Self Absorption 1
Self Absorption 1
Self Absorption 1
Pacific University
CommonKnowledge
School of Professional Psychology Theses, Dissertations and Capstone Projects
7-1-2013
Recommended Citation
Sasiain, Michael (2013). Mindfulness and self-absorption: Examining the relationship between non-elaborative attention and shifts in
rigidity of self-consciousness (Master's thesis, Pacific University). Retrieved from:
http://commons.pacificu.edu/spp/470
This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Theses, Dissertations and Capstone Projects at CommonKnowledge. It has been accepted
for inclusion in School of Professional Psychology by an authorized administrator of CommonKnowledge. For more information, please contact
CommonKnowledge@pacificu.edu.
Mindfulness and self-absorption: Examining the relationship between
non-elaborative attention and shifts in rigidity of self-consciousness
Abstract
While researchers have explored the relationship between mindfulness and self-consciousness, none have
investigated the relationship between mindfulness and self-absorption. This study examined the relationship
between mindfulness and private and public self-absorption. The sample consisted of 224 individuals
anonymously recruited from various locations. We found a negative relationship between mindfulness and
both private and public self-absorption. We did not find any significant differences between the number of
years that participants had been practicing mindfulness on the one hand and the relation of mindfulness to
self-absorption on the other. Implications for psychopathology, attentional flexibility, and the processing of
information with reference to the self are discussed.
Degree Type
Thesis
Rights
Terms of use for work posted in CommonKnowledge.
Comments
Library Use: LIH
A THESIS
OF
PACIFIC UNIVERSITY
HILLSBORO, OREGON
BY
MICHAEL SASIAIN
OF
JULY, 2013
APPROVED:
While researchers have explored the relationship between mindfulness and self-
consciousness, none have investigated the relationship between mindfulness and self-absorption.
This study examined the relationship between mindfulness and private and public self-
absorption. The sample consisted of 224 individuals anonymously recruited from various
locations. We found a negative relationship between mindfulness and both private and public
self-absorption. We did not find any significant differences between the number of years that
participants had been practicing mindfulness on the one hand and the relation of mindfulness to
self-absorption on the other. Implications for psychopathology, attentional flexibility, and the
ii
Acknowledgments
I would like to express my appreciation to Dr. James Lane for his patience, guidance, and
shared passion for acceptance-based interventions and open-minded views. I would also like to
thank my father and mother for their unending support of my spiritual aspirations, physical well-
being, and existential dreams. And finally, I wish to thank my partner, Angelica, and friends who
iii
Table of Contents
Page
ABSTRACT.................................................................................................................................. ii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS.......................................................................................................... iii
LIST OF TABLES..........................................................................................................................v
INTRODUCTION.......................................................................................................................... 1
MINDFULNESS............................................................................................................................ 1
Definitions of Mindfulness................................................................................................. 2
Emotional, Psychological, and Physiological Benefits of Mindfulness............................. 4
Emotional Benefits…...........................................................................................................4
Psychological Benefits….................................................................................................... 5
Physiological Benefits…..................................................................................................... 6
SELF-CONSCIOUSNESS............................................................................................................. 6
Private Self-Consciousness................................................................................................. 7
Public Self-Consciousness.................................................................................................. 7
Mechanisms of Self-Consciousness.................................................................................... 8
Schemas and Self-Focused Attention….................................................................. 9
Self-Consciousness as Self-Reference…................................................................ 9
SELF-ABSORPTION................................................................................................................... 12
PURPOSE AND HYPOTHESES OF STUDY............................................................................. 13
METHOD..................................................................................................................................... 15
RESULTS...................................................................................................................................... 17
DISCUSSION............................................................................................................................... 21
REFERENCES............................................................................................................................. 25
APPENDICES.............................................................................................................................. 31
iv
List of Tables
Page
TABLE 1: Descriptive Statistics for the MAAS and the Private and Public subscales of the
Self-Absorption Scale...................................................................................................19
TABLE 2: Frequencies, Pearson Correlations, and Values of Statistical Significance for
Participants Categorized by Years of Mindfulness Practice.........................................20
TABLE 3: Z-scores for the Correlations Between Groups……………........................................21
v
Introduction
In the present study I investigated the relationship between mindfulness and self-
absorption. Mindfulness refers to a deliberate process in which individuals are open to and focus
conceptual elaboration on their experience (Grossman, 2011; Brown & Ryan, 2003). Self-
absorption refers to a maladaptive self-focus that is excessive, sustained, and inflexible in its
his or her attention inwardly or outwardly with reference to the self (Trapnell & Campbell, 1999;
Fenigstein, 2009).
While a number of researchers have examined the relationship between mindfulness and
self-consciousness (Creswell, Way, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2007; Brown & Ryan, 2003;
Teasdale & Green, 2004; Walach et al., 2006; Evans, Baer, & Segerstrom, 2009) none have yet
explored the link between mindfulness and self-absorption. In the sections below I present and
review the literature on each construct and provide a justification for the possible relationship
Mindfulness
gained acceptance within the field of clinical psychology as both an intervention and a domain of
research. Since its debut into western psychology, results of a vast number of studies have
rumination on negative thoughts (Ma & Teasdale, 2004; Teasdale et al., 2000), decreased
1
reactivity to negative emotions (Hill & Updegraff, 2012), an increased ability to tolerate physical
pain (Perlman, Salomons, Davidson, & Lutz, 2010; Wong et al., 2011), and a reduction of daily
western psychology. In the following section I will therefore review three operational definitions
Definitions of Mindfulness
Bishop et al. (2004) theorize that mindfulness emerges from a reciprocal interplay
between the (1) self-regulation of attention and a (2) particular attitude towards one's experience.
