Chapter 5
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
MATTHEW D. LIEBERMAN
Who we are as humans has a lot to do with what happens
between our ears. What happens between our ears has a lot
to do with the social world we traverse, engage, and react
to. The former has been the province of neuroscience and
the latter the province of social psychology for nearly a
century. Recently, scientists have begun to study the social
mind by literally looking between the ears using the tools
of neuroscience. Social cognitive neuroscience uses the tools
of neuroscience to study the mental mechanisms that create, frame, regulate, and respond to our experience of the
social world. On its worst days, social cognitive neuroscience is phrenological, cataloguing countless brain regions
involved in the vast array of social processes. On its best
days, social cognitive neuroscience enhances our understanding of the social mind as well as any other method.
The goals of this handbook chapter are to give an
overview of the human history of this research area
(Section I), to summarize the techniques common to this
approach (Section II), to survey the functional neuroanatomy of social cognition (Section III), and to discuss
how brain research can make specific contributions to the
social psychological enterprise (Section IV). A special
note to social psychologists with little intrinsic interest in
the brain trying to determine whether social cognitive neuroscience is worth getting acquainted with: Go straight to
Section IV.
have become leaders in the field, despite few having published social cognitive neuroscience findings at that point.
There were introductory talks on social cognition and cognitive neuroscience by Neil Macrae and Jonathan Cohen,
respectively, along with symposia on stereotyping (William
Cunningham, Jennifer Eberhardt, Matthew Lieberman,
and Wendy Mendes), self-control (Todd Heatherton, Kevin
Ochsner, and Cary Savage), emotion (Ralph Adolphs,
Turhan Canli, Elizabeth Phelps, and Stephanie Preston),
imitation and social relations (Alan Fiske, Marco Iacoboni,
David Perrett, and Andrew Whiten), and theory of mind
(Chris Ashwin, Josep Call, Vittorio Gallese, and Kevin
McCabe). If this meeting represented the first time that all
of the ingredients of social cognitive neuroscience were
mixed together in a single pot, the water was already boiling when the ingredients were tossed in. To appreciate
how the pot got this way, several historical strands must
be highlighted.
In the early 1990s, John Cacioppo used the term “social
neuroscience” (Cacioppo, 1994) to characterize how the
social world affects the nervous system. Work in this area
was mostly health relevant (Berntson, Sarter, & Cacioppo,
1998; Kiecolt-Glaser & Glaser, 1989; Segerstrom, Taylor,
Kemeny, & Fahey, 1998) or animal research (Carter, 1998;
Insel & Winslow, 1998; Panksepp, 1998) examining the
impact of social factors on the autonomic, neuroendocrine,
and immune systems (Blascovich & Mendes, this volume).
In other words, early social neuroscience primarily focused
on how the social world affects the peripheral nervous system
and other bodily systems. Although neurocognitive mechanisms clearly fall under the umbrella of social neuroscience,
there were few investigations linking social processes with
brain processes during the 1990s. Social cognitive neuroscience represented a new arm of social neuroscience that
primarily focused on the neurocognitive mechanisms of
I. HISTORY
The Oxford Dictionary of Psychology (Colman, 2006)
identifies a 2001 conference, held at the University of
California, Los Angeles, as a starting point for social cognitive neuroscience. This was the first formal meeting on
social cognitive neuroscience, and many of the attendees
I would like to thank Naomi Eisenberger and members of the UCLA Social Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory for various discussions
about the contents of this chapter.
143
CH05.indd 143
12/22/09 5:33:29 PM
144
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
everyday social cognition. Subsequently, the terms “social
cognitive neuroscience” and “social neuroscience” have
largely become synonymous because the domains and methods of study have merged.
Although social cognitive neuroscience reached its
boiling point around 2001, with numerous scientists beginning to use neuroscience methods to study social cognition, there were isolated programs of research focusing on
social cognitive neuroscience in the 1990s. Antonio and
Hannah Damasio’s work on the socioemotional changes in
individuals with ventromedial prefrontal cortex (PFC; see
Table 5.1 for a list of acronyms and neuroscience terms
used in this chapter) (Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, &
Anderson, 1994) sparked great interest in social cognitive
neuroscience, affective neuroscience (Panksepp, 1998),
and neuroeconomics (Camerer, Loewenstein, & Prelec,
2005). Chris and Uta Frith began an extremely fruitful line
of research on the neural bases of theory of mind in the
mid-1990s (Fletcher et al., 1995), a topic that is foundational within social cognitive neuroscience. Stan Klein and
John Kihlstrom examined self-knowledge by examining a
patient with temporary amnesia, providing the best early
example of how neuroscience could provide constraints on
social psychological theories (Klein, Loftus, & Kihlstrom,
1996). Research on the neural bases of face and biological motion processing were relatively advanced in this
period (McCarthy, Puce, Gore, & Allison, 1997), but not
yet in a way that resonated with traditional social psychological questions. Finally, Cacioppo, Crites, and Gardner
(1996) examined the neural bases of attitudes and evaluative processing using event-related potentials (ERPs)
and demonstrated important dissociations between social
Table 5.1 Acronyms and Jargon in Social Cognitive Neuroscience
PFC
Prefrontal Cortex
STS
Superior Temporal Sulcus
TPJ
Tempoparietal Junction
FFA
Fusiform “Face” Area
ACC
Anterior Cingulate Cortex
Anterior
Towards the front of the brain
Posterior
Towards the back of the brain
Rostral
Towards the front of the brain
Caudal
Towards the back of the brain
Dorsal
Towards the top of the brain
Ventral
Towards the bottom of the brain
Superior
Towards the top of the brain
Inferior
Towards the bottom of the brain
Lateral
Away from the middle of the brain
Medial
Towards the middle of the brain
CH05.indd 144
cognitive processes that were seemingly similar. These
lines of research are the precursors of social cognitive neuroscience and served as inspiration for many who would
go on to work in this area.
Finally, a great deal of human capital was spent bringing social cognitive neuroscience into existence. Influential
scientists already doing social neuroscience, such as John
Cacioppo and Ralph Adolphs, helped promote funding for
and publication of social cognitive neuroscience research.
Established top-notch social psychologists including Todd
Heatherton, Mahzarin Banaji, Neil Macrae, and Susan
Fiske began conducting social cognitive neuroscience
research and lent much-needed credibility to the fledgling area of research. Finally, Steve Breckler and Carolyn
Morf, program officers at the National Science Foundation
(NSF) and National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH),
respectively, had the vision to fund young scientists in this
area, before the area even existed.
Stir all these ingredients together and drop in a generous
helping of motivated graduate students and, voilà: social
cognitive neuroscience. In 2000, the term “social cognitive
neuroscience” first appeared in two papers (Lieberman,
2000; Ochsner & Schachter, 2000), and the first functional
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study examining a
traditional social psychology topic was published (Phelps
et al., 2000). In 2001, the first review of social cognitive neuroscience was published (Ochsner & Lieberman,
2001), although, in truth, the paucity of published research
at that time made this review as much a promissory note as
a progress report.
In the decade since, social cognitive neuroscience has
gone through an explosion. In 2001, a Google search for
“social cognitive neuroscience” returned 6 hits. In 2009,
the same search returned over 52,000 hits (see Figure 5.1).
Similarly, the number of empirical social cognitive neuroscience articles published each year has steadily increased
from 2000 through 2008 (see Figure 5.1). There have been
numerous literature reviews of social cognitive neuroscience (Adolphs, 2001; Amodio & Frith, 2006; Bechara,
2002; Blakemore, Winston, & Frith, 2004; Lieberman,
2007a; Ochsner, 2004, 2007), not to mention a few critiques (Cacioppo et al., 2003; Kihlstrom, 2006; Vul, Harris,
Winkielman, & Pashler, 2009; Willingham & Dunn, 2003).
There have been special issues on social cognitive neuroscience in several journals, including Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology (2003), Neuropsychologia (2003),
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience (2004), Neuroimage
(2005), Brain Research (2006), New York Academy of
Sciences (2007), Group Processes and Intergroup Relations
(2008), and Child Development (2009). Two new journals were founded in 2006 to focus on this area of study:
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience (SCAN) and
12/22/09 5:33:29 PM
Methods and Analysis
145
Google hits for SCN (cumulative)
60000
52600
Number of Hits
42700
40000
29500
21600
20000
3530
6
115
346
922
2001
2002
2003
2004
0
2005
Year
2006
2007
2008
Empirical SCN publications (per year)
(Aug. 08)
200
Number of Publications
2009
196
162
150
142
100
92
94
2004
Year
2005
53
50
42
33
20
0
2000
2001
2002
2003
2006
Social Neuroscience. Several funding agencies have had
special funding initiatives for social cognitive neuroscience; these agencies include the National Institute of
Mental Health, National Institute of Drug Addiction,
National Institute of Aging, and the National Institute of
Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Finally, there have been
a series of social cognitive neuroscience preconferences
and small meetings, and now a yearly Social and Affective
Neuroscience (SAN) conference. In this decade, social
cognitive neuroscience has gone from virtually nonexistent to having an increasingly firm foundation and the
other accoutrements of a scientific discipline.
II. METHODS AND ANALYSIS
Social Cognitive Neuroscience Methods
Before jumping into a review of what has been learned with
the tools of social cognitive neuroscience it is important
CH05.indd 145
2007
2008
Figure 5.1 Growth of social cognitive neuroscience. The top panel displays the number of hits returned from a Google search of
“social cognitive neuroscience” on January 1
of each year from 2001–2009. The bottom
panel displays the number of social cognitive neuroscience empirical articles published
each year from 2000–2009. Note that the 196
articles indicated for 2008 were from January
through August.
to understand the tools themselves (this section) and the
techniques (next section) used to draw inferences about
social psychological processes in the brain. The primary
tools used are neuroimaging techniques (fMRI, PET, ERP)
and lesion studies.
Positron Emission Tomography
The earliest neuroimaging that focused on functional brain
localization was PET. In PET, the subject is either injected
with or inhales radioactive tracers that attach to biologically active molecules. Gamma rays from these tracers can
then be detected with PET, allowing for the identification
of where the tracers are traveling in the brain during different kinds of mental activity. Typically, PET scans have
a temporal resolution of about a minute (i.e., one aggregate data point per minute) and a spatial resolution of
about a cubic centimeter. Apart from being the first form
of functional neuroimaging of the whole brain, PET’s
greatest advantage is that different kinds of molecules can
12/22/09 5:33:30 PM
146
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
be tagged by tracers, thereby allowing studies to examine
not just blood flow in the brain but also the distribution of
neurochemical processes.
brain region and thus it is difficult to make inferences to a
specific region.
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) allows for the
creation of temporary lesions to a particular region of cortex and thereby overcomes some of the limitations of lesion
studies. TMS relies on electromagnetic pulses, which stimulate the neurons in a small area of cortex. This is typically done to excite the neurons until they stop operating
efficiently. Functionally speaking, this repetitive TMS will
take a brain region offline for several minutes, allowing
researchers to determine which temporary lesions produce
performance deficits on tasks of interest.
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) is a noninvasive neuroimaging technique that has replaced PET as the
dominant mode of functional neuroimaging largely because
of its better temporal resolution (1 to 2 seconds) and spatial
resolution (approximately 3 mm3). Most fMRI studies use
blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) fMRI to determine
which brain regions are more or less active during any psychological task. BOLD fMRI works on the principle that
the blood flowing to an active region is more oxygenated
than blood elsewhere, and oxygenated blood has different magnetic properties than deoxygenated blood: fMRI
can detect the spatial location of these different magnetic
properties and reconstruct where blood was flowing to.
A limitation of fMRI is that each condition of interest must
typically be represented by several trials, which can lead
to habituation and contamination effects. Also, nearly
all fMRI analyses are comparisons between experimental
conditions within a subject, typically aggregated across
subjects. Between-group analyses are the exception, not
the norm, and even these are between-group comparisons
of within-subject comparisons. Various social psychological findings become difficult to replicate with fMRI if subjects are exposed to all task conditions.
Event-Related Potentials
Event-related potentials (ERPs) are derived from an electroencephalograph (EEG), which measures the summated
electrical activity from neurons firing in the outer layers
of the cortex. ERPs are the reliable responses that occur
time-locked to a stimulus or response, averaged over several trials. The two primary advantages of the ERPs are
that they directly measure the brain’s electrical activity and
have millisecond temporal resolution, allowing for exquisite measurement of time course. Two weaknesses of ERPs
are that only the outer cortex can be reliably assessed and
the spatial resolution of ERPs is quite poor.
Lesions
By examining individuals with damage to different brain
regions and observing the ensuing psychological deficits,
one can determine the contributions of the damaged regions
to psychological function. The great advantage of lesion
studies over neuroimaging methods is that neuroimaging
only identifies regions active during psychological processes but cannot establish their causal relevance, whereas
lesion studies yield causal inferences. The main limitation
of lesion studies is that damage is rarely limited to one
CH05.indd 146
Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation
Neuroimaging Analyses
Most published social cognitive neuroscience research has
used fMRI, and thus it is worth describing in more detail
how analyses are conducted with fMRI data (also see
Lazar, 2008). This section is provided with an eye toward
the social psychologist who may want to know a bit more
about the steps involved in inferring that “region X is
more active during task A than during task B,” without
having to mire through too much jargon.
Preprocessing
fMRI data are typically preprocessed before analyses
are conducted. What this means is that various things are
done to the raw data that are obtained during scanning to
make the information suitable for analysis. One can think
of it a bit like statistically normalizing scales before combining them or applying log transformations to make a
distribution more normal. In fMRI studies, realignment,
normalization, and smoothing are the standard components
to preprocessing. It should be noted that each of these steps
introduces some noise to the signal while improving the
signal in other ways. Assumptions go into how each of
these steps is performed, and the practical implementation
of these assumptions is never perfect.
Realignment is a process that corrects the brain images
to account for the motion of a subject’s head while in the
scanner. Small movements of a few millimeters in any
direction can alter whether the signal appears to be coming from one brain structure or another. Realignment uses
structural features of the brain to determine how the brain
has moved and then “puts the brain back” in the same space
as the brain was in during a reference scan. When successful, realignment ensures that the amygdala, for instance,
shows up in the same place in the acquired brain images
throughout the entire data collection.
Whereas realignment tries to ensure that an individual’s
brain maintains its own constant “coordinate space,” the
12/22/09 5:33:30 PM
Methods and Analysis
goal of normalization is to put all of the different subjects’
realigned brain scans into a single coordinate space so
that the brain structures line up across subjects. Brains
come in all shapes and sizes, and normalization essentially
morphs different brains into a common space. Different
programs do this in different ways, and no method does
this perfectly.
Spatial smoothing is the last key step in preprocessing.
Smoothing involves averaging over adjacent “voxels” (i.e.,
three-dimensional [3D] pixels) in the brain images. This
provides a number of benefits in terms of enhancing the
detection of certain kinds of signals, but this is done at
the expense of diminishing the likelihood of detecting other
kinds of signals. Usually this is a desirable trade-off, but it
again demonstrates that the data analyzed in fMRI studies
are far from their raw state and represent a series of decisions and transformations that render the data more analyzable, while sometimes introducing problems when the data
do not conform to the assumptions behind the transformations. In many ways this differs little from the assumptions
that are made in statistical analyses but are often untested in
our behavioral studies (heteroscedasticity anyone?).
Whole-Brain Analyses
The great majority of analyses reported in fMRI research
are whole-brain analyses comparing brain activations under
two task conditions across all of the voxels in the brain.
For instance, imagine a study in which the subject spends
alternating 30-second periods looking at pictures of ingroup
members and then outgroup members, for a total of 3 minutes. Say we want to know which brain regions are differentially activated under these two conditions. The MRI
scanner may collect a full brain volume (i.e., a set of images
taken at roughly the same time that, stacked together, cover
the entire brain) every 3 seconds, and thus there are a total
of 60 volumes takes over the 3-minute scan. Each of the
60 volumes represents a time point; thus, at each voxel in
the brain there is a 60-point time series reflecting the relative activation of each voxel. The statistical tools convolve
a hypothetical BOLD response (i.e., a model of how the
blood oxygenation typically rises and falls over time in an
active area) with the experimental design to create a hypothetical time series of what a brain region’s activity would
look like if it were differentially sensitive to the two conditions of the experiment. This hypothesized time series is
then regressed against the actual time series at every voxel
in the brain to see which voxels in the brain show a pattern of activation consistent with the hypothesized pattern.
When several contiguous voxels from a brain region all
show the hypothesized pattern across time, it is generally
inferred that this region of the brain is more active under
one condition than another. The brain images in published
CH05.indd 147
147
articles that show yellow and orange “blobs” typically
represent the regions that cross some threshold (e.g., 10
contiguous voxels all with regression values of p < .001)
for consistency with the experimental regressor.
These analyses yield the brain regions for a single subject
that are sensitive to task demands. Our interest is usually
in generalizing to the population at large, so we combine
single-subject whole-brain analyses across subjects to
determine which brain regions are reliably active across
subjects. This is done by computing one sample t-test at
each voxel, using the parameter estimates (i.e., regression
coefficients) from each subject at the same voxel. If the
average parameter estimate from each subject in a particular region is large enough, it will emerge as significant in
this random effects analysis.
Region of Interest Analyses
Neuroimaging studies commonly report the results of
region of interest (ROI) analyses. Such analyses reflect the
search within a specific region of the brain for significant
activations. ROI analyses can serve several different purposes in a study. One benefit of searching within a smaller
region of the brain is that it reduces the number of simultaneous statistical tests and thus reduces the burden of correcting for multiple comparisons. It also allows for a priori
hypothesis testing by intentionally searching within brain
regions thought to be relevant to the comparison. In some
ways, this is analogous to performing one-tailed rather than
two-tailed t-tests where a more lenient test can be performed
because a precise hypothesis is specified. One unfortunate
side effect of papers that rely solely on the ROI approach is
that they can give the inadvertent impression that only the
examined regions are involved in a process of interest.
Whatever the purpose of an ROI analysis, it is important to know exactly what kind of ROI analysis is being
reported. There are at least two kinds of distinctions to
be drawn between different ROI analyses. First, an ROI
can be either functionally or anatomically defined. An
anatomically defined ROI involves trying to find the true
borders of a brain structure on the brain images. Functional
ROIs ignore anatomical boundaries and instead use some
existing pattern of activation to identify the ROI. For
instance, one might run a “localizer scan” (Saxe, Brett, &
Kanwisher, 2006) to define an ROI using a task well known
to activate a particular brain structure and then examine
what that ROI does in some new experimental condition.
The second kind of distinction among ROI analyses
concerns whether the ROI is treated as a “supervoxel” or
a “search space.” Some ROI analyses treat the ROI as a
space within which significant clusters of activation can
be detected. Other ROI analyses treat the ROI as a single
entity that is either significant as a whole or not.
12/22/09 5:33:31 PM
148
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
Each kind of ROI analysis described here is valid, and
there are more kinds that were not described. Nevertheless,
it is critical to know which kind of ROI is being used
because each supports different kinds of inferences and has
different limitations.
Connectivity Analyses
Researchers are increasingly interested in the relationships
between brain regions, rather than focusing on what each
brain region is doing independently. Connectivity analyses
provide an estimate of the extent to which brain regions are
showing coordinated activity under particular task conditions. Inverse connectivity is also of interest within social
cognitive neuroscience because this indicates that two
brain regions show a pattern consistent with one region
regulating the other. These analyses do not establish causality, as they are entirely correlational; however, the correlations do point to the regions that are good candidates to
have causal effects.
There are two main kinds of connectivity analyses
that correspond roughly to between-subjects and withinsubjects analyses. Between-subjects connectivity analyses
are much easier to conduct, but they are less likely to be able
to provide strong evidence that brain regions are actually
working together or at odds with one another. Such analyses involve correlating a single estimate of activity for
each subject in one brain region with a single estimate of
activity for each subject in another brain region. What such
analyses reveal is whether the extent to which a person
activates brain region X more during task A than task B
is associated with the activity in brain region Y during the
same comparison of task A and B. For instance, is the magnitude of activity in a region of prefrontal cortex for each
subject during attempts at self-control, compared with a
baseline task, inversely associated with the magnitude of
amygdala activity across subjects as well. The limitation
of this procedure is that a single average estimate of activity
during the task is used (i.e., how much did a subject activate
the prefrontal region averaged across all self-control trials
of the task?) and thus it says nothing about the temporal
dynamics of the brain regions.
The second type of connectivity, functional connectivity, addresses this issue by examining the extent to which
the time series of activation in two regions are correlated
with one another. Specifically, functional connectivity
assesses whether the time series of activation between
brain regions X and Y are more strongly correlated under
task A than under task B. This analysis must be carried
out on each subject individually and then aggregated
across subjects. The conceptual limitation of these analyses is that they typically assess only how brain regions
are correlated at the same moment in time. One can easily
CH05.indd 148
imagine that the true dynamics between some regions
involve time lags of up to a few seconds (e.g., 2 seconds
of prefrontal effort toward self-control might be needed
before downstream reductions in amygdala area are
observed). A between-subjects connectivity analysis might
still capture this effect because it does not make assumptions about the temporal dynamics, but a functional connectivity analysis would probably miss the effect. Solutions to
these problems, allowing for hypothesis-driven time lags,
are being worked on (Formisano et al., 2002).
Regression Analyses
Because social psychologists are interested in how social
and personality factors interact to affect task behavior,
social cognitive neuroscience commonly uses regression
analyses in fMRI. Regression analyses are straightforward
to run in most fMRI statistical packages. Here, a vector of
regressor values, one value per subject, is entered into a
whole-brain comparison of two task conditions. The output
will look like any whole-brain analysis with p-values for
each voxel, indicating the reliability for the correlation, and
brain maps displaying clusters of activation. For discussion
of the characterization of such analyses as “voodoo,” see
papers by Vul et al. (2009) and Lieberman, Berkman, and
Wager (2009).
With use of this technique, any trait-level or selfreport variable can be used to examine whether it is
associated with the pattern of activity across subjects.
Socioeconomic status, neuroticism, and rejection sensitivity are just a few of the trait variables whose relation
to neural responses have been examined. One can also
assess behavior that occurs after the scanning procedures
to examine the relation of that behavior to neural responses
during a relevant task in the scanner. For instance, one
could examine whether individual differences in automatic
mimicry in a laboratory setting are associated with individual differences in the magnitude of imitation-related
brain activity in an fMRI scanning session. The betweensubjects connectivity analyses described earlier are actually just a special application of this kind of regression
analysis.
One can also use physiological, behavioral, or selfreport responses obtained during the scanning session itself
as a regressor at the single-subject level. Here, as with functional connectivity, the regressor of interest is correlated
with the time series of activity to determine whether the
two are related. For instance, a study might involve the presentation of 50 works of art and obtain the subject’s rating
of desirability for each. These ratings can then be entered as
a regressor unfolding over time to determine, within a subject, which brain regions have activity that rises and falls
with this psychological response.
12/22/09 5:33:32 PM
Functional Neuroanatomy
Reverse Inference
Reverse inference refers to a particular difficulty in
drawing psychological inferences from neuroimaging data
(Poldrack, 2006). Ideally, neural activations could serve
as markers that a particular psychological process has
occurred. If we could confidently assert that every time the
amygdala is activated some form of fear processing has
occurred, this would be a boon to social psychologists
for whom the limitations of self-report and introspection
are well-known (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). Unfortunately,
the amygdala is activated under numerous task conditions,
including, for instance, getting a reward. Without a oneto-one correspondence between function and structure,
reverse inferences become far less reliable (Ochsner, 2007).
In truth, reverse inference is a part of almost every
study and will continue to be. It is only slightly different
outside of fMRI research. For instance, reaction times can
vary for any number of reasons, and thus it is problematic to assume that it necessarily reflects the number of
underlying operations or the difficulty of each operation.
Realistically, reverse inference will always be a potential
inferential problem, but several steps can be taken to minimize the problem.
First, a focus on networks of brain regions rather than
a single brain region can help dramatically. For instance,
the dorsomedial PFC, posterior superior temporal sulcus
(STS), and temporal poles are commonly coactivated
when subjects perform theory of mind or mentalizing
tasks (i.e., thinking about the psychological states and
characteristics of another; Frith & Frith, 2003). Although
the temporal poles may be activated under various task
conditions (e.g., semantic processing), there is little evidence that all three regions are coactivated under conditions that do not involve mentalizing (Cabeza & Nyberg,
2000). Thus, the presence of any one of the three regions
may not be a valid marker for mentalizing, but the three
together may constitute a marker. Connectivity analyses
can also suggest that these regions are working in concert
with each other during a particular task, strengthening the
inference further. Localizer scans can help as well. If each
subject performs an explicit mentalizing task prior to a
second task where we would like to surreptitiously assess
whether mentalizing is occurring, functionally defined
ROIs can be created for each subject in the particular
regions used for mentalizing. It is then possible to determine whether those same ROIs are activated during the
subsequent task.
Eye Movement Confounds
Another consideration before leaving this section concerns
how eye movement may dramatically alter our interpretation of neuroimaging (and for that matter, behavioral) data.
CH05.indd 149
149
It has been observed that autistic individuals, compared
with healthy matched control subjects, show less amygdala
activity when presented with emotional faces (BaronCohen et al., 1999; Pelphrey, Morris, McCarthy, & LaBar,
2007). The initial inference drawn was that the amygdalae
of autistic persons were less sensitive to faces or the emotional content of faces. However, autistic individuals also
spend less time looking at the eyes of a face than do healthy
individuals, and the eyes are extremely important for identifying emotional expressions (Adolphs et al., 2005). When
eye gaze differences were accounted for, using eye-tracking
equipment in the scanner, there were no remaining differences in amygdala responses of autistic versus control
subjects (Dalton et al., 2005). Similarly, when a patient
with amygdala damage who was impaired at recognizing
fear expressions (Adolphs, Tranel, Damasio, & Damasio,
1994) was retested with instructions to attend to the eyes
of the target faces, the patient performed at normal levels
(Adolphs et al., 2005). These results change the interpretation of the original findings, suggesting that the amygdala
directs eye gaze to important cues in the environment and
that autistic individuals and those with amygdala damage
are less likely to spontaneously do this. Another possibility is the amygdalae of autistic persons are hypersensitive,
rather than hyposensitive, to distressing social information
and therefore look less at these stimuli. It is natural to think
that the extent to which a region of the brain responds to a
stimulus presented in the scanner reflects that brain region’s
sensitivity to that class of stimuli. Knowing where the subjects are looking, what they are attending to, or what they
are thinking about while processing the stimulus can lead to
very different interpretations.
III. FUNCTIONAL NEUROANATOMY
Welcome to the “lite-brite” portion of the chapter. This section reviews the known neural bases of social cognition,
self-processes, and processes specific to social interactions.
“Lite-brite” is a pejorative term, based on a toy from the
1960s, for studies that examine social psychological processes in the scanner and see what lights up. This is also
referred to as brain mapping and has gotten something of
a bad rap. Social psychologists have rightly pointed out
that knowing where a process occurs in the brain does not
in itself add one iota to psychological theories. But sometimes, such studies lead to other studies that do add an iota
or two to our theories. Sometimes, several brain mapping
studies considered together can suggest new divisions and
commonalities between processes that might not have been
obvious from other behavioral and self-report methods (see
Section IV).
12/22/09 5:33:32 PM
150
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
Social Perception
Humans and other primates are sensitive to a wide array of
nonverbal cues of social significance. We may not always
reflect on the meaning of these cues, but ongoing social
perception invariably influences our thoughts, feelings, and
behaviors. Basic capacities of social perception are taken for
granted in many models of social cognition, yet it is these
basic capacities that received the most attention in the early
days of social cognitive neuroscience. Cognitive neuroscientists have extensively studied the neural bases of face and
body perception, biological motion, action observation,
and emotion recognition. Each of these social perception
processes is reviewed in this section (see Figure 5.2).
Face and Body Perception
Face perception research has been a major topic for
neuroimaging research since the mid-1990s. The primary
question has been whether there are regions of the brain that
are tuned specifically for the processing of faces or whether
faces are one of many entities decoded through a common set
of perception processes. A number of neuroimaging studies
have converged on a region of the fusiform gyrus, which
links the occipital and temporal cortices, that is selectively
Social Perception
1 posterior superior temporal
sulcus
2 fusiform “face” area
3 extrastriate “body” area
4 occipital “face” area
Biological Motion
5 amygdala
6 inferior parietal lobule
7 ventrolateral PFC – pars opercularis
8 ventrolateral PFC – pars orbitalis
Figure 5.2 The brain regions involved in social perception (face
and body perception [2–4], biological motion perception [1], action
observation [6, 7], and emotion recognition [5, 8]). Numbers in
brackets correspond to the regions in the figure reliably associated
with a particular aspect of social perception.
CH05.indd 150
and maximally activated by facial stimuli (Kanwisher,
McDermott, & Chun, 1997; McCarthy et al., 1997). This
region has been dubbed the fusiform face area (FFA) by
Kanwisher and colleagues (1997). A second, more posterior
region that also shows face selectivity has been called the
occipital face area (OFA; Hoffman & Haxby, 2000).
A significant challenge to the equating of the FFA and
OFA with face processing came from Gauthier, Skudlarski,
Gore, and Anderson (2000). Gauthier argued that the FFA
is specialized for expert visual processing and that face
processing is just one obvious application of this region’s
computations. Gauthier created fictional animals (“greebles”) and found that increased perceptual experience
with greebles led to increases in FFA activity. Similarly,
car and bird experts show significant activity in the FFA
and OFA to cars and birds, respectively (Gauthier et al.,
2000). Kanwisher notes that across these studies, the FFA
still shows the greatest activation to faces (Grill-Spector,
Knouf, & Kanwisher, 2004).
Another approach (Haxby et al., 2001) suggests that
although the FFA may be most attuned to faces, whereas
other regions of occipitotemporal cortex are more responsive to nonface objects, this is not the only metric that
matters. Regardless of what class of object each of these
regions is most responsive to, the activity in each of
these regions still discriminates between the presence and
absence of numerous kinds of stimuli. Thus, the FFA may
be most relevant to processing faces and yet still participate, along with other regions in a distributed network, in
the processing of various nonface stimuli.
Just as the FFA is particularly responsive to the presence
of faces, another region in occipital cortex, referred to as
the extrastriate body area (EBA), is more active when subjects are presented with bodies than when shown faces or
other stimuli (Downing, Yuhong, Shuman, & Kanwisher,
2001). Interestingly, the response of the EBA is greater
when the head is occluded than when the head is visible
(Morris, Pelphrey, & McCarthy, 2006). The EBA is also
more active when subjects view bodies from a distance,
allocentrically, rather than from an egocentric perspective
typically associated with viewing one’s own body directly
(Chan, Peelen, & Downing, 2004).
The fact that even infants have the ability to discriminate
between biological motion (i.e., movements consistent
with the biomechanics of biological organisms) and nonbiological motion (Fox & McDaniel, 1982) suggests that
the brain may have dedicated support for processing biological motion. Like many aspects of nonverbal decoding,
biological motion simply appears to us in perception as
qualitatively different from nonbiological motion.
12/22/09 5:33:33 PM
Functional Neuroanatomy
Biological motion is detectable from “point light
walkers” (Johansson, 1973) in which only a handful of
points identifying a target’s joint locations are shown
as the target moves. Several fMRI studies have shown that
the posterior STS (see Figure 5.2) is more active during the presentation of point light walkers than various
control stimuli (Grèzes et al., 2001; Grossman & Blake,
2002; Vaina, Solomon, Chowdhury, Sinha, & Belliveau,
2001). Additionally, increased posterior STS activity
to point light walker stimuli over a period of training is
associated with improvements in behavioral performance
(Grossman, Blake, & Kim, 2004). Even sounds of people
walking activate the posterior STS (Bidet-Caulet, Voisin,
Bertrand, & Fonlupt, 2005; Saarela & Hari, 2008). Finally,
lesions to this region produce deficits in processing point
light walkers (Saygin, 2007). Together, these findings suggest a strong link between this form of biological motion
detection and the posterior STS (cf. Noguchi, Kancoke,
Kakigi, Tanabe, & Sadato, 2005). The inferior parietal lobule (IPL) and FFA have also been implicated in a subset of
point light walker studies (Grèzes et al., 2001; Grossman &
Blake, 2002; Grossman et al., 2004; Vaina et al., 2001).
