Jungian Psychology Active Imagination An
Jungian Psychology Active Imagination An
Jungian Psychology Active Imagination An
VOLUME 1
C.G. JUNG &
GUIDED IMAGERY IN
PSYCHOTHERAPY
BY BRIAN DAMIEN DIETRICH
BRIAN DAMIEN DIETRICH
© 2018 Brian Damien Dietrich
EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY
Wundt, who is commonly identified as "the founding
father of experimental psychology" (Araujo, 2016, p. vii),
developed a method of self-observation called
introspectionism, which entailed a researcher's careful
attention to his subjective experience, including the interior
process of imagery. Titchener, Like Galton before him,
believed images were the primary elements from which
complex mental states are constructed. By way of illustration,
in his 1897 book An Outline of Psychology Titchener wrote:
We cannot think, unless we have ideas in which to
think; ideas are built up from impressions . . . thus most
of us remember, imagine, dream, and think in terms of
sight . . . we see it occurring in our ‘mind’s eye’; when
we ‘imagine’ an experience, we have a mental ‘image’
of it, we seem to see it take place; when we dream . . .
we see ourselves or our friends engaged in this action
or in that; and when we think, we often see the words
in which we are thinking, as if they were printed or
written on an imagined page. (p. 19)
Experiences of mental imagery, he argued, could be
analyzed with dispassionate objectivity by psychologists
trained in the subtle art of introspection (Holt, 1964). Whereas
Titchener emphasized the introspective study of conscious
experience and, like Galton and Binet, he ardently believed in
the cognitive importance of imagery, Külpe rejected Wundtian
introspection on the grounds that it failed to provide a full
explanation of the human mind and its achievements (Holt,
1964). And, in contradistinction to Titchener, he maintained
thought was imageless and ultimately irreducible to
combinations of sensations (Singer, 1974). Through laboratory
studies conducted between 1901 and 1908, Külpe and his
students determined the essential operations of even the
most basic thought processes occurred below the threshold
of consciousness, and they insisted this lacuna demanded
psychology shift its focus away from the subjective
introspection of conscious thought. And while Jon Roeckelein
(2004) opines that the debate between Titchener and Külpe
and their competing notions (i.e., image dependent vs.
imageless thought) was ultimately “salubrious for the
development of a scientific and experimental psychology” (p.
155), in response to the Wurzburg School’s demand for
disciplinary realignment, two very different psychological
solutions were proffered in the years just prior to the Great
War: behaviorism and psychoanalysis. Robert Holt (1964)
contends that although the two schools shared an emphasis
on behavior, and neither put stock in descriptions of
conscious contents, Freudian psychoanalysis did not impact
the psychological mainstream to the degree behaviorism did
for many years.
In contradistinction to Holt’s arguably overly simplistic
conflation between these diverging branches of
psychological thought, I will show specific features from the
historical development of psychoanalysis, in fact, played a
crucial role on the emergence and proliferation of
psychotherapeutic applications of imagery in Europe. The
most significant of these are (1) the influence of hypnosis
including its prototypical expression as magnetism and (2)
Jung’s radical reconceptualization of the unconscious, his
method of Active Imagination, and the role imagery plays in
psychic functioning and development.
BEHAVIORISM
Regarding the transition from experimental psychology to
behaviorism, the triumph of Külpe and the Würzberg School
effectively marginalized interest in the subjective study of
inner experience and imagery from serious scholarly
consideration for decades—especially in the United States,
where psychological behaviorism flourished (Holt, 1964;
Singer, 1974). Founded by John Watson and developed by
such prominent figures as Ivan Pavlov and B. F. Skinner,
psychological behaviorism rejected subjectivism and
introspective methods and instead embraced objectivity,
measurement, and precise definitions of stimulus and
response (Singer, 1974). Reformulated by behaviorism, the
psychological task turned from subjective introspection to the
objective description or explanation of an organism’s
observed (i.e., external) behavior. Introspectivism became
irrelevant then, and psychology’s disciplinary emphases
shifted from cognitive ephemera to concrete problem solving
(Holt, 1964). During this period, Robert Holt (1964) contends,
“imagery, attention, states of consciousness and other such
central concepts of the old era were anathematized as
‘mentalistic’ and cast into outer darkness” (p. 257). In fact,
Holt argues, between the years 1910 and 1970, imagery was
banished from serious scientific consideration (p. 263). Even
worse, having fallen out of fashion in the United States and
Britain, for some behaviorists like Watson inner experience
and imagery became topics worthy of mockery and derision
(Singer, 1974). This attitude stands in stark contrast to the vital
development of imagery techniques that proliferated in
Europe during the same period. However, before turning to
these various guided imagery practices, I will first provide a
summary of dynamic psychiatry’s historical development and
emphasize the influence magnetism, hypnotism,
psychoanalysis, and analytical psychology had on the
emergence guided imagery and its various therapeutic
applications across Continental Europe.
3 AN UNBROKEN CHAIN:
INCUBATION,
MAGNETISM,
HYPNOTISM, AND PSYCHOANALYSIS
SPIRITUALISM
According to several sources (Charet, 2015; Ellenberger,
1970; Watkins, 1976/1984; Webb, 1974), concurrent with
Mesmer’s disgrace and hypnotism’s increased scientific
recognition and acceptance was the countervailing rise of
Spiritualism, a movement that began in the United States in
the 1840s and quickly spread to England, France, and
Germany in the 1850s. The term Spiritualism, according to F.
X. Charet (2015), “refer[s] to a belief in the communication
between the living and the dead . . . [and] the various
experiences and ideas that became attached to this belief”
(p. 1), some of which included mediumistic communion with
spirits, which were “directly contingent upon the
phenomenon of trance states” (p. 33), crystal/mirror gazing,
automatic writing/talking/drawing, and divination. According
to James Webb (1974), “To a large extent [Spiritualism] grew
out of the Mesmeric movement, and the motley collection of
ideas which had fastened themselves to Mesmer” (p. 44).
Webb avers it was also connected closely with the millenarian
expectations of the mid-century. Millenarianism, briefly stated,
is an apocalyptic vision signaled by war, famine, and natural
disasters; it is a vision that heralds the second coming of
Christ, who will vanquish all evil and usher in a golden age
(Herbermann, 1911, p. 308). Underscoring the significance of
spiritualism in the history of depth psychology Ellenberger
(1970) obliquely asserts, spiritualism exposed psychologists to
novel approaches to the mind and provided a variety of
methods, such as automatic writing, by which to explore the
unconscious.
AMBROISE-AUGUSTE LIÉBEAULT,
HIPPOLYTE BERNHEIM, AND JEAN-
MARTIN CHARCOT
Returning to the psychological study of hypnotism, its role
in the emergence of depth psychology and its influence on
guided imagery recall the Scottish surgeon and hypnotist
James Braid. His demystifying mental-suggestive theory
canalized along two divergent lines, which were represented
on one side by the French hypnotists Ambroise-Auguste
Liébeault (1823–1904) and Hippolyte Marie Bernheim (1840–
1919), co-founders of the Nancy School on one side, and on
the other by Jean-Martin Charcot (1823–1893) who headed
the Salpêtrière School. Whereas Liébeault /Bernheim
emphasized suggestion and the psychological nature of
hypnosis, which they viewed in nonpathological terms,
Charcot conversely stressed the physiological and
neurological dimension of hypnosis, which he viewed in
morbid terms akin to hysteria. Contra Liébeault /Bernheim,
Charcot maintained that only proto-hysterics or hysterics
could be hypnotized. In a way similar to Mesmer before him,
Charcot was an avowed materialist. Hence, his physiological
account of hypnosis can be viewed as a refinement of
Mesmer’s physicalist/materialist/fluidic assumptions (Kelly,
1991). According to Kelly (1991), Liébeault /Bernheim
introduced “medical psychology to new dimensions of the
human psyche, to the phenomena of posthypnotic
suggestion, multiple personality, amnesias, paralyses,
sensory and muscular dysfunction . . . [all] related to the
psychology of the unconscious” (p. 28). Charcot, meanwhile,
demonstrated that hypnosis could produce neurological
symptoms and mitigate certain neurological disorders. His
rigorous scientific approach won for hypnotism legitimacy,
respectability, and acceptance from the French Académie
des Sciences and validated the psychological study of
hypnotic phenomena (Kelly, 1991). Moreover, despite his
physicalist leanings, Mitchell and Black (1995) argue that
Charcot’s study of hysteria along with his demonstrations that
hypnotic ideas could produce symptoms and bring about
temporary cures effected Freud by “fatefully shifting his focus
from brain to mind” (p. 2).
PIERRE JANET
Like Meyers and Flournoy, Pierre Janet (1859–1947)
combined hypnosis and spiritualistic techniques such as
automatic writing in his own philosophically informed
psychological investigations. Influenced partly by
experimental psychology and his study with Wilhelm Wundt in
America, Janet emphasized a strictly scientific method that in
his view entailed not only analysis but also synthesis
(Ellenberger, 1970). Janet’s significance to the discovery of
the unconscious and the treatment of psychopathology
cannot be overstated (Ellenberger, 1970; Kelly, 1991; Micale,
2014). In fact, some discoveries for which Freud is credited
were ideas previously set forth by Janet, such as the cathartic
cure for psychoneurosis effected through an elucidation of
historical trauma, or Janet’s recognition that analysands
idealized their analysts and endowed them with significant
powers of influence. Janet called this phenomenon the
rapport which Freud later christened transference
(Ellenberger, 1970; Micale, 2014). Further substantiating
Janet’s significance to depth psychology, Kelly (1991) avers
that Janet’s study of hypnotism, and hypnoid states, which
drew upon both clinical observation and experimental
research, “earned him a significant place alongside Freud
and Jung in the founding and development of the psychology
of the unconscious” (p. 29). Emphasizing Janet’s contributions
to imaginal processes such as active imagination and guided
imagery, Watkins (1974) argues that Janet’s psychological
investigations led him to conclude that the unconscious was
made up of fragmentary subpersonalities similar to the di-
psychism or double ego (Le double moi) described by Max
Dussoire, or the poly-psychism (many souls) annunciated by
the magnetist Durand (de Gros) (Ellenberger, 1970). Watkins
(1974) avers that Janet sought to uncover the essential ideas
or images that constituted the foundational core and hidden
unconscious source of his patients’ psychopathology. In his
1889 book, L’automatisme psychologique (Psychological
Automatism), Janet identified what he called “partial
automatisms” (p. 223) (i.e., split-off parts) of his patient’s
personalities that followed an autonomous process of
unconscious development which he termed subconscious
“idées fixes” (fixed ideas). These unconscious organizations
developed autonomous psychological existences and were
understood by Janet as both the cause and effect of
psychopathology. Although Janet understood hysterical fits to
be enactments of subconscious fixed ideas and he examined
dreams for indications of them, his primary method for rooting
them out was hypnosis combined with other spiritualist
practices such as automatic writing, automatic talking, and
crystal gazing (Ellenberger, 1970, p. 373). Through crystal
gazing, Watkins (1976/1984) asserts, “inner conflicts, hidden
identities and scenes with which the personality was dealing
were projected into the [crystal] ball and experienced as
images and scenes coming from the ball” (p. 35). Conceived
of as originating in the subconscious, Janet believed these
split-off subpersonalities became distinctly real entities, which
held power to subvert a patients’ orientation to ordinary
reality. According to Watkins (1976/1984), Janet viewed the
alternate mythic reality represented by autonomous
subpersonalities in decidedly negative terms. She contends
that his goal was not merely to bring a patient’s unconscious
and pathogenic alternate reality to light so much as it was to
destroy it, or at a minimum to fundamentally alter it “by means
of dissociation or transformation” (p. 35). To achieve this end
required abaissement du niveau mental, a spontaneous or
induced lowering of a patient’s conscious awareness and a
regression to a mental state that was both narrowed and
disunited (Dell & O’Neil, 2015). According to Watkins
(1976/1984), Janet became actively involved in his patients’
imaginal enactments and inserted himself as a character in
their narrative dramas to influence their outcomes. In one
famous example of his intrusive directivity, Janet (1889) asked
a hypnotized woman stricken by hysterical blindness to
imagine herself as a young girl sleeping not by a girl afflicted
by facial impetigo—which he had deduced caused her
tramatogenic blindness—but rather that she was sleeping
beside a “very nice child who was not sick” (p. 440). Janet’s
method, which may be characterized as a directive form of
imagery substitution, is today commonly referred to in the
field of cognitive behavioral therapy as “Imagery Rescripting”
(Homes, Arntz, & Smucker, 2007, p. 298). Janet was of the
opinion a patient could only remember dissociated
experience occluded by trauma by inducing a similar state of
consciousness to that which the traumatic incident itself
provoked. He therefore used hypnosis not only to access
subconscious memories and ideas that caused a patient’s
maladaptive behavior and emotional pain, but also to
transform traumatic images into narratives of personal history
(van der Kolk & van der Hart, 1989). Confirming the efficacy of
Janet’s approach, Ellenberger (1970) asserts that “the most
effective method proved to be [a] substitution, that is, [the]
suggestion of a gradual transformation of the hallucinatory
picture” (pp. 367–368). Watkins (1976/1984) meanwhile
stresses the power of the mythic imagination and asserts that
Janet accomplished his cures by means of fantasy, which
gradually transformed the patient’s personal myth. She avers
that by engaging patients at the imaginal level, Janet could
alter the essential psychic discontinuities caused by the
original traumatic experience. Because Janet achieved his
cures by means of the rapport, which is to say, a combination
of his own authority and suggestive power, one could argue
Janet’s approach to imagery was directive in the extreme
(Watkins, 1976/1984). Summarizing Janet’s approach to
images Watkins (1976/1984) specifies:
1. Janet worked with hysterics who experienced inner
dramas that were like dreams. By lowering the threshold
of a patient’s consciousness inner dramas could be
activated, accessed, and analyzed by the doctor.
2. The emergent fantasy material evinced “other than
‘worldly’ personalities, situations and ideas (believed to
be contained in the unconscious)” (p. 48).
3. These subpersonalities and otherworldly ideas and
situations were conceptualized as resulting from
traumatic events in a patient’s life and were thought to
express the nature of the problem that needed a cure.
4. The doctor entered the patients fantasy drama and
altered the pathological symbolic situation.
