Neo 71
Neo 71
Neo 71
Data from diverse sources indicate that the Late Cretaceous (100 to 65 million years ago)
climate was milder than today's because of shallow seas covering the continents. At the
end of the Cretaceous, the geological record shows that these seaways retreated from the
continents back into the major ocean basins. No one knows why. Over a period of about
100,000 years, while the seas pulled back, climates around the world became dramatically
more extreme: warmer days, cooler nights; hotter summers, colder winters. Perhaps
nonavian dinosaurs (that is, all the dinosaurs except those belonging to the Ave or bird
family) could not tolerate these extreme temperature changes and became extinct.
If true, though, why did cold-blooded animals, such as snakes, lizards, turtles, and
crocodiles survive the freezing winters and torrid summers? Those animals are at the
mercy of the climate to maintain a livable body temperature. It's hard to understand why
they would not be affected, whereas nonavian dinosaurs were left too crippled to cope,
especially if some dinosaurs were warm-blooded. Critics also point out that the shallow
seaways had retreated from and advanced on the continents numerous times during the
Mesozoic. So why did the nonavian dinosaurs survive the climatic changes associated with
the earlier fluctuations but not with this one? Although initially appealing, the hypothesis of
a simple climatic change related to sea levels is insufficient to explain all the data.
Volcanism has also been implicated in dinosaur extinction. The end of the Cretaceous
coincided with a great increase in volcanic activity throughout the world. Lava flooded large
areas of India, and explosive eruptions in the South Atlantic and the midwestern United
States hurled ash over much of the globe. These eruptions could have spewed great
quantities of poisonous gases into the atmosphere, causing acid rain and more acidic
waters in the surface layers of the ocean. Over the short term, a cooling would result from
the airborne volcanic ash, which would cut off sunlight. Climatic changes from increased
volcanism may have caused the extinction of many nonavian dinosaurs, but they do not
satisfactorily explain the selective patterns of extinction in the fossil record.
Iridium is no longer common on Earth's surface. Because it usually exists in a metallic state,
it was preferentially incorporated into Earth's core as the planet cooled and consolidated.
Iridium is found in high concentrations in some meteorites, in which the solar system's
original chemical composition is preserved. Even today, microscopic meteorites continually
bombard Earth, falling on both land and sea. By measuring how many of these meteorites
fall to Earth over a given period of time, scientists can estimate how long it might have
taken to deposit the observed amount of iridium in the boundary clay. These calculations
suggest that a period of about one million years would have been required. On the basis of
other evidence, however, scientists know the deposition of the boundary clay could not
have lasted one million years, so the unusually high concentration of iridium seemed to
require a special explanation.
Data from diverse sources indicate that the Late Cretaceous (100 to 65 million years ago)
climate was milder than today's because of shallow seas covering the continents. At the
end of the Cretaceous, the geological record shows that these seaways retreated from the
continents back into the major ocean basins. No one knows why. Over a period of about
100,000 years, while the seas pulled back, climates around the world became dramatically
more extreme: warmer days, cooler nights; hotter summers, colder winters. Perhaps
nonavian dinosaurs (that is, all the dinosaurs except those belonging to the Ave or bird
family) could not tolerate these extreme temperature changes and became extinct.
climate conditions that were more extreme than today's climate
a reduction in the size of the major ocean basins around the world
temperatures that probably made it more difficult for some dinosaurs to survive
cooler summer temperatures and warmer winter temperatures
2. In paragraph 2, why does the author bring up the fact that snakes, lizards,
turtles, and crocodiles all survived extinction when nonavian dinosaurs did not?
If true, though, why did cold-blooded animals, such as snakes, lizards, turtles, and
crocodiles survive the freezing winters and torrid summers? Those animals are at the
mercy of the climate to maintain a livable body temperature. It's hard to understand why
they would not be affected, whereas nonavian dinosaurs were left too crippled to cope,
especially if some dinosaurs were warm-blooded. Critics also point out that the shallow
seaways had retreated from and advanced on the continents numerous times during the
Mesozoic. So why did the nonavian dinosaurs survive the climatic changes associated with
the earlier fluctuations but not with this one? Although initially appealing, the hypothesis of
a simple climatic change related to sea levels is insufficient to explain all the data.