To be more precise, the self-regulation of attention, also known as decentering, consists of three
sub-components: (a) a sustained attention to, and thereby awareness of, one's moment-to-
moment internal and external experience, (b) the ability to intentionally switch one's attention
from one object to another in a flexible manner, and (c) an inhibition of elaborative processing
that naturally emerges from the focus of attention on the process of sensations, thoughts, and
subjective experiences, and the objects found therein, with curiosity, acceptance, and openness.
that underlie one's psychological processes or external behavior (Lau et al., 2006). In addition,
mindfulness is regarded as being a mode of awareness that can be generated and sustained
through the regulation of one's attention. Thus, mindfulness is a dispositional skill that can be
exercised and strengthened and is not exclusive to a particular form of contemplative practice.
2
In a second conceptualization, Brown and Ryan (2003) suggest that mindfulness may be
mindfulness is an awareness that (a) is open and receptive to experience, (b) is undivided while
attending to internal and external stimuli, and (c) emphasizes the quality of consciousness over
the content of thoughts, feelings, and sensations. Furthermore, mindful awareness is related to
consciousness that does not conceptualize itself. In contrast, mindlessness may be construed as a
compulsive or automatic behaviors that occur in the absence of full awareness and the
an unpleasant experience.
A third perspective has been proposed by Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, and Toney
(2006) who view mindfulness as being composed of five facets: (a) observing, (b) describing, (c)
acting with awareness, (d) accepting without judgment, and (e) non-reactivity to inner
external phenomena, such as bodily sensations, sights, and sounds. The second skill of
describing refers to an individual’s recognition and labeling of emotions, thoughts, and bodily
sensations. The third skill, acting with awareness, arises when a person allocates his or her full
attention to an activity which may be contrasted with being distracted while engaging in a task.
The fourth skill, accepting without judgment, refers to an individual’s attitude in which he or she
does not evaluate thoughts or feelings as being either good or bad. And last, non-reactivity to
inner experiences refers to individuals allowing thoughts and feelings to be fully experienced
3
Emotional, Psychological, and Physiological Benefits of Mindfulness
Emotional Benefits
Higher levels of mindfulness have been linked with an increased ability to label and
differentiate among emotions without judgment (Creswell, Baldwin, Way, Eisenberger, &
Lieberman, 2007; van den Hurk et al, 2011; Hill & Updegraff, 2012), decrease in emotional
reactivity (Hill & Updegraff, 2012), and an increase psychological well-being and adjustment
(Josefsson, Larsman, Broberg, & Lundh, 2011). With regard to depression, mindfulness practice
has also been shown to significantly decrease maladaptive rumination in individuals with a
history of major depression (Teasdale et al., 2000; Ma & Teasdale, 2004) while likewise
decreasing the chronic retrieval of episodic (Alberts & Thewissen, 2011) and autobiographical
memories (Williams, Teasdale, Segal, & Soulsby, 2000) associated with negative emotions.
Mindfulness can boost emotional resiliency and working memory capacity in socially and
physically demanding environments. For instance, after recruiting a sample of U.S. Marine Corp
Reservists and civilians contracted with the U.S. military, Jha, Stanley, Kiyonaga, Wong, and
Gelfand (2010) assigned participants to one of three groups: the military control group (MC), the
civilian control group (CC), and the military training group (MT). Members of the MT group
underwent 24-hrs of formal instruction in mindfulness over an 8-week period and were asked to
log the number of hours spent meditating outside of mindfulness training sessions. Conversely,
4
participants in the MC and CC groups were not provided with any training or interventions.
In order to assess the efficacy of mindfulness training, the investigators compared the
working memory capacity (WMC) of participants before and after predeployment – a period of
intense, specialized combat training preceding military deployment. The results show that (a) the
WMC of the CC group remained unchanged, (b) the WMC of the MC group decreased, (c) the
WMC of participants in the MT group with low practice hours decreased, and (d) the WMC of
participants in the MT group with high practice hours increased. Furthermore, the number of
hours spent practicing mindfulness was negatively correlated with reports of negative emotions.
The authors concluded that by increasing the working memory capacity of individuals,
mindfulness training may be effective in protecting people from the impact of negative affect
Psychological Benefits
Higher levels of mindfulness have also been linked with reduced frequencies of and
internal reactions to repetitive thoughts (Feldman, Greeson, & Senville, 2010) and distressing
emotions (Josefsson, Larsman, Broberg, & Lundh, 2011), an increased ability to cognitively
2011), and a decreased experience of analgesic pain via the skills of observation and non-
likewise correlated greater degrees of mindfulness with decreased resting activity in both the
amygdala and areas that process information with reference to the self (Way, Creswell,
Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2010), increased cortical activity in the prefrontal cortex (Creswell,
Baldwin, Way, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2007; Chiesa, Brambilla, & Setterri, 2010), and
5
increased activity in the anterior cingulate cortex and neural areas related to attention (Chiesa,
Physiological Benefits
And last, with regard to health higher levels of mindfulness have been associated with
decreased heart rates and blood pressure (Chiesa, Brambilla, & Setterri, 2010), reduced negative
emotions, depressive symptoms, and smoking behaviors in individuals with nicotine dependence
(Rogojanski, Vettese, & Antony, 2011), decreased frequency of substance abuse in prison
inmates (Bowen et al., 2006), and increased self-efficacy and engagement with health-oriented
behaviors, such as a physical activity and the consumption of fruits and vegetables (Gilbert &
Waltz, 2010).
