BrewerGrafandWillnatPrimingorFraming2
BrewerGrafandWillnatPrimingorFraming2
BrewerGrafandWillnatPrimingorFraming2
net/publication/245518065
CITATIONS READS
196 6,108
3 authors, including:
All content following this page was uploaded by Joseph Graf on 11 July 2017.
PRIMING OR FRAMING
Media Influence on Attitudes toward Foreign Countries
Abstract / This study examines two routes for media effects on the standards by which people
evaluate foreign countries. The first is indirect: a news story about an issue in a domestic context
may heighten the cognitive accessibility of thoughts about the issue, thereby priming audience
members to base their evaluations of foreign nations on those thoughts. The second is direct: a news
story that presents a frame linking an issue to a foreign nation in a way that suggests a particular
evaluative implication may shape how audience members judge that nation. An experiment revolv-
ing around media coverage of two issues and attitudes toward four nations found evidence for media
influence along the second route but not the first.
Keywords / attitudes toward foreign countries / cognitive accessibility / media effects / news frames
/ priming
Most people are heavily dependent on the mass media for information about
international affairs. As a result, the media can play an important role in
shaping mass perceptions of other nations. Studies have found that exposure to
news coverage increases knowledge about and can significantly influence public
opinion toward foreign nations (Albritton and Manheim, 1983, 1985; Manheim
and Albritton, 1984; Perry, 1985, 1987). Such perceptions, in turn, have
important implications in a number of areas, ranging from the nature of
personal interactions among people of differing nations to mass attitudes about
foreign policy to the practice of public diplomacy (Bartels, 1995; Manheim,
1991, 1994; Peffley and Hurwitz, 1992). Thus, it is not surprising that actors
in the international arena often undertake considerable efforts in order to mold
the content of media coverage.
But how, exactly, do the media influence attitudes toward other nations, and
under what specific circumstances will they do so? To date, there has been rela-
tively little research into the mechanisms that govern media influence on these
attitudes. The present study takes a step to fill this gap by testing whether news
stories help determine which standards of judgment citizens use to form atti-
tudes about foreign nations. Specifically, it tests for evidence of two different
sorts of media effects. First, can the media prime standards for judging foreign
nations by highlighting issues on the domestic agenda? Second, can the media
alter the judgment process by providing news frames that directly link issues to
specific foreign nations? The evidence for answering these questions comes from
494 GAZETTE VOL. 65 NO. 6
With all of this in mind, we have two goals. One is to test whether spread-
ing activation can produce priming effects along indirect routes, as the simple
accessibility-based model of priming suggests. The other is to test whether the
media will influence how citizens form judgments about other nations when –
and perhaps only when – they provide news frames that explain why one should
evaluate a specific country in terms of a particular issue and how one should
connect them.
drugs – even news within a domestic context – could influence how accessible
thoughts about these issues are when these audience members evaluate foreign
nations. Thus, exposure to media coverage of domestic terrorism and domestic
illegal drug use may prime audience members to use their attitudes about terror-
ism and drugs to form judgments of the four target countries. Accordingly, we
suggest the following priming hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1: Compared to people not exposed to any news about terrorism, people primed
with news stories about domestic terrorism should be more likely to judge Iran and Libya
based on associations with anti-terrorism efforts.
Hypothesis 2: Compared to people not exposed to any news about illegal drugs, people primed
with news stories about domestic illegal drug use should be more likely to judge Colombia and
Mexico based on associations with the war on drugs.
In the case of Hypothesis 1, participants who read news stories about terror-
ism in the US should be more likely to evaluate Libya and Iran based on their
attitudes toward terrorism than participants who did not read any stories about
terrorism. Given the nature of prior media coverage, this would be reflected in
the activation of associations between terrorism and the target nations (with
citizens seeing these countries either as supporters of state terrorism or targets
of anti-terrorism efforts). In the case of Hypothesis 2, stories about illegal drugs
in the US might activate associations between the war on drugs and the target
nations (with citizens seeing these countries either as drug traffickers or allies
in the war on drugs).
A second set of hypotheses revolves around whether exposure to stories that
directly link the issues to the target nations will influence the judgment process.
More specifically, they center on the effects of media coverage framing Iran and
Libya as sponsors of terrorism or Colombia and Mexico as allies in the war on
drugs. Note that these frames not only suggest which issue audience members
should consider in forming their judgments of the target nations; they also
suggest particular evaluative implications. Consequently, we state the following
framing hypotheses:
Hypothesis 3: Compared to people not exposed to any news about terrorism, people exposed
to news stories framing Iran and Libya as sponsors of terrorism should be more likely to judge
these nations based on negative associations with anti-terrorist efforts.
