LIFE SPAN DEVELOPMENT
UNIT I
Lesson 1 - Foundations of Development
Lesson 2 - Recent Theoretical Perspectives - Methods
Lesson 3 - Genetic Foundation - Reproductive Choices - Environmental Contexts
for Development
Lesson 4 - Prenatal Development - Characteristics - Importance of Conception -
Prenatal Environmental Influences
Lesson 5 - Attitudes of Significant People - Medical Interventions - Hazard during
the Prenatal Period
Unit II
Lesson 6- Infancy and Toddlerhood -Characteristics - Adjustments - Learning
Capacities
Lesson7- Piaget’s Cognitive Developmental Theory- Information Processing -
Individual Differences in Early Mental Development
Lesson 8- Language Development - Developmental Tasks
Lesson 9- Erikson's Theory of Infant and Toddler Personality – Emotional
Development - Temperament and Development - Development of
Attachment
Lesson 10- Self-Development - Hazards
Unit III
Lesson 11- Early Childhood - Characteristics - Developmental Tasks - Body Growth
- Brain Development - Influences On Physical Growth and Health
Lesson 12- Motor Development - Piaget's Theory: The Preoperational Stage -
Vygotsky's Sociocultural Theory - Erikson’s Theory: Initiative Versus
Guilt - Child Rearing and Emotional and Social Development
Lesson 13- Self-Understanding - Peer Relations - Foundations of Morality - Gender
Typing
Lesson 14- Late Childhood - Piaget's Theory: The Concrete Operational Stage -
Language Development - Learning in School. Erikson's 'Theory: Industry
versus Inferiority
Lesson 15- Self- Understanding – Emotional Development - Peer Relations - Gender
Typing - Family Influences - Problems of Development
Unit IV
Lesson 16- Puberty - Body Changes -Growth Spurt - Consequences of Abstract
Thought
Lesson 17- Effects Of Puberty Changes - Deviant Maturing – Hazards during
Puberty
Lesson 18- Adolescence - Characteristics - Developmental Tasks - Piaget's Theory:
The Formal Operational Stage - Cognitive Development
Lesson 19- Emotionality - Social Interests and Morality Changes during
Adolescence - Sex Interests - Erikson’s Theory: Identity Versus Identity
Confusion
Lesson 20- Changes in Morality during Adolescence - Sex Interests - Erikson’s
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
2
Theory: Identity Versus Identity Confusion
Unit V
Lesson 21- Early Adulthood - Characteristics - Sex - Role Adjustment - Erikson's
Theory: Intimacy versus Isolation – Family Life Cycle
Lesson 22- Diversity of Adult Lifestyles - Career Development -Middle Adulthood -
Erikson’s Theory: Generativity versus Stagnation
Lesson 23- Changes in Mental Abilities - Adapting To Physical Changes in Midlife -
Relationship at Midlife - Vocational Life
Lesson 24- Late Adulthood - Life Expectancy - Physical Changes - Memory
Erikson's Theory: Ego Integrity versus Despair
Lesson25- Change in Self Concept and Personality - Psychological Well-Being -
Relationships - Retirement and Leisure
UNIT I
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
3
Lesson – 1
FOUNDATIONS OF DEVELOPMENT
Contents
1.0 Aims and Objectives
1.1 Introduction
1.1.1 Early Approaches
1.1.2 Studying the life Span
1.2 Meaning of Development changes
1.2.1 Goal of Life span Changes
1.2.2 Research on Life span Change
1.2.3 Attitudes toward Life span Changes
1.2.4 Some aspects that influence attitudes toward Life span changes
1.3. Basic Issues
1.3.1 Continuous or Discontinuous Development
1.3.2 Course of Development
1.3.3 Nature or Nurture
1.4 Obstacles in Studying Life-Span Development
1.4.1 Representative Samples of Subjects
1.4.2 Establishing Rapport with Subjects
1.4.3 Methodology
1.4.4 Accuracy of Data Obtained
1.4.5 Ethical Aspects of Research
1.5 Let Us Sum Up
1.6 Check your Progress
1.7 Lesson – End Activities
1.8 References
1.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help understand the foundations of life span development which is
fascinating and ever changing.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
i) describe the early approaches to life span development
ii) list the meaning of developmental changes
iii) mention the basic issues of development
iv) state the obstacles in studying life span development
1.1 INTRODUCTION
Life Span Development or Developmental psychology is the branch of psychology that
studies intraindividual changes and interindividual changes within these intraindividual
changes. Its task, as La Bouvie has pointed out, is "not only description but also explication
of age-related changes in behavior in terms of antecedent-consequent relationships".
Developmental psychologists study developmental change covering the life span horn
conception to death. By so doing, they attempt to give a complete picture of growth and
decline. Others cover only a segment of the life span-childhood, adulthood, or old age. In this
book an attempt will be made to cover all segments and show the important developmental
changes at different periods during the entire life span.
Siegel during the early years, as has explained, "Life span psychology was preoccupied
with ages and stages. Investigators sought to learn the typical age at which various stages of
development occurred". The areas in which research was mainly concentrated were those
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
4
considered significant for human evolutional adaptation. For the most part, research studies
were concentrated on preschool and school-age children and on adolescents. Only later did
research extend downward, first to birth and then to conception, and later upward, to
adulthood, old age, and finally to middle age.
The two major reasons for the uneven emphasis of developmental psychology, (1), the
study of a particular period in the developmental pattern has been greatly influenced by the
desire to solve some practical problem or problems associated with that period. Research in
the area of middle age, for example, is an outgrowth of the realization that good adjustments
in the latter years of life depend on how well one has adjusted to the physical and psychological
changes that normally occur in the middle years.
Since the focus of interest in life span psychology has changed over the years, there are
gaps in our knowledge of the different developmental phenomena characteristic of the
different periods. These gaps are also due in part to difficulties in studying the different
patterns of behavior characteristic of a given period, especially difficulties in getting representative
samplings of subjects of a given age and in finding a suitable method for the study
of behavior patterns.
(2) The reason for the uneven emphasis is that it is- harder to study people at some stages
of life than at others. Getting middle-aged and elderly subjects, for example, is far more
difficult than getting preschool or school-age children or even adolescents.
Life Span psychologists have six major objectives: (1) to find out what are the common
and characteristic age changes in appearance, in behavior, in interests, and in goals from one
developmental period to another; (2) to find out when these changes occur; (3) to find out
what causes them; (4) to find out how they influence behavior; (5) to find out whether they
can or cannot be predicted; and (6) to find out whether they are individual or universal.
1.1.1 Early Approaches
Early forerunners of the scientific study of development were baby biographies, journals
kept to record the early development of a child. One early journal, published in 1787 in
Germany, contained Dietrich Tiedemann's (1897/1787) observations of his son's sensory,
motor, language, and cognitive behavior during the first 21/2 years. Typical of the speculative
nature of such observations was Tiedemann's erroneous conclusion, after watching the infant
suck more on a cloth tied around something sweet than on a nurse's finger, that sucking
appeared to be "not instinctive, but acquired".
It was Charles Darwin, originator of the theory of evolution, who first emphasized the
developmental nature of infant behavior. In 1877 Darwin published notes on his son Doddy's
sensory, cognitive, and emotional development during his first twelve months. Darwin's
journal gave "baby biographies" scientific respectability; about thirty more were published
during the next three decades.
By the end of the nineteenth century, several important trends in the western world were
preparing the way for the scientific study of development. Scientists had unlocked the
mystery of conception and were arguing about the relative importance of "nature" and
"nurture" (inborn characteristics and experiential influences). The discovery of germs and
immunization made it possible for many more children to survive infancy. Laws protecting
children from long workdays let them spend more time in school, and parents and teachers
became more concerned with identifying and meeting children's developmental needs. The
new science of psychology taught that people could understand themselves by learning what
had influenced them as children. Still, this new discipline had far to go. For example,
adolescence was not considered a separate period of development until the early twentieth
century, when G. Stanley Hall, a pioneer in child study, published a popular (though
unscientific) book called Adolescence (1904/1916).
Hall also was one of the first psychologists to become interested in aging. In 1922, at age
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
5
78, he published Senescence: The Last Half of Life. Six years later, Stanford University
opened the first major scientific research unit devoted to aging. But not until a generation
later did the study of aging blossom. Since the late 1930s a number of important long-term
studies discussed in the second half of this book, such as those of K. Warner Schaie, George
Vaillant, Daniel Levinson, and Ravenna Helson, have focused on intelligence and personality
development in adulthood and old age.
1.1.2 Studying the life Span
Today most developmental scientists recognize that development goes on throughout life.
This concept of a lifelong process of development that can be studied scientifically is known
as life-span development.
Life-span studies grew out of research designed to follow children through adulthood.
The Stanford Studies of Gifted Children (begun in 1921 under the direction of Lewis M.
Terman) trace the development of people (now in old age) who were identified as unusually
intelligent in childhood. Other major studies that began around 1930-the Fels Research
Institute Study, the Berkeley Growth and Guidance Studies, and the Oakland (Adolescent)
Growth Study-have given us much information on long-term development.
Because human beings are complex, the study of life-span development is interdisciplinary,
drawing on many fields, or disciplines. These include psychology, psychiatry,
sociology, anthropology, biology, genetics (the study of inherited characteristics), family
science (the study of family processes), education, history, philosophy, and medicine.
1.2 MEANING OF DEVELOPMENTAL CHANGES
Development means a progressive series of changes that occur as a result of
maturation and experience. As Van den Daele has pointed out, "development implies
qualitative change". This means that development does not consist merely of adding inches to
one's height or of improving one's ability. Instead, it is a complex process of integrating many
structures and functions.
There are two essentially antagonistic processes in development take place
simultaneously throughout life -growth, or evolution, and atrophy, or involution. Both begin
at conception and end at death. In the early years growth predominates, even though atrophic
changes occur as early as embryonic life. In the latter part of life, atrophy predominates,
though growth does not stop; hair continues to grow, and cells continue to be replaced. With
aging, some parts of the body and mind change more than others.
The human being is never static. From conception to death, change is constantly taking
place in physical and psychological capacities. As Piaget has explained, structures are "far
from being static and given from the start." Instead, a maturing organism undergoes
continued and progressive changes in response to experiential conditions, and these result in a
complex network of interaction.
As development is continuous, as Bower has pointed out, in the sense that it is a cyclic
process with competences developing, and then disappearing, only to appear at a later age, it
is not continuous in the sense that it increases constantly but rather in a series of waves with
whole segments of development reoccurring repetitively. Bower has explained, newborns
walk, if held, and then this ability disappears only to reappear at eight or ten months of age.
He explains that the "various explanations of repetitive processes in development thus seem
to differ depending on the specific repetition to be explained. What all the explanations have
in common, however, is that they preserve the assumption that psychological growth, in spite
of its apparent reversals, is a continuous and additive process". When regression to an earlier
stage occurs, there is usually a cause for it, as in the regression to awkwardness that occurs
with the rapid growth at puberty. .
The pattern of change resembles a bell shaped curve, rising abruptly at the start and then
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
6
flattening out during the middle years, only to decline slowly or abruptly in old age. It is
important to recognize that at no time can this pattern be represented by a straight line,
though plateau periods of short or long duration may occur in the development of different
capacities.
1.2.1 Goal of Life span Changes
It is to enable people to adapt to the environment in which they live. To achieve this
goal, self-realization, or, as it is sometimes called, 'Self-actualization," is essential. However,
this goal is never static. It may be considered an urge-the urge to do what one is fitted to do,
the urge to become the person, both physically and psychologically, that one wants to be.
The way people express this urge depends on the individual's innate abilities and
training, not only during the early, formative years of childhood but also as he or she grows
older and comes under greater pressures to conform to social expectations.
Since self-realization plays an important role in mental health, people who 'make good
personal and social adjustments must have opportunities to express their interests and desires
in ways that give them satisfaction but, at the same time, conform to accepted standards. Lack
of these opportunities will result in frustrations and generally negative attitudes toward
people and toward life in general.
1.2.2 Research on Life span Change
Research on developmental changes during childhood and adolescence has been far more
extensive than studies of changes that occur during the later years. Among the reasons for this
uneven emphasis is the fact that the many prevailing traditional beliefs about children and
adolescents have acted as a spur to researchers, who have set up studies designed to prove or
disprove these beliefs. Traditional beliefs concerning the post adolescent years are less
numerous, and have had less impact on the direction of research. Further, developmental
changes occurring at middle age were regarded as purely physiological and, therefore, outside
the scope of psychological research. Changes occurring in old age affected a relatively small
percentage of the population and were thus considered less important than changes that occur
during the early years. It is now recognized that changes occurring at any developmental
stage are worthy of study.
The most important incentive to research about developmental changes has been the
nature-nurture controversy which has raged for decades. How important a role-maturation
based on genetic factors plays in bringing about developmental changes as compared with
environmental pressures and experiences has been the focal point of interest, and many
research studies have been devoted to trying to find a satisfactory solution to this controversy.
The research on developmental changes at all stages has been the emergence of a large
number of new theories about the causes and effects of such changes. These theories are not
always backed up by adequate evidence, 'and a great deal of research is motivated by the
desire to substantiate or refute material that has widespread acceptance in the field. Any new
theory can lead to controversy and experimentation, but of all theories, none have provided a
more powerful incentive to research than Piaget’s developmental theories, especially his
theories about 'cognitive development. Other views that have inspired numerous studies are
Kohlberg’s stages of moral development and Gesell’s stages of equilibrium and
disequilibrium.
1.2.3 Attitudes toward Life span Changes
Changes of a physical or psychological nature are constantly taking place; many people
are only vaguely aware of them unless they occur abruptly or markedly affect the pattern of
their lives. The changes of old age, for example, usually occur at a much slower pace than
those of childhood or adolescence. However, they still require readjustments on the part of all
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
7
individuals. But, when individuals can make these adjustments relatively slowly, they
themselves or others may not be conscious of them.
Thus when changes are rapid, on the other hand, the individual is only too well aware of
them, as are others. During the puberty growth spurt at the end of childhood and the
beginning of adolescence, such comments as "My, how you have grown since I last saw
you!" are evidence of how others notice these changes.
Similarly, in senescence, when the downward movement begins to accelerate, the elderly
are aware of the fact that their health is "failing" and that their minds are "slipping." Constant
readjustment to these changes is necessary in the scheduled pattern of their lives. They must
slow down as the incapacities and infirmities of old age catch up with them and they must
frequently forgo some of the activities that formerly played important roles in their lives.
As there is, a tendency for most people to regard the past as better than the present. And
even though most children look forward to the day when they will be "teenagers," when that
time comes they often long for the carefree days of their childhood. Similarly, many men
who look forward to retirement wish, when the mandatory age for retirement arrives, that
they could go back to earlier years when their usefulness and prestige were recognized by the
social group.
As and when people become aware of the changes taking place in them, they develop
definite attitudes toward these changes. Whether these attitudes will be favorable or
unfavorable depends on a number of factors, the most important of which are described
below.
1. 2. 4 Some aspects that influence attitudes toward Life span changes
Appearance: Developments that improve one’s appearance are welcome and lead to
favorable attitudes while those that detract from one’s appearance are resisted and every
possible attempt is made to camouflage them.
Behaviour: As and when behaviour changes are disconcerting, as during puberty and
senescence, they affect attitudes toward the changes unfavorably. The reverse is true when
changes are favorable, as occurs, for example, when the helplessness of babyhood gradually
gives way to the independence of childhood.
Cultural Stereotypes: From mass media, people learn cultural stereotypes associated with
different ages and they use these stereotypes to judge people of those ages.
Cultural Values: Each culture has certain values associated with different ages. Because
maximum productivity is associated with young through early middle-age adulthood,
attitudes toward this age group are more favorable than attitudes toward other ages.
Role Changes: Attitudes toward people of different ages are greatly influenced by the roles
they play. When people change their roles to less favorable ones, as in the case of retirement
or widowhood, social attitudes toward them are less sympathetic.
Personal Experiences: Personal experiences have a profound effect on an individual’s
attitude toward developmental changes. Since the authority and prestige of middle-aged
executive’s decreases as they approach retirement, their attitudes toward aging are, for
example, unfavorably affected. These attitudes are intensified by unfavorable social attitudes.
Check Your Progress 1
State some of the aspects that influence attitudes toward Life span changes.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
1. 3 BASIC ISSUES
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
8
Research on human development is a relatively recent endeavor. Studies of children did
not begin until the early part of the twentieth century. Investigations into adult development,
aging, and change over the life course emerged only in the 1960s and 1970s. Nevertheless,
ideas about how people grow and change have existed for centuries. As these speculations
combined with research, they inspired the construction of theories of development. A theory
is an orderly, integrated set of statements that describes, explains, and predicts behaviour. For
example, a good theory of infant-caregiver attachment would (1) describe the behaviors of
babies of 6 to 8 months of age as they seek the affection and comfort of a familiar adult, (2)
explain how and why infants develop this strong desire to bond with a caregiver, and (3)
predict the consequences of this emotional bond for relationships throughout life.
Theories are vital tools for two reasons. First, they provide organizing frameworks for
our observations of people. In other words, they guide and give meaning to what we see.
Second, theories that are verified by research provide a sound basis for practical action. Once
a theory helps us understand development, we are in a much better position to know what to
do in our efforts to improve the welfare and treatment of children and adults.
Theories are influenced by the cultural values and belief systems of their times. But
theories differ in one important way from mere opinion and belief: A theory's continued
existence depends on scientific verification. This means that the theory must be tested with a
fair set of research procedures agreed on by the scientific community and that its findings
must endure, or be replicated, over time.
In the field of human development, there are many theories with very different ideas
about what people are like and how they change. The study of development provides no ultimate
truth because investigators do not always agree on the meaning of what they see. In
addition, humans are complex beings; they change physically, mentally, emotionally, and socially.
As yet, no single theory has explained all these aspects. However, the existence of
many theories helps advance knowledge because researchers are continually trying to support,
contradict, and integrate these different points of view.
There are some major theories of human development and research strategies used to test
them. We can easily organize them, since almost all take a stance on three basic issues: (1) Is
the course of development continuous or discontinuous? (2) Does one course of development
characterize all people, or are there many possible courses? (3) Are genetic or environmental
factors more important in determining development? Let's look closely at each of these
issues.
1.3.1 Continuous or Discontinuous Development?
The best way to describe the differences in capacities between small infants, young
children, adolescents, and adults is how the major theories recognize two possibilities.
One view holds that infants and preschoolers respond to the world in much the same way
as adults do. The difference between the immature and mature being is simply one of amount
or complexity. For example, when Shanthi was a baby, her perception of a piano melody,
memory for past events, and ability to sort objects into categories may have been much like
our own. Perhaps her only limitation was that she could not perform these skills with as much
information and precision as we can. If this is so, then change in her thinking must be
continuous-a process of gradually augmenting the same types of skills that were there to
begin with.
A second view regards infants and children as having unique ways of thinking, feeling,
and behaving, ones quite d i f ferent from adults'. In other words, developme n t i s
discontinuous-a process in which new and different ways of interpreting and responding to
the world emerge at particular time periods. From this perspective, infant Shanthi was not yet
able perceive and organize events and objects as a mature person could. Instead, she moved
through a series of developmental steps, each with unique features, until she reached the
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
9
highest level of functioning.
Theories that accept the discontinuous perspective regard development as taking place in
stages-qualitative changes thinking, feeling, and behaving that characterize specific periods
of development. In stage theories, development is much like climbing a staircase, with each
step corresponding to a more mature, reorganized way of functioning. The stage concept also
assumes that people undergo periods of rapid transformation as they step up from one stage
to the next. In other words, change is fairly sudden rather than gradual and ongoing.
1.3.2 Course of Development
Stage theorists assume that people everywhere follow same sequence of development.
Yet the field of human development is becoming increasingly aware that children and adults
live in distinct contexts or unique combinations personal and environmental circumstances
that can result different paths of change. For example, a shy individual, fears social
encounters develops in very different contexts from those of a social agemate who readily
seeks out other people. Children and adults in village societies encounter experiences in their
families and communities that differ sharply from those of people in large cities. These
different circumstances foster different intellectual capacities, social skills, and feelings about
the self and others.
Contemporary theorists regard the contexts that shape development as many-layered and
complex. On the personal side, they include heredity and biological makeup. On the
environmental side, they include immediate settings, such as home, school, and
neighborhood, as well as circumstances more remote from people's everyday lives-community
resources, societal values, and historical time period. Finally, a special interest in culture
has made researchers more conscious than ever before of diversity in development.
1.3.3 Nature or Nurture
Each theory describes the course of human development, and also each theory takes a
stance on a major question about its underlying causes: Are genetic or environmental factors
important in determining development? This is the age-old nature-nurture controversy. By
nature, we mean inborn biological givens-the hereditary information we receive from our
parents at the moment of conception. By nurture, we mean the complex forces of the
physical and social world that influence our biological makeup and psychological experiences
before and after birth.
Even though all theories grant at least some role to both nature and nurture, they vary in
emphasis. For example, consider the following questions: Is the developing person's ability to
think in more complex ways largely the result of an inborn timetable of growth? Or is it
primarily influenced by stimulation from parents and teachers? Do children acquire language
rapidly because they are genetically predisposed to do so or because parents tutor them from
an early age? And what accounts for the vast individual differences among people-in height,
weight, physical coordination, intelligence, personality, and social skills? Is nature or nurture
more responsible?
The theories take a stand on nature versus nurture affect their explanations of individual
differences. Some theorists emphasize stability- that individuals who are high or low in a
characteristic (such as verbal ability, anxiety, or sociability) will remain so at later ages.
These theorists typically stress the importance of heredity. If they regard environment as
important, they usually point to early experiences as establishing a lifelong pattern of
behavior. Powerful negative events in the first few years, they argue, cannot be fully
overcome by later, more positive ones. Other theorists take a more optimistic view. They
emphasize plasticity-that change is possible and likely if new experiences support it.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
10
Check Your Progress 2
State basic issues involved in the Life span development.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
1.4 OBSTACLES IN STUDYING LIFE-SPAN DEVELOPMENT
All studies of the Life span are beset by obstacles in varying degrees. The five most common
and most serious of these are discussed below.
1.4.1 Representative Samples of Subjects
The first obstacle scientists’ encounter in studying development during the life span is
securing representative samples of subjects at different age levels, although it is relatively
easy to get representative samplings of subjects from among schoolchildren and college
students. In the case of newborn infants, however, researchers often meet with strong parental
objections. Getting older adolescents and young adults who are not attending school to
volunteer as subjects is also difficult because they may not be available for study at anyone
particular place.
This difficulty increases with advancing age, which is why so many of the studies
relating to the latter years of life have been made on men and women living in institutions,
people who unquestionably are not representative of the general population.
Recruiting young adults, middle-aged adults, or the elderly as voluntary participants in
experiments has likewise been a difficult task, even when they are paid for their time. Many
persons shy away from any testing program, partly because of lack of personal interest but
mainly because they are afraid they will not do well and, as a result, create an unfavorable
impression. Relying on those who are willing to participate may be creating a bias just as
using institutional cases does.
1.4.2 Establishing Rapport with Subjects
The second obstacle scientists’ encounter in studying development during the life span is
establishing rapport with subjects at different age levels. There is no guarantee that scientists
will be able to elicit the information they are seeking from any group unless they are able to
establish rapport with their subjects. Therefore, there is no guarantee that the data they obtain
is as accurate or as comprehensive as it might have been had a better relationship existed
between subjects and experimenters.
The reason for this is that obtaining information from subjects of any age is extremely
difficult because most people resent having a stranger pry into their personal affairs. Even
schoolchildren and college students, who often take tests or fill out questionnaires as part of
their classroom work, show their resentment by being uncooperative or even by falsifying the
information they give. This is even truer of adults of all ages. Their resentment at
participating in a scientific study may be partially overcome if they are paid to do so, but they
tend to regard the experimenter as an invasion of privacy.
As a result, it is questionable whether data obtained from many studies is a true picture
of the involved individuals' attitudes, feelings, and values. Only when good rapport can be
established with the subjects and when there is evidence of cooperation on their part can great
confidence be placed in the results of these studies.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
11
1.4.3 Methodology
The third obstacle scientists’ encounter in studying development during the life span
is securing a satisfactory method. This is because no one method can be used satisfactorily
for studying people at all ages or for investigating all areas of development. Some of the
methods that must be resorted to, for lack of better ones, are of dubious scientific value.
Because of the wide age range of subjects and the variety of different areas of
development that must be studied to give a composite picture, assorted methods have had to
be used. Some have been borrowed from medicine, from the physical sciences, and from
related social sciences, especially anthropology and sociology. Some have made use of laboratory
settings, and others of the naturalistic settings of the home, school, community, or
work environment. Some are regarded as reliable, while others, especially the retrospective
and introspective techniques, are of questionable value.
Regardless of the method used, most of the studies have been cross-sectional
comparisons of the same abilities at different stages of development. As such, they do not
give evidence about developmental trends or about intraindividual variability. Nor is it
possible, when using cross-sectional comparisons, to assess the relative b e h a vior
constellations of individuals at an early age and similar behavior in adult life.
One of the most serious problems connected with the cross-sectional approach is that it
is almost impossible to get comparable groups of subjects for study at different age levels.
This can bias the result of studies, especially studies of old age. When mental abilities are
studied using the cross-sectional approach, mental decline is reported to be far greater than
when the same mental abilities are studied using the longitudinal approach. This, in turn, has
given scientific backing to the popular belief that mental decline in old age is not only great
but also universal.
Another serious problem associated with the cross-sectional approach is that it does not
take into consideration cultural changes which always play a major role in the patterns of
physical and mental development. This results in a tendency to interpret any change that may
appear as an age change.
Cultural changes affect values, among other things. A comparison of adolescents of
today with members of the older generation showed that the latter tend to disapprove more
strongly of extravagance than adolescents do. This might be interpreted to mean that
members of the older generation have become rigid with age. In reality, the difference is one
of cultural values. When members of the older generation were growing up, high value was
placed on a prudent spending of money and on having a nest egg for the proverbial rainy day.
Today, adolescents are growing up in a culture dominated by the philosophy of “earn more
and spend more”. Because of the rapid change in cultural values taking place at the present
time, children often consider their parents' values old-fashioned.
1.4.4 Accuracy of Data Obtained
The fourth obstacle scientists’ encounter in studying development during the life span is
ensuring that the data obtained from the studies will be accurate. Inaccuracies may result
when a biased sampling of subjects gives a false picture of the normal developmental pattern
at a particular age. This can happen, for example, when institutionalized elderly people are
used for the study and the subjects try to present as favorable a picture of them as they can
and, either consciously or unconsciously, distort their introspective or retrospective reports. It
can also occur when the only method available for studying a certain area of development is
less than satisfactory.
In the measurement of intelligence it is still questionable if the results are accurate for
the first two years of life. There is even controversy about the accuracy of intelligence tests
for older age levels. Observational techniques for the study of behavior during the preschool
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
12
years are questioned for accuracy because of the tendency of observers to draw inferences
from their study of children's behavior and speech.
Studying well-being, life satisfaction, or happiness is very difficult because only
subjective measures can be used. The accuracy of such measures is open to question. In the
study of fears by means of oral or written checklists, it was found that subjects often do not
identify fear as different from anxiety or worry. Furthermore, when parents report their children's
fears, they often describe different fears than their children do. In addition, it is
impossible to identify the intensity of fears using only a checklist.
Even though the longitudinal approach has a methodological advantage over the crosssectional
approach, the problem of accuracy is still ever present. Unless such studies are
started when the subjects are very young, information about their earlier lives must be
supplied by the subjects themselves or by parents, teachers, and peers, who tend to interpret
the data they report in terms of their own attitudes and experiences.
1.4.5 Ethical Aspects of Research
The fifth obstacle scientists’ encounter in studying development during the life span
involves the ethical aspects of research. Today there is a growing trend to take this into
account, and it has been a stumbling block to certain kinds of studies, which, in the past, were
made without consideration of their fairness to the subjects studied. With the trend now a
days toward considering the rights of subjects, emphasis in being placed on asking their
consent to participate in experiments, or, for the very young, the consent of their parents or
guardians. Such consideration also applies to high school and college students; they no longer
are expected to take time from their studies to participate in experiments unless they are paid
to, do so. Thus there is a tendency to bias the sampling because only those who need the
money or those who feel that the money is worth their while are willing to accommodate the
researcher.
Consideration of the rights of subjects is therefore bound to lead to gaps in present-day
knowledge of development. Aside from preschool and school age children, no other group of
individuals has been taken greater advantage of by scientific researchers' than the
institutionalized elderly. Just as preschool and school-age children formerly were not
consulted about their willingness to participate in an experiment, so the wishes of the
institutionalized elderly were ignored. They were expected to take tests or answer questions,
regardless of how they felt about the matter, because they were receiving public assistance.
Even in privately supported institutions the inmates were not always consulted.
Such procedures are now regarded as an invasion of privacy and as ethically questionable:
As a result, many experimenters are taking into consideration the wishes of those they try to
recruit as subjects. They are also taking a new attitude toward institutionalized middle-aged
people, recognizing their rights to participate or refuse to participate in any research study.
While these new attitudes toward ethical standards have resulted in fairer treatment of
institutionalized middle-aged and older people, they have made it more difficult to get
subjects for scientific research among the older age groups.
Check Your Progress 3
Write the five most common and most serious obstacles in studying life-span development.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
13
1.5 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Early Approaches in studying the life Span
ii) Meaning of Development changes
iii) Goal of Life span Changes
iv) Research on Life span Change
v) Attitudes toward Life span Changes
vi) Some aspects that influence attitudes toward Life span changes
vii) Continuous or Discontinuous Development
viii) Obstacles in Studying Life-Span Development
1.6 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. Your answer may include the description of
i) Appearance
ii) Behaviour
iii) Cultural Stereotypes
iv) Cultural Values
v) Role Changes
vi) Personal Experiences
2. The Basic issues that are to be dealt are:
i) Continuous or Discontinuous Development
ii) Course of Development
iii) Nature or Nurture
3. The five most common and most serious obstacles in studying life-span development
could be:
i) Representative Samples of Subjects
ii) Establishing Rapport with Subjects
iii) Methodology
iv) Accuracy of Data Obtained
v) Ethical Aspects of Research
1.7 Lesson - End Activities
(1) Have a discussion with parents and list down the factors have high influence on
development.
(2) Develop miniature life situations tells to find out the continuous and discountinuous
development.
1.8 References
1. Levin, M.J., Psychology – A Biographical Approach, New York: McGraw Hill, 1978.
2. Symonds, P., The Dynamics of Human Adjustment, New York: Appleton, 1976.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
14
LESSON – 2
RECENT THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES – METHODS
Contents
2.0 Aims and Objectives
2.1 Introduction
2.2 Mid twentieth century theories
2.2.1 The Psychoanalytic Perspective
2.2.2 Behaviorism and Social Learning Theory
2.2.3 Piaget's Cognitive-Developmental Theory
2.3 Recent Theoretical perspectives
2.3.1 Information Processing
2.3.2 Ethology and Evolutionary Developmental Psychology
2.3.3 Vygotsky's Sociocultural Theory
2.3.4 Ecological Systems Theory
2.4 Methods
2.4.1 Common Research Methods
2.4.2 General Research Designs
2.4.3 Designs for Studying Development
2.5 Let Us Sum Up
2.6 Check your Progress
2.7 Lesson – End Activiti3es
2.8 References
2.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This lesson will provide a glimpse into the various old and new theories of life span
development. It also covers the research methods in studying the developmental aspects
of a human being.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
i) state the twentieth century theories that helped to put a foundation in studying
development.
ii) understand the recent theories of life span development.
iii) list the research methods most commonly used.
iv) highlight the general research designs employed in psychology.
v) explain the special designs for studying development.
2.1 INTRODUCTION
One of our human characteristics is that we seek logical explanations of things that
happen. The scientific method involves formulating a problem, developing a hypothesis,
testing it, and then drawing conclusions that are stated in the form of a theory. A theory
organizes the data, ideas, and hypothesis and states them in coherent, interrelated, general
propositions, principles or laws. These propositions, principles or laws are useful in
explaining and predicting phenomena, now and in the future. The theories may be arranged
into two major categories. They are I. Mid twentieth century theories 1. Psychoanalytic 2.
Learning 3. Cognitive and II. Recent Theoretical Perspectives 4. Information Processing
5. Ethology and Evolutionary Developmental Psychology 6. Vygotsky's Sociocultural Theory
7. Ecological Systems Theory
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
15
2.2 MID TWENTIETH CENTURY THEORIES
2.2.1 The Psychoanalytic Perspective
According to the psychoanalytic perspective, people move through a series of stages in
which they confront conflicts between biological drives and social expectations. The way
these conflicts are resolved determines the individual's ability to learn, to get along with
others, and to cope with anxiety.
Freud's Theory. Freud, a Viennese physician, saw patients in his practice with a variety
of nervous symptoms, such as hallucinations, fears, and paralyses that appeared to have no
physical basis. Seeking a cure for these troubled adults, Freud found that their symptoms
could be relieved by having patients talk freely about' painful events of their childhoods. On
the basis of adult remembrances, he examined the unconscious motivations of his patients
and constructed his psychosexual theory, which emphasized that how parents manage their
child's sexual and aggressive drives in the first few years is crucial for healthy personality
development.
Three Parts of the Personality. In Freud's theory, three parts of the personality-id, ego,
and superego-become integrated during five stages, summarized in Table 1. The id, the
largest portion of the mind, is the source of basic biological needs and desires. The ego-the
conscious, rational part of personality-emerges in early infancy to redirect the id's impulses so
they are discharged on appropriate objects at acceptable times and places. For example, aided
by the ego, the hungry baby of a few months of age stops crying when he sees his mother
unfasten her clothing for breast-feeding.
Between 3 and 6 years of age, the superego, or conscience, develops from interactions
with parents, who insist that children conform to the values of society. Now the ego faces the
increasingly complex task of reconciling the demands of the id, the external world, and
conscience. For example, when the ego is tempted to gratify an id impulse by hitting a
playmate to get an attractive toy, the superego may warn that such behavior is wrong. The
ego must decide which of the two forces (id or superego) will win this inner struggle, or it
must work out a compromise, such as asking for a turn with the toy. According to Freud, the
relations established among the id, ego, and superego during the preschool years determine
the individual's basic personality.
Psychosexual Development. Freud believed that during childhood, sexual impulses shift
their focus from the oral to the anal to the genital regions of the body. In each stage of
development, parents walk a fine line between permitting too much or too little gratification
of their child's basic needs. If parents strike an appropriate balance, then children grow into
well-adjusted adults with the capacity for mature sexual behavior, investment in family life,
and rearing of the next generation. Freud's psychosexual theory highlighted the importance of
family relationships for children's development. It was the first theory to stress the role of
early experience.
Erikson's Theory. Although Erikson (1950) accepted Freud's basic psychosexual
framework, he expanded the picture of development at each stage. In his psychosocial
theory, Erikson emphasized that the ego does not just mediate between id impulses and
superego demands. At each stage, it acquires attitudes and skills that make the individual an
active, contributing member of society. A basic psychological conflict, which is resolved
along a continuum from positive to negative, determines healthy or maladaptive outcomes at
each stage. As Table 2 shows, Erikson's first five stages parallel Freud's stages, but Erikson
added three adult stages. Finally, unlike Freud, Erikson pointed out that normal development
must be understood in relation to each culture's life situation.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
16
Table 1 Freud's Psychosexual Stages
Psychosexual
Stage
Period of
Development
Description
Oral
Anal
Phallic
Latency
Genital
Birth-1 year
1-3 years
3-6 years
6- 11 years
Adolescence
The new ego directs the baby's sucking activities toward breast or bottle. If oral needs are
not met appropriately, the individual may develop such habits as thumb sucking,
fingernail biting, and pencil chewing in childhood and overeating and smoking in later
life.
Toddlers and preschoolers enjoy holding and releasing urine and feces. Toilet training
becomes a major issue between parent and child. If parents insist that children be trained
before they are ready or make too few demands, conflicts about anal control may appear
in the form of extreme orderliness and cleanliness or messiness and disorder.
Id impulses transfer to the genitals, and the child finds pleasure in genital stimulation.
Freud's Oedipus conflict for boys and Electra conflict for girls arise, and young children
feel a sexual desire for the other sex parent. To avoid punishment, they give up this desire
and, instead, adopt the same-sex parent's characteristics and values. As a result, the
superego is formed, and children feel guilty each time they violate its standards. The
relations among id, ego, and superego established at this time determine the individual's
basic personality.
Sexual instincts die down, and the superego develops further. The child acquires new
social values from adults outside the family and from play with same-sex peers.
Puberty causes the sexual impulses of the phallic stage to reappear. If development has
been successful during earlier stages, it leads to marriage, mature sexuality, and the birth
and rearing of children.
Table 2
Erikson's Psychosocial Stages, with Corresponding Psychosexual Stages
Psychosocial
Stage
Period of
Development
Description
Basic trust
versus mistrust
(Oral)
Autonomy
versus shame
and doubt
(Anal)
Initiative versus
guilt (Phallic)
Industry versus
diffusion
(Latency)
Identity versus
identity
confusion
(Genital)
Intimacy versus
isolation
Generativity
versus
stagnation
Ego integrity
versus despair
Birth-1 year
1-3 years
3-6 years
6- 11 years
Adolescence
Young
adulthood
Middle
adulthood
Old age
From warm, responsive care, infants gain a sense of trust, or confidence,
that the world is good. Mistrust occurs when infants have to wait too long
for comfort and are handled harshly,
Using new mental and motor skills, children want to choose and decide
for themselves. Autonomy is fostered when parents permit reasonable
free choice and do not force or shame the child.
Through make-believe play, children experiment with the kind of person
they can become. Initiative-a sense of ambition and responsibilitydevelops
when parents support their child's new sense of purpose. The
danger is that parents will demand too much self-control, which leads to
over control, meaning too much guilt.
At school, children develop the capacity to work and cooperate with
others. Inferiority develops when negative experiences at home, at school,
or with peers lead to feelings of incompetence.
The adolescent tries to answer the question, Who am I, and what is my
place in society? Self-chosen values and vocational goals lead to a lasting
personal identity. The negative outcome is confusion about future adult
roles.
Young people work on establishing intimate ties to others. Because of
earlier disappointments, some individuals cannot form close relationships
and remain isolated.
Generativity means giving to the next generation through child rearing,
caring for other people, or productive work. The person who fails in these
ways feels an absence of meaningful accomplishment.
In this final stage, individuals reflect on the kind of person they have
been. Integrity results from feeling that life was worth living as it
happened. Older people who are dissatisfied with their lives fear death.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
17
2.2.2 Behaviorism and Social Learning Theory
According to behaviorism, directly observable events stimuli and responses-are the
appropriate focus of study.
Traditional Behaviorism. John Watson (1878-1958) Watson was inspired by studies of
animal learning carried out by famous Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov. Pavlov knew that
dogs release saliva as an innate reflex when they are given food. But he noticed that his dogs
were salivating before they tasted any food-when they saw the trainer who usually fed them.
The dogs, Pavlov reasoned, must have learned to associate a neutral stimulus (the trainer)
with another stimulus (food) that produces a reflexive response (salivation). As a result of
this association, the neutral stimulus could bring about a response resembling the reflex.
Eager to test this idea, Pavlov successfully taught dogs to salivate at the sound of a bell by
pairing it with the presentation of food. He had discovered classical conditioning.
Watson wanted to find out if classical conditioning could be applied to children's
behavior. In a historic experiment, he taught Albert, a 11-month-old infant, to fear a neutral
stimulus-a soft white rat-by presenting it several times with a sharp, loud sound, which
naturally scared the baby. Little Albert, who at first had reached out eagerly to touch the furry
rat, began to cry and turn his head away when he caught sight of it. In fact, Albert's fear was
so intense that researchers eventually challenged the ethics of studies like this one. Consistent
with Locke's tabula rasa, Watson concluded that environment is the supreme force in
development. Adults can mold children's behavior, he thought, by carefully controlling
stimulus-response associations. And development is a continuous process, consisting of a
gradual increase in the number and strength of these associations.
Another form of behaviorism was B. F. Skinner's (19041990) operant conditioning
theory. According to Skinner, behavior can be increased by following it with a wide variety
of reinforcers, such as food, praise, or a friendly smile. It can also be decreased through
punishment, such as disapproval or withdrawal of privileges. As a result of Skinner's work,
operant conditioning became a broadly applied learning principle.
Social learning Theory. Albert Bandura, emphasized modeling, otherwise known as
imitation or observational learning, as a powerful source of development. Bandura (1977)
recognized that children acquire many favorable and unfavorable responses simply by
watching and listening to others around them. The baby who claps her hands after her mother
does so, the child who angrily hits a playmate in the same way that he has been punished at
home, and the teenager who wears the same clothes and hairstyle as her friends at school are
all displaying observational learning.
Bandura's recent theory stresses the importance of cognition, or thinking. The theory
places such strong emphasis on how we think about ourselves and other people that he calls it
a social-cognitive rather than a social learning approach. According to this view, children
gradually become more selective in what they imitate. From watching others engage in selfpraise
and self-blame and through feedback about the worth of their own actions, children
develop personal standards for behavior and a sense of self-efficacy-the belief that their own
abilities and characteristics will help them succeed. These cognitions guide responses in
particular situations.
2.2.3 Piaget's Cognitive-Developmental Theory
Piaget's Stages. Piaget's view of development was greatly influenced by his early training in
biology. Central to his theory is the biological concept of adaptation. Just as the structures of
the body are adapted to fit with the environment, so the structures of the mind develop to better
fit with, or represent, the external world. In infancy and early childhood, children's
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
18
understanding is different from adults'. As the brain develops and children's experiences
expand, they move through four broad stages, each characterized by qualitatively distinct
ways of thinking. In the sensorimotor stage, cognitive development begins with the baby's
use of the senses and movements to explore the world. These action patterns evolve into the
symbolic but illogical thinking of the preschooler in the preoperational stage. Then cognition
is transformed into the more organized reasoning of the school age child in the concrete
operational stage. Finally, in the formal operational stage, thought becomes the complex,
abstract reasoning system of the adolescent and adult.
2.3 RECENT THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES
New ways of understanding the developing person are constantly emerging-questioning,
building on, and enhancing the discoveries of earlier theories. Today, a burst of fresh
approaches and research emphases is broadening our understanding of the lifespan.
2.3.1 Information Processing
During the 1970s, researchers turned to the field of cognitive psychology for ways to
understand the development of thinking. The design of digital computers that use mathematically
specified steps to solve problems suggested to psychologists that the human mind
might also be viewed as a symbol-manipulating system through which information flows-a
perspective called information processing. From presentation to the senses at input to
behavioral responses at output, information is actively coded, transformed, and organized.
Information-processing researchers often use flowcharts to map the precise series of steps
individuals use to solve problems and complete tasks, much like the plans devised by
programmers to get computers to perform a series of "mental operations."
Like Piaget's cognitive-developmental theory, information processing regards people as
active, sense-making beings. But unlike Piaget's theory, there are no stages of development.
Rather, the thought processes studied are -perception, attention, memory, planning strategies,
categorization of information, and comprehension of written and spoken prose-are regarded
as similar at all ages but present to a lesser or greater extent. Therefore, the view of
development is one of continuous change.
2.3.2 Ethology and Evolutionary Developmental Psychology
Ethology is concerned with the adaptive, or survival, value of behavior and its
evolutionary history. Its roots can be traced to the work of Darwin. Two European
Zoologists, Konrad Lorenz and Niko watching diverse animal species in their natural
habitats, observed behavior patterns that promote survival. The best known of these is imprinting,
the early following behavior of certain baby birds, such as geese, that ensures that
the young will stay close to the mother and be fed and protected from danger. Imprinting
takes place during an early, restricted time period of development. If the mother goose is not
present during this time but an object resembling her in important features is, young goslings
may imprint on it instead.
Observations of imprinting led to a major concept in human development: the critical
period. It refers to a limited time span during which the individual is biologically prepared to
acquire certain adaptive behaviors but needs the support of an appropriately stimulating
environment. Many researchers have conducted studies to find out whether complex
cognitive and social behaviors must be learned during certain time periods. For example, if
children are deprived of adequate food or physical and social stimulation during their early
years, will their intelligence be impaired? If language is not mastered; during early childhood,
is the capacity to acquire it reduced?
The term sensitive period applies better to human development than does the strict notion
of a critical period. A sensitive period is a time that is optimal for certain capacities to emerge
and in which the individual is especially responsive to environmental influences. However,
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
19
its boundaries are less, defined than are those of a critical period. Development may occur
later, but it is harder to induce.
British psychoanalyst John Bowlby (1969) argued that infant smiling, babbling, grasping,
and crying are built-in social signals that encourage the parent to approach, care for, and
interact with the baby. By keeping the mother near, these behaviors help ensure that the
infant will be fed, protected from danger, and provided with the stimulation and affection
necessary for healthy growth. The development of attachment in humans is a lengthy process
involving changes in psychological structures that lead the baby to form a deep affectional tie
with the caregiver. Bowlby (1979) believed that this bond has lifelong consequences,
affecting relationships "from cradle to grave".
Observations by ethologists have shown that many aspects of social behavior, including
emotional expressions, aggression, cooperation, and social play, resemble those of our
primate relatives. Recently, researchers have extended this effort in a new area of research
called evolutionary developmental psychology. It seeks to understand the adaptive value of
species-wide cognitive, emotional, and social competencies as those competencies change
with age.
Evolutionary psychologists are not just concerned with the biological basis of
development. They are also interested in how individuals learn because learning lends
flexibility and greater adaptiveness to behavior. The evolutionary selection benefits of
behavior are believed to be strongest in the first half of life-to ensure survival, reproduction,
and effective parenting. As people age, social and cultural factors become increasingly
important in generating and maintaining high levels of functioning. Vygotsky's sociocultural
theory, serves as an excellent complement to ethology because it highlights social and
cultural contexts for development.
2.3.3 Vygotsky's Sociocultural Theory
Cross-cultural and multicultural research helps us untangle the contributions of
biological and environmental factors to the timing, order of appearance, and diversity of children's
and adults' behaviors. However, this approach can lead us to conclude incorrectly that
one culture is superior in enhancing development, whereas another is deficient. In addition, it
does not help us understand the precise experiences that contribute to cultural differences in
behavior.
Today, more research is examining the relationship of culturally specific practices to
development. The contributions of Russian psychologist Lev Vygotsky (1896-1934) have
played a major role in this trend. Vygotsky's perspective is called sociocultural theory. It
focuses on how culture-the values, beliefs, customs, and skills of a social group-is transmitted
to the next generation. According to Vygotsky, social interaction-in particular,
cooperative dialogues with more knowledgeable members of society-is necessary for children
to acquire the ways of thinking and behaving that make up a community's culture. Vygotsky
believed that as adults and more expert peers help children master culturally meaningful
activities, the communication between them becomes part of children's thinking. As children
internalize the essential features of these dialogues, they can use the language within them to
guide their own thought and actions and to acquire new skills.
Vygotsky agreed with Piaget that children are active, constructive beings. But unlike
Piaget, who emphasized children's independent efforts to make sense of their world,
Vygotsky viewed cognitive development as a socially mediated process-as dependent on the
support that adults and more mature peers provide as children try new tasks.
In Vygotsky's theory, children undergo certain stage wise changes. For example, when
they acquire language, their ability to participate in dialogues with others is greatly enhanced,
and mastery of culturally valued competencies surges forward. When children enter school,
they spend much time discussing language, literacy, and other academic concepts-
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
20
experiences that encourage them to reflect on their own thinking. As a result, they show
dramatic gains in reasoning and problem solving.
2.3.4 Ecological Systems Theory
Ecological systems theory views the person as developing within a complex system of
relationships affected by multiple levels of the surrounding environment. Since the child's
biological dispositions join with environmental forces to mold development, Bronfenbrenner
recently characterized his perspective as, bioecological model.
Bronfenbrenner envisions the environment as a series of nested structures that includes
but extends beyond the home, school, neighborhood, and workplace settings in which people
spend their everyday lives. Each layer of the environment is viewed as having a powerful
impact on development.
A) The Microsystem. The innermost level of the environment is the microsystem, which
consists of activities and interaction patterns in the person's immediate surroundings.
Bronfenbrenner emphasizes that to understand development at this level, we must keep in
mind that all relationships are bidirectional. For example, adults affect children's behavior
but children's biologically and socially influenced characteristics - their physical attributes,
personalities, and capacities-also affect adults' behavior. When these bidirectional
interactions occur often over time, they have an enduring impact on development.
At the same time, other individuals in the microsystem affect the quality of any twoperson
relationship. If they are supportive, then interaction is enhanced. For example, when
parents encourage one another in their child-rearing roles, each engages in more effective
parenting. In contrast, marital conflict-is associated with inconsistent discipline and hostile
reactions toward children. In response, children typically become hostile, and both parent and
child adjustment suffers.
B) The Mesosystem. The second level of Bronfenbrenner's model, the mesosystem,
encompasses connections between microsystems. For example, a child's academic progress
depends not just on activities that take place in classrooms. It is also promoted by parent
involvement in school life and by the extent to which academic learning is carried over into
the home. Among adults, how well a person functions as spouse and parent at home is
affected by relationships in the workplace, and vice versa.
C) The Exosystem. The exosystem refers to social settings that do not contain the
developing person but nevertheless affect experiences in immediate settings. These can be
formal organizations, such as the board of directors in the individual's workplace or health
and welfare services in the community. For example, flexible work schedules, paid maternity
and paternity leave and sick leave for parents whose children are ill are ways that work
settings can help parents’ rear children and, indirectly, enhance the development of both adult
and child. Exosystem supports can also be informal, such as social networks-friends and
extended-family members who provide advice, companionship, and even financial assistance.
Research confirms the negative impact of a breakdown in exosystem activities.
D) The Macrosystem. The outermost level of Bronfenbrenner's model, the macrosystem, is
not a specific context. Instead, it consists of cultural values, laws, customs, and resources.