Processing the gaze direction of others has also
reliably activated the posterior STS, particularly in the
right hemisphere (Cloutier, Turk, & Macrae, 2008;
Hoffman & Haxby, 2000; Hooker et al., 2003; Mosconi,
Mack, McCarthy, & Pelphrey, 2005; Pelphrey, Morris, &
McCarthy, 2005; Pelphrey, Singerman, Allison, & McCarthy,
2003; Pelphrey, Viola, & McCarthy, 2004; Wicker, Perrett,
Baron-Cohen, & Decety, 2003). Young children show this
effect (Mosconi et al., 2005), whereas individuals with
lesions to the superior temporal region have gaze-processing deficits (Akiyama, Kato, Muramatsu, Saito, Nakachi,
et al., 2006; Akiyama, Kato, Muramatsu, Saito, Umeda, et al.,
2006). Hoffman and Haxby (2000) observed that the presentation of faces showing different gaze cues could modulate
FFA or posterior STS activity depending on whether subjects were instructed to attend to the targets’ identity or
gaze, respectively. Similar to the observation of walking, gaze
perception also modulates activity in the IPL (Hoffman &
Haxby, 2000; Pelphrey et al., 2003).
Action Observation
Action observation involves the perception of biological motion that implies a specific action is being enacted
intentionally. Most action observation studies have examined the neural responses to “reaching to grasp” actions or
other hand actions. These studies have commonly observed
increased activity in the left IPL and left posterior ventrolateral PFC (bleeding into the contiguous region of ventral
premotor cortex) during action observation compared with
control stimuli (Chong, Williams, Cunnington, & Mattingley,
CH05.indd 151
151
2008; Decety et al., 1997; Johnson-Frey et al., 2003; Lamm,
Batson, & Decety, 2007; Lotze et al., 2006; Molnar-Szakacs,
Kaplan, Greenfield, & Iacoboni, 2006; Pierno et al., 2009).
One study examining the effects of cognitive load on action
observation found that IPL and posterior STS activity were
unaffected by load but that ventrolateral PFC responses
to action were absent during load (Chong et al., 2008).
In addition, the posterior STS and temporoparietal junction
(TPJ) have been observed in some of these studies as well
(Chong et al., 2008; Lamm, Batson, et al., 2007; Liljeström
et al., 2008).
Emotion Recognition
Recognizing the emotional displays of other people is one
of the most frequent and important forms of nonverbal
decoding performed by humans. Such displays provide
relatively automatic, prereflective access into the psychological state of others, although it should be noted that
the bare perception of these displays does not necessarily
imply that those psychological states are being explicitly
represented or processed.
A number of brain regions have been implicated in
the processing of emotional facial expressions; however, the
vast majority of studies have focused on the amygdala.
The amygdala has been a central focus of study in affective
neuroscience more generally, in part because of its clear
causal role in fear conditioning in rodents (LeDoux, Iwata,
Cicchetti, & Reis, 1988) and its frequent activation in neuroimaging studies of fearful faces (Morris et al., 1996).
Since these early studies, it has become clear that the
amygdala can respond to both positively and negatively
valenced stimuli (Hamann, Ely, Hoffman, & Kilts, 2002),
as long as they are high in arousal (Anderson, Christoff,
Panitz, De Rosa, & Gabrieli, 2003; Cunningham, Raye, &
Johnson, 2004), as well as various facial expressions
(Fitzgerald, Angstadt, Jelsone, Nathan, & Luan Phan, 2006;
van der Gaag, Minderaa, & Keysers, 2007). An increasingly common view is that the amygdala serves as a detector of potential emotional significance of things in the
environment. Consistent with this view, the amygdala is
responsive to novelty, regardless of valence or arousal,
as new things may provide as yet unidentified reward or
threat (Schwartz et al., 2003).
If the amygdala is part of the brain’s advance scout team
determining what is important to focus on and react to, one
would expect this region to operate very efficiently. There
is now converging evidence to suggest that the amygdala
processes the emotional significance of perceptual stimuli automatically. First, the amygdala responds to threat
stimuli presented subliminally (Morris, Öhman, & Dolan,
1998; Whalen et al., 1998) or in binocular rivalry paradigms (Pasley, Mayes, & Schultz, 2004; Williams, Morris,
12/22/09 5:33:34 PM
152
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
McGlone, Abbott, & Mattingley, 2004). Second, individuals
who have damage to visual pathways still produce activation of the amygdala to emotional stimuli (Anders et al.,
2004; Hamm et al., 2003; Vuilleumier et al., 2002). Third,
intracranial recordings of amygdala activity suggest that it
responds to emotional stimuli within 200 ms of their presentation (Krolak-Salmon, Hénaff, Vighetto, Bertrand, &
Mauguière, 2004). Finally, amygdala activity to emotional
stimuli is preserved under some forms of cognitive load
(Anderson et al., 2003; Vuilleumier, Armony, Driver, &
Dolan, 2001), although not always (Pessoa, McKenna,
Gutierrez, & Ungerleider, 2002).
With respect to facial expressions, the role of the amygdala has been most clearly established in lesion studies by
Adolphs, Tranel, Damasio, and Damasio (1995) demonstrating that damage to the amygdala produces deficits in identifying emotional expressions, particularly fear. Lesions to
the insula (Calder, Keane, Manes, Antoun, & Young, 2000),
basal ganglia (Calder, Keane, Lawrence, & Manes, 2004),
and ventromedial PFC (Heberlein, Padon, Gillihan, Farah, &
Fellows, 2008) have also been shown to impair identification of one or more facial expressions.
The FFA is also modulated by emotional expressions
compared with neutral faces; however, a series of studies by
Vuilleumier and colleagues have demonstrated that this
response is likely due to feedback from the amygdala after
the amygdala has already processed the facial expression.
First, the pattern of activity in amygdala and FFA under
dual-task conditions is more consistent with the amygdala’s
influence over FFA than visa versa (Vuilleumier et al.,
2001; Vuilleumier, Mohr, Valenza, Wetzel, & Landis, 2003).
Second, patients with amygdala lesions do not show
greater FFA activity to emotional than nonemotional faces
(Vuilleumier, Richardson, Armony, Driver, & Dolan, 2004).
The right ventrolateral PFC is another region that is
commonly activated during emotion recognition. This
activity may be specifically related to explicitly identifying an emotional expression (Lieberman et al., 2007;
Nomura et al., 2004), as this region is less often observed
during passive viewing of emotional faces and is typically
absent if attention is directed toward nonemotional aspects
of emotional faces. This parallels the finding of decreased
activity in the ventrolateral PFC during action observation
under cognitive load, described earlier, and other similar
findings in the domains of visual self-recognition (Sugiura
et al., 2000) and imitation (Lee, Josephs, Dolan, &
Critchley, 2006), described later.
Although the lion’s share of emotion recognition has
focused on facial expressions, some studies have examined
emotional prosody (i.e., tone of voice) as well as body position and movements as indicators of emotional state. Passive
presentations of emotional compared with nonemotional
CH05.indd 152
prosody have been shown to activate the right superior
temporal gyrus or STS in a region anterior to the region
commonly observed in studies of biological motion
(Beaucousin et al., 2007; Wiethoff et al., 2007). When the
emotional tone heard is explicitly labeled, there is still STS
activity along with activity in right or bilateral ventrolateral
PFC (Bach et al., 2008; Ethofer et al., 2006; Wildgruber,
Pihan, Ackermann, Erb, & Grodd, 2002; Wildgruber et al.,
2005). Identifying emotion from bodies has been shown
to activate the right posterior STS, right TPJ, EBA, amygdala, and bilateral temporal pole each in one of three studies (de Gelder, Snyder, Greve, Gerard, & Hadjikhani, 2004;
Grèzes, Pichon, & de Gelder, 2007; Peelen, Atkinson,
Andersson, & Vuilleumier, 2007), with only the bilateral
ventrolateral PFC appearing in multiple studies. Finally,
one study has used a standardized test of nonverbal decoding ability, the Profile of Nonverbal Sensitivity (Rosenthal,
Hall, DiMatteo, Rogers, & Archer, 1979), and observed
posterior STS, left IPL, left TPJ, and bilateral ventrolateral
PFC activity while labeling the emotional state of the targets. Additionally, those self-reporting greater social skills
produced larger increases in right ventrolateral PFC, dorsomedial PFC, and basal ganglia.
Social Inference
Social inference has been at the heart of social cognition
for more than three decades. Social inference encompasses
a variety of processes invoked as we form representations
of the psychological states, traits, and preferences of others. These inferences can be made using inferential algebra (Jones & Harris, 1967), covariation analyses (Kelley,
1973), stereotype-based inferences (Ames, 2004; Fiske &
Neuberg, 1990), or by projecting oneself onto the target
(Ross, Greene, & House, 1977). Some of these processes
occur automatically, whereas others occur slowly guided
by specific inferential intentions that require cognitive
resources and effort (Gilbert, Pelham, & Krull, 1988).
Despite social psychology’s focus on a deficit in the ability
of humans to make sense of other minds (Gilbert & Malone,
1995), the vast majority of social cognitive neuroscience
studies of social inference have been inspired by the study of
children developing the ability to make sense of other minds
(i.e., “mentalizing”). Wimmer and Perner (1983) first used
false-belief tests to determine when children begin to show
basic mentalizing competence. These tasks are usually of the
following form: Person A knows that X is true (e.g., Sally
knows her marble is in the box on the left); while Person A is
absent, things are changed such that X is no longer true (e.g.,
while Sally is out of the room, Anne moves the marble to the
box on the right). The subject is then asked what Person A
now believes about X. The subject knows that X is no longer
12/22/09 5:33:35 PM
Functional Neuroanatomy
true, but Person A does not and therefore the subject should
indicate that Person A believes X is true. Good performance
is thought to indicate that the child has a theory of other
minds (i.e., theory of mind; Premack & Woodruff, 1978) and
that other minds can represent the world differently from our
own. Most children master this basic mentalizing skill by age
three or four.
Mentalizing
To isolate the neural correlates of mentalizing, several
researchers have used verbal stimulus materials, including variants of the false-belief paradigm described earlier
(Gobbini, Koralek, Bryan, Montgomery, & Haxby, 2007;
Grèzes, Berthoz, & Passingham, 2006; Grèzes, Frith, &
Passingham, 2004; Mitchell, 2008; Perner & Aichhorn,
2006; Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003; Saxe, Moran, Scholz, &
Gabrieli, 2006; Saxe, Schulz, & Jiang, 2006). Other verbal tasks have used short stories that require mentalizing to
explain a target’s behavior, but do not specifically depend
on a false belief (Fletcher et al., 1995; Gallagher et al., 2000;
Happé et al., 1996; Hynes, Baird, & Grafton, 2006; Saxe &
Kanwisher, 2003; Völlm et al., 2005). Also, some verbal tasks
are used to assess the ability to infer other individual’s feelings, rather than thoughts (Hynes et al., 2006; Shamay-Tsoory,
Tibi-Elhanany, & Aharon-Peretz, 2006; Shamay-Tsoory &
Aharon-Peretz, 2007; Vollm et al., 2006).
Other tasks induce mentalizing nonverbally. Several
studies have used animations of geometric shapes inspired
by the classic Heider and Simmel (1944) fighting triangles
video (Castelli, Frith, Happé, & Frith, 2002; Gobbini et al.,
2007; Moriguchi et al., 2006; Ohnishi et al., 2004; Schultz,
Imamizu, Kawato, & Frith, 2004), which promote anthropomorphism and mental state attributions to the shapes.
Some nonverbal tasks require inferences to be drawn about
mental states from a target’s eyes (Baron-Cohen et al.,
1999; Platek, Keenan, Gallup, & Mohamed, 2004) or use
nonverbal cartoons in which subjects choose a final panel
based on their understanding of the target’s mental state
from the earlier panels (Brunet, Sarfati, Hardy-Baylé, &
Decety, 2000; Gallagher et al., 2000).
A third type of mentalizing study examines judgments of
enduring psychological characteristics of others via impression formation, for example, by asking what characteristics
the person has (Harris, Todorov, & Fiske, 2005; Heberlein &
Saxe, 2005; Mitchell, Banaji, & Macrae, 2005a, 2005b;
Mitchell, Cloutier, Banaji, & Macrae, 2006), and via conceptual perspective-taking, for example, by asking how
the person would judge topic X (Ruby & Decety, 2003,
2004). Inferences about momentary intentions have also
been examined in paradigms that require subjects to infer
the intentions of others (Ciaramidaro et al., 2007; German,
Niehaus, Roarty, Giesbrecht, & Miller, 2004; Kampe,
CH05.indd 153
153
Frith, & Frith, 2003; Walter et al., 2004) or to determine
what one’s own intention would be in particular situations
(Blakemore, den Ouden, Choudhury, & Frith, 2007; den
Ouden, Frith, & Blakemore, 2005).
A final set of mentalizing studies has examined online
mentalizing as it occurs in the context of interaction with
other people (although not face-to-face). In three studies,
subjects played strategy games (e.g., prisoner ’s dilemma)
against a person or computer, under the assumption that
mentalizing should occur only when playing against a
person (Fukui, Murai, Shinozaki, 2006; Gallagher, Jack,
Roepstorff, & Frith, 2002; Rilling, Sanfey, Aronson,
Nystrom, & Cohen, 2004). In a fourth study, subjects
believed they were either collaborating on a task with the
experimenter or working alone (Gilbert et al., 2007). In a
fifth study, professional taxi drivers drove a simulated taxi
in a virtual reality environment in which they interacted
with numerous other characters whose mental states were
relevant (Spiers & Maguire, 2006).
In addition to these different methods for studying mentalizing, there have been a handful of studies that have
reported on the neural bases of irony and idiom comprehension (Lauro, Tettamanti, Cappa, & Papagano, 2008;
Wakusawa et al., 2007; Wang, Lee, Sigman, & Dapretto,
2006a, 2006b). Comprehension of irony and idiom involves
understanding of communicative intent and requires distinguishing literal from contextually suggested meanings.
Therefore, these tasks probably require similar, if not identical, processes as those used for mentalizing.
Across 45 tasks/studies,1 three regions were present in
more than half of the studies (see Table 5.2 and Figure 5.3).
The dorsomedial PFC (Brodmann areas [BA] 8/9) was
reported in 91% of mentalizing tasks, whereas the TPJ and
temporal pole were reported in 59% and 52%, respectively.
The posterior STS and precuneus were each observed in
39% of studies, and the medial PFC (BA 10) was observed
in 33%. In approximately half of the studies reporting
temporal pole, posterior STS, and TPJ activations, these
activations were bilateral. In those studies in which
these regions were reported in only one hemisphere, only
the posterior STS was reliably lateralized, appearing in the
right hemisphere in 88% of these nonbilateral studies.
Table 5.2 also breaks down the activations by mentalizing induction type for any method that has been used at least
four times (false belief, story, animation, impression formation, intention inference, online mentalizing, and irony and
idiom comprehension). There are three notable conclusions.
First, the dorsomedial PFC is the only region that is reliably
1
Studies including runs of more than one method are counted
separately for each method’s results.
12/22/09 5:33:36 PM
154
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
Table 5.2 Activations from 45 Mentalizing Studies
False belief (n⫽8)
Verbal stories (n⫽6)
Animations (n⫽5)
DMPFC
TPJ
Temporal Pole
88%
88%
25%
83%
100%
100%
0%
pSTS
Precuneus
MPFC
IFG
VMPFC Fusiform Gyrus
25%
63%
25%
25%
0%
67%
17%
33%
17%
17%
17%
0%
100%
100%
20%
20%
40%
20%
80%
0%
Impression formation (n⫽6)
100%
66%
33%
33%
66%
33%
33%
50%
0%
Intention inference (n⫽6)
100%
67%
67%
67%
67%
67%
33%
17%
17%
Online mentalizing (n⫽5)
100%
40%
40%
20%
20%
40%
0%
0%
0%
Irony & idioms (n⫽4)
75%
25%
75%
75%
0%
50%
25%
50%
0%
Total (n⫽45)
91%
59%
52%
39%
39%
33%
24%
15%
13%
Mentalizing
Mentalizing
Mirror System
Empathy
1 dorsomedial PFC
2 precuneus/posterior cingulate
3 temporal junction
4 posterior superior temporal sulcus
5 temporal pole
6 ventrolateral PFC – pars opercularis and
ventral premotor cortex
7 inferior parietal lobule
8 dorsal anterior cingulate cortex
9 anterior insula
10 medial PFC
activated by each mentalizing paradigm. Second, animation-induced mentalizing consistently recruits the temporal
pole and posterior STS, but not the TPJ; verbally induced
mentalizing via false belief and other verbal stories consistently recruits the TPJ, but not the temporal pole and posterior STS. This is consistent with the notion that the STS and
TP are involved in nonreflective social cognition, whereas
the TPJ, as part of lateral parietal cortex, is involved in more
reflective aspects of social cognition (Satpute & Lieberman,
2006; Liberman, 2009b). Finally, although fusiform gyrus
was observed in 13% of the studies overall, it was present in
80% of the animation-based studies.
CH05.indd 154
Figure 5.3 The brain regions involved in
social inference. The top row of images
displays the regions commonly activated in
mentalizing and theory of mind tasks. The
bottom left image displays the mirror system. The bottom right image displays brain
regions identified in studies of empathy.
Note: Anterior insula is displayed on the medial
wall for presentation purposes, but is actually
between the medial and lateral walls of the
cortex.
These results suggest that the dorsomedial PFC may
play a central role in mentalizing in general, with other
subsets of regions being recruited for particular kinds of
materials or task demands. There is at least some evidence
to suggest that dorsomedial PFC activation is modulated
by an explicit mentalizing goal and can be taken offline by
cognitive load in dual-task paradigms. Conversely, the
posterior STS and temporal pole can be activated to
mentalizing-relevant materials in the absence of a mentalizing goal and are still activated to the same degree with and
without cognitive load (den Ouden, U. Frith, C. Frith, &
Blakemore, 2005; Mason, Banfield, & Macrae, 2004;
12/22/09 5:33:36 PM
Functional Neuroanatomy
Mitchell, Macrae, & Banaji, 2004; van Duynslaeger, van
Overwalle, & Verstraeten, 2007).
A number of lesion studies have also helped to identify
the regions that causally contribute to mentalizing. These
studies can be subdivided into those that focus on the prefrontal cortex, TPJ, or amygdala. Several lesion studies
have demonstrated prefrontal involvement in mentalizing;
however, these studies do not provide much anatomical
specificity (Channon & Crawford, 2000; Stone, BaronCohen, & Knight, 1998; Stuss, Gallup, & Alexander, 2001).
One study did find that left ventrolateral PFC was associated with impairments in making personality judgments
(Heberlein, Adolphs, Tranel, & Damasio, 2004).
Bird, Castelli, Malik, and Husain (2004) reported a
case study of a patient with focal dorsomedial PFC and
medial PFC damage. Despite the strong links in the fMRI
literature between the dorsomedial PFC and mentalizing,
this patient demonstrated no mentalizing impairments. It is
worth noting that developmental mentalizing studies have
consistently reported decreasing dorsomedial PFC activity with age, suggesting that it may play a greater causal
role in adolescence than in adulthood (Blakemore et al.,
2007; Wang et al., 2006a; see also Pfeifer, Lieberman, &
Dapretto, 2007). This would be consistent with the dorsomedial PFC playing a controlled processing role in
mentalizing that may be less needed as elements of mentalizing are increasingly automated.
In another case study, a patient with focal right ventrolateral
PFC damage experienced mentalizing deficits under specific
circumstances (Samson, Apperly, Kathirgamanathan, &
Humphreys, 2005). The patient was capable of reasoning
about a target’s false belief if the story was crafted to indicate that the target had a false belief without revealing
what the true state of affairs was. In contrast, if the patient
knew the true state of affairs, he consistently projected this
knowledge onto the target. Samson and colleagues interpreted these findings as indicating an impaired ability to
inhibit one’s own perspective and knowledge, rather than a
deficit in belief reasoning per se (see also Lamm, Nusbaum,
Meltzoff, & Decety, 2007). This interpretation is supported by
developmental findings that mentalizing abilities in children
are correlated with inhibitory skill as well (Carlson & Moses,
2001). Three studies examining left TPJ lesions (Apperly,
Samson, Chiavaino, & Humphreys, 2004; Heberlein et al.,
2004; Samson, Apperly, Chiavarino, & Humphreys, 2004)
also demonstrated significant mentalizing impairments associated with this region (cf. Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2006).
Finally, there has been an ongoing debate about the
role of the amygdala in mentalizing. The amygdala figured prominently in early theories of mentalizing (BaronCohen et al., 2000), but it was reported in only 2 of the 45
neuroimaging studies of mentalizing reviewed. Although
CH05.indd 155
155
the results of studies looking at mentalizing in individuals
with amygdala lesions is mixed (Han, Jiang, Humphreys,
Zhou, & Cai, 2005; Shaw et al., 2007; Stone, Baron-Cohen,
Calder, Keane, & Young, 2003), a study by Shaw et al. (2004)
may help explain the amygdala’s role in mentalizing and
why it does not appear in most neuroimaging studies. Shaw
et al. (2004) compared 15 subjects with congenital amygdala damage from birth or early childhood to 11 subjects
with amygdala lesions that developed in adulthood. Early
damage was associated with a variety of mentalizing deficits, whereas late damage was not. Moreover, the subject’s
age at the time the lesion developed was strongly correlated
with overall mentalizing performance. This suggests that
the amygdala may play a critical role in bridging between
early and mature forms of mentalizing. More sophisticated
forms of mentalizing may not specifically depend on the
amygdala; however, they may develop in the first place
only if simpler amygdala-based mentalizing skills are in
place to be built upon (see Machado, Snyder, Cherry,
Lavenex, & Amaral, 2008).
Attempts are being made to determine the functional
contributions of particular brain regions to mentalizing,
but most of the results are quite tentative at this point
(Decety & Lamm, 2007; Gallagher & Frith, 2003; Saxe &
Wexler, 2005). The posterior STS responds to biological
motion cues (e.g., gaze shifts, lip movements) that are likely
to provide raw perceptual material for drawing inferences
about the mental states of others. Temporal poles are commonly activated when seeing the faces or names of familiar people (Sugiura et al., 2006), and some have suggested
that this region represents semantic information in the social
domain (Lambon Ralph, Pobric, & Jefferies, 2009). Saxe and
colleagues have suggested that the TPJ is specifically responsible for belief-related cognition (Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003;
Saxe & Wexler, 2005); however, others have suggested that
the TPJ is responsible for directing attention to salient cues
in the environment (Decety & Lamm, 2007; Mitchell, 2008)
rather than having a specific role in mentalizing.
Although the dorsomedial PFC is by far the most commonly activated region during mentalizing, there is not yet
an agreed-upon account of its function (Amodio & Frith,
2006; Saxe & Powell, 2006). One relatively unexplored
idea suggests an analogy to working memory processes
where the dorsolateral PFC is thought to orchestrate working memory using various “slave” systems in the lateral
parietal cortex (Baddeley, 2002) and elsewhere (Postle,
2006). In the context of mentalizing, the dorsomedial PFC
would orchestrate cognition about mental states with the
help of more simplistic slave systems in the TPJ, posterior
STS, and temporal poles. Such a model would be relatively
straightforward to test with modified working memory
paradigms.
12/22/09 5:33:38 PM
156
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
Mentalizing About Similar Others
Even if the exact role of the dorsomedial PFC is not yet
specified, a recent series of studies have helped clarify
how the dorsomedial and medial PFCs differentially contribute to mentalizing. Mitchell and colleagues (Mitchell
et al., 2005b; Mitchell, Macrae, & Banaji, 2006) have
demonstrated that the subjective similarity between a
target and oneself determines which PFC region is most
strongly associated with mentalizing. In most mentalizing studies, there is little basis for even evaluating the
similarity of targets to oneself, and these studies reliably
recruit the dorsomedial PFC. In Mitchell’s studies, to
the extent that targets are rated as dissimilar to the self, the
dorsomedial PFC is again the region most activated by
mentalizing. However, to the extent that targets are rated
as similar to oneself, a more ventral region in the medial
PFC is increasingly activated. Mitchell has suggested that
for similar targets, subjects are projecting themselves onto
the other person to answer questions about the target. Selfreferential processing is strongly associated with medial
PFC activity (Lieberman, 2007), and thus this account
makes intuitive sense.
Other qualitative distinctions might contribute to a
split between the contributions of the dorsomedial PFC
and medial PFC in mentalizing. Mentalizing is typically
equated with theory of mind processes in a broad fashion.
Yet people have a theory of “minds in general” as well as
theories of “specific minds.” It could be the case that the
dorsomedial PFC supports the general theory of mind,
including rules for understanding how the average person
is likely to experience and respond to different situations
and events. In contrast, the medial PFC might support idiosyncratic theories of specific minds, including our own
mind and those close to us. To this end, van Overwalle
(2009) published a meta-analysis suggesting that mentalizing about close others does reliably recruit the medial PFC
(cf. Heatherton et al., 2006). From this perspective, the
similarity findings from Mitchell and colleagues (2005b)
may be a special case of applying a specific theory of
mind (i.e., one’s specific theory of one’s own mind) to a
similar other.
Imitation and the Mirror Neuron System
In the early 1990s, Rizzolatti and colleagues (di Pellegrino,
Fadiga, Fogassi, Gallese, & Rizzolatti, 1992; Gallese, Fadiga,
Fogassi, & Rizzolatti, 1996) discovered a set of neurons
in the ventral premotor cortex in monkeys that was active
both when the monkey performed a goal-directed action
(e.g., grabbing a raisin) and when the monkey watched
someone else perform the same goal-directed action. Later
researchers observed similar effects in the anterior section
of the IPL (Gallese, Fogassi, Fadiga, & Rizzolatti, 2002).
CH05.indd 156
Together, the ventral premotor cortex and anterior IPL
form, in monkeys, what has been called the mirror neuron
system (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004).
Although no human research has identified single
neurons in these regions that respond both when observing and when performing an action, there is compelling
fMRI data to suggest that a homologous mirror system
exists in humans. Iacoboni and colleagues (1999) provided the first evidence by having subjects observe and
imitate finger tapping while in a scanner. They found
three brain regions that were active during both observation and imitation: left posterior ventrolateral PFC,2 right
anterior IPL, and right anterior intraparietal sulcus. The
bilateral posterior ventrolateral PFC and bilateral anterior
IPL have been identified as the regions central to the
mirror system (Chaminade & Decety, 2002; Hamilton,
Wolpert, Frith, & Grafton, 2006; Heiser, Iacoboni, Maeda,
Marcus, & Mazziotta, 2003; Urgesi, Moro, Candid, &
Aglioti, 2006). One critical difference between the human
and monkey mirror systems is that in monkeys, only hand
actions that are observed in the presence of the object to
be manipulated produce activity in the mirror neurons
(Gallese et al., 1996). In contrast, for humans a variety of
hand actions that do not involve an object or involve an
occluded object still produce mirror system activity (Liu
et al., 2008; Montgomery, Isenberg, & Haxby, 2007). Also,
the human mirror system is active when observing goaldirected actions performed by robots whose action paths
differ from human actions (Engel, Burke, Fiehler, Bien, &
Rösler, in press; Engel, Burke, Fiehler, Bien, & Rösler, 2008;
Gazzola, Rizzolatti, & Keysers, 2008; cf. Tai, Scherfler,
Brooks, Sawamoto, & Castiello, 2004).
Beyond the original studies of hand–object actions,
there have been a number of extensions regarding the
classes of actions that activate the mirror system in humans.
Communicative hand gestures and mimed actions both activate this system (Liu et al., 2008; Montgomery et al., 2007).
Hearing actions activate the ventral premotor cortex (Kohler
et al., 2002). In addition, being touched or watching another
person being touched produces mirror-like effects in the IPL
(Keysers et al., 2004). A series of studies have also determined that the mirror system is activated during observation
and imitation of facial expressions (Carr, Iacoboni, Dubeau,
Mazziotta, & Lenzi, 2003; Hennenlotter et al., 2005;
2
For the remainder of this chapter, “posterior ventrolateral PFC”
is used to refer to the pars opercularis region of the inferior frontal gyrus and the neighboring ventral premotor region commonly
found in imitation studies. “Ventrolateral PFC” is used to refer to
mid-ventrolateral and anterior ventrolateral areas, including the
pars triangularis, pars orbitalis, and lateral BA 10.
12/22/09 5:33:38 PM
Functional Neuroanatomy
Lee et al., 2006; Leslie, Johnson-Frey, & Grafton, 2004;
Pfeifer, Iacoboni, Mazziotta, & Dapretto, 2008).
Hennenlotter et al. (2009) found that amygdala activity in
response to angry faces was reduced in subjects after Botox
injections into their foreheads; they also observed that the
reduction in amygdala activity correlated with the reduction
in “frown muscle” activity. These results suggest that spontaneous imitation of the observed facial expressions contributes to the strength of one’s own limbic responses. Finally,
somewhat counter to the notion that the same representation
for action is activated both when seeing and when performing an action, performing actions that complement
an observed action activate the mirror system more than
actually imitating the action (Newmann-Norlund, van
Schie, van Zuijilen, & Bekkering, 2007).
The discovery of mirror neurons in primates and the
homologous mirror system in humans has produced enormous excitement within the scientific community and
beyond. This system is proposed to be at the root of our language abilities, the ability to learn through imitation, a basis
for social ingratiation through unconscious mimicking,
and a mechanism critical to automatic nonverbal encoding and decoding, mental state inference, and empathy.
Faith in the significance of the mirror system has led some
to “predict that mirror neurons will do for psychology
what DNA did for biology. . . . They will provide a unifying framework and help explain a host of mental abilities
that have hitherto remained mysterious” (Motluck, 2001).
In contrast, Gopnik (2007) has argued that much like the
left-brain/right-brain notions that took root in popular
culture in the 1970s and still retain a myth-like status, the
mirror neuron mania is promising much more than it has
delivered. Publications of articles in the mainstream media
with titles such as “Cells That Read Minds” (Blakeslee,
2006) oversimplify the findings and give an inaccurate characterization of what these neurons are known to be doing.
Beyond responding both when an action is observed and
performed, what are the functional properties of the mirror
system? One open question is whether this is a system that
supports vicarious learning of new behaviors or is tuned to
respond to actions that are already well established in one’s
behavioral repertoire. Supporting the latter interpretation,
professional pianists show greater mirror system activity
when listening to music than do nonmusicians (Bangert
et al., 2006) and professional dancers show greater mirror system activity when watching a dance performance in
their own style of dance than a performance from another
tradition (Calvo-Merino, Glaser, Grèzes, Passingham, &
Haggard, 2005). In contrast, two studies have found that
observation of unknown guitar chords, for which no motor
representation already exists, produced mirror system
activity (Buccino et al., 2004; Vogt et al., 2007), with one
CH05.indd 157
157
of these finding greater mirror system activity for unknown
than for known chords (Vogt et al., 2007). One resolution
to these conflicting findings centers on the observer ’s goal.
In the studies in which only known actions activated the
mirror system, subjects did not have the goal of subsequently performing these unknown actions. In the studies
in which unknown actions produced robust mirror system
activity, subjects were required to subsequently perform
these actions. Thus, having the explicit goal of learning to
perform an action can bring the mirror system online even,
or perhaps especially, while observing unknown actions.
A final study relevant to the role of the mirror system in
known and unknown actions scanned dancers before and
after 5 days of training on particular dance routines (Cross,
Kraemer, Hamilton, Kelley, & Grafton, 2009). Subjects
were scanned while watching several dance routines—
some that would be learned and others that would not. For
both kinds of dances, the mirror system was at its most active
before the training period. Those that were then practiced
for 5 days retained nearly the same level of activation in the
mirror system, whereas the untrained dances produced far
less mirror system activity at the posttest. These data suggest
that having a preexisting action representation contributes
less to mirror system activity than the motivational relevance of the actions to oneself. The fact that food-grasping
behavior produces more mirror system activity in hungry
subjects than in satiated subjects is consistent with this
motivational account (Chen, Meltzoff, & Decety, 2007).
Is Mirroring Automatic?
It is generally assumed that the mirror neuron system operates automatically, converting third-person observations of
actions into embodied first-person experiences, and therefore into an understanding of the mental states (intentions,
thoughts, feelings, desires) of others. Taking the automaticity claim first (with the second claim addressed in the
next section), the best evidence for this comes from a study
in which some subjects were instructed to explicitly focus
on an actor ’s actions and intentions and other subjects
were instructed simply to watch the video clips (Iacoboni
et al., 2005). Similar levels of mirror system activity were
found in both sets of subjects, leading to the conclusion
that mirror system activity is automatic. The difficulty with
this interpretation is that the subjects not receiving action
observation instructions (1) were free to explicitly focus on
the intentions and actions in the clips and (2) viewed clips
in which there was little else to attend to but the actions.