Hypothesis 4: Compared to people not exposed to any news about illegal drugs, people
exposed to news stories framing Colombia and Mexico as allies in the war on drugs should
be more likely to judge these nations based on positive associations with the war on drugs.
and transforming the resulting index so that 1 indicated the maximum possible
support for the war on drugs and 0 indicated the minimum. The reliability of
the scale was .81 (Cronbach’s alpha); its mean was .51.
In addition, the pretest measured students’ gender and foreign affairs
knowledge. The inclusion of controls for these variables did not influence the
results of the analyses and they are therefore not reported.
Participants were randomly assigned to a control condition or one of four
experimental conditions. The treatments were based on actual newspaper
articles, though these were rewritten to be between 400 and 500 words long.
Care was taken in making the articles appear as real as possible, and they were
presented with New York Times bylines. The participants were told that the
project dealt with student opinions toward the news; to further disguise the
purpose of the study, the post-test began with a set of unrelated questions about
how interesting, credible and objective the articles were.
Some participants were randomly assigned to read two articles about
terrorism; others read two articles about the war on drugs. More specifically,
some participants read stories that explicitly framed Libya and Iran as sponsors
of terrorism (framing group 1) or Mexico and Colombia as US allies in the war
on drugs (framing group 2), whereas other participants read stories about
domestic terrorism (priming group 1) or the domestic war on drugs (priming
group 2). Participants in the control condition read stories about computer use
and the Euro, topics presumably unrelated in citizens’ minds to either the issues
at hand or the target nations.
Findings
To reiterate, this study tested for two types of media effects on how participants
evaluated the target nations: effects produced by stories that focused on issues
within a domestic context without mentioning a foreign nation (media priming)
and effects produced by stories that linked the foreign nations to issues through
news frames (media framing). In the first analysis, which examined the impact
of the terrorism stories on post-test evaluations of Libya and Iran, the partici-
pants who read articles about drug use or the control articles were grouped into
the control condition. Table 1 reports the means and standard deviations for
the pretest and post-test measures of attitudes toward Iran and Libya by con-
dition. As the table shows, these attitudes tended toward negativity across the
board, although the post-test attitudes of participants who read the stories that
directly linked Libya and Iran to terrorism were particularly negative.
According to Hypothesis 1, participants who read news stories about
domestic terrorism should have been more likely to evaluate Libya and Iran
based on their attitudes toward terrorism than participants who did not read
any stories about terrorism. This should have led to interactions between
exposure to the domestic terrorism treatment and support for a war on terror-
ism (presumably negative interactions, given the likely nature of any previously
learned associations). Hypothesis 3 points to another expectation: compared to
participants in the control condition, participants who read the stories framing
Libya and Iran as sponsors of terrorism should have been more likely to draw
BREWER ET AL.: PRIMING OR FRAMING 501
TABLE 1
Pretest and Post-test Attitudes toward Libya and Iran, by Condition
Libya Iran
———————— ——————————
Condition Pre Post Pre Post
Note: Table entries are means within conditions. Standard deviations are in parentheses.
negative associations between anti-terrorist efforts and these nations. Thus, one
would expect negative interactions between exposure to this treatment and
support for a war on terrorism.
The following ordinary least square regression model provided a test of both
hypotheses:
TABLE 2
The Impact of Terrorism Stories and Attitudes about Terrorism on Attitudes toward
Libya and Iran
R2 .52 .46
N 193 194
Notes: Table entries are unstandardized regression coefficients. Standard errors are in paren-
theses. Significance tests are based on two-tailed tests except where noted in the text.
** p < .01, * p < .05.
participants who read these stories did not differ discernibly from control
participants in the way that they used their attitudes about terrorism to form
evaluations of the nations.
On the other hand, the results produced partial support for Hypothesis 3.
Exposure to news stories that explicitly portrayed Libya and Iran as sponsors
of terrorism significantly altered the impact of anti-terrorist attitudes on partici-
pants’ evaluations of Iran (p < . 05, one-tailed test). Moreover, the interaction
had the anticipated negative sign. In this instance, participants did seem to
adopt the frame of reference provided by the media in evaluating Iran. The
same did not hold true in the case of Libya: here, the effect of support for a war
on terrorism did not significantly differ as a result of exposure to the stories
linking Libya and Iran to terrorism. In sum, for Libya and Iran there was little
evidence of priming along the indirect route and mixed evidence of influence
produced by news frames for the nations at hand.