Self-Consciousness
either toward thoughts, feelings, and sensations that reference the intrapersonal aspects of the
self, “I'm aware of the way my mind works when I work through a problem”, or the interpersonal
aspects of the self, “I usually worry about making a good impression.” It is a construct for
assessing motivational differences between individuals (Fenigstein, Scheier, & Bus, 1975;
Fenigstein, 2009). Originally conceived as a unitary construct, factor analysis has instead shown
consciousness and public self-consciousness (Fenigstein et al., 1975), the operations of which
6
Private Self-Consciousness
and cognitions that are oriented towards the self. In addition, the authors theorize that
individuals who are oriented towards private self-consciousness are motivated by an intrinsic
need to understand themselves (Fenigstein et al., 1975) and tend to attribute the cause of their
behavior to internal factors over external conditions (Fenigstein, 2009). Other researchers have
found that such individuals exhibit a greater congruence between implicit and explicit social
The literature on self-consciousness also suggests that individuals who score high (versus
low) in the domain of private self-consciousness are more reliable in providing self-reports
across an extended period of time (Nasby,1989b), are less likely to exaggerate positive traits
when describing themselves to others with the aim of appearing confident or self-reliant
(Lalwani, Shrum, & Chiu, 2009), and may rely on social norms to guide the appropriateness of
self-disclosures when meeting a person for the first time (Shaffer & Tomareli, 1989). It is also
interesting to note that sorority members who present with an elevated private self-consciousness
exhibit a higher probability of abusing alcohol when compared to their peers while, conversely,
the likelihood of a fraternity member abusing alcohol decreases as their private self-
Public Self-Consciousness
Public self-consciousness is the habitual focus on the self as a social object and
corresponding 'outward' aspects of the self that are directly accessible to others. Put differently,
7
phenomena that present within a social context such as one's physical appearance, spoken and
written words, gestures, and actions assume the central focus within this type of self-
consciousness. Thus, Fenigstein (2009) theorized that individuals with a dispositional public
self-consciousness monitor the reactions of others to themselves, are keenly aware of the
impressions that they convey to others, and are invested in gaining and maintaining interpersonal
relationships.
The literature on self-consciousness suggests that individuals who are oriented towards a
high (versus low) public self-consciousness tend to be sensitive to rejection by peers and are less
likely to affiliate with said group following social rejection (Fenigstein et al., 1975), may
describe their actions in an inflated, positive light in order to minimize or avoid negative
judgments by others (Lalwani et al., 2009), and reciprocate self-disclosures when meeting a
person for the first time with the aim of fostering a positive social image (Shaffer & Tomareli,
correlated with a self-referential cognitive bias in which individuals assume that the perspective
of others mirror their own (Fenigstein & Abrams, 1993) and/or tend to perceive themselves as a
Mechanisms of Self-Consciousness
Several theories accounting for the individual differences in cognition and behavior
between private and public self-consciousness have been proposed. In the section below I
briefly review two theories, namely self-consciousness as (a) self-focused attention and as (b) an
8
Schemas and Self-Focused Attention
Schemas are cognitive frameworks that encode, represent, and retrieve information with
toward stimuli, schemas associated with the stimulus become primed which then, in turn,
facilitate the assimilation of information (e.g. sensations, thoughts, emotions, memories, etc.)
with similar information derived from the past. Furthermore, once a schema activates, relevant
even ignored within an individual's conscious experience. For example, reading in one's native
language requires considerably less effort than reading in a language that one has recently begun
to acquire.
network of schematic associations become activated which facilitate the encoding and retrieval
decisions for specific behaviors by comparing the incoming information with internal models of
action. Carver (1979) suggests that the varying models of actions and their associated content
may account for the differential behaviors associated with private and public self-consciousness.
As such, individuals with high self-consciousness are characterized by a high focus of attention
on themselves while individuals with low self-consciousness focus a low degree of attention on
themselves (Carver & Scheier, 1978). Researchers have likewise suggested that individuals with
high private self-consciousness can be distinguished from individuals with high public self-
consciousness by the aspects of themselves that they have articulated and represented
Support for the hypothesis of self-focused attention has been generated by researchers
9
examining the relationship between information processing and self-consciousness (Nasby,
1989b; Sneed & Whitbourne, 2003; Teasdale & Green, 2004; Silvia, Eichstaedt, & Phillips,
2005; Gendolla, Abele, Andrei, Spurk, & Richter, 2005). For instance, Nasby (1989a) utilized a
recognition task to determine whether self-schemas would interfere with the ability to accurately
recollect information. The investigators presented two lists to participants: List A contained
adjectives relating to personal traits while List B contained a mixture of words that were
identical to and different from those found in the first list. After participants read List A the
investigators asked participants to survey List B and identify whether words were 'new' or 'old.'
The results indicate that individuals high (versus low) in private or public self-consciousness
engage in significantly more false alarms by mistakenly identifying new words as old.
Moreover, the traits associated with the false alarms corresponded with the type of self-
consciousness of the participant. Therefore, the investigators concluded that private and public
self-consciousness correspond with schemas that articulate and represent different aspects of the
self.
Self-Consciousness as Self-Reference
relevance to the self (Hull & Levy, 1979; Hull, Slone, Meteyer, & Matthews, 2002). Put
differently, self-consciousness is nothing less than the total organization and encoding of
information that is referenced to the self. Thus, the information that is selected, encoded, and
retrieved is not only obtained from explicit contexts, such as thoughts, feelings, sensations, and
interpersonal situations, but may likewise be derived from implicit environmental cues whose
10
recognition operate below the threshold of consciousness (Fejfar & Hoyle, 2000; Hull et al.,
For example, in a series of experiments Hull et al. (2002) investigated whether subliminal
visual cues could influence the performance of individuals high in private self-consciousness. In
the first and second study, participants completed a scrambled sentence task containing words
associated with old age such as “Florida”, “bingo”, “retired”, “wrinkle”, and so on. Participants
who scored high in private self-consciousness walked significantly slower than their peers
'failure' group and asked to complete 100 trials of a lexical decision task in which strings of
letters were judged as being either words or non-words. In addition, the word “success” was
flashed to success group while “failure” was flashed to the failure group 17ms before every trial.