Stronger tests of automaticity have thus far come down on
the side of the mirror system being relatively intentional
and controlled. For instance, Lee and colleagues asked
subjects to look at emotionally expressive faces and to
imitate the emotional expressions in one set of trials and
12/22/09 5:33:39 PM
158
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
to make gender discriminations in the other set of trials
(Lee et al., 2006). In both types of trials, subjects were
attending to the faces, but only the imitate trials produced
mirror system activity. If the mirror system responds automatically, there should have been activity in both conditions. Another study found that when simulated biological
motion was viewed along with a task to determine whether
the motion was biologically plausible, there was more
mirror system activity than if the same motion was observed
with an instruction to focus on the colors of the moving
elements (Engel et al., 2008). A third study used a working memory paradigm to examine neural responses when
one, two, or three actions had to be held in memory for
several seconds; mirror system activity in this study was
found to increase linearly with the number of actions to be
remembered (Engel et al., in press). This suggests that the
mirror system may operate as a working memory system
for action, which is consistent with a controlled processing
account. Given the paucity of studies examining whether
the mirror system functions automatically, the answer is
not yet clear, but the evidence thus far does suggest that the
mirror system may not function automatically.
Mentalizing Versus Mirroring
Most studies examining individuals’ ability to infer the
contents of another ’s mind (i.e., mental state inference)
have typically come from the theory of mind tradition
(Wimmer & Perner, 1983). The mirror system is thought to
represent a neural substrate for a second way of understanding the mental states of others characterized by simulation
theory (Goldman, 1989). According to simulation theory,
“we understand others’ thoughts by pretending to be in their
‘mental shoes’ and by using our own mind/body as a model
of the minds of others” (Gallese, Ferrari, & Umiltà, 2002,
p. 36). In terms of the mirror system, this suggests that we
understand the mental states that lead a person to perform a
certain action because seeing this action activates the motor
representations we possess for performing the same action.
This then allows us to use our own activated mental states to
understand the other individual’s mind.
This is an appealing account of understanding others in
an embodied way. The open question is whether the mirror
system contributes to understanding others, and if so, in
what ways? Despite the claim that mirror neurons provide
a unifying “basis of social cognition” (Gallese, Keysers, &
Rizzolatti, 2004), studies of the mirror system almost never
assess the social understanding supposedly obtained as a
result of mirror system activity, and studies that examine
social cognition overtly (i.e., mentalizing studies) rarely
report activity in the mirror system.
The limitation of previous studies to address this issue
is that mentalizing and mirroring studies each leave out
CH05.indd 158
a critical element that would lead the “other team” to cry
foul. On one hand, imitation studies that successfully
recruit the mirror system do not ask subjects to draw inferences about the mental states of the observed target or
check whether they have. On the other hand, mentalizing
studies, which almost always have an abstract detached
quality to them, do not lend themselves to mirror system
involvement. Reading vignettes or watching abstract shapes
move around are not the kinds of real-life experiences that
simulation theory focuses on.
Two studies have attempted to address these multiple
concerns in a single study. In a 2 ⫻ 2 study design by
Wheatley, Milleville, and Martin (2007), subjects were
shown object animations. The researchers varied whether
the animations looked like animate or inanimate entities
and whether subjects were watching or imagining the
movements. The mirror system was activated, and to a
similar degree, during all four trial types. In contrast, the
brain regions that selectively responded to animacy were
almost all mentalizing regions and none were mirror system regions, except for the posterior STS, which is the one
region that sometimes appears in both networks. Judging
animacy is not the same as mental state inference, but it is
certainly a step in that direction.
In another study, Spunt, Satpute, and Lieberman (in press)
presented subjects with video clips of an actor performing
simple everyday goal-directed behaviors (e.g., brushing his
teeth) but manipulated the subject’s inferential goal along
an action identification hierarchy (Vallacher & Wegner,
1987). On different trials, subjects were asked to think
about what the target was doing (medium action identification level; “brushing his teeth”), how the target was
performing the behavior (low action identification level;
“moving his arm”), or why the target was performing the
behavior (high action identification level; “maintaining
oral hygiene”). Critically higher levels of action identification require a greater focus on the internal mental states
of the actor, and lower levels shift attention away from mental states and focus more on the external mechanics of the
behavior. Similar to the results of the study by Wheatley
et al. (2007), performing each of the identification tasks
activated the mirror system to the same degree, suggesting that differential needs for mental state inference did
not differentially engage the mirror system. In contrast,
multiple regions in the mentalizing network produced
parametric increases in activity that tracked increases in
action identification level. Given that these were everyday kinds of behaviors that could or could not be used
to draw inferences about the mental states of the actor,
depending on the subject’s goals, it is difficult to raise the
abstraction argument that applies to previous mentalizing
tasks.
12/22/09 5:33:39 PM
Functional Neuroanatomy
At this point, it appears that the mirror system is primarily
involved in understanding observed behaviors externally
as behaviors (i.e., behavior identification) but may not be
involved in consciously understanding or representing the
mental states of others. In contrast, the mentalizing network thus far appears to be more central to mental state
inference. Interestingly, at rest the mentalizing and mirror
system networks are negatively correlated with one another
(Fox et al., 2005).
Empathy
Empathy has quickly become a major area of study within
social cognitive neuroscience. One of the first studies in
this area involved subjects being scanned while alternately
receiving painful stimulation and observing their romantic
partner receiving painful stimulation (Singer et al., 2004).
Analogous to the mirror system’s common response to performing and observing an action, Singer and colleagues
found that the pain distress regions of the brain, the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the anterior insula,
were activated while receiving and observing another
receive painful stimulation. Eight studies have now almost
all shown the dorsal ACC and anterior insula to be active in
studies of empathy for physical pain (Botvinick et al., 2005;
Morrison, Peelen, & Downing, 2007; Ochsner et al., 2008;
Singer et al., 2004, 2006), distressing loud noises (Lamm,
Batson, et al., 2007), and disgusting odors (Jabbi, Swart, &
Keysers, 2007; Wicker, Keysers et al., 2003) in which subjects were both observers and receivers of the distressing
experience.
Although this is an extremely robust set of findings, it is
unclear how they relate to the broader concept(s) of empathy.
More than any other domain in social cognitive neuroscience, there seems to be little agreement about what
empathy is and what psychological processes it involves.
Lamm, Batson, et al. (2007) recently defined empathy in
terms of three components: “(1) an affective response to
another person, which some believe entails sharing that
person’s emotional state; (2) a cognitive capacity to take
the perspective of the other person; and (3) some monitoring mechanisms that keep track of the origins (self vs.
other) of the experienced feelings” (p. 42). This definition
gives a seat at the table to each of several different existing
approaches to empathy.
Another way to arrive at the same definition is to consider three things that empathy is not. Empathy is not a mere
cognitive understanding of the emotional state of another
without having any emotional reaction of one’s own. For
instance, one could see a picture of Hitler wincing in pain
and be able to accurately indicate his level of pain without
necessarily feeling a similar emotional response of one’s
own (Singer et al., 2006). We would not want to label this
CH05.indd 159
159
as an empathic response. Similarly, empathy is not merely
being in the same emotional state as another person. If one
were to see another in pain and become so distressed that
one began to ruminate on one’s own distress and how such
painful episodes could be avoided by oneself in the future,
this also would not be an empathic response (Batson,
1991). Along similar lines, having a positive emotional
response to the sight of one’s favorite food being served to
someone who despises that dish does not seem empathetic
either. Here, one would be focused on one’s own response
rather than the other person’s.
Thus, there are open questions as to what common brain
activations during the observation and experience of painful stimulation means with respect to empathy. Because
self-reported empathic feelings have not been correlated
with neural responses in the more than four dozen fMRI
studies of empathy, it is difficult to know whether subjects’
distress is related to feeling bad for the observed target or
if subjects are experiencing a self-focused type of distress.
One study has found that dorsal ACC and anterior insula
activity is modulated by whether the target receiving painful stimulation has previously been observed treating
others unfairly or not (Singer et al., 2006). If pain observation were only leading to self-focused distress, the moral
assessment of the pain recipient would probably be of little
consequence. Thus, this study provides some evidence that
the mirrored pain response may reflect empathic responses
rather than self-focused responses.
Another issue is whether the dorsal ACC and anterior insula activations reported during visual observation
of pain and distress generalizes to other kinds of empathy inductions (e.g., linguistic) and empathy inductions
focused on different domains of experience (e.g., sharing
in another ’s success). Humans are capable of empathizing
with an endless variety of experiences, but thus far pain has
been the primary experience examined. It is plausible that
dorsal ACC and anterior insula activity is a consequence
of already feeling empathic toward a person who now happens to be in pain and that other brain regions would be
activated if one watched an empathized-with person win
the lottery.
Although a number of studies have begun to address
these issues, few conclusions have emerged, because there
has been little consensus across different studies. For instance,
when subjects are exposed to stories or scenarios meant
to induce empathic responses (Decety & Chaminade,
2003; Farrow et al., 2001; Shamay-Tsoory, Tomer, Berger,
Goldsher, & Aharon-Peretz, 2005), the dorsal ACC and anterior insula are not commonly activated. Instead, mentalizing regions such as the dorsomedial PFC and temporal pole
tend to be activated along with amygdala. As mentioned,
no study has obtained self-reported empathy to presented
12/22/09 5:33:40 PM
160
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
stimuli that can be used to correlate with neural responses;
however, several studies have assessed trait empathy and
correlated this with neural responses. Here, the brain region
most commonly associated with trait empathy is the medial
PFC (BA 10; Ranklin et al., 2006; Shamay-Tsoory, Tomer,
Berger, & Aharon-Peretz, 2003; Shamay-Tsoory, Lester
et al., 2005; Singer et al., 2004). Other regions, including the
dorsal ACC, anterior insula, ventrolateral PFC (both mirror
system and non–mirror system areas), dorsomedial PFC,
and ventral striatum, have each been identified in at least
two studies using trait empathy measures (Chakrabarti,
Bullmore, & Baron-Cohen, 2006; Kaplan & Iacoboni,
2006; Pfeifer et al., 2008; Ranklin et al., 2006; SchulteRüther, Markowitsch, Fink, & Piefke, 2007; Singer et al.,
2004; Shamay-Tsoory, Lester et al., 2005).
Perhaps most clarifying in light of the tripartite empathy
definition given previously are the two studies by Lamm and
colleagues (Lamm, Batson, et al., 2007; Lamm, Nussbaum,
et al., 2007) that assessed trait emotional contagion, the
tendency to mirror what others are feeling. These studies
both found that activity in the dorsal ACC, anterior insula,
and mirror system was related to trait emotional contagion,
suggesting that these regions may be specifically involved
in the bottom-up emotion matching that often occurs with
empathy, rather than the top-down components of empathy
(i.e., perspective taking and keeping focus on the other
rather than on the self).
One of these studies in particular helps bolster this interpretation. Lamm, Nussbaum et al. (2007) had subjects view
two sets of pictures that depicted needles going through
the skin of a person’s hand; however, for one set, subjects
were informed that the “hand had already been numbed for
a biopsy.” The bottom-up visual inputs from both sets of
images appear painful, but top-down cognitive appraisal
should drive very different empathy responses to the two
stimuli. Pain regions including the dorsal ACC, anterior
insula, and somatosensory cortex were strongly activated
by both sets of pictures. In contrast, regions involved in
mentalizing (the medial PFC, dorsomedial PFC, ventromedial PFC, and precuneus) and self-control (the right ventrolateral PFC) were differentially activated to the different
sets of pictures. These regions may play a role in contextualizing empathic responses to take account of what the
experience of the other is likely to be, based on knowledge
of their situation (e.g., numbed hand) or personality (e.g.,
masochist?).
A number of studies have now examined what differs
in the brain as one considers another ’s distress rather than
one’s own. These studies are an important complement to
those that reveal the commonalities. Some of the studies
that reported commonalities also reported what was greater
during experiencing or observing something distressing
CH05.indd 160
(Ochsner et al., 2008; Singer et al., 2004; Wicker, Keysers,
et al., 2003). Other studies manipulated the subjects’ perspective to focus on a target’s experience or their own
experience (Jackson, Brunet, Meltzoff, & Decety 2006;
Preston et al., 2007; Schulte-Rüther et al., 2007). Although
no brain region was observed as being active in a majority
of these studies, some regions were more involved in selfor other-focused attention. Specifically, the dorsal ACC,
anterior insula, and posterior ventrolateral PFC were more
active only during self-focused or personal experience
conditions. In contrast, the ventromedial PFC, precuneus,
posterior STS, TPJ, IPL, and amygdala tended to be more
active during other-focused or target observation conditions. In a connectivity analysis, Zaki, Ochsner, Hanelin,
Wager, and Mackey (2007) observed stronger connectivity
between the dorsal ACC and medial PFC, posterior STS,
precuneus, and IPL during the observation, relative to the
experience, of pain. This suggests a role for the mentalizing network in empathy.
Thus, the tentative conclusion that may be drawn at this
point is that the dorsal ACC and anterior insula are activated both when a person is observing and experiencing
painful stimulation, potentially supporting an internal mirroring of another ’s affective response. In contrast, self and
social cognition regions, including the medial PFC, dorsomedial PFC, ventromedial PFC, and precuneus, may support processes supporting focusing on and making sense of
another ’s experience as it would feel for them.
Attributions of Morality and Trustworthiness
Although most neuroimaging studies examining the processes whereby the psychological states and traits of others
are inferred have focused on this process generically, there
has been some work focusing on domain-specific attributions. Two commonly studied domain-specific attributions
are for morality and trustworthiness.
The most significant finding in the domain of morality
judgments is that personal, relative to impersonal, moral
decisions recruit more regions associated with mentalizing and self-referential processing, including the medial
PFC, precuneus, and TPJ (Greene, Sommerville, Nystrom,
Darley, & Cohen, 2001). Moral reasoning in general also
invokes elements of the mentalizing and self-reference
network, including the medial PFC, ventromedial PFC, TPJ,
and posterior STS (Moll, de Oliveira-Souza, Bramati, &
Grafman, 2002; Moll, de Oliveira-Souza, Eslinger et al.,
2002). Several studies have now shown modulation of these
regions as factors related to moral attributions are manipulated, including the actor ’s intentions (Berthoz, Armony,
Blair, & Dolan, 2002; Borg, Hynes, Horn, Grafton, & SinnottArmstrong, 2006) and beliefs about the consequences of
the action (Young, Cushman, Hauser, & Saxe, 2007), actual
12/22/09 5:33:40 PM
Functional Neuroanatomy
consequences (Borg et al., 2006; Young et al., 2007), and
whether an audience is present to the actions (Finger,
Marsh, Kamel, Mitchell, & Blair, 2006). Additionally,
damage to the ventromedial and medial PFCs has been
associated with impaired ability to make personal, but
not impersonal, moral judgments (Ciaramelli, Muccioli,
Ladavas, & di Pellegrino, 2007; Mendez, Anderson, &
Shapira, 2005).
In contrast, judgments of trustworthiness have been
almost exclusively linked to amygdala activity across studies.
Adolphs, Tranel, and Damasio (1998) observed that
patients with bilateral amygdala damage, relative to controls, were heavily biased to rate faces as more trustworthy.
Similarly, an early fMRI study found that the amygdala
was more active when the subject was presented with
untrustworthy faces than with trustworthy faces (Winston,
Strange, O’Doherty, & Dolan, 2002). Interestingly, Engell,
Haxby, and Todorov (2007) observed that amygdala
responses more closely tracked consensus judgments of
trustworthiness for different faces than the subjects’ own
ratings for those faces.
Self-Processes
The self has been a central topic within social psychology for
decades, because many theories regarding the development,
maintenance, and regulation of the self suggest that these
are profoundly social processes and because self-processes
continuously influence our social cognition and behavior. It
is little surprise then that the self has been one of the most
actively researched topics within social cognitive neuroscience. In the following sections, I discuss in turn the functional
neuroanatomy of agency, self-recognition, self-reflection and
self-knowledge, and self-control (see Figure 5.4).
161
Agency
Agency refers to the sense that one was causally responsible for
a particular behavior and forms one of the phenomenological
cores of selfhood. The neural correlates of agency have been
examined in two different ways. First, a number of studies
have compared the neural bases of freely chosen self-initiated actions to externally triggered actions. Although these
studies do not assess the experience of agency, they operate
under the assumption that freely chosen actions are associated with a sense of agency and free will. Perhaps the
most famous of these studies is Libet’s, which has been
referred to time and again to argue that free will is an
illusion (Libet, Wright, & Gleason, 1982).
In Libet’s study, subjects freely chose when to make a
response and were asked to watch a clock and remember
the precise time when they formed the intention to respond.
Intriguingly, Libet observed a neural response, the readiness
potential thought to emanate from the supplementary motor
area, a few hundred milliseconds prior to when subjects
claimed to have formed an intention. Libet argued that the
neural responses that would ultimately trigger a behavior
were causing an intention to be formed rather than an intention setting the motor response in motion (see also Fried
et al., 1991). The neuroimaging studies that have followed
have commonly observed supplementary motor area activity, along with the dorsal ACC, lateral PFC, medial PFC,
and precuneus (Babiloni et al., 2008; Brass, Derrfuss, &
von Cramon, 2005; Brass, Zysset, & von Cramon, 2001;
C. Frith, Friston, Liddle, & Frackowiak, 1991; Hunter
et al., 2003; Lau, Rogers, Haggard, & Passingham, 2004;
Lau, Rogers, Ramnani, & Passingham, 2004). One study
observed that supplementary motor area activity that occurs
just prior to intention formation predicts the timing of selfreported intention formation, whereas activity in the medial
Self-Processes
1 medial PFC
2 dorsomedial PFC
3 precuneus/posterior cingulate
4 rostral ACC
CH05.indd 161
5 supplementry motor area
6 ventrolateral PFC
7 inferior parietal lobule
Figure 5.4 The brain regions involved in
self-processes (agency processing [1, 3, 5, 7],
self-recognition [6, 7], self-reflection [1–3],
and self-control [4, 5, 6]). Numbers in brackets
correspond to the regions in the figure reliably
associated with a particular self process.
12/22/09 5:33:41 PM
162
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
PFC and precuneus up to 10 seconds prior to intention
formation predicts the timing of self-reported intention formation (Soon, Brass, Heinze, & Haynes, 2008).
These studies are not without limitations. Waiting for an
extended time until one has an intention to press a button
is an artificial task that may well involve processes distinct from those involved in intention–action connections
in more naturalistic settings. These studies do not merely
examine intention formation but rather intention formation
while in the mind-set of reflecting on and detecting one’s
own intention formation. This would seem to have all of
the usual issues with introspection (Nisbett & Wilson,
1977). One can imagine monitoring one’s own intention
formation, particularly in such an artificial task, to be more
of a signal detection task than a direct read-off of one’s
own psychological states. When attending to one’s own
thoughts, a variety of fleeting thoughts are likely to occur,
and the individual must decide which rise to the level of
full-blown intentions and which do not. In the study by
Soon et al. (2008), it is not hard to imagine that a partially
formed and vaguely conscious intention to press a button
occurs at one point but does not meet one’s threshold for
declaring that an intention has occurred. Nevertheless,
this subthreshold intention may set in motion a series of
psychological events that trigger the full-blown intention
several seconds later. If the subthreshold intention is not
reported, its neural correlates would appear to predict the
subsequent above-threshold intention, thus subverting the
apparent order of events. Consequently, it is unclear at this
point whether neural events causally precede all intentions or just those intentions that we reflectively recognize
as intentions.
The second approach to the study of agency involves creating discrepancies between one’s behavior and the visual
presentations of one’s behavior. Typically, these studies
manipulate visual feedback such that one’s arm movements
appear to move in a different trajectory than intended, or a
delay is used such that one’s hand movements are seen a few
hundred milliseconds after they are produced. Across these
studies, the most common finding is that the IPL, in the area
of the TPJ, increases in activity as the mismatch between
produced and observed behavior increases (Blakemore,
Oakley, & Frith, 2003; Farrer, et al., 2003; Farrer et al., 2008;
Leube et al., 2003; Shimada, Hiraki, & Oda, 2005). Studies
have used TMS applied to this area to disrupt agency judgments (Preston & Newport, 2008; Tsakiris, Costantini, &
Haggard, 2008). Bilateral activity in this region has also
been observed when hearing delayed playback of one’s own
voice (Hashimoto & Sakai, 2003).
Similar to these findings, schizophrenic patients and those
with related experiences of external control of one’s actions
tend to produce greater right IPL activity during normal
CH05.indd 162
behavior than do control subjects, with the effect increasing
with symptom strength (Franck, O’Leary, Flaum, Hichwa, &
Andreasen, 2002; Ganesan, Hunter, & Spence, 2005;
Spence, Brooks, Hirsch, Liddle, & Grasby, 1997). Lastly,
lesion-induced out-of-body experiences have been localized
to the IPL–TPJ region (Blanke, Landis, Spinelli, & Seeck,
2004), with intracranial stimulation and TMS to this region
producing out-of-body–like experiences (Blanke, Ortigue,
Landis, & Seeck, 2002; Blanke et al., 2005). Together, these
results suggest that this region may code for the mismatch
between intention and action, with quiescence in this region
resulting during normal personal agency.
Overall, these two experimental approaches suggest
that forming an intention to act and assigning agency to
an observed behavior may depend on different neural systems. Forming an intention appears to rely largely on structures on the medial walls of the cortex, whereas evaluating
whether the behavior that results is one’s own involves a
lateral region of parietal cortex.
Self-Recognition
The canonical test for whether an animal or human baby has
self-awareness is the mirror self-recognition test (Gallup,
1970). In this test, colored ink or powder is applied to the
subject’s forehead while the subject is asleep. Once awake,
the subject is placed in front of a mirror. If upon noticing the colored patch in the mirror, the subject proceeds
to touch its own forehead where the color is, the subject is
then said to have passed the mirror self-recognition test.
A number of neuroimaging studies have now established
the network of brain regions involved in recognizing oneself from pictures. Nine of ten neuroimaging studies using
“pictures of the self” observed increased right ventrolateral
PFC activity (Devue et al., 2007; Hodzic, Muckli, Singer, &
Stirn, 2009; Kaplan, Aziz-Zadeh, Uddin, & Iacoboni, 2008;
Morita et al., 2008; Platek et al., 2004, 2006; Suguira
et al., 2000, 2005, 2008). About half of these also reported
increased right IPL activity (cf. Morita et al., 2008). One
of these studies (Kaplan et al., 2008) found that identifying the self from pictures or voice recordings activated the
same region of right ventrolateral PFC. Additionally, TMS
applied to right IPL was found to reduce subjects’ sensitivity to self–other distinctions (Uddin, Molnar-Szakacs,
Zaidel, & Iacoboni, 2006). One study of note (Suguira
et al., 2000) compared active and passive responses to
self-images. In the conjunction of these two tasks, right
IPL activity was observed, whereas right ventrolateral
PFC activity was observed only in the comparison of the
two tasks such that it was more active when subjects were
explicitly identifying their own faces. Thus, right IPL activity may be involved in lower-level visual processing of the
self, whereas right ventrolateral PFC activity may be more
12/22/09 5:33:42 PM
Functional Neuroanatomy
involved in intentional self-recognition. Interestingly, in East
Asian subjects, right ventrolateral PFC activity is more
active for one’s own face relative to a coworker ’s face, if
they are primed with an independent self-construal (Sui &
Han, 2007), whereas an interdependent self-construal
produces similarly strong activations for both faces in this
region.
Self-Reflection and Self-Knowledge
The ability to reflect on one’s current and past experiences,
preferences, traits, and abilities is one of the signature
achievements of the human brain. Although some other species have shown evidence of rudimentary self-awareness,
perhaps as evidenced by the mirror self-recognition test, no
other species has such an overdeveloped self-awareness as to
need aisle after aisle of self-help books. A few dozen neuroimaging and lesion studies have now examined the processes
by which we focus our attention internally on ourselves.
Free-form reflection on the self has been found to
produce activity in the medial PFC and the contiguous
regions of the precuneus and posterior cingulate cortex
(jointly referred to in this section as precuneusPCC) relative to control tasks; in addition, there is more activity in
the medial PFC (BA 10) relative to free-form reflection
on another individual (D’Argembeau et al., 2005; Farb
et al., 2007; Johnson et al., 2006; Kjaer, Nowak, & Lou,
2002). The involvement of the medial PFC is of particular
interest given that this is the only region of the prefrontal
cortex known definitively to be disproportionately larger
in humans than in other primate species (Semendeferi,
Schleicher, Zilles, Armstrong, & Van Hoesen, 2001). Trait
self-consciousness has also been specifically associated
with medial PFC activity (Eisenberger, Lieberman, &
Satpute, 2005). Similarly, explicitly attending to one’s preferences, relative to a non–self-reflective control task, has
reliably been associated with medial PFC and dorsomedial
PFC activity (Goldberg, Harel, & Malach, 2006; Gusnard,
Akbudak, Shulman, & Raichle, 2001; Johnson et al., 2005;
Lane, Fink, Chau, & Dolan, 1997; Ochsner, Knierim,
et al., 2004). Interestingly, mindfulness meditation training that attempts to shift self-processing from linguistic
self-evaluation to a more experiential basic awareness has
been shown to diminish this medial PFC activity (Farb
et al., 2007). Another study (Johnson et al., 2006) found
that reflecting on the self with a promotion or prevention
focus (Higgins, 1998) was associated with either increased
medial PFC or precuneusPCC activity, respectively.
Additionally, the medial and ventromedial PFCs have
both been associated with self-insight processes. For
instance, patients with damage to these regions were less
aware of whether their behavior constituted social transgressions compared with patients with damage to the
CH05.indd 163
163
lateral PFC (Beer, John, & Knight, 2006; see also Beer,
Heerey, Keltner, Scabini, & Knight, 2003). Similarly,
activity in the medial and ventromedial PFCs was greater
when subjects successfully predicted whether they would
be able to retrieve particular words from memory (Schnyer,
Nicholls, & Verfaellie, 2005). Although there have been
only a few neuroscience investigations of self-insight,
these studies are particularly important because they link
neural processes to adaptive outcomes of self-reflection.
It is one thing to identify the medial PFC’s involvement
when people try to reflect on themselves, but it is quite
another to determine that activating the medial PFC during these attempts is associated with something useful and
accurate about oneself.
The great majority of self-reflection studies have
focused on trait self-knowledge. In these studies, subjects
are typically asked to indicate whether trait words or
phrases are descriptive of themselves, are descriptive
of another person, or have some textual or semantic feature
(Craik et al., 1999; D’Argembeau, Xue, Lu, Van der Linden, &
Bechara, 2008; Fossati et al., 2003, 2004; Gutchess,
Kensinger, & Schacter, 2007; Heatherton et al., 2006;
Johnson et al., 2002; Kelley et al., 2002; Kircher et al., 2002;
Lou et al., 2004; Macrae, Moran, Heatherton, Banfield, &
Kelley, 2004; Moran, Macrae, Heatherton, Wyland, & Kelley,
2006; Pfeifer et al., 2007; Saxe, Moran, et al., 2006;
Schmitz & Johnson, 2006; Schmitz, Kawahara-Baccus, &
Johnson, 2004; Seger, Stone, & Keenan, 2004; Turner,
Simons, Gilbert, Frith, & Burgess, 2008; Vanderwal, Hunyadi,
Grupe, Connors, & Schultz, 2008; Zhang et al., 2006; Zhu,
Zhang, Fan, & Han, 2007). All but one of these studies
has shown increased medial PFC activity during selfjudgments relative to either other-judgments or control judgments, with precuneusPCC and dorsomedial PFC
activations also present in several studies. Two studies have
found that the medial PFC is more active while judging
positive self-traits than negative self-traits (Fossati et al.,
2003, 2004), and a third found that the medial PFC was
not sensitive to this distinction and that the subgenual
ACC was activated by positive self-traits relative to negative self-traits (Moran et al., 2006). Multiple studies have
also linked the medial PFC to subsequent memory for selfrelevant traits (Fossati et al., 2004; Macrae et al., 2004),
which is consistent with the association of the medial PFC
with autobiographical memory relative to episodic memory more generally (Gilboa, 2004).
Given that self-knowledge and self-concepts change
over time, it is important to determine the neural processes
involved in the developmental and experience-driven
changes in these processes. One developmental fMRI
study (Pfeifer et al., 2007) found that the medial PFC was
significantly more active in 9-year-old children than in
12/22/09 5:33:42 PM
164
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
adults when making trait self-judgments. In contrast, the
levels of medial PFC activity in young adults and older
adults were similar when making trait self-judgments
(Gutchess et al., 2007). A study on self-schemas compared
trait self-judgments in domains for which subjects were or
were not self-schematic (i.e., had substantial experience)
(Lieberman, Jarcho, & Satpute, 2004). Judgments made in
the self-schematic domain produced greater activity in the
ventromedial PFC, ventral striatum, amygdala, lateral temporal cortex, and precuneusPCC than judgments from the
nonschematic domain. In contrast, nonschematic judgments produced greater activity in the dorsomedial PFC
and medial temporal lobe. These results suggest that schematics may recruit more automatic affective processes than
nonschematics in making these judgments.
Change over time has also been examined by asking
subjects to take different temporal perspectives on the self.
Studies comparing the present perspective of the self to
future (Ersner-Hershfield, Wimmer, & Knutson, in press)
and past (D’Argembeau et al., 2008) perspectives of the self
have both observed greater medial PFC activity when individuals focus on the self as it is currently constituted rather
than on the self at other time points. These data are consistent with the notions that there is a greater identification
with the current self and that future and past selves may be
treated in some ways as if they are altogether different individuals from oneself (Libby, Eibach, & Gilovich, 2005).
Other open questions include whether the medial PFC is
similarly active for self- and other-judgments and whether
the medial and dorsomedial PFCs are each involved in
both self and social cognition. As to the first question,
some studies have reported greater medial PFC activity for
self-judgments relative to other-judgments (Kelley et al.,
2002; Lou et al., 2004), although some have not (Schmitz
et al., 2004; Seger et al., 2004). One criticism of those that
have shown a difference is that in these studies the self is a
far better known target than nonself targets (e.g., the queen
of Denmark or the president of the United States). One
study (Heatherton et al., 2006) specifically compared selfjudgments to judgments of a close friend and still found
significantly greater medial PFC activity for self-versus
other-judgments; however, others have found similar
medial PFC activity for self-judgments and judgments of a
significant other or mother (Ochsner et al., 2005; Schmitz
et al., 2004; Vanderwal et al., 2008).
With respect to the relative involvement of the medial
PFC, dorsomedial PFC, and precuneusPCC, across all of the
self-reflection and self-knowledge studies, medial PFC
activations were present in 94% of the studies, whereas
dorsomedial PFC and precuneusPCC activations were present in 53% and 63% of studies, respectively. Thus, activations of the dorsomedial PFC and precuneusPCC are
CH05.indd 164
common; however, these activations are not as reliably
invoked by self-reflection processes as is medial PFC
activity. This is almost the mirror image of the pattern from
mentalizing studies in which dorsomedial PFC activations
were present in 91% of studies and medial PFC and precuneusPCC activations were present in 33% and 39% of studies, respectively.
Finally, classic theories of self-knowledge have proposed that self-concepts develop when individuals take
the perspective of others on themselves (Cooley, 1902;
Mead, 1934). Reflected appraisals constitute one person’s
assessment of what another person thinks of him or her.
Three studies of adults have now examined the neural correlates of reflected appraisals of the self (“what I think you
think of me”) compared with direct appraisals of the self
(“what I think of me”), and each have found similar
levels of medial PFC and dorsomedial PFC activity in
the two forms of appraisals (D’Argembeau et al., 2007;
Ochsner et al., 2005; Pfeifer et al., 2009). One of these
studies (Pfeifer et al., 2009) focused primarily on adolescents, because this is a critical period of self-concept
development. The TPJ, a region that commonly appears
in mentalizing tasks, was strongly activated during
reflected appraisals in adolescents and adults. Given
that reflected appraisals involve mentalizing about the
belief another person holds toward oneself, this is not a
surprising result. Perhaps more surprising was the strong
activation of the TPJ during direct appraisals in adolescents, but not in adults. This suggests the possibility that
adolescents, but not adults, are spontaneously drawing
upon social sources of information when asked to generate direct appraisals. Consistent with this notion, a number of regions involved in mentalizing about others were
more active during direct appraisals in adolescents than
in adults, including the dorsomedial PFC, posterior STS,
and precuneusPCC.