The picture became clearer in the second analysis, which examined the
effects of the stories about illegal drugs on post-test attitudes toward Mexico
and Colombia. In this analysis, the participants who read articles about terror-
ism or the control articles were grouped into the control condition. Table 3
reports the means and standard deviations for the pretest and post-test
measures of attitudes toward Mexico and Colombia by condition. Participants
in all three conditions tended to rate Mexico rather favorably and Colombia
slightly unfavorably both before and after the experimental procedure.
By the logic of Hypothesis 2, participants who read news stories about the
domestic drug war should have been more likely to base their post-test evalu-
ations of Mexico and Colombia on their attitudes about the war on drugs than
BREWER ET AL.: PRIMING OR FRAMING 503
TABLE 3
Pretest and Post-test Attitudes toward Mexico and Colombia, by Condition
Mexico Colombia
———————— ——————————
Condition Pre Post Pre Post
Note: Table entries are means within conditions. Standard deviations are in parentheses.
participants who did not read any stories about drugs. This could have
produced interactions between exposure to the domestic treatment and support
for a war on drugs. Hypothesis 4 leads to another expectation: compared to
participants in the control condition, participants who read the stories framing
Mexico and Colombia as allies in the war on drugs should have been more likely
to draw positive associations between this war and the two nations. Here, then,
one would specifically expect positive interactions between exposure and
support for a war on drugs.
A second regression model tested Hypotheses 2 and 4. Again, dichotomous
variables captured the effects of the treatments. The index for support for the
war on drugs was also included in the model, as were its interactions with the
two treatments. The pretest measure of the dependent variable completed the
model:
TABLE 4
The Impact of Drug Stories and Attitudes about Drugs on Attitudes toward Mexico
and Colombia
R2 .39 .30
N 194 195
Notes: Table entries are unstandardized regression coefficients. Standard errors are in paren-
theses. Significance tests are based on two-tailed tests except where noted in the text.
** p < .01, * p < .05.
Conclusion
The findings presented here show that the media can influence the standards
by which people evaluate foreign nations. At the same time, the data also
suggest that there are limits to this influence. When members of our audience
read stories that offered a direct link between an issue and a nation that carried
a specific evaluative implication, they tended to adopt this frame of reference
in their own thinking. In three out of four cases, frames that provided this sort
of link shaped how people formed judgments. On the other hand, none of our
four tests produced evidence that priming effects work along an indirect route.
When participants read about issues on the domestic front, they did not carry
their thoughts over to the international realm. Influence along one route suc-
ceeded, whereas influence along the other route failed.
Some caution is warranted in drawing these conclusions. The absence of
priming effects along the indirect path might be due in part to the statistical
power afforded by the sample size or the imperfections of the measures used to
BREWER ET AL.: PRIMING OR FRAMING 505
capture attitudes toward the target nations and attitudes about the issues at
hand. The participants in the experiment also read the treatments in the context
of an experiment; hence they may have behaved differently than they would
have in a more natural setting. Furthermore, participants in this experiment
were undergraduate students and not necessarily typical of American citizens,
let alone citizens of other nations. Still, the methods that failed to capture
priming effects along the indirect route succeeded in capturing influence
produced by more explicit story lines. Care was taken to craft realistic treat-
ments and to disguise the purpose of this study. And while students may differ
in how they think about foreign nations compared to the general public, this
study focuses on the psychological processes by which people form these evalu-
ations – which both students and the general public should share, regardless of
nationality.
What accounts, then, for the failure of priming along the indirect path? One
partial explanation could lie in the power of stories to activate conflicting associ-
ations between issues and nations. It may be that audience members drew both
positive and negative associations between the war on drugs and the two target
nations for this issue. Such ambiguity could have interfered with the emergence
of a clear, consistent priming effect for the domestic drug use stories. On the
other hand, it seems less likely that the domestic terrorism stories would prime
both positive and negative associations between terrorism and the target nations
for that issue. Thus, this explanation, by itself, is probably insufficient.
