Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

Indecent - Influence - The - Positive - Effects - of - Obsceni (Sürüklenen) 9

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

SOCIAL INFLUENCE, 2006, 1 (2), 138–146

Indecent influence: The positive effects of obscenity


on persuasion

Cory R. Scherer and Brad J. Sagarin


Northern Illinois University, DeKalb, IL, USA

This experiment examined the effects of judicious swearing on persuasion in a


pro-attitudinal speech. Participants listened to one of three versions of a
speech about lowering tuition that manipulated where the word ‘‘damn’’
appeared (beginning, end, or nowhere). The results showed that obscenity at
the beginning or end of the speech significantly increased the persuasiveness of
the speech and the perceived intensity of the speaker. Obscenity had no effect
on speaker credibility.

In 1939, David Selznick, producer of Gone With the Wind, was fined $5,000
by the Hollywood Production Code Commission for the profane word that
ended Rhett Butler’s famous line, ‘‘Frankly my dear, I don’t give a damn’’
(Vertres, 1997). Sixty-five years later, US Vice President Dick Cheney used a
substantially stronger word when he told Vermont Democratic Senator
Patrick Leahy to, in the words of The Washington Times, ‘‘perform an
anatomical sexual impossibility’’ (Simms, 2004, p. 98). The statement
garnered no fine, and Cheney offered no apology. Indeed, in an interview
with Neil Cavuto of Fox News, Cheney expressed no regrets, explaining
instead that he ‘‘felt better afterwards’’ (FOXNews.com, 2004, "101).
Clearly, society’s stance against swearing has become more relaxed in
recent years. The increasing acceptability of swearing raises the possibility
that obscenity could have a positive effect on the perceptions of the speaker.
In fact, Cheney’s use of obscenity actually endeared him to some. As blogger

Address correspondence to: Cory R. Scherer, Department of Psychology, Northern Illinois


University, DeKalb, IL 60115, USA. Email: cscherer@niu.edu
The authors wish to thank Jeremy D. Heider and John J. Skowronski for helpful comments
on earlier versions of this manuscript.
The findings reported here were initially presented at the 2005 annual meeting of the Society
of Personality and Social Psychologists in New Orleans, LA, and at the 2005 annual meeting of
the Midwestern Psychological Association in Chicago, IL, USA.

# 2006 Psychology Press Ltd


http://www.psypress.com/socinf DOI: 10.1080/15534510600747597
INDECENT INFLUENCE 139

Ravenwood explained, ‘‘The more I hear about Vice President Dick Cheney
telling Senator Patrick Leahy to go fuck himself, the better I like Cheney’’
(Ravenwood’s Universe, 2004, "1). Along these lines, a poster to the bulletin
board on PromoteLiberty.org argued that Cheney’s comment ‘‘shows a
remarkable degree of restraint on the part of the Vice President yet a
willingness to stand up for his personal honor and convictions’’ (FMeekins,
2004, "4).
The present experiment was designed to examine the effects of obscenity
on the perceptions of a speaker and the persuasiveness of a speech.
However, as the reactions to Cheney’s statement suggest, obscenity may
have its most positive effect when targeted at a congenial audience. Given
this, the present experiment examined the persuasive impact of a single
swear word incorporated into a pro-attitudinal speech. The present
experiment also examined the effects of obscenity on the perceived intensity
and credibility of the speaker.

SWEARING AND PERSUASION


What are the effects of swearing on the influence process? Past research
suggests two possibilities: (a) increasing the perceived intensity of the
communicator and (b) decreasing credibility.

Intensity
Hamilton, Hunter, and Burgoon (1990) defined intensity as a stylistic
feature of language that is expressed through emotionality and specificity.
Emotional intensity is the degree of affect in the source’s language. Obscene
language can be seen as a form of intense language (Bradac, Bowers, &
Courtright, 1979).
A study examining motivation for why people swear had female and male
college students complete a survey to determine their beliefs about the
common motives for their use of obscenity and why others use obscenity
(Fine & Johnson, 1984). The study examined 10 possible motives: to express
anger, to emphasize feelings, out of habit, peer pressure, to relieve tensions
and frustrations, because the word is taboo, to act cool, to get attention,
because the word is acceptable, and lack of another word. Across gender,
the motives of expressing anger and emphasizing feelings were found to be
of greatest importance.1

1
Although people swear to express anger, they also swear to express other emotions such as
happiness. At the 2003 Grammy awards, for example, Bono of the rock group U2 used an
obscenity to express how happy he was with the fact that his band had just won an award.
140 SCHERER AND SAGARIN