According to Watkins (1976/1984), a central feature of
Janet’s method was that the doctor aggressively intruded
upon the imaginal setting and forcefully directed characters—
eliminated some, introduced new ones, and even dispensed
with the patient’s fantasy altogether by imposing his own
narrative, which is to say, “The imaginal structure of action
and image were radically disrupted according to the wishes
of an ‘outsider’” (Watkins 1976/1984, p. 49). Predicated on
doctor-centered directivity and suggestion, Watkins’s critical
evaluation of Janet’s authoritative manipulation of his
patients’ fantasy material is also an essential feature of
hypnotism, which exploits the hypersuggestivity of induced
hypnoid states to affect behavioral change. Hippolyte
Bernheim (1880) the famed hypnotist, underscored this point
in his book Suggestive Therapeutics: A Treatise on the
Nature and uses of Hypnotism (De la Suggestion et de son
Application à la Thérapeutique) when he declared,
“Suggestion rules the greater part of hypnotic manifestations”
(p. xiii). And Ellenberger (1970) expands on the kyriarchal
power differential inherent in hypnosis, which he
characterizes as:
the quintessence of the relation of dependency of one
individual upon another. It is a surrender of one’s will
to the will of another and is more likely to occur when
there is a considerable psychological or social
distance between the two individuals, the one
endowed with power and prestige, the other, passive
and submissive. (p. 190)
Based on a doctor’s authoritative suggestion, this
hierarchically structured and directive approach to fantasy
also characterized Breuer and Freud’s early collaborative
work with hypnotism during the years 1887-1896 and
arguably persisted in the ensuing “psychoanalytic tradition
[which] grew out of Freud’s early experiences with hypnosis”
(Hall et al., 2006, p. 11).
EUROPEAN STUDIES
According to Singer (1974), Holt (1964), and Sheikh (2011),
dispassionate scientific discourse and controlled
experimentation which constrained American and British
psychological investigations of interior phenomena were not
as determinative, or as limiting, as they were in Europe. This
was arguably due, at least partly, to sociopolitical variances
between the two regions at the beginning of the 19th century.
Whereas Europe had been devastated by war and social
upheaval, in America, the technological zeitgeist (i.e.,
mechanization, materialism, mass production, consumerism,
and empiricism) came to inform American psychology’s
emphasis on observable and measurable (i.e. external)
responses such that—in the American academy—inner
experience was collectively repressed in a way that did not
take place in Europe (Singer, 1974). Several authors
(Crampton, 1974/2005; Hall et al., 2006; Holt, 1964; Sheikh,
2011; Singer, 1974; Watkins, 1976/1984) have identified
additional factors that account for European psychologists’
persistent interest in human subjectivity and inner
explorations of imagery, and Anees Sheikh (2011) summarily
annunciates these considerations:
1. Experimental psychologists (i.e., the forerunners of
behaviorism) who were opposed to introspective
methods fled Europe in droves during the 1st and 2nd
World Wars.
2. Inaugurated by Jung, subjective investigations of the
inner world of imagery influenced numerous European
clinicians.
3. French and German phenomenology (i.e. Bachelard and
Husserl) deeply informed European science and clinical
psychology.
4. Eastern philosophical systems based on subjective
explorations of consciousness influenced the clinical
practice of European psychologists.
One further explanation for the proliferation of mental
imagery approaches in Europe is, there remained among
Europeans an irrepressible and enduring romantic sensibility
regarding the world of imagination and inner experience
(Singer, 1974; Watkins, 1976/1984), which arguably had always
been a part of Europe’s intellectual climate.
Before turning to the respective founders of the various
forms of guided imagery practice in Europe, it should be
noted some contemporary guided imagery researchers
(Achterberg, 1985; Miller, 2016; Rossman, 2003; Sheikh,
Kunzendorf, & Sheikh, 2003) trace guided imagery’s earliest
origins in Europe to ancient sources in Greece—specifically to
Hippocrates, the legendary father of medicine, and still
further through the mists of time to the mythic figure of
Asclepius, to whom I shall now turn.
HIPPOCRATES
Sir William Osler, in his 1921 book The Evolution of Modern
Medicine, provides a eulogistic introduction to Hippocrates
who he calls “The Father of Medicine” (p. 66). Citing Émile
Littré (1840) from his book Oeuvres complètes d’ Hippocrate
(Complete works of Hippocrates), Osler (1921) emphasizes the
importance of Hippocrates’s formulation of humoral
pathology that he extrapolated from Empedocles’s
speculations regarding the elements of Fire, Earth, Air, and
Water as “the root of all things” (p. 67). Whereas the
Macrocosm of the world is comprised of these four
fundamental elements, Hippocrates conjectured that the
Microcosm of the human body was made up of four humors:
“blood, phlegm, yellow bile (or choler) and black bile (or
melancholy), which corresponded to the four qualities of
matter, heat, cold, dryness and moisture” (p. 38). Regarding
the four humors, in his treatise Nature of Man, which he
composed sometime in the 5th century BCE (Jouanna, 2012,
p. 335), Hippocrates (trans. 1931) wrote:
The body of man contains in itself blood and phlegm
and yellow bile and black bile, which things are in the
natural constitution of his body, and the cause of
sickness and of health. He is healthy when they are in
proper proportion between one another as regards
mixture and force and quantity, and when they are well
mingled together; he becomes sick when one of these
is diminished or increased in amount, or is separated
in the body from its proper mixture, and not properly
mingled with all the others. (p. 11)
Constituting a proto-psychological theory, Hippocrates’s
four humors were correlated with four different personality
types: melancholic, phlegmatic, sanguine, and choleric which
each represented a preponderance of a particular vital fluid:
black-bile, phlegm, blood and yellow-bile respectively.
Following Asclepius and Aristotle, the notion that imagination
exerted a material effect on the body was a commonly held
belief in 5th century Athens (Rossman, 2003), and although it
is nowhere specifically stated in the Corpus Hippocraticum,
Martin Rossman (2003) suggests that images and imagination
moved the four humors toward balance or imbalance;
sickness or health. This feature of humoral medicine, that
images and emotions influence physiology, persisted well
into the 17th century. For example, in J. B. van Helmont’s
(1683) idea of the psychophysiological principle of the
archeus influus (i.e. the spirit of life) or the idea morbosa (i.e.
the hurtful idea) in which disease was essentially imagined,
though not unreal (Fischer-Homberger, 1979). Rossman
(2003) explains that at the time of Hippocrates, it was
believed that the human heart “took in reality, subtracted its
matter and took the remainder into the psyche (soul) where it
formed images.” These images, “stimulated emotional
reactions . . . moved the four humors . . . [and] balance[d]
health” (Rossman, 2003, p. 90). Pincus and Sheikh (2009)
meanwhile underscore Hippocrates’s focus on the role of the
physician to activate “the body-mind toward intrinsic healing”
(p. 42). And Achterberg (1985) asserts that Hippocrates
championed the Asclepian values of loving concern,
gentleness, and dignity. Predicated on the notion there exists
a link between images, emotions, and health, contemporary
guided imagery, such as that practiced by Rossman, Pincus,
Sheikh, and Achterberg, shares an affinity with Hippocrates’s
humoral medicine. Mental imagery expresses the central
nervous system’s simultaneous information processing
system which is a primary encoding system that affects all
major physiological functions: respiration, heart-rate, blood-
pressure, metabolic rates in cells, gastro-intestinal motility
and secretion, sexual function and immune response
(Bressler & Rossman, 2002, p. 627). Extending in a
contiguous line from the ancient Greek cult of Asclepius and
Hippocratic medicine, one could argue RGI represents a
contemporary iteration of these earliest forms of mind-body
medicine.
CARL HAPPICH
Happich was a devout Christian who began investigating
mental imagery as early as 1920 (Crampton, 1974/1977). He
combined Eastern spiritual practices with the insights of
modern depth psychology and created imagery techniques
that he believed facilitated psycho-spiritual growth and
transformation. As stated by Hjalmar Sundén (2014), Happich
articulated his fundamental principles regarding guided
imagery, the practice of which he called Mediation, in two
journal articles and one short introductory monograph: his
1932 “Bildbewusstsein als Ansatzstelle psychischer
Behandlung” (Image Consciousness as the Point of
Psychological Treatment); his 1939 “Bildbewusstsein und
schopferische situation” (Symbolic consciousness and the
creative situation); and his 1938 manual Anleitung zur
Meditation (Guide to Meditation) which was released in
English translation in 2015. According to Happich (1938/2015),
“meditation takes place in an ancient part of our conscious, to
which our sense of logic and reason have no access; it is a
place of images and feelings” (p. 25). He further asserted,
“genuine meditation . . . is accomplished in the realm of the
Soul . . . in the mental realm that is known as image-
consciousness” (p. 25). Wolfgang Kretschmer (1951, 1962,
1951/1965, 1969)—in his essay “Die meditativen Verfahren in
der Psychotherapie” (“Meditative Techniques in
Psychotherapy”), which was republished as a chapter in
Roberto Assagioli’s 1965 Psychosynthesis: A Collection of
Basic Writings and in Charles Tart’s 1969 Altered States of
Consciousness—provides an overview of Happich’s work. As
reported by Kretschmer (1951/1965), Happich identified a level
of “symbolic consciousness” (p. 305) situated between
ordinary waking consciousness and the unconscious.
Expressed through images and dynamic symbolism, and
accessible through meditation (i.e., guided imagery), Happich
believed that this ancient dimension of consciousness was
the locus of all creative production and healing. The efficacy
of Happich’s practical techniques, Kretschmer (1951/1965)
contends, resulted from his “religious attitude . . . [which was]
based on sound psychological principles . . . confirmed by the
work of the Jungian school” (p. 308). Happich’s (1938/2015)
method, which was adopted mainly by theologians, entailed
the following:
1. Happich maintained that relaxation rather than a
suspension of consciousness or an abaissement du
niveau mental, allowed the subject to access the realm
of imagery or “the spiritual domain” (p. 29). The
relaxation state, he believed, was amenable to an
“associative mode of thinking rather than the logical,
analytical and critical mode of thinking” (p. 28).
2. In contradistinction to Jung’s method of active
imagination, Happich prescribed “a conscious form of
breathing” (p. 55) before and during the therapeutic
session, which Kretschmer (1951/1965) characterized as
a “passivity of respiration” (p. 306).
3. Happich (1938/2015) argued that inner work should be
overseen by a teacher or guide to “safeguard
against . . . dangers [or] . . . being led astray” (p. 56). The
imagery guide, in Happich’s view, helped one “make
sense of . . . images and . . . bring order into the inner
[realm] so that [one] may gain in strength and vigor” (p.
56).
4. Happich assigned predetermined imagery scenes such
as a meadow, a mountain, or a Chapel. He quietly
observed his subjects who described their unfolding
imagery experiences to him. And he utilized their verbal
accounts diagnostically and therapeutically (Crampton,
1974/1977).
Two crucial elements of Happich’s method which are still
employed in RGI include the use of breathing techniques to
promote a relaxed and inward focused state of attention and
the dyadic relationship between the imagining subject and
the imagery guide.
EUGENE CASLANT
Edward Casey (2000) identifies the occultist Eugene
Caslant as one of the pioneering figures in imagery studies in
France. As a means of cultivating a subject’s capacity for what
he believed were extrasensory powers (i.e., clairvoyance or
double vision, and premonitory or retrospective vision),
Caslant (1921/1937) developed imagery techniques
themetized around imaginal ascent and descent. His method,
like Happich’s, was directive, such that he would instruct the
imagining subject to follow a path of either ascent or descent
that he theorized triggered various emotions that advanced
the subject’s level of psychic development. He would also
impose specific images on the subject’s imaginal experience,
which in his view facilitated upward and downward
movement such as staircases or ladders (Watkins, 1976/1984).
During these directed inner experiences, a subject would
encounter a variety of energy centers or conscious beings
who populated these imaginary worlds, some of whom were
enlightening guides, while others were decisively more
deceptive or misleading (Caslant 1921/1937). Crampton
(1974/2005) avers that Caslant (1921/1937) believed
archetypal figures encountered during visualization, which he
called “extra-terrestress” (p. 42), (i.e., extraterrestrials or
aliens), were higher order beings than humans.
Reminiscent of Jung (1952/1967), who distinguished two
kinds of fantasy, Caslant (1921/1937) distinguished two kinds
of imagination, which he termed active imagination and
passive imagination. His terms and the phenomena they
describe, however, are arguably the inverse of Jung’s, such
that their theorizing is in greater consonance then semantics
would suggest. Whereas Jung emphasized active imagination
and conscious engagement with spontaneously arising
images from the unconscious and viewed passive fantasy in
terms of indolent daydreaming, Caslant instead extoled the
occult power of the passive imagination, which in his view
provided the means by which a subject could access
supernormal (archetypal) levels of consciousness. Passive
imagination, Caslant (1921/1937) explained, entails the
spontaneous appearance of images, that associatively link
with other images to become “coherent scenes that embrace
the whole field of consciousness” (p. 15). Another vehicle for
passive imagination and its spontaneous images Caslant
opined, is telepathy, which bridges the consciousness of
people and establishes a spontaneous subconscious
connection between them. It is this form of passive
imagination, Caslant asserted, that accounts for “intuition,
hunches . . . perception of atmosphere [auras], cases of
double vision, mind reading and other anomalous
phenomena” (p. 15). In contrast to Jung’s notion of fantasy,
passive imagination in Caslant’s usage emphasized the
spontaneous creativity of the unconscious, while active
imagination—which for Jung entailed the collaboration
between the ego and Self—for Caslant, referred only to the
ego’s willful machinations. According to Caslant (1921/1937):
Active imagination is the faculty that willfully and
purposively engages internal images. Through active
imagination the writer constructs his novels, the artist
establishes his musical composition or painting, or the
scientist prepares his laboratory and combines
mathematical operations. It is the source of
understanding phenomena, creation and evolution. It
is the ego that serves as the basis of judgment, that
forms ideas, and a great number of mental
phenomena. (p. 14)
Caslant (1921/1937) believed a subject’s imagery held
prognostic value. Accordingly, the level that a subject could
achieve in the imagination not only revealed but also
delimited the subject’s degree of attainable “facultiés supra-
normales” (p. 13), or supernormal powers. Building on the
work of his teacher, Charles Henry, Caslant was convinced
expansion or inhibition of imagination corresponded to
directional movement (Watkins, 1976/1984, p. 76). For
example, moving upward from left to right or downward from
right to left seemed to enhance a subject’s generation of
images, while the reverse movements (i.e., right to left, or
downward from left to right) seemed to inhibit a subject’s
production of imagery. Crampton (1974/2005) meanwhile
clarifies that in Caslant’s view, the subject could only ascend
as high in imaginal space as the subject’s level of
development would allow. Caslant’s emphasis on
directionality, ascension, and descension was later
appropriated by his student Robert Desoille in his method of
Directed Daydreaming, which I will discuss momentarily. First,
though, I will underscore several elements of Caslant’s
technique which have been retained as structural features in
contemporary guided imagery practice. These include the
following:
1. The imagery guide facilitates a psychophysiological
state of calm by means of deep, slow breathing.