A second explanation fits both sets of cases. It may be that audience
members simply did not associate terrorism with Iran and Libya or illegal drugs
with Mexico and Colombia prior to reading the stories about domestic terror-
ism and drug use. Thus, they may not have seen their thoughts about these
issues as applicable to the target nations. This explanation would fit well with
recent accounts (Miller and Krosnick, 1996; Price and Tewksbury, 1997) that
emphasize the importance of applicability or relevance in mediating priming
effects. It would also suggest that priming along the indirect route is generally
unlikely to emerge in the realm of judging foreign nations. The examples in this
study were deliberately chosen to reflect linkages between issues and nations
drawn within a real-world mass media discourse. If even these associations were
too weak within the minds of citizens to allow for spreading activation, then it
seems doubtful that priming effects produced by stories about domestic affairs
would exert a pervasive influence on mass judgments of foreign nations.
An alternative (or additional) explanation is that the study produced no
evidence of spreading activation because this is not the mechanism that
produces priming effects in the first place. The failure to find priming along the
indirect route is also consistent with the argument made by Miller and Krosnick
(2000) that priming effects result from learning about what trusted media
sources see as important, rather than from the automatic machinery of cogni-
tive accessibility. Under this explanation, one would not necessarily expect the
media to influence how people think about foreign countries unless they provide
stories that explain why one should associate particular issues with particular
nations.
When the media provide such stories, audience members do respond to
506 GAZETTE VOL. 65 NO. 6
them. Reading stories that linked Iran to terrorism led participants to judge Iran
in terms of their attitudes about fighting terrorism; similarly, reading stories that
linked Mexico and Colombia to the war on drugs led participants to judge both
countries in terms of their attitudes about the war on drugs. Moreover, the
stories led participants to use their thoughts about the issues in a particular way.
In the case of Iran, the stories led them to see support for a war on terrorism as
incompatible with favorable attitudes toward the nation. Meanwhile, in the
cases of Mexico and Colombia, the stories led participants to see support for a
war on drugs as a reason for favoring the nations. Thus, a moral of this study
is that one needs to provide a clear story – a frame that explains what issue is
at stake and on which side of the issue the foreign nation stands – in order to
shape how a mass audience judges another country.
That lesson has important implications. One is that actors in international
affairs may not need to worry too much about the impact of domestic news on
how mass publics view other nations. The evidence presented in this study
suggests that media audiences do not necessarily connect domestic issues such
as illegal drugs to foreign nations unless the media explicitly draw that con-
nection. Thus, nations that fear the indirect consequences of ‘guilt by associ-
ation’ should rest more easily.
However, it may be in the interests of some nations to attend to – and
perhaps attempt to shape – the specific frames that the news media provide for
them. As our study has shown, news frames are readily adopted by media audi-
ences and can influence their views of foreign nations. While this can have
undesirable consequences for foreign nations, it also suggests that certain story
lines that tie popular (or unpopular) stands to a nation could be a useful tool
in public diplomacy (see Manheim, 1991, 1994).
Note
1. The content analysis of the New York Times was conducted using keyword searches of the
Lexis/Nexis database and included both full-length stories and news briefs. The search terms
used were ‘drugs’ with Colombia and Mexico, and ‘terrorism’ with Libya and Iran. A handful
of stories involving the taking of performance-enhancing drugs by athletes were ignored for
purposes of this analysis.
References
Albritton, R.B. and J.B. Manheim (1983) ‘News of Rhodesia: The Impact of a Public Relations
Campaign’, Journalism Quarterly 60: 622–8.
Albritton, R.B. and J.B. Manheim (1985) ‘Public Relations Efforts for the Third World: Images in
the News’, Journal of Communication 35: 43–59.
Anderson, J.R. and G.H. Bower (1973) Human Associative Memory. Washington, DC: Winston.
Bartels, L. (1995) ‘The American Public’s Defense Spending Preferences in the Post-Cold War Era’,
Public Opinion Quarterly 58: 479–508.
Cappella, J.A. and K.H. Jamieson (1997) Spiral of Cynicism. New York: Oxford University Press.
Collins, A.M. and E.F. Loftus (1975) ‘A Spreading Activation Theory of Semantic Processing’,
Psychological Review 82: 407–28.
Domke, D., D.V. Shah and D.B. Wackman (1998) ‘Media Priming Effects: Accessibility, Associ-
ation, and Activation’, International Journal of Public Opinion Research 10: 51–74.
Entman, R. (1993) ‘Framing: Toward Clarification of a Fractured Paradigm’, Journal of Com-
munication 43: 51–8.
