Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
Ethology Biphonation May Function to Enhance Individual Recognition in the Dhole, Cuon alpinus Elena V. Volodina*, Ilya A. Volodin* , Irina V. Isaeva & Carolyn Unckà * Scientific Research Department, Moscow Zoo, Moscow, Russia Department of Biology, Lomonosov Moscow State University, Moscow, Russia à University of Western Ontario, London, Canada Correspondence Elena V. Volodina, Scientific Research Department, Moscow Zoo, B. Gruzinskaya, 1, Moscow, 123242, Russia. E-mail: volodinsvoc@yahoo.com Received: November 21, 2005 Initial acceptance: December 24, 2005 Final acceptance: December 24, 2005 (J. Lazarus) doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01231.x Abstract Biphonation (two independent fundamental frequencies in a call spectrum) represents one of the most widespread nonlinear phenomena in mammalian vocalizations. Recently, the structure of biphonations was described in detail; however, their functions are poorly understood. For the dhole (Cuon alpinus), biphonic calls represent a prominent feature of vocal activity. In this species, the biphonic call is composed of two frequency components – the high-frequency squeak and the low-frequency yap, which also occur alone as separate calls. In this study, we test the hypothesis that the complication of call structure, resulting from the joining of these calls into the biphonic yap–squeak may enhance the potential for individual recognition in the dhole. We randomly selected for analysis 30 high-frequency squeaks, 30 low-frequency yaps and 30 biphonic yap–squeaks per animal from five subadult captive dholes (450 calls in total). Discriminant analysis, based on 10 squeak parameter values, showed 80.7% correct assignment to a predicted individual. For 10 yap parameters, the correct assignment was only 44.7%. However, the analysis based on 10 parameters of the biphonic yap–squeak, selected as best contributing to discrimination, showed 96.7% correct assignment to a predicted individual. The results provide strong support for the hypothesis tested showing that the joining of two independent calls into a common vocalization may function to enhance individual recognition in the dhole. Introduction Biphonation is one of the nonlinear phenomena in mammalian vocalizations that evoked a burst of research interest in recent years (Wilden et al. 1998; Riede et al. 2000; Fitch et al. 2002). Biphonation may be recognized: (1) by appearance of two independent fundamental frequencies in a sound spectrum, (2) by different contours of frequency modulation in some frequency bands on a spectrogram, (3) by the appearance of additional frequency bands, representing linear combinations of two independent frequencies, which may be calculated using the formula n Æ f + m Æ g (f and g are two independent Ethology 112 (2006) 815–825 ª 2006 The Authors Journal compilation ª 2006 Blackwell Verlag, Berlin frequencies; n and m are integers) (Wilden et al. 1998; Volodin & Volodina 2002). At least four probable mechanisms for production of biphonic vocalizations have been proposed: (1) asynchronous vibration pattern of the left and right vocal fold (Berry et al. 1996; Tigges et al. 1997); (2) involvement of vocal fold extensions (vocal membranes) in the production of a second fundamental frequency (Brown & Cannito 1995; Mergell et al. 1999; Riede et al. 2000); (3) vortex-shedding at the glottal constriction inducing a whistle-like sound (Solomon et al. 1995; Herzel & Reuter 1997; Wilden et al. 1998); and (4) source-tract coupling (Herzel & Reuter 1996; Mergell & Herzel 1997). These proposals 815 Biphonation for Individual Recognition in the Dhole come from three sources: from analysis of actual animal and human calls (Brown & Cannito 1995; Herzel & Reuter 1996, 1997; Tigges et al. 1997; Wilden et al. 1998; Riede et al. 2000), from experiments on excised larynges (Solomon et al. 1995; Berry et al. 1996; Brown et al. 2003), and from computer simulation models (Mergell & Herzel 1997; Mergell et al. 1999). However, research of the role of various mechanisms in production of biphonic calls in particular species is limited, and sound production mechanisms in nonhuman mammals remain poorly understood (Peters et al. 2002). Functional interpretations of nonlinear phenomena in vocalizations are also scarce, and nearly entirely absent with concern to biphonation. These phenomena are not under central nervous system control, but arise from the physics of a mammalian sound production apparatus and thus may not have an adaptive meaning for a caller (Wilden et al. 1998; Fitch et al. 2002). For example, subharmonics, chaos and biphonations occur in unhealthy voices both in humans (Herzel 1993; Herzel et al. 1995; Herzel & Reuter 1996) and in nonhuman mammals, such as Japanese macaque Macaca fuscata (Riede et al. 1997), domestic dog Canis familiaris (Riede et al. 2001) and domestic cat Felis catus (Riede & Stolle-Malorny 1999). Although some nonlinear phenomena in voices may not have evolved as a structural feature with a communicative meaning a priori, they may have been adopted subsequently (Fitch et al. 2002). Moreover, there are some data confirming the communicative functions of biphonation. In two penguin species – the king penguin Aptenodytes patagonicus and the emperor penguin A. forsteri – beating generated by interaction of two close frequencies enhances the ability of calls to propagate through obstacles of numerous penguin bodies in a colony and provides additional cues for both parent–chick and mate–mate recognition (Aubin et al. 2000; Lengagne et al. 2001; Aubin & Jouventin 2002). In the killer whale Orcinus orca, differences in degradation and directionality of the lower- and higher-frequency components in biphonic calls provide information about direction of movement of a caller (Miller 2002). In addition, among proposed functions of nonlinear phenomena are accenting call formant structure for delivering information about size of a caller and breaking of monotony in vocal sequences in order to escape habituation