The results indicate that participants high in private self-consciousness in the success group
completed the task the fastest while those in the failure group were the slowest. The speed of
participants with low private self-consciousness, regardless of the direction in which they were
primed, was in the middle. In a final set of experiments participants completed a lexical decision
task similar to the one described above but instead utilizing the priming words of “angry” and
“relax.” Furthermore, the heart rates of participants were then measured after completing the
task. The results indicate that participants high in private self-consciousness in the angry group
exhibited the fastest heart rate while those in the relax group presented with the lowest. In
contrast, the heart rates of participants with low private self-consciousness, regardless of the
direction in which they were primed, were in the middle. The authors concluded that self-
consciousness does not require conscious processes, such as attention, to activate associated
11
schemata. Moreover, because the schemata that were elicited appeared to be incongruent with
the appearance of participants, such as being of a relatively young age yet walking slower
because an 'elder' schema was activated, it was also concluded that self-consciousness is not self-
Self-Absorption
being diagnosed with a wide spectrum of disorders, namely depression, social and generalized
anxiety, schizophrenia or alcohol abuse along with the probability of presenting with test anxiety,
account for this finding Ingram (1990) developed a theory of attention and information
Attention is constituted by three parameters: direction, duration, and flexibility. The first
emotions, or bodily sensations or outwardly towards the environment. In this case an adaptive
awareness consists of a fluid, alternating balance between the internal and external domains. The
second parameter, duration, corresponds with the length of time that attention is sustained in an
internal or external direction. And third, the authors hypothesized that humans have a limited
reserve of cognitive resources that we can allocate to tasks but can overcome this restriction in
our ability to switch attention between stimuli. Thus, flexibility refers to the degree to which an
individual can switch the direction of his or her attention in response to a stimulus or
psychological intention.
12
Self-absorption, then, refers to a maladaptive self-focus in which attention is excessive in
direction, sustained in duration, and inflexible in its alternation between internal and external
foci. Hence, Ingram (1990) theorized that self-absorption may lead to maladjustment in
with dysfunctional content are elicited when attention is excessively focused on the self. Once
elicited, the dysfunctional schemas of individuals remain primed for an extended period of time
thereby facilitating the articulation of these maladaptive schemas through the automatic selection
of relevant information. And last, because individuals are unable to switch their attention from
their sense of self and associated schemas the process of self-absorption becomes locked into a
mindfulness and self-absorption. With regard to mindfulness I utilized the operational definition
associated with the mindfulness awareness attention scale (MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003),
namely that mindful awareness (a) is open and receptive to experience, (b) is undivided while
attending to internal and external stimuli, and (c) emphasizes the quality of consciousness over
the content of thoughts, feelings, and sensations. Self-absorption, on the other hand, refers to a
maladaptive self-focus that is excessive, sustained, and inflexible in its operation. Moreover, it
should be noted that the self-absorption scale (SAS; McKenzie & Hoyle, 2008), a measure that I
used in this study, separates self-absorption into a private and public domain analogous to the
Previous researchers have not provided direct evidence for or against the link between
13
mindfulness and self-absorption. However, theoretically speaking it appears that the constructs
of mindfulness and self-absorption can be bridged through their mutual emphasis on (a) the role
of attention in the processing of information and (b) the role, or lack thereof, of the self as a point
of reference for incoming information. Furthermore, in considering the literature of private self-
consciousness the constructs of internal state awareness (ISA) and self-reflectiveness (SR) seem
motivation to understand his or her self through conceptual elaboration. Research has
demonstrated positive associations of ISA with an adaptive attention on the self and SR with a
maladaptive attention on the self (Burnkrant & Page, 1984; Watson, Morris, Ramsey, Hickman,
& Waddell, 1996). For instance, Watson et al. (1996) found ISA to be negatively, and SR
positively, correlated with shame, guilt, social anxiety, private and public self-consciousness, and
a preoccupation with the thoughts of others regarding oneself. Furthermore, in developing the
MAAS Brown and Ryan (2003) found mindfulness to be positively correlated with ISA and
negatively with SR. In contrast, in developing the SAS McKenzie and Hoyle (2008) found
private and public self-absorption to be positively correlated with SR and negatively with ISA.
As a final consideration, research indicates that individuals with a high ISA are resistant to
behavioral priming effects while individuals with a high SR are more likely to enact particular
behaviors when relevant schemas are environmentally primed (Wheeler et al., 2008).
With these considerations in mind I proposed two hypotheses for the present study. First,
mindfulness will negatively correlate with both public and private self-absorption. And second,
as the number of years that individuals have been practicing mindfulness increases, mindfulness
14
will also increase while private and public self-absorption will decrease. Should I obtain a
significant statistical relationship between mindfulness and self-absorption then the results may
Method
Participants
participate in the study. Additional details are provided in the Procedures section below.
Measures
The Mindfulness Awareness Attention Scale (MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003) is a 15-item
questionnaire that measures mindfulness associated with everyday life. Responses are scored on
a 6-point Likert scale ranging from “almost always” to “almost never.” Items tap into
mindlessness, for instance “It seems I'm “running on automatic” without much awareness of
what I'm doing,” and therefore, higher scores on the MAAS reflect a greater degree of
mindfulness. The MAAS has good psychometric properties with an alpha coefficient of .81 and
excellent test-retest reliability ranging between .80 and .87 with a month delay between
administrations. The MAAS also has adequate discriminant and convergent validity and
correlates positively with measures of positive mood and other mindfulness measures and
negatively with measures of anxiety, neuroticism, and negative affect (Appendix B).
15
The Self-Absorption Scale
The Self-Absorption Scale (SAS; McKenzie & Hoyle, 2008) is a 17-item questionnaire
that measures the degree to which individuals are self-absorbed in the domains of public and
private self-consciousness. Responses are scored on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “not at
all like me” to “very much like me.” Higher scores on the SAS indicate higher degrees of self-
absorption. For example, sample items include “Sometimes I am so deep in thought about my
life I am not aware of my surroundings” and “It upsets me when people I meet don’t like me.”