2. The imagery guide offers an environment conducive to
inner work that includes soft light and the reduction of
external sounds to facilitate a relaxed and inward focus.
3. The imagery guide provides a safe holding environment
characterized by empathic attunement.
4. The imagery guide assists with the elicitation of images.
5. The imagery guide poses questions and discourages
judgment, interpretation, or rational analysis of inner
experience.
6. The imagery guide eases return to ordinary, outer-world
consciousness.
Of the foregoing, one could argue that the most crucial
element of Caslant’s method is that the guide remains
present and empathically attuned to the subject’s imaginal
experience. And employing the imagery of Mesmeric fluid,
Caslant described how the guide can immersively attend to
and vicariously resonate with a subject’s inner experience.
According to Caslant (1921/1937):
One can achieve this by attentively observing him, in
other words, by remaining with him in thought. As
every thought produces fluidic emissions, one can
create in this manner, a current on which the subject
can support himself in order to observe and retain his
images. The experimenter will note, in fact, that every
distraction on his part, will be accompanied by some
weakening of the subject’s vision and in general by
descent to a lower plain. (pp. 75–76)
ROBERT DESOILLE
Desoille, as noted, was a student of Caslant’s who had
developed mental imagery techniques with the intention of
inducing supernormal phenomena. It was from Caslant, with
whom he studied in 1923 but whom he failed to credit in his
own work that Desoille adopted the themes of imaginary
ascent and descent, which he believed generated imagery
independent from perceptual memory and made
transpersonal experience possible. More specifically, Desoille
associated step-wise ascending images with the incremental
sublimation of instinct and a concomitant activation of a
subject’s higher spiritual and ethical qualities, which resulted
in archetypal encounters with numinous figures and, at the
highest reaches of ascension, led to a particular state of
mystical union in which a subject merged with or was
surrounded by celestial light. Desoille (1945) insisted that the
attainment of mystical experience was paramount because it
constituted the “only experience of creative value . . . which
can be a point of departure for the reconstruction of the
personality” (p. 379). Simply stated: Desoille believed
transformation and healing through ecstatic or mystically
inspired states was only possible by means of imagined
ascent. According to Desoille (1945):
Images, associated with a euphoric state, should only
be sought through symbolic ascent. Although it is
possible to bring about euphoric states through
symbolic descent, these states, however attractive
they may be, do not lead to sublimation or a desire for
greater contact what reality: rather they encourage the
patient to take refuge in an unhealthy type of reverie.
The search for these euphoric states through symbolic
ascent is a necessary condition for ultimate
sublimation, despite their apparently regressive
quality. It seems that such state, which take the subject
back to the infantile level, give him a corrective affect
of experience in a short time, thereby compensating
for the distorted affective education of his childhood.
(1945, p. 378)
Descent imagery, meanwhile, in which the subject was
directed downward though imagined holes or fissures in the
earth, or beneath watery surfaces—aside from providing
access to symbolic material to be sublimated and
transformed through ascent, led the subject to confront
primitive instinctual nature, which was symbolically expressed
as a subterranean or under-sea encounter with theriomorphic
figures, chthonic beings or other shadowy denizens of the
underworld. According to Watkins (1976/1984), Desoille
associated ascent imagery with the collective unconscious
which produced sensations of light and induced feelings of
soothing calm, somatic ease, and even euphoria. Desoille
conversely linked descent imagery with the personal
unconscious which provoked feelings of anxiety, dyspnea,
fear, trembling, and tachycardia. Watkins (1976/1984) avers
that horizontal movement also held significance for Desoille.
She states, for example, that Desoille believed left to right
movement in imaginal situations had to do with the future,
whereas a right to left movement provoked images of a
subject’s past. Continuing in this vein, Desoille (1966)
suggested that imaginal movement of the right arm signified
optimism, altruism, and struggle, whereas the imaginal
drawing-in of the left arm indicated to him avoidance and fear
or that a subject wished to internally withdraw.
It was Desoille’s view that monstrous underworld figures
encountered by a subject through terrestrial or aquatic
descent represented regressive dimensions of the subject’s
psyche. And he encouraged subjects to bravely face sinister
archetypal forms. He even counseled subjects to kill
imagined figures when he deemed it necessary (Crampton,
1974/2005, p. 4). Crampton (1974/2005) argues that the
cultivation of higher spiritual values that facilitated the
realization of a person’s full potential was the central concern
of Desoille, who insisted it was only through the refinement of
a spiritual attitude that one could confidently embrace one’s
destiny in service to others and thus realize what he called
the “oblative state” (p. 5) of selfless generosity.
Desoille authored three major works: Exploration de
l’affectivité subconsciente par la méthode du rêve-éveillé:
Sublimation et acquisitions psychologiques (Exploration of
Subconscious Affectivity Using the Waking Dream Method:
Sublimation and Psychological Findings) (1938); Le rêve-
éveillé en psychothérapie: Essai sur la fonction de régulation
de l’inconscient collectif (The Waking Dream in
Psychotherapy: Essay on the Function and Regulation of the
Collective Unconscious) (1945); and Théorie et pratique du
rêve-éveillé dirigé (Theory and Practice of the Directed
Waking Dream) (1961). Because these texts are not available
in English translation, most English speakers know Desoille’s
work through a series of his lectures translated in 1965 by the
psychologist Frank Haronian for the Psychosynthesis
Research Foundation (Hardy, 1987). Whereas Desoille’s first
book relied heavily on the works of Freud and Janet and his
second book drew primarily from Jung, his third book, which
represents a curious departure from dynamic psychology,
appealed to Pavlov and the behaviorist school.
In the first of his three translated lectures, “The Directed
Daydream”—a name Crampton (1974/2005) notes he
appropriated from the French journalist and novelist Léon
Daudet (1926)—Desoille referred to the particular kind of
imagery he sought to evoke by means of his technique Le
Rêve Éveillé Dirigé (R.E.D) (i.e., The Directed Daydream)
which, without credit or citation, drew heavily from Caslant’s
theorizing (Crampton, 1974/2005; Hall et al., 2006; Sheikh &
Jordan, 1983; Singer, 1974; Watkins, 1976/1984). Desoille
(1965) specified:
The style of the imagery . . . [of interest to him]
progressively departs from one’s memories of reality
and from the habitual imagery of nocturnal dreams . . .
[to] an entirely new world that was unknown to both
Freud and Adler. Only Jung, the visionary, managed to
catch a glimpse of this domain. (p. 12)
Although this sentence suggests that Desoille held Jung in
higher regard than Freud or Adler, his opinion of him was not
altogether positive, which is evidenced in the same
paragraph when he states Jung only described the uncharted
vistas of this new world of imagination “in a very sketchy
fashion by drawing on traditional legends” (p. 12). Although
Desoille referenced Jung and appropriated his concepts and
terminology—especially, as noted, in his second book Le
rêve-éveillé en psychothérapie: Essai sur la fonction de
régulation de l’inconscient collectif —it is apparent, based on
his questionable statements regarding Jung and his
psychology he lacked an intimate understanding of Jung’s
work. In the second of his three 1965 lectures, for instance,
Desoille claimed that although “the Jungian school [attends
to] the imagery of fables,” they are only familiar with such
images “as they have arisen spontaneously from folklore
traditions.” He further stated, “They have no methods for
intentionally evoking them so that they can be studied in vivo
and used therapeutically [emphasis added]” (p. 38). This
assertion, however, is counterfactual given Jung had already
described his experiments with the process he would come
to call “active imagination” in rudimentary terms in a series of
lectures he provided in English at the Psychologischer Club
Zürich, in July 1925 (Shamdasani, 2011). In his 6th seminar
lecture transcribed by Cary Baynes, for example, Jung
(1925/1989) stated:
I devised such a boring method by fantasizing that I
was digging a hole, and by accepting this fantasy as
perfectly real. This is naturally somewhat difficult to do
—to believe so thoroughly in a fantasy that it leads you
into further fantasy, just as if you were digging a real
hole and passing from one discovery to another. But
when I began on the hole I worked and worked so
hard that I knew something had to come of it—that
fantasy had to produce, and lure out, other fantasies.
(p. 49)
Joan Chodorow meanwhile states that Jung wrote his first
professional paper about active imagination, “The
Transcendent Function,” in 1916. In it, Chodorow (1997)
specifies, Jung “sets forth both his new psychotherapeutic
method and . . . describes the stages of active imagination
and some of its many forms, he also links active imagination
to work with dreams and the transference relationship” (p. 4).
Although this work was not published until 1958, it was
nevertheless available in Jungian circles 7 years before
Desoille’s inaccurate claim that Jung articulated no method
by which to engage mental imagery. Jung first used the term
active imagination publicly in his 1935 Tavistock lectures
delivered in London, which in 1936 were privately distributed
by the Analytical Psychology Club of London and published
by Dr. Roland Cahen in French in his 1944 release of Jung’s
L’Homme à la découverte de son âme (Modern Man in
Search of his Soul)—still 1 year before the publication of
Desoille’s (1945) Le rêve-éveillé en psychothérapie: Essai sur
la fonction de régulation de l’inconscient collectif, which drew
heavily on Jung’s theorizing. A second instance of Desoille’s
arguably shallow understanding of Jung that is evident in his
lectures is his assertion Jung believed the symbol of a dragon
referred singularly to “the mother who refuses to give herself
to her son” (p. 10) while Desoille believed the symbol of the
dragon represented “all the prohibitions imposed on the
subject by his cultural milieu . . . the family. . . the patient’s
social class and . . . vocational commitments” (p. 10).
Desoille’s faulty interpretation of Jung’s view—and indeed his
suggestion the image of a dragon is tantamount to the
Freudian superego—confuses a sign which is a knowable
thing for a symbol which points beyond itself to what is
unknown, partly known, or unknowable. Jung drew a clear
distinction between these terms and their meaning in his
1921/1971 work Psychological Types, of which Desoille
seemed unaware. While Desoille’s comment could
generically refer to Jung’s (1955-1956/1970) account of hero
myths (p. 531 [CW 14, para. 756]), reflecting the terrible and
devouring aspect of the dual mother (Jung, 1952/1967, p. 366
[CW 5, para. 522]), a quick review of the General Index to the
Collected Works of C. G. Jung shows that Jung referred to
dragons and dragon symbology hundreds of times in many
different ways, which in contrast to Desoille’s own arguably
concretized interpretation, underscores the fact that for Jung
(1921/1971), “a symbol . . . signifies something more and other
than itself which eludes our present knowledge” (CW 6, para.
817), which is to say it points beyond what can be fully known.
Further, despite Desoille’s transpersonal emphasis on
mystical experience and Crampton’s (1974/2005) insistence,
Desoille embraced Jung’s conception of the psyche’s
dynamic structure wherein personality integration is only
possible by shifting the center of identity from the ego to the
Self (p. 5), Desoille (1965) expressly rejected the existence of
the Self archetype “to which Jung attributes almost
superhuman qualities” because, he proclaimed, it shifts
“psychology to metaphysics [which is] in inimical to scientific
research” (p. 43).
Signaling an understanding of Jung’s psychology arguably
as dubious and imprecise as Desoille’s, Wolfgang Kretschmer
(1951/1965) wrote that “Desoille’s valuation of the ‘Collective
Unconscious’”—a theoretical construct he clearly
appropriated from Jung— “is more radical and consequential
than Jung’s in that [Desoille] holds that the meeting with the
‘Collective Unconscious’ is a decisive and unavoidable
presupposition of the therapeutic process” (p. 310). This,
Kretschmer argued, is because, in Desoille’s method, a
subject must learn to consciously “control the ‘Archetypes’
within himself, to be free of them, and . . . lose his fear of
them” (p. 310). Aside from his attribution to the archetypes of
the collective unconscious—which may be inferred through
images but remain ultimately unknowable—the status
knowable things, Kretschmer failed to recognize the central
feature of Jung’s (1928/1966e) analytical psychology is
individuation, “which is a process of differentiation, having for
its goal the development of the individual personality” (p. 155
[CW 7, para. 242]). Because individuation entails an expansion
of consciousness through the ego’s progressive
differentiation from the archetypes and the individual from
the collective, which liberates one from “unconscious
contamination . . . [and] compulsion” (p. 225 [CW 7, para.
373]), one could argue—in contradistinction to Kretschmer’s
assertion above—Jung’s psychology certainly requires
confrontation with, and differentiation from, the archetypes of
the collective unconscious which as Kretschmer notes works
to increase psychological freedom and mitigate fear.
It is possible Desoille’s facile understanding of Jung,
reinforced by Kretschmer’s diminished misapprehension of
his work contributed to Jung’s influence on the development
of guided imagery becoming obscured or unacknowledged
by some contemporary researches. Jung’s ambivalence
regarding publication of his 1916 findings though may also
have contributed to the blurring of his importance. Because
Jung’s methods for working with fantasy images were
privately circulated among his acolytes and not widely
distributed, Singer (1974) asserts, “the major thrust of modern
imagery techniques came from Desoille . . . [who] formalized
many of the procedures still used with variations by
practitioners” such as Hanscarl Leuner, Frétigny and Virel,
and Roberto Assagioli, “all of whom had formal medical or
psychological training (p. 34). One could also argue that these
researchers, each of whom achieved notoriety in the field of
imagery research, emphasized their technical variations over
and against any antecedent influences. Singer (1974) further
states that Desoille’s methods, which were subsequently
elaborated by scientific researchers, paved the way for
experimental investigations and the application of scientific
methods in the field which may also have contributed to
Jung’s opacity and lack of recognition. Because, as Singer
(1974) notes, “the major impetus to the development of
mental imagery techniques in Europe came from the work of
Desoille” (p. 69) who, as discussed, not only misunderstood
Jung but came to disregard depth psychology for being
“much too literary for it to claim to be a science” (Desoille,
1965, p. 33), it is not surprising guided imagery’s intellectual
paternity, which I have shown is traceable to Jung, became at
this significant juncture discontiguous and obscure.
