Thinking Styles - Jieqiong Fan
Thinking Styles - Jieqiong Fan
Thinking Styles - Jieqiong Fan
Thinking Styles:
By
Jieqiong Fan
Thinking Styles: Identity, Value, and Malleability
By Jieqiong Fan
All rights for this book reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced,
stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means,
electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, without
the prior permission of the copyright owner.
Table 1.2: The Distribution of the Sample in the Main Study .................. 23
Thinking styles, one of the latest theories in the field of intellectual styles,
are defined as one’s preferred way of using abilities. Fifteen years ago, I
was so excited when I first learned the concept of thinking styles in a lecture.
It has been claimed that the construct of thinking styles is an additional
factor that can explain individual differences in performance beyond ability
and personality. Furthermore, unlike ability and personality which are at
least half determined by genetic factors, thinking styles were considered to
be more modifiable by environments and experience. Based on these
contentions, it seemed very likely that one can successfully improve
students’ performance by cultivating their thinking styles. It has also been
argued that thinking styles are value free, which means that there are no
good or bad styles and that only “style match” matters. This argument is
also tempting.
A few years later, I began my doctoral study and chose thinking styles as
my research area. I was so surprised when I found out that the above
contentions were mostly conceptual arguments rather than evidence-based
conclusions. Likewise, many arguments with regard to other style models in
the field of intellectual styles were also widely disseminated to educational
practitioners before being fully examined. The hasty dissemination of the
concept of styles combined with the myriad of style models in the field
inevitably attracted plenty of criticism and doubts, which, at one time, made
the field stagnate.
and value are revisited and discussed in more detail. In doing so, this book
helps researchers and educators to have a deeper understanding of the nature
of styles and their role in student development; it also provides practical
suggestions for parents, teachers, and other educational practitioners.
Hopefully, this book will contribute to the advancement of the field of styles
and facilitate the application of style theories and research in educational
practice.
This book is aimed at academics and graduate students who wish to research
styles and its relevant fields, such as educational psychology, school
psychology, higher education, student development, curriculum design and
instruction, and career counselling. Educators, teachers, counsellors, and
other practitioners in schools, universities, and other institutions will also
be interested in this book because it discusses the practical implications of
research findings on cultivating adaptive thinking styles and promoting
student development.
It should be noted that this book’s major work is rooted in my PhD research
project and partially draws on some of my recent research articles. Many
people have provided generous help and support during the research process
and the publication of this book. I would like to take this opportunity to
express my appreciation to them. First and foremost, my deepest gratitude
goes to my PhD supervisor, Professor Li-fang Zhang. She possesses all the
attributes required for a fantastic supervisor. Professor Zhang has provided
me with prompt guidance and unconditional help whenever I needed it. Her
feedback and comments on my work are always constructive and insightful.
I am also deeply grateful to Professor Zhang for her constant encouragement
and trust in my abilities. Without her, I would not have made such
tremendous progress. Furthermore, Professor Zhang’s excellent academic
abilities, as well as her conscientious, persistent, and dedicated attitude to
work, have deeply influenced me and will definitely continue to inspire me
throughout my entire academic career.
I would also like to thank my teachers, colleagues, and friends both inside
and outside the Faculty of Education in the University of Hong Kong.
Discussions with them are always illuminating. I am deeply grateful to
Professor Stephen Rayner, Professor Carol Chan, and Dr. W. W. Ki for their
constructive feedback on my PhD dissertation. Special thanks are also given
to Professor Chen Chen and Dr. Yanbi Hong who have provided me with
generous support. Their valuable advice and comments have been a great
help in my research. Their empathy and encouragement have motivated me
on my way forward.
xiv Preface
Moreover, my sincere thanks go to Dr. Guohong Wu, Ms. Shu Gao, and Mr.
Mingbo Liu for their facilitation of my research’s data collection over the
years. I would also like to thank the teachers who arranged time in their
classes for me and the students who participated in my research. In addition,
I am deeply grateful to my Cambridge Scholars Publishing editors, Helen
Edwards and Adam Rummens, as well as my proofreader, Dr. Joanne Ella
Parsons, for their competent assistance when contracting the book,
reviewing the book proposal, and polishing the manuscript into its published
form. I also want to thank my typesetting manager, Amanda Miller, for her
constant help during the publishing process.
FDI: Field-dependence/independence
Some people have an excellent performance in school but then fail in their
career. A person could be judged as less intelligent in one setting, but may
also be judged as having better abilities in another. A person may not have
excellent performance when he is as an athlete, but he could be a brilliant
coach. In our society, students have to go through fierce competitions in
order to enter the next stage in the educational system: primary school,
junior school, high school, and university. Some students, even though they
have a great performance at the beginning, do increasingly badly in these
successive stages. This is not surprising as the schools in each stage are highly
selective when recruiting students. It is very possible that one person will
become confronted with people in successive stages who have stronger
abilities than those in the last stage. Therefore, it is very difficult for them to
outperform other people every time. However, students can still improve at
every point. Robert Sternberg, who proposed the theory of thinking styles, was
one of them. Like many other scholars, he noticed that the above phenomena
could not be simply explained by abilities or personality. For decades,
researchers, educators, psychologists, and philosophers have been trying to
solve this mystery. During this process, the construct of styles emerged. After
the concept of styles was firstly introduced by Gordon Allport (1937), various
style constructs and theories proliferated (Zhang & Sternberg, 2006). Style
theorists believe that people who have identical abilities may have different
styles, which leads to different performances contingent to their environment.
One is cognitive styles, which refers to the ways in which people prefer to
process information (Armstrong, Peterson, & Rayner, 2012). Typical
examples of cognitive styles are field-dependent/independent (Witkin,
1954), intuitive-thinking (Myers, 1962), reflective-impulsive (Jerome
2 Chapter One
Another term is learning styles, which refers to the ways in which people
prefer to respond to learning tasks (Armstrong et al., 2012). Typical
constructs that are classified into learning styles include instructional
preference (Friedman & Stritter, 1976), learning preference (Rezler &
Rezmovic, 1974), study process (Biggs, 1979), and approach to study
(Entwistle & Ramsden, 1983).
The third term is thinking styles, which was proposed by Sternberg (1988,
1997). Compared with the previous two, thinking styles refer to the ways
that people prefer to think about the information as they are learning it or
after they already know it. In recent studies, thinking styles are considered
to be more general than cognitive or learning styles, because they include
all three traditions (cognition-centered, personality-centered, and activity-
centered) in the field of styles (Zhang & Fan, 2011).
However, the relationships between the various style labels are still puzzling
for many scholars. For example, after a careful review, Zhang and Sternberg
(2006) found cognitive styles and learning styles have much in common,
and some scholars have used them interchangeably (e.g., Campbell, 1991;
Tennant, 1997). In addition, there are still disputes over some style
constructs, such as Kolb’s (1976) learning style (Zhang & Sternberg, 2006).
Aiming to further clarify the relationships between styles, scholars have
made endeavors beyond the aforementioned three terms, which
significantly contribute to the integration of the existing studies in the field.
Taking “onion” as a metaphor, Curry’s (1983) model claimed that there are
three layers of styles. The innermost layer involves personality dimensions,
the middle layer involves information processing, and the outermost layer
involves instructional preferences. Miller (1987) proposed a model of
cognitive processes and styles where cognitive styles are seen as individual
differences in three types of cognitive processes: perception, memory, and
thought. He also suggested that all cognitive styles can be analyzed by one
bipolar dimension: analytic-holistic. Riding and Cheema (1991) adopted
An Introduction to Thinking Styles 3
Thinking
Dimension Key Characteristics
Styles
One prefers to work on tasks that require creative
Function Legislative (I) strategies; one prefers to choose one's own
activities.
One prefers to work on tasks with clear
Executive (II) instructions and structures; one prefers to
implement tasks with established guidelines.
One prefers to work on tasks that allow for one's
Judicial (I) evaluation; one prefers to evaluate and judge the
performance of other people.
Hierarchical One prefers to distribute attention to several tasks
Form
(I) prioritized according to one's valuing of the tasks.
Monarchic One prefers to work on tasks that allow complete
(II) focus on one thing at a time.
One prefers to work on multiple tasks in the
Oligarchic
service of multiple objectives, without setting
(III)
priorities.
One prefers to work on tasks that allow flexibility
Anarchic (III)
as to what, where, when, and how one works.
One prefers to pay more attention to the overall
Level Global (I)
picture of an issue and to abstract ideas.
One prefers to work on tasks that require working
Local (II)
with concrete details.
One prefers to work on tasks that allow one to
Scope Internal (III)
work as an independent unit.
One refers to work on tasks that allow for
External (III)
collaborative ventures with other people.
One prefers to work on tasks that involve novelty
Leaning Liberal (I)
and ambiguity.
One prefers to work on tasks that allow one to
Conservative
adhere to the existing rules and procedures in
(II)
performing tasks.
Note. Extracted from Zhang, 2003, p. 630; I = Type I thinking style; II = Type II
thinking style; and III = Type III thinking style
First, how do styles and personality overlap? With regard to this aspect,
many studies have been undertaken to examine the correlations between
personality and styles or the percentages of the variance in styles that can
be explained by personality (e.g., Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2009;
Kwang & Rodrigues, 2002; Larson et al., 2002; Zhang, 2006b). The extent
of the overlap between personality and styles found in these studies vary
depending on the different personality constructs and style models adopted.
However, apart from several dimensions, such as the extraversion-
introversion dimension in Myers and McCaulley’s (1988) personality types
(one model of intellectual styles), the correlations found between
personality and styles are moderate at most (see Chapter 2 for more detail),
which means that the overlap of personality and styles is limited.
Third, another difference between styles and personality that scholars argue
about is the question of malleability. Some scholars believe that styles are
more changeable than personality (e.g., Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham,
2009; Zhang & Sternberg, 2005). However, there is a lack of empirical
studies that directly examine this argument.
However, the issue of style socialization is still one of the most controversial
in the field (Zhang & Sternberg, 2006). Some scholars consider styles to be
a product of nature. For example, Riding and Rayner (1998) argue that
An Introduction to Thinking Styles 9
There are three main lines of research that have contributed to the
socialization issue of styles. The first line examines style differences based
on certain personal factors (e.g., age, gender, socioeconomic status, and
personal experience, etc.). The second line explores the relationships
between certain environmental factors (e.g., disciplines, culture, and work
environment, etc.) and styles. The third line consists of longitudinal studies
that mostly examine the effects of training (e.g., program, course, and
therapy, etc.) on styles. The details of these studies are described below.
The most widely examined socialization factors in the field of styles are age
and gender. In Zhang and Sternberg’s (2006) and Fer’s (2012) reviews,
although there are some inconsistent results from empirical studies, they
found that most of them had concluded that age and gender play an important
role in the development of styles. For example, Jonassen and Grabowski
(1993) found the developing curve of field dependence/independence. Style
differences based on other theoretical frameworks were also found to be a
function of age, such as Biggs’s (1978) learning approaches (e.g., Hilliard,
1995; Richardson, 1995; E. Sadler-Smith, 1996), Kirton’s (1961,1976)
adaption-innovation decision making and problem solving styles (e.g.,
Hayward & Everett, 1983; C. M. Jacobson, 1993), and Sternberg’s (1988,
1997) thinking styles (e.g., Zhang, 1999, 2004d; Zhang & He, 2003). By the
same token, gender differences in styles are also found in many kinds of
styles (see the review in Zhang and Sternberg, 2006; Fer, 2012). Though
there were different findings on the specific relationships between gender
and particular styles, it was consistently proved that gender plays an
important role in the development of styles. Zhang and Sternberg (2006)
even argued that the inconsistent results on the gender differences can also
be seen as evidence of socialization, because it reflects the influence of
different cultures on gender stereotypes and socialization.
10 Chapter One
The style studies on personal experience include three major aspects. First,
style differences related to education or training have already been studied.
For example, In Fer’s (2012) review of the relationships between
educational level and intellectual styles, she found that people with higher
levels of education tended to have more creativity-generating styles (Type
I) than their counterparts. Lee (2002) conducted a study among teachers and
found differences in their thinking styles by comparing teachers with and
without professional training. Second, in addition to learning experience,
studies on non-academic settings and styles show that work experience
plays an important role in the socialization of individuals’ styles. Examples
include the length of work experience (e.g., P. A. Holland, Bowskill, &
Bailey, 1991; Sternberg & Grigorenko, 1995; Zhang & Sachs, 1997), work
positions (e.g., R. T. Keller & Holland, 1978a; Zhang & Higgins, 2008),
and job functions (e.g., Foxall, Payne, & Walters, 1992; Kirton & Pender,
1982).
The most widely studied factor that has been used to infer environmental
effects on styles is people’s disciplines. Student styles vary based on
different majors (e.g., Bin, 2009; Fer, 2007; Jones, Reichard, & Mokhtari,
2003; Skogsberg & Clump, 2003; S. N. Smith & Miller, 2005; Tucker,
1999) or different specialized colleges (e.g., Barnhart, 2003; Mitchell &
An Introduction to Thinking Styles 11
scores in the legislative, executive, local, and liberal thinking styles than
people in Hong Kong. All of these studies supported the important role of
culture in the socialization of people’s intellectual styles.
In fact, the most direct and most convincing pathway to examine the issue
of style malleability is via a longitudinal study. However, there has been
limited attention paid to this type of study.
such as the local thinking style; the surface learning approach; field
dependence; and the analytic style) (e.g., Alborzi & Ostovar, 2007;
Dollinger, Palaskonis, & Pearson, 2004; Kim & Michael, 1995; A. L.
Miller, 2007; S.-K. Park, Park, & Choe, 2005; Torrance & Reynolds, 1978;
Yang & Lin, 2004; Zhang, 2002b, 2003b, 2004c, 2005a).
However, most of the studies that examined the relationships between styles
and cognitive outcomes directly focused on academic achievement and task
performance. The results from these studies seem more complicated than
the ones from studies on abilities, creativity, cognitive development, and
gifted students mentioned above. Some studies found that students with
Type I styles outperformed students with Type II styles. For example, field-
independent (Type I style) students were found to have better levels of
academic achievement (e.g., Bagley & Mallick, 1998; Cameron & Dwyer,
2005; Hite, 2004), problem-solving skills (Williams, 2001), and programming
performances (Johnson & Kane, 1992; D. J. Wilson, Mundy-Castle, &
Sibanda, 1990). Huang and Chao’s (2000) study also showed that students
with learning disabilities had higher scores in field dependence than those
without learning disabilities. The results from some studies on other style
constructs, such as Biggs’ (1978) learning approaches, Myers and
McCaulley’s (1988) personality types, Kagan and his colleagues’ (1964)
reflective-impulsive styles, and Sternberg’s (1997) thinking styles also
support the belief that Type I styles have more positive value in terms of
academic achievement and task performance, which resonates with the
findings on field-dependence/independence (Chamorro-Premuzic &
Furnham, 2008; Fisher, 1994; Furnham, Christopher, Garwood, & Martin,
2007; Nachmias & Shany, 2002; Stahl, Erickson, & Rayman, 1986;
Sternberg & Grigorenko, 1993). Even in non-academic settings, the results
on the positive relationship between Type I styles and task performance
have been also found among reflective-impulsive styles (Sayles-Folks &
Harrison, 1989), field-dependence/independence (Wegner, 1980), and
adaption-innovation styles (Foxall & Hackett, 1992; Gelade, 1995).
approach) had higher scores in academic tests (Varma & Thakur, 1992;
Zhang, 2000). Kern and Matta (1988) found that when students were tested
on their performance on software use, students with a sensing (Type II)
preference did better than students with an intuition (Type I) preference. In
a traditional learning mode, the analytic style (Type II) was also found to be
positively related to academic performance (Liao & Chuang, 2007).
Zhang and Sternberg (2006) argued that these inconsistent results were due
to the different cultures, disciplines, learning materials, tasks and
assessments, and school levels involved in the studies. In spite of this, all of
them have still proved that styles are value laden rather than value free.
Most studies in this field focus on the relationship between styles and
personality. Type I styles have been found to be more related to adaptive
personality traits, such as openness and conscientiousness (Busato et al.,
1999; Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2007; Shokri, Kadivar, Farzad, & Sangari,
2007; Zhang, 2002a, 2002d; Zhang & Huang, 2001). As for other aspects
of personality in a much wider sense (such as moral, emotional, and
psychosocial aspects), Type I styles have also been proved to be more
related to certain developmental outcomes that always are seen as positive
attributes. For example, field independence (Type I), reflectivity (Type I)
and Type I thinking styles were associated with higher levels of moral
maturity (S.-K. Park et al., 2005; Schleifer & Douglas, 1973). Field
independent people also exhibit better control of their behaviors (Leventhal
& Sisco, 1996). Moreover, the innovative style (Type I) was found to be
negatively related to pessimism and positively related to tolerance of
ambiguity (R. T. Keller & Holland, 1978b; Wunderley, Reddy, & Dember,
1998). People with the innovative style and Type I thinking styles also had
more positive attitudes towards new technologies (Chakraborty, Hu, & Cui,
2008; Zhang, 2007c; Zhang & He, 2003). Meanwhile, Type I thinking styles
were also found to be positively related to a more adaptive type of academic
motivation (i.e., motivation to approach success) (W. Fan & Zhang, 2009).
