ISSN
OFFICERS AND EXECUTIVE BOARD
(January 2014 ~ December 2015)
President
Jeong-Ryeol Kim (Korea National University of Education)*
Vice Presidents
Journal Editing & Publication
Young-Joo Jeon (Mokwon University)*
Conference & Homepage
Rob Waring (Notre Dame Seishin University, Japan)*
Planning & Coordination
Scott Miles (Daegu Haany University)*
Public Relations
Seok-Chae Rhee (Yonsei University)*
Research & Development
Kyungsook Chang
(Korea
Evaluation)*Secretary General
Treasurer
Institute
for
Curriculum
and
Jeyoung Lee (Korea National University of Education)*
Myeong-gi Jeong (Song Jeong Elementary School)*
Editors Jyun Bang (Busan University of Foreign Studies)*
Eunsol Shin (Sogang University)*
Koun Choi (Cambridge University, UK)
Benjamin Garrido (Mokwon University)
Conference & Homepage
Dong Joo Lee (Korea National University of Education)*
Daniel Stewart (The Kaisei Academy)
Jeong-Jee Whang (Pyeongtack University)
Planning & Coordination
Yoonhee Choi (Chongshin University)*
Aaron Jolly (Visang Corporation)
Carl Dusthimer (Korea National University of Education)
Mike Long (Yonsei University)
Public Relations
Hoyeol Ryu (Hankyong National University)*
Kyeong-chulk Kim (Dongbu elementary school)
Tae Hyeok Moon (Haun Elementary School)
Ralph Cousins (Pai Chai University)
Research & Development
Sunmi Park (Brigham Young University of Hawaii)*
Heeseong Ahn (Ministry of Education)
Seonmin Huh (Woosong University) Becky Elliott (WnB Press)
Auditor Yong Joon Yang (Jeju National University)
Advisors (January 2014 ~ December 2015)
Richard R. Day (University of Hawai’i, USA)*
Rod Ellis (University of Auckland, New Zealand)*
Alan Maley (Oxford University Press, UK)*
Paul Nation (Victoria University of Wellington, New Zealand)*
Doo-Bon Bae (Hankuk University of Foreign Studies)*
Do-Seon Eur (Korea University)*
Editorial Committee Members (January 2014 ~ December 2015)
Marc Helgesen (Miyagi Gakuin Women's University, Japan)*
Keiko Koda (Carnegie Mellon University, USA)*
Thomas N. Robb (Kyoto Sangyo University, Japan)*
Averil Coxhead (Victoria University of Wellington, New Zealand)*
Melanie Gobert (Higher Colleges of Technology, United Arab Emirates)*
Mark Brierley (Shinshu University, Japan)*
Sang-Soo Yeo (Mokwon University)
Jien Kim (Kwandong University)
Kyungsook Chang (Korea Institute for Curriculum and Evaluation)
Hye Young Kang (National Institute for International Education)
Kyeong-hoon Kim (Kwangju Women’s University)
Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language
Korean English Extensive Reading Association
Vol. 1, No. 1 Winter 2014
________________________________________________________________
Tetsushi Takemori,
Yoko Kodama,
Kriss Lange
How a Japanese Junior College Added
ER to Its English Curriculum
Bjorn Fuisting
Principles and Practice of
Extensive Reading
1
15
Eunsol Shin
Extensive Reading and the English Section
21
of the Korean Scholastic Aptitude Test:
A Diary Study of a Korean High School Student
Sakae Onoda
Investigating Effects of Extensive Listening on 43
Listening Skill Development in EFL Classes
Kiyomi Yoshizawa
Atsuko Takase
Kyoko Otsuki
Will Extensive Reading Help
L2 Learners’ Use of Context
While Processing Text?
57
1
Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language
Vol. 1, No. 1 Winter 2014
How a Japanese Junior College Added ER to its English
Curriculum
TetsushiTakemori
Miyagi University of Education
Yoko Kodama
University of Shimane Junior College
Kriss Lange
University of Shimane Junior College
Takemori, T., Kodama, Y., &Lange, K (2013), How a Japanese Junior College Added
ER to its English Curriculum.Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language.
1(1), 1-13.
ABSTRACT
We started a new English curriculum including ER in 2012. We will report the history and
results of our ER program. Starting as an extracurricular ER activity, we continued to
develop ways to encourage students to read more by developing our program. We discuss
how we made use of ER in our regular classes such as American and British Literature,
English Writing, and Graduation Project. We also discuss our new courses such as
Extensive Reading A and B, and Extensive Listening with the results of improvements in
the students’ English proficiency and in their attitudes toward reading. Our results are based
on survey results, class observations, and proficiency test scores. (110 words)
I. General Background of English Education at Our Two-Year
College
In 2007, we restructured the department and build a new curriculum. The general
trend was toward fewer English classes. Reading classes, including literature classes
especially, were cut back. Before restructuring, there were 8 Literature courses and after
2
Tetsushi Takemori·Yoko Kodama·Kriss Lange
restructuring, only 4 were offered in our two-year curriculum. However, other practical
skill courses such as speaking, listening and writing courses were maintained at 4
courses for speaking, 3 for listening and 4 for writing.These curriculum changes were in
response to the perceived needs of our students who seemed to be more interested in
practical English courses.
II. Changes We have Found in Students’ English Ability
Our most reliable and constant means of evaluating our students’ English proficiency
is with the TOEIC test that we give twice a year. The results of the TOEIC test before
restructuring our curriculum show that a larger part of the gains made were in the
reading section. For example, in 2006 our first and second year student total gains on the
two tests were 11.1 points,with 9.4 pointsgained on the reading section, but only 1.7
points on the listening section.
The same thing can be said in 2007 where the total gains on the two tests were 19.4
points, with 14.3 gained in the reading section and just 5.1 in the listening section.
These proportions clearly favor gains in reading (see Table 1).
TABLE 1
TOEIC Score Gains Over 6 Month Period in 2006 and 2007
1st and 2nd Year
Listening
Reading
Total
2006
1.7
9.4
11.1
2007
5.1
14.3
19.4
Students
After restructuring, the proportion of TOEIC reading and listening score gains began
to change. We began to notice lower gains in TOEIC reading scores. For example, in
2008, almost all of the gains made between the first and second TOEIC tests were from
the listening section (32.8 points) and students only gained an average of 0.4 points on
the reading section. Again in 2010,of the 21.5 points gained on the second TOEIC, 24.7
points came from listening and there was a negative gain of 3.1 points on the reading
sections (see Table 2).1We presumed that our students’ reading proficiency was in
decline, possibly due to factors such as the restructuring of the curriculum with the
decrease of English reading courses.
3
How a Japanese Junior College Added ER to Its English Curriculum
TABLE 2
TOEIC Score Gains Over 6 Month Period in 2008 and 2010
1st and 2nd Year
Students
Listening
Reading
Total
2008
32.8
0.4
33.2
2010
24.7
-3.1
21.5
III. History of Using ER at this Junior College
1. Starting ER with graduation project group
Professor Lange started using ER in his graduation project seminar with a group of 6
students. The results were very promising. The average reading amount was 608,000
words a year, which means 304,000 words in one semester. One student read 1,291,000
words in one year, while another read 809,000 words and both of their TOEIC reading
scores increased by about 80 points during the year. In addition, the average gains on the
TOEIC reading section of the 6 graduation project students were on average 41 points
higher than the average gains of the rest of the second year English majors (see Table 3).
TABLE 3
2009 ER Graduation Project Students’TOEIC Results
Students
One Year Gains on
TOEIC Tests
GP
2008.12.03
A
545
B
435
C
245
D
455
E
305
F
325
315
230
300
135
145
100
305
150
215
90
230
95
2009.06.27
530
420
405
530
290
305
265
265
255
165
200
205
310
220
195
95
205
100
TOEIC
2009.11.25
615
560
345
615
335
235
305
310
345
215
235
110
360
255
225
110
140
95
Total
Words
L.S.
R.S.
T.S.
Read
-10
80
70
1,291,047
45
80
125
809,340
90
10
100
690,906
55
105
160
478,635
10
20
30
325,786
-90
0
-90
52,194
4
Tetsushi Takemori·Yoko Kodama·Kriss Lange
GP
Averages
385
2nd Year
Ave.
413
133
262
English
Major
252
238
451
175
251
441
183
16.7
49.2
65.8
-3.4
6.6
3.4
607,985
258
436
179
268
444
193
186
(n=23)
2. Extracurricular Activities from the 2nd semester of 2009 to the 1st
semester of 2011
Being encouraged by the results of the graduation project, Takemori, Kodama, and
Lange moved toward wider implementation of ER for our students. We received a
special research subsidy from the Universityof Shimane in 2009 and 2010, with which
we could provide our students with more graded readers and leveled readers for ER.
From 2011 to 2013, we received a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the
government. With this grant we increased the books for ER in our library (see Table 4).
TABLE 4
ER Library Publisher and Number of Titles
Publisher
Stages
Titles
Cambridge U. Press
0 to 6
83
Footprints Reading Library
1 to 2
26
Foundations Reading Library
1 to 7
42
I Can Read
0 to 4
195
Ladder Series
1 to 5
128
Macmillan Readers
1 to 6
160
Oxford Reading Tree
2 to 11
196
Oxford Bookworms
0 to 6
242
Penguin Readers
0 to 6
337
Our first activity was to introduce ER to the students through an extracurricular
activity. The following is the history of the extracurricular activities from 2009 to 2011.
1) ER Club 1
In the second semester of 2009, we organized the ER club and advertised it in various
How a Japanese Junior College Added ER to Its English Curriculum
5
English related courses. The activity of the club was simply to get together in a reading
room in the college library and read graded readers and leveled readers using the SSS
style ER method. SSS style means Start with Simple Stories and students were
encouraged to read a lot of easy books without using dictionaries, skipping parts they
didn’t understand and switching to a different book at any time if they wanted.
The number of participants was 14 with 9 English majors and 5 Japanese majors.
We gathered twice a week for 45 minutes, 28 times a semester. Two meetings were used
for the pre and post tests.They generally liked reading in this way so they came to the
library regularly and read. The average reading amount was 62,000 words with the
highest of 85,000 and the lowest of 42,000. We figured out the reading speed as 76
words per minute (wpm), calculating the words read and the time spent (See Table 5).
We gave the students the pre and post Cloze tests to measure the increase in their reading
proficiency.The students made average gains of 6 points on the post test. The 9 English
majors who participated regularly made an average gain of 27.2 points on the reading
section of the TOEIC test.
The average rate of attendance was 81%. Some students came regularly but others
didn’t. This may be because we met after classes and it may have been a little too late for
some of them as one student commented in a questionnaire. We realized that it was
important to consider the meeting times to maximize attendance.
2)
ER Club 2
The activity period for ER Club 2 was in the first semester of 2010. We gathered twice
a week for 45 minutes, 26 times a semester. Two meetings were used for the pre and
post tests as in Club 1. The number of participants was 8 first year students with 7
English majors and 1 Japanese major. One second year Japanese major continued from
Club 1. She was the only one to continue even though many students from Club 1
indicated that they would like to continue doing ER in the questionnaire. The average
reading amount was 57,000 words with the highest of 87,000 and the lowest of 30,000.
Using the same calculation as in Club 1, we figured out the reading speed as 86 wpm.
The one second year student who continued from Club 1 read at 100 wpm. This gain
from the average of 76 wpm in Club 1 indicates the importance of continuing extensive
reading.
On the same Cloze test we used in Club 1, students made an average gain of 15 points.
The one second year continuing student from Club 1 made the highest gain of 27 points
on the post-test. This student read over 150,000 words in Club 1 and 2. Since she was a
Japanese major who took very few English courses, we may be able to attribute these
gains to ER.
The average rate of attendance was 61%. The meeting times were the same as that of
6
Tetsushi Takemori·Yoko Kodama·Kriss Lange
Club 1. Due to scheduling constraints we had to set the club meeting times after classes
again. It was especially difficult to adjust the meeting times to the needs of all students
of different majors and years (see Table 5).
TABLE 5
ER Club from 2009 -2010
ER Club
Year
Number
of
Students
Number
of
Meetings
(45 min)
Attendan
Word
ce
Totals
Rate
(aver)
Highest
Lowest
Total
Total
Reading
Speed
(wpm)
Club 1
(2nd sem.
14
28
81%
62,000
85,000
42,000
76
9
26
61%
57,000
87,000
30,000
86
2009)
Club 2
(1st sem.
2010)
3)
ER Club 3 and ER Club 4
ER Club 3 was in the second semester of 2010. We tried to make several
improvements in Club 3 to increase attendance and amount of reading. After seeing the
results of rather low attendance rates of Club 2, we set the meeting session at 4 times a
week to give choices for the students to fit their own class schedules. But having so
many options for meetings was actually detrimental to attendance. The easier they could
come, the fewer times they came. We also tried a new test to measure the effectiveness
of ER. The Cloze test that we had been using took about 60 minutes and students felt this
was too long. So, we used a new comprehension test from the publisher of Penguin
Readers. Although we gave the pre comprehension test to the students, we could not give
them the post test due to the scarce attendance of the students.
For the activity of Club 3, we made an original ER recording booklet called ‘The First
Stage’ which already includes the YL (readability score for Japanese students), word
numbers and titles of the ER books we have in our library. The students could save
recording time especially of the lower level books by using this booklet. They also used
ready-made, blank recording books after they finished ‘The First Stage’ booklet.
Club 4 was in the first semester of 2011. We gathered twice a week for 45 minutes, 29
times a semester. We tried to find the best possible times for students to meet between
classes. The attendance rate was only 36%.We think this illustrates just how difficult it
can be to maintain attendance in a club activity.
7
How a Japanese Junior College Added ER to Its English Curriculum
3. In-class Reading in Writing Classes and Making Use of Graded
Readers in Literature Classes
1) Writing Classes
In order to share the beneficial effects of ER with more students Lange added in-class
ER to his writing courses for first and second year students in the first semester of the
2010 academic year. Writing 1 for first year students had 14 students. Writing 3 for
second year students had 8 students. Students were given 20 minutes for reading in class.
The first year students’ average in reading amount was about 46,000 words for the
semester while the second year students read about 31,000 wordson average. If we
roughly figure that students read 80 wpm (based on the ER Club’s calculations of 76 and
86 wpm) during the 20 minute in-class ER time, students would have read about 21,000
words, so both groups must have also spent time reading outside of class. Some of the
students taking the course were also regular ER Club members.
Similar implementation of ER was also carried out in Lange’s 2011 writing courses.
Students were given over twice as much time, 45 minutes, in the first year writing
classes for in-class ER. This increase in time for reading, however, did not lead to a
proportional increase in words read. The average reading amount was 60,000 words. If
students read at 80 wpm using the same calculation above to find how many of those
words were read in class,we see that about 47,000 words could have been read in class.
This figure is only 1000 words more than the first year students’ average of 46,000
words in the 2010 Writing Class,even though they had half as much time to read in class.
The first year writing course students in 2011 did in-class ER for 45 minutes but spent
less time reading outside of class compared with those of 2010 who did in-class ER for
20 minutes (see Table 6).
TABLE 6
ER in Writing Courses in 2010 and 2011
Number
Year and Course
of
Students
Expected
Actual
Time for
SSR word
Word
SSR (min.)
totals(80
Totals
wpm)
(aver)
20,800
45,745
24,945
20,800
30,654
9,854
Words
read out
of class
2010
Writing 1 (1st year, 1st
sem.)
Writing 3
(2nd
year, 1st sem.)
2011
16
8
13 classes X
20 min.
8
Tetsushi Takemori·Yoko Kodama·Kriss Lange
Writing 1
(1st
year, 1st sem.)
