Applied Psycholinguistics, page 1 of 29, 2013
doi:10.1017/S0142716413000222
An investigation of morphological
awareness and processing in adults
with low literacy
ELIZABETH L. TIGHE
Florida State University
KATHERINE S. BINDER
Mount Holyoke College
Received: May 8, 2012
Accepted for publication: October 11, 2012
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE
Elizabeth Tighe, Department of Psychology, Florida State University, 1107 West Call Street,
Tallahassee, FL 32306-4301. E-mail: tighe@psy.fsu.edu
ABSTRACT
Morphological awareness, which is an understanding of how words can be broken down into smaller
units of meaning such as roots, prefixes, and suffixes, has emerged as an important contributor to
word reading and comprehension skills. The first aim of the current study was to investigate the
contribution of morphological awareness independent of phonological awareness and decoding to
the reading comprehension abilities of adults with low literacy. Results indicated that morphological
awareness was a significant unique predictor of reading comprehension. A second aim of the study
was to investigate the processing of morphologically complex words of adults with low literacy in
both an oral reading passage and a single-word naming task. Adults’ accuracy and response times
were measured on different types of morphologically complex words and compared with control
words matched on frequency in both the passage and the naming tasks. Results revealed that adults
were vulnerable to morphological complexity: they performed more accurately and faster on matched
control words versus morphologically complex word types. The educational implications for Adult
Basic Education programs are discussed.
Literacy, the ability to extract meaning from written text, is an invaluable skill
enabling individuals to function in daily life. The 2003 National Assessment
of Adult Literacy, administered to a nationally representative sample of almost
20,000 adults, reports that approximately 14% of American adults read below the
basic literacy level and an additional 22% read at the basic literacy level (Kutner
et al., 2007). Research has shown that poor literacy skills are perpetuated through
generations. Children of adults with low literacy skills are disadvantaged upon
school entrance, which eventuates in a higher probability of dropping out (Kirsch,
Jungeblut, Jenkins, & Kolstad, 1993). To most effectively curb this problem, we
© Cambridge University Press 2013 0142-7164/13 $15.00
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
2
must understand the process by which adults acquire language and develop reading
skills.
Adult Basic Education (ABE) programs are designed to help diminish the
problem of adult low literacy by providing adults (ages 16 and older), who are
not concurrently enrolled in kindergarten to Grade 12 education, with instruction
and coursework to earn a General Educational Development (GED) certificate.
These programs serve approximately 2.6 million adults annually; however, this is
just a small fraction of the approximately 90 million US adults with low literacy
(National Research Council, 2012). Despite the prevalence of low literacy skills
among adults, there is a paucity of rigorous research investigating the reading skills
and best instructional approaches for this population. Several factors exacerbate
the need for high-quality research on adult reading skills and the efficacy of ABE
programs: a lack of systematic curriculum and testing materials, high attrition
rates, and heterogeneous demographics of the adult population.
The current study enhances the existing body of literature by investigating
morphological awareness, a conscious understanding of how words can be broken down into smaller units of meaning, and reading comprehension in adults
enrolled in ABE programs (Carlisle, 2000). In order to effectively evaluate the
literacy skills for this population, it is important to understand that adults might
follow a unique developmental trajectory when acquiring language and reading
skills (Perin, 1988; Thompkins & Binder, 2003). Unfortunately, most research
has focused exclusively on children’s acquisition of literacy skills. Thus, many
ABE programs utilize testing materials, instructional methods, and models of
reading that were developed for children. Although research on children can help
guide literacy research for struggling adult readers, adults and children differ in
several important reading areas such as exposure to printed word, experience with
language, and ability to use higher order cognitive functioning (Adams, 1990;
Hoffman, 1978; Perin, 1988; Thompkins & Binder, 2003).
READING SKILLS OF ADULTS WITH LOW LITERACY
The limited amount of existing literature on adults with low literacy has suggested
that adults across a range of skill levels in ABE programs have deficient decoding, phonological, receptive vocabulary, fluency, rapid automatized naming, and
reading comprehension skills (Greenberg, Ehri, & Perin, 1997, 2002; MacArthur,
Konold, Glutting, & Alamprese, 2010; Mellard & Fall, 2012; Mellard, Woods, &
Fall, 2011; National Research Council, 2012; Sabatini, 2002; Sabatini, Sawaki,
Shore, & Scarborough, 2010; Thompkins & Binder, 2003). Greenberg et al. (1997,
2002) reported that ABE students outperformed reading-achievement matched
children on word recognition tasks requiring orthographic knowledge (such as
sight-word reading). Conversely, children outperformed ABE students on word
recognition tasks requiring phonological knowledge (such as nonword reading).
Similarly, Thompkins and Binder (2003) reported that adult literacy students had
weaker phonological skills and more advanced orthographic knowledge than children matched on reading achievement levels. Comparing ABE students to skilled
adult readers, Binder and Borecki (2008) found that adults with low literacy relied
more on orthographic information and contextual clues than on phonological
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
3
information when silently reading connected texts that contained homophone
pairs.
To date, no studies with the adult low literacy population have investigated the
construct of morphological awareness. Morphological awareness falls under the
general umbrella term linguistic awareness, which also encompasses phonological
and orthographic awareness. All three linguistic awareness skills are necessary to
facilitate reading development and word learning (Berninger, Abbott, Nagy, &
Carlisle, 2010). Adult literacy research has found that ABE students have particularly strong orthographic abilities and have consistent deficiencies in decoding and
phonological abilities. However, the third component, morphological awareness,
has not been investigated in the context of adult literacy. Thus, the current study
wants to expand the body of literature by assessing morphological processing
and the contribution of morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and
decoding to reading comprehension in adults with low literacy.
MORPHOLOGICAL AND PHONOLOGICAL AWARENESS
Phonological awareness, the ability to distinguish and manipulate the sound structure of language, has consistently been found to be an important predictor of children’s reading comprehension (Adams, 1990; Good, Gruba, & Kaminski, 2001).
The English orthography does not rely exclusively on the alphabetic principle
because grapheme to phoneme correspondences are not always mapped perfectly
one to one. Instead, English has a deep orthography, which implies that a single
sound (phoneme) can be represented by several graphemes (e.g., bare and bear).
Thus, the English orthography is considered morphophonemic, meaning words are
characterized simultaneously by sounds (phonemes) and meanings (morphemes).
Phonemes are individual sound units that can build complex words, decompose
complex words, or be moved around to form new words. Similarly, morphemes
are the smallest units of meaning (e.g., prefixes and suffixes), which can be used
to build complex words, decompose complex words, and be moved to form new
words. If the English language were purely based on phonetics, words would
be spelled the way in which they sound (e.g., the past tense of trap would be
trapt). Since English spelling encompasses both morphological and phonological
components, readers understand that trap becomes trapped.
Several studies have tried to disentangle phonological awareness and morphological awareness and assess the unique contributions of these two constructs to
children’s reading skills. Jarmulowicz, Hay, Taran, and Ethington (2008) constructed a model to assess the relationship between phonological and morphological awareness and found that they are correlated yet distinct literacy skills.
Phonological awareness has a greater impact on reading skills up until third grade,
whereas morphological awareness builds on phonological abilities and becomes
a more important predictor of reading skills after third grade and through the
high school years. Berninger et al. (2010) conducted a longitudinal study investigating growth in phonological, orthographic, and morphological awareness for
first through sixth graders. The study reported that phonological and orthographic
awareness exhibited the greatest growth during the earlier elementary school
years. Growth in morphological awareness was primarily during first through
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
4
fourth grades; however, growth in derivational morphological awareness showed
considerable growth beyond the fourth-grade level. These researchers concluded
that phonological awareness is necessary but not sufficient on its own to account for
learning to read. Thus, they deduced that orthographic and morphological awareness should be included in reading models. Moreover, morphological awareness
displayed the longest developmental trajectory, with growth on different facets
of morphological awareness spanning from first through sixth grades. Finally,
several studies have investigated whether morphological awareness is predictive
of reading skills (both comprehension and word-level reading) after controlling
for phonological awareness (Deacon & Kirby, 2004; Kirby et al., 2012; Singson,
Mahony, & Mann, 2000). This research highlights the importance of distinguishing between phonological and morphological awareness and their independent
contributions to reading comprehension.