Distinct from other European imagery researchers and
practitioners reviewed in this section, Desoille was by trade
an engineer with no formal training as either a psychologist or
a psychiatrist. His lack of clinical training and renegade
practice of teaching imagery techniques to lay-people in the
community he called “guides” provoked ire and disapproval
from professionally trained members of the academy who
sharply criticized Desoille, not only for his lack of theoretical
sophistication, but for his failure to acknowledge his
predecessors in the field of mental imagery (Frétigny & Virel,
1969).
Desoille presented his technique to subjects in a simple
and straightforward way (Singer 1974). He would instruct his
subject to lie in a recumbent position in a semi-darkened
room with eyes closed and then lead the subject through a
process of progressive relaxation. Once he had induced his
subject to a relaxed and inward focused state, Desoille (1965)
would instruct the subject to envision one of six specific
images representing six different themes, which he believed
“had the subject face every possible kind of life situation” (p.
6). These scenarios, he averred, allowed him to explore all his
subject’s habitual responses. Desoille’s six imagery situations
include the following:
1. A preliminary session in which subjects were directed to
face themselves, through what today would be
considered stereotypical images expressing
retrogressive gender reifications (i.e., swords for males,
vessels for females).
2. The second situation either aquatic or terrestrial
descent was designed to bring a person into a
confrontation with hidden or suppressed character
traits, which arguably correspond with Jung’s
conception of the shadow.
3. The third situation was intended to reconcile unresolved
issues with the opposite sex parent.
4. The fourth situation was designed to resolve issues with
the same-sex parent. In the third and fourth situations,
Desoille once again employed images that by today’s
standards represented sexist gender reifications. Males,
for example, were instructed to venture down into a
cave to meet a witch, whom Desoille conjectured
represented the mother and a wizard for the father.
Women meanwhile were instructed to follow this same
pattern only reversed, encountering first the father (a
wizard) and then the mother (a witch).
5. The fifth imagery theme focused on inhibitive societal
constraints. And the stock image for both sexes in this
imagery situation was a dragon.
6. The sixth scenario was intended to address Oedipal
issues. Both sexes were instructed to venture into
Sleeping Beauty’s castle. And in keeping with the same
arguably stereotypical sex roles, men were told to
identify with Prince Charming whereas women were
instructed to imagine themselves as Sleeping Beauty.
(Desoille, 1966)
Salient features of Desoille’s technique become clear
based on the foregoing which include an extremely high
degree of therapist directivity; the imposition onto the
subject’s imaginal exploration of pre-formulated images
based on gender reifications; and a significant power
differential wherein the therapist, as God-like authority,
governs the subject’s imagery process, determines what
images the subject will encounter, and defines the concrete
meaning of codified images. Problems associated with these
characteristics features of Desoille’s method are astutely
observed by Watkins (1976/1984) who wrote:
Desoille’s techniques arising from his theoretical
notions can end, I think, by often imposing on patient’s
inner worlds, a structure and set of values not
necessarily their own. The possible beneficial results
of having a person establish contact with his imagery
and learn to move in that realm is compromised by a
detailed schedule of places to get to and things to be
accomplished that the therapist thought were
important. . . . Desoille believed that his directiveness
was justified because he understood the patient to be
in need of alternatives for movement. However, there
is harm . . . in not being willing to wait for the client to
generate his own symbolic situations and modes of
being. (pp. 79-80)
Extending Watkins’s critique, one could argue, Desoille’s
inclination to rigidly control every aspect of his subject’s
imagery, including his tendency to lead subjects to imagery
he considered more positive or hopeful, not only overlooked
the value of his subject’s spontaneous imagery he dismissed
as “negative,” but it also arguably underscores the adverse
consequences of his lack of clinical training. Specifically,
Desoille’s reflexive impulse to rescue his subjects
accentuates what one could argue was his inability to
effectively manage his countertransference reactivity, which
resulted in his imposing value judgments on to his subject’s
imagery experience to quell his anxiety. If this was indeed the
case, not only did Desoille fail to stick with the image, by
foreclosing his subject’s imaginal process, he arguably
betrayed it.
HANSCARL LEUNER
Themes of therapist authority and directivity in therapeutic
applications of imagery in European psychotherapy continue
in Leuner’s approach to mental imagery, which Singer and
Pope (1978) extolled as “the most systematic of the European
mental imagery or waking-dream approaches that reflect the
influences among others of Jung’s active imagination
method, Schultz’s autogenic training, and Desoille’s Reve
Éveillé method” (p. 212). Although several guided imagery
historians provide summary presentations of Leuner and his
work (Crampton, 1974/2005; Frétigny & Virel, 1969; Singer,
1974; Singer & Pope, 1978; Watkins, 1976/1984), Leuner
himself offered an epigrammatic introduction to his method
for working with mental imagery in an abbreviated version of
his 1966 lecture to the New Jersey Neuropsychiatric Institute
in Princeton, New Jersey, which published them in The
American Journal of Psychotherapy in 1969. In this lecture,
Leuner asserted he began longitudinal experimental studies
in 1948 to assess psychotherapeutic uses of guided imagery
and its efficacy in clinical practice. Leuner’s (1954) first
professional article, “Kontrolle der Symbolinterpretation im
experimentellen Verfahren” (Control of Symbolic
Interpretation in Experimental Procedures) presented his
techniques for utilizing imagery diagnostically and as a means
for measuring progress in psychotherapy. At that time, Leuner
(1969) referred to his method as “Experimentelies katathymes
Bilderleben (EkB),” which in English is “experimentally
induced catathymic imagery” (p. 2). And he defined
catathymic imagery— a term coined by H. W. Maier in his 1912
journal article titled, “Über Katathyme Wahnbildung und
Paranoia”—as “inner visions which occur . . . with and are
related to affect and emotions” (Leuner, 1969, p. 1). From the
Greek kata which means dependent and thymos, which
means soul, Meloy (2010) asserts that the derived term
catathymic can most easily be understood as “in accordance
with emotion” (p. 1).
Leuner developed his diagnostic and assessment imagery
techniques into a comprehensive psychotherapeutic system
he first designated as Symboldrama which William Swartley
(1965) introduced in the United States as Initiated Symbol
Projection (Hardy, 1987). Leuner subsequently branded his
method Guided Affective Imagery (GAD) (Leuner, 1969). In his
lecture to the New Jersey Psychoanalytic Institute—in which
he provided his first description of GAD in English—Leuner
(1969) claimed to have “formulated and crystallized a
sensitive system of psychotherapy which can provide the
psychodynamic material needed for a genuine depth
psychotherapy” (p. 2) His therapeutic approach, he asserted,
was “able to relieve acute neurotic disturbance in a short
time,” and treat chronic illness “in much less time than is
usually needed for psychoanalysis” (p. 2). Further extolling
the virtues of his method, Leuner reported that even
seemingly intractable psychoanalytic cases “as long as 15
years duration have been treated successfully” (p. 2),
whereas positive outcomes of his method “persisted for
follow-up periods of as long as six years” (p. 2). Even more
impressive, Leuner claimed, was the fact that “the average
treatment took 40 hours, and the range was from one to 160
hours” (p. 2).
As with Desoille and Happich, a central feature of Leuner’s
approach entailed the use of relaxation techniques,
specifically those developed by the psychiatrist Johannes
Heinrich Schultz, which he published in his 1932 book
Autogenous Training. Later, in 1969, Schultz developed his
methods into a comprehensive psychotherapeutic system
with Wolfgang Luthe, with whom he co-authored a six-volume
series of textbooks titled Autogenic Therapy. It is beyond the
scope of this literature review to provide a detailed analysis
of their methods. Luthe (1979), however, provided an
epigrammatic summary in his essay “About the Methods of
Autogenic Therapy,” which essentially describes Autogenic
Training as a method of progressive relaxation that employs a
combination of breathing techniques and somatically focused
autosuggestion designed to induce a deeply relaxed and
inward focused state.
In Leuner’s approach patients were instructed to lie on a
couch and relax using Schultz’s (1932) autogenic training
techniques. He advised GAI practitioners to reduce external,
potentially distracting stimuli, and to dim the lights prior to
initiating the patient’s guided imagery. Leuner developed 10
standard imagery themes, and five principle techniques
designed to encourage the generation of mental imagery,
which also informed his interpretations of his patient’s
images. Singer (1974) describes these techniques as “the
training method, the diagnostic method, the symbol dramatic
method, and psychoanalytic method” (p. 83). Leuner
identified six techniques, which he found especially valuable:
1. The intrapsychic peacemaker. A method used to give
subjects control over the inception of their fantasy
material and affects. (Provides training in assertiveness,
control, and mastery).
2. Confrontation—a kind of desensitization technique.
(Elements of implosive therapy and desensitization).
3. Feeding. Employed to appease scary monster-like
images that may arise. (Desensitization).
4. Reconciliation. Similar to the behavioral technique of
symbolic modeling, this technique allowed the patient to
play out alternative ways of relating.
5. Exhausting and Killing. Imaginal abreaction or discharge
of aggression.
6. Magic Fluids. A combination of imaginal analgesia
associated with experiences of maternal love and
soothing). (as cited in Singer, 1974, p. 83)
In his lecture to the New Jersey Neuropsychiatric Institute,
Leuner also prescribed ten standard imagery scenarios the
first three of which—including specific tasks and techniques
directed by the therapist—comprised the basic tools of his
method. These include the following:
1. The Meadow (adopted from Happich). Leuner claimed
that an open meadow is a beginning place conducive to
visualization which can also serve as a screen upon
which patients project their current mood or most
distressing problems. Possibly symbolizing a
supralapsarian paradise, a return to nature, and spiritual
renewal for Leuner, the meadow represented the
foundational source of an individual’s life (i.e., the
introjected maternal object relationship) (Leuner, 1969)
2. The Mountain (also adopted from Happich). Leuner
instructed his patients to climb a mountain and describe
the view from its summit. He believed that this situation
symbolically indicated the degree to which patients felt
capable of influencing the course their lives. It also
suggested to him the likelihood of patients’ success or
failure in their vocational pursuits. Additionally, Leuner
opined, it might reveal a patient’s repressed desire for
success, notoriety, or fame.
3. The Stream. After descending from the mountaintop and
returning to the meadow Leuner directed his patients to
find a brook or stream and follow it, either to its source
upstream, or downstream where it emptied into the
ocean. To Leuner the stream not only symbolized
constriction or flow of a patient’s psychic energy, but
also the patient’s capacity for emotional growth. In
regard to neurotic patients, Leuner conjectured the
stream never flowed all the way to the ocean without
meeting some form obstruction. Further, water which
was blocked or dammed suggested to him psychic
resistance, the clearing of which he believed led to
insight, enjoyment, and higher development.
4. The House. Evincing Freud’s influence, Leuner saw the
image of a house as a symbol for his patients or their
personalities. Either appearing spontaneously or firmly
suggested by the therapist, Leuner believed patients
could project all their fears and hopes onto the image of
a house. He maintained, for instance, if a patient
envisioned a magnificent palace that it could suggest an
inflated sense of grandiosity whereas a modest dwelling
might indicate low self-regard. Leuner also explored the
layout and different areas of the house (i.e., the kitchen,
bathroom, bedrooms, as well as the size and proximity
of beds). Further exhibiting his fidelity to psychoanalytic
theory, Leuner followed Oedipal themes and suggested,
for example, that a female patient might locate her
clothes in or near her father’s closet. Attics and
basements, meanwhile, were thought by him to lead
back to early developmental experiences and the
possible evocation of repressed traumatic scenes from
childhood.
5. A Close Relationship. The fifth standardized situation
asks patients to imagine their parents, siblings, or some
other intimate relative. Leuner instructed patients to
observe their family members from a distance and to
describe family members’ appearances, attitudes, and
behaviors—especially when family members or relatives
approached the patient. This situation, Leuner believed,
indicated of kind and quality of relationship patients had
with their early caregivers and other important
attachments in their lives. Leuner stated parental figures
could appear in familiar anthropic form or appear as
theriomorphic figures, which he believed helped
circumvent patients’ resistance to directly confronting
their parents. Leuner, for example, specifically linked the
father and mother to an elephant or a cow respectively.
6. A Sexual Scene. Leuner asked his patients to visualize
images and scenes designed to elicit erotic feelings and
sexual fantasies. Like Desoille, Leuner prescribed
arguably heteronormative and sexist stock images
based on reified notions of gender and sexuality. A
woman, for example, was told to imagine that she was
alone and vulnerable on a long walk or a country drive
when her car breaks down. She was then instructed to
imagine a handsome man driving up to rescue her from
this vulnerable situation by offering her a ride. Although
Leuner claimed there existed innumerable possible
outcomes to this scenario, those he specified included:
no car appears, the castrated symbol of a small boy in a
toy care appears, or a sinister, sexually inclined man
drives the woman into a dark forest. A man, meanwhile,
was instructed by Leuner to find a rosebush in the
meadow, to describe the soft pink or red of its
blossoms, touch the delicate petals, and pluck a flower
from the bush to take as his own.
7. A Fierce Beast. In Leuner’s seventh structured
visualization he subtly suggested or directly
commanded his patients to envision a lion or some
other fierce beast. He employed this technique to reveal
a patient’s instinctual nature and aggressive tendencies.
Leuner would, for instance, direct a patient to imagine a
lion or some other ferocious animal and then imagine a
person the patient didn’t like to appear facing the lion in
the same setting. The patient was instructed to describe
the ensuing scene, which often included violent images
of wild carnage.
8. An Ego Ideal. In his eighth standardized imagery
exercise, Leuner directed his patients to state the name
of a person of their same gender without thought or
reflection, and then imagine the figure of a person who
would bear the name the patient blurted out. Leuner
claimed the figure that appeared usually represented
the patient’s idealized self—or ego ideal—which is the
person the patient wished to be. This was helpful, he
averred, in working through issues concerning the
consolidation of identity.
9. A Dark Forest or Cave. Leuner believed certain symbols,
like a cave or darkened woods elicited images that
symbolized more deeply repressed psychological
material. Peering into the dark forest for example, or a
dark cave (which culled material from an even deeper
psychic level) often resulted with a sinister figure
emerging from the dark such as a frightening witch or a
gruesome ogre. Typically, the dark figure was of the
same sex as the patient. Such figures, Leuner averred,
represented the patients early object relationships,
which are internalized representations of a patient’s
early caregivers, along with neurotic relational patterns,
behaviors, and affects.
10.A Swamp. In Leuner’s tenth and final standardized
visualization, the patient was instructed to imagine a
swamp somewhere near the meadow. Leuner told his
patient a figure would emerge out of the swamp’s muck
and mire. This figure, which he specified might be
reptilian, amphibian, fish, or anthropoid symbolically
represented, he claimed, the patient’s most deeply
repressed psychic material and the archaic dimensions
of the patient’s instinctual drives.