The priority that the macrosystem gives to the needs of children and adults affects the support
they receive at inner levels of the environment. For example, in countries that require highquality
standards for child care and workplace benefits for employed parents, children are
more likely to have favorable experiences in their immediate settings. And when the
government provides a generous pension plan for retirees, it supports the well-being of the
elderly.
E) A Dynamic, Ever-Changing System. According to Bronfenbrenner, the environment is
not a static force that affects people in a uniform way. Instead, it is dynamic and everchanging.
Whenever individuals add or let go of roles or settings in their lives, the breadth of
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
21
their microsystems changes. These shifts in contexts, or ecological transitions, as
Bronfenbrenner calls them, take place throughout life and are often important turning points
in development. Starting school, entering the workforce, marrying, becoming a parent,
getting divorced, moving, and retiring are examples.
Bronfenbrenner refers to the temporal dimension of his model as the chronosystem (the
prefix chrono means "time"). Changes in life events can be imposed externally. Alternatively,
they can arise from within the person, since individuals select, modify, and create many of
their own settings and experiences. How they do so depend on their age; their physical,
intellectual, and personality characteristics; and their environmental opportunities. Therefore,
in ecological systems theory, development is neither controlled by environmental
circumstances nor driven by inner dispositions. Instead, people are products and producers of
their environments, so both people and their environments form a network of interdependent
effects.
Check Your Progress 1
Give an overall picture of the theoretical perspectives involved in life span development.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
2.4 METHODS
In every science, theories, like those we have just reviewed, guide the collection of
information, its interpretation, and its application to real-life situations. In fact, research
usually begins with a prediction about behavior drawn from a theory, or what we call a
hypothesis. But theories and hypotheses are only the beginning of the many activities that
result in sound evidence on human development. Investigators must decide which participants,
and how many, to include. Then they must figure out what the participants will be
asked to do and when, where, and how many times each will have to be seen. Finally, they
must examine and draw conclusions from their data.
The research strategies commonly used to study human development are, the research
methods-the specific activities of participants, such as taking tests, answering questionnaires,
responding to interviews, or being observed. Then the research designs overall plans for
research studies that permit the best possible test of the investigator's hypothesis. Finally, the
ethical issues involved in doing research with human participants.
There are two reasons for doing research. First, each of us must be a wise and critical
consumer of knowledge. Knowing the strengths and limitations of various research strategies
becomes important in separating dependable information from misleading results. Second,
individuals who work directly with children or adults may be in a unique position to build
bridges between research and practice by carrying out research, either on their own or in
partnership with experienced investigators. Currently, communities and researchers are
collaborating in designing, implementing, and evaluating interventions that enhance lifespan
development.
2.4.1 Common Research Methods
Common methods include systematic observation, self-reports (such as questionnaires
and interviews), clinical or case studies of a single individual, and ethnographies of the life
circumstances of a specific group of people.
2.4.1a Systematic Observation. To find out how people actually behave, a researcher may
choose systematic observation. Observations can be made in different ways. One approach is
to go into the field, or natural environment, and observe the behaviour of interest – a method
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
22
called naturalistic observation.
Observing 3- and 4-year-olds in child-care centers, the researchers recorded each instance
of crying and the reactions of nearby children-whether they ignored, watched curiously,
commented on the child's unhappiness, scolded or teased, or shared, helped, or expressed
sympathy. Caregiver behaviors, such as explaining why a child was crying, mediating
conflict, or offering comfort, can be noted. The great strength of naturalistic observation is
that investigators can see directly the everyday behaviors they hope to explain.
Naturalistic observation also has a major limitation: Not all individuals have the same
opportunity to display a particular behavior in everyday life. Researchers commonly deal
with this difficulty by making structured observations, in which the investigator sets up a
laboratory situation that evokes the behaviour of interest so that every participant has an
equal opportunity. But its great disadvantage is that people do not necessarily behave in the
laboratory as they do in everyday life.
The procedures used to collect systematic observations vary, depending on the nature of
the research problem. Some investigators must describe the entire stream of behavior
everything said and done over a certain time period. In other studies, only one or a few
kinds of behavior are needed, and it is not necessary to preserve the entire behavior stream. In
these instances, researchers use more efficient observation procedures in which they record
only certain events or mark off behaviors on checklists. Systematic observation provides
invaluable information on how children and adults actually behave, but it tells us little about
the reasoning behind their responses. For this kind of information, researchers must turn to
self-report techniques.
2.4.1b Self-Reports. Self-reports are instruments that ask participants to provide
information on their perceptions, thoughts, abilities, feelings, attitudes, beliefs, and past
experiences. They range from relatively unstructured interviews to highly structured
interviews, questionnaires, and tests.
In a clinical interview, a flexible, conversational style is used to probe for the participant's
point of view. Piaget questioned a 5-year-old child about his understanding of dreams:
Where does the dream come from?-I think you sleep so well that you dream.-Does it come
from us or from outside?-From outside.- What do we dream with?-I don't know.- With the
hands? .. With nothing?- Yes, with nothing.- When you are in bed and you dream, where is
the dream?-In my bed, under the blanket. I don't really know. If it was in my stomach, the
bones would be in the way and I shouldn't see it .-Is the dream there when you sleep?-Yes, it
is in the bed beside me.
Notice how Piaget encouraged the child to expand his ideas. Although a researcher
conducting clinical interviews with more than one participant would typically ask the same
first question to ensure a common task, individualized prompts are given to evoke a fuller
picture of each person's reasoning.
The clinical interview has two major strengths. First, it permits people to display their
thoughts in terms that are as close as possible to the way they think in everyday life. Second,
the clinical interview can provide a large amount of information in a fairly brief period. For
example, in an hour-long session, we can obtain a wide range of information on child rearing
from a parent or on life circumstances from an elder-much more than we could capture by
observing for the same amount of time.
A major limitation of the clinical interview has to do with the accuracy with which people
report their thoughts, feelings, and experiences. Some participants, desiring to please the
interviewer, may make up answers that do not represent their actual thinking. When asked
about past events, they may have trouble recalling exactly what happened. And because the
clinical interview depends on verbal ability and expressiveness, it may underestimate the
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
23
capacities of individuals who have difficulty putting their thoughts into words.
The clinical interview has also been criticized because of its flexibility. When questions
are phrased differently for each participant, responses may be due to the manner of interviewing
rather than to real differences in the way people think about a certain topic.
Structured interviews, in which each participant is asked the same set of questions in the
same way, can eliminate this problem.
2.4.1c The Clinical or Case Study, Method. An outgrowth of psychoanalytic theory, which
stresses the importance of understanding a single life history, the clinical, or case study,
method brings together a wide range of information on one person, including interviews,
observations, and sometimes test scores. The aim is to obtain as complete a picture as possible
of that individual's psychological functioning and the experiences that led up to it.
The clinical method is well suited to studying the development of individuals who are few
in number and who vary widely in characteristics. For example, the method has been used to
find out what contributes to the accomplishments of prodigies extremely gifted children who
attain adult competence in a field before age 10. Consider Ashok, a boy who read, wrote, and
composed musical pieces before he was out of diapers. By age 4, Ashok was deeply involved
in mastering human symbol systems-BASIC for the computer, Sanskrit, Hindi, Tamil,
Malayalam, English, music, and mathematics. Ashok’s parents provided a home rich in
stimulation and reared him with affection, firmness, and humor. They searched for schools in
which he could both develop his abilities and form rewarding social relationships. He
graduated from college at age 18 and continues to pursue musical composition. Ashok would
not have realized his abilities without the chance combination of his special gift with
nurturing, committed parents.
The clinical method yields richly detailed case narratives that offer valuable insights into
the multiplicity of factors that affect development. Nevertheless, like all other methods, it has
drawbacks. Information is often collected unsystematically and subjectively, permitting too
much leeway for researchers' theoretical preferences to bias their observations and interpretations.
In addition, investigators cannot assume that their conclusions apply to anyone other
than the person studied. Even when patterns emerge across several cases, it is wise to try to
confirm them with other research strategies.
2.4.1d Methods for Studying Culture. A growing interest in the impact of culture has led
researchers to adjust the methods just considered as well as tap procedures specially devised
for cross-cultural and multicultural research. Which approach investigators choose depends
on their research goals.
Sometimes researchers are interested in characteristics that are believed to be universal
but that vary in degree from one society to the next. These investigators might ask, do parents
make greater maturity demands of children in some cultures than in others? How strong are
gender stereotypes in different nations? In each instance, several cultural groups will be
compared, and all participants must be questioned or observed in the same way. Therefore,
researchers use the self-report and observational procedures, adapting them through
translation so they can be understood in each cultural context. For example, to study cultural
variation in parenting attitudes, the same questionnaire, asking for ratings on such items as "If
my child gets into trouble, I expect him or her to handle the problem mostly by himself or
herself;' is given to all participants.
At other times, researchers want to uncover the cultural meanings of children's and adults'
behaviors by becoming as familiar as possible with their way of life. To achieve this goal,
researchers rely on a method borrowed from the field of anthropology-ethnography. Like the
clinical method, ethnographic research is largely a descriptive, qualitative technique. But
instead of aiming to understand a single individual, it is directed toward understanding a
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
24
culture or a distinct social group, achieving its goals t hrough participant observation.
Typically, the researcher lives with the cultural community for a period of months or years,
participating in all aspects of its daily life. Extensive field notes are gathered, consisting of a
mix of observations, self-reports from members of the culture, and careful interpretations by
the investigator. Later, these notes are put together into a description of the community that
tries to capture its unique values and social processes.
Ethnographers strive to minimize their influence on the culture they are studying by
becoming part of it. Nevertheless, at times their presence does alter the situation. And as with
clinical research, investigators' cultural values and theoretical commitments sometimes lead
them to observe selectively or misinterpret what they see. In addition, the findings of
ethnographic studies cannot be assumed to apply, or generalize, beyond the people and
settings in which the research was conducted.
Check Your Progress 2
State the common research methods.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
2.4.2 General Research Designs
In deciding on a research design, investigators choose a way of setting up a study that
permits them to test their hypotheses with the greatest certainty possible. Two main types of
designs are used in all research on human behavior: correlational and experimental.
2.4.2a Correlational Design. In a correlational design, researchers gather information on
already-existing groups of individuals, generally in natural life circumstances, and make no
effort to alter their experiences. Then they look at relationships between participants’
characteristics and their behaviour or development. For example, does the arrival of a baby
influence a couple's marital satisfaction? Does the death of a spouse in old age affect the
surviving partner's physical health and psychological well-being? In these and many other
instances, the conditions of interest are difficult or impossible to arrange and control and
must be studied as they currently exist.
Correlational studies have one major limitation: We cannot infer cause and effect. For
example, if we find that parental interaction is related to children's intelligence, we still do
not know whether parents' behavior actually causes intellectual differences among children.
In fact, the opposite is certainly possible. The behaviors of highly intelligent children may be
so attractive that they cause parents to interact more favorably. Or a third variable that we did
not even think about studying, such as amount of noise and distraction in the home, may be
causing both maternal interaction and children's intelligence to change.
2.4.2b Experimental Design. An experimental design permits inferences about cause and
effect because researchers use an evenhanded procedure to assign people to two or more
treatment conditions. In an experiment, the events and behaviors of interest are divided into
two types: independent and dependent variables. The independent variable is the one the
investigator expects to cause changes in another variable. The dependent variable is the one
the investigator expects to be influenced by the independent variable. Cause-and-effect
relationships can be detected because the researcher directly controls or manipulates changes
in the independent variable by exposing participants to the treatment conditions. Then the
researcher compares their performance on measures of the dependent variable.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
25
I n a laboratory experiment, investigators can explore the impact of adults' angry
interactions on children's adjustment. The hypothesis that, “the angry encounters will have a
significant (independent variable) effect on children's emotional reactions (dependent
variable)”. Four- and 5-year-olds were brought one at a time to a laboratory, accompanied by
their mothers. One group was exposed to an unresolved-anger treatment, in which two adult
actors entered the room and argued but did not work out their disagreements. The other group
witnessed a resolved-anger treatment, in which the adults ended their disputes by apologizing
and compromising. During a follow-up adult conflict, children in the resolved-anger
treatment showed less distress, as measured by anxious facial expressions, freezing in place,
and seeking closeness to their mothers. The experiment revealed that anger resolution can
reduce the stressful impact of adult conflict on children.
In experimental studies, investigators must take special precautions to control for
participants' characteristics that could reduce the accuracy of their findings. For example, in
the study just described, if a greater number of children from homes high in parental conflict
ended up in the unresolved anger treatment, we could not tell whether the independent
variable or the children's backgrounds produced the results. To protect against this problem,
researchers engage in random assignment of participants to treatment conditions. By using an
unbiased procedure, such as drawing numbers out of chits or flipping a coin, investigators
increase the likelihood that participants' characteristics will be equally distributed across
treatment groups.
2.4.2c Modified Experimental Designs: Field and Natural Experiments. M o s t
experiments are conducted in laboratories where researchers can achieve the maximum -
possible control over treatment conditions. But, findings obtained in laboratories may not
always apply to everyday situations. The ideal solution to this problem is to do experiments
in the field as a complement to laboratory investigations. In field experiments, investigators
capitalize on rare opportunities to assign people randomly to treatment conditions in natural
settings. In the laboratory experiment i t can be concluded that the emotional climate
established by adults affects children's behavior in the laboratory.
Another study the research was carried out in a child-care center. A caregiver deliberately
interacted differently with two groups of preschoolers. In one condition (the nurturant
treatment), she modeled many instances of warmth and helpfulness. In the second condition
(the control, since it involved no treatment), she behaved as usual, with no special emphasis
on concern for others. Two weeks later, the researchers created several situations that called
for helpfulness. For example, a visiting mother asked each child to watch her baby for a few
moments, but the baby's toys had fallen out of the box. The investigators found that children
exposed to the nurturant treatment were much more likely to return toys to the baby than
were those in the control condition.
Often researchers cannot randomly assign participants and manipulate conditions in the
real world. Sometimes they can compromise by conducting natural experiments. Treatments
that already exist, such as different child-care centers, schools, workplaces, or retirement
homes, are compared. These studies differ from correlational research only in that groups of
participants are carefully chosen to ensure that their characteristics are as much alike as
possible. In this way, investigators rule out as best they can alternative explanations for their
treatment effects. But despite these efforts, natural experiments are unable to achieve the
precision and rigor of true experimental research.
2.4.3 Designs for Studying Development
Scientists interested in human development require information about the way research
participants change over time. To answer questions about development, they must extend
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
26
correlational and experimental approaches to include measurements at different ages.
Longitudinal and cross-sectional designs are special developmental research strategies. In
each, age comparisons form the basis of the research plan.
2.4.3a The Longitudinal Design. In a longitudinal design, a group of participants are
studied repeatedly at different ages, and changes are noted as the participants mature. The
time spanned may be relatively short (a few months to several years) or very long (a decade
or even a lifetime). The longitudinal approach has two major strengths. First, because it
tracks the performance of each person over time, researchers can identify common patterns of
development as well as individual differences. Second, longitudinal studies permit
investigators to examine relationships between early and later events and behaviors.
Researchers want to know whether children who display extreme personality styles-either
angry and explosive or shy and withdrawn-retain the same dispositions when they become
adults. In addition, the researchers wanted to know what kinds of experiences promote
stability or change in personality and what consequences explosiveness and shyness have for
long-term adjustment. The researchers searched into the archives of the Guidance Study, a
well-known longitudinal investigation initiated in 1928 at the University of California,
Berkeley, and continued for several decades.
Results revealed that the two personality styles were only moderately stable. Between
ages 8 and 30, a good number of individuals remained the same, whereas others changed
substantially. When stability did occur, it appeared to be due to a "snowballing effect;' in
which children evoked responses from adults and peers that acted to maintain their
dispositions. In other words, explosive youngsters were likely to be treated with anger and
hostility (to which they reacted with even greater unruliness), whereas shy children were apt
to be ignored.
Persistence of extreme personality styles affected many areas of adult adjustment. For
men, the results of early explosiveness were most apparent in their work lives, in the form of
conflicts with supervisors, frequent job changes, and unemployment. Since few women in
this sample of an earlier generation worked after marriage, their family lives were most
affected. Explosive girls grew up to be hotheaded wives and parents who were especially
prone to divorce. Sex differences in the long-term consequences of shyness were even
greater. Men who had been withdrawn in childhood were delayed in marrying, becoming
fathers, and developing stable careers. Because a withdrawn, unassertive style was socially
acceptable for females, women who had shy personalities showed no special adjustment
problems.
Problems in Conducting Longitudinal Research. Despite their strengths, longitudinal
investigations pose a number of problems. For example, participants may move away or drop
out of the research for other reasons. This changes the original sample so it no longer
represents the population to whom researchers would like to generalize their findings. Also,
from repeated study, people may become "test-wise:' Their performance may improve as a
result of practice effects--better test-taking skills and increased familiarity with the test-not
because of factors commonly associated with development.
But the most widely discussed threat to longitudinal findings is cohort effects: Individuals
born in the same time period are influenced by a particular set of historical and cultural
conditions. Results based on one cohort may not apply to people developing in other times.
Similarly, a longitudinal study of the lifespan would probably result in quite different
findings if it were carried out in the first decade of the twenty-first century, around the time
of World War II, or during the Great Depression of the 1930s.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
27
2.4.3b The Cross-Sectional Design. The length of time it takes for many behaviors to
change, even in limited longitudinal studies, has led researchers to turn toward a more
convenient strategy for studying development. In the cross-sectional design, groups of people
differing in age are studied at the same point in time.
A study in which students in standards 3, 6, 9, and 12 filled out a questionnaire about their
sibling relationships is a good example. Findings revealed that sibling interaction was
characterized by greater equality and less power assertion with age. Also, feelings of sibling
companionship declined during adolescence. The researchers thought that several factors
contributed to these age differences. As later-born children become more competent and
independent, they no longer need, and are probably less willing to accept, direction from
older siblings. In addition, as adolescents move from psychological dependence on the family
to greater involvement with peers, they may have less time and emotional need to invest in
siblings.
2.4.3c Problems in Conducting Cross-Sectional Research. The cross-sectional design is an
efficient strategy for describing age-related trends. Because participants are measured only
once, researchers need not be concerned about such difficulties as participant dropout or
practice effects. But evidence about change at the level at which, it actually occurs-the individual-
is not available. For example, in the cross-sectional study of sibling relationships,
comparisons are limited to age-group averages. We cannot tell if important individual
differences exist. Indeed, longitudinal findings reveal that adolescents vary considerably in
the changing quality of their sibling relationships, many becoming more distant but some
becoming more supportive and intimate.
Cross-sectional studies--especially those that cover a wide age span-have another
problem. Like longitudinal research, they can be threatened by cohort effects. For example,
comparisons of 10-year-old cohorts, 20-year-old cohorts, and 30-year-old cohorts-groups
born and reared in different years-may not really represent age-related changes. Instead, they
may reflect unique experiences associated with the historical period in which each age group
grew up.
2.4.3d Improving Developmental Designs. To overcome some of the limitations of
longitudinal and cross-sectional research, investigators sometimes combine the two
approaches. One way of doing so is the longitudinal-sequential design, in which a sequence
of samples (two or more age groups) is followed for a number of years.
The design has two advantages. First, it permits us to find out whether cohort effects are
operating by comparing people of the same age who were born in different years. We can
compare the three samples at ages 20, 30, and 40. If they do not differ, we can rule out cohort
effects. Second, we can make longitudinal and cross-sectional comparisons. If outcomes are
similar in both, then we can be especially confident about our findings.
To date, only a handful of longitudinal sequential studies have been conducted. Yet the
design permits researchers to profit from the strengths of both longitudinal and cross
sectional strategies. And in uncovering cohort effects, it also helps explain diversity in
development.
Check Your Progress 3
A. Bring out the general research designs
B. State the special designs for studying Life span development.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
28
2.5 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) The difference between mid twentieth century theories and Recent Theoretical
perspectives
ii) Since research forms the core of any field is the methods like Common Research
Methods, General Research Designs and the special Designs for Studying
Development are discussed.
2.6 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. The theoretical perspectives involved in life span development using
i) The Psychoanalytic Perspective
ii) Behaviorism and Social Learning Theory
iii) Piaget's Cognitive-Developmental Theory
iv) Information Processing
v) Ethology and Evolutionary Developmental Psychology
vi) Vygotsky's Sociocultural Theory
vii) Ecological Systems Theory
2. The Common research methods include
i) Systematic observation,
ii) Self-reports (such as questionnaires and interviews),
iii) Clinical or case studies of a single individual, and
iv) Ethnographies of the life circumstances of a specific group of people.
3. AThe two main types of general designs used in all research on human behavior include
i) Correlational and
ii) Experimental
C. The extension of Correlational and experimental research are known as special
developmental research strategies they include
i) Longitudinal and
ii) Cross-sectional designs.
2.7 Lesson – End Activities
1) State the limitations of longitudinal design in studying development.
2) Explain how the correlation design are more useful in studying development.
2.8 References
1) Wexberg, E., Individual Psychology (Trans. By W.B. Wolf) London: Allen & Unwin,
1987.
2) Woodworth, R.S., Contemporary Schools and Psychology, London: Methuen : 1985.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
29
LESSON – 3
GENETIC FOUNDATION - REPRODUCTIVE CHOICES -
ENVIRONMENTAL CONTEXTS FOR DEVELOPMENT
Contents
3.0 Aims and Objectives
3.1 Introduction
3.2 The Genetic Code
3.3 The Sex Cells
3.4 Male or Female
3.5 Multiple Births
3.6 Patterns of Genetic Inheritance
3.6.1 Dominant-Recessive Inheritance
3.6.2 Codominance
3.6.3 X-Linked Inheritance
3.6.4 Genetic Imprinting
3.6.5 Mutation
3.6.6 Polygenic Inheritance
3.7 Chromosomal Abnormalities
3.7.1 Down Syndrome
3.7.2 Abnormalities of the Sex Chromosomes
3.8 Reproductive Choices
3.8.1 Genetic Counseling
3.8.2 Prenatal Diagnosis and Fetal Medicine
3.8.3 Genetic Testing
3.8.4 Adoption
3.9 Environmental Contexts of Development
3.9.1 The Family
3.9.2 Socioeconomic Status and Family Functioning
3.9.3 The Impact of Poverty
3.9.4 Cultural Values and Practices
3. 10 Let Us Sum Up
3.11 Check your Progress
3.12 Lesson – End Activities
3.13 References
3.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This lesson provides an over view of genetic foundations for the development of a life
and the choices parents have to reproduce a child. It will also focus on significance of the
environment influence on development.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
i) address the genetic foundation of development
ii) mention the chromosomal abnormalities during development
iii) state the choices that a parent has in reproducing a child
iv) list the role environment in development
3.1 INTRODUCTION
Each and every human being is made up of trillions of separate units called cells.
Inside every cell is a control center, or nucleus, that contains rod like structures called
chromosomes which store and transmit genetic information. Human chromosomes come in
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
30
23 matching pairs. Each pair member corresponds to the other in size, shape, and genetic
functions. One is inherited from the mother and one from the father.
3.2 THE GENETIC CODE
Chromosomes are made up of a chemical substance called deoxyribonucleic acid, or
DNA. DNA is a long, double-stranded molecule that looks like a twisted ladder. Each rung of
the ladder consists of specific pair chemical substances called bases, joined together between
the two sides. It is this sequence of bases that provides genetic instructions. A gene is a
segment of DNA along the length of the chromosome. Genes can be of different lengthsperhaps
100 to several thousand ladder rungs long. An estimated 30,000 genes lie along the
human chromosomes.
We share some of our genetic makeup with even the simplest organisms, such as bacteria
and molds, and most of it with other mammals, especially primates. Between 98 and percent
of 99 percent of chimpanzee and human DNA is identical. This means that only a small
portion of our heredity is responsible for the traits that make us human, from our upright gait
to our extraordinary language and cognitive capacities. And the genetic variation from one
human to the next is even less. Individuals around the world are about 99.1 percent
genetically identical. Only a tiny quantity of DNA contributes to human variation in traits and
capacities.
A unique feature of DNA is that it can duplicate itself through a process caused mitosis.
This special ability permits the one-celled fertilized ovum to develop into a complex human
being composed of a great many cells. During mitosis, the chromosomes copy themselves. As
a result, each new body cell contains the same number of chromosomes and identical genetic
information.
Genes accomplish their task by sending instructions for making a rich assortment of
proteins to the cytoplasm, the area surrounding the cell nucleus. Proteins, which trigger
chemical reactions throughout the body, are the biological foundation on which our
characteristics are built. How do humans, with far fewer genes than scientists once thought
(only twice as many as the worm or fly), manage to develop into such complex beings? The
answer lies in the proteins our genes make, which break up and reassemble in staggering
variety-about 10 to 20 million altogether. In simpler species, the number of proteins is far
more limited. Furthermore, the communication system between the cell nucleus and cytoplasm,
which fine-tunes gene activity, is more intricate in humans than in simpler organisms.
Within the cell, a wide range of environmental factors modify gene expression. So even at
this microscopic level, biological events are the result of both genetic and non genetic forces.
3.3 THE SEX CELLS
New individuals are created when two special cells called gametes, or sex cells - the
sperm and ovum--combine. A gamete contains only 23 chromosomes, half as many as a regular
body cell. Gametes are formed through a cell division process called meiosis, which
ensures that a constant quantity of genetic material is transmitted from one generation to the
next. When sperm and ovum unite at conception, the cell that results, called a zygote, will
again have 46 chromosomes.
In meiosis, the chromosomes pair up and exchange segments, so that genes from one are
replaced by genes from another. Then chance determines which member of each pair will
gather with others and end up in the same gamete. These events make the likelihood of non
twin offspring of the same two parents being genetically the same extremely slim-about 1 in
700 trillion. Therefore, meiosis helps us understand why siblings differ from each other, even
though they have features in common because their genotypes come from the same pool of
parental genes.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
31
3.4 MALE OR FEMALE
Note that 22 of the 23 pairs of chromosomes are matching pairs, called autsomes. The
twenty-third pair consists of sex chromosomes. In females, this pair is called XX; in males, it
is called XY. The X is a relatively long chromosome, whereas the Y is short and carries little
genetic material. When gametes are formed in males, the X and Y chromosomes separate into
different sperm cells. In females, all gametes carry an X chromosome. Therefore, the sex of
the new organism is determined by whether an X-bearing or a Y-bearing sperm fertilizes the
ovum.
3.5 MULTIPLE BIRTHS
Unlike most animals, the human baby usually comes into the world alone. Exceptionsmultiple
births-occur in two different ways.
One mechanism occurs when the woman's body releases two ova within a short time of each
other, and both are fertilized. The two babies that are conceived are called fraternal, two-egg,
or dizygotic twins. Since they are created by different ova and different sperm cells, they are
no more alike in genetic makeup than any other siblings. They may be of the same sex or
different sexes.
The other mechanism is the division of a single ovum after fertilization. Identical, One-egg,
or monozygotic twins, which results from this cell division, have the same genetic heritage.
Any differences they will later show must be due to the influences of environment. They are
always of the same sex. Triplets, quadruplets, and other multiple births result from either one
of these processes or a combination of both.
Identical twins seem to be the result of an "accident" in prenatal development, unrelated to
either genetic or environmental influences. They account for one-fourth to one-third of all
twins.
Fraternal twins are more common under some circumstances. More are being born these
days because of fertility drugs that stimulate ovulation and often cause the release of more
than one Ovum. These twins are more likely to be born to women who have had two or more
pregnancies, to older women, in families with a history of fraternal twins, and in various
ethnic groups. Fraternal twin births are most common among African Americans (1 in 70
births), East Indians, and northern Europeans, and least common among Asians other than
East Indians (1 in 150 births among the Japanese and 1 in 300 among the Chinese). These differences
are probably due to hormonal differences in women. Monozygotic twins seem to be
born through an accident of prenatal life and their incidence is about the same in all ethnic
groups. They are always of the same sex and have exactly the same genetic heritage: these
children look so much alike.
3.6 PATTERNS OF GENETIC INHERITANCE
Patterns of genetic inheritance - the way genes from each parent interact-explains the color of
hair, skin etc. Except for the XY pair in males, all chromosomes come in corresponding pairs.
Two forms of each gene occur at the same place on the autosomes, one inherited from the
mother and one from the father. If the genes from both parents are alike, the child is
homozygous and will display the inherited trait. If the genes are different, then the
heterozygous, and relationships between the genes determine the trait that will appear.
3.6.1 Dominant-Recessive Inheritance. In many heterozygous pairings, dominant-recessive
inheritance occurs: one gene affects the child's characteristics. It is called dominant; the
second gene, which has no effect, is called recessive. Hair color is an example. The gene for
dark hair is dominant (we can represent it with a capital D), whereas the one for blond hair is
recessive (symbolized by a lowercase b). A d who inherits a homozygous pair of dominant
genes (DD); and a child who inherits a heterozygous pair (Db) will be d. haired, even though
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
32
their genotypes differ. Blond hair can result only from having two recessive genes (bb). Still,
heterozygous individuals with just one recessive (Db) can pass that trait to their children.
Therefore they are called carriers of the trait.
Some human characteristics that follow the rules of dominant-recessive inheritance are
given in Table 1 and Table 2. Many disabilities and diseases are the product of recessive
genes. One of the most frequently occurring recessive disorders is phenylketonuria, or PKU.
It affects the way the body breaks down proteins contained in many foods. Infants born with
two recessive genes lack an enzyme that converts one of the basic amino acids that make up
proteins (phenylalanine) into a by-product essential for body functioning (tyrosine). Without
this enzyme, phenylalanine quickly builds to toxic levels that damage the central nervous
system. By 1 year, infants with PKU are permanently retarded.
Only rarely are serious diseases due to dominant genes. Children who inherit the
dominant gene always develop the disorder. They seldom live long enough to reproduce, and
the harmful dominant gene is eliminated from the family's heredity in a single generation.
Some dominant disorders, however, do persist. One of them is Huntington disease, a
condition in which the central nervous system degenerates. Its symptoms usually do not
appear until age 35 or later, after the person has passed the dominant gene to his or her
children.
Table 1 Examples of Dominant and Recessive Characteristics
Dominant Recessive
Dark hair Normal hair
Normal hair
Curly hair
Nonred hair
Facial dimples
Normal hearing
Normal vision
Farsightedness
Normal vision
Normally pigmented skin
Double-jointedness
Type A blood
Type B blood
Rh-positive blood
Blond hair Pattern baldness
Pattern baldness
Straight hair
Red hair
No dimples
Some forms of deafness
Nearsightedness
Normal vision
Congenital eye cataracts
Albinism
Normal joints
Type 0 blood
Type 0 blood
Rh-negative blood
Table 2 Examples of Dominant and Recessive Diseases
Disease Description Mode of
Inheritance
Incidence Treatment
Autosomal
Diseases
Cooley's
anemia
Cystic
fibrosis
Phenylketonuria
(PKU)
Pale appearance, retarded
physical growth, and lethargic
behavior begin in infancy.
Lungs, liver, and pancreas
secrete large amounts of thick
mucus, leading to breathing and
digestive difficulties.
Inability to metabolize the
amino acid phenylalanine,
contained in many proteins,
Recessive
Recessive
Recessive
1 in 500 births to
parents of Mediterranean
descent
1 in 2,000 to 2,500
Caucasian births; 1
in 16,000 births to
North Americans
of African descent
1 in 8,000 births
Frequent blood transfusion; death
from complications usually
occurs by adolescence.
Bronchial drainage, prompt
treatment of respiratory infection,
dietary management. Advances in
medical care allow survival with
good life quality into adulthood.
Placing the child on a special diet
results in average intelligence and
normal lifespan. Subtle
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
33
Sickle cell
anemia
Tay-Sachs
disease
Huntington
disease
Marfan
syndrome
X-Linked
Diseases
Duchenne
muscular
dystrophy
Hemophilia
Diabetes
insipidus
causes severe central nervous
system damage in the first year
of life.
Abnormal sickling of red blood
cells causes oxygen deprivation,
pain, swelling, and tissue
damage. Anemia and
susceptibility to infections, especially
pneumonia, occur.
Central nervous system
degeneration, with onset at
about 6 months, leads to poor
muscle tone, blindness, deafness,
and convulsions.
C e n t r a l n e r v o us s y s t e m
degeneration leads to muscular
coordination difficulties, mental
deterioration, and personality
changes. Symptoms usually do
not appear until age 35 or later.
Tall, slender build; thin,
elongated arms and legs. Heart
defects and eye abnormalities,
especially of the lens. Excessive
lengthening of the body results
in a variety of skeletal defects.
Degenerative muscle disease.
Abnormal gait, loss of ability to
walk between 7 and 13 years of
age.
Blood fails to clot normally. Can
lead to severe internal bleeding
and tissue damage.
Insufficient production of the
hormone vasopressin results in
excessive thirst and urination.
Dehydration can cause central
nervous system damage.
Recessive
Recessive
Dominant
Dominant
Recessive
Recessive
Recessive
1 in 500 births to
North Americans
of African descent
1 in 3,600 births to
Jews of European
descent and to
French Canadians
1 in 18,000 to
25,000 births
1 in 20,000 births
1 in 3,000 to 5,000
male births
1 in 4,000 to 7,000
male births
1 in 2,500 male
births
difficulties with planning and
problem-solving are often
present.
B l o o d t r a n s f o r m a t i o n s ,
painkillers, prompt treatment of
infection. No known cure; 50
percent die by age:
None. Death by 3 to 4 years of
age.
None. Death occurs 10 to 20
years after symptom onset.
Correction of heart and eye
defects, sometimes possible.
Death from heart failure in young
adulthood is common..,
None. Death from respiratory
infection. or weakening of the
heart muscle usually occurs in
adolescence.
Blood transfusions. Safety
precautions to prevent injury.
Hormone replacement.
3.6.2 Codominance. In some heterozygous circumstances, the dominant-recessive
relationship does not hold completely. Instead, we see Codominance, a pattern of inheritance
in which both genes influence the person’s characteristics.
The sickle cell trait, a heterozygous condition present in many black Africans, provides
an example. Sickle cell anemia (Table 2) occurs in full form when a child inherits two
recessive genes. They cause the usually round red blood cells to become sickle (or crescentmoon)
shaped, especially under low-oxygen conditions. The sickled cells clog the blood
vessels and block the flow of blood. Individuals who have the disorder suffer severe attacks
involving intense pain, swelling, and tissue damage. They generally die in the first 20 years
of life; few live past age 40. Heterozygous individuals are protected from the disease under
most circumstances. However, when they experience oxygen deprivation-for example, at
high altitudes or after intense physical exercise-the single recessive gene asserts itself, and a
temporary, mild form of the illness occurs.
The sickle cell gene is common among black Africans for a special reason. Carriers of it
are more resistant to malaria than are individuals with two genes for normal red blood cells.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
34
In Africa, where malaria is common, these carriers survived and reproduced more frequently
than others, leading the gene to be maintained in the black population.
3.6.3 X-Linked Inheritance. Males and females have an equal chance of inheriting recessive
disorders carried on the autosomes, such as PKU and sickle cell anemia. But when a harmful
recessive gene is carried on the X chromosome, X-linked inheritance applies. Males are more
likely to be affected because their sex chromosomes do not match. In females, any recessive
gene on one X chromosome has a good chance of being suppressed by a dominant gene on
the other X. But the Y chromosome is only about one-third as long and therefore lacks many
corresponding genes to override those on the X. A well-known example is hemophilia, a
disease in which the blood fails to clot normally.
Besides X-linked disorders, many sex differences reveal the male to be at a disadvantage.
Rates of miscarriage, infant and childhood deaths, birth defects, learning disabilities, behavior
disorders, and mental retardation are greater for boys. It is possible that these sex
differences can be traced to the genetic code. The female, with two X chromosomes, benefits
from a greater variety of genes. Nature, however, seems to have adjusted for the male's
disadvantage. Worldwide, about 106 boys are born for every 100 girls, and judging from
miscarriage and abortion statistics, an even greater number of males are conceived.
Nevertheless, in recent decades the proportion of male births has declined in many
industrialized countries, including Canada, Denmark, Germany, Finland, the Netherlands,
Norway, and the United States. Some researchers blame increased occupational and
community exposure to pesticides for a reduction in sperm counts overall, especially Ybearing
sperm. But the precise cause is unknown.
3.6.4 Genetic Imprinting. More than 1,000 human characteristics follow the rules of
dominant-recessive and codominant inheritance. In these cases, whichever parent contributes
a gene to the new individual, the gene responds in the same way. Geneticists, however, have
identified some exceptions. In genetic imprinting, genes are imprinted, or chemically marked,
in such a way that one member of the pair (either the mother's or the father's) is activated,
regardless of its makeup. The imprint is often temporary: It may be erased in the next
generation, and it may not occur in all individuals.
Imprinting helps us understand certain puzzling genetic patterns. For example, children
are more likely to develop diabetes if their father, rather than their mother, suffers from it.
And people with asthma or hay fever tend to have mothers, not fathers, with the illness.
Imprinting may also explain why Huntington disease, when inherited from the father, tends to
emerge at an earlier age and to progress more rapidly.
Genetic imprinting can also operate on the sex chromosomes, as fragile X syndrome
reveals. In this disorder, an abnormal repetition of a sequence of DNA bases occurs on the X
chromosome, damaging a particular gene. Fragile X syndrome is the most common inherited
cause of mild to moderate mental retardation. It has also been linked to 2 to 3 percent of cases
of autism, a serious emotional disorder of early childhood involving bizarre, self-stimulating
behavior and delayed or absent language and communication. Research reveals that the
defective gene at the fragile site is expressed only when it is passed from mother to child.
3.6.5 Mutation. The harmful genes created by mutation, a sudden but permanent change in a
segment of DNA. A mutation may affect only one or two genes, or it may involve many
genes, as in the chromosomal disorders. Some mutations occur spontaneously, simply by
chance. Others are caused by hazardous environmental agents in our food supply or the air
we breathe.
Although nonionizing forms of radiation-electromagnetic waves and microwaves-have no
demonstrated impact on DNA, ionizing (high-energy) radiation is an established cause of
mutation. Women who receive repeated doses before conception are more likely to miscarry
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
35
or give birth to children with hereditary defects. Genetic abnormalities, such as physical
malformations and childhood cancer, are also higher when fathers are exposed to radiation in
their occupations. However, infrequent and mild exposure to radiation does not cause genetic
damage. Instead, high doses over a long period impair DNA.
3.6.6 Polygenic Inheritance. The individual differences are much easier to trace to their
genetic origins than are characteristics that vary continuously among people, such as height,
weight, intelligence, and personality. These traits are due to polygenic inheritance, in which
many genes determine the characteristic in question. Polygenic i nheritance is complex, and
much about it is still unknown.
Check Your Progress 1
Discuss some of the aspects of genetic foundations and its implications on development of a
normal child.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
3.7 CHROMOSOMAL ABNORMALITIES
Besides harmful recessive genes, normalities of the chromosomes are a major cause of
serious developmental p roblems. Most chromosomal defects result from mistakes during
meiosis, w hen the ovum and sperm are formed. A chromosome pair does not separate
properly or part of a chromosome breaks off. Because these errors involve more DNA than
problems due to single genes, usually produce many physical and mental symptoms.
3.7.1 Down Syndrome. The most common chromosomal disorder, occurring in 1 out of
every 800 live births, is Down syndrome. In 95 percent of cases it results from a failure of the
twenty first pair of chromosomes to separate during meiosis, so the new individual inherits
three of these chromosomes rather the normal two. In other less frequent forms, an extra
broken piece of a twenty first chromosome is present. Or an error occurs during the early
stages of mitosis, causing some but not all body cells to have the defective chromosomal
makeup (called a mosaic pattern). In these instances, because less genetic material is
involved, symptoms of the disorder are less extreme.
The consequences of Down syndrome include mental retardation, memory and speech
problems, limited vocabulary and slow motor development. Affected individuals also have
distinct physical features – a short, stocky build, a flattened face, a protruding tongue,
almond-shaped eyes, and an unusual crease running across the palm of the hand. In addition,
infants with Down syndrome are often born with eye cataracts and heart and intestinal
defects. Three decades ago, most died by early adulthood. Today because of medical
advances, many survive into their sixties and beyond. 1
Infants with Down syndrome smile less readily, s how poor eye-to-eye contact, and
explore objects less persistently. But when parents encourage them to engage with their
surroundings, Down syndrome children develop more favorably. They also benefit from
infant and preschool intervention programs, although emotional, social, and motor skills
improve more than intellectual performance. Thus, environmental factors affect how well
children with Down syndrome fare.
The risk of Down syndrome rises with maternal age, from 1 in 1,900 births at age 20, to 1
in 300 at age 35, to 1 in 30 at age 45. Geneticists believe that the ova, present in the woman's
body since her own prenatal period, weaken over time. As a result, chromosomes do not
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
36
separate properly as they complete the process of meiosis at conception. In about 5 to 10
percent of cases, the extra genetic material originates with the father. However, Down
syndrome and other chromosomal abnormalities are not related to advanced paternal age. In
these instances, the mutation occurs for other unknown reasons.
3.7.2 Abnormalities of the Sex Chromosomes. Disorders of the autosomes other than Down
syndrome usually disrupt development so severely that miscarriage occurs. When such babies
are born, they rarely survive beyond early childhood. In contrast, abnormalities of the sex
chromosomes usually lead to fewer problems. In fact, sex chromosome disorders often are
not recognized until adolescence when, in some of the deviations, puberty is delayed. The
most common problems involve the presence of an extra chromosome (either X or Y) or the
absence of one X in females.
A variety of myths exist about individuals with sex chromosome disorders. For example,
males with XYY syndrome are not necessarily more aggressive and antisocial than XY males.
And most children with sex chromosome disorders do not suffer from mental retardation.
Instead, their intellectual problems are usually very specific. Verbal difficulties-for example,
with reading and vocabulary-are common among girls with triple X syndrome (XXX) and
boys with Klinefelter syndrome (XXY), both of whom inherit an extra X chromosome. In contrast,
girls with Turner syndrome (XO), who are missing an X, have trouble with spatial
relationships-for example, drawing pictures, telling right from left, following travel
directions, and noticing changes in facial expressions.
3.8 REPRODUCTIVE CHOICES
In the past, many couples with genetic disorders in their families chose not to bear a child
at all rather than risk the birth of an abnormal baby. Today, genetic counseling and prenatal
diagnosis help people make informed decisions about conceiving, carrying a pregnancy to
term, or adopting a child.
3.8.1 Genetic Counseling
Genetic counseling is a communication process designed to help couples assess their
chances of giving birth to a baby with a hereditary disorder and choose t he best course of
action in view of risks and family goals. Individuals likely to seek counseling are those who
have had difficulties bearing children, such as repeated miscarriages, or who know that genetic
problems exist in their families. In addition, women who delay childbearing past age 35
are candidates for genetic counseling. After this time, the overall rate of chromosomal abnormalities
rises sharply, from 1 in every 190 to as many as 1 in every 10 pregnancies at age 48.
If a family history of mental retardation, physical defects, or inherited diseases exists, the
genetic counselor interviews the couple and prepares a pedigree, a picture of the family tree
in which affected relatives are identified. The pedigree is used to estimate the likelihood that
parents will have an abnormal child, using the genetic principles. In the case of many
disorders, blood tests or genetic analyses can reveal whether the parent is a carrier of the
harmful gene. Carrier detection is possible for all of the recessive diseases listed in Table 2,
as well as others, and for fragile X syndrome.
When all the relevant information is in, the genetic counselor helps people consider
appropriate options. These include "taking a chance" and conceiving, choosing from among a
variety of reproductive technologies like donor insemination and In vitro fertilization, or
adopting a child.
3.8.2 Prenatal Diagnosis and Fetal Medicine
If couples who might bear an abnormal child decide to conceive, several prenatal
diagnostic methods -medical procedures that permit detection of problems before birth-are
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
37
available (Table 3). Women of advanced maternal age are prime candidates for amniocentesis
or chorionic villus sampling. Except for ultrasound and maternal blood analysis, prenatal
diagnostic methods should not be used routinely, as they have some chance of injuring the
developing organism.
Table 3
Method Description
Amniocentesis
Chorionic
villus sampling
Fetoscopy
Ultrasound
Maternal blood
analysis
Preimplantation
genetic
diagnosis
The most widely used technique. A hollow needle is inserted through the abdominal
wall to obtain a sample of fluid in the uterus. Cells are examined for genetic defects.
Can be performed by 11 to 14 weeks after conception but safest after 15 weeks; 1 to
2 more weeks are required for test results. Small risk of miscarriage.
A procedure that can be used if results are desired or needed very early in
pregnancy. A thin tube is inserted into the uterus through the vagina, or a hollow
needle is inserted through the abdominal wall. A small plug of tissue is removed
from the end of one or more chorionic villi, the hair l ike projections on the
membrane surrounding the developing organism. Cells are examined for genetic
defects. Can be performed at 6 to 8 weeks after conception, and results are available
within 24 hours. Entails a slightly greater risk of miscarriage than does
amniocentesis. Also associated with a small risk of limb deformities, which
increases the earlier the procedure is performed.
A small tube with a light source at one end is inserted into the uterus to inspect the
fetus for defects of the limbs and face. Also allows a sample of fetal blood to be
obtained, permitting diagnosis of such disorders as hemophilia and sickle cell
anemia as well as neural defects. Usually performed between 15 and 18 weeks after
conception, but can be done as early as 5 weeks. Entails some risk of miscarriage.
High-frequency sound waves are beamed at the uterus; their reflection is translated
into a picture on a video screen that reveals the size, shape, and placement of the
fetus. By itself, permits assessment of fetal age, detection of multiple pregnancies,
and identification of gross physical defects. Also used to guide amniocentesis,
chorionic villus sampling, and fetoscopy. When used five or more times, may
increase the chances of low birth weight.
By the second month of pregnancy, some of the developing organism's cells enter
the maternal bloodstream. An elevated level of alpha-fetoprotein may indicate
kidney disease, abnormal closure of the esophagus, or neural tube defects, such as
anencephaly (absence of most of the brain) and spina bifida (bulging of the spinal
cord from the spinal column). Isolated cells can be examined for genetic defects,
such as Down syndrome.
After in vitro fertilization and duplication of the zygote into a cluster of about eight
to ten cells, one or two cells are removed and examined for hereditary defects. Only
if that sample is free of detectable genetic disorders is the fertilized ovum implanted
in the woman's uterus.
Advances in genetic engineering also offer hope for correcting hereditary defects. As part
of the Human Genome Project an ambitious, international research program aimed at deciphering
the chemical makeup of human genetic material (genome)-researchers have mapped
the sequence of all human DNA base pairs. Using that information, they are "annotating" the
genome-identifying all its genes and their functions, including their protein products and
what they do. A major goal is to understand the estimated 4,000 human disorders, those due
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
38
to single genes and those resulting from a complex interplay of multiple genes and
environmental factors.
Already, thousands of genes have been identified, including those involved in hundreds of
diseases, such as cystic fibrosis, Huntington disease, Duchenne muscular dystrophy, Marfan
syndrome, and some forms of cancer. As a result, new treatments are being explored, such as
gene therapy-delivering DNA carrying a functional gene to the cells, thereby correcting a
genetic abnormality. In recent experiments, gene therapy relieved symptoms in hemophilia
patients and patients with severe immune system dysfunction. Another approach is
proteomics, modifying gene-specified proteins involved in biological aging and disease.
3.8.3 Genetic Testing
For those who harbor genes that might lead to later-emerging disorders, a few predictive
tests exist, such as those for breast and colon cancer. Scientists predict that by the year 2010
many more such tests will be available, permitting people to find out about their genetic risks
and, hopefully, take steps to reduce them-through medical monitoring, lifestyle changes, or
drug therapy.
Although its potential benefits are great, at present genetic testing raises serious social,
ethical, and legal concerns. A major controversy involves testing children and adults who are
at risk but who do not yet show disease symptoms. Delay between the availability of
predictive tests and effective interventions mean that people must live with the knowledge
that they might become seriously ill. A related concern is the need for greater knowledge of
genetics, by both health professionals and the general public. Without this understanding,
doctors and patients may misinterpret genetic risk. For example, genes associated with breast
cancer were first identified in families with a high incidence of the disease.
3.8.4 Adoption
Adults who cannot bear children, who are likely to pass along a genetic disorder, or who
are older and single but want a family are turning to adoption in increasing numbers.
Adoption agencies usually try to find parents of the same ethnic and religious background as
the child.. Because the availability of healthy babies has declined (young unwed mothers give
up their babies today than in the past), more people are adopting from other countries or
taking children who are older or who have developmental problems.
Adopted children and adolescents-whether born in another country or in the country of
their adoptive parents-have more learning and emotional difficulties than other children, a
difference that increases with the child's age at time of adoption, The biological mother may
have been unable to care for the child because of emotional problems believed to be partly
genetic, such as alcoholism or severe depression. She may have passed this tendency to her
offspring. Or perhaps she experienced stress, poor diet, or inadequate medical care during
pregnancy-factors that can affect the child. Furthermore, children adopted after infancy are
more likely than their nonadopted peers to have a history of conflict-ridden family
relationships, lack of parental affection, and neglect and abuse. Finally, adoptive parents and
children, who are genetically unrelated, are less alike in intelligence and personality than
biological relatives-differences that may threaten family harmony. But despite these risks,
most adopted children fare surprisingly well.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
39
Check Your Progress 2
A. Bring out the disorders that is due to chromosomal abnormalities
B. State the reproductive choices that a parent has in having a child.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
3.9 ENVIRONMENTAL CONTEXTS OF DEVELOPMENT
Just as complex as the genetic inheritance that sets the stage for development is the
surrounding environment-a many-layered set of influences that combine to help or hinder
physical and psychological well-being.
The Bronfenbrenner's ecological systems theory emphasizes that the environments
extending beyond the microsystem, or the immediate settings powerfully affect development.
The impact is so pervasive that we seldom stop to think about it in our daily lives. This is the
macrosystem, or broad social climate of society-its values and programs that support and
protect human development. All people need help with the demands of each phase of the
lifespan-through well-designed housing, safe neighborhoods, good schools, well-equipped
recreational facilities, affordable health services, and high-quality child care and other
services that permit them to meet both work and family responsibilities. And some people,
because of poverty or special tragedies, need considerably more help than others.