17
13 classes X
45 min.
46,800
60,000
13,200
This in-class ER was a good chance for more of our students to experience ER. The
average word totals were not that large but some students developed the habit of reading
extensively with one student reaching over 180,000 words.
2) Literature Classes
In 2010 and 2011, ‘Reading British and American Literature B’were offered to second
year students in the second semester. About 35 students took the courses. TheChristmas
Carol (Oxford Bookworms Stage 3, 10,385 words) was assigned as homework over the
winter vacation in both years. After the vacation students took a comprehension test on
the book. The average score was 80%.
In 2011, ‘Reading British and American Literature C’was offered to second year
students in the first semester. 15 students took the course. Moby Dick (Macmillan
Readers Level 6, 34,000 words) was used as the textbook for the course. In class
students focused on understanding the plot, characters and general content of the novel.
In the questionnaires given during the last class period, 60% of the students felt that they
could understand more than 80% of the story. About 70% of them enjoyed reading the
story.
In 2012, ‘Introduction to American Literature’ was offered to the first year students in
the second semester.About 35 students took the course. Pocahontas (Oxford Bookworms
Stage 1, 5,300 words) was assigned as homework over the winter vacation. After the
vacation students took a comprehension test on the book (see Table 7).
TABLE 7
ER in Literature Courses from 2010 to 2012
Year /
Course
Book Title
2nd year,
Reading British and
A Christmas
2nd Sem.
American Literature B
Carol
2nd year,
Reading British and
A Christmas
2nd Sem.
American Literature B
Carol
Sem.
Publisher. /
Word
Homework
Stage
Total
/ In-Class
Oxford 3
10,385
Homework
Oxford 3
10,385
Homework
2010
2011
9
How a Japanese Junior College Added ER to Its English Curriculum
2nd year,
Reading British and
2nd Sem.
American Literature C
Moby Dick
Macmillan
34,000
In-Class
Oxford 3
10,385
Homework
Oxford 1
5,320
Homework
6
2012
2nd year,
Reading British and
A Christmas
2nd Sem.
American Literature B
Carol
1st year,
Introduction to American
2nd Sem.
Literature
Pocahontas
In these literature courses we expected that they could enjoy the story while
improving their reading proficiency by reading famous works of literature.
4. Findings through ER Club activities from 2009 to 2011
Through ER Club activities since 2009, we could obtain the basic information about
reading rate and average amount of words students could read. For example, based on
the club data, the average number of words they can read if meeting twice a week for 45
minutes, 25 times in a semester is 60,000 words. Student average reading speed is about
80 wpm.
We gave a questionnaire to determine student attitudes towards ER and any effects
they might have noticed. Some of their responses were consistently high. For example,
many students felt that their resistance to reading English lessened, reading books was
fun and they’d like to continue to do ER. Nevertheless, student rates of attendance to the
ER Clubs were rather low after Club 1. Also, we should take note that only one student
continued from Club 1 to Club 2. Thismight have been partly because of the difficulty in
arranging an ER club meeting time when students could consistently attend.Another
reason we can think of is that second year students at a two year college must focus on
job-hunting or transferring to a four year university so their time is limited and
continuing extra-curricular activities is difficult.
It is said in Tadoku-Tacho Magazine(vol. 19, pp.10-28), if students read more than
100,000 words, they will notice that their reading comprehension will improve. And,
reading over 150,000 words, reading speed will increase. One student who read over
150,000 words in ER Club 1 and 2 showed higher reading speeds and better reading
comprehension.So, an important first step to continue ER is to help students read over
100,000 words.
10
Tetsushi Takemori·Yoko Kodama·Kriss Lange
IV. New Courses for ER and Extensive Listening
1. Establishing the New Courses
Starting in 2012 we put ER Course A in the first semester for first year students, and
ER Course B and Extensive Listening (EL) in the second semester for first year students
into our curriculum. Through ER club activities since 2009, we could gather the basic
information on what we can expect students to do. For example, the average number of
words they can read over a semester, average gains we can expect on proficiency tests,
how much students can maintain interest in ER and to continue with ER. This
information was valuable when designing these new courses.
By creating courses for ER we could concentrate more directly on motivating students
to read and ensure enough time in class for reading the minimum amount of words. By
setting ER courses in the first and second semesters, students have a chance to continue
reading throughout the year. This creates the continuity needed to reach word totals as
high as 100,000.
2. Implementing the Courses
We started ‘Extensive Reading A’, ‘Extensive Reading B’ and ‘Extensive Listening’
classes for the 2012 entering students. Almost all of our students majoring in English
took these courses but there was a wide range in the number of total words read in each
course.The total reading/listening goal for each course was roughly 100,000 words.
There were a few students who were able to achieve this goal.
Extensive Reading courses A and B both give students about 45 minutes of in-class
reading time. Based on the average reading speed of the students, we figured out the
minimum number of words they can read just by attending classes. From this number we
set the minimum word total requirement for Extensive Reading A at 35,000 words,
Extensive Reading B at 40,000 words and Extensive Listening at 20,000 words. These
totals, plus the words read from literature courses for first year students, should bring the
minimum total number of words to 100,000.
In ER Course A there were 25 students who read an average of 49,000 words. In ER
Course B and Extensive Listening which are offered in the second semester the
combined average word total was 89,000 words. 28 students attended ER Course B and
31 students took EL (see Table 8).
11
How a Japanese Junior College Added ER to Its English Curriculum
TABLE 8
ER Courses Results from 2012
Number
2012 ER/ EL Courses
of
Students
ER Course A
(first semester 2012)
ER Course B
(second semester 2012)
Extensive Listening Course
(second semester 2012)
25
28
31
Expected
Actual
Time for
word total
Word
SSR (min.)
from SSR (at
Totals
80 wpm)
(aver.)
46,800
48,893
13 classes X
45 min.
13 classes X
45 min.
13 classes
X 9 min.
46,800
89,074
9,360
Out of the 22 students who took all three courses, 15 achieved the minimum word
goal of 100,000 words. There were two who read over 300,000 and 6 who read over
200,000. On the other hand, 7 students couldn’t achieve the minimum goal. One of our
challenges from now on is how to help students who couldn’t reach 100,000 words
achieve the minimum word goal.
We examined the TOEIC scores for the first year students according to the average
number of words they read. We found that those who read more than 200,000 words
made average gains of 43 points in the reading section and 78 points in their total score
from the first test in July to the second test in December in 2012. Then on the third
TOEIC test they gained an average of 63 points on the reading section and 120 for the
total score from their first test scores. Students who read between 190,000 and 100,000
words made average gains of 20 points in the reading section and 5 points in their total
score from the first test in July to the second test in December in 2012. Then on the third
TOEIC test they gained an average of 27.5 points on the reading section and 35 for the
total score from their first test scores. These TOEIC scores seem to clearly reflect the
amount of reading the students completed(see Figure 1).
12
Tetsushi Takemori·Yoko Kodama·Kriss Lange
FIGURE 1
One Year TOEIC Gains Grouped by Amount of Words Read
140.0
120.0
120.0
ER Word Total
Groups
100.0
80.0
60.0
57.1
35.0
40.0
20.0
read > 200,000 words
62.9
27.5
7.5
3.8
190,000 - 100,000
words
read < 100,000 words
6.3
2.5
0.0
Listening
Reading
Total
V. Conclusion
Returning to the topic of gains on the TOEIC tests, our students continued to show a
lower percentage of gains on the reading section since the restructuring of the curriculum
as discussed in section II. However, the gains for the 2012 entering students, who began
attending our college after Extensive Reading A, B and EL were added to the curriculum,
are encouraging. Their reading score went up an average of 26.5 points from July 2012
to July 2013. Listening scores also increased by 19 points resulting in a total gain of 45.5
points on the TOEIC in one year. After one year, we could see the expected results of
using ER. Our present challenge is to help more students reach the minimum word goal.
13
How a Japanese Junior College Added ER to Its English Curriculum
APPENDIX
Content Notes for Reference
1
TOEIC Score Summary from 2006 to 2010
1st TOEIC Scores
1st and 2nd year
2nd TOEIC Scores
students
n
L
R
T
n
L
R
T
2006
101
253.3
154.5
407.7
73
254.9
163.9
418.8
2007
97
239.5
164.5
404
84
244.6
178.8
423.3
1st TOEIC Scores
1st year students
2nd TOEIC Scores
n
L
R
T
n
L
R
T
2008
35
220.9
165.4
386.3
30
253.7
165.8
419.5
2010
24
246.7
170.4
417.1
22
271.4
167.3
438.6
Key words: Extensive reading, English curriculum, Japanese Junior College, TOEIC scores
The Authors
TetsushiTakemori
Faculty of Education, Miyagi University of Education
149, Aramaki-aza-Aoba, Aobaku,
Sendai, 980-0845, Japan
Tel: 022-214-3496
E-mail: takemori@staff.miyakyo-u.ac.jp
Yoko Kodama
Department of Arts and Sciences, University of Shimane Junior College
Kriss Lange
Department of Arts and Sciences, University of Shimane Junior College
24-2 Hamanogi 7-chome, Matsue
Shimane, 690-0044, Japan
Tel: 0852-26-5525/0852-20-0228
Fax: 0852-21-8150
E-mail: y-kodama@matsue.u-shimane.ac.jp / k-lange@matsue.u-shimane.ac.jp
This research has been subsidized by a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the government from 2011
to 2013.
15
Principles and Practice of Extensive Reading
Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language
Vol. 1, No. 1 Winter 2014
Principles & Practice of Extensive Reading
Bjorn Fuisting
Ritsumeikan University
Fuisting, Bjorn (2013), Principles & Practice of Extensive Reading. Journal of
Extensive Reading in Foreign Language. 1(1), 15-20.
ABSTRACT
Starting or revamping an Extensive Reading (ER) program can be a daunting task. There
are several factors to consider, including reading materials, objectives, grades, and the
role of the teacher among others. Richard Day and Julian Bamford (2002) have outlined
ten fundamental principles of ER but how can they be used to implement an ER program
from the ground up? This article discusses these ten principles of ER and suggests how
they can be practically applied when planning an ER program, as well as provides tips for
avoiding pitfalls encountered when planning/implementing an ER course. (95 words)
I. Introduction
Extensive Reading (ER) comes in many shapes and forms. A single definition is hard
to come by but in 1990 Susser and Robb summarized various research into their working
definition of “reading (a) of large quantities of material or long texts; (b) for global or
general understanding; (c) with the intention of obtaining pleasure from the text”
(Susser& Robb, 1990, p.165). The definition has since been expanded and explained in
more details and Richard Day and Julian Bamford (2002) have listed ten fundamental
principles of ER. The Day and Bamford principles were inspired by Williams (1986) and
have been developed over the considerable time they have been involved in researching
and promoting ER. This article will discuss these ten principles and suggest how they
can be practically applied when planning an ER program, as well as provide tips for
avoiding pitfalls encountered when planning/implementing an ER course.
16
Bjorn Fuisting
II. Top Ten Principles for Teaching Extensive Reading
These are the top ten principles as originally listed by (Bamford& Day (2002).
1 The reading material is easy
2 A variety of reading material on a wide range of topics must be available
3 Learners choose what they want to read
4 Learners read as much as possible
5The purpose of reading is usually related to pleasure, information and general
understanding
6 Reading is its own reward
7 Reading speed is usually faster rather than slower
8 Reading is individual and silent
9 Teachers orient and guide their students
10 The teacher is a role model of a reader
When the list was published in 2002, two responses where published in the same issue
of Reading in a Foreign Language (Prowse, 2002, and Robb, 2002). Whilst Prowse
mainly concurs with Day and Bamford, he also advocates the use of recordings, Robb
disagrees with the 6th principle ‘Reading is its own reward’. He claims that there need to
be some kind of control function and that reading “to satisfy a course requirement”
might be necessary in order to motivate students to continue to read (Robb, 2002, p.
146).
1. Issues and Advice
The ten ER principles are a good starting point but when putting them into practice some
issues arise, especially when it comes the reading material, covered by principle 1 and 2,
and the purpose, principle 5and6.
1) Reading Material
In addition to Day and Bamford’s principles of easy and varied material, Williams
claims that “In the absence of interesting texts, very little is possible” (1986, p. 42)and
Prowse (2002) stresses that texts should be easy and engaging. This makes selecting
books one of the most crucial aspects of any ER program. In the author’s experience, the
largest difficulties have been in securing enough funds and convincing faculty and staff
to supply material that is easy enough for the students. It is recommended that there
should be few or no unfamiliar items of vocabulary. Hu and Nation (2002) state it is
Principles and Practice of Extensive Reading
17
necessary for readers to have a 98% coverage rate of the vocabulary. However, this may
mean that some college students should be reading very simple texts, such as Reading
Foundation Library or even Building Blocks Library. Both these series are excellent for
lower level students but were rejected to be purchased by a mid-level private university
in Japan due to being deemed beneath university level.As a result, the author had to use
his private funds as well as donated samples to secure enough books at an appropriate
level for the ER program. Once students are able to read at the 200-250 headword level
the choice of material greatly increases, but it can still be a challenge to have your
institution purchase enough books. Having students buy and donate books and using
library or material budgets are other ways of securing funds (see more suggestions in
Waring, 2000). For a successful program you need to have a minimum of three books
per student in the program, preferably more (Extensive Reading Foundation [ERF],
2011). There is also a lot of free materials available that can inspire and improve your
ER program (Fuisting, 2010).
Following the principles of having a variety of topics and engaging material is getting
easier with more and more material being published for the EFL market. The Language
Learner Literature (LLL) awards, administrated by the Extensive Reading Foundation,
can serve as a good guide to high quality books written for learners of English. An
excellent example of a low level but extremely engaging, and sadly always current topic,
is Phil Prowse’s Why? (2008). It deals with the subject of war but is written at a beginner
level of 250 headwords. In terms of variety, it is recommended to include several
publishers’ series, and both fiction and non-fiction, original stories and adaptations, as
well as a variety of genres (ERF, 2011).
2) Purpose
Day and Bamford (2002) state ‘Reading is its own reward’ and other ER advocates
(Prowse, 2002; Williams, 1986) strongly discourage the use of the quizzes and other
forms of tests to check if students have read the books they claim to have completed.
Book talks, reviews and discussions could instead be used to measure if the students
have engaged with the book (Bamford& Day, 2003). Depending on the nature of the
class this approach might be suitable. However, most educational institutions require
some kind of control function and it can be argued that passing a quiz and thereby
increasing the number of words read can be motivational (Robb, 2002). In the author’s
experience the Moodle Reader, developed by Tom Robb at Kyoto Sangyo University,
Japan, is one of the best and easiest to use ER monitoring systems. It can be used for
individual classes or institutional wide ER programs. It covers and impressive 3000+
graded readers and books for young readers and is based on the amount of words read
18
Bjorn Fuisting
(see www.moodlereader.org).
III. Conclusion
The principles stated by Day and Bamford are a very good guide and inspiration for
how to do ER but each educator should look at his or her situation and adapt these
principles as necessary. The students’ and the institution’s needs and capabilities should
be taken into account when deciding how to adjust the principles. The author has broken
down the original ten ER principles into five areas and added some brief advice for each
one that has proven to be successful in the 10 years he has been teaching ER at junior
high school, senior high school and university in Japan.