Self-Control
Self-control, or the ability to regulate, manipulate, or control one’s prepotent thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, has
been extensively examined using various tools of neuroscience. Explicit attempts at self-control across various
domains commonly recruit a network of brain regions,
including the lateral PFC and the contiguous regions of
the dorsal ACC, presupplementary motor area (BA 6),
and posterior dorsomedial PFC (BA 8). It should be noted
that the dorsal ACC is typically thought to serve a conflict detection function indicating the need for self-control,
whereas the lateral PFC is thought to be more involved in
implementing control or inhibiting prepotent responses
(MacDonald, Cohen, Stenger, & Carter, 2000). Lesion data
support the latter claim regarding the lateral PFC (Aron,
12/22/09 5:33:43 PM
Functional Neuroanatomy
Robbins, & Poldrack, 2004), but they are less supportive
of the former claim regarding the dorsal ACC (Fellows &
Farah, 2005).
Most relevant to social psychology are the more than
30 neuroimaging studies of affect and emotion regulation (Ochsner & Gross, 2005; for relevant cognitive
studies, see Goel & Dolan, 2003; Mitchell et al., 2007).
These studies can be divided according to whether emotion regulation is the explicit goal of the task or whether
emotion regulation occurs incidentally as a consequence
of another process not intended to produce emotion regulation effects. Explicit emotion regulation tasks include
reappraisal (Banks, Eddy, Angstadt, Nathan, & Luan Phan,
2007; Beauregard, Levesque, & Bourgouin, 2001; Eippert
et al., 2007; Goldin, McRae, Ramel, & Gross, 2007;
Harenski & Hamann, 2006; Herwig et al., 2007; Kim &
Hamann, 2007; Luan Phan et al., 2005; McRae, Ochsner,
Mauss, Gabrieli, & Gross, 2008; Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, &
Gabrieli, 2002; Ochsner, Ray, et al., 2004; Schaefer
et al., 2003; Urry et al., 2006; Wager, Davidson, Hughes,
Lindquist, & Ochsner, 2008), suppression (Goldin et al.,
2007; Lee, Dolan, & Critchley, 2008; Ohira et al., 2006),
detachment (Kalisch et al., 2005; Levesque et al., 2003),
and self-distraction (Kalisch, Wiech, Herrmann, &
Dolan, 2006).
Across 19 neuroimaging studies, task conditions that
invoked explicit emotion regulation efforts were commonly associated with activations in right ventrolateral
PFC (63% of studies), left ventrolateral PFC (63% of studies), the contiguous regions of the presupplementary motor
area and posterior dorsomedial PFC (47%), and left dorsolateral PFC (32% of studies). Approximately half of these
studies also reported on frontal regions whose activity was
associated with regulatory success either in terms of selfreported affect or limbic activity. Although there is not an
entirely consistent pattern among these analyses, right and
left ventrolateral PFCs do appear more often than other
regions.
Most of these studies have examined the regulation
of negative affect. Although a few studies have looked at
regulation during the presentation of positively valenced
images (Kim & Hamann, 2007; Ohira et al., 2006), it is
unclear whether such images produce a similarly intense
emotional response to the negative images typically
used. A study by Delgado, Gillis, and Phelps (2008) examined reappraisal in the context of financial reward and
observed increased left ventrolateral and left dorsolateral
PFC activity along with diminished ventral striatum activity during reappraisal.
More than a dozen studies have examined incidental
emotion regulation using affect-based conflict resolution,
placebo, and affect labeling paradigms. In placebo studies,
CH05.indd 165
165
subjects are led to believe that their pain or anxiety will be
alleviated by a pill or cream that is in fact pharmacologically inert. Although there is no instruction to intentionally regulate one’s pain or anxiety, subjects often report
less distress in placebo conditions. In the five neuroimaging studies (Kong et al., 2006; Lieberman, Jarcho, Berman
et al., 2004; Petrovic et al., 2005; Wager et al., 2004, studies
1 and 2) that have related neural responses to placeborelated distress reductions, four have reported right ventrolateral PFC activity and two have reported activity in
left ventrolateral PFC, right dorsolateral PFC, and rostral
ACC. Five studies employed conflict resolution tasks in
which emotional cues must be ignored to successfully perform the task (Enger, Etkins, Gale, & Hirsch, 2008; Etkin,
Enger, Peraza, Kandel, & Hirsch, 2006; Felmingham
et al., 2007; Most, Chun, Johnson, & Kiehl, 2006; Ochsner,
Hughes, Robertson, Cooper, & Gabrieli, in press). Here, the
regulation of emotional responses is secondary to the main
task of making a fast categorical judgment about another
stimulus; thus, regulation is secondary to the main task.
In all five of these studies, the rostral ACC was associated with successful regulation of the emotional distracter.
Lastly, four fMRI studies (Altshuler et al., 2005; Hariri,
Bookheimer, & Mazziotta, 2000; Lieberman, Hariri, Jarcho,
Eisenberger, & Bookheimer, 2005; Lieberman et al., 2007)
have examined the neural basis of why putting feelings into
words can dampen emotional responses (Pennebaker &
Beall, 1986). In these studies, subjects chose affective
labels to characterize the negative emotional images. In
each of these studies, right ventrolateral PFC was the primary brain region active during “affect labeling,” relative
to control conditions. In addition, in each of these studies,
right ventrolateral PFC activity was associated with diminished amygdala responses to the negative stimuli. During
these studies, emotion regulation was incidental; subjects
were not trying to regulate their emotional responses.
Across all of the incidental emotion regulation studies, right
ventrolateral PFC and rostral ACC activations were present in 57% and 50% of these studies, respectively. Across
both intentional and incidental emotion regulation studies,
right ventrolateral PFC activity was reported most often
(59%), followed by left ventrolateral PFC activity (41%).
Although right ventrolateral PFC activity was equally
likely to be present in intentional and incidental emotion
regulation studies (63% vs. 57%), left ventrolateral PFC
activity was far more likely to be present in intentional than
in incidental regulation studies (63% vs. 14%), as was the
case for the contiguous regions of the presupplementary
motor area and posterior dorsomedial PFC (47% vs. 0%).
In contrast, the rostral ACC was much more likely to
be invoked during incidental regulation studies (50%) than
in intentional regulation studies (5%).
12/22/09 5:33:44 PM
166
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
Social Interaction
Trust, Cooperation, and Fairness
Building relationships of any kind and effectively working with others depends on mutual trust, a willingness to
cooperate, and a sense that rewards and responsibilities
are being distributed fairly. Using paradigms created by
behavioral economists, social cognitive neuroscientists
and neuroeconomists have been examining these different
social adhesives (see Figure 5.5).
Several fMRI studies have used variants of the “trust
game” (Berg, Dickhaut, & McCabe, 1995) to examine the
neural processes invoked when deciding whether to trust a
stranger. In the trust game, there are two players: decision
maker 1 (DM1) and decision maker 2 (DM2). DM1, also
called the investor, is given a sum of money (e.g., $10). This
money can be kept or invested. If invested, the money is
moved to DM2, also called the trustee. Any money received
by the trustee is increased by a known and predetermined
factor (e.g., multiplied by 4). DM2 then decides how much
money to transfer back to DM1. In the case of mutual trust
and repeated games with the same individual, it would be in
both players’ interest for DM1 to invest the entire sum and
for DM2 to return half of the proceeds. However, if DM1
does not trust DM2 to return a fair share, DM1 is less likely
to invest as much of the initial endowment. Additionally, in
a one-shot game where each player will make only a single
Fairness, Trust, & Helping
decision with the other player, it is considered irrational for
DM2 to return any money to DM1.
To examine the neural correlates of trusting another
person in a one-shot trust game (McCabe, Houser, Ryan,
Smith, & Trouard, 2001), in contrast to mere investing
phenomena, subjects played some rounds with a human
DM2 and some with a computer DM2. The researchers
observed that the medial PFC was more active for DM1
when DM1 decided to transfer the funds over to DM2. It is
possible that the medial PFC represents the DM1’s feeling
of similarity to DM2 (Mitchell, Macrae, & Banaji, 2006)
and thus DM1’s willingness to cooperate. In another type
of cooperative game, Decety and colleagues also found the
medial and ventromedial PFCs to be more active when a person was being cooperative (Decety, Jackson, Sommerville,
Chaminade, & Meltzoff, 2004). In their trust game study,
Delgado, Frank, and Phelps (2005) observed greater ventral striatum and left TPJ activity in DM1 when that person
chose to trust. King-Casas and colleagues (2005) examined
multiple games played between the same DM1 and DM2 and
found that when a DM1 responded to DM2’s untrustworthy
behavior by investing even more on the next round of the
game, rather than less, activity in the caudate in the dorsal
striatum of DM1 increased. Finally, Krueger and colleagues
(2007) observed greater dorsomedial PFC, ventral striatum,
and septal activity in DM1 when that person chose to trust.
Thus, although there is substantial variability across studies,
Unfairness & Social Rejection
Unfairness & Social Rejection
1 medial PFC
2 ventromedial PFC
3 ventral striatum
4 dorsal ACC
5 anterior insula
6 ventrolateral PFC
Figure 5.5 The brain regions involved in
social interactions. The top left image displays brain regions activated in studies of
fairness, trust, and helping. The top right
and bottom right images display brain
regions activated in studies of unfairness
and social rejection.
Note: Anterior insula is displayed on the lateral
wall for presentation purposes, but is actually
between the medial and lateral walls of the
cortex.
CH05.indd 166
12/22/09 5:33:44 PM
Functional Neuroanatomy
these findings do suggest that different regions on the
medial prefrontal wall (the dorsomedial PFC, medial PFC,
and ventromedial PFC) and in the striatum (dorsal and ventral) are more active during the decision to trust.
Brain-based oxytocin levels are also associated with
DM1’s trust behavior in the trust game. In the first study to
examine this, a DM1 receiving an intranasal dose of oxytocin transferred more money to DM2 than those who had
received a placebo (Kosfeld, Heinrichs, Zak, Fischbacher, &
Fehr, 2005). In another study (Baumgartner, Heinrichs,
Vonlanthen, Fischbacher, & Fehr, 2008), a dozen oneshot trust games were played after an oxytocin or placebo
induction; however, subjects received feedback about game
dynamics after the first six games had been played. At this
point, subjects who were in the role of DM1 were informed
that in 50% of the prior games, DM2 had not transferred
money back to them. Knowing that future betrayals were
likely, placebo DM1 subjects reduced their later transfers
to DM2. In contrast, DM1’s who had received oxytocin
actually increased their transfers to DM2 after receiving
the feedback. These oxytocin findings make sense in light
of the known role of oxytocin in social attachment and pair
bonding in animals (Insel & Shapiro, 1992).
Two studies have examined the neural correlates associated with finding out that another person has failed
to reciprocate one’s own trusting behavior. One study
(Rilling, Dagenais, Goldsmith, Glenn, & Pagnoni, 2008)
used the “prisoner ’s dilemma” game in which DM1’s and
DM2’s financial outcomes are each dependent on both
their own and the other player ’s decision. If both DM1 and
DM2 choose to cooperate, they receive equitable outcomes
that maximize their joint reward total. However, for each
decision maker, given a particular decision by the other
player, defecting will produce a greater personal reward
than cooperating. Rilling found that if DM1’s cooperation
was unreciprocated by DM2, DM1 produced greater insula
and reduced ventral striatum activity. Similarly, in a trust
game, Delgado and colleagues (2005) found that DM2’s
choice not to transfer funds back to DM1 led to reduced
ventral striatum activity in DM1. This might have been
due to the diminished financial reward associated with this
outcome; however, Delgado also showed that this effect
was absent when DM2 was believed by DM1 to be of high
moral character. This suggests that the diminished ventral
striatum activity was at least in part due to social factors.
Finally, multiple studies have examined the decision
to punish those who exhibit unfair behavior. A trust game
study using PET (de Quervain et al., 2004) found that if
DM1 was given the opportunity to punish DM2 when DM2
did not transfer money back, DM1 showed increased activity in the dorsal striatum, and the magnitude of this activity
was correlated with the size of the punishment delivered.
CH05.indd 167
167
Other studies have used the “ultimatum game” (Fehr &
Schmidt, 1999) to examine punishment for unfair treatment.
In this game, DM1 is given an endowment (e.g., $10) and
makes a proposal for how DM1 and DM2 should split the
endowment (e.g., DM1 will keep $7 and DM2 will receive $3).
If DM2 accepts the proposal, both players receive what DM1
has proposed. If DM2 rejects the proposal, both players get
nothing. At one time, economists supposedly argued that
DM2 should accept any nonzero offer, being better than zero,
and thus DM1 should always offer one penny and DM2 should
accept. In actual play, DM1 usually offers 30% to 50% and
DM2 will reject many of the offers lower than 30%. Sanfey
and colleagues published the first neuroimaging study of
the ultimatum game and found that subjects in the DM2
role showed greater anterior insula activity to unfair offers
($1 or $2 out of $10) than to fair offers, but only if DM1 was a
person, not a computer (Sanfey, Rilling, Aronson, Nystrom, &
Cohen, 2003). Additionally, the magnitude of anterior insula
activity was associated with the tendency to reject the offer.
Given that anterior insula activity has been associated with
feelings of disgust, Sanfey suggested that this activity may
represent the sense of insult or injustice associated with an
unfair offer. A second fMRI study of the ultimatum game
(Tabibnia, Satpute, & Lieberman, 2008) equated the material payoff of fair and unfair offers, comparing, for instance,
a fair offer of $5 out of $10 to an unfair offer of $5 out
of $23. As in the study by Sanfey and colleagues, anterior
insula activity was associated with the tendency to reject
unfair offers.
The study by Tabibnia et al. (2008) also examined the
psychological struggle that can occur when an offer is
simultaneously unfair and financially desirable (e.g., $5
out of $23). Subjects who more frequently accepted these
unfair but desirable offers showed increased activity in
right ventrolateral PFC, a region that’s been associated
with emotion regulation and self-control more generally,
and also showed a correlated decrease in anterior insula
activity. In contrast, two studies (Knoch, Pascual-Leone,
Meyer, Treyer, & Fehr, 2006; van’t Wout, Khan, Sanfey, &
Aleman, 2005) observed less frequent rejection of unfair
offers when TMS was applied to right dorsolateral PFC,
presumably reducing the contribution of this region to
decision processes during this task.
Two other studies using the ultimatum game have
identified causal neural mechanisms contributing to an
enhanced tendency to reject unfair offers. In one of these
studies (Koenigs & Tranel, 2007), patients with damage to
the ventromedial PFC and right ventrolateral PFC were
more likely to reject unfair offers. In the second study
(Crockett, Clark, Tabibnia, Lieberman, & Robbins, 2008),
pharmacological reduction of serotonin levels also led to
more frequent rejection of unfair offers. Reduced serotonin
12/22/09 5:33:45 PM
168
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
levels have been shown to diminish ventrolateral PFC
activity during a motor inhibition task (Evers et al., 2005),
and thus it is plausible that regulation of one’s sense of
insult is less effective due to serotonergic depletion effects
on the ventrolateral PFC.
Social Rewards and Helping
An interesting finding that has emerged from fMRI studies
of two-person economic games described in the previous
section is that people show evidence of reward activation
when they participate in good interactions involving trusting and fair behavior, even when this treatment confers
no additional financial benefit to them or even leads to a
loss. In a prisoner ’s dilemma study (Rilling et al., 2002),
subjects showed greater ventral striatum activity during mutual cooperation than during any other combination of responses. This is striking in light of the fact that
mutual cooperation is not the most financially rewarding
outcome possible. This suggests that against their own
financial interest, there is a hedonic benefit to participating in a reciprocated trusting behavior. Similarly, Tabibnia
et al. (2008) observed that fair offers produced greater
activity in the ventral striatum and ventromedial PFC than
unfair offers that would yield the same material benefit.
A number of behavioral studies have yielded results consistent with those of the fMRI studies, suggesting that being
treated fairly is rewarding above and beyond the material
benefits that fair treatment often brings (De Cremer &
Alberts, 2004; Tyler, 1991). It has been suggested that this
makes sense evolutionarily because fair treatment can be
considered a proxy for whether one is valued by others
in a group. From an evolutionary perspective, continued
inclusion in social groups has been critical to receiving a
share of needed resources and even to survival; thus, any
cue that one has met this inclusion criterion is likely to be
rewarding.
Indeed, simple signs of social acceptance have been
associated with ventral striatum activity in a number of
recent studies. Izuma, Saito, and Sadato (2008) found
that a person’s ventral striatum was similarly activated
by financial rewards and by being informed that others
view that person in a positive light. In a developmental
social neuroscience study (Scott, Dapretto, Ghahremani,
Poldrack, & Bookheimer, under review), children’s good
performance on each trial of a task was rewarded by either
financial reward or a smiling female face with the words
“that’s correct” next to it. Similar increases in ventral striatum activity were observed whether the reward was financial or social.
Another set of studies has shown that helping behavior in the form of charitable giving also generates reward
activity. Moll and colleagues (2006) asked people to accept
CH05.indd 168
or reject each of a series of propositions that would yield
positive, neutral, or negative financial outcomes for oneself and/or for different charities (money really went to
these charities in this study). Trials in which subjects could
gain money for themselves with no negative consequence
for the charity unsurprisingly led to increased ventral striatum activity. What was surprising is that trials in which
the charity would gain while the subject would lose money
(i.e., a donation) led to a higher level of ventral striatum
activity than receiving money oneself. Additionally, the
magnitude of ventral striatum activity during donation
decisions was associated with the tendency to accept donation propositions during the task. All of these studies taken
together suggest that enacting or being the recipient of
prosocial behavior activates the ventral striatum, a region
that has been commonly associated with reward responses
to primary reinforcers and to nonsocial secondary reinforcers such as money, drug cues for addicts, and erotic
images (Lieberman & Eisenberber, 2009).
Social Rejection
The study of social rejection and ostracism has been a
major area of social psychological research in the past
decade (Williams, 2007). Being excluded or rejected represents some of the most distressing experiences that people
have, and fear of rejection is a powerful motivator that may
help explain a wide array of classic findings of conformity
and obedience to authority (Williams, Bernieri, Faulkner,
Grahe, & Gada-Jain, 2000). Based initially on animal studies (Panksepp, Herman, Conner, Bishop, & Scott, 1978), it
has been suggested that there may be an overlap in the way
that the brain represents experiences of physical pain and
social pain (i.e., the pain of social rejection, exclusion, or
isolation) (Eisenberger & Lieberman, 2004; MacDonald &
Leary, 2005).
In humans, the neural components of the physical “pain
matrix” are fairly well understood, including the dorsal
ACC, anterior insula, somatosensory cortex, and periaqueductal gray (Price, 2000). Of these regions, the dorsal ACC
has been most reliably associated with the distress of physical pain (Rainville, Duncan, Price, Carrier, & Bushnell,
1998), in contrast to the somatosensory cortex, which
has been primarily associated with the sensory aspects of
physical pain (e.g., identifying where on the body the pain
is felt). For instance, after surgical lesioning of the dorsal
ACC for chronic pain, patients typically report that they
can identify the location of a painful stimulus on their body
and how intense the stimulus is, but they also report that
the pain no longer bothers them (Foltz & White, 1968).
Finally, as described earlier, right ventrolateral PFC and
rostral ACC have both been associated with the regulation
of physical pain distress.
12/22/09 5:33:46 PM
Functional Neuroanatomy
Eisenberger and colleagues have conducted a series of
neuroimaging studies that suggest that social pain processes
largely rely on this same physical pain network (Eisenberger,
Gable, & Lieberman, 2007; Eisenberger, Lieberman, &
Williams, 2003; Masten, Telzer, & Eisenberger, under
review; Way, Taylor, & Eisenberger, 2009). In these studies,
subjects believe they are playing a simulated ball-tossing
game on the Internet while they and two other subjects are
all in MRI scanners. Once in the scanner, the subjects actually play against computer players programmed to include
the subject for a certain amount of time and then stop throwing the ball to the subject for the remainder of the scan.
Self-reported social distress during this exclusion episode is
associated with greater dorsal ACC activity, whereas lower
distress reports are associated with increased right ventrolateral PFC activity (Eisenberger et al., 2003; Eisenberger,
Way, Taylor, Welch, & Lieberman, 2007). Other studies have
also observed increased dorsal ACC activity in response to
rejection-themed images (Kross, Egner, Ochsner, Hirsch, &
Downey, 2007) and video clips of disapproving facial
movements (Burklund, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2007).
Additionally, dorsal ACC activity during exclusion in the
scanner correlates with daily experiences of social disconnection outside the scanner (Eisenberger, Gable et al.,
2007). Thus, the distress of social pain in the dorsal ACC
and the regulation of social pain in right ventrolateral PFC
closely parallel the findings from the physical pain literature. The animal literature supports these findings as well,
having shown that electrical stimulation of the dorsal ACC
increases and surgical lesions of ACC decrease distress
vocalizations associated with social isolation in nonhuman
mammals (MacLean & Newman, 1988; Smith, 1945).
One criticism of these findings (Sommerville,
Heatherton, & Kelley, 2006) focuses on the common view
that the dorsal ACC is responsible for cognitive processes,
whereas the rostral ACC is responsible for corresponding
affective processes. This viewpoint suggests that the social
rejection findings may reflect a violation of cognitive
expectations of inclusion and that the dorsal ACC is therefore activated because of cognitive conflict monitoring.
However, this perspective does not account for the activity
correlating with the self-reported distress of the experience
(Eisenberger et al., 2003). Furthermore, it does not account
for the increased dorsal ACC activity in rejection-sensitive
individuals to cues of rejection (Burklund et al., 2007),
because these individuals expect rejection more and yet
show more dorsal ACC activity in response to it. Finally,
the strong linkage between an opioid polymorphism and the
dorsal ACC response to rejection is hard to square with a
purely cognitive account (Way et al., 2009).
It is worth considering where this critique comes from
historically. The belief that the dorsal and rostral ACCs are
CH05.indd 169
169
involved in cognitive and affective processes, respectively, is
largely a consequence of an influential review paper (Bush,
Luu, & Posner, 2000). In this study the researchers reviewed
dozens of cognitive conflict studies and found that these
tended to activate the dorsal ACC, whereas a study of emotional conflict detection in an emotional Stroop paradigm and
other clinical symptom provocation studies produced rostral
ACC activity. First, it is important to note that this literature
review included no studies of physical pain. Even though the
dorsal ACC has been repeatedly associated with the emotional distress of physical pain, this finding was not accounted
for in their analysis. Second, subsequent emotional conflict
monitoring has found activity in the dorsal ACC (Davis
et al., 2005; Ochsner, Hughes, Robertson, Cooper, & Gabrieli,
2009). Third, numerous neuroimaging studies have shown
dorsal ACC activity associated with anxiety and other affective
processes (Ehrsson, Weich, Weiskopf, Dolan, & Passingham,
2007; McRae, Reiman, Fort, Chen, & Lane, 2008; Simmons
et al., 2008; Straube, Mentzel, & Miltner, 2007). Fourth, neuropsychological lesion data are more supportive of the dorsal
ACC’s role in pain distress than cognitive conflict monitoring, because dorsal ACC lesions are commonly found to
diminish pain distress (Foltz & White, 1968), whereas cognitive conflict monitoring is often spared (Baird et al., 2006;
Fellows & Farah, 2005; Stuss, Floden, Alexander, Levine, &
Katz, 2001).
One way to reconcile these notions of dorsal ACC function is to think of it functioning like an alarm (Eisenberger &
Lieberman, 2004). Consider the typical smoke alarm. To
work successfully, it must fuse two functions together. On
one hand, it must have a mechanism capable of detecting when a critical threshold for smoke particles has been
met—a mechanism conceptually analogous to cognitive
conflict monitoring. On the other hand, in order to notify
people that there’s a fire, it must have a mechanism that
can sound an audible alarm after the first mechanism has
detected the smoke. This latter process resembles the function that pain distress plays in our lives, experientially
notifying us that some harm may come to us. From this
perspective, determining the function of the dorsal ACC
may not be an either/or decision. Rather conflict monitoring and pain distress may reflect coordinated cognitive and
experiential components of a single alarm mechanism.
Attachment and Close Relationships
A number of imaging studies have begun to examine how
the brain responds to the people we love (spouse, partner,
child, parent). Across these studies, most have observed
limbic activations (e.g., amygdala, striatum, dorsal ACC,
insula), although some report widespread activity in the
mentalizing network (Leibenluft, Gobbini, Harrison, &
Haxby, 2004; Seifritz et al., 2003). Hearing a child crying
12/22/09 5:33:46 PM
170
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
has been associated with dorsal ACC activity (Lorberbaum
et al., 2002; Seifritz et al., 2003), whereas seeing pictures
of one’s own child or infant tends to activate the amygdala,
dorsal ACC, anterior insula, and bilateral lateral PFCs
(Bartels & Zeki, 2004; Leibenluft et al., 2004; MinagawaKawai et al., 2008; Ranote et al., 2004). One study of
mothers viewing pictures of their infant has shown ventral striatum activity (Strathearn, Li, Fonagy, & Montague,
2008), but this result has not yet been replicated. Only
one study has examined the interaction of viewing one’s
own infant or another ’s, either in distress or not; this study
revealed strong dorsal ACC and dorsomedial PFC activity when mothers viewed their own infant in distress relative to the other conditions (Noriuchi, Kikuchi, & Senoo,
2008). A recent study using near-infrared spectroscopy
(Minagawa-Kawai et al., 2008) has examined infants’ neural responses to their mother ’s face and observed greater
medial PFC activity in response to their mother smiling
(relative to the mother not smiling and a stranger smiling or
not smiling).
Viewing pictures of one’s romantic attachments has
typically produced dorsal striatum activity (Aron et al.,
2005; Bartels & Zeki, 2000); however, one study that subliminally primed the name of one’s loved one has reported
increased ventral striatum activity (Ortigue, BianchiDemicheli, Hamilton, & Grafton, 2007). Similar to the
network associated with seeing cues associated with one’s
own child, adult attachment studies have observed relationships between anxious attachment style and activity in
the amygdala and dorsal ACC during relationship distress
or hostile feedback paradigms (Gillath, Bunge, Shaver,
Wendelken, & Mikulincer, 2005; Lemche et al., 2006). In
addition, another study reported that avoidant attachment
was associated with diminished ventral striatum feedback during supportive feedback from a stranger (Vrticka,
Andersson, Grandjean, Sander, & Vuilleumier, 2008).
Finally, a few studies have examined grief responses by
prompting individuals to think about the recent loss of a
significant other (e.g., mother recently dying of cancer or
a romantic relationship that recently ended). These studies
have typically observed greater activity in the dorsal ACC
and anterior insula, consistent with a social pain account
of grief, and in the posterior cingulate (Gundel, O’Connor,
Littrell, Fort, Lane, 2003; O’Connor et al. 2008). One
study (O’Connor et al., 2008) examined the neural differences among individuals who were showing a normal
level of recovery from grief compared with those with
complicated grief, which refers to a persistent grief that is
not following the normal recovery pattern. Complicated
grief was associated with increased ventral striatum activity, relative to noncomplicated grief, when responding
to cues related to the deceased. This activity was also
CH05.indd 170
associated with self-reported yearning for the deceased,
suggesting that ventral striatum activity may reflect current desires for connection with the deceased that typically abate over the course of several months of normal,
noncomplicated grief.
Attitudes and Attitude Change
Attitudes are one of social psychology’s oldest constructs
(Thurstone, 1928). People’s attitudes are of great interest
because they are believed to predict an individual’s behavior in a variety of attitude-relevant situations. In contrast to
our intuitions, self-reported attitudes are often poor indicators of subsequent behaviors. This has led researchers to
examine the existence and predictive efficacy of implicit
attitudes (Fazio & Williams, 1986), to assess attitudes in
the aggregate (Ajzen, 2001), and to identify the critical
role of behavioral intentions linking attitudes to behaviors
(Gollwitzer, 1999). To date, the neuroscience of attitudes
has largely focused on the neural correlates of attitudinal
evaluation and the neural correlates of attitude change.
Attitudinal Evaluation
Several studies have examined which brain regions are
more active when expressing attitudinal evaluations (e.g.,
how good is it?) compared with when control judgments
are made (e.g., how symmetrical is it?). There is substantial variability in the activations reported across studies of
attitudinal evaluation. This may be a result of the diversity
of attitude objects examined in different studies. The
objects examined include geometric shapes (Jacobsen,
Slotkin, Westerveld, Mencl, & Pugh, 2006), paintings
(Kawabata & Zeki, 2004), music (Brattico, Tervaniemi, &
Picton, 2003), social concepts (Cunningham et al., 2004),
unfamiliar faces (O’Doherty et al., 2003), famous names
(Cunningham, Johnson, Gatenby, Gore, & Banaji, 2003;
Zysset, Huber, Ferstl, & von Cramon, 2002), and current political candidates (Kaplan, Freedman, & Iacoboni,
2007). The most frequently observed activations in these
studies occur in the bilateral ventrolateral PFC, along with
a host of mentalizing and self-referential brain regions,
including the medial PFC, dorsomedial PFC, posterior
cingulate, TPJ, and temporal pole.
When subjects report their evaluations, it is difficult to
know what psychological processes are occurring to generate this evaluation. For instance, evaluations are sometimes constructed in the moment, and other times they are
retrieved from memory. Sometimes people feel comfortable expressing their attitudes, and other times they engage
in effortful mental processes to shape the expression of an
attitude for public consumption. Evaluations also vary in
valence and arousal, and therefore task materials that vary
12/22/09 5:33:47 PM
Functional Neuroanatomy
on these dimensions across studies could produce different
results as well. Some of these elements have been examined. With respect to valence, positive and negative attitudes have been associated with left and right lateral PFCs,
respectively (Cunningham, Espinet, DeYoung, & Zelazo,
2005). In contrast, attitudinal intensity or arousal has
been associated with the amygdala and ventromedial PFC
(Cunningham et al., 2004). Self-reported efforts to control
one’s evaluation have been associated with activity in the
ventrolateral PFC, dorsolateral PFC, dorsal ACC, medial
PFC, and precuneus. In contrast, being exposed to liked or
disliked attitude objects without expressing an evaluation
has been associated with activity in the ventral striatum
(Aharon et al., 2001) and amygdala (Cunningham et al.,
2003), respectively, suggesting that these regions may play
a role in implicit attitudes.
More recently, an area of research referred to as neuromarketing has begun examining branding effects that
bear a close relationship to attitude processes. The most
significant of these studies recreated the Pepsi challenge
inside the scanner (McClure et al., 2004). In the classic
advertising campaign from the 1970s, it was found that
despite overwhelming self-reported preference for Coke,
when each drink was tasted without labels, Pepsi was
more often preferred. The implication is that Coke is preferred because of the brand association rather than its taste.
In this study, subjects tasted Coke and Pepsi on a series
of trials, but could see the brand labels on only some of
the trials. They observed that in the absence of labels,
ventromedial PFC activity was associated with drink preference, consistent with this region’s common association
with hedonic experience (Trepel, Fox, & Poldrack, 2005).
In contrast, when the brand labels were available, preferences were associated with dorsolateral PFC and hippocampal activity, suggesting a role for higher cognitive and
memory processes.
Attitude Change
The first neuroscience investigation of attitude change
explored cognitive dissonance processes in patients with
anterograde amnesia (Lieberman, Ochsner, Gilbert, &
Schacter, 2001). Cognitive dissonance reduction usually
refers to the change in attitudes or beliefs that occur when
one has freely chosen to engage in a behavior that conflicts
with a previously held attitude or belief. For instance, in the
free choice paradigm, an individual ranks his or her preferences for several items in a category (e.g., kitchen appliances; Brehm, 1956) and then chooses which of two closely
ranked items he or she would like to own; the subject then
finally re-ranks all of the items. The classic finding is that
the selected item goes up in the re-rankings, whereas the
unselected item goes down in the re-rankings. The dissonance
CH05.indd 171
171
account suggests that choosing between evenly liked items
is at odds with previously ranking them as similar and that
by “spreading the alternatives” in one’s updated rankings,
the selected items comes to look as though it was an obvious choice all along. Of course, to outsiders, this looks like
post hoc rationalization.
Several early accounts of cognitive dissonance processes suggested that dissonance reduction processes were
relatively explicit and slow, occurring over a long period
of time after the conflictual behavior occurred (Festinger,
1964; Hovland & Rosenberg, 1960; Steele, Spencer, &
Lynch, 1993). According to this model, an individual must
be consciously aware that he or she has engaged in counterattitudinal behavior, attribute the resulting dissonance feelings to this specific conflict, and then engage in effortful
processing to change this attitude over time. Lieberman et al.