Fine and Johnson’s (1984) results demonstrate that the emphasis of feelings
is an important motive for swearing. Furthermore, people recognize that
other people swear, in part, to emphasize feeling. In this regard, Mulac (1976)
found that a speaker can demonstrate strong emphasis about a topic by using
obscene language, but the obscene language detracts from other aspects of
how the speaker is perceived. Nonetheless, in regards to persuasion, Fine and
Johnson’s results suggest that if an audience hears a speaker swear when
giving a speech on a particular topic, then the audience might infer that the
speaker is emphasizing feelings. Acknowledgement of such a point might
motivate the audience to take particular note of the argument and, quite
possibly, to be especially influenced by the communication.
In fact, research supports the idea that speakers can increase persuasion
by increasing the intensity of their language. According to a causal model
by Bradac, Bowers, and Courtright (1980) based on reinforcement
expectancy theory, language intensity influences attitude change through
two steps: language intensity affects source evaluation and source evaluation
affects attitude change. If swear words act as strong or intense language,
then obscenity may increase persuasion in the same way as other
forms of intense language. However, unlike some other forms of intense
language, swearing may negatively impact source evaluation by reducing
credibility.

Credibility
In Cursing in America, Jay (1992) claims that cursing at an inappropriate
time will reduce a speaker’s credibility, persuasiveness, and perceived
professionalism. Therefore, Jay cautions that swearing for persuasive
reasons should be used only when the speaker has nothing to lose.
Past research on obscenity and persuasion supports Jay’s (1992) concern.
For example, Bostrom, Baseheart, and Rossiter (1973) examined reactions
to people who swear. This experiment looked at the persuasive effects
of three types of profane language: religious (e.g., damn), excretory
(e.g., shit), and sexual (e.g., fuck) obscenity. The participants listened
to a tape-recorded interview about a topic and evaluated the topic before
and after listening to the tape. Overall, Bostrom et al. (1973) did not find
support for the prediction that obscenity would increase persuasion.
Another study conducted by Hamilton (1989) found that obscenity
increased audience disgust with the message and negative perceptions of
the source.
However, the lack of persuasion effects in these studies may have
stemmed from the choice of topics, which were counter-attitudinal for most
participants. For counter-attitudinal topics, listeners may use swearing as an
excuse to reject the message. On the other hand, swearing may increase
INDECENT INFLUENCE 141

persuasion for pro-attitudinal topics. Nevertheless, given Jay’s (1992)


caution, the present experiment examined the possible detrimental effects
of obscenity on the credibility of the speaker.

THE CURRENT EXPERIMENT


The current experiment examined the effects of swearing on the persuasive
impact of a speech and the intensity and credibility of the speaker. Because
of the dearth of evidence for the persuasive power of obscenity, the present
experiment used swearing in a manner optimized for its effectiveness: one
relatively mild swear word (‘‘damn’’) was placed at the beginning or end of a
pro-attitudinal speech.

Method

Participants. A total of 88 introductory psychology students from a large


Midwestern university participated in partial fulfillment of a course
requirement.

Design and procedure. The participants were randomly assigned to one of


three conditions (no swear word, swear word at the beginning of the speech,
swear word at the end). After giving informed consent, participants were
seated in front of a computer and instructed to follow the instructions on the
computer.
The computer played a 5-minute videotaped speech about the topic of
lowering tuition at a different university, a pro-attitudinal topic of low
relevance to the participants. When the participants finished watching the
speech, they completed scales that measured their attitudes on the topic and
their perceptions of the speaker. After the participants finished, they were
probed for suspicion and debriefed.

Materials. There were three speeches of similar length. The speeches


discussed the topic of lowering tuition at a different university. The speeches
had a mixture of strong and weak arguments. Strong arguments included
how students have to take into account how much school will cost when
deciding where to go and how the school will be saving the students money.
Weak arguments included how the school could use lowering tuition as a
selling point and how the community will be more attractive to businesses
because the students would have more money to spend in the town.
Judicious swearing was operationalized as a single instance of the relatively
inoffensive word ‘‘damn.’’ The swear word appeared either at the beginning
(‘‘… that lowering of tuition is not only a great idea, but damn it, also the
most reasonable one for all parties involved.’’) or end (‘‘Damn it, I think
142 SCHERER AND SAGARIN

lowering tuition is a great idea.’’) of the speech. The control speech was the
same speech without the swear word.2
The speeches were delivered in a video format on a computer screen using
Medialab experimental software (Jarvis, 2002). The male speaker could be
seen from mid-chest up in front of a neutral background. The speaker
attempted to maintain the same tone for every speech.
There were two surveys that assessed the participants’ attitudes about the
speaker and the speech. The first survey was a nine-item scale that asked
questions about the participants’ attitudes toward the speaker. The
questions of most interest were the three questions about the intensity of
the speaker (how passionately, strongly, and enthusiastically did the speaker
feel) and three questions about the credibility of the speaker (how credible,
trustworthy, and knowledgeable the audience found the speaker). There
were an additional three questions about how similar the speaker was to the
participant (was the speaker like them, similar to them, and akin to them)
that were used for further study. The second survey was a four-item scale
that asked about the participants’ attitudes about lowering tuition (how
much did they like the idea of lowering tuition, how much did they think it
was a good idea at the school that was implementing the plan, would they
implement such a plan at their school, and did the speech make them feel
more positive or negative towards the idea). All questions used similar
seven-point response options with all points labeled (e.g., not at all credible,
not credible, somewhat not credible, neutral, somewhat credible, credible,
very credible).