Singer (1974) argues that although “Leuner was clearly
influenced in some of his symbolism by the more mystical
Jung” (p. 112), he was more Freudian in his approach which,
Singer claims, is evinced by Leuner’s focus on the personal
level of a patient’s life (i.e., areas of intrapsychic conflict that
emphasized “relationships among striving, ego, ideals, and
parental figures” (p. 112). Leuner’s objective was to make
unconscious personal material conscious and to discharge
repressed material related to a patient’s childhood, not delve
into the mystery of archetypes with the hope of evoking
spiritual experiences. And although he did directly
acknowledge Jung, Leuner (1969) declared, “Although I have
learned a lot about symbolism from the Jungian school, I do
not see any reason to employ its rather mystical theory as
long as there is an adequate theoretical framework which is
more down to earth” (p. 8).
Regarding his general attitude toward imagery and the
unconscious, Watkins (1976/1984) importantly underscores
Leuner, like Desoille before him, segregated images by what
he determined was their positive or negative value. The
problem with imposed valuations of this sort, though, is not
only that it evidences exogenous missionary-like piety, but
the therapist’s influence can align with patients’ own biases
concerning their imagery. Such collusion can censor the
creative unconscious and stifle its poesies. In this way, depth
psychology is reduced to a goal-driven ego-psychology that
intends to bypass negative emotions in singular pursuit of
positive feelings. Consequently, a person’s depth and full
range of images and affects are disregarded in compliance
with the therapist’s imposed authority. Watkins (1976/1984)
aptly notes Leuner’s method is heroic. It follows an arguably
monomythic pattern of descent into the underworld,
confrontation with and the killing of monsters, ascent and
return to the outer world of ego bearing treasures purloined
from the unconscious. One could argue that such imagery
colonizes and exploits the unconscious for outer world gain
and results in the unconscious expressing itself in
symptomatic or destructive ways. To wit, Watkins contends,
“in this view of the conscious relation to the unconscious all
that tends to keep the person in the unconscious, that
refuses to be brought up, becomes threatening [and]
monstrous indeed” (p. 88).
Founded on practitioner authority, objectivity, and
rationality, our first four philosophically informed imagery
practitioners (i.e., Happich, Caslant, Desoille, Leuner) evince
starkly modernist values which, especially in the case of
Desoille and Leuner, contestably devolved into a sort of
psychic colonialism. Their mutual tendency to omnisciently
insert their imagery onto a subject’s imaginal experiences as
deus ex machina demonstrates that they understood the
indigenous psyche to be dangerous, dark, and primitive. It
follows from this shared belief that imaginal work, in their
view, required a practitioner’s strict moral guidance to ensure
their patients’ salvation, which is to say their patients’ proper
psychic development and maturation. Diverging from these
first four European imagery practitioners, our next
researchers: Frétigny and Virel, Roberto Assagioli, Martha
Crampton and Mary Watkins each devised more egalitarian
approaches to guided imagery which arguably expressed the
values of humanistic psychology. Simply stated, this means
their approaches were comparatively more client-centered,
nondirective, and permissive than those previously surveyed.
4
of Singer’s tripartite structural framework in this literature
review.
RECENT APPLICATIONS:
ROBERTO ASSAGIOLI
AND PSYCHOSYNTHESIS
Roberto Assagioli was an early transpersonal psychologist
and the founder of Psychosynthesis. In 1905, he was among
the first Italians to join the psychoanalytic movement.
Assagioli (1974) corresponded with but never met Freud. He
did, however, meet Jung, and by his own account not only
exchanged letters with him for decades but maintained what
he described as a “cordial relationship” (as cited in Keen,
1974) with Jung from their first 1907 meeting in Zurich until
Jung’s death in 1961. Massimo Rosselli and Duccio Vanni
(2014) traced their “long term professional and friendly
relationship” (p. 7) in their article “Roberto Assagioli and Carl
Gustav Jung,” which was published in The Journal of
Transpersonal Psychology. Drawing on Assagioli’s three 1967
transcribed lectures titled Jung and Psychosynthesis and
“papers and other materials in the Assagioli Archives” (p. 31),
Rosselli and Vanni trace Jung’s influence on Assagioli and
document what they contend are parallel developments in
their psychological systems rather than Assagioli’s
appropriations of Jung’s ideas. Aside from their stated
purpose—to distinguish Assagioli’s psychosynthesis from
Jung’s analytical psychology and advance the case that
Assagioli was a pioneer equal to Jung in the founding and
expansion of transpersonal psychology, their article also
arguably demonstrates Jung’s indelible influence on
Assagioli, who along with many other psychosynthesis writers
reviewed (Crampton, 1969, 1975, 1977; Gerard, 1967;
Haronian, 1976; Vargiu, 1977) all seem incapable of describing
psychosynthesis without repeated references to Jung and his
seminal ideas.
According to Martha Crampton (2001), psychosynthesis
refers to Assagioli’s theory and practice of transpersonal
development, which aims to unify a patient’s personality with
the foundational indwelling source of meaning, direction, and
purpose Assagioli called “the transpersonal Self.” Understood
as a wellspring of wisdom, love, and spiritual inspiration, the
transpersonal Self, she states, not only functions to organize
and integrate the individual personality, but also propels “the
will to meaning and service” (p. 567). Assagioli’s presented
his system in two published volumes, Psychosynthesis: A
Collection of Basic Writings (1965) and The Act of Will (1973).
Two additional works by him were published posthumously,
Transpersonal Development: The Dimension Beyond
Psychosynthesis (1993) and Freedom in Jail (2016). Assagioli
believed that depth psychology, while important, only
captured part of the fullness of human life. Wholeness
required a “height” psychology to compensate the depth
perspective and make it fully representative of the sublime
dimensions of human experience (Cortright, 1997). Thus,
Assagioli developed a series of techniques by which the “I”
(i.e., the conscious personality) could gain access to and align
with the superconscious (i.e., the higher, spiritual region of the
psyche), leading to realization and identification with the
transpersonal Self (i.e., the timeless ontological center of life).
According to Assagioli and Vargiu (n.d.), “the Self is
unchanging in essence, yet it sends out its energies, which
are stepped down in intensity and transmitted through the
Superconscious, and received, absorbed and utilized by the
personality” (p. 2). Utilizing guided imagery as the primary
technique, the psychosynthetic process entails a series of
identifications and disidentifications with contents from the
lower unconscious called “subpersonalities” (p. 2). These
split-off and disavowed aspects of the personality are similar
to Jung’s (1969/1948) notion of complexes, which he
characterized as autonomous personalities independently
operating in the sphere of consciousness (p. 96 [CW 8, para.
201]). By disidentifying from these lower parts, one begins to
make contact with ever higher levels of consciousness that
culminate in identification with the transpersonal Self
(Cortright, 1997). Before turning to the specific ways mental
imagery is conceived of and utilized in psychosynthesis, I will
analyze Rosselli and Vanni’s (2014) juxtaposition of Jung and
Assagioli’s transpersonal psychologies and present key
theoretical differences identified by them.
Rossi and Vanni compare Jung and Assagioli’s
psychological systems in a variety of ways, including their
respective views on archetypes, psychic structure, and
models of the unconscious; conceptual entities (i.e., the ‘I’, the
Self archetype and the transpersonal Self); their views
regarding psychological growth and development (i.e.,
individuation vs. psychosynthesis); and psychotherapeutic
methods (Rosselli & Vanni, 2014). Regarding the collective
unconscious and the nature of archetypes, Rosselli and Vanni
(2014) specify that Assagioli was in basic agreement with
Jung—that there exists “a collective-archetypal unconscious
beyond the personal unconscious” (p. 9). Yet more than Jung,
they insist, Assagioli developed a structured approach to
these transpersonal dimensions that emphasized a direct
experience of archetypal contents. Although one might agree
that Jung was less than systematic in his description of
individuation and provided little operational guidance in
terms of specific techniques one might use to facilitate the
process, his account of intense engagement with archetypal
images emerging from the creative unconscious—such as
those he documented in his The Red Book—were published a
full seven years before Rosselli and Vanni’s article. Contrary
to their assertion, therefore, direct confrontation with
archetypal contents (i.e., numinous archetypal images) is the
sine qua non of Jungian psychology. Moreover, as Slater
notes, many of the precepts of Jung’s psychology,
appropriated by Assagioli and delineated by Rosselli and
Vanni, were well established without reference to his imaginal
experiences long before The Red Book’s publication (G.
Slater, personal communication, December 8, 2016), not the
least of which was Jung’s emphasis on the dynamic
relationship between the conscious and the unconscious
mediated by images, which is arguably the cornerstone of
Jung’s psychology. Rossi and Vanni next criticize Jung for
what they argue was his lack of conceptual clarity, especially
for what they see as his failure to carefully distinguish the
various strata of the collective unconscious, namely what
Assagioli described as the archaic lower layer associated with
early childhood issues; the middle layer—where experience is
integrated into a nonreflexive but coherent personal
expression; and the higher layer called the transpersonal
unconscious—or superconscious—which is related to
aesthetics, peak experiences, and inspired creativity (Firman
& Gila, 2002). Rossi and Vanni (2014) also charge that Jung
committed what transpersonal psychologist Ken Wilber (2011)
termed the “pre- trans- fallacy” which simply stated means
that he did not clearly differentiate pre-personal/pre-rational
and trans-personal/trans-rational levels of consciousness.
When reductively muddled, authentic mystical experiences
are viewed as forms of regression to states of “infantile
narcissism, oceanic adualism [and] indissociation” (p. 88). One
could argue that Freud committed this error in his theorizing.
Alternately, when elevated, undifferentiated prepersonal
states are confusedly assigned a grandiose status so that
primary infantile narcissism is conflated with transrational
mystical union. And this error, Assagioli (1974) and Wilber
(2011) claim, is more commonly committed by Jung and his
followers. Noting a tendency among transpersonal
psychologists to devalue the primal and instinctual psyche for
the soulless transcendent fantasy of pure spirit, Slater refutes
the charge that Jung conflated lower and higher dimensions
of the collective psyche (G. Slater, personal communication,
December 8, 2016). As evidence, he points to Jung’s
(1954/1969a) infrared/ultraviolet analogy, which Jung used to
describe the dual nature of archetypes. Jung’s illustrative
analogy conceives of archetypes as existing on a visible
spectrum of light bracketed by two invisible poles: infrared
light on one side and ultraviolet light on the other (pp. 211–
215 [CW 8, paras. 414-420]). In this symbolic model, ultraviolet
light is seen as the spiritual/psychic dimension of the
archetype, whereas the infrared is understood as the
archetypes instinctual/physical (psychoid) dimension. This
analogy, he rightly contends, not only illustrates Jung’s
differentiation of the lower and higher psychic realms contra
Rossi and Vanni’s claims but demonstrates his understanding
that lower and higher dimensions of the psyche must
collaborate and finally be drawn together (G. Slater, personal
communication, December 3, 2016).
Assagioli believed that expanded consciousness hinged
not only on engagement with the numinous psyche but more
specifically with “direct experience of the transpersonal Self,”
which he understood as “an experiential reality [and] a core
point of identity” (Rosselli & Vanni, 2014, p. 9). This distinction,
Assagioli’s conception of the Self as empirical and
personalistic, evinces that for him, the transpersonal Self was
not an archetype beyond consciousness as it was for Jung
but was rather seen by him as an ontological reality that
could be fully realized through human experience. Assagioli’s
experiential conception of the Self illuminates what is
arguably the problematic nature of Jung’s commitment to
Kantian epistemology (examined in detail in Volume 2), which
differentiated phenomenal reality from unknowable numinal
entities and informed Jung’s division between archetypes as
a priori unknowable (numina) and archetypal images
disclosed to consciousness which are a posteriori
(phenomenal) experiences. Kant’s philosophy comported
neatly with the values of scientific materialism, which in turn
conferred legitimacy on what was then the burgeoning field
of dynamic psychology. And while Jung’s Kantianism shielded
him from charges of metaphysical speculation or mysticism, it
left him open to critiques like Rosselli and Vanni’s (2014) who
assert that “for Jung, the Self, as archetype, could not be
experienced and remained unconscious” (p. 9). By arguably
bypassing the subtlety of Jung’s notion of psychological
reality, Rosselli and Vanni proudly distinguish Assagioli’s
psychosynthesis over and against Jung’s analytical
psychology. Based on Assagioli’s belief they trumpet, “for
Assagioli the transpersonal Self is a reality that can be
directly experienced” (p. 9). It would seem, however, that
Rosselli and Vanni misconstrue Jung’s idea of the archetype
per se. Conceived of by Jung, the archetype per se is a
noumenon, a mere potential, proclivity, or form that must be
filled with empirical content to be experienced. Properly
understood, Rosselli and Vanni’s suggestion that Assagioli—
by means of psychosynthesis—could directly experience the
transpersonal Self is nonsensical, because “by definition,
there is nothing to experience if one encounters this empty
imaginative predisposition” (G. Slater, personal
communication, December 8, 2016).
Further describing the phenomenology of the
psychosynthetic Self, Rosselli and Vanni (2014) remark that
“the Self can be experienced both as ‘personal self or I’ at the
center of the personality and as ‘transpersonal Self’ at higher
levels of consciousness” (p. 9). Because the Self, in
Assagioli’s view, is an all-inclusive ontological reality, they
argue his view of the transpersonal Self cannot be limited to,
and indeed far surpasses, Jung’s conception of psychic
totality (i.e., the combined spheres of the conscious and the
unconscious).
In the parlance of Jungian psychology, the personal self is
simply called ego, and its superordinate ground, from which it
emerges, differentiates, and ultimately returns is called The
Self. Beyond semantic inflection, Rosselli and Vanni seem to
suggest, in contrast to Jung, that for Assagioli there is no
other—no objective dimension of the psyche. One could
argue this characterization not only denies phenomenological
experience of the unconscious (i.e., the seeming objectivity of
dreams or the intrusive disruption of complexes), it also paints
the unconscious in cozy terms of sameness, and thus
reduces the psyche from a system of propulsive alterity to
one of bipolar (i.e., either inflated or mundane) ego states.
Notwithstanding their conflation, Rosselli and Vanni’s
account of the Self calls to mind Edward Edinger’s (1972)
description of the Jungian Self as “the ordering and unifying
center of the total psyche (conscious and unconscious) just as
the ego is the center of the conscious personality” (p. 3).