(2001) compared attitude changes in amnesics and healthy
controls because it is unlikely that amnesics would recognize that they have engaged in a behavior that conflicts with
a previously expressed attitude. Despite this impairment,
amnesics showed as much attitude change as control subjects, suggesting that the conventional account of cognitive
dissonance effects relies too heavily on controlled processing mechanisms being deployed slowly over time. Multiple
electroencephalograph (EEG) studies also suggest that dissonance effects may occur more quickly than previously
assumed (Harmon-Jones, Gerdjikov, & Harmon-Jones,
2006; Harmon-Jones, Harmon-Jones, Fearn, Sigelman, &
Johnson, 2008).
Stereotyping and Intergroup Processes
Perceiving Race
Neuroscience research on stereotyping and related intergroup processes represents a microcosm of the larger
social cognitive neuroscience landscape, including studies
of social perception, implicit attitudes, self-like processing of others, and self-control. This is also one of the areas
of social cognitive neuroscience where ERP studies vastly
outnumber fMRI studies (for review, see Amodio, 2008;
Bartholow & Dickter, 2007; Kubota & Ito, 2009).
A number of fMRI studies have examined the perception of Black and White faces. Across these studies, inverse
affective and perceptual effects have emerged. On one
hand, greater amygdala activity in response to Black versus White faces (Lieberman et al., 2005; Ronquillo et al.,
2007) suggests a possible negative evaluative response
to or greater emotional evocativeness of Black faces. In
contrast, greater activity in the FFA to ingroup versus outgroup faces (Golby, Gabrieli, Chiao, & Eberhardt, 2001;
Lieberman et al., 2005) has been interpreted as reflecting greater perceptual expertise with ingroup faces.
12/22/09 5:33:47 PM
172
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
These results are paralleled by ERP studies (Ito & Urland,
2003), which reveal some early components that are more
responsive to outgroup faces (N100, P200) and another
early component that is more responsive to ingroup faces
(N200), which has been linked to FFA activity (Allison
et al., 1994).
Although these early ERP components are not modulated by race-related encoding goals (Ito & Urland, 2005),
both race-based categorization and individuation goals
have been associated with diminished amygdala responses
to Black faces in fMRI studies. Wheeler and Fiske (2005)
observed diminished amygdala activity when subjects
judged a target’s food preference. In contrast, Lieberman
et al. (2005) observed diminished amygdala activity during
the labeling of a target’s race, similar to the effects of affect
labeling. Along the same lines, a study of stigma (Krendl,
Macrae, Kelley, Fugelsang, & Heatherton, 2006) reported
less amygdala activity when subjects’ judgments were
explicitly focused on the stigma, compared with when they
were not. In addition, amygdala responses to race have
been modulated by skin darkness (Ronquillo et al., 2007)
and the direction of a target’s eye gaze (Richeson, Todd, &
Trawalter, 2008).
Implicit Attitudes
Other neuroimaging studies have examined the relationship between attitudes and amygdala responses to Black
faces, relative to White faces. Most notably, an early fMRI
study (Phelps et al., 2000) observed that amygdala activity
to Black faces was correlated with the strength of negative
implicit attitudes toward Blacks but was not correlated
with an explicit measure of racism. Similarly, another study
(Cunningham, Johnson et al., 2004) reported greater amygdala activity to Black faces versus White faces only when
the faces were presented subliminally, suggesting potential
self-regulation under supraliminal conditions. In this study,
implicit attitudes were associated with amygdala activity
during subliminal presentations but not during supraliminal presentations. Somewhat surprisingly, a patient with
amygdala damage showed normal implicit racial attitudes
(Phelps, Cannistraci, & Cunningham, 2003), although the
lesion was acquired in adulthood and other social processes
have been spared for amygdala lesions acquired in adulthood (Shaw et al., 2004). In contrast, patients with ventromedial and medial PFC damage do not produce implicit
attitude effects (Milne & Grafman, 2001).
Controlling Bias
Given that stereotype-based expectations can lead to systematically biased behavior (Payne, 2001) and given that
most individuals are motivated to be or appear nonbiased,
self-regulation processes are often brought online in order to
CH05.indd 172
guard against having biased thoughts, feelings, or behaviors
toward outgroup members. Multiple fMRI studies have
observed a network almost identical to those seen in other
forms of self-control (ventrolateral PFC, dorsolateral PFC,
dorsal ACC, supplementary motor area) more active in conditions in which subjects are exposed to Black faces under
conditions where bias could be revealed (Cunningham,
Johnson et al., 2004; Richeson et al., 2003). In addition,
work with ERPs (Amodio et al., 2004; cf. Bartholow
et al., 2005) has shown evidence of a fast response in the
dorsal ACC, called the error-related negativity response,
during the Weapons Identification Task (Payne, 2001) on
trials that reveal bias. Critically, the dorsal ACC response
on a particular trial predicted greater controlled processing
during the subsequent trial. This suggests that this activation is an internal indicator of potential bias and the need to
be more careful on ensuing trials.
Naturally, there are situations in which individuals do
not mind acting on the basis of ingroup favoritism. People
want members of their ingroups to succeed and obtain their
fair share of resources, at a minimum. One study observed
neural responses associated with this ingroup bias in the
absence of pressure to be unbiased (Rilling, Dagenais,
et al., 2008). Subjects were separated into groups using a
minimal group paradigm manipulation; they then played
prisoner dilemma games with ingroup and outgroup members. Approximately one third of the subjects reported
feeling differently when playing against an ingroup member than an outgroup member. This subsample, but not
the sample as a whole, produced greater activity in the
dorsomedial PFC and right TPJ, both regions in the mentalizing network, when playing with an ingroup rather than
with an outgroup member. In other words, playing with an
ingroup player may have produced more mentalizing about
the perspective of the other player.
Being the Target of Prejudice
Although the vast majority of intergroup studies, both
behavioral and neuroimaging, have examined the perceiver ’s side of bias, a handful have examined the reactions
of the targets of prejudice. In the behavioral literature, stereotype threat (Steele & Aronson, 1995) is the most widely
used paradigm for examining the effect of stereotypes on
the target of those stereotypes. In these studies, subjects for
whom a stereotype exists (e.g., females are bad at math)
perform a stereotype-relevant task (e.g., a math test) that
either is characterized as measuring their ability or is characterized in nonability terms (e.g., it is a game). The standard finding is that stereotype targets perform worse on
these tasks when they believe the task is diagnostic of their
ability, and these results are explained in terms of anxiety
over confirming the stereotype. In other words, if a female
12/22/09 5:33:48 PM
How Social Cognitive Neuroscience Contributes to Social Psychology
is anxious or distracted, thinking that poor performance on
a math test will confirm negative math stereotypes about
women, this may limit the woman’s ability to focus on
task, thus creating a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Two fMRI studies have examined the neural correlates of
stereotype threat (Krendl, Richeson, Kelley, & Heatherton,
2008; Wraga, Helt, Jacobs, & Sullivan, 2006; see also
Masten et al., under review). In both studies, increased stereotype threat was associated with increased rostral ACC
activity. Given that this region has been associated both
with emotional experience and with the regulation of emotions, it is difficult to interpret the significance of this common activation from just these two studies. In one of the
studies (Wraga et al., 2006), increased activity in the rostral
ACC was marginally associated with poorer task performance. However, this could be explained either as distress
interfering with task performance or as attention to regulating one’s distress interfering with task performance. An
ERP study (Forbes, Schmader, & Allen, 2008) found that
those in a stereotype threat condition who responded to the
task by devaluing its significance produced smaller errorrelated negativity responses to their own errors, suggesting
less self-monitoring as a consequence of devaluing.
IV. HOW SOCIAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE
CONTRIBUTES TO SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
Now that we have reviewed where dozens of social psychological processes occur in the brain, anyone would be
forgiven for believing that social cognitive neuroscience is
little more than phrenology. Knowing that social processes
can be localized within the brain is not all that interesting.
What is the alternative hypothesis? That they will be localized in your elbow? A cognitive neuroscientist who has
taken a shine to the social side of things might respond that
brain mapping is essential to understanding what different brain regions do. How can we really understand what
a brain region does if it is examined using only abstract
decontextualized stimuli that cognitive psychologists typically use? A complete understanding of the brain will be
constituted only if the brain is studied while situated in all
its social psychological glory.
A social psychologist would likely respond that it is all
fine and well that neuroscientists want to probe their favorite brain regions using social psychological paradigms to
figure out what those regions do. But what does that do for
social psychology? Is our social psychology improved at
all by looking at the brain? Are there social psychological
theories that should be updated in light of social cognitive
neuroscience data? Is there conventional wisdom in our
field that needs to be reconsidered or looked at in a fresh
CH05.indd 173
173
light because of brain data? Do neuroscience methods
allow us to ask social psychological questions that have
gone unanswered for years? If the answer to any of these
questions is yes, then social psychology needs the tools of
neuroscience just as surely as it needed the tools of cognitive psychology a few decades ago.
Can social cognitive neuroscience answer all of social
psychology’s questions? Of course not. No method can.
Neuroimaging is no more a panacea than reaction time measures or introspective self-reports. Indeed, for most of the
interesting findings from the history of social psychology,
neuroimaging would have been a far worse tool than those
already used by social psychology. During a typical fMRI
session, a person lays prone in the scanner wearing goggles
that allow the subject to see a video feed; the subject responds
during tasks almost exclusively with button boxes limited
to a few buttons, and there are constant loud noises during
scanning. Finally, experimental trials from each condition of
interest often must be repeated dozens of times, meaning that
any task for which trial repetition will necessarily contaminate the psychological phenomenon is off limits. Despite
these limitations, there are specific ways in which neuroscience can contribute to our social psychological enterprise
that should matter even to social psychologists uninterested
in the brain. The remainder of this section discusses some
of the ways that social cognitive neuroscience can and has
contributed to the mission of social psychology.
Brain Mapping
Knowing where social psychological processes occur
in the brain does matter for at least a few reasons. First,
animal research and cognitive neuroscience have made
significant progress in figuring out the computations performed by particular brain regions. This knowledge can
be drawn on to generate preliminary inferences about the
kinds of subprocesses subserving macrolevel social processes. That is, social processes usually encompass multiple
component processes simultaneously or in rapid sequence,
and identifying the involvement of brain regions with wellcharacterized functions can help us identify which corresponding psychological processes may contribute to the
total mental act. For instance, imagine that when individuals watch one person greeting another person, a region of
the lateral temporal cortex known to be primarily involved
with semantic processing (Noppeney & Price, 2004) was
activated, compared with some control task. One might
infer that watching this social episode is comprehensible to
us because we retrieve social scripts from semantic memory.
Alternatively, imagine that watching this greeting activates
the mirror system. This finding might suggest that people
understand social episodes through simulation rather than
12/22/09 5:33:48 PM
174
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
semantic coding. Incidentally, it could be the case that both
the mirror system and semantic processes are activated
when observing the greeting. One of the advantages of neuroimaging over standard behavioral testing is that multiple
systems can be interrogated simultaneously and often without eliciting a behavioral response from subjects that would
require particular instructions and an attentional set that
might contaminate the natural attitude of the subject.
Brain mapping discoveries are the beginning, not the
end, of the process for social cognitive neuroscience. Once
the regions involved in a social process are identified,
one can then more carefully interrogate those regions in
future studies that focus on hypothesis testing. As social
psychologists, we are used to our everyday experiences
serving as the anecdotal database from which we design
studies. Brain mapping studies are the way that social cognitive neuroscientists create an anecdotal database.
In many cases, it might be argued that brain mapping is
telling us something we already know from other existing
behavioral research, and that is a fair criticism. However,
we should ask ourselves what the value of a neuroimaging
study would have been, had it come first. Would it have
updated our social psychological theories just as the behavioral research did? If so, it indicates that neuroimaging data
can constrain our theories (Kihlstrom, 2006); it is just a historical accident that the behavioral study came first. Surely
in the future there will be times when the neuroimaging
study will come first and make significant contributions.
Convergences
Although social cognitive neuroscience is still a young discipline, one of its most exciting contributions is a series of
findings in which two experiences that seem quite different
from each other phenomenologically, or were thought to be
only metaphorically related, actually rely on overlapping
neural processes. The assumption is that if two processes
rely on common brain regions, then they rely on common
computational processes as well. It is exceedingly difficult
to demonstrate that two psychological events that feel different from each other share a great deal at the computational level (Kosslyn, 1999). Yet such demonstrations are
a critical component to advancing social psychological
theory. We group psychological phenomena into domains
of study based on whether phenomena feel similar or meet
some set of logical criteria; however, additional progress
would be made if psychological phenomena were grouped
based on their deep structure.
Social Metaphors Are Not So Metaphorical
A number of social psychological phenomena have now
been linked to nonsocial phenomena in ways that raise the
CH05.indd 174
possibility that descriptions of social experience may be far
less metaphorical than once thought. Social rewards such
as positive social feedback or being treated with respect
(Izuma et al., 2008; Tabibnia et al., 2008) activate the ventral striatum in much the same way that winning money
or eating chocolate does. The experience and regulation of
social pain are associated with brain regions involved in the
experience and regulation of physical pain (Eisenberger &
Lieberman, 2004). The sense of insult in response to
unfair treatment and the experience of disgust in response
to sensory stimuli are both associated with activity in the
anterior insula (Borg, Lieberman, & Kiehl, 2008; Calder
et al., 2000; Hsu, Anen, & Quartz, 2008; Sanfey, Rilling,
Aronson, Nystrom, & Cohen, 2003; Wicker, Keysers,
et al., 2003). In each of these cases, the social phenomenon seems less abstract and more embodied in light of
these linkages. In addition, these unexpected convergences
have led to behavioral studies that would not have been done
otherwise. For instance, behavioral studies have examined
the relationship between social and physical pain sensitivity (DeWall & Baumeister, 2006; Eisenberger, Jarcho,
Lieberman, & Naliboff, 2006), with one recent experiment
finding that taking Tylenol reduced self-reported feelings
of social rejection (DeWall et al., in press).
The linkage of social to physical pain changes our conceptual understanding of social rejection and the need for
social connection. Maslow’s (1943) hierarchy of needs
orders our needs (from most basic to least basic) as biological, safety, belonging, esteem, and self-actualization.
In other words, biological and safety needs are critical to
survival, and the rest are more or less gravy. However,
deficits in social connection cause a form of pain just
as deficits in other survival needs cause a form of pain (e.g.,
hunger, thirst, cold). It seems that evolution has a special
painful place for deficits in basic survival needs, and social
connection has made the cut. It has been speculated that
because mammalian young are born relatively helpless,
incapable of securing their own food, water, and shelter,
continued social connection with their caregiver(s) is their
primary means of survival. Knowing that social rejection
activates the same pain processes as other survival need
deficits allows us to think differently about social connection’s place in our hierarchy of needs (Baumeister & Leary,
1995; Lieberman & Eisenberger, 2009).
Using the Self to Understand Similar Others
Other work has shown convergences within social cognition that have been hypothesized but never clearly demonstrated. For instance, although it is not surprising that
people would use their knowledge of themselves to make
sense of others, until recently there had been no hard evidence one way or the other. Studies by Mitchell, Macrae,
12/22/09 5:33:49 PM
How Social Cognitive Neuroscience Contributes to Social Psychology
and Banaji (2006) provide compelling evidence that we
do use ourselves to make sense of at least some people.
Specifically, they showed that the same region of the medial
PFC is active when making self-referential judgments
and judgments about a similar other but that this region
is not active when making judgments about a dissimilar
other. Such findings open up a variety of opportunities to
hypothesize about how targets will be differentially understood and treated based on the relative contributions of the
medial PFC or dorsomedial PFC (Harris et al., 2005).
Empathy
Knowing that experiencing physical pain and seeing others in physical pain recruit the same neural systems makes
an important contribution to empathy research (Singer
et al., 2004). When someone says, “I feel your pain,” we
can certainly quibble about whose pain they are feeling, but
for the first time there is evidence they are really feeling
someone’s pain rather than merely entertaining an abstract
idea. This often-replicated overlap also provides an experimental paradigm for testing various important aspects of
empathy theories in the future because the modulation
of this overlap by situational and personality factors can
be easily assessed (Singer et al., 2006).
Direct and Reflected Self-appraisals
Social psychologists and sociologists have long hypothesized about the role that others’ evaluations of us have
on our own self-views (Cooley, 1902; Mead, 1934). As
compelling and influential as this symbolic interactionist
account has been over the years, there has been surprisingly little empirical evidence to support it. Behavioral
research has focused on the overlap in the content of direct
and reflected self-appraisals. Neuroimaging, however,
allows us to examine the overlap in the structures supporting different kinds of appraisals. It might be expected that
asking a 12-year-old boy what his best friend thinks of
him would recruit brain regions known to be involved in
self-referential processing and also brain regions known to
be involved in mentalizing. Here, the adolescent is being
asked to reflect on the mental state of another person and
to derive a self-evaluation from this. The fact that adolescents recruit both of these systems when asked to make
a direct appraisal of themselves (e.g., what do you think
of yourself?) is more surprising. This finding constitutes
preliminary evidence of reflected appraisals being spontaneously generated even when they have not been asked
for (Pfeifer et al., 2009). Adults do not show broad activation of the social cognition network when making selfreferential judgments. Note that if asked to make a direct
self-appraisal, neither adolescent nor adult is likely to spontaneously use reflected appraisal language in their replies,
CH05.indd 175
175
but these neuroimaging data suggest that adolescents are
doing something social when making direct self-appraisals.
What this something is requires further investigations.
To be sure, these convergences are open to multiple interpretations. They are new findings that need further interrogation. However, each suggests new conceptual understandings
of social phenomena and may inspire a variety of behavioral
and neuroscience studies to follow up on these leads. New
findings are rarely ends in themselves. However, each of
these findings is part of a social psychological conversation,
and suggests that neuroscience can indeed have a seat at the
table and even have something worth saying to social psychologists now and then. It is also worth noting that in each
of the preceding examples, knowing which brain regions are
involved is relatively superfluous to the relevance of the findings for social psychology. One need not have an interest in
neuroanatomy to find an overlap in how the brain processes
social and nonsocial rewards quite compelling. One need
never know that the ventral striatum is the point of convergence for this to be relevant. The anatomy can be left to the
anatomists, but the investigation of such overlaps provides a
method for conceptual advances within social psychology.
Dissociations
A basic tenet of all psychological research is that if two processes or performances can be dissociated on some dependent measure such as reaction time, then the processes are
distinct from one another. Neuroscience research is no different. When lesion studies observe that damage to region
A produces deficits in task X but not in task Y, compared
with damage to region B, which produces deficits in task
Y but spared performance in task X, this is taken as strong
evidence that task X and Y rely on different psychological
processes. Similarly, when an fMRI study reveals that
different brain structures tend to be active during tasks
X and Y, this too suggests different psychological processes may be at work. In some cases, these differences
are quite relevant to social psychological theories.
Social Cognition Is Special
Perhaps the single best example of a neuroimaging study
challenging the traditional understanding of a social psychological finding comes from Mitchell et al. (2004). In a classic behavioral study, subjects read passages with the goal of
either memorizing the material for later testing or forming
a social impression of the target in the passage (Hamilton,
Katz, & Leirer, 1980). The surprising finding was that the
impression formation goal led to better performance on a
subsequent memory test, even though those with an impression formation goal did not know the test was coming and
those in the memorization condition did. The generally
12/22/09 5:33:49 PM
176
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
accepted explanation of these results was depth of processing (Craik & Tulving, 1975), such that social encoding was
believed to be a deeper, more elaborative form of encoding
than encoding with a memorization goal.
Mitchell and colleagues (2004) replicated this paradigm
in the scanner and discovered what those earlier studies could
not. Social and nonsocial encoding do not just differ quantitatively on a depth of processing dimension. Rather, they
rely on qualitatively dissociable processes. Countless studies have shown that successful memorization (i.e., encoding that leads to later retrieval success) is associated with
activity in left ventrolateral PFC and the medial temporal
lobes (Wagner et al., 1998). Mitchell found that activity in
these regions did predict retrieval success in the memorization condition but did not predict retrieval success in the
social encoding condition. Instead, retrieval success in the
social encoding condition was associated with activity in
the dorsomedial PFC. This finding strongly calls into question the depth of processing account and instead suggests that
there is something qualitatively different about social encoding. Regardless of how one evaluates the significance of this
problem, it is a clear case in which the inference from the
behavioral data was wrong and the neuroimaging evidence
provided a clear and compelling case for distinct processes
operating in social and nonsocial encoding. Upon learning
the results of this study, one must update one’s understanding of this phenomenon based on these neuroimaging data.
Social by Default
One of the most significant discoveries in the past decade
of cognitive neuroscience research is the default network.
These regions are highly activated when a subject is at rest
(i.e., when not being given any experimental task to perform)
(Raichle et al., 2001) and show highly coordinated activity with each other at rest (Fox et al., 2005). They become
less active when cognitive tasks are performed (Greicius,
Krasnow, Reiss, & Menon, 2003; Shulman et al., 1997),
to the extent that the cognitive tasks are more demanding (McKiernan, Kaufman, Kucera-Thompson, & Binder,
2003), but when active during cognitive tasks, they tend to be
associated with producing errors (Boly et al., 2007; Li, Yan,
Bergquist, & Sinha, 2007; Weissman, Roberts, Visscher, &
Woldorff, 2006). At rest, these regions produce activity that
is inversely correlated with activity in brain regions supporting common cognitive tasks (Fox et al., 2005).
What is striking is that this default network could easily
be mistaken for a self and social cognition network. All of
the regions that are highly active at rest (dorsomedial PFC,
medial PFC, ventromedial PFC, precuneus, TPJ in almost
all studies, with fusiform gyrus and temporal poles also
appearing with some frequency) are among the regions that
figure most prominently in this review of social cognitive
CH05.indd 176
neuroscience. The implication is obvious. When left to
their own devices, people think about themselves and their
social lives (D’Argembeau et al., 2005; Gusnard et al.,
2001; Iacoboni et al., 2004; Mason et al., 2007; Wicker,
Ruby, Royet, & Fonlupt, 2003).
Put a different way, the brain’s default focus is social.
Only when something nonsocial, like a working memory
task, requires it to direct its resources elsewhere does it
momentarily stop focusing on the social. Social psychologists might find this to be obvious, but to funding agencies, the media, and your grandparents this kind of finding
really helps to firm up the significance of what we study
(the fact that the size of the prefrontal cortex across species
correlates the typical group size in each species is a good
one to throw out there too; Dunbar, 1998).
It should be noted that it was recently reported that anesthetized unconscious monkeys still had increased activity in
the default regions (Vincent et al., 2007; see also Fransson
et al., 2007). This raises a fascinating issue, one that should
be relevant to social psychologists (and not just impress
their grandparents). Does the brain show these social cognition activations at rest because this is what we choose to
think about in our spare time? Or is it the case that we tend
to focus on social and self-related thinking in our spare time
because high baseline activity in these regions biases us, in
a sense priming us, to think about these things? Has evolution progressed in such a way that it has proved adaptive to
have our spare thought biased toward processing and reprocessing information about ourselves and the social world?
Automaticity and Control
At the end of the 1990s, great attention was being
devoted to the Implicit Association Test (Greenwald,
McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) as a method for assessing
implicit attitudes. On one hand, large numbers of social
psychologists were conducting Implicit Association Test
studies because among implicit measures it was straightforward to use and produced strong experimental effects
with relatively modest sample sizes. On the other hand,
there was a great deal of controversy over what the Implicit
Association Test measured and whether what it measured could legitimately be called implicit. At one point,
so the story goes, the Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology had a moratorium on publishing any additional Implicit Association Test papers until it was clear
that it really assessed implicit attitudes. When Phelps and
colleagues (2000) reported that the strength of amygdala
responses to images of Black faces was strongly associated
with Implicit Association Test scores but not with explicit
attitude scores, this was generally received as significantly
strengthening the case that the Implicit Association Test
truly measured implicit attitudes. The amygdala has long
12/22/09 5:33:50 PM
How Social Cognitive Neuroscience Contributes to Social Psychology
been thought to primarily engage in automatic processes,
given its phylogenetic history, its early position in the
visual processing stream, its role in fear conditioning in
rodents, and the fact that subliminal presentations of fear
expressions activate this region. If the Implicit Association
Test scores, but not explicit attitudes, are associated with
amygdala responses, then there is a good chance the
Implicit Association Test is measuring something implicit.
Thus, neuroimaging findings help distinguish implicit from
explicit attitudes and clarify the interpretation of one of the
most commonly used social psychological instruments.
As with implicit and explicit attitudes, several dualprocess models within social psychology (Chaiken & Trope,
1999) posit some combination of automatic and controlled
processes believed to share the work in various domains
(e.g., persuasion, attribution, self-knowledge, empathy).
Automatic processes are fast, resistant to interruption,
independent of conscious intention, or outside of awareness, whereas controlled processes are slow, interruptible,
intention-driven, and accessible to awareness (Wegner &
Bargh, 1998). There are several remaining important questions about dual-process models. For instance, are automaticity and control two ends of a spectrum in which the
same processes and representations are employed but with
differing levels of efficiency? Or are there distinct automatic and controlled processes that differ qualitatively
and may be sensitive to different types of inputs, store information differently, and respond differently as a function
of context? If there are separate processes, how many sets of
dual-processes exist? One scientist (Kruglanski et al.,
2003) hyperbolically suggested that there might be 30 sets
of dual-processes based on the fact that a contemporary
volume on dual-process models (Chaiken & Trope, 1999)
had 30 chapters, each putting forth a dual-process model
with only minimal connections made between the different
models. Because experiences in different domains of social
psychology feel so different from one another and have
such different outcomes, it is hard to assess whether dualprocess models in these domains (e.g., persuasion and stereotyping) rely on common processes. Similarly, because
different underlying processing architectures can produce
the same behaviors, it can be difficult to identify which processing architectures are really at work (Gilbert, 1999).
Neuroimaging has been quite informative in general
in helping to tease apart processes that are implicit, automatic, nonconscious, or reflexive from those that are
explicit, controlled, conscious, or reflective (Lieberman,
2009a; Satpute & Lieberman, 2006). For instance, explicit
learning is impaired in anterograde amnesiacs but not in
patients with Parkinson’s disease, whereas implicit learning
is impaired in patients with Parkinson’s disease but not in
anterograde amnesiacs (Knowlton, Mangels, & Squire, 1996).
CH05.indd 177
177
Based on the neural deficits associated with each neuropsychological impairment, neuroimaging studies have shown
that implicit learning is associated with basal ganglia activations, whereas explicit learning has been associated
with medial temporal lobe activations (Lieberman, Chang,
Chiao, Bookheimer, & Knowlton, 2004; Poldrack et al.,
2001). Moreover, these regions appear to be in competition
such that if one region is relatively active during task performance, the other tends to be correspondingly deactivated.
In one particularly elegant study, Foerde, Knowlton, and
Poldrack (2006) trained subjects on two tasks known to be
learnable using both implicit and explicit processes. For one
task, subjects were trained under cognitive load; the other
task was learned without cognitive load. When there was no
cognitive load task, thus facilitating explicit learning strategies, activity in the medial temporal lobe during training
was associated with performance accuracy at a follow-up
test session. When there was cognitive load during training, thus interfering with explicit learning strategies, activity
in the medial temporal lobe during training was associated
with performance at test; instead, activity in the basal ganglia was associated with later performance. Critically, the
behavioral performances were equivalent in both conditions. In other words, behaviorally there was no evidence
that different underlying psychological processes were supporting performance at test, but neuroimaging revealed that
there were indeed different processes at work. These results
strongly suggest that there are two separate processes that
operate at different times and in different contexts. Although
there may be a smooth transition in observable performance
as learning and performance switch from being controlled to
automatic, the underlying neural responses argue for qualitatively distinct processes.
Although social cognitive neuroscience research has
rarely set out to compare automatic and controlled variants
of social cognition, a number of studies have had conditions
that would at least roughly meet the criteria allowing for such
a comparison. Lieberman (2007) reviewed the findings from
several domains of social cognition. Six brain regions were
reliably invoked during controlled, but not automatic, forms
of social cognition; these regions included the lateral PFC,
lateral parietal cortex, medial PFC, dorsomedial PFC, precuneus, and medial temporal lobe. Four regions were reliably
invoked during automatic, but not controlled, forms of social
cognition; these regions included the amygdala, ventromedial PFC, lateral temporal cortex, and ventral striatum.
These results suggest an answer to the first of the lingering dual-process questions: Are there really separate
automatic and controlled social processes? The findings are
more consistent with an account of separate automatic and
controlled processes, rather than an account wherein single
processes are called automatic when they operate efficiently
12/22/09 5:33:50 PM
178
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
and called controlled when they operate inefficiently. Rather,
it appears that with training, the brain regions responsible
for automatic processes slowly develop computational algorithms to support task performance, and as these processes
come online, brain regions supporting controlled processing
are needed less and less.
These data also speak to the second lingering question of how many sets of dual-processes exist. Although
no definitive answer is available, the review (Lieberman,
2007) found that brain regions involved in automatic or
controlled processes tended to each be involved in a
variety of automatic or controlled processes. For instance,
the ventromedial PFC has been associated with automatic
aspects of self-knowledge, decision making, emotional
experience, and attitudes, whereas right ventrolateral PFC
has been associated with inhibitory control over behavior,
thought, emotion, attitudes, and perspective (Cohen &
Lieberman, in press). Thus, it appears that the same networks responsible for automatic and controlled processing
in one social psychological domain may deal with automatic and controlled processing in other domains as well.
The phenomenologically different inputs in each social
domain may produce different outputs but still make use of
a shared dual-process architecture. This may help explain
phenomena such as ego depletion (Baumeister, Bratslavsky,
Muraven, & Tice, 1998), in which self-control efforts in one
domain undermine subsequent self-control efforts in another
domain. From the perspective of the brain, the processing
resources from the same brain regions may be required for
both tasks, and thus the brain is not starting fresh when moving from one task to another.
Such neuroscience findings may also help update our
understanding of the relationship between automaticity and control more broadly. Similar to the implicit and
explicit learning findings, in a number of the reviewed studies (Lieberman, in press), increasing activity in controlled
processing regions was associated with decreased activity
in automatic processing regions such as the amygdala. For
instance, looking at an emotional picture nonreflectively
leads to reliable amygdala activity. However, labeling the
emotional content of the same picture reflectively leads
to reliable right ventrolateral PFC activity and correlated
decreases in amygdala activity. From the typical view of
automaticity, it is difficult to explain how amygdala activity
in response to an emotional picture would be diminished by
the addition of a conscious reflective process. The amygdala response occurs when such pictures are presented subliminally (Morris et al., 1998; Whalen et al., 1998), a gold
standard for automaticity. Automatic processes are believed
not to rely on the common pool of controlled processes
resources; thus, conscious reflective processing should not
take away any resources that the amygdala needs to respond.
CH05.indd 178
Additionally, by definition, automatic processes that can be
triggered without one’s intentions (e.g., through subliminal
presentations) are believed to be immune to interruption
from conscious processing. Finally, the controlled process
in question directs attention to the emotional aspects of the
stimulus and thus is unlikely to reduce amygdala activity
through distraction effects.
Although difficult to explain from a social cognition perspective, from a cognitive neuroscience perspective, these
results are quite amenable to explanation. There are brain
regions that, independent of one another, show evidence
of possessing the operating characteristics of automatic or
controlled processes, as commonly defined. However, these
brain regions are also interconnected in a multitude of ways,
including functionally inhibitory connections. In other
words, the amygdala may process emotional inputs in an
automatic fashion that requires no controlled processing
resources to operate; however, the amygdala may also be
anatomically linked with regions of the prefrontal cortex
that can inhibit the amygdala’s functioning if they are activated. Although such results do not in themselves demand
a rewriting of all the rules of automaticity and control, they
do suggest aspects that are worth reconsidering and testing
as this new channel of data becomes available.
Internal and External Self-Focus
The mirror self-recognition test (Gallup, 1970) is used to
test whether a particular species possesses self-awareness.
Consequently, it is rather surprising that the network of
brain regions involved in recognizing oneself in a picture
and the network of brain regions involved in reflecting on
one’s feelings, preferences, and traits are completely nonoverlapping networks (Lieberman, 2007). External selffocus (i.e., visual self-recognition) is reliably associated
with a lateral frontoparietal network in the right hemisphere, whereas internal self-focus (i.e., reflecting on one’s
psychological characteristics) is reliably associated with a
medial frontoparietal network. What’s more, the activity in
these two networks at rest tend to be inversely correlated
with one another (Fox et al., 2005). This separation of the
neural networks supporting internal and external self-focus
calls into question whether the mirror self-awareness test is
actually an index of the ability to reflect on the psychological aspects of oneself or is limited to an ability to recognize
the physical manifestations of oneself, perhaps a precursor
to, rather than evidence of, true self-awareness.