3.9.1 The Family
In power and breadth of influence, no context equals the family. The family creates bonds
between people that are unique. Attachments to parents and siblings usually last a lifetime
and serve as models for relationships in the wider world of neighborhood, school, and
community. Within the family, children learn the language, skills, and social and moral
values of their culture. And at all ages, people turn to family members for information,
assistance, and interesting and pleasurable interaction. Warm, gratifying family ties predict
physical and psychological health throughout development. In contrast, isolation or alienation
from the family is often associated with developmental problems.
Contemporary researchers view the family as network interdependent relationships. The
ecological systems theory that has bidirectional influences exist in which the behaviour of
each family member affect those of others. Indeed, the very term system implies that the
responses of all family members are related. These system influences operate both directly
and indirectly.
3.9.1a Direct Influences. When we observe family members interacting, it can be seen that
kind, patient communication evokes cooperative, harmonious responses, whereas harshness
and impatience engender angry, resistive behavior. Each of these reactions, in turn, forges a
new link in the interactive chain. In first instance, a positive message tends to follow; in the
second, a negative or avoidant one tends to occur. For example, when parents' requests are
accompanied by warmth and affection children tend to cooperate. And when children
willingly comply, their parents are likely to be warm and gentle in the future. In contrast,
parents who discipline with harshness and impatience have children who refuse and rebel.
And because, children's misbehavior is stressful for parents, they may increase their use of
punishment, leading to more unruliness by the child.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
40
3.9.1.b Indirect Influences. Bronfenbrenner calls the indirect influences of others as the
effect of third parties.
Third parties can serve as supports for development. For example, when parents' marital
relationship is warm and considerate, mothers and fathers praise and stimulate their children
more and nag and scold them less. In contrast, when a marriage is tense and hostile, parents
tend to be less responsive to their children's needs and to criticize, express anger, and punish.
Similarly, children can affect their parents' relationship in powerful ways. For example, some
children show lasting emotional problems when their parents divorce.
When family relationships are strained by third parties, other members like grand parents
may help restore effective interaction. They can promote children's development in many
ways-both directly, by responding warmly to the child, and indirectly, by providing parents
with child-rearing advice, models of child-rearing skill, and even financial assistance. Of
course, like any indirect influence, grandparents can sometimes be harmful. When
quarrelsome relations exist between grandparents and parents, parent-child communication
may suffer.
3.9.1.c Adapting to Change. Bronfenbrenner's theory - chronosystem, the interplay of forces
within the family is dynamic and ever-changing. Important events, such as the birth of a
baby, a change of jobs, or an elderly parent joining the household due to declining health,
create challenges that modify existing relationships. The way such events affect family
interaction depends on the support provided by other family members as well as on the
developmental status of each participant.
In recent decades, a declining birth rate, a high divorce rate, and expansion of women's
roles have led to a smaller family size. This combined with a longer lifespan, means that
more generations are alive with fewer members in the youngest ones, leading to a "topheavy"
family structure. Consequently, young people today are more likely to have older
relatives than at any time in history-a circumstance that can be enriching as well as a source
of tension.
3.9.2 Socioeconomic Status and Family Functioning
Socioeconomic status (SES) combines three interrelated, but not completely overlapping,
variables:(l) years of education and (2) the prestige of and skill required by one's job, both of
which measure social status, and (3) income, which measures economic status. As
socioeconomic status rises and falls, people face changing circumstances that profoundly
affect family functioning.
SES affects the timing and duration of phases of the family life cycle. People who work
in skilled and semiskilled manual occupations (machinists, truck drivers) tend to marry and
have children earlier as well as give birth to more children than people in white-collar and
professional occupations. The two groups also differ in values and expectations. For example,
when asked about personal qualities they desire for their children, lower-SES parents tend to
emphasize external characteristics, such as obedience, politeness, neatness, and cleanliness.
In contrast, higher-SES parents emphasize psychological traits, such as curiosity, happiness,
self-direction, and cognitive and social maturity. In addition, fathers in higher-SES families
tend to be more involved in child rearing and household responsibilities. Lower-SES fathers,
partly because of their gender stereotyped beliefs and partly because of economic necessity,
focus more on their provider role.
Education also contributes to SES differences in family interaction. Higher-SES parents'
interest in providing verbal stimulation and nurturing inner traits is supported by years of
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
41
schooling, during which they learned to think about abstract, subjective ideas. Furthermore,
the greater economic security of higher-SES families permits them, to devote more time,
energy, and material resources to nurturing their children's psychological characteristics.
3.9. 3 The Impact of Poverty
When families slip into poverty, development is seriously threatened. Joblessness, a high
divorce rate, a lower remarriage rate among women than men, widowhood, and inadequate
government programs to meet family needs is responsible for developmental problems. The
child poverty rate is higher than that of any age group. These circumstances are particularly
worrisome because the earlier poverty begins, the deeper it and the longer it lasts, the more
devastating its effects on physical and mental health and school achievement. The constant
stresses that accompany poverty gradually weaken the family system. Poor families have
many daily hassles- no food, improper clothing, no shelter etc. When daily crises arise,
family members become depressed, irritable, and distracted, and hostile interactions increase.
These outcomes are especially severe in families that must live in poor housing and
dangerous neighborhoods-conditions that make everyday existence more difficult, while
reducing social supports that help people cope with economic hardship.
Most homeless families consist of women with children under age 5. Besides health
problems (which affect most homeless people), homeless children suffer from developmental
delays and serious emotional stress.
3.9. 4Cultural Values and Practices. Cultures shape family interaction and community
settings beyond the home-in short, all aspects of daily life. Many of us remain blind to aspects
of our own cultural heritage until we see them in relation to the practices of others.
Although many people value independence and privacy, not all citizens share the same
values. Some are part of sub cultures-groups of people with beliefs and customs that differ
from those of the larger culture. Many minority groups have cooperative family structures,
which help protect their members from the harmful effects of poverty. Within the extended
family, grandparents play meaningful roles in guiding younger generations; adults with
employment, marital, or child-rearing difficulties receive assistance and emotional support;
and care giving is enhanced for children and the elderly. Broad dimension on which cultures
and subcultures differ a r e : the extent to which collectivism versus individualism is
emphasized. In collectivist societies, people define themselves as part of a group and stress
group goals over individual goals. In individualistic societies people think of themselves as
separate entities and are largely concerned with their own personal needs. Although
individualism tends to increase as cultures become more complex, cross-national differences
remain. The United States is strongly individualistic, and India is a collectivist.
Check Your Progress 3
State the role of Environment in the development of a child.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
42
3.10 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) The Genetic foundations with Code, Sex Cells, Multiple Births and the Patterns of
Genetic Inheritance.
ii) The significant impact of Chromosomal Abnormalities causing Down Syndrome
and Abnormalities in the Sex Chromosomes
iii) Reproductive Choices using Genetic Counseling, Prenatal Diagnosis and Fetal
Medicine, Genetic Testing and Adoption are the some of the aspects which enable
the parents to have a normal child.
iv) The Environmental Contexts of Development such as the family, socioeconomic
status and family functioning were dealt.
3.11 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. Your answer may include the description of
i) The Genetic Code
ii) The Sex Cells
iii) Male or Female
iv) Multiple Births
v) Patterns of Genetic Inheritance
vi) Dominant-Recessive Inheritance
vii) Codominance
viii) X-Linked Inheritance
ix) Genetic Imprinting
x) Mutation
xi) Polygenic Inheritance
2. A. The disorders due to Chromosomal Abnormalities include
i. Down Syndrome
ii. Abnormalities of the Sex Chromosomes
B. Reproductive Choices which a parent in having a child are
i. Genetic Counseling
ii. Prenatal Diagnosis and Fetal Medicine
iii. Genetic Testing and
iv. Adoption
3. The Environmental Contexts in which a child Develops are:
i. The Family
ii. Socioeconomic Status and Family Functioning
iii. The Impact of Poverty
iv. Cultural Values and Practices of the family
3.12 Lesson – End Activities
1. Describe about the genetic influence on development.
2. Explain about the patterns of genetic inheritance.
3.13 References
1. Kohlberg, L. and Turiel, E., Research and Moral Development: A Cognitive Developments
Approach, New York, Wiley, 1971.
2. Piaget, J., The Origins of Intelligence in Children, New York : 1952.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
43
LESSON - 4
PRENATAL DEVELOPMENT - CHARACTERISTICS -
IMPORTANCE OF CONCEPTION - PRENATAL
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES
Contents
4.0 Aims and Objectives
4.1 Introduction
4.2 Characteristics of the Prenatal Period
4.3 Importance of Conception
4.3.1 Hereditary Endowment
4.3.2 Sex
4.3.3 Number of Offspring
4.3.4 Some Common Developmental Characteristics of Twins
4.3.5 Ordinal Position
4.4 Characteristics of Prenatal Environment
4.4.1 Maternal Factors
4.4.2 Prenatal Nourishment
4.4.3 Other Maternal Factors
4.4.4 Paternal Factors
4.5 Let Us Sum Up
4.6 Check your Progress
4.7 Lesson – End Activities
4.8 References
4.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This lesson will give an overall picture of the Characteristics of prenatal development
and the importance of the external environment on the infant.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
i) Discuss the characteristics of prenatal period
ii) Explain how the heredity, sex and birth position play an important role in the
development of child
iii) Understand the influence of the prenatal environment on the child
iv) State the role of father in the development of a child
4.1 INTRODUCTION
There is extensive evidence to show how conditions in the prenatal environment can and
do affect development before birth. This has justified beginning the study of development
from the moment of conception rather than from the time of birth.
Most of the development that takes place before birth has been investigated by
physiologists and members of the medical profession, and the results of these studies have
been extensively borrowed by developmental psychologists. Their contributions have, for the
most part, been to supplement the physiological and physical data with evidence of the effects
of psychological states on the pattern of development and the long-term effects of
attitudes of significant people.
The major happenings during the nine months before birth are to be explained, to
emphasize the significance of the moment of conception, and to show what environmental
and psychological factors affect the course of development.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
44
4.2 CHARACTERISTICS OF THE PRENATAL PERIOD
In spite of the fact that the first developmental period in the life span is next to the
shortest of all-the shortest is the period of the newborn or infancy- it is in many respects one
of the most, if not the most, important period of all. This period, which begins at conception
and ends at birth, is approximately 270 to 280 days in length, or nine calendar months.
Although it is relatively short, the prenatal period has six important characteristics, each of
which has a lasting effect on development during the lire span. They are as follows:
1. The hereditary endowment, which serves as the foundation for later development, is fixed,
once and for all, at this time. While favorable or unfavorable conditions, both before and after
birth may and probably will affect to some extent the physical and psychological traits that
make up this hereditary endowment, the changes will be quantitative not qualitative.
2. Favorable conditions in the mother's body can foster the development of hereditary
potentials while unfavorable conditions can stunt their development, even to the point of
distorting the pattern of future development. At few if any other times in the life span are
hereditary potentials so influenced by environmental conditions as they are during the
prenatal period.
3. The sex of the newly created individual is fixed at the time of conception and conditions
within the mother's body will not affect it, as is true of the hereditary endowment. Except
when surgery is used in sex transformation operations, the sex of the individual, determined
at the time of conception, will not change. Such operations are rare and only partially
successful.
4. Proportionally greater growth and development take place during the prenatal period than
at any other time throughout the individual's entire life. During the nine months before birth,
the individual grows from a microscopically small cell to an infant who measures
approximately twenty inches in length and weights, on the average, 7 pounds. It has been
estimated that weight during this time increases eleven million times. Development is likewise
phenomenally rapid. From a cell that is round in shape, all the bodily features, both
external and internal, of the human being develop at this time. At birth, the newly born infant
can be recognized as human even though many of the external features are proportionally
different from those of an older child, an adolescent, or an adult.
5. The prenatal period is a time of many hazards, both physical and psychological. While it
cannot be claimed that it is the most hazardous period in the entire life span-many believe
that infancy's more hazardous-it certainly is a time when environmental or psychological
hazards can have a marked effect on the pattern of later development or may even bring
development to an end.
6. The prenatal period is the time when significant people form attitudes toward newly
created individuals. These attitudes will have a marked influence on the way these individuals
are treated, especially during their early, formative years. If the attitudes are heavily
emotionally weighted, they can and often do play havoc with the mother's homeostasis and,
by so doing, upset the conditions in the mother's body that are essential to the normal
development of the newly created individual.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
45
Check Your Progress 1
Discuss the six characteristics of the prenatal period
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
4.3 IMPORTANCE OF CONCEPTION
At the time of conception, four important conditions are determined that influence the
individual's later development. What role each of these conditions plays in the individual's
development will explain why the time of conception is probably the most important period in
the life span.
4.3.1 Hereditary Endowment
The first important happening at the time of conception is the determination of the newly
created individual's hereditary endowment. The contributions to this endowment from both
parents and from both maternal and "paternal ancestors. Because the hereditary endowment is
determined once and for all at the time of conception, its importance is far greater than it
would be if it were subject to later change.
The determination of hereditary endowment affects later development in two ways. First,
heredity places limits beyond which individuals cannot go. If prenatal and postnatal conditions
are favorable, and if people are strongly motivated, they can develop their inherited physical
and mental traits to their maximum potential, but they can go no further. Montagu has stressed,
"Where we control the environment, we to some extent control heredity. Heredity, it has been
said, determines what we can do, and environment what we do do".
The second important thing about the hereditary endowment is that it is entirely a matter of
chance: there is no known way to control the number of chromosomes from the maternal or
paternal side that will be passed on to the child. Scheinfeld has pointed out that the birth of a
given individual depends on the union of a particular ovum with a particular sperm.
4.3.2 Sex
Determination of the individual’s sex happens during conception. It is known that the
sperm cell-that is, the father-determines the sex of a child. At conception, the zygote receives
23 chromosomes from the sperm and 23 from the ovum. (Figure 1 and 2)They align
themselves in pairs: 22 pairs are autsomes, or nonsex chromosomes; the twenty third pair is
sex chromosomes, which determine if the new human being will be male or female. In
females, this pair is called XX; in males, it is called XY. The X is a relatively long
chromosome, whereas the Y is short and carries little genetic material. When gametes are
formed in males, the X and Y chromosomes separate into different sperm cells. In females,
all gametes carry an X chromosome. Therefore, the sex of the new organism is determined by
whether an X-bearing or a Y-bearing sperm fertilizes the ovum.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
46
Figure 1
Figure 2
Sex depends on the kind of spermatozoon that unites with the ovum. Once the male and
female cells have united, nothing can be done to change the sex of the newly formed
individual. Whether this individual is male or female will have a lifelong effect on the
individual's patterns of behavior and personality.
There are three reasons why the sex of an individual is important to lifelong
development. First, each year children come under increasing cultural pressures from parents,
teachers, their peer group, and society at large to develop attitudes and behavior patterns that
are considered appropriate for members of their sex. Children who learn to behave in ways
that are considered appropriate for their sex are assured of social acceptance. By contrast,
children who fail to conform are subjected to criticism and social ostracism.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
47
Second, learning experiences are determined by the individual's sex. In the home, at
school, and in play groups, children learn what is considered appropriate for members of their
sex. A boy who learns to play girls' games is labeled a "sissy" and girls who prefer boys'
games are known as "tomboys."
Third, and perhaps most important of all, is the attitude of parents and other significant
family members toward individuals because of their sex. Studies of sex preferences for
offspring have revealed that the traditional preference for a boy, especially for the firstborn,
still persists. Strong preferences for a child of a given sex have marked influences on parents'
attitudes, which in turn affect their behavior toward the child and their relationships with the
child.
4.3.3 Number of Offspring
The third important happening at the time of conception or shortly thereafter is the
determination of the number of offspring there will be. While most humans are singletons,
multiple births also occur. Meredith has reported that lout of 80 births is twins, 1 out of every
9,000 is triplets, and 1 out of every 570,000 is quadruplets. There are more frequent multiple
births among blacks and fewer among Chinese, Japanese, and other Mongoloid-race groups
than there are among whites.
When a ripe ovum is fertilized by one spermatozoon, the result will be a singleton,
unless the fertilized ovum (zygote) splits into two or more distinct parts during the early
stages of cell cleavage. When this happens, the result will be identical (uniovular) twins,
triplets, or other multiple births. If two or more ova are released simultaneously and are
fertilized by different spermatozoa, the result will be nonidentical (also called biovular or
fraternal) twins, triplets, or other multiple births.
Approximately one-third of all twins are identical. Because the chromosomes and genes
of the two or more zygotes from which individuals of nonidentical multiple birth develop are
not the same, their mental and physical make ups are different. By contrast, those of identical
multiple births come from the same zygote, and consequently they have the same assortment
of chromosomes and genes. Children of identical multiple births are always of the same sex,
while those of nonidentical multiple births may be of the same or opposite sex.
Effects on Development. Most studies of the effects of multiple births on development have
been limited to twins for the reason that triplets, quadruplets, and other multiple births occur
very infrequently and the mortality rates among them are much higher than among twins,
thus making studies of them difficult if not impossible.
However, there is reason to assume that the effects of multiple birth on triplets,
quadruplets, and other multiples is much the same as on twins though the former feel these
effects to a greater extent. The reason that multiple births affect the pattern of development is
not only that there are differences in heredity but that both the prenatal environment and the
postnatal environment of singletons are different from those of children of multiple births.
This contributes to different patterns of development, different patterns of behavior, and
differences in personality.
Before birth, the singleton has the mother's uterus to itself, and thus development is not
affected by crowding, a factor in multiple births that will be discussed more fully in the
following chapter.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
48
There are also differences in the postnatal environments of singletons and those of
multiple births. While the mother can give her undivided attention to the care of a singleton,
those of multiple birth must share it. Thus during the early years, when the foundations of the
personality pattern are laid, babies of multiple birth receive less mothering than singletons,
and consequently they may feel unloved or actually rejected.
4.3.4 Some Common Developmental Characteristics of Twins
Developmental lag: In physical, mental, motor, and speech development, twins tend to lag
behind singletons of the same age. Lag in motor and speech development may be due to brain
damage or to prematurity but it is more likely to be due to parental over protectiveness.
Physical Development: Twins tend to be smaller, age for age, than singletons. This is
generally due to the fact that they are premature. They also suffer from brain damage and
other physical defects more often than singletons.
Mental Development: Mental similarities between identical twins are much greater than
between nonidentical twins and this persists into old age. Identical twins also show strong
similarities in terms of special abilities, such as musical and artistic aptitudes.
Social Development: Twins tend to compete for adult attention, to imitate each other's
speech and behavior, and to depend on each other for companionship during the preschool
years. As they grow older, sibling rivalry and competition develop. One twin usually takes on
the role of leader, forcing the other into the role of follower. This affects their relationships
with other family members and with outsiders.
Personality Development: Many twins have difficulty in developing a sense of personal
identity. This is especially true of identical twins and of nonidentical twins of the same sex.
Others enjoy the close relationship of twinship and the attention they receive as a result of
their similarity in appearance. This leads to self-satisfaction and self-confidence.
Behavior Problems: Behavior problems have been reported to be more common among
twins than among singletons of the same ages. It is thought that this is a result of the way
twins are treated, both at home and outside the home. Behavior problems have also been
reported to be more common among nonidentical than among identical twins. It has been
suggested that this is because rivalry is stronger between nonidentical than identical twins.
Long-Term Effects of Twinship Those that have been made rarely go beyond the tenth year
of the twins' lives, have indicated the following long-term effects. There is a tendency for the
developmental lag in physical development to end before children reach puberty and often
much earlier. Generally the firstborn twin continues to be larger, brighter, and better adjusted
socially throughout the childhood years. The smaller the twins at birth, the longer the
developmental lag tends to persist.
The mutual dependency or "twinning relationship" so common among young twins and
the one sided dependency of the smaller on the larger twin generally give way to social
relationships similar to those of singletons before the twins enter school. Those who attend
day-care centers or preschools tend to abandon these patterns of dependency earlier than
twins whose environment is limited to the home. F raternal twins are more vulnerable to
external pressures and to have less support from the twinship relationship than do identical
twins not only when they are young but also as they grow older.
4.3.5 Ordinal Position
The fourth thing that happens at the time of conception is the establishment of the new
child's ordinal position among siblings. While this may change within a year or two after
birth, the child's ordinal position remains fairly static from then on. For example, a secondborn
child may be the "baby of the family" or hold a last-born ordinal position for a year or
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
49
more after birth but then be replaced by a newly born sibling. Shifting from the "baby-of-the
family" position to a middle-born's position may be upsetting for a short time, but young
children or even babies tend to adjust to these changes.
There is evidence to conclude that it is not ordinal position per se that leaves its mark on
the individual's personality and patterns of behavior but rather circumstances in life related to
this position-such as the role the individual plays in the family and the treatment he or she
receives from significant family members and their attitudes. Since roles, attitudes, and
treatment are far more likely to persist than to change, the individual constantly receives
reinforcements which, in time, result in firmly established habits.
Bigner has described specifically how being a second-born or a firstborn in a family
affects the child's development. He contends, "A second-born child plays a 'satellite' role in
many of his interactions with the older, since the firstborn may act as the natural leader of the
siblings in the family constellation".
How ordinal position will affect the individual will depend on a number of conditions,
the two most important of which are the sex of the individual and how individuals feel about
the roles they are expected to play.
A firstborn girl, for example, who is expected to help with the housework and with the
care of young siblings may resent the fact that the boys in the family have fewer domestic
duties and are granted privileges and given opportunities denied to her. A second- or laterborn
boy may resent being "bossed" by an older female sibling or being treated as the "baby
of the family" while his female siblings are given more privileges and freedom than he is
given.
Some individuals enjoy the role they are expected to play as a result of their ordinal
position while others do not. A firstborn child, for example, may resent the pressures of
parents to live up to their expectations or having to act as a model for younger siblings. On
the other hand, the firstborn may derive personal satisfaction from serving as a role model for
younger siblings.
Long-Term Effects of Ordinal Position Older children, adolescents, and adults of different
ordinal positions give clues as to how ordinal position may become a persistent factor in
determining the kind of personal and social adjustments the individual will make throughout
the life span.
There is evidence that firstborns tend to be brighter and to be higher achievers than their
later born siblings. There is little or no evidence, on the other hand, that this is due to
hereditary difference but rather to environmental conditions that foster the child's intellectual
development. Firstborns are not only given more intellectual stimulation than later-borns but
they are also given more opportunities to develop their intellectual abilities in school and
college. As Zajonc has pointed out "Parents and psychologists have always regarded firstborn
children as different and special and, as a result, have given them greater intellectual
stimulation and opportunities to develop their intellectual capacities than their later-born
siblings have had".
Because of the greater opportunities they are given and because of the special treatment
they receive, firstborns have been found to outnumber later borns in leadership roles as early
as elementary school. On the other hand, because of greater over protectiveness and parental
concern about their physical welfare, firstborns tend to be more concerned about their health
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
50
and to consult doctors more frequently than do later-borns. They also tend to be more
cautious and take fewer risks as children and as adults than do siblings of other ordinal
positions.
The effects on' marital adjustments of ordinal position in the childhood family have
revealed that the best marital adjustments were in families where the husbands were the
oldest brothers with younger sisters while the poorest adjustments and the greater number of
divorces were in families where the husbands were the younger brothers with older sisters
and where the wives had, during childhood, learned to be bossy as a result of playing
surrogate-mother roles. By contrast, husbands who, as firstborns in their childhood homes,
learned to take responsibility made better adjustments to marriage.
Check Your Progress 2
A. State the four important conditions that determine the influence on the individual's later
development during conception.
B. Explain some of the Developmental Characteristics of Twins
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
4.4 CHARACTERISTICS OF PRENATAL ENVIRONMENT
Only recently have we become aware of some of the myriad environmental influences that
can affect the developing fetus. The role of the father, for example, used to be almost ignored.
Today we know that various environmental factors can affect a man's sperm-and the children
he conceives.
While the mother's role has been recognized far longer, we are still discovering many
elements that can affect her fetus.
4.4.1 Maternal Factors
Most of our knowledge about prenatal hazards comes from animal research or from studies in
which mothers reported on such factors as what they had eaten while pregnant, what drugs
they had taken, how much radiation they had been exposed to, and what illnesses they had
contracted. Both these methods have limitations: it is not always accurate to apply findings
from animals to human beings, and people do not always remember what they did in the past.
Various influences in the prenatal environment affect different fetuses differently. Some
environmental factors that are teratogenic, or birth defect-producing, in some cases have little
or no effect in others. Research suggests that the timing of an environmental event, its
intensity, and its interaction with other factors are all relevant.
4.4.2 Prenatal Nourishment
Nutrition: Babies develop best when their mothers eat well. A woman's diet before as well as
during pregnancy is crucial to her child's future health. Diet during pregnancy may be even
more vital. Pregnant women who gain between 22 and 46 pounds are less likely to miscarry
or to bear stillborn or low birth weight babies.
Well-nourished mothers bear healthier babies, while mothers with inadequate diets are more
likely to bear premature or low-birth weight infants, babies who are stillborn (born dead) or
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
51
die soon after birth, or babies whose brains do not develop normal. In low-income families,
other kinds of deprivation may aggravate the effects of poor nutrition.
Malnourished pregnant women who take dietary supplements have bigger, healthier, more
active, and more visually alert infants. In addition, better-nourished mothers tend to
breastfeed longer, thus benefiting their babies. Furthermore, recent findings point to at
reduced risk of neural tube defects in the babies of women who received supplements of folic
acid (a vitamin in the B group) even before pregnancy, leading to the recommendation that all
women of childbearing age receive this vitamin.
A well-balanced daily diet for pregnant women includes foods from each of the following
categories: protein (meat and meat alternatives), dairy products, bread and cereals, fruits and
vegetables rich in vitamin C, dark-green vegetables, other fruits and vegetables (including
yellow ones rich in vitamin A), and fats and oils. Women need to eat more than usual when
pregnant: typically, 300 to 500 more calories a day, including extra protein. Teenagers,
women who are ill or undernourished or under stress, and those who took birth control pills
until shortly before pregnancy need extra nutrients.
Physical Activity Fortunately, not all things an expectant mother does or is exposed to are
harmful to a fetus. She can continue to jog, cycle, swim, play tennis, and so forth, since
moderate exercise does not seem to endanger the fetuses of healthy women. Regular exercise
prevents constipation and improves respiration, circulation, muscle tone, and skin elasticity,
all of which contribute to a more comfortable pregnancy and an easier, safer delivery.
However, pregnant women should avoid activities that could cause a high degree of
abdominal trauma.
Employment during pregnancy generally entails no special hazards. However, strenuous
working conditions, occupational fatigue, and long working hours may be associated that
could cause a greater risk of premature birth.
It is recommended that women in low- risk pregnancies be guided by their own abilities
and stamina. The safest course seems to be for pregnant women to exercise moderately, not
pushing themselves and not raising their heart rate above 150, and, as with any exercise, to
taper off at the end of each session rather than stop abruptly.
Drug Intake: Practically everything the mother takes in makes its way to the new life in her
uterus. Drugs may cross the placenta, just as oxygen, carbon dioxide, and water do. Each year
as many as 375,000 infants may be affected by their mothers' drug abuse during pregnancy.
The organism is especially vulnerable in its first few months, when development is most
rapid. Thus drugs taken early in pregnancy have the strongest effects.
A number of extremely serious problems have shown up in a mother's-or father's-use of
drugs. Some of these problems cart be treated if the presence of a drug in a newborn baby's
body can be detected early. But it is often difficult to determine exactly which drugs a person
has taken, since doctors usually have to rely on the parents' own, often inaccurate, testimony.
A new test for the presence of drugs in a newborn's system analyzes the baby's meconium,
the fetal waste matter that is excreted during the first few days after birth. Babies whose
mothers used cocaine during pregnancy begin life with massive problems. Many are preterm
and small, many have neurological problems.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
52
Medical Drugs: Drugs known to be harmful include the antibiotics streptomycin and tetracycline;
the sulfonamides; excessive amounts of vitamins A, B6, C, D, and K; certain
barbiturates, opiates, and other central nervous system depressants; several hormones,
including birth control pills, progestin, diethylstilbestrol (DES), androgen, and synthetic
estrogen; Accutane, a drug often prescribed for severe acne and even aspirin. It is
recommended that no medication be prescribed for a pregnant or breastfeeding woman unless
it is essential for her own or her child's health.
The effects of taking a drug during pregnancy do not always show up immediately. The
synthetic hormone diethylstilbestrol (DES) which was widely prescribed (ineffectually, as it
turned out) to prevent miscarriage. Years later, when the daughters of women who had taken
DES during pregnancy reached puberty, about 1 in 1000 developed a rare form of vaginal or
cervical cancer. "DES daughters" also have had more trouble bearing their own children, with
higher risks of miscarriage or premature delivery and "DES sons" seem to show a higher rate
of infertility and reproductive abnormalities.
Alcohol: There are babies born 'with alcohol-related birth defects. Many babies suffer from
fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS), a combination of slowed prenatal and postnatal growth, facial
and bodily malformations, and disorders of the central nervous system. Central nervous
system problems can involve poor sucking response, brain-wave abnormalities, and sleep
disturbances in infancy; and, throughout childhood, a short attention span, restlessness,
irritability, hyperactivity, learning disabilities, and motor impairments.
Some of the problems of FAS recede after birth; but problems like retardation, learning
disabilities and hyperactivity persist into adulthood, and some malformations require surgery.
Even moderate drinking may harm the fetus. Growth retardation increases if the mother has
even one or two drinks a day. The effect increased sharply with heavier alcohol intake; taking
less than one drink a day had a minimal effect.
Marijuana Evidence is mounting that heavy marijuana use by pregnant women can lead to
birth defects. A mother's heavy use affects her infant's nervous system. Transient neurological
disturbances, like tremors and startles, and higher rates of premature and small-fordate.
There is link between marijuana use just before and during pregnancy and a childhood
cancer-acute lymphoblastic leukemia-possibly because of pesticide contamination of the
cannabis leaves. In sum, women of childbearing age should not use marijuana.
Nicotine Pregnant smokers are at higher risk than nonsmokers of bearing preterm and smallfor-
date babies, and of complications ranging from bleeding during pregnancy to death of the
fetus or newborn. However, women who reduce smoking during pregnancy tend to have
bigger babies than those who continue to smoke at previous levels.
Smoking in pregnancy seems to have some of the same effects on school-age children as
drinking in pregnancy: poor attention span, hyperactivity, learning problems, perceptualmotor
and linguistic losses, social maladjustment, poor IQ scores, low grade placement, and
minimal brain dysfunction.
Those whose mothers smoked at least a pack a day after pregnancy were twice as likely to
be anxious, disobedient, or hyperactive or to exhibit some other behavior problem as were
children of nonsmokers. The effect was dose-related; that is, it was more pronounced in
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
53
children whose mothers smoked more than a pack a day. And the risk was not lessened if the
mother had stopped smoking during pregnancy but resumed afterward. It is possible that
smoking during pregnancy may alter the child's brain structure or function, with resulting
long-term effects on behavior; that passive exposure to cigarette smoke after birth may affect
a child's central nervous system; that smoking may alter the mother's behavior, thus affecting
her child's; or that mothers who smoke may be less tolerant of their children's behavior.
Opiates: Women addicted to such drugs as morphine, heroin, and codeine are likely to bear
premature, addicted babies who show effects until at least age 6. Addicted newborns are
restless and irritable and often have tremors, convulsions, fever, vomiting, and breathing
difficulties; they are twice as likely to die soon after birth as non addicted babies. As older
babies, they cry often and are less alert and less responsive. And in early childhood-from approximately
ages 3 to 6-they weigh less, are shorter, are less well adjusted, and score lower
on tests of perceptual and learning abilities. Long-term follow-up studies on these children
have found that they tend not to do well in school, are unusually anxious in social situations,
and have trouble making friends.
Cocaine: Cocaine (including crack, its smokable form) is reported to be the number one
illicit drug used by pregnant women in the United States, its effects are of grave importance.
Although the immediate medical results of a pregnant woman's use of cocaine (such as
prematurity, low birth weight, and smaller head circumference) are well known, little is
known about later developmental consequences. It seems, though, that organizational and
language skills and secure emotional attachment may be affected for the worse.
Although babies whose mothers stopped using cocaine early in pregnancy grew as
normally as babies of drug-free mothers, many were less alert and responsive. Cocaineexposed
newborns also showed more stress behaviors, such as tremors, restlessness,
irritability, abnormal reflex behaviors, and excessive high-pitched crying.
Caffeine: Can the caffeine that a pregnant woman swallows in coffee, tea, cola, or chocolate
cause trouble for her fetus? A recent study suggests that the amount of caffeine in 11/2 to 3
cups of coffee a day may nearly double the risk of miscarriage, and drinking more than 3
cups nearly triples the risk. It is recommended that pregnant women avoid or use sparingly
any food, beverages, or drugs that contain caffeine.
4.4.3 Other Maternal Factors
Illness: A number of illnesses can have serious effects on the developing fetus, depending
partly on when a pregnant woman gets sick.
Rubella (German measles) before the eleventh week of pregnancy is almost certain to cause
deafness and heart defects in the baby; but the chance of these consequences drops to about 1
in 3 between 13 and 16 weeks of pregnancy and is almost nil after 16 weeks. The syndrome
can be prevented by immunizing women before pregnancy-ideally, by immunizing girls
before puberty.
Diabetes, tuberculosis, and syphilis have also led to problems in fetal development, and
both gonorrhea and genital herpes can have harmful effects on the baby at the time of
delivery.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
54
Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome (AIDS): may be contracted by a fetus if the
mother has the disease or even has the human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) in her blood.
The contents of the mother's blood are shared with the fetus through the placenta, and blood
is a carrier of the virus that causes AIDS. HIV disproportionately affects disadvantaged
women and children end is most often contracted by intravenous drug use or by sexual intercourse
with a drug user.
Premature babies are more susceptible, perhaps because they miss the protection of
antibodies that may not appear until the last 3 months of gestation. Other associations were
found with such birth complications as use of forceps and episiotomy. Babies delivered by
cesarean surgery were less likely to be affected. Breastfed infants of these mothers are more
likely to develop the infection, but, still, in most populations breastfeeding is still to be
preferred, unless the prevalence of HIV infection is very high or the difference in death rates
between breastfed and bottle-fed babies is very low.
Incompatibility of Blood Types: A problem resulting from the interaction of heredity with
the prenatal environment is incompatibility of blood type between mother and baby. When a
fetus's blood contains the Rh factor - a protein substance-but the mother's blood does not,
antibodies in the mother's blood may attack the fetus and possibly bring about spontaneous
abortion, stillbirth, jaundice, anemia, heart defects, mental retardation, or death. Usually the
first Rh-positive baby is not affected, but with each succeeding pregnancy the risk becomes
greater. A vaccine can now be given to an Rh-negative mother; when it is administered
within 3 days after childbirth or abortion, it will prevent her body from making antibodies.
Babies affected with Rh disease can be treated by repeated blood transfusions, sometimes
before birth.
Medical X-rays: It is known for more than 60 years that radiation can cause gene mutations,
minor changes that alter a gene to produce a new, often harmful characteristic. Although it is
not known what exact dosage of x-rays will harm a fetus, the greatest potential for harm
seems to occur early in pregnancy. Radiation exposure should be avoided, especially during
the first 3 months. With the availability of medical x-rays are less necessary and less
prevalent today than they were in the past.
Maternal Age: What is the best age to have a baby? Complications for teenage mothers and
their babies, most of which are social rather than medical. The concerns for mothers past 30,
however, have historically been for the physical well-being of mother and child. In recent
years, as more women have delayed childbearing until the mid-thirties or even the forties,
researchers have focused on the risks involved, and have come up with largely encouraging
findings. Well educated nonsmokers who received prenatal care, women over 35 had only a
slightly higher risk of bearing unusually small babies and were no more likely to deliver
prematurely or to have stillbirths than were younger first-time mothers. However, older
mothers were twice as likely to have such complications of pregnancy as diabetes and high
blood pressure. Furthermore, as women age, they become less fertile, are more likely to have
miscarriages, and are more at risk of having children with birth defects.
Environmental Hazards: Anything that affects a pregnant woman can affect her fetus:
chemicals, radiation, extremes of heat and humidity, and other hazards of modern life. For
example, babies whose mothers ate fish contaminated with chemicals used in industry had
babies weight less at birth, had smaller heads, and showed weaker reflexes and more jerky
movements than infants whose mothers did not eat the fish; and they showed poor visual-
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
55
recognition memory at 7 months and poor memory for words and numbers at 4 years. At age
4, such children also tended to be less able to discriminate between visual stimuli and to have
more problems with short-term memory.
Women who took saunas or soaked in hot tubs early in their pregnancies seem to run a
higher risk of bearing babies with neural-tube defects. Infants exposed to high levels of lead
prenatally scored lower on intelligence tests than those exposed to low or moderate levels.
Children exposed prenatally to heavy metals showed higher rates of childhood illness and
lower levels of performance. Women who worked with chemicals widely used in manufacturing
semiconductor chips had about double the rate of miscarriages as did women workers
who did not handle the chemicals, suggesting a potential health risk.
Radiation is especially dangerous. It affected Japanese infants after the atomic bomb
explosions in Hiroshima and Nagasaki and German infants after the spill-out at the nuclear
power plant at Chernobyl in the Soviet Union. In utero exposure to radiation has been linked
to greater risk of mental retardation, small head size, chromosomal malformations, Down
syndrome, seizure, and poor performance on IQ tests and in school. The critical period seems
to be 8 through 15 weeks after fertilization.
4.4.4 Paternal Factors: Environmental Influences Transmitted by the Father
The father, too, can transmit environmentally caused defects. Exposure to lead, marijuana and
tobacco smoke, large amounts of alcohol and radiation and certain pesticides may result in
the production of abnormal sperm. Associations have appeared between nervous system
tumors in children and such occupations of their fathers as electrical or electronic worker,
auto mechanic, miner, printer, paper or pulp mill worker, and aircraft industry worker.
Paternal diet low in vitamin C could cause birth defects and certain types of cancers in the
children.
A harmful influence on both mother and baby is nicotine from a father's smoking. Babies
of fathers who smoked were lighter at birth by about 4 ounces per pack of cigarettes smoked
per day by the father (or the cigar or pipe equivalent). Children of male smokers were twice
as likely as other children to contract cancer as adults.
A man's use of cocaine can also cause birth defects in his children, since cocaine seems to
attach itself to his sperm. This cocaine-bearing sperm then enters the ovum at the time of
conception. Fathers must share the responsibility for such birth defects-not only those caused
by cocaine, but also those caused by other toxins, such as lead and mercury, which might also
"hitchhike" onto sperm in the same way.
And it now seems that some drug-induced abnormalities may occur much earlier than
previously thought, even before the fertilized ovum is implanted in the uterus. One route for
the transmission of cocaine from the sperm to the baby may still lie with the mother,
however. It is possible that when a cocaine-using woman has sexual intercourse, the man's
sperm in her reproductive tract can pick up the drug and carry it to the ovum, where it will do
its damage.
A later paternal age (average in the late thirties) is associated with increases in several rare
conditions, including one type of dwarfism; Marfan's syndrome (deformities of the head and
limbs); and a kind of bone malformation. The father's age may also be a factor in about 5 per-
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
56
cent of cases of Down syndrome. More male cells than female ones undergo mutations and
mutations may increase with parental age.
Check Your Progress 3
Write about some of the myriad environmental influences that can affect the developing
fetus.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
4.5 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Six characteristics of the Prenatal Period and its role in development
ii) Stated the importance of Conception and the factors contributing to grow a normal
child
iii) Dealt some six common developmental characteristics of twins
iv) The four prenatal environmental characteristics influencing the fetus
4.6 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. The answer may include the following six characteristics
i) A brief about the foundation laid by the hereditary endowment to be stated.
ii) Favorable mother's body conditions to foster the development of a child.
iii) As to how the sex of the newly created individual is fixed at the time of
conception.
iv)Write about growth and development that takes place during the prenatal period.
v) Hazards, both physical and psychological during the prenatal period
vi) Effect of the significant people’s attitudes toward newly created individuals.
2.A. The following four factors can be explained
i) Hereditary Endowment
ii)Sex
iii) Number of Offspring
iv) Ordinal Position
B.The six developmental characteristics of twins are to be explained briefly
i)Developmental lag
ii)Physical Development
iii)Mental Development
iv) Social Development
v)Personality Development
vi)Behavior Problems
3. The environmental influence on the fetus could through the following four sources
i)Maternal Factors
ii)Prenatal Nourishment
iii)Other Maternal Factors
iv) Paternal Factors
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
57
4.7 Lesson – End Activities
1) Mention few important characteristics of Prenatal environment.
2) Briefly describe the maternal factors influencing development.
4.8 References
1. Previn, Lawrence, A., Personality, New York: Wiley 1984.
2. Stanger, Ross, Psychology of Personality, New York: McGraw – Hill, 1974.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
58
LESSON – 5
ATTITUDES OF SIGNIFICANT PEOPLE - MEDICAL INTERVENTIONS -
HAZARD DURING THE PRENATAL PERIOD
Contents
5.0 Aims and Objectives
5.1 Introduction
5.2 Attitudes of Significant people - Origin of Attitudes
5.2.1 Conditions Influencing Attitudes
5.2.2 Persistence of Attitudes
5.2.3 Effects of Attitudes on Children
5.2.4 Effects of Attitudes on Family Relationships
5.3 Natural and Prepared Childbirth
5.4 Medical Interventions
5.4.1 Fetal Monitoring
5.4.2 Labor and Delivery Medication
5.4.3 Cesarean Delivery
5.5 Hazards during the Prenatal Period
5.5.1 Physical Hazards
5.5.2 Psychological Hazards
5.5.3 Some Common Unfavorable Attitudes toward an Unborn Child
5.6 Let Us Sum Up
5.7 Check your Progress
5.8 Lesson – End Activities
5.9 References
5.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help understand the role of people in the development of a fetus.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
i) Mention the effect of attitudes of significant people on child as such
ii) State the significance of natural birth
iii) Examine the various kinds of medical interventions that aid in the birth of a child
iv) Discuss the physical and psychological hazards during the prenatal period
v) List the common unfavorable attitudes toward an unborn child
5.1 INTRODUCTION
Until the early 1940s, psychological interest in the prenatal period was concentrated on
the physical conditions in the mother's body that might affect development and on the
persistence of these effects into postnatal life. The work of Sontag and his associates, for
example, called attention to the fact that the mother's emotional state can affect the
development of the unborn child.
However, psychologists are interested in finding out what is responsible for maternal
and other family-member attitudes toward the developing child; how persistent these attitudes
are; and what effects they have on the relationships of different family members with children
during their postnatal lives, especially during the early formative years when the significant
people in their world are members of their families. While relatively recent in origin, these
studies have revealed the important information summarized below.
5.2 ATTITUDES OF SIGNIFICANT PEOPLE - ORIGIN OF ATTITUDES
Attitudes toward children and parenthood are usually formed early in life, though they
may crystallize when the individual knows that he or she will soon become a parent.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
59
Many factors influence the formation of attitudes toward children. First, young people’s
earlier experience with children have a marked effect on how they feel about them in general
and about their own impending roles as parents. A woman, for example, who had to help care
for younger brothers and sisters may have an unfavorable attitude toward children, or a
woman who grew up as an only child may want many children to make up for the loneliness
she felt when she was young.
Second, the experiences of friends, either in the past or at present, color the individuals
attitudes. For example, a young man who hears his friends complain about the financial
burdens of parenthood may decide that he would rather not have children.
Third, parent or grandparent who loves children and who pities people who are childless
can influence a person's attitudes favorably. Fourth, a person’s attitude toward the sex of the
unborn child can be influenced by stereotyped ideas-that boys are "a handful," for example.
Fifth, the mass media tend to glamorize family and the parental role. The attitudes of an
adult whose own experiences with children have been limited may be profoundly influenced
by "family shows" on television.
5.2.1 Conditions Influencing Attitudes
Many conditions affect the attitudes-both favorably and unfavorably-of parents, siblings,
and grandparents toward a child. The most commonly reported of these are summarized in
Table 1.
A careful study of the conditions listed in this table will show that different conditions
affect the attitudes of different significant people. For example, the attitudes of siblings are
affected by conditions that are different from those of the mother or of the father or of the
grandparents-just as the mother's attitudes are affected by conditions different from those of
the father, of the siblings, or of the grandparents.
Table 1
CONDITIONS AFFECTING ATTITUDES OF SIGNIFICANT PEOPLE
Mother's Attitude
. Love of children
. Desire for companionship
. Desire to please her husband or improve a poor marital relationship
. Desire to be like her friends who have children.
. Feelings of inadequacy for the parental role
. Resentment at having to give up a career
. Fear of childbirth or of having a defective child
. Resentment at the physical discomforts and weight gain associated with pregnancy
. Resentment at being overworked or tied down
Father's Attitude
. Desire for a son to carryon the family name or be associated with him in business
. A need to prove his virility to himself and others.
. Feelings of inadequacy for the parental role. Resentment at interference with educational or vocational plans
. Worry about the financial burdens of raising a child
. Resentment at being tied down
Siblings' Attitudes
. Desire for a playmate
. Desire to have as many siblings as their friends
. Fear of losing parental affection and attention.
. Fear of having to share a room or toys with the new sibling or having to help care for it
. Desire for sympathy from friends who com plain about their own siblings
Grandparents' Attitudes
. Desire for a grandchild to carryon the family name
. Love of children
. Desire to feel useful by helping care for the grandchild
. Fear of being imposed on for financial or other help
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
60
5.2.2 Persistence of Attitudes
Likes, dislikes, prejudices, and attitudes, once formed, tend to persist, though slight
changes are possible. The changes that do occur are usually in the form of modifications of
existing attitudes; these attitudes become less or more favorable than they originally were.
Thus changes in attitudes are quantitative rather than qualitative. For example, a teenage
boy's hero worship of a well-known football player may diminish when he discovers that his
idol has faults not readily apparent at first. Similarly, a person's dislike for someone of a
different race, religion, or socioeconomic background may mellow somewhat with personal
contacts. Such changes are modifications of already-existing attitudes.
There are two reasons for persistence of attitudes. First, attitudes tend to persist because
they are based on beliefs the individual considers to be valid and justified. After all, the heroworshiping
teenager contends, his idol certainly must be someone special if he has become a
hero to others too.
Siblings and other significant people in the life of the unborn child have reasons for
wanting or not wanting the child, and they consider these reasons valid. Hence their attitudes,
like those of parents, tend to persist, though they too may be modified.
The second reason for the persistence of attitudes toward a child, formed before the
child’s birth, is that they are usually highly emotionally toned. And, like all emotional
attitudes, they are difficult if not impossible to change. A woman, for example, who as a girl
resented having to give up some of the time she wanted to spend with her friends to help with
the care of younger siblings, is likely to resent being tied down again with the care of a child,
even if it is her own.
A man, for example, who is upset and resentful at the privations fatherhood brings, may
and often will tell others that he is delighted at the prospects of having a child. At home, he
may accuse his wife of "being careless" and allowing herself to become pregnant, but, to
those outside the home, the camouflage of his true feelings will usually be adequate to make
others believe that he is delighted at the prospect of becoming a father and, later, in his role of
father.
5.2.3 Effects of Attitudes on Children
The mother's attitude can have an effect on her unborn baby-not through the umbilical
cord, which is' the only direct connection between the two-but as a result of endocrine
changes which can and do occur if the mother-to-be is subjected to severe and prolonged
stress which normally accompanies persistently unfavorable attitudes. Favorable attitudes, by
contrast, will lead to good body homeostasis and this will favor normal development during
the prenatal period.
After birth, the mother's attitudes, most of which were formed before the baby's birth,
have an influence because they are reflected in the way the child is treated. For example, a
mother who wanted a boy will have a less favorable attitude toward a daughter. If she is
disappointed in having a daughter, she may feel guilty and compensate for this by being
overprotective and overindulgent of the child. If a later child should be the hoped-for son,
either consciously or unconsciously she may show favoritism toward him, and her treatment
of her daughter will be colored by rejection.
The attitudes of other family members--the father, siblings, and grandparents-can also
affect the child. Before the child's birth they may affect it indirectly through the mother, for
example, if family members let her know that they do not welcome the idea of its birth and
thus cause her to become nervous and upset. By contrast, favorable attitudes on the part of
other family members reinforce the mother's favorable attitudes or lessen any emotional
stress she may be under if her own attitude is not favorable.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
61
Like maternal attitudes, the attitudes of other family members tend to persist though they
may change slightly, depending partly on whether the child conforms to their expectations
and partly on how he or she treats them. Grandparents, for example, may have favorable
attitudes toward very young children but may feel differently about them if, as the children
grow older, they treat their grandparents with less respect and less affection.
5.2.4 Effects of Attitudes on Family Relationships
The attitudes of different family members-the foundations of which have generally been
laid before a child is born-have a profound influence not only on the child but also on family
relationships. This influence may be favorable or unfavorable, depending not on the attitude
of one family member but on the attitudes of all family members.
If favorable attitudes toward a new baby could be counted on to persist and if
unfavorable attitudes could be counted on to become less unfavorable or even favorable, they
would not represent a threat to family relationships. Unfortunately, favorable attitudes often
become less favorable after the child's birth, and unfavorable attitudes tend to persist, even
though they may be so cloaked that they appear to have changed for the better. .
Sooner or later children become aware of the way different family members feel about
them, and this influences their attitudes toward family members and toward themselves as
well. Feeling loved and wanted will motivate a child to behave in a way that will intensify
favorable family attitudes and relationships. If, on the other hand, children sense, suspect, or
know that they are disappointments to their fathers, a burden to their already overworked
mothers, and a nuisance to their siblings, they will show their resentment by behaving in
ways that will intensify their unfavorable attitudes and worsen family relationships. This
often is the starting point of personality maladjustments and problem behavior that can
plague children for years-often throughout life.