Reading Material
1 The reading material is easy
2 A variety of reading material on a wide range of topics must be available
Choose material below students’ normal reading level
Few or no unfamiliar items of vocabulary or grammar
Sets of books can be good but have a variety of series
You need a minimum of 3 books per student
Include a variety of genres including non-fiction
Learner’s choice & goals
3 Learners choose what they want to read
4 Learners read as much as possible
Allow students to choose both which level and which books to read
One book per week or a set amount of words per semester/course
Purpose
5The purpose of reading is usually related to pleasure, information and
general understanding
6 Reading is its own reward
Use follow-up activities such as book talks, recommendations & discussions
There are no or minimal tests and/or book reports
If quizzes must be used, the Moodle Reader is highly recommended
Principles and Practice of Extensive Reading
19
Reading style
7 Reading speed is usually faster rather than slower
8 Reading is individual and silent
Start by having sustained silent reading (SSR) during class time
Gradually move to set the reading as homework
Consider adding a Speed Reading course to your class
Teacher’s role
9 Teachers orient and guide their students
10 The teacher is a role model of a reader
Help students select books
Read yourself during SSR to show your love of reading
Apart from the above recommendations on the implementation of the principles of
ER there are also a lot of resources and advice available for starting an ER program. The
Japan Association for Language Teaching (JALT) ER Special Interest Group has
collected some of the major ones on their website (www.ersig.org/drupal-ersig/links) and
an in 2010 Oxford published an excellent book written by ten different researcher and
practitioners on different aspects of ER (Day et al, 2010). Good luck with your ER
program.
REFERENCES
Bamford, J., & Day, R. R. (Eds.).(2003). Extensive reading activities for teaching
language.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Day, R., Bassett, J., Parminter, S., Bullard, N., Furr, M. Prentice, N. Mahmood, M.,
Stewart, D., & Robb, T. (2010).Bringing extensive reading into the classroom: A
practical guide to introducing extensive reading and its benefits to the learner.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Day, R., &Bamford, J. (2002).Top ten principles for teaching extensive reading.Reading
in a Foreign Language 14(2), 136-141.
Extensive Reading Foundation (2011).The Extensive Reading Foundation’s Guide to
Extensive Reading. Japan: Extensive Reading Foundation.
Fuisting, B. (2010). Free ER resources from publishers. ExtensiveReading in Japan 3(1),
20
Bjorn Fuisting
13-14.
Hu, M., & Nation, I. S. P. (2000).Vocabulary density and reading
comprehension.Readingin a Foreign Language 13(1), 403-430.
Prowse, P. (2002). Top ten principles for teaching extensive reading: A response.
Reading in a Foreign Language 14(2), 142-145.
Prowse, P. (2008). Why? Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Robb, T. (2002).Extensive reading in the Asian context -- An alternative view.Reading
in a Foreign Language 14(2), 146-147.
Susser, B., & Robb, T. N. (1990).EFL Extensive reading instruction: Research and
procedure. JALT Journal 12(2) 161-185.
Waring, R. (2000) Guide to the ’Why’ and ’How’ of using graded readers. Japan:
Oxford University Press.
Williams, R. (1986). "Top ten" principles for teaching reading.ELT Journal40(1), 42-45.
Applicable levels: Junior high school, senior high school, university
Key words: Extensive Reading, principles, implementation, practical advice
Author
Bjorn Fuisting,
RitsumeikanUniversityBiwako-Kusatsu Campus.
1-1-1 Noji-higashi,Kusatsu
Shiga 525-8577
JAPAN
Tel: +81-77-561-3946
bjornfuisting@gmail.com
21
Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language
Vol. 1, No. 1 Winter 2014
Extensive Reading and the English Section of the Korean
Scholastic Aptitude Test: A Diary Study of a Korean High
School Student
Shin Eunsol
Korea National University of Education
Eunsol, Shin (2013), Extensive Reading and the English Section of the
Korean Scholastic Aptitude Test: A Diary Study of a Korean High School
Student. Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language.1(1), 21 -41.
ABSTRACT
In the process of learning English, many Korean students feel pressure to achieve good
grades to enter a prestigious university. In such a situation, extensive reading may serve to
encourage students to read English books without pressure.
Consequently, an 8-week
extensive reading individual case study was conducted with an 18-year-old high school
student to assess how it can influence performance on the English section of the Korean
Scholastic Aptitude Test (KSAT). During 8 weeks, a participant read 16 books according
to his level and interest, keeping his reading journals. It was found that the volume of
words is expanded, reading speed and comprehension is enhanced, and grades from mock
tests are improved. The participant also expressed that he could overcome the fear to read
books and solve questions from the Englishsection of the KSAT and raise motivation to
study English through the influence of extensive reading. These findings suggest that
incorporating extensive reading into theexisting English education system in Korea
provides a viable mechanism through which to improve English language learning. (169
words)
22
Shin Eunsol
I. INTRODUCTION
For most Korean high school students, gaining high scores in the Korean Scholastic
Aptitude Test, the most crucial exam to enter the top universities in Korea, could be the
strongest motivating factor for studying English.Yet, many students in Korea are
burdened by studying English, approaching it with tangible results and achievement in
their mind rather than enjoyment.A recent survey of 28,762 elementary, middle, and high
school students conducted by the Seoul Metropolitan Office of Education (2011),
suggests that students are reluctant to study English because they find it boring and
difficult.For example, high school students found English class too boring (33.4%) and
hard to handle (20.5%).However, this is a natural outcome because the high school
curriculum concentrates on preparing students for the college entrance exams, which
have many difficult questions often beyond the ability of the students. Moreover, it
employs a textbook-based approach focusing on grammar teaching, vocabulary learning,
and reading comprehension leading students to harbor a defensive and negative attitude
towards English (Heo, Choi, & Lee, 2009), which suggests thatthe future of English
education in Korea does not look too promising. Another notable result is that students’
responses about improving the effectiveness of English classes included more interesting
classes, improvement of the current classroom environment, and various after-school
programs. From the survey by the Seoul Metropolitan Office of Education (2011), 18.2%
of students also responded that a reading program withEnglish books is needed as the
most efficient way of studying English. Thus, to deal with problems facing the Korean
English education system, a great deal of change is needed, including encouraging
students to study and read English books, as opposed to textbooks, without undue
pressure and a governmentlevel improvement to fix the overall picture of English
education.
For several years, there have been numerous studies indicating that extensive reading
affects reading comprehension and speed (Bell, 2001; Robb & Susser, 1989), vocabulary
gains (Cho & Krashen, 1994; Horst, 2005, Pitts, White, & Krashen, 1989), writing
(Hafiz & Tudor, 1990; Tsang, 1996), and even listening proficiency (Elley & Mangubhai,
1983). These studies offer educators the chance to consider and implement extensive
reading programs in classroom settings. These are quantitative studies with a focus on
whether extensive reading influences students positively.But, there are few qualitative
studies which show what exactly learners are going through and gaining, through an
extensive reading program, sometimes losing the opportunity to observe learners’
situation and viewpoint.
Besides, there have only been few studies specifically focusing on the relevance
between the KSAT and extensive reading until now, except for extensive reading studies
Extensive Reading and the English Section of the Korean Scholastic Aptitude Test:
A Diary Study of a Korean High School Student
23
regarding classroom environment (Lee & Kim, 2007), vocabulary gains (Cha, 2009), and
motivation (Shin & Ahn, 2006) focusing on Korean English education. Consequently,
the present study examines the core relationship between extensive reading and the
English section of the KSAT in a form of a learner’s diary study, which is defined as “a
first-person account of a language learning or teaching experience, documented through
regular, candid entries in a personal journal and then analyzed for recurring patterns or
salient events”(Bailey, 1990, p.215). In other words, this study aims to investigate both
the benefits and challenges that one might have when engaging in extensive reading and
find out how extensive reading affects solving questions from English section in KSAT.
II. EXTENSIVE READING AND KSAT
1. Extensive Reading
The increasing attention to extensive reading is based on Krashen’s (1993) theory
which states that “when second language learners read for pleasure, they can continue to
improve in their second language without classes, without teachers, without study and
even without people to converse with.”Additionally, Krashen’s Input Hypothesis (1982,
1997) and Comprehension Hypothesis (2004) may support the value of extensive
reading, asserting that comprehensible input and low affective filter should be
considered as the prime need. Krashen also suggests that reading materials must be
abundantly available, comprehensible, and of a slightly high-level input.
Furthermore, extensive reading acts as an implicit learning system (Grabe, 2009, p. 63).
Table 1, drawn from Grabe (2009)outlines the way that reading skills are acquired as
implicit knowledge through extensive reading.
Table 1
Reading Tasks that Involve Implicit and Explicit Learning
Implicit Learning
Explicit Learning
Improving word recognition with known
Learning new words on first encounters
words through fluent text reading and
through explicit definitions
rereading
Stabilizing and expanding word meanings by
Learning new word meanings by noticing
being exposed to words again in similar and
new uses of words and figuring out their
new contexts
meanings
24
Shin Eunsol
Expanding word meanings through multiple
Learning
incidental contacts with a word in consistent
intentionally making inferences from context
new
word
meanings
by
supporting contexts
information
Processing letter-sounds correspondences by
Attending to letter-sound correspondence by
reading easy texts
direct instruction
Increasing word reading fluency through large
Attending to words that are not well
amounts of extensive reading
established by noting words that reappear of
intentionally using the word
Increasing text reading fluency by rereading
Intentionally practicing passage reading to
texts and by extensive reading with easy texts
improve reading accuracy
2. Korean SAT
The Korean College Scholastic Aptitude Test measures students’aptitudein the areas
necessary for college education and makes students choose what subjects they take for
the test, as a credible and objective resource for universities to select students while also
aiming to improve the academic standards of public school system. The English section
ofthe KSAT exam demands that students have comprehensive linguistic ability. It
assesses communicative competence and thinking skills for college-level inference
ability at the same time. There are four types of questions in the test: listening, speaking,
reading, and writing to assess language abilityaround vocabulary, grammar, and thinking
skill, emphasizing both accuracy and fluency. Examination questionsaim to make the
best use of all textbooks published for the year and to avoid introducing a particular
subject and textbook. The level of vocabulary is decided on according to frequency in
optional subjects or intensive classes. In other words, the English section ofthe KSAT
exam demands that students have comprehensive linguistic ability. Thus, whether a
single method of teaching English is appropriate for such a wide variety of skills is
unclear. Consequently, some language instructors in Korea are increasingly seeking
alternative approaches to teaching English but the problem still remains that many
students struggle to concentrate on target language and finding effective ways to produce
better grades in the KSAT. Introducing extensive reading may complement the existing
approach and increase student’s performance in the KSAT.
There have been various papers about the effectiveness of extensive reading and
analysis of KSAT, but the purpose of this experimentation is to let students, parents, and
teachers know how extensive reading influences students, as an initial attempt to relate
extensive reading to KSAT.
Extensive Reading and the English Section of the Korean Scholastic Aptitude Test:
A Diary Study of a Korean High School Student
25
Here are research questions related with what is arranged above.
1. How does the extensive reading help a low-ranking high school student to improve
vocabulary, reading speed and comprehension, and especially KSAT scores?
2. What challenges does a student confront in the extensivereading process and how did
the learner deal with these challenges?
With these research questions, the present study sought to take an exploratory approach
to the effectiveness of extensive reading in relation to the KSAT, adopting an in-depth
case study with a single participant. A case study provides an illuminating, in depth
approach to the understanding of a phenomenon (Rowley, 2000). Consequently, this
approach was adopted as it was considered most likely to provide a thorough
understanding of how and why extensive reading may or may not support Korean
English Language teaching presently.
III. METHOD
1. The participant
The participant in this case study is 18-year-old high school student. He comes from a
middle class family in Korea. He has lived in Korea all his life and never been to any
English-speaking countries before. For the last four years, he has spent his time and
energy concentrating on athletic achievement such as boxing and taekwondo to get into
college on his merits as a sportsman.Even though he has been studying English for about
eight years from elementary school and knows the alphabet, basic words, and some
phrases, he has always remained in the lowest ranks of his school and been reluctant to
study English. In school, he has studied English, using a textbook according to the
regular curriculum of his school and an extra workbook which deals with listening and
reading comprehension for the KSAT.
2. Procedure
The participant was met twice a weekfor 8 weeks from September 17 to November 10
of 2012. Two graded readers a week were provided as homework. Each book has several
chapters, from a minimum of five to a maximum of seven chapters.In extensive reading,
students are asked to divide these chapters into seven parts to provide a reading for each
day of the week. Each session began by checking what he had studied for a week in and
out of school and inspecting his assignment of reading graded readers, providing him
26
Shin Eunsol
with worksheets and asking him the contents of what he read in order to check whether
he actually read the books or not. After reading each book, the student should write short
essay in Korean regarding what he read as a form of review and his reaction to the story.
He was also supposed to write all the unknown words from the books in his journal. The
books he read were borrowed from Sogang University library and classified according to
their level.
The following table shows the books the participant was supposed to read on a daily
basis.
Table 2
Book Lists
Publisher
Level
Words Num.
Simon and the Spy
Penguin Readers
Beginner
200
Give us the Money
OXFORD
Starter
250
Survive!
OXFORD
Starter
250
The White Stones
OXFORD
Starter
250
Oranges in the Snow
OXFORD
Starter
250
Orca
OXFORD
Starter
250
Starman
OXFORD
Starter
250
Sally’s Phone
OXFORD
Starter
250
OXFORD
Starter
250
OXFORD
Starter
250
Penguin Readers
Beginner
300
The Adventures of Tom Sawyer
Penguin Readers
Beginner
300
The Wrong Man
Penguin Readers
Beginner
300
Amazon Rally
Penguin Readers
Beginner
300
Penguin Readers
Beginner
300
Penguin Readers
Beginner
300
Connecticut Yankee In King
Arthur’s Court
Mystery in London
The Gift of the Magi and Other
Stories
Marcel and the Shakespeare
Letters
A Biker’s Ghost
Whenchoosing what he would read, he was allowed to pick what he felt attracted to
from a number of books presented to him. However, he was guided to choose books with
95% of known words and 5% of unknown words (Dubin & Olshtain, 1993) in order not
to be confused by a number of unknown words, encouraging him to select books of as
different a genre and topics as possible. Uponfinding unknown words, he wrote them
down on the reading journal and was not allowed to look up dictionary immediately after
Extensive Reading and the English Section of the Korean Scholastic Aptitude Test:
A Diary Study of a Korean High School Student
27
encountering unknown words. Instead, he was encouraged to figure out what these
words were from the context, and allowed to search the dictionary after reading the
whole story.
When he felt bored toward the book he selected before starting to read in earnest, he
could change to recharge his enthusiasm. To be a good model for the student, the
researcher read the same books he picked and talked about the contents of the books
after reading with him. For pleasure reading, a heavy burden of testing was not imposed
on him and it was explained to him that extensive reading was more about what he read
and experienced as beneficial tools for his linguisticability rather than to judge him on
his capability, even though I prepared after the reading activity and reading journal for
each graded reader. It was also emphasized that reading the graded reader silently as
homework was like having a sort of relationship with books and experience of extensive
reading affected how he learns a foreign language deeply related to his personal life.
3. Instruments and Analysis
There were five methods to estimate what the participant has gained during the whole
period of experimentation: vocabulary tests (pre and post tests), reading speed (pre and
post tests), reading comprehension tests (pre and post tests), mock tests (pre and post
tests), and his daily journals. Under conditions of dealing with graded readers for 8
weeks, vocabulary expansion, progress in reading speed and comprehension, improved
KSAT mock test scores, and confidence regarding both exams and the language itself
would be defined as improved English. Tools for the tests of vocabulary, reading speed
and comprehension are extracted from the English exams in the KSAT, and mock tests
from professional organizations which are the most similar to the KSAT for judging if a
student is influenced by the graded reader books so as to deal with English test. During
the experimentation, the participant was not allowed to take other official mock tests. As
this study has a purpose of expressing how extensive reading approach influences on a
participant in detail, followingstudent’s reading experience with his journals and
observing his change is meaningful to the results.
