of Clinical Neuroscience. 10 (2): 146–
157. doi:10.1016/S0967-5868(02)00164-9. PMID 12637040. S2CID 3236567
2.
16. ^ Benton A (2000). History of Neuropsychology: Selected Papers. US: Oxford.
17. ^ Finger 2000, p. 151
18. ^ Viney W (2003). A History of Psychology: Ideas and Context (3rd ed.).
Boston: Pearson.
19. ^ Cubelli R, De Bastiani P (February 2011). "150 years after Leborgne: why is
Paul Broca so important in the history of neuropsychology?". Cortex; A Journal
Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 47 (2): 146–
147. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.11.004. PMID 21112584. S2CID 45646738.
20. ^ "Carl Wernicke | Encyclopedia.com". www.encyclopedia.com.
Retrieved 2022-04-21.
21. ^ Jump up to:a b Javed, Kinaan; Reddy, Vamsi; M Das, Joe; Wroten, Michael
(2022), "Neuroanatomy, Wernicke Area", StatPearls, Treasure Island (FL):
StatPearls Publishing, PMID 30422593, retrieved 2022-04-21
22. ^ Lanczik, M.; Keil, G. (June 1991). "Carl Wernicke's localization theory and its
significance for the development of scientific psychiatry". History of
Psychiatry. 2 (6): 171–180. doi:10.1177/0957154X9100200604. ISSN 0957-
154X. PMID 11613217. S2CID 20908696.
23. ^ Carmichael L (May 1959). "Karl Spencer Lashley, experimental
psychologist". Science. 129 (3360): 1410–
1412. Bibcode:1959Sci...129.1410C. doi:10.1126/science.129.3360.1410. PM
ID 13658968.
24. ^ "Learning and Memory (Section 4, Chapter 7)". Neuroscience Online: An
Electronic Textbook for the Neurosciences. Retrieved 15 March 2022. {{cite
book}}: |work= ignored (help)
25. ^ "What is Experimental Neuropsychology?". www.allpsychologycareers.com.
Retrieved 2018-09-25.
26. ^ "Clinical and experimental neuropsychology". UKEssays. Retrieved 2018-
09-25.
27. ^ Cohen D (2008). "Neuropsychology". Dr Doug Cohen.
28. ^ Stebbins GT (2007). "Neuropsychological Testing". Textbook of Clinical
Neurology. Elsevier. pp. 539–557. doi:10.1016/b978-141603618-0.10027-
x. ISBN 978-1-4160-3618-0.
29. ^ Hall J, O'Carroll RE, Frith CD (2010). "Neuropsychology". Companion to
Psychiatric Studies (8th ed.). Elsevier. pp. 121–140. doi:10.1016/b978-0-
7020-3137-3.00007-3. ISBN 978-0-7020-3137-3.
30. ^ Garson J (2018). "Connectionism". In Zalta EN (ed.). The Stanford
Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2018 ed.). Metaphysics Research Lab,
Stanford University. Retrieved 2018-09-25.
31. ^ Roalf DR, Gur RC (November 2017). "Functional brain imaging in
neuropsychology over the past 25 years". Neuropsychology. 31 (8): 954–
971. doi:10.1037/neu0000426. PMC 5822440. PMID 29376672.
32. ^ Boyle GJ, Saklofske DH, Matthews G, eds. (2012). SAGE Benchmarks in
Psychology. Vol. 3: Psychological Assessment. London: SAGE. ISBN 978-0-
85702-270-7.
33. ^ Sherman, Elisabeth (May 6, 2020). "Multidimensional Malingering Criteria for
Neuropsychological Assessment: A 20-Year Update of the Malingered
Neuropsychological Dysfunction Criteria". Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology. 35 (6): 735–
764. doi:10.1093/arclin/acaa019. PMC 7452950. PMID 32377667.
34. ^ Etcoff, L. M.; Kampfer, K. M. (December 1996). "Practical guidelines in the
use of symptom validity and other psychological tests to measure malingering
and symptom exaggeration in traumatic brain injury cases". Neuropsychology
Review. 6 (4): 171–201. doi:10.1007/BF01874896. ISSN 1040-
7308. PMID 9159771. S2CID 11046587.
35. ^ Alexander AL, Lee JE, Lazar M, Field AS (July 2007). "Diffusion tensor
imaging of the brain". Neurotherapeutics. 4 (3): 316–
329. doi:10.1016/j.nurt.2007.05.011. PMC 2041910. PMID 17599699.
36. ^ Wei W, Wang XJ (December 2016). "Inhibitory Control in the Cortico-Basal
Ganglia-Thalamocortical Loop: Complex Regulation and Interplay with Memory
and Decision Processes". Neuron. 92 (5): 1093–
1105. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.031. PMC 5193098. PMID 27866799.
37. ^ Bauer RM, Iverson GL, Cernich AN, Binder LM, Ruff RM, Naugle RI (May
2012). "Computerized neuropsychological assessment devices: joint position
paper of the American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology and the National
Academy of Neuropsychology". Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 27 (3):
362–373. doi:10.1093/arclin/acs027. PMC 3499090. PMID 22382386.