Results
The purpose of the present experiment was to examine the effects of
swearing on the perceptions of the speaker and the persuasiveness of the
speech. These were tested using a series of univariate ANOVAs comparing
the three conditions on each dependent variable (speaker intensity, speaker
credibility, and attitude about topic; see Table 1). Each dependent variable
displayed good internal consistency (intensity: a 5 .87, credibility: a 5 .83,
and attitude about topic: a 5 .82). Speaker intensity was correlated with
speaker credibility (r 5 .28, p 5 .009) and with attitude about the topic
(r 5 .35, p , .001). Speaker credibility did not correlate with attitude about
the topic (r 5 .12, p 5 .267).

2
The experiment contained an additional condition with swearing in the middle of the
speech. Unfortunately, this condition inadvertently confounded the placement of the swear
word with its use (‘‘… then the alumni may feel that the damn school already has taken enough
money from them.’’). This condition did not differ from the control condition on persuasion,
speaker intensity, or speaker credibility, but given the confound, it is unclear whether the lack of
an effect was due to the placement of the swear word or its use.
INDECENT INFLUENCE 143

TABLE 1
Effects of one swear word on persuasiveness of a speech and perceptions of the
speaker

No obscenity Obscenity at the Obscenity


(control) beginning at the end
Attitude about lowering 4.14 SD 5 0.40 4.42a SD 5 0.45 4.34a SD 5 0.41
tuition
Speaker intensity 4.40 SD 5 1.03 4.89a SD 5 0.81 5.02a SD 5 0.98
Speaker credibility 4.91a SD 5 0.73 4.91a SD 5 0.75 4.98a SD 5 0.76
Scales range from 1–7 with higher values indicating more persuasion, greater intensity, and
greater credibility. Means within a row that share a superscript are not significantly different.

Swearing had a significant effect on participants’ attitudes about lowering


tuition, F(2, 85) 5 3.751, p 5 .027. Follow-up contrasts showed that the
speeches with the swear word at the beginning or end were significantly
more persuasive than the control speech (see Table 1). The speeches with the
swear word in the beginning and end did not significantly differ from each
other. Swearing also had a significant effect on participants’ perceptions of
the intensity of the speaker, F(2, 85) 5 3.473, p 5 .035. Follow-up contrasts
revealed the same pattern as for attitudes about lowering tuition: swearing
at the beginning or end of the speech led to significantly higher perceptions
of speaker intensity than no swearing. Swearing did not significantly impact
perceptions of speaker credibility, F(2, 85) 5 0.052, p 5 .945.3

Mediational analysis. To test whether the effects of swearing on persuasion


were fully or partially mediated by increased intensity, three regression
analyses were conducted (Baron & Kenny, 1986). In each of these
regressions, the three conditions were represented by two contrast vectors.
Contrast vector 1 (CV1) compared the beginning and end conditions against
the control condition. Contrast vector 2 (CV2) compared the beginning
condition against the end condition. CV1 represented the comparison of
interest. CV2 was included to fully represent the three conditions in the
regression equations.
In the first regression, intensity was regressed on the contrast vectors.
Consistent with the ANOVA results above, CV1 was a significant predictor
of intensity, B 5 2.395, SEB 5 .152, b 5 2.270, t 5 22.599, p 5 .011. In the
second regression, attitude toward lowering tuition was regressed on the
contrast vectors. Also consistent with the ANOVA results, CV1 was a
significant predictor of attitude toward lowering tuition, B 5 2.938,

3
When looking at the expertise and trustworthiness components of credibility separately, the
results were similarly nonsignificant—expertise: F(2, 85) 5 0.184, p 5 .832, trustworthiness: F(2,
85) 5 0.224, p 5 .800.
144 SCHERER AND SAGARIN

SEB 5 .352, b 5 2.276, t 5 22.661, p 5 .009. In the third regression, attitude


toward lowering tuition was regressed on the contrast vectors and intensity.
CV1 remained a significant predictor, B 5 2.067, SEB 5 .031, b 5 2.228,
t 5 22.131, p 5 .036, although the beta was reduced somewhat, suggesting
partial mediation. Speaker intensity approached significance, B 5 .075,
SEB 5 .046, b 5 .174, t 5 1.628, p 5 .107. It should be noted, however, that
although the experimental manipulation allows a causal interpretation of
the effect of swearing on intensity and persuasion, the relationship between
intensity and persuasion is correlational, and the data are consistent with
other possible causal relationships.