Firstly, the vocabulary test was comprised of a word test developed by Lauferand
Nation (1999) through a website for word testing to verify the general vocabulary ability
and a vocabulary test form with actual words on the English exam of the KSAT from
2009 to 2011, which was devised by the researcher. In order to avoid the participant
getting a hint from similar word forms arranged together, words were mixed as randomly
as possible. Secondly, reading speed and comprehension ability was measured
respectively with exam sheets designed by the researcher, which consisted of 33 random
questions extracted from English section of KSAT in 2009– 2011. Because questions set
28
Shin Eunsol
at those examinations were taken from careful consideration of eminent scholars in
Korea and verified already, the intension of assessing the participant by these questions
seems fair. His scholastic level was also measured by mock tests on April 10 from
Visangedu (English education institution), June 7 from Korea Institute for Curriculum
and Evaluation (KICE,), and November 14 from Gyeonggido Office Education. The last
method to judge what he earned from the treatment was his daily journals which
included his whole process and history of reading experience – words entries, summary,
and reaction to the story in English. Iparticularly guided him to write his feelings in
terms of development in his reading in Korean, because it could be the most important
standard to write his candid reaction to the language for judging his English ability from
what he was saying.
This study focuses on the question about how reading graded readers actually affects a
student’s grades on the English test of the KSAT. That is why tools are the tests of
vocabulary, reading speed and comprehension extracted from the English exams in the
KSAT, and mock tests from professional organizations which are the most similar to the
KSAT for judging if a student is influenced by the graded reader books so as to deal with
English test. During the experimentation, the participant was not allowed to take other
official mock tests. As this study has a purpose of expressing how extensive reading
approach influences on a participant in detail, thestudent’s reading experience was
chronicled with his journals and by observing his change is meaningful to the results.
Figure 1
Five Instruments According to Time Order
Extensive Reading and the English Section of the Korean Scholastic Aptitude Test:
A Diary Study of a Korean High School Student
29
IV. RESULTS
1. Vocabulary
During the treatment, the student read two books per week, totaling 16 books. For the
first three weeks, he found more than 45 unknown words, about 3 to 4 words per page,
from each book and there seemed to be little difference about word gains. However, it
was 3 weeks later that the number of unknown words started to reduce gradually. As the
experiment entered the sixth week, the numbers seemed to increase to 95 words but the
books he read, The Gift of the Magi and Other Stories and The Adventures of Tom
Sawyer, have 26 pages and 300 headwords, meaning this discrepancy can be explained
by asignificant increase in length of readings.Before arriving at the sixth week, he had
read books of 250 headwords. To sum up, when taking a close look at the trend of words
gain through the whole experimentation, it is easy to verify how he could accumulate
vocabulary from graded readers.
When analyzing his journals concerning unknown words, several characteristics are
found. First, the pattern of unknown words he displayed, including all kinds of
inflectional morphemes, lasted for 3 weeks. For example, he could not distinguish the
difference between catch and catches, long and longest, asking the meaning of shine,
shining, and shines. Secondly, in the first week, he could not recognize what is a proper
noun or a common noun. He wrote Skaran and Langrun, which are the name of places,
as unknown words in the second week. Yet, he stopped writing those words from the
fourth week and presumably could sense the difference. Finally, in his journals, some
unidentified words are repeated over and over again, such as suddenly, thief, liquid,
nobody, through, etc. Once he was informed that he wrote the same words several times
in the third week, he started to pay attention to what he wrote before taking notes and did
not write the same words on his journal whatsoever from the fourth week.
2. Reading speed and comprehension
In the reading material for the pre-test, there were five questions with 630 words and
the participant was asked to check the answer without being pressured by time limitation,
having enough time to solve each question. Five post-test reading materials have 678
words and the environment for solving the questions is uniform with that of the pre-test.
In the pretest, it took the participant more than 10 minutes to complete the answers and 8
minutes in the post test. Out of 5 questions, he checked 3 correct answers in both tests. In
the situation that time is one of the most critical elements in the exams, he reduced more
than 2 minutes, which draws a conclusion that he shows marked improvement in this
section and possibility of completing questions on time as well.
30
Shin Eunsol
Figure 2
Comparison of Pre- and Post-Reading Speed in wpm
Reading Speed (wpm)
100
80
60
40
20
0
Reading Speed
pretest
posttest
58.1
76.75
Reading comprehension test was proceeded by exam papers. Reading materials were
made especially for both pre and post test including 33 questions (the same number of
questions in KSAT) respectively. Questions were taken out of 2009 – 2011 KSAT
English section test papers and they are mixed spontaneously, abiding by the order of
questions in KSAT. The number of correct answers and the number of the whole
questions are subjected to the following formula:
# correct answers
# of the whole questions (33)
× 100 = reading comprehension scores
Figure 3
Reading Comprehesion
Score
Reading Comprehension Score
80
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
Reading
Comprehension
pretest
posttest
27
69
Extensive Reading and the English Section of the Korean Scholastic Aptitude Test:
A Diary Study of a Korean High School Student
31
When taking a close look at his journals, especially in terms of reading comprehension,
there is a gradual improvement. It can be seen from the following journal entries, which
are translated to English by the researcher:
Journal for week 1
“WhenI solved questions in the reading comprehension test from a teacher and questions
from mock tests in school, they were too difficult for me to handle, because I did not
know most of the words in the paragraphs and honestly, I did not understand what the
paragraphs were about. I also felt difficulty in decoding meaning of words in the books
the research gave me.”
Journal for week 3
“Compared to questions from mock tests or workbook in school, words in the books I
read are quite easy for me to deal with. Until last week, I had had hard time
understanding and processing what I read, but I think I could grasp the flow of contents
in the graded readers.”
Journal for week 5
“I like reading comic books rather than normal books with just texts. I also enjoy seeing
pictures on certain pages, because I can guess the next episode. As I read the graded
readers for the last few weeks, I think the books I read are quite appropriate for my
level.”
Journal for week 8
“I used to loathe studying English only because I could not understand what it is about.
But I think I have confidence from reading graded readers in that I could actually
understand contents from the context. I could read one chapter without pausing and
concentrate on what I read without distraction, but only if contents were interesting.”
Improvement of reading comprehension did not occur in a single day. He received the
opportunity to practice and adjust reading materials that he is dealing with from the
experience of extensive reading for eight weeks. He often expressed that he was getting
better when it comes to reading comprehension and showed satisfaction with graded
readers because they did not force him to read against his will. As far as contents are
easy and interesting, reading graded readers is effective for reading comprehension and
makes learners have confidence about the target language.
3. Mock Test
The participant took a mock test as a pre-test before experimentation and one mock
test as a post-test for confirming the difference of grades between pre- and post-test.
32
Shin Eunsol
Themock test in June proved to be his second examination as a mock test in this year. He
confessed that he did not actually solve the questions from the test sheets, checked the
answers randomly and fell asleep, even though he controlled himself not to spoil his
exam.
Table 3
Mock Test Grades Before and After the Treatment
Date
June 7, 2012
November 14, 2012
Scores out of 100
29
67
After the whole experiment, he scored a 67 on the posttest. He mentioned that he had
concentrated on the test felt strained unlike the other times because he took this exam
after a committed attempt to study English. He was confident that he could do welland
expected better results this time. When compared to the grades of the test in June, 38
points, his achievements in the post-test could provide a greater opportunity to apply for
universities because it would be difficult to be accepted by universities with such low
grades. In the interview, he expressed excitement and enthusiasm when taking the last
exam in that he could actually read questions and solve them with low inhibition and
higher confidence. He also mentioned in the following journal entry that doing extensive
reading helped him to read fast and see the whole context of the paragraphs he deals with
even when he solved questions on mock tests.
Journal for week 6
“WhenI solve the questions from workbook in school, I used to translateparagraphs into
Korean in my head, because I cannot help but do it. If not, I could not process what I
read clearly. In contrast, when I read graded reader, I do not have to translate contents I
read in Korean. It sometimes confuses me in that I can read many English words and
phrases in graded reader books without stopping and specific translation but when
dealing with questions for preparing KSAT, I need to concentrate on the text much more
thoroughly and scrutinize even one sentence for a few minutes.”
That is, his attitude toward reading materials in English seems to be different from the
initial phase of the experiment. Of course, difficult questions require a learner to
translate them into Korean and graded readers do not. After experimentation he pointed
out that he did feel confusion when dealing with different texts of the KSAT questions
and contents in graded reader books. However, he also emphasized that even
whenstudyingEnglish questions in school and taking mock test in November, he felt the
ratio of reading English texts in English had climbed up. After the period of adjustment,
Extensive Reading and the English Section of the Korean Scholastic Aptitude Test:
A Diary Study of a Korean High School Student
33
reading habit without translation can be earned by experience of extensive reading and
the learner’s desperate effort.
V. DISCUSSION
This study found that extensive reading facilitated achievement within the English
language section of the KSAT. The participant achieved increased performance across all
sections of the test and illustrated more confidence and interest in English through his
learning diary.
The relevance between extensive reading and the English section of the KSAT should
be discussed here in terms of research questions regarding vocabulary, reading speed and
comprehension, and miscellaneous aspects of learning English with results in this study.
First, a low-ranking learner’s limited lexical ability which particularly lacks delexical
verbs, collocations, and basic words could be filled with an experience of reading graded
readers in addition to intensive drills of difficult and professional words for gaining high
grades on the KSAT. Secondly, regarding reading speed and comprehension, the
situation that students should squeeze in more accurate answers of the KSAT in a smaller
time frame needs a new idea of applying extensive reading into the current intensive
reading program. That is because the benefits followed by extensive reading makes a
slow reader like the study participant exposed to a repetitious training of reading a wide
range of materials, graded readers, as an eclectic way of teaching English. Lastly, while
the English section of the KSAT contains many questions related to cultural factors,
students at low levels including the participant in this study were not used to
understanding cultural points from each question. Also, without motivation and
confidence to read texts in English boosted by extensive reading, it would be very
challenging to solve many questions in the exam of KSAT as well. Thus, the results say
that books with lot of cultural information and driving force to read eventually help the
subject to reach advanced levels.
In addition to the implication of this study, measuring the effectiveness of extensive
reading in practical research should be debated to provide a challenge as researchers aim
to measure effective in a realistic and understandable way of parents and educators alike.
For instance, the present study investigating relationship between extensive reading and
TOEIC scores implies that extensive reading intrigues automaticity and reading fluency
with TOEIC scores increased over the 10 weeks of the investigation (Storey, Gibson, &
Williamson, 2006). Also,influence of extensive reading on TOEFL score progress was
shown to be effective (Constantino, Lee, Cho, &Krashen, 1997). However, this approach
was chosen as it was considered the most productive method to garner attention towards
34
Shin Eunsol
extensive reading if visible benefits were found. Specifically, the goal of this study was
to consider extensive reading as a way of improving scores for the KSAT
becauseEnglish is commonly studied for practical reasons, such as gaining good grades
on the KSAT. In Japan, Furukawa (2008) examinedthe effects of extensive reading on
the Japanese equivalent of the KSAT. The results from that study lead many Japanese
language instructors to adoptextensive reading as their approach to teaching English and
they intuitively realize that their students need a large quantity of English input through
extensive reading (Schmidt, 1996). Thus, extensive reading research focusing on English
proficiency tests including the KSAT and school examination should be done so as to
grab the attention of Korean students, parents, and teachers.
However, some limitations of this study exist. Foremost among these is the use of a
single participant as a case study. For example, in single-subjects design it is not
uncommon for researchers to do the treatment for a month and check progress, then
withdraw the treatment, for two weeks, and see how progress has changed, before
restarting the treatment for another two weeks or one month. This is called an A-B-A
design in single-subjects research (Christensen, 1941). So, not doing this is another
limitation of this study. There could have been a two week period where the student did
not engage in extensive reading class with me, seeing if his results dipped again, and
then restarting the meetings with him, and measuring if it worked. Without comparing to
baseline1 responses recorded before and after the treatment for certain period of time,
his response to the treatment condition cannot be fully guaranteed. Thus, extra
observation is necessary for seeing whether his improved English ability lasts long after
the treatment. Another limitation might be that extra time taking English classes with the
researcher could have caused him to become more passionate and more interested in
English during his class times. So while extensive reading provided a benefit, some of
his improvement could also, potentially, be attributed to his increased concentration in
class or situation of being tested.
Jones (1994) suggests that if the purpose of a case study is to find out what is involved
via the process of learning, records from a subject are what subjectivity represents.
Fortunately, this studyprovides positive results about extensive reading in a short period
of time in anenvironment of learning that is optimized for a single student. Nevertheless,
further studies of extensive reading that increase the number of participants in a variety
of situations will provide a more assertive and substantial exploration of the
effectiveness of extensive reading.
1
The target behavior of the subject in its naturally occurring stage or prior to
presentation of the treatment condition
Extensive Reading and the English Section of the Korean Scholastic Aptitude Test:
A Diary Study of a Korean High School Student
35
VI. CONCLUSION
This case study examined the impact of extensive reading on vocabulary, reading
comprehension, and grades of the English test in the KSAT from the perspective of a
low-ranking Korean high school student.Reading materials with 16 graded readers were
offered to a student for 8 weeks and words and questions from an English test of the
KSAT were used to measure the impact of extensive reading on student performance.
Since this research is a qualitative study, conclusions cover what a student has gained
from experimentation in a close association with the researcher. Results are discussed
according to vocabulary gains, reading speed and comprehension, and relevance of the
KSAT with extensive reading.
Regardingvocabulary gains, the participant seems to add steadily to his stock of words
during the 8 week intervention. Throughout this time, he kept his diaries in a
conscientious manner, writing down unknown words from the books. As a result of his
limited initialEnglish vocabulary, he illustrated word recognition patterns at the
elementary school level, such as confusion regarding inflectional morpheme,
classification of nouns, and the repetition of writing down the same words. Over time,
and especially from the fifth week, these patterns seemed to fade away and he could
understand words which used to be challenging for him without pausing while reading.
Improvement was also shown in the measurement test using common words and
authentic vocabulary from KSAT. This suggests that beginning reading graded readers
allowed him to accumulate words from the very beginning level.
Secondly, his improvement was manifest in term of reading speed and comprehension.
His journals suggest that he gradually grasped the meaning of the context from an initial
state of no understanding to that of overall comprehension. When taking the
measurement tests of reading speed and comprehension with actual reading questions
from KSAT, he demonstrated progress on both speed and comprehension by showing
that reading speed increased from 58.1 wpm to 76.75 wpm and reading scores from 27 to
69. It means that reading graded readers lead him to gain speed reading with a better
understanding of text.
Finally, his grades on an English section of a mock test indicated progress, showing a
large gap between pre and post tests. When he took mock test as a pretest in June, he
belonged to the sixth grade according to the graded classification of KSAT2. After the
treatment, he got a score of 67, which corresponds to the fourth grade, on the English
section of mock test in November. Even though he trained himself in preparing for the
KSAT and the mock test with workbooks in school, his progress on mock test can also
2
The graded classification of KSAT is to divide all KSAT applicants into nin
e groups of grade from one (prime) to nine (the lowest).
36
Shin Eunsol
be attributed to experiencing extensive reading to a certain degree. That is because he
strongly expressed reading graded reader helped him go smoothly on the test which he
used to be stuck in with many obstacles such as difficult words, structure, and long
passages. His experience of extensive reading enabled him to overcome his fear to
comprehend the overwhelming amount of text for the KSAT in that he had to deal with
one book as a whole, even though it is a very easy one, not a single paragraph drill.
Overall, the experience of extensive reading from the viewpoint of the once poor
student who did not have motivation to study English suggests thought-provoking issue.