Check Your Progress 1
Bring out attitudes toward children and parenthood which are usually formed early in life.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
5.3 NATURAL AND PREPARED CHILDBIRTH
In 1914 a British physician, Dr. Grantly Dick-Read, claiming that fear causes most of the
pain in childbirth, put forth the theory of natural childbirth. This method aims to eliminate
fear by educating women in the physiology of reproduction and delivery and training them in
breathing, relaxation, and physical fitness. Dr. Fernand Lamaze was using the psycho
prophylactic method-prepared childbirth,-substituting new breathing and muscular responses
to the sensations of uterine contractions for the old responses of fear and pain.
The Lamaze method of prepared childbirth instructs women in anatomy to remove fear of
the unknown and trains them to vary their patterns of breathing to match the strength of
contractions and to concentrate on sensations other than the contractions. The mother learns
to relax her muscles as a conditioned response to the voice of her "coach" (usually the father
or a friend). Social support is also a factor: The coach attends classes with the expectant
mother, takes part in the delivery, and helps with the exercises-enhancing her sense of selfworth
and reducing her fear of being alone at the time of birth.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
62
5.4 MEDICAL INTERVENTIONS
If a two-year-old child walks with a halting, lumbering gait and has difficulty keeping her
balance. She has cerebral palsy, a general term for a variety of impairments in muscle
coordination that result from brain damage before, during, or just after birth.
Like 10 percent of youngsters with cerebral palsy, this child’s brain damage could be
caused by anoxia, or inadequate oxygen supply, during labor and delivery. Her mother got
pregnant accidentally, was frightened and alone, and arrived at the hospital at the last minute.
The girl was in breech position, turned so that the buttocks or feet would be delivered first,
and the umbilical cord was wrapped around her neck. Had her mother come to the hospital
earlier, doctors could have monitored the girls condition and delivered her surgically as soon
as squeezing of the umbilical cord led to distress, reducing the damage or preventing it
entirely. In cases like this, medical interventions during childbirth are clearly justified. But in
others, they can interfere with delivery and even pose new risks. In the following sections, we
examine some commonly used medical techniques.
5.4.1 Fetal Monitoring
Fetal monitors are electronic instruments that track the baby's heart rate during labor. An
abnormal heartbeat may indicate that the baby is in distress due to anoxia and needs to be
delivered immediately. Most hospitals require continuous fetal monitoring; it is used in over
80 percent of births. In some countries, continuous monitoring is usually reserved for babies
at risk for birth complications. The most popular type of monitor is strapped across the
mother's abdomen throughout labor. A second, more accurate method involves threading a
recording device through the cervix and placing it directly under the baby's scalp.
Fetal monitoring is a safe medical procedure that has saved the lives of many babies in
high-risk situations. Nevertheless, the practice is controversial. In healthy pregnancies, it does
not reduce the rate of infant brain damage or death. Critics also worry that fetal monitors
identify many babies as in danger that, in fact, are not. Monitoring is linked to an increased rate
of cesarean (surgical) deliveries. In addition, some women complain that the devices are
uncomfortable, prevent them from moving easily, and interfere with the normal course of labor.
Still, it is likely that fetal monitors will continue to be used routinely in the hospitals,
even though they are not necessary in most cases. Today, doctors can be sued for malpractice
if an infant dies or is born with problems and they cannot show that they did everything
possible to protect the baby.
5.4.2 Labor and Delivery Medication
Some form of medication is used in 80 to 95 percent of births. Analgesics, drugs used to
relieve pain, may be given in mild doses during labor to help a mother relax. Anesthetics are
a stronger type of painkiller that blocks sensation. A regional anesthetic may be injected into
the spinal column to numb the lower half of the body.
Although pain-relieving drugs enable doctors to perform essential life-saving medical
interventions, they can cause problems when used routinely. Anesthesia weakens uterine
contractions during the first stage of labor and interferes with the mother's ability to feel
contractions and push during the second stage. As a result, labor is prolonged. In addition,
since labor and delivery medication rapidly crosses the placenta, the newborn baby may be
sleepy and withdrawn, suck poorly during feedings, and be irritable when awake.
Some researcher’s claims that use of medication during childbirth have a lasting impact
on physical and mental development. Anesthesia may be related to other risk factors that
could account for long-term consequences in some studies, but more research is needed to
sort out these effects.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
63
5.4.3 Cesarean Delivery
A cesarean delivery is a surgical birth; the doctor makes an incision in the mother's
abdomen and lifts the baby out of the uterus. Thirty years ago, cesarean delivery was rare.
Since then, the cesarean rate has climbed.
Cesareans have always been warranted by medical emergencies, such as Rh
incompatibility, premature separation of the placenta from the uterus, or serious maternal
illness or infection (for example, the herpes simplex 2 virus, which can infect the baby during
a vaginal delivery). However, surgical delivery is not always needed in other instances. For
example, although the most common reason for a cesarean is a previous cesarean, the
technique used today-a small horizontal cut in the lower part of the uterus-makes vaginal
birth safe in later pregnancies. Cesareans are often justified in breech births, in which the
baby risks head injury or anoxia. But the infant's exact position makes a difference. Certain
breech babies fare just as well with a normal delivery as with a cesarean. Sometimes the
doctor can gently turn the baby into a head-down position during the early part of labor.
When a cesarean delivery does occur, both mother and baby need extra support. Although
the operation is quite safe, it requires more time for recovery. Because anesthetic may have
crossed the placenta, newborns are more likely to be sleepy and unresponsive and to have
breathing difficulties.
Check Your Progress 2
Explain how medical interventions during childbirth are clearly justified.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.............................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
........
5.5 HAZARDS DURING THE PRENATAL PERIOD
At no other time during the life span are there more serious hazards to development-or
hazards" of a more serious nature-than during the relatively short period before birth. These
may be physical or psychological. Physical hazards have received more scientific attention
because they are more easily recognized.
However, psychological hazards are sometimes as serious as physical hazards since they
affect the attitudes of significant people toward the developing child. Furthermore, they often
intensify physical hazards.
5.5.1 Physical Hazards
Each of the three major subdivisions of the prenatal period involves particular
physical hazards. While these do not affect all individuals by any means, they do occur with
some frequency and can be serious enough to affect the development of the individual
throughout life. Davis and
5.5.1.a Common Physical Hazards during the Prenatal Period
Period of the Zygote
Starvation: The zygote will die of starvation if it has too little yolk to keep it alive until it can
lodge itself in the uterine wall or if it remains too long in the tube.
Lack of Uterine Preparation: Implantation cannot occur if, as a result of glandular
imbalance, the uterine walls are not prepared in time to receive the zygote.
Implantation in the Wrong Place: If the zygote becomes attached to a small fibroid tissue in
the uterine wall or to the wall of the Fallopian tube, it cannot get nourishment and will die.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
64
Period of the Embryo
Miscarriages: Falls, emotional shocks, malnutrition, glandular disturbances, vitamin
deficiency, and serious diseases, such as pneumonia and diabetes, can cause the embryo to
become dislodged from its place in the uterine wall, resulting in a miscarriage. Miscarriages
that are due to unfavorable conditions in the prenatal environment are likely to occur between
the tenth and eleventh weeks after conception.
Developmental Irregularities: Maternal malnutrition; vitamin and glandular deficiencies;
excessive use of drugs, alcohol, and tobacco; and diseases, such as diabetes and German
measles, interfere with normal development, especially that of the embryonic brain.
Period of the Fetus
Miscarriages: Miscarriages are always possible up to the fifth month of pregnancy; the most
vulnerable time is when the woman's menstrual period would normally occur.
Prematurity: Fetuses who weigh less than 2 pounds 3 ounces have less chance of surviving
than heavier fetuses and a greater chance of developing malformations.
Complications of Delivery: Maternal stress affects uterine contractions and is likely to lead to
complications during birth.
Developmental Irregularities: Any of the unfavorable environmental conditions present
during the period of the embryo will also affect the development of fetal features and retard
the whole pattern of fetal development.
Conditions Influencing Physical Hazards: Certain conditions have been found to increase
the likelihood that physical hazards will occur or accentuate them. The first of these
conditions is the timing of their appearance. It has been recognized by doctors for many years
that if the mother-to-be contracts rubella during the first trimester of pregnancy the chances
of developmental irregularities in her unborn child, especially in the form of eye or or a
malformation of the heart, will occur.
Female hormones, such as estrogen and progestin, when taken in the early stages of
pregnancy may disturb the normal cardiovascular development of the fetus and cause
congenital heart diseases. It is reported that the second and third lunar months, when the heart
is developing rapidly, are the most serious times. This is not true if these hormones are taken
after the fourth lunar month.
The second condition that increases the likelihood of physical hazards is if the condition
is intense or greater than is normal. Some conditions that are known to affect the developing
child during the prenatal period are described below; others are suspected of affecting
development.
Maternal malnutrition can play havoc with normal development, especially the
development of the fetal brain. Excessive smoking and drinking are detrimental to normal
development, especially during the periods of the embryo and fetus. This is true also of
taking drugs.
Maternal age has been reported to be a condition that intensifies the possibility of
physical hazards during the prenatal period. The reason for this is that as women approach the
menopause, they frequently have endocrine disorders which slow down the development of
the embryo and fetus, causing such developmental irregularities as cretinism, Down’s
syndrome, heart malformations and hydrocephalus all of which involve physical and mental
defects. The incidence of Down's syndrome increases as age advances in women. Older
women also tend to have smaller babies and to have more complications at birth than do
younger women. While paternal age may likewise cause developmental irregularities or
stillbirths, this is likely to happen only when paternal age is over sixty years.
Certain kinds of work are more likely to disturb the prenatal development than others.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
65
Chemicals and other hazards faced by women working in such places as hospitals, beauty
parlors, and factories may be responsible for the increasing number of birth defects and
miscarriages during recent years.
Multiple births are more hazardous than single births. Fetuses of multiple births are
crowded during the prenatal period, and this inhibits the normal fetal activity essential for
development. Prematurity is also more likely in the case of multiple births, as is the possibility
of developmental irregularities. Because multiple births are more common among
blacks than among whites, this may account in part for the higher infant mortality rate and the
greater incidence of developmental irregularities among blacks than among whites.
Long-Term Effects: If developmental irregularities are serious and if the embryo or fetus
does not miscarry or die at birth or shortly afterward, the individual will be deformed in some
way. One of the serious aspects of developmental irregularities is that they are sometimes not
diagnosed as such until months or even years after birth. Epilepsy, cerebral palsy, and mental
deficiency, for example may not show up until babyhood or even early in childhood.
Parents who believe that their baby is normal at birth find it difficult to accept a
defective child and often blame themselves for having caused the defect. This leads to strong
feelings of guilt and a tendency to overprotect defective children or to refuse to accept the
fact that they are, as defective as they are.
It is now known that malnutrition during pregnancy may damage the developing fetal
brain, causing learning difficulties in school, especially reading disabilities. Damage to the
fetal brain, whatever the cause, will have effects on the individual's behavior that become
more and more apparent as children grow older and are compared with other children of the
same age.
A chromosomal abnormality, especially in an X chromosome, has been found to lead to
physical abnormalities that can predispose the individual to abnormal behavior if they make it
difficult for him or her to adjust to social expectations.
5.5.2 Psychological Hazards
The psychological hazards can have persistent effects on the individual's development and
can influence the postnatal environment and the treatment the child receives from significant
people during the early, formative years. The three most important psychological hazards are
traditional beliefs about prenatal development, maternal stress during the prenatal period and
unfavorable attitudes toward the unborn child on the part of people who will play significant
roles in the child’s life.
Traditional Beliefs: Perhaps there are more traditional, and more damaging, beliefs about the
prenatal developmental period than about any other period in the life span. Such beliefs can
and do affect parents' treatment of their children and often have an effect on their attitudes
toward each other.
In spite of scientific evidence to the contrary, many people, for example, still believe
that it is within their power to control the sex of their offspring. They believe they can do this
by intercourse at certain periods during the menstrual cycle, by producing an acid
environment in the woman's reproductive organs if a girl is desired and an alkaline
environment for a boy or by artificial insemination after chemically abstracting sperm cells
that would produce a child of the desired sex and then implanting them in the woman's
reproductive organs.
The effects of such a belief are more serious than most people realize. When parents are
convinced that they can produce an offspring of the sex they want, they are generally bitterly
disappointed when the child turns out to be of the opposite sex. This disappointment may
wane and disappear in time, but it frequently leaves its imprint upon the parents' attitudes
toward the child. Furthermore, many men feel that it is the woman who has the power to
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
66
control the sex of the child, and if she does not produce an offspring of the sex her husband
wants, his attitude toward her may be seriously affected. To date, the only known way to
predict with high accuracy the sex of an unborn child is by amniocentesis.
There are also traditional beliefs about the causes of developmental irregularities. Some
of these emphasize heredity, but most stress the role played by maternal impressions. There
are two lines of medical evidence to disprove these beliefs about maternal impressions. First,
there is evidence that the same types of abnormalities found in humans are also found in the
lower animals whose low level of mental development would make them incapable of
maternal impressions. Second, there is no direct nervous connection between the mother and
the embryo. There are no nerves in the umbilical cord, and thus the mother's thoughts,
feelings, and emotions could have no direct influence on the embryo.
In the past, twins were believed to be caused by evil sprits and thus were feared and
rejected by the social group. Today, only the most uncivilized cultures hold such beliefs,
although many people still think that it is "animal-like" to have twins and that twins are less
desirable and less acceptable than singletons.
Maternal Stress: The second important psychological hazard associated with the prenatal
period is maternal stress - heightened general emotionality over a prolonged period of time.
Stress can be the result of fear, anger, grief, jealousy, or envy.
There are many causes of maternal stress during pregnancy, the most common of which
are the following: not wanting a child because of marital or economic difficulties or because
having a child will interfere with educational or vocational plans; physical discomforts that
are severe and frequent enough to make the mother to be nervous, irritable, and generally
emotionally disturbed; feelings of inadequacy for the parental role; and fears that the child
will be physically deformed or mentally deficient-fears that are often heightened by mass
media reports of the frequency of birth defects and of specific causes of birth defects, such as
rubella and thalidomide. Some women have fantasies and dreams about giving birth to
deformed babies which intensify such fears.
Prolonged and extreme maternal stress during the period of the fetus frequently causes
more illness during the first three years of the child's life than is experienced by children who
had a more favorable fetal environment. Children whose mothers were under great stress
during pregnancy also show more "free-floating anxiety"; although they can still perform
their daily routines, such anxiety has an adverse effect on their ability to learn, to remember,
and to reason to their full capacities. As a result, they seem to be less bright than they actually
are.
Unquestionably one of the most serious effects of maternal stress during pregnancy is on
children's postnatal adjustments to family members. Because of their hyperactivity, excessive
crying, and other indications of poor adjustment to postnatal life, they are regarded as
"difficult" babies. Attitudes of family members toward them are then far less favorable than
they would have been had they made better adjustments to postnatal life.
As they grow older, children sense these unfavorable attitudes on the part of family
members and later, on the part of peers, teachers, and other outsiders. Feeling unloved and
rejected, they often show below average physical development, hyperactivity, lags in
developing motor skills and speech, and learning problems. All these lead to poor personal
and social adjustments.
Unfavorable Attitudes on the Part of Significant People: The third common psychological
hazard during the prenatal period is unfavorable attitudes on the part of significant people in
the child's life. This is, in many respects, the most serious and far-reaching in its influence
because once attitudes are developed they tend to persist with little if any real change or
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
67
modification. There is evidence that many unfavorable attitudes toward children begin to
develop when their potential arrival becomes known to parents, siblings, relatives, and
neighbors. The most common and most serious of these attitudes are listed below.
5.5.3 Some Common Unfavorable Attitudes toward an Unborn Child
Not Wanting the Child: The mother may not want the child because it is illegitimate,
because it will interfere with her career, because it will tie her down, or because she is
already overworked caring for other children. The father may not want the child because he
does not want to be forced to marry the mother, because of the financial burden the child will
represent, because he does not want to be tied down, or because he does not want his wife to
be preoccupied with child care and neglect him. Siblings may not want the child because they
resent the restrictions a baby will place on their activities or because they do not want to
share their possessions or their mother's time and attention with the new baby.
Not Wanting the Child at This Time: The parents may not want the child now because it
will interfere with their educational and vocational plans, because they feel they are too
young and inexperienced to care for a child, because they cannot afford it, or because they do
not want to assume parental responsibilities so soon. Grandparents may feel that the young
couple cannot afford the baby and may fear that they will have to provide financial and other
help.
Preference for a Child of a Particular Sex: The father and the grandparents usually want
the firstborn to be a boy; if there are already boys in the family, they may want a girl. The
mother may want a boy to please her husband, or she may prefer a girl, who she feels will be
more of a companion to her. Siblings generally prefer a child of their own sex, whom they
regard as more likely to be a playmate.
Dream-Child Concept: All family members have a dream-child concept that colors their
attitudes toward the unborn baby. Parents and grandparents want the baby to be perfect
mentally, emotionally, and physically - bright, obedient, beautiful-and siblings want an ideal
playmate, one who will do whatever they want to do and who will never rival or outstrip
them.
Not Wanting Children of Multiple Birth: Many adults, even today, regard multiple births
as animal-like or accept the traditional belief that children of multiple birth are doomed to be
physical and mental weaklings. Others believe that multiple-birth children make too much
work for all family members and dread the added expense for hospital care which is
inevitable if they are premature. These unfavorable attitudes are intensified if conditions after
birth are similar to those they dreaded before birth.
Wanting to Have a Miscarriage or an Abortion: When a baby is unwanted, regardless of
the reason, some women hope they will have a miscarriage or they plan an abortion. If the
developing baby's life is ended, either by miscarriage or abortion, women often feel guilty
and this unfavorable attitude carries over to any children they may have in the future. Should
they decide against an abortion or should there be no miscarriage, they may feel guilty and
express their guilt in over protectiveness and overindulgence of the child they had hoped not
to have.
Scorn for the Child: Relatives, friends of the family, and neighbors may have an
unfavorable attitude toward the child-to be because it is illegitimate, because of some stigma
in the lives of one or both parents, or because it is the child of an interracial or inter religious
marriage. As a result, the parents may become defensive and treat the child in an
overprotective or overindulgent way to compensate for these unfavorable attitudes or they
may reject the child because they feel embarrassed and ashamed.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
68
Unfavorable attitudes toward children of multiple births are often stronger and more
persistent than those toward singletons. These attitudes are intensified when multiple births
come unexpectedly and parents have not had time to adjust to them. Even when parents
welcome the idea of a multiple birth, their attitudes may become unfavorable when they are
faced with the realities of the babies’ care and the expense involved.
Check Your Progress 3
A. Explain some of the Physical and psychological hazards during prenatal period.
B. Discuss some common unfavorable attitudes toward an unborn child
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
5.6 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Origin of Attitudes
ii) Natural and Prepared Childbirth
iii) Medical Interventions
iv) Hazards during the Prenatal Period
v) Some Common Unfavorable Attitudes toward an Unborn Child
5.7 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. Many factors influence the formation of attitudes toward children, some of them are:
i.) Conditions Influencing Attitudes
ii) Persistence of Attitudes
iii) Effects of Attitudes on Children
iv) Effects of Attitudes on Family Relationships
2. The use of the following methods as medical interventions can be discussed with
justifications
i) Fetal Monitoring
ii) Labor and Delivery Medication
iii) Cesarean Delivery
3. A Physical hazards
i) Lack of Uterine Preparation
ii) Implantation in the Wrong Place
iii) Miscarriages
iv) Complications of Delivery
Psychological hazards
i)Traditional Beliefs
ii) Maternal Stress
iii) Unfavorable Attitudes on the Part of Significant People
B. Common unfavorable attitudes towards prenatal period
i) Not Wanting the Child
ii) Not Wanting the Child at This Time
iii) Preference for a Child of a Particular Sex
iv) Dream-Child Concept
v) Not Wanting Children of Multiple Birth
vi) Wanting to Have a Miscarriage or an Abortion
vi) Scorn for the Child
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
69
5.7 Lesson – End Activities
1. Briefly describe how emotional shocks affect the embryo development.
2. Mention about the effect of multiple birth.
5.8 References
1. Roazen, P. Freud and His Followers, New York: Alfred Knopf, 1975.
2. Stanger, Ross, A. Biographical Approach, New York: McGraw – Hill, 1978.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
70
UNIT II
LESSON – 6
INFANCY AND TODDLERHOOD -CHARACTERISTICS - ADJUSTMENTS -
LEARNING CAPACITIES
Contents
6.0 Aims and Objectives
6.1 Introduction
6.2 Characteristics of Infancy
6.2.1 Infancy Is the Shortest of All Developmental Periods
6.2.2 Infancy Is a Time of Radical Adjustments
6.2.3 Infancy Is a Plateau in Development
6.2.4 Infancy Is a Preview of Later Development
6.2.5 Infancy Is a Hazardous Period
6.3 Adjustments of Infancy
6.4 Conditions Influencing Adjustment to Postnatal Life
6.4.1 Prenatal Environment
6.4.2 Kind of Birth
6.4.3 Experiences Associated with Birth
6.4.4 Length of Gestation Period
6.4.5 Parental Attitudes
6.4.6Postnatal Care
6.5 Characteristics of Toddlerhood
6.5.1 Toddlerhood is the True Foundation Age
6.5.2 Toddlerhood Is an Age of Rapid Growth and Change
6.5.3 Toddlerhood Is an Age of Decreasing Dependency
6.5.4 Toddlerhood Is the Age of Increased Individuality
6.5.5 Toddlerhood Is the Beginning of Socialization
6.5.6 Toddlerhood Is the Beginning of Sex-Role Typing
6.5.7 Toddlerhood Is an Appealing Age
6.5.8 Toddlerhood Is the Beginning of Creativity
6.5.9 Toddlerhood Is a Hazardous Age
6.6 Learning Capacities
6.6.1 Classical Conditioning
6.6.2 Operant Conditioning
6.6.3 Habituation
6.6.4 Imitation
6.7 Let Us Sum Up
6.8 Check your Progress
6.9 Lesson – End Activities
6.10 References
6.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help understand the Characteristics of Infancy and Toddlerhood their
adjustments patterns and learning capacities.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
i) describe the various characteristics of infancy period
ii) state the adjustments made during infancy
ii) list the conditions influencing adjustment to postnatal life
iii) mention the characteristics of toddlerhood
iv) obstacles in studying life span development
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
71
6.1 INTRODUCTION
Infancy, or the period of the newborn, is, according to standard dictionaries, the
beginning or the early period of existence as an individual rather than as a parasite in the
mother's body. Dictionaries also define an infant as a child in the first period of life.
According to legal standards, an infant is an individual who is a minor until reaching the age
of legal maturity, which, in America today, is eighteen years. According to medical
terminology, an infant is a young child, but no specific age limits are placed on when the
individual ceases to be an infant and becomes a child.
Many psychologists use the word infant in much the same way as members of the
medical profession do and, like them, fail to set an age limit on infancy. This gives the period
an ambiguous status in the life span. The word infant suggests extreme helplessness, and it
will be limited to the first few weeks of life. During this period, the newborn's complete
helplessness gradually gives way to increasing independence.
Further toddlerhood occupies the first two years of life following the brief two-week
period of infancy. During the toddlerhood months, there is a gradual but pronounced decrease
in helplessness. This does not mean that helplessness quickly disappears and is replaced by
independence. Instead, it means that every day, week, and month the individual becomes
more independent so that, when toddlerhood ends with the second birthday, the individual is
a quite different person than when toddlerhood began.
Because "baby" suggests too many people a helpless individual, it is becoming increasingly
common to apply the label toddler to the individual during the second year of toddlerhood. A
toddler is a baby who has achieved enough body control to be relatively independent.
6.2 CHARACTERISTICS OF INFANCY
Each period in the life span is characterized by certain developmental phenomena that
distinguish it from the periods that precede and follow it. While some of these phenomena
may be associated with other periods, they appear in a distinctive form during infancy.
Following are the five most important characteristics of this period.
6.2.1 Infancy Is the Shortest of All Developmental Periods
Infancy begins with birth and ends when the infant is approximately two weeks old,
by far the shortest of all developmental periods. It is the time when the fetus must adjust to
life outside the uterine walls of the mother where it has lived for approximately nine months.
According to medical criteria, the adjustment is completed with the fall of the umbilical cord
from the navel; according to physiological criteria, it is completed when the infant has
regained the weight lost after birth; and according to psychological criteria, it is completed
when the infant begins to show signs of developmental progress in behavior. Although most
infants complete this adjustment in two weeks or slightly less, those whose birth has been
difficult or premature require more time.
In spite of its shortness, infancy is generally subdivided into two periods: the period of
the Partunate and the period of the neonate.
Period of the Partunate (from birth to fifteen to thirty minutes after birth): This period
begins when the fetal body has emerged from the mother's body and lasts until the umbilical
cord has been cut and tied. Until this is done, the infant continues to be a parasite and makes
no adjustments to the postnatal environment the environment outside the mother's body.
Period of the Neonate (from the cutting and tying of the umbilical cord to approximately the
end of the second week of postnatal life): The infant is now a separate, independent
individual and is no longer a parasite. During this period, the infant must make adjustments to
the new environment outside the mother's body. Even in difficult births, it seldom takes more
than forty-eight hours for the fetus to emerge from the mother's body. By contrast, it requires
approximately two weeks adjusting to the new environment outside the mother's body.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
72
6.2.2 Infancy Is a Time of Radical Adjustments
Although the human life span legally begins at the moment of birth, birth is merely an
interruption of the developmental pattern that started at the moment of conception. It is the
graduation from an internal to an external environment. Like all graduations, it requires
adjustments on the individual's part. It may be easy for some infants to make these
adjustments but so difficult for others that they will fail to do so. Miller has commented, "In
all the rest of his life, there will never be such a sudden and complete change of locale".
6.2.3 Infancy Is a Plateau in Development
The rapid growth and development which took place during the prenatal period suddenly
come to a stop with birth. In fact, there is often a slight regression, such as loss of weight and
a tendency to be less strong and healthy than at birth. Normally this slight regression lasts for
several days to a week, after which the infant begins to improve. By the end of the infancy
period, the infant's state of development is usually back to where it was at the time of birth.
The halt in growth and development, characteristic of this plateau, is due to the necessity
for making radical adjustments to the postnatal environment. Once these adjustments have
been made, infants resume their growth and development.
While a plateau in development during infancy is normal, many parents, especially those
of firstborn children, become concerned about it and fear that something is wrong with their
child. Consequently, the infancy plateau may become a psychological hazard, just as it is a
potential physical hazard.
6.2.4 Infancy Is a Preview of Later Development
It is not possible to predict with even reasonable accuracy what the individual's future
development will be on the basis of the development apparent at birth. However, the
newborn's development provides a clue as to what to expect later on.
6.2.5 Infancy Is a Hazardous Period
Infancy is a hazardous period, both physically and psychologically. Physically, it is
hazardous because of the difficulties of making the necessary radical adjustments to the
totally new and different environment. The high infant mortality rate is evidence of this.
Psychologically, infancy is hazardous because it is the time when the attitudes of
significant people toward the infant are crystallized. Many of these attitudes were established
during the prenatal period and may change radically after the infant is born, but some remain
relatively unchanged or are strengthened, depending on conditions at birth and on the ease or
difficulty with which the infant and the parents adjust.
Check Your Progress 1
Bring out the general characteristics of infancy
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
6.3 ADJUSTMENTS OF INFANCY
Infants must make four major adjustments before they can resume their developmental
progress. If they do not make them quickly, their lives will be threatened. While these
adjustments are being made, there is no developmental progress. Instead, the infant remains
on a plateau or may even regress to a lower stage of development. These adjustments are:
1. Temperature Changes: There is a constant temperature of 100°F in the uterine sac, while
temperatures in the hospital or home may vary from 60 to 70°F.
2. Breathing: When the umbilical cord is cut, infants must begin to breathe on their own.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
73
3. Sucking and Swallowing: The infant must now get nourishment by sucking and
swallowing, instead of receiving it through the umbilical cord. These reflexes are imperfectly
developed at birth, and the infant often gets less nourishment than is needed and thus loses
weight.
4. Elimination: The infant's organs of elimination begin to work soon after birth; formerly,
waste products were eliminated through the umbilical cord.
Every newborn infant finds adjustment to postnatal life difficult at first. Some have
trouble adjusting to temperature changes and develop colds, which may turn into pneumonia.
Others find breathing difficult and must be given oxygen. Most choke when they attempt to
suck and swallow, and many regurgitate more than they are able to retain, in which case they
get less nourishment than they need to grow or even to retain their birth weight. Few have
any real trouble eliminating urine, but many have difficulties with fecal elimination.
6.4 Conditions Influencing Adjustment to Postnatal Life
Many conditions influence the success with which infants make the necessary adjustments to
postnatal life. The most important of these, are the kind of prenatal environment, the type of
birth and experiences associated with it, the length of the gestation period, parental attitudes,
and postnatal care.
6.4.1 Prenatal Environment
The first condition that influences the kind of adjustment infants make to postnatal
life is the kind of prenatal environment they had. A healthy prenatal environment will
contribute to good adjustments to postnatal life.
On the other hand, there are many kinds of intrauterine disturbance that can and often do
cause an infant to be born, as Schwartz has pointed out, "with severe injuries and then be
subject to a miserable life". Inadequate prenatal care of the mother, as a result of either
poverty or neglect, is often responsible for the development of unfavorable conditions in the
intrauterine environment which affect the developing child and lead to complications during
childbirth, both of which affect the kind of adjustment the infant makes.
Unquestionably one of the most important conditions that contribute to difficulties in
postnatal adjustment is a prenatal environment characterized by prolonged and intense
maternal stress. As was mentioned earlier, this leads to complications during pregnancy and
childbirth. Maternal stress also causes the fetus to become hyperactive during the last months
of pregnancy, and this condition tends to persist after birth, manifesting itself in feeding
difficulties, failure to gain weight, sleep problems, general irritability, distractibility, and a
host of other conditions that make adjustment to postnatal life difficult.
6.4.2 Kind of Birth
The second condition that influences the kind of adjustment that will be made to postnatal
life is the kind of birth the infant experiences. Many traditional beliefs about birth and how it
affects the individual's adjustments to life persist even today. For example, there are many
beliefs about auspicious and inauspicious times to be born. There is also the belief that the
ease or difficulty of birth affects postnatal adjustments and the belief that a premature baby
will never be as strong as one born at full term or make as successful an adjustment to life.
Even with our modern medical techniques, birth is a hazardous experience. Jeffcoate has
pointed out that the "most hazardous journey made by any individual is through the four
inches of the birth canal". Schwartz further emphasized the hazardous nature of birth when he
said, "Birth is almost without exception a brutal process which endangers the life and health
of the child".
There are five kinds of birth they are:
Natural, or Spontaneous, Birth: In a natural birth, the position of the fetus and its size in
relation to the mother's reproductive organs allow it to emerge in the normal, headfirst
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
74
position.
Breech Birth: In a breech birth, the buttocks appear first, followed by the legs and finally the
head.
Transverse Birth: In a transverse presentation, the fetus is positioned crosswise in the
mother's uterus. Instruments must be used for delivery unless the position can be changed
before the birth process begins.
Instrument Birth: When the fetus is too large to emerge spontaneously or when its position
makes normal birth impossible, instruments must be used to aid in delivery.
Caesarean Section: I f x -rays taken during the latter part of pregnancy indicate that
complications may result if the infant emerges through the birth canal, the baby is brought
into the world through a slit made surgically in the mother's abdominal wall.
The infant who has been born spontaneously usually adjusts more quickly and more
successfully to the postnatal environment than one whose birth has been difficult enough to
require use of instruments or caesarean section.
More hazards are associated with instrument births and caesarean sections than with
spontaneous births. The more difficult the birth, the greater the chance of damage and the
more severe the damage. Babies born by caesarean section are the quietest, crying less than
those born spontaneously or with the aid of instruments and showing greater lethargy and
decreased reactivity. As a result, they normally make better adjustments to their postnatal
environment-unless they have had difficulty establishing respiration, which may cause
temporary or permanent brain damage.
6.4.3 Experiences Associated with Birth
The third conditions that influence the kind of adjustments infants make to postnatal life
are experiences associated with birth. Regardless of the kind of birth, two birth experiences
have a major effect on postnatal adjustments. They are the extent to which the mother is
medicated during the birth process and the ease or difficulty with which the infant establishes
respiration.
Infants whose mothers are heavily medicated during labor show drowsiness and
disorganized behavior for three or more days after birth, as compared with one or two days
for those whose mothers are lightly medicated or receive no medication at all. Furthermore,
infants whose mothers are heavily medicated lose more weight and take longer to regain their
lost weight than infants whose mothers have less medication, Federman and Yang, for
example, have reported that the effects on the infant's adjustment to postnatal life may persist
during the first month after birth.
The ease or difficulty with which infants start to breathe after birth likewise affects their
postnatal adjustments. When there is interruption of the oxygen supply to the brain before or
during birth-anoxia the infant may die. Infants who live may be temporarily or permanently
brain damaged, although this may not be apparent for months or even years after birth,
While anoxia may occur in any birth, it is especially likely to occur in precipitate laborlabor
lasting less than two hours. When this occurs, the infant is introduced to oxygen too
suddenly and is not yet ready to start to breathe. How much brain damage there will be and
how permanent its effects will be depend largely on how quickly the infant can establish
respiration.
6.4.4 Length of Gestation Period
The fourth condition that influences infants' adjustments to postnatal life is the length of
the gestation period. Very few infants are born exactly 280 days after conception. Those who
arrive ahead of time are known as prematures -often referred to in hospitals as "preemies"-
while those who arrive late are known as postmatures, or postterm babies.
Postmaturity occurs less often than in the past because it is now possible to induce labor
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
75
when x-rays/scanning show that the fetus is large enough and well-enough developed to
adjust successfully to postnatal life. Induced labor is also used as a means of preventing
possible birth complications and birth injuries, especially brain damage, which can result if
the fetal head is allowed to grow too large.
It is now recognized that birth weight alone is not enough to determine prematurity.
Instead, gestation age, body length, bone ossification, head circumference, irritability, reflex,
nutritional state, and neurological assessment are also used.
When infants are 20 or more inches long and weigh 8 or more pounds, they are
considered postmature. It they are less than 19 inches long and weigh 5 pounds 8 ounces or
less, they are regarded as premature. The more they deviate from the norm for their sex and
racial group on the minus side, the more premature they are considered to be. On the other
hand, the more they deviate on the plus side, the more postmature they are considered to be.
Unless damaged at birth, the postmature infant usually adjusts more quickly and more
successfully to the postnatal environment than the infant born at full term. However, because
the chances of birth damage increase as Postmaturity increases, the advantages that come
from the speed and ease of adjustment are far outweighed by the possibilities of birth
damage.
Prematurely born babies usually experience complications in adjusting to the postnatal
environment, and these may have a serious effect on future adjustment. Furthermore, every
difficulty that the normal, full-term infant faces in adjusting to the new environment is
magnified in the case of the premature baby.
6.4.5 Parental Attitudes
How quickly and how successfully newborn infants will adjust to postnatal life is greatly
influenced by parental attitudes. This is the fifth condition that influences the kind of
adjustments infants make to postnatal life.
When parental attitudes are unfavorable, for whatever the reason, they are reflected in
treatment of the infant that militates against successful adjustments to postnatal life. By
contrast, parents whose attitudes are favorable treat the infant in ways that encourage good
adjustment. Parent-infant interactions are not characterized by the emotional tension and
nervousness that are normally present when parental attitudes are unfavorable. A relaxed
mother, for example, usually produces more milk than one who is tense and nervous, and this
helps the infant adjust to a new method of taking nourishment.
While maternal attitudes are, unquestionably, more important than paternal attitudes in
determining the newborn infant's adjustment to postnatal life, paternal attitudes cannot be
disregarded. Indirectly, they are important because of the effect they have on maternal
attitudes. Directly, they are important because of the effect they have on the way fathers
handle their newborn infants and on the way they assist in their care after they are brought
home from the hospital. Fathers who are present during delivery usually have more favorable
attitudes toward their children than do those who do not share the childbirth experience with
their partners.
Parental attitudes toward the newborn infant are influenced by attitudes developed during
the prenatal period, by conditions associated with birth, and by the care given the infant after
leaving the hospital. Some conditions have a greater effect on maternal attitudes while others
have a greater impact on paternal attitudes.
6.4.6 Postnatal Care
The sixth influential condition is the kind of postnatal care the newborn receives during
the infancy period. For the most part, care during the first three or four days after birth will be
by hospital personnel. After that, care will be in the home, usually given by the mother with
some assistance from the father, relatives, or paid domestic help brought into the home for a
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
76
week or more after the mother leaves the hospital.
While the overall quality of the postnatal care is important in determining the kind of
adjustments the infant will make to postnatal life, three aspects of this care are especially
important. They are the amount of attention infants receive to ensure that their needs will be
met satisfactorily and relatively promptly, the amount of stimulation they receive from the
time of birth, and the degree of confidence their parents, especially their mothers, have in
meeting their needs.
First, newborn infants, accustomed to a stable environment before birth in which their
bodily needs were automatically met with no effort on their part, must now depend on the
people in their new environment to meet these needs for them. Because of their
neurophysiological immaturity, these needs will not necessarily arise at given times.
Furthermore, newborns cannot tell those around them what they want or need. All they can
do is cry. While most normal, full-term infants suffer no serious or lasting effects as a result
of this impersonal care, there is evidence that it delays their adjustment to postnatal life.
The second aspect of postnatal care that influences the infant's adjustments to postnatal
life is the type and amount of stimulation given. Because of the little time nurses can devote
to each newborn in the hospital nursery, most infants receive minimal stimulation during the,
first few days of their lives. Also, because many parents, especially parents of firstborns, are
afraid that handling them will damage them in some way, infants are often deprived at home
of the stimulation they formerly had in the uterus from the constant movements of the fetal
body. Unfortunately, they are usually handled, rocked, talked to, and in other ways stimulated
as little as possible.
When, on the other hand, newborn infants are stimulated, they regain their lost birth
weight earlier, they overcome the dazed state characteristic of the first days of postnatal life
sooner, and they are more alert and responsive to their new environment. This is true of
prematures just as it is of full-term infants. As Marcus has explained, "The warmth and
affection a mother shows when cuddling her baby apparently does more than demonstrate her
love; it may actually stimulate the infant's neurological development. Lack of loving
stimulation could contribute to the disabilities often suffered by premature babies".
The third condition associated with postnatal care is the degree of confidence parents,
especially mothers; have in performing their parental tasks satisfactorily. Many parents lack
confidence in their abilities to take care of their infants once they are released from the
hospital. This is especially true of firstborns or infants who are premature or suffer from some
physical defect.
Recognizing that a mother's self-confidence aids her infant's adjustments to postnatal life,
some hospitals are giving the new mother an opportunity to share in the care of her infant
through the "rooming-in plan," One of the most difficult problems mothers face, especially in
the care of a firstborn, is to know what the infant's different cries mean. The greater the
mother's confidence in this ability, the better she can care for her infant and the better the
infant's postnatal adjustments will be.
Check Your Progress 2
State the conditions influencing adjustment to postnatal life.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
....................................................................................................................................................
6.5 CHARACTERISTICS OF TODDLERHOOD
Certain characteristics of toddlerhood, while similar to characteristics of other periods
in the life span, are of particular importance during the toddlerhood years. They distinguish
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
77
toddlerhood from the periods preceding it and those that follow it. Following are the most
important characteristics.
6.5.1 Toddlerhood is the True Foundation Age
While the whole of childhood, but especially the early years, are generally regarded as
the foundation age, toddlerhood is the true foundation period of life because, at this time,
many behavior patterns, many attitudes, and many patterns of emotional expression are being
established.
Early scientific interest in the importance of these foundations came from the work of
Freud, who maintained that personality maladjustments in adulthood had their origins in
unfavorable childhood experiences. Erikson also contended that "childhood is the scene of
man's beginning as man, the place where our particular virtues and vices slowly but clearly
develop and make themselves felt." According to Erickson, how babies are treated will determine
whether they will develop "basic trust" or "basic distrust"-viewing the world as safe,
reliable, and nurturing or as full of threat, unpredictability, and treachery. The first two years
are critical in setting the pattern for personal and social adjustments. "Providing a rich social
life for a twelve- to fifteen-month-old child is the best thing you can do to guarantee a good
mind".
There are four reasons why foundations laid during the toddlerhood years are important.
First, contrary to tradition, children do not outgrow undesirable traits as they grow older.
Instead, patterns established early in life persist regardless of whether they are good or bad,
harmful or beneficial. Second, if an undesirable pattern of behavior or unfavorable beliefs
and attitudes have started to develop, the sooner they can be corrected the easier it will be for
the child. Third, because early foundations quickly develop into habits through repetition,
they will have a lifelong influence on a child's personal and social adjustments. And, fourth,
because learning and experience play dominant roles in development, they can be directed
and controlled so that the development will be along lines that will make good personal and
social adjustments possible.
6.5.2 Toddlerhood Is an Age of Rapid Growth and Change
Babies grow rapidly, both physically and psychologically. With this rapid growth comes
a change not only in appearance but also in capacities. Babies gradually become less topheavy
than they were at birth and their limbs develop in better proportion to the large head.
Changes in body proportions are accompanied by growth in height and weight. While growth
is rapid during the entire toddlerhood period, it is especially so during the first year of
toddlerhood.
Intellectual growth and change parallel physical growth and change. Perhaps in no area
is change more apparent than in the baby's ability to recognize and respond to people and
objects in the environment. Before toddlerhood has come to an end, babies are able to
understand many things and can communicate their needs and wants in ways that others can
understand.
6.5.3 Toddlerhood Is an Age of Decreasing Dependency
The decrease in dependency on others results from the rapid development of body
control which enables babies to sit, stand, and walk and to manipulate objects. The random,
mass movements of the infant give way to coordinated movements, which make it possible
for babies to do things for themselves which formerly they had to rely upon others to do for
them. Independence also increases as babies become able to communicate their needs to
others.
With decreased dependency comes a rebellion against being "babied." No longer are
babies willing to let others do things for them that they can or believe they can do for
themselves. If they are not permitted to try to be independent when they want to be, they
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
78
protest. This protest takes the form of angry outbursts and crying and soon develops into
negativism -one of the outstanding characteristics of the closing months of toddlerhood.
6.5.4 Toddlerhood Is the Age of Increased Individuality
Perhaps the most significant thing about increased independence is that it permits babies
to develop along lines suited to their interests and abilities. As a result, the individuality
apparent at birth increases as toddlerhood draws to a close. Individuality is shown in
appearance and in patterns of behavior. Even identical multiple births show individuality.
As individuality increases so does the necessity for treating each baby as an individual.
No longer can all babies be expected to thrive on the same food or the same schedules for
eating and sleeping. Nor can the same child-training techniques be expected to work equally
well for all babies. Most parents discover, even before babies reach the first birthday, that
they are individuals and must be treated as such.
6.5.5 Toddlerhood Is the Beginning of Socialization
The egocentrism, characteristic of the very young baby, quickly gives way to a desire to
become a part of the social group. Babies show their desire to become a part of the social
group by putting up protests when they are left alone for any length of time and by trying to
win the attention of others in any way they can.
One of the ways in which babies show their interest in becoming a part of the social
group is by attachment behavior. Because they can count on the attention and affection of
their mothers or mother substitutes more than on other family members or outsiders, they
develop strong emotional ties with their mothers long before toddlerhood comes to a close. It
is from the satisfaction of this attachment behavior that the desire to establish warm and
lasting relationships with others develops.
6.5.6 Toddlerhood Is the Beginning of Sex-Role Typing
Almost from the moment of birth, boys are treated as boys and girls as girls. Boys, for
example, are dressed in blue clothes, covered with blue blankets, and live in a room that lacks
the frills and ruffles of a girl's room. Toys are selected that are appropriate for boys, and they
are told stories about boys and their activities. The same sex-identifying traditions apply to
girls.
But while sex-role typing is part of a girl's early training, the pressures on her to be sexappropriate
even as a baby are not as strong as they are on a boy. However, indirectly girls
are sex-role typed in toddlerhood by being permitted to cry and show other signs of "female
weakness" which are discouraged in boy babies.
6.5.7 Toddlerhood Is an Appealing Age
Even though all babies are disproportionate, according to adult standards, they are
appealing because of their big heads, protruding abdomens, small, thin limbs, and tiny hands
and feet. When they are dressed in baby clothes and wrapped in baby blankets, they become
even more appealing.
Older children as well as adults find small babies appealing because of their helplessness
and dependency. Gradually, as babies' dependency is replaced by their ability to do things for
themselves, and their appearance becomes less appealing as it changes from the small, dolllike
body covered with baby garments to a larger, lankier body covered with sturdier, plainer
clothes, they become less easy to manage and more resistant to help from others.
6.5.8 Toddlerhood Is the Beginning of Creativity
Because of their lack of muscle coordination and their inability to control their
environment, babies are incapable of doing anything that can be regarded as original or
creative. They are learning, however, in these early months of life to develop interests and
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
79
attitudes that will lay the foundations for later creativity or for conformity to patterns set by
others. And this will be largely determined by the treatment they receive from others,
especially their parents.
6.5.9 Toddlerhood Is a Hazardous Age
While there are hazards at every age during the life span, certain hazards are more
common during toddlerhood than at other ages. Some of these are physical and some
psychological.
Among the physical hazards, illnesses and accidents are the most serious because they
often lead to permanent disabilities or to death. Since behavior patterns, interests, and
attitudes are established during toddlerhood, serious psychological hazards can result if poor
foundations are laid at this time.
6.6 LEARNING CAPACITIES
Learning refers to changes in behavior as the result of experience. Babies come into the
world with built-in learning capacities that permit them to profit from experience
immediately. Infants are capable of two basic forms of learning,: classical and operant
conditioning. They also learn through their natural preference for novel stimulation. Finally,
shortly after birth, babies learn by observing others; they can soon imitate the facial expressions
and gestures of adults.
6.6.1 Classical Conditioning
Newborn reflexes, make classical conditioning possible in the young infant. In this form
of learning, a new stimulus is paired with a stimulus that leads to a reflexive response. Once
the baby's nervous system makes the connection between the two stimuli, the new stimulus
produces the behavior by itself.
Classical conditioning is of great value to infants because it helps them recognize which
events usually occur together in the everyday world. As a result, they can anticipate what is
about to happen next, and the environment becomes more orderly and predictable. Let's take
a closer look at the steps of classical conditioning.
As Rekha settled down in the rocking chair to nurse her baby Mala, she often stroked the
babies’ forehead. Soon Rekha noticed that each time the babies’ forehead was stroked, the
baby Mala made sucking movements. The baby had been classically conditioned. Here is
how it happened:
1. Before learning takes place, an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) must consistently produce a
reflexive, or unconditioned, response (UCR). I n Mala's case, sweet breast milk (UCS)
resulted in sucking (UCR).
2. To produce learning, a neutral stimulus that does not lead to the reflex is presented just
before, or at about the same time as, the UCS. Rekha stroked Mala's forehead as each nursing
period began. The stroking (neutral stimulus) was paired with the taste of milk (UCS).
3. If learning has occurred, the neutral stimulus by itself produces a response similar to the
reflexive response. The neutral stimulus is then called a conditioned stimulus (CS), and the
response it elicits is called a conditioned response (CR). We know that Mala has been
classically conditioned because stroking her forehead outside the feeding situation (CS)
results in sucking (CR).
If the CS is presented alone enough times, without being paired with the UCS, the CR
will no longer occur. In other words, if Rekha strokes Mala's forehead again and again
without feeding her, Mala will gradually stop sucking in response to stroking. This is referred
to as extinction.
Young infants can be classically conditioned most easily when the association between two
stimuli has survival value. Mala learned quickly in the feeding situation, since learning the
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
80
stimuli that accompany feeding improves the infant's ability to get food and survive. In
contrast, some responses are very difficult to classically condition in young babies. Fear is
one of them. Until infants have the motor skills to escape from unpleasant events, they do not
have a biological need to form these associations. But after 6 months of age, fear is easy to
condition.
6.6.2 Operant Conditioning
In classical conditioning, babies build expectations about stimulus events in the
environment, but they do not influence the stimuli that occur. In operant conditioning, infants
act (or operate) on the environment, and stimuli that follow their behaviour change the
probability that the behavior will occur again. A stimulus that increases the occurrence of
a response of a response is called a reinforcer. For example, sweet liquid reinforces the
sucking response in newborn babies. Removing a desirable stimulus or presenting an
unpleasant one to decrease the occurrence of a response is called punishment. A sourtasting
fluid punishes newborn babies' sucking response. It causes them to purse their lips
and stop sucking entirely.
Because the young infant can control only a few behaviors, successful operant
conditioning in the early weeks of life is limited to sucking and head-turning responses.
However, many stimuli besides food can serve as reinforcers. For example, researchers have
created special laboratory conditions in which the baby's rate of sucking on a nipple produces
a variety of interesting sights and sounds. Newborns will suck faster to see visual designs or
hear music and human voices. As these findings suggest, operant conditioning has become a
powerful tool for finding out what stimuli babies can perceive and which ones they prefer.
Operant conditioning soon modifies parents' and babies' reactions to each other. As the
infant gazes into the adult's eyes, the adult looks and smiles back, and then the infant looks
and smiles again. The behavior of each partner reinforces the other, and as a result, both
parent and baby continue their pleasurable interaction.
6.6.3 Habituation
At birth, the human brain is set up to be attracted to novelty. Infants tend to respond more
strongly to a new element that has entered their environment. Habituation refers to a gradual
reduction in the strength of a response due to repetitive stimulation. Looking, heart rate, and
respiration rate may all decline, indicating a loss of interest. Once this has occurred, a new
stimulus-some kind of change in the environment--causes responsiveness to return to a high
level, an increase called recovery. For example, when you walk through a familiar space, you
notice things that are new and different, such as a recently purchased picture on the wall or a
piece of furniture that has been moved. Habituation and recovery enable us to focus our
attention on those aspects of the environment we know the least about. As a result, learning is
more efficient.
