Applied_Metacognition
Applied_Metacognition
Applied_Metacognition
net/publication/238730504
Applied Metacognition
CITATIONS READS
169 1,929
2 authors, including:
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Timothy John Hollins on 17 October 2014.
Edited by
Timothy J. Perfect
Bennett L. Schwartz
The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, CB2 1RP, United Kingdom
C Cambridge University Press 2002
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library
vii
viii Contents
Conclusions
11. Metacognition research: an interim report 261
Index 287
1 Introduction: toward an applied
metacognition
1
2 Bennett L. Schwartz and Timothy J. Perfect
latency, allocation of study time, and decisions about which items to study
(e.g. Son and Metcalfe, 2000; Thiede and Dunlosky, 1999).
The idea of control processes is crucial to the development of applied
metacognition. If control processes exist and influence human behavior
and cognition, it may be possible to improve or alter control processes in
ways which will improve human learning. Consider the work of Thiede
and Dunlosky, 1999 (see also Son and Schwartz, this volume). Thiede
and Dunlosky asked students to make judgments of learning ( JOLs) on
paired-associate word pairs, that were either concrete nouns (e.g. dog-
spoon) or abstract words (e.g. democracy–gravity). Later, they were given
the option to chose items for re-study. When time pressure to learn was
high, the students chose those that had been given high JOLs. When
time pressure was low, the students chose the harder items to study, that
is, those given low JOLs. Thus, depending on the context, the students
made different control decisions. Although conducted in a lab under
controlled settings, the applied value of this study is obvious: perhaps we
can teach students to use adaptive and flexible control strategies. Thus,
the introduction of the concept of control radically altered the kinds of
questions that could be asked about how to apply metacognitive findings.
Domains of application
Our goal in this volume is to establish the importance of metacognition
to a variety of applied concerns, and specifically, to demonstrate how
metacognition can inform these areas. It is our belief that metacognition
has made a good start at being applied, as a number of researchers in
the field are actively engaged in applying their results. In addition, many
metacognition researchers ask questions that are applied in nature to
begin with.
We consider that the following chapters will make important contri-
butions to two applied domains, namely (a) education, specifically appli-
cations toward improving learning and training; and (b) legal contexts,
specifically in the self-evaluation of eyewitness reports. The chapters here
also touch on applications to other domains including counseling (self-
evaluation of coping strategies, beliefs in recovered memories), human
factors (self-monitoring of job performance), and recovery from brain
injuries.
The current volume contains at least one chapter on each of these
topics. Educational applications predominate, as most applied metacog-
nition has had some form of learning as its focus. Maki and McGuire
(this volume) discuss the growing literature on the monitoring of compre-
hension. Many studies find that students, even capable college students,
Toward an applied metacognition 7
represented here, Bjork and his colleagues have been applying metacog-
nition research to a variety of training situations from the military to
sports teams (see Bjork, 1994; 1999). Nelson and his colleagues have
been applying metacognitive research to the learning of foreign language
vocabulary (Nelson et al., 1994). Thus, we anticipate a bright future for
metacognition research.
Conclusions
Hermann (1998) distinguished between four types of research method-
ologies, three of which were related to applied research. The first group
are theory-based researchers, for whom application is not initially rel-
evant. Rather, their goal is to understand basic processes. The second
group of researchers are basic researchers who stress ecological validity,
also known as the “everyday memory” movement, in memory research.
These researchers are fundamentally interested in basic questions of
theory, but wish to address them in a way that has direct validity to
everyday life. The important point about “ecologically valid” research
is that, although it tells us something about ordinary life, it may not be
directly applicable.
The third type of research is labeled “applicable research” by Hermann
(1998). Applicable research implies ecologically valid research, but re-
search for which applications follow naturally. A typical example is that
Loftus’ misinformation-effect studies clearly suggest that investigators
should avoid leading and misleading questions when questioning a wit-
ness or suspect. Finally, Hermann’s fourth type of research is “applica-
tion research.” This involves research on specific instruments, products,
or services which have been designed to work in specific domains. Thus,
application research might involve experimenting on whether a specific
form of therapy helps amnesic patients remember more of what they have
been taught.
The chapters in this book are mostly of the “applicable research”
variety. The researchers have not yet designed specific interventions to im-
prove learning, to ameliorate Alzheimer’s, or to assist judges and juries.
However, because the research is ecologically valid and application is
kept in mind, the potential application of much research on metacog-
nition falls naturally out of the studies used to explore it. Thus, the
schoolteacher looking for specific methods to improve metacognitive
understanding may find these chapters premature, but the researcher
looking to design specific application tools should find these chapters
invaluable.
10 Bennett L. Schwartz and Timothy J. Perfect
Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the training
of human beings. In J. Metcalfe and A. P. Shimamura (eds.), Metacognition:
knowing about knowing, pp. 185–205. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
(1999). Assessing our own competence: heuristics and illusions. In D. Gopher
and A. Koriat (eds.), Attention and performance XVII. Cognitive regulation of
performance: interaction of theory and application. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bothwell, R. K., Deffenbacher, K. A., and Brigham, J. C. (1987). Correlation of
eyewitness accuracy and confidence: optimality hypothesis revisited. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 72, 691–695.
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognitive and cognitive monitoring: a new area of
cognitive developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.
Gruneberg, M. M., and Monks, J. (1974). “Feeling of knowing” and cued recall.
Acta Psychologica, 38, 257–265.
Hacker, D. J. (1998). Definitions and empirical foundations. In D. J. Hacker,
J. Dunlosky, and A. C. Graesser (eds.), Metacognition in educational theory
and practice, pp. 1–24. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hacker, D. J., Dunlosky, J., and Graesser, A. C. (1998). Metacognition in educa-
tional theory and practice. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hart, J. T. (1965). Memory and the feeling-of-knowing experience. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 56, 208–216.
(1967). Memory and the memory-monitoring process. Journal of Verbal Learn-
ing and Verbal Behavior, 6, 685–691.
Herrmann, D. J. (1998). The relationship between basic research and applied
research in memory and cognition. In C. P. Thompson, D. J. Hermann,
D. Bruce, J. D. Read, D. G. Payne, and M. P. Toglia (eds.), Autobiographical
memory: theoretical and applied perspectives, pp. 13–27. New Jersey: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Kuhn, D. (2000). Metacognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 9, 178–181.
Metcalfe, J. (1993). Novelty monitoring, metacognition, and control in a com-
posite holographic associative recall model: interpretations for Korsakoff
amnesia. Psychological Review, 100, 3–22.
Neisser, U. (1978). Memory: what are the important questions? In M. M.
Gruneberg, P. Morris, and R. L. Sykes (eds.), Practical aspects of memory,
pp. 3–24. London: Academic Press.
Nelson, T. O. (1988). Predictive accuracy of feeling of knowing across different
criterion tasks and across different subject populations and individuals. In
M. Gruneberg, P. Morris, and R. Sykes (eds.), Practical aspects of memory:
current research and issues, Volume 1, pp. 190–196. New York: Wiley.
(1993). Judgments of learning and the allocation of study time. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 269–273.
(1996). Consciousness and metacognition. American Psychologist, 51, 102–
116.
Nelson, T. O., Dunlosky, J., Graf, A., and Narens, L. (1994). Utilization of
metacognitive judgments in the allocation of study during multitrial learning.
Psychological Science, 5, 207–213.
Toward an applied metacognition 11