In other words, there is a possibility for extensive reading to exert a strong influence on
many students who are going through learning difficulties, especiallyrelated to studying
English. The use of interesting contents with an abundant amount of cultural exposure
from appropriate level books without inhibition are beneficial to a student to improve
vocabulary and reading comprehension. Furthermore, the positive habits gained through
extensive reading also affect grades on theEnglishsection of the KSAT. In this manner, if
recognition grows of the effectiveness of extensive reading for studying a second
language, there could be many viable options for a proper system of English educationin
Korea.
REFERENCES
Board of Education in Seoul (2011).Research and Development of English Education of
Schools in Seoul-focused on human resources and placementAhn, B. K., & Shin, I. S. (2006). The effects of different types of extensive reading
materials on reading amount, attitude, and motivation. English Teaching, 61(1),
67-88.
Bell, T. (2001). Extensive reading: Speed and comprehension. The Reading Matrix, 1.
Retrieved from
http://www.readingmatrix.com/archives/archives_vol1_no1.html
Bailey, K. M. (1990). The use of diary studies in teacher education programs. In J. C.
Richards & D.Nunan (Eds.), Second language teacher education (pp. 215-226).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cha, J. E. (2009). The effects of extensive reading on enhancing vocational high school
students' L2 vocabulary &reading rates.English Teaching, 64(3), 3-30.
Cho, K..&Krashen, S. (1994). Acquisition of vocabulary from the Sweet Valley Kids
Extensive Reading and the English Section of the Korean Scholastic Aptitude Test:
A Diary Study of a Korean High School Student
37
series: Adult ESL acquisition. Journal of Reading, 37(8), 662-667.
Christensen, L. (1941). Experimental methodology.Allyn and Bacon.
Constantino, R., Lee, S., Cho, K. S.&Krashen, S. (1997). Free voluntary readingas a
predictor of TOEFL scores. Applied Language Learning, 8, 111–118.
Dubin, F.,&Olshtain, E. (1993).Predicting word meanings from contextual clues:
Evidencefrom L1 readers. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation.
Elley, W. B., &Mangubhai, F. (1983).The impact of reading on second language
learning.Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 53–67.
Furukawa, A. (2008). Extensive reading from the first day of English learning.Extensive
Reading in Japan, 1(2), 11-14.
Grabe, W. (2009).Reading in a second language. New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Hafiz, F.,& Tudor, I. (1990).Graded readers as an input medium in L2
learning.System,18(1), 31-42.
Heo, Y. S., Choi, J. Y., & Lee, W. J. (2009).Comparing motivation to learn of reading
classes between Korean and American middle school students.The Journal of
Yeolin Education 17(4), 45-63.
Horst, M. (2005).Learning L2 vocabulary through extensive reading: A measurement
study.The Canadian Modern Language Review, 61, 355–382.
Jones,F. R. (1994). The lone language learner: A diary study. System, 22(4), 441-454.
Krashen, S. D. (1982). Principles and practice in second language acquisition. New
York: Prentice-Hall.
Krashen, S. (1993).The power of reading: Insights from the research. Englewood, Co.:
Libraries Unlimited.
Krashen, S. (2004).Applying the comprehension hypothesis: Some
suggestions.International Journal of Language Teaching, 1, 21-29.
Laufer, B.,& Nation, P. (1999).A vocabulary size test of controlled productive
ability.Language Testing 16(1), 33-51.
Lee, H. J.,& Kim, M. J. (2007).A case study of implementing a level-basedextensive
reading program in school settings.Modern English Education, 8(1),151-170.
Mason, B., &Krashen, S. (1997). Extensive reading in English as a foreign
language.System, 25(1), 91-102.
Pitts, M., White, H., &Krashen, S. D. (1989).Acquiring second language vocabulary
38
Shin Eunsol
through reading: A replication of the clockwork orange study using second
language acquirers.Reading in a Foreign Language, 5, 271–275.
Robb, T. N., &Susser, B. (1989).Extensive reading vs. skills building in an EFL
context.Reading in a foreign language, 5(2), 239-251.
Schmidt, K. (1996). Extensive reading in English: Rationale and possibilities for a
program at Shirayurigakuen.Sendai ShirayuriGakuen Journal of General
Research, 24, 81–92.
Shin, I. S.,&Ahn, B. K. (2006). The effects of different types of extensivereading
materials on reading amount, attitude, and motivation. English Teaching,61(1),
67-88.
Storey, C., Gibson, K., & Williamson, R. (2006). Can extensive reading boostTOEIC
scores? In K. Bradford-Watts, C. Ikeguchi, & M. Swanson (Eds.), JALT2005
conference proceedings, 1004–1018. Tokyo, Japan: JALT.
Tsang, W.K. (1996). Comparing the effects of reading and writing on writing
performance.Applied Linguistics, 17, 210–233.
APPENDIX
A Journal Example by the Participant
Extensive Reading and the English Section of the Korean Scholastic Aptitude Test:
A Diary Study of a Korean High School Student
39
40
Shin Eunsol
Extensive Reading and the English Section of the Korean Scholastic Aptitude Test:
A Diary Study of a Korean High School Student
Applicable levels: high school and university levels
Key words: extensive reading, Korean scholastic aptitude test, low level language learner, diary
study
Author
Eunsol Shin
Korea National University of Education,
San 7 DarakriGangnaemyeonChengWonGunChungBuk 363-791, Korea
Tel: 82-010-6656-5254
Fax: 82-43-223-2207
E-mail: dizzyraining@gmail.com
41
43
Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language
Vol. 1, No. 1 Winter 2014
Investigating effects of extensive listening on listening skill
development in EFL classes
Sakae Onoda
Kanda University of International Studies, Japan
Onoda, Sakae (2013), Investigating effects of extensive listening on listening skill
development in EFL classes. Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language. 1(1),
43-55.
ABSTRACT
This study investigated the effects of an extensive listening project on the development of
listening skills and confidence improvement of university English majors over one
academic year. The research draws on the listening skill development instruction
suggested by the four strands of teaching (Nation & Newton, 2009). Nation and Newton
claim that listening skills will be improved by engaging in meaning-focused input and
fluency development tasks using easy and interesting materials for extended periods of
time. In the present study, 30 first-year intermediate-level English majors at a university
engaged in an extensive listeningactivity: they were required to listen to or watch
materials they selected from a large selection of online listening materials for 30 minutes
a day. The results show that the approach improved student listening skills as measured
using a listening test and confidence as measured using a questionnaire. (141 words)
.
I. INTRODUCTION
The importance of extensive listening is well documented in language learning. L2
literature (Dunkel, 1986; Ellis: 2008; Rost, 2002; Vandergrift, 2007) indicates that a
large amount of exposure to input, whether visual or aural, is vital for language
acquisition. This seems to be supported by Nation and Newton (2009) who argue that
44
Sakae Onoda
listening is the natural precursor to speaking and that the early stages of language
development, whether the first language or other languages, are dependent on listening.
Thus, the important role listening plays suggests that listening instruction should aim to
improve listening fluency (i.e., the ability to automatically understand the main points of
texts and interpret speakers’ feelings using top-down-strategies) as well as to promote
basic listening skills (i.e., the ability to identify words and phrases using bottom-up
skills).
In this regard, Nation and Newton’s (2009) four strands of teaching approach is worth
considering and employing in order to effectively improve language skills. The four
strands are meaning-focused input, language-focused learning, meaning-focused output,
and fluency development. They suggest that a language course ideally has about 25
percent of each of these four strands. Different kinds of activities can be used in each
component to promote language learning. For the development of listening fluency, tasks
from meaning-focused input and fluency-development strands such as listening to easy
stories, repeated listening to CDs, and extensive listening are effective; all of these
promote automaticity of particular language items through multiple encounters and
retrieval of them, therefore enhancing listening fluency. Among them, extensive listening
(i.e., listening to easy and interesting materials for extended periods of time) that
subsumes the first two tasks appears to be most promising. According to Nation and
Newton (2009), extensive listening must meet the following four requirements of the
meaning-focused input and fluency strands:
(1) learners listen to what is largely familiar to them in terms of vocabulary, content, and discourse
features (i.e., encouraging the use of top-down skills),
(2) learners focus on meaning (i.e., intrinsically interesting texts),
(3) some pressure or encouragement is applied on the learners to perform at a faster than usual
speed, and
(4) learners are exposed to a large amount of input (i.e., repeated practice of language items).
Thus, Nation and Newton’s four strands of teaching appear to substantiate the use of
an extensive listening approachto improve student listening fluency and confidence.
Here note that in extensive listening, learners are exposed to language items
repeatedly, which promotes automatization. It is well documented that automatization is
necessary for all kinds of skill development. Anderson’s (1983) ACT theory postulates
that the development of skills entails the transformation of conscious describable types
of knowledge into more unconscious and automatic types of knowledge that can be
available at the time of use. The theory implies that it is repeated practice that can
facilitate unconscious and automatic processing, and thereby, skill acquisition (DeKeyser,
Investigating Effects of Extensive Listening on Listening Skill Development in EFL Classes
45
2007). It appears that the theory can apply to L2 language learning, particularly listening
fluency development. Abundant literature holds that automatization, facilitated by
multiple encounters and retrievals of language items (Nation, 2007; Schmitt & Carter,
2004), enhances L2 listening and speaking fluency (Nation & Newton, 2009; Wood,
2001). Automatic linguistic processing requires little working memory capacity when a
particular language unit is recognized and retrieved in its entirety. Therefore, attentional
resources allow for the processing of other information and enable the listener and
speaker to process larger pieces of information efficiently.
Although there has been limited research on the effects of extensive listening, some
studies indicate that it is effective for improving listening fluency and confidence in
language learning.
One of them is Millet’s (2010) pedagogical intervention utilizing the QuickListen
approach that adopted easy story listening, a task categorized by Nation and Newton as
meaning-focused input and fluency development. The results indicate that listening to
easy and interesting stories over the course of a year enhances both listening skills and
confidence.
Similar results were yielded in Onoda’s study (2012). He investigated the effects of
QuickListens (Millet, 2010) in tandem with extensive listening assignments on listening
skill and confidence development in university English majors over a term. The
intervention drew on meaning-focused input and fluency development tasks suggested in
the four strands of teaching (Nation & Newton, 2009) and the results of Millet’s study
(2010) on the employment of the QuickListens approach. Thirty first-year intermediate
level English majors engaged in a QuickListens activity (i.e., listened to an easy and
interesting story with time pressure) during every class meeting, and were also required
to listen to or watch self-selected materials for 30 minutes on a daily basis outside the
classroom. The results show that the intervention improved student listening skills as
measured using a listening test and confidence as measured using a questionnaire.
These study results substantiated Nation and Newton and Millet’s postulation that
student listening skills and confidence improve when they listen extensively to easy texts
that arouse their interests.
It may be that the practice of extensive listening can be corroborated by several
studies to date that investigated the effects of extensive reading on language learning.
Elley and Magunbhai (1981) used “book flood” with primary school students and
reported that participants improved their abilities to understand sentence structures and
memorize them as well as their reading and listening skills. Renandya, Rajan, and Jacobs’
(1999) showed that through extensive reading adult learners in Vietnam improved their
general English proficiency. Davis (1995) comments that extensive reading makes
learners more positive about reading, which implies enhanced motivation and confidence
46
Sakae Onoda
in L2 reading. Summarizing the results of extensive reading studies, Harmer (2001)
argues that extensive reading has a number of benefits for the development of language
skills, and that the effects of extensive reading are echoed by effects of extensive
listening: “The more students listen, the more language they acquire, and the better they
get at listening activities in general” (p. 204).
Of equal importance, support comes from the literature on the use of listening
strategies and self-regulation strategies in L2 learning. The use of extensive listening for
listening skill development is congruent with strategies reported by successful English
learners, especially those who improved listening skills effectively (Onoda, 2012; White,
2008). In these studies, various strategies were used to increase exposure to spoken
language by talking to peers in and out of the class, going to a self-access learning center,
and listening to self-selected listening materials at home or while using public
transportation. In addition, self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 2000) and social
cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986) explicate the effects of intrinsic motivation and
self-efficacy on language skill development. Learners can regulate their learning when
they are intrinsically motivated and self-efficacious, and self-regulated learning thus
affectively enhanced yields academic achievement, which in turn improves self-efficacy.
The theoretical underpinning is corroborated by a large number of studies in educational
psychology (e.g., Pintich & De Groot, 1990) and a few studies in second language
acquisition (Onoda, 2012).
From these findings, it appears that extensive listening will improve listening skill and
possibly overall English skills, promote incidental vocabulary learning, and enhance
confidence in and motivation for learning English.
II. RESEARCH QUESTION
Drawing on suggestions by Nation and Newton, the results of extensive reading and
listening studies, and self-regulation study results, a large amount of exposure to
listening material that is easy and interesting is expected to improve listening skills of
the participants who have been exposed to limited amounts of spoken English. This line
of thought leads to the research question: To what extent is the use of daily extensive
listening assignments effective for listening skill and confidence development in
university English majors over one academic year?
Investigating Effects of Extensive Listening on Listening Skill Development in EFL Classes
47
III. METHOD
1. Participants
The participants were 60 first-year English majors at a university in eastern Japan.
Thirty of these students participated in 2009 and the other 30 participated in 2012.
Participants demonstrated an intermediate English proficiency as measured by the
TOEFL. The 2009 group was composed of 24 female and 6 male students including
five repeaters of the course, and the 2012 group comprised 22 female and 8 male
students including four repeaters. They had studied English for six years at secondary
schools, but because reading and grammar are mainly taught using Japanese and the
development of listening and speaking skills is still on the periphery of curricula
(Murphey, Onoda, Sato, & Takaki, 2012), they still had difficulties understanding and
interpreting naturally spoken English in a timely manner and wanted to improve
listening and speaking skills.
2. Research Design
Based on the fact that there were no statistically significant differences between the
two groups (the 2009 group and 2012 group) in terms of listening skills measured by a
listening comprehension test and listening and reading skills measured by the TOEFL at
the beginning of the study in April, 2009 and 2012, listening skill improvement was
compared between the two groups using a listening comprehension test at the end of
each year. The listening comprehension test was composed of 50 questions (monologues
and dialogues), lasting for 40 minutes (See Appendix) and demonstrated a high
reliability (α= .87). At these times confidence in L2 listening was also measured using a
questionnaire, and interviews were conducted in order to elicit feelings about the
extensive listening task in which the experimental group had engaged.
The classroomtasks and assignments were designed based on Nation and Newton’s
(2009) four strands of teaching. Both groups were engaged in learning using TOEFL
listening, reading, and grammar exercises (a language-focused learning task) in class, an
Academic Word List quiz (another language-focused learning task), and easy story
reading and discussions based on it (meaning-focused input and output tasks) . However,
the assignments given to them were different. The 2009 control group was given VOA
(Voice of America) listening and TOEFL listening practices as an assignment (a
language-focused learning task) to do for 30 minutes a day whereas the 2012
experimental group was assigned to do extensive listening practices using
48
Sakae Onoda
EnglishCentral(a fluency development task)for 30 minutes a day outside the classroom.
The extensive listening assignment was designed based on some of the principles of
extensive reading posited by Day and Bamford (2002):
1. The reading material is easy,
2. A variety of reading material on a wide range of topics must be available,
3. Learners choose what they want to read,
4. Learners read as much as possible,
5. The purpose of reading is usually related to pleasure, information, and general
understanding,
6. Reading is its own reward,
7. Reading speed is usually faster rather than slower,
8. Reading is individual and silent.
It is important to note that while student engagement in the listening assignment (i.e.,
VOA listening and TOEFL listening practices) was checked by their completed listening
diaries to be handed in every two weeks, their engagement in the extensive listening
assignment was monitored by the teacher on the EnglishCentral website and by reading
their completed listening diaries to be handed in every two weeks.