By studying infants' habituation and recovery, we can explore their understanding of the
world. For example, a baby who first habituates to a visual pattern (a photo of a baby) and
then recovers to a new one (a photo of a bald ma) appears to remember the first stimulus and
perceive the second one as new and different from it. This method of studying infant
perception and cognition can be used with newborn babies, including those who are preterm.
It has even been used to study the fetus's sensitivity to external stimuli-for example, by
measuring changes in fetal heart rate when various repeated sounds are presented. The
capacity to habituate and recover is evident in the third trimester of pregnancy.
6.6.4 Imitation
Newborn babies come into the world with a primitive ability to learn through imitation-by
copying the behavior of another person. The newborn's capacity to imitate extends to certain
gestures, such as head movements, and has been demonstrated in many ethnic groups and
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
81
cultures.
Some regard the capacity as little more than an automatic response, much like a reflex.
Others claim that newborns imitate many facial expressions even after short delays-when the
adult is no longer demonstrating the behavior. These observations suggest that the capacity is
flexible and voluntary.
Infants' capacity to imitate improves greatly over the first 2 years. But however limited it
is at birth, imitation is a powerful means of learning. Using imitation, young infants explore
their social world, getting to know people by matching their behavioral states. In the process,
babies notice similarities between their own actions and those of others and start to find out
about themselves. Furthermore, by tapping into infants' ability to imitate, adults can get
infants to express desirable behaviors, and once they do, adults can encourage these further.
Finally, caregivers take great pleasure in a baby who imitates their facial gestures and actions.
Imitation seems to be one of those capacities that help get the infant's relationship with
parents off to a good start.
Check Your Progress 3
A. Explain the significant characteristics of Toddlerhood
B. Mention the learning capacities of toddler hood.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
6.7 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Characteristics of Infancy
ii) Adjustments of Infancy
iii) Conditions Influencing Adjustment to Postnatal Life
iv) Characteristics of Toddlerhood
v) Learning Capacities of toddler hood
6.8 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. Your answer may include the description of the characteristics of infancy
i) it is the shortest of all developmental periods
ii) it is a time of radical adjustments
iii) it has a plateau in development
iv) it is a preview of later development
v) it is a hazardous period
2. The conditions that could influence adjustment to postnatal life are:
i) Prenatal Environment
ii) Kind of Birth
iii) Experiences Associated with Birth
iv) Length of Gestation Period
v) Parental Attitudes
vi) Postnatal Care
6.9 Lesson – End Activities
1. A. The characteristics of toddlerhood are:
i) the True Foundation Age
ii) an Age of Rapid Growth and Change
iii) an Age of Decreasing Dependency
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
82
iv) the Age of Increased Individuality
v) the Beginning of Socialization
vi) the Beginning of Sex-Role Typing
vii) an Appealing Age
viii) the Beginning of Creativity
ix) a Hazardous Age
B. Learning capacities of toddler hood can be explained with reference to:
i) Classical Conditioning
ii) Operant Conditioning
iii) Habituation
iv) Imitation
6.10 References
1. Roazen, P. Freud and His Followers, New York: Alfred Knopf, 1975.
2. Stanger, Ross, A. Biographical Approach, New York: McGraw – Hill, 1978.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
83
UNIT II
LESSON – 7
PIAGET’S COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENTAL THEORY- INFORMATION
PROCESSING - INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN EARLY MENTAL
DEVELOPMENT
Contents
7.0 Aims and Objectives
7.1 Introduction
7.1.1 Piaget's Ideas about Cognitive Change
7.1.2 The Sensorimotor Stage
7.2 Information Processing
7.2.1 Structure of the Information-Processing System
7.2.2 Attention
7.2.3 Memory
7.2.4 Categorization
7.3 Individual Differences in Early Mental Development
7.3.1 Infant Intelligence Tests
7.3.2 Early Environment and Mental Development
7.3.3 Early Intervention for At-Risk Infants and Toddlers
7.4 Let Us Sum Up
7.5 Check your Progress
7.6 Lesson – End Activities
7.7 References
7.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help you understand Piaget’s stages of cognitive development, the
information processing process and the individual differences in early mental development of
infants
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
v) Mention the stages of cognitive development through life span
vi) State the Piaget's ideas about cognitive change
vii) Explain the structure of the information-processing system
viii) Describe the use of infant intelligence tests
ix) Bring out the relationship of environmental factors to infant and toddler mental test
scores
x) Spell out the intervention for at-risk infants and toddlers
7.1 INTRODUCTION
Swiss theorist Jean Piaget inspired a vision of children as busy, motivated explorers
whose thinking develops as they act directly on the environment. Influenced by his
background in biology, he believed that the child's mind forms and modifies psychological
structures so they achieve a better fit with external reality. Piaget's theory focuses on four
stages(Table 1) of cognitive development, which is stated below. This will be dealt
individually in the subsequent lessons.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
84
Table 1 Piaget’s Stages of Cognitive Development
Stage Period of
Development
Description
Sensorimotor
Preoperational
Concrete
operational
Formal
operational
Birth-2 years
2-7 years
7-11 years
11 years on
Infants "think" by acting on the world with their eyes, ears, hands,
and mouth. As a result, they invent ways of solving sensorimotor
problems, such as pulling a lever to hear the sound of a music box,
finding hidden toys, and putting objects in and taking them out of
containers.
Preschool children use symbols to represent their earlier
sensorimotor discoveries. Development of language and makebelieve
play takes place. However, thinking lacks the logical
qualities of the two remaining stages.
Children's reasoning becomes logical. School-age children
understand that certain amount of lemonade or play dough remains
the same even after its appearance changes. They also organize
objects into hierarchies of classes and subclasses. However, thinking
falls short of adult intelligence. It is not yet abstract.
The capacity for abstraction permits adolescents to reason with
symbols that do not refer to objects in the real world, as in advanced
mathematics. They can also think of all possible outcomes in a
scientific problem, not just the most obvious ones.
Children move through four stages between infancy and adolescence. During those stages, all
as, cognition develops in an integrated fashion, changing in similar way at about the same
time. The first stage is the sensorimotor stage, which spans the first 2 years of life.
As the name of this stage implies, Piaget believed infants and toddlers "think" with their
eyes, ears, hands and other sensorimotor equipment. They cannot yet, carry out many
activities inside their heads. But by the end of to toddlerhood, children can solve practical,
everyday problems and represent their experiences in speech, gesture, and play. To appreciate
Piaget's view of how these vast changes take let's consider some important concepts.
7.1.1 Piaget's Ideas about Cognitive Change
According to Piaget, specific psychological structures - organized ways of making sense
of experience called schemes-change with age. At first, schemes are sensorimotor action
patterns. For example, at 6 months, a child dropped objects in a fairly rigid way, simply by
letting go of a rattle and watching with interest. By age 18 months his "dropping scheme" had
become much more deliberate and creative. He tossed all sorts of objects down the basement
stairs, throwing some up in the air, bouncing others off walls, releasing some gently and
others forcefully. Soon his schemes will move from an action-based level to a mental level.
Instead of just acting on objects, he will show evidence of thinking before he acts. This
change marks the transition from sensorimotor to preoperational thought.
In Piaget's theory, two processes account for changes in schemes: adaptation and
organization.
Adaptation It involves building schemes through direct interaction with the environment.
It consists of two complementary activities: assimilation and accommodation. During
assimilation, we use our current schemes to interpret the external world. For example, when
the child dropped objects were assimilating them all into his sensorimotor "dropping
scheme.” In accommodation, we create new schemes or adjust old ones after noticing that our
current ways of thinking do not fit the environment completely. When the child dropped
objects in different ways, he modified his dropping scheme to take account of the varied
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
85
properties of objects.
According to Piaget, the balance between assimilation and accommodation varies over
time. When children are not changing much, they assimilate more than they accommodate.
Piaget called this a state of cognitive equilibrium, implying a steady comfortable condition.
During rapid cognitive change, however, children are in a state of disequilibrium, or
cognitive discomfort. They realize that new information does not match their current
schemes, so they shift away from assimilation toward accommodation. Once they modify
their schemes, they move back toward assimilation, exercising their newly changed structures
until they are ready to be modified again.
Each time this back-and-forth movement between equilibrium and disequilibrium occurs,
more effective schemes are produced. Because the times of greatest accommodation are
earliest ones, the sensorimotor stage is Piaget's most complex period of development.
Organization Schemes also change through organization, a process that takes place
internally, apart from direct contact with the environment. Once children form new schemes,
they rearrange them, linking them with other schemes to create a strongly interconnected
cognitive system. For example, eventually a child will relate "dropping" to "throwing" and to
his developing understanding of "nearness" and "farness:' According to Piaget, schemes reach
a true state of equilibrium when they become part of a broad network of structures that can be
jointly applied to the surrounding world.
7.1.2 The Sensorimotor Stage
The difference between the newborn baby and the 2-yearold child is so vast that the
sensorimotor stage is divided into six substages (Table 2). According to Piaget, at birth
infants know so little about the world that they cannot purposefully explore their
surroundings. The circular reaction provides a special means of adapting first schemes. It
involves stumbling onto a new experience caused by the baby’s own motor activity. The
reaction is “circular” because the infant tries to repeat the event again and again. As a result,
a sensorimotor response that first occurred by chance becomes strengthened into a new
scheme.
Table 2- Piaget’s Sensorimotor Substages
Sensorimotor Substage Adaptive Behaviors
1. Reflexive schemes
(birth to 1 month)
2. Primary circular reactions
(1-4 months)
3. Secondary circular
reactions (4-8 months)
4. Coordination of secondary
Circular reactions (8-12
months)
5. Tertiary circular reactions
(12-18 months)
6. Mental representation
(18 months-2 years)
Newborn reflexes
Simple motor habits centered around the infant's own body;
limited anticipation of events
Actions aimed at repeating interesting effects in the
surrounding world; imitation of familiar; behaviors
Intentional, or goal-directed, behavior; ability to find a hidden
object in the first location in which it is hidden (object
permanence); improved anticipation of events; imitation of
behaviors slightly different from those the infant usually
performs
Exploration of the properties of objects by acting on them in
novel ways; imitation of unfamiliar behaviors; ability to
search in several locations for a hidden object (accurate A-B
search)
Internal depictions of objects and events, as indicated by
sudden solutions to problems, ability to find an object that has
been moved while out of sight (invisible displacement),
deferred imitation, and make-believe play
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
86
During the first 2 years, the circular reaction changes in several ways. At first it centers
around the infant's own body. Later, it turns outward, toward manipulation of objects. Finally,
it becomes experimental and creative, aimed at producing novel effects in the environment.
Young children's difficulty inhibiting new and interesting behaviors may underlie the circular
reaction. But this immaturity in inhibition seems to be adaptive! It helps ensure that new
skills will not be interrupted before they consolidate. Piaget considered revisions in the
circular reaction so important that he named the sensorimotor substages after them.
Repeating Chance Behaviors According to Piaget, newborn reflexes are the building
blocks of sensorimotor intelligence. At first, in Substage 1, babies suck, grasp, and look in
much the same way, no matter what experiences they encounter.
Around 1 month, as babies enter Substage 2, they start to gain voluntary control over their
actions through the primary circular reaction, by repeating chance behaviors largely motivated
by basic needs. This leads to some simple motor habits, such as sucking their fists or
thumbs. Babies of this substage also begin to vary their behavior in response to
environmental demands. For example, they open their mouths differently for a nipple than for
a spoon. Young infants also start to anticipate events. For example, at 3 months, when
Gautham awoke from his nap, he cried out with hunger. But as soon as Ganga entered the
room, his crying stopped. He knew that feeding time was near.
During Substage 3, which lasts from 4 to 8 months, infants sit up and reach for and
manipulate objects? These motor achievements play a major role in turning their attention
outward toward the environment. Using the secondary circular reaction, they try to repeat
interesting events caused by their own actions. For example, 4-month-old Manthra
accidentally knocked a toy hung in front of her, producing a fascinating swinging motion.
Over the next 3 days, Manthra tried to repeat this effect, at first by grasping and then by
waving her arms. Finally she succeeded in hitting the toy and gleefully repeated the motion.
She had built the sensorimotor scheme of "hitting.” Improved control over their own behavior
permits infants to imitate the behavior of others more effectively. However, 4- to 8 montholds
cannot adapt flexibly and quickly enough to imitate novel behaviors. Therefore, although
they enjoy watching an adult demonstrate a game they are not yet able to participate.
Intentional Behavior In Substage 4, 8- to 12-montb olds combine schemes into new,
more complex action sequences. As a result, actions that lead to new schemes no longer have
a random, hit-or-miss quality-accidentally, bringing the thumb to the mouth or happening to
hit the toy. Instead, 8- to 12-month-olds can engage in intentional, or goal-directed, behavior,
coordinating schemes deliberately to solve simple problems. The clearest example is
provided by Piaget's famous object-hiding task, in which he shows the baby an attractive toy
and then hides it behind his hand or under a cover. Infants of this substage can find the object.
In doing so, they coordinate two schemes-"pushing" aside the obstacle and "grasping" the
toy. Piaget regarded these action sequences as the foundation for all problem solving.
Retrieving hidden objects reveals that infants have begun to master object permanence,
the understanding that objects continue to exist when out of sight. But awareness of object
permanence is not yet complete. If the baby reaches several times for an object at a first
hiding place (A) and sees it moved to a second (B), she will still search for it in the first
hiding place (A). Because babies make this A-not-B search error, Piaget concluded that they
do not have a clear image of the object as persisting when hidden from view.
Substage 4 brings additional advances. First, infants can better anticipate events, so they
sometimes use their capacity for intentional behavior to try to change those events. At 10
months, Kaushik crawled after Ganga when she put on her chappals, whimpering to keep her
from leaving. Second, babies can imitate behaviors slightly different from those they usually
perform. After watching someone else, they try to stir with spoon, push a toy car, or drop
raisins in a cup. Once again they draw on intentional behavior, purposefully modifying
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
87
schemes to fit an observed action.
In Substage 5, which lasts from 12 to 18 months, the territory circular reaction emerges.
Toddlers repeat behaviors with variation, provoking new results. Kaushik when he drops an
object over the basement steps, trying this, then that, and then another action. Because they
approach the world in this deliberately exploratory way, 12- to 18-month-olds are far better
sensorimotor problem solvers than they were before. For example Kathambari figured out
how to fit a shape through a hole in a container by turning and twisting it until it fell through,
and she discovered how to use a stick to get toys that were out of reach. According to Piaget,
this capacity to experiment leads to a more advanced understanding of object permanence.
Toddlers look several locations to find a hidden toy, displaying an accurate A -B search.
Their more flexible action patterns also permit them to imitate many more behaviors, such as
stacking blocks, scribbling on paper, and making funny faces.
Mental Representation. Substage 6 culminates with the ability to create mental
representations-internal depictions of information that the mind can manipulate. Our most
powerful mental representations are of two kinds: (1) images, or mental pictures of objects,
people, and spaces, and (2) concepts, or categories in which similar objects or events are
grouped together. Using a mental image, we can retrace our steps when we've misplaced
something. Or we can imitate another's behavior long after we've observed it. And by
thinking in concepts and labeling them (for example, ball for all rounded, movable objects
used in play), we become more efficient thinkers, organizing our diverse experiences into
meaningful, manageable, and memorable units.
Piaget noted that in arriving at solutions suddenly rather than through trial-and-error
behavior, 18- to 24-month-olds seem to experiment with actions inside their heads-evidence
that they can mentally represent their experiences. For example, at 19 months Kathambari
received a new push toy. As she played with it for the first time, she rolled it over the carpet
and ran into the sofa. She paused for a moment, as if to "think,” and then immediately turned
the toy in a new direction. Representation results in several other capacities. First, it enables
older toddlers to solve advanced object permanence problems involving invisible
displacement-finding a toy moved while out of sight, such as into a small box while under a
cover. Second, it permits deferred imitation-the ability to remember and copy the behavior
of models who are not present. Finally, it makes possible make-believe play, in which
children act out everyday and imaginary activities. Like Kathambari's pretending to go to
sleep, the toddler's make-believe is very simple. Make-believe expands greatly in early
childhood and is so important for psychological development that we will return to it again.
In sum, as the sensorimotor stage draws to a close, mental symbols have become major
instruments of thinking.
Check Your Progress 1
Describe the Piaget’s cognitive developmental theory with special reference to
Sensorimotor Stage
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
....................................................................................................................................................
7.2 Information Processing
Information-processing theorists agree with Piaget that children are active, inquiring
beings, but they do not provide a single, unified theory of cognitive development. Instead,
they focus on many aspects of thinking, from attention, memory, and categorization skills to
complex problem solving.
The information-processing approach frequently relies on computer like flowcharts to
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
88
describe the human cognitive system. The computer model of h uman thinking is attractive
because it is explicit and precise. Information-processing theorists are not satisfied with general
concepts, such as assimilation and accommodation, to describe how children think.
Instead, they want to know-exactly what individuals of different ages do when faced with a
task or problem.
7.2.1 Structure of the Information-Processing System
Most information-processing researchers assume that we hold information in three parts
of the mental system for processing: the sensory register; working, or short-term, memory;
and long-term memory. As information flows through each, we can operate on and transform
it using mental strategies, increasing the chances that we will retain information and use it
efficiently. To understand this more clearly, let's look at each aspect of the mental system.
FIGURE 1 Store model of the human information-processing system.
First, information enters the sensory register. Here, sights and sounds are represented
directly and stored briefly. Look around you, and then dose your eyes. An image you saw
persists for a few seconds, but then it decays, or disappears, unless you use mental strategies
to preserve it. For example, you can attend to some information more carefully than to other
information, increasing the chances that it will transfer to the next step of the informationprocessing
system.
The second part of the mind i s working, or short-term, memory, where we actively
“work” on a limited amount of information, applying mental strategies. For example, if you
are studying this book effectively, you are taking notes, repeating information to yourself, or
grouping pieces of information together. Think, for a moment, about why you apply these
strategies. The sensory register, although limited, can take in a wide panorama of
information. The capacity of working memory is more restricted. But by meaningfully
Stimulus Input
Sensory
Register
Represents
sights and
sounds directly
and stores them
briefly
Attention
Working or
Short-Term
Memory
Holds limited
a m o u n t o f
information
which is worked
on to facilitate
m e m o r y a n d
problem solving
Storage
Retrieval
Response Output Response
Generator
Long-Term
Memory
Stores
information
permanently
Central Executive
Coordinates incoming
information with information
in the system
Controls attention
Selects, applies, and monitors
the effectiveness of strategies
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
89
connecting pieces of information into a single representation, we reduce the number of pieces
we must attend to, thereby making room in working memory for more. Also, the more
thoroughly we learn information, the more automatically we use it. Automatic cognitive
processing expands working memory by permitting us to us to focus on other information
simultaneously.
To manage its complex activities, a special part of working memory called the central
executive directs the flow of information with information already in the system, and selects,
applies, and monitors strategies. The central executive is the conscious, reflective part of our
mental system.
The longer we hold information in working memory, the greater the likelihood that it will
transfer to the third, and largest storage area - long – term memory, o ur permanent
knowledge base, which is limitless. In fact, we store so much in long-term memory that we
sometimes have problems with retrieval, or getting information back from the system. To aid
retrieval, we apply strategies, just as we do in working memory. Information in long-term
memory is categorized according to a master plan based on contents, much like a library
shelving system. As a result, we can retrieve it easily by following the same network of
associations used to store it in the first place.
Information processing researchers believe that the basic structure of the mental system is
similar throughout life. However, the capacity of the system-the amount of information that
can be retained and processed at once and the speed with which it can be processed-increases,
making possible more complex forms of thinking with age. Gains in information-processing
capacity are partly due to brain development and partly due to improvements in strategies,
such as attending to information and categorizing it effectively. The development of these
strategies is already under way in the first 2 years of life.
7.2.2 Attention
Attention develops in early infancy between 1 and 2 months of age, infants shift from
attending to a single high-contrast feature of their visual world to exploring objects and
patterns more thoroughly. Besides attending to more aspects of the environment, infants
gradually become more efficient at managing their attention, taking in information more
quickly with age. Habituation research reveals that preterm and newborn babies require a
long time to habituate and recover to novel visual stimuli-about 3 or 4 minutes. But by 4 or 5
months, infants require as little as 5 to 10 seconds to take in a complex visual stimulus and
recognize that it differs from a previous one.
One reason that very young babies' habituation times are so long is that they have
difficulty disengaging their attention from interesting stimuli. Once, Ambika held a doll
dressed in red-and-white checked overalls in front of 2-month-old Manthra, who stared
intently until, unable to break her gaze, she burst into tears. Just as important as attending to a
stimulus is the ability to shift attention from one stimulus to another. By 4 to 6 months,
infants' attention becomes more flexible.
During the first year, infants attend to novel and eye-catching events. With the transition
to toddlerhood, children become increasingly capable of intentional behavior. Consequently,
attraction to novelty declines (but does not disappear) and sustained attention improves,
especially when children play with toys. When a toddler engages in goal-directed behavior
even in a limited way, such as stacking blocks or putting them in a container, attention must
be maintained to reach the goal. As and activities become more complex, so does the duration
of attention.
7.2.3 Memory
Habituation research provides a window into infant memory. Studies show that
infants gradually make finer distinction among visual stimuli and remember them longer-at 3
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
90
months, for about 24 hours; by the end of the first year, for several days and, in the case of
some stimuli (such as a photo oft human face), even weeks. Yet recall that what babies know
about the stimuli to which they habituate and recover is not always clear. Some researchers
argue that infants' understanding is best revealed through their active efforts to master their
environment. Consistent with this view, habituation research greatly underestimates infants'
memory when compared with methods that rely on their active exploration of objects.
So far, we have discussed only recognition – noticing when a stimulus is identical or
similar to one previously experienced. This is the simplest form of memory because all
babies have to do is indicate (by looking or kicking) that a new stimulus is identical or similar
to a previous one. Recall is more challenging because it involves remembering something in
the absence of perceptual support. To recall, you must generate a mental image of the past
experience. By the end of the first year infants can engage in recall. We know because they
find hidden objects and imitate the actions of others hours or days after they observed the
behavior.
7.2.4 Categorization
As infants gradually remember more information, they store it in a remarkably orderly
fashion. In fact, young babies categorize stimuli on the basis of shape, size, and other
physical properties at such an early age that categorization is among the strongest evidence
that babies' brains are set up from the start to represent and organize experience in adult like
ways.
Habituation/recovery has also been used to study infant categorization. Researchers show
babies a series of stimuli belonging to one category and then see whether they recover to
(look longer at) a picture that is not a member of the category. Findings reveal that 7- to 12-
month-olds structure objects into an impressive array of meaningful categories-food items,
furniture, birds, animals, vehicles, kitchen utensils, plants, and. Besides organizing the
physical world, infants of this age categorize their emotional and social worlds. They sort
people and their voices by gender and age, have begun to distinguish emotional expressions,
and can separate the natural movements of people from other motions.
The earliest categories are perceptual-based on similar overall appearance or prominent
object part, such as legs for animals and wheels for vehicles. But by the end of the first year,
more categories are conceptual-based on common function and behavior. For example, lyear-
olds group together kitchen utensils because each is used to prepare food for eating.
In the second year, children become active categorizers. Around 12 months, they touch
objects that go together, without grouping them. Sixteen-month-olds can group objects into a
single category. For example, when given four balls and four boxes, they put all the balls
together but not the boxes. Around 18 months, they sort objects into two classes. Compared
with habituation/recovery, touching, sorting, and other play behaviors better reveal the
meanings that toddlers attach to categories because they are applying to those meanings in
their everyday activities. For example, after having watched an adult give a toy dog a drink
from a cup, 14-month-olds shown a rabbit and a motorcycle usually offer the drink only to
the rabbit. Their behavior reveals a clear understanding that certain actions are appropriate for
some categories of items (animals) and not others (vehicles).
Check Your Progress 2
Explain the information processing approach to problem solving
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
91
7.3 Individual Differences in Early Mental Development
The testing approach is very different from the cognitive theories, which try to explain
the process of development-how children's thinking changes over time. In contrast, designers
of mental tests focus on cognitive products. They seek to measure behaviors that reflect
mental development and to arrive at scores that predict future performance, such as later
intelligence, school achievement, and adult vocational success. This concern with prediction
arose nearly a century ago, when French psychologist Alfred Binet designed the first
successful intelligence test, which predicted school achievement. It inspired the design of
many new tests, including ones that measure intelligence at very early ages.
7.3.1 Infant Intelligence Tests
Accurately measuring the intelligence of infants is a challenge because they cannot
answer questions or follow directions. All we can do is present them with stimuli, coax them
to respond, and observe their behavior. As a result, most infant tests emphasize perceptual
and motor responses, along with a few tasks that tap early language and cognition. For
example, the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, a commonly used test for children
between 1 month and 31/2 years, consists of two parts: (1) the Mental Scale, which includes
such items as turning to a sound, looking for a fallen object, building a tower of cubes, and
naming pictures; and (2) the Motor Scale, which assesses gross and fine motor skills, such as
grasping, sitting, drinking from a cup, and jumping.
Predicting Later Performance from Infant Tests. Many people assume, incorrectly, that
IQ is a measure of inborn ability that does not change with age. Despite careful construction,
most infant tests predict later intelligence poorly. Longitudinal research reveals that the
majority of children show substantial fluctuations in IQ between toddlerhood and
adolescence- 10 to 20 points in most cases and sometimes much more.
Because infants and toddlers are especially likely to become distracted, fatigued, or bored
during testing, their scores often do not reflect their true abilities. In addition, the perceptual
and motor items on infant tests differ from the tasks given to older children, which emphasize
verbal, conceptual and problem-solving skills. Because of concerns that infant test scores do
not tap the same dimensions of intelligence measured at older ages, they are conservatively
labeled developmental quotients, or DQs, rather than IQs.
7.3.2 Early Environment and Mental Development
Intelligence is a complex blend of hereditary and environmental influences. There is a
relationship of environmental factors to infant and toddler mental test scores.
Home Environment. The Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME)
is a checklist for gathering information about the quality of children's home lives through
observation and interviews with parents. The Caregiving Concerns Table3 below lists factors
measured by HOME during the first 3 years. Each is positively related to toddlers' mental test
performance. Regardless of SES and ethnicity, an organized, stimulating physical setting and
parental encouragement, involvement, and affection repeatedly predict infant and early
childhood IQ. The extent to which parents talk to infants and toddlers is particularly
important. Heredity does not account for all of the correlation between home environment
and mental test scores. Family living conditions continue to predict children's IQ beyond the
contribution of parental IQ and it was found that infants and children growing up in less
crowded homes had parents who were far more verbally responsive to them-a major
contributor to language, intellectual, and academic progress.
Table 3
The Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME): Infancy and Toddler Subscales
SUBSCALE SAMPLE ITEM
Emotional and verbal
responsiveness of the
Parent caresses or kisses child at least once during observer’s visit.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
92
parent
Acceptance of the Child
Organization of the
physical environment
Provision of appropriate
play materials
Parental involvement with
the child
Variety in daily situation
Parent does not interfere with child’s actions or restrict child’s movements more
than three times during observer’s visit.
Child’s play environment appears safe and free of hazards.
Parent provides toys or interesting activities for child during observer’s visit.
Parent tends to keep child within visual range and to look at child often during
observer’s visit.
Child eats at least one meal per day with mother or father, according to parental
report.
Infant and Toddler Child Care. Home environments are not the only influential settings in
which young children spend their days. Today, more than 60 percent of mothers with a child
under age 2 are employed in cities. Child care for infants and toddlers has become common,
and its quality has a major impact on mental development. Research consistently shows that
infants and young children exposed to poor-quality child care, regardless of whether they
come from middle- or low-SES homes, score lower on measures of cognitive and social
skills.
In contrast, good child care can reduce the negative impact of a stressed, poverty-stricken
home life, and it sustains the benefits of growing up in an economically advantaged family.
Entering high-quality child care in infancy and toddlerhood was associated with cognitive,
emotional, and social competence in middle childhood and adolescence.
7.3.3 Early Intervention for At-Risk Infants and Toddlers
Children living in poverty are likely to show gradual declines in intelligence test scores
and to achieve poorly when they reach school age. These problems are largely due to stressful
home environments that undermine children's ability to learn and increase the chances that
they will remain poor throughout their lives. A variety of intervention programs have been
developed to break this tragic cycle of poverty. Although most begin during the preschool
years, a few start during infancy and continue through early childhood.
Some interventions are center-based; children attend an organized child-care or preschool
program where they receive educational, nutritional, and health services, and child-rearing
and other social-service supports are provided to parents as well. Other interventions are
home-based. A skilled adult visits the home and works with parents, teaching them how to
stimulate a very young child's development. In most programs, participating children score
higher on mental tests by age 2 than do untreated controls. These gains persist as long as the
program lasts and occasionally longer. The more intense the intervention, (full-day, yearround
high-quality child care plus support services for parents) the greater children’s
cognitive, and academic performance throughout childhood and adolescence.
Without some form of early intervention, many children born into economically
disadvantaged families will not reach their potential. Recognition of this reality led to provide
limited funding for intervention services directed at infants and toddlers who already have
serious developmental problems or who are at risk for problems because of poverty. At
present, available programs are not nearly enough to meet the need. Nevertheless, those that
exist are a promising beginning.
Check Your Progress 3
Explain the testing approach to assess the individual differences in early mental development
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
93
7.4 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Piaget's Ideas about Cognitive Change
ii) Piaget’s Sensorimotor Stage
iii) Structure of the Information-Processing System
iv) Individual Differences in Early Mental Development
v) Infant Intelligence Tests
vi) Early Environment and Mental Development
vii) Early Intervention for At-Risk Infants and Toddlers
7.5Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. Your answer may include :
i) Piaget’s Stages of Cognitive Development
ii) Piaget's Ideas about Cognitive Change
iii) The Sensorimotor Stage
iv) Repeating Chance Behaviors
v) Intentional Behavior
vi) Mental Representation
2. The focus of information processing approach includes:
i) structure of the information-processing system
ii) aspects of thinking, attention, memory, and
iii) categorization skills to complex problem solving
3. Assessing early mental development and include the:
i) Infant Intelligence Tests
ii) Early Environment and Mental Development
iii) Early Intervention for At-Risk Infants and Toddlers
7.6 Lesson – End Activities
1. Critically evaluate Piaget’s theory.
2. Discuss the major cognitive change during concrete operational stage”.
7.7 References
1. Papalia, D.E. and Olds, S.W., Psychology, New York : McGraw – Hill, 1987,
2. Piaget, J., Judgement and Reasoning in the Child, New York : 1926.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
94
LESSON – 8
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT - DEVELOPMENTAL TASKS
Contents
8.0 Aims and Objectives
8.1 Introduction
8.2 Three Theories of Language Development
8.2.1 The Behaviorist Perspective
8.2.2 The Nativist Perspective
8.2.3 The Interactionist Perspective
8.2.4 Getting Ready to Talk
8.2.5 First Words
8.2.6 The Two-Word Utterance Phase
8.2.7 Individual and Cultural Differences
8.2.8 Supporting Early Language Development
8.3 Developmental Tasks of Toddlerhood
8.3.1 Physical Development
8.3.2 Pattern of Physical Development during Toddlerhood
8.3.3 Physiological Functions
8.3.4 Muscle Control
8.3.5 Toddlerhood Skills
8.3.6 Beginning of Handedness
8.3.7 Developments in Socialization
8.3.8 Pattern of Development of Social Behavior
8.3.9 Beginnings of Interest in Play
8.3.10 Play Development Follows a Pattern
8.3.11 Value of Play in Toddlerhood
8.3.12 Development of Understanding
8.4 Let Us Sum Up
8.5 Check your Progress
8.6 Lesson – End Activities
8.7 References
8.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help you understand language development and developmental tasks of
infancy period.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
i) State the three theories of language development
ii) Mention the progress of talking by toddler hood
iii) List the developmental tasks of toddlerhood in the various areas
8.1 INTRODUCTION
As perception and cognition improve during infancy, they pave the way for an
extraordinary human achievement-language. On average, children say their first word at 12
months of age, with a range of about 8 to 18 months. Once words appear, language develops
rapidly. Sometime between 1 1/2 and 2 years, toddlers combine two words. By age 6, they
have a vocabulary of about 10,000 words, speak in elaborate sentences, and are skilled
conversationalists.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
95
8.2 THREE THEORIES OF LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT
In the 1950s, researchers did not take seriously the idea that very young children might be
able to figure out important properties of the language they hear. As a result, the two theories
of how children acquire language were extreme views. One behaviorism, regards language
development as entirely due to environmental influences. The second, nativism assumes that
children are "prewired" to master the intricate rules of their language.
8.2.1 The Behaviorist Perspective
Behaviorist B. F. Skinner (1957) proposed that language, like any other behavior, is
acquired through operant conditioning. As the baby makes sounds, parents reinforce those
that are most like words with smiles, hugs, and speech in return. For example, at 12 months,
when a child makes a sound, and if the parent utters a word about the article pointed, the
child would repeat the same word.
Some behaviorists say children rely on imitation to rapidly acquire complex utterances,
such as whole phrases and sentences. And imitation can combine with reinforcement to
promote language, as when a parent coaxes.
8.2.2 The Nativist Perspective
Linguist Noam Chomsky (1957) proposed a nativist theory that regards the young, child’s
amazing language skill as etched into the structure of the human brain. Focusing on grammar,
Chomsky reasoned that the rules of sentence organization are much too complex to be
directly taught to or discovered by a young child. Instead, he argued, all children are born
with a language acquisition device (LAD), an innate system that contains a set of rules
common to all languages. It permits children, no matter which they hear, to understand and
speak in a rule-oriented fashion as soon as they pick up enough words.
Newborn babies are remarkably sensitive to speech sounds and prefer to listen to the
human voice. In addition, children the world over reach major language mile stones in a
similar sequence. Isolated and abused children who experienced little contact in childhood
reveal lasting deficits in language, especially grammar and communication skills--evidence
indicating that childhood is a sensitive period for language learning, although the precise
boundary of that period is unclear. This is what Chomsky's idea of a biologically based
language program.
8.2.3 The Interactionist Perspective
In recent years, new ideas about language development have arisen, emphasizing interactions
between inner capacities and environmental influences. Although several interactionist
theories exist, all stress the social context of language learning. An active child, well endowed
for acquiring language, observes and participates in social exchanges. From these
experiences, children gradually discover the functions and regularities of language.
According to the interactionist position, native capacity, a strong desire to interact with
others, and a rich language and social environment combine to help children build a
communicative system. And because genetic and environmental contributions vary across
children, the interactionist perspective predicts individual differences in language learning.
Even among interactionists, debate continues over the precise nature of innate language
abilities. Some theorists accept a modified view of Chomsky's position. They believe that
children are primed to acquire language but that they form and refine hypotheses about its
structure on the basis of experiences with language. Others believe that children make sense
of their complex language environments by applying powerful cognitive strategies rather than
strategies specifically tuned to language.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
96
The course of early language growth supports the interactionist position. But none of
these theories has yet been fully tested. In reality, biology, cognition, and social experience
may operate in different balances with respect to various aspects of language: pronunciation,
vocabulary, grammar, and communication skills. Table 1 provides an overview of early
language milestones.
Table 1
Milestones of Language Development During the First Two Years
Approximate Age Milestone
2 months
4 months on
8-12 months
12 months
18-24 months
20-26 months
Infants coo, making pleasant vowel sounds.
Infants babble, adding consonants to their cooing sounds and
repeating syllables. By 7 months, babbling of hearing infants
starts to include many sounds of mature spoken languages.
Infants and parents establish joint attention, and parents often
verbally label what the baby is looking at.
Interaction between parents and baby includes turn-taking games,
such as pat-a-cake and peek-a-boo. By 12 months, babies
participate actively.
Babbling contains consonant-vowel and intonation patterns of the
infant's language community. Infants begin using preverbal
gestures, such as showing and pointing, to influence the behavior
of others. Word comprehension first appears.
Toddlers say their first recognizable word.
Vocabulary expands from about 50 to 200 words.
Toddlers combine two words.
8.2.4 Getting Ready to Talk
Before babies say their first word, they are preparing for language in many ways. They
listen attentively to human speech and make speech like sounds. And as adults, we can hardly
help but respond.
Cooing and Babbling: Around 2 months, babies begin to make vowel-like noises, called
cooing because of their pleasant "00” quality. Gradually, consonants are added, and around 4
months, babbling appears, in which infants repeat consonant-vowel combinations in long
strings, such as "bababababa" or "nanananana."
The timing of early babbling seems to be due to maturation because babies everywhere
start babbling at about the same age and produce a similar range of early sounds. But for
babbling to develop further, infants must be able to hear human speech. If a baby's hearing is
impaired, these speech like sounds are greatly delayed or, in the case of deaf infants, totally
absent.
As infants listen to spoken language, babbling expands to include a broader range of
sounds. At around 7 months, it starts to include many sounds of mature spoken languages.
And by 1 year, it contains the consonant-vowel and intonation patterns of the infant's
language community. Deaf infants exposed to sign language from birth babble with their
hands in much the same way hearing infants do through speech. Furthermore, hearing babies
of deaf, signing parents produce babble like hand motions with the rhythmic patterns of
natural language. Infants' sensitivity to language rhythm, evident in both spoken and signed
babbling, may help them discover and produce meaningful language units. And through
babbling, babies seem to experiment with a great many sounds that can be blended into their
first words.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
97
Becoming a Communicator: Besides responding to cooing and babbling, adults interact
with infants in many other situations. Around 4 months, infants start to gaze in the same
direction adults are looking, a skill that becomes more accurate between 12 and 15 months of
age. Adults also follow the baby's line of vision and comment on what the infant sees,
labeling the environment for the baby. Infants and toddlers who often experience this joint
attention comprehend more language, produce meaningful gestures and words earlier, and
show faster vocabulary development.
Around 4 to 6 months, interaction between parent and baby begins to include give-andtake,
as in turn-taking games, such as pat-a-cake and peek-a-boo. At first, the parent starts the
game and the baby is an amused observer. Nevertheless, 4-month-olds are sensitive to the
structure and timing of these interactions, smiling more to an organized than a disorganized
peek-a-boo exchange. By 12 months, babies participate actively, trading roles with the parent.
As they do so, they practice the turn-taking pattern of human conversation, a vital context for
acquiring language and communication skills. Infants' play maturity and vocalizations during
games predict advanced language progress between and 2 years of age.
At the end of the first year, as infants become capable of intentional behavior, they use
preverbal gestures to influence the behavior of others. For example, Deepa held up a toy to
show it and pointed to cupboard when she wanted a cookie. Mother responded to her gestures
and also labeled them {"Oh, you want a chocolate”). In this way, toddlers learn that using
language leads desired results. Soon they utter words along with their reaching and pointing
gestures, the gestures recede, and spoken language is under way.
8.2.5 First Words
In the middle of the first year, infants begin to understand word meanings. When 6-
month-olds listened to the words “mommy” and "daddy" while looking at side-by-side videos
of their parents, they looked longer at the video of the named parent. First spoken words,
around 1 year, build on the sensorimotor foundations Piaget described on categories children
form during their first 2 years. Usually they refer to important people ("Mama;' "Dada"), objects
that move ("car;' "ball;' "cat"), familiar actions ("bye-bye; “up," "more"), or outcomes of
familiar actions ("dirty," "wet;' hot"). In their first 50 words, toddlers rarely name things that
just sit there, like "table" or "vase".
Besides cognition, emotion influences early word learning. At first, when acquiring a new
word for an object, person, or event, 1 1/2-year-olds say it neutrally; they need to listen
carefully to learn, and strong emotion diverts their attention. As words become better learned,
toddlers integrate talking and expressing feelings. "Shoe!" said one enthusiastic 22-month-old
as her mother tied her shoelaces before an outing. At the end of the second year, children
begin to label their emotions with words like "happy;' "mad;' and "sad".
When young children first learn words, they sometimes apply them too narrowly, an error
called underextension. For example, at 16 months, Deepa used "bear" to refer only to the
worn and tattered bear that she carried around much of the day. A more common error is
overextension-applying a word to a wider collection of objects and events than is
appropriate. For example, Geetha used "car" for buses, trains, trucks, and fire engines.
Toddlers' overextensions reflect their sensitivity to categories. They apply a new word to a
group of similar experiences, such as "car" to wheeled objects and "open" to opening a door,
peeling fruit, and undoing shoelaces. This suggests that children sometimes overextend
deliberately because they have difficulty recalling or have not acquired a suitable word. As
their vocabularies enlarge, overextensions disappear.
Overextensions illustrate another important feature of language development: the
distinction between language production (the words children use) and language
comprehension (the words children understand). Children overextend many more words in
production than they do in comprehension. That is, a 2-year-old may refer to trucks, trains,
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
98
and bikes as "car" but look at or point to these objects correctly when given their names. At
all ages, comprehension develops ahead of production. This tells us that failure to say a word
does not mean that toddlers do not understand it. If we rely only on what children say, we
will underestimate their knowledge of language.
8.2.6 The Two-Word Utterance Phase
At first, toddlers add to their vocabularies slowly, at a rate of 1 to 3 words a month.
Between 18 and 24 months, a spurt in vocabulary growth often takes place. As speed of
identifying words in spoken sentences and memory and categorization improve, many
children add 10 to 20 new words a week. When vocabulary approaches 200 words, toddlers
start to combine two words, saying, for example, "Mommy shoe;' "go car;' and "more
chocolates:' These two-word utterances are called telegraphic speech because, like a
telegram, they leave out smaller and less important words. Children, the world over use them
to express an impressive variety of meanings.
Two-word speech is largely made up of simple formulas, such as "want + X" and "more +
X;' with many different words inserted in the X position. Although toddlers rarely make gross
grammatical errors (such as saying "chair my" instead of "my chair"), they can be heard
violating the rules. For example, at 20 months, Geetha said "more hot" and "more read;'
combinations that are not acceptable in English grammar. The word-order regularities in
toddlers' two-word utterances are usually copies of adult word pairings, as when the parent
says, "That's my book," or "How about more sandwich?”. But it does not take long for
children to figure out grammatical rules. The beginnings of grammar are in place by age 21/2.
8.2.7 Individual and Cultural Differences
Each child's progress in acquiring language results from a complex blend of biological
and environmental influences. The most common biological explanation is girls' faster rate of
physical maturation, believed to promote earlier development of the left cerebral hemisphere,
where language is housed. But perhaps because of girls' slight language advantage, mothers
also talk more to toddler-age girls than boys, so girls add vocabulary more quickly for both
genetic and environmental reasons.
Besides the child's sex, personality makes a difference. Reserved, cautious toddlers often
wait until they understand a great deal before trying to speak. When they finally do speak,
their vocabularies grow rapidly. In the week after her adoption, 16-month-old Gomathi spoke
only a single Tamil word. For the next 2 months, Gomathi listened to English conversation
without speaking-a "silent period" typical of children beginning to acquire a second language.
Around 18 months, words came quickly-first "Eli:' then "doggie;' "kitty;' "Mama:' "Dada;'
"book;' "ball;' "car:' "cup;' "clock;' and "chicken:' all within a single week.
Young children have unique styles of early language learning. Geetha and Gomathi, like
most toddlers, used a referential style; their early vocabularies consisted mainly of words
that referred to objects. A smaller number of toddlers use an expressive style; compared with
referential children, they produce many more pronouns and social formulas, such as "stop it:'
"thank you;' and "I want it.' These styles reflect early ideas about the functions of language.
Gomathi, for example, thought words were for naming things. In contrast, expressive-style
children believe words are for talking about people's feelings and needs. The vocabularies of
referential-style children grow faster because all languages contain many more object labels
than social phrases. Expressive-style children tend to be highly sociable, and parents more
often use verbal routines ("How are you?" “Its no trouble") that support social relationships.
8.2.8 Supporting Early Language Development
There is little doubt that children are specially prepared for acquiring language, since no
other species can develop as flexible and creative a capacity for communication as we can.
Yet consistent with the interactionist view, a rich social environment builds on young
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
99
children's natural readiness to speak their native tongue.
Adults in many cultures speak to young children in child directed speech (CDS), a form of
communication made up short sentences with high-pitched, exaggerated expression, clear
pronunciation, distinct pauses between speech segments, and repetition of new words in a
variety of contexts ("See the ball.' "The ball bounced!"). Deaf parents use a similar style of
communication when signing their deaf babies. CDS builds on several communicative
strategies we have already considered: joint attention, turn-taking, and caregivers' sensitivity
to children’s preverbal gestures.
From birth on, children prefer to listen to CDS over other k inds of adult talk, and by 5
months they are more emotionally responsive to it. And parents constantly fine-tune it,
adjusting the length and content of their utterances to fit their children's needs-adjustments
that promote language comprehension and also permit toddlers to join in conversation.
Conversational give-and-take between parent and toddler is one of the best predictors of
early language development and academic competence during the school years. Impatience
with and rejection of children's efforts to talk lead them to stop trying and result in immature
language skills.
Check Your Progress 1
A. Explain the three significant theories of language development
B. Discuss the how babies gradually get ready to talk fluently?
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
8.3 DEVELOPMENTAL TASKS OF TODDLERHOOD
As the pattern of development is predictable even though different babies reach
important landmarks in this pattern at slightly different ages, it is possible to set up standards
of social expectations in the form of developmental tasks. All babies, for example, are
expected to learn to walk, to take solid foods, to have their organs of elimination under partial
control, to achieve reasonable physiological stability (especially in hunger rhythm and sleep),
to learn the foundations of speech, and to relate emotionally to their parents and siblings to
some extent instead of being completely self-bound, as they were at birth. Most of these
developmental tasks will not, of course, be completely mastered when toddlerhood draws to a
close, but the foundations for them should be laid.
When toddlerhood ends, all normal babies have learned to walk, though with varying
degrees of proficiency. They have also learned to take solid foods and they have achieved a
reasonable degree of physiological stability. The major tasks involving the elimination of
body wastes are well under control and will be completely mastered within another year or
two.
While most babies have built up a useful vocabulary, can pronounce the words they use
reasonably correctly, can comprehend the meaning of simple statements and commands, and
can put together several words into meaningful sentences, their ability to communicate with
others and to comprehend what others say to them is still on a low level. Much remains to be
mastered before they enter school.
The rapid development of the nervous system, the ossification of the bones, and the
strengthening of the muscles make it possible for babies to master the developmental tasks of
toddlerhood. However, their success in this regard depends to a large extent upon the
opportunities they are given to master them and the help and guidance they receive.
Babies who lag behind their age-mates in mastering the developmental tasks of
toddlerhood will be handicapped when they reach the early childhood years and are expected
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
100
to master the developmental tasks for these years. A poor foundation in motor skills or in
speech, for example, will make it difficult for young children to master the skills in these
areas of development. Good mastery of these developmental tasks, by contrast, gives babies
the foundations needed for successful mastery of speech, motor skills, and other forms of
body control that are essential to becoming a part of the peer group-one of the important
developmental tasks of the early childhood years.
8.3.1 Physical Development
Toddlerhood is one of the two periods of rapid growth during the life span; the other
comes at puberty. During the first six months of life, growth continues at the rapid rate
characteristic of the prenatal period and then begins to slow down. In the second year, the rate
of growth decelerates rapidly. During the first year of life, the increase in weight is
proportionally greater than the increase in height; during the second year, the reverse is true.
If the rapid growth characteristic of the prenatal and early postnatal periods did not
decelerate soon after birth, the child would grow into a giant. It has been estimated that if
weight increased at the same rate it did during the first year of life, a child who weighed
seven pounds at birth would weigh 230,029 pounds at eleven years of age.
While the general pattern of growth and development is similar for all babies, there are
variations in height, weight, sensory capacities, and other areas of physical development.
Some babies start life smaller and less well developed than the norm. This may be due to
prematurity or to a poor physical condition resulting from maternal malnutrition, stress, or
some other unfavorable condition during the prenatal period. As a result, such babies tend to
fall behind their age-mates during the toddlerhood years.
The pattern of physical growth in toddlerhood is much the same for boys and girls.
However, within the sex groups there are marked variations. Throughout the first year of life,
there is little difference in height and weight between black and white babies of comparable
economic levels. Differences begin to appear in the second year however, because black
children are, typically, of a more slender build than white children.
There are also variations in body size of babies of different socioeconomic levels.
Babies whose parents are of the lower socioeconomic levels tend to be smaller, in both
weight and height, than those whose parents come from the higher socioeconomic levels.
Body build, which begins to be apparent during the second year of life, also contributes to
variations in height and weight.
Throughout the toddlerhood period variations n o t only continue but become more
pronounced. At all times variations in weight are greater than variations in height. This is
because variations in weight are dependent partly on body build and partly on eating habits
and diets. However, in spite of variations in physical growth and development, it is possible
to get a general picture of the pattern of growth and development during the toddlerhood
years.
8.3.2 Pattern of Physical Development during Toddlerhood
Weight: At the age of four months, the baby's weight has normally doubled. At one year,
babies weigh, on the average, three times as much as they did at birth, or approximately 21
pounds. At the age of two, the typical baby weighs 25 pounds. Increase in weight during
toddlerhood comes mainly from an increase in fat tissue.
Height: At four months, the baby measures between 23 and 24 inches; at one year, between
28 and 30 inches; and at two years, between 32 and 34 inches.
Physical Proportions: Head growth slows down in toddlerhood, while trunk and limb
growth increases. Thus the baby gradually becomes less top-heavy and appears more slender
and less chunky by the end of toddlerhood.
Bones: The number of bones increases during toddlerhood. Ossification begins in the early
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
101
part of the first year, but is not completed until puberty. The fontanel, or soft spot on the
skull, has closed in approximately 50 percent of all babies by the age of eighteen months, and
in almost all babies by the age of two years.
Muscles and Fat: Muscle fibers are present at birth but in very undeveloped forms. They
grow slowly during toddlerhood and are weak. By contrast, fat tissue develops rapidly during
toddlerhood, due partly to the high fat content of milk, the main ingredient in a baby's diet.
Body Builds: During the second year of life, as body proportions change, babies begin to
show tendencies toward characteristic body builds. The three most common forms of body
build are ectomorphic, which tends to be long and slender, endomorphic, which tends to be
round and fat, and mesomorphic which tends to be heavy, hard, and rectangular.