MORPHOLOGICAL AWARENESS AND READING ABILITIES
Several studies have documented that morphological awareness is an important
predictor of children’s reading comprehension across a range of grades (Carlisle,
1988, 2000; Deacon & Kirby, 2004; Kirby et al., 2012; Nagy, Berninger, & Abbott,
2006; Nagy, Berninger, Abbott, Vaughan, & Vermeulen, 2003). Carlisle (2000)
found that morphology tasks accounted for 43% of variance in reading comprehension for third graders and 55% of the variance in reading comprehension for
fifth graders. Nagy et al. (2006) investigated the role of morphological awareness,
phonological memory, and phonological decoding in predicting reading comprehension, reading vocabulary, and spelling in fourth/fifth graders, sixth/seventh
graders, and eighth/ninth graders. For the two younger groups, morphological
awareness emerged as the only unique predictor of reading comprehension. For
the eighth and ninth graders, all three skills (morphological awareness, phonological memory, and phonological decoding) were unique predictors of reading
comprehension; however, morphological awareness still accounted for the greatest
percentage of variance.
In addition, morphological awareness has been found to be a strong of predictor
of single-word reading with children across a wide range of grades (Deacon
& Kirby, 2004; Roman, Kirby, Parrila, Wade-Woolley, & Deacon, 2009; Singson
et al., 2000). Singson et al. (2000) found that morphological awareness contributed
uniquely to reading abilities (word reading and decoding skills) for third through
sixth graders over and above phonological awareness and receptive vocabulary
knowledge. Kirby et al. (2012) reported that independent of verbal and nonverbal
IQ and phonological awareness, morphological awareness was a significant predictor of word reading accuracy and speed as well as reading comprehension in
first through third graders. The current study investigated accuracy and response
times on morphologically complex words embedded in a passage as well as
morphologically complex words in a single-word recognition task. In addition,
because morphological awareness has been found to be a strong predictor of both
reading comprehension and single-word decoding, the present study looked at
the contribution of morphological awareness over and above the contributions of
decoding and phonological awareness to reading comprehension.
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
5
MORPHOLOGY AND CONTEXT
Contextual clues contained in printed texts have been found to promote the processing and understanding of unfamiliar morphologically complex words (Sternberg,
1987). Anglin (1993) reported that context effects begin to play a more important
role in word learning after the third grade because children are able to apply
morphological analysis to infer the meaning of new words. The average fifth
grader reads approximately 1 million words of text in a year and of those words,
approximately 15,000 to 55,000 different words are novel to the reader (Nagy,
Herman, & Anderson, 1985). Moreover, morphologically complex words account
for more than 60% of the vocabulary words children encounter during reading after fourth grade (Egan & Pring, 2004; Nagy & Anderson, 1984; Nagy, Anderson,
Schommer, Scott, & Stallman, 1989). Thus, the ability to engage in morphological
decomposition and utilize contextual clues in printed text becomes important at
increasing grade levels to identifying novel morphologically complex words.
Wysocki and Jenkins (1987) tested fourth, sixth, and eighth graders on their
abilities to use morphological and contextual information to define unfamiliar
words. The researchers investigated the use of sentence context by presenting
unfamiliar words in strong and weak contexts. A strong context was defined by
including clues that would help students infer the meaning of the word, whereas
a weak context included little or no indication of word meaning. The sixth and
eighth graders were better at combining contextual and morphological clues than
were the fourth graders, showing significant improvement in identifying unfamiliar
word meanings in both strong and weak contexts. The ability of older students
to combine information from both morphological rules and context demonstrates
a hypothesis set out by Nagy and Anderson (1984) that morphological rules and
context work together. Several studies have suggested that adults with low literacy
might compensate for poor word decoding skills by developing compensatory
strategies such as utilizing contextual clues when reading connected text (Binder
& Borecki, 2008; Blalock, 1981; Read & Ruyter, 1985). The current study investigated the ability of adults with low literacy to utilize contextual clues by measuring
accuracy on morphologically complex words presented in connected text versus
words presented in isolation in a word recognition task.
DEVELOPMENT OF INFLECTIONAL AND DERIVATIONAL
MORPHOLOGICAL RULES
Morphological research differentiates between two types of morphologically complex words: inflected and derived (Carlisle, 2003). Inflectional morphemes alter
the tense or pluralize the root word but keep the word class intact (e.g., pull to
pulled). Derivational morphemes can change the meaning of the root word and
can also alter the word class (e.g., help to helpless). Children are able to apply
inflectional morphology as early as the preschool years (Berko, 1958; Brittain,
1970; Clark & Hecht, 1982). Although preschoolers have some knowledge of
inflectional endings, they have more difficulty with more complex inflectional
endings, such as knowing to apply –es rather than just –s (Berko, 1958). Studies
have reported that young children have a tendency to produce overregulation errors
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
6
when applying the past tense inflection –ed (e.g., runned; Brown, 1973; Marcus
et al., 1992). These types of errors demonstrate that children implicitly understand
inflectional morphological rules even though these words are orthographically and
phonologically incorrect.
Knowledge and awareness of derivational morphology begins as early as
preschool but rapidly increases from first to fourth grades and continues to develop
until adulthood (Carlisle, 1988, 2003; Clark, 1982). Preschoolers have the ability
to understand simple derivational affixes such as adding the –er agentive suffix
to words (e.g., teacher); however, they have more difficulty applying derivational
affixes to words that undergo orthographic and/or phonological shifts (Anglin,
1993). Most studies of morphological awareness have focused exclusively on
inflected morphology in younger children or exclusively on derivational morphology in older children; however, in order to understand morphological awareness
as a broader construct, it is important to incorporate both inflected and derived
morphology within a single study.
Some studies with adult literacy students matched with children on achievement
level have reported that adults consistently make morpheme ending errors when
spelling both inflected and derived words (Liberman, Rubin, Duques, & Carlisle,
1985; Viise, 1996; Worthy & Viise, 1996). For example, Worthy and Viise (1996)
reported that adults made many omissions of inflectional endings—crack for
cracking. Furthermore, the adults made guesses on unfamiliar words that were
not related semantically. These findings suggest that adults with low literacy skills
have not yet mastered inflected and derived morphologically complex words;
therefore, it is imperative to include both word types in morphological tasks for
this population.
MORPHOLOGY AND WORD FREQUENCY AND TRANSPARENCY
Morphological research has also investigated the role of word frequency and
familiarity in determining a reader’s awareness of the morphological structure
of words (Carlisle, 2003; Carlisle & Katz, 2006; Egan & Pring, 2004; Reichle
& Perfetti, 2003). Carlisle and Katz (2006) found that the frequency of derived
words, root words, and the size of the word family are important in assisting with
word recognition. All words have frequency values determined by the Standard
Frequency Index (SFI; Carroll, Davies, & Richman, 1971). Words with greater
SFIs are more familiar to elementary school children and, thus, more rapidly
identified and processed than those with lower SFIs (Larsen & Nippold, 2007).
In addition, the word characteristics of transparency and opaqueness are important in determining a reader’s awareness of the morphological structure of
words (Carlisle, 2000, 2003; Carlisle & Stone, 2005). Phonological transparency
refers to base words that are intact in the derived form of the word: growth is
phonologically transparent because the root word, grow, retains its pronunciation.
In contrast, phonological opaqueness refers to a pronunciation change from the
base word to the derived word form: finality represents a phonological shift because
the pronunciation of final changes.
Carlisle and Stone (2005) utilized measures of frequency and transparency in
their study examining the role of morphemes in the speed and accuracy with which
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
7
lower and upper elementary students read derived words. In the first part of the
study, students were presented with individual words transparent in both spelling
and sound. Words contained two morphemes (hilly) and single-morpheme, pseudocomplex derived words (silly) and were matched for spelling, word length, and
word frequency. The students were also presented with low-frequency derived
words, all of which contained high-frequency base forms (puzzlement), to investigate the role of base word familiarity on speed and accuracy in word recognition.
Results indicated that all of the students read derived two-morpheme words more
accurately and faster than they did single-morpheme words. In addition, upper
and lower elementary students were more accurate and faster at identifying highfrequency derived two-morpheme words as compared with derived low-frequency
words with high-frequency bases. However, there was a greater difference in
accuracy and speed for high- versus low-frequency words in lower elementary
students compared to upper elementary students. Thus, upper elementary students were better able to recognize the high-frequency bases in order to facilitate
recognition of low-frequency words.