The SAS has good psychometric properties with an alpha coefficient of .81 and .89 for private
and public self-absorption, respectively, and test-retest of .60 for private and .73 for public self-
absorption with a 7 week delay between administrations. Internal consistency estimates were
A full set of loadings for confirmatory factor analysis were found to be significant (p <
.001) with an average loading of .58 for items measuring private self-absorption and .66 for
items measuring public self-absorption. The inter-factor correlation was .67. The SAS has good
discriminant and convergent validity with private self-absorption correlating positively with
measures of rumination and depressive symptoms and negatively with measures of self-esteem
and self-efficacy and pubic self-absorption correlating positively with measures of social anxiety
and negatively with measures of extraversion and social desirability (McKenzie & Hoyle, 2008).
It should likewise be noted that McKenzie and Hoyle (2008) found that women (M =
1.74) on average presented with significantly higher scores than men (M = 1.66) on private self-
absorption. However, the difference between men and women was small (effect size r = .07).
No significant difference between men and women was found in the domain of public self-
16
Procedures
A web-based internet survey, hosted on surveygizmo.com, was used to collect the data.
Approval from Pacific University's Institutional Review Board approved the research prior to
collected. An invitation for participation was electronically sent to the Laughing Buddha
Sangha, the University Buddhist Association of UCLA, the mindfulness-based stress reduction
program associated with Yoga Hillsboro, and various online locations (e.g., Facebook, Reddit,
etc.) (Appendix D). Participants read and agreed to an informed consent before completing the
age or older to participate. Experience with mindfulness was not a requisite for participation.
Results
Of the 277 who responded to my invitation for participation, 226 completed the full
battery of measures. In addition, the data associated with 2 participants were identified as
outliers and were consequently omitted from the final analysis (refer to the Preliminary Analysis
section for additional details). Thus, the data used in this study was comprised by a sample of
224 participants. Of this sample, 82 (36.6%) self-identified as male, 141 (63%) as female, and 1
(0.4%) as other. The age of participants ranged from 18 to 69 years, with a mean age of 41.4
years (SD = 12.3 years). The distribution of self-reported racial identity was 84.4% Caucasian,
4% Asian, 1.3% American Indian/Alaskan Native, 0.5% as Hawaiian Native/Pacific Islander, and
education completed was 0.5% some high school, 4% high school diploma, 21.9% some college
17
or technical school, 8.9% 2-year degree, 19.2% 4-year degree, 10.7% some graduate school,
28.1% master's degree, and 6.7% doctoral degree. The participants had the following self-
reported length of mindfulness practice: 6.6% had never meditated or practiced mindfulness,
27% had been practicing for less than one year, 20.4% had one to two years of experience, 12%
had two to four years of experience, 11.5% had four to six years’ experience, and 22.6% had
Preliminary Analysis
Prior to analyzing the data, I examined each variable’s compliance with assumptions of
parametric statistical analysis. Each measure was inspected with reference to mean, standard
deviation, distribution of scores, skewness, and kurtosis. Upon inspection of the skewness and
kurtosis of private self-absorption both variables were found to exceed the absolute value of 1.
The data of two participants, in relation to private self-absorption, were found to exceed the z-
score of 3.29 and were therefore removed from the total data set.
Mindfulness scores, D(224) = 0.05, p = .20, were normal. Private self-absorption, D(224)
= 0.12, p < .05, and public self-absorption, D(224) = 0.11, p < .05, scores were both significantly
non-normal. Given the large sample size employed in this study violations of normality are to be
expected and should therefore not adversely impact the results. Table 1 provides an overview of
the descriptive statistics for the MAAS and the SAS as divided into the domains of private and
public self-absorption.
18
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics for the MAAS and the Private and Public subscales of the Self-Absorption
Scale
Confidence Intervals
Main Analysis
relationship between mindfulness, on the one hand, and private and public self-absorption on the
other. In line with the first hypothesis moderate, negative correlations were obtained between
mindfulness and private self-absorption, r = -.33, p < .01, and public self-absorption, r = -.39, p <
.01. Thus, higher levels of mindfulness corresponded with lower levels of both private and
public self-absorption.
I also assessed the relationship between mindfulness scores, self-absorption scores, and
the number of years that participants had been practicing mindfulness. Based on participants'
self-reports of the number of years that they had been practicing mindfulness I assigned
participants to one of five groups: (a) no experience with mindfulness practice, (b) 2 years or less
practice, or (e) 7 or more years of mindfulness practice. Correlations between MAAS scores and
SAS scores were then generated for each group; private and public self-absorption scores, on this
note, were combined into a composite score in order for independent sample correlations to be
19
obtained for each group. And last, I tested the correlations to determine whether a significant
difference existed between the groups. No significant differences were found thereby providing
support against my second hypothesis. The frequencies, Pearson correlations, and values of
significance for each independent group are presented in Table 2. The z-scores and values of
significance for the differences between each group are presented in Table 3.
Table 2
Frequencies, Pearson Correlations, and Values of Statistical Significance for Participants
Categorized by Years of Mindfulness Practice
Number of Years Practicing Mindfulness n r p
No Experience 15 -.30 .28
2 years or less 107 -.39 .00*
2 to 4 years 26 -.08 .71
4 to 6 years 26 -.52 .01*
7 or more years 50 -.42 .00*
Note. *p < .01 (2-tailed), n = Number of participants in each group, r = Pearson correlation
coefficient.
20
Table 3
Z-scores for the Correlations Between Groups (with Values of Statistical Significance in
Parentheses)
Years Practicing
Mindfulness 1 2 3 4 5
1. No Experience --- .324 (.75) -.649 (.52) .760 (.45) .434 (.66)
Discussion
The purpose of this thesis was two-fold: (a) to determine whether a relationship exists
between mindfulness and self-absorption and (b) to determine the number of years that
individuals have been practicing mindfulness influences the relationship between mindfulness
and self-absorption. Commensurate with the aims of this study I formulated two hypotheses:
First, that mindfulness would be negatively correlated with both public and private self-
absorption. And second, that the negative correlation between mindfulness and self-absorption
would become stronger as the number of years that individuals had been practicing mindfulness
increased.