Volcanism has also been implicated in dinosaur extinction. The end of the Cretaceous
coincided with a great increase in volcanic activity throughout the world. Lava flooded large
areas of India, and explosive eruptions in the South Atlantic and the midwestern United
States hurled ash over much of the globe. These eruptions could have spewed great
quantities of poisonous gases into the atmosphere, causing acid rain and more acidic
waters in the surface layers of the ocean. Over the short term, a cooling would result from
the airborne volcanic ash, which would cut off sunlight. Climatic changes from increased
volcanism may have caused the extinction of many nonavian dinosaurs, but they do not
satisfactorily explain the selective patterns of extinction in the fossil record.
Energetic
Scientific
Continuous
Productive
Iridium is no longer common on Earth's surface. Because it usually exists in a metallic state,
it was preferentially incorporated into Earth's core as the planet cooled and consolidated.
Iridium is found in high concentrations in some meteorites, in which the solar system's
original chemical composition is preserved. Even today, microscopic meteorites continually
bombard Earth, falling on both land and sea. By measuring how many of these meteorites
fall to Earth over a given period of time, scientists can estimate how long it might have
taken to deposit the observed amount of iridium in the boundary clay. These calculations
suggest that a period of about one million years would have been required. On the basis of
other evidence, however, scientists know the deposition of the boundary clay could not
have lasted one million years, so the unusually high concentration of iridium seemed to
require a special explanation.
It was present at the time the solar system originally formed.
There is very little iridium found in Earth's core.
Iridium on meteorites is rarely in its metallic state.
At high levels, iridium is toxic enough to have caused mass extinctions.
Iridium is no longer common on Earth's surface. Because it usually exists in a metallic state,
it was preferentially incorporated into Earth's core as the planet cooled and consolidated.
Iridium is found in high concentrations in some meteorites, in which the solar system's
original chemical composition is preserved. Even today, microscopic meteorites continually
bombard Earth, falling on both land and sea. By measuring how many of these meteorites
fall to Earth over a given period of time, scientists can estimate how long it might have
taken to deposit the observed amount of iridium in the boundary clay. These calculations
suggest that a period of about one million years would have been required. On the basis of
other evidence, however, scientists know the deposition of the boundary clay could not
have lasted one million years, so the unusually high concentration of iridium seemed to
require a special explanation.
meteorites had stopped fling to Earth while the boundary clay was being deposited
the thin bed of boundary clay was formed over a period of about one million years
the thin bed of boundary clay layer was older than they previously thought
the iridium in the boundary clay could not have been deposited through the usual
9. Look at the four squares [■] that indicate where the following sentence could be
added to the passage.
These large bodies of water kept the temperature of the nearby air relatively
constant from day to night, summer to winter.
Data from diverse sources indicate that the Late Cretaceous (100 to 65 million years ago)
climate was milder than today's because of shallow seas covering the continents.■ At the
end of the Cretaceous, the geological record shows that these seaways retreated from the
continents back into the major ocean basins.■ No one knows why.■ Over a period of about
100,000 years, while the seas pulled back, climates around the world became dramatically
more extreme: warmer days, cooler nights; hotter summers, colder winters.■ Perhaps
nonavian dinosaurs (that is, all the dinosaurs except those belonging to the Ave or bird
family) could not tolerate these extreme temperature changes and became extinct.
10. Direction: An introductory sentence for a brief summary of the passage is provided
below. Complete the summary by selecting the THREE answer choices that express the
most important ideas in the passage. Some sentences do not belong in the summary
because they express ideas that are not presented in the passage or are minor ideas in the
passage. This question is worth 2 points.
The nonavian dinosaurs became extinct at the end of the Cretaceous period, and
scientists have proposed several theories to explain why.
A. There is evidence that calls into question the hypothesis that the seas retreated from the
continents at the end of the Cretaceous period.
B. Increased volcanic activity led to acid rain, a short-term cooling of the atmosphere, and
other harmful effects, but it does not explain the selective patterns of extinction.
D. Loss of continental seas made the climate more extreme, but some cold- blooded
animals survived the change and the seas had retreated before without massive
extinctions.
F. Evidence of volcanic activity during the Cenozoic period supports the theory that Earth
was being struck by meteorites when the extinctions happened.
Infant-Directed Speech
Infant-directed speech is a style of speech directed toward infants. This type of speech
pattern was previously called motherese because it was assumed that it applied only to
mothers. However, that assumption was wrong, and the gender-neutral term
infant-directed speech is now used more frequently.
Infant-directed speech changes as children become older. Around the end of the first year,
infant-directed speech takes on more adultlike qualities. Sentences become longer and
more complex, although individual words are still spoken slowly and deliberately. Pitch is
also used to focus attention on particularly important words.