The second part of the Carlisle and Stone (2005) study investigated the speed
and accuracy in which middle and high schoolers read derived words that differed
in phonological transparency (shift and stable words). The researchers compiled a
list of phonological shift words (e.g., majority) and stable words (e.g., maturity),
which controlled for spelling, word length, base frequency, and derived-word
frequency. Previous research indicates that words undergoing phonological shifts
present difficulties for children learning to read (Carlisle, 2000). The researchers
hypothesized that middle school students would read stable words faster and more
accurately than shift words, but they posited that this difference would disappear
by high school. However, the results indicated that both middle and high schoolers
were more accurate on stable versus shift words.
CURRENT STUDY
The present study had two primary aims. First, we investigated the contribution of
morphological awareness independent of phonological awareness and decoding to
reading comprehension in adults enrolled in ABE programs. Within an adult literacy framework, past research has investigated the linguistic awareness constructs
of phonological and orthographic knowledge. We wanted to expand the existing
research by looking at the third component of linguistic awareness: morphological
awareness. Previous research with children has found that morphological awareness is an important predictor of reading comprehension after controlling for
phonological awareness (Deacon & Kirby, 2004; Kirby et al., 2012). We wanted
to assess if morphological awareness was also a unique contributor to reading
comprehension for adults with low literacy.
Second, the other aim of the current study was to examine morphological
processing in adults with low literacy by measuring accuracy and response times
on different types of printed morphologically complex words presented in an
oral reading passage and a single-word naming task. If morphological awareness
was found to be an important contributor to reading comprehension for this
population, we wanted to assess what aspects of morphological complexity adults
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
8
were sensitive to. In accordance with a broader definition of morphological
awareness, we included both inflected and derived word types. In addition, the
study incorporated derived words that were phonologically transparent, opaque,
and included high- and low-frequency words from the Carlisle and Stone (2005)
study. By including a passage and a single-word recognition task, we were able
to look at the role of context in the accuracy and response times of identifying
morphologically complex words.
The present study addressed four research questions:
1. Does morphological awareness account for additional variance, independent of
phonological awareness and decoding skills, for adults with low literacy skills?
2. How accurate and how fast are response times of adults with low literacy skills
on inflected morphologically complex words compared with single-morpheme
matched control words in an oral reading passage and a single-word naming task?
3. How accurate and how fast are response times on derived morphologically complex words compared with matched control words in an oral reading passage and
a single-word naming task?
4. Does context influence accuracy?
We hypothesized that similar to research with children, morphological awareness would make a unique contribution to reading comprehension independent of
phonological awareness and decoding skills for adults with low literacy (Deacon
& Kirby, 2004; Kirby et al., 2012). We also hypothesized that adults would be
more accurate and faster at responding to matched control words as compared
with morphologically complex words on all word types. A difference in accuracy
and response times between morphologically complex words and matched control
words would indicate sensitivity to morphological complexity. Similar to Carlisle
and Stone (2005), we predicted that adults would be less accurate and show slower
response times on phonological shift compared to stable words and low-frequency
words compared to high-frequency words. Based on spelling research with adult
literacy students, we hypothesized lower accuracy on inflected endings compared
to single-morpheme matched control words (Viise, 1996; Worthy & Viise, 1996).
Previous research has found that children rely on contextual clues and morphological awareness to decipher novel words (Nagy & Anderson, 1984). Further,
adult literacy research has shown that adults rely on contextual clues during reading to mask word-decoding difficulties (Binder & Borecki 2008; Read & Ruyter,
1985). Thus, we hypothesized that context would aid ABE students in recognizing
unfamiliar morphologically complex words.
METHOD
Participants
Participants included 57 adults from an urban ABE program located in western
Massachusetts. In this program, adult learners spend 3 hr a day, 5 days a week
in the classroom. The majority of this time is spent with a classroom instructor.
Most of the instructional time is devoted to teaching the whole group, but some
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
9
time is spent in breakout groups in which a tutor might work with the group
or individuals to develop their skills. This site also has access to a computer
lab, so if software is available, the students spend their time on software that is
relevant to their lessons. The participants consisted of 39% males (n = 22) and
61% females (n = 35). Their ages ranged from 18 to 57 years (M = 26.9). The
racial and ethnic background of the participants was varied: 42% had a Hispanic
background, 37% were African American, 18% were Caucasian, and 3% were
Asian. There were a few participants (14%) who reported that they had been
previously diagnosed with a learning disability. The participants included 12 from
the English for speakers of other languages (ESOL) Level 3 class, 13 from the
pre-GED (equivalent of 5th- to 8th-grade levels) class, and 32 from the GED (9thto 12th-grade level). All of the pre-GED adults included in the study were reading
at the higher end of the grade level based on their Test of Adult Basic Education—
Reading scores. We collected data on their phonological decoding skills and their
passage comprehension abilities, and the mean grade level equivalency for these
skills were 5.8 (SD = 3.31) and 4.4 (SD = 2.11), respectively. In addition, all
ESOL participants were native Spanish speakers and all were literate in their
native language.1 The educational background of the participants was varied:
7.7% had fewer than 6 years of formal education, 23% had received some middle
school exposure, and 69.2% had spent some time in high school. Our participant
demographics are consistent with those reported from ABE programs across the
United States and thus considered to be a representative sample (see National
Research Council, 2012). Of the 57 participants, only 52 completed both days of
testing. Participants received $15.00 as compensation for their time.
Materials
Participants were administered a battery of literacy assessments measuring morphological, phonological, decoding, and reading comprehension skills. In addition,
an experimenter-designed oral reading passage and a single-word naming task
were administered.
Morphological awareness
We incorporated the three tests described below to assess morphological awareness. These morphological awareness tasks were chosen because they were consistently found throughout the morphology literature and consisted of both real-word
and nonword tasks. Because these three measures were highly correlated, we created a composite score, referred to as morphological awareness for our analyses.
The Cronbach α coefficient for the measures was 0.82.
Base form morphology task
The base form morphology task, adapted from Leong (2000) and Carlisle (1988),
aimed to assess morphological structure by recognition of the root word of derived
target words. The participant deconstructed derivational target words into base
words. The examiner read aloud a derived target word, which served as a prime for
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
10
the participant. Next, the examiner read a short sentence with a blank at the end. The
participant was expected to provide the base word of the target word given in the
beginning of the sentence. For example, “Growth. She wanted her plant to _____”;
“grow”. The task included four different conditions: no-change (growth–grow),
orthographic-change (foggy–fog), phonological-change (popularity–popular), and
a both-change (width–wide). The task was adapted from previous studies to include
equal numbers of no change, orthographic shift, phonological shift, and both shift
items. A correct answer received 1 point and an incorrect or no answer received
0 points. Participants were given a practice round of 2 items and then completed
seven sentences in each condition for a total of 30 items. The test was discontinued
if the participant made six errors. The Cronbach α coefficient for the base form
morphology task was 0.97 for the sample.
Derived form morphology task
This task, adapted from Leong (2000) and Carlisle (1988), was similar in layout to
the base form morphology task. Again, the task was adapted to have equal numbers
of no change, orthographic shift, phonological shift, and both shift items. The task
assessed an individual’s ability to transform a base word into a derived word. The
examiner provided participants with a base word, followed by a sentence concluding with a blank. The participant was asked to fill in the blank with the proper
derived form of the word. For example, “Explain. His excuse was a bad _______”;
“explanation.” The same four conditions were used: no change, orthographic
change, phonological change, and both change. Correct answers received one point
and incorrect or no answers received zero points. A practice round of 2 items was
included and participants completed seven sentences in each condition for a total
of 30 items. The test ended if the participant made six errors. The Cronbach α reliability coefficient for the derived form morphology task was 0.97 for the sample.
Derivational Suffix Choice Test of Pseudowords
This test was designed to assess an individual’s ability to manipulate morphemes
using nonwords (Mahony, 1994; Singson et al., 2000). The test was given to the
participants in written form and administered orally to avoid reading difficulties.
The test displayed a sentence with a blank, and the participant was prompted
to select the appropriate answer from four listed choices. For example, “Our
teacher taught us how to _______ long words. Answer choices included jittling,
jittles, jittled, and jittle. The correct response, jittle, received one point, while an
incorrect answer or no answer resulted in zero points. Participants were given 14
of these items; however, the test was stopped if the participant made six errors.
