Social Competence
Social Competence
Social Competence
Relationships within the family may also affect a childs social behavior.
Behavior that is appropriate or effective in one culture may be less so in
another culture. Children from diverse cultural and family backgrounds
thus may need help in bridging their differences and in finding ways to
learn from and enjoy one another. Teachers can help by creating classroom
communities that are open, honest, and accepting of differences.
Much research suggests that pretend play can contribute to young
childrens social and intellectual development. When children pretend to
be someone or something else, they practice taking points of view other
than their own. When they pretend together, children often take turns and
make deals and decisions cooperatively. Such findings suggest that
children in early childhood programs ought to have regular opportunities
for social play and pretend play. Teachers can observe and monitor the
childrens interactions.
.
How can we evaluate a childs social competence?
Social workers, psychologists, and other professionals may need to use formal
evaluations of preschoolers social competence. These measures include the Social
Skills Rating System (SSRS), the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment
(ASEBA) Preschool (Ages 15), and the Strengths and Difficulties questionnaire.
Many of these involve observations of the child, input from adults who know the child,
and may also include interviews with the child. But teachers and caregivers may not
want or need such formal (sometimes costly) measures to gain a sense of the social
competence of the children in their classrooms.
The Social Attributes checklist below was created to help teachers and caregivers
check to see whether a childs social competence is developing well. The intent of
this informal checklist is not to prescribe correct social behavior or diagnose a
problem but rather to help teachers observe, understand, and support children whose
social skills are still forming. The list is based on research on elements of young
childrens social competence and on studies comparing behavior of well-liked children
with that of children who are not as well liked (Bierman, Kalvin, & Heinrichs, 2015;
Katz & McClellan, 1997; Ladd & Profilet, 1996; McClellan & Kinsey, 1999).
Development of social competence in childhood has become an area of interest for researchers,
mainly because of its undeniable constructive role in shaping adjustment abilities both in childhood
and adulthood. As a multidimensional phenomenon, social competence has been defined as a
construct involving variety of qualities and traits such as positive self-image, social assertion,
frequency of interaction, social cognitive skills, popularity with peers, etc. (1).
The term social competence covers a broader domain than does the term social skills. The
term social skills is basically based on behavior and refers to particular behavior types, which a
person should perfect to participate successfully in a variety of social settings. Standing up to group
or peer pressure, asking for help and solving problems could be considered as instances of these
behavior. Person's awareness of how his behavior affects his surroundings and his sensitivity to the
needs of others are the differentiating points of social competence (2 and 3). Greater social
competence is generally related to peers acceptance, emotional health, and capability as
established in school such as school readiness, interpersonal relationships, and social adjustment.
Children who are socially assertive, cooperative, and friendly are likely to do well in social and
academic areas and demonstrate a higher psychological resilience. In contrast with what was
mentioned above, there is positive association between the lack of social competence, such as
empathy, cooperation, and conflict resolution skills on the one hand, and children's negative
behaviors and problems in their social interaction skills on the other hand (4 and 5).
The social interactions in which young children engage in the second year of their lives
have complementary and reciprocal structures. The peers in play exchange both turns and
roles such as run and chase, hide and seek, offer and receive. The toddler period is of
special significance for the child to develop their capacity both in terms of initiation and
responsiveness. This period marks the formation and stabilization of friendships, which are
the beginning of affective relationships among peers. There is evidence suggesting that
friendship among peers can provide social and emotional support to young children (4). In
the third year, the increased language skills, together with children's ability to assume a
more active role, lead to higher levels of social responsiveness and initiative (9). Once the
children pass this age, their social competence develops most noticeably in the form of the
communication of meaning. The process includes sharing meaning or themes, knowledge
of social rules, negotiation in a cooperative fashion, etc. Children's ability to communicate
meaning enables them to participate in a wider variety of games and to vary the themes of
games, as well as make early forms of social pretension. In the latter toddler period,
children acquire increased flexibility in the choice of friends and playmates, and as a result
make more friends including both long and short term ones (4).
Although much research has been carried out to investigate the development of
children's social competence from infants to preschoolers, the developmental
sequence of social competence and behavior problems during the preschool period
has not been studied as much. Empirical studies on this period might shed light on
the characteristics of children's social competence as well as helping one to discover
the effects of social incompetence on behavior problems especially regarding the
development period of the 2-to 6-year-old children. Furthermore, more intimate
knowledge on the development of social competence and various aspects of the
construct in preschoolers may better contribute to the ones who are seeking to help
out the young children with lower level of social competence, or problems associated
with it (e.g., conduct problems).
The aims of the present study were twofold. In this study it was attempted to (1)
investigate social competence and behavioral / emotional problems in Iranian
preschool children of 42 years, and (2) to examine whether factors such as
children's age, gender, socio-economic status (SES) of the family were related to the
children's social competence and behavioral/emotional problem score