in listeners and to attract their attention to a caller (Fitch & Hauser 2002; Fitch et al. 2002). Biphonic calls have been recorded in a few terrestrial mammalian taxa, and besides such specialized group as bats (Kanwal et al. 1994), they have been 816 E. V. Volodina, I. A. Volodin, I. V. Isaeva & C. Unck recorded primarily in primates (Brown & Cannito 1995; Brown et al. 2003) and canids (Wilden et al. 1998; Volodin & Volodina 2002). Biphonation has been recorded in timber wolf Canis lupus (Nikol’skii & Frommolt 1989; Frommolt 1999), domestic dog (Solomon et al. 1995; Volodin et al. 2005b; Volodina et al. 2005), dog–wolf hybrid (Riede et al. 2000) and jackal Canis aureus (A. Pojarkov, pers. comm.), but are especially prominent both in the African wild dog Lycaon pictus (Wilden 1997; Wilden et al. 1998; Robbins 2000) and in the dhole (Volodin & Volodina 2002). In canids, biphonic calls may occur as irregular events and not in all individuals, or attend specific states, such as frustration in domestic dogs (Volodin et al. 2005b; Volodina et al. 2005). But, in African wild dogs and dholes, biphonic calls occur regularly among calls attending short-distant affiliative interactions in a pack and make up 60% and 44% of vocal emissions in this context, respectively. Moreover, they occurred in all individuals in these species (Wilden et al. 1998; Volodin & Volodina 2002). The dhole is a pack-living canid, communally hunting on large prey and inhabiting areas with complex relief in mountains and in locations with dense vegetation, with primary breeding by a dominant pair and other group members functioning as helpers (Cohen 1977; Johnsingh 1982; Karanth & Sunquist 1995; Venkataraman et al. 1995; Venkataraman 1998). Dholes typically show very high vocal activity, that attend all, even small movements of pack members and all contacts among the animals (Sosnovskii 1967; Cohen 1977, 1985; Johnsingh 1982). In captivity, the dhole vocal repertoire includes 11 call types, based on three vocal components: the low-frequency tonal (with fundamental frequency varying from 0.5 to 1.4 kHz), the highfrequency tonal (with fundamental frequency varying from 5.5 to 10.8 kHz), and the pulsed component. Only one call type has a biphonic structure, resulting from simultaneous production of the highand low-frequency components (Volodin et al. 2001). Biphonic calls (call type yap–squeak), alongside with ‘clear’ yaps and squeaks and frequency jumps from squeak to yap, occurred primarily during peaceful interactions among group members and in the context of spontaneous movements in an enclosure (Volodin et al. 2001; Volodin & Volodina 2002). In these situations, the occurrence of biphonic calls (yap–squeaks) among contact calls (yaps, squeaks and yap–squeaks) varied in 14 individual dholes from 20% to 92%, and was not related significantly to age, sex or litter membership (Volodin & Volodina 2002). Ethology 112 (2006) 815–825 ª 2006 The Authors Journal compilation ª 2006 Blackwell Verlag, Berlin E. V. Volodina, I. A. Volodin, I. V. Isaeva & C. Unck It is not clear if yap–squeak is the same call type known as ‘mixed yip-yack cackle’ after Johnsingh (1982). Moreover, it is difficult to relate it to other call types reported onomatopoetically in earlier literature, because of the absence of spectrograms in these papers. Moreover, biphonic calls are difficult to discern from some other call types by ear. Therefore, based on the context of usage of these calls in captivity, we propose their function to be as peaceful vocalizations and that these calls may promote individual recognition in dholes. Furthermore, we propose that the complexity of the call type, composed from two independent frequencies, may play a special role, enhancing reliability of the recognition. Here we test using discriminant analysis, if the biphonic calls, composed of the high- and low-frequency components, provide better potential for individual identification, than the monophonic calls consisting of one of these components. Animals and Methods Calls were tape-recorded from five subadult dholes (aged 7.5–11 months) from two litters born in captivity. The first litter of one male and two females (no. 10, 11, 12) was born in March 19, 1999 in Moscow Zoo (Russia). The second litter of three males was born in April 24, 1999 in Volokolamsk Moscow Zoo Brooder, but only two of them (no. 14, 15) provided the necessary number of calls to be included in the analysis. All the recordings were made during November 2, 1999 to February 21, 2000. All the animals were housed with their littermates. Parents were housed together with pups (first litter) or separated from them by wire mesh (second litter). The sound recordings were made with a SONY WM-D6C recorder (Sony Corp., Tokyo, Japan) and MCE-100 unidirectional microphone (LOMO, St. Petersburg, Russia). Frequency responses of both systems were 40–15 000 Hz. Distance to animals during the recordings varied from 2 to 8 m. The sounds were produced spontaneously without stimulation from observers. All individuals from the same litters could be reliably identified by their coloration pattern. Simultaneous video recordings to identify calling individuals were made with a SONY TRV-65E video camera. For analysis, we randomly selected 90 high-quality calls per individual (30 yaps, 30 squeaks and 30 biphonic yap–squeaks, 450 calls for five dholes in total). Ownership of each call was confirmed independently by two observers during recordings which Ethology 112 (2006) 815–825 ª 2006 The Authors Journal compilation ª 2006 Blackwell Verlag, Berlin Biphonation for Individual Recognition in the Dhole was confirmed additionally on the basis of video recordings. All spectrograms of these calls were analyzed with Avisoft SASLab software (ª R. Specht). Digital processing used fast Fourier transform (FFT) with 22.05 kHz sampling frequency, Hamming window, FFT length 512 points, frame 50%, overlap 93.8%, that provided 1.45 ms time resolution and 43 Hz frequency resolution. Biphonic calls of dholes comprise two independent vocal components