Whereas the psychosynthetic conception arguably combines
ego and Self, Jung considered the ego to be “the seat of
subjective identity,” and the Self to be “the seat of objective
identity . . . [and] the point where transpersonal energies flow
into personal life” (pp. 3–4). Beyond these theoretical
distinctions, which arguably become irrelevant during
phenomenological (in vivo) experiences of the numinous,
Rosselli and Vanni stress that “in psychosynthesis . . . the Self
and . . . integration of the transpersonal are both the goal
and the means . . . [to achieve] the creation of a connection
between the transpersonal and the personal” (p. 9). This
description of Assagioli’s Self as both object and aim, though,
is arguably no real departure from Jung’s understanding of
individuation as the full and unique development of the
personality mediated by the transcendent function (described
in detail in Volume 2) which, briefly stated, is a dialogical
process between the conscious (ego) and the unconscious
(Self) that works to symbolically transcend the opposites and
balance the psyche. In his comparison of Jung’s and
Assagioli’s respective psychologies, the transpersonal
psychologist Brant Cortright (2007) asserts that though their
systems may have originated independently, their maps of
the psyche are “very similar.” However, “instead of Jung’s
Self,” for Assagioli “there is the Transpersonal Self to which
the I or ego is to subordinate itself.” Additionally, in place of
Jung’s “collective unconscious and archetypal energies, there
is the higher unconscious and the transpersonal will.”
Notwithstanding these differences, “In both systems the path
to transcendence lies through the personal wounds,
complexes and subpersonalities of the unconscious” (p. 95).
Regarding Assagioli and Jung’s respective ideas about
individual and transpersonal development guided by the Self,
Assagioli admitted, “the goals are the same . . . [and] the
methods used in the process of individuation are partly the
same and inclusive” (as cited in Rosselli, 2014, p. 10). These
methods included “dream work and self-exploration by
means of symbols, creativity and the imagination” (p. 30).
Concerning the promotion of synthesis and personality
integration, because Assagioli emphasized consciousness
and will, which he saw as the primary function of the Self,
Rosselli and Vanni contend that his directed, structured, and
active techniques (i.e., guided imagery and visualization) are
superior to Jung’s method of active imagination, which in
their view is a more unpredictable synthetic methodology
(Rosselli & Vanni, 2014). Assagioli meanwhile fully
acknowledged the synthetic dimension of Jung’s psychology
and observed, the shared goal of analytical psychology and
psychosynthesis is the reconstruction and integration of the
personality. He wrote, “what Jung called the ‘process of
individuation’ [is] a goal shared by psychosynthesis itself” (as
cited in Rosselli and Vanni, 2014, p. 20). Rosselli and Vanni
make these final points concerning the similarities between
Jung and Assagioli’s psychologies: both systems, they write,
frame “the human existential journey,” as a sort of “spiritual
quest,” which progresses through “various stages of
transformation and inner growth,” towards the realization of
one’s “essential uniqueness and wholeness.” One becomes
oneself through this process and integrates “all the different
conscious and unconscious elements of the psyche” (p. 28).
Assagioli first met Jung in August 1907 during his first trip
to the Burghölzi hospital in Zurich, where he worked on an
association study with Edouard Claparéde and Theodore
Flournoy (Berti, 1988). In July 1909 Assagioli returned to the
Burghölzi to complete his doctoral dissertation, which he
titled “La Psiconalisi” (Psychoanalysis). Three months earlier,
on April 2, 1909 Jung wrote to Freud, “There must be [a]
special complex . . . having to do with the prospective
tendencies in man. If there is a ‘psychoanalysis’ there must
also be a ‘psychosynthesis’ which creates future events
according to the same laws” (McGuire, 1994, p. 216). Timing
alone would suggest that Jung originated the term
psychosynthesis, which Assagioli appropriated as the
moniker for his psychological system. And this argument
becomes even more compelling when one considers
Assagioli’s wider embrace of Jung’s ideas (i.e. the collective
unconscious). However, citing Gary Lachman’s (2010) book
Jung the Mystic: The Esoteric Dimensions of Jung’s Life and
Teachings, Rosselli and Vanni (2014) intimate the opposite
direction of influence and assert that “Lachman put forward
the view ‘Jung got the idea of psychosynthesis from Roberto
Assagioli, whose work he admired’” (p. 30). This paraphrased
quotation comes from a footnote in Lachman’s book, which
amounts to an off-hand remark with no real evidentiary
support. Moreover, Lachman’s actual words are, “it is possible
Jung got the idea of psychosynthesis from Assagioli whom
Jung met 1909, and whose work he admired” (p. 223). On its
face, this statement is more conjecture than argument.
Furthermore, after raising the question of influence and
dubiously implying Assagioli originated the term, Rosselli and
Vanni (2014) admit that “it is a plausible hypothesis that Jung .
. . was . . . the first to propose the term ‘psychosynthesis’”
(30). Regarding Jung and his influence, the final word goes to
Assagioli (1974) who pronounced:
Jung . . . [was] an explorer of the vast and little known
territory of the psyche . . . he was a courageous and
brilliant pioneer who opened up new paths and gave
new dimensions to the human mind. . . . He
contributed greatly to the freeing of psychology from
the narrow trammels of a purely descriptive
objectivism and expanded immensely its field by
demonstrating the existence and the value of the
higher psychic functions, of spiritual levels and
need. . . . Moreover . . . he points the way to liberation
from the conditioning pressures on the personality and
from the powerful influences exerted by the images
and structures of the collective unconscious. (p. 7)
PSYCHOSYNTHESIS IMAGERY
TECHNIQUES
Assagioli (1965) devised “techniques of visualization” (p.
145) to promote both personal psychosynthesis and spiritual
psychosynthesis, which is to say the integration of both
personal and transpersonal levels of the psyche. Though he
wrote about visualization as a discrete practice, imagination
and imagery are aspects of a many of his therapeutic
techniques. Personal psychosynthesis techniques that utilize
images and imagination include catharsis, self-identification,
and disidentification. The catharsis technique asks patients to
re-live distressing emotional situations or scenes by
imagining they are in the original situation living the
experience in the present. This process can be augmented
by having patients express themselves to an imagined person
or persons related to the re-vivified scene (Roberto Assagioli,
1965). Self-Identification and Disidentification are paired
techniques that also employ images and imagination and are
related to patients’ imagined explorations of their processes
of introspection, affect, and thought. Assagioli (1965) asserted
evocation of images by means of visualization and the
recruitment of all the subtle senses of the imagination—visual,
auditory, tactile, kinesthetic, and olfactory—is “one of the
most important and spontaneously active functions of the
human psyche, both in its conscious and in its unconscious
aspect or levels” (p. 144). Which is why, he maintained—
emphasizing the ego’s domestication of it— that the
imagination “has to be controlled when excessive or
dispersed; to be trained when weak, and to be utilized owing
to its great potency” (p. 144). Assagioli differentiated between
consciously selected images he called “reproductive
imagination,” and spontaneous images arising from the
unconscious he termed “creative imagination” (p. 146).
Because he emphasized strengthening the imaginal senses
by means of training a patient’s will—one could argue—he
favored reproductive (and highly directive) techniques by
which he essentially told a patient what images to see and
what actions to take (Assagioli, 1965). Ideal Model Imagery is
another important imagination-based psychosynthesis
technique. After identifying and deconstructing a patient’s
faulty life model—usually fashioned from external
expectations and social imperatives—the therapist facilitates
the patient’s construction of an ideal model which expresses
“a dynamic, inner creative pattern” (p. 168).
Citing the mental imagery practices of Happich (1938) and
Desoille (1945), Assagioli states that Symbol Utilization is
another psychosynthetic technique that utilizes images and
imagination. One could argue, however, that his version of
this technique might be more accurately described as Sign
Utilization for the seemingly univocal meanings of the images
he prescribes. Referring to a compendium of stock images
used in psychosynthesis, Assagioli states, for example, “only
symbols of a positive value are presented to the patient and
are therefore listed here” (p. 183). These images include a
variety of “Nature Symbols . . . animal Symbols . . . Human
Symbols . . . Man Made Symbols . . . Religious and
Mythological Symbols . . . Abstract Symbols . . . and Individual
or Spontaneous Symbols” (pp. 181–182). His belief that these
cataloged images are uniformly positive gives lie to their
status as symbols whose definitive meaning, Jung insisted—
unlike signs—can never be fully known. Recalling Watkins’s
(1976/1984) critique of Desoille, one could further argue
Assagioli’s moral valuations regarding the positive or
negative value of images has no place in a truly client-
centered method of working with images and imagination
which psychosynthesis purports to be (Roberto Assagioli &
Vargiu, n.d., p. 2).
Techniques that Assagioli employed to facilitate a patient’s
spiritual psychosynthesis include, once again, specific
“symbols” that “can indicate or evoke the spiritual Self” (p.
203). These comprise geometric mandala-like symbols,
nature symbols like the Sun, and importantly—in relationship
to both Jungian psychology and contemporary RGI practices
—personified wisdom figures (Assagioli, 1965). Similar to
Jung’s (1963) dialogical engagement with “ghostly gurus” like
“Philemon . . . [who] represented the spiritual aspect,” or “Ka
[who] represented a kind of earth demon” (pp. 184–185)—and
anticipating contemporary guided imagery’s inner advisor
technique—personified wisdom figures in Assagioli’s system
were thought to mediate the relationship between the
personal and transpersonal Self by means of inner dialogue
(Assagioli, 1965). Assagioli’s methods designed to facilitate
spiritual psychosynthesis are as directive as those he
employed at the personal level and include exercises based
on the Grail Legend, Dante’s Divine Comedy, and the image
of a rose blossoming (Assagioli, 1965). These exercises
address the higher spiritual dimensions of human experience,
which Assagioli attributed to “the superconscious” whose
functional elements include “aesthetic, ethical, religious
experiences, intuition, inspiration [and] states of mystical
consciousness” (p. 6). It is important here to underscore
Assagioli’s distinction between the superconscious and the
Self. The superconscious is a transpersonal region of the
psyche and the energies and functions associated with it. The
Self meanwhile is the focal point and cause of
superconscious processes. A simple analogy clarifies this
distinction: “The Self is to the superconscious as the ‘I’, or
personal self, is to the elements and functions of the
personality” (Assagioli & Vargiu, n.d., p. 2). Additional mental
imagery procedures utilized by Assagioli include techniques
to develop intuition; induce states of tranquil serenity; evoke
personal relationships; and control, sublimate, and transform
sexual energy (Assagioli, 1965).
Because Assagioli strenuously emphasizes consciousness
and will as the means to integrate the personality, he seems
to invite Hillman’s (1975) critique of the “Modern Disciplines of
the Imagination” (p. 38), which in Hillman’s view uniformly
work to depotentiate autonomous images and strengthen the
ego. The aim of such techniques, Hillman argues, “is less the
realization of the images . . . than it is the realization of the
personality performing the exercise, i.e., the ego.” And this
fact, he avers, exposes the “disciplines of the imagination” for
what, in his view, they truly are—misguided despotic efforts of
“disciplining of the image” (p. 39). While one may sympathize
with Hillman’s characterization of modern imagery disciplines
as vehicles for the pedestrian ego to trammel over and spoil
pristine imaginal lands and exploit its resources, his depiction
is arguably one-sided and hyperbolic. Contrary to Hillman’s
contestably projected neo-colonial fantasy, one can, I argue,
approach the imaginal word with ecological reverence,
openness, and receptivity. Hillman’s withering reaction to
various disciplines of the imagination (i.e., Desoille’s, Leuner’s,
Assagioli’s), although illuminating, can be understood as a
necessary compensation to what was modern Western
psychology’s arguably monocular emphasis on ego
consolidation, which reached cartoon-like proportions in
some new-age applications of image work which Tacey
(2004) describes as “positively infantile and deeply
regressed” (p. 24). Hillman’s revisioning, inverted the ego’s
dominion and subordinated its heroic autonomy to the
imaginal soul. After this reactive penance, though, I will argue
for a more ecologically balanced approach, one that
acknowledges the reciprocal need for a relationship between
the ego and the inhabitants of the imagination who emerge
ex nihlo from the autonomous psyche that is their creative
source.
PSYCHOSYNTHETIC VISUALIZATION:
THE ENVELOPING EGO AND ITS
LEGATEES.
Assagioli’s heirs (Crampton, 1969, 1974/1977, 1974/2005;
Ferrucci, 2009; Gerard, 1967; Rowan, 1989; Vargiu, 1977) all
developed mental imagery techniques under the aegis of
psychosynthesis in order to access “regions of our being
which are completely unavailable to our analytical mind,” and
can only be disclosed through the “faculty…[of] the intuition”
(Ferrucci, 2009, p. 118). Consonant with Assagioli’s arguably
ego-centric approach to images, Robert Gerard’s (1967)
method of Symbolic Visualization also emphasizes the willful
“development and control of imaginative processes” (p. 1).
Gerard insists by using “Controlled Symbolic Visualization” (p.
1), a patient can acquire “control over imaginative processes,”
and strengthen “his sense of self-identity as a directing agent
over his inner and outer life” (p. 2). Employing stock images,
Gerard directs his patients’ imagery to ensure specific
outcomes he believes to be therapeutic. If his patient fails, as
instructed by him, to imagine a flower blossoming to full
bloom, for example, he insists that the patient repeatedly
undertake the procedure until the patient is able to “produce
a beautiful rose and . . . realize that the whole process is a
significant symbol of . . . inner growth” (p. 1). Given that the
images to which he refers seem to possess only one
meaning, Gerard, like his teacher Assagioli, mischaracterizes
as symbols what are actually signs. The image of a sunflower
with an inner core surrounded by petals, for example, is
defined by him as an image of “synthesis . . . integration and
balance” (p. 2). A pair of clasping hands, he definitively
claims, are a symbol of relational harmony. And dusting off
Desoille’s reified gender tropes, Gerard defines swords and
cup-like receptacles as symbols of masculinity and femininity
respectively. According to him, even colors signify a one-to-
one relationship to specific affects. He claims, for instance,
that in his psychophysiological studies the color “blue was
found to have a tranquilizing effect” (pp. 2–3). Although
“Spontaneous Symbolic Visualization” (p. 4) is briefly
mentioned by Gerard, he frames this counterpoint to
controlled visualization in terms of disruption to those
therapist-directed methods he prefers. Gerard cites Jellinek
(1949) and Goldberger (1957) as clinicians who utilized
spontaneous symbolic visualization. A quick review of their
published articles, however, suggests that while they may
have entertained elements of spontaneous imagery, their
methods were nevertheless quite directive. Take Jellinek
(1949), for example: although she did invite a patient to
visualize a spontaneous image of his speech impediment—
which he did by imagining his stuttering as a small dwarf
sitting on his shoulder—she told him, “If you speak slowly, you
will starve the little demon and it will die” (p. 380). Next,
insofar as he would instruct his patients to visualize images of
specific verbal thoughts, Goldberger (1957) was similarly
directive in his approach. On a positive note, though,
Goldberger did caution therapists to make every “effort to
avoid suggesting to the subject what he should see” (p. 128).