The Cronbach α reliability coefficient for the Derivational Suffix Choice Test of
Pseudowords was 0.84 for the sample.
Phonological awareness
Dynamic indicators of basic early literacy skills phoneme segmentation fluency.
The phoneme segmentation fluency subtest measures phonological awareness
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
11
by testing the ability to break real words into their subsequent phonemes. The
examiner presented a word orally to the participant and asked the participant to
say all the sounds in the word. For example, if given the word mop, the correct
response would be “/m/ /o/ /p/.” Participants were timed for 1 min. Participants
were required to say each individual sound to receive full credit. The correct
number of phonemes per minute determined the phoneme segmentation fluency
rate (Good & Kaminski, 2002). Binder, Snyder, Ardoin, and Morris (2011) used
this test in an ABE setting and obtained a Cronbach α of 0.87.
Decoding
We utilized two measures of decoding, which are described below. These two
measures were highly correlated and thus we utilized a composite score referred
to as decode in our analyses. The Cronbach α coefficient for the two measures was
0.79.
Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests—Revised (WRMT-R) word attack and letter word
ID. The word attack subtest of the WRMT-R assesses an individual’s ability to
decode nonwords (Woodcock, 1987). Each participant was presented with 45
nonwords, such as nat or ib, and asked to read them aloud. A correct response
elicited a point only if the whole word was pronounced correctly. No response,
incorrect syllable pronunciation, or reading the syllables disjointedly resulted in
no points. The test was discontinued if the participant answered 6 incorrectly. This
test has a reliability of 0.92 (Woodcock, 1987). Binder et al. (2011) used this test
in an ABE setting and obtained a Cronbach α of 0.94.
The letter word identification subtest of the WRMT-R assesses participant’s
abilities to recognize and pronounce individual letters and words. Participants
were shown a binder containing pages with letters and groups of single words
and asked to identify specific words/letters. The words increased in difficulty as
the task progressed. The test was suspended if the participant answered six words
incorrectly. This test has a reliability of 0.97 (Woodcock, 1987). Binder et al.
(2011) reported a Cronbach α of 0.95 with ABE students.
Reading comprehension
The passage comprehension subtest of the WRMT-R is designed to measure
readers’ abilities to understand words and sentences by asking participants to rely
on pictures and contextual cues (Woodcock, 1987). Participants were presented
with a series of pictures with written words and asked to pick the picture that was
described by the written words. Next, the participant was presented with sentences,
each with a missing word, which participants were asked to supply. Either a picture
or contextual cues within the sentence were provided to enable the participant to
fill in the missing word. For example, “The drums were pounding in the distance.
We could ____ them.” Participants were expected to supply the correct answer,
“hear,” and testing was suspended if participants provided six incorrect responses.
This test has a reliability of 0.93 (Woodcock, 1987). Binder et al. (2011) reported
a Cronbach α of 0.92 with the ABE students.
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
12
Oral reading passage
A narrative passage containing 327 words was constructed with particular attention to different inflectional endings and types of derived words. For inflected
endings, the passage included eight –s words, eight –ed words, and six –ing
words, each of which was matched based on frequency with single-morpheme
control words utilizing the SFI (e.g., times and since; walked and stay; smiling
and marry; Appendix A). The passage also included 34 derived words from the
Carlisle and Stone (2005) study: low-frequency derived words with high-frequency
bases matched with high-frequency, high-frequency base words (e.g., flowery and
lucky); stable versus phonological shift words matched on frequency and word
length (e.g., cultural and natural); and high-frequency, two-morpheme words
matched with pseudocomplex derived, single-morpheme words (e.g., hilly and
silly). The derived words from the Carlisle and Stone (2005) study were matched
based on spelling, word length, base frequency, and derived-word frequency (Appendix A). The target words were not predicted from the semantic context of the
passage. Thus, for the most part, the target words were in a neutral context, but
certainly the syntax of the passage aided word identification. This was probably
most important for the derived words because the affixes often alter the part of
speech.
Participants were asked to read the entire passage aloud while the researcher
tape-recorded and marked errors on a score sheet. The same passage was administered to all participants. Number of errors were recorded on a score sheet—“1”
for correct and “0” for incorrect. A single scorer recorded all errors during test
administration. A random 20% of the passage errors were rescored utilizing the
tape-recording to ensure that scoring was accurate. Accuracy was 100% between
scoring checks.
Naming task
The experimenter-designed naming task utilized the same word types as those used
in the oral reading passage. All words were matched based on frequency with the
corresponding word types in the oral reading passage. For inflected endings, the
naming task included three –s words, three –ed words, and three –ing words, each
of which was matched based on frequency with single-morpheme control words
utilizing the SFI (Appendix B). For derived words, the naming task included 30
words from the Carlisle and Stone (2005) study including: low-frequency derived
words with high-frequency bases matched with high-frequency, high-frequency
base words (e.g., beastly and icy); stable versus phonological shift words matched
on frequency and word length (e.g., precision and confusion); and high-frequency,
two-morpheme words matched with pseudocomplex derived, single-morpheme
words (e.g., windy and candy). The derived words from the Carlisle and Stone
(2005) study were matched based on spelling, word length, base frequency, and
derived-word frequency (Appendix B).
Participants were shown a total of 48 words on a computer screen, each of which
was presented in isolation for up to 5 s. Participants read each word aloud and the
E-Prime computer program recorded the response time data while the researcher
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
13
Table 1. Correlations between literacy assessments
Tasks
1. DMORPH
2. BMORPH
3. Suffix choice
4. MA total
5. DIBELS PSF
6. Word attack
7. Letter word
8. Decode total
9. Passage comp
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
— .68** .74** .92** .47** .58** .75** .73** .77**
— — .66** .90** .45** .46** .60** .59** .71**
— —
— .84** .39** .63** .66** .71** .69**
— —
—
— .49** .60** .74** .74** .81**
— —
—
—
— .54** .49** .55** .57**
— —
—
—
—
— .68** .89** .51**
— —
—
—
—
—
— .94** .60**
— —
—
—
—
—
—
— .61**
— —
—
—
—
—
—
—
—
Note: DMORPH, derived form morphology task; BMORPH, base form
morphology task; MA, morphological awareness; DIBELS PSF, dynamic
indicators of basic early literacy skills phoneme segmentation fluency subtest. N
= 55 for DMORPH, word attack, DIBELS, letter word ID, and decode total; N
= 54 for BMORPH and suffix choice; N = 52 for MA.
**p < .01.
kept track of errors. A random 20% of the naming task was rescored by listening
to the tape-recording and accuracy was 100%.
Procedure
The tasks were administered individually to the participants in two 30-min sessions
over a 3-day span. The order of the sessions and the order of the tasks within the
sessions were counterbalanced. Participants were recruited on a voluntary basis,
and the only requirement for participation was a signed informed consent form.
Testing took place in a quiet classroom at the center.
RESULTS
Regression of morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and
decoding on reading comprehension
Table 1 presents the correlations between all morphological awareness, phonological awareness, decoding, and reading comprehension measures. All of these
literacy assessments were positively correlated.
We performed a multiple regression with the composite decoding score, composite morphological awareness score, and phonological awareness as predictor
variables and passage comprehension as the outcome variable. We expected that
morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and decoding would be significant predictors of reading abilities for adults with low literacy skills. The
regression equation was significant, F (3, 48) = 37.70, p < .001, accounting for
70.2% of the variance. Both morphological and phonological awareness were
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
14
Table 2. Regression analysis of phonological and morphological
awareness on reading comprehension tasks
Tasks
β
SE
t
Phonological awareness
Morphological awareness
Decode
0.25*
0.75***
−0.08
0.048
0.038
0.050
2.66
6.30
−0.67
Note: N = 52. R2 = .702.
*p < .05. ***p < .001.
significant unique predictors, but decoding was not a unique predictor of reading
comprehension (see Table 2).
We also wanted to determine how much additional variance morphological
awareness would contribute to the explanation of reading comprehension above
phonological awareness. We performed a hierarchical regression with passage
comprehension as the outcome measure. In the first block, we entered only phonological awareness, and the regression was significant, F (1, 50) = 24.24, p <
.001, accounting for 32.6% of the variance. In the second block, we included
morphological awareness, to see if this addition significantly altered R2 . The
overall regression was significant, F (2, 49) = 56.97, p < .001. Furthermore,
morphological awareness explained 37.3% of the variance beyond phonological
awareness.