Is infant-directed speech similar across cultures? In some respects, it clearly is. According to
a growing body of research, there are basic similarities across cultures in the nature of
infant-directed speech. Consider, for instance, a comparison of the major characteristics of
speech directed at infants used by native speakers of English and Spanish. Of the ten most
frequent features, six are common to both: exaggerated intonation, high pitch, lengthened
vowels, repetition, lower volume, and instructional emphasis (that is, heavy stress on
certain key words, such as emphasizing the word "ball" in the sentence, "No, that's not a
ball."). Similarly, mothers in the United States, Sweden, and Russia all exaggerate and
elongate the pronunciation of the three vowel sounds of "ee," "ah," and "oh" when
speaking to infants in similar ways, despite differences in the languages in which the
sounds are used. Even deaf mothers use a form of infant-directed speech: When
communicating with their infants, deaf mothers use sign language at a significantly slower
tempo than when communicating with adults, and they frequently repeat the signs.
The cross-cultural similarities in infant-directed speech are so great, in fact, that they
appear in some facets of language specific to particular types of interactions. For instance,
evidence comparing American English, German, and Mandarin Chinese speakers shows
that in each of the languages, pitch rises when a mother is attempting to get an infant's
attention or produce a response, while pitch falls when she is trying to calm an infant. Why
do we find such similarities across very different languages? One hypothesis is that the
characteristics of infant-directed speech activate innate responses in infants. As we have
noted, infants seem to prefer infant-directed speech over adult-directed speech, suggesting
that their perceptual systems may be more responsive to such characteristics. Another
explanation is that infant-directed speech facilitates language development, providing cues
as to the meaning of speech before infants have developed the capacity to understand the
meanings of words.
Despite the similarities in the style of infant-directed speech across diverse cultures, there
are some important cultural differences in the quantity of speech that infants hear from
their parents. For example, although the Gusii of Kenya care for their infants in an
extremely close, physical way, they speak to them less than American parents do.
Infant-directed speech is a style of speech directed toward infants. This type of speech
pattern was previously called motherese because it was assumed that it applied only to
mothers. However, that assumption was wrong, and the gender-neutral term
infant-directed speech is now used more frequently.
simple sentences
frequent shifts in intonation
saying the same words several times
lower pitch
Infant-directed speech changes as children become older. Around the end of the first year,
infant-directed speech takes on more adultlike qualities. Sentences become longer and
more complex, although individual words are still spoken slowly and deliberately. Pitch is
also used to focus attention on particularly important words.
4. Which of the sentences below best expresses the essential information in the
highlighted sentence in the passage? Incorrect choices change the meaning in
important ways or leave out essential information.
Infant-directed speech plays an important role in infants' acquisition of language.
Newborns respond to such speech more readily than they do to regular language, a
fact that suggests that they may be particularly receptive to it. Furthermore, some
research suggests that unusually extensive exposure to infant-directed speech early
in life is related to the comparatively early appearance of first words and earlier
linguistic competence in other areas.
Is infant-directed speech similar across cultures? In some respects, it clearly is. According to
a growing body of research, there are basic similarities across cultures in the nature of
infant-directed speech. Consider, for instance, a comparison of the major characteristics of
speech directed at infants used by native speakers of English and Spanish. Of the ten most
frequent features, six are common to both: exaggerated intonation, high pitch, lengthened
vowels, repetition, lower volume, and instructional emphasis (that is, heavy stress on
certain key words, such as emphasizing the word "ball" in the sentence, "No, that's not a
ball."). Similarly, mothers in the United States, Sweden, and Russia all exaggerate and
elongate the pronunciation of the three vowel sounds of "ee," "ah," and "oh" when
speaking to infants in similar ways, despite differences in the languages in which the
sounds are used. Even deaf mothers use a form of infant-directed speech: When
communicating with their infants, deaf mothers use sign language at a significantly slower
tempo than when communicating with adults, and they frequently repeat the signs.
Moderate intonation
Repeated words and phrases
Speaking more quietly
Emphasis on key words
6. In paragraph 5, why does the author discuss deaf mothers in connection with
infant-directed speech?
Is infant-directed speech similar across cultures? In some respects, it clearly is. According to
a growing body of research, there are basic similarities across cultures in the nature of
infant-directed speech. Consider, for instance, a comparison of the major characteristics of
speech directed at infants used by native speakers of English and Spanish. Of the ten most
frequent features, six are common to both: exaggerated intonation, high pitch, lengthened
vowels, repetition, lower volume, and instructional emphasis (that is, heavy stress on
certain key words, such as emphasizing the word "ball" in the sentence, "No, that's not a
ball."). Similarly, mothers in the United States, Sweden, and Russia all exaggerate and
elongate the pronunciation of the three vowel sounds of "ee," "ah," and "oh" when
speaking to infants in similar ways, despite differences in the languages in which the
sounds are used. Even deaf mothers use a form of infant-directed speech: When
communicating with their infants, deaf mothers use sign language at a significantly slower
tempo than when communicating with adults, and they frequently repeat the signs.