Teeth: The average baby has four to six of the twenty temporary teeth by the age of one and
sixteen by the age of two. The first teeth to cut through are those in the front, the last to
appear are the molars. The last four of the temporary teeth usually erupt during the first year
of early childhood.
Nervous System: At birth, brain weight is one-eighth of the baby's total weight. Gain in
brain weight is greatest during the first two years of life, thus accounting for the baby's topheavy
appearance. The cerebellum, which plays an important role in body balance and
postural control, triples in weight during the first year of postnatal life. This is true also for
the cerebrum. Immature cells, present -at birth, continue to develop after birth but relatively
few new cells are formed.
Sense Organ Development: By the age of three months, the eye muscles are well-enough
coordinated to enable babies to see things clearly and distinctly and the cones are wellenough
developed to enable them to see colors. Hearing develops rapidly during this time.
Smell and taste, which are well developed at birth, continue to improve during toddlerhood.
Babies are highly responsive to all skin stimuli because of the thin texture of their skin and
because all sense organs relating to touch, pressure, pain, and temperature are present in welldeveloped
forms.
8.3.3 Physiological Functions
Toddlerhood is the time when the fundamental physiological patterns of eating,
sleeping, and elimination should be established, even though the habit formation may not be
completed when toddlerhood ends.
Sleep Patterns: During the first year of toddlerhood, the mean duration of night sleep
increases from 8 1/2 hours at three weeks to 10 hours at twelve weeks and then remains
constant during the rest of that year. During the first three months, the decline in day sleep is
balanced by an increase in night sleep. Throughout the first year, wakefulness-sleep cycles of
approximately one hour in length occur in both day and night sleeps, with deep sleep lasting
only about twenty-three minutes.
Eating Patterns: From birth until four or five months of age, all eating is in the infantile form
of sucking and swallowing. Food, as a result, must be in a liquid form. Chewing generally
appears in the developmental pattern a month later than biting. But, like biting, it is in an
infantile form and requires much practice before it becomes serviceable.
Food dislikes, which begin to develop during the second year, are frequently the result of
the prolongation of infantile eating patterns. After being accustomed to food in liquid form, it
is difficult for babies to adjust to a semisolid form. This adds to their revolt against the food,
even though they may like its taste.
Patterns of Elimination: Bowel control begins, on the average, at six months, and bladder
control begins between the ages of fifteen and sixteen months. In the case of the former,
habits of control are established by the end of toddlerhood, though temporary lapses may be
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
102
expected when the baby is tired, ill, or emotionally excited. Bladder control, on the other
hand, is in a rudimentary state at the close of toddlerhood. Dryness during the daytime can be
expected for a major part of the time except when deviations from the scheduled routine of
the day, illness, fatigue, or emotional tension interfere. Dryness at night cannot be achieved in
the average child until several years later.
8.3.4 Muscle Control
At first, the baby's body is in more or less constant motion similar to the mass activity of
the newborn infant. This is true even during sleep. Gradually this random, meaningless
movement becomes more coordinated, thus making control of the muscles possible.
Maturation and learning work together in the development of muscle control. As a result
of the maturation of the muscles, bones, and nerve structures, and because of the change that
takes place in body proportions, babies are able to use their bodies in a coordinated manner.
They must, however, be given an opportunity to learn how to do so. Until this state of
readiness is present, teaching will be of little or no value.
Development of control over the muscles follows a definite and predictable pattern
governed by the laws of developmental direction. According to these laws, muscle control
sweeps over the body from head to foot and from trunk to extremities. This means that the
muscles in the head region come under voluntary control first, and those in the leg region last.
Babies who sit early walk earlier than babies who start to sit later. It is possible to
predict with a fair degree of accuracy when babies will start to walk if one knows what their
rate of development is in other motor coordinations. A fairly accurate way to predict the age
at which babies will start to walk alone is to multiply the age at which they begin to creep by
1 1/2 or the age at which they sit alone by 2.
8.3.5 Toddlerhood Skills
On the foundations laid through maturation of muscle coordinations, babies begin,
before the end of the first year of life, to develop skills-fine coordinations in which the
smaller muscles play a major role. To develop skills, however, there are three essentials: an
opportunity for practice, an incentive to learn, and a good model to copy with guidance to
ensure that the copying will be correct. How important imitating a model is has been shown
by the fact that, in babies blind from birth, there is a delay in their gross motor development
and in the acquisition of skills.
Before toddlerhood is over, babies acquire many skills that are useful to them in their
daily activities. At first, they are unable to integrate the different parts of a skill, with the
result that the skill is of little value to them. With practice integration takes place. None of
these skills will be well learned in the relatively short span of toddlerhood, but they serve as
the foundation of skills that will be refined and more completely learned as babies emerge
into the childhood years. The skills that all babies can be expected to learn are usually
divided into two major categories hand skills and leg skills.
Because there is a rapid increase in the use of the hands during the early weeks of life,
hand coordinations develop rapidly. As each new hand skill develops, it absorbs babies' interests
and activities, and they devote much of their waking time to the use of their hands. This
further increases their control over them. By contrast, because the major part of toddlerhood
is devoted to developing the ability to walk, leg skills are only in a rudimentary state of
development by the end of this period. The new leg skills acquired during toddlerhood are
learned mainly during the last part of the second year.
8.3.6 Beginning of Handedness
Learning to use one hand in preference to the other-handedness -is an important
aspect of the development of hand skills during toddlerhood. During the early months of life,
a baby is ambidextrous, with no preference for either hand. By the time they are eight months
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
103
old, babies who are above average in mental and motor development show a greater degree of
hand preference than those who are less well advanced, and this preference is usually for the
right hand. However, most babies shift from the use of one hand to the other, depending
largely on the position of the person or object they want to reach. If the object is closer to the
right hand, that is the hand the baby will use.
Shifting likewise occurs during the second year but not as frequently as during the
first. Thus, during toddlerhood, babies are neither dominantly left- nor dominantly righthanded,
though they show, especially in the second year, a tendency to use one hand more
than the other.
8.3.7 Developments in Socialization
Early social experiences play a dominant role in determining the baby's future social
relationships and patterns of behavior toward others. And because the baby's life is centered
around the home, it is here that the foundations for later social behavior and attitudes are laid.
There is little evidence that people are inherently social or antisocial. Instead, whether they
become outer- or inner-bound-extroverted or introverted-depends mainly on their early social
experiences.
Studies of the social adjustments of older children and even adolescents show the
importance of the social foundations laid in toddlerhood. There are two reasons for the
importance of these early foundations. First, the type of behavior babies show in social
situations affects their personal and social adjustments. A smiling child is much more likely
to provoke intensive maternal feelings and to become a good partner in the relation between
him and his mother or other care takers and to draw more attention of the adult than others
who smile less. The two- to three-year-olds might establish an attachment for some object-a
favorite toy or a blanket, for example-these '''attachment objects, whether animate or
inanimate, may serve as anxiety reducers. When a preschooler is accompanied by an
attachment object, it reduces anxiety in a novel situation and facilitates adjustment to this
situation".
The second reason why early social foundations are important is that, once established,
they tend to be persistent as children grow older. Children who cried excessively as babies
tend to be aggressive and to show other attention-getting behavior. By contrast, friendly,
happier babies usually become socially better adjusted as they grow older.
8.3.8 Pattern of Development of Social Behavior
Early social behavior follows a fairly predictable pattern, though variations can and do
occur as a result of health or emotional states or because of environmental conditions. At
birth, infants are non gregarious in the sense that it makes no difference to them who attends
to their physical needs. In fact, young babies can be soothed as well by a hot-water bottle or a
soft pillow as by human caresses. But at around the age of six weeks, a true social smile-or a
smile in response to a person rather than to a tactile stimulus applied to the lips, which
produces a reflex smile-appears, and this is regarded as the beginning of socialization.
The pattern of social responses to adults differs from that of social responses to other
babies. The first social responses are to adults, while those to other babies appear slightly
later. During the first year of toddlerhood, babies are in a state of equilibrium which makes
them friendly, easy to handle, and pleasant to be with. Around the middle of the second year,
equilibrium gives way to disequi1ibrium, and babies then become fussy, uncooperative, and
difficult to handle. Before toddlerhood is over, however, equilibrium is restored and babies
again exhibit pleasant and social behavior.
8.3.9 Beginnings of Interest in Play
There are certain characteristics of toddlerhood play which are distinctive and which
make it different from the play of young children and certainly from the play of older
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
104
children and adolescents. First, in toddlerhood play there are no rules or regulations. Consequently,
it may be regarded as free, spontaneous play. Babies play how and when they wish
without any preparation for or restrictions on, the way they play.
Second, throughout toddlerhood, play is more often solitary than social. Even when
playing with the mother, the baby is "often a plaything, while the mothering one is the player.
In time, both the child and the mothering one are mutually players and playthings". When
babies are with other babies or with children, there is little interaction or cooperation. Instead,
play is "onlooker play" in which babies watch what the others are doing, or "parallel play" in
which they play in their own way without regard to what the other is doing. When there is
any interaction, it consists mainly of grabbing or snatching toys from another baby. There is
little or no social give-and-take.
Third, because play is dependent on physical, motor, and intellectual development, the
kinds of play depend on the babies' patterns of development in these areas. As these patterns
unfold, play becomes more varied and complex.
Fourth, toys and other play equipment per se are less important now than they will be
later. This means that babies' play can be carried out with any object that stimulates curiosity
and exploration: regular toys are not needed at this period.
And, fifth, babies' play is characterized by much repetition and little variation. The
reason for this is that babies lack the skills which make the wider play repertoire of
preschoolers and older children possible.
8.3.10 Play Development Follows a Pattern
Play during the toddlerhood years is greatly influenced by the baby's physical, motor, and
mental development. And because these patterns of development are similar for all babies,
the pattern of play is similar and predictable.
For example, at six months babies play with one object at a time. By the time they are
nine months, they combine or relate two separate objects in their play and show an interest in
similarities among objects. By the time they are two years old, they show evidence of
pretending in their play.
Different play patterns likewise follow predictable patterns. This is true of manipulative
play and play with toys. In playing with toys, babies first explore them and later use them to
make things or to supplement their make-believe play. Regardless of environment and of
individual differences, certain patterns of play are found more or less universally.
8.3.11 Value of Play in Toddlerhood
During toddlerhood, play is for pleasure and "not for any end result, it makes important
contributions to .the baby's development. As Bruner has said, "Play is serious business." He
further explains that it provides opportunities for many forms of learning, two of which are
especially important-problem solving and creativity. Without play, the foundations for
creativity as well as the foundations for problem solving would not be laid before children
developed habits of dealing with their environments in noncreative, stereotyped ways.
Play gives to information babies about their environments and the people and things in
their environments. It is through exploration that the infant learns about the world of people
as well as of things. Unquestionably, this learning would ultimately take place without play
but play hastens the learning and adds to its enjoyment.
Play is the enjoyment babies derive from it. Were it not for opportunities for play,
equipment to stimulate it, and guidance in how to use the equipment, babies would become
bored and spend their time crying for attention. The self-confidence that comes with selfsufficiency
helps children to cope with the various problems that face them as they grow
older.
Creativity is only in its most rudimentary forms during toddlerhood, the satisfaction the
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
105
individual derives from opportunities and encouragement to do things in a creative way
provides an incentive to further creativity as the child emerges from the restricted environment
of toddlerhood and has more opportunities to do things in an original way.
Much play in toddlerhood is solitary; some of it is carried out with others, mainly
members of the family-siblings, parents, or grandparents. Learning to play with others
encourages babies to be cooperative instead of self-bound, an essential to good social
relationships when toddlerhood comes to a close. Like other foundations, if foundations in
cooperation are properly laid in toddlerhood, adjustment to the demands of childhood will be
easier to meet successfully.
8.3.12 Development of Understanding
All babies begin life with no comprehension of the meaning of the things they come in
contact with in their environment, they must acquire, through maturation and learning, an
understanding of what they observe. What meanings babies acquire depends partly on the
level of their intelligence and partly on their previous experiences. As new meanings are acquired,
babies interpret new experiences in terms of their memories of previous ones. The
association of meanings with objects, people, and situations results in the development of
concepts.
Normal babies show, that concepts develop rapidly. Babies show recognition of familiar
people and objects in their environment through pleasurable responses, just as they regard
strange people and objects with fear. Conditioning of the emotions is so easy and so common
during toddlerhood. Watson conditioned a baby to fear a rabbit by associating a loud, harsh
noise with the rabbit. Later, Watson reported that the baby showed fear of white stuffed animals,
a white muff, and even a person in a Santa Claus costume with a flowing white beard.
Check Your Progress 2
Explain the significant developmental tasks of toddlerhood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
8.4 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
vi) The three theories of achievement-language
vii) As to how toddlers get ready to talk
viii) Tasks that toddlers had to overcome
ix) The significance of socialization and play in development
8.5 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. A. Your answer may include the following three theories
i) The Behaviorist Perspective
ii) The Nativist Perspective
iii) The Interactionist Perspective
A. The steps babies take to talk fluently are:
i) Getting Ready to Talk
ii) First Words
iii) The Two-Word Utterance Phase
2. The developmental tasks could include
i) Pattern of Physical Development
ii) Physiological Functions
iii) Muscle Control
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
106
iv) Toddlerhood Skills
v) Beginning of Handedness
vi) Developments in Socialization
vii) Pattern of Development of Social Behavior
viii) Beginnings of Interest in Play
8.6 Lesson – End Activities
1. Discuss the pattern of physical development during Toddlerhood.
2. Briefly explain Toddlerhood skills.
8.7 References
1. Erickson, E., Childhood and Society, New York: Norton, 1968, P. 263.
2. Piaget, J., The Oriigns of Intelligence in Childre, New York: International University
Press, 1982.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
107
LESSON - 9
ERIKSON'S THEORY OF INFANT AND TODDLER PERSONALITY -
EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT - TEMPERAMENT AND DEVELOPMENT -
DEVELOPMENT OF ATTACHMENT
Contents
9.0 Aims and Objectives
9.1 Introduction
9.1.1 Basic Trust versus Mistrust
9.1.2 Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt
9.2 Emotional Development
9.2.1 Development of Some Basic Emotions
9.2.2 Understanding and Responding to the Emotions of Others
9.2.3 Emergence of Self-Conscious Emotions
9.2.4 Beginnings of Emotional Self-Regulation
9.3 Temperament and Development
9.3.1 The Structure of Temperament
9.3.2 Measuring Temperament
9.3.3 Stability of Temperament
9.3.4 Genetic Influences
9.3.5 Environmental Influences
9.3.6 Temperament and Child Rearing: The Goodness-of-Fit Model
9.4 Development of Attachment
9.4.1 Ethological Theory of Attachment
9.4.2 Measuring the Security of Attachment
9.4.3 Stability of Attachment and Cultural Variations
9.4.4 Factors That Affect Attachment Security
9.4.5 Multiple Attachments
9.4.6 Attachment and Later Development
9.5 Let Us Sum Up
9.6 Check your Progress
9.7 Lesson – End Activities
9.8 References
9.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help understand the toddler personality, emotional, temperamental and
attachment development.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
xi) State the Erikson’s theory of infant and toddler personality
xii) Mention the emotional development in infant and toddlers
xiii) Discuss the temperament development
xiv) Development of Attachment in infant and toddlers
9.1 INTRODUCTION
Erikson divided human development into eight stages ( Discussed in Unit I - Lesson
2) and said that the individual has a psychosocial task to master during each stage. The
confrontation with each task during each stage is mastered, a positive quality is built into the
personality and further development takes place. If the task is not mastered, and the conflict
is unsatisfactorily resolved, the ego is damaged because a negative quality is incorporated in
it. The overall task of the individual is to acquire a positive identity as he or she moves from
one stage to the next. The following are with regard to the Infancy (Basic trust Vs Mistrust)
and Toddlerhood (Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt).
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
108
9.1.1 Basic Trust versus Mistrust
Freud called the first year the oral stage and regarded gratification of the infant's need for
food and oral stimulation vital. Erikson accepted Freud's emphasis on feeding; but he
expanded and enriched Freud's view. A healthy outcome during infancy, Erikson believed,
does not depend on the amount of food or oral stimulation offered but rather on the quality of
the caregiver's behavior. A mother who supports her baby’s development relieves discomfort
promptly and sensitively. For example, she holds the infant gently during feedings, patiently
waits until the baby has had enough milk, and weans when infant shows less interest in the
breast or bottle.
Erikson recognized that no parent can be perfectly in tune with the baby's needs. Many
factors affect her responsiveness - feelings of personal happiness, current life conditions (for
example, additional young children in the family), and culturally valued child-rearing
practices. But when the balance of care is sympathetic and loving, the psychological conflict
of the year-basic trust versus mistrust-is resolved on the positive side. The trusting infant
expects the world to be good and gratifying, so he feels confident about venturing out and
exploring it. The mistrustful baby cannot count on the kindness and compassion of others, so
she protects herself by withdrawing from people and things around her.
9.1.2 Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt
In the second year, during Freud's anal stage, instinctual energies shift to the anal region
of the body. Freud viewed toilet training, in which children must bring their anal impulses in
line with social requirements, as crucial for personality development. Erikson agreed that the
parent's manner of toilet training is essential for psychological health. But he viewed it as
only one of many important experiences for newly walking, talking toddlers. Their familiar
refrains-"No!" and "Do it Myself” -reveal that they have entered a period of budding
selfhood. Toddlers want to decide for themselves-not just in toileting but in other situations
as well. The great conflict of toddlerhood, autonomy versus shame and doubt, is resolved
favorably when parents provide young children with suitable guidance a n d r e a s o n a ble
choices. A self-confident, secure 2 year old has been encouraged not just to use the toilet but
to eat .with a spoon and to help pick up his toys. His parents do not criticize or attack him
when he fails at these new skills. And they meet his assertions of independence with
tolerance and understanding. For example, they grant him an extra 5 minutes to finish his
play before leaving for the grocery store and wait patiently while he tries to zip his jacket.
According to Erikson, the parent who is over- or under controlling in toileting is likely to
be so in other aspects of the toddler's life. The outcome is a child who feels forced and
ashamed and doubts his ability to control his impulses and act competently on his own. In
sum, basic trust and autonomy grow out of warm, sensitive parenting and reasonable
expectations for impulse control starting in the second year.
Check Your Progress 1
Explain the Erikson’s theory of infant and toddler personality
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
...............................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
......
9.2 EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
Emotions play a powerful role in organizing the attainments that Erikson regarded as
so important: social relationships, exploration of the environment, and discovery of the self.
Because infants cannot describe their feelings, determining exactly which emotions they are
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
109
experiencing is a challenge. Although vocalizations and body movements provide some information,
facial expressions offer the most reliable cues. Emotional expressions are built-in
social signals, has inspired researchers to analyze infants' facial patterns to determine the
range of emotions they display at different ages.
9.2.1 Development of Some Basic Emotions
Basic emotions are universal in humans and other primates, have a long evolutionary
history of promoting survival, and can be directly inferred from facial expressions. They include
happiness, interest, surprise, fear, anger, sadness, and disgust. Do infants come into the
world with the ability to express basic emotions? Although signs of some emotions are
present, babies' earliest emotional life consists of little more than two global arousal states:
attraction to pleasant stimulation and withdrawal from unpleasant stimulation. Over time,
emotions become clear, well-organized signals.
Around 6 months, face, voice, and posture form organized patterns that vary meaningfully
with environmental events. For example, Suja typically responded to her parents' playful interaction
with a joyful face, pleasant cooing, and a relaxed posture, as if to say, "This is fun!"
In contrast, an unresponsive parent often evokes a sad face, fussy vocalizations, and a
drooping body (sending the message, "I'm despondent") or an angry face, crying, and "pick -
me- up" gestures (as if to say, "Change this unpleasant event!") . If parental depressive
signals continue, they can profoundly disrupt emotional and social development. In sum, by
the middle of the first year, emotional expressions are well organized and specific- and
therefore tell us a great deal about the infant's internal state (Table 1). Three basic emotionshappiness,
anger, and fear-have received the most research attention.
Table 1
Milestones of Emotional Development During the First Two Years
Approximate
Age
Milestone
Birth
2-3 months
3-4 months
6-8 months
8-12 months
18-24 months
Infants' emotions consist largely of two global arousal states: attraction to
pleasant stimulation and withdrawal from unpleasant stimulation.
Infants engage in social smiling and respond in kind to adults' facial
expressions.
Infants begin to laugh at very active stimuli.
Expressions of basic emotions are well organized and vary meaningfully
with environmental events. Infants start to become angry more often and in
a wider range of situations. Fear, especially stranger anxiety, begins to rise.
Attachment to familiar caregivers is clearly evident, and separation anxiety
appears. Infants use familiar caregivers as a secure base for exploration.
Infants perceive facial expressions as organized patterns, and meaningful
understanding of them improves. Social referencing appears. Infants laugh
at subtle elements of surprise.
Self-conscious emotions of shame, embarrassment, guilt, and pride appear.
A vocabulary for talking about feelings develops rapidly, and emotional
self-regulation improves. Toddlers appreciate that others' emotional
reactions may differ from their own. First signs of empathy appear.
Happiness. Happiness-first in terms of blissful smiles later through exuberant laughtercontributes
to many aspects of development. Infants smile and laugh when they achieve new
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
110
skills, expressing their delight in motor and cognitive mastery. The smile also encourages
caregivers to be affectionate and stimulating, so the baby smiles even more. Happiness binds
parent and baby into a warm, supportive relationship and fosters the infant's developing
competencies.
During the early weeks, newborn babies smile when full, during sleep, and in response to
gentle touches and sounds, such as stroking the skin, rocking, and the mother's soft, high
pitched voice. By the end of the first month, infants start to smile at interesting sights, but
these must be dynamic and eye catching, such as a bright object jumping suddenly across the
baby’s field of vision. Between 6 and 10 weeks, the human face evokes a broad grin called
the social smile. These changes in smiling parallel the development of infant perceptual
capacities-in particular, babies' increasing sensitivity to visual patterns, including the human
face.
Laughter, which first occurs around 3 to 4 months, reflects faster processing of
information than does smiling. Around the middle of the first year, infants smile and laugh
more when interacting with familiar people, a preference that strengthens the parent-child
bond. Like adults, 10- to 12-month-olds have several smiles, which vary with context. They
show a broad, "cheek-raised" smile in response to a parent's greeting; a reserved, muted smile
in response to a friendly stranger; and a "mouth-open" smile during stimulating play.
Anger and Fear. Newborn babies respond with generalized distress to a variety of
unpleasant experiences, including hunger, painful medical procedures, changes in body
temperature, and too much or too little stimulation. From 4 to 6 months into the second year,
angry expressions increase in frequency and intensity. Older babies also show anger in a
wider range of situations-for example, when an object is taken away, the caregiver leaves for
a brief time, or they are put down for a nap.
The most frequent expression of fear is to unfamiliar adults, a response called stranger
anxiety. But if the adult sits still while the baby moves around and a parent is nearby, infants
often show positive and curious behavior. The stranger's style of interaction-expressing
warmth, holding out an attractive toy, playing a familiar game, and approaching slowly rather
than abruptly-reduces the baby's fear.
9.2.2 Understanding and Responding to the Emotions of Others
Infants' emotional expressions are closely tied to their ability to interpret the emotional
cues of others. Babies match the feeling tone of the caregiver in face-to-face communication.
Early on, infants detect others' emotions through a fairly automatic process of emotional
contagion, just as we tend to feel happy or sad when we sense these emotions in others.
Between 7 and 10 months, infants perceive facial expressions as organized patterns, and
they can match the emotion in a voice with the appropriate face of a speaking person.
Responding to emotional expressions as organized wholes indicates that these signals have
become meaningful to babies. As skill at detecting what others are looking at and reacting to
improves, infants realize that an emotional expression not only has meaning but is also a
meaningful reaction to a specific object or event.
Once these understandings are in place, infants engage in social referencing, in which
they actively seek emotional information from a trusted person in an uncertain situation.
Many studies show that the caregiver's emotional expression (happy, angry, or fearful)
influences whether a 1-year-old will be wary of strangers, play with an unfamiliar toy, or
cross the deep side of the visual cliff. Social referencing gives infants and toddlers a powerful
means for learning. By responding to caregivers' emotional messages, they can avoid harmful
situations.
9.2.3 Emergence of Self-Conscious Emotions
Besides basic emotions, humans are capable of a second, higher-order set of feelings,
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
111
including shame, embarrassment, guilt, envy, and pride. These are called self-conscious
emotions because each involves injury to or enhancement of our sense of self. For example,
when we are ashamed or embarrassed, we feel negatively about our behavior, and we want to
retreat so others will no longer notice our failings. In contrast, pride reflects delight in the
self's achievements, and we are inclined to tell others what we have accomplished.
Self-conscious emotions appear in the middle of the second year, as the sense of self
emerges. Shame and embarrassment can be seen as 18- to 24-month-olds lower their eyes,
hang their heads, and hide their faces with their hands. Guilt like reactions is also evident.
Besides self-awareness, self-conscious emotions require an additional ingredient: adult
instruction in when to feel proud, ashamed, or guilty. Parents begin this tutoring early when
they say, "My, look at how far you can throw that ball!" or, "You should feel ashamed for
grabbing that toy!".
9.2.4 Beginnings of Emotional Self-Regulation
Besides expressing a wider range of emotions, infants and toddlers begin to manage their
emotional experiences. Emotional self-regulation refers to the strategies we use to adjust
our emotional state to a comfortable level of intensity so we can accomplish our goals. If you
drank a cup of coffee to wake up this morning, reminded yourself that an anxiety-provoking
event would be over soon, or decided not to see a scary horror film, you were engaging in
emotional self-regulation. A good start regulating emotion during the first 2 years contributes
greatly to autonomy and mastery of cognitive and social skills.
In the early months of life, infants have only a limited capacity to regulate their emotional
states. Although they can turn away from unpleasant stimulation and can mouth and suck
when their feelings get too intense, they are easily overwhelmed. They depend on the
soothing interventions of caregivers-lifting the distressed baby to the shoulder, rocking, and
talking softly.
As caregivers help infants regulate their emotions, they contribute to the child's style of
self-regulation. Parents who read and respond sympathetically to the baby's emotional cues
have infants who are less fussy, more easily soothed, and more interested in exploration. In
contrast, parents who wait to intervene until the infant has become extremely agitated reinforce
the baby's rapid rise to intense distress. When caregivers fail to regulate stressful
experiences for babies who cannot yet regulate them for themselves, brain structures that
buffer stress may fail to develop properly, resulting in an anxious, reactive temperament.
In the second year, growth in representation and language leads to new ways of regulating
emotions. A vocabulary for talking about feelings, such as "happy;' "love;' "surprised;'
"scary;' "yucky;' and "mad;' develops rapidly after 18 months. Children of this age are not yet
good at using language to comfort themselves. But once they can describe their internal
states, they can guide caregivers to helping them.
Check Your Progress 2
Mention how emotional development plays a powerful role among infants and toddlers
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
9.3 TEMPERAMENT AND DEVELOPMENT
When we describe one person as cheerful and "upbeat,” another as active and energetic,
and still others as calm, cautious, or prone to angry outbursts, we are referring to
temperament - stable individual differences in quality and intensity of emotional reaction,
activity level, attention, and emotional self-regulation. The temperamental differences among
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
112
children are a great concern because the psychological traits that make up temperament are
believed to form the cornerstone of the adult personality.
As temperament increases, the chances that a child will experience psychological
problems or alternatively, be protected from the effects of a highly stressful home life. It was
found that, parenting practices can modify children's emotional styles considerably.
9.3.1 The Structure of Temperament
Thomas and Chess's nine dimensions, listed in Table 2, served as the first influential
model of temperament and inspired all others that followed. When detailed descriptions of
infants' and children's behavior obtained from parental interviews were rated on these
dimensions, certain characteristics clustered together, yielding three types of children:
Table 2 - Two Models of Temperament
Thomas and Chess Rothbart
Dimension
Activity level
Rhythmicity
Distractibility
Approach/
withdrawal
Adaptability
Attention span
and persistence
Intensity of
reaction
Threshold of
responsiveness
Quality of mood
Description
Ratio of active periods to inactive ones
Regularity of body functions, such as
sleep, wakefulness, hunger, and
excretion
Degree to which stimulation from the
environment alters behavior-for
example, whether crying stops when a
toy is offered
Response to a new object, food, or
person
Ease with which child adapts to changes
in the environment, such as sleeping or
eating in a new place
Amount of time devoted to an activity,
such as watching a mobile or playing
with a toy
Energy level of response, such as
laughing, crying, talking, or gross motor
activity
Intensity of stimulation required to
evoke a response
Amount of friendly, joyful behavior as
opposed to unpleasant, unfriendly
behavior
Dimension
Activity level
Soothability
Attention span/
persistence
Fearful distress
Irritable distress
Positive affect
Description
Level of gross motor activity
Reduction of fussing, crying, and
distress in response to caregiver's
soothing
Duration of orienting or interest
Wariness and distress in response to
intense or novel stimuli, including
time to adjust to new situations
Extent of fussing, crying, and
distress when desires are frustrated
Frequency of expression of
happiness and pleasure
a. The easy child quickly establishes regular routines in infancy, is generally cheerful,
and adapts easily to new experiences.
b. The difficult child is irregular in daily routines, is slow to accept new experiences,
and tends to react negatively and intensely.
c. The slow-to-warm-up child is inactive, shows mild, low-key reactions to environmental
stimuli, is negative in mood, and adjusts slowly to new experiences.
Of the three types, the difficult pattern has sparked the most interest, since it places
children at high risk for adjustment problems-both anxious withdrawal and aggressive behavior
in early and middle childhood. Compared with difficult children, slow-to-warm-up
children do not present many problems in the early years. However, they tend to show excessive
fearfulness and slow, constricted behavior in the late preschool and school years,
when they are expected to respond actively and quickly in classrooms and peer groups.
A second model of temperament, devised by Mary Rothbart, is also shown in Table.2. It
combines overlapping dimensions of Thomas and Chess. For example, "distractibility" and
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
113
"attention span and persistence”, considered opposite ends of the same dimension, are called
"attention span/persistence.” It also includes a dimension not identified by Thomas and
Chess-"irritable distress"-that taps emotional self-regulation. And it omits overly broad dimensions,
such as "Rhythmicity:' "intensity of reaction” and "threshold of responsiveness". A
child who is rhythmic in sleeping is not necessarily rhythmic in eating or bowel habits. And a
child who smiles and laughs quickly and intensely is not necessarily quick and intense in fear
or irritability.
9.3.2 Measuring Temperament
Temperament is often assessed through interviews or questionnaires given to parents.
Behavior ratings by pediatricians, teachers, and others familiar with the child and direct
observations by researchers have also been used. Parental reports have been emphasized
because of their convenience and parents' depth of knowledge about the child. At the same
time, information from parents has been criticized as being biased and subjective.
Nevertheless, parent ratings are moderately related to observations of children's behavior.
And parent perceptions are useful for understanding the way parents view and respond to
their child.
To explore the biological basis of temperament, physiological measures are used. Most
efforts have focused on inhibited, or shy, children, who react negatively to and withdraw
from novel stimuli, and uninhibited, or sociable, children, who react positively to and
approach novel stimuli. Heart rate, hormone levels, and electrical brain-wave recordings in
the frontal region of the cerebral cortex differentiate children with inhibited and uninhibited
temperaments.
9.3.3 Stability of Temperament
Even though there is long-term stability of temperament. It would be difficult to claim
that temperament really exists if children's emotional styles were not stable over time. Infants
and young children who score low or high on attention span, irritability, sociability, or
shyness are likely to respond similarly when assessed again a few years later and,
occasionally, even into the adult years.
When the evidence as a whole is examined carefully, however, temperamental stability
from one age period to the next is generally low to moderate. Although quite a few children
remain the same, a good number have changed when assessed again. In fact, some
characteristics, such as shyness and sociability, are stable over the long term only in children
at the extremes-those who are very inhibited or very outgoing to begin with.
A major reason as to why temperament is not more stable is that temperament itself
develops with age. The early months are a period of irritability and activity level, fussing and
crying for most babies. As infants can better regulate their attention and emotions, many who
initially seemed irritable become calm and content. At first, an active, wriggling infant tends
to be highly aroused and uncomfortable, whereas an inactive baby is often alert and attentive.
As infants begin to move on their own, the reverse is so! An active crawler is usually alert
and interested in exploration, whereas a very inactive baby might be fearful and withdrawn.
The changes shown by many children suggest that experience can modify biologically
based temperamental traits (although children rarely change from one extreme to another-that
is, a shy toddler practically never becomes highly sociable).
9.3.4 Genetic Influences
The word temperament implies a genetic foundation for individual differences in
personality. It is known that identical twins are more similar than fraternal twins across a
wide range of temperamental and personality traits. Heritability estimates suggest a moderate
role for heredity in temperament and personality: About half of the individual differences can
be traced to differences in genetic makeup.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
114
Consistent ethnic and sex differences in early temperament exist, again implying a role
for heredity. Asian babies tend to be less active, less irritable, less vocal, more easily soothed
when upset, and better at quieting themselves. From an early age, boys tend to be more active
and daring and girls more anxious and timid-a difference reflected in boys' higher injury rates
throughout childhood and adolescence.
9.3.5 Environmental Influences
Heredity and environment often combine to strengthen the stability of temperament, since
a child's approach to the world affects the experiences to which she is exposed. Japanese
mothers usually say that babies come into the world as independent beings that must learn to
rely on their mothers through close physical contact. North American mothers are likely to
believe just the opposite-that they must wean babies away from dependence into autonomy.
Asian mothers interact gently, soothingly, and gesturally and discourage strong emotion in
their babies, whereas Caucasian mothers use a more active, stimulating, verbal approach.
These behaviors enhance cultural differences in temperament.
A similar process seems to contribute to sex differences in temperament. Within the first
24 hours after birth (before they have had much experience with the baby), parents already
perceive boys and girls differently. Sons are rated as larger, better coordinated, more alert,
and stronger. Daughters are viewed as softer, more awkward, weaker, and more delicate.
Gender-stereotyped beliefs carryover into the way parents treat their infants and toddlers.
Parents more often encourage sons to be physically active and daughters to seek help and
physical closeness. These practices promote temperamental differences between boys and
girls.
In families with several children, an additional influence on temperament is at work.
Parents often look for and emphasize personality differences in their children. This is
reflected in the comparisons parents make: "She's a lot more active," or "He's more sociable”.
Each child, in turn, evokes responses from caregivers that are consistent with parental views
and with the child's actual temperamental style.
9.3.6 Temperament and Child Rearing: The Goodness-of-Fit Model
It is a known fact that the temperaments of many children change with age. This suggests
that environments do not always sustain or intensify a child's existing temperament. If a
child's disposition interferes with learning or getting along with others, adults must gently but
consistently counteract the child's maladaptive behavior.
Thomas and Chess (1977) proposed a goodness-of-fit model to explain how
temperament and environment can together produce favorable outcomes. Goodness of fit
involves creating child-rearing environments that recognize each child's temperament while
encouraging more adaptive functioning.
Goodness of fit helps explain why difficult children (who withdraw from new
experiences and react negatively and intensely) are at high risk for later adjustment problems.
These children, at least in Western middle-SES families, frequently experience parenting that
fits poorly with their dispositions. As infants, they are far less likely to receive sensitive
caregiving. By the second year, parents of difficult children tend to resort to angry, punitive
discipline, and the child reacts with defiance and disobedience. Then parents often behave
inconsistently, rewarding the child's noncompliance by giving in to it, although they initially
resisted. These practices maintain and even increase the child's irritable, conflict-ridden style.
In contrast, when parents are positive and involved and engage in the sensitive, face-to face
play that helps infants regulate emotion, difficultness declines by age 2.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
115
Check Your Progress 3
State the significant the role of temperament in a child’s life
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
9.4 DEVELOPMENT OF ATTACHMENT
Attachment is the strong, affectional tie we have with special people in our lives that
leads us to experience pleasure and joy when we interact with them and to be comforted by
their nearness during times of stress. By the second half of the first year, infants have become
attached to familiar people who have responded to their needs. Watch babies of this age, and
notice how they single out their parents for special attention. For example, when the mother
enters the room, the baby breaks into a broad, friendly smile. When she picks him up, he pats
her face, explores her hair, and snuggles against her. When he feels anxious or afraid, he
crawls into her lap and clings closely.
Freud first suggested that the infant's emotional tie to the mother is the foundation for all
later relationships. Erikson's theory states how the psychoanalytic perspective regards feeding
acts as the primary context in which caregivers and babies build their close emotional bond.
Behaviorism, too, emphasizes the importance of feeding, but for different reasons. According
to a well-known behaviorist account, as the mother satisfies the baby's hunger, infants learn
to prefer her soft caresses, warm smiles, and tender words of comfort because these events
have been paired with tension relief.
Although feeding is an important context for building a close relationship, attachment
does not depend on hunger satisfaction. In the 1950s, a famous experiment showed that rhesus
monkeys reared with terry-cloth and wire-mesh "surrogate mothers" clung to the soft
terry-cloth substitute, even though the wire-mesh "mother" held the bottle and infants had to
climb on it to be fed. Similarly, human infants become attached to family members who seldom
feed them, including fathers, siblings, and grandparents.
9.4.1 Ethological Theory of Attachment
It recognizes the infant's emotional tie to the caregiver as an evolved response that
promotes survival, is the most widely accepted view. Contact with the parent also ensures
that the baby will be fed, but John Bowlby (1969) pointed out that feeding is not the basis for
attachment. Instead, the attachment bond has strong biological roots.
According to Bowlby, the infant's relationship with the parent begins as a set of innate
signals that call the adult to the baby's side. Over time, a true affectional bond develops,
which is supported by new cognitive and emotional capacities as well as by a history of
warm, sensitive care. Attachment develops in four phases:
1. The preattachment phase (birth to 6 weeks). Built-in signals-grasping, smiling, crying,
and gazing into the adult's eyes - help bring newborn babies into close contact with other
humans. Once an adult responds, infants encourage her to remain nearby because closeness
comforts them. Babies of this age recognize their own mother's smell and voice. But they are
not yet attached to her since they do not mind being left with an unfamiliar adult.
2. The "attachment-in-the-making" phase (6 weeks to 8 months). During this phase, infants
respond differently to a familiar caregiver than to a stranger. As infants learn that their own
actions affect the behavior of those around them, they begin to develop a sense of trust- the
expectation that the caregiver will respond when signaled. But even though they recognize
the parent, babies still do not protest when separated from her.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
116
3. The phase of "clear-cut" attachment (6-8 months to 18 months-2 years). Now attachment
to the familiar caregiver is clearly evident. Babies display separation anxiety, becoming upset
when the adult whom they have come to rely on leaves. Separation anxiety does not always
occur; like stranger anxiety, it depends on infant temperament and the current situation.
Besides protesting the parent's departure, older infants and toddlers try hard to maintain her
presence. They approach, follow, and climb on her in preference to others. And they use the
familiar caregiver as a secure base or point from -which to explore, venturing into the
environment and then returning for emotional support.
4. Formation of a reciprocal relationship (18 months-2 years and on). By the end of the
second year, rapid growth in representation and language permits toddlers to understand
some of the factors that influence the parent's coming and going and to predict her return. As
a result, separation protest declines. Now children start to negotiate with the caregiver, using
requests and persuasion to alter her goals.
According to Bowlby, out of their experiences during these four phases, children
construct an enduring affectional tie to the caregiver that they can use as a secure base in the
parents' absence. This inner representation becomes a vital part of personality. It serves as an
internal working model, or set of expectations about the availability of attachment figures
and their likelihood of providing support during times of stress. This image becomes the
model, or guide, for all future relationships.
9.4.2 Measuring the Security of Attachment
Although virtually all family-reared babies become attached to a familiar caregiver by the
second year, the quality of this relationship differs from child to child. A widely used
technique for assessing the quality of attachment between 1 and 2 years of age is the Strange
Situation. Mary Ainsworth and her colleagues said t h a t securely attached infants and
toddlers should use the parent as a secure base from whic h t o explore an unfamiliar
playroom. In addition, when the parent leaves, an unfamiliar adult should be less comforting
than the parent.
Observing the responses of infants researchers have identified a secure attachment pattern
and three patterns of insecurity.
Secure attachment. These infants use the parent as a secure base from which to explore.
When separated, they may or may not cry, but if they do, it is because the parent is absent and
they prefer her to the stranger. When the parent returns, they actively seek contact, and their
crying is reduced immediately.
Avoidant attachment These infants seem unresponsive to the parent when she is present.
When she leaves, they are usually not distressed, and they react to the stranger in much the
same way as to the parent. During reunion, they avoid or are slow to greet the parent, and
when picked up, they often fail to cling.
Resistant attachment Before separation, these infants often seek closeness to the parent and
often fail to explore. When she returns, they display angry, resistive behavior, sometimes
hitting and pushing. Many continue to cry after being picked up and cannot be comforted
easily.
Disorganized - disoriented attachment This pattern reflects the greatest insecurity. At
reunion, these infants show a variety of confused, contradictory behaviors. They might look
away while being held by the parent or approach her with flat, depressed emotion. A few cry
out after having calmed down or display odd, frozen postures.
Infants' reactions in the Strange Situation resemble their use of the parent as a secure
base and their response to separation at home. For this reason, the procedure is a powerful
tool for assessing attachment security.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
117
9.4.3 Stability of Attachment and Cultural Variations
Quality of attachment is usually secure and stable for middle-SES babies experiencing
favorable life conditions. And infants who move from insecurity to security typically have
well-adjusted mothers with positive family and friendship ties. Perhaps many became parents
before they were psychologically ready but, with social support, grew into the role. In
contrast, for low-SES families with many daily stresses, attachment usually moves away from
security or changes from one insecure pattern to another.
These indicate that securely attached babies more often maintain their attachment status
than do insecure babies. Cross-cultural evidence indicates that, German infants show
considerably more avoidant attachment than American babies do. Japanese mothers rarely
leave their babies in the care of unfamiliar people, so the Strange Situation probably creates
far greater stress for them than for infants who frequently experience maternal separations.
Also, Japanese parents value the infant clinginess and attention seeking that is part of
resistant attachment, considering them to be normal indicators of infant closeness and
dependence. Despite these cultural variations, the secure pattern is still the most common
pattern of attachment in all societies studied to date.
9.4.4 Factors That Affect Attachment Security
There are four important factors that might influence attachment security: (1) opportunity
to establish a close relationship, (2) quality of caregiving, (3) the baby's characteristics, and
(4) family context.
Opportunity for Attachment When a baby does not have the opportunity to establish an
affectional tie to a caregiver, for example institutionalized infants who had been given up by
their mothers between 3 and 12 months of age. The babies when placed on a large ward
where they shared a nurse with at least seven other babies. In contrast to the happy, outgoing
behavior they had shown before separation, they wept and withdrew from their surroundings,
lost weight, and had difficulty sleeping. If a consistent caregiver did not replace the mother
the depression deepened rapidly.
These institutionalized babies had emotional difficulties because they were prevented
from forming a bond with one or a few adults. When they grow up they were likely to display
emotional and social problems desire for adult attention, over-friendliness to unfamiliar
adults and peers and few friendships.
Quality of Caregiving Sensitive caregiving-responding promptly, consistently and
appropriately to infants and holding them tenderly and carefully - is moderately related to
attachment security in diverse cultures. In contrast, insecurely attached infants tend to have
mothers who engage in less physical contact, handle them awkwardly, behave in a "routine"
manner, and are sometimes negative, resentful, and rejecting.
A special form of communication c a l led interactional synchrony separated the
experiences of secure and insecure babies. It is best described as a sensitively tuned
"emotional dance," in which the caregiver responds to infant signals in a well-timed,
rhythmic, appropriate fashion. In addition, both parents match emotional states, especially the
positive ones.
Infant Characteristics Since attachment is the result of a relationship that builds
between two partners, infant characteristics should affect how easily it is established. In
stressed, poverty-stricken families, prematurity, birth complications, and newborn illness are
linked to attachment insecurity. But when parents have the time and patience to care for a
baby with special needs and view their infants positively, at-risk newborns fare quite well in
attachment security
Babies who are irritable and fearful may simply react to brief separations with intense
anxiety, regardless of the parent's sensitivity to the baby. Consistent with this view,
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
118
emotionally reactive, difficult babies are more likely to develop later insecure attachments.
Family Circumstances Job loss, a failing marriage, financial difficulties, and other
stressors can undermine attachment by interfering with the sensitivity of parental care. Or
they can affect babies' sense of security directly, by exposing them to angry adult interactions
or unfavorable child-care arrangements. The availability of social supports, especially assistance
in caregiving, reduces stress and fosters attachment security.
Parents bring to the family context a long history of attachment experiences, out of which
they construct internal working models that they apply to the bonds established with their
babies. Internal working models are reconstructed memories affected by many factors,
including relationship experiences over the life course, personality, and current life
satisfaction. Longitudinal studies show that negative life events can weaken the link between
an individual's own attachment security in infancy and a secure internal working model in
adulthood. And insecurely attached babies who become adults with insecure internal working
models often have lives that, based on adulthood self-reports, are filled with family crises.
Our early rearing experiences do not destine us to become sensitive or insensitive parents.
Rather, the way we view our childhoods-our ability to come to terms with negative events, to
integrate new information into our working models, and to look back on our own parents in
an understanding, forgiving way-is much more influential in how we rear our children than is
the actual history of care we received.
9.4.5 Multiple Attachments
Babies develop attachments to a variety of familiar people-not just mothers but also fathers,
siblings, grandparents, and professional caregivers. Although Bowlby made room for
multiple attachments in his theory, he believed that infants are predisposed to direct their
attachment behaviors to a single special person, especially when they are distressed.
Fathers Like mothers' sensitive caregiving, fathers' sensitivity predicts secure
attachment-an effect that becomes stronger the more time they spend with their babies. But as
infancy progresses mothers and fathers in many cultures-Australia, India, Israel, Italy, Japan,
and the United States-relate to babies in different ways. Mothers devote more time to
physical care: expressing affection. Fathers spend more time in playful interaction. Mothers
and fathers also play differently with babies. Fathers tend to engage in more exciting, highly
physical bouncing and lifting games, especially with their infant sons. However, this picture
of "mother as caregiver" and "father as playmate" has changed in some families due to the
revised work status of women.
Siblings Despite a declining family size, 80 percent of children still grow up with at least
one sibling. The arrival of a baby brother or sister is a difficult experience for most
preschoolers, who quickly realize that now they must share their parents' attention and
affection. They often become demanding and clingy for a time and engage in deliberate
naughtiness. And their security of attachment typically declines, more so if they are over age
2 (old enough to feel threatened and displaced) and if the mother is under stress due to
marital or psychological problems.
An older child can also be seen kissing, patting, and calling out, "Mom, he needs you;'
when the baby cries-signs of growing affection. By the end of the baby's first year, siblings
typically spend much time together. Infants of this age are comforted by the presence of their
preschool-age brother or sister during short parental absences. And in the second year, they
often imitate and actively join in play with the older child
9.4.6 Attachment and Later Development
According to psychoanalytic and ethological theories, the inner feelings of affection and
security that result from a healthy attachment relationship support all aspects of psychological
development. P reschool teachers viewed children who were securely attached as babies as
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
119
high in self-esteem, socially competent, cooperative, and popular. In contrast, they viewed
avoidantly attached agemates as isolated and disconnected and resistantly attached agemates
as disruptive and difficult.
Secure attachment in infancy causes improved cognitive, emotional, and social
competence in later years. When parents respond sensitively not just in infancy but also
during later years, children are likely to develop favorably. In contrast, parents who react
insensitively for a long time have children who establish lasting patterns of avoidant,
resistive, or disorganized behavior and are at greater risk for academic, emotional, and social
difficulties. At the same time, infants and young children are resilient beings. A child whose
parental caregiving improves or who has compensating, affectionate ties outside the family is
likely to fare well.
Check Your Progress 4
State why attachment is the strong and create affectional tie among infants and toddlers
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
9.5 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Erikson’s Basic Trust versus Mistrust
ii) Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt
iii) Emotional Development
iv) Temperament and Development.
v) Development of Attachment
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
x) Characteristics of Infancy
9.6 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. Your answer may include the discussion of Erikson’s theory
i) Basic Trust versus Mistrust
ii) Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt
2. Emotional development among infant and toddlers are:
i) Development of Some Basic Emotions
ii) Understanding and Responding to the Emotions of Others
iii) Emergence of Self-Conscious Emotions
iv) Beginnings of Emotional Self-Regulation
3. The temperament involving development are:
i) The Structure of Temperament
ii) Measuring Temperament
iii) Stability of Temperament
iv) Genetic Influences
v) Environmental Influences
vi) Temperament and Child Rearing: The Goodness-of-Fit Model
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
120
4. Development of Attachment includes
i) Ethological Theory of Attachment
ii) Measuring the Security of Attachment
iii) Stability of Attachment and Cultural Variations
iv) Factors That Affect Attachment Security
v) Multiple Attachments
vi) Attachment and Later Development
9.7 Lesson – End Activities
1. Briefly discuss about the effect of attachment.
2. “Clear – cut” attachement phase – obser children and write the major changes in the pase.
9.8 References
1. Erickson, E., Childhood and Society, New York: Norton, 1968, p. 263.
2. Erickson, Moral Stages and Moralinration: The Cognitive Development Approach.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
121
LESSON – 10
SELF-DEVELOPMENT - HAZARDS
Contents
10.0 Aims and Objectives
10.1 Introduction
10.1.1 Self-Awareness
10.1.2 Categorizing the Self
10.1.3 Emergence of Self-Control
10.2 Hazards Of Infancy
10.2.1 Physical Hazards
10.2.2 Psychological Hazards
10.3 Hazards In Toddlerhood
10.3.1 Physical Hazards
10.3.2 Psychological Hazards
10.3.3 Social Hazards
10.4 Let Us Sum Up
10.5 Check your Progress
10.6 Lesson – End Activities
10.7 References
1.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help understand the development of self and hazards that are common
during infancy and toddler hood.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
xv) State the emergence of self among infants
xvi) Mention the Self-Awareness and early emotional and social development
xvii) Put forth the emergence of Self-Control
xviii)List the hazards during the infancy and toddler hood
10.1 INTRODUCTION
Infancy is a rich formative period for the development of physical and social
understanding. Infants develop an appreciation of the permanence of objects, over the first
year, infants recognize and respond appropriately to others' emotions and distinguish familiar
from unfamiliar people. That both objects and people achieve an independent, stable
existence for the infant implies that knowledge of the self as a separate, permanent entity
emerges around this time.