Potentially the greatest implication of this dissociation
is that it may help explain why nearly all human beings
maintain some intuitive belief in mind–body dualism,
even when rationally admitting that dualism is a nonstarter
logically (Lieberman, 2009a). Although the broad strokes
of Descartes’ brand of dualism focused on the existence of
12/22/09 5:33:51 PM
How Social Cognitive Neuroscience Contributes to Social Psychology
two strata—the material and the immaterial, the impact of
dualism largely follows from imputing material and immaterial aspects to each individual (i.e., mind and body). Part
of the reason that this discredited theory is so compelling
is that everyone has experiences that feel like a struggle
between two aspects of the self. When we “drag ourselves
out of bed,” this fits nicely with the notion that there is a
mind that somehow forces the unwilling body out of bed.
However, the clean division between the brain regions
involved in internal self-focus (i.e., focusing on one’s
mind) and external self-focus (i.e., focusing on one’s body)
suggests that mind–body dualism may be a particularly
sticky notion because our brain cleaves our perceptions
of ourselves into these components whether we ask our
brain to or not. Just as sights and sounds are automatically
processed by separate neural networks and give rise to
irreducibly distinct sensations, perhaps the separate processing streams for reflecting on one’s own mind and body
produce the irreducible experience of dualism.
Future Questions
Expected or unexpected convergences and dissociations in
the brain regions responsible for particular social processes
help group these processes into the appropriate psychological bins. Neuroscience techniques allow for other kinds
of insights and hypothesis testing as well, although at this
point, very little of this work has been done. For instance,
as cognitive neuroscientists refine their understanding of
the basic computations performed by different regions,
activation in different networks can serve as an indicator
that certain psychological processes have been invoked
(c.f. Poldrack, 2006). This is not to suggest that we will
be able to look at the brain and know whether someone is
reading Haruki Murakami or Italo Calvino anytime soon,
but we may be able to have some idea of whether a person
is at least recruiting self-processes in a very general way,
which would be useful.
Starting in the 1970s, a variety of self-serving or egocentric biases were reported on. For instance, people who
live together each tend to believe they are responsible for
a disproportionate amount of the housework that gets done
(Ross & Sicoly, 1975). Similarly, after being asked if they
would walk around wearing a giant sign saying “Eat at
Joe’s” for a small payment, regardless of the choice they
made, subjects tended to believe most other people would
make the same choice as they did (Ross et al., 1977). Rival
accounts of these self-serving biases (Greenwald, 1980;
Nisbett & Ross, 1980) led to countless studies attempting
to show whether these effects were due to motivational
processes intended to justify a person’s own behavior and
positions or were due to cognitive processes that tended
CH05.indd 179
179
to be biased as a result of the structure of information
processing and the information sample available for consideration (e.g., a person is aware of all the housework
done by oneself but only a portion done by a roommate).
Because studies often provided positive evidence for their
position without providing evidence against the alternative
account, the debate eventually lost steam and was believed
by many to be irresolvable (Tetlock & Levi, 1982). If
neuroimaging can assess the extent to which self-related
or motivational processes are at work, it should be possible to fruitfully revisit this debate. In all likelihood, both
motivational and cognitive processes can contribute to
these effects, but neuroimaging might reveal individual
differences in the source of these biases across individuals,
which in turn might relate to different psychological consequences (e.g., resistance to being challenged).
In the 1990s, research on automatic goals, motives, and
behavior was (and continues to be) enormously influential (Dijksterhuis & Bargh, 2001). The fact that priming
“impression” leads people to act as if they have an impression formation goal (Chartrand & Bargh, 1996), that
priming “succeed” can produce an array of motivational
phenomena (Bargh, Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai, Barndollar, &
Trötschel, 2001), and that priming “elderly” can lead people to walk more slowly (Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996)
are extraordinary findings. Nevertheless, it is unclear from
these findings alone whether automatic and nonautomatic
variants of these processes are in fact one and the same.
The assumption within this literature is that they are the
same, but this has remained an assumption. Neuroimaging
may be relatively uniquely positioned to address this
question because it can clearly show whether two putative processes are relying on common or distinct neural
networks.
Automatic goals, motives, and behaviors fall into the
broader category of phenomena that are real but seem a bit
magical. There are other linkages that always seem a
bit magical as well, such as the functioning of placebo
effects, hypnosis, and the impact of social support on
health (after controlling for specific health care provided
by supporters). In each of these cases, it’s hard to tell a
straightforward compelling story about why the phenomena occur because each is at odds with our basic dualistic
notions that beliefs can change beliefs and overt behavior
but beliefs cannot change low-level perceptual or physiological responses (i.e., our more mechanistic processes).
In each case, neuroimaging data are starting to reveal
where in the brain the magic happens (Eisenberger, Taylor,
Gable, Hilmert, & Lieberman, 2007; Kosslyn, Thompson,
Costantini-Ferrando, Alpert, & Spiegel, 2000; Wager
et al., 2004), and this will allow for further interrogation of
these brain regions and how their neurocognitive function
12/22/09 5:33:52 PM
180
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
might produce the observed results. Neuroscience is hardly
a cure-all, but these are the kinds of problems for which
neuroscience methods may shed new light and prompt new
programs of behavioral research.
V. CONCLUSIONS AND THE NEXT DECADE
This chapter has provided a history of social cognitive neuroscience, the neural landmarks that have been laid down
for hypothesis testing in various domains of social psychology, and an exploration of the specific ways in which
social cognitive neuroscience directly contributes to the
mission of social psychology. Given the number of pages
devoted to each of these sections, there is no denying that
the emphasis of the past decade has been on brain mapping
far more than hypothesis testing. This is not surprising,
because for neuroimaging research, the hypothesis testing phase generally follows the brain mapping phase. But
a more significant factor in the relative balance between
brain mapping and hypothesis testing is that doing the
kind of social cognitive neuroscience studies that ask and
answer the questions of social psychology is hard, much
harder than doing a brain mapping study to see what lights
up. Indeed, making truly meaningful contributions to
social psychology using any methodology is hard because
our phenomena are counterintuitive, our subjects are moving targets trying to figure out the purpose of our experiments, and our experiments must recreate just the right
ecologically valid experiences within ethically acceptable
limits, while still assessing the appropriate dependent variables. All of this is made that much harder when subjects
are essentially lying in coffin-like confinement, unable to
move, unable to speak, and needing several repetitions of
each trial type to extract detectable signals from the noise.
Social cognitive neuroscience studies that address
social psychological questions will only be carried out
to the extent that social psychologists want to ask those
questions and make a commitment to conducting social
cognitive neuroscience studies, either on their own or with
collaborators. Cognitive neuroscientists who are interested
in using social psychological paradigms to clarify what
different brain regions do have every right to do so. They
are pursuing their intellectual passion and they should.
There is no reason why they should suddenly care about
the enduring questions of social psychology anymore than
social psychologists should suddenly care about the enduring issues in neuroscience.
It is incumbent upon social psychologists to make use
of neuroscience for their own ends. And this is nothing new
for social psychologists. In the 1970s, social psychology
reinvented itself in large measure by co-opting the methods
CH05.indd 180
of cognitive psychology for its own purposes. Whether
social psychologists choose to embrace the methods of neuroscience to pursue our mission is still an open question.
Nevertheless, this alone will determine whether the next
review of social cognitive neuroscience, a decade from now,
will have a better balance between brain mapping studies
and studies that move social psychology forward.
REFERENCES
Adolphs, R. (2001). The neurobiology of social cognition. Current Opinion
in Neurobiology, 11, 231–239.
Adolphs, R., Gosselin, F., Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., Schyns, P., &
Damasio, A. R. (2005). A mechanism for impaired fear recognition
after amygdala damage. Nature, 433, 68–72.
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1998). The human amygdala in
social judgment. Nature, 393, 470–471.
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (1994). Impaired
recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage
to the human amygdala. Nature, 372, 669–672.
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1995). Fear and
the human amygdala. The Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 5879–5891.
Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., O’Connor, E., &
Breiter, H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI
and behavioral evidence. Neuron, 32, 537–551.
Ajzen, I. (2001). Nature and operation of attitudes. Annual Review of
Psychology, 52, 27–58.
Akiyama, T., Kato, M., Muramatsu, T., Saito, F., Nakachi, R., & Kashima, H.
(2006). A deficit in discriminating gaze direction in a case with right
superior temporal gyrus lesion. Neuropsychologia, 44, 161–170.
Akiyama, T., Kato, M., Muramatsu, T., Saito, F., Umeda, S., & Kashima, H.
(2006). Gaze but not arrows: A dissociative impairment after right
superior temporal gyrus damage. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1804–1810.
Allison, T., Ginter, H., McCarthy, G., Nobre, A. C., Puce, A., Luby, M.,
et al. (1994). Face recognition in human extrastriate cortex. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 71, 821–825.
Altshuler, L., Bookheimer, S.,Townsend, J., Proenza, M.A., Eisenberger, N. I.,
Sabb, F., et al. (2005). Blunted activation in orbitofrontal cortex
during mania: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
Biological Psychiatry, 58, 763–769.
Ames, D. R. (2004). Inside the mind reader ’s tool kit: Projection and stereotyping in mental state inference. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 87, 340–353.
Amodio, D. M. (2008). The social neuroscience of intergroup relations.
European Review of Social Psychology, 19, 1–54.
Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of the minds: The medial
frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature, 7, 268–277.
Amodio, D. M., Harmon-Jones, E., Devine, P. G., Curtin, J. J.,
Hartley, S. L., & Covert, A. E. (2004). Neural signals for the detection
of unintentional racial bias. Psychological Science, 15, 88–93.
Anders, S., Birbaumer, N., Sadowski, B., Erb, M., Mader, I., Grodd, W.,
et al. (2004). Parietal somatosensory association cortex mediates
affective blindsight. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 339–340.
Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Panitz, D., De Rosa, E., & Gabrieli, J. D. E.
(2003). Neural correlates of the automatic processing of threat facial
signals. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 5627–5633.
Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., Chiavaino, C., & Humphreys, G. W. (2004).
Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind:
Neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced lan-
12/22/09 5:33:52 PM
References 181
guage and executive demands. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16,
1773–1784.
Aron, A. R, Fisher, H., Mashek, D. J., Strong, G., Li, H., & Brown, L. L.
(2005). Reward, motivation, and emotion systems associated with earlystage intense romantic love. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 327–337.
Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right
inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 170–177.
Babiloni, C., Vecchio, F., Bares, M., Brazdil, M., Nestrasil, I., Eusebi, F.,
et al. (2008). Functional coupling between anterior prefrontal cortex
(BA10) and hand muscle contraction during intentional and imitative
motor acts. NeuroImage, 39, 1314–1323.
Bach, D. R., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., Herdener, M., Strik, W. K., &
Seifritz, E. (2008). The effect of appraisal level on processing of emotional prosody in meaningless speech. NeuroImage, 42, 919–927.
Baddeley, A. D. (2002). Fractionating the central executive. In D. T. Stuss &
R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function. (pp. 246–260).
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baird, A., Dewar, B., Critchley, H., Gilbert, S. J., Dolan, R. J., & Cipolotti, L.
(2006). Cognitive functioning after medial frontal lobe damage including
the ACC: A preliminary investigation. Brain and Cognition, 60, 166–175.
Bangert, M., Peschel, T., Schlaug, G., Rotte, M., Drescher, D., Hinrichs, H.,
et al. (2006). Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in
professional pianists: Evidence from fMRI conjunction. NeuroImage,
30, 917–926.
Banks, S. J., Eddy, K. T., Angstadt, M., Nathan, P. J., & Luan Phan, K.
(2007). Amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotion regulation.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 303–312.
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social
behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on
action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–244.
Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Trötschel, R.
(2001). The automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of
behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,
1014–1027.
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Bullmore, E. T., Wheelwright, S.,
Ashwin, C., & Williams, S. C. R. (2000). The amygdala theory of
autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 355–364.
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E. T.,
Brammer, M. J., Simmons, A., et al. (1999). Social intelligence in
the normal and autistic brain: An fMRI study. European Journal of
Neuroscience, 11, 1891–1898.
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic love.
Neuroreport, 11, 3829–3834.
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and
romantic love. NeuroImage, 21, 1155–1166.
Bartholow, B. D., & Dickter, C. L. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience
of person perception: A selective review focused on the event-related
brain potential. In P. Winkielman & E. Harmon-Jones (Eds.), Social
neuroscience: Integrating biological and psychological explanations
of social behavior (pp. 376–400). New York: Guilford Press.
Bartholow, B. D., Pearson, M. A., Dickter, C. L., Sher, K. J., Fabiani, M., &,
Gratton, G. (2005). Strategic control and medial frontal negativity:
Beyond errors and response conflict. Psychophysiology, 42, 33–42.
Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological
answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego
depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265.
Baumeister, R. F. & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire
for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation.
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
Baumgartner, T., Heinrichs, M., Vonlanthen, A., Fischbacher, U., &
Fehr, E. (2008). Oxytocin shapes the neural circuitry of trust and trust
adaptations in humans. Neuron, 58, 639–650.
CH05.indd 181
Beaucousin, V., Lacheret, A., Turbelin, M.-R., Morel, M. I., Mazoyer, B., &
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2007). fMRI study of emotional speech
comprehension. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 339–352.
Beauregard, M., Levesque, J., & Bourgouin, P. (2001). Neural correlates
of conscious self-regulation of emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 21,
RC165.
Bechara, A. (2002). The neurology of social cognition. Brain, 125, 1673–1675.
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994).
Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human
prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.
Beer, J. S., Heerey, E. A., Keltner, D., Scabini, D., & Knight, R. T. (2003).
The regulatory function of self-conscious emotion: Insights from
patients with orbitofrontal damage. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 85, 594–604.
Beer, J. S., John, O. P., & Knight, R. T. (2006). Orbitofrontal cortex and
social behavior: Integrating self-monitoring and emotion cognition
interactions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 871–879.
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social
history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10, 122–142.
Berntson, G. G., Sarter, M., Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Anxiety and cardiovascular reactivity: The basal forebrain cholinergic link. Behavioural
Brain Research, 94, 225–248.
Berthoz, S., Armony, J. L., Blair, R. J. R., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). An fMRI
study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of
social norms. Brain, 125, 1696–1708.
Bidet-Caulet, A., Voisin, J., Bertrand, O., & Fonlupt, P. (2005). Listening
to a walking human activates the temporal biological motion area.
NeuroImage, 28, 132–139.
Bird, C. M., Castelli, F., Malik, U., & Husain, M. (2004). The impact of
extensive medial frontal lobe damage on “theory of mind” and cognition. Brain, 127, 914–928.
Blakemore, S.-J., den Ouden, H., Choudhury, S., & Frith, C. (2007).
Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about
intentions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 130–139.
Blakemore, S.-J., Oakley, D. A., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Delusions of alien
control in the normal brain. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1058–1067.
Blakemore S.-J, Winston, J, & Frith, U. (2004). Social cognitive neuroscience: Where are we heading? Trends in Cognitive Science, 8,
216–222.
Blakeslee, S. (2006, January 10). Cells that read minds. New York Times.
Blanke, O., Landis, T., Spinelli, L., & Seeck, M. (2004). Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of neurological origin. Brain, 127, 243–258.
Blanke, O., Mohr, C., Michael, C. M., Pascual-Leone, A., Brugger, P.,
Seeck, M., et al. (2005). Linking out-of-body experience and self processing to mental own-body imagery at the temporoparietal junction.
The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 550–557.
Blanke, O., Ortigue, S., Landis, T., & Seeck, M. (2002). Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions. Nature, 419, 269–270.
Boly, M., Balteau, E., Schnakers, C., Degueldre, C., Moonen, G., Luxen, A.,
et al. (2007). Baseline brain activity fluctuations predict somatosensory perception in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 104, 12187–12192.
Borg, J. S., Hynes, C., Horn, J. V., Grafton, S., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W.
(2006). Consequences, action, and intention as factors in moral judgments:
An fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 803–817.
Borg, J. S., Lieberman, D., & Kiehl, K. A. (2008). Infection, incest, and
iniquity: Investigating the neural correlates of disgust and morality.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1529–1546.
Botvinick, M., Jha, A. P., Bylsma, L. M., Fabian, S. A., Solomon, P. E., &
Prkachin, K. M. (2005). Viewing facial expressions of pain engages
cortical areas involved in the direct experience of pain. NeuroImage,
25, 312–319.
12/22/09 5:33:53 PM
182
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
Brass, M., Derrfuss, J., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2005). The inhibition of
imitative and overlearned responses: A functional double dissociation.
Neuropsychologia, 43, 89–98.
Brass, M., Zysset, S., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). The inhibition of imitative response tendencies. NeuroImage, 14, 1416–1423.
Brattico, E., Tervaniemi, M., & Picton, T. W. (2003). Effects of brief
discrimination-training on the auditory N1 wave. NeuroReport, 14,
2489–2492.
Brehm, J. W. (1956). Post-decision changes in the desirability of alternatives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 384–389.
Brunet, E., Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Baylé, M.-C., & Decety, J. (2000). A PET
investigation of the attribution of intentions with a nonverbal task.
NeuroImage, 11, 157–166.
Buccino, G., Vogt, S., Ritzl, A., Fink, G. R., Zilles, K., Freund, H.-J., et al.
(2004). Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand actions:
An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 42, 323–334.
Burklund, L. J., Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). The face of
rejection: Rejection sensitivity moderates dorsal anterior cingulated activity
to disapproving facial expressions. Social Neuroscience, 2, 238–253.
Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulated cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4,
215–222.
Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review
of 275 PET and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
12, 1–47.
Cacioppo, J. T. (1994). Social neuroscience: Automatic, neuroendocrine,
and immune responses to stress. Psychophysiology, 31, 113–128.
Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Lorig, T. S., Norris, C. J., Rickett, E., &
Nusbaum, H. (2003). Just because you’re imaging the brain doesn’t
mean you can stop using your head: A primer and set of first principles.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 650–661.
Cacioppo, J. T., Crites, S. L., Jr., & Gardner, W. L. (1996). Attitudes to
the right: Evaluative processing is associated with lateralized late positive event-related brain potentials. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 22, 1205–1219.
Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Lawrence, A. D., & Manes, F. (2004). Impaired
recognition of anger following damage to the ventral striatum. Brain,
127, 1958–1969.
Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Manes, F., Antoun, N., & Young, A. W. (2000).
Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury.
Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1077–1078.
Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P.
(2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study
with expert dancers. Cerebral cortex, 15, 1243–1249.
Camerer, C., Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (2005). Neuroeconomics: How
neuroscience can inform economics. Journal of Economic Literature,
18, 9–64.
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72,
1032–1053.
Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M.-C., Mazziotta, J. C., & Lenzi, G. L.
(2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, USA, 100, 5497–5502.
Carter, C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and
love. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 779–818.
Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger
syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to
animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999). Dual-process theories in social
psychology. New York: Guilford Press.
Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Empathizing
with basic emotions: Common and discrete neural substrates. Social
Neuroscience, 1, 364–384.
CH05.indd 182
Chaminade, T., & Decety, J. (2002). Leader or follower? Involvement of
the IPL in agency. NeuroReport, 13, 1975–1978.
Chan, A. W. Y., Peelen, M. V., & Downing, P. E. (2004). The effect of viewpoint on body representation in the extrastriate body area. NeuroReport,
15, 2407–2410.
Channon, S., & Crawford, S. (2000). The effects of anterior lesions on
performance on a story comprehension test: Left anterior impairment
on a theory of mind-type task. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1006–1017.
Chartrand, T., & Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automatic activation of impression
formation and memorization goals: Nonconscious goal priming reproduces effects of explicit task instructions. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 71, 464–478.
Chen, Y., Meltzof, A. N., & Decety, J. (2007). Motivation modulates the
activity of the human mirror-neuron system. Cerebral Cortex, 17,
1979–1986.
Chong, T. T.-J., Williams, M. A., Cunnington, R., & Mattingley, J. B.
(2008). Selective attention modulates inferior frontal gyrus activity
during action observation. NeuroImage, 40, 298–307.
Ciaramelli, E., Muccioli, M., Ladavas, E., & di Pellegrino, G. (2007).
Selective deficit in personal moral judgment following damage
to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Social Cognitive and Affective
Neuroscience, 2, 84–92.
Ciaramidaro, A., Adenzato, M., Enrici, I., Erk, S., Pia, L., Bara, B. G.,
et al. (2007). The intentional network: How the brain reads varieties of
intentions. Neuropsychologia, 45, 3105–3113.
Cloutier, J., Turk, D. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2008). Extracting variant and
invariant information from faces: The neural substrates of gaze detection and sex categorization. Social Neuroscience, 3, 69–78.
Cohen, J. R., & Lieberman, M. D. (in press). The common neural basis of
exerting self-control in multiple domains. In Y. Trope, R. Hassin, &
K. N. Ochsner (Eds.), Self-control.
Colman, A. M. (2006). Oxford Dictionary of Psychology (2nd ed.).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and social order. New York: Scribner’s.
Craik, F. I. M., Moroz, T. M., Moscovitch, M., Stuss, D. T., Winocur, G.,
Tulving, E., et al. (1999). In search of the self: A positron emission
tomography study. Psychological Science, 10, 26–34.
Craik, F. I., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention
of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
104, 268–294.
Crockett, M. J., Clark, L., Tabibnia, G., Lieberman, M. D., & Robbins, T. W.
(2008). Serotonin modulates behavioral reactions to unfairness.
Science, 30, 1739.
Cross, E. S., Kraemer, D. J. M., Hamilton, A. F. C., Kelley, W. M., &
Grafton, S. T. (2009). Sensitivity of the action observation network to
physical and observational learning. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 315–326.
Cunningham, W. A., Espinet, S. D., DeYoung, C. G., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005).
Attitudes to the right and left: Frontal ERP asymmetries associated
with stimulus valence and processing goals. NeuroImage, 28, 827–834.
Cunningham, W. A., Johnson, M. K., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., &
Banaji, M. R. (2003). Neural components of social evaluation. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 639–649.
Cunningham, W. A., Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Gatenby, J. C.,
Gore, J. C., & Banaji, M. R. (2004). Separable neural components in the
processing of black and white faces. Psychological Science, 15, 806–813.
Cunningham, W. A., Raye, C. L., & Johnson, M. K. (2004). Implicit and
explicit evaluation: fMRI correlates of valence, emotional intensity, and control in the processing of attitudes. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 16, 1717–1729.
D’Argembeau, A., Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Laureys, S., Del Fiore, G.,
Degueldre, C., et al. (2005). Self-referential reflective activity and its
relationship with rest: A PET study. NeuroImage, 25, 616–624.
D’Argembeau, A., Ruby, P., Collette, F., Degueldre, C., Balteau, E.,
Luxen, A., et al. (2007). Distinct regions of the medial prefrontal cortex
12/22/09 5:33:54 PM
References 183
are associated with self-referential processing and perspective taking.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 935–944.
D’Argembeau, A., Xue, G., Lu, Z.-L., Van der Linden, M., & Bechara, A.
(2008). Neural correlates of envisioning emotional events in the near
and far future. NeuroImage, 40, 398–407.
Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S.,
Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., et al. (2005). Gaze fixation and
the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience,
8, 519–526.
Eisenberger, N. I., Jarcho, J. J., Lieberman, M. D., & Naliboff, B. D.
(2006). An experimental study of shared sensitivity to physical pain
and social rejection. Pain, 126, 132–138.
Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts:
A common neural alarm system for physical pain and social pain.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 294–300.
Davis, K. D., Taylor, K. S., Hutchison, W. D., Dostrovsky, J. O.,
McAndrews, M. P., Richter, E. O., & Lozano, A. M. (2005). Human
anterior cingulate cortex neurons encode cognitive and emotional
demands. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 8402–8406.
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Satpute, A. B. (2005). Personality
from a controlled processing perspective: An fMRI study of neuroticism, extraversion, & self-consciousness. Cognitive, Affective, and
Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 169–181.
Decety, J., & Chaminade, T. (2003). Neural correlates of feeling sympathy.
Neuropsychologia, 41, 127–138.
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290–292.
Decety, J., Grèzes, J., Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Procyk, E.,
et al. (1997). Brain activity during observation of actions influence of
action content and subject’s strategy. Brain, 120, 1763–1777.
Eisenberger, N. I., Taylor, S. E., Gable, S. L., Hilmert, C. J., & Lieberman, M. D.
(2007). Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress response. NeuroImage, 35, 1601–1612.
Decety, J., Jackson, P. L., Sommerville, J. A., Chaminade, T., & Meltzoff, A. N.
(2004). The neural bases of cooperation and competition: An fMRI
investigation. NeuroImage, 23, 744–751.
Eisenberger, N. I., Way, B. M., Taylor, S. E., Welch, W. T., & Lieberman, M. D.
(2007). Understanding genetic risk for aggression: Clues from the
brain’s response to social exclusion. Biological Psychiatry, 61,
1100–1108.
Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2007). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: How low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition. The Neuroscientist, 13, 580–593.
De Cremer, D., & Alberts, H. J. E. M. (2004). When procedural fairness
does not influence how positive I feel: The effects of voice and leader
selection as a function of belongingness need. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 34, 333–344.
Delgado, M. R., Frank, R. H., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). Perceptions of moral
character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game.
Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1611–1618.
Delgado, M. R., Gillis, M. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2008). Regulating the
expectation of reward via cognitive strategies. Nature Neuroscience,
11, 880–881.
den Ouden, H. E., Frith, U., Frith, C., & Blakemore, S.J. (2005). Thinking
about intentions. NeuroImage, 28, 787–796.
Devue, C., Collette, F., Balteau, E., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., Maquet, P.,
et al. (2007). Here I am: The cortical correlates of visual self-recognition.
Brain Research, 1143, 169–182.
Engel, A., Burke, M., Fiehler, K., Bien, S., & Rösler, F. (2008). What
activates the human neuron system during observation of artificial movements: Bottom-up visual features or top-down intentions?
Neuropsychologia, 46, 2033–2042.
Engel, A., Burke, M., Fiehler, K., Bien, S., & Rösler, F. (in press). How
moving objects become animated: The human mirror neuron system
assimilates non-biological movement patterns. Social Neuroscience.
Engell, A. D., Haxby, J. V., & Todorov, A. (2007). Implicit trustworthiness
decisions: Automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1508–1519.
Enger, T., Etkins, A., Gale, S., & Hirsch, J. (2008). Dissociable neural systems resolve conflict from emotional versus nonemotional distracters.
Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1475–1484.
Ethofer, T., Anders, S., Erb, M., Herbert, C., Wiethoff, S., Kissler, J.,
et al. (2006). Cerebral pathways in processing of affective prosody:
A dynamic causal modeling study. NeuroImage, 30, 580–587.
DeWall, C. N., MacDonald, G., Webster, G. D., Masten, C., Baumeister, R. F.,
Powell, C., et al. (in press). Tylenol reduces social pain: Behavioral and
neural evidence. Psychological Science.
Etkin, A., Enger, T., Peraza, D. M., Kandel, E. R., & Hirsch, J. (2006).
Resolving emotional conflict: A role for the rostral anterior cingulated cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron, 51,
871–882.
DeWall, C. N., & Baumeister, R. F. (2006). Alone but feeling no pain:
Effects of social exclusion on physical pain tolerance and pain threshold, affective forecasting, and interpersonal empathy. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 1–15.
Evers, E. A., Cools, R., Clark, L., van der Veen, F. M., Jolles, J.,
Sahakian, B. J., et al. (2005). Serotonergic modulation of prefrontal
cortex during negative feedback in probabilistic reversal learning.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 30, 1138–1147.
di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G.
(1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiology study.
Experimental Brain Research, 91,176–80.
Farb, N. A. S., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., McKeon, D, Fatima, Z.,
et al. (2007). Attending to the present: Mindfulness meditation reveals
distinct neural modes of self-reference. Social Cognitive and Affective
Neuroscience, 2, 313–322.
Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The perception-behavior expressway. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 1–40.
Downing, P. E., Yuhong, J., Shuman, M., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). A cortical
area selective for visual processing of the human body. Science, 293,
2470–2473.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary
Anthropology, 6, 178–190.
Ehrsson, H. H., Weich, K., Weiskopf, N., Dolan, R. J., & Passingham, R. E.
(2007). Threatening a rubber hand that you feel is yours elicits a cortical
anxiety response. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
104, 9828–9833.
Eippert, F., Veit, R., Weiskopf, N., Erb, M., Birbaumer, N., & Anders, S.
(2007). Regulation of emotional responses elicited by threat-related
stimuli. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 409–423.
CH05.indd 183
Eisenberger, N. I., Gable, S. L., & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). fMRI
responses relate to differences in real-world social experience.
Emotion, 7, 745–754.
Farrer, C., Franck, N., Georgieff, N., Frith, C. D., Decety, J., & Jeannerod, M.
(2003). Modulating the experience of agency: A positron emission
tomography study. NeuroImage, 18, 324–333.
Farrer, C., Frey, S. H., Van Horn, J. D., Tunik, E., Turk, D., Inati, S., et al.
(2008). The angular gyrus computes action awareness representations.
Cerebral Cortex, 18, 254–261.
Farrow, T. F., Zheng, Y., Wilkinson, I. D., Spence, S. A., Deakin, J. F.,
Tarrier, N., et al. (2001). Investigating the functional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness. NeuroReport, 12, 2433–2438.
Fazio, R. H., & Williams, C. J. (1986). Attitude accessibility as a moderator
of the attitude-perception and attitude-behavior relations: An investigation of the 1984 presidential election. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 51, 505–514.
12/22/09 5:33:55 PM
184
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and
cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114, 817–868.
Fellows, L. K., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Is ACC necessary for cognitive control? Brain, 128, 788–796.
Felmingham, K., Kemp, A., Williams, L., Das, P., Hughes, G., Peduto, A.,
et al. (2007). Changes in anterior cingulate and amygdala after cognitive behavior therapy of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological
Science, 18, 127–129.
Festinger, L. (1964). Conflict, decision, and dissonance. Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press.
Finger, E. C., Marsh, A. A., Kamel, N., Mitchell, D. G. V., & Blair, J. R.
(2006). Caught in the act: The impact of audience on the neural
response to morally and socially inappropriate behavior. NeuroImage,
33, 414–421.
Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of
information and motivation on attention and interpretation. Advances
in Experimental Social Psychology, 23, 1–74.
Fitzgerald, D. A., Angstadt, M., Jelsone, L. M., Nathan, P. J., & Luan Phan, K.
(2006). Beyond threat: Amygdala reactivity across multiple expressions of facial affect. NeuroImage, 30, 1441–1448.
Fletcher, P. C., Happe, F., Frith, U., Baker, S. C., Dolan, R. J.,
Frackowiak, R. S. J., et al. (1995). Other minds in the brain: A functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension.
Cognition, 57, 109–128.
Foerde, K., Knowlton, B. J., & Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, USA, 103, 11778–11783.
Foltz, E. L., & White, L. E. (1968). The role of rostral cingulotomy in
“pain” relief. International Journal of Neurology, 6, 353–373.
Forbes, C. E., Schmader, T., & Allen, J. J. B. (2008). The role of devaluing and discounting in performance monitoring: A neurophysiological study of minorities under threat. Social Cognitive and Affective
Neuroscience, 3, 253–261.
Formisano, E., Linden, D. E. J., Di Salle, F., Trojano, L., Esposito, F.,
Sack, A. T., et al. (2002). Tracking the mind’s image in the brain I: Timeresolved fMRI during visuospatial mental imagery. Neuron, 35, 185–194.
Fossati, P., Hevenor, S. J., Graham, S. J., Grady, C., Keightley, M. L.,
Craik, F., et al. (2003). In search of the emotional self: An fMRI study
using positive and negative emotional words. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 160, 1938–1945.
Fossati, P., Hevenor, S. J., Lepage, M., Graham, S. J., Grady, C.,
Keightley, M. L., et al. (2004). Distributed self in episodic memory:
Neural correlates of successful retrieval of self-encoded positive and
negative personality traits. NeuroImage, 22, 1596–1604.
Fox, R., & McDaniel, C. (1982). The perception of biological motion by
human infants. Science, 218, 486–487.
Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., &
Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized
into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, USA, 102, 9673–9678.
Franck, N., O’Leary, D. S., Flaum, M., Hichwa, R. D., & Andreasen, N. A.