18 Chapter One
It has to be admitted that there are still a few studies on social and emotional
development that have inconsistent results, though they are much less
common than the studies on academic achievement. For example, the
holistic style (Type I) was found to be positively related to anxiety in Jong,
Merckelbach, and Nijman’s (1995) study, and the analytic style (Type II)
was found to be associated with better self-control in Gadzella’s (1990)
study. However, according to the above review, most studies on the
relationship of styles with social and emotional development support the
argument that styles are value-laden and Type I styles are more adaptive
than Type II.
The value issue of intellectual styles can also be inferred from the research
on styles and teacher-student interactions in educational settings. First,
teachers with Type I styles are more likely to have effective teaching
behaviors. For example, it was found that teachers with Type I thinking
styles tended to exhibit more student-centered teaching behaviors and use
more beneficial humor styles in teaching, while teachers with Type II
thinking styles tended to exhibit more teacher-centered teaching behaviors
and use more detrimental humor styles (G. H. Chen, 2007; Lee, 2002; D.
M. Yu, 2008; Zhang, 2001a). Similar results were also found in research on
other style constructs. For instance, more field independent (Type I) and
more reflective (Type I) teachers have more positive attitudes to teaching
and students, and also display teaching behaviors which are normally
considered to be beneficial for students, such as more encouragement, more
nurturing, more democracy, and less criticism (see the review in Zhang &
Sternberg, 2006). Meanwhile, effective teachers’ MBTI profiles always
included intuition (Type I) rather than sensing (Type II) (Mills, 2003;
Rushton, Morgan, & Richard, 2007). Second, teachers prefer students with
Type I styles (Zhang, Fu, & Jiao, 2008). Third, students also prefer teachers
with Type I styles (Zhang, 2004e, 2006a, 2008a; Zhang, Huang, & Zhang,
2005) and tend to have better course satisfaction if their teachers use Type
I styles (Betoret, 2007; G. H. Chen, 2007). All of the evidence indicates that
Type I styles are more valued than Type II styles in educational settings,
although in some situations Type II styles are probably encouraged by
academic assessments in a practical sense (as previously discussed).
It is noticeable that most style training programs aim to help people develop
certain types of styles rather than others. Type I styles, such as field
20 Chapter One
In the following three chapters (i.e., Chapter 2, 3, and 4), each of the three
controversial issues will be discussed with a review of previous empirical
evidence in literature, as well as the major findings from the present research
project. In Chapter 5, all of the findings relevant to the three issues are
summarized and further discussed within the broader context of intellectual
styles. The final chapter highlights the conclusions, as well as discusses
their contributions and implications for theories, research, and educational
practice.
Participants
than the dropouts. The results of the main study should be understood in the
light of these group differences.
Time 1 Time 2
Grade freshmen 609 504
juniors 317 239
missing 0 0
Major humanities and social sciences 282 224
science and technology 643 518
missing 1 1
Gender male 520 414
female 405 328
missing 1 1
Total 926 743
Measures
Seven inventories were adopted in this study: a) the Parenting Style Index
(Steinberg, Lamborn, Darling, Mounts, & Dornbusch, 1994) for assessing
perceived parenting styles; b) the Inventory of Students’ Perceived Learning
Environment (self-designed) for assessing the perceived learning
environment; c) the NEO Five-Factor Inventory-3 (McCrae & Costa, 2007)
for assessing students’ personality traits; d) the Thinking Style Inventory-
Revised II (Sternberg, Wagner, & Zhang, 2007) for assessing thinking
styles; e) Career Decision-Making Self-Efficacy Scale––Short Form (Betz,
Klein, & Taylor, 1996) for assessing career decision self-efficacy; f) the
Satisfaction With Life Scale (E. Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985)
and g) the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson, Clark, &
Tellegen, 1988) for assessing subjective well-being. All of the measures
have been validated in the research. More details about each measure can
be found in Appendix 2 and Appendix 3.
Procedures
At the beginning of the academic year, all the participants were asked to
respond to the questionnaire, which include all the inventories mentioned
above and some necessary demographic information (including gender, age,
grade, major, hometown, and socioeconomic status) in class in the presence
24 Chapter One
of the researcher and their teachers. It should be noted that at the time of the
data collection, the freshmen were asked to recall their experience in high
school when they responded to the Inventory of Students’ Perceived
Learning Environment, while the juniors were asked to recall their
experience in university. In addition, all the participants were asked to recall
their experiences with their parents during the first 16 years of their lives
when they responded to the Parenting Style Index.
One year after the first data collection, the participants were asked to
respond again to the same questionnaire, which include all above
inventories and some necessary demographic information. It should be
noted that, at the time of the data collection, all of them were asked to
respond to the Inventory of Students’ Perceived Learning Environment
according to their experience in university during the period between the
two data collections. They were also asked to respond to the Parenting Style
Index according to their experience with their parents in the period between
the two data collections.
Participants
Twenty-nine participants were selected from students who had taken part in
both of the main study’s data collections and had been found to have
significant changes in their thinking styles over the course of a year
according to the results of the Reliable Change Index, which is a statistical
technique that can detect significant individual change by excluding
measure errors.
Interview Protocol
The interview protocol consisted of three parts. The first part included
questions for ice-breaking, where the interviewer attempted to obtain the
An Introduction to Thinking Styles 25
Procedures
Some scholars argue that styles are subordinate to personality (e.g., Jackson
& Lawty-Jones, 1996; von Wittich & Antonakis, 2011), whereas other
scholars insist that personality and styles are distinct constructs and they
both make unique contributions to the understanding of individual
differences (e.g., Li & Armstrong, 2015; Zhang, 2006b). From an empirical
perspective, the relationship between personality and styles can be
understood from the following three lines of research: (1) research that
examines the extent of the overlap between personality and styles; (2)
research that reveals the unique contributions of styles to other outcome
variables beyond personality; and (3) research that compares the difference
in changeability between personality and thinking styles. This chapter
discusses the association between personality and thinking in three sections,
which focus on the aforementioned three lines of research. In each section,
I will review the relevant literature in the field of intellectual styles and
present the empirical evidence from my research project.
1The present chapter draws heavily on the work by Zhang, L. F., Chen, C., and
myself, titled “Thinking styles: Distinct from personality?”, published in Personality
and Individual Differences (Fan, Zhang, & Chen, 2018).
Personality and its Relationship with Thinking Styles 27
The inconsistent findings are partially due to the different style constructs
examined in various studies (Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2009).
Grigorenko and Sternberg (1995) classified various style models into three
traditions: cognition-centered, personality-centered, and activity-centered.
From this conceptual perspective, personality-centered styles are supposed
to be closer to personality than cognition-centered and activity-centered
styles. To some extent, the previous findings supported this assumption.
Activity-centered styles, such as Biggs’ (1978) learning approaches and
Kolb’s (1976) learning styles, only had a low to moderate overlap with
personality. Cognition-centered styles, such as Witkin's (1962) field
dependence/independence, had no or a low overlap with personality, while
the strongest overlaps between personality and styles were found in
personality-centered styles, such as the personality types defined by Myers
and McCaulley (1988) and the KAI styles specified by Kirton (1961, 1976).
Studies based on a more comprehensive style model that includes all of the
aforementioned three traditions would deepen our understanding of the
relationship between styles and personality. Unlike binary models of styles
(e.g., Witkin’s field-dependence/independence; Kirton’s adaption-innovation
styles), the model of thinking styles (also known as the theory of mental
28 Chapter Two
Zhang and her colleagues (Zhang, 2002a, 2002d, 2006b; Zhang & Huang,
2001) conducted a series of studies showing that personality traits had a
weak to moderate correlation with thinking styles, and the variance in
thinking styles that could be explained by personality traits was less than
35%. Most of these studies adopted correlations or ordinary least squares
(OLS) regressions to analyze the relationships between thinking styles and
personality traits. However, Von Wittich and Antonakis (2011) argue that
correlations and OLS regressions tend to underestimate the overlap between
variables because these methods of data analysis do not take measurement
errors into consideration. More rigorous data analysis methods, such as
errors-in variables (EIV) least squares regression models, should be used to
avoid attenuating coefficient estimation.
Neuroticism -.10** .12*** -.04 -.06 .03 -.14*** .21*** -.13*** .10** .03 .08 .04 -.17***
Extraversion .17*** .10** .23*** .16*** .06 .21*** -.04 .27*** -.03 .19*** .10** - .53***
.14***
Openness .40*** .06 .37*** .13*** .09** .36*** -.12*** .20*** .10** .06 .03 .19*** .26***
Agreeableness -.02 .00 -.01 - .05 .01 -.08 .03 -.06 .10** -.06 - .22***
.13*** .18***
Conscient- .29*** .22*** .24*** .05 .21*** .21*** .05 .54*** .19*** .16*** .03 .16*** .26***
iousness
Notes: Control variables: gender, grade, hometown, and SES; and **p<.01, ***p<.001.
30 Chapter Two
R2Total 0.33 0.14 0.27 0.12 0.09 0.26 0.14 0.39 0.12 0.12 0.06 0.28 0.44
R2demo 0.05 0.00 0.04 0.02 0.02 0.05 0.02 0.01 0.02 0.01 0.02 0.03 0.02
R2Personality 0.28 0.14 0.23 0.11 0.07 0.21 0.12 0.38 0.10 0.11 0.04 0.26 0.41
F 35.95*** 10.96*** 28.13*** 9.47*** 7.70*** 27.19*** 11.35*** 51.33*** 10.53*** 8.93*** 5.24*** 24.33*** 59.12***
df 915 915 915 915 915 915 915 915 915 915 915 915 915
ȕneuroticism -.12* .38*** .17** -.16** .39*** .11* .15** .34*** .20*** -.17*** .26***
ȕextraversion -.11* .16** .12* .20*** -.15* .33*** .26*** -.48*** .75***
ȕconscientiousness .22*** .38*** .13** .32*** .25*** .72*** .36*** .17** .31***
Notes: R2Total = contribution of gender, grade, hometown, SES, and personality to thinking styles; R2demo = contribution of gender,
grade, hometown, and SES to thinking styles; R2Personality = unique contribution of personality to thinking styles; and *p<.05,
**p<.01, ***p<.001. For greater clarity, insignificant regression coefficients are not shown.
Personality and its Relationship with Thinking Styles 31
Table 2.1 shows the correlations between thinking styles and personality
with the above control demographics. In order to balance type I and type II
errors, .01 was used as the cut-off value for the level of statistical
significance in partial correlations between thinking styles and personality
traits. 41 of the 65 correlations were significant (r = .09–.54).
The results from the EIV models (Table 2.2) showed that the percentages of
the variance in thinking styles explained by personality traits ranged from
4%–41%, with only two exceeding 30%: the variances in the external and
hierarchical thinking styles accounted for by personality traits. Among the
44 significant regression coefficients, almost two-thirds of the coefficients
were under .30, while four coefficients exceeded .50. The largest regression
coefficients were the ones for the extraversion personality trait for the
external thinking style and the conscientiousness personality trait for the
hierarchical thinking style.
CDSE is defined as one’s beliefs about how well he/she can perform in
career choice tasks (Taylor & Betz, 1983). CDSE has been proven to be an
important predictor for one’s career performance (Choi et al., 2012).
Meanwhile, career development is a major issue that university students
have to deal with because they are just about to begin their working life (J.
Fan, 2016). Furthermore, studies have found a close relationship between
thinking styles and CDSE (J. Fan, 2016), as well as a significant relationship
between personality and CDSE (Hartman & Betz, 2007). It is reasonable to
Personality and its Relationship with Thinking Styles 33
put thinking styles and personality into one regression model in order to
identify the amount of variance in CDSE which can be explained by styles
beyond personality.
The most accepted concept of subjective well-being is currently the one that
defines subjective well-being as “a person’s cognitive and affective
evaluations of his or her life as a whole” (E. Diener, Oishi, & Lucas, 2009,
p.187). According to this definition, subjective well-being includes a
cognitive judgment of life that is always indicated by life satisfaction and
emotional reactions to life events that are always indicated by positive affect
and negative affect. Furthermore, this definition also implies the subjective
nature of well-being, which refers to one’s personal perception of his/her
own well-being. Top-down theories of SWB contend that people’s
predispositions influence their experiences and the perceptions of their lives.
Among all of the personal predispositions, personality is the most
extensively studied variable. For example, following a meta-analysis of 137
studies, DeNeve and Cooper (1998) found that personality variables, such
as repressive-defensiveness, trust, emotional stability, locus of control,
desire for control, hardiness, self-esteem, and tension, are all associated with
subjective well-being. Specific to the Big Five personality traits, extraversion
(as a positive predictor) and neuroticism (as a negative predictor) are the
most repeatedly reported traits that are closely related to subjective well-
being. In addition, agreeableness and conscientiousness were also found to
have positive correlations with subjective well-being in some other studies
(Lucas & Diener, 2008). In contrast, intellectual styles have received little
attention in the field of subjective well-being. Despite this, a few studies on
the relationships between intellectual styles and mental health (e.g., Wise et
al., 1978; Chen & Zhang, 2010) have indicated that styles probably also play
a functional role in subjective well-being. Therefore, it is also reasonable to
put thinking styles and personality into one regression model to identify the
amount of variance in SWB which can be explained by styles beyond
personality.
1999; Fjell & Walhovd, 2004; Zhang & Sternberg, 2006). The findings of
this study help to clarify the position of thinking styles in the field of
individual differences. It suggests that investigating thinking styles beyond
personality is worthwhile in order to achieve a comprehensive understanding
of individual differences in human performance and development.
CHAPTER THREE
As one of the most controversial issues, style malleability has been hotly
debated for a long time (Zhang, 2013; Zhang & Sternberg, 2006). Some
scholars believe that styles are inborn characteristics and that they are
relatively static (e.g., Riding & Rayner, 1998), while others argue that they
are a joint product of nature and nurture, which means they can be cultivated.
For example, Sternberg (1994; Sternberg, 1997) pointed out in his mental
self-government theory that thinking styles are at least in part socialized.
Although some scholars tend to stress the genetic effect on the development
of styles (e.g., Riding & Rayner, 1998), Zhang and Sternberg (2005) claim
that styles represent states rather than traits based on the accumulating
findings from abundant studies. This means, to some extent, that styles can
be socialized and modified by the environment in which people reside.
However, it has to be admitted that research on the socialization of thinking
styles is still in its infancy.
2The present section draws heavily on the work by Zhang, L. F., Hong, Y., and
myself, titled “The Malleability of Thinking Styles Over One Year”, published in
Educational Psychology (Fan, Zhang, & Hong, 2020).
42 Chapter Three
At the population level (i.e., the group level), there are two types of changes
that are frequently examined in longitudinal studies. One is mean-level
change, and the other is rank-order consistency. Mean-level change (also
called normative change) refers to people’s general trends of change over
time (Roberts, Wood, & Caspi, 2008). This type of change is always
analyzed by comparing the difference between mean scores of certain
characteristics across two points in time. Rank-order consistency (also
called differential stability) is used to explore whether people remain stable
in the level of certain characteristics relative to their age cohorts. This
consistency is always indicated by correlation coefficients between two
occasions with the higher correlation coefficients meaning less changes to
one’s relative status in the level of certain characteristics (Donnellan,
Conger, & Burzette, 2007; Roberts et al., 2008).
The Socialization of Thinking Styles 43
This section presents the results from the examination of style change using
the above four analytic approaches (i.e., mean-level change, rank-order
consistency, individual stability, and ipsative consistency). The data was
extracted from the two waves of the present research with a one-year
interval. 743 of the 926 university students responded to the same
questionnaire at the second wave. The attrition analysis conducted by the t-
tests showed that there was no significant difference in any dimension of
thinking styles between the students who participated in both data
collections and those who dropped out. Therefore, the analyses of the
change of thinking styles were based on the sample of 743 participants who
completed both waves of data collection. The reasons that the present
research was conducted among Chinese university students were based on
the following concerns. When students enter university, they experience a
transition in both family and school life. With regard to family life, they
become more distant from home and learn to be more independent from
44 Chapter Three
Figure 3.1 Grade Differences in the Change of Six Thinking Styles Freshmen
Juniors
The Socialization of Thinking Styles 47
The results showed that nine of the 13 thinking styles had an increase at the
mean-level. Given that the participants were experiencing the transition
from adolescence to young adulthood, stronger preferences for most of the
thinking styles after a year could help students to be more flexible to deal
with different types of tasks. Furthermore, the change of thinking styles
showed significant grade differences. On the one hand, the results showed
that freshmen had a larger increase in the internal thinking style than juniors
over one year. During the first year in Chinese universities, the majority of
the courses in which freshmen were enrolled were in a traditional lecture
format and required significant self-study efforts. This might enhance the
freshmen’s preferences for working independently (i.e., internal). On the
other hand, compared with the freshmen, the juniors had a significantly
larger increase in the judicial, global, liberal, oligarchic, and external
thinking styles. This suggests that juniors had become more creative when
working on tasks (i.e., liberal), thought more critically when evaluating or
judging something (i.e., judicial), paid more attention to the overall picture
(i.e., global), had become more flexible dealing with multiple tasks (i.e.,
oligarchic), and had increased their preference to work with people (i.e.,
external) over the duration of a year. These results make sense because when
transitioning from juniors to seniors over the course of a year, students begin
to access more advanced courses in their disciplines. Teachers may require
students to possess more critical thinking skills and ask them to apply what
they have learned to manage small projects. During this period, students
also need to prepare themselves for their coming careers, so they may have
more diverse experiences rather than just academic learning. All of these
environments and experiences could be reinforcing their use of the above
thinking styles.
to moderate after one year, and students’ relative status in the levels of the
liberal, hierarchical, and external thinking styles were even more stable.