10.1.1 Self-Awareness
As early as the first few months, babies smiled and returned friendly behaviors to their
image in the mirror realized that gazing and grinning back was really itself.
Emergence of the I-Self and Me-Self Researchers have exposed infants and toddlers to
images of themselves in mirrors, on videotapes, and in photos. When shown two side-by-side
video images of their kicking legs, one from their own perspective (camera behind the baby)
and one from an observer's perspective (camera in front of the baby), 3-month-olds look
longer at the observer's view. This suggests that within the first few months, infants seem to
have some sense of their own body as a distinct entity, since they have habituated to it, as
indicated by their interest in novel images.
The earliest aspect of the self to emerge is the I – self, or sense of self as agent, involving
awareness that the self is separate from the surrounding world and can control its own
thoughts and actions. According to many theorists, the beginnings of the I -self lie in infants'
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
122
recognition that their own actions cause objects and people to react in predictable ways. In
support of this idea, babies whose parents encourage them to explore and respond to their
signals consistently and sensitively are advanced in constructing a sense of self as agent. For
example, between 1 and 2 years of age, they display more complex self-related actions during
make-believe play, such as making a doll labeled as the self take a drink or kiss a teddy bear.
Smiling and vocalizing at a caregiver who smiles and vocalizes back helps specify the
relation between self and social world. And watching the movements of one's own hand
provides still another kind of feedback-one under much more direct control than other people
or objects. The second aspect of the self is the me-self, a sense of self as an object of
knowledge and evaluation. It consists of all qualities that make the self unique, including
physical characteristics, possessions and (as child gets older) attitudes, beliefs, and
personality traits. During the second year, toddlers start to construct a me-self; they become
aware of the self's features. In one study, 9- to 24-month-olds were placed in front of a
mirror. Then, under the pretext of wiping the baby's face, each mother was asked to rub red
dye on her infant's nose. Younger infants touched the mirror as if the red mark had nothing to
do with them. But by 15 months, toddlers rubbed their strange-looking red noses. They were
keenly aware of their unique appearance. By age 2, almost all children recognize themselves
in photos and use their name or a personal pronoun ("I" or "me") to refer to themselves.
Like the I-self, the me-self seems to be fostered by sensitive caregiving. Securely
attached toddlers display more complex knowledge of their own and their parents' features
than do their insecurely attached agemates.
Self-Awareness and Early Emotional and Social Development Self-awareness quickly
becomes a central part of children's emotional and social lives. Self-conscious emotions
depend on an emerging sense of self. As the self strengthens in the second year, toddlers
show the beginnings of self-conscious behavior: bashfulness and embarrassment. Selfawareness
also leads to the child's first efforts to appreciate another's perspective. It is
accompanied by the first signs of empathy-the ability to understand another's emotional state
and feel with that person, or respond emotionally in a similar way. For example, toddlers start
to give to others what they themselves find comforting-a hug, a reassuring comment, or a
favorite doll or blanket. At the same time, they demonstrate clearer awareness of how to
upset others. One 18-month-old heard her mother comment to another adult, "Amudha
(sibling) is really frightened of spiders. In fact, there's a particular toy spider that we've got
that she just hates". The innocent-looking toddler ran to get the spider out of the toy box,
returned, and pushed it in front of Amudha's face!
10.1.2 Categorizing the Self
When children have a me-self, they use their representational and language capacities to
create a mental image of themselves. One of the first signs of this change is that toddlers
compare themselves to other people. Between 18 and 30 months, children categorize
themselves and others on the basis of age ("baby;' "boy;' or "man"), sex ("boy" versus "girl"),
physical characteristics ("big;' "strong"), and even goodness and badness ("I a good girl."
"Tarun mean!"). Toddlers' understanding of these social categories is limited, but they use
this knowledge to organize their own behavior. For example, children's ability to label their
own gender is associated with a sharp rise in gender-stereotyped responses.
10.1.3 Emergence of Self-Control
Self-awareness provides the foundation for self-control, the capacity to resist an impulse
to engage in socially disapproved behavior. Self-control is essential for morality, another
dimension of the self that will flourish during childhood and adolescence. To behave in a
self-controlled fashion, children must have some ability to think of themselves as separate,
autonomous beings who can direct their own actions. And they must have the
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
123
representational and memory capacities to recall a caregiver's directive and apply it to their
own behavior.
As these capacities emerge, the first glimmerings of self control appear between 12 and 18
months as compliance. Toddlers show clear awareness of caregivers' wishes and expectations
and can obey simple requests and commands. And as every parent knows, they can also
decide, to do just the opposite! One way toddlers assert their autonomy is by resisting adult
directives. But among toddlers who experience warm parenting, opposition is far less
common than compliance with an eager, willing spirit, which suggests that the child is
beginning to adopt the adult's directive as his own. Around 18 months, the capacity for selfcontrol
appears, it improves steadily into early childhood.
As self-control improves, mothers increase the rules they require toddlers to follow, from
safety and respect for property and people to family routines, manners, and simple chores.
Still, toddlers' control over their own actions depends on constant parental oversight and
reminders.
Check Your Progress 1
Explain the significant role of formative period in the self development
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
10.2 HAZARDS OF INFANCY
In spite of its short duration, infancy is one of the most hazardous periods in the life span.
Hazards at this time may be physical, psychological, or both and they can affect both present
and future adjustment. In the case of the plateau in development, the physical adjustments
may take place too slowly, thus threatening the infant's life. Psychologically, this plateau is
hazardous because it may cause parents to become anxious and fearful about the infant's
development, feelings that can persist and lead to over protectiveness in later years.
10.2.1 Physical Hazards
Some of the physical hazards of infancy are of only temporary significance, while others
can affect the individual's entire life pattern. The most serious physical hazards are those
relating to an unfavorable prenatal environment, a difficult and complicated birth, a multiple
birth, postmaturity, and prematurity, and conditions leading to infant mortality.
Unfavorable Prenatal Environment: As a result of unfavorable conditions in the prenatal
environment, the infant may have difficulty adjusting to postnatal life. Excessive smoking on
the part of the mother, for example, can affect the development of the fetus. Prolonged and
intense maternal stress is another important factor, causing the infant to be tense and nervous.
Difficult and Complicated Birth: A difficult or complicated birth frequently results in
temporary or permanent brain damage. If the birth requires the use of instruments, as in the
case where the fetus is so large that it has to be aided in its passage down the birth canal or if
the fetus lies in a foot first or a transverse position, the chances of brain damage from the use
of instruments to aid delivery are always present.
A caesarean section or a precipitate birth, on the other hand, is likely to result in anoxia,
a temporary loss of oxygen to the brain. If anoxia is severe, the brain damage will be far
greater than if anoxia lasts for only a few seconds. The more complicated the birth and the
more damage there is to the brain tissue, the greater the effect on the infant's postnatal life
and adjustments.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
124
Severe and persistent brain damage will have adverse effects on all adjustments during
infancy and often into childhood or even throughout life. The effects of brain damage are
most frequently shown in uncoordinated behavior, hyperactivity, learning difficulties, and
emotional problems.
Multiple Birth Children of multiple births are usually smaller and weaker than singletons as a
result of crowding during the prenatal period, which inhibits fetal movements. These babies
tend to be born prematurely, which adds to their adjustment problems.
Postmaturity: It is hazardous only when the fetus becomes so large that the birth requires the
use of instruments or surgery, in which case the hazards are due to the conditions associated
with birth rather than to postmaturity per se. One study of babies born more than three weeks
after term reported that they experienced neonatal adjustment problems and were also
socially maladjusted and required special schooling by the age of seven.
Prematurity: Prematurity causes -more neonatal deaths than any other condition. Prematurely
born infants are also especially susceptible to brain damage at birth because the skull is not
yet developed enough to protect the brain from pressures experienced during birth. Anoxia is
another common problem since the premature baby's respiratory mechanism is not fully
developed.
The problems of adjustment every newborn infant must face are exaggerated in the
prematures. For example, they require nearly three times as much oxygen as full-term infants
because their breathing is characterized by jerks and gasps. They often have difficulty in
expanding their lungs, and muscular weakness makes breathing difficult.
Because sucking and swallowing reflexes are underdeveloped, the premature infant will
require special feeding with a medicine dropper or tube. The premature's body temperature is
not yet properly controlled, and special equipment is needed to duplicate as nearly as possible
the constant temperature of intrauterine life.
Infant Mortality Unquestionably the most serious of the physical hazards of infancy is infant
mortality. The most critical times for death during the period of infancy are the day of birth
(when two-thirds of all neonatal deaths occur) and the second and third days after birth.
Neonatal deaths have been reported to be most common during the months of June and July
but, to date, no satisfactory explanation for this has been given.
The causes of infant mortality are numerous and varied. Some neonatal deaths are due to
conditions that detrimentally affected the prenatal environment and thus impaired normal
development. Some are the result of difficult and complicated births, such as those requiring
the use of instruments or caesarean section. Some are the result of brain damage, anoxia, or
excessive medication of the mother during labor. And some-but fewer than in the past-are
due to unfavorable conditions in the postnatal environment; a radical temperature change may
cause pneumonia, for example, or a substitute for the mother's milk may cause diarrhea or
other digestive disturbances.
10.2.2 Psychological Hazards
Even though psychological hazards tend to have less effect on the infant's adjustment to
postnatal life than physical hazards, they are nonetheless important because of their long-term
effects. Psychological scars acquired during infancy can cause the individual lifelong
adjustment problems.
Relatively few of the potential psychological hazards of infancy have received more
attention.
Traditional Beliefs about Birth Difficult births, for example, are believed to result in
"difficult children" -those who are hard to handle and whose behavior tends to deviate from
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
125
that of children born with a minimum of difficulty. For centuries it has been believed that
children of multiple births have to be different and inferior to singletons and that prematures
are doomed to be physical and mental weaklings.
Time of birth on the future development of the child, while there is little scientific evidence
to substantiate the belief that there is a "best time" to be born, there is evidence that,
because the mother's health plays an extremely important role during the prenatal period, any
unfavorable condition during her pregnancy may and often does prove to be hazardous to her
unborn child.
A baby who is born within a year after the birth of a sibling is subject to a less favorable
prenatal environment than would have been the case had the interval between births been
longer. The mother has not had time to recover fully from the previous birth, and such an
infant tends to be lethargic at birth which affects postnatal adjustments. Throughout the early
years of life, babies born soon after the birth of an older sibling are likely to receive less of
their much needed attention and stimulation because of the other demands on the mother's
time.
Helplessness To some parents the helplessness of the newborn infant is appealing while, to
most, it is frightening. So long as the infants are in the hospital and under the care of doctors
and nurses, parents are not too concerned about their helplessness. However, when they take
them home from the hospital and assume the responsibility for their care, infantile
helplessness becomes a serious psychological hazard. The reason for this is that parents
wonder if they are capable of assuming the care of their newborn babies and this, in turn,
makes them nervous and anxious.
Anxiety and insecurity are quickly transmitted to the infants through the way mothers
handle them and this affects their postnatal adjustments. The helplessness of the newborn is
more of a psychological hazard in the case of firstborns than of later-born children. By the
time parents have had several children; they accept the helplessness of the newborn in a more
relaxed way and are not so likely to be disturbed by it as they are for the firstborn infant.
Individuality of the Infant To most adults, being different is interpreted as being inferior.
When parents steep themselves in child-care literature before the arrival of their first child, or
when they set up norms of behavior based on what their earlier-born children did at different
ages, they tend to judge a newborn infant in these terms.
Parental concern is then expressed in their treatment of the infant. This, in turn, affects
the infant's adjustments to postnatal life and tends to increase the severity of the problems
that concerned the parents. Under such conditions, the infant's individuality becomes a
psychological hazard which, unless parents accept individuality as normal, will play havoc
with the adjustments made not only during infancy but also as childhood progresses.
Developmental Lag Some infants lag behind are those born prematurely or those who were
injured at birth. Instead of regaining lost birth weight by the end of the first week or sooner,
they may continue to lose weight or rest on a plateau with no improvement at all. Even
worse, they may show such a pronounced lag that instead of being allowed to go home with
their parents three to four days after birth, as is usual, they are kept in the hospital and may
even have to have special nursing care.
Even a healthy, full-term infant may show developmental lag should there be some
minor and temporary illness or should the mother's milk be inadequate and the formula
substituted is not suited to the infant's needs.
Plateau in Development Even though a plateau in development is normal immediately after
birth, many first-time parents are unaware of this. As a result, they are concerned when their
baby seems to be making no progress.
However, it often leaves some psychological obstacles, three of which are common and
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
126
serious. First, it makes parents believe their infant is delicate and, as a result, should have
extra care and attention. This encourages over protectiveness which, once developed, often
persists as a habit. Second, it weakens parents' confidence in their ability to assume full care
of the infant after leaving the hospital. If by then the infant has not regained lost birth weight,
this lack of confidence is greatly increased. And third, parents feel that they must handle the
infant as little as possible and with great care to prevent further loss of weight or failure to
gain weight. As a result, they deprive the infant of one of the essentials of development,
stimulation of the different areas of the body.
Lack of Stimulation There is increasing scientific evidence that newborn infants need
stimulation of different areas of their bodies and of different sense organs if they are to
develop as they should. This, of course, is not a "new-fangled idea" because, in the days
when babies were born at home, they were picked up, rocked, talked to, and sung to as part of
their routine care.
Because it is customary today for babies to be born in hospitals, they are often deprived
of the stimulation received by babies born at home or even by those whose mothers have
them in their hospital rooms under the rooming-in plan. And, until very recently, premature
infants were kept in isolettes or incubators where they received only the minimum of
stimulation.
New-Parent Blues States of depression, often called "new-parent blues," are almost universal
among new parents. These depressive states tend to be more pronounced in mothers than in
fathers and in parents of first babies than in those who have already had one or more children.
In new mothers, depressive states are partly physical and partly psychological. The
glandular changes accompanying pregnancy and childbirth, fatigue from labor and childbirth,
and the generally weakened condition that persists even after normal childbirth all contribute
to maternal states of depression.
For most fathers, new-parent blues are more psychological than physiological. They are
often concerned about the extra expenses they must meet, especially if the mother must give
up her job. Many men also are concerned about how the pattern of their lives will change as a
result of parenthood and what effect this will have on their marital relationship.
New-parent blues can and often do play havoc with the infant's adjustments to postnatal
life. The new baby senses the tensions of the parents, especially of the mother, and this makes
it nervous and prone to crying. Many infants cry more after they get home than they did in
the hospital.
Unfavorable Attitudes on the Part of Significant People Even though parents, siblings, and
grandparents may have favorable attitudes toward the unborn baby during most of the
pregnancy, their feelings may change as the birth draws nearer and they become increasingly
aware of the new responsibilities they will have to face.
Disappointment about the infant's sex and its appearance, its excessive crying and
difficulties in taking nourishment, and its extreme helplessness which is often frightening to
new parents are just a few of the many reasons. Unfavorable attitudes are often intensified by
birth complications, the unexpected arrival of twins or triplets, and the new-parent blues
described above.
The mother's attitudes are especially important because they can directly affect the care
the baby receives. Thus the infant's behavior also influences the mother's behavior; if this is
unfavorable to begin with, it will become increasingly so, with the result that the infant's
adjustment problems are worsened.
Names Because it is legally established shortly after birth, a child's name can be classified as
one of the important potential psychological hazards of infancy. Their names do not become
real hazards until children are old enough to respond to how people outside the family react
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
127
to them (usually during the preschool years). Names become real psychological hazards only
if they cause the children embarrassment-or sometimes even humiliation-if their friends think
their names are "funny" or regard them as sex-inappropriate. While it is impossible to predict
during infancy how individuals will react to their names as they grow older, certain kinds of
names are almost universally hazardous.
Check Your Progress 2
State the physical and psychological hazards of infancy
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
10.3 HAZARDS IN TODDLERHOOD
As toddlerhood is the foundation age, it is an especially hazardous time. The hazards
may be physical, psychological, or both, as is true of infancy. For example, excessive crying
is both physically and psychologically damaging to the baby and to the home atmosphere. It
leads to gastrointestinal disturbances, regurgitation of food, night waking, and general nervous
tension. Furthermore, excessive crying leads to feelings of insecurity which affect the
baby's developing personality. In addition, excessive crying affects the baby's relationships
with the parents and other family members unfavorably. This, in turn, indirectly affects
personality development. In the first year of toddlerhood, physical hazards tend to be more
numerous and more serious than psychological ones, while the reverse is true during the
second year.
10.3.1 Physical Hazards
Physical hazards are serious for all babies but especially for those who are born
prematurely; those' who suffer from brain damage or other birth defects; and those whose
physical development and general physical condition at birth are poor.
Mortality Greater mortality occurs during the first three months of toddlerhood than laterwith
approximately two-thirds of all deaths during the first year of life occurring during the
first month. During the first year of toddlerhood, death is usually caused by serious illness
while, during the second year, death is more often due to accidents. Throughout toddlerhood,
more boys die than girls.
Crib Death Apparently normal, healthy babies are sometimes the victims of sudden and
unexpected death-referred to in medical circles as crib death. Usually crib death occurs after
a long period of sleep.
To date, medical science has been unable to find the exact cause or causes of crib death.
There is some evidence that it is common in babies who experience abnormalities in
breathing or who have had some abnormal condition at birth, such as jaundice. It is also more
common in babies who have had oxygen therapy during the newborn period.
More crib death occurs during the first year of toddlerhood than during the second; and
more during the first six months of the first year than in the second six months. There is no
evidence, to date, that crib death is more common among boys than among girls, or among
babies of lower socioeconomic groups as compared with those of higher groups.
Illnesses While it is true that many deaths during the first few months of postnatal life are
due to such illnesses as gastrointestinal or respiratory complications, the number of deaths
due to serious illness then declines rapidly because most babies today are given inoculations
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
128
and vaccinations to immunize them against diseases which, in the past, often proved to be
fatal.
However, minor illnesses such as colds and digestive upsets are common. Prompt
diagnosis and proper medical care can keep these from causing serious harm, but if they are
neglected, as frequently happens in the case of colds, serious disturbances can develop
rapidly, especially ear infections. Prolonged illnesses, even though not terminal, can interfere
with the normal growth pattern.
Accidents Although accidents are infrequent during the first year of life, owing to the fact
that babies are carefully protected in their cribs, play pens, and carriages, they are far more
frequent during the second year, when babies can move about more freely and are not as well
protected. Some toddlerhood accidents, such as bruises and scratches, are minor and have no
permanent effects. Others, such as blows on the head or cuts, may be serious enough to leave
permanent scars or may even be fatal. Even minor accidents, however, leave psychological
scars. Babies are often conditioned to fear situations similar to those in which accidents
occurred or they develop a generalized timidity as a result of frequent accidents.
Malnutrition May come from inadequate food intake or from an unbalanced diet, can play
havoc not only with physical growth but also with mental development. It not only causes
stunted growth but also leads to physical defects such as carious teeth, bowed legs, and a
tendency to suffer from more or less constant illnesses.
Because the brain grows and develops at such an accelerated rate during toddlerhood, it
can be seriously impaired by malnutrition. The first two years of postnatal life have been
called the critical period in brain growth because of the marked increase in internal
development of the brain cells at this time. As a result, it is the time when the brain is most
vulnerable to damage. If babies suffer from malnutrition at this time, there is no evidence that
the development that normally would take place will do so later.
Foundations of Obesity Many parents equate health in toddlerhood with plumpness and do
all they can to see that their babies are chubby. There is evidence that fat babies tend to have
obesity problems as they grow older while thin babies do not. This is because the number and
size of the fat cells of the body are established early in life.
There are three critical periods of fat-cell development. The first occurs during the three
months before birth, the second during the first three years of postnatal life, and the third
during the early part of adolescence. If babies are overfed, they are likely to have an obesity
problem for the rest of their lives.
The reason for this is that even if children slim down later, they still have the same
number of cells capable of storing fat-a built-in potential for becoming obese. Similarly,
babies who are fed large amounts of carbohydrates during this critical period of fat-cell
development are not only overweight as babies but are more subject to diabetes and heart disease
as they grow older. Bottle-fed babies are more likely to be overfed than breast-fed
babies, and thus lay the foundation for obesity problems.
Physiological Habits The foundations of the important physiological habits-eating, sleeping,
and eliminating-are established during toddlerhood, and thus a common physical hazard of
this period is the establishment of unfavorable attitudes on the baby's part toward these
habits.
10.3.2 Psychological Hazards
The most serious psychological hazards of toddlerhood involve the baby's failure to
master the developmental task for that age. Mastery of these tasks is important for two
reasons. First, the sooner babies gain control over their bodies, the sooner they can become
independent of help from others. Second, mastery of these tasks provides the foundations on
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
129
which mastery of later developmental tasks will be built. The better the mastery of
toddlerhood tasks, the more easily and quickly will the child be able to master the tasks of
early childhood.
Most of the serious psychological hazards of toddlerhood are related, either directly or
indirectly, to the failure to master the developmental tasks of toddlerhood.
Hazards in Motor Development When motor development is delayed, babies will be at a
great disadvantage when they begin to play with age-mates. The more they lag behind the
group in motor control, the slower they are likely to be in acquiring the skills other children
possess. Furthermore, because the desire to be independent makes its appearance early in the
second year, babies whose motor development lags behind that of their peers become
frustrated when they try to do things for themselves and fail.
Speech Hazards Delayed speech, like delayed motor control, is serious in toddlerhood
because, at this age, the foundations are being laid for the development of the tools of
communication that will be needed later as social horizons broaden. In early childhood, when
interest in people outside the home begins to awaken, children whose speech lags markedly
behind that of other children find themselves in the role of outsiders.
There are a number of reasons for delayed speech, the most common of which are low
level of intelligence, lack of stimulation (especially during the first year of life), and multiple
births. When parents and other caretakers fail to stimulate babbling and early attempts to
speak, most babies lose interest in trying to speak. The result is that their speech is often
markedly delayed. By contrast, when babies are encouraged to babble and to learn to say
words, their speech development conforms to the normal pattern and is often accelerated. The
more novelty there is in the environment, the greater the baby's motivation to vocalize.
Emotional Hazards There are four common psychological hazards that frequently arise in
relation to emotional development during the toddlerhood years.
1. Emotional Deprivation: Babies who are not given the opportunity to experience the
normal emotions of toddlerhood-especially affection, curiosity, and joy-do not thrive
physically. If emotional deprivation is severe and prolonged, it inhibits the secretion of the
pituitary hormones, including the growth hormone, and this may lead to what has been called
"deprivation dwarfism." In addition, emotional deprivation in toddlerhood often causes
babies to be backward in their motor and speech development and they do not learn how to
establish social contacts or show affection. They usually become listless, depressed, and
apathetic and often develop nervous mannerisms such as thumb-sucking.
2. Stress: Stress-a prolonged unpleasant emotional state, such as fear or anger-can cause
endocrine changes which upset body homeostasis. This, then, is reflected in eating and
sleeping difficulties, in nervous mannerisms such as excessive thumb sucking, and in
excessive crying. There are many causes of stress-poor health, parental neglect and poor
environmental conditions that interfere with proper sleeping and eating-but constant and
close association with a nervous, tense mother is a particularly important factor.
3. Too Much Affection: Parents who are oversolicitious or over demonstrative encourage
their babies to focus their attention on themselves and to become self-bound and selfish.
Babies thus expect others to show affection for them but they do not reciprocate.
4. Dominant Emotions: Conditions in the baby's environment encourage the development of
certain emotions to the exclusion of others, and these eventually become dominant unless
conditions change and the development of other emotions is encouraged. Timidity may
persist long after toddlerhood if a shy or fearful chi Id is exposed to too many strangers or too
many frightening situations.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
130
10.3.3 Social Hazards
The major social hazard o f toddlerhood is lack of opportunity and motivation to learn to
become social. This encourages the prolongation of egocentrism, which is characteristic of all
babies, and leads to the development of introversion. Being deprived of opportunities for
social contacts is detrimental at any age, but it is especially so from the ages of six weeks to
six months-the critical time in the development of attitudes which affect the pattern of socialization.
While social attitudes can and do change, many individuals who formed
unfavorable social attitudes as babies continue to be socially less well adjusted as they grow
older.
Play Hazards P l a y i n toddlerhood is potentially hazardous, both physically and
psychologically. Many toys can inflict cuts and bruises or cause the baby to choke on a part
that has come loose. The major psychological hazard is that the baby may come to rely too
much on the toys themselves for amusement, instead of learning to play in ways that involve
interaction with others. Television; used as a built-in babysitter, also discourages the baby
from taking an active role in play. When playing games with adults or older siblings, babies
are usually allowed to win. As a result, they find competition with other children difficult as
they grow older and are not able to lose graciously.
Hazards in Understanding Even though understanding is in a rudimentary stage of
development, it presents one serious psychological hazard. In the development of concepts, it
is relatively easy to replace wrong meanings associated with people, objects, or situations
with correct meanings. However, all concepts have some emotional weighting, and this is
where the potential hazard lies. If, for example, the baby learns to associate sweets with
rewards for good behavior and to think of vegetables as a form of punishment, the emotional
weighting of these concepts may lead to persistent food likes and dislikes.
Hazards in Morality No one expects babies to be moral in the sense that their behavior
conforms to the moral standards of the social group or that they will feel guilt and shame if
they fail to do so. However, a serious psychological hazard to future moral development
occurs when babies discover that they get more attention when they do things to annoy and
antagonize others than when they behave in a more socially approved way.
During toddlerhood, the patterns of behavior that present the greatest problems for
caretakers are dawdling, defiance, and disobedience-the "three D's" of morality. While not
one of these unsocial patterns of behavior is well developed even as toddlerhood draws to a
close, the foundations are often laid at this time.
Family-Relationship Hazards Because the family constitutes the main social environment,
any unfavorable condition in family relationships or in the baby's relationships with different
family members leads to psychological hazards with serious and far-reaching consequences.
Of the many potential hazards in family relationships, the following six are the most common
and the most far-reaching in their effects.
1. Separation from Mother: Unless a stable and satisfactory substitute is provided, babies
who are separated from their mothers develop feelings of insecurity which are expressed in
personality disturbances that may lay the foundation for later maladjustments.
2. Failure to Develop Attachment Behavior: Babies who fail to establish attachment
behavior with their mothers, or some stable mother-substitute, suffer from feelings of
insecurity similar to those associated with separation from their mothers. In addition, they do
not experience the pleasures that come from close, personal relationships. This handicaps
them in establishing friendships as they grow older.
3. Deterioration in Family Relationships: This a l most always occurs during the second
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
131
year of life is psychologically hazardous because babies notice that family members have
changed attitudes toward them and treat them differently. As a result, they usually feel
unloved and rejected-feelings which lead to resentment and insecurity.
4. Overprotectiveness: Babies who are overprotected and prevented doing what they are
capable of doing become overdependent and afraid to do what other babies of their ages do.
This, in time, is likely to lead to the abnormal fear of school-school phobia-and excessive
shyness in the presence of strangers.
5. Inconsistent Training: Inconsistent child-training methods-which can be the result of
permissiveness or of parents' feelings of inadequacy in the parental role-provide poor
guidelines for babies. This slows down their learning to behave in approved ways.
6. Child Abuse: When parents' are unhappy in their parental roles or when a frictional
relationship exists between them, some babies become the targets of anger and resentment.
The babies are either neglected or abused. The second year of life is a more common time for
child abuse than the first because babies are more troublesome to their parents and this triggers
the outlet of anger, resentment, and other unpleasant emotions engendered in the
relationship of the parents.
Hazards in Personality Development The developing self-concept is in large part a mirror
image of what babies believe significant people in their lives think of them. As family
relationships deteriorate during the second year of toddlerhood, self-concepts reflect the
unfavorable attitudes babies think family members have toward them. They then express
these unfavorable self-concepts in aggressive, resentful, negativistic, or withdrawn behavior,
all of which make them seem less endearing to family members than they were earlier. The
changed attitudes of family members are then reflected in their treatment of the babies. This
reinforces the unfavorable self-concepts that the baby is in the process of developing.
Check Your Progress 3
Explain the various hazards in toddlerhood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
10.4 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
xi) Self Development
xii) Categorizing the Self
xiii) Self-Control emergence in infancy
xiv) Hazards of infancy and toddler hood
10.5 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. Your answer may include the following
i) Development of Self-Awareness
ii) Categorizing the Self
iii) Emergence of Self-Control in infancy
2. The hazards of infancy include
I. Physical Hazards
i) Multiple Birth
ii) Postmaturity
iii) Prematurity
iv) Infant Mortality
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
132
v) Psychological Hazards
vi) Individuality of the Infant
vii) Helplessness
II Psychological Hazards
i) Helplessness
ii) Developmental Lag
iii) Lack of Stimulation
3. The Hazards of toddler hood are:
I Physical Hazards
i) Mortality
ii) Crib Death
iii) Illnesses
iv) Accidents
v) Malnutrition
II Psychological Hazards
i) Speech Hazards
ii) Emotional Hazards
III Social Hazards
i) Play Hazards
ii) Hazards in Understanding
iii) Hazards in Morality
iv) Family-Relationship Hazards
v) Hazards in Personality Development
10.6 Lesson – End Activities
1. Mention the Hazards of Infamy.
2. Write few remedies measure for speech hazards.
10.7 References
1. Erickson, E., Childhood and Society, New York : Norton, 1950
2. Piaget, J., The Origins of Intelligence in Children, New York : Norton, 1988.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
133
UNIT III
LESSON - 11
EARLY CHILDHOOD - CHARACTERISTICS - DEVELOPMENTAL TASKS -
BODY GROWTH - BRAIN DEVELOPMENT - INFLUENCES ON PHYSICAL
GROWTH AND HEALTH
CONTENTS
11.0 Aims and Objectives
11.1 Introduction
11.2.1 Names Used by Parents
11.2.2 Names Used by Educators
11.2.3 Names Used by Psychologists
11.3 Developmental Tasks of Early Childhood
11.4 Body Growth
11.4.1 Skeletal Growth
11.4.2 Asynchronies in Physical growth
11.5 Brain Development
11.5.1 Handedness
11.5.2 Other Advances in Brain Development
11.6 Influences on Physical Growth and Health
11.6.1 Heredity and Hormones
11.6.2 Emotional Well-Being
11.6.3 Nutrition
11.7 Let Us Sum Up
11.8 Check your Progress
11.9 Lesson – End Activities
11.10 References
11.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help understand the characteristics and developmental tasks with
reference to body and brain development.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
i) understand the characteristics of early childhood
ii) discuss the developmental tasks
iii) state the body growth and brain development of a child
iv) explain the factors that influence the physical and health of a child
11.1 INTRODUCTION
Childhood begins when the relative dependenc y o f toddlerhood is over, at
approximately the age of two years, and extends to the time when the child becomes sexually
mature, at approximately thirteen years for the average girl and fourteen years for the average
boy. After children become sexually mature, they are known as adolescents.
During this long period of time-roughly eleven years for girls and twelve years for boys
marked changes take place in the child both physically and psychologically. Today it is
widely recognized that childhood should be subdivided into two separate periods, early and
late childhood. Early childhood extends from two to six years, and late childhood extends
from six to the time the child becomes sexually mature. Thus early childhood begins at the
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
134
conclusion of toddlerhood-the age when dependency is practically a thing of the past is being
replaced by growing independence-and ends at about the time the child enters first grade in
school.
The new pressures and expectations that accompany the child's formal entrance into
school result in changes in patterns of behavior, interests, and values. As a result, children
become "different people from what they were earlier.
11.2 CHARACTERISTICS OF EARLY CHILDHOOD
Just as certain characteristics of toddlerhood make it a distinctive period in the life span,
so certain characteristics of early childhood set it apart from other periods. These
characteristics are reflected in the names that parents, educators, and psychologists
commonly apply to this period.
11.2.1 Names Used by Parents
Early childhood is considered as a problem age or a troublesome age. While toddlerhood
presents problems for parents, most of these centers around the baby's physical care. With the
dawn of childhood, behavior problems become more frequent and more troublesome than the
physical-care problems of toddlerhood.
As to why the behavior problems dominate the early childhood years is that young
children are developing distinctive personalities and are demanding an independence which,
in most cases, they are incapable of handling successfully. In addition young children are
often obstinate, stubborn, disobedient, negativistic, and antagonistic. They have frequent
temper tantrums, they are often bothered by bad dreams at night and irrational fears during
the day, and they suffer from jealousies.
Due to these problems, early childhood seems less appealing age than toddlerhood to
many parents. The dependency of the baby, so endearing to parents a s well as to older
siblings, is now replaced by resistance on the child's part to their help and a tendency to reject
demonstrations of their affection. F urthermore, few young children are as cute as babies,
which also make them less appealing.
Early childhood is referred to the toy age because young children spend much of their
waking time playing with toys. Studies of children's play have revealed that toy play reaches
its peak during the early childhood years and then begins to decrease when children reach the
school age. This, of course, does not mean that interest in playing with toys ends abruptly
when the child enters school. Instead, with entrance into first grade, children are encouraged
to engage in games and modified forms of sports, none of which require the use of toys.
When alone, however, children continue to play with their toys well into the third or even
fourth standard.
11.2.2 Names Used by Educators
The early childhood years is referred as preschool age to distinguish it from the time
when children are considered old enough, both physically and mentally, to cope with the
work they will be expected to do when they begin their formal schooling Even when children
go to nursery school or kindergarten, they are labeled preschoolers rather than
schoolchildren. In the home, day-care center, nursery school, or kindergarten, the pressures
and expectations young children are subjected to are very different from those they will
experience when they begin their formal education in the first standard. The early childhood
years, either in the home or in a preschool, are a time of preparation.
11.2.3 Names Used by Psychologists
A number of different names are used to describe the outstanding characteristics of the
psychological development of children during the early years of childhood. One of the most
commonly applied names is the pregang age, the time when children are learning the
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
135
foundations of social behavior as a preparation for the more highly organized social life they
will be required to adjust to when they enter first standard.
Since the major development that occurs during early childhood centers around gaining
control over the environment, many psychologists refer to early childhood as the exploratory
age, a label which implies that children want to know what their environment is, how it
works, how it feels, and how they can be a part of it. This includes people as well as
inanimate objects. One common way of exploring in early childhood is by asking questions:
thus this period is often referred to as the questioning age.
Imitation of the speech and actions of others are more pronounced during early
childhood. For this reason, it is also known as the imitative age. However, in spite of this
tendency, most children show more creativity in their play during early childhood than at any
other time in their lives. For that reason, psychologists also regard it as the creative age.
11.3 DEVELOPMENTAL TASKS OF EARLY CHILDHOOD
Although the foundations of some of the developmental tasks young children are expected
to master before they enter school are laid in toddlerhood, much remains to be learned in the
relatively short four-year span of early childhood.
When toddlerhood ends, all normal babies have learned to walk, though with varying
degrees of proficiency; have learned to take solid foods; and have achieved a reasonable
degree of physiological stability. The major task of learning to control the elimination of
body wastes has been almost completed and will be fully mastered within another year or
two.
While most babies have built up a useful vocabulary, have reasonably correct
pronunciation of the words they use, can comprehend the meaning of simple statements and
commands, and can put together several words into meaningful sentences, their ability to
communicate with others and to comprehend what others say to them is still on a low level.
Much remains to be mastered before they enter school.
Similarly, they have some simple concepts of social and physical realities, but far too
few to meet their needs as their social horizons broaden and as their physical environment
expands. Few babies know more than the most elementary facts about sex differences, and
even fewer understand the meaning of sexual modesty. It is questionable whether any babies,
as they enter early childhood, actually know what is sex-appropriate in appearance, and they
have only the most rudimentary understanding of sex-appropriate behavior.
This is equally true of concepts of right and wrong. What knowledge they have is limited
to home situations and must be broadened to include concepts of right and wrong in their
relationships with people outside the home, especially in the neighborhood, in school, and on
the playground.
One of the most important and, for many young children, one of the most difficult of the
developmental tasks of early childhood, is learning to relate emotionally to parents, siblings,
and other people. The emotional relationships that existed during toddlerhood must be
replaced by more mature ones. The reason for this is that relationships to others in
toddlerhood a r e based on babyish dependence on others to meet their emotional needs,
especially their need for affection. Young children, however, must learn to give as well as to
receive affection. In short, they must learn to be outer-bound instead of self bound.
Check Your Progress 1
State the how characteristics of early child hood are reflected
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
136
11.4 BODY GROWTH
The rapid increase in body size of the first 2 years tapers off into a slower growth pattern
during early childhood. On average, children add 2 to 3 inches in height and about 5 pounds
in weight each year. Boys continue to be slightly larger than girls. At the same time, the
"baby fat" that began to decline in toddlerhood drops off further. The child gradually
becomes thinner, although girls retain somewhat more body fat, and boys are slightly more
muscular. As the torso lengthens and widens, internal organs tuck neatly inside, and the spine
straightens. By age 5 the top-heavy, bowlegged, potbellied toddler has become a more
streamlined, flat-tummied, longer-legged child with body proportions similar to those of
adults. Consequently, posture and balance improve-changes that support the gains in motor
coordination. Individual differences in body size are even more apparent during early
childhood than in infancy and toddlerhood.
11.4.1 Skeletal Growth
The skeletal changes that are under way in infancy continue throughout early childhood.
Between ages 2 and 6, approximately 45 new epiphyses, or growth centers in which cartilage
hardens into bone, emerge in various parts of the skeleton. Other epiphyses will appear in
middle childhood. X-rays of these growth centers enable doctors to estimate children's
skeletal age, or progress toward physical maturity. During early and middle childhood,
information about skeletal age is helpful in diagnosing growth disorders.
Parents and children are especially aware of another aspect of skeletal growth: By the end
of the preschool years, children start to lose their primary, or "baby;' teeth. The age at which
they do so is heavily influenced by genetic factors. For example, girls, who are ahead of boys
in physical development, lose their primary teeth sooner. Environmental influences,
especially prolonged malnutrition, can delay the appearance of permanent teeth.
Even though primary teeth are temporary, dental care is important. Diseased baby teeth can
affect the health of permanent teeth. Brushing consistently, avoiding sugary foods, drinking
fluoridated water, and getting topical fluoride treatments and sealants (plastic coatings that
protect tooth surfaces) prevent cavities. Unfortunately, childhood tooth decay remains high,
especially among low-SES children.
11.4.2 Asynchronies in Physical growth
Body systems differ in their unique, carefully timed patterns of maturation. Physical
growth is asynchronous. Body size (as measured by height and weight) and a variety of
internal organs follow the general growth curve, which involves rapid growth infancy, slower
gains in early and middle childhood and rapid growth again during adolescence. Yet there are
exceptions to this trend. The genitals develop slowly from birth to age 4, change little
throughout middle childhood, and then grow rapidly during adolescence. In contrast, the
lymph glands grow at an astounding pace in infancy and childhood, and then their rate of
growth declines in adolescence. The lymph system helps fight infection and assists with
absorption of nutrients, thereby supporting children's health and survival. During the first few
years, the brain grows more rapidly than any other part of the body.
11.5 BRAIN DEVELOPMENT
Between ages 2 and 6 years, the brain increases from 70 to 90 percent of its adult weight.
Brain-imaging studies reveal that energy metabolism in the cerebral cortex reaches a peak
around age 4. By this time, many cortical regions have overproduced synapses, resulting in a
high energy need. As formation of synapses, myelinization, and synaptic pruning continue,
preschoolers improve in a wide variety of skills-physical coordination, perception, attention,
memory, language, logical thinking, and imagination.
Recall that the cerebral cortex is made up of two hemispheres. Measures of neural activity
in various cortical regions reveal especially rapid growth from 3 to 6 years in frontal-lobe
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
137
areas devoted to planning and organizing behavior. Furthermore, for most children, the left
hemisphere is especially active between 3 and 6 years and then levels off. In contrast, activity
in the right hemisphere increases steadily throughout early and middle childhood.
These findings fit nicely with what we know about several aspects of cognitive
development. Language skills (typically housed in the left hemisphere) increase at an
astonishing pace in early childhood, and they support children's increasing control over
behavior. In contrast, spatial skills (such as finding one's way from place to place, drawing
pictures, and recognizing geometric shapes) develop gradually over childhood and adolescence.
Differences in rate of development between the two hemispheres suggest that they are
continuing to lateralize (specialize in cognitive functions).
11.5.1 Handedness
One morning on a visit to the preschool, I watched 3year-old Moira as she drew pictures,
worked puzzles, joined in snack time, and played outside. Unlike most of her classmates,
Moira does most things-drawing, eating, and zipping her jacket-with her left hand. But she
uses her right hand for a few activities, such as throwing a ball. Hand preference is evident in
10 percent of l-year-olds and strengthens during early childhood. Ninety percent of 5-yearolds
prefer one hand over the other
A Strong hand preference reflects the greater capacity of one side of the brain- the
individual’s dominant cerebral hemisphere – to carry out skilled motor action. Other important
abilities may be located on the dominant side as well. In support of this idea, for righthanded
people, who make up 90 percent of the population in Western nations, language is
housed with hand control in the left hemisphere. For the remaining left-handed 10 percent,
language is often shared between the hemispheres. This indicates that the brains of lefthanders
tend to be less strongly lateralized than those of right-handers. Consistent with this
idea, many left-handed individuals are ambidextrous. Although they prefer their left hand,
they sometimes use their right hand skillfully as well.
Is handedness hereditary? Researchers disagree on this issue. Left-handed parents show
only a weak tendency to have left-handed children. One genetic theory proposes that most
children inherit a gene that biases them for right-handedness and a left -dominant cerebral
hemisphere. However, that bias is not strong enough to overcome experiences that might
sway children toward a left-hand preference.
Research confirms that experience can profoundly affect handedness. Both identical and
fraternal twins are more likely than ordinary siblings to differ in handedness. The hand preference
of each twin is related to body position in the uterus; twins usually lie in opposite
orientations. According to one theory, the way most fetuses lie-turned toward the left-may
promote greater postural control by the right side of the body. Also, wide cultural differences
exist in rates of left-handedness. For example, in Tanzania, Africa, children are physically
restrained and punished for favoring their left hand. Less than 1 percent of Tanzanians are
left-handed.
Perhaps you have heard that left-handedness is more frequent among severely retarded
and mentally ill people than it is in the general population. Although this is true, recall that
when two variables are correlated, one does not necessarily cause the other. Atypical
lateralization is probably not responsible for the problems of these individuals. Instead, they
may have suffered early damage to the left hemisphere, which caused their disabilities and
also led to a shift in handedness. In support of this idea, left-handedness is associated with
prenatal and birth difficulties that can result in brain damage, including prolonged labor,
prematurity, Rh incompatibility, and breech delivery .
Keep in mind, however, that only a small number of lefthanders show developmental
problems. In fact, unusual lateralization may have certain advantages. Left- and mixedhanded
youngsters are more likely than their right-handed agemates to develop outstanding
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
138
verbal and mathematical talents. More even distribution of cognitive functions across both
hemispheres may be responsible.
11.5.2 Other Advances in Brain Development
Besides the cortex, other parts of the brain make strides during early childhood. As we
look at these changes, you will see that all involve establishing links between different parts
of the brain, increasing the coordinated functioning of the central nervous system.
At the rear and base of the brain is the cerebellum, a structure that aids in balance and
control of body .movement. Fibers linking the cerebellum to the cerebral cortex begin to
myelinate after birth, but they do not complete this process until about age 4. This change
undoubtedly contributes to dramatic gains in motor control, so that by the end of the
preschool years children can play hopscotch, pump a playground swing, and throw a ball with
a well-organized set of movements.
The reticular formation, a structure of the brain that maintains alertness and
consciousness, myelinates throughout childhood and into adolescence. Neurons in the
reticular formation send out fibers to the frontal lobes of the cortex, contributing to
improvements in sustained, controlled attention.
The corpus callosum is a large bundle of fibers that connects the two cortical
hemispheres. Myelinization of the corpus callosum does not begin until the end of the first
year of life. Between 3 and 6 years, it grows rapidly and then enlarges at a slower pace into
adolescence. The corpus callosum supports integration of many aspects of thinking, including
perception, attention, memory, language, and problem solving. The more complex the task,
the more crucial communication between the hemispheres becomes.
Check Your Progress 2
Write a note on the body growth and brain development
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
11.6 INFLUENCES ON PHYSICALGROWTH AND HEALTH
While discussing growth and health in early childhood, heredity remains important,
environmental factors continue to play crucial roles. Emotional well-being, good nutrition,
relative freedom from disease, and physical safety are essential.
11.6.1 Heredity and Hormones
The impact of heredity on physical growth is evident throughout childhood. Children's
physical size and rate of growth are related to those of their parents. Genes influence growth
by controlling the body's production of hormones. The pituitary gland, located at the base of
the brain, plays a critical role by releasing two hormones that induce growth.
The first is growth hormone (GH), which from birth on is necessary for development of
all body tissues except the central nervous system and genitals. Children who lack GH reach
an average mature height of only 4 feet, 4 inches. When treated with injections of GH starting
at an early age, these GH-deficient children show catch-up growth and then grow at a normal
rate, reaching a height much greater than they would have without treatment.
The second pituitary hormone affecting children's growth, thyroid-stimulating hormone
(TSH), stimulates the thyroid gland (located in the neck) to release thyroxine, which is
necessary for normal development of the nerve cells of the brain and for GH to have its full
impact on body size. Infants born with a deficiency of thyroxine must receive it at once or
they will be mentally retarded. At later ages, children with too little thyroxine grow at a
below-average rate. By then, the central nervous system is no longer affected because the
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
139
most rapid period of brain development is complete. With prompt treatment, such children
catch up in body growth and eventually reach normal size.
11.6.2 Emotional Well-Being
In childhood as in infancy, emotional well-being can have a profound effect on growth
and health. Preschoolers with very stressful home lives (due to divorce, financial difficulties,
or a change in their parents' employment status) suffer more respiratory and intestinal
illnesses and more unintentional injuries than others.
Extreme emotional deprivation can interfere with the production of GH and lead to
psychosocial dwarfism, a growth disorder that appears between 2 and 15 years of age.
Typical characteristics include very short stature, decreased GH secretion, immature skeletal
age, and serious adjustment problems, which help distinguish psychosocial dwarfism from
normal shortness. When such children are removed from their emotionally inadequate
environments, their GH levels quickly return to normal, and they grow rapidly. But if
treatment is delayed, the dwarfism can be permanent.
11.6.3 Nutrition
With the transition to early childhood, many children become unpredictable and choosy
eaters. This decline in appetite is normal. It occurs because growth has slowed. Furthermore,
preschoolers' wariness of new foods may be adaptive. By sticking to familiar foods, they are
less likely to swallow dangerous substances when adults are not around to protect them.
Parents need not worry about variations in amount eaten from meal to meal. Preschoolers
compensate for a meal in which they ate little with a later one in which they eat more.
Even though they eat less, preschoolers need a high-quality diet. They require the same
foods adults do-only smaller amounts. Fats, oils, and salt should be kept to a minimum because
of their link to high blood pressure and heart disease in adulthood. Foods high in sugar
should also be avoided. In addition to causing tooth decay, they lessen young children's
appetite for healthy foods and increase their risk of overweight and obesity.
The social environment powerfully influences young children's food preferences.
Children tend to imitate the food choices of people they admire-adults as well as peers. A
pleasant mealtime climate also encourages healthy eating. Repeated exposure to a new food
(without any direct pressure to eat it) increases children's acceptance. Sometimes parents
bribe their children, saying, "Finish your vegetables, and you can have an extra cookie." This
practice causes children to like the healthy food less and the treat more. Too much parental
control over children's eating limits their opportunities to develop self-control.
11.6.4 Infectious Disease
In well-nourished children, ordinary childhood illnesses have no effect on physical
growth. But when children are undernourished, disease interacts with malnutrition in a
vicious spiral, and the consequences for physical growth can be severe.
Infectious Disease and Malnutrition. Illnesses such as measles and chicken pox, which
typically do not appear until after age 3 in industrialized nations, occur much earlier. Poor
diet depresses the body's immune system, making children far more susceptible to disease. Of
the 10 million annual worldwide deaths in children under age 5, 99 percent are in developing
countries and 70 percent are due to infectious diseases.
Disease, in turn, is a major cause of malnutrition and, through it, hinders physical growth.
Illness reduces appetite and limits the body's ability to absorb foods. These outcomes are
especially severe in children with intestinal infections. In developing countries, diarrhea is
widespread and increases in early childhood because of unsafe water and contaminated foods,
leading to growth stunting and several million childhood deaths each year.
Most growth retardation and deaths due to diarrhea can be prevented with nearly cost free
oral rehydration therapy (ORT), in which sick children are given a glucose, salt, and water
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
140
solution that quickly replaces fluids the body loses. Since 1990, public health workers have
taught nearly half the families in the developing world how to administer ORT. As a result,
the lives of more than 1 million children are being saved annually.
Immunization. In industrialized nations, childhood diseases have declined dramatically
during the past half-century, largely due to widespread immunization of infants and young
children. All children were guaranteed free immunizations, a program that has led to a steady
improvement in early childhood immunization rates.
Inability to pay for vaccines, however, is only one cause of inadequate immunization.
Misconceptions also contribute for example, the notion that vaccines do not work or that they
weaken the immune system. Furthermore, some parents have been influenced by media
reports suggesting that the measles-mumps-rubella vaccine has contributed to a rise in
number of children diagnosed with autism. Yet large-scale studies show no association
between immunization and autism. Public education programs directed at increasing parental
knowledge about the importance and safety of timely immunizations are badly needed.