Accuracy on inflected morphologically complex words
One of our primary questions addressed the word identification accuracy on
inflected morphologically complex words as compared with single-morpheme
matched control words between words in context (oral reading passage) and
isolated words (naming task). We performed a 2 Morphological Complexity (morphologically complex vs. single-morpheme matched control words)×2 Task Type
(oral reading passage vs. naming task) repeated measures analysis of variance
(ANOVA) to investigate accuracy levels within participants. Percent variables
were calculated to adjust for different total numbers of inflected and matched
control words between the passage and naming tasks. A Bonferroni corrected α
level of 0.025 was utilized to determine significant effects.
We found a significant main effect of morphological complexity, such that
participants were significantly more accurate at reading the matched control words
(M = 95.6%) as compared with the inflected complex words (M = 85.7%), F (1,
51) = 45.55, p < .001. As expected, this finding demonstrates that adults with low
literacy skills are vulnerable to morphologically complex words in print, and they
experience difficulty with these words. There was no significant main effect of
task type, F (1, 51) = 2.52, p > .05. Participants performed similarly on inflected
words and matched control words in both the oral reading passage (M = 91.4%)
and the naming task (M = 89.9%).
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
15
There was a significant two-way interaction between morphological complexity
and task type, F (1, 51) = 19.48, p < .001. Within both the oral reading passage
and the naming task, single-morpheme matched control words (Ms = 93.9% and
97.2%, respectively) were read more accurately than morphologically complex
words (Ms = 88.9% and 82.5%, respectively; p = .002, p < .001). Interaction
contrasts revealed that the magnitude of difference in accuracy between inflected
complex words and single-morpheme matched controls was significantly larger
in isolation as compared with the magnitude of difference in accuracy between
inflected complex and single-morpheme matched control words in context, F
(1, 51) = 19.48, p < .001 (see Figure 1, panel 1). These findings demonstrate
sensitivity to morphological complexity in printed words, such that ABE students
performed more accurately on single-morpheme matched control words versus
inflected complex words. Further, ABE students were better at identifying words
presented in context as compared to words presented in isolation.
Accuracy on derived morphologically complex words
We were also interested in printed word identification accuracy on types of derived morphologically complex words versus matched control words presented in
context and in isolation. We performed a separate 2 Morphological Complexity
(morphologically complex vs. matched control words)×2 Task Type (oral reading
passage vs. naming task) repeated measures ANOVA for each of our three derived
word types. Percent variables were calculated to adjust for different total numbers
of types of derived words between the oral reading passage and the naming task. A
Bonferroni corrected α level of 0.025 was utilized to determine significant effects.
Low-frequency, high-frequency base versus high-frequency, high-frequency base
words. We found a significant main effect of morphological complexity, such
that the high-frequency base matched control words (M = 89.3%) were read more
accurately than the low-frequency, high-frequency base words (M = 73.8%),
F (1, 51) = 52.12, p < .001. As predicted, there was a significant main effect of
task type such that participants were more accurate at reading words in context
(M = 86%) than at reading them in isolation (M = 77.1%), F (1, 51) = 17.76,
p < .001. This indicates that context aided the adults with low literacy skills in
identifying morphologically complex words.
There was a significant interaction between morphological complexity and task
type for low-frequency versus high-frequency base matched control words, F (1,
51) = 37.76, p < .001. Within the naming task, participants were significantly
more accurate on the matched control, high-frequency words (M = 90.8%) as
compared with the low-frequency words (M = 63.5%); however, in context there
was no significant difference in accuracy between low-frequency (M = 84.1%)
and high-frequency base matched control words (M = 87.9%; p < .001, p =
.090, respectively; see Figure 1, panel 2). This finding once again demonstrates
that adults with low literacy skills are vulnerable to morphological complexity in
print and that the adults are more accurate at recognizing words in context than in
isolation.
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
Figure 1. The mean accuracy on all word types in context and in isolation.
16
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
17
Phonological shift versus stable words. We found a significant main effect of
morphological complexity, such that stable matched control words (M = 83.9%)
were read more accurately than phonological shift words (M = 57.3%), F (1, 51) =
103.32, p < .001. There was also a significant main effect of task type, such that
printed words were read more accurately in context (M = 82.8%) than words in
isolation (M = 58.4%), F (1, 51) = 62.95, p < .001.
There was a significant interaction between morphological complexity and task
type for phonological shift versus stable words, F (1, 51) = 75.36, p < .001.
Within each task type participants were more accurate at identifying stable words
(M = 86.5% passage; M = 81.3% naming) than shift words (M = 79.1% passage;
M = 35.6% naming; p = .001, p < .001, respectively). Interaction contrasts
demonstrated that the magnitude of difference in accuracy between the shift words
and the stable matched control words in isolation was significantly larger than the
magnitude of difference in accuracy between the shift and stable words in context,
F (1, 51) = 75.36, p < .001 (see Figure 1, panel 3). These results indicate that
adults performed more accurately on the stable matched control words as compared
with the shift words and were more accurate reading words in context versus in
isolation.
Complex versus pseudocomplex words. We found a significant main effect of
morphological complexity such that participants were significantly more accurate
at reading the pseudocomplex matched control words (M = 94.4%) than the
morphologically complex words (M = 89.3%), F (1, 51) = 15.65, p < .001. This
indicates that adults with low literacy skills are vulnerable to derived morphologically complex words, responding more accurately to pseudocomplex matched
control words. There was not a significant main effect of task type, indicating
that participants performed similarly reading words in isolation (M = 93.7%) and
when reading words in context (M = 90.1%), F (1, 51) = 4.43, p < .05. There was
also no significant interaction between task type and morphological complexity
for morphologically complex versus pseudocomplex matched control words, F (1,
51) = 0.21, p > .05 (see Figure 1, panel, 4).
Response times on the oral reading passage and naming task
Response time data were collected for both the oral reading passage and the
naming task; however, the way in which response time data were collected differed
between the two tasks. For the naming task, the response time was measured from
the presentation of the word to the time the participant made his first articulation
of the word. For the oral reading passage, the response time was the length of time
needed to read the entire word. These data were extracted from the sound files
using PRATT software.
Participants’ times for each of the word types were averaged, and any words
read incorrectly were eliminated. For the naming task only, data points related to
equipment malfunctions were removed: words under 200 ms or above 3000 ms.
This resulted in the removal of 3.86% of the words. Finally, for both tasks we
purged outliers above 2 SD from the mean: an additional 4.69% of words in the
naming task and 3.32% of words in the passage.
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
18
Although the two measures of response times were different, we ran a 2 Morphological Complexity×2 Task Type repeated measures ANOVA for our inflected
word type, similar to our analysis of the accuracy data. Similarly, we ran 2 Morphological Complexity × 2 Task Type repeated measures ANOVAs for the three
derived word types. We wanted to see if the matched control words were read faster
than the morphologically complex target words, regardless of which response type
measure was used. In addition, we wanted to examine the interaction between
morphological complexity and task type to see if the magnitude of difference in
response times was smaller in the passage than in the naming task. If this turned
out to be the case, it would indicate that contextual clues played an important role.
It is important to note that for the main effect of task type, we could not directly
compare differences between the naming task and the passage because response
time data were collected differently. Instead, we expected that the response times
would always be faster in the passage than in the naming task for two reasons:
the context was useful in the accuracy data, and the response type measure in the
passage did not measure the time it took participants to process the word. Results
indicated that this was the case for all word types.
Inflected versus inflected matched control words. We found a significant main
effect of morphological complexity, such that participants were significantly faster
at reading the inflected matched control words (M = 568.43 ms) as compared with
the inflected complex words (M = 603.33 ms), F (1, 47) = 20.41, p < .001.
As expected, this finding demonstrates that adults with low literacy skills are
vulnerable to morphologically complex words and have difficulty with them. The
interaction between morphological complexity and task type was not significant,
F (1, 47) = 1.21, p > .05 (see Figure 2, panel 1). Thus, the magnitude of difference
in response times between inflected complex words and single-morpheme matched
controls was no different in isolation as compared with the magnitude of difference
in response times between inflected complex words and single-morpheme matched
controls in context.