To his credit, Gerard (1967) acknowledges Jung’s “‘active
imagination’ technique . . . in which . . . the patient is asked to
continue an interrupted dream or imagine a dialogue with the
figures of the dream” (p. 4) as being perhaps the most
unstructured techniques of spontaneous symbolic
visualization. The accuracy of his description notwithstanding,
Gerard rightly identifies the nondirective and spontaneous
nature of Jung’s method.
Although in his chapter on symbolic visualization Piero
Ferrucci (2009) refers to Jung’s idea of symbols that “point to
something that is very little known or completely unknown”
(p. 118), he nevertheless asserts that by means of
identification with a symbol, one can “understand the symbol
from within . . . the formless reality the symbol represents,” so
that it becomes a “reservoir of revelation” (p. 119). Insofar as
Ferrucci is suggesting that an image can reveal to the
imagining person considering it a specific meaning related to
that person’s specific imaginal inquiry—which is to say that
one can ask an image what it means and allow the image
itself to answer—I am in agreement. However, because he
goes on to prescribe a series of stock images designed to
induce particular experiences (i.e., The Lighthouse; The
Butterfly; The Sun; The Flame; The Fount; The Diamond; The
Sky; The Ship; The Bell, The Arrow; and The Villa), it would
appear that Ferrucci too falls prey to a one-sided emphasis
on ego development in his approach to mental imagery.
Of particular relevance to this IPA study on the relationship
between Jungian active imagination and RGI are
subpersonality techniques developed by James Vargiu (1977)
and John Rowan (1989). Based on the notion the psyche is
multiple, comprised of what William James (1890) described
as “various selves” (p. 315) and range from persona-like roles
that coalesce around the ego to frank dissociative states,
these guided imagery techniques may be applied to working
with parts, polarities, and conflict. Analogous to Jung’s (Jung
& Riklin, 1904/1973) notion of “emotionally charged
complexes” (p. 72 [CW 2, para. 167]), Rowan (1989) defines a
subpersonality as “a semi-permanent, and semi-autonomous
region of the personality capable of acting as a person” (p. 8).
This definition accords with Jung’s (1948/1969c) description of
a complex, which possesses “a powerful inner coherence . . .
wholeness and . . . a relatively high degree of autonomy so
that . . . it behaves like an animated foreign body within the
sphere of consciousness (p. 96 [CW 8, para. 201]). The
experience of a subpersonality, according to Rowan, feels like
“being ‘taken over’ by a part of ourselves . . . we didn’t know
was there” and makes one say to oneself “‘I don’t know what
got into me’” (p. 7). Jung (1948/1969c) meanwhile avers,
because they “behave like independent beings” (p. 121 [CW 8,
para. 253]), “There is no difference in principle between a
fragmentary personality and a complex” (p. 97 [CW 8, para.
202]). To that, one could argue, neither is there much
difference between a complex and a subpersonality. Vargiu
(1977) specifies that subpersonality integration takes place
around “a higher order center . . . the ‘I,’ the personal center if
identity” (p. 7) by means of “a ‘guided daydream,’” which he
states is “a means of establishing two-way communication
with the unconscious.” This dialogical process is facilitated by
a guide, he says, who “encourages . . . and helps [one] to
move on and face and resolve problem areas, usually on a
symbolic level” (p. 9). The guided daydream, Vargiu further
specifies, can be utilized to access the transpersonal
dimension of the superconscious and channel
superconscious energies toward subpersonality
harmonization, which entails recognition, reciprocal
abreaction, acceptance, coordination, identification,
disidentification, empathy, and synthesis (Vargiu, 1977).
Martha Crampton, perhaps more than any of Assagioli’s
successors, was instrumental not only in contextualizing
psychosynthetic visualization in the history of mental imagery
techniques but also in developing a method of working with
images that arguably informs most contemporary approaches
to RGI. Because she and the Jungian analyst Mary Watkins
developed similar perspectives regarding image work based
on their homologous genealogical analyses of guided
imagery and its antecedent influences, I will present their
approaches to imagery and image work—which may be
characterized as client-centered, nondirective, and
permissive—under the same heading. This pairing arguably
not only represents a kind of rapprochement between
psychosynthesis and analytical psychology, but it also more
consciously re-locates contemporary RGI practices in the
history of Jungian and archetypal psychology, which I argue
became obscured, in part, due to a pseudo clade divergence
inaugurated by Robert Desoille and his followers.
Concomitant with his rejection of Jung’s psychology for
Pavlovian behaviorism, the widespread dissemination of
Desoille’s technique Le rêve éveillé dirigé (The Directed
Waking Dream), and the varied European methods it
spawned arguably filled the methodological vacuum created
by Jung’s early ambivalence regarding public disclosure of
his imaginal experiences and his seeming disregard for
operationalizing his method of active imagination.
HENRY CORBIN
In his essay “Towards a Chart of the Imaginal,” which is a
prelude to the second edition of Corps Spirituel et Terre
celeste de l’Iran Mazdeen a l’Iran Sh’ite (Spiritual body and
celestial Earth: From Mazdean Iran to Shi’ite Iran), Henry
Corbin (1977/1989) further elaborates the idea of a third
distinct area of psychic reality “between sense perceptions
and the intuitions or categories of the intellect.” This third all-
but-forgotten ontological region, he argues, is neither the
intellectual world of conceptual abstraction nor the empirical
world of sensuality. Rather, he avers, it is a “suprasensual
world,” and can only be accessed by means of the mediating
power “Active Imagination,” or “agent imagination,” which in
the modern West, he laments, has “been left to the poets” (p.
vii). Whether imagination has indeed been lost or merely
trivialized by Western rationalism, to even speak of this
subtle, intermediate realm, Corbin insisted, required a
neologism to differentiate it from the frivolous sense of
unreality suggested by the debased word imaginary. Thus, he
coined the Latinate neologism mundus imaginalis (p. ix) (i.e.,
the imaginal realm), to describe this real but forgotten
dimension, and he provided a detailed explication of it in his
three major works translated to English: Avicenna and the
Visionary Recital (1960), Creative Imagination in the Sufism of
Ibn ‘Arabi (1969), and Spiritual Body and Celestial Earth
(1977/1989). In these hermeneutic exegeses, Corbin
elucidates the medieval Muslim philosophies of Avicenna
(981-1037), Suhrawardi (1155-1191), Ibn ‘Arabi (1165-1240), and
Mulla Sadra (1571-1640).
Corbin maintained that the mundus imaginalis is
synonymous with several Arabic and Persian phrases,
including “malakût (the subtle world of the souls), the
barzakh (the interworld), hurqalya (the world of the celestial
Earth), na koja abad (the land of nowhere), and ‘alam al-
mithal (the world of images and archetypal ideas)” (Mahmoud,
2005, p. 1). Though he coined the term mundus imaginalis, he
attributed the idea of the intermediate imaginal realm to
Suhrawardi, the founder of the Illuminationist School of
Persian mysticism, who provided an ontological foundation
for the imaginal realm in his “Book of Conversations.” In
keeping with Suhwardi’s spiritual cosmology, Corbin
(1972/1995) specifies, there exists a hierarchy of three worlds,
each of which expresses a distinct level of reality. At the
lowest level is Molk, which is the earthly plane of existence
(i.e., the physical world of the senses and material objects).
The next, median level is the Malakût, the land of souls,
images, and archetypal ideas, which is synonymous with
Corbin’s (1989/1997) mundus imaginalis. The highest level,
meanwhile, is Jabarut, which is the world of candescent
beings, “pure Intellectual forms” (p. 3), and “pure archangelic
Intelligences” (Corbin, 1972/1995, p. 8). Corresponding to
Suhwardi’s tripartite structure, Corbin (1972/1995) specifies,
are “three organs of knowledge: the senses, the imagination,
and the intellect,” and these in turn correspond to “the triad of
anthropology,” which is to say the three levels human being:
“body, soul, spirit” (p. 8). Pursuant to Suhwardi’s cosmology,
between the dualistic Cartesian worlds of abstract
understanding and the empirical sensation and indeed,
seamlessly conjoining them, is “an intermediate world . . . of
the Image, mundus imaginalis: a world as ontologically real as
the world of the senses and the world of the intellect” (p. 9).
Corbin (1989/1997) emphasizes, “the mundus imaginalis and
the Imaginal Forms [are] . . . defined by their median and
mediation situation,” in between these other two discrete and
mutually abstracted ontological and epistemological modes.
Regarding the function of the mundus imaginalis, he avers,
“On the one hand [the imaginal realm] immaterialises the
Sensible Forms, on the other it ‘imaginalises’ the Intellectual
Forms to which it gives shape and dimension” (p. 2), which
essentially means that the imagination and images link,
mediate, and synthesize ideas of the intellect and percepts of
the senses.
This idea of an intermediate, mediating, and synthesizing
dimension is further explained by the “great master” of
Sufism Ibn al-’Arabi (1165-1240), who, William Chittick (1989)
asserts, “considered imagination as the underlying stuff of
both the universe and the human soul and insisted upon
placing imaginal perception on equal footing with rational
understanding” (p. 99). A century after Suhwardi, Ibn ‘Arabi
more fully elaborated the implications of the in-between
realm of the mundus imaginalis, which is at once spiritual,
intelligible, and unseen and corporeal, sensible, and visible.
This median dimension, he proclaimed, is the place where
spiritual beings are corporealized and corporeal beings are
spiritualized. In the hierarchy of being, al-’Arabi considered
the imaginal realm to be more real than the physical realm,
but less real than spiritual realm, which possessed the
highest ontological status. He insisted that the existence of
this median and mediating realm is the only way to account
for prophetic revelations, theophanic visions, or the existence
angelic and demonic beings—all of which are nonphysical
and therefore dismissed by philosophers, while nevertheless
being sensory experiences (i.e., disclosed to the subtle
imaginal senses).
Ibn al- ‘Arabi differentiated the ontological dimensions of
imaginal perception and rational cognition. Whereas rational
ideas are thought about in the form of abstract concepts,
images rather are sensually experienced through the
imagination, so that one tastes, smells, hears, sees, and
touches the inner world. Whereas the thinker reflects on
abstract ideas disengaged from sensual attributes, the poet
instead experiences various “loci of vision” (p. 103), which are
inwardly perceived and sensually disclosed. Poetry, properly
conceived, Chittick (1989) insists, is not thought about; rather
it is “something which is seen with the inward eye, and heard
with the inward ear and only then described” (p. 103). In al-
‘Arabi’s, hierarchy of existences, God represents the ultimate
metaphysical reality, which humans can never fully know
because God in his absolute essence is utterly invisible and
indefinable. However, in a way similar to the relationship
between Jung’s archetypes and archetypal images, Chittick
(1989) asserts, humans can apprehend manifestations of
God’s through God’s own self-revealing, which “takes place
within a form, which is the locus . . . in which spiritual vision
occurs.” These “loci of vision” (i.e., manifestations of God’s
self-disclosure) are variously designated as “‘imaginal form’ . .
. ‘locus of witnessing’. . . [or] ‘locus of manifestation’” (p. 102).
Although Ibn al-Arabi outlined imagination in terms of four
interrelated cosmological modes: (1) as the entire universe; (2)
the realm situated between bodies and spirits; (3) the mortal
soul; or (4) the soul’s native function (Chittick, 1989)—all of
which must be understood in relationship to the sacred reality
from which they issue—of particular relevance to my own
study are al-’Arabi’s last three forms (i.e., imagination as the
realm between body and spirit, the human soul itself which is
an intermediary between luminous divine spirit and dense
bodily physicality, and imagination as a faculty of the soul).
According to Chittick (1989), because imagination is
positioned between the spiritual and physical worlds,
imagination is thought to bring spiritual beings into
relationships with corporeal creatures. Thus envisioned,
imagination makes possible human apprehension of real
spiritual entities who represent a higher order of existence.
And this metaphysical interpretation is one way of
understanding the nature of imaginal persons such as Jung’s
psychopomp Philemon, or the various imaginal wisdom
figures experienced by subjects through RGI. In addition,
Chittick (1989) avers, the soul was created by God to serve as
an “interworld between spirit and body.” Situated in this
middle ground, the soul is “both one and many, luminous and
dark, subtle and dense, high and low, visible and invisible,”
which means residing between duality, “the soul [itself] . . . is
built of imagination” (p. 106). Meanwhile, as a faculty of the
soul, imagination “spiritualizes the corporeal things perceived
by the senses and stores them in memory . . . [and] it
‘corporalizes’ the spiritual things perceived in the heart by
giving them shape and form” (p. 107). Corbin (1977/1989)
described the function of the mundus imaginalis in similar
terms which is to create symbols from sensible and
intellectual forms (p. ix).
One could argue that in its “median and mediating
situation between the intellectual and sensible worlds”
(Corbin, 1977/1989, p. ix), the mundus imaginalis is like Jung’s
(1953/1969) psychoid archetype postulate, which may be
thought of as an interpenetrating, liminal space and point of
convergence between matter and mind. One could further
argue that Corbin’s account on the imaginal realm, along with
Jung’s idea there exists a third median area where, “psyche
and matter are two different aspects of one and the same
thing” (p. 215 [CW 8, para. 418]), provide guided imagery with
both an imaginal locus and an explanatory principle for its
mechanism of healing in mind/body medicine.
It is in the imaginal world that all theophanic visions and
alchemical transformations occur; where “the events of
sacred history are perceived by means of [imagination] that
open onto this world and its myriad beings of light”
(Cheetham, 2005, p. 65).
By reinforcing an imaginal ontology and providing a
phenomenology of the subtle realm, Corbin’s (1977/1989)
work arguably adds weight and scholarly dimension to
guided imagery’s interior explorations and its foundational
safe space technique, which may be understood as a vehicle
providing access to the intermediary world of archetypal
imagination the Sufis call “Malakût, the world of the Soul” (p.
ix).