The results reveal that thinking styles had moderate to strong rank-order
stability, which means that students’ relative status among their counterparts
in the level of each thinking style was retained over one year. It is interesting
that the liberal thinking style had a significant change at the mean level
while, at the same time, it showed a strong rank-order consistency. This
pattern indicated that the liberal thinking styles may have a normative
increase over one year, but most students may have similar amounts of
increase, which leads to their relative rank status staying stable.
Chi-square
TS Decreased Stayed stable Increased (df = 2, N =
743)
legislative 1.70% 96.00% 2.30% 1.83
executive 2.40% 94.60% 3.00% 0.69
judicial 1.50% 95.40% 3.10% 4.15
global 1.90% 94.80% 3.40% 3.38
local 2.20% 95.70% 2.20% 0.71
liberal 1.60% 95.40% 3.00% 2.96
conservative 2.00% 94.80% 3.20% 2.30
hierarchical 1.90% 95.70% 2.40% 1.14
monarchic 2.00% 95.80% 2.20% 1.05
oligarchic 2.00% 93.30% 4.70% 15.62***
anarchic 1.10% 93.50% 5.40% 31.12***
internal 1.10% 95.40% 3.50% 9.01*
external 2.40% 95.30% 2.30% 0.14
Notes. TS = thinking styles; Decreased = Reliable Chang Index below -1.96; Stayed
stable = Reliable Change Index between -1.96 and 1.96; Increased = Reliable
Change Index above 1.96. The Chi-square tests were used to examine if the observed
distribution of Reliable Change Index was significantly different from 2.5%
(decreasing), 95% (stayed stable), and 2.5% (increasing). * p<.05. ** p < .01. *** p
< .001.
Table 3.3: The Distribution of the Reliable Change Index Among Two Grades
Further examination was conducted to see if gender, grade, and major made
differences in the ipsative consistency of thinking styles. Because D2 scores
were not normally distributed, Mann-Whitney U tests were adopted. The
results showed that there was no significant group difference in D2 based on
gender, grade, and major, suggesting that the configuration of thinking
styles changed in similar patterns across gender, grade, and major.
In summary, given the results from the four types of changes examined,
thinking styles have a moderate to high degree of stability, but this does not
mean that they are fixed characteristics because, even though it is at a small
magnitude, thinking styles are still changeable. The research findings
largely supported Zhang and Sternberg's (2005) view on style malleability:
styles are stable states rather than permanent traits.
High school teachers gave you homework every day and told you exactly
what you needed to do step-by-step. In university, there is no homework at
all! Teachers also do not provide any instruction on what you need to do. It
makes me feel like my life has no direction […] but it also allows me to read
whatever I like. (WHN)
In high school, everyone did the same thing and you always had teachers
keep an eye on you. In university, we all have different goals. There are
various modes of life open for you to choose. […] No one guides you. You
have great freedom and you have to consider everything for yourself. You
need to choose your own goal and your own way. You have to be global.
(MX)
It seems that, unlike the experience in high school where students were
restricted by an organized schedule set by teachers, as well as by the
pressure of the National College Entrance Examination, in university,
students become the master of their own lives; this autonomy provides many
opportunities for them to practice Type I styles. Meanwhile, university
learning activities rewarded Type I styles more than the ones in high schools.
The detailed descriptions provided by interviewees about what they had
experienced in the university environment resonate with the findings from
the quantitative study concerning the influence of learning environments on
students’ thinking styles, which will be discussed in detail in the third
section of this chapter. In general, the finding that students became more
likely to use the creativity-generating styles (Type I styles) when they
entered higher levels in the educational system echoes Fer’s (2012) review
of the studies on the correlations between educational level and intellectual
styles, which also indicates that the higher levels encourage the
development of Type I styles.
Furthermore, in terms of style change trends, the results from the main study
also showed that the increase of Type I thinking styles among juniors was
significantly greater than among freshmen. Based on the interviewees’
description, this is probably because of the differences between the
experiences that freshmen and juniors had during the research interval.
Although the university environment generally encouraged Type I thinking
styles, juniors seemed to experience more opportunities that promote Type
I thinking styles. It is interesting that, based on the quantitative study, the
learning environment that freshmen experienced and the one that the juniors
had experienced in the year between the pretest and the posttest did not
54 Chapter Three
differ significantly, but the interviewees still claimed some differences that
may not have been clearly detected by the inventory of learning
environment in the quantitative study.
First, even though both the learning environments for freshmen and juniors
encouraged constructivist learning, juniors become more deeply involved in
this type of learning than freshmen, which means that they were somewhat
more influenced by it than the freshmen. Some interviewees mentioned that,
in the first year of university life, freshmen had just left the harsh levels of
studying in high schools behind and, therefore, they were attracted to the
variety of university activities on offer. During this period, academic study
lost its core status in the students’ lives. Although the learning environment
seemed to create a climate that facilitates students’ Type I thinking styles,
the freshmen did not spend much time and energy becoming involved in it.
As one of the interviewees mentioned,
I spent most of my time in student activities and associations during the first
year [in university]. I only worked hard at learning at the end of the semester,
in order to deal with the exams. I know a lot of students like me. We all
embrace “Buddha's feet” at the last moment [we would just be cramming for
exams] (QBK).
On the contrary, the juniors were already over the period of being curious
about the novelty of university life and they had, instead, become gradually
aware of the importance of academic performance in their later career
development. For example,
My GPA (Grade Point Average) in the first year after I entered the university
was embarrassing. […] Last year, I decided to go abroad after my graduation
and I realized that GPA is a key element when applying for good overseas
universities. I am now working much harder than before and my GPA has
improved. (QBK)
Therefore, when the juniors adjusted their time and energy allocation, and
became more involved in academic learning, they increased their likelihood
of being influenced by learning environments.
During the first year of my university life, I did a lot of detailed things. For
example, when I was a junior officer in student association, I usually
delivered posters and persuaded students to participate in our activities. I
never designed an activity myself. This kind of general planning thing is
usually done by senior officers or managers.
The type of job roles that freshmen played in student associations and
organizations probably enabled them to practice their Type II styles instead
of encouraging Type I styles. In addition, the learning environment that
freshmen experienced may not be as constructivist as the one that the juniors
experienced, although the differences were not explicitly revealed in the
quantitative data. It may be because the students have used their previous
experiences as a baseline when responding to the questionnaires, in which
case, freshmen and juniors perceive their learning environments as almost
equally constructivist when the extent of the discrepancy between the
current learning environment and the one they experienced previously (high
school for freshmen and lower grades in university for juniors) were
perceived to be similar. Therefore, the quantitative data did not reveal the
differences between the learning environment perceived by freshmen and
the one perceived by juniors. However, the results from the qualitative study
did show that, although the learning environment that the freshmen
experienced in the first year of the university life was more constructivist
than the one in high schools, it was not as constructivist as the one
experienced by the juniors. For example, during the first year of university
life, although the students were impressed by certain courses taught in a
constructivist way, they still had a great number of elementary courses that
largely pertained to basic information on particular subject matters, whereas
the number of constructivist-oriented courses increased among juniors.
Meanwhile, the freshmen’s assessments included open-ended questions as
well as information recitation. In contrast, the portion of assessments that
completely valued deep thinking increased for juniors. For example,
Generally speaking, teachers of specialized courses tend to implement class
discussions more and encourage students to think critically and deeply. In
the first two years of university life, we did not have too many specialized
courses. At that time, we also had many basic courses. In basic courses, it
was usually just the teacher who did the talking and almost 100 students in
the same classroom were just listening. (ZJL)
Therefore, the mixed nature of the courses and assessments that freshmen
experienced may have also confounded the trend of developing Type I
thinking styles to some extent.
Third, there are probably some factors outside the campus that also exert an
impact on the students’ development of thinking styles. During the
interviews, many juniors mentioned that they undertook internships during
the third year of their university life. As they were brand-new experiences,
the internships provided new ways of viewing themselves and the world
around them, which also significantly affected their thinking styles. For
example,
I think the internship had a great impact on me. I worked for a liberal
magazine. The atmosphere was liberal and creative. You could bring out
every idea that came into your mind. In addition, the fact that you need to
handle multiple tasks simultaneously helped you to learn how to manage
stress and become increasingly better at multitasking. (ZJL)
Netter, & Keehn, 2007), mental health and well-being (Fletcher, Walls,
Cook, Madison, & Bridges, 2008), personality (Reti et al., 2002), academic
achievement (Spera, 2005), and social behaviors (Steinberg, 2001).
Current findings
In the first round of data collection, participants were asked to recall their
experiences with their parents during the first 16 years of their lives when
they responded to the Parenting Style Index. In the second round of data
collection, which took place a year later, they responded to the Parenting
Style Index according to their experience with their parents in the period
between the two occasions of data collection. This enables us to see both
remote and recent influences of parenting styles on thinking styles. As in
other analyses, I controlled the relevant demographic factors in the
subsequent analyses. In the first wave of data, gender, grade, and hometown
made significant differences in thinking styles, while gender and major made
significant differences in parenting styles. In the second wave of data, thinking
styles differed based on gender and grade, while parenting styles differed
based on gender and hometown. Furthermore, socioeconomic status (SES)
was significantly associated with both parenting styles and thinking styles in
both waves of data. As SES is one of the familial factors, the relationships
between SES and thinking styles were highlighted (see Table 3.4).
The legislative, judicial, global, liberal, and hierarchical thinking styles that
were found to have significant positive relationships with SES all belong to
Type I thinking styles, which means that students from families with a
higher socioeconomic status were more likely to have styles characterized
by higher cognitive complexity and creativity. This finding was consistent
with earlier studies (Ho, 1998; Sternberg & Grigorenko, 1995; Tse, 2003;
Yang & Lin, 2004; Zhang & Postiglione, 2001).
Table 3.5: Predicting Thinking Styles from the Three Dimensions of Parenting Styles (Time 1)
TS Type I Type II Type III
Leg Jud Glo Lib Hie Exe Loc Con Mon Oli Ana Int Ext
2
R Total 0.07 0.07 0.04 0.06 0.07 0.08 0.04 0.08 0.06 0.09 0.05 0.03 0.12
R2demo 0.04 0.03 0.02 0.05 0.01 0.00 0.01 0.02 0.02 0.01 0.02 0.02 0.02
R2PS 0.03 0.04 0.02 0.01 0.06 0.08 0.03 0.08 0.04 0.08 0.03 0.01 0.10
F 12.30*** 11.22*** 5.77*** 10.26*** 12.10*** 12.65*** 5.91*** 13.60*** 9.52*** 15.31*** 8.72*** 5.30*** 20.81***
df 6,918 6,918 6,918 6,918 6,918 6,918 6,918 6,918 6,918 6,918 6,918 6,918 6,918
*** *** *** ** *** *** *** *** *** *** ***
ȕinvol .19 .20 .14 .11 .26 .24 .13 .17 .16 .29 .16 .34***
ȕpsy_au -.10** -.09** -.13*** -.09** -.16*** -.12*** -.14*** -.18*** -.10**
Notes: TS = thinking styles; Leg = Legislative; Jud = Judicial; Glo = Global; Lib = Liberal; Hie = Hierarchical; Exe = Executive; Loc
= Local; Con = Conservative; Mon = Monarchic; Oli = Oligarchic; Ana = Anarchic; Int = Internal; Ext = External; R2Total = the
contribution of gender, grade, SES, and parenting styles to thinking styles; R2demo = the contribution of gender, grade, and SES to
thinking styles; R2PS = the unique contribution of parenting styles to thinking styles; invol = acceptance/involvement; psy_au =
psychological autonomy-granting; super = strictness/supervision; and *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001.
The Socialization of Thinking Styles 63
Table 3.6: Predicting Thinking Styles from the Three Dimensions of Parenting Styles (Time 2)
2
R demo 0.03 0.03 0.05 0.07 0.01 0.01 0.02 0.02 0.03 0.02 0.04 0.04 0.01
R2PS 0.06 0.06 0.03 0.05 0.09 0.08 0.06 0.08 0.06 0.14 0.04 0.02 0.13
F 8.37*** 8.03*** 6.62*** 10.76*** 9.55*** 7.88*** 6.87*** 8.94*** 7.39*** 15.08*** 7.25*** 5.14*** 12.45***
df 9,724 9,724 9,724 9,724 9,724 9,724 9,724 9,724 9,724 9,724 9,724 9,724 9,724
*** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** **
ȕinvol .29 .28 .14 .27 .32 .27 .23 .21 .24 .36 .13 .40***
ȕpsy_au -.15*** -.15** -.09* -.11** -.18** -.20*** -.17*** -.21*** -.21*** -.15*** -.08*
Notes: TS = thinking styles; Leg = Legislative; Jud = Judicial; Glo = Global; Lib = Liberal; Hie = Hierarchical; Exe = Executive; Loc
= Local; Con = Conservative; Mon = Monarchic; Oli = Oligarchic; Ana = Anarchic; Int = Internal; Ext = External; R2Total = the
contribution of gender, grade, hometown, SES, and parenting styles to thinking styles; R2demo = the contribution of gender, grade,
hometown, and SES to thinking styles; R2PS = the unique contribution of parenting styles to thinking styles; invol =
acceptance/involvement; psy_au = psychological autonomy-granting; super=strictness/supervision; and *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001.
64 Chapter Three
Table 3.7: Style Differences Based on the Types of Parenting Styles (MANCOVA) (Time 1)
Thinking Styles Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Partial Eta Squared
Type I Legislative 19.95 3 6.65 8.20 .000 .03
Judicial 21.92 3 7.31 8.32 .000 .03
Hierarchical 40.82 3 13.61 14.50 .000 .05
Type II Executive 37.53 3 12.51 14.70 .000 .05
Local 13.70 3 4.57 4.35 .005 .01
Conservative 20.40 3 6.80 6.54 .000 .02
Monarchic 15.66 3 5.22 5.00 .002 .02
Type III Oligarchic 40.10 3 13.37 14.76 .000 .05
External 78.36 3 26.12 24.27 .000 .07
The Socialization of Thinking Styles 65
Table 3.8: Style Differences Based on the Types of Parenting Styles (MANCOVA) (Time 2)
Thinking Styles Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Partial Eta Squared
Type I Legislative 36.53 3 12.18 18.68 .000 .072
Judicial 21.35 3 7.12 8.48 .000 .034
Liberal 36.65 3 12.22 12.01 .000 .047
Hierarchical 52.07 3 17.36 22.11 .000 .084
Type II Executive 29.45 3 9.82 15.59 .000 .060
Local 17.42 3 5.81 5.97 .001 .024
Conservative 11.90 3 3.97 4.73 .003 .019
Monarchic 14.92 3 4.97 5.30 .001 .021
Type III Oligarchic 47.93 3 15.98 18.90 .000 .072
External 75.45 3 25.15 26.36 .000 .098
66 Chapter Three
The results based on the data from Time 1 and Time 2 were consistent,
which means that the contribution of parenting styles in either the first 16
years of the students’ lives or in the past year to thinking styles was quite
similar. In terms of the dimensional approach, it was found that parenting
styles could explain 1–10% and 2–14% of the variance in thinking styles at
Time 1 (Table 3.5) and at Time 2 (Table 3.6), respectively. At both Time 1
and Time 2, it was found that parental acceptance/involvement was positively
associated with all but the internal style, which partially supported the
hypothesis. Meanwhile, also partially in line with expectations,
psychological autonomy-granting was negatively related to some Type I
styles, Type II styles, and Type III styles. In addition, perceived parents’
supervision positively predicted some Type II styles at both Time 1 and
Time 2, and it also negatively contributed to two Type I styles at Time 2,
which was totally consistent with expectations.
indulgent parenting styles also scored higher than those who perceived their
parents as using the authoritarian parenting style. The only differences
between the results at Time 1 and Time 2 were that, at Time 1, style
differences based on types of parenting styles were also found in the judicial
style and the conservative style, while at Time 2, there were also differences
in the liberal style based on types of parenting styles.