With regard to the first hypothesis, a significant negative relationship between mindfulness
and private and public self-absorption was obtained. This finding suggests that higher levels of
mindfulness correspond with an increased ability for individuals to voluntarily switch their
attention between internal thoughts, feelings, and bodily sensations increases as well.
21
Conversely, lower levels of mindfulness correspond with a lower degree of cognitive flexibility.
With consideration to Ingram's (1990) theory of self-absorption this finding suggests that given
their strong attentional flexibility, individuals who are high in mindfulness are more resistant to
schizophrenia, and alcohol abuse, than their peers with a lower level of mindfulness. Indeed,
psychological robustness (Goldin & Gross, 2010; Jha et al., 2010; Greco, Baer, & Smith, 2011).
Thus, the hypothesis that increases in mindfulness may lead to increases in cognitive flexibility
In addition, the confirmation of the first hypothesis likewise suggests that people with high
(versus low) levels of mindfulness are less likely to employ their self as a schematic point of
reference when processing information. This notion is congruous with previous findings that
greater degrees of mindfulness correspond with decreases in the encoding and retrieval of
autobiographical memories (Williams et al., 2000) and resting activity in neurological areas that
process information with reference to the self (Way et al., 2010). Moreover, in considering the
field, it is plausible that individuals with higher levels of mindfulness may present with lower
levels of private and public self-consciousness and consequently exhibit a greater ability for
social desirability, and be more resilient to implicit environmental primes that are associated with
self-schemas.
With regard to the second hypothesis, the lack of a significant difference between the
22
groups was surprising. To elaborate, research has demonstrated a positive relationship between
mindfulness and the amount of time that individuals invest into practicing mindfulness (Jha et
al., 2010; Josefsson et al., 2011). In addition, given the evidence supporting the first hypothesis
it still appears logical that as mindfulness increased with the years that individuals spent
However, several methodological limitations, which will be discussed below, may have
contributed to this result. Therefore, while the time spent practicing mindfulness may truly bear
no relation to the relationship between mindfulness and self-absorption this interpretation should
The first limitation of this study is that self-absorption is a construct that corresponds with
abnormalities of attentional focus and psychopathological disorders. Thus, given that the sample
was not drawn from a clinical population the range of scores on private and public self-
absorption may not represent the full possible range of self-absorption. A second limitation is
that although participants were asked for the number of years that they had been practicing
mindfulness, I failed to inquire into (a) the average time of formal mindfulness practice spent per
day and (b) whether the number of years reported were contiguous, on the one hand, or separated
In conclusion, this is the first study to explore the relationship between mindfulness and
self-absorption and provide a nomological network that can bridge these two distinct constructs.
Future research can investigate the efficacy of mindfulness to reduce psychological disorders
associated with self-absorption. Furthermore, the theoretical link between mindfulness and self-
absorption, and self-consciousness by extension, provides evidence for the role that the self as an
epistemic point of reference may play in the effectiveness of mindfulness and the susceptibility
23
of individuals to certain psychological disorders.
24
25
References
Alberts, H., & Thewissen, R. (2011). The effect of a brief mindfulness intervention on memory
for positively and negatively valenced stimuli. Mindfulness, 2, 73–77. doi:
10.1007/s12671-011-0044-7
Baer, R.A., Smith, G.T., Hopkins, J, Krietemeyer, J, & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report
assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13(1), 27–45. doi:
10.1177/1073191105283504
Bishop, S.R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N.D., Carmody, J., Segal, Z.V., Abbey,
S., Speca, M., Velting, D., & Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed Operational
Definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), 230–241. doi:
10.1093/clipsy.bph077
Bowen, S., Witkiewitz, K., Dillworth, T.M., Chawla, N., Simpson, T.L., Ostafin, B.D., Larimer,
M.E., Blume, A.W., Parks, G.A., & Marlatt, G.A. (2006). Mindfulness meditation and
substance use in an incarcerated population. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20(3),
343–347. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.20.3.343
Brown, K.W., & Ryan, R.M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in
psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
Burnkrant, R.E., & Page, T.J. (1984). A modification of the Fenigstein, Scheier, and Buss self-
consciousness scales. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48(6), 629–637. doi:
10.1207/s15327752jpa4806_10
Carver, C.S. (1979). A cybernetic model of self-attention processes. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 37(8), 1251–1281. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.37.8.1251
Carver, C.S., & Scheier, M.F. (1978). Self-focusing effects of dispositional self-consciousness,
mirror presence, and audience presence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
36(3), 324–332. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.36.3.324
Chiesa, A., Brambilla, P., & Serretti, A. (2010). Functional neural correlates of mindfulness
meditations in comparison with psychotherapy, pharmacotherapy and placebo effect. Is
there a link? Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 22(3), 104–117. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-
5215.2010.00460.x
Creswell, J.D., Way, B.M., Eisenberger, N.I. & Lieberman, M.D. (2007). Neural correlates of
dispositional mindfulness during affect labeling. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69(6), 560–
565. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3180f6171f
26
Evans, S.R., Baer, R.A., & Segerstrom, S.C. (2009). The effects of mindfulness and self-
consciousness on persistence. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(4), 379–382.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.03.026
Fejfar, M.C., & Hoyle, R.H. (2000). Effect of private self-awareness on negative affect and self-
referent attribution: A quantitative review. Personality and Social Psychology Review,
4(2), 132–142. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0402_02
Feldman, G., Greeson, J., & Senville, J. (2010). Differential effects of mindful breathing,
progressive muscle relaxation, and loving-kindness meditation on decentering and
negative reactions to repetitive thoughts. Behavior Therapy and Research, 48(10), 1002–
1011. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.006
Fenigstein, A. (2009). Private and public self-consciousness. In Leary, M.R. & Hoyle, R.H.