Low-frequency, high-frequency base versus high-frequency, high-frequency base
words. There was a significant main effect of morphological complexity, such
that the high-frequency base matched control words (M = 580.66 ms) were read
faster than low-frequency, high-frequency base words (M = 787.97 ms), F (1,
46) = 276.34, p < .001. There was not a significant interaction between morphological complexity and task type for low-frequency versus high-frequency base
matched control words, F (1, 46) = 0.018, p > .05 (see Figure 2, panel 2). Therefore, the magnitude of difference in response times between the low-frequency
words and the high-frequency base matched control words in the isolated word
naming task was approximately the same as the magnitude of difference in response times between the low-frequency and the high-frequency base matched
control words found in context.
Phonological shift versus stable words. There was not a main effect of mor-
phological complexity, F (1, 30) = 3.25, p = .082, but there was a significant
interaction between morphological complexity and task type for phonological
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
Figure 2. The mean response times on all word types in context and in isolation.
19
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
20
shift versus stable matched control words, F (1, 30) = 4.82, p = .036. Interaction
contrasts demonstrated that the magnitude of difference in response times between
the shift words (829.51 ms) and the stable matched control words (749.97 ms)
was significantly different in the naming task, but the difference in response times
between the shift (676.68 ms) and the stable words (682.30 ms) was eliminated
when the words were encountered in context, F (1, 30), = 4.82, p = .036 (see
Figure 2, panel 3).
Complex versus pseudocomplex words. There was a significant main effect of
morphological complexity, such that the pseudocomplex matched control words
(M = 563.66 ms) were read faster than the complex words (M = 591.54 ms), F (1,
49) = 10.49, p = .002. In addition, there was a significant interaction between morphological complexity and task type for complex versus pseudocomplex matched
control words, F (1, 49) = 10.15, p = .003. Interaction contrasts revealed that the
magnitude of difference in response times between complex words (M = 729.20
ms) and pseudocomplex words (M = 674.89 ms) was significantly different in the
naming task, but the difference in response times between complex (453.89 ms)
and pseudocomplex words (452.42 ms) was eliminated in context, F (1, 49), =
10.15, p = .003 (see Figure 2, panel 4).
DISCUSSION
The purpose of this study was to examine morphological skills in a sample of
adults with low literacy. First, we wanted to assess the contribution of morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and decoding to reading comprehension.
Second, we wanted to investigate the accuracy and response times on different
types of printed morphologically complex words as compared to matched control
words in an oral reading passage and a word naming task. Results indicated that the
majority of our hypotheses were supported. Both morphological and phonological
awareness were found to be unique predictors of reading comprehension. Further,
morphological awareness contributed additional variance beyond phonological
awareness. For morphological processing, adults were faster and more accurate
at reading matched control words than morphologically complex words in both
the passage and the naming task. Moreover, words presented in context were
read faster and more accurately than words presented in isolation. Similar to
research that has been done with children, these findings suggest that morphological awareness is an important contributor to reading comprehension in adults
with low literacy. The findings also indicate that adults with low literacy skills
are vulnerable to the morphological complexities of printed words. This study is
the first to examine morphological awareness and morphological processing in
adults with low literacy and has important implications for understanding adults’
acquisition of literacy skills and for instructional practices in ABE programs.
Predictors of reading comprehension
Morphological awareness has been found to play a significant role in reading
comprehension, after controlling for phonological awareness and word decoding
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
21
skills in children (Deacon & Kirby, 2004; Kirby et al., 2012). Although there
have been no studies with ABE students looking at morphological awareness as
a predictor of reading comprehension, we hypothesized that similar to studies
of children, morphological, phonological, and decoding skills as a set would
predict adults’ reading comprehension. Results revealed that phonological and
morphological awareness were unique predictors of reading comprehension. Further, these predictors accounted for a large proportion of the variance in reading
comprehension, suggesting the importance of morphological and phonological
skills to reading comprehension in this population. Although decoding did not
emerge as a significant unique predictor of reading comprehension, we do not take
this to mean that decoding is not an important contributor to adults’ reading comprehension. Decoding was moderately to highly correlated (r = .61) with reading
comprehension. Instead, we believe that there is a high degree of multicollinearity
among phonological, morphological, and decoding skills. Therefore, decoding
skills are unable to account for additional variance in reading comprehension
beyond phonological and morphological skills.
Previous literature on phonology and morphology has contested the degree to
which phonological awareness and morphological awareness are separate, individual predictors of reading skills. Until recently, phonological awareness was
thought of as the single most important predictor of reading achievement in children (Carlisle, 2003). Consistent with research with children, we predicted that
after controlling for phonological awareness, morphological awareness would
be a unique predictor of adults’ reading comprehension skills. We found that
morphological awareness accounted for additional variance beyond phonological
awareness. This is an important finding because it establishes that morphological
awareness is an important contributor to the reading comprehension skills of
adults enrolled in ABE programs. The educational implications of this finding are
discussed in a section on implications below.
Accuracy and response times on inflected words
Because we found morphological awareness to be an important contributor to reading comprehension, we wanted to investigate different characteristics of printed
morphologically complex words to which adults may be sensitive. We investigated accuracy and response times on inflected morphologically complex words
compared to single-morpheme matched control words. Few studies with older
children have incorporated words with inflected endings because knowledge of
inflectional morphology is mastered by the early elementary school years (Anglin,
1993; Berko, 1958; Wysocki & Jenkins, 1987). Worthy and Viise (1996) compared
adults in literacy classes with children matched on achievement level for spelling
and reported that adults had particular difficulties with inflectional endings. Thus,
we expected slight deviations from the children’s research: we proposed that
inflectional morphology presents more of a challenge for the population of adults
with low literacy skills. Our results demonstrated this trend: adults were less
accurate and slower on inflected complex words compared to single-morpheme
matched control words in reading a passage and in a naming task. Therefore, adults
with low literacy struggle with inflected complex words in print. Incorporating
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
22
explicit teaching of inflectional endings in ABE programs is imperative to help
adults parse complex words.
Accuracy and response times on derived words
Next, we looked at the morphological characteristics of word frequency and word
transparency in derived morphologically complex words compared to matched
control words. We hypothesized that participants would demonstrate greater accuracy and respond faster to all three of the derived matched control word types
as compared to the three types of derived complex word types in both the oral
reading passage and the naming task. Results indicated that for all derived word
types, adults with low literacy skills were more accurate on matched control
words than on complex words. In addition, participants were faster at responding
to matched control words than to complex words for two of the word types: highfrequency, high-frequency base words and pseudocomplex words. Response times
on the phonological transparency word type were the only ones not supported
by our hypothesis: stable matched control words were read as quickly as shift
words.
In general, these findings are consistent with past literature, which reports
that frequency, familiarity, and transparency of words play an important role in
determining a reader’s awareness of morphological structure. Carlisle and Stone
(2005) found that derived words that were phonologically transparent and high
frequency were read more accurately and faster than phonological shift and lowfrequency words for children. These findings are consistent with the present study
for adults: stable and high-frequency words were processed more accurately than
were phonological shift and low-frequency words.
Our finding that there were no differences in response times between phonological shift and stable words deviated from Carlisle and Stone’s (2005) results.
Looking at this finding, we noticed that there was a large discrepancy in response
times within the naming task such that shift words were responded to at a much
slower rate than were stable words. There was no difference in response times for
shift and stable words within the oral reading passage even though participants
were more accurate on stable words in both the passage and the naming task. We
think that this difference in response times is eliminated in the passage because
adults utilized contextual clues to help them read words faster. The shift and
stable words were the most difficult words within the tasks; therefore, many of
the adults who struggled with these words were eliminated from the response
time data because they did not produce any correct words. Thus, adults who
actually were able to read some of these words probably performed better on
the task and utilized contextual clues to help them identify the words faster. In
isolation, the adults did not have the clues to help them figure out the more difficult
phonological shift words, and they therefore spent longer trying to identify the
words.
Carlisle and Stone (2005) also found that high-frequency words were read
more accurately than pseudocomplex words. This study did not replicate that
finding: the adults read the pseudocomplex words both more accurately and faster
than the high-frequency complex words. In contrast to the Carlisle and Stone
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
23
(2005) study, we hypothesized that the pseudocomplex words would be read
more accurately and faster because we proposed that adults possess the ability to
engage in morphological decomposition as opposed to processing complex words
holistically. Thus, adults with low literacy skills recognized the base in the highfrequency complex word forms but had less developed morphological awareness
to efficiently process and accurately identify the derived form. Participants did not
need to break down the single-morpheme pseudocomplex words and instead read
these words holistically. Both the high-frequency complex and pseudocomplex
were high-frequency words; we surmise that adults struggled to decompose and
produce the derived complex form.