The above results revealed that parenting styles had a significant influence
on students’ thinking styles. Specifically, parental involvement positively
contributed to the majority of thinking styles while the psychological
autonomy (from parents) that students perceived negatively contributed to
the majority of thinking styles, which partially supported the specific
research hypotheses. In addition, it was found that, exactly as hypothesized,
parental strictness/supervision negatively contributed to Type I thinking
styles and positively contributed to Type II styles. The results regarding
types of parenting styles also repeatedly found that students raised under the
authoritative and the indulgent parenting styles are more likely to develop
Type I thinking styles and some of Type III thinking styles than those raised
by parents using the authoritarian and the neglectful parenting styles.
68 Chapter Three
Table 3.9: Multiple Comparisons for Thinking Styles Based on Types of Parenting Styles (Time 1)
95% CI
Thinking Styles PS(I) PS(J) Mean Difference(I-J) Std. Error Sig.
Lower Bound Upper Bound
Type I Legislative 1 4 0.35 0.08 .000 0.13 0.56
3 4 0.32 0.09 .008 0.06 0.58
Judicial 1 4 0.36 0.08 .000 0.14 0.58
3 4 0.42 0.10 .000 0.15 0.69
Hierarchical 1 2 0.33 0.09 .003 0.08 0.59
1 4 0.53 0.08 .000 0.31 0.76
3 4 0.48 0.10 .000 0.21 0.75
Type II Executive 1 4 0.48 0.08 .000 0.26 0.69
Conservative 1 4 0.33 0.09 .002 0.09 0.58
Type III Oligarchic 1 4 0.47 0.08 .000 0.25 0.70
3 4 0.36 0.10 .003 0.09 0.63
External 1 2 0.57 0.10 .000 0.30 0.84
1 4 0.63 0.09 .000 0.39 0.88
3 2 -0.58 0.11 .000 0.27 0.90
3 4 0.65 0.10 .000 0.36 0.94
Notes: PS = parenting styles; 1 = authoritative; 2 = authoritarian; 3 = indulgent; 4 = neglectful.
The Socialization of Thinking Styles 69
Table 3.10: Multiple Comparisons for Thinking Styles Based on Types of Parenting Styles (Time 2)
95% CI
Thinking Styles PS(I) PS(J) Mean Difference(I-J) Std. Error Sig.
Lower Bound Upper Bound
Type I Legislative 1 2 0.33 0.09 .006 0.07 0.59
1 4 0.41 0.07 .000 0.21 0.62
3 4 0.42 0.09 .000 0.17 0.68
Liberal 1 2 0.43 0.12 .004 0.10 0.76
3 2 0.52 0.14 .002 0.14 0.90
Hierarchical 1 2 0.51 0.10 .000 0.23 0.79
1 4 0.61 0.08 .000 0.39 0.83
3 4 0.46 0.10 .000 0.19 0.74
Type II Executive 1 4 0.47 0.07 .000 0.27 0.66
Type III Oligarchic 1 4 0.49 0.08 .000 0.26 0.73
3 4 0.47 0.10 .000 0.18 0.76
External 1 2 0.59 0.11 .000 0.28 0.91
1 4 0.56 0.09 .000 0.32 0.81
3 2 0.68 0.13 .000 0.32 1.04
3 4 0.64 0.11 .000 0.34 0.95
Notes: PS = parenting styles; 1 = authoritative; 2 = authoritarian; 3 = indulgent; 4 = neglectful.
70 Chapter Three
Data from the follow-up interviews showed that students were not fully
aware of the influence of parenting styles on thinking styles. When they
were asked to respond to “what factors caused the change of your thinking
styles”, they talked a lot about learning environments but did not voluntarily
mention parenting. However, when they were asked the question “what
about the influence from the parenting you experienced”, they all responded
to the question without any hesitation: “of course it has an influence;
actually, a very important influence on my thinking styles”. But when it
came to the question of “how”, they all failed to address it specifically. This
is probably because, first, after entering university, students spent most of
their time in learning environments and their interactions with their parents
only filled a minimal portion of their life, which made them less likely to
instantly recall the factor of parenting. After all, the memories about their
learning environments were fresher. Second, parents seem to influence
children through osmosis. It is subtle and gradually formed. Furthermore,
people tend to not be as sensitive to the influence of a relatively constant
environment (e.g., family environment) than to the influence of a
changeable environment (e.g., learning environment).
However, when the interviewees were asked to just describe how their
parents parented them and how they responded to this parenting, the
connection between parenting and thinking styles emerged. This finding
further explains the results obtained from the main study.
Second, students who perceived that their parents had given them high
psychological autonomy had different responses to this kind of parenting.
On the one hand, some students said that, because their parents always
respected them and encouraged them to make decisions by themselves, they
had never thought that it was necessary to obey authorities. Therefore, these
students were less likely to develop Type II styles. On the other hand, some
students also considered psychological autonomy to be an indicator of their
parents’ trust and that this should be reciprocal. Therefore, they were more
The Socialization of Thinking Styles 71
willing to be a “standard good” child and they usually tried to avoid doing
anything that might make their parents worried or anxious. The concept of
“not being rebellious as a repayment for parents’ trust” (MSJ) probably led
these students to suppress the development of Type I styles, which are
creativity-oriented and fond of novelty. The ambivalence in the ways the
students treated the psychological autonomy obtained from their parents
may partially explain the results from the main study, which indicated that
students who perceived higher levels of psychological autonomy from their
parents had lower scores on both Type I and Type II thinking styles.
Actually, some studies (e.g., Greenfield, Keller, Fuligni, & Maynard, 2003;
Qin, Pomerantz, & Wang, 2009) have suggested that psychological
autonomy granted by parents may not benefit child development in China
as much as it does in Western countries due to the cultural differences in
attitudes to the value of independence and interdependence. The students’
complicated emotions and feelings when they responded to the
psychological autonomy granted by their parents may be ascribed to the
mixed influence of their increasing need for autonomy during the process
of growth and the cultural beliefs about interdependence and harmony
within the family.
3 This section partially draws on the work by Zhang, L. F. and myself, titled “The
role of learning environments in thinking styles”, published in Educational
Psychology (Fan & Zhang, 2014).
The Socialization of Thinking Styles 73
There are also several studies that assess intellectual styles using Witkin’s
(1962) theory of field dependence/independence to examine the influence
of a constructivist learning environment on intellectual styles. Luk (1998)
found that in a self-directed learning environment created by a distance-
74 Chapter Three
1) Constructivist learning
2) Student autonomy
The measure that assessed the learning environments in the present research
aimed to reflect the above four characteristics. Meanwhile, this measure was
supposed to be able to assess students’ perception of the general learning
environment rather than just a specific course. However, to the best of my
knowledge, most of the existing inventories on learning environments were
designed to assess learning environments within particular courses.
Furthermore, many inventories focus on a limited range of dimensions in
teaching, such as course design, assessments, and teaching methods, which
have been criticized for being too teacher-centered (e.g., Centra, 1993;
The Socialization of Thinking Styles 79
Kolitch & Dean, 1999). Fortunately, some inventories that have been
recently developed have noticed other dimensions in learning environments.
For example, Pace and Kuh (2007) designed a questionnaire to assess
students’ experience of activities on campus, while Wierstra, Kanselaar, van
der Linden, & Lodewijks (1999) added the dimension of student
participation in their inventory. Others included teacher-student and
student-student relationships (e.g., Entwistle, McCune, & Hounsell, 2003;
Kember & Leung, 2009). However, although these inventories succeeded
in expanding the breadth of the dimensions included in the questionnaires
that assess the learning environment to some extent, none of them was
comprehensive enough to provide a systematic examination of the general
learning environment. Therefore, given that the existing inventories on
learning environments are either too class-specific or only include a limited
range of dimensions, it became meaningful to develop an encompassing
learning environment inventory that can assess students’ general perception
of various learning dimensions both inside and outside classroom.
Current findings
In the literature it has been shown that a constructivist learning environment
usually aims to encourage students to experience the process of knowledge
construction through exploration, as well as to be autonomous in their
learning. These objectives are in line with the characteristics of Type I styles
that manifest autonomy, initiative, and creativity, but they are contrary to
the characteristics of Type II styles that are instruction-conforming and
conservative. Therefore, it was hypothesized that, statistically, constructivist
learning environments would positively contribute to Type I thinking styles
(Hypothesis 1) and negatively contribute to Type II thinking styles
(Hypothesis 2). As the nature of Type III styles is context-dependent, no
specific hypothesis concerning Type III styles was made. The research
findings revealed that some of the above hypotheses were supported while
others were not.
It should be noted that during the data collection of Time 1 in the main study,
freshmen were asked to recall their experiences in high schools while
juniors were asked about their university experiences. Separate analyses of
The Socialization of Thinking Styles 81
Leg Jud Glo Lib Hie Exe Loc Con Mon Oli Ana Int Ext
2
R Total 0.15 0.12 0.05 0.15 0.15 0.09 0.11 0.06 0.07 0.13 0.07 0.05 0.32
R2demo 0.04 0.03 0.02 0.05 0.01 0.00 0.02 0.03 0.02 0.01 0.02 0.03 0.02
R2LE 0.11 0.09 0.03 0.10 0.14 0.09 0.10 0.03 0.05 0.12 0.05 0.03 0.30
F 12.20*** 9.80*** 3.43*** 12.33*** 11.97*** 6.95*** 8.83*** 4.32*** 5.49*** 9.96*** 5.50*** 3.98*** 32.29***
df 15,899 15,899 15,899 15,899 15,899 15,899 15,899 15,899 15,899 15,899 15,899 15,899 15,899
*
ȕcons .10
ȕasse .12** .14*** .22*** .14*** .16*** .09* .11** .19*** .09**
ȕfaci .10*
2 2
Notes: TS = thinking styles; R Total = the contribution of gender, grade, major, hometown, SES, and learning environments to thinking styles; R demo = the contribution of
gender, grade, major, hometown, and SES to thinking styles; R2LE = the unique contribution of learning environments to thinking styles; cons = constructivist-oriented
teaching; goal = clear goals and coherence of curricula; auto = student autonomy; asse = assessment and assignment; tsin = teacher-student interaction; ssco = student-student
cooperation; peer = peer morale and identities; faci = learning facilities; and *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001.
The Socialization of Thinking Styles 83
Table 3.12: Predicting Thinking Styles from Learning Environments (Time 2)
TS
Type I Type II Type III
Leg Jud Glo Lib Hie Exe Loc Con Mon Oli Ana Int Ext
2
R Total 0.25 0.20 0.12 0.19 0.22 0.17 0.17 0.10 0.13 0.22 0.11 0.10 0.33
2
R demo 0.04 0.03 0.05 0.07 0.01 0.01 0.02 0.02 0.02 0.03 0.04 0.04 0.02
2
R LE 0.21 0.18 0.07 0.12 0.21 0.16 0.15 0.07 0.11 0.19 0.07 0.06 0.31
*** *** *** ** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** ***
F 19.72 15.42 8.20 13.95 17.42 12.04 12.28 6.63 9.08 16.65 7.27 6.61 29.74***
df 12,728 12,728 12,728 12,728 12,728 12,728 12,728 12,728 12,728 12,728 12,728 12,728 12,728
ȕauto
ȕtsin
Notes: TS = thinking styles; R2Total = the contribution of gender, grade, major, SES, and learning environments to thinking styles; R2demo = the contribution of gender, grade,
major, and SES to thinking styles; R2LE = the unique contribution of learning environments to thinking styles; cons = constructivist-oriented teaching; goal = clear goals and
coherence of curricula; auto = student autonomy; asse = assessment and assignment; tsin = teacher-student interaction; ssco = student-student cooperation; peer = peer morale
and identities; faci = learning facilities; and *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001.
84 Chapter Three
Second, the results also indicated that students whose classmates and friends
were proactive in learning and interested in knowledge connection and
application (i.e. had higher scores in peer morale and identities) tended to
think critically (judicial style) and globally (global style), tended to
demonstrate open-mindedness (liberal style), and preferred unstructured
thinking (legislative style). Compared with peer influence, the contribution
of teacher-student interaction was tenuous, especially in the university
environment. Interviewees in the follow-up qualitative study claimed that
the frequency and the intensity of teacher-student interaction decreased
dramatically from high school to university. Instead, the time spent with
peers in learning occasions or daily life significantly increased. For example,
The Socialization of Thinking Styles 85
Most teachers just showed up during lectures. If you are not proactive
enough, it is then hard to approach them out of the class. The undergraduate
courses seem too simple and basic for teachers, and they are busy doing their
cutting-edge research. If we encounter difficulties in studying, we usually
need to seek help through the internet or discuss it with our peers. We seldom
look to teachers. (MSJ)
Apart from the above dimensions, it was also found that the dimension of
student-student cooperation played a more salient role in their Type III
styles. The results indicated that students who perceived more support and
cooperation from classmates tended to select their tasks based on others’
suggestions (oligarchic style) and to work with others (external style) rather
than to do things alone (internal style). This finding makes sense because
students who experienced more student cooperation would value others’
suggestions to a greater degree and would be more willing to work with
others. Nonetheless, an alternative explanation could be that students with
the oligarchic and external styles rely more on others, which leads them to
interact more with others; this then enables them to perceive more student-
student cooperation than their counterparts. Students with the internal
thinking style are more independent, so they tend to interact with others less,
which would make them perceive less student-student cooperation than
their counterparts. Although the two explanations both seem tempting, the
interview data provided plausible evidence to support the former
explanation. For example, as one interviewee stated,
The Socialization of Thinking Styles 87
I was quite introverted before, mostly because I was not confident enough, I
think. During university life, after experiencing many student cooperation
projects and class discussions, I found that even if some students could not
express themselves very well or their arguments did not make sense, they
were still able to confidently give their opinion and other people listened to
them with great tolerance. I gradually realized that I can do this too. (ZJL)
One dimension that was found to contribute to all types of thinking styles
(but only at Time 2) was learning facilities. In terms of learning facilities,
both quantitative data and qualitative data indicated that learning facilities
in universities were significantly more abundant than in high schools.
Therefore, it is not surprising that the associations between learning
facilities and thinking styles were weak based on the analysis of the first
wave of the data set, which included the data of high school environments.
However, the analysis of the second wave of data set, where the research
totally focused on one-year experience in university, found that learning
facilities played a salient role in all three types of thinking styles. This may
be because the nature and features of the available resources and
opportunities on campus are diverse. The diversity of learning facilities and
the different ways that students used them provided them with opportunities
to develop diverse thinking styles. For example, some students may use
library resources to download information, while some others may use them
to design a project. Some laboratories may provide students with
opportunities to apply their knowledge as research assistants and invite them
to participate in the process of knowledge construction, while others may
only ask them to maintain equipment and clean. In order to achieve a better
understanding of how specific learning facilities influence students’
thinking styles, further research is needed to refine the exploration of
different learning facilities and interrogate the ways in which students use
resources and opportunities, rather than merely examining their availability.
It is necessary to note that, the results from the follow-up interviews also
indicated that there were other factors in university life beyond the range of
dimensions examined in the quantitative study which also exerted an
influence on their thinking styles. These include experiences in student
associations, internships outside campus, and the management of daily life
(see “The change of thinking styles” section in this chapter for more details).
Leg Jud Glo Lib Hie Exe Loc Con Mon Oli Ana Int Ext
2
R Total 0.29 0.24 0.11 0.26 0.35 0.18 0.15 0.18 0.14 0.19 0.12 0.20 0.46
R2demo 0.04 0.03 0.02 0.05 0.01 0.00 0.02 0.03 0.03 0.01 0.02 0.03 0.02
2
R PS 0.03 0.04 0.02 0.01 0.07 0.08 0.03 0.06 0.04 0.09 0.03 0.01 0.10
R2LE 0.08 0.06 0.02 0.09 0.08 0.05 0.08 0.02 0.03 0.07 0.05 0.03 0.20
R2Pers 0.13 0.11 0.05 0.10 0.19 0.06 0.03 0.08 0.05 0.03 0.02 0.14 0.13
F 15.43*** 12.26*** 4.79*** 13.55*** 20.77*** 8.72*** 6.93*** 8.55*** 6.54*** 8.84*** 5.07*** 9.75*** 33.34***
df 23,891 23,891 23,891 23,891 23,891 23,891 23,891 23,891 23,891 23,891 23,891 23,891 23,891
ȕpsy_au -.08* -.07* -.08** -.11** -.10** -.12*** -.10** -.12*** -.14*** -.08*
ȕcons
ȕfaci .09*
Notes: TS = thinking styles; Leg = Legislative; Jud = Judicial; Glo = Global; Lib = Liberal; Hie = Hierarchical; Exe = Executive; Loc
= Local; Con = Conservative; Mon = Monarchic; Oli = Oligarchic; Ana = Anarchic; Int = Internal; Ext = External; R2Total = the
contribution of gender, grade, major, hometown, SES, parenting styles, learning environments, and personality to thinking styles;
R2demo = the contribution of gender, grade, major, hometown, and SES to thinking styles; R2PS = the unique contribution of parenting
styles to thinking styles; R2LE = the unique contribution of learning environments to thinking styles; R2Pers = the unique contribution of
personality to thinking styles; invol = acceptance/involvement; psy_au = psychological autonomy-granting; super =
strictness/supervision; cons = constructivist-oriented teaching; goal = clear goals and coherence of curricula; auto = student autonomy;
asse = assessment and assignment; tsin = teacher-student interaction; ssco = student-student cooperation; peer = peer morale and
identities; faci = learning facilities; neuro = neuroticism; extra = extraversion; open = openness to experience; agree = agreeableness;
cient = conscientiousness; and *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001.