(2002). Cerebral blood flow changes associated with Schneiderian
first-rank symptoms in schizophrenia. Journal of Neuropsychiatry
Clinical Neuroscience, 14, 277–282.
Fransson, P., Skiold, B., Horsch, S., Nordell, A., Blennow, M., Lagercrantz, H.,
et al. (2007). Resting-state networks in the infant brain. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 15531–15536.
Fried, I., Katz, A., McCarthy, G., Sass, K. J., Williamson, P., Spencer, S. S.,
et al. (1991). Functional organization of human supplementary motor
cortex studied by electrical stimulation. The Journal of Neuroscience,
11, 3656–3666.
CH05.indd 184
Frith, C. D., Friston, K., Liddle, P. F., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1991). Willed
action and the prefrontal cortex in man: A study with PET. Proceedings
of the Royal Society of London, 244, 241–246.
Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London, B:
Biological Sciences, 358, 459–473.
Fukui, H., Murai, T., Shinozaki, J., Aso, T., Fukuyama, H., Hayashi, et al.
(2006). The neural basis of social tactics: An fMRI study. NeuroImage,
32, 913–920.
Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of “theory of
mind.” Trends in Cognitive Psychology, 7, 77–83.
Gallagher, H. L., Happé, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, U., &
Frith, C. D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: An
fMRI study of “theory of mind” in verbal and nonverbal tasks.
Neuropsychologia, 38, 11–21.
Gallagher, H. L., Jack, A. I., Roepstorff, A., & Frith, C. D. (2002).
Imagining the intentional stance in a competitive game. NeuroImage,
16, 814–521.
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.
Gallese, V., Ferrari, P. F., & Umilità, M. A. (2002). The mirror matching
system: A shared manifold for intersubjectivity. Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 25, 35–36.
Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Action representation and the IPL. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common
mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and performance
(Vol.19, pp. 247–266). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the
basis of social cognition. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,
8, 396–403.
Gallup, G. G., Jr. (1970). Chimpanzees: Self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–87.
Ganesan, V., Hunter, M. D., & Spence, S. A. (2005). Schneiderian firstrank symptoms and right parietal hyperactivation: A replication using
fMRI. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 1545.
Gauthier, I., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J. C., & Anderson, A. W. (2000).
Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 191–197.
Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., & Keysers, C. (2007). The anthropomorphic
brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic
actions. NeuroImage, 35, 1674–1684.
de Gelder, B., Snyder, J., Greve, D., Gerard, G., & Hadjikhani, N. (2004).
Fear fosters flight: A mechanism for fear contagion when perceiving
emotion expressed by a whole body. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, USA, 101, 16701–16706.
German, T. P., Niehaus, J. L., Roarty, M. P., Giesbrecht, B., & Miller, M. B.
(2004). Neural correlates of detecting pretense: Automatic engagement
of the intentional stance under covert conditions. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 16, 1805–1817.
Gilbert, D. T. (1999). What the mind’s not. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope
(Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 3–11).
New York: Guilford Press.
Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias.
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 21–38.
Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busyness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 733–740.
Gilbert, S. J., Williamson, I. D. M., Dumontheil, I., Simons, J. S., Frith, C. D., &
Burgess, P. W. (2007). Distinct regions of medial rostral prefrontal cortex supporting social and nonsocial functions. Social Cognitive and
Affective Neuroscience, 2, 217–226.
Gilboa, A. 2004. Autobiographical and episodic memory—one and the
same? Evidence from prefrontal activation in neuroimaging studies.
Neuropsychologia, 42, 1336–1349.
12/22/09 5:33:56 PM
References 185
Gillath, O., Bunge, S. A., Shaver, P. R., Wendelken, C., & Mikulincer, M.
(2005). Attachment-style differences in the ability to suppress negative
thoughts: Exploring the neural correlates. NeuroImage, 28, 835–847.
Gutchess, A. H., Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Aging,
self-referencing, and medial prefrontal cortex. Social Neuroscience, 2,
117–133.
Gobbini, M. I., Koralek, A. C., Bryan, R. E., Montgomery, K. J., & Haxby, J. V.
(2007). Two takes on the social brain: A comparison of theory of mind
tasks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1803–1814.
Hamilton, A. F. de C., Wolpert, D. M., Frith, U., & Grafton, S. T. (2006).
Where does your own action influence your perception of another person’s action in the brain? NeuroImage, 29, 524–535.
Goel, V., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Explaining modulation of reasoning by
belief. Cognition, 87, B11–B22.
Hamilton, D. L., Katz, L. B., & Leirer, V. O. (1980). Cognitive representations of personality impressions: Organizational processes in first
impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
39, 1050–1063.
Golby, A. J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Chiao, J. Y., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2001).
Differential responses in the fusiform region to same-race and otherrace faces. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 845–850.
Goldberg, I. I., Harel, M., & Malach, R. (2006). When the brain loses its
self: Prefrontal inactivation during sensorimotor processing. Neuron,
50, 329–339.
Goldin, P. R., McRae, K., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. J. (2007). The neural
bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative
emotion. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 577–586.
Goldman, A. I. (1989). Interpretation psychologized. Mind and Language,
4, 161–185.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.
Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Hoffman, J. M., & Kilts, C. D. (2002). Ecstasy
and agony: Activation of the human amygdala in positive and negative
emotion. Psychological Science, 13, 135–141.
Han, S., Jiang, Y., Humphreys, G. W., Zhou, T., & Cai, P. (2005). Distinct
neural substrates for the perception of real and virtual visual worlds.
NeuroImage, 24, 928–935.
Happé, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., Johansson, M., Gillberg, C.,
et al. (1996). “Theory of mind” in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan
study of Asperger syndrome. NeuroReport, 8, 197–201.
Gopnik, A. (2007). Cells that read minds? What the myth of mirror neurons
get wrong about the human brain. Slate. www.slate.com/id/2165123.
Retrieved September 4, 2009.
Harenski, C. L., & Hamann, S. (2006). Neural correlates of regulating negative emotions related to moral violations. NeuroImage, 30, 313–324.
Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D.
(2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293, 2105–2108.
Hariri, A. R., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2000). Modulating
emotional responses: Effects of a neurotical network on the limbic system. NeuroReport, 11, 43–48.
Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of
personal history. American Psychologist, 35, 603–618.
Harmon-Jones, E., Gerdjikov, T., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2006). The effect of
induced compliance on relative left frontal cortical activity: A test
of the action-based model of dissonance. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 36, 1–11.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring
individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association
test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.
Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2003). Functional
connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default
mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
100, 252–258.
Grèzes, J., Berthoz, S., & Passingham, R. E. (2006). Amygdala activation
when one is the target of deceit: Did he lie to you or to someone else?
NeuroImage, 30, 601–608.
Grèzes, J., Fonlupt, P., Bertenthal, B., Delon-Martin, C., Segebarth, C., &
Decety, J. (2001). Does perception of biological motion rely on specific
brain regions? NeuroImage, 13, 775–785.
Grèzes, J., Frith, C. D., & Passingham, R. E. (2004). Inferring false beliefs
from the actions of oneself and others: An fMRI study. NeuroImage,
21, 744–750.
Grèzes, J., Pichon, S., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Perceiving fear in dynamic
body expressions. NeuroImage, 35, 959–967.
Grill-Spector, K., Knouf, N., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). The fusiform face
area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 555–562.
Grossman, E. D., & Blake, R. (2002). Brain areas active during visual perception of biological motion. Neuron, 35, 1167–1175.
Grossman, E. D., Blake, R., & Kim, C. Y. (2004). Learning to see biological motion: Brain activity parallels behavior. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 16, 1669–1679.
Gundel, H., O’Connor, M. F., Littrell, L., Fort, C., & Lane, R. D. (2003).
Functional neuroanatomy of grief: An FMRI study. American Journal
of Psychiatry, 160, 1946–1953.
Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2001).
Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation
to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, USA, 98, 4259–4264.
CH05.indd 185
Hamm, A., Weike, A. I., Schupp, H. T., Treig, T., Dressel, A., & Kessler, C.
(2003). Affective blindsight: Intact fear conditioning to a visual cue in
a cortically blind patient. Brain, 126, 267–275.
Harmon-Jones, E., Harmon-Jones, C., Fearn, M., Sigelman, J. D., &
Johnson, P. (2008). Left frontal cortical activation and spreading of
alternatives: Tests of the action-based model of dissonance. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 1–15.
Harris, L. T., Todorov, A., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). Attributions on the brain:
Neuro-imaging dispositional inferences, beyond theory of mind.
NeuroImage, 28, 763–769.
Haxby, J. V., Gobbini, M. I., Furey, M. L., Ishai, A., Schouten, J. L., &
Pietrini, P. (2001). Distributed and overlapping representations of faces
and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science, 293, 2425–2430.
Hashimoto, Y., & Sakai, K. L. (2003). Brain activations during conscious
self-monitoring of speech production with delayed auditory feedback:
An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 20, 22–28.
Heatherton, T. F., Wyland, C. L., Macrae, C. N., Demos, K. E., Denny, B. T., &
Kelley, W. M. (2006). Medial prefrontal activity differentiates self
from close others. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1,
18–25.
Heberlein, A. S., & Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2004). Cortical
regions for judgments of emotions and personality traits from pointlight walkers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1143–1158.
Heberlein, A. S., Padon, A. A., Gillihan, S. J., Farah, M. J., & Fellows, L. K.
(2008). Ventromedial frontal lobe plays a critical role in facial emotion
recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 721–733.
Heberlein, A. S., & Saxe, R. R. (2005). Dissociation between emotion and
personality judgments: Convergent evidence from functional neuroimaging. NeuroImage, 28, 770–777.
Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent
behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–259.
Heiser, M., Iacoboni, M., Maeda, F., Marcus, J., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2003).
The essential role of Broca’s area in imitation. European Journal of
Neuroscience, 17, 1123–1128.
12/22/09 5:33:57 PM
186
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
Hennenlotter, A., Dresel, C., Castrop, F., Castrop Baumann, A. O.,
Wohlschläger, A. M., & Haslinger, B. (2009). The link between facial
feedback and neural activity within central circuitries of emotion-new
insights from botulinum toxin-induced denervation of frown muscles.
Cerebral Cortex, 19, 537–542 .
Hennenlotter, A., Schroeder, U., Erhard, P., Castrop, F., Haslinger, B.,
Stoecker, D., et al. (2005). A common neural basis for receptive and
expressive communication of pleasant facial affect. NeuroImage, 26,
581–591.
Herwig, U., Baumgartner, T., Kaffenberger, T., Bruhl, A., Kottlow, M.,
Schreiter-Gasser, U., et al. (2007). Modulation of anticipatory emotion and perception processing by cognitive control. NeuroImage, 37,
652–662.
Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a
motivational principle. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,
30, 1–46.
Hodzic, A., Muckli, L., Singer, W., & Stirn, A. (2009). Cortical responses
to self and others. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 951–962.
Hoffman, E. A., & Haxby, J. V. (2000). Distinct representations of eye gaze
and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception.
Nature Neuroscience, 3, 80–84.
Hooker, C. I., Paller, K. A., Gitelman, D. R., Parrish, T. B., Mesulam, M. M., &
Reber, P. J. (2003). Brain networks for analyzing eye gaze. Cognitive
Brain Research, 17, 406–418.
Hovland, C. I., & Rosenberg, M. J. (1960). Summary and further theoretical issues. In M. J. Rosenberg, C. I. Hovland, W. J. McGuire, R.
P. Abelson, & J. W. Brehm (Eds.), Attitude organization and change:
An analysis of consistency among attitude components (pp. 198–235).
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Hsu, M., Anen, C., & Quartz, S. R. (2008). The right and the good:
Distributive justice and neural encoding of equity and efficiency.
Science. 320, 1092–1095.
Hunter, M. D., Farrow, T. F. D., Papadakis, N. G., Wilkinson, I. D.,
Woodruff, P. W. R., & Spence, S. A. (2003). Approaching an ecologically valid functional anatomy of spontaneous “willed” action.
NeuroImage, 20, 1264–1269.
Hynes, C. A., Baird, A. A., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Differential role of the
orbital frontal lobe in emotional versus cognitive perspective-taking.
Neuropsychologia, 44, 374–383.
Iacoboni, M., Lieberman, M. D., Knowlton, B. J., Molnar-Szakacs, I.,
Moritz, M., Throop, C. J., & Fiske, A. P. (2004). Watching social interactions produces dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD
fMRI signal increases compared to a resting baseline. NeuroImage, 21,
1167–1173.
Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., &
Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own
mirror neuron system. PLoS Biology, 3, e79.
Iacoboni, M., Woods, R., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., &
Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation.
Science, 286, 2526–2528.
Insel, T. R., & Shapiro, L. E. (1992). Oxytocin receptors and maternal behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 652,
122–141.
Insel, T. R., & Winslow, J. T. (1998). Serotonin and neuropeptides in affiliative behaviors. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 207–219.
Ito, T. A., & Urland, G. R. (2003). Race and gender on the brain:
Electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of multiply categorizable individuals. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 85, 616–626.
Ito, T. A., & Urland, G. R. (2005). The influence of processing objectives
on the perception of faces: An ERP study of race and gender perception. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 21–36.
Izuma, K., Saito, D. N., & Sadato, N. (2008). Processing of social and
monetary rewards in the human striatum. Neuron, 58, 284–295.
CH05.indd 186
Jabbi, M., Swart, M., & Keysers, C. (2007). Empathy for positive and
negative emotions in the gustatory cortex. NeuroImage, 34, 1744–1753.
Jackson, P. L., Brunet, E., Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2006). Empathy
examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how
I feel versus how you feel pain. Neuropsychologia, 44, 752–761.
Jacobsen, L. K., Slotkin, T. A., Westerveld, M., Mencl, W. E., & Pugh., K. R.
(2006). Visuospatial memory deficits emerging during nicotine withdrawal
in adolescents with prenatal exposure. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31,
1550–1561.
Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model
for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–211.
Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Mitchell, K. J., Touryan, S. R., Greene, E. J., &
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2006). Dissociating medial frontal and posterior cingulated activity during self-reflection. Social Affective and
Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 56–64.
Johnson, S. C., Baxter, L. C., Wilder, L. S., Pipe, J. G., Heiserman, J. E., &
Prigatano, G. P. (2002). Neural correlates of self-reflection. Brain, 125,
1808–1814.
Johnson, S. C., Schmitz, T. W., Kawahara-Baccus, T. N., Rowley, H. A.,
Alexander, A. L., Lee, J., et al. (2005). The cerebral response during
subjective choice with and without self-reference. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 17, 1897–1906.
Johnson-Frey, S. H., Maloof, F. R., Newman-Norlund, R., Farrer, C., Inati, S., &
Grafton, S. T. (2003). Actions or hand-object interactions? Human
inferior frontal cortex and action observation. Neuron, 39, 1053–1058.
Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 1–24.
Kalisch, R., Wiech, K., Critchley, H. D., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P.,
Oakley, D. A., et al. (2005). Anxiety reduction through detachment:
Subjective, physiological, and neural effects. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 17, 874–883.
Kalisch, R., Wiech, W., Herrmann, K., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Neural correlates of self-distraction from anxiety and a process model of cognitive emotion regulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18,
1266–1276.
Kampe, K. K. W., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2003). “Hey John:” Signals
conveying communicative intention toward the self activate brain
regions associated with “mentalizing,” regardless of modality. The
Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 5258–5263.
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face
area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 4302–4311.
Kaplan, J. T., Aziz-Zadeh, L., Uddin, L. Q., & Iacoboni, M. (2008). The
self across the senses: An fMRI study of self-face and self-voice recognition. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3, 218–223.
Kaplan, J. T., Freedman, J. T., & Iacoboni, M. (2007). Us versus them:
Political attitudes and party affiliation influence neural responses to
faces of presidential candidates. Neuropsychologia, 45, 55–64.
Kaplan, J. T., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Getting a grip on other minds: Mirror
neurons, intention, understanding, and cognitive empathy. Social
Neuroscience, 1, 175–183.
Kawabata, H., & Zeki, S. (2004). Neural correlates of beauty. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 91, 1699–1705.
Kelley, H. H. (1973). The process of causal attribution. American
Psychologist, 28, 107–128.
Kelley, W. M. C., Macrae, C. N., Wyland, C. L., Caglar, S., Inati, S., &
Heatherton, T. F. (2002). Finding the self? An event-related fMRI
study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 785–794.
Keysers, C., Wicker, B., Gazzola, V., Anton, J.-L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V.
(2004). A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and
experience of touch. Neuron, 42, 335–346.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K, & Glaser, R. (1989). Psychoneuroimmunology: Past,
present, and future. Health Psychology, 8, 677–682.
12/22/09 5:33:57 PM
References 187
Kihlstrom, J. F. (2006). Does neuroscience constrain social-psychological
theory? Dialogue, 21, 16–17.
Kim, S. H., & Hamann, S. (2007). Neural correlates of positive and negative emotion regulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19,
776–798.
King-Casas, B., Tomlin, D., Anen, C., Camerer, C. F., Quartz, S. R., &
Montague, P. R. (2005). Getting to know you: Reputation and trust in a
two-person economic exchange. Science, 308, 78–83.
Kircher, T. T. J., Brammer, M., Bullmore, E., Simmons, A., Bartels, M., &
David, A. S. (2002). The neural correlates of intentional and incidental
self processing. Neuropsychologia, 40, 683–692.
Lambon Ralph, M. A., Pobric, G., & Jefferies, E. (2009). Conceptual
knowledge is underpinned by the temporal pole bilaterally: Convergent
evidence from rTMS. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 832–838.
Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., & Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrates of
human empathy: Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 42–58.
Kjaer, T. W., Nowak, M., & Lou, H. C. (2002). Reflective self-awareness
and conscious states: PET evidence for a common midline parietofrontal core. NeuroImage, 17, 1080–1086.
Lamm, C., Nusbaum, H. C., Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2007). What are
you feeling? Using functional magnetic resonance imaging to assess
the modulation of sensory and affective responses during empathy for
pain. PLoS ONE, 2, e1292.
Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1996). Self-Knowledge of an
amnesic patient: Toward a neuropsychology of personality and social
psychology. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 125, 250–260.
Lane, R. D., Fink, G. R., Chau, P. M.-L., & Dolan, R. J. (1997). Neural
activation during selective attention to subjective emotional responses.
NeuroReport, 8, 3969–3972.
Knoch, D., Pascual-Leone, A., Meyer, K., Treyer, V., & Fehr, E. (2006).
Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal
cortex. Science, 314, 829–832.
Lau, H. C., Rogers, R. D., Haggard, P., & Passingham, R. E. (2004a).
Attention to intention. Science, 303, 1208–1210.
Knowlton, B. J., Mangels, J. A., & Squire, L. R. (1996). A neostriatal habit
learning system in humans. Science, 273, 1399–1402.
Koenigs, M., & Tranel, D. (2007). Irrational economic decision-making
after ventromedial prefrontal damage: Evidence from the ultimatum
game. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 951–956.
Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., &
Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action
representations in mirror neurons. Science, 297, 846–848.
Kong, J., Gollub, R. L., Rosman, I. S., Webb, J. M., Vangel, M. G., Kirsch, I.,
et al. (2006). Brain activity associated with expectancy-enhanced placebo analgesia as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging.
The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 381–388.
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005).
Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435, 673–676.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1999). If neuroimaging is the answer, what is the question?
Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 354, 1283–1294.
Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., Costantini-Ferrando, M. F.,
Alpert, N. M., & Spiegel, D. (2000). Hypnotic visual illusion alters
color processing in the brain. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157,
1279–1284.
Krendl, A. C., Macrae, C. N., Kelley, W. M., Fugelsang, J. A., &
Heatherton, T. F. (2006). The good, the bad, and the ugly: An fMRI
investigation of the functional anatomic correlates of stigma. Social
Neuroscience, 1, 5–15.
Krendl, A. C., Richeson, J. A., Kelley, W. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (2008).
The negative consequences of threat: A functional magnetic resonance
imaging investigation of the neural mechanisms underlying women’s
underperformance in math. Psychological Science, 19, 168–175.
Krolak-Salmon, P., Hénaff, M. A., Vighetto, A., Bertrand, O., & Mauguière, F.
(2004). Early amygdala reaction to fear spreading in occipital, temporal, and frontal cortex: A depth electrode ERP study in human. Neuron,
42, 665–676.
Kross, E., Egner, T., Ochsner, K., Hirsch, J., & Downey, G. (2007). Neural
dynamics of rejection sensitivity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
19, 945–956.
CH05.indd 187
Kubota, J. T., & Ito, T. A. (2009). You were always on my mind: How
event-related potentials inform impression formation research.
In T. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 333–346). New York: Psychology Press.
Lau, H. C., Rogers, R. D., Ramnani, N., & Passingham, R. E. (2004b).
Willed action and attention to the selection of action. NeuroImage, 21,
1407–1415.
Lauro, L. J. R., Tettamanti, M., Cappa, S. F., & Papagano, C. (2008). Idiom
comprehension: A prefrontal task? Cerebral Cortex, 18, 162–170.
Lazar, N. A. (2008). The statistical analysis of functional MRI data.
New York: Springer.
LeDoux, J. E., Iwata, J., Cicchetti, P., & Reis, D. J. (1988). Different projections of the central amygdaloid nucleus mediate autonomic and behavioral
correlates of conditioned fear. Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 2517–2529.
Lee, T.-W., Dolan, R. J., & Critchley, H. D. (2008). Controlling emotional
expression: Behavioral and neural correlates of nonimitative emotional responses. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 104–113.
Lee, T.-W., Josephs, O., Dolan, R. J., & Critchley, H. D. (2006). Imitating
expressions: Emotion-specific substrates in facial mimicry. Social
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 122–135.
Leibenluft, E., Gobbini, M. I., Harrison, T., & Haxby, J. V. (2004). Mothers’
neural activation in response to pictures of their children and other children. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 225–232.
Lemche, E., Giampietro, V. P., Surguladze, S. A., Amaro, E. J., Andrew, C. M.,
et al. (2006). Human attachment security is mediated by the amygdala: Evidence from combined fMRI and physophysiological measures.
Human Brain Mapping, 27, 623–635.
Leslie, K. R., Johnson-Frey, S. H., & Grafton, S. T. (2004). Functional
imaging of face and hand imitation: Toward a motor theory of empathy.
NeuroImage, 21, 601–607.
Leube, D. T., Knoblich, G., Erb, M., Grodd, W., Bartels, M., & Kircher, T. T. J.
(2003). The neural correlates of perceiving one’s own movements.
NeuroImage, 20, 2084–2090.
Levesque, J., Eugene, F., Joanette, Y., Paquette, V., Mensour, B., et al.
(2003). Neural circuitry underlying voluntary suppression of sadness.
Biological Psychiatry, 53, 502–510.
Li, C.-S. R., Yan, P., Bergquist, K. L., & Sinha, R. (2007). Greater activation
of the “default” brain regions predicts stop signal errors. NeuroImage,
38, 640–648.
Krueger, F., McCabe, K., Moll, J., Kriegeskorte, N., Zahn, A., & Grafman, J.
(2007). Neural correlates of trust. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, USA, 104, 20084–20089.
Libby, L. K., Eibach, R. P., & Gilovich, T. (2005) Here’s looking at me:
The effect of memory perspective on assessments of personal change.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 50–62.
Kruglanski, A. W., Chun, W. Y., Erb, H. P. Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., &
Spiegel, S. (2003). A parametric unimodel of human judgment. In
J. P. Forgas, K. D. Williams, & W. von Hippel (Eds.) Social judgments:
Implicit and explicit processes (pp. 137–161). New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Libet, B., Wright, E. W., & Gleason, C. A. (1982). Readiness-potentials
proceeding unrestricted “spontaneous” vs. pre-planned voluntary acts.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 54, 322–335.
Lieberman, M. D. (2000). Intuition: A social cognitive neuroscience
approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 109–137.
12/22/09 5:33:58 PM
188
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core
processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 259–289.
Lieberman, M. D. (in press) Why symbolic processing of affect can
disrupt negative affect: Social cognitive and affective neuroscience investigations. In A. Todorov, S. T. Fiske, & D. Prentice (Eds.),
Social neuroscience: Toward understanding the underpinnings of
the social mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
Lieberman, M. D. (2009a). What makes big ideas sticky. In M. Brockman
(Ed.), What’s next: Dispatches on the future of science (pp. 90–103).
New York: Vintage Books.
Lieberman, M. D. (2009b). What zombies can’t do: A social cognitive approach to the irreducibility of reflective consciousness. In
J. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual process and beyond
(pp. 293–316). New York: Oxford University Press.
Lieberman, M. D., Berkman, E. T., & Wager, T. D. (2009). Correlations in
social neuroscience aren’t voodoo: A reply to Vul et al. Perspectives
on Psychological Science, 4, 299–307.
Lieberman, M. D., Chang, G. Y., Chiao, J., Bookheimer, S. Y., &
Knowlton, B. J. (2004). An event-related fMRI study of artificial grammar learning in a balanced chunk strength design. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 16, 427–438.
Lieberman, M. D., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2009). Pains and pleasures of
social life. Science, 323, 890–891.
Lieberman, M. D., Eisenberger, N. I., Crockett, M. J., Tom, S. M.,
Pfeifer, J. H., & Way, B. M. (2007). Putting feelings into words: Affect
labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli.
Psychological Science, 18, 421–428.
Lieberman, M. D., Hariri, A., Jarcho, J. M., Eisenberger, N. I., &
Bookheimer, S. Y. (2005). An fMRI investigation of race-related amygdala activity in African-American and Caucasian-American individuals.
Nature Neuroscience, 8, 720–722.
Lieberman, M. D., Jarcho, J. M., Berman, S. B., Naliboff, B. D.,
Suyenobu, B. Y., Mandelkern, M., et al. (2004). The neural correlates
of placebo effects: A disruption account. NeuroImage, 22, 447–455.
Lieberman, M. D., Jarcho, J. M., & Satpute, A. B. (2004). Evidencebased and intuition-based self-knowledge: An fMRI study. Journal of
Social and Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 421–435.
Lieberman, M. D., Ochsner, K. N., Gilbert, D. T., & Schacter, D. L.
(2001). Do amnesiacs exhibit cognitive dissonance reduction? The role
of explicit memory and attention in attitude change. Psychological
Science, 12, 135–140.
Liljeström, M., Tarkiainen, A., Parviainen, T., Kujala, J., Numminen, J.,
Hiltunenm, J., et al. (2008). Perceiving and naming actions and objects.
NeuroImage, 41, 1132–1141.
Liu, F., Buccino, G., Duzzi, D., Benuzzi, F., Crisi, G., Baraldi, P., et al.
(2008). Neural substrates for observing and imagining non-objectdirected actions. Social Neuroscience, 3, 261–275.
Lorberbaum, J. P., Newman, J. D., Horwitz, A. R., Dubno, J. R.,
Lydiard, R. B., et al. (2002). A potential role for thalamocingulate circuitry in human maternal behavior. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 431–445.
Lotze, M., Heymans, U., Birbaumer, N., Veit, R., Erb, M., Flor, H., et al.
(2006). Differential cerebral activation during observation of expressive gestures and motor acts. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1787–1795.
Lou, H. C., Luber, B., Crupainm, M., Keenan, J. P., Nowak, M., Kjare, T. W.,
et al. (2004). Parietal cortex and representation of the mental self.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 101,
6827–6832.
Luan Phan, K., Fitzgerald, D. A., Nathan, P. J., Moore, G. L., Uhde, T. W., &
Tancer, M. E. (2005). Neural substrates for voluntary suppression
of negative affect: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
Biological Psychiatry, 57, 210–219.
MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000).
Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulated cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288, 1835–1838.
CH05.indd 188
MacDonald, G., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social exclusion hurt?
The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychological
Bulletin, 131, 202–223.
Machado, C. J., Snyder, A. Z., Cherry, S. R., Lavenex, P., & Amaral, D. G.
(2008). Effects of neonatal amygdala or hippocampus lesions on resting brain metabolism in the macaque monkey: A microPET imaging
study. NeuroImage, 39, 832–846.
MacLean P. D., & Newman, J. D. (1988). Role of midline frontolimbic
cortex in production of the isolation call of squirrel monkeys. Brain
Research, 45, 111–123.
Macrae, C. N., Moran, J. M., Heatherton, T. F., Banfield, J. F., & Kelley, W. M.
(2004). Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. Cerebral
Cortex, 14, 647–654.
Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological
Review, 50, 370–396.
Mason, M. F., Banfield, J. F., Macrae, C. N. (2004). Thinking about
actions: The neural substrates of person knowledge. Cerebral Cortex,
14, 209–214.
Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., &
Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: The default network and
stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315, 393–395.
Masten, C. L., Telzer, E. H., & Eisenberger, N. I. (under review). Being the
target of racial discrimination: An fMRI study.
McCabe, K., Houser, D., Ryan, L., Smith, V., & Trouard, T. (2001).
A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal
exchange. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA,
98, 11832–11835.
McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Gore, J. C., & Allison, T. (1997). Face-specific
processing in the human fusiform gyrus. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 9, 605–610.
McClure, S. M., Li, J., Tomlin, D., Cypert, K. S., Montague, L. M., &
Montague, P. R. (2004). Neural correlates of behavioral preference for
culturally familiar drinks. Neuron, 44, 379–387.
McKiernan, K. A., Kaufman, J. N., Kucera-Thompson, J., & Binder, J. R.
(2003). A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced
deactivation in functional neuroimaging. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 15, 394–408.
McRae, K., Ochsner, K. N., Mauss, I. B., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Gross, J. J. (2008).
Gender differences in emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive
reappraisal. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 11, 143–162.
McRae, K., Reiman, E. M., Fort, C. L., Chen, K., & Lane, R. D. (2008).
Asssociation between trait emotional awareness and dorsal anterior
cingulate activity during emotion is arousal-dependent. NeuroImage,
41, 648–655.
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society (C. W. Morris, Ed.). Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Mendez, M. F., Anderson, E., & Shapira, J. S. (2005). An investigation
of moral judgement in frontotemporal dementia. Cognitive Behavioral
Neurology, 18, 193–197.
Milne, E., & Grafman, J. (2001). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions
in humans eliminate implicit gender stereotyping. Journal of
Neuroscience, 21, 1–6.
Minagawa-Kawai, Y., Matsuoka, S., Dan, I., Naoi, N., Nakamura, K., &
Kojima, S. (2008). Prefrontal activation associated with social attachment: Facial-emotion recognition in mothers and infants. Cerebral
Cortex, 19, 284–292.
Mitchell, J. P. (2008). Activity in the right temporo-parietal junction is not
selective for theory of mind. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 262–271.
Mitchell, J. P., Banaji, M. R., & Macrae, C. N. (2005a). General and specific contributions of the medial prefrontal cortex to knowledge about
mental states. NeuroImage, 28, 757–762.
Mitchell, J. P., Banaji, M. R., & Macrae, C. N. (2005b). The link between
social cognition and self-referential thought in the medial prefrontal
cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1306–1315.
12/22/09 5:33:59 PM
References 189
Mitchell, J. P., Cloutier, J., Banaji, M. R., & Macrae, C. N. (2006). Medial
prefrontal dissociations during processing of trait diagnostic and
nondiagnostic person information. Social Cognitive and Affective
Neuroscience, 1, 49–55.
Mitchell, J. P., Heatherton, T. F., Kelley, W. M., Wyland, C. L.,
Wegner, D. M., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Separating sustained from
transient aspects of cognitive control during thought suppression.
Psychological Science, 18, 292–297.
Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, C. N., & Banaji, M. R. (2004). Encoding-specific
effects of social cognition on the neural correlates of subsequent memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 4912–4917.
Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, C. N., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). Dissociable medial
prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others.
Neuron, 50, 655–663.
Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I. E., & Grafman, J. (2002).
Functional networks in emotional moral and nonmoral social judgments. NeuroImage, 16, 696–703.
Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal
reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.
Noguchi, Y., Kancoke, Y., Kakigi, R., Tanabe, H. C., & Sadato, N. (2005).
Role of the superior temporal region in human visual motion perception. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1592–1601.
Nomura, M., Ohira, H., Haneda, K., Iidaka, T., Sadato, N., Okada, T.,
et al. (2004). Functional association of the amygdala and ventral prefrontal cortex during cognitive evaluation of facial expressions primed
by masked angry faces: An event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 21,
352–363.
Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Eslinger, P. J., Bramati, I. E., MouraoMiranda, J., Andreiuolo, P. A., et al. (2002). The neural correlates of
moral sensitivity: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions. Journal of Neuroscience, 22,
2730–2736.