(Eds.) (Ed.), Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior (pp. 495–511). NY:
Guilford Press.
Fenigstein, A., & Abrams, D. (1993). Self-attention and the egocentric assumption of shared
perspectives. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29(4), 287–303. doi:
10.1006/jesp.1993.1013
Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M.F., & Buss, A.H. (1975). Public and Private Self-Consciousness:
Assessment and Theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43(4), 522–527.
doi: 10.1037/h0076760
Garland, E.L., Gaylord, S.A., & Fredrickson, B.L. (2011). Positive reappraisal mediates the
stress-reductive effects of mindfulness: An upward spiral process. Mindfulness,
2(1), 59–67. doi: 10.1007/s12671-011-0043-8
Gendolla, G.H., Abele, A.E., Andrei, A., Spurk, D., & Richter, M. (2005). Negative mood, self-
focused attention, and the experience of physical symptoms: The joint impact hypothesis.
Emotion, 5(2), 131–144. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.2.131
Gilbert, D., & Waltz, J. (2010). Mindfulness and health behaviors. Mindfulness, 1(4), 227–234.
doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12671-010-0032-3
Goldin, P.R., & Gross, J.J. (2010). Effects of Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) on
emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder. Emotion, 10(1), 83–91. doi:
10.1037/a0018441
27
Grant, J.A., & Rainville, P. (2009) Pain sensitivity and analgesic effects of mindful states in zen
meditators: A cross-sectional study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 71(1), 106–114. doi:
10.1097/PSY.0b013e31818f52ee
Greco, L.A., Baer, R.A., & Smith, G.T. (2011). Assessing mindfulness in children and
adolescents: Development and validation of the Child and Adolescent Mindfulness
Measure (CAMI). Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 606–614. doi: 10.1037/a0022819
Grossman, P., & van Dam, N.T. (2011). Mindfulness, by any other name…: Trials and
tribulations of sati in western psychology and science. Contemporary Buddhism, 12(1),
219-239. doi: 10.1080/14639947.2011.564841
Gschwedner, T., Hofmann, W., & Schmitt, M. (2006). Synergistic moderator effects of situation
and person factors of awareness and adjustment on the consistency of implicit and
explicit attitudes. Journal of Individual Differences, 27(1), 47-56. doi: 10.1027/1614-
0001.27.1.47
Hill, C.L.M., & Updegraff, J.A. (2012). Mindfulness and its relationship to emotional regulation.
Emotion, 12(1), 81–90. doi: 10.1037/a0026355
Hull, J.G., & Levy, A.S. (1979). The organizational functions of the self: An alternative to the
Duval and Wicklund model of self-awareness. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 37(5), 756–768. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.37.5.756
Hull, J.G., Slone, L.B., Meteyer, K.B., & Matthews, A.R. (2002). The nonconsciousness of self-
consciousness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 406–424. doi:
10.1037//0022-3514.83.2.406
Ingram, R.E. (1990). Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: Review and a conceptual
model. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 156–176. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.107.2.156
Jha, A.P., Stanley, E.A., Kiyonaga, A., Wong, L., & Gelfand, L. (2010). Examining the protective
effects of mindfulness training on working memory capacity and affective experience.
Emotion, 10(1), 54–64. doi: 10.1037/a0018438
Josefsson, T., Larsman, P., Broberg, A.G., & Lundh, L.G. (2011). Self-reported mindfulness
mediates the relation between meditation experience and psychological well-being.
Mindfulness, 2(1), 49–58. doi: 10.1007/s12671-011-0042-9
Lalwani, A.K., Shrum, L.J., & Chiu, C. (2009). Motivated response styles: The role of cultural
values, regulatory focus, and self-consciousness in socially desirable responding. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(4), 870–882. doi: 10.1037/a0014622
Lau, M.A., Bishop, S.R., Segal, Z.V., Buis, T., Anderson, N.D., Carlson, L., Shapiro, S., &
28
Carmody, J. (2006). The Toronto mindfulness scale: Development and validation. Journal
of Clinical Psychology, 62(12), 1445–1487. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20326
Ma, S.H., & Teasdale, J.D. (2004). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression:
Replication and exploration of differential relapse prevention effects. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72(1), 31–40. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.1.31
McKenzie, K. S., & Hoyle, R. H. (2008). The Self-Absorption Scale: Reliability and validity in
non-clinical samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 726–731. doi:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.07.020
Nasby, W. (1989a). Private and public self-consciousness and the articulation of the self-schema.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 117–123. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.56.1.117
Park, A., Sher, K.J., & Krull, J.L. (2006). Individual differences in the "Greek effect" on risky
drinking: The role of self-consciousness. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20(1), 85–
90. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.20.1.85
Perlman, D.M., Salomons, T.V., Davidson, R.J., & Lutz, A. (2010). Differential effects on pain
intensity and unpleasantness of two meditation practices. Emotion, 10(1), 65–71. doi:
10.1037/a0018440
Rogojanski, J., Vettese, L.C., & Antony, M.M. (2011). Role of sensitivity to anxiety symptoms in
responsiveness to mindfulness versus suppression strategies for coping with smoking
cravings. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67(4), 439–445. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20774
Shaffer, D.R., & Tomarelli, M.M. (1989). When public and private self-foci clash: Self-
consciousness and self-disclosure reciprocity during the acquaintance process. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 56(5), 765–776. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.56.5.765
Shapiro, S.L., Astin, J.A., Bishop, S.R., & Cordova, M. (2005). Mindfulness-based stress
reduction for health care professionals: Results from a randomized trial. International
Journal of Stress Management, 12(2), 164–176. doi: 10.1037/1072-5245.12.2.164
Silvia, P. J., Eichstaedt, J., & Phillips, A. G. (2005). Are rumination and reflection types of self-
focused attention? Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 871–881. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2004.06.011
Sneed, J.R., & Whitbourne, S.K. (2003). Identity processing and self-consciousness in middle
29
Teasdale, J.D., & Green, H.A.C. (2004). Ruminative self-focus and autobiographical memory.