that also occur as separate vocalizations. The fundamental of the low-frequency component f0 is about 1 kHz, and the fundamental of the high-frequency component g0 is higher than 5 kHz (Fig. 1). We measured seven frequency and three temporal parameters for each component occurring alone as yap and squeak calls and 20 parameters for each biphonic yap–squeak (Table 1) For the biphonic calls, the high- and low-frequency component parameters were measured after highand low-pass filtration of 5 kHz, applied alternately. Temporal parameters were measured from the spectrogram window using a standard marker cursor. Fundamental frequency parameters for each component were measured using a free reticule cursor. All measurements were exported automatically into an Excel database. The number of fundamental frequency extrema were counted visually from spectrograms according to Tooze et al. (1990). Frequency parameters of the high-frequency components were measured by g0 only, whereas those of the low-frequency components by the most well-expressed frequency band (f0, f1 or f2). Values of peak frequency and bandwidth of peak frequency (at distance )10 dB from peak) of the corresponding component Fig. 1: Spectrograms illustrating three call types of the dhole: left – the high-frequency call or squeak; middle – the low-frequency call or yap; right – the biphonic call or yap–squeak. Designations: g0 – fundamental frequency of the high-frequency component; f0 – fundamental frequency of the low-frequency component; f1 and f2 – harmonics of f0; g0–f0 – the linear combination of f0 and g0 817 Biphonation for Individual Recognition in the Dhole E. V. Volodina, I. A. Volodin, I. V. Isaeva & C. Unck Table 1: Call parameters used in the statistical analyses Call parameters High-frequency component Low-frequency component Start fundamental frequency (kHz) End fundamental frequency (kHz) Maximum fundamental frequency (kHz) Minimum fundamental frequency (kHz) Peak frequency (frequency with the maximum amplitude in the power spectrum) (kHz) Bandwidth of peak frequency (Hz) Number of fundamental frequency extrema (total number of peaks and depressions) Duration from start to maximum frequency point of a component (s) Duration from maximum frequency point to end of a component (s) Proportion: duration from start to maximum frequency point of a component/duration of a component g0_ini g0_end g0_max g0_min g_peak g_bandw g_extrem g_dur_inc g_dur_dec g_k_max f0_ini f0_end f0_max f0_min f_peak f_bandw f_extrem f_dur_inc f_dur_dec f_k_max were taken automatically from the mean power spectrum. For the high-frequency component, the peak frequency band coincided with fundamental frequency g0, otherwise, for the low-frequency component, it could fall on one of the harmonic bands from f0 to f2, predominately on f1. We also used one calculated parameter: the duration from the start to the maximum frequency point of a component divided by the duration of the component (k_max) (Table 1). For each of the three call types, values were normally distributed for most parameters (Kolmogorov– Smirnov test). As parametrical anova and discriminant analysis are relatively robust to departures from normality (Dillon & Goldstein 1984), this was not an obstacle to the application of these tests. We performed one-factor anova, with ‘individual’ as the grouping variable, to compare variability of the parameters within and between individuals for each call type. Then we used the standard discriminant analysis procedure based on 10 parameters for each call to determine whether calls could be assigned to the correct caller. Because the biphonic calls contained a double set of parameters, 10 from the low frequency, and 10 from the high frequency, we had to reduce this number to 10 to escape increasing discriminability of biphonic calls simply because of an increase in the number of parameters entered into the analysis. To select 10 of the 20 parameters available for biphonic calls, we conducted a stepwise discriminant analysis and took the 10 parameters that best contributed to the discrimination. For statistical comparison of correct assignment values resulting from discriminant analyses for the biphonic and non-biphonic calls, we used a 2 · 2 chi-squared test. To validate results of discriminant analysis, we performed cross-validation analysis and randomization. For cross-validation analysis, call samples for 818 each dhole were randomly split half-and-half, providing a training set (75 calls) and a test set (75 calls) for each call type. Then the classification of one half of the dataset was made, with the discriminant function derived from the other half. Randomization was applied in order to calculate the expected level of correct assignment by discriminant analysis if the calls we analyzed were randomly distributed between individuals. For this procedure, we created five randomization groups for each call type. Each group of 30 calls consisted of six randomly selected calls taken from each of five dholes. After that, we conducted a standard discriminant analysis and calculated the probabilities of correct assignment of calls to the randomization groups. These probabilities were taken as random values for each call type. Differences between the random and actual values of correct assignment were tested with a 2 · 2 chi-squared test. All the analyses were performed in STATISTICA, version 6.0 (StatSoft, Inc, Tulsa, OK, USA). Results The one-way anova revealed the highly significant individual differences in all call parameters for individuals both for squeaks (10 parameters) and yap– squeaks (20 parameters), but only for four call parameters of yaps (f0_end, f0_min, f_dur_dec, f_k_max). For the other six call parameters of yaps, interindividual variability did not exceed intra-individual variability. Table 2 shows mean values and standard deviations for some call parameters (for four parameters both for the high-frequency squeak and the low-frequency yap as well as for eight parameters of biphonic yap–squeak calls) and results of anova-based