IV. RESULTS
This study investigated the effects of extensive listening assignments on the
development of listening skills and confidence improvement of university English
majors by comparing the achievements of the control and experimental groups over an
academic year. The descriptive statistics for the listening comprehension test scores and
the confidence questionnaire results are presented in Tables 1 and 2.
Listening skill improvements by the control group and experimental group as
measured by the listening comprehension test were analyzed using t-tests, and it was
found that the mean of the experimental group (M = 65.57, SD = 11.81) was significantly
different from the mean of the control group (M = 57.56, SD = 10.14), t(29) = 3.21,
p< .001. In addition, results of the questionnaire conducted in both groups in April 2009
and in January 2010, and in April 2012 and January 2013 indicated that confidence in L2
listening increased in the experimental group but not in the control group. On a 5
pointLikertscale (1 = not confident at all to 5 = very confident), confidence in L2
learning did not improve in the control group (M = 2.91, SD = 0.75, in April 2009 and M
=3.25, SD = 0.44, in January, 2010) as much as in the experimental group (M = 2.86, SD
Investigating Effects of Extensive Listening on Listening Skill Development in EFL Classes
49
= 0.57 in April 2012 and M = 3.77, SD = 0.61, in January 2013).
TABLE 1
Descriptive Statistics for the Listening Comprehension Test Scores
Groups
Tests
Control group (n=-30)
Experimental group (n=30)
Pre-test
Post-test
Pre-test
Post-test
M
53.50
57.56
54.10
65.57
SE
0.84
0.84
0.84
0.84
[51.00, 58.21]
[52.31, 562.50]
[2.10, 59.20]
[89.23, 72.45]
SD
9.82
10.14
9.95
11.81
Skewness
0.92
-0.83
-0.89
-105
SES
0.40
0.40
0.41
0.41
Kurtosis
0.85
-.079
0.41
2.01
SEK
0.80
.0.80
0.81
0.81
95% CI
Note: CI = Confidence interval.
The skewness and kurtosis statistics for the listening comprehension test scores was
acceptable, and no outliers were identified. In addition, the listening comprehension test
demonstrated a high reliability coefficient of α= .87.
TABLE2
Descriptive Statistics for the Confidence Questionnaire Results
Groups
Tests
M
SE
Control group (n=-30)
Experimental group (n=30)
Pre-test
Post-test
Pre-test
Post-test
2.91
3.25
2.86
3.77
0.04
0.04
0.04
0.04
[2.54, 3.43]
[2.78, 3.81]
[2.56,3.51]
[3.32, 4.21]
SD
0.57
0.44
0.57
0.61
Skewness
0.52
0.56
0.41
0.59
SES
0.14
0.14
0.15
0.15
Kurtosis
0.36
0.41
0.33
0.54
SEK
0.28
0.28
0.29
0.29
95% CI
Note: CI = Confidence interval.
The skewness and kurtosis statistics for the confidence questionnaire was acceptable,
50
Sakae Onoda
and no outliers were identified.
V. DISCUSSION
Results show that the extensive listening practice (a fluency development task) helped
improve listening skills significantly and improve confidence in listening to naturally
spoken English to a greater extent than the VOA and TOEFL listening practice (a
language-focused learning task). The results of this study on extensive listening seem to
parallel some of the benefits of extensive reading reported in L2 literature: it is fun and
provides a sense of achievement, improving motivation for and confidence in listening to
naturally spoken English. Student feedback may help clarify the reasons for improved
listening skills and listening confidence. Fifteen students in the experimental group
discussed the value of the extensive listening practice and described the benefits: they
took pleasure in understanding the listening materials, had stronger motivation for and
confidence in listening, and in fact learned expressions. Seven students said they had
practiced shadowing to memorize sentences and five students said that they had felt
some of the English sentences echoing in their head. Eight students commented that
using the TOEFL practice book (language-focused learning) in addition to the extensive
listening assignment (fluency-development) felt well balanced. The TOEFL reading,
listening, and grammar exercises were a little difficult, and the students had to read,
listen, and think very attentively. Although they had to study for TOEFL tests, they were
often discouraged because they could not answer the questions correctly. However,
students could relax and easily follow the storyline when engaged in theextensive
listening task. This was not the case with the control group, who reported that it was
good to practice TOEFL listening exercises at home, but that there were a lot of difficult
vocabulary included in the TOEFL listening exercises and sometimes they felt
discouraged and lost motivation to continue the study.
Also, student feedback revealed the value of the material used for the research and
their proactive and possibly self-regulating learning behaviors. EnglishCentral provided
a wide range of listening materials from TV news clips to movies and TV dramas, and
students were free to choose the materials that were suited to their interests and English
proficiencies. The materials in each genre were changed every few weeks by the website
organizer so that students could keep watching the same types of texts (such as TV
dramas, movies, and TV news clips) and maintain their interest in and motivation for
learning English for a long time. Also, they could watch the same material as many times
as they wanted to within the few weeks, check their understanding by looking at the
transcripts and subtitles, and learn words and phrases they were interested in by using
Investigating Effects of Extensive Listening on Listening Skill Development in EFL Classes
51
explanations given by the texts. Using EnglishCentral, twelve students in the
experimental group reported to have improved their listening skills and to have
expanded their English vocabulary, enhancing their confidence in naturally spoken
English. Eight students utilized various listening strategies such as watching movies
without subtitles first, then watching them with subtitles, and finally discussing the
content with their friends or teachers, in order to improve their understanding.
More importantly, student feedback appears to substantiate some of the benefits of
Nation and Newton’s (2009) four strands of teaching; they postulate that four strands be
employed in language teaching to develop student language skills effectively. The
control group was engaged in three strands or types of tasks that did not include fluency
development whereas the experimental group experienced the four strands or types of
tasks including a fluency development task. The inclusion of a fluency development task,
particularly extensive listening, appears to be better balanced and more cognitively and
motivationally beneficial for student language learning.
The results can also be supported by self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 2000)
and social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986) that postulate that intrinsic motivation and
self-efficacy have a deep impact on academic achievement. These theories imply that
improved self-efficacy appears to be related to improved listening skills (Onoda, 2012;
Pintrich, & De Groot, 1990).
VI. CONCLUSION
The use of the extensive listening task appears to be effective for the improvement of
listening skills and confidence in listening to naturally spoken English in university
English majors; extensive listening seems to have similar effects as extensive reading.
However, given the small sample size (n = 30), some caution should be exercised in the
interpretation of the results. Replication with a larger sample size with more rigid control
of the control group’s engagement in a different task may provide more substantial data
to verify these findings. Finally, it might yield more accurate and pedagogically useful
data if the improvement of student listening skills and confidence in listening is
investigated by using a growth curve model.
52
Sakae Onoda
REFERENCES
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The
HarvardUniversity Press.
architecture
of
cognition.
Cambridge,
MA:
Bandura, A. (1986).Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive
theory.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Davis, C. (1995). Extensive reading: An expensive extravagance? ELT Journal 49(4).
Day, R. R., (2008). Teaching foreign language fluency.The Language Teacher 32 (7).
Day, R. R., &Bamford, J. (2002).Top ten principles for teaching extensive
reading.Reading in a Foreign Language, 14,136–141.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The "what" and "why" of goal pursuits: Human needs
and the self-determination of behavior. Psychology Inquiry, 11(4), 227-268.
DeKeyser, R. (2007). Practice in a second language: Perspectives from applied
linguistics and cognitive psychology.Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity Press.
Dunkel, P. A. (1986). Developing listening fluency in L2: Theoretical principles and
pedagogical considerations.Modern Language Journal 70, 99-106.
Ellis, R. (2008). The study of second language acquisition.Oxford: OxfordUniversity
Press.
Elley, W. B., &Mangubhai, F. (1981).The impact of a book flood in Fuji primary
schools.New Zealand Council for Educational Research and Institute of
Education: University of South Pacific.
Harmer, J. (2001).The practice of English language teaching.Essex: Pearson Education.
Mason, B., &Krashen, S. D. (1997).Extensive reading in English as a foreign
language.System, 25, 91–102.
Millet, S. (2010). Quicklistens. Retrieved from
http://www.victoria.ac.nz/lals/staff/Publications/The_No__1_Ladies_Detective
Agency.pdf.
Murphey, T., Onoda, S., Sato, K., &Takaki, M. (2012).Language of Interaction in
Japanese EFL JHSs & SHSs,” presented at the JALT Conference, Shizuoka,
Japan, 2012.
Nation, P (2007). The four strands. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching,
1(1),1-11.
Nation,
I.
S.
P.,
&
Newton,
J.
(2009).Teaching
ESL/EFL listening
and
Investigating Effects of Extensive Listening on Listening Skill Development in EFL Classes
53
speaking.Newcastle, UK: Routledge.
Onoda, S. (2000).Introducing productive activities into Media English teaching— with a
primary focus on group projects.JACES, 39, 87-102.
Onoda, S. (2012).Self-regulation and its relation to motivation and proficiency.
Unpublished Ed.D dissertation, TempleUniversity: Philadelphia.
Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. (1990).Motivational and self-regulated learning
components of classroom academic performance.Journal of Educational
Psychology, 82, 33-40.
Renandya, W. A. Rajan, B. R. S., & Jacobs, G. M. (1999).Extensive reading with adult
learners of English as a second language.RELC Journal, 30(1), 39-61.
Rost, M. (2002).Teaching and researching listening. London, UK: Longman.
Vandergrift, L. (2007). Recent developments in second and foreign language listening
comprehension research.Language Teaching, 40, 191-210.
White, G. (2008). Listening and good language learners.Cambridge:
CambridgeUniversity Press.
Williams, M., & Burden, R. L. (2005).Psychology for language teachers.Cambridge:
CambridgeUniversity Press.
Wood, D. (2001). In search of fluency: What is it and how can we teach it? The
Canadian Modern Language Review, 57(4), 573-589.
54
Sakae Onoda
APPENDIX
Listening comprehension test
Monologues:
(Script)
1.
American alligators once almost completely died out. In 1967, the United States
government began to protect them. Today there are over one million alligators in the
United States. They are found mainly in the southern party of the United States,
especially in the states of Florida and Louisiana. They love in rivers, lakes, ponds, or
wetland areas.
These animals are great swimmers. They have strong feet and tails that help them swim
quickly through the water. An average male alligator is three to five meters long. Half of
its length is its tail. An average male alligator weighs between 220 and 270 kilograms.
Females are usually smaller than males.
Female alligators are gentle mothers to their young. A mother alligator makes a nest on
shore, where she lays her eggs. Then she guards her eggs until they’re ready to hatch.
When the baby alligators come out of their eggs, she gently carries them in her mouth to
the water nearby. Their mother protects them from animals that might hurt then,
including other alligators. The young alligators stay with their mother for up to two years,
until they are able to take care of themselves.
Question 1: What is true about American alligators?
a) They are poor swimmers.
b) They rarely love in lakes or ponds.
c) They love mainly in Florida and Louisiana.
d) They are especially found in northern rivers and lakes.
Question 2: What is true about baby alligators?
a) They are born from eggs.
b) They are born in the water.
c) They protect their mother.
d) They live with their mother for over three years.
Dialogues:
(Script):
1.
W: Did you know that we need at least 8 hours of sleep every day?
Investigating Effects of Extensive Listening on Listening Skill Development in EFL Classes
55
M: Don’t people say different things about the amount of sleep we need?
W: Right, but most seem to agree that teenagers don’t sleep enough for their
schooling.
M: Well at least that explains the low scores on my exams.
Question 1: What are the man and the woman talking about?
a) The results of the exams
b) The necessary amount of sleep
c) Teenagers’ school behavior
d) Sleeping students during class hours
2.
M: So, where are we going to watch the program? At your place or my place?
W: What program?
M: I told you yesterday. There’s going to be a TV program about famous doctors in
Japan.
W: Ah, I forgot. Sure. I7ll come to your place. What time does it start>?
M: In an hour.
Question 2: What are the man and the woman going to do next?
a) Go to the man’s place.
b) Go to a hospital.
c) Go out to see a movie.
d) Go to the woman’s house.
Applicable levels: high school and university levels
Key words: extensive listening, confidence-building, the four strands of teaching, listening
fluency development, listening skill development
Sakae Onoda
Dept. of English,
KandaUniversity of International Studies
1-4-1 Wakaba, Mihama-ku, Chiba-shi,
Chiba-ken, 261-0014 , Japan
Tel: 0081-43-273-1320
Fax: 0081-43-273-1322
E-mail: onoda@kanda.kuis.ac.jp
57
Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language
Vol. 1, No. 1 Winter 2014
Will extensive reading help L2 learners’ use of context
while processing text?
KiyomiYoshizawa
Atsuko Takase
Kansai University
Kyoko Otsuki
Hiroshima Shudo University
Yoshizawa, K., Takase, A., &Otsuki, K. (2013), Will extensive reading help L2 learners’
use of context while processing text?Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign
Language.1(1), 57 -69.
ABSTRACT
It is hypothesized that extensive reading (ER) helps learners to learn grammar in context
and to expand the scope of text to processwhen they read. This study examines whether
EFL learners expand the scope of text to process as they engage themselves in reading.
Data was collected over three academic years. 341 Japanese EFL learners participated in
all the data collection sessions, and they took a cloze test developed to place learners and
monitor their progress.Each deletion of the cloze test was classified into four types,
depending on the amount of information required for a successful task completion
(Bachman, 1985). In order to examine how the learners’ responses to the items in each of
the four categories changed over time, a linear growth model was applied. The results
indicate that the participants’ rate of change was rather similar to each other across
different types of items. (148 words)
I. INTRODUCTION
Extensive reading (ER) has been widely recognized as one of the effective ways to
learn languages for the last two decades. In Japan, more institutional organizations,
58
Kiyomi Yoshizawa·Atsuko Takase·Kyoko Otsuki
including secondary schools, colleges and universities, have implemented ER to
improve their students’ English ability. This trend is backed by an abundance of research,
which demonstrates that ER contributes to improvement in various aspects of learners’
language learning: reading proficiency (e.g. Elley & Mangubhai, 1981; Mason &
Krashen, 1997; Yamashita, 2008), vocabulary acquisition (Cirocki, 2009; Horst, 2005),
reading rate and fluency (Beglar, Hunt,& Kite, 2011; Iwahori, 2008), writing ability
(Irvine, 2011; Janopoulos, 1986;), gains on the TOEIC scores (Nishizawa, Yoshioka, &
Fukada, 2010), and improvement of general English proficiency (Takase, 2008, 2012;
Takase & Otsuki, 2012).
In addition to these various effects of ER on linguistic improvement, ER has great
impacts on learners’ positive attitudes towards learning English and self-confidence
(Mason & Krashen, 1997; Takase, 2004, 2007, 2009).
Among these reports on effectiveness of ER on development of various language
aspects, research on grammar is scarce. To our knowledge, Maruhashi’s research (2011)
is the only one, in which she reported 137 university students’ improvement on some
grammatical aspects after three months of ER. Meanwhile, Takase (2008) reported
that after three months of ER, over 40 % of the unmotivated reluctant repeaters
in university perceived improvement in their grammar knowledge such as the
structures of English sentences or role of adjectives. The participants in the study
had been acquainted with those grammatical points, but they had not completely
understood them since their early stage of studying English at secondary school.