Importance of context
This study expanded on the Carlisle and Stone (2005) study by examining accuracy
of word reading by looking at words presented in context versus words presented
in isolation. The exposure to the interactive nature of spellings, pronunciations,
syntax, and meanings present in a passage could influence accuracy and speed of
word recognition. Adults have more exposure to language and printed materials
than children. Adults with low literacy skills are no exception; this group may
know a spoken word but may not be able to identify the written form of that
word. We expected that context would be a positive factor for word identification
because context provides clues to infer meaning and help recognize unfamiliar
words. Results supported this hypothesis: adults were more accurate at identifying
morphologically complex words in context for three of our word types. This
finding was especially salient for phonological shift words: for words presented
in isolation, only 34 participants got at least one word correct and averaged a
36.5% word accuracy level. Conversely, for words presented in context, all 55
participants were included and averaged a 79.2% word accuracy level.
The only word type that did not follow this trend was the high-frequency
complex words versus pseudocomplex matched control words. We propose that
the words were too familiar to the adults because complex and pseudocomplex
words were high frequency and relatively easier words in relation to the other
derived word types in this study. Thus, the presentation of the words (in context
vs. in isolation) was irrelevant because overall the participants had high accuracy
levels on both complex and pseudocomplex words.
Limitations
There are a few limitations of this study that should be addressed. First, the
diversity and the size of the sample posed a limitation: the 57 participants spanned
an array of ages, language experience, and class levels. The study included both
ESOLs and native English speakers. Further, the study did not yield enough
power to be able to divide the participants by class: only 11 ESOL students, 12
pre-GED students, and 29 GED students completed all tasks. While it would
have been beneficial to examine differences across groups, it is important to
note that our sample is representative of the population of adults enrolled in
ABE programs across the United States (National Research Council, 2012). In
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
24
addition, in a recent study, Binder et al. (2011) used a similar population of
adults with low literacy skills and found no differences between the native and
nonnative reader groups in how different component skills (e.g., phonological
and orthographic awareness and oral reading fluency) predicted reading ability.
However, we recognize that this continues to be an issue that warrants further
investigation. Second, there were difficulties with the naming task. Many of the
word types included fewer words than the oral reading passage. By including
fewer words in the naming task, there was little room for errors: if all words were
missed, an entire participant was eliminated from the analyses. Third, response
time data were measured differently between the passage and the naming task,
and therefore it was difficult to interpret the findings. It would be beneficial and
more useful to directly compare response times between the two task types to see
if accuracy data corresponded with response time data.
Future research
There are several important directions to consider for future research on morphological awareness and processing and reading abilities in adults with low literacy
skills. It would be interesting to differentiate by ability levels and investigate
morphological skills from adults with low literacy skills in beginning literacy
programs through those in GED-level classes. Comparing adults with low literacy
skills to children based on achievement level could allow one to assess differences in the developmental trajectory of acquiring morphological rules. Our study
found that adults with low literacy skills struggled with both inflected and derived
complex words. Few studies have included inflected and derived words and none
assessing both children and adults with low literacy. This type of study could
have important implications for the most effective approaches to teaching adults
about morphological rules. Finally, because past research has investigated the relationship among phonology, morphology, and orthography in children, it would
be worthwhile assessing these relationships in adults by including orthographic
knowledge.
Educational implications
The results of the current study indicate that morphological awareness is an important predictor of adults’ reading comprehension over and above phonological
awareness. In addition, the study indicates that adults are vulnerable to morphological complexity. This study enhances the existing literature on adults with low
literacy and demonstrates that morphological awareness is an important component skill of reading comprehension for this population. This could have important
implications for ABE programs because explicitly teaching adults to understand
morphological rules and how to decompose words into constituent morphemes
could improve their morphological awareness, vocabulary knowledge, and subsequently reading comprehension skills. For children, understanding morphemic
structure aids them in understanding and figuring out unfamiliar words and directly corresponds to spelling. The importance of rules regarding morphology
and spelling contradicts some of the phoneme–grapheme correspondences when
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
25
alternative pronunciations exist for word endings that are spelled the same way.
For example, the past tense inflection, –ed, presents a challenge because kissed
(/t/ sound), killed (/d/ sound), and waited (/-əd/ sound) are all spelled the same
but pronounced differently (Carlisle, 2010). Nunes and Bryant (2009) proposed
a reading program emphasizing spelling abilities by directly teaching morphemic
spelling rules. This type of instruction would allow learners to focus more on word
structure (learning prefixes and suffixes) and enable them to apply these rules
to promote reading comprehension and expand vocabulary knowledge. Explicit
morphological instruction (in both inflectional and derivational morphology) may
be beneficial for adults with low literacy skills because they struggle with morphologically complex words. Spelling research has found that this group suffers
from poor understanding of morphological structure and underdeveloped phonological skills; however, they have better orthographic knowledge in comparison
with children matched on achievement level (Viise, 1996; Worthy & Viise, 1996).
Our study finds that adults also struggle with morphologically complex words in
passage reading and when identifying words presented in isolation. Therefore,
an integrative instructional approach encompassing phonological, morphological,
and orthographic knowledge should be explored in adult literacy programs.
APPENDIX A
Oral reading passage
Bold words are from the Carlisle and Stone (2005) study; italicized words are inflected
types.
Long before colonial times, in the hilly region of France, there was a silly young prince in
search of a lover. All his wealth did not make a difference because he was very lonely in
his large, empty palace. He wanted a lady to cook him dinner and clean his dirty home.
He needed to add flavor to his cookery and style to his dull house. But first he had to make
a confession—before he was a prince, he was a beggar and a thief. He entered the lottery
and became a winner. Despite his financial security, he was afraid and secretive. It was a
fearsome worry that someone would discover his past.
One day a pretty woman wearing a stylish dress with a flowery design walked by the
castle. She was singing a beautiful melody. He looked at her with intensity and liked her
sparkly earrings and blonde hair. He was drawn to her natural beauty. He went up to her
with confidence and invited her to the annual music convention. Realizing his sincerity
and charming good looks, she said yes. They sat on the grass beneath a shady tree to enjoy
the serenity of the afternoon and discuss their mutual preference for classical music. He
took her to the opera and the ballet.
The prince could not stop smiling because he felt so lucky. The couple began spending
every moment together. The prince’s friends took his dependence on her for stupidity and
lack of maturity. He was hurt by the severity of their opinions. But he did hover and cling
to her the majority of the time. He gave her a hundred presents and asked her to marry
him. She explained her cultural beliefs—since she was the youngest daughter, she had to
stay home and take care of her mother. He looked at her in puzzlement and his heart broke
to pieces. He was alone again, still in search of his princess.
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
26
APPENDIX B
Naming task
Inflected/inflected matched words:
Eyes
Minutes
Tastes
Falling
Swirling
Enjoying
Worked
Noted
Colored
Soon
Speed
Shame
Volume
Senior
Garbage
Pattern
Sink
Bottle
High-frequency/pseudocomplex words
Icy
Windy
Batter
Robber
Mower
Mercy
Candy
Chapter
Rubber
Tower
Low-frequency, high-frequency base/high-frequency, high-frequency base
Pailful
Beastly
Queendom
Idealize
Corrective
Odorous
Icy
Windy
Batter
Robber
Mower
Phonological shift/stable words
Confusion
Conformity
Activity
Oddity
Precision
Finality
Mortality
Locality
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The project described was supported by Grant R15HD067755 from the Eunice Kennedy
Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development awarded (to K.S.B.).
The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the
official views of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development or the National Institutes of Health.
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
27
NOTE
1.
Note that we reran all of our analyses excluding the 12 ESOL participants specifically
from the ESOL class. Although this reduces power (n = 45), we were still able to
replicate all of the same results for both accuracy and response times with these
participants omitted.
REFERENCES
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society
for Research in Child Development, 58, 1–166.
Berko, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150–177.
Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Nagy, W., & Carlisle, J. (2010). Growth in phonological, orthographic,
and morphological awareness in grades 1 to 6. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 39, 141–
163.