92 Chapter Three
Table 3.14: Predicting Thinking Styles from Parenting Styles, Learning Environments, and Personality Traits (Time
2)
TS Type I Type II Type III
Leg Jud Glo Lib Hie Exe Loc Con Mon Oli Ana Int Ext
2
R Total 0.39 0.33 0.20 0.34 0.42 0.25 0.22 0.20 0.21 0.30 0.17 0.26 0.47
R2demo 0.04 0.03 0.05 0.07 0.01 0.02 0.02 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.04 0.04 0.02
2
R PS 0.06 0.06 0.03 0.05 0.09 0.08 0.06 0.08 0.06 0.14 0.04 0.02 0.13
R2LE 0.16 0.13 0.06 0.09 0.14 0.10 0.11 0.04 0.08 0.10 0.05 0.05 0.21
2
R Pers 0.13 0.10 0.06 0.13 0.17 0.05 0.03 0.06 0.05 0.03 0.03 0.15 0.12
F 19.32*** 14.94*** 7.45*** 15.57*** 22.18*** 10.02*** 8.51*** 7.78*** 8.39*** 13.35*** 6.27*** 10.84*** 27.69***
df 23,709 23,709 23,709 23,709 23,709 23,709 23,709 23,709 23,709 23,709 23,709 23,709 23,709
ȕpsy_au -.10** -.08* -.07* -.11** -.12** -.10** -.17*** -.12** -.13***
ȕcons .12*
ȕtsin
ȕneuro .11** .08* .21*** .11** .23*** .10* .16*** .11** .13***
ȕextra .10* .10* .12** .08* -.09* .16*** .13** -.30*** .41***
ȕagree -.11*** -.13*** -.16*** -.17*** -.07* -.13*** .12*** -.13*** -.25*** .09**
ȕcient .12*** -.18*** .08* .42*** .17*** .15*** .19*** .13*** -.07*
Notes: TS = thinking styles; Leg = Legislative; Jud = Judicial; Glo = Global; Lib = Liberal; Hie = Hierarchical; Exe = Executive; Loc
= Local; Con = Conservative; Mon = Monarchic; Oli = Oligarchic; Ana = Anarchic; Int = Internal; Ext = External; R2Total = the
contribution of gender, grade, major, hometown, SES, parenting styles, learning environments, and personality to thinking styles;
R2demo = the contribution of gender, grade, major, hometown, and SES to thinking styles; R2PS = the unique contribution of parenting
styles to thinking styles; R2LE = the unique contribution of learning environments to thinking styles; R2Pers = the unique contribution of
personality to thinking styles; invol = acceptance/involvement; psy_au = psychological autonomy-granting; super =
strictness/supervision; cons = constructivist-oriented teaching; goal = clear goals and coherence of curricula; auto = student autonomy;
asse = assessment and assignment; tsin = teacher-student interaction; ssco = student-student cooperation; peer = peer morale and
identities; faci = learning facilities; neuro = neuroticism; extra = extraversion; open = openness to experience; agree = agreeableness;
cient = conscientiousness; and *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001.
94 Chapter Three
There are two types of change models. One is called the unconditional
change model where the change of the dependent variable is predicted by
the independent variable at Time 1. This change model assumes that the
change of the dependent variable is irrelevant to the initial score of the
dependent variable, which is seldom true in reality. Therefore, the
conditional change score model that controls for the initial score of the
dependent variable is always preferred in research to capture the factors that
really cause the change.
to have higher scores at posttest. (Taris, 2000). Although there was the
“regression toward the mean” effect, the parenting styles and the learning
environments that students experienced in the year between the pretest and
the posttest still had made unique contributions to the change of thinking
styles. Specifically, parental involvement contributed to the increase of
most thinking styles, while psychological autonomy from parents
contributed to a decrease in most thinking styles, which partially supported
the hypotheses. Parental supervision mainly decreased Type I thinking
styles, which was in line with expectations. To further explore the
interactions between parenting dimensions, the role of parenting styles in
the change of thinking styles was further examined based on the typology
of parenting styles by conducting MANOVAs. It was found that the types
of parenting styles that students experienced during the year between the
pretest and the posttest did not make any significant difference in the change
of thinking styles.
The results from the conditional change models (Table 3.15) about the
contribution of learning environments to the change of thinking styles
partially supported the relevant hypotheses. Specifically, constructivist-
oriented teaching and peer morale mainly promoted Type I styles, while
clear goals and coherence of curricula primarily increased Type II styles. In
addition, student-student cooperation mainly contributed to the change of
Type III styles, and learning facilities had an effect on all three Types of
thinking styles. This pattern was quite similar to the one found in the cross-
sectional data.
power differed. In the first wave of data collection, the predictive power of
parenting styles that students experienced in the first 16 years in their lives
was examined with previous experience in learning environments and
personality traits together. The results from this set of data indicated that the
parenting styles that students experienced in the first 16 years of their lives
were the major predictors for two of the 13 thinking styles (executive and
oligarchic). However, in the longitudinal data analysis where the influence
of parenting styles that students experienced in the year between the pretest
and the protest was examined with learning environments in the same
research interval and personality traits at the initial stage of the research, it
was found that parenting styles were not a major predictor for any of the
thinking styles. These findings indicated that, although parenting styles had
made unique contributions to students’ thinking styles, they were not
powerful predictors for thinking styles; in addition, the influence of
parenting styles on thinking styles decreased over one year. It is
understandable that the influence of parenting styles on students’
development decreased when students went into university education
because, during this period, they lived away from their parents and had
limited communication with them.
Considering all of the above findings, I would like to address two points.
First, the specific relationships of the various dimensions of parenting styles
and learning environments to thinking styles found in longitudinal models
were similar to the ones identified earlier in the separate regression models
of each predictor based on the cross-sectional data. This means that the
relationships of parenting styles and learning environments to thinking
styles are more likely to be causal than occasional. Second, even though
personality traits served as the major predictors for the majority of thinking
styles, the two environmental factors (parenting styles and learning
environments) still made unique contributions beyond personality traits to
thinking styles, although the magnitude of the predictive power was not
large. It seems that personality traits provide a baseline of thinking styles
and the environmental factors then adjust them. These adjustments may not
be dramatic, but they do happen. This finding once again supported the
argument that styles are stable status rather than permanent traits.
98 Chapter Three
df 24,708 24,708 24,708 24,708 24,708 24,708 24,708 24,708 24,708 24,708 24,708 24,708 24,708
Parenting Styles
ȕinvol .09** .11** .13*** .13*** .10** .10** .10** .11** .15*** .14***
ȕpsy_au -.09** -.09** -.08* -.06* -.11*** -.12*** -.11*** -.14*** -.13*** -.12***
ȕsuper -.07* -.11*** -.07* -.09** -.09**
Learning Environments
ȕcons .14** .17*** .11* .10*
ȕgoal .10* .09* .11*
ȕauto .09* .11**
ȕasse .16*** .11**
ȕtsin .11*
ȕssco .09* .11** .12** -.13** .27***
ȕpeer .11** .09*
The Socialization of Thinking Styles 99
ȕfaci .16*** .10* .09* .18*** .10* .14*** .09*
Personality
ȕneuro .08* .07*
ȕextra -.07* .07* -.08* .11**
ȕopen -.08** -.09** -.08** .07* -.07*
ȕagree -.07* -.07*
ȕcient .08* .07* .08*
TS
ȕtime1 -.66*** -.63*** -.62*** -.65*** -.67*** -.70*** -.64*** -.66*** -.67*** -.67*** -.59*** -.63*** -.73***
Notes: C-TS = Change of thinking styles; Leg = Legislative; Jud = Judicial; Glo = Global; Lib = Liberal; Hie = Hierarchical; Exe =
Executive; Loc = Local; Con = Conservative; Mon = Monarchic; Oli = Oligarchic; Ana = Anarchic; Int = Internal; Ext = External;
R2Total = the contribution of gender, grade, major, hometown, SES, parenting styles at Time 2, learning environments at Time 2,
personality at Time 1, and thinking styles at Time 1 to the change of thinking styles from Time 1 to Time 2; R2demo = the contribution
of gender, grade, major, hometown, and SES to the change of thinking styles; R2PS = the unique contribution of parenting styles during
the year between the pretest and the posttest to the change of thinking styles; R2LE = the unique contribution of learning environments
during the year between the pretest and the posttest to the change of thinking styles; R2Pers = the unique contribution of personality at
Time 1 to the change of thinking styles; R2TS1 = the unique contribution of thinking styles at Time 1 to the change of thinking styles;
invol = acceptance/involvement; psy_au = psychological autonomy-granting; super = strictness/supervision; cons = constructivist-
oriented teaching; goal = clear goals and coherence of curricula; auto = student autonomy; asse = assessment and assignment; tsin =
teacher-student interaction; ssco = student-student cooperation; peer = peer morale and identities; faci = learning facilities; neuro =
neuroticism; extra = extraversion; open = openness to experience; agree = agreeableness; cient = conscientiousness; and *p<.05,
**p<.01, ***p<.001. The number of R2 in bold means the F change was significant after the predictor was added into the regression
model.
CHAPTER FOUR
The value of styles can be reflected through the line of research that
examines the relationships between styles and individual developmental
outcomes. If some specific styles have been very often found to play more
positive roles than others in individual developmental outcomes, then styles
can be regarded as value-laden. If styles have been found to play a diverse
role in different contexts, they can be considered to be value-differentiated.
The research findings are also complex in terms of thinking styles. On the
one hand, Type I thinking styles (e.g., legislative, judicial, and hierarchical)
were found to play more positive roles in academic achievement in contrast
to Type II thinking styles (e.g., executive and conservative) in some studies
(W. Fan et al., 2010; Grigorenko & Sternberg, 1997; Zhang, 2004d). On the
other hand, some studies conducted in Hong Kong, the Philippines, and
102 Chapter Four
Spain found that some Type II thinking styles (e.g., the conservative,
executive, and local styles) are positively related to academic achievement
(Bernardo et al., 2002; Cano-Garcia & Hughes, 2000; Varma & Thakur,
1992; Zhang, 2002e, 2004b, 2007b).
4The section draws partially on my work titled, “The role of thinking styles in career
decision-making self-efficacy among university students”, published in Thinking
Skills and Creativity (Fan, 2016).
104 Chapter Four
(e.g., Bosacki et al., 1997; Phan, 2007; Tierney, 1997; Watkins & Dahlin,
1997). Moreover, field-independent (Type I) people were found to have
higher levels of job satisfaction (Hageman, 1990). In contrast, some studies
revealed that Type II styles played a negative role in certain social and
emotional outcomes. For example, Type II styles (e.g., field-dependent,
impulsive) were also easily found among patients with depressive symptoms,
borderline personality disorder, and Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity
Disorder (Calamari et al., 2000; López-Villalobos et al., 2003).
In terms of Type III styles, previous studies showed that Type III thinking
styles play a positive role in some developmental outcomes sometimes but
a negative role in others (e.g., Zhang, 2001d, 2008b; Zhang, 2010). These
findings resonate with Zhang and Sternberg’s (2006) contention that Type
III styles are value-differentiated, because they manifest the nature of either
Type I or Type II styles depending on the specific context.
The theory of career decision self-efficacy, which was derived from the
integration of Bandura’s (1977) self-efficacy theory and Crites’ (1961, 1965)
career maturity theory, and its subsequent research have contributed to the
development of the social cognitive career theory (SCCT), which was
proposed by Lent, Brown, and Hackett (1994). As one of the integral social
106 Chapter Four
Many studies have found that personal factors, such as gender, ethnicity,
social status, personality, identity, and self-reliance, play a role in individual
differences in career decision self-efficacy (e.g., Gianakos, 2001; Gloria &
Hird, 1999; Hartman & Betz, 2007; Nauta & Kahn, 2007; Thompson &
Subich, 2006). For example, in terms of one of the widely studied personal
inputs––personality traits––Hartman and Betz (2007) found that
conscientiousness and extraversion were positively related to career
decision self-efficacy, while neuroticism was negatively related. Similar
results were also found in Page, Bruch, and Haase’s (2008) study and Jin,
Watkins, and Yuen’s (2009) research. However, although intellectual styles
are seen as the interface between personality and ability (Zhang & Sternberg,
2005), their role in career decision self-efficacy has been under-researched.
I analyzed the relationship between thinking styles and career decision self-
efficacy based on two sets of data (Time 1 and Time 2) to see if the
relationship between the two variables remained the same across time.
Furthermore, to exclude the possible influence of confounding variables, the
relationship between thinking styles and career decision self-efficacy was
examined with parenting styles, learning environments, personality traits,
and relevant demographic factors (i.e., gender, grade, major, hometown, and
SES) as controls. The results based on hierarchical regressions showed that
thinking styles had made unique statistical contributions to CDSE beyond
parenting styles, learning environments, and personality traits. Specifically,
thinking styles uniquely explain 7% to 9% of the variance in CDSE at Time
1 (Table 4.1) and 2% to 4% of the variance in CDSE at Time 2 (Table 4.2).
108 Chapter Four
Table 4.1: Predicting Career Decision Self-Efficacy from Thinking Styles with Parenting Styles, Learning
Environments, Personality Traits, and Relevant Demographic Factors in Control (Time 1)
Type II ȕexe
ȕloc .08* .11**
ȕcon
ȕmon -.08*
Type III ȕoli
ȕana
ȕint .12** .11*
ȕext .11* .09* .10*
Notes: CDSE = career decision self-efficacy; Total = the contribution of gender, grade, major, hometown, SES, parenting styles,
R2
learning environments, personality traits, and thinking styles to career decision self-efficacy; R2demo = the contribution of gender, grade,
major, hometown, and SES to career decision self-efficacy; R2PS = the unique contribution of parenting styles to career decision self-
efficacy; R2LE = the unique contribution of learning environments to career decision self-efficacy; R2Personality = the unique contribution
of personality to career decision self-efficacy; R2TS = the unique contribution of thinking styles to career decision self-efficacy; leg =
legislative; jud = judicial; glo = global; lib = liberal; hie = hierarchical; exe=executive; loc = local; con = conservative; mon = monarchic;
oli = oligarchic; ana = anarchic; int = internal; ext = external; *p<.05; **p<.01; and ***p<.001.
110 Chapter Four
Table 4.2: Predicting Career Decision Self-Efficacy from Thinking Styles with Parenting Styles, Learning
Environments, Personality Traits, and Relevant Demographic Factors in Control (Time 2)
ȕautonomy .10*
ȕassessment .08*
ȕs-s cooper .17*** .10* .18*** .15*** .16***
ȕpeer -.11** -.10** -.08* -.18***
ȕfacilities .08* .11**
Personality ȕneuro .07*
ȕextra .09* .09*
Thinking Styles
Type I ȕleg .10* .10* .09*
ȕhie .09* .12** .08*
Type II ȕmon -.08*
CDSE ȕtime1 -.68*** -.68*** -.63*** -.70*** -.69***
2
Notes: CDSE = career decision self-efficacy; R Total = the contribution of gender, grade, major, hometown, SES, parenting styles at Time 2, learning environments at Time 2,
personality at Time 1, thinking styles at Time 1, and career decision self-efficacy at Time 1 to the change of career decision self-efficacy from Time 1 to Time 2; R2demo = the
contribution of gender, grade, major, hometown, and SES to the change of career decision self-efficacy; R2PS = the unique contribution of parenting styles during the year
between the pretest and the posttest to the change of career decision self-efficacy; R2LE = the unique contribution of learning environments during the year between the pretest
and the posttest to the change of career decision self-efficacy; R2Personality = the unique contribution of personality at Time 1 to the change of career decision self-efficacy; R2TS
= the unique contribution of thinking styles at Time 1 to the change of career decision self-efficacy; R2Time1 = the unique contribution of career decision self-efficacy at Time
1 to the change of career decision self-efficacy; invol = acceptance/involvement; psyc_auto = psychological autonomy-granting; super = strictness/supervision; constru =
constructivist-oriented teaching; clear goal = clear goals and coherence of curricula; autonomy = student autonomy; assessment = assessment and assignment; t-s inter =
teacher-student interaction; s-s cooper = student-student cooperation; peer = peer morale and identities; facilities = learning facilities; neuro = neuroticism; extra = extraversion;
open = openness to experience; agree = agreeableness; conscien = conscientiousness; leg = legislative; jud = judicial; glo = global; lib = liberal; hie = hierarchical; exe =
executive; loc = local; con = conservative; mon = monarchic; oli = oligarchic; ana = anarchic; int = internal; ext = external; *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001; and the number of
R2 in bold means that the F change was significant after the predictor was added into the regression model. Due to space limitations, the dimensions of the predictors that
were not significantly related to any of the change of CDSE were omitted.