Noppeney, U., & Price, C. J. (2004). Retrieval of abstract semantics.
NeuroImage, 22, 164–170.
Moll, J., Krueger, F., Zahn, R., Pardini, M., de Oliveira-Souza, R., &
Grafman, J. (2006). Human fronto-mesolimbic networks guide decision about charitable donation. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences, USA, 103, 15623–15628.
Ochsner, K. N. (2004). Current directions in social cognitive psychology.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 254–258.
Molnar-Szakacs, I., Kaplan, J., Greenfield, P. M., & Iacoboni, M. (2006).
Observing complex action sequences: The role of the fronto-parietal
mirror neuron system. NeuroImage, 33, 923–935.
Montgomery, K. J., Isenberg, N., & Haxby, J. V. (2007). Communicative
hand gestures and object-directed hand movements activated the mirror
neuron systems. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2,
114–122.
Moran, J. M., Macrae, S. N., Heatherton, T. F., Wyland, C. L., &
Kelley, W. M. (2006). Neuroanaotomical evidence for distinct cognitive
and affective components of self. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
18, 1586–1594.
Moriguchi, Y., Ohnishi, T., Lane, R. D., Maeda, M., Mori, T., Nemoto, K.,
et al. (2006). Impaired self-awareness and theory of mind: An fMRI
study of mentalizing in alexithymia. NeuroImage, 32, 1472–1482.
Morita, T., Itakura, S., Saito, D. N., Nakashita, S., Harada, T., Kochiyama, T.,
et al. (2008). The role of the right prefrontal cortex in self-evaluation
of the face: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Social
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 20, 342–355.
Morris, J. P., Pelphrey, K. A., & McCarthy, G. (2006). Occipitotemporal
activation evoked by the perception of human bodies is modulated by the presence or absence of the face. Neuropsychologia, 44,
1919–1927.
Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., Rowland, D., Young, A. W.,
Calder, A. J., et al. (1996). A differential neural response in the human
amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature, 383, 812–815.
Morris, J. S., Öhman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Conscious and unconscious
emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature, 393, 467–470.
Morrison, I., Peelen, M. V., & Downing, P. E. (2007). The sight of others’
pain modulates motor processing in human cingulate cortex. Cerebral
Cortex, 17, 2214–2222.
CH05.indd 189
Motluck, A. (2001). Read my mind. New Scientist, 169, 22.
Newmann-Norlund, R. D., van Schie, H. T., van Zuijilen, M. J., &
Bekkering, H. (2007). The mirror neuron system is more active during
complementary compared with imitative action. Nature Neuroscience,
10, 817–818.
Noriuchi, M., Kikuchi, Y., & Senoo, A. (2008). The functional neuroanatomy
of maternal love: Mother ’s response to infant’s attachment behaviors.
Biological Psychiatry, 63, 415–423.
Ochsner, K. N. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: Historical development, core principles, and future promise. In Kruglanksi, A., &
Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Social Psychology: A Handbook of Basic
Principles (2nd ed., pp. 39–66). New York: Guilford Press.
Ochsner, K. N., Beer, J. S., Robertson, E. R., Cooper, J. C., Gabrieli, J. D. E.,
Kihstrom, J. F. (2005). The neural correlates of direct and reflected
self-knowledge. NeuroImage, 28, 797–814.
Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002).
Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 1215–1229.
Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 242–249.
Ochsner, K. N., Hughes, B. L., Robertson, E., Cooper, J. C., & Gabrieli, J.
(2009). Neural systems supporting the control of cognitive and affective conflict. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1842–1855.
Ochsner, K. N., Knierim, K., Ludlow, D. H., Haneline, J., Ramachandran, T.,
Glover, G., et al. (2004). Reflecting upon feelings: Systems supporting the attribution of emotion to self and other. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 16, 1746–1772.
Ochsner, K. N., & Lieberman, M. D. (2001). The emergence of social cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 56, 717–734.
Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Chopra, S.,
Gabrieli, J. D. E., et al. (2004). For better or for worse: Neural systems
supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion.
NeuroImage, 23, 483–499.
Ochshner, K. N., & Schacter, D. L. (2000). A social cognitive neuroscience
approach to emotion and memory. In J. C. Borod (Ed.), The neuropsychology of emotion (pp. 163–193). New York: Oxford University
Press.
Ochsner, K. N., Zaki, J., Hanelin, J., Ludlow, D. H., Knierim, K.,
Ramachandran, T., et al. (2008). Your pain or mine? Common and
distinct neural systems supporting the perception of pain in self
and other. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3, 144–160.
Mosconi, M. W., Mack, P. B., McCarthy, G., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2005).
Taking an “intentional stance” on eye-gaze shifts: A functional neuroimaging study of social perception in children. NeuroImage, 27,
247–252.
O’Connor, M.-F., Wellisch, D. K., Stanton, A. L., Eisenberger, N. I.,
Irwin, M. R., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008). Craving love? Enduring
grief activates brain’s reward center. NeuroImage, 42, 969–972.
Most, S. B., Chun, M. M., Johnson, M. R., & Kiehl, K. A. (2006).
Attentional modulation of the amygdala varies with personality.
NeuroImage, 31, 934–944.
O’Doherty, J., Winston, J., Critchley, H., Perrett, D., Burt, D. M., &
Dolan, R. J. (2003). Beauty in a smile: The role of medial orbitofrontal
cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 41, 147–155.
12/22/09 5:33:59 PM
190
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
Ohira, H., Nomura, M., Ichikawa, N., Isowa, T., Iidaka, T., Sato, A., et al.
(2006). Associations of neural and physiological responses during voluntary emotion suppression. NeuroImage, 29, 721–733.
Ohnishi, T., Moriguchi, Y., Matsuda, H., Mori, T., Hirakata, M., Imabayashi, E.,
et al. (2004). The neural network for the mirror system and mentalizing in normally developed children: An fMRI study. NeuroReport,
15, 1482–1487.
Ortigue, S., Bianchi-Demicheli, F., Hamilton, A. F. de C., & Grafton, S. T.
(2007). The neural basis of love as a subliminal prime: An event-related
functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 19, 1218–1230.
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human
and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.
Panksepp, J., Herman, B., Conner, R., Bishop, P., & Scott, J. P. (1978). The
biology of social attachments: Opiates alleviate separation distress.
Biological Psychiatry, 13, 607–618.
Pasley, B. N., Mayes, L. C., & Schultz, R. T. (2004). Subcortical discrimination of unperceived objects during binocular rivalry. Neuron, 42,
163–172.
Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and
controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 81, 181–192.
Peelen, M. P., Atkinson, A. P., Andersson, F., & Vuilleumier, P. (2007).
Emotional modulation of body-selective visual areas. Social Cognitive
and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 274–283.
Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., & McCarthy, G. (2005). Neural basis of eye
gaze processing deficits in autism. Brain, 128, 1038–1048.
Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., McCarthy, G., & LaBar, K. S. (2007).
Perception of dynamic changes in facial affect and identity in autism.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 140–149.
Pelphrey, K. A., Singerman, J. D., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (2003).
Brain activation evoked by perception of gaze shifts: The influence of
context. Neuropsychologia, 41, 156–170.
Pelphrey, K. A., Viola, R. J., & McCarthy, G. (2004). When strangers pass:
Processing of mutual and averted social gaze in the superior temporal
sulcus. Psychological Science, 15, 598–603.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Beall, S. K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic event:
Toward an understanding of inhibition of disease. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 95, 274–281.
Perner, J., & Aichhorn, M. (2006). Thinking of mental and other representations: The role of left and right temporo-parietal junction. Social
Neuroscience, 1, 245–258.
Pessoa, L., McKenna, M., Gutierrez, E., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002).
Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 99, 11458–11463.
Petrovic, P., Dietrich, T., Fransson, P., Andersson, J., Carlsson, K., & Igvar, M.
(2005). Placebo in emotion processing—induced expectations of
anxiety relief activate a generalized modulatory network. Neuron, 46,
957–969.
Pfeifer, J. H., Iacoboni, M., Mazziotta, J. C., & Dapretto, M. (2008).
Mirroring others’ emotions relates to empathy and interpersonal competence in children. NeuroImage, 39, 2076–2085.
Pfeifer, J. H., Lieberman, M. D., & Dapretto, M. (2007). “I know you are
but what am I?!”: Neural bases of self- and social knowledge in children and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1323–1337.
Pfeifer, J. H., Masten, C. L., Borofsky, L. A., Dapretto, M.,
Lieberman, M. D., & Fuligni, A. J. (2009). Neural correlates of
direct and reflect self-appraisals in adolescents and adults. Child
Development, 80, 1016–1038.
Phelps, E. A., Cannistraci, C. J., & Cunningham, W. A. (2003). Intact performance on an indirect measure of race bias following amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia, 41, 203–208.
CH05.indd 190
Phelps, E. A., O’Connor, K. J., Cunningham, W. A., Funayama, E. S.,
Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., et al. (2000). Performance on indirect
measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 729–738.
Pierno, A. C., Tubaldi, F., Turella, L., Grossi, P., Barachino, L., Gallo, P.,
et al. (2009). Neurofunctional modulation of brain regions by the
observation of pointing and grasping actions. Cerebral Cortex, 19,
367–374.
Platek, S. M., Keenan, J. P., Gallup, G. G., Jr., & Mohamed, F. (2004).
Where am I? The neurological correlates of self and other. Cognitive
Brain Research, 19, 114–122.
Platek, S. M., Loughead, J. W., Gur, R. C., Busch, S., Ruparel, K., Phend, N.,
et al. (2006). Neural substrates for functionally discriminating self-face
from personally familiar faces. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 91–98.
Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 59–63.
Poldrack, R. A., Clark, J., Pare-Blagoev, E. J., Shohamy, D., Creso Moyano, J.,
Myers, C., et al. (2001). Interactive memory systems in the human
brain. Nature, 414, 546–550.
Postle, B. R. (2006). Working memory as an emergent property of the mind
and brain. Neuroscience, 139, 23–38.
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory
of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.
Preston, A. D., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Grabowski, T. J., Stansfield, R. B.,
Mehta, S., et al. (2007). The neural substrates of cognitive empathy.
Social Neuroscience, 2, 254–275.
Preston, C., & Newport, R. (2008). Misattribution of movement agency following right parietal TMS. Social Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 26–32.
Price, D. D. (2000). Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective
dimension of pain. Science, 288, 1769–1772.
de Quervain, D. J.-F., Fischbacher, U., Treyer, V., Schellhammer, M.,
Schnyder, U., Buck, A., et al. (2004). The neural basis of altruistic
punishment. Science, 305, 1254–1258.
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J.,
Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 676–682.
Rainville, P., Duncan, G. H., Price, D. D., Carrier, B., & Bushnell, M. C.
(1998). Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulated but not
somatosensory cortex. Science, 277, 968–971.
Ranklin, K. P., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Allison, S. C., Stanley, C. M., Glenn, S.,
et al. (2006). Structural anatomy of empathy in neurodegenerative disease. Brain, 129, 2945–2956.
Ranote, S., Elliott, R., Abel, K. M., Mitchell, R., Deakin, J. F. W., &
Appleby, L. (2004). The neural basis of maternal responsiveness to
infants: An fMRI study. Neuroreport, 15, 1825–1829.
Richeson, J. A., Baird, A. A., Gordon, H. L., Heatherton, T. F., Wyland, C. L.,
Trawalter, S., et al. (2003). An fMRI investigation of the impact of
interracial contact on executive function. Nature Neuroscience, 16,
1323–1328.
Richeson, J. A., Todd, A. R., & Trawalter, S. (2008). Eye-gaze direction modulates race-relate amygdala activity. Group Processes and
Intergroup Relations, 11, 233–246.
Rilling, J. K., Dagenais, J. E., Goldsmith, D. R., Glenn, A. L., & Pagnoni, G.
(2008). Social cognitive neural networks during in-group and outgroup interactions. NeuroImage, 41, 1447–1461.
Rilling, J. K., Goldsmith, D. R., Glenn, A. L., Jairam, M. R., Elfenbein, H. A.,
Dangenais, J. E., et al. (2008). The neural correlates of the affective
response to unreciprocated cooperation. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1256–1266.
Rilling, J. K., Gutman, D. A., Zeh, T. R., Pagnoni, G., Berns, G. S., &
Kilts, C. D. (2002). A neural basis for social cooperation. Neuron, 35,
395–405.
Rilling, J. K., Sanfey, A. G., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. A.
(2004). The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal
interactions. NeuroImage, 22, 1694–1703.
12/22/09 5:34:00 PM
References 191
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual
Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.
Ronquillo, J., Denson, T. F., Lickel, B., Lu, Z.-L., Nandy, A., & Maddox, K. B.
(2007). The effects of skin tone on race-related amygdala activity:
An fMRI investigation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,
2, 39–44.
Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., Rogers, P. L., & Archer, D.
(1979). Sensitivity to nonverbal communication: The PONS Test.
Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The “false consensus effect”: An
egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 13, 279–301.
Ross, M., & Sicoly, F. (1975). Egocentric biases in availability and attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 322–337.
Ruby, P., & Decety, J. (2003). What you believe versus what you think
they believe: A neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective-taking.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 2475–2480.
Ruby, P., & Decety, J. (2004). How would you feel versus how do you
think she would feel? A neuroimaging study of perspective-taking with
social emotions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 988–999.
Saarela, M. V., & Hari, R. (2008). Listening to humans walking together
activates the social brain circuitry. Social Neuroscience, 3, 401–409.
Samson, D., Apperley, I. A., Chiavarino, C., & Humphreys, G. W. (2004).
Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone
else’s belief. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 499–500.
Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Kathirgamanathan, U., & Humphreys, G. W.
(2005). Seeing it my way: A case of a selective deficit in inhibiting selfperspective. Brain, 128, 1102–1111.
Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D.
(2003). The neural basis of decision-making in the ultimatum game.
Science, 300, 1755–1758.
Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2006). Integrating automatic and
controlled processing into neurocognitive models of social cognition.
Brain Research, 1079, 86-97.
Saxe, R., Brett, M., & Kanwisher, N. (2006). Divide and conquer:
A defense of functional localizers. NeuroImage, 30, 1088–1096.
Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people:
The role of temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind.” NeuroImage,
19, 1835–1842.
Saxe, R., Moran, J. M., Scholz, J., & Gabrieli, J. (2006). Overlapping and
non-overlapping brain regions for theory of mind and self reflection
in individual subjects. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,
1, 229–234.
Saxe, R., & Powell, L. J. (2006). It’s the thought that counts: Specific brain
regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychological Science,
17, 692–699.
Saxe, R., Schulz, L. E., & Jiang, Y. V. (2006). Reading minds versus following rules: Dissociating theory of mind and executive control in the
brain. Social Neuroscience, 1, 284–298.
Saxe, R., & Wexler, A. (2005). Making sense of another mind: The
role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia, 43,
1391–1399.
Saygin, A. P. (2007). Superior temporal and premotor brain areas necessary
for biological motion perception. Brain, 130, 2452–2461.
Schaefer, A., Collette, F., Philippot, P., der Linden, M. V., Laureys, S.,
Delfiore, G., et al. (2003). Neural correlates of “hot” and “cold” emotional processing: A multilevel approach to the functional anatomy of
emotion. NeuroImage, 18, 938–949.
Schmitz, T. W., & Johnson, S. C. (2006). Self-appraisal decisions evoke
dissociated dorsal-ventral aMPFC networks. NeuroImage, 30,
1050–1058.
Schmitz, T. W., Kawahara-Baccus, T. N., & Johnson, S. C. (2004).
Metacognitive evaluation, self-relevance, and the right prefrontal
cortex. NeuroImage, 22, 941–947.
CH05.indd 191
Schnyer, D. M., Nicholls, L., & Verfaellie, M. (2005). The role of VMPC in
metamemorial judgments of content retrievability. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 17, 832–846.
Schulte-Rüther, M., Markowitsch, H. J., Fink, G. R., & Piefke, M. (2007).
Mirror neuron and theory of mind mechanisms involved in face-toface interactions: A functional magnetic resonance imaging approach
to empathy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1354–1372.
Schultz, J., Imamizu, H., Kawato, M., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Activation
of the human superior temporal gyrus during observation of goal attribution by intention objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16,
1695–1705.
Schwartz, C. E., Wright, C. I., Shin, L. M., Kagan, J., Whalen, P. J.,
McMullin, K. G., et al. (2003). Differential amygdalar response to
novel versus newly familiar neutral faces: A functional MRI probe
developed for studying inhibited temperament. Biological Psychiatry,
53, 854–862.
Scott, A. A., Dapretto, M., Ghahremani, D., Poldrack, R. A., &
Bookheimer, S. Y. (under review). Social learning deficits relate to
decreased neural response to rewards.
Seger, C. A., Stone, M., & Keenan, J. P. (2004). Cortical activations during
judgments about the self and another person. Neuropsychologia, 42,
1168–1177.
Segerstrom, S. C., Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., & Fahey, J. L. (1998).
Optimism is associated with mood, coping, and immune change in
response to stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
1646–1655.
Seifritz, E., Esposito, F., Neuhoff, J. G., Luthi, A., Mustovic, H., Dammann, G.,
et al. (2003). Differential sex-independent amygdala response to
infant crying and laughing in parents versus nonparents. Biological
Psychiatry, 54, 1367–1375.
Semendeferi, K., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., Armstrong, E., & Van
Hoesen, G. W. (2001). Evolution of the hominoid prefrontal cortex:
Imaging and quantitative analysis of area 10. American Journal of
Physical Anthropology, 114, 224–241.
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Dissociable prefrontal
networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: A lesion study.
Neuropsychologia, 45, 3054–3067.
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Lester, H., Chisin, R., Israel, O., Bar-Shalom, R.,
Peretz, A., et al. (2005). The neural correlates of understanding the other ’s distress: A positron emission tomography investigation of accurate
empathy. NeuroImage, 27, 468–472.
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tibi-Elhanany, Y., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2006). The
ventromedial prefrontal cortex is involved in understanding affective but not cognitive theory of mind stories. Social Neuroscience,
1, 149–166.
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Berger, B. D., & Aharon-Peretz, J.
(2003). Characterization of empathy deficits following prefrontal brain
damage: The role of the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 324–337.
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Berger, B. D., Goldsher, D., &
Aharon-Peretz, J. (2005). Impaired “affective theory of mind” is
associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cognitive and
Behavioral Neurology, 18, 55–67.
Shaw, P., Lawrence, E., Bramham, J., Brierley, B., Radbourne, C., &
David, A. S. (2007). A prospective study of the effects of anterior
temporal lobectomy on emotion recognition and theory of mind.
Neuropsychologia, 45, 2783–2790.
Shaw, P., Lawrence, E. J., Radbourne, C., Bramham, J., Polkey, C. E., &
David, A. S. (2004). The impact of early and late damage to the human
amygdala on “theory of mind” reasoning. Brain, 127, 1535–1548.
Shimada, S., Hiraki, K., & Oda, I. (2005). The parietal role in the sense of
self-ownership with temporal discrepancy between visual and proprioceptive feedbacks. NeuroImage, 24, 1225–1232.
12/22/09 5:34:00 PM
192
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
Shulman, G. L., Fiez, J. A., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R. L., Miezin, F. M.,
Raichle, M. E., et al. (1997). Common blood flow changes across
visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 9, 648–663.
Simmons, A., Matthews, S. C., Feinstein, J. S., Hitchcock, C., Paulus, M. P., &
Stein, M. B. (2008). Anxiety vulnerability is associated with altered
anterior cingulate response to an affective appraisal task. NeuroReport,
19, 1033–1037.
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R., & Frith, C. D.
(2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1162.
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., &
Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathetic neural responses are modulated by the
perceived fairness of others. Nature, 439, 466–469.
Smith, W. (1945). The functional significance of the rostral cingular cortex as revealed by its responses to electrical excitation. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 8, 241–255.
Sugiura, M., Watanabe, J., Maeda, Y., Matsue, Y., Fukuda, H., &
Kawashima, R. (2005). Cortical mechanisms of visual self-recognition.
NeuroImage, 24, 143–149.
Sui, J., & Han, S. (2007). Self-construal priming modulates neural substrates of self-awareness. Psychological Science, 18, 861–866.
Tabibnia, G., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008). The sunny side
of fairness: Preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry). Psychological
Science, 19, 339–347.
Tai, Y. F., Scherfler, C., Brooks, C. D., Sawamoto, N., & Castiello, U.
(2004). The human premotor cortex is “mirror” only for biological
actions. Current Biology, 14, 117–120.
Tetlock, P. E., & Levi, A. (1982). Attribution bias: On the inconclusiveness
of the cognition-motivation debate. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 18, 68–88.
Thurstone, L. L. (1928). Attitudes can be measured. American Journal of
Sociology, 33, 529–554.
Sommerville, L. H., Heatherton, T. F., & Kelley, W. M. (2006). ACC
responds differentially to expectancy violation and social rejection.
Nature Neuroscience, 9, 1007–1008.
Trepel, C., Fox, C. R., Poldrack, R. A. (2005). Prospect theory on the brain?
Toward a cognitive neuroscience of decision under risk. Cognitive
Brain Research, 23, 34–50.
Soon, C. S., Brass, M., Heinze, H.-J., & Haynes, J.-D. (2008). Unconscious
determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience,
11, 543–545.
Tsakiris, M., Costantini, M., & Haggard, P. (2008). The role of the right
temporo-parietal junction in maintaining a coherent sense of one’s
body. Neuropsychologia, 46, 3014–3018.
Spence, S. A., Brooks, D. J., Hirsch, S. R., Liddle, P. F., & Grasby, P. M.
(1997). A PET study of voluntary movement in schizophrenic patients
experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control). Brain,
120, 1997–2011.
Turner, M. S., Simons, J. S., Gilbert, S. J., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W.
(2008). Distinct roles for lateral and medial rostral prefrontal
cortex in source monitoring of perceived and imagined events.
Neuropsychologia, 46, 1442–1453.
Spiers, H. J., & Maguire, E. (2006). Spontaneous mentalizing during an interactive real word task: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1674–1682.
Tyler, T. R. (1991). Psychological models of the justice motive: Antecedents
of distributive and procedural justice. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 67, 850–863.
Spunt, R. P., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (in press). Identifying the
what, why, and how of an observed action: An fMRI study of mentalizing and mechanizing during action observation. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience.
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual
test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 69, 797–811.
Steele, C., Spencer, S., & Lynch, M. (1993). Self-image resilience and dissonance: The role of affirmational resources. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 64, 885–896.
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., Calder, A., Keane, J., & Young, A. (2003).
Acquired theory of mind impairments in individuals with bilateral
amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia, 41, 209–220.
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 640–656.
Strathearn, L., Li, J., Fonagy, P., & Montague, P. R. (2008). What’s in a smile?
Maternal brain responses to infant facial cues. Pediatrics, 122, 40–51.
Straube, T., Mentzel, H., & Miltner, W. H. R. (2007). Waiting for spiders: Brain activation during anticipatory anxiety in spider phobics.
NeuroImage, 37, 1427–1436.
Stuss, D. T., Floden, D., Alexander, M. P., Levine, B., & Katz, D. (2001).
Stroop performance in focal lesion patients: Dissociation of processes
and frontal lobe lesion location. Neuropsychologia, 39, 771–786.
Uddin, L. Q., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Zaidel, E., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). rTMS
to the right IPL disrupts self-other discrimination. Social Cognitive
and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 65–71.
Urgesi, C., Moro, V., Candid, M., & Aglioti, S. M. (2006). Mapping implied
body actions in the human motor system. Journal of Neuroscience, 26,
7942–7949.
Urry, H. L., van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., Kalin, N. H., Thurow, M. E.,
Schaefer, H. S., et al. (2006). Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal
cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and
predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults.
Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 4415–4425.
Vaina, L. M., Solomon, J., Chowdhury, S., Sinha, P., & Belliveau, J. W.
(2001). Functional neuroanatomy of biological motion perception in
humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 98,
11656–11661.
Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they’re
doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological
Review, 94, 3–15.
Van der Gaag, C., Minderaa, R. B., & Keysers, C. (2007). The BOLD
signal in the amygdala does not differentiate between dynamic facial
expressions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 93–103.
Sugiura, M., Kawashima, R., Nakamura, K., Okada, K., Kato, T.,
Nakamura, A., et al. (2000). Passive and active recognition of one’s
own face. NeuroImage, 11, 36–48.
van Duynslaeger, M., van Overwalle, F., & Verstraeten, E. (2007).
Electrophysiological time course and brain areas of spontaneous and intentional trait inferences. Social Cognitive and Affective
Neuroscience, 2, 174–188.
van Overwalle, F. (2009). Social cognition and the brain: A meta-analysis.
Human Brain Mapping, 30, 829–858.
Sugiura, M., Sassa, Y., Jeong, H., Horie, K., Sato, S., & Kawashima, R.
(2008). Face-specific and domain-general characteristics of cortical
responses during self-recognition. NeuroImage, 42, 414–422.
van’t Wout, M., Kahn, R. S., Sanfey, A. G., & Aleman, A. (2005). Repetitive
transcranial magnetic stimulation over the right dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex affects strategic decision-making. NeuroReport, 16, 1849–1852.
Sugiura, M., Sassa, Y., Watanabe, J., Akitsuki, Y., Maeda, Y., Matsue, Y.,
et al. (2006). Cortical mechanisms of person perception: Recognition
of famous and personally familiar names. NeuroImage, 31, 853–860.
Vanderwal, T., Hunyadi, E., Grupe, D. W., Connors, C. M., & Schultz, R. T.
(2008). Self, mother, and abstract other: An fMRI study of reflective
social processing. NeuroImage, 41, 1437–1446.
Stuss, D. T., Gallup, G. G., Jr., & Alexander, M. P. (2001). The frontal lobes
are necessary for “theory of mind.” Brain, 124, 279–286.
CH05.indd 192
12/22/09 5:34:01 PM
References 193
Vincent, J. L., Patel, G. H., Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Baker, J. T., Van
Essen, D. C., et al. (2007). Intrinsic functional architecture in the
anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature, 447, 83–86.
Vogt, S., Buccino, G., Wohlschläger, A. M., Canessa, N., Shah, N. J., Zilles, K.,
et al. (2007). Prefrontal involvement in imitation learning of hand
actions: Effects of practice and expertise. NeuroImage, 37, 1371–1383.
Vollm, B. A., Taylor, A. N. W., Richardson, P., Corcoran, R., Stirling, J.,
McKie, S., et al. (2006). Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and
empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. NeuroImage, 29, 90–98.
Wheatley, T., Milleville, S. C., & Martin, A. (2007). Understanding animate agents: Distinct roles for the social network and mirror systems.
Psychological Science, 18, 469–474.
Wheeler, M. E., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). Controlling racial prejudice:
Social-cognitive goals affect amygdala and stereotype activation.
Psychological Sciences, 16, 56–63.
Vrticka, P., Andersson, F., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P.
(2008). Individual attachment style modulates human amygdala and
striatum activation during social appraisal. PLoS One, 6, e2868.
Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J.-P., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G.
(2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of
seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40, 655–664.
Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Clarke, K., Husain, M., Driver, J., &
Dolan, R. J. (2002). Neural responses to emotional faces with and
without awareness; Event-related fMRI in a parietal patient with visual
extinction and spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia, 40, 2156–2166.
Wicker, B., Perrett, D. I., Baron-Cohen, S., & Decety, J. (2003). Being the target of another’s emotion: A PET study. Neuropsychologia, 41, 139–146.
Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of
attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: An eventrelated fMRI study. Neuron, 30, 829–841.
Wiethoff, S., Wildgruber, D., Kreifekts, B., Becker, H., Herbert, C., Grodd, W.,
et al. (2007). Cerebral processing of emotional prosody—influence of
acoustic parameters and arousal. NeuroImage, 39, 885–893.
Vuilleumier, P., Mohr, C., Valenza, N., Wetzel, C., & Landis, T. (2003).
Hyperfamiliarity for unknown faces after left lateral temporo-occipital
venous infarction: A double dissociation with prosopagnosia. Brain,
126, 889–907.
Wildgruber, D., Pihan, H., Ackermann, H., Erb, M., & Grodd, W. (2002).
Dynamic brain activation during processing of emotional intonation: Influence of acoustic parameters, emotional valence, and sex.
NeuroImage, 15, 856–869.
Vuilleumier, P., Richardson, M. P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J.
(2004). Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation
during emotional face processing. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 1271–1278.
Wildgruber, D., Riecker, A., Hertich, I., Erb, M., Grodd, W., Ethofer, T., et
al. (2005). Identification of emotional intonation evaluated by fMRI.
NeuroImage, 24, 1233–1241.
Vul, E., Harris, C., Winkielman, P., & Pashler, H. (2009). Puzzlingly high
correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 274–290.
Williams, K. D. (2007). Ostracism. Annual Review of Psychology, 58,
425–452.
Wager, T. D., Davidson, M. L., Hughes, B. L., Lindquist, M. A., &
Ochsner, K. N. (2008). Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron, 59, 1037–1050.
Wicker, B., Ruby, P., Royet, J.-P., & Fonlupt, P. (2003). A relation between
rest and the self in the brain? Brain Research Reviews, 43, 224–230.
Williams, K. D., Bernieri, F., Faulkner, S., Grahe, J., & Gada-Jain, N.
(2000). The Scarlet Letter Study: Five days of social ostracism. Journal
of Personal and Interpersonal Loss, 5, 19–63.
Wager, T. D., Rilling, J. K., Smith, E. E., Sokoloik, A., Casey, K. L.,
Davidson, R. J., et al. (2004). Placebo-induced changes in fMRI in the
anticipation and experience of pain. Science, 303, 1162–1167.
Williams, M. A., Morris, A. P., McGlone, F., Abbott, D. F., & Mattingley, J. B.
(2004). Amygdala responses to fearful and happy facial expressions under conditions of binocular suppression. The Journal of
Neuroscience, 24, 2898–2904.
Wagner, A. D., Schacter, D. L., Rotte, M., Koutstaal, W., Maril, A., Dale, A. M.,
et al. (1998). Building memories: Remembering and forgetting of verbal
experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science, 281, 1188–1191.
Willingham, D. T., & Dunn, E. W. (2003). What neuroimaging and brain
localization can do, cannot do, and should not do for social psychology.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 662–671.
Wakusawa, K., Sugiura, M., Sassa, Y., Jeong, H., Horie, K., Sato, S., et al.
(2007). Comprehension of implicit meanings in social situations involving irony: A functional MRI study. NeuroImage, 37, 1417–1426.
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and
constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.
Walter, H., Adenzato, M., Ciaramidaro, A., Enici, I., Pia, L., & Bara, B. G.
(2004). Understanding intentions in social interactions: The role of the
anterior paracingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16,
1854–1863.
Winston, J. S., Strange, B. A., O’Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002).
Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 277–283.
Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006a). Developmental
changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. Social
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 107–121.
Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006b). Neural basis
of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody
and context. Brain, 129, 932–943.
Way, B. M., Taylor, S. E., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2009). Variation in the
k-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) is associated with dispositional and
neural sensitivity to social rejection. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Science. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812612106.
Wegner, D. M., & Bargh, J. A. (1998). Control and automaticity in social
life. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of
social psychology (pp. 446–496). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Weissman, D. H., Roberts, K. C., Visscher, K. M., & Woldorff, M. G.
(2006). The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nature
Neuroscience, 9, 971–978.
CH05.indd 193
Whalen, P. J., Raunch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., &
Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. The
Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 411–418.
Wraga, M., Helt, M., Jacobs, E., & Sullivan, K. (2006). Neural basis of
stereotype-induced shifts in women’s mental rotation performance.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 12–19.
Young, L., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Saxe, R. (2007). The neural
basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 8235–8240.
Zaki, J., Ochsner, K. N., Hanelin, J., Wager, T. D., & Mackey, S. C. (2007).
Different circuits for different pain: Patterns of functional connectivity
reveal distinct networks for processing pain in self and others. Social
Neuroscience, 2, 276–291.
Zhang, L., Tiangang, Z., Zhang, J., Liu, Z., Fan, J., & Ying, Z. (2006). In
search of the Chinese self: An fMRI study. Science in China, 49, 89–96.
Zhu, Y., Zhang, L., Fan, L., & Han, S. (2007). Neural basis of cultural
influence on self-representation. NeuroImage, 34, 1310–1316.
Zysset, S., Huber, O., Ferstl, E., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). The anterior frontomedian cortex and evaluative judgment: An fMRI study.
NeuroImage, 15, 983–991.
12/22/09 5:34:02 PM