Binder, K. S., & Borecki, C. (2008). The use of phonological, orthographic, and contextual information
during reading: A comparison of adults who are learning to read and skilled adult readers.
Reading and Writing, 21, 843–858.
Binder, K. S., Snyder, M. A., Ardoin, S. P., & Morris, R. K. (2011). Dynamic indicators of basic early
literacy skills: An effective tool to assess adult literacy students? Adult Basic Education and
Literacy Journal, 5, 150–160.
Blalock, W. J. (1981). Persistent problems and concerns of young adults with learning disabilities. In
W. M. Cruickshank & A. A. Silver (Eds.), Bridges to tomorrow: The best of ACDL (pp. 35–55).
Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press.
Brittain, M. (1970). Inflectional performance and early reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 6, 34–38.
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Carlisle, J. F. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth, and
eighth graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 247–266.
Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words:
Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, 12, 169–190.
Carlisle, J. F. (2003). Morphology matters in learning to read: A commentary. Reading Psychology,
24, 291–322.
Carlisle, J. F. (2010). Review of Nunes, T., and Bryant, P. (2006). Improving literacy by teaching
morphemes. Reading and Writing, 23, 257–262.
Carlisle, J. F., & Katz, L. A. (2006). Effects of word and morpheme familiarity on reading of derived
words. Reading and Writing, 19, 669–693.
Carlisle, J. F., & Stone, C. A. (2005). Exploring the role of morphemes in word reading. Reading
Research Quarterly, 40, 428–449.
Carroll, J. B., Davies, P., & Richman, B. (1971). Word frequency book. New York: American
Heritage.
Clark, E. V. (1982). The young word maker: A case study of innovations in the child’s lexicon. In
E. Wanner & L. Gleitman (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 390–425).
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Clark, E. V., & Hecht, B. F. (1982). Learning to coin agent and instrument nouns. Cognition, 12,
1–24.
Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of
morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics,
25, 223–238.
Egan, J., & Pring, L. (2004). The processing of inflectional morphology: A comparison of children
with and without dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 17, 567–591.
Good, R. H., Gruba, J., & Kaminski, R. A. (2001). Best practices in using Dynamic Indicators of Basic
Literacy Skills (DIBELS) in an outcome driven model. Best Practices in School Psychology,
4, 679–700.
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
28
Good, R. H., & Kaminski, R. A. (Eds.). (2002). Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills (6th
ed.). Eugene, OR: Institute for the Development of Educational Achievement. Retrieved from
http://dibels.uoregon.edu/
Greenberg, D., Ehri, L. C., & Perin, D. (1997). Are word reading processes the same or different in
adult literacy students and 3rd–5th graders matched for reading level? Journal of Educational
Psychology, 89, 262–275.
Greenberg, D., Ehri, L. C., & Perin, D. (2002). Do adult literacy students make the same word-reading
and spelling errors as children matched for word-reading age? Scientific Studies of Reading, 6,
221–243.
Hoffman, L. (1978). Reading errors among skilled and unskilled adult readers. Community/ Junior
College Research Quarterly, 2, 151–162.
Jarmulowicz, L., Hay, S. E., Taran, V. L., & Ethington, C. A. (2008). Fitting derivational morphophonology into a developmental model of reading. Reading and Writing, 21, 275–297.
Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. H., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Woolley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012).
Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing, 25, 389–410.
Kirsch, I. S., Jungeblut, A., Jenkins, L., & Kolstad, A. (1993). Adult literacy in America: A first
look at the results of the National Adult Literacy Surve (NCES 93-275). Washington, DC:
National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, US Department of
Education.
Kutner, M., Greenberg, E., Jin, Y., Boyle, B., Hsu, Y., & Dunleavy, E. (2007). Literacy in everyday life:
Results from the 2003 National Assessment of Adult Literacy (NCES 2007–480). Washington,
DC: National Center for Education Statistics, US Department of Education.
Larsen, J. A., & Nippold, M. A. (2007). Morphological analysis in school-age children: Dynamic
assessment of a word learning strategy. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,
38, 201–212.
Leong, C. K. (2000). Rapid processing of base and derived forms of words and grades 4, 5, and 6
children’s spelling. Reading and Writing, 12, 277–302.
Liberman, I. Y., Rubin, H., Duques, S., & Carlisle, J. (1985). Linguistic abilities and spelling proficiency in kindergarteners and adult poor spellers. In D. B. Gray & J. F. Kavanaugh (Eds.),
Biobehavioral measures of dyslexia (pp. 163–176). Parkton, MD: York Press.
MacArthur, C. A., Konold, T. R., Glutting, J. J., & Alamprese, J. A. (2010). Reading component skills
of learners in adult basic education. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 108–121.
Mahony, D. L. (1994). Using sensitivity to word structure to explain variance in high school and
college level reading ability. Reading and Writing, 6, 19–44.
Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J., & Xu, F. (1992). Overregulation
in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57,
1–182.
Mellard, D. F., & Fall, E. (2012). Component model of reading comprehension for adult education
participants. Learning Disability Quarterly, 35, 10–23.
Mellard, D., Woods, K., & Fall, E. (2011). Assessment and instruction of oral reading fluency among
adults with low literacy. Adult Basic Education and Literacy Journal, 5, 3–14.
Nagy, W. E., & Anderson, R. C. (1984). The number of words in printed school English. Reading
Research Quarterly, 19, 304–330.
Nagy, W. E., Anderson, R., Schommer, M., Scott, J. A., & Stallman, A. C. (1989). Morphological
families and word recognition. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 262–282.
Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contribution of morphology beyond phonology
to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 98, 134–147.
Nagy, W. E., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Vaughan, K., & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of
morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk second-grade readers and
at-risk fourth-grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 730–742.
Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. A., & Anderson, R. C. (1985). Learning words from context. Reading Research
Quarterly, 20, 233–253.
National Research Council. (2012). Improving adult literacy instruction: Options for practice and
research. In A. M. Lesgold & M. Welch-Ross (Eds.), Committee on learning sciences: Foundations and applications to adolescent and adult literacy. Washington, DC: National Academies
Press.
Applied Psycholinguistics
Tighe & Binder: An investigation of morphological awareness
29
Nunes, T., & Bryant, P. (2009). Children’s reading and spelling: Beyond the first steps. West Sussex:
Wiley–Blackwell.
Perin, D. (1988). Combining schema activation and cooperative learning to promote reading comprehension in adult literacy students. Journal of Reading, 32, 54–68.
Read, C., & Ruyter, L. (1985). Reading and spelling skills in adults of low literacy. Remedial and
Special Education, 6, 43–52.
Reichle, E. D., & Perfetti, C. A. (2003). Morphology in word identification: A word experience model
that accounts for morpheme frequency effects. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 219–237.
Roman, A. A., Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R. K., Wade-Woolley, L., & Deacon, S. H. (2009). Toward a
comprehensive view of the skills involved in word reading in Grades 4, 6, and 8. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 96–113.
Sabatini, J. (2002). Efficacy in word reading of adults: Ability group comparisons. Scientific Studies
of Reading, 6, 267–298.
Sabatini, J. P., Sawaki, Y., Shore, J. P., & Scarborough, H. S. (2010). Relationships among reading
skills of adults with low literacy. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 122–138.
Singson, M., Mahony, D., & Mann, V. (2000). The relation between reading ability and morphological
skills: Evidence from derivational suffixes. Reading and Writing, 12, 219–252.
Sternberg, R. J. (1987). Most vocabulary is learned from context. In M. G. McKeown & M. E. Curtis
(Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 89–106). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Thompkins, A. C., & Binder, K. S. (2003). A comparison of the factors affecting reading performance
of functionally illiterate adults and children matched by reading level. Reading Research
Quarterly, 38, 236–255.
Viise, N. M. (1996). A study of the spelling development of adult literacy learners compared with that
of classroom children. Journal of Literacy Research, 28, 561–587.
Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Mastery Reading Tests—Revised. Circle Pines, MN: AGS.
Worthy, J., & Viise, N. M. (1996). Morphological, phonological, and orthographic differences between
the spelling of normally achieving children and basic literacy adults. Reading and Writing, 8,
139–159.
Wysocki, K., & Jenkins, J. R. (1987). Deriving word meanings through morphological generalization.
Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 66–81.