The students in the study actually showed a significant improvement on the
postEdinburgh Project on Extensive Reading Placement/Progress Test (EPER
PPT, hereafter). EPER PPT is acloze test developed by the Institute for Applied
Language Studies in the University of Edinburgh in 1992 in order to measure
general English proficiency including grammar in context, and to place learners
into appropriate reading levels. It alsomonitors their progress. The cloze test, by
its nature, requires learners to process text at various levels, ranging from clauses to
across sentences. On the other hand, research reports that poor L2 readers focus on a
limited range of text when they read for comprehension (e.g., Yoshizawa, 2000). Thus,
obtaining high marks in the EPER PPT could indicate that learners are capable of
processing wider range of context required for giving answers. Considering these
observations and the report from Takase (2008), it might be assumed that while learners
are engaged in extensive reading, processing unit in context will be expanding, which is
reflected in the improvement of their performance on different types of items deleted in
the EPER PPT. The present study aims to examine how learners’ rates of change grow
across different types of cloze items over time. Thus, we posed the following research
questions:
Will Extensive Reading Help L2 Learners’Use of Context While Processing Text?
59
1) Will extensive reading help learners expand the scope of text to process?
2) Do learners differ in their estimated rate of change across different types of
EPER items?
3) Is there any relationship between the learners’ initial language proficiency and
their rate of change?
II. METHOD
1. Participants
A total of 526 second-year Japanese EFL university students participated in the
study. Out of 526 participants, 341 participated in all the data collection sessions. Their
majors were commerce, economics, law, and literature. Their English proficiency varied
from false beginner to advanced level, which can be converted to approximately CFER
A1 to B2. Each student was engaged in ER in a reading class for one academic year,
choosing books appropriate to their English proficiency levels and their interest.
2. Materials
The participants took the EPER Placement/Progress Test (Form A) three times
during the course. The EPER PPT, mentioned above, is a cloze test which comprises 12
short passages (approximately 80 words on average) taken from different levels of
graded readers and arranged in an ascending order of difficulty. There are 141 deleted
items at the rate of 4 to 12 words (M = 6.30, SD = 1.16).
Among language testing researchers, the type of language ability measured by
cloze tests is inconclusive. According to Bachman (1985), cloze tests can measure
both lower level (e.g. within clause) and higher level (e.g. across sentences)
reading abilities. Also, empirical research demonstrated that cloze tests can
measure learners’ proficiency level (Chihara et al. 1994; Sasaki 2000; Yamashita
2008) including micro-level linguistic competence such as vocabulary, spelling
and morphosyntax, as scoring criterion is limited to the list of given answers. As
12 short passages in the EPER PPT were all taken from obsolete graded readers,
the EPER PPT is considered as an appropriate test to measure general English
proficiency and improvement of the learners who use various graded readers for
ER.
Each deletion in the EPER Form A test was classified into four types based on
60
Kiyomi Yoshizawa·Atsuko Takase·Kyoko Otsuki
Bachman’s classification (1985), reflecting four different levels of context required for
closure:
Type 1 requires context within the same clause where the closure is located;
Type 2 requires context across clauses, within a sentence;
Type 3 requires context across sentence, within a text;
Type 4 requires extra-textual knowledge.
Type 1 items require grammatical knowledge and respondents need to use context
within the same clause where a particular blank is located. From Type 2 to Type 4,
context required for answering items expands from sentence level to text level and
ultimately world knowledge. The following are examples of deletion of each type.
Simon looks at the people in the station. (1) can see students in jeans, and men (2)
suits. He can see families and children. He cannot see any spies. Simon’s train
goes (3) 11.00, and it is 10.57 now. Simon (4) to the train. There is an old woman
with an umbrella near Simon. She is walking very fast. Simon does not see her. He
does not see her bag. (Simon and the Spy, Penguin Readers)
In order to answer item 1, learners should read the previous sentence and know that
Simon is a name for men (world knowledge), which leads to the answer, the third person
singular masculine pronoun he. Thus, answering item 1 requires the context across
sentences (Type 3) and world knowledge (Type 4) as well as grammatical knowledge. In
order to answer item 2, learners need to look at the sentence in which item 2 is included.
Since two types of clothes jeans and clothes are juxtaposed, the preposition in will come
here. Because the information called for the closure is from two clauses within the
sentence, this is the Type 2 deletion. Item 3 serves as an example of Type 1 items: only
grammatical knowledge is required to reach the correct answer for item 3. The word
which comes before the time expression (i.e., 11:00) will be at. To fill in (4), learners
need to know how Simon is engaged in the train in this context. Based on the previous
sentence, learners know that the train is going to leave soon, which leads them to
consider that Simon is in a rush. Therefore, item 4 is categorized into Type 3.
According to the scheme explained above, deleted items were classified by two of
the authors and another person from outside, resulting in the high degree of consistency.
The distribution of deletion types in EPER (A) is as follows: Type 1 counts for 27.0%;
Type 2, 19.9%; Type 3, 47.5%; and Type 4, 5.7%.
Will Extensive Reading Help L2 Learners’Use of Context While Processing Text?
61
3. Procedure
At the onset of the course, Edinburgh Project on Extensive Reading (EPER)
Placement/Progress Test (PPT) (Form A) was administered as the pre-test in order to
investigate the participants’ general English proficiency. The same EPER Test was
administered as a mid-test at the end of the first semester (14 weeks after the pre-test),
and as a post-test at the end of the academic year (9 months after the beginning of the
course including summer vacation) in order to examine the learners’ improvement in
their general English proficiency.
The class met once a week for 15 sessions each semester. During the 90-minute
session, participants were provided with 40 - 45 minutes for in-class reading including
the time to keep their reading log and to exchange books delivered to class by the
instructor, meaning that participants were engaged in Sustained Silent Reading (SSR,
hereafter) for approximately 30 - 35 minutes in each session. They were also encouraged
to read as many books as possible outside class and required to keep a reading log each
time they finished a book.
Concerning the reading materials, the participants used two types of materials:
graded readers that are written for EFL and ESL learners; and leveled readers and young
readers that are written for L1 children of English. Major graded readers participants
read include Cambridge English Readers, Foundations Reading Library, Macmillan
Readers, Oxford Bookworms, Penguin Readers, and Scholastic ELT Readers. Leveled
readers include Oxford Reading Tree, Longman Literacy Land Story Street, Rookie
Readers, Scholastic Reading, Step Into Reading, Capstone Series, Nate the Great, A to Z
Mysteries, Magic Tree House, etc.
4. Data Analysis
The EPER PPT data were analyzed using RaschUnidimensional Measurement
Model software (RUMM Laboratory). The main purpose of conducting Rasch analysis
was to convert the data into an interval scale. The dichotomous model was used. In order
to confirm that the data fit the dichotomous model, the following aspects were examined
prior to obtaining the participants’ ability scores (i.e., pre-, mid- and post-test scores) in
logits: the item-total statistics fit of items and persons to the model, the assumption of
the local item independence, unidimensionality, targeting of the scale, and reliability.
By the end of the Rasch analyses, 23 items were deleted as misfitting items. Table
1 shows the result of the final analysis. When the data fits the model, the mean of the fit
residuals becomes close to zero and the standard deviation becomes close to one. The
results show a fairly good fit to the model. However, the means of the items and persons
62
Kiyomi Yoshizawa·Atsuko Takase·Kyoko Otsuki
indicate that item difficulty has a higher logit than the person logit, meaning that items
were rather difficult for the participants. Also, a Rasch item reliability was .93.
TABLE 1
The Summary Statistics of the Rasch Analysis
Items
Mean
SD
Persons
Location
Fit residuals
location
Fit residuals
0.000
2.405
-0.167
1.644
-0.670
0.993
-0.233
0.769
FIGURE 1.
Figure 1 shows the person-item threshold distribution. The upper graph shows the
distribution of the persons (i.e., participants); the lower graph shows the distribution of
items. Although the distribution of the items is greater than that of the persons, indicating
that there are easier and more difficult items than the abilities of the participants, the
items are well targeted.
In the second part of the data analysis, we applied the linear growth curve
modeling to examine whether ER would help learners expand the scope of text to
process and what type of closure would improve most. We examined whether
individuals differ in their estimated rate of change across different types of EPER
items.
63
Will Extensive Reading Help L2 Learners’Use of Context While Processing Text?
III. RESULT
Table 2 shows the descriptive statistics for the standard scores of the EPER
administered at the beginning, in the middle, and at the end of the extensive reading
course. The mean score of the EPER at the beginning of the course was 50.69 (SD =
13.86), 60.81 (SD = 13.31) in the middle of the course, and 61.70 (SD = 13.66) at the
end of the course, respectively. Figure 2 shows the mean scores of the EPER PPT at
three administrations. Those results show that the participants scored about 10 points
higher at the end of the first semester (Time 2) than they did at the beginning of the ER
course (Time 1). On the other hand, they scored only about one point higher at the end of
the course (Time 3) than they did at the end of the first semester (Time 2).
Based on the last Rasch analysis where 23 test items were deleted, a separate
analysis was conducted for each of the four types of items. For each type of items, it was
confirmed that the data fit the Rasch model. Then, person measures were created based
on the items of the same type. Similar to the person measures based on all the items,
person measures were converted to standard scores ranging from zero to 100. Table2
shows the descriptive statistics of four types of items. On the table, MIN 0 refers to the
minimum score and MAX 100 refers to the maximum score, respectively. The results
show patterns similar to those described about the person measures based on all the
items.
TABLE 2
The Descriptive Statistics for the EPER at Three Administrations
Time 1
Mean
SD
MIN
MAX
Time 2
Mean
SD
MIN
MAX
Time 3
Mean
SD
MIN
MAX
ALL
TYPE 1
TYPE 2
TYPE 3
TYPE 4
50.69
13.88
0
92
41.98
12.57
0
76.21
51.54
15.65
0
93.58
50.48
14.08
0
89.9
52.26
14.75
0
91.1
60.81
13.31
3
90
51.12
12.66
13.88
100
58.60
15.21
0
100
60.65
13.17
0
94.78
60.82
14.41
0
100
61.7
13.66
14
100
51.91
12.28
13.88
85.67
59.30
15.74
9.77
100
61.39
13.77
18.88
100
61.98
14.39
18.43
100
64
Kiyomi Yoshizawa·Atsuko Takase·Kyoko Otsuki
FIGURE 2.
The Mean Scores of the EPER at Three Administrations
In order to answer our research question, we applied linear individual growth
models to examine the change of the learners’ reading performance over time. Linear
growth models include two components: level-1 and level-2 submodels. The former
describes how individuals change over time, i.e., within-person change; the latter
describes how these changes differ across individuals, i.e., between-person differences in
change. In this study, we focus only on the level-1 submodel. We write the level-1
submodel as:
Yij= π0i+π1i (Time)ij + eij (Equation 1).
In the study, we applied liner growth models to estimate (a) an intercept (i.e., an initial
status at the onset of the ER course); (b) a mean growth curve and the extent of
individual variation around it; (c) the correlation between an initial status and rate of
change. Four types of items were analyzed separately, using HLM version 6 software
(Scientific Software International, Inc.). Table 3 shows the results of fitting a liner
growth model for change to the EPER data. The estimated average initial status and rate
of growth were 43.43 and 5.01 for Type 1 items, 47.96 and 4.31 for Type 2 items, 45.85
and 5.98 for Type 3 items, and 47.49 and 5.43 for Type 4 items, respectively. In other
words, the average score of Type 1 items at the pretest time were estimated to be 43.43
and the participants were gaining 5.01 points per year, for example. Also,both the
65
Will Extensive Reading Help L2 Learners’Use of Context While Processing Text?
intercept and the slope were significant and both parameters were necessary to explain
the mean growth trajectories across four types of items.
Next, we examined the individual variation in growth trajectories from the mean
curve. The variance components in Table 3 indicate the amount of variability left after
fitting the model. The estimates for the variances of individual growth parameters (i.e.,
the initial status and the rate of change) were 99.11 and 0.50 for Type 1 items, 209.73
and 12.42 for Type 2 items, 142.16 and 0.79 for Type 3 items, and 135.68 and 0.82 for
Type 4 items, respectively. The variance of the intercept was significant for all four types.
This indicates that the participants varied significantly in their abilities to endorse each
type of items at the onset of the ER course. On the other hand, the variance of the growth
rate indicates different patterns. The variance of the growth rate was significant for Type
2 items, but insignificant for other types of items. This means a lot of individual
differences were involved in the growth rate of Type 2 items.
Further, we examined the correlations between the initial state and the growth rate.
In general, negative correlations ranging from -0.07 to -0.49 were observed between the
initial status and the growth rate.
TABLE 3
Results of Fitting aLiner Growth Model for Change to Four Types of Items
Fixed effect
Estimate
s.e.
Type 1
initial status
43.43
0.67
rate of change
5.01
0.24
Level 1 error
-correlation
-0.27
Type 2
initial status
47.96
0.94
rate of change
4.31
0.36
Level 1 error
-correlation
-0.49
Type 3
initial status
45.85
0.76
rate of change
5.98
0.26
Level 1 error
-correlation
-0.07
Type 4
initial status
47.49
0.89
rate of change
5.43
0.34
Level 1 error
-correlation
-0.26
* P< .05 ** p< .01 *** p< .001.
Variance components
χ2
df
p
Estimate
p
***
***
99.11
0.50
42.53
813.23
470.40
467
467
***
0.45
***
***
209.73
12.42
73.58
941.11
604.67
467
467
***
***
***
***
142.16
0.79
49.11
941.33
479.61
467
467
***
0.33
***
***
135.68
0.82
87.51
700.55
446.64
467
467
***
0.50
66
Kiyomi Yoshizawa·Atsuko Takase·Kyoko Otsuki
IV. DISCUSSION
1. Main findings
It is hypothesized that extensive reading (ER) helps learners to learn grammar in
context and to expand the scope of text to process (across sentences or entire
paragraphs) when they read. Our first research questionis as follows: Will extensive
reading help learners expand the scope of text to process? Based on the results of the
linear growth modeling, we can conclude that the learners’ growth rates were quite
similar across four types of items. The growth rates range from 4.31 (Type 2) to 5.98
(Type 3). Although the slope of Type 3 items seems to be slightly higher than the rest,
the difference is quite small.
The second research question is “Do learners differ in their estimated rate of change
across different types of EPER items?” We hypothesized that learners would show
greater growth rates in Type 2 and Type 3 items than Type 1 items. Learners have to use
context across clauses within a sentence to answer Type 2 items correctly and context
across sentences within text to answer Type 3 items correctly. However, the data
analyses show a different picture. That is, the learners’ growth rates were quite similar
across four types of items. This may indicate that the participants improved their implicit
knowledge of grammar through ER. Table3 shows that the growth rate of Type 1 items is
similar to those of the other types of items. This result may provide additional support
for what is reported in Takase (2008, 2012). She reports that over 40% of the repeaters
in her ER class perceived their improvement in grammar. The participants in the current
study might have improved their knowledge of grammar through ER and this is shown
in the growth rate of Type 1 items.
Further, the results of the data analysis direct us to a future direction of our research.
Even though learners’ growth rates were very similar to each other, Type 2 items show
rather large variability. This indicates that learners show individual differences in their
growth rates. In the current study, we focused on only the level-1 submodel and
examined the within-person changes over time. Other time-varying predictors should be
able to give a clearer picture about learners’ growth trajectories.
The third research question is whether there is any relationship between the
learners’ initial language proficiency and their rate of change. There was a weak negative
correlation between the learners’ initial status and their growth rate especially for Type 2
items. This indicates that the learners who had limited language proficiency at the
beginning of the ER course, measured by Type 2 items, tended to gain at rather a faster
rate than those who had higher language proficiency.
Will Extensive Reading Help L2 Learners’Use of Context While Processing Text?