The cross-cultural similarities in infant-directed speech are so great, in fact, that they
appear in some facets of language specific to particular types of interactions. For instance,
evidence comparing American English, German, and Mandarin Chinese speakers shows
that in each of the languages, pitch rises when a mother is attempting to get an infant's
attention or produce a response, while pitch falls when she is trying to calm an infant. Why
do we find such similarities across very different languages? One hypothesis is that the
characteristics of infant-directed speech activate innate responses in infants. As we have
noted, infants seem to prefer infant-directed speech over adult-directed speech, suggesting
that their perceptual systems may be more responsive to such characteristics. Another
explanation is that infant-directed speech facilitates language development, providing cues
as to the meaning of speech before infants have developed the capacity to understand the
meanings of words.
aspects
adaptations
rules
combinations
8. Paragraph 6 supports which of the following ideas about how infants respond
to infant-directed speech?
The cross-cultural similarities in infant-directed speech are so great, in fact, that they
appear in some facets of language specific to particular types of interactions. For instance,
evidence comparing American English, German, and Mandarin Chinese speakers shows
that in each of the languages, pitch rises when a mother is attempting to get an infant's
attention or produce a response, while pitch falls when she is trying to calm an infant. Why
do we find such similarities across very different languages? One hypothesis is that the
characteristics of infant-directed speech activate innate responses in infants. As we have
noted, infants seem to prefer infant-directed speech over adult-directed speech, suggesting
that their perceptual systems may be more responsive to such characteristics. Another
explanation is that infant-directed speech facilitates language development, providing cues
as to the meaning of speech before infants have developed the capacity to understand the
meanings of words.
Infants may not respond to exaggerated pitch if it does not match the situation.
Infants may respond more readily to speech adapted to their perceptual systems.
Infants may respond only when they can attach conceptual meaning to the words
they hear.
Infants may not respond if the nature of the interaction does not require it.
9. Look at the four squares [■] that indicate where the following sentence could be
added to the passage.
In particular, there are very interesting accounts of special ways words are spoken to
infants and they have been documented among quite distinct languages.
Is infant-directed speech similar across cultures? In some respects, it clearly is. According to
a growing body of research, there are basic similarities across cultures in the nature of
infant-directed speech.■ Consider, for instance, a comparison of the major characteristics
of speech directed at infants used by native speakers of English and Spanish.■ Of the ten
most frequent features, six are common to both: exaggerated intonation, high pitch,
lengthened vowels, repetition, lower volume, and instructional emphasis (that is, heavy
stress on certain key words, such as emphasizing the word "ball" in the sentence, "No,
that's not a ball."). ■ Similarly, mothers in the United States, Sweden, and Russia all
exaggerate and elongate the pronunciation of the three vowel sounds of "ee," "ah," and
"oh" when speaking to infants in similar ways, despite differences in the languages in which
the sounds are used ■. Even deaf mothers use a form of infant-directed speech: When
communicating with their infants, deaf mothers use sign language at a significantly slower
tempo than when communicating with adults, and they frequently repeat the signs.
10. Direction: An introductory sentence for a brief summary of the passage is provided
below. Complete the summary by selecting the THREE answer choices that express the
most important ideas in the passage. Some sentences do not belong in the summary
because they express ideas that are not presented in the passage or are minor ideas in the
passage. This question is worth 2 points.
When adults speak to infants, they typically employ a special type of speech pattern
technically known as "infant-directed speech."
B. There are many different features of infant-directed speech, but six of them occur across
all language groups.
D. There are striking cross-cultural similarities both in the way adults speak to infants and
in how certain linguistic features are associated with situations.
E. Infant-directed speech may stimulate innate responses in infants or contribute to
language development by providing cues to the meaning of Speech.
F. While infant-directed speech is found in all cultures, cultures vary in the amount of
speech infants hear that is not specifically directed to them.
Answers:
1. D
2. D
3. A
4. A
5. A
6. D
7. D
8. C
9. A
10. BCD
11. B
12. D
13. A
14. D
15. A
16. A
17. A
18. B
19. A
20. ADE