The Role of Thinking Styles in Student Development 115
The results from the analysis of two sets of cross-sectional data and that of
the longitudinal data confirmed the important role that thinking styles play
in the students’ career maturity, as represented by career decision self-
efficacy. Actually, the contributions of thinking styles to career decision
self-efficacy (CDSE) were even beyond those contributed by parenting
styles, learning environments, and personality traits. Specifically speaking,
the results from both the cross-sectional data analysis and the longitudinal
data analysis demonstrated that Type I thinking styles were positively
associated with CDSE, which was completely in line with the hypothesis.
For example, the legislative style and the hierarchical style were found to
be significant predictors for CDSE. It is understandable that if students
prefer to deal with unstructured tasks and arrange multiple tasks in an
efficient way, they would be more confident in coping with career-related
tasks, such as collecting occupational information, making career plans, and
solving problems during career development.
In terms of Type II styles, although the local style was found to be positively
related to career decision self-efficacy (CDSE) based on the analyses of data
at Time 1, this relationship was not confirmed by the results from the
longitudinal data analyses, thereby indicating that the relationship found at
Time 1 was more likely to be covariant than causal. However, the negative
relationship between the monarchic style and CDSE found in the cross-
sectional data analysis (Time 1) was confirmed by the results from the
longitudinal data analyses, thereby indicating that students who prefer to
deal with one task at one time (monarchic) did not have enough confidence
when dealing with tasks relevant to their career development, which may
require multiple foci. This result supports the argument that Type II styles
are less adaptive.
116 Chapter Four
In terms of Type III styles, although some of the relationships between Type
III thinking styles and CDSE were found based on the cross-sectional data
analyses, none of them were confirmed based on the longitudinal data
analyses. This indicates that Type III styles did not predict students’ CDSE
in certain way, which suggests that Type III styles’ value is not explicit and
it may, instead, depend on specific situations.
Type II ȕexe
ȕloc
ȕcon
ȕmon
Type III ȕoli
ȕana .09** .07*
ȕint
ȕext -.09*
Notes: SWB = subjective well-being; R2Total = the contribution of gender, grade, major, hometown, SES, parenting styles, learning
environments, personality traits, and thinking styles to subjective well-being; R2demo = the contribution of gender, grade, major,
hometown, and SES to subjective well-being; R2PS = the unique contribution of parenting styles to subjective well-being; R2LE = the
unique contribution of learning environments to subjective well-being; R2Personality = the unique contribution of personality to subjective
well-being; R2TS = the unique contribution of thinking styles to subjective well-being; leg = legislative; jud = judicial; glo = global; lib
= liberal; hie = hierarchical; exe = executive; loc=local; con = conservative; mon = monarchic; oli = oligarchic; ana = anarchic; int =
internal; ext = external; *p<.05; **p<.01; and ***p<.001.
124 Chapter Four
Table 4.6: Predicting the Change of Subjective Well-being from Thinking Styles (Time 1) in the Presence of Parenting
Styles (Time 2), Learning Environments (Time 2), and Personality Traits (Time 1)
The analyses of the quantitative data from the main study demonstrated that
thinking styles play an important role in subjective well-being (SWB).
However, just as they were not aware of the relationships between thinking
styles and career maturity when they were asked about the relationships
between the two, the interviewees were also not fully aware of the
relationships between thinking styles and SWB. The connection only
emerged when they were encouraged to freely discuss anything about their
subjective well-being, which further confirmed the results from the
quantitative data, as well as providing possible explanations for them.
Some of my classmates seemingly know a lot. They have read beyond the
things taught in class, even before they enter university. They have had
abundant experiences. During the class discussions, they present their own
ideas confidently and clearly. This makes me feel inferior and stressed. (MX)
Furthermore, the longitudinal data also indicated that the hierarchical style
was a positive predictor for students’ positive affect after one year. As some
interviewees described, the university environment is a multitasking
environment. For instance,
In high school, I only needed to focus on one thing because my parents had
done other things for me. In university, I have to take care of a lot of things
myself, such as how to allocate money, when to buy new clothes, and other
detailed things in daily life […] Furthermore, besides dealing with difficult
courses, I also have to prepare for TOFEL and GRE. In addition, a lot of
stuff needs to be done in the student associations I participate in. I feel upset
that my energy is so limited but there are too many things to do. (QBC)
Although positive relationships between some Type II styles and SWB were
found in the cross-sectional data analyses at Time 1, the same positive
relationships were not confirmed in the longitudinal data analyses. In fact,
in the longitudinal data analyses, just like the hypothesis, some Type II
styles (e.g., the executive style and the monarchic style) were found to
negatively contribute to subjective well-being. It is probably because in the
university environment where direct instructions from teachers and parental
supervision decrease, students who prefer to follow others’ instructions
(executive) do not have many resources to depend on, which probably leads
them to sometimes feel helpless, which would influence their subjective
well-being. During interviews, it was also found that students who scored
higher in the executive style expressed more confusion during their adaption
to university life. When there was no explicit instruction and step-by-step
guidance in university, they felt “lost (㏞ⲉ)” (WHN) for a while until they
managed to successfully adapt. In addition, as previously mentioned, the
university is a multi-tasking environment where students who prefer to do
only one task at a time (monarchic) are likely to feel difficulties during the
128 Chapter Four
In terms of Type III styles, although some relationships between Type III
thinking styles and subjective well-being were found during the cross-
sectional analyses, none of them were confirmed from the longitudinal data
analyses. It indicated that Type III styles did not predict students’ subjective
well-being in a certain way, which suggests that Type III styles’ value
probably varies depending on the particular context.
Therefore, the present research examined the mediating role that thinking
styles may play in the relationships of parenting styles, learning
environment, and personality to students’ career decision self-efficacy and
subjective well-being. The analyses were based on two-wave longitudinal
data.
There are three approaches for the mediation analysis with two-wave
longitudinal data (Roth & MacKinnon, 2012). The first one is raw change
score analysis. The path of this approach is from the predictor(s) to the
change of mediator(s) to the change of outcome(s). As mentioned before,
this approach overlooks the effect of the initial score on the mediator(s) and
the outcome(s). Without considering the initial scores of the mediator(s) and
the outcome(s), it would be insufficient to infer causal relationships (Cole
& Maxwell, 2003). The second approach, which is called baseline-adjusted
change score analysis, atones for this shortcoming. It controls the
relationships due to the initial differences of the mediator(s) and the
130 Chapter Four
outcome(s) by putting the initial scores of the mediator(s) and the outcome(s)
into the analysis model. The third approach is autoregressive model analysis,
where the path is from the predictor(s) to the mediator(s) at posttest to the
outcome(s) at posttest with the initial scores of the mediator(s) and the
outcome(s) being controlled. As for the total effect, the second approach
and the third approach are identical, but the mediation effect achieved by
the third approach is usually bigger than the one from the second approach
because, in autoregressive model analysis, the calculation of the unique
contribution of the mediator(s) to the outcome(s) includes both within-
subject covariation and between-subjects covariation, while the baseline-
adjusted change score analysis restricts itself to the within-subjective
covariation. The second approach is recommended when the research focus
is pure change within individuals (Roth & MacKinnon, 2012). Therefore,
the baseline-adjusted change score analysis was adopted in the present
research.
There are four prerequisites for establishing mediation models. First, the
independent variable (IV) should significantly predict the mediator. Second,
the IV should significantly predict the dependent variable (DV) without the
mediator. Third, the mediator should have made a unique contribution to
the DV with the presence of the IV. Fourth, compared with the contribution
of the IV to the DV in the absence of the mediator, the contribution of the
IV to the DV after the mediator is added would decrease or even disappear.
A series of regressions (see Table 3.15 and Appendices 5–8) were
conducted to determine the relationships that met the four conditions.
Eventually, 13 potential mediating relationships for career decision self-
efficacy (CDSE) and six potential mediating relationships for subjective
well-being (SWB) were found (Table 4.7); however, no statistical mediating
effect of thinking styles in the relationships between personality traits and
the outcome variables was found. The 19 possible mediating relationships
were put into two path models (one for CDSE and the other for SWB) to
demonstrate the mediating effect through the baseline-adjusted change
score analysis (Figure 4.1 and Figure 4.2).
The model fit indices of the CDSE path model (Figure 4.1) are as follows:
Chi-square (df = 236, N = 743) = 416.80, p<.001, RMSEA = .032, SRMR
= .069, NFI = .98, NNFI = .99, and CFI = .99. Three Type I thinking styles
played significant roles as mediators in the relationships of parenting styles
and learning environments to CDSE. Specifically, the judicial style
significantly mediated the influence of parental acceptance/involvement on
self-efficacy of occupational information collection, the influence of
psychological autonomy-granting from parents on career planning, the
The Role of Thinking Styles in Student Development 131
Table 4.7: Testing Longitudinal Relationships that are Potentially Mediated by Thinking Styles
Mediat
IV DV Path Model Testing
or
indirect effect of direct effect of IV
PS TS CDSE a b a*b
IV on DV on DV
occupational
acceptance/involvement jud .07* .08*** .01
information
.01* .20***
occupational
acceptance/involvement ext .11*** .02 .00
information
acceptance/involvement jud planning .07* .08*** .01 .01 .27***
acceptance/involvement jud problem solving .07* .08*** .01 .01 .25***
psychological autonomy-
jud planning -.06* .08*** -.01 -.01* -.06***
granting
acceptance/involvement lib self-appraisal .08** .09*** .01 .01* .26***
acceptance/involvement hie goal selection .16*** .05* .01 .01* .23***
indirect effect of direct effect of IV
LE TS CDSE a b a*b
IV on DV on DV
constructivist-oriented teaching jud problem solving .15*** .08*** .01 .01** .09***
occupational
learning facilities jud .07* .08*** .01 .01* .07**
information
learning facilities jud problem solving .07* .08*** .01 .01* .08***
peer morale and identities lib self-appraisal .13*** .09*** .01 .01** .03
clear goal and coherence of
hie goal selection .22*** .05* .01 .01* .01
curricula
occupational
student-student cooperation ext .34*** .02 .01 .01 .03
information
The Role of Thinking Styles in Student Development 133
indirect effect of direct effect of IV
PS TS SWB a b a*b
IV on DV on DV
acceptance/involvement lib positive affect .11*** .16*** .02
.03*** .19***
acceptance/involvement hie positive affect .14*** .08** .01
strictness/supervision lib positive affect -.06 .16*** -.01 -.01 -.07*
psychological autonomy-
loc negative affect -.07** .03 -.00
granting
-.02** -.29***
psychological autonomy-
ana negative affect -.12*** .11*** -.01
granting
indirect effect of direct effect of IV
LE TS SWB a b a*b
IV on DV on DV
constructivist-oriented teaching hie positive affect .20*** .08** .02 .02** .20***
Notes: PS = parenting styles; LE = learning environments; TS = thinking styles; SWB = subjective well-being; CDSE = career decision
self-efficacy; jud = judicial; lib = liberal; hie = hierarchical; loc = local; ana = anarchic; ext = external.
134 Chapter Four
The model fit indices of SWB’s path model (Figure 4.2) were as follows:
Chi-square (df = 90, N = 743) = 262.07, p<.001, RMSEA = .051, SRMR
= .044, NFI = .93, NNFI = .93, and CFI = .95. Three thinking styles
significantly mediated the influence of parenting styles and learning
environments on SWB. Specifically, two Type I styles, the liberal style and
the hierarchical style, partially mediated the influence of parental
acceptance/involvement on positive affect. The hierarchical style also
partially mediated the influence of constructivist-oriented teaching on
positive affect. In addition, the mediating effect of one Type III style, the
anarchic style, in the relationship between psychological autonomy-
granting from parents and negative affect was also significant.
The results based on the longitudinal data analysis showed that thinking
styles played the role of partial mediators in the relationships of two
environmental predictors (parenting styles and learning environments) to
career decision self-efficacy (CDSE) and subjective well-being (SWB),
where the mediating function was mostly found among Type I and Type III
thinking styles. Although the indirect effect of parenting styles and learning
environments on CDSE and SWB through thinking styles was not very
strong, the mediating function of thinking styles is still statistically
significant.
For example, three Type I styles partially mediated the influence of one
dimension of parenting styles and two dimensions of learning environments
on the students’ development of CDSE. These results indicated that, if the
students perceived their parents as caring and involved (parental
acceptance/involvement), they would be more likely to develop Type I
thinking styles that are characterized as thinking critically (judicial),
favoring novel tasks or issues (liberal), and handling multiple tasks in an
orderly way (hierarchical), which accordingly promoted their self-efficacy
in dealing with tasks relevant to career development. In addition, the results
also indicated that, when students experienced a learning environment
where teachers taught in a constructivist-oriented way, or when sufficient
learning facilities could be accessed, they also tended to develop the judicial
style (Type I), which might have increased their self-efficacy in their career
development. In addition, Type I thinking styles also completely mediated
the influence of another two dimensions of learning environments on CDSE.
It was indicated that when peers were proactive in learning, or when
curricula were designed with clear goals, students would be more likely to
develop the liberal style (Type I) or the hierarchical style (Type I), which
subsequently increased their self-efficacy in dealing with career-related
tasks.
Style Identity
For decades, the field of intellectual styles has suffered from a blurred
identity in the broader context of psychology and education (Zhang, 2013).
140 Chapter Five
style changed over one year while extraversion in personality traits did not
change, although the two dimensions highly overlapped according to the
analysis of the bivariate relationship. Again, this result suggests that the
examination of bivariate relationships between styles and other constructs
of individual differences based on simple correlations or regressions is
insufficient to clarify the identity issue. Unfortunately, this is exactly the
case in most of the existing research that has tried to distinguish styles from
personality or abilities. Therefore, more studies anchored in other style
models are required to both examine the incremental validity of styles and
compare their nature and the targeted constructs based on longitudinal data.
This will hopefully allow us to achieve a clear picture of the identity of
styles in the broader context of psychology and education.
For now, based on the current research’s findings on thinking styles and
personality traits, a tentative agreement can be reached that personality traits
and thinking styles are two distinct constructs and that it is worthwhile
studying thinking styles in order to have a comprehensive understanding of
student performance. However, is it possible to apply this conclusion to
other style models? Further studies of other style models that focus on the
incremental validity of styles, as well as comparing the malleability of styles
and personality, are also required.
Style Malleability
Another major piece of puzzle in the field of styles is the controversial issue
of style malleability. Can styles be socialized or modified? It is especially
vital to clarify this issue for educators, because it is meaningless to apply
the notion of styles in practice if they are fixed traits. Like the other
controversial issues mentioned in this book, this matter has been strongly
debated for decades. Many discourses on the issue were at the conceptual
level and the research evidence was not scientifically sound (Zhang, 2013).
not in juniors. This suggests that the influence of the cultural climate in the
students’ hometowns on their thinking styles may vanish after experiencing
university life for a number of years. However, it is also probably due to the
cohort effect. But which speculation is true? The results from the analysis
of the longitudinal data supported the former speculation. The effect of the
students’ hometowns on thinking styles became insignificant after a year.
This finding suggests that university experiences and environments may
play a role in shaping students’ thinking styles. The follow-up interview
study further confirmed this speculation. In the interviews, students mainly
attributed these style changes to the change of learning environment,
personal involvement in the learning environment, changes in daily life, and
other personal experiences (e.g., involvement in student organizations and
internships). It revealed that students’ thinking styles changed when they
made an effort to adapt to their changeable environments. Moreover, the
findings from the quantitative, longitudinal study on the role of learning
environments in students’ thinking styles provided more evidence that
consolidate this conclusion. Despite the unexpected results on some specific
relationships, all of the findings demonstrated that learning environments
significantly contributed to the development of thinking styles. The
quantitative findings on the hometown’s disappearing effect on thinking
styles after the students entered university, the quantitative analyses of the
role of various dimensions of learning environments in students’ thinking
styles, and the qualitative findings from interviews construct a triangulation.
This makes the conclusion that learning environments play a significant role
in shaping students’ thinking styles more solid and compelling. Based on
all of the above evidence, the dynamic nature of thinking styles is
indisputable.
methodology of most studies could have been more rigorous, the findings
indicating the dynamic nature of styles based on various style models cannot
just be coincidence. Therefore, she asserted that styles are malleable.