interindividual comparison for each call type for five individuals. All other parameters of squeaks and yap–squeaks, not included in Table 2, showed significant differences at the p < 0.001 level. Ethology 112 (2006) 815–825 ª 2006 The Authors Journal compilation ª 2006 Blackwell Verlag, Berlin E. V. Volodina, I. A. Volodin, I. V. Isaeva & C. Unck Biphonation for Individual Recognition in the Dhole Table 2: Values (mean  SD) of high-frequency call (squeak), low-frequency call (yap) and biphonic call (yap–squeak) parameters for five dholes and results of ANOVA-based interindividual comparisons for each call type Call parameters Female 10 Squeak (n ¼ 150, 30 per animal) g0_max, kHz 8.76  0.91 g_dom, kHz 8.43  1.01 g_dur_inc, ms 52  36 g_dur_dec, ms 50  44 Yap (n ¼ 150, 30 per animal) f0_max, kHz 0.94  0.11 f_dom, kHz 1.62  0.77 f_dur_inc, ms 38  18 f_dur_dec, ms 33  13 Yap–squeak (n ¼ 150, 30 per animal) g0_max, kHz 9.27  0.49 g_dom, kHz 8.62  0.88 g_dur_inc, ms 29  27 g_dur_dec, ms 83  38 f0_max, kHz 1.02  0.09 f_dom, kHz 1.57  0.62 f_dur_inc, ms 29  21 f_dur_dec, ms 52  18 Male 11 Female 12 Male 14 Male 15 F4,145 8.08 7.87 80 35     0.26 0.26 32 40 7.51 7.13 99 21     0.30 0.28 40 30 5.92 5.72 78 44     0.14 0.13 38 34 8.13 7.88 38 69     0.16 0.13 34 31 169** 139** 13** 7** 1.00 1.54 40 29     0.15 0.80 21 19 1.00 1.67 34 32     0.18 0.94 16 19 1.01 1.72 42 21     0.12 0.76 18 13 0.95 2.07 37 20     0.10 0.84 14 13 1.6; p ¼ 0.16 1.8; p ¼ 0.13 1.0; p ¼ 0.42 4.1* 7.91 7.74 41 75 1.22 1.90 37 49         0.14 0.14 39 44 0.10 0.61 24 12 7.27 6.84 71 47 1.13 1.67 8 42         0.16 1.20 40 36 0.14 0.66 14 16 5.98 5.76 55 63 1.16 2.08 17 39         0.29 0.19 35 41 0.09 0.54 14 14 8.25 7.76 26 88 1.27 1.50 48 29         0.25 0.10 37 47 0.09 0.52 14 11 511** 78** 8** 5* 27** 5** 24** 13** *p < 0.01; **p < 0.001. For yaps, significant individual differences did occur also in f0_end (p < 0.001), f0_min (p < 0.05) and in f_k_max (p < 0.05). Three discriminant analyses were performed, based on: (1) 10 parameters of the squeak; (2) 10 parameters of the yap; and (3) 10 parameters of the yap–squeak, selected using a stepwise discriminant analysis. For the squeak, 80.7% correct assignment was achieved (Table 3, Fig. 2a), significantly more than the random value of 30.7% (v2 ¼ 73.96, d.f. ¼ 1, p < 0.001), being calculated by using the randomization procedure. The first discriminant function correlated basically with fundamental frequency parameters, primarily with g0_max and g0_end, as well as with g_peak, and explained 85.72% of the variance. Contribution of other parameters was small. The second discriminant function described 8.88% of the variance and correlated mainly with g_dur_inc, g_k_max and g0_ini (Table 4). Cross-validation analysis showed 84.0% correct assignment for the training call set (n ¼ 75, 15 calls per individual), with 66.7–100% for particular individuals. Correct assignment for the test call set (n ¼ 75, other 15 calls per individual) did not differ significantly from the training percentage of assignment, and showed 72.0%, varying from 33.3% to 100% among individuals (v2 ¼ 2.49, d.f. ¼ 1, p ¼ 0.11). For the yap, the discriminant analysis showed only 44.7% correct assignment to individual (Table 3, Ethology 112 (2006) 815–825 ª 2006 The Authors Journal compilation ª 2006 Blackwell Verlag, Berlin Table 3: Assignment of dhole calls to a predicted individual, based on discriminant analysis of the squeak, yap and yap–squeak call parameter values Predicted group membership Actual group Squeak Female 10 Male 11 Female 12 Male 14 Male 15 Total Yap Female 10 Male 11 Female 12 Male 14 Male 15 Total Yap–squeak Female 10 Male 11 Female 12 Male 14 Male 15 Total 10 11 12 14 15 Total Correctly classified (%) 17 0 0 0 0 17 4 22 1 0 1 28 5 3 25 0 2 35 0 0 1 30 0 31 4 5 3 0 27 39 30 30 30 30 30 150 56.7 73.3 83.3 100 90.0 80.7 13 12 4 1 3 33 8 5 4 5 2 24 2 6 16 3 1 28 4 6 2 11 2 25 3 1 4 10 22 40 30 30 30 30 30 150 43.3 16.7 53.3 36.7 73.3 44.7 30 0 0 0 0 30 0 28 2 0 1 31 0 1 28 0 0 29 0 0 0 30 0 30 0 1 0 0 29 30 30 30 30 30 30 150 100 93.3 93.3 100 96.7 96.7 Fig. 2b). This value of correct assignment did not differ significantly from the random value of 33.3% (v2 ¼ 3.59, d.f. ¼ 1, p ¼ 0.06). Both frequency and 819 Biphonation for Individual Recognition in the Dhole E. V. Volodina, I. A. Volodin, I. V. Isaeva & C. Unck Table 4: Values of correlation between squeak call parameters and the two first discriminant functions; eigenvalues and percent variance, described by each function Parameters Root 1 Root 2 g0_ini g0_end g0_max g0_min g_peak g_bandw g_extrem g_dur_inc g_dur_dec g_k_max 0.503 0.730 0.915 0.511 0.827 0.142 0.062 )0.107 0.036 )0.059 )0.593 0.074 )0.158 )0.528 )0.169 0.215 )0.246 0.680 )0.537 0.672 Eigenvalue Percent variance 5.56 85.72% 0.58 8.88% Table 5: Values of correlation between yap call parameters and the two first discriminant functions; eigenvalues and percent variance, described by each function Fig. 2: Scatterplots showing separation produced by the first two discriminant functions of three call types for five dholes: (a) based on parameters of the high-frequency call or squeak; (b) based on parameters of the low-frequency call or yap; (c) based on parameters of the biphonic call or yap–squeak temporal parameters contributed to discrimination. The first discriminant function was related primarily to parameters f0_end, f_dur_dec and f_k_max, that 820 Parameters Root 1 Root 2 f0_ini f0_end f0_max f0_min f_peak f_bandw f_extrem f_dur_inc f_dur_dec f_k_max )0.112 0.559 )0.100 0.318 0.301 )0.131 0.229 0.065 )0.484 0.423 )0.007 )0.235 0.205 )0.294 0.141 0.444 )0.202 )0.261 )0.037 )0.043 Eigenvalue Percent variance 0.42 61.23% 0.17 24.24% described 61.23% of the variance only. The second discriminant function was founded on f_bandw, f0_min and f_dur_inc and described 24.24% of the variance (Table 5). Cross-validation analysis showed 53.3% correct assignment for the training call set (n ¼ 75, 15 calls per individual), with 40.0–60.0% for particular individuals. Correct assignment for the test call