In his book Avicenna and the Visionary Recital, Corbin
(1960) describes the archetypal transformation of the Persian
mystics Avicenna and Suhrawardi as revealed in their
dramaturgical récits—each verse of which represents their
ever-widening theophanic vision of God’s self-revealing in the
imaginal world. According to Mahmoud (2005), “the visionary
recital is the visionary’s account of his soul’s voyage into the
mundus imaginalis under the guidance of [his] Angel-guide”
(p. 1). Corbin (1960) writes that the personal figure of the
angel, “symbolize[ing] the soul’s most intimate depths,” is in
fact, the soul’s counterpart, with whom “it forms a totality that
is dual in structure . . . and may be called ego and Self” (p.
20). According to Corbin, “the event [of their meeting] will
take place, in a mental vision [or] a waking dream” (p. 20).
The parallel to Jung’s psychology is clear: through the
process of individuation, the ego encounters the archetypes
mediated by images and is brought into subordinate
relationship to the Self, which Jung (1954/1969a) described as
the “Imago Dei” (p. 193 [CW 8, para. 390]), or the image of
God in man. Less obvious is the similarity the angelic
encounter shares with RGI inner guide technique, wherein
one enters an altered state of consciousness characterized
by a relaxed and inward focus and interacts with a loving and
wise inner being to gain insight and understanding. One
could argue that RGI is a core spiritual technology that
facilitates direct experiences of the numinosum, which Jung
(1940/1969) described as a “dynamic agency . . . independent
of . . . will . . . that causes a peculiar alteration of
consciousness” (p. 7 [CW 11, para. 6]) Jung understood all
spiritual experience as an expression of the Self archetype.
Though the Self archetype can neither be directly known nor
fully understood, one can nevertheless discern whether or
not one is in contact with the archetypal unconscious, or
transpersonal Self by a specific quality of experience Rudolf
Otto (1917/1950) described as numinous, meaning:
A state of mind which is . . . perfectly sui generis and
irreducible to any other [mental state] . . . Like every
absolutely primary and elementary datum [that] . . .
cannot be strictly defined . . . it can only be evoked [or]
awakened in the mind; as everything that comes ‘of
the spirit’ must be awakened. (p. 7)
Otto (1917/1950) characterized the numinous as a
“mysterium tremendum et facinans” (p. 12), which means a
mystery that is both tremendous and fascinating. And
because numinous experience has been linked to
metaphysical and religious ideas from ancient times, Jung
(1973a) argued in all attempts to understand it, “use must be
made of certain parallel religious or metaphysical ideas . . . to
formulate and elucidate it” (p. 547). RGI offers a way of living
a spiritually rich and meaningful life without embracing any
particular creed or dogma, and it offers an experiential form
of spirituality that refuses to dictate what form the divine
should take. Given RGI’s numinous potentials, Students’
accounts of their guided imagery experiences with safe
space and inner guide imagery may share some features with
Persian mystical récits that documented their personal
transformations. This is not to say every RGI experience
results in a direct experience of the divine, or that the
imaginal experiences of disciplined Sufi masters are not of an
entirely different order, than students’ preliminary
explorations of the imaginal realm. It only suggests RGI holds
the potential of providing such experiences. An analogy may
prove helpful here: On a grand piano, one can play “chop-
sticks,” or through practice, discipline, and passionate
devotion develop enough skill to play Chopin’s Etudes, or
even compose a sonata of one’s own.
JAMES HILLMAN
James Hillman (1975) was a reformer and revolutionary
whose psychological revisioning sought to return soul to
psychology by seeing through its enshrined dogmas and
doctrinal literalism. Noting that Enlightenment positivism and
secularism had banished “soul” from psychological discourse,
Hillman advanced a “poetic basis of mind,” and argued for a
“psychology of soul based on a psychology of image” (p. xvii)
grounded not in brain physiology, linguistic structures, social
organization, or behaviorism, but the very process of
imagining itself (p. xvii). Hillman’s major works include The
Myth of Analysis (1960), Suicide and the Soul (1965/2011),
Revisioning Psychology (1975), and Archetypal Psychology: A
Brief Account (1983). And in each of these books, he
sustained his career-spanning deconstructive critique of
psychological literalism, his emphasis on myth, imagination,
soul, and soul-making.
Soul—Hillman’s (1965/2011) root metaphor for depth
psychology (p. 47)—is a perspective whose primary activity is
imagining. Comprised of its own autochthonic images,
Hillman believed that soul is revealed, understood, and
created by venturing into the depths the poet John Keats
(1819) called, “the vale of soul-making” (para. 4). By adding
depth and turning events into experience, Hillman (1975)
contends that it is soul that makes all meaning possible (p.
xvi), and it is the image we experience directly that itself
confers meaning. Hillman claimed:
Images [are] the basic givens of psychic life, self-
originating, inventive, spontaneous, complete and
organized in archetypal patterns…[they] are the raw
materials and finished products of psyche, and they
are the privileged mode of access to knowledge of
soul. Nothing is more primary. (p. xvii)
According to Hillman (1975), fantasy images are the
ultimate facts of the psyche upon which consciousness
depends. Ideas, sensations, perception of the physical world,
emotions, beliefs, and desires must all first present
themselves to consciousness as images in order to be
experienced (p. 23). For Hillman, images are not symbols,
signs, allegories, or representations. Images are rather the
irreducible facts of psychic reality and are thus the sine qua
non of archetypal psychology. Disclosed to subtle inner
senses that apprehend the median dimension of psyche and
soul, Hillman, following Corbin believed images—unlike
concepts—must be intimately experienced, relationally
engaged, and felt rather than interpreted, explained or simply
thought about.
Although Hillman (1983) maintained that images constitute
the essential data of archetypal psychology (p. 14), it was
Jung (1929/1967) who first expressed, “Image is psyche” (p.
50 [CW 13, para. 75]) and explicitly underscored their
equivalency. Noting that “psyche consists essentially of
images” (Jung, 1926/1969, p. 325 [CW 8, para. 618]), and that
“every psychic process is an image and an imagining” (Jung,
1939/1969, p. 544 [CW 11, para. 889]), Jung effectively
“resuscitated images” (Avens, 1980, p. 89), which had been
deprecated and compressed into icons by the church fathers
at the Counsel of Nicaea, who treated images as re-
presentations or allegories instead of numinous presences of
the divine (p. 189). While Jung spoke of images and symbols
interchangeably, in actual practice, especially among Jung’s
orthodox heirs, symbols nevertheless came to be viewed as
“abstractions from images” (Hillman, 1977, p. 65), or
conceptual proxies. Straying from the image’s
phenomenological presentation (i.e., its particular mood, tone,
and context), the symbol instead came to suggest “a higher
order, a metaphysical archetype or noumena, outside or
beyond the presenting phenomena” (Butler, 2014, p. 39).
Instead of drawing one’s attention to the image’s concrete
particularity, the symbol served as a bridge or pointer to
something over and above the image itself. Thus, in a way
dissimilar to orthodox Jungians who moved away from the
image itself to a larger collective reality through symbolic
amplification to glean supplemental meaning from cross-
cultural and cross-temporal comparisons, Hillman’s (1975,
1977) imagistic approach more vehemently insisted on the
experience-near process of sticking with the
phenomenological presence of the image itself. Hillman’s
critical realignment demands that images not be reified or
reduced to formulaic tropes. Symbols, according to Hillman,
only “become images when they are particularized by a
specific context, mood, and scene” (p. 62). By the same
token, he averred, “symbols . . . only can appear in images
and as images” (p. 65).
The philosopher Edward Casey has specified, “The image
is not what you see but the way you see” (as cited in Hillman,
1979b, p. 176). It is a mode of perception, an “operation of
insight” (p. 176), or a subtle sensing…of relations among
events” (p. 176). Neither pictures, nor visualizations, images
are specific “mood[s], scene[s], and context[s]” (p. 175)
apprehended through a “sensing…intuition” (p. 177), which
may be understood as de-literalized, subtle (imaginal) senses.
Hillman’s (1975) soul-making procedural moves:
personifying—or imagining things, pathologizing—or falling
apart, psychologizing—or seeing through, and dehumanizing
—or soul-making all serve to free depth psychology from
literalism, absolutism, and staid orthodoxy. Hillman was
especially critical of the orthodoxy exhibited by Jung’s
classical heirs, which in his view theologized Jung’s
psychology. The lionization of Jung by his followers,
conversion of his theories to sanctioned doctrine, and
institute driven apostolic succession are all problematic,
Hillman (1971) argued, because they end up “producing
dogmas, propitiatory rites, priesthoods and worship” (p. 197).
Hillman found Jung’s monotheistic temperament and
seemingly unconscious enactment of the Christian mythos in
his psychology especially disconcerting, and he attacked one
idol of worship in particular—Jung’s monotheistic notion of
the Self, which prized “Unity, integration and individuation,”
over and against psychic “multiplicity and diversity” (p. 194).
Whereas Jung favored “the self of psychological wholeness
[which] more clearly reflects the God of monotheism and the
Senex archetype,” Hillman instead championed psychological
polytheism in which “the soul [may] serve in its time many
Gods” (p. 201). According to Hillman (1983), “the soul is
constituted of images…[and] is primarily an imagining activity”
(p. 14). It cannot, therefore, be limited to an ideology of
psychic monotheism, which views the many persons of the
psyche as pathological expressions in need of either cure or
excision. By insisting that the soul is polytheistic and above all
imaginative, Hillman recognized that all perspectives,
including medical and scientific dogmas, are products of
imagination. And he argued that myth, rather than medicine
or science, provides a superior reflection of the polyvalent
soul and its poesies.
Hillman (1983) described Jung and Corbin as the first and
second “immediate father[s] of archetypal psychology” (p. 10),
respectively. He credited Jung with providing him with the
idea that “the fundamental structure of the psyche, its formal
patterns and relational configurations, are archetypal . . .
[which] like psychic organs . . . are given with the psyche
itself” (p. 10). He credited Corbin for recognizing that
archetypes present themselves as images, whose essential
nature is first discernible by means of imagination. For
Hillman, Corbin’s ontological placement of the archetypes in
the imagination means “the entire procedure of archetypal
psychology as a method is imaginative” (p. 12). By situating
Jung’s work in the venerable tradition of imaginative
psychology and embracing him as archetypal psychology’s
first father (Hillman, 1983, p. 10), Hillman refined and
deepened Jung’s thought (Avens, 1980), and he affirmed
imaginal knowing and soul as the “primary and…ultimate
realities” (p. 187).
Hillman (1983) chose the term archetypal instead of Jung’s
term analytical to describe his own psychological re-visioning
because it de-emphasized the stultifying language of
scientific rationalism, and instead accentuated the humanities
(i.e., that discipline which addresses the question of what it
means to be human imaginatively through literature, arts,
language, and music) (p. 9). Diverging from Jung’s
conceptually abstract view of archetypes which inclines to
metaphysical and philosophical speculation, Hillman (1983)
argues archetypes are images that disclose meaning, depth,
and value phenomenologically and his psychology sticks with
the image by staying firmly rooted in the imagination (p 18).
Whereas Jung created active imagination to engage the
persons of the psyche as images, Hillman’s (1977) approach
emphasized poesies, the angelic function of words as
transmitters of images, and imaginative metaphoric play,
which he described in his essay “An Inquiry into Image,” as
“talking with the image and letting it talk” (p. 81). Working with
dream images using rhetorical iteratio and metaphorical
analogy, Hillman averred, allowed the image itself to speak,
and in speaking reveal its “hidden connections” (p. 83).
Whereas Jung’s main concern was relating to the image,
Hillman’s emphasis was protecting the polysemous potential
of the image by not interpreting it or drawing any singular
conclusion from it. A case can be made that Hillman’s (1977)
method contributes a form of imaginative amplification that
complements active imagination through “iteratio of the
prima materia” (p. 74). Taken together, Hillman and Jung’s
methods inform an approach to image work that honors the
delicate ecology between worlds. This approach applies to
images spontaneously arising from the unconscious or
recalled images from dreams, and it allows the image as
subject situated in the imaginal world to weave its poetry. The
method in question, arguably originated by Jung and
adopted by RGI, entails communicating with the image as a
sentient other. This means, as Hillman described, talking with
and listening to the image, deepening affective engagement
with it, and fully honoring its person by directly asking the
image (1) What it wants, (2) what it needs, (3) and what it has
to offer. Identifying (4) how one feels talking with the image,
(5) expressing one’s feelings to the image, (6) allowing the
image to respond, (7) engaging in open dialogue with the
image, (8) asking permission to touch the image, and finally,
(9) becoming the image itself.
What Corbin called the mundus imaginalis, Hillman
(1965/2011) alternatively called soul. Though he remarked that
soul is a purposely vague concept that defies definition (p.
46), he formulated the following description nevertheless: “By
soul, I mean the imaginative possibility in our natures, the
experiencing through reflective speculation, dream, image
and fantasy—that mode which recognizes all realities as
primarily symbolic or metaphorical” (Hillman, 1975, p. xvi). By
arguing that “human existence even at its basic vital level is a
metaphor” (Hillman, 1972/2007, p. 30), Hillman asserted the
primacy of images and the imagination for psychology—the
logos of the soul—which for him is a psychic perspective
which precedes all other domains of knowledge. According
to Hillman (1975):
To live psychologically is to imagine things. . . . To be in
soul is to experience the fantasy in all realities and the
basic reality of fantasy . . . man is primarily an image
maker and our psychic substance consists of images;
our experience is imagination. We are indeed such
stuff as dreams are made on. (p. 23)
Although Jung (2012b) wrote that “the [psyche] is no
longer a content in us . . . we [are] contents of it” (p. 57), one
could argue that he still sought acceptance from the wider
scientific community, and because of his lingering allegiance
to scientific rationalism, his work often straddled an
uncomfortable borderland between science and spirituality
(Cheetham, 2012). In contradistinction, Hillman’s re-visioning
liberates the soul from the vestigial limitations of the natural
sciences, which reductively understand the psyche as a
function of brain encased in human skulls.
According to archetypal psychology, image is the primary
irreducible datum. “In the beginning is image; first imagination
then perception; first fantasy then reality” (Hillman, 1975, p.
23). Fantasy images are disclosed immediately, viscerally, and
directly, and from them people fashion their basic sense of
reality. Consciousness itself, Hillman argues, is mediated by
images. Everything else—ideas of the intellect, physical
sensations, and perceptual experiences of the world “must
present themselves [first] as images in order to become
experienced” (p.23).
Archetypal psychology’s imaginal ontology and its
epistemology based on images, integrating the work of Jung
and Corbin, provides the imaginal lens through which I will
analyze the imagery experiences of research participants in
the third and final volume of this series.