Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

Complexity in Asynchronous CMC Agreement and Disagreement

• So6llo (2000) Students in ACMC devoted 49% of their pos6ngs to responding to the teacher.• Student responses to each other,“suggested agreement and disagreement in indirect ways”(p. 104).• Anecdotal observa6on of superficial ACMC• No explicit previous research into agreement and disagreement in online language learning

Complexity in Asynchronous CMC Agreement and Disagreement CALICO 2011 Greg Kessler Ohio University General research into Agreement & Disagreement • General preference for agreement (or appearance of agreement) in oral exchanges (Brown & Levinson, 1987) • Foppa (1995) Disagreement always requires jusQficaQon…& it contains no informaQon, but rather the informaQon is contained within the jusQficaQon. • Disagreement in online exchanges can easily be seen as impolite (Angouri & Tseliga, 2010) Agreement & Disagreement Research • Morand and Ocker (2003) idenQfy disagreement as one of many face threatening act. • Scollon & Scollon (1983) idenQfy the significance of solidarity in agreement. • Holtgraves (1997) employed politeness theory to explore agreement, avoidance of disagreement, and seeking common ground in discussion of controversial topics. Disagreement in Online CommunicaQon • Chu‐Carroll & Carberry (1998) observed a]empts at reconciliaQon following disagreement. • Walker (1996) focused on the acceptance and rejecQon of disagreements. • Di‐Eugenio (2001) uQlized a holisQc perspecQve to idenQfy soluQons to disagreements. Agreement & Disagreement in Language Learning • Discussion boards can support students’ ability to agree and disagree within language learning contexts (Opp‐ Beckman & Kieffer, 2004). • SoQllo (2000) Students in ACMC devoted 49% of their posQngs to responding to the teacher. • Student responses to each other, “suggested agreement and disagreement in indirect ways” (p. 104). • Anecdotal observaQon of superficial ACMC • No explicit previous research into agreement and disagreement in online language learning Guiding Research Regarding Complexity • Various approaches to invesQgaQng CAF • ACMC and SCMC comparison, SoQllo (2000) – ACMC Promotes reflecQon & opportunity for elaboraQon • MulQdimensional “organic” perspecQves, Norris & Ortega (2009) – Comparison of measures – CauQon in interpretaQon/extrapolaQon • Balanced complexity, Schulze (2010) – Combining measures QuesQons • What is the nature of (dis)agreement and directness in online ACMC? • What is the relaQonship between syntacQc complexity and (dis)agreement in ACMC? • How do complexity measures compare across these discourse funcQons? • How does human vs automated complexity compare? Methodology • 40 NNES in 15 week online course • Weekly discussions on culture related topics • All response posts were included • 1662 response posts consisQng of 228,060 words • Two independent trained raters idenQfied direct and indirect agreement and disagreement acts (as outlined by B & L,1998) Methodology • SyntacQc complexity was observed in terms of three convenQonal measures: – General Complexity (Mean length of T‐unit) – Hunt, 1965 – Phrasal ElaboraQon (Mean length of clause) – Elder & Iwashita, 2005 – SubordinaQon (Clauses per T‐unit) – Sco], 1988 • Anova across the agreement types & measures • MulQcollinearity correlaQon comparing measures • Discriminant analysis Results • 1010 instances of agreement and disagreement – 964 of agreement • 831 direct • 133 indirect – 46 of disagreement • 31 indirect • 15 direct Results Results • Significant difference in complexity between agreement and disagreement (sub: clauses/ t‐ unit) • Indirect agreement was more syntacQcally complex than direct agreement across all measures • Indirect disagreement was more syntacQcally complex than direct disagreement Results • Results vary according to selected measure • Consistent with Norris & Ortega, 2009 • Complexity measures demonstrated significant mulQcollinearity • Mean length of T‐unit and mean length of clause correlated at .665 • Comparing complexity measures revealed redundancy of use of measures • Length of clause removed from study • Discriminant analysis provided insight into comparison across agreement types and measures Conclusions Q1 • Students may benefit from expectaQons for demonstraQng disagreement • Students may benefit from suggesQons & guidance on how to appropriately disagree • Students may benefit from explicit exposure to findings of SoQllo (2000) Conclusions Q2 • Complexity varies across acts of agreement and disagreement • Statements of disagreement are more complex than statements of agreement • Indirect statements are more complex than direct statements Conclusions Q3 • Different complexity measures offer different results • Varied measures should be employed • CombinaQons of complexity measures can be beneficial or redundant • Measure choice should be influenced by nature of discourse funcQons Looking at the entire text… …or organized by discourse function Scale and orientation can also influence interpretation Conclusions Q4 • Automated analysis of complexity is reliable Length T unit Length Clause Clauses per T unit IRR 0.95 IRR 0.97 IRR 0.98 Automated SyntacQc Complexity h]p://aihaiyang.com/synlex/syntacQc/ Lu, Xiaofei (2010). Automatic analysis of syntactic complexity in second language writing. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 15(4):474-496. " Thanks! Questions? http:call.ohio.edu/calico11a.ppt References • Angouri, J., & Tseliga, T. (2010). “you HAVE NO IDEA WHAT YOU ARE TALKING ABOUT!” A study of impoliteness strategies in two online fora. Angouri & Tseliga. Journal of Politeness Research 6(1), 57‐82. Special Issue on Politeness and Computer‐ Mediated CommunicaQon. Edited by Miriam Locher. • Brown, P. and Levinson, S.C. (1987) Politeness: Some Universals in Language Use. • Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. • Foppa, K. (1995). On mutual understanding and agreement in dialogues. In I. Marková, C. Graumann, & K. Foppa (Eds.), MutualiQes in dialogue (pp. 149–175). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. • Lu, Xiaofei (2010). AutomaQc analysis of syntacQc complexity in second language wriQng. InternaQonal Journal of Corpus LinguisQcs, 15(4):474‐496. • Norris, J. & Ortega, L. (2009). Towards an organic approach to invesQgaQng CAF in instructed SLA: The case of complexity. Applied LinguisQcs, 30(4), 555‐ 578. • Ogden, R. (2006). PhoneQcs and social acQon in agreements and disagreements. In Journal of PragmaQcs. • Opp‐Beckman, L. & Kieffer, C. (2004). A CollaboraQve Model for Online InstrucQon in the Teaching of Language and Culture. In S. Fotos and C.M.Browne (Eds.) New PerspecQves on CALL for Second Language Classrooms. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. • Schultze, M. (2010). Measuring textual complexity in student wriQng. American AssociaQon of Applied LinguisQcs Conference, Atlanta. • SoQllo, S. (2000). Discourse funcQons and syntacQc complexity in synchronous and asynchronous communicaQon. Language Learning & Technology, 4(1), 82‐119. Retrieved February 1, 2011, from h]p://llt.msu.edu/volumen4number1/soQllo/