set (n ¼ 75, other 15 calls per individual) was only 34.7% (26.7–46.7% between individuals), significantly lower than the results for the training set (v2 ¼ 4.57, d.f. ¼ 1, p < 0.05). So, the yap parameters showed low ability to discriminate between individuals. For the yap–squeak, the stepwise discriminant analysis selected three parameters of the high-frequency component and seven parameters of the low-frequency component that contributed mostly to discrimination of calls to individuals (Table 6). Ethology 112 (2006) 815–825 ª 2006 The Authors Journal compilation ª 2006 Blackwell Verlag, Berlin E. V. Volodina, I. A. Volodin, I. V. Isaeva & C. Unck Biphonation for Individual Recognition in the Dhole Table 6: Values of correlation between 10 yap–squeak call parameters, selected with the stepwise discriminant procedure, and the two first discriminant functions; eigenvalues and percent variance, described by each function Parameters Root 1 Root 2 g0_end g0_max g_bandw f0_ini f0_max f_bandw f_extrem f_dur_inc f_dur_dec f_k_max 0.850 0.792 0.075 )0.086 )0.057 0.032 0.092 0.087 0.051 0.058 )0.077 0.170 )0.117 )0.040 0.509 )0.031 0.059 0.437 )0.321 0.542 Eigenvalue Percent variance 22.27 87.27% 2.12 8.30% With these 10 parameters, standard discriminant analysis provided 96.7% correct assignment (Table 3, Fig. 2c). This value is significantly higher than the random value of 32.0% (v2 ¼ 133.88, d.f. ¼ 1, p < 0.001). In this case, the first discriminant function described 87.27% of the variance and was correlated only with fundamental frequency parameters of high-frequency component (g0_end and g0_max). The second discriminant function was based on the low-frequency component parameters, both temporal (f_k_max and f_dur_inc), and frequency (f0_max), and described as little as 8.3% of the variance (Table 6). Thus, the high-frequency component parameters contributed more to individual discrimination of biphonic calls, and the same parameters (g0_end and g0_max) that contributed mainly to discrimination of yap–squeaks were among the three that contributed mainly to discrimination of squeaks (Tables 4 and 6). Therefore, the first discriminant function of the biphonic call relied on the high-frequency parameters, whereas the second one on the low-frequency parameters (Table 6, Fig. 2), that resulted in a small increase in the percentage of correct assignment to individuals. Crossvalidation analysis showed 94.7% correct assignment for the training call set (n ¼ 75, 15 calls per individual), with 86.7–100% for particular individuals. Correct assignment for the test call set (n ¼ 75, other 15 calls per individual) did not differ from the training percentage of assignment, and also showed 94.7%, varying from 86.7% to 100% among individuals. Finally, a comparison of discrimination percentages to individuals between the yap, squeak and yap–squeak showed that the discriminability was Ethology 112 (2006) 815–825 ª 2006 The Authors Journal compilation ª 2006 Blackwell Verlag, Berlin significantly higher for the squeak than for the yap (v2 ¼ 40.02, d.f. ¼ 1, p < 0.001) and for the yap– squeak than for the yap (v2 ¼ 95.84, d.f. ¼ 1, p < 0.001) and for the squeak (v2 ¼ 17.55, d.f. ¼ 1, p < 0.001). Discussion The presented data support the hypothesis that the biphonic calls, representing a combination of the high- and the low-frequency components, enhances the potential for individual discrimination in the dhole. However, the high-frequency squeak had substantially higher potential for individual discrimination than the low-frequency yap. The yap did not provide cues to individuality at all, showing a discrimination ability that did not differ significantly from the random values. For many canids, discriminant analysis-based research has suggested a potential for individual recognition by long-distance calls. Such data were reported for howling of timber wolves (Tooze et al. 1990), for bark series of arctic foxes Alopex lagopus (Frommolt et al. 1997, 2003) and swift foxes Vulpes velox (Darden et al. 2003), for hoo-calls of African wild dogs (Hartwig 2005) and corresponding to hoocall vocalization of dholes (Durbin 1998). Probably, the cues to individuality in distant calls of canids compensate for the absence of visual and olfactory stimuli that provide cues to individuality in close proximity. Our study showed the presence of individual cues in short-distance, low-intensity calls in the dhole. For short-distance calls, the necessity of individual cues is questionable, because the roles of visual and olfactory channels are considered as much more meaningful for short-distance communication. However, Owren & Rendall (1997, 2001) showed, that for group-living primates with a complex system of subordination, individual cues may also be important for short-distance calls, because calls of particular individuals adopt a role of conditioned stimuli, evoking pleasant or unpleasant effects. Consistent with this model, dholes might use individually distinctive short-distance calls to support stable social relationships within a pack. Biphonic calls are emitted in peaceful short-distance interactions (Volodin et al. 2001), and thus their production itself may evoke positive affiliative effects in pack fellows and result in very low intrapack aggression, a characteristic for this species (Johnsingh 1982; Ludwig & Ludwig 2000). Such an effect of vocalizations has been shown for complex primate 821 Biphonation for Individual Recognition in the Dhole societies: if high-ranking animals emit groomingassociated affiliative calls when approaching subordinates, positive interactions occur more often, than if they are silent (Bauers & de Waal 1991; Cheney et al. 1995). Although the effect-conditioning model outlines the role of vocal tract formants as cues to individual identity in short-distance primate calls, such as lowfrequency non-biphonic grunt and ‘coo’ calls (Owren & Rendall 1997, 2001; Rendall et al. 1998), in the dhole the individual cues are based on a very complex call structure, resulting from appearance of a second fundamental