BREWER ET AL.: PRIMING OR FRAMING 507
Fiske, S.T. and S.E. Taylor (1984) Social Cognition. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Gamson, W.A. (1992) Talking Politics. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Herr, P.M., S.J. Sherman and R.H. Fazio (1983) ‘On the Consequences of Priming: Assimilation
and Contrast Effects’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 19: 323–40.
Higgins, E.T. and G.A. King (1981) ‘Accessibility of Social Constructs: Information-Processing
Consequences of Individual and Contextual Variability’, in N. Cantor and J. F. Kihlstrom (eds)
Personality, Cognition, and Social Interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Higgins, E.T., W.S. Rhodes and C.R. Jones (1977) ‘Category Accessibility and Impression
Formation’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 13: 141–54.
Higgins, E.T., J.A. Bargh and W. Lombardini (1985) ‘The Nature of Priming Effects on Categoriz-
ation’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 11: 59–69.
Iyengar, S. (1991) Is Anyone Responsible? Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Iyengar, S. and D.R. Kinder (1987) News that Matters: Television and American Opinion. Chicago,
IL: University of Chicago Press.
Iyengar, S. and V. Ottati (1994) ‘Cognitive Perspective in Political Psychology’, pp. 143–87 in R.S.
Wyer and T.K. Srull (eds) Handbook of Social Cognition, Vol. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Krosnick, J.A. and L.A. Brannon (1993) ‘The Impact of War on the Ingredients of Presidential
Evaluations: George Bush and the Gulf Conflict’, American Political Science Review 87: 963–75.
Krosnick, J.A. and D.R. Kinder (1990) ‘Altering the Foundations of Support for the President
through Priming’, American Political Science Review 84: 497–511.
Manheim, J. B. (1991) All of the People, All the Time: Strategic Communication and American
Politics. New York: M.E. Sharp.
Manheim, J. B. (1994) ‘Strategic Public Diplomacy: Managing Kuwait’s Image during the Gulf
Conflict’, pp. 131–48 in W.L. Bennett and D.L. Paletz (eds) Taken by Storm: The Media, Public
Opinion, and Foreign Policy in the Gulf War. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Manheim, J.B. and R.B. Albritton (1984) ‘Changing National Images: International Public
Relations and Media Agenda Setting’, American Political Science Review 78: 641–57.
Miller, J.M. and J.A. Krosnick (1996) ‘News Media Impact on the Ingredients of Presidential Evalu-
ations: A Program of Research on the Priming Hypothesis’, pp. 79–100 in D.C. Mutz, P.M.
Sniderman and R.A. Brody (eds) Political Persuasion and Attitude Change. Ann Arbor: The Uni-
versity of Michigan Press.
Miller, J.M. and J.A. Krosnick (2000) ‘News Media Impact on the Ingredients of Presidential Evalu-
ations: Politically Knowledgeable Citizens Are Guided by a Trusted Source’, American Journal of
Political Science 44: 295–309.
Nelson, T.E., R.A. Clawson and Z.M. Oxley (1997) ‘Media Framing of a Civil Liberties Conflict
and its Effect on Tolerance’, American Political Science Review 91: 567–83.
Peffley, M. and J. Hurwitz (1992) ‘International Events and Foreign Policy Beliefs: Public Responses
to Changing Soviet–US Relations’, American Journal of Political Science 36: 431–61.
Perry, D.K. (1985) ‘The Mass Media and Inference About Other Nations’, Communication Research
12: 595–614.
Perry, D.K. (1987) ‘The Image Gap: How International News Affects Perceptions of Nations’, Jour-
nalism Quarterly 64: 416–21.
Price, V. and D. Tewksbury (1995) ‘News Values and Public Opinion: A Theoretical Account of
Media Priming and Framing’, paper presented at the annual conference of the International Com-
munication Association, Albuquerque, NM.
Price, V. and D. Tewksbury (1997) ‘News Values and Public Opinion: A Theoretical Account of
Media Priming and Framing’, pp. 173–212 in G. Barnett and F.J. Foster (eds) Progress in Com-
munication Sciences. Greenwich, CT: Ablex.
Srull, T.K. and R.S. Wyer (1980) ‘Category Accessibility and Social Perception: Some Implications
for the Study of Person Memory and Interpersonal Judgments’, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 38: 841–56.
Wyer, R.S. and T.K. Srull (1986) ‘Human Cognition in its Social Context’, Psychological Review
93: 322–59.
Wyer, R.S. and T.K. Srull (1989) Memory and Cognition in its Social Context. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
508 GAZETTE VOL. 65 NO. 6