However, intellectual styles based on different style models may be
malleable to varying degrees. This could be discussed within the context of
Curry’s “onion” model and Grigorenko and Sternberg’s (1995) three-
tradition model. Taking the “onion” as a metaphor, Curry’s (1983) model
claimed that there are three layers of styles. The innermost layer involves
personality dimensions, the middle layer involves information processing,
and the outermost layer involves instructional preferences. Theoretically, it
is expected that styles which fall into the innermost layer are less changeable
than the ones in the middle layer, while styles that fall into the outermost
layer are the most changeable among the three “onion” layers. According to
Grigorenko and Sternberg’s (1995) model, all of the existing style
constructs fall into three traditions: cognition-centered, personality-centered,
and activity-centered. It is also speculated that personality-centered styles
are the least changeable while activity-centered styles are the most
changeable. The above hypotheses are partially supported by a few studies.
For example, Low and his colleagues (2005) found that the rank-order
stability of vocational interests (also called career personality types, which
is a style model that falls in the innermost layer of the “onion” and in the
personality-centered tradition) were even stronger than personality traits. In
contrast to other style models, learning approaches (styles falling in the
outermost layer of the “onion” and in the activity-centered tradition) are the
most widely examined style constructs regarding the issue of style
malleability in longitudinal studies. Although the direction of the change of
learning approaches is inconsistent across studies, most of the existing
research supported the idea that learning approaches are changeable.
However, as Zhang (2013) pointed out in her book, the existing literature is
insufficient to make a reliable comparison between different style models
in terms of their degree of malleability due to the paucity of longitudinal
studies in contrast to the large number of various style models and the lack
of methodological rigor in most studies. Therefore, it is recommended that
further research should be undertaken based on other style models with the
following features: 1) longitudinally tracing style change; 2) excluding the
effect of measurement errors; 3) involving a large sample and keeping the
rate of attrition low; 4) providing a fuller account of confounding factors
while detecting the effect of specific environmental factors; and 5) reporting
effect sizes. It will only be possible to compare the degree of style change
and ascertain if the malleability of styles is contingent upon their distance
from the center of Curry’s “onion” and their positions in Grigorenko and
The Response to Three Controversial Issues 149
Style Value
Just like other controversial matters in the field of intellectual styles, the
issue of style value has gone through many conceptual arguments. Some
scholars contend that there are no good or bad styles (e.g., Kwang &
Rodrigues, 2002; Messick, 1994; Riding, 1997). The value of styles depends
on the different contexts where they are used. When Sternberg (1997)
proposed the concept of thinking styles, he also stated that they are value-
free. However, he gradually changed his opinion based on the accumulating
evidence. In his work with Zhang, he supported the argument that styles are
value-laden rather than value-free in today’s society (Zhang & Sternberg,
2009). In Zhang’s (2017) recent book, she explicitly stated that Type I styles
that manifest cognitive complexity and creativity are more adaptive than
Type II styles that manifest norm-conformity. However, as I mentioned in
the preceding chapter, the range of developmental outcomes examined with
intellectual styles is very narrow, and it is particularly restricted to academic
outcomes. Studies examining the role of styles in non-academic
development are still limited (Gebbia & Honigsfeld, 2012). This prevents a
comprehensive understanding of the value of intellectual styles in student
development.
In general, the present research’s findings are in line with Zhang’s (2017)
judgment about the value of intellectual styles. As expected, Type I thinking
styles positively contribute to students’ career decision self-efficacy and
150 Chapter Five
Nevertheless, there was one exception. The local style was still found to
positively contribute to subjective well-being in the analyses of the
longitudinal data. In the preceding chapter, I mentioned that it might be
relevant to the association between the local style and academic
performance. The assessment and evaluation in Chinese universities might
reward students using the local style, whereas a good academic performance
is probably one of the important origins of subjective well-being for
students. Besides this, there might be an alternative explanation from a
much broader perspective. Although success could be positively related to
happiness, the attributes that are desirable for success and happiness might
be slightly different. People who like paying attention to detail (the local
style) might not achieve great career success, but they could still have
positive emotions in daily life. However, as only one weak link was found
between the local style and subjective well-being, more studies are needed
to further examine this speculation.
Despite this small exception, the findings of the present research generally
suggest that Type I thinking styles have a greater positive value in student
development. One may naturally wonder if the superiority of Type I
thinking styles is culturally-specific or universal.
Zhang and Sternberg (2005) have pointed out that intellectual styles are
sociological, which means that “the use of a style is affected by the
preferences of the society in which one lives for various ways of thinking
(p. 2)”. At the conceptual level, it is understandable that culture has an
influence on shaping people’s styles. Based on this theoretical consideration
and Hofstede’s (1980) theory of cultural dimensions, Zhang (2013)
proposed a hypothesis that countries characterized by low power distance
(i.e., the unequal distribution of power is less acceptable), low uncertainty
avoidance (i.e. society is more tolerant of ambiguity), individualism (i.e.,
individual thoughts and behaviors are more respected), and masculinity (i.e.,
traditional masculine attributes, such as assertiveness and decisiveness, are
more valued) would encourage people to develop Type I styles, whereas
countries characterized by high power distance (i.e., the unequal distribution
of power is more acceptable), high uncertainty avoidance (i.e. society is less
tolerant of ambiguity), collectivism (i.e. conformity with other members of
the society is more appreciated), and femininity (i.e., traditional feminine
attributes, such as rule-following and obedience, are more valued) would
encourage people to develop Type II styles. After reviewing existing
research on the basis of several style constructs, such as field
dependence/independence, learning approaches, and career personality type,
Zhang (2013) concluded that, in spite of a few exceptions, the research
152 Chapter Five
Second, social norms and culture are gradually changing along with
economic evolution and the process of modernization (Matsumoto, 2002).
Although traditional Chinese societies manifested characteristics which
indicated high power distance, high uncertainty avoidance, collectivism,
and femininity, many things have been changing in the process of rapid
economic growth and accelerated modernization in Chinese society in
recent decades. Social norms and culture in contemporary Chinese society
is in the process moving from one polar to the other in Hofstede’s (1980)
culture dimensions. Accordingly, society’s attitudes to some individual
attributes are also changing. For example, in a series of studies conducted
by Chen and his colleagues in 1990, they found that shyness in Chinese
children was associated with a good adjustment, but this association had
become weaker in 1998. In 2002, a reverse correlation was even found
between shyness and child adjustment, which means that shy Chinese
children appear to be poorly adjusted (X. Chen, Cen, Li, & He, 2005). The
researchers claimed that this trend could be attributed to the rapid expansion
of a competitive market-oriented economy in recent decades. Under this
economic and social background, assertiveness and sociability are required
for success and therefore become more valued and encouraged by society.
By the same token, the change of culture and social norms may be also
responsible for the superiority of the Type I thinking styles found in the
present research. In contemporary Chinese society, characteristics linked
The Response to Three Controversial Issues 153
CONCLUDING REMARKS
Compared with the majority of studies in the field of styles, the present
research has advantages in various aspects, such as its longitudinal design
and its full consideration of the potential confounding variables. This has
led to solid and sound findings. Based on the findings of the present research
and the discussion in preceding chapters, some of the conclusions are well
grounded. First of all, this chapter will summarize the conclusions of the
research undertaken. In the second section, the theoretical contributions, as
well as its implications for both practice and research, will be elucidated.
This will be followed by a discussion of possible future directions in the
field of intellectual styles.
Conclusions
The present research addressed three major controversial issues in the field
of styles. It explored the socialization of students’ thinking styles and traced
their changes over the course of a year. It also examined the role of thinking
styles in terms of developmental outcomes with regard to career decision
self-efficacy and subjective well-being. This led to the following four major
conclusions.
Although styles are relatively stable, they are still changeable. Moreover,
Concluding Remarks 155
Theoretical contributions
First, and most importantly, the present research has further clarified the
nature of styles by addressing three major controversial issues. Bewildering
information and the lack of consensus have been seen as the major obstacles
that impede the advancement of the theory of styles (Peterson et al., 2009).
This research has provided empirical evidence to address three of the major
controversial issues in the field: how personality traits and styles are related
to each other; whether styles are malleable; and whether styles are value-
laden. The empirical evidence provided has profound significance for the
clarification of these controversial issues due to its three major strengths.
First, most of the relevant studies in the existing literature are based on
cross-sectional data, which limits the interpretation of the relationships
within covariations. The longitudinal design provides greater credibility
when interpreting the possible directions of the examined relationships.
Second, the present research examined personal and environmental factors
simultaneously and across time, which provides a comprehensive and
dynamic picture of the relationships involved. Third, the present research
adopted mixed methods where the qualitative data was used to triangulate
and further interpret the findings of quantitative studies, which makes its
conclusions more convincing. This compelling evidence significantly
contributes to the clarification of three of the controversial issues in the field
of styles and facilitates a better understanding of the nature of intellectual
styles, thereby advancing the field.
Concluding Remarks 157
Second, the present research provides empirical support for certain theories.
For instance, the research discovered the combined functions of personality
traits, parenting styles, and learning environments on the development of
students’ thinking styles. This supports Bronfenbrenner’s bioecological
theory (Bronfenbrenner, 1979; Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994), which
claims that human development is a joint function of person and context.
The research also revealed the mediating function of thinking styles in
student development; this supports Biggs’ 3P model (1990), which states
that presage factors (experiences) influence product factors (performance)
through their effect on process factors (styles). Furthermore, as the 3P
model was initially proposed to predict students’ academic performance, the
present research not only supports this model but also extends it to non-
academic performance in various areas (e.g., career maturity and subjective
well-being).
First, the value of thinking styles identified in the present research provides
information for parents, teachers, administrators in educational institutions,
and students on what styles they should encourage and cultivate. Based on
the findings of the present research, Type I thinking styles (which are
characterized as creativity-generating and having higher levels of cognitive
complexity) seem to be more beneficial to student development. Keeping
this point in mind during daily educational practice has profound
implications, especially when traditional Chinese culture treats norm-
conformity positively. Parents and teachers should transfer the emphasis
from obeying authority to encouraging creativity and autonomy.
Educational administrators could conduct some workshops or courses that
aim at cultivating students’ Type I styles. In addition, students should be
aware of practicing their Type I styles in daily life.
Second, while the findings on the value of thinking styles provide insights
into the type that should be cultivated by parents and educational
practitioners, the findings on the malleability of thinking styles indicate that
158 Appendices
Future Directions
The field of styles has suffered from the bewildering array of style
constructs for decades. Although efforts to integrate these theories have
been made in recent decades, the criticisms of the field are still ongoing.
One of the reasons for this is that some of the fundamental issues that
threaten the identity and practical value of style in education remain
unsolved. This book reviewed the existing literature and discussed the
findings of a recent longitudinal study on thinking styles. It demonstrated
that 1) thinking styles are distinct from personality rather than subordinate
to it; 2) thinking styles are stable to some extent but still changeable; and 3)
thinking styles are value-laden with Type I thinking styles being more
adaptive. As the model of thinking styles is one of the most recent style
theories and because it has several strengths compared with many other
style constructs (as discussed in Chapter 1), the findings related to it provide
considerable insights, which enable us to understand the nature of
intellectual styles. I believe that the research findings disclosed in this book
help fortify the credibility of the field of styles and, at the same time, instill
more confidence in educational practitioners.
In the concluding remarks of the final chapter as well as the whole book, I
would like to remind readers that, although the present research only
provides a piece of the puzzle for intellectual styles, the findings elucidated
are compelling and intriguing. Given the well-established facts that thinking
styles explain students’ performance beyond personality and that they are
more changeable than personality, educational practitioners should attempt
to enhance student performance by encouraging them to develop particular
thinking styles. It is also exciting news because it does not mean that
teachers need to tailor their instructions to meet every student’s
individualized stylistic needs in order to optimize their performance.
Research suggests that teachers should make concerted efforts with parents
to foster students’ Type I styles, which would benefit their long-term
development.
APPENDICES
It is noted that, due to its poor performance in terms of reliability and the
validity in the pilot study, item 12 was modified in the main study. Besides,
it should be also noted that, at Time 1 (pretest) in the main study, students
were asked to respond to the PSI according to their experience during the
first 16 years of their lives, while at Time 2 (posttest) students were asked
to respond to the PSI according to their experience in the year between the
pretest and the posttest. Because all the students resided on campus instead
164 Appendices
of at home after they entered university, some items in the PSI, especially
the items in the scale of strictness/supervision were no longer suitable for
their situation. Therefore, relevant items were modified slightly in terms of
their wording for investigation at Time 2. Because the test instructions and
some items were different between Time 1 and Time 2, the psychometric
properties of the PSI for both Time 1 and Time 2 were analyzed.
Factor Structure
The result of the EFA for the PSI is favorable. Principal Component
Analysis with Varimax Rotation was carried out. Three factors were
forcibly extracted according to the theoretical structure. At Time 1, three
factors explained 39.72% of the variance in the data. Apart from item 12,
which cross-loaded on the dimensions of psychological autonomy granting
and acceptance/involvement, all items loaded on the theoretically expected
factors. At Time 2, three factors explained 46.62% of the variance in the
data. All items loaded on the theoretically expected factors and none of them
were cross-loaded.
Reliability
The Cronbach’s alpha coefficients of the PSI’s three scales for Time 1
were .73 (acceptance/involvement), .75 (psychological autonomy-granting),
and .81 (strictness/supervision). The Cronbach’s alpha coefficients of the
PSI’s three scales for Time 2 were .82 (acceptance/involvement), .81
(psychological autonomy-granting), and .89 (strictness/supervision). These
results indicated that the reliability of the PSI was respectable. The current
internal consistency of the PSI was comparable with the results in Chen’s
(2010) study of Chinese adolescents.
There are 32 items in this inventory in total, with four items for each
dimension. Some items were borrowed and modified from the four
inventories mentioned above (i.e., Student Engagement Questionnaire,
Experiences of Teaching and Learning Questionnaire, College Student
166 Appendices
Factor Structure
The results from CFA for the ISPLE preliminarily supported the eight-factor
model. According to the three criteria of good indices proposed by Hu and
Bentler (1999) (irrelevant to sample size; punish complex models; and
sensitive to misfit models), RMSEA, NNFI, and CFI are considered to be
good indicators of a model fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999; Wen, Hau, & Marsh,
2004). Hu and Bentler identified the cutoff values for RMSEA, NNFI, and
CFI as .06, .95, and .95, respectively. However, Marsh, Hau, and Wen (2004)
criticized Hu and Bentler’s (1999) decision, as they felt that the rules were
too stringent and may increase Type I errors. They recommended that
RMSEA <.08 is acceptable, and so are NNFI and CFI >.90. Marsh, Hau,
and Wen (2004) also claimed that, in large samples, RMSEA, NNFI, and
CFI are better indices than the Chi-square. The model fit indices for the
ISPLE eight-factor model were as follows: Chi-square (df = 435, N = 926)
= 2459.22, p<.001, RMSEA = .071, NNFI = .94, and CFI = .95. According
to this, the ISPLE’s eight-factor model was acceptable.
Reliability
Factor Structure
The results from the CFA for the NEO-FFI-3 showed that the data did not
fit the five-factor model very well. The model fit indices were as follows:
Chi-square (df = 1700, N = 926) = 11557.48, p<.001, RMSEA = .079, NNFI
= .84, and CFI = .84. The misfit of the five-factor model has been
demonstrated in previous studies using NEO-FFI (e.g., Egan, Deary, &
Austin, 2000; Holden & Fekken, 1994). The current CFA results from the
NEO-FFI-3 were consistent with these previous findings. McCrae et al.
(1996) argued that the model misfit was because of a problem in Structural
Equation Modeling, while Gignac et al. (2007) claimed that the problem
was more likely to be in the five-factor structure than in the method. The
issue of the misfit of the NEO-FFI’s five-factor model calls for further
research.
168 Appendices
Reliability
Both the TSI (Sternberg & Wagner, 1992) and the Thinking Style
Inventory-Revised (TSI-R, Sternberg, Wagner, & Zhang, 2003) have been
found to have acceptable reliability and validity in previous studies in
several cultures, including Hong Kong, mainland China, the United
Kingdom, and the United States (Zhang, 2004a, 2004d, 2005b, 2008b,
2008c; Zhang & Higgins, 2008), apart from its relatively low reliability for
the Anarchic scale. As the latest version, TSI-R2 has been applied in the
Chinese context (e.g., Zhang, 2009, 2010), and it was found that the internal
consistency for the Anarchic scale had improved. The psychometric
properties of the TSI-R2 in the present research are described below.
Factor Structure
The results from the CFA for the TSI-R2 preliminarily supported the 13-
factor model. The model fit indices were as follows: Chi-square (df = 1937,
N = 926) = 9021.39, p<.001, RMSEA = .063, NNFI = .93, and CFI = .94.