frequency. With the absence of formant cues in dhole calls (Volodin et al. 2001), just the use of a second fundamental frequency allows dholes to enhance strongly the potential for individual recognition of short-distance calls. The key role of two frequencies in a call spectrum for both parent–chick and mate–mate recognition was also demonstrated for two penguin species (Aubin et al. 2000; Lengagne et al. 2001; Aubin & Jouventin 2002). Furthermore, exploitation of calls with two frequencies, lying far apart from each other, may provide additional advantages: cues to orientation and direction of a movement of pack members emitting these calls. These proposals come from physical frameworks, suggesting that high frequencies, propagated in the environment, attenuate much more strongly, than low frequencies (Wiley & Richards 1978; Roberts et al. 1980; Owings & Morton 1998; Naguib & Wiley 2001). For canids, this effect was experimentally confirmed for the domestic dog (Frommolt & Gebler 2004). Our recent data showed that both the biphonic yap–squeaks and non-biphonic yaps provide information about orientation of a caller to a listener: when dholes called toward a microphone, the proportion of energy in the higher part of the call spectrum (above 5 kHz) was significantly higher than when calls were emitted in an orientation away from a microphone (Volodin et al. 2005a). The data, available for two dolphin species, also showed equivocal relations between directionality and presence of two frequencies in call spectra. For the killer whale, only using biphonic calls provides reliable information about orientation of a caller to a listener, whereas calls consisting exclusively of the low-frequency component, do not provide such information (Miller 2002). On the other hand, for the Hawaiian spinner dolphin Stenella longirostris, non-biphonic calls provided information about direction of a caller’s movement just as a consequence of the difference in directionality and propagation 822 E. V. Volodina, I. A. Volodin, I. V. Isaeva & C. Unck ability of fundamental frequency and higher harmonics (Lammers & Au 2003). Taken together, the available data show that a wide frequency spectrum with widely spaced frequency bands alone is sufficient for coding orientation of a caller to a listener. However, as the amplitude of higher-ordered harmonics decreases about 6–12 dB per octave (Titze 1994; Owren & Bernacki 1998), an addition of a second higher fundamental frequency, lying apart from the first one, makes the biphonic call structure especially suitable for coding orientation. Indeed, both Miller’s data on the killer whale and our data on the dhole showed a tendency for better performance of biphonic calls in coding orientation of a caller to a listener in comparison with monophonic calls (Miller 2002; Volodin et al. 2005a). Therefore, in the dhole, the high-frequency squeak, occurring singly, possesses the ability to discriminate between individuals, although less well than the biphonic yap–squeak, but it could not provide cues to the orientation of a caller, because the high-frequency narrow-band calls are the most difficult to locate (Marler 1955; Klump & Shalter 1984). On the other hand, the low-frequency yap, occurring singly, possesses the ability to encode the orientation of a caller to a listener that is comparable with, but not as good as, the biphonic yap–squeak, and at the same time shows poor discrimination ability. Joined together into a biphonic call, they perform better in both respects. The combination of enhanced potential to code individuality with enhanced potential to code orientation of a caller to a listener makes biphonic calls especially appropriate for delicate communication in a pack with complex subordination between animals, living in close vicinity. This conclusion is in accordance with our previous data, suggesting a very high occurrence of biphonic calls in the dhole (Volodin & Volodina 2002). Moreover, for the African wild dog, a second extremely social canid species, a very high level of occurrence of biphonic contact calls was reported (Wilden 1997; Wilden et al. 1998). Only two canid species – the dhole and the African wild dog – show such high percentages of production of biphonic calls, although many other canids are able to produce two fundamentals simultaneously (Nikol’skii & Frommolt 1989; Solomon et al. 1995; Riede et al. 2000; Volodin et al. 2005b). It is probable, that in conditions of dense vegetation and large social groups, under which these species live, the acoustical channel, providing information about individuality and spacing of animals, becomes Ethology 112 (2006) 815–825 ª 2006 The Authors Journal compilation ª 2006 Blackwell Verlag, Berlin E. V. Volodina, I. A. Volodin, I. V. Isaeva & C. Unck preferable for prompt communication even over short distances, releasing the visual channel from communicative load (e.g. Morton & Shalter 1977; Lamprecht et al. 1985; Manser 1999). However, further research is necessary to reveal the more clear communicative meaning of biphonic calls, using playbacks and studying auditory perception. Unfortunately, to date, there are no data concerning the possible mechanism of production of the high-frequency squeak-like calls in canids. Our observations of dholes and domestic dogs (E. Volodina, I. Volodin, unpubl. data) showed that during emission of the squeak the mouth is closed, with sound passing exclusively through the nose. As soon as the animal begins adding a low-frequency yap, it opens its mouth. Thus the high frequency is emitted into the environment through the nose, whereas the lower is emitted through the mouth. These observations are in accordance with X-ray video data on vocalizing domestic dogs, showing that highfrequency whines were produced nasally, whereas low-frequency barks were produced with open mouth (Fitch 2000). Acknowledgements We are grateful to Leon Durbin, Robert Robbins, KarlHeinz Frommolt, Andrew Poyarkov and Olga Filatova for valuable discussion, to Igor Pavlinov and Andrew Babitsky for consulting in statistics, to Egor Bazykin for providing the necessary literature, and to John Lazarus, Gustav Peters and an anonymous referee for comments and improvements of the manuscript. This work was supported by grant no. 03-04-48919 from the Russian Foundation for Basic Research. Literature Cited Aubin, T. & Jouventin, P. 2002: How to vocally identify kin in a crowd: the penguin model. Adv. Study Behav. 