Discussion
The purpose of this experiment was to examine the effects of judicious
swearing on persuasion in a pro-attitudinal speech. Results demonstrated
that swearing at the beginning and at the end of the speech led to more
positive attitudes about the topic and greater perceptions of speaker
intensity. These results provide the first demonstration of the persuasive
power of obscenity, and they suggest that judiciously used obscenity can
increase persuasion, at least within the context of a pro-attitudinal speech.
Mediational analyses suggested that speaker intensity partially mediated
the effects of swearing on persuasion, although the effect of intensity on
persuasion in the final regression equation was not statistically significant.
This may be due to a lack of statistical power. Additional research should be
conducted to further test the mediational effect. These findings are
congruent with the idea that language intensity can lead to higher levels
of attitude change and they suggest that swear words can be used in a similar
way to other forms of intense language.
In the present experiment, swearing had no impact on speaker credibility.
In regards to credibility, it is possible that swearing may be affecting
credibility both positively and negatively, leading to an overall null effect.
Obscenity could impact credibility positively because the use of obscenity
could make a credible speaker appear more human. Consistent with this,
Aune and Kikuchi (1993) found that language intensity increased source
credibility in a pro-attitudinal message. However, obscenity could also
impact credibility negatively because the use of obscenity could be seen as
inappropriate for a credible speaker. Future work is needed to tease apart
the relationship between swearing and the different aspects of credibility:
expertise and trustworthiness. It is also possible that credibility would have
greater importance in a counter-attitudinal speech in which the audience
might be motivated to reject the speech by derogating the qualities of the
speaker.
INDECENT INFLUENCE 145

Limitations and future directions. As described above, the present experiment


was designed to examine the persuasive power of obscenity in an optimal
setting: a pro-attitudinal speech containing a single, relatively mild swear
word. Future studies could examine whether obscenity’s persuasive effect is
limited to this domain. Are there situations in which obscenity can increase
persuasion even in a counter-attitudinal speech? Would obscenity be more
or less useful if the message arguments are all strong or weak? What would
be the effects of using stronger (and potentially more offensive) swear
words? Would an increase in the number of swear words increase their
persuasive impact? It might be the case that the effects of swearing on
persuasion are curvilinear; additional swear words may increase a message’s
persuasive impact only to the extent that they are perceived as appropriate.
Once the swearing becomes excessive, however, it may backfire.

Manuscript received 11 April 2005


Manuscript accepted 11 April 2006

REFERENCES
Aune, R. K., & Kikuchi, T. (1993). Effects of language intensity similarity on perceptions of
credibility, relational attributions, and persuasion. Journal of Language and Social
Psychology, 12, 224–238.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social
psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.
Bostrom, B. N., Baseheart, J. R., & Rossiter, C. M. (1973). The effects of three types of profane
language in persuasive messages. The Journal of Communication, 23, 461–475.
Bradac, J., Bowers, J., & Courtright, J. (1980). Lexical variations in intensity, immediacy and
diversity: An axiomatic theory and causal model. In R. N. St. Clair & H. Giles (Eds.), The
social and psychological contexts of language (pp. 294–317). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Bradac, J., Bowers, J. A., & Courtright, J. (1979). Three language variables in communication
research: Intensity, immediacy and diversity. Human Communication Research, 5, 257–269.
Fine, M. G., & Johnson, F. L. (1984). Female and male motives for using obscenity. Journal of
Language and Social Psychology, 3, 59–74.
FMeekins (2004, July 3). Hillary high horse. Message posted to http://www.promoteliberty.org/
phpbb2/viewtopic.php?t 5 65
FOXNews.com (2004). Transcript: Interview with Dick Cheney, Retrieved September 20, 2004,
from http://www.foxnews.com/printer_friendly_story/0,3566,123792,00.html
Hamilton, M., Hunter, J., & Burgoon, M. (1990). An empirical investigation of an axiomatic
model of the effect of language intensity on attitude change. Journal of Language and Social
Psychology, 9, 235–255.
Hamilton, M. A. (1989). Reactions to obscene language. Communication Research Reports, 6,
67–69.
Jarvis, B. (2002). MediaLab2001 (Version 2002.1.4) [Computer software]. New York:
Empirisoft Corporation.
Jay, T. (1992). Cursing in America. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Mulac, A. (1976). Effects of obscene language upon three dimensions of listener attitude.
Communication Monographs, 43, 300–307.

You might also like