67
2. Limitations
The results of the present study should be viewed taking the following limitations
into consideration. First, the participants of the present study were Japanese EFL learners
at a university. The generalizability of the results is limited to the learners in similar
learning context. Second, the participants were in the ER class for one academic year. If
they had continued to read extensively for a longer time period, clearer trends might
have been obtained across different types of cloze items. Third, we need to confirm the
degree in which Bachman’s classification of items on a cloze test would match the way
test-takers would respond each of the items on the EPER PPT.
3. Educational Implication
As the results of this research illustrate, even one year of engagement in extensive
reading help learners, from low- to high-level university students, improve their general
English proficiency by processing texts using grammar in context, context within and
across sentences, or extra-textual knowledge. With implementation of extensive reading
into school curriculum at an early stage of formal English education in elementary
school or secondary school, these skills could be developed faster and better than
translation practice and explicit knowledge of grammar rules, which have been practiced
extensively in the learning context where the current study took place.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
This study was supported by JSPS Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (C) Grant
Number 24520678.
REFERENCES
Bachman, L.F. (1985). Performance on cloze tests with fixed-ratio and rational
deletions.TESOL Quarterly 19(3), 535-569.
Beglar, D., Hunt, A., & Kite, Y. (2011).The effect of pleasure reading on Japanese
university EFL learners’ reading rates.Language Teacher, 61(4), 1-39.
Chihara, T., Oller, J.W., Jr., Weaver, K.,&Chavey-Oller, M.A. (1994). Are cloze items
sensitive to constraints across sentences? Language learning,27, 63-73.
Cirocki, A. (2009). Implementing the ER approach to literature in the EFL
secondaryclassroom: An action research study.In A. Cirocki (Ed.), Extensive Reading
68
Kiyomi Yoshizawa·Atsuko Takase·Kyoko Otsuki
inEnglish Language Teaching (pp.521–546). Munich, Germany: Lincom.
Elley, W. B., &Mangbhai, F.(1981).The impact of a book flood in Fiji primary schools.
New Zealand Council for Educational Research and Institute of Education, NZ:
University of South Pacific.
Grabe, W. (2009).Reading in a second language: Moving from theory to practice.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Horst, M. (2005).Learning L2 vocabulary through extensive reading: A measurement
study.The Canadian Modern Language Review, 61, 355–82.
Irvine, A. (2011). Extensive Reading and the Development of L2 Writing. Paper
presented at the First Extensive Reading World Congress, Kyoto.
Iwahori, Y. (2008). Developing reading fluency: A study of extensive reading in EFL.
Reading in a Foreign Language, 20(1), 70-91.
Janopoulos, M. (1986).The relationship of pleasure reading and second language writing
proficiency.TESOL Quarterly, 20 (4), 763-768.
Laird, E. (2008).Simon and the spy. Harlow, England: Pearson Longman.
Maruhashi, K. (2011, March). The effect of extensive reading on Japanese EFL learners’
grammatical competence. Paper presented at the American Association for Applied
Linguistics Annual Conference, Chicago, Illinois.
Mason, B., &Krashen, S. (1997). Extensive reading in English as a foreign
language.System, 25(1), 99–102.
Nishizawa, H., Yoshioka, T., &Fukada, M.(2010).The impact of a 4-year extensive
reading program.JALT2009 Conference Proceedings, 632-640.
Sasaki, M. (2000). Effects of cultural schemata on students’ test-taking process for cloze
tests: A multiple data source approach. Language Testing,17, 85-114.
Takase, A. (2004). Investigating students’ reading motivation through interviews.Forum
for Foreign Language Education, 3, 23-38.
Takase, A. (2007). Japanese high school students’ motivation for extensive L2
reading.Reading in a Foreign Language, 19(1), 1-17.
Takase, A. (2008). The two most critical tips for a successful extensive reading.Kinki
University English Journal, 1, 119-136.
Takase, A. (2009). The effects of SSR on learners’ reading attitudes, motivation, and
achievement: A quantitative study. In A. Cirocki (Ed.), Extensive Reading in English
Language Teaching (pp.547-560). Munich, Germany: Lincom.
Takase, A. (2010). Eigotadoku/tachoshido manual [Teaching Manual for Extensive
Reading and Listening].Tokyo, Japan: Taishukan Shoten.
Takase, A. (2012). The effectiveness of sustained silent reading in becoming autonomous
learners.Forum for Foreign Language Education, 11, 1-14.
Will Extensive Reading Help L2 Learners’Use of Context While Processing Text?
69
Takase, A., &Otsuki, K. (2012).New challenges to motivate remedial EFL students to
read extensively.Apples – Journal of Applied Language Studies, 6(2), 75–94.
Yamashita, J. (2008). Extensive reading and development of different aspects of L2
proficiency.System, 36, 661-672.
Yoshizawa, K. (2000). Dokkaifushinsha-no shido [Teaching poor readers]. In T.
Takanashi& Y. Ushiro (Eds.),EigoRiidinguJiten [English Reading Encyclopedia] (pp.
120-133). Tokyo, Japan: Kenkyusha.
Applicable levels: All practitioners and learners
Key words: Extensive Reading, Grammar in context, Implicit knowledge of grammar, Text
processing
KiyomiYoshizawa
Kansai University
3-3-35 Yamate-cho, Suita-shi, Osaka 564-8680 Japan
Tel: +81-6-6368-0827
Fax: +81-6-6368-0036
yoshizaw@kansai-u.ac.jp
AtsukoTakase
Kansai University
3-3-35 Yamate-cho, Suita-shi, Osaka 564-8680 Japan
Tel: +81-6-6368-1121
Fax: +81-6-6368-0036
atsukot@jttk.zaq.ne.jp
KyokoOtsuki
Hiroshima Shudo University Faculty of Law
1-1-1 Otsuka Higashi, Azaminami-ku, Hiroshima-shi, Hiroshima 731-3195 Japan
Tel: +81-82-830-1123
otsuki_kyoko@hotmail.com
Information for Contributors of English Manuscripts
-‘Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language’Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language is a referred, quarterly
–published journal of the Korean English Extensive Reading
Association(KEERA).
1. Content
The content of the manuscript to be submitted to Journal of Extensive
Reading in Foreign Language should concern theories and/or practices
in the field of extensive reading in foreign language education. The
manuscript may be on an experimental analysis, a new proposal, or a
critique of theories and/or practices in the field. The manuscript dealing
with topics in linguistics, literature or interdisciplinary areas must have
implications for extensive reading in foreign language education.
The manuscript is accepted for review with the understanding that
the same work has not been submitted elsewhere, or previously published.
Time for Publication :
KEERA publishes four issues of
" Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language " Journal per year:
(1) Spring issue, March 31st;
(2) Summer issue, June 30th;
(3) Fall issue, September 30th;
(4) Winter issue, December 31st.
2. Specifications
1) The length of the manuscript should not exceed 20 pages of
Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language .
2) The manuscript should be prepared in MS Word (2000 or a higher version)
in the A4 - size paper setting.
3) The left and right margins should be 4.2 cm; the top margin, 5 cm, and the bottom margin,
5.2 cm; the top header, 4.2 cm and the bottom header, 1 cm. The line spacing of the
title, the main text, and the reference list should be fixed 14.5-point, while that of the
abstract and direct quotations should be fixed 12.5-point, and footnotes should be fixed
10-point. The main text should be Times New Roman 10-point and the letter spacing
should be 100% in 0.1- point narrow spacing.
4) The title of the article should not exceed two lines.
5) The abstract should be composed of approximately 200 words.
(Please indicate how many words are included in parentheses at its end.)
6) The subheadings should be numbered in the following manner: I. 1. 1) (1) ①
7) The dash should be presented as “.” rather than “--”.
8) Quotation marks should be in the format of . . or “ ”.
9) A new line in the body of text should begin with letters not with symbols or
punctuations such as dashes or hyphens. For example, if there is a hyphen in the
beginning of a new line, the hyphen needs to be moved to the previous line.
10) Korean words cannot be mingled with English ones for a reference, a title
or any other information, English words need to be used or Korean words
should be romanized.
11) A list of references, tables, and figures should conform to the style
of Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (6th ed.) (2010).
12) A list of references should be made using reverse indentation. For a book,
the place of publication and the publisher should be provided. When the place
of publication is a city in the U.S., specify the name of the state by writing the
abbreviation of the state (e.g., TX for Texas). For other places in other countries,
provide both the name of the city and of the country. When citing an article or
a chapter in an edited book, include its page numbers. When citing a revised book,
cite the edition used with an Arabic numeral in parentheses.
13) Korean references need to be presented in English. If the title of the article
and of the journal is not known in English, romanize it, including its
English translation in brackets.
14) In the list of references, single-author citations precede multiple-author
citations regardless of when the citations were published.
15) “Applicable levels” and “key words” should be listed at the end of the article.
16) After the list of the key words, the author’s name, affiliation, mailing
address, home/office phone number, fax number, email address, and cell
phone number should be provided.
17) When two or more authors submit an article, the degree of authorial participation should be indicated such as a co-author, a corresponding author, a first
author, a second author, etc.
3. Style Guidelines
All submissions to Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language should
conform to the requirements of Publication Manual of the American
Psychological Association (6th ed.). The following are some important examples
taken from the manual.
1) Text Citation
(1) One Work by a Single Author:
The last name of the author and the year of publication are inserted in the text at
the appropriate point.
— He stated, “[t]he .placebo effect. disappeared when [his own and other.s]
behaviors were studied in this manner” (Smith, 1982, p. 276).
— Smith (1982) found that “[t]he .placebo effect. disappeared when [his own
and other.s] behaviors were studied in this manner” (p. 276).
(2) One Work by Two Authors:
Cite both names every time the reference occurs in the text.
— Gardner and Lambert (1976) found that their previous study (Gardner &
Lambert,
1972) could not . . .
(3) One Work by 3-5 Authors:
Cite all authors the first time the reference occurs; in subsequent citations
include only the surname of the first author followed by “et al.” and the year in
parentheses.
— First citation: William, Jones, Smith, Bradner, and Torrington (1983) found
that . . .
— Second citation: William et al. (1983) studied the effect of . . .
(4) One Work by More Than Six Authors:
Cite only the surname of the first author followed by “et al.” and the year in the
first and subsequent citations. However, in the reference list, the names of all six
or more authors should be spelled out.
(5) Two or More Works Within the Same Parentheses:
Arrange two or more works by the same author by year of publication, and list
two or more works by different authors in alphabetical order by the first authors.
surnames. Separate the citations with semicolons.
— Oxford (1986, 1989, 1994) reported that many studies (Gosden, 1992;
Hanania & Akhtar, 1985;Hopkins, 1987, 1988a, 1989; Tarone et al., 1981)
used strategy training . . .
(6) Authors with the Same Surname:
If a reference list includes publications by two or more authors with the same
surname, include the authors’ initials in all text citations to avoid confusion,
even if the year of publication differs.
— H. D. Brown (1993) and J. D. Brown (1994) agreed that . . .
(7) Personal Communications:
Personal communications may be private letters, memos, some electronic
communications (e.g., e-mail, messages from non-archived discussion groups, or
electronic bulletin boards), personal interviews, telephone conversations and the
like. Because they do not provide recoverable data, personal communications are
not included in the reference list. Give the initials as well as the surname of the
communicator, and provide as exact a date as possible.
— T. K. Lutes (personal communication, April 18, 2010)
— (V.G. Nguyen, personal communication, September 28, 2009)
2) Reference List
All sources cited in the text must be included alphabetically in the reference list.
Below are examples of the entries: journal articles, books, articles or chapters in
the edited book, magazines or newspaper articles, reports, proceedings of
meetings and symposia, and doctoral dissertations or master’s theses. Reference
examples of online sources are added in each entry.
(1) Journal Articles
— Setter, J., & Jenkins, J. (2005). Pronunciation. Language Teaching, 38,
1-17.
— Sillick, T. J., & Schutte, N. S. (2006). Emotional intelligence and
self-esteem medicate between perceived early parental love and adult
happiness. E-Journal of Applied Psychology, 2(2), 38-48. Retrieved from
http://ojs.lib.swin.edu.au/index.php/ ejap
(2) Books
—Bachman, L. F. (2004). Statistical analyses for language assessment.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
—Strunk, W., Jr., & White, E. B. (Eds.). (1979). The elements of style. New
York, NY: Macmillan.
— American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
(3) Articles or Chapters in the Edited Book
— O’Grady, W. (2003). The radical middle: Nativism without universal
grammar. In C. Doughty & M. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second
language acquisition (pp. 43-103). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
(4) Magazines or Newspaper Articles
— Gardner, H. (1981, December). Do babies sing a universal song?
Psychology Today, pp. 70-76.
— Clay, R. (2008, June). Science vs. ideology: Psychologists fight back
about the misuse of research. Monitor on Psychology, 39(6). Retrieved from
http://www.apa.org/ monitor/
— Schwartz, J. (1993, September 30). Obesity affects economic, social
status. The Washington Post, pp. A1, A4.
— Brody, J. E. (2007, December 11). Mental reserves keep brain agile. The
New York Times, pp. 2-3. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com
(5) Reports
— Newport, E. L. (1975). Motherese: The speech of mothers to young
children (Tech. Rep. No. 53). San Diego, CA: University of California,
Center for Human Information Processing.
— Gottfredson, L. S. (1980). How valid are the reinforcement pattern
scores? (Report No. CSOS-R-292). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins
University, Center for Social Organization of Schools. (ERIC Document
Reproduction Service No. ED 182 465)
(6) Proceedings of Meetings and Symposia
— Miyagi, T. (2006, May). Technology-enhanced collaborative projects and
internet-based instruction. Paper presented at the conference of the
MATESOL program, San Francisco State University, San Francisco.
— Wells, C. G. (1984). Lexical-grammatical features of child language.
Proceedings of the Fourth International Symposium on Child Language (pp.
83-92). Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada: Mitchell Press.
— Muellbauer, J. (2007, September). Housing, credit, and consumer
expenditure. In S. C. Ludvigson (Chair), Housing and consumer behavior.
Symposium conducted at the meeting of the Federal Reserve Bank of Kansas
City, Jackson Hole, WY.
(7) Doctoral Dissertations or Master.s Theses
— Kevins, G. M. (1981). An analysis of ESL learners’ discourse patterns.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation. McGill University, Montreal.
— Appelbaum, L. G. (2005). Three studies of human information
processing: Texture amplification, motion representation, and figure-ground
segregation. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B. Sciences and
Engineering, 65(10), 5428.
— Foster, M. E. (1982). An analysis of the relationship between preservice
teacher training and directed teaching performance (Doctoral dissertation,
University of Chicago, 1981). Dissertation Abstract International, 42,
4409A.
— Pendar, J. E. (1982). Undergraduate psychology majors: Factors
influencing decisions. Dissertation Abstracts International, 42,
4370A-4371A. (University Microfilms No. 82-06, 181)
— Bruckman, A. (1997). MOOSE Crossing: Construction, community, and
learning in a networked virtual world for kids (Doctoral dissertation,
Massachusetts Institute of Technology). Retrieved from
http://www-static.cc.gatech.edu/~asb/thesis/
Journal of Extensive Reading in Foreign Language
Publisher
publication
Email
Homepage
Date of
publication
Jeong-Ryeol Kim
The Korean English Extensive Reading Association
317-1 English Education Dept. Teachers’ College
21 Mokwon-gil, Seo-gu, Daejeon
302-729, Korea
Tel. +82-42-829-7448
Fax. +82-42-829-7038
Mobile. +82-10-4132-2048
e-mail: yjjeon@mokwon.ac.kr
http://keera.or.kr/
31st of Jan., 2014
ISSN
(print)
ISSN
(online)