31, 243—277. Aubin, T., Jouventin, P. & Hildebrand, C. 2000: Penguins use the two-voice system to recognize each other. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 267, 1081—1087. Bauers, K. A. & de Waal, F. B. M. 1991: ‘‘Coo’’ vocalizations in stumptailed macaques: a controlled functional analysis. Behaviour 119, 143—160. Berry, D. A., Herzel, H., Titze, I. R. & Story, B. H. 1996: Bifurcations in excised larynx experiments. J. Voice 10, 129—138. Brown, C. H. & Cannito, M. P. 1995: Modes of vocal variation in Sykes’s monkey (Cercopithecus albogularis) squeals. J. Comp. Psychol. 109, 398—415. Ethology 112 (2006) 815–825 ª 2006 The Authors Journal compilation ª 2006 Blackwell Verlag, Berlin Biphonation for Individual Recognition in the Dhole Brown, C. H., Alipour, F., Berry, D. A. & Montequin, D. 2003: Laryngeal biomechanics and vocal communication in the squirrel monkey (Saimiri boliviensis). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113, 2114—2126. Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M. & Silk, J. B. 1995: The role of grunts in reconciling opponents and facilitation interactions among adult female baboons. Anim. Behav. 50, 249—257. Cohen, J. A. 1977: A review of biology of the dhole or Asiatic wild dog (Cuon alpinus Pallas). Anim. Regulat. Stud. 1, 141—158. Cohen, J. A. 1985: A note on the behaviour of captive dholes (Cuon alpinus). J. Bombay Nat. Hist. Soc. 82, 183—187. Darden, S. K., Dabelsteen, T. & Pedersen, S. B. 2003: A potential tool for swift fox Vulpes velox conservation: individuality of long-range barking sequences. J. Mammal. 84, 1417—1427. Dillon, W. R. & Goldstein, M. 1984: Multivariate Analysis: Methods and Applications. Wiley, New York. Durbin, L. S. 1998: Individuality in the whistle call of the Asiatic wild dog Cuon alpinus. Bioacoustics 9, 197—206. Fitch, W. T. 2000: The phonetic potential of nonhuman vocal tracts: comparative cineradiographic observations of vocalizing animals. Phonetica 57, 205—218. Fitch, W. T. & Hauser, M. D. 2002: Unpacking ‘‘honesty’’: vertebrate vocal production and the evolution of acoustic signals. In: Acoustic Communication (Simmons, A., Fay, R. R. & Popper, A. N., eds). Springer, New York, pp. 65—137. Fitch, W. T., Neubauer, J. & Herzel, H. 2002: Calls out of chaos: the adaptive significance of nonlinear phenomena in mammalian vocal production. Anim. Behav. 63, 407—418. Frommolt, K.-H. 1999: Sidebands – facts and artefacts. Bioacoustics 10, 219—224. Frommolt, K.-H. & Gebler, A. 2004: Directionality of dog vocalizations. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 116, 561—565. Frommolt, K.-H., Kruchenkova, E. P. & Russig, H. 1997: Individuality of territorial barking in Arctic foxes, Alopex lagopus (L., 1758). Z. fur Säugetierkunde 62 (Suppl. 2), 66—70. Frommolt, K.-H., Goltsman, M. E. & Macdonald, D. W. 2003: Barking foxes, Alopex lagopus: field experiments in individual recognition in territorial mammal. Anim. Behav. 65, 509—518. Hartwig, S. 2005: Individual acoustic identification as a non-invasive conservation tool: an approach to the conservation of the African wild dog Lycaon pictus (Temminck, 1820). Bioacoustics 15, 35—50. Herzel, H. 1993: Bifurcation and chaos in voice signals. Appl. Mech. Rev. 46, 399—413. 823 Biphonation for Individual Recognition in the Dhole Herzel, H. & Reuter, R. 1996: Biphonation in voice signals. In: Nonlinear, Chaotic, and Advanced Signal Processing Methods for Engineers and Scientists (Katz, R. A., Frison, T. W., Kadke, J. B. & Bulsara, A. R. eds). American Institute of Physics, Woodbury, pp. 644—657. Herzel, H. & Reuter, R. 1997: Whistle register and biphonation in a child’s voice. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 49, 216—224. Herzel, H., Berry, D., Titze, I. R. & Steinecke, I. 1995: Nonlinear dynamics of the voice: signal analysis and biomechanical modelling. Chaos 5, 30—34. Johnsingh, A. J. T. 1982: Reproductive and social behaviour of the dhole, Cuon alpinus (Canidae). J. Zool. Lond 198, 443—463. Kanwal, J. S., Matsumura, S., Ohlemiller, K. & Suga, N. 1994: Analysis of acoustic elements and syntax in communication sounds emitted by mustached bats. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 96, 1229—1254. Karanth, K. U. & Sunquist, M. E. 1995: Prey selection by tiger, leopard and dhole in tropical forests. J. Anim. Ecol. 64, 439—450. Klump, G. M. & Shalter, M. D. 1984: Acoustic behaviour of birds and mammals in the predator context. Z. Tierpsychol. 66, 189—226. Lammers, M. O. & Au, W. W. L. 2003: Directionality in the whistles of Hawaiian spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris): a signal feature to cue direction of movement? Marine Mam. Sci. 19, 249—264. Lamprecht, J., Kaiser, A., Peters, A. & Kirchgessner, C. 1985: Distance call duets in bar-headed geese (Anser indicus): co-operation through visual relief of the partner? Z. Tierpsychol. 70, 211—218. Lengagne, T., Lauga, J. & Aubin, T. 2001: Intra-syllabic acoustic signatures used by the king penguin in parent-chick recognition: an experimental approach. J. Exp. Biol. 204, 663—672. Ludwig, W. & Ludwig, C. 2000: Beobachtungen zur sozialen Organisation eines Rudels Rothunde (Cuon alpinus) im Zoo Dresden. Zool. Garten N.F. 70, 39—59. Manser, M. B. 1999: Response of foraging group members to sentinel calls in suricates, Suricata suricata. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 266, 1013—1019. Marler, P. 1955: Characteristics of some animal calls. Nature 176, 6—8. Mergell, P. & Herzel, H. 1997: Modelling biphonation – the role of the vocal tract. Speech Communication 22, 141—154. Mergell, P., Fitch, W. T. & Herzel, H. 1999: Modelling the role of non-human vocal membranes in phonation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 105, 2020—2028. Miller, P. J. O. 2002: Mixed-directionality of killer whale stereotyped calls: a direction of movement cue? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 52, 262—270. 824 E. V. Volodina, I. A. Volodin, I. V. Isaeva & C. Unck Morton, E. S. & Shalter, M. D. 1977: Vocal response to predators in pair-bonded Carolina wrens. Condor 79, 222—227. Naguib, M. & Wiley, H. 2001: Estimating the distance to a source of sound: mechanisms and adaptations for longrange communication. Anim. Behav. 62, 825—837. Nikol’skii, A. A. & Frommolt, K.-H. 1989: Vocal Activity in the Timber Wolf. Moscow State Univ. Press, Moscow. [In Russian]. Owings, D. H. & Morton, E. S. 1998: Animal Vocal Communication: A New Approach. Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge. Owren, M. J. & Bernacki, R. H. 1998: Applying linear predictive coding (LPC) to frequency-spectrum analysis of animal acoustic signals. In: Animal Acoustic Communication (Hopp, S. L., Owren, M. J. & Evans, C. S. eds). Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp. 129—162. Owren, M. J. & Rendall, D. 1997: An affect-conditioning model of nonhuman primate vocal signalling. In: Perspectives in Ethology, Vol. 12 (Owings, D. H., Beecher, M. D. & Thompson, N. S., eds). Plenum Press, New York, pp. 299—346. Owren, M. J. & Rendall, D. 2001: Sound on the rebound: bringing form and function back to the forefront in understanding non-human primate vocal signalling. Evol. Anthropol. 10, 58—71. Peters, G., Koenig, A., Herzel, H. P., Riede, T., East, M. & Hofer, H. 2002: Powerful sound analysis tools and rudimentary knowledge of sound production mechanisms in mammals – does this discrepancy matter? Adv. Ethol. 37, 63. Rendall, D., Owren, M. J. & Rodman, P. S. 1998: The role of vocal tract filtering in identity cueing in rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) vocalizations. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103, 602—614. Riede, T. & Stolle-Malorny, A. 1999: The vocal change of a kitten with craniocerebellar trauma – a case study. Bioacoustics 10, 131—141. Riede, T., Wilden, I. & Tembrock, G. 1997: Subharmonics, biphonations, and frequency jumps – common components of mammalian vocalization or indicators for disorders. Z. fur Säugetierkunde 62 (Suppl. 2), 198—203. Riede, T., Herzel, H., Mehwald, D., Seidner, W., Trumler, E., Tembrock, G. & Böhme, G. 2000: Nonlinear phenomena and their anatomical basis in the natural howling of a female dog-wolf breed. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 108, 1435—1442. Riede, T., Herzel, H., Hammerschmidt, K., Brunnberg, L. & Tembrock, G. 2001: The harmonic-to-noise ratio applied to dog barks. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 110, 2191—2197. Robbins, R. L. 2000: Vocal communication in free-ranging African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus). Behaviour 137, 1271—1298. Ethology 112 (2006) 815–825 ª 2006 The Authors Journal compilation ª 2006 Blackwell Verlag, Berlin E. V. Volodina, I. A. Volodin, I. V. Isaeva & C. Unck Roberts, J. P., Kacelnik, A. & Hunter, M. J. 1980: Some consequences of sound interference patterns for birdsong. Acoustic Lett. 3, 141—146. Solomon, N. P., Luschei, E. & Kang, L. 1995: Fundamental frequency and tracheal pressure during three types of vocalizations elicited from anaesthetized dogs. J. Voice 9, 403—412. Sosnovskii, I. P. 1967: Breeding the red dog or dhole Cuon alpinus at Moscow Zoo. Int. Zoo Yearbook 7, 120—122. Tigges, M., Mergell, P., Herzel, H., Wittenberg, Th. & Eysholdt, U. 1997: Observation and modelling of glottal biphonation. Acoustica 83, 707—714. Titze, I. R. 1994: Principles of Voice Production. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ. Tooze, Z. J., Harrington, F. H. & Fentress, J. C. 1990: Individually distinct vocalizations in timber wolves, Canis lupus. Anim. Behav. 40, 723—730. Venkataraman, A. B. 1998: Male-biased adult sex-ratios and their significance for cooperative breeding in dhole, Cuon alpinus, packs. Ethology 104, 671—684. Venkataraman, A. B., Arumugam, R. & Sukumar, R. 1995: The foraging ecology of dhole (Cuon alpinus) in Mudumalai-Sanctuary, southern India. J. Zool. 237, 543—561. Volodin, I. A. & Volodina, E. V. 2002: Biphonation as a prominent feature of the dhole Cuon alpinus sounds. Bioacoustics 13, 105—120. Volodin, I. A., Volodina, E. V. & Isaeva, I. V. 2001: Vocal repertoire in the dhole Cuon alpinus (Carnivora, Cani- Ethology 112 (2006) 815–825 ª 2006 The Authors Journal compilation ª 2006 Blackwell Verlag, Berlin Biphonation for Individual Recognition in the Dhole dae) in captivity. Russian J. Zool. 80, 1252—1267. [in Russian, translated into English in Entomological Review, 2001, 81, (Suppl. 2), S346–S361]. Volodin, I., Nagailik, M. & Volodina, E. 2005a: Cues to a direction of movement in biphonic calls of the dhole (Cuon alpinus). In: XX Congress of International BioAcoustic Council, Book of Abstracts (Trilar, T., ed.). Portoroz, Slovenia, p. 36. Volodin, I. A., Volodina, E. V. & Filatova, O. A. 2005b: Structural peculiarities, occurrence and functional significance of nonlinear phenomena in calls of terrestrial mammals. J. General Biol. 66, 346—362. [In Russian]. Volodina, E., Volodin, I. & Filatova, O. 2005: Quantitative analysis of nonlinear phenomena in whines of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). In: XX. Congress of International BioAcoustic Council, Book of Abstracts (Trilar, T., ed.). Portoroz, Slovenia, p. 37. Wilden, I. 1997: Phonetische Variabilität in der Lautgebung Afrikanischer Wildhunde (Lycaon pictus) und deren frühe Ontogenese. Aachen, Shaker Verlag. Wilden, I., Herzel, H., Peters, G. & Tembrock, G. 1998: Subharmonics, biphonation, and deterministic chaos in mammal vocalization. Bioacoustics 9, 171—196. Wiley, R. H. & Richards, D. B. 1978: Physical constraints on acoustic communication in the atmosphere: implication for the evolution of animal vocalizations. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 3, 69—94. 825