Thinking Styles: Identity, Value, and Malleability 169
Reliability
Factor Structure
The results from the CFA for the CDSES-SF indicated that the 5-factor
model was acceptable. The model fit indices were as follows: Chi-square
(df = 264, N = 926) = 1604.27, p<.001, RMSEA = .074, NNFI = .96, and
170 Appendices
CFI = .97. The values of RMSEA were similar to those (RMSEA = .07)
found in another study conducted among Chinese postgraduates (Jin et al.,
2009). The value of CFI was higher in the present study than those (CFI
= .86) found in Jin et al.'s (2009) study.
Reliability
The Satisfaction with Life Scale and The Positive and Negative
Affect Schedule
The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; E. Diener et al., 1985) is a widely
used inventory that assesses the cognitive component of subjective well-
being and life satisfaction. The SWLS contains five items in total with a 7-
point scale (1 = not at all true of me; 7 = exactly true of me).
The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988)
has also been widely used to assess the affective component of subjective
well-being. The inventory consists of two 10-item subscales for positive
affect and negative affect. Participants were asked to respond to a 7-point
scale (1 = not at all true of me; 7 = exactly true of me) to indicate their
affective experience in the past month. Satisfactory psychometric properties
have been supported by various studies in different cultures (e.g., Krohne,
Egloff, Kohlmann, & Tausch, 1996; Melvin & Molloy, 2000; Paradowski,
2001).
The SWLS and the PANAS were used together in the present research as
the indicators for subjective well-being (SWB). Thus, the two inventories
Thinking Styles: Identity, Value, and Malleability 171
were put together for factor analysis. The psychometric properties of the
two inventories are shown below.
Factor Structure
Reliability
The Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were .83/.83 (Time 1/Time 2) for the
scale of life satisfaction, .89/.90 (Time 1/Time 2) for positive affect,
and .91/.91 (Time 1/Time 2) for the scale of negative affect. These results
indicated that the reliability of the measures for SWB were very good. The
results were very consistent with those in Ye’s (2008) study among Chinese
samples, indicating that the two measures for SWB were stable and reliable.
172 Appendices
Q: Do you have any questions about your thinking styles report? Is anything
unclear? Or is there any information you would like?
Q: Do you think that your thinking styles have changed? When did these
changes happen? (Did you have any changes in your thinking styles last
year?) What kind of changes? Could you provide some examples about your
changes in thinking styles? How have these changes happened? (Followed
by further enquiries about the factors that the research focuses on.)
–– Are your parents strict? Or do they treat you with a lot of care and
make few demands? Do your parents insist that you adopt their
opinions, or do they understand that you have different ones? Did
the parenting you experienced, such as the above aspects, influence
your development of thinking styles? Could you provide some
examples on how your thinking styles were influenced by your
parenting?
–– Did your parents’ attitude to you and their parenting change after
you went to university? Did these changes in parenting styles
contribute to the change in your thinking styles? How?
high school and the one in university? What are the differences?
Did these differences contribute to the change of your thinking
styles? Which aspects of learning environments affect your
thinking styles? How?
–– Are there one or two courses that have influenced your thinking
styles? What are the characteristics of these courses?
Q: Among these factors, how do you think that your thinking styles affect
your career maturity?
––Is any thinking style beneficial when you are dealing with tasks
related to career development (e.g., career planning, self-appraisal,
and goal selection)? What type of thinking styles? Could you provide
some examples?
Thinking Styles: Identity, Value, and Malleability 177
–– Is there any thinking style that you believe has prevented you from
effectively dealing with tasks related to career development? What
type of thinking styles? Could you provide some examples?
Q: Are you satisfied with your life? Are you happy? Why?
Q: Among these factors, how do you believe your thinking styles have
affected your subjective well-being?
Chen, X., Cen, G., Li, D., & He, Y. (2005). Social Functioning and
Adjustment in Chinese Children: The Imprint of Historical Time. Child
Development, 76(1), 182-195. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00838.x
Cheng, H., & Furnham, A. (2004). Perceived parental rearing style, self-
esteem and self-criticism as predictors of happiness. Journal of
Happiness Studies, 5(1), 1-21.
Cheung, F. (2002). Thinking styles and achievement in mathematics and
language learning. Unpublished manuscript. The University of Hong
Kong.
Choi, B. Y., Park, H., Yang, E., Lee, S. K., Lee, Y., & Lee, S. M. (2012).
Understanding career decision self-efficacy: A meta-analytic approach.
Journal of Career Development, 39(5), 443-460. doi:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0894845311398042
Coffield, F., Moseley, D., Hall, E., & Ecclestone, K. (2004). Learning styles
and pedagogy in post-16 learning: A systematic and critical review.
London: Learning & Skills Research Centre.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral science.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cohen, N. J. (1981). valuation of the relative effectiveness of
methylphenidate and cognitive behavior modification in the treatment
of kindergarten-aged hyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology: An official publication of the International Society for
Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 9(1), 43-54.
Collings, J. N. (1985). Scientific thinking through the development of
formal operations: Training in the cognitive restructuring aspect of field-
independence. Research in Science & Technological Education, 3(2),
145-152.
Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., &
Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The case
for nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218-232.
Collis, B., & Winnips, K. (2002). Two scenarios for productive learning
environments in the workplace. British Journal of Educational
Technology, 33, 133-148.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory
manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO Personality Inventory-
Revised(NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI)
professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources
Inc.
Courneya, K. S., & Hellsten, L.-A. M. (1998). Personality correlates of
exercise behavior, motives, barriers and preferences: An application of
Thinking Styles: Identity, Value, and Malleability 191
Fletcher, A. C., Walls, J. K., Cook, E. C., Madison, K. J., & Bridges, T. H.
(2008). Parenting style as a moderator of associations between maternal
disciplinary strategies and child well-being. Journal of Family Issues,
29(12), 1724-1744.
Flouri, E. (2004). Subjective well-being in midlife: The role of involvement
of and closeness to parents in childhood. Journal of Happiness Studies,
5(4), 335-358.
Foxall, G. R. (1990). An empirical analysis of mid-career managers'
adaptive-innovative cognitive styles and task orientations in three
countries. Psychological Reports, 66, 1115-1124.
Foxall, G. R., & Hackett, P. M. W. (1992). Cognitive style and extent of
computer use in organizations: Relevance of sufficiency of originality,
efficiency and rule-conformity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 74, 491-
497.
Foxall, G. R., Payne, A. F., & Walters, D. A. (1992). Adaptive-innovative
cognitive styles of Australian managers. Australian Psychologist, 27(2),
118-122.
Francis, L. J., & Jones, S. H. (2000). Psychological type and happiness: A
study among adult churchgoers. Journal of Psychological Type, 54, 36-
41.
Fraser, B. J. (1998). The birth of a new journal: Editor's introduction.
Learning Environments Research, 1, 1-5.
Friedman, C. P., & Stritter, F. T. (1976). An empirical inventory comparing
instructional preferences of medical and other professional students.
Research in Medical Education Processing, 15th Annual conference,
November, San Francisco, 85-90.
Furnham, A. (1996). The big five versus the big four: the relationship
between the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) and NEO-PI five
factor model of personality. Personality and Individual Differences,
21(2), 303-307. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(96)00033-5
Furnham, A. (2012). Intelligence and intellectual styles. In L. F. Zhang, R.
J. Sternberg, & S. Rayner (Eds.), Handbook of intellectual styles:
Preferences in cognition, learning, and thinking. (pp. 173-192). New
York, NY, US: Springer Publishing Co, New York, NY.
Furnham, A., & Cheng, H. (2000). Perceived parental behaviour, self-
esteem and happiness. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology,
35(10), 463-470.
Furnham, A., Christopher, A. N., Garwood, J., & Martin, G. N. (2007).
Approaches to learning and the acquisition of general knowledge.
Personality and Individual Differences, 43(6), 1563-1571.
Thinking Styles: Identity, Value, and Malleability 195
Hayward, G., & Everett, C. (1983). Adaptors and innovators: Data from the
Kirton Adaptor-Innovator Inventory in a local authority setting. Journal
of Occupational Psychology, 56, 339-342.
Helwig, A. A., & Myrin, M. D. (1997). Ten-year stability of Holland codes
within one family. Career Development Quarterly, 46(1), 62-71.
Retrieved from
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=aph&AN=971
2194161&site=ehost-live
Hess, R. D., & Azuma, M. (1991). Cultural support of schooling: Contrasts
between Japan and the United States. Educational Researcher, 20, 2-8.
Hilliard, R. I. (1995). How do medical students learn: Medical student
learning styles and factors that affect these learning styles. Teaching and
Learning in Medicine, 7(4), 201-210.
Hite, C. E. (2004). Expository content area texts, cognitive style and gender:
Their effects on reading comprehension. Reading Research and
Instruction, 43(4), 41-74.
Ho, H. K. (1998). Assessing thinking styles in the theory of mental self-
government: A mini validity study in a Hong Kong secondary school.
Unpublished manuscript. The University of Hong Kong.
Hofstede, G. H. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences
in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Holden, R. R., & Fekken, G. C. (1994). The NEO five-factor inventory in a
Canadian context: Psychometric properties for a sample of university
women. Personality and Individual Differences, 17(3), 441-444.
Retrieved from
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0191886994902917
Holland, J. L. (1973). Making vocational choices: A theory of careers.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Holland, P. A., Bowskill, I., & Bailey, A. (1991). Adaptors and innovators:
Selection versus induction. Psychological Reports, 68, 1283-1290.
Holmes, C. S. (1981). Reflective training and causal attributions in
impulsive mildly retarded children. Journal of Clinical Child
Psychology, 10(3), 194-199.
Hommerding, L. (2003). Thinking style preferences among the public
library directors of Florida. Dissertation Abstracts International
(Section B): The Sciences and Engineering, 63(11B), 5545.
Huang, J., & Chao, L. (2000). Field dependence versus field independence
of students with and without learning disabilities. Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 90(1), 343-346.
Huppert, F. A., Abbott, R. A., Ploubidis, G. B., Richards, M., & Kuh, D.
(2010). Parental practices predict psychological well-being in midlife:
198 References
Long, L., Fang, L., & Ling, W. (2000). Advancement in the theory and
measurement of career maturity. Psychological Science (China), 23(5),
595-598.
Loo, R. (1997). Evaluating change and stability in learning style scores.
Educational Psychology, 17(1/2), 95. Retrieved from
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=aph&AN=970
6014967&site=ehost-live
López-Villalobos, J. A., Pintado, I. S., Sánchez-Mateos, J. D., Rodríguez, J.
M. M., Azón, M. I. S., & Sanz, F. R. (2003). Attention deficit
hypeactivity disorder: Development of reflexive-impulse, reflexive-
rigid and field dependence-independence cognitive styles. Revista de
Psiquiatría Infanto-Juvenil, 20(4), 166-175.
Low, K. S. D., Yoon, M., Roberts, B. W., & Rounds, J. (2005). The Stability
of Vocational Interests From Early Adolescence to Middle Adulthood:
A Quantitative Review of Longitudinal Studies. Psychological Bulletin,
131(5), 713-737. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.713
Loyens, S. M. M., & Gijbels, D. (2008). Understanding the effects of
constructivist learning environments: Introducing a multi-directional
approach. Instructional Science, 36(5-6), 351-357.
Loyens, S. M. M., Rikers, R. M. J. P., & Schmidt, H. G. (2008).
Relationships between students' conceptions of constructivist learning
and their regulation and processing strategies. Instructional Science,
36(5-6), 445-462.
Loyens, S. M. M., Rikers, R. M. J. P., & Schmidt, H. G. (2009). Students'
conceptions of constructivist learning in different programme years and
different learning environments. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 79, 501-514.
Lucas, R. E. (2005). Time does not heal all wounds: A longitudinal study of
reaction and adaptation to divorce. Psychological Science, 16(12), 945-
950. Retrieved from http://pss.sagepub.com/content/16/12/945.abstract
Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2004).
Unemployment alters the set point for life satisfaction. Psychological
Science, 15(1), 8-13. Retrieved from
http://pss.sagepub.com/content/15/1/8.abstract
Lucas, R. E., & Diener, E. (2008). Personality and subjective well-being. In
O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of
personality psychology: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 795-814).
New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
Luk Suet Ching, W. (1998). The influence of a distance-learning
environment on students' field dependence/independence. Journal of
Experimental Education, 66(2), 149-160. Retrieved from
Thinking Styles: Identity, Value, and Malleability 203
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=ulh&AN=496
443&site=ehost-live
Lykken, D., & Tellegen, A. (1996). Happiness is a stochastic phenomenon.
Psychological Science, 7(3), 186-189.
Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. (1983). Socialization in the context of the
family: Parent-child interaction. In E. M. Hetherington & P. H. Mussen
(Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Socialization, personality, and
social development (Vol. 4, pp. 1-101). New York: Wiley.
Mandelman, S. D., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2012). The etiology of intellectual
styles: Contributions from intelligence and personality. In L. F. Zhang,
R. J. Sternberg, & S. Rayner (Eds.), Handbook of intellectual styles:
Preferences in cognition, learning, and thinking. (pp. 89-107). New
York, NY, US: Springer Publishing Co, New York, NY.
Maton, K. I. (1990). Meaningful involvement in instrumental activity and
well-being: Studies of older adolescents and at risk urban teen-agers.
American Journal of Community Psychology, 18(2), 297-320. Retrieved
from http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00931306
Matsumoto, D. (2002). The new Japan: debunking seven cultural
stereotypes. Yarmouth, ME: Intercultural Press.
Maynard, M. J., & Harding, S. (2010). Perceived parenting and
psychological well-being in UK ethnic minority adolescents. Child
Care, Health & Development, 36(5), 630-638.
McCartney, K., & Rosenthal, R. (2000). Effect size, practical importance,
and social policy for children. Child Development, 71, 173-180. doi:
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00131
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (2007). Brief versions of the NEO-PI-3.
Journal of Individual Differences, 28(3), 116-128. doi:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001.28.3.116
McCrae, R. R., Zonderman, A. B., Costa Jr, P. T., Bond, M. H., &
Paunonen, S. V. (1996). Evaluating replicability of factors in the
Revised NEO Personality Inventory: Confirmatory factor analysis
versus Procrustes rotation. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 70(3), 552-566. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.552
McGinn, L. K., Cukor, D., & Sanderson, W. C. (2005). The Relationship
Between Parenting Style, Cognitive Style, and Anxiety and Depression:
Does Increased Early Adversity Influence Symptom Severity Through
the Mediating Role of Cognitive Style? Cognitive Therapy & Research,
29(2), 219-242. Retrieved from
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=aph&AN=169
02735&site=ehost-live
204 References
Wu, X., & Zhang, H. C. (1999). The preliminary application of the Thinking
Style Inventory in college students. Psychological Science China, 22(4),
293-297.
Wunderley, L. J., Reddy, W. B., & Dember, W. N. (1998). Optimism and
pessimism in business leaders. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
28(9), 751-760.
Xie, Q. (2015). Intellectual styles: Their associations and their relationships
to ability and personality. Journal of Cognitive Education and
Psychology, 14(1), 63-76. doi:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/1945-8959.14.1.63
Yang, S. C., & Lin, W. C. (2004). The relationship among creative, critical
thinking and thinking styles in Taiwan high school students. Journal of
Instructional Psychology, 31(1), 33-45.
Ye, S. (2008). A Longitudinal Study of Subjective Well-being among
Chinese University Students: the Roles of Personality, Attribution, and
Coping. Unpublished Doctorial Dissertation. The University of Hong
Kong. Hong Kong.
You, Z., & Jia, F. (2008). Do they learn differently? An investigation of the
pre-service teachers from US and China. Teaching and Teacher
Education, 24(4), 836-845.
Yu, D. M. (2008). Teachers' teaching styles and interpersonal behaviors.
Unpublished manuscript.
Yu, T. M. (2012). Do teachers’ interaction styles affect secondary school
students’ thinking styles and academic performance? . Unpublished
doctoral dissertation. The University of Hong Kong.
Zhang, L. F. (1999). Further cross-cultural validation of the theory of mental
self-government. The Journal of Psychology, 133(2), 165-181. doi:
https://doi.org/10.1080/00223989909599731
Zhang, L. F. (2000). University students' learning approaches in three
cultures: An investigation of Biggs's 3P model. The Journal of
Psychology, 134(1), 37-55.
Zhang, L. F. (2001a). Approaches and thinking styles in teaching. Journal
of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 135(5), 547-562.
Zhang, L. F. (2001b). Do styles of thinking matter among Hong Kong
secondary school students? Personality and Individual Differences,
31(3), 289-301.
Zhang, L. F. (2001c). Do thinking styles contribute to academic
achievement beyond abilities? The Journal of Psychology, 135(6), 621-
637.
Zhang, L. F. (2001d). Thinking styles, self-esteem, and extracurricular
experiences. International Journal of Psychology, 36(2), 100-107.
216 References