Personality and Individual Differences, 36(8), 1933–1943. doi:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2003.08.022
Teasdale, J.D., Segal, Z.V., Williams, J.M.G., Ridgeway, V.A., Soulsby, J.M., & Lau, M.A.
(2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based
cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68(4), 615–623. doi:
10.1037/0022-006X.68.4.615
Trapnell, P.D., & Campbell, J.D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of
personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 76(2), 284–304. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.76.2.284
Van den Hurk, P.A.M., Wingens, T., Giommi, F., Barendregt, H.P., Speckens, A.E.M., & van
Schie, H.T. (2011). On the relationship between the practice of mindfulness meditation
and personality — An exploratory analysis of the mediating role of mindfulness skills.
Mindfulness, 2(3), 194–200. doi: 10.1007/s12671-011-0060-7
Walach, H., Buchheld, N., Buttenmuller, V., Kleinknecht, N., & Schmidt, S. (2006). Measuring
mindfulness: The Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI). Personality and Individual
Differences, 40(8), 1543–1555. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.025
Watson, P.J., Morris, R.J., Ramsey, A., Hickman, S.E., & Waddell, M.G. (1996). Further contrasts
between self-reflectiveness and internal state awareness factors of private self-
consciousness. The Journal of Psychology, 139(2), 183–192. doi:
10.1080/00223980.1996.9915000
Way, B.M., Creswell, J.D., Eisenberger, N.I., & Lieberman, M.D. (2010). Dispositional
mindfulness and depressive symptomatology: correlations with limbic and self-referential
neural activity during rest. Emotion, 10(1), 12–24. doi: 10.1037/a0018312
Wheeler, S.C., Morrison, K.R., DeMarree, K.G., & Petty, R.E. (2008). Does self-consciousness
increase or decrease priming effects? It depends. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 44(3), 882–889. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2007.09.002
Williams, J.M., Teasdale, J.D., Segal, Z.V., & Soulsby, J. (2000). Mindfulness-based cognitive
therapy reduces overgeneral autobiographical memory in formerly depressed patients.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(1), 150–155. doi: 10.1037//0021-843X.109.1.150
30
Wong, S.Y., Chan, F.W., Wong, R.L., Chu, M.C., Lam, Y.Y., Mercer, S.W., & Ma, S.H. (2011).
Comparing the effectiveness of mindfulness-based stress reduction and multidisciplinary
intervention programs for chronic pain: A randomized comparative trial. Clinical Journal
of Pain, 27(8), 724–734. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0b013e3182183c6e
31
Appendix A
Demographics Questionnaire
What is your age? ____
Hispanic or Latino
Not Hispanic or Latino
What state do you live in? (Drop box list of all US states)
I am currently:
Employed
Unemployed and not a student
An undergraduate student
A graduate student
32
Other ___________
I am:
Married or Partnered
In a relationship
Single
Appendix B
Below is a collection of statements about your everyday experience. Using the 1–6 scale below,
please indicate how frequently or infrequently you currently have each experience. Please answer
according to what really reflects your experience rather than what you think your experience should
be.
_____ 1. I could be experiencing some emotion and not be conscious of it until sometime later.
_____ 2. I break or spill things because of carelessness, not paying attention, or thinking of
something else.
_____ 4. I tend to walk quickly to get where I’m going without paying attention to what I
experience along the way.
_____ 5. I tend not to notice feelings of physical tension or discomfort until they really grab my
attention.
_____ 6. I forget a person’s name almost as soon as I’ve been told it for the first time.
_____ 7. It seems I am “running on automatic” without much awareness of what I’m doing.
_____ 9. I get so focused on the goal I want to achieve that I lose touch with what I am doing right
now to get there.
_____ 10. I do jobs or tasks automatically, without being aware of what I’m doing.
_____ 11. I find myself listening to someone with one ear, doing something else at the same time.
_____ 12. I drive places on “automatic pilot” and then wonder why I went there.
_____ 13. I find myself preoccupied with the future or the past.
34
Appendix C
Self-Absorption Scale
Please read each statement carefully before answering. To the left of each item, indicate how
often you behave in the stated manner, using the following scale:
Almost Almost
never always
1 2 3 4 5
_____ 1. I find myself wondering what others think of me even when I don’t want to.
_____ 2. I have difficulty focusing on what others are talking about because I wonder what
they’re thinking of me.
_____ 3. I feel like others are constantly evaluating me when I’m with them.
_____ 8. I am very aware of what others think of me, and it bothers me.
_____ 9. My mind never focuses on things other than myself for very long.
_____ 10. When I start thinking about how others view me, I get all worked up.
_____ 11. I cannot stop my head from thinking thoughts about myself.
_____ 12. Sometimes I am so deep in thought about my life I am not aware of my surroundings.
_____ 14. I do not spend long amounts of time thinking about myself.
36
_____ 15. When I think about my life, I keep thinking about it so long I cannot turn my attention
to tasks that need to be done.
_____ 16. When I’m about to meet someone for the first time, I worry about whether they’ll like
me.
_____ 17. After being around other people, I think about what I should have done differently
when I was with them.
37
Appendix D
Email Invitation
Hello,
Participation in the study involves filling out three questionnaires (a total of 42 items). You must
be 18 years old or older to participate. Your participation should take no longer than 10 minutes.
If you would like to participate in this study, please click the following link to begin:
http://edu.surveygizmo.com/s3/1146592
Michael Sasiain
M.S. Candidate
Pacific University
School of Professional Psychology