Diseases that spread most rapidly are diarrhea and respiratory infections the illnesses most
frequently suffered by young children. The risk that a respiratory infection will result in otitis
media, or middle ear infection, is almost double that of children remaining at home.
11.6. 5 Childhood Injuries
Unintentional injuries - auto collisions, pedestrian accidents, drownings, poisonings,
firearm wounds, burns, falls, and swallowing of foreign objects-are the leading cause of
childhood mortality in industrialized countries. Among injured children and youths who
survive, thousands suffer pain, brain damage, and permanent physical disabilities.
Auto and traffic accidents, drownings, and burns are the most common injuries during
early childhood. Motor vehicle collisions are by far the most frequent source of injury at all
ages, ranking as the leading cause of death among children more than 1 year old.
Factors Related to Childhood Injuries. We are used to thinking of childhood injuries as
"accidental.” But a close look reveals that meaningful causes underlie them, and we can,
indeed, do something about them.
Individual differences exist in the safety of children's behaviors. Because of their higher
activity level and greater willingness to take risks during play, boys are more likely to be
injured than girls. Temperamental characteristics-irritability, inattentiveness, and negative
mood-are also related to childhood injuries. Children with these traits present special childrearing
challenges. They are likely to protest when placed in auto seat restraints, to refuse to
take a companion's hand when crossing the street, and to disobey even after repeated adult
instruction and discipline.
Poverty, low parental education, and more children in the home are also strongly
associated with injury. Parents who must cope with many daily stresses often have little time
and energy to monitor the safety of their youngsters. And their homes and neighborhoods
pose further risks. Noise, crowding, and confusion characterize run-down, inner-city
neighborhoods with few safe places to play.
Poverty, rapid population growth, overcrowding in cities, and heavy road traffic
combined with weak safety measures are major causes. Safety devices, such as car safety
seats and bicycle helmets, are neither readily available nor affordable in most developing
countries. This indicates that besides reducing poverty and teenage pregnancy and upgrading
the status of child care, additional steps must be taken to ensure children's safety.
Preventing Childhood Injuries. Childhood injuries have many causes, so a variety of
approaches are needed to control them. Laws prevent many injuries by requiring car safety
seats, child-resistant caps on medicine bottles, flameproof clothing, and fenced in backyard
swimming pools.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
141
Communities can help by modifying their physical environments. Inexpensive and widely
available public transportation can reduce the time that children spend in cars. Playgrounds, a
common site of injury, can be covered with protective surfaces, such as rubber matting, sand,
and wood chips. Free, easily installed window guards can be given to families in high-rise
apartment buildings to prevent falls. And widespread media and information campaigns can
inform parents and children about safety issues.
Nevertheless, even though they know better, many parents and children behave in ways
that compromise safety. Preschoolers spontaneously recall only about half of their parents'
home safety rules; they need prompting to remember others and supervision to ensure that
they comply even with well-learned rules. A variety of programs based on behavior
modification (modeling and reinforcement) have improved safety practices. In one,
counselors helped parents identify dangers in the home-fire hazards, objects that young
children might swallow poisons, firearms, and others. Then they demonstrated specific ways
to eliminate the dangers. Some interventions reward parents and children with prizes if the
children arrive at child care or school restrained in car seats.
Efforts like these have been remarkably successful, yet their focus is fairly narrow-on
decreasing specific environmental risks and risky behaviors. Attention must also be paid to
family conditions that can prevent childhood injury: relieving crowding in the home,
providing social supports to ease parental stress, and teaching parents to use effective
discipline.
Check Your Progress 3
Write the factors that influences on physical growth and health during early child hood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
11.7 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Characteristics of Early Childhood
ii) Skeletal Growth
iii) Asynchronies in Physical growth
iv) Handedness
11.8 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. Characteristics of early child hood are reflected in the names used by
i) Parents
ii) Educators
iii) Psychologists
2. The body growth and brain development involves
i) Skeletal Growth
ii) Asynchronies in Physical growth
iii) Handedness
3. The factors that could influence physical growth and health are:
i) Heredity and Hormones
ii) Emotional Well-Being
iii) Nutrition
iv) Heredity and Hormones
v) Emotional Well-Being
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
142
11.9 Lesson – End Activities
1. Discuss about infectious disease.
2. Mention few criteria for high quality food.
11.10 References
1. Kohlberg, L., Moral Stage3s and Moralization : A Cognitive Developmental Approach,
New York: 1958.
2. Piaget, J., The Origins of Intelligence in Children, New York : International university
Press, 1952.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
143
LESSON – 12
MOTOR DEVELOPMENT - PIAGET'S THEORY: THE PREOPERATIONAL
STAGE - VYGOTSKY'S SOCIOCULTURAL THEORY - ERIKSON’S THEORY:
INITIATIVE VERSUS GUILT - CHILD REARING AND EMOTIONAL AND
SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT.
Contents
12.0 Aims and Objectives
12.1 Introduction
12.1.1 Gross Motor Development
12.1.2 Fine Motor Development
12.1.3 Individual Differences in Motor Skills
12.2 Piaget's Theory: The Preoperational Stage
12.2.1 Advances in Mental Representation
12.2.2 Make-Believe Play
12.2.3 Limitations of Preoperational Thought
12.3 Piaget and Education
12.4 Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory
12.4.1 Private Speech
12.4.2 Social Origins of Early Childhood Cognition
12.5 Erikson's Theory: Initiative versus Guilt
12.6 Child Rearing and Emotional and Social Development
12.6.1 Child-Rearing Styles
12.6.2 Authoritative Child Rearing
12.6.3 Authoritarian Child Rearing
12.6.4 Permissive Child Rearing
12.6.5 Uninvolved Parenting
12.6.6 What Makes Authoritative Child Rearing So Effective?
12.6.7 Cultural Variations
12.6.8 Child Maltreatment
12.6.9 Origins of Child Maltreatment
12.6.10 Consequences of Child Maltreatment
12.6.11 Preventing Child Maltreatment
12.7 Let Us Sum Up
12.8 Check your Progress
12.9 Lesson – End Activities
12.10 References
12.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help understand the motor development, preoperational stage, child
rearing and emotional and social development.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
v) understand motor skills in early childhood
vi) mention the Piaget's theory of preoperational stage
vii) discuss the Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory
viii) explain Erikson's theory of initiative versus guilt
ix) list the child rearing, emotional and social development
12.1 INTRODUCTION
You will see that an explosion of new motor skills occurs in early childhood, each of
which builds on the simpler movement patterns of toddlerhood. The same principle that
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
144
governs motor development during the first 2 years of life continues to operate during the
preschool years. Children integrate previously acquired skills into more complex, dynamic
systems of action. Then they revise each new skill as their bodies grow larger and stronger,
their central nervous systems develop, and their environments present new challenges.
12.1.1 Gross Motor Development
As children's bodies become more streamlined and less top heavy, their center of gravity
shifts downward, toward the trunk. As a result, balance improves greatly, paving the way for
new motor skills involving large muscles of the body. By age 2, preschoolers' gaits become
smooth and rhythmic-secure enough that soon they leave the ground, at first by running and
later by jumping, hopping, galloping, and skipping.
As children become steadier on their feet, their arms and torsos are freed to experiment with
new skills-throwing and catching balls, steering tricycles, and swinging on horizontal bars
and rings. Then upper- and lower-body skills combine into more refined actions. Five- and 6-
year-olds simultaneously steer and pedal a tricycle and flexibly move their whole body when
throwing, catching, hopping, and jumping. By the end of the preschool years, all skills are
performed with greater speed and endurance. Table 1 provides a closer look at gross motor
development in early childhood.
Table 1 Changes in Gross and Fine Motor Skills during Early Childhood
Age Gross Motor Skills Fine Motor Skills
2-3 years
3-4 years
4-5 years
5-6 years
Walks more rhythmically; hurried walk
changes to run.
A jump, hops, throw, and catches with rigid
upper body. Pushes riding toy with feet;
little steering.
Walks up stairs, alternating feet, and
downstairs, leading with one foot.
Jumps and hops, flexing upper body.
Throws and catches with slight involvement
of upper body; still catches by trapping ball
against chest.
Pedals and steers tricycle.
Walks downstairs, alternating feet.
Runs more smoothly.
Gallops and skips with one foot.
Throws ball with increased body rotation
and transfer of weight on feet; catches ball
with hands.
Rides tricycle rapidly, steers smoothly.
Increases running speed.
Gallops more smoothly; engages in true
skipping. Displays mature throwing and
catching pattern. Rides bicycle with training
wheels.
Puts on and removes simple items
of clothing.
Zips and unzips large zippers.
Uses spoon effectively.
Fastens and unfastens large
buttons.
Serves self food without
assistance.
Uses scissors.
Copies vertical line and circle.
Draws first picture of person, using
tadpole image.
Uses fork effectively.
Cuts with scissors following line.
Copies triangle, cross, and some
letters.
Uses knife to cut soft food.
Ties shoes.
Draws person with six parts.
Copies some numbers and simple
words.
12.1.2 Fine Motor Development
Like gross motor development, fine motor skills take a giant leap forward during the
preschool years. Because control of the hands and fingers improves, young children put
puzzles together, build with small blocks, cut and paste, and string beads. To parents, fine
motor progress is most apparent in two areas: (1) children's care of their own bodies, and (2)
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
145
the drawings and paintings that fill the walls at home.
12.1.2.a Self - Help Skills. Young children gradually become self-sufficient at dressing and
feeding, although parents need to be patient about these abilities (Table 1). When tired and in
a hurry, young children often revert. And the 3-year-old who dresses himself sometimes ends
up with his shirt on inside out, his pants on backwards, and his left shoe on his right foot!
Perhaps the most complex self-help skill of early childhood is shoe tying, mastered around
age 6. Success requires a longer attention span, memory for an intricate series of hand
movements, and the dexterity to perform them. Shoe tying illustrates the close connection
between motor and cognitive development. Drawing and writing are additional examples.
12.1.2.b From Scribbling to Pictures. A variety of factors combine with fine motor control
to influence the development of children's artful representations. These include cognitive advances-
the realization that pictures can serve as symbols and gains in planning skills and
spatial understanding, which result in a move from a focus on separate objects to a broader
visual perspective. The emphasis that the child's culture places on artistic expression also
makes a difference.
Typically, drawing progresses through the following sequence:
1. Scribbles. Children begin to draw during the second year. At first, gestures rather than the
resulting scribbles contain the intended representation.
2. First representational forms. By age 3, children's scribbles start to become pictures. Often
this happens after they make a gesture with the crayon, notice that they have drawn a
recognizable shape, and then decide to label it.
A major milestone in drawing occurs when children use lines to represent the boundaries of
objects. This enables 3- and 4-year-olds to draw their first picture of a person.
3. More realistic drawings. Young children do not demand that a drawing be realistic. But as
cognitive and fine motor skills improve, they learn to desire greater realism. As a result, they
create more complex drawings. The drawings might contain more conventional figures, in
which the head and body are differentiated.
12.1.2.c Cultural Variations in Children's Drawings. In cultures with rich artistic
traditions, children's drawings reflect the conventions of their culture and are more elaborate.
In cultures with little interest in art, even older children and adolescents produce simple
forms. Once children realize that lines must evoke human features, they find solutions to
figure drawing that vary somewhat from culture to culture but, overall, follow the sequence
described earlier.
12.1.2.d Early Printing. As preschoolers experiment with lines and shapes, notice print in
storybooks, and observe people writing, they try to print letters and, later, words. Often the
first word printed is the child's name. Initially, it may be represented by a single letter.
12.1.3 Individual Differences in Motor Skills
A child with a tall, muscular body tends to move more quickly and to acquire certain
skills earlier than a short, stocky youngster. Researchers believe that body build contributes
to the superior performances who have longer limbs, since they have better leverage.
Sex differences in motor skills are evident in early childhood. Boys are slightly ahead of
girls in skills that emphasize force and power. By age 5, they can jump slightly farther, run
slightly faster, and throw a ball much farther (about 5 feet farther). Girls have an edge in fine
motor skills and in certain gross motor skills that require a combination of good balance and
foot movement, such as hopping and skipping. Boys' greater muscle mass and (in the case of
throwing) slightly longer forearms may contribute to their skill advantages. And girls' greater
overall physical maturity may be partly responsible for their better balance and precision of
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
146
movement.
When children have play spaces appropriate for running, climbing, jumping, and
throwing and have access to puzzles, construction sets, and art materials that promote manipulation,
drawing, and writing, they respond eagerly to these challenges.
Check Your Progress 1
Bring out the motor skills development during early child hood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
12.2 PIAGET'S THEORY: THE PREOPERATIONAL STAGE
As children move from the sensorimotor to preoperational stage, which spans the
years 2 to 7, the most obvious change is an extraordinary increase in representational, or
symbolic, activity. Recall that infants and toddlers have some ability to represent their world.
During early childhood, this capacity blossoms.
12.2.1 Advances in Mental Representation
Piaget acknowledged that language is our most flexible means of mental representation.
By detaching thought from action, it permits far more efficient thinking than was possible
earlier. When we think in words, we overcome the limits of our momentary experiences. We
can deal with past, present, and future at once and combine concepts in unique ways, as when
we think about a hungry caterpillar eating bananas or monsters flying through the forest at
night.
Despite the power of language, Piaget did not believe that it plays a major role in
cognitive development. Instead, he believed that sensorimotor activity leads to internal
images of experience, which children then label with words. In support of Piaget's view, the
first words toddlers use have a strong sensorimotor basis. In addition, toddlers acquire an
impressive range of categories long before they use words to label them.
12.2.2 Make-Believe Play
Make-believe play is another excellent example of the development of representation
during early childhood. Piaget believed that through pretending, young children practice and
strengthen newly acquired representational schemes. Drawing on his ideas, several
investigators have traced the development of make-believe during the preschool years.
12.2.2.a Development of Make-Believe. David a 3 year old boy visited a neighbor’s house.
David wandered around, picked up the receiver of a toy telephone, said, "Hi, Mommy: and
then dropped it. In the housekeeping area, he found a cup, pretended to drink, and then
toddled off again.
A comparison of David’s pretend illustrates three important changes. It reflects the preschool
child's growing symbolic mastery:
A. Over time, play increasingly detaches from the real-life conditions associated with it. In
early pretending, toddlers use only realistic objects-for example, a toy telephone to talk into
or a cup to drink from. Most of these first pretend acts imitate adults' actions and are not yet
flexible. Children younger than age 2, for example, will pretend to drink from a cup but
refuse to pretend a cup is a hat.
After age 2, children pretend with less realistic toys, such as a block standing in for a
telephone receiver. And during the third year, they can flexibly imagine objects and events,
without any support from the real world.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
147
B. Play becomes less self-centered with age. At first, make-believe is directed toward the
self-for example, David pretends to feed only himself. A short time later, children direct
pretend actions toward other objects, as when the child feeds a doll. And early in the third
year, they become detached participants who make a doll feed itself. Make-believe gradually
becomes less self-centered as children realize that agents and recipients of pretend actions
can be independent of themselves.
C. Play gradually includes more complex scheme combinations. David can pretend to drink
from a cup but he does not yet combine pouring and drinking. Later, children combine
schemes with those of peers in sociodramatic play, the make-believe with others that is
under way by age 21/2 and increases rapidly during the next few years. By the end of early
childhood, children have a sophisticated understanding of role relationships and story lines.
The appearance of complex sociodramatic play indicates that children do not just represent
their world; they are aware that make-believe is a representational activity-an understanding
that increases between ages 4 and 8. Listen closely to preschoolers as they assign roles and
negotiate make-believe plans: "You pretend to be the Doctor; I'll act like I'm a patient!" In
communicating about pretend, children think about their own and others' fanciful
representations. This indicates that they have begun to reason about people's mental activities.
12.2.2.b Benefits of Make-Believe. Today, Piaget's view of make-believe as mere practice
of representational schemes is regarded as too limited. Play not only reflects but also contributes
to children's cognitive and social skills. Compared with social non pretend activities
(such as drawing or putting puzzles together), during sociodramatic play preschoolers' interactions
last longer, show more involvement, draw larger numbers of children into the
activity, and are more cooperative.
12.2.3 Limitations of Preoperational Thought
Piaget described preschoolers in terms of what they cannot, rather than can, understand.
He compared them to older, more competent children in the concrete operational stage, as the
term preoperational suggests. According to Piaget, young children are not capable of
operations--mental actions that obey logical rules. Instead, their thinking is rigid, limited to
one aspect of a situation at a time, and strongly influenced by the way things appear at the
moment.
12.2.3.a Egocentrism. For Piaget, the most serious deficiency of preoperational thinking, the
one that underlies all others, is egocentrism - failure to distinguish the symbolic viewpoints
of others from one's own. He believed that when children first mentally represent the world,
they tend to focus on their own viewpoint. Hence, they often assume that others perceive,
think, and feel the same way they do.
Egocentrism, Piaget pointed out, shows up in other aspects of children's reasoning. He
regarded egocentrism as responsible for preoperational children's animistic thinking-the
belief that inanimate objects have lifelike qualities, such as thoughts, wishes, feelings, and
intentions. The 3-year-old who charmingly explains that the sun is angry at the clouds and
has chased them away is demonstrating this kind of reasoning. According to Piaget, because
young children egocentrically assign human purposes to physical events, magical thinking is
common during the preschool years. Piaget argued that preschoolers' egocentric bias prevents
them from accommodating, or reflecting on and revising their faulty reasoning in response to
their physical and social worlds.
12.2.3.b Inability to Conserve. Conservation refers to the idea that certain physical characteristics
of objects remain the same, even when their outward appearance changes. At
snack time, Preethi and Samy each had identical boxes of cereals, but after Preethi opened
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
148
hers out on the table, Samy was convinced that she had more.
Another type of conservation task involves liquid. The child is shown two identical tall
glasses of water and asked if they contain equal amounts. Once the child agrees that they do,
the water in one glass is poured into a short, wide container, changing the appearance of the
water but not its amount. Then the child is asked whether the amount of water is the same or
has changed. Preoperational children think the quantity has changed. They explain, "There is
less now because the water is way down here" (that is, its level is so low) or, "There is more
now because it is all spread out."
Preoperational children's inability to conserve highlights several related aspects of their
thinking. First, their understanding is centered, or characterized by centration. They focus on
one aspect of a situation, neglecting other important features. In conservation of liquid, the
child centers on the height of the water, failing to realize that all changes in height are
compensated for by changes in width. Second, children are easily distracted by the perceptual
appearance of objects. It looks like there is less water in the short, wide container, so it must
have less water. Third, children treat the initial and final states of the water as unrelated
events, ignoring the dynamic transformation (pouring of water) between them.
The most important illogical feature of preoperational thought is its irreversibility, an
inability to mentally go through a series of steps in a problem and then reverse direction,
returning to the starting point. Reversibility is part of every logical operation. After Preethi
spills her box of cereals, Samy cannot reverse by thinking to himself, "I know that Preethi
doesn't have more cereals than I do. If we put them back in that little box, her raisins and my
cereals would look just the same.”
12.2.3.c Lack of Hierarchical Classification. Lack of logical operations leads
preschoolers to have difficulty with hierarchical classification-the organization of objects
into classes and subclasses on the basis of similarities and differences. Piaget's famous class
inclusion problem, demonstrates this limitation. Children are shown 16 flowers, 4 of which
are blue and 12 of which are yellow. Asked, "Are there more yellow flowers or flowers?" the
preoperational child responds, "More yellow flowers," failing to realize that both yellow and
blue flowers are included in the category "flowers." Preoperational children center on the
overriding perceptual feature of yellow. They do not think reversibly by moving from the
whole class (flowers) to the parts (yellow and blue) and back again.
12.3 PIAGET AND EDUCATION
Piaget's theory has had a major impact on education, especially during early childhood.
Three educational principles derived from his theory continue to have a widespread influence
on teacher training and classroom practices:
Discovery learning: In a Piagetian classroom, children are encouraged to discover for
themselves through spontaneous interaction with the environment. Instead of presenting
ready-made knowledge verbally, teachers provide a rich variety of activities designed to
promote exploration-art materials, puzzles, table games, dress-up clothing, building blocks,
books, measuring tools, musical instruments, and more.
Sensitivity to children's readiness to learn: A Piagetian classroom does not try to speed up
development. Instead, Piaget believed that appropriate learning experiences build on
children's current thinking. Teachers watch and listen to their students, introducing
experiences that enable them to practice newly discovered schemes and that are likely to
challenge their incorrect ways of viewing the world. But teachers do not impose new skills
before children indicate they are interested and ready.
Acceptance of individual differences: Piaget's theory assumes that all children go through
the same sequence of development, but at different rates. Therefore, teachers must plan
activities for individual children and small groups rather than just for the whole class. In
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
149
addition, teachers evaluate educational progress by comparing each child to that child's own
previous development. They are less interested in how children measure up to normative
standards, or the average performance of same-age peers.
Check Your Progress 2
Spell out the significant aspects of the Piaget’s preoperational stage
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
12.4 VYGOTSKY’S SOCIOCULTURAL THEORY
Piaget's emphasis on language as a source of cognitive development brought on yet
another challenge, this time from Vygotsky's sociocultural theory, which stresses the social
context of cognitive development. During early childhood, rapid growth in language
broadens preschoolers' ability to participate in social dialogues with more knowledgeable individuals,
who encourage them to master culturally important tasks. Soon children start to
communicate with themselves in much the same way they converse with others. This greatly
enhances the complexity of their thinking and their ability to control their own behavior.
12.4.1 Private Speech
Watch preschoolers as they go about their daily activities, and you will see that they
frequently talk out loud to themselves. For example, as Samy worked a puzzle, he said,
"Where's the red piece? Now, a blue one. No, it doesn't fit. Try it here.'
Piaget called these utterances egocentric speech, reflecting his belief that young children
have difficulty taking the perspectives of others. For this reason, he said, their talk is often
"talk for self" in which they run off thoughts in whatever form they happen to occur,
regardless of whether a listener can understand. Piaget believed that cognitive maturity and
certain social experiences-namely, disagreements with peers-eventually bring an end to
egocentric speech. Through arguments with agemates, children repeatedly see that others
hold viewpoints different from their own. As a result, egocentric speech declines.
Vygotsky voiced a powerful objection to Piaget's conclusions. He reasoned that children
speak to themselves for self-guidance. Because language helps children think about their
mental activities and behavior and select courses of action, Vygotsky viewed it as the
foundation for all higher cognitive processes, including controlled attention, deliberate
memorization and recall, categorization, planning, problem solving, and self-reflection. As
children get older and find tasks easier, their self-directed speech is internalized as silent,
inner speech-the verbal dialogues we carry on with ourselves while thinking and acting in
everyday situations.
Children's self-directed speech is now called private speech instead of egocentric speech.
Research shows that children use more of it when tasks are difficult and they are confused
about how to proceed. Also, as Vygotsky predicted, private speech goes underground with
age, changing into whispers and silent lip movements. Finally, children who freely use
private speech during a challenging activity are more attentive and involved and do better
than their less talkative agemates.
12.4.2 Social Origins of Early Childhood Cognition
Vygotsky believed children's learning takes place within a zone of proximal
development-a range of tasks too difficult for the child to do alone but possible with the help
of others. Consider the joint activity of Samy and his mother, who assists him in putting
together a difficult puzzle.
Samy's mother keeps the puzzle within his zone of proximal development, at a
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
150
manageable level of difficulty. To do so, she engages in scaffolding-adjusting the support
offered during a teaching session to fit the child's current level of performance. When the
child has little notion of how to proceed, the adult uses direct instruction and breaks the task
into manageable units. As the child's competence increases, effective scaffolders gradually
and sensitively withdraw support, turning over responsibility to the child. Gradually, children
take the instructions and make it part of their private speech, and use that speech to organize
their independent efforts.
12.4.3 Vygotsky and Education
Piagetian and Vygotskian classrooms clearly have features in common. Both emphasize
active participation and acceptance of individual differences. Yet a Vygotskian classroom
goes beyond independent discovery. It promotes assisted discovery. Teachers guide children's
learning with explanations, demonstrations, and verbal prompts, carefully tailoring their
efforts to each child's zone of proximal development. Assisted discovery is also helped along
by peer collaboration. Teachers group together classmates of differing abilities and
encourage them to teach and help one another.
Vygotsky saw make-believe play as the ideal social context for fostering cognitive
development in early childhood. As children create imaginary situations, they learn to follow
internal ideas and social rules rather than their immediate impulses. For example, a child
pretending to go to sleep follows the rules of bedtime behavior. Another child imagining
himself to be a father and a doll to be a child conforms to the rules of parental behavior.
According to Vygotsky, make-believe play is a unique, broadly influential zone of proximal
development in which children tryout a wide variety of challenging activities and acquires
many new competencies.
12.5 ERIKSON'S THEORY: INITIATIVE VERSUS GUILT
Erikson described early childhood as a period of "vigorous unfolding." Once children
have a sense of autonomy, they become less contrary than they were as toddlers. Their
energies are freed for tackling the psychological conflict of the preschool years: initiative
versus guilt. As the word initiative suggests, young children have a new sense of
purposefulness. They are eager to tackle new tasks, join in activities with peers, and discover
what they can do with the help of adults. And they also make strides in conscience
development.
Erikson regarded play as a central means through which young children find out about
themselves and their social world. Play permits preschoolers to tryout new skills with little
risk of criticism and failure. It also creates a small social organization of children who must
cooperate to achieve common goals. Around the world, children act out family scenes and
highly visible occupations-police officer, doctor, and nurse.
It is known Erikson's theory builds on Freud's psychosexual stages. In Freud's wellknown
Oedipus and Electra conflicts, to avoid punishment and maintain the affection of
parents, children form a superego, or conscience, by identifying with the same-sex parent.
That is, they take the parent's characteristics into their personality and, as a result, adopt the
moral and gender-role standards of their society. Each time the child disobeys standards of
conscience, painful feelings of guilt occur.
For Erikson, the negative outcome of early childhood is an overly strict superego that
causes children to feel too much guilt because they have been threatened, criticized, and
punished excessively by adults. When this happens, preschoolers' exuberant play and bold
efforts to master new tasks break down.
Although Freud's Oedipus and Electra conflicts are no longer regarded as satisfactory
explanations of conscience development, Erikson's image of initiative captures the diverse
changes in young children's emotional and social lives. The preschool years are, indeed, a
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
151
time when children develop a confident self-image, more effective control over their emotions,
new social skills, the foundations of morality, and a clear sense of themselves as boy or
girl.
Check Your Progress 3
Discuss the Vygotsky's sociocultural theory with reference to language development.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
12.6 CHILD REARING AND EMOTIONAL AND SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT
Parents can foster children's competence-through warmth and sensitivity to children's
needs, by serving as models and reinforcers of mature behavior, by using reasoning and
inductive discipline, and by guiding and encouraging children's mastery of new skills.
12.6.1 Child-Rearing Styles
Child-rearing styles are combinations of parenting behaviors that occur over a wide range
of situations, creating an enduring child-rearing climate. Information on child rearing by
watching parents interact with their preschoolers reveal three features that consistently
differentiate an authoritative style from less effective styles: (1) acceptance and involvement,
(2) control, and (3) autonomy granting.
Table 1 Features of Child Rearing styles
Child-Rearing
Styles
Acceptance and
Involvement
Control Autonomy Granting
Authoritative
Authoritarian
Permissive
Uninvolved
Is warm, attentive,
and sensitive to the
child's needs
Is cold and rejecting
a n d f r e q u e n t l y
degrades the child
Is warm but
o v e r i n d u l g e n t o r
inattentive
I s e m o t i o n a l l y
d e t a c h e d a n d
withdrawn
Makes reasonable
demands for maturity,
a n d c o n s i s t e n tly
enforces and explains
them
Makes many demands
coercively by yelling,
commanding, and
criticizing
Makes few or no
demands
Makes few or no
demands
Permits the child to
make decisions
in accord with
readiness
Makes decisions f o r
the child Rarely listens
to the child's point of
view
Permits the child to
make many decisions
before the child is
ready
Is indifferent to the
child's decision making
and point of view
Table 1 shows how child-rearing styles differ in these features. Let's discuss each style in
turn.
12.6.2 Authoritative Child Rearing. The authoritative style - the most successful approach
to child rearing-involves high acceptance and involvement, adaptive control techniques, and
appropriate autonomy granting. Authoritative parents are warm, attentive, and sensitive to
their child's needs. They establish an enjoyable, emotionally fulfilling parent-child relationship
that draws the child into close connection. At the same time, authoritative parents
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
152
exercise firm, reasonable control; they insist on mature behavior and give reasons for their
expectations. Finally, authoritative parents engage in gradual, appropriate autonomy granting,
allowing the child to make decisions in areas where he is ready to make choices. Throughout
childhood and adolescence, authoritative parenting is linked to many aspects of competence,
such as upbeat mood, self-control, task persistence, cooperativeness, high self-esteem, social
and moral maturity, and favorable school performance.
12.6.3 Authoritarian Child Rearing. Parents who use an authoritarian style are low in
acceptance and involvement, high in coercive control, and low in autonomy granting.
Authoritarian parents appear cold and rejecting; they frequently degrade their child by putting
her down. To exert control, they yell, command, and criticize. "Do it because I said so!" is the
attitude of these parents. If the child disobeys, authoritarian parents resort to force and
punishment. In addition, they make decisions for their child and expect the child to accept
their word in an unquestioning manner. If the child does not, authoritarian parents resort to
force and punishment. Children of authoritarian parents are anxious and unhappy. When
interacting with peers, they tend to react with hostility when frustrated. Boys, especially show
high rates of anger and defiance. Girls are dependent, lacking in exploration, and
overwhelmed by challenging tasks.
12.6.4 Permissive Child Rearing The permissive style of child rearing is warm and
accepting. But rather than being involved, such parents are overindulging or inattentive.
Permissive parents engage in little control of their child's behavior. And instead of gradually
granting autonomy, they allow children to make many of their own decisions at an age when
they are not yet capable of doing so. Their children can eat meals and go to bed when they
feel like it and watch as much television as they want. They do not have to learn good
manners or do any household chores. Children of permissive patents are impulsive, disobedient,
and rebellious. They are also overly demanding and dependent on adults, and they show
less persistence on tasks than children whose parents exert more control. The link between
permissive parenting and dependent, non achieving behavior is especially strong for boys
12.6.5 Uninvolved Parenting. T h e uninvolved style combines low acceptance and
involvement with 1ittle control and general indifference to autonomy granting. Often these
parents are emotionally detached and depressed and so overwhelmed by life stress that they
have little time and energy for children. At its extreme, uninvolved parenting is a form of
child maltreatment called neglect. Especially when it begins early, it disrupts virtually all
aspects of development, including attachment, cognition, and emotional and social skills.
12.6.6 What Makes Authoritative Child Rearing So Effective?
Authoritative child rearing seems to create an emotional context for positive parental
influence. First, warm, involved parents who are secure in the standards they hold for their
children provide models of caring concern as well as confident, self-controlled behavior.
Second, control that appears fair and reasonable to the child, not arbitrary, is far more likely
to be complied with and internalized. Finally, authoritative parents make demands and
engage in autonomy granting that fits with their children's ability to take responsibility for
their own behavior. As a result, these parents let children know that they are competent
individuals who can do things successfully for themselves, thereby fostering high self-esteem
and cognitive and social maturity.
12.6.7 Cultural Variations
Ethnic groups often have distinct child-rearing beliefs and practices. For example,
compared with Native Americans, Chinese adults describe their parenting as more
controlling. They are more directive in teaching and scheduling their children's time, as a way
of fostering self-control and high achievement. In most instances, Chinese parents combine
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
153
control with high warmth. But when control becomes coercive, it is harmful in Chinese as
well as Western cultures. A great many factors contribute to good parenting: personal
characteristics of the child and parent, SES, access to extended family and community
supports, cultural values and practices, and public policies.
12.6.8 Child Maltreatment
Child maltreatment is as old as human history, but only recently has there been
widespread acknowledgement that the problem exists and research aimed at understanding it.
Perhaps public concern has increased because child maltreatment is especially common in
large industrialized nations. There are many children who were identified as victims. Because
most cases go unreported, the true figures could be high.
Child maltreatment takes the following forms:
Physical abuse: assaults on children that produce pain, cuts, welts, bruises, burns, broken
bones, and other injuries
Sexual abuse: sexual comments, fondling, intercourse, and other forms of exploitation
Neglect: living conditions in which children do not receive enough food, clothing,
medical attention, or supervision
Psychological abuse: Failure of caregivers to meet children's needs for affection and
emotional support, and actions-such as ridicule, humiliation, or terrorizing-that damage
children's cognitive, emotional, or social functioning
Psychological and sexual abuses are the most destructive forms. The rate of psychological
abuse may be the highest, since it accompanies most other types. About 10 percent of
confirmed maltreatment victims are sexually abused. And here again, many more children are
affected, but they are too frightened to seek help or are pressured into silence. Although
children of all ages are targets of sexual abuse, the largest numbers of victims are identified
in middle childhood.
12.6.9 Origins of Child Maltreatment
Early it was believed that child maltreatment was rooted in adult psychological
disturbance. But it soon became clear that although child maltreatment is more common
among disturbed parents, a single "abusive personality type" does not exist. Sometimes even
"normal" parents harm their children! Also, parents who were abused as children do not
necessarily repeat the cycle with their own children.
To understand child maltreatment, we now turn to ecological systems theory. The
interacting variables-at the family, community, and cultural levels-promote child abuse and
neglect.
The Family. Within the family, certain children-those whose characteristics make them more
of a challenge to rear-are more likely to become targets of abuse. These include premature or
very sick babies and children who are temperamentally difficult, are inattentive and
overactive, or have other developmental problems. But whether such children actually are
maltreated depends on parents' characteristics.
Maltreating parents are less skillful than other parents in handling discipline
confrontations. They also suffer from biased thinking about their child. Abusive parents react
to stressful situations with high emotional arousal. At the same time, low income, unemployment,
marital conflict, overcrowded living conditions, frequent moves, and extreme
household disorganization are common in abusive homes.
The Community. The majority of abusive and neglectful parents are isolated from both
formal and informal social supports. This social isolation has at least two causes. First,
because of their own life histories, many of these parents have learned to mistrust and avoid
others. They do not have the skills necessary for establishing and maintaining positive
relationships with friends and relatives. Second, maltreating parents are more likely to live in
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
154
unstable, run-down neighborhoods that provide few links between family and community,
such as parks, child-care centers, preschool programs, recreation centers, and churches. For
these reasons, they lack "lifelines" to others and have no one to turn to for help during
stressful times.
The Larger Culture. Cultural values, laws, and customs profoundly affect the chances that
child maltreatment will occur when parents feel overburdened. Societies that view violence
as an appropriate way to solve problems set the stage for child abuse. The Supreme Court has
given directions against the right of school officials to use corporal punishment. Some
countries have policies or laws that prohibit physical punishment of children, rates of child
abuse are low.
12.6.10 Consequences of Child Maltreatment
The family circumstances of maltreated children impair the development of emotional
self-regulation, empathy and sympathy, self concept, social skills, and academic motivation.
Over time, these youngsters show serious learning and adjustment problems, including
academic failure, severe depression, aggressive behavior, peer difficulties, substance abuse,
and delinquency. The home lives of abused children abound with opportunities to learn to use
aggression as a way of solving problems.
The demeaning parental messages, in which children are ridiculed, humiliated, rejected,
or terrorized, result in low self-esteem, high anxiety, self-blame, aggression, and efforts to
escape from extreme psychological pain-at times severe enough to lead to attempted suicide
in adolescence. At school, maltreated children are serious discipline problems. Their
noncompliance, poor motivation, and cognitive immaturity interfere with academic
achievement an outcome that further undermines their chances for life success.
12.6.11 Preventing Child Maltreatment
Because child maltreatment is embedded in families, communities, and society as a
whole, efforts to prevent it must be directed at each of these levels. Many approaches have
been suggested, including interventions that teach high-risk parents effective child-rearing
strategies, high school child development courses that include direct experience with
children, and broad social programs aimed at bettering economic conditions for low-SES
families.
Parents Anonymous, an organization that has as its main goal helping child-abusing
parents learn constructive parenting practices, does so largely through social supports. It
offers self-help group meetings, daily phone calls, and regular home visits to relieve social
isolation and teach responsible child-rearing skills. Even with intensive treatment, some
adults persist in their abusive acts. When parents are unlikely to change their behavior, taking
the drastic step of separating parent from child and legally terminating parental rights is the
only reasonable course of action.
Check Your Progress 4
A. State the Child-rearing styles as combinations of parenting behaviors.
B. List the types of Child maltreatment and its origin.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
12.7 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Motor development during early child hood
ii) Piaget's theory of Preoperational Stage
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
155
iii) Vygotsky's Sociocultural Theory
iv) Erikson’s Theory of initiative versus guilt
v) Child Rearing and emotional and social development.
12.8 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. The motor skills development during early child hood could include:
i) Gross Motor Development
ii) Fine Motor Development
iii) Individual Differences in Motor Skills
2. The significant aspects of Piaget’s pre operational stage are:
i) Advances in Mental Representation
ii) Development of Make-Believe play
iii) Egocentrism
iv) Inability to Conserve
v) Lack of Hierarchical Classification
3. Vygotsky's sociocultural theory points at:
i) Private Speech
ii) egocentric speech
iii) zone of proximal development
iv) scaffolding
v) assisted discovery
vi) peer collaboration
4. A. Child-rearing styles are:
i) Authoritative Child Rearing
ii) Authoritarian Child Rearing
iv) Permissive Child Rearing
v) Uninvolved Parenting
B. The types of Child maltreatment and its origin are:
i) Physical abuse
ii) Sexual abuse
a. Neglect
b. Psychological abuse
Origin
i) The Family
ii) The Community
iii) The Larger Culture
12.9 Lesson – End Activities
1. Explain the Educational Implications of Vygotsky’s Socio Cultural Theory.
2. Describe about the origins of Child Mal treatment.
12.10 References
1. Kohlberg, L., and Turiel, E., Research and Moral Development : A Cognitive
Developmental Approach, New York: Wiley, 1971.
2. Levin, M.J., Psychology : A Biographical Approach, New York: McGraw – Hill, 1978.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
156
LESSON – 13
SELF-UNDERSTANDING - PEER RELATIONS - FOUNDATIONS OF MORALITY -
GENDER TYPING
Contents
13.0 Aims and Objectives
13.1 Introduction
13.2 Self understanding
13.3 Peer Relations
13.3.1 Advances in Peer Sociability
13.3.2 First Friendships
13.4 Foundations of Morality
13.4.1 The Psychoanalytic Perspective
13.4.2 Social Learning Theory
13.4.3 The Cognitive-Developmental Perspective
13.4.4 The Other Side of Morality: Development of Aggression
13.5 Gender Typing
13.5.1 Gender-Stereotyped Beliefs and Behavior
13.5.2 Genetic Influences on Gender Typing
13.5.3 Environmental Influences on Gender Typing
13.5.4 Gender Identity
13.5.5 Reducing Gender Stereotyping in Young Children
13.6 Let Us Sum Up
13.7 Check your Progress
13.8 Lesson – End Activities
13.9 References
13.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help understand the self understanding, peer relations, development of
morality and gender identity during early child hood.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
x) Denote the self-concept development
xi) Discuss the kind of peer relations children get into
xii) State the emphasizes of different theories on the aspect of morality
xiii) Mention the process of developing gender roles
13.1 INTRODUCTION
During early childhood, new powers of representation permit children to reflect on
themselves. Language enables them to talk about the, I – self - their own subjective experience
of being. As the, I -self becomes more firmly established, children focus more
intently on the, me-self-knowledge and evaluation of the self's characteristics. They start to
develop a self-concept, the set of attributes, and values that an individual believes defines
who he or she is.
13.2 SELF UNDERSTANDING
Foundations of Self-Concept: The preschoolers mention observable characteristics, such
as their name, physical appearance, possessions, and everyday behaviors these indicate selfconcepts
are very concrete.
By age 3 1/2, preschoolers also describe themselves in terms of typical emotions and
attitudes, as in "I'm happy when I play with my friends" or "I don't like being with grownups".
This suggests that they have some awareness of their unique psychological
characteristics. As further support for this budding grasp of personality, when given a trait
label, such as "shy" or "mean;' 4-year-olds infer appropriate motives and feelings. But
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
157
preschoolers do not refer directly to traits when describing themselves. This capacity must
wait for greater cognitive maturity.
The adult-child conversations about personally experienced events contribute to the
development of an autobiographical memory. Parents often use these discussions to impart
evaluative information about the child's actions, as when they say, "You were a big boy when
you did that!" Consequently, these narratives serve as a rich source of early self-knowledge
and, as the Cultural Influences, as a major means through which the young child's selfconcept
is imbued with cultural values.
13.2.1 Emergence of Self-Esteem
Another aspect of self-concept emerges in early childhood: Self-esteem, t h e
judgments we make about our own worth and the feelings associated with those judgments.
Self esteem ranks among the most important aspects of self-development, since evaluations
of our own competencies affect our emotional experiences, future behavior, and long-term
psychological adjustment.
By age 4, preschoolers have several self-esteems, such as learning things well in school,
trying hard at challenging tasks, making friends, and treating others kindly. However, their
understanding is not as differentiated as that of older children and adults. And usually they
rate their own ability as extremely high and underestimate the difficulty of tasks.
High self-esteem contributes greatly to preschoolers' initiative during a period in which
they must master many new skills. Nevertheless, by age 4, some children give up easily when
faced with a challenge, such as working a hard puzzle or building a tall block tower. They are
discouraged after failure and conclude that they cannot do the task.
13.3 PEER RELATIONS
As children become increasingly self-aware, more effective at communicating, and
better at understanding the thoughts and feelings of others, their skill at interacting with peers
improves rapidly. Peers provide young children with learning experiences they can get in no
other way. Because peers interact on an equal footing, they must assume responsibility for
keeping a conversation going, cooperating, and setting goals in play. With peers, children
form friendships-special relationships marked by attachment and common interests. Let's
look at how peer interaction changes over the preschool years.
13.3.1 Advances in Peer Sociability
The peer sociability among 2- to 5-year-olds, when noticed had a dramatic rise with age
in joint, interactive play. It can be concluded that social development proceeds in a three-step
sequence. It begins with nonsocial activity-unoccupied, onlooker behavior and solitary play.
Then it shifts to parallel play, in which a child plays near other children with similar
materials but does not try to influence their behavior. At the highest level are two forms of
true social interaction. One is associative play, in which children engage in separate
activities, but they exchange toys and comment on one another’s behaviour. The other is
cooperative play, a more advanced type of interaction in which children orient toward a
common goal, such as acting out a make believe theme or building a sand castle.
13.3.1a Recent Evidence on Peer Sociability. Play forms emerge in the order suggested by
Parten, but they do not form a developmental sequence in which later-appearing ones replace
earlier ones. Instead, all types coexist during the preschool years. Furthermore, although
nonsocial activity declines with age, it is still the most frequent form among 3to 4-year-olds.
Even among kindergartners it continues to take up as much as a third of children’s free-play
time. Also, solitary and parallel play remain fairly stable from 3 to 6 years, accounting for as
much of the young child's playas highly social, cooperative interaction.
It is the type, rather than the amount, of solitary and parallel play that changes during
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
158
early childhood. Often parents wonder if a preschooler who spends much time playing alone
is developing normally. Only certain kinds of nonsocial activity - aimless wandering,
hovering near peers, and functional play involving immature, repetitive motor action-are
cause for concern. Children who behave in these ways usually are temperamentally inhibited
preschoolers who have not learned to regulate their high social fearfulness. Often their
parents have overprotected them rather than encouraged them to approach other children.
13.3.1b Cultural Variations. Peer sociability in collectivist societies takes different forms
than in individualistic cultures. For example, children in India generally play in large groups
that require high levels of cooperation. Much of their behavior during sociodramatic play and
early games is imitative, occurs in unison, and involves close physical contact.
Cultural beliefs about the importance of play also affect early peer associations. Adults
who view play as mere entertainment is less likely to provide props and encourage pretend
than those who value its cognitive and social benefits.
13.3.2 First Friendships
As preschoolers interact, first friendships form that serve as important contexts for
emotional and social development. The word friendship means to most is a mutual
relationship involving companionship, sharing, understanding of thoughts and feelings, and
caring for one another in times of need. In addition, mature friendships endure over time and
survive occasional conflicts.
Preschoolers understand something about the uniqueness of friendship. They know that a
friend is someone "who likes you" and with whom you spend a lot of time playing. Yet their
ideas about friendship are far from mature. Four to 7-year-olds regard friendship as
pleasurable play and sharing of toys. As yet, friendship does not have a long-term, enduring
quality based on mutual trust.
Nevertheless, interactions between young friends are unique. Preschoolers give twice as
much reinforcement, in the form of greetings, praise, and compliance, to children they identify
as friends, and they also receive more from them. Friends are also more emotionally
expressive-talking, laughing, and looking at each other more often-than non friends.
Furthermore, early childhood friendships offer social support..
13.3.3 Parental Inf1uences on Early Peer Relations
It is within the family that children first acquire skills for interacting with peers. Parents
influence children's peer sociability both directly, through attempts to influence children's
peer relations, and indirectly, through their child-rearing practices and play behaviors.
13.3.3.a Direct Parental Influences. Preschoolers whose parents frequently arrange informal
peer play activities tend to have larger peer networks and be more socially skilled. In
providing play opportunities, parents show children how to initiate their own peer contacts.
Parents also offer guidance on how to act toward others. Their skillful suggestions for solving
peer problems-such as managing conflict, discouraging teasing, and entering a play group-are
associated with preschoolers' social competence and peer acceptance.
13.3.3.b Indirect Parental Influences. Many parenting behaviors are aimed at promoting
peer sociability but nevertheless spill over into peer relations. For example, secure
attachments to parents are linked to more responsive, harmonious peer interactions. The
emotionally expressive and supportive communication that contributes to attachment security
may be responsible. H ighly involved, emotionally positive parent-child conversations and
play were linked to children's pro social behavior and positive peer relations.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
159
Check Your Progress 1
State how peer interaction changes over the preschool years?
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
13.4 FOUNDATIONS OF MORALITY
If you watch children's behavior and listen in on their conversations, you will find many
examples of their developing moral sense. By age 2, they react with distress to acts that are
aggressive or that otherwise might do harm, and they use words to evaluate behavior as
"good” or "bad".
Throughout the world, adults take note of this budding capacity to distinguish right from
wrong. Some cultures have special words for it. By the end of early childhood, children can
state a great many moral rules, such as "You're not supposed to take things without asking" or
"Tell the truth!” In addition, they argue over matters of justice, as when they say, "You sat
there last time, so it's my turn;' or, "It's not fair. He got more!”
All theories of moral development recognize that conscience begins to take shape in early
childhood. And most agree that at first, the child's morality is externally controlled by adults.
Gradually, it becomes regulated by inner standards. Truly moral individuals do not just do
the right thing when authority figures are around. Instead, they have developed a
compassionate concern for others and principles of good conduct, which they follow in a
wide variety of situations.
Although points of agreement exist among major theories, each emphasizes a different
aspect of morality. Psychoanalytic theory stresses the emotional side of conscience
development in particular, identification and guilt as motivators of good conduct. Social
learning theory focuses on moral behavior and how it is learned through reinforcement and
modeling. And the cognitive-developmental perspective emphasizes thinking - children's
ability to reason about justice and fairness.
13.4.1 The Psychoanalytic Perspective
According to Freud, young children form a superego, or conscience, by identifying with
the same-sex parent, whose moral standards they adopt. Children obey the superego to avoid
guilt, a painful emotion that arises each time they are tempted to misbehave. Moral
development, Freud believed, is largely complete by 5 to 6 years of age, at the end of the
phallic stage.
A special type of discipline called induction, which helps the child notice feelings by
pointing out the effects of the child's misbehavior on others, supports conscience formation.
For example, a parent might say, "If you keep pushing him he'll fall down and cry;' or, "She
feels so sad because you won't give back her doll". As long as the explanation matches the
child's capacity to understand, induction is effective with children as young as 2 years of age.
Preschoolers whose parents use it are more likely to make up for their misdeeds, and they
more often display prosocial behavior.
Furthermore, Freud's theory places a heavy burden on parents, who must ensure through
their disciplinary practices that children develop an internalized conscience. Although good
discipline is crucial, children's characteristics can affect the success of parenting techniques.
Freud was correct that guilt is an important motivator of moral action. Inducing empathy
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
160
- based guilt (expressions of personal responsibility and regret, such as "I'm sorry I hurt
him") by explaining how the child's behavior is harmful is a way to influence children
without using coercion. Empathy-based guilt reactions are consistently associated with
stopping harmful actions, repairing damage, and acting prosocially. But guilt is not the only
force that compels us to act morally. And contrary to what Freud believed, moral
development is not complete by the end of early childhood. Instead, it is a far more gradual
process, extending into adulthood.
13.4.2 Social Learning Theory
Social learning theory does not regard morality as a special human activity with a unique
course of development. Instead, moral behavior is acquired just like any other set of
responses through reinforcement and modeling.
13.4.2.a The Importance of Modeling. Operant conditioning - following up children's good
behavior with reinforcement in the form of approval, affection, and other rewards-is not
enough for children to acquire moral responses. For a behavior to be reinforced, it must first
occur spontaneously. Yet many prosocial acts, such as sharing, helping, or comforting an
unhappy playmate, do not occur often enough at first for reinforcement to explain their rapid
development in early childhood. Instead, social learning theorists believe that children largely
learn to behave morally through modeling-by observing and imitating people who
demonstrate appropriate behavior. Once children acquire a moral response, such as sharing or
telling the truth, reinforcement in the form of praise increases its frequency.
Many studies show that models who behave helpfully or generously increase young
children's prosocial responses. The following characteristics of models affect children's
willingness to imitate:
A. Warmth and responsiveness. Preschoolers are more likely to copy the prosocial actions of
an adult who is warm and responsive than those of an adult who is cold and distant. Warmth
seems to make children more attentive and receptive to the model, and it is itself an example
of a prosocial response.
B. Competence and power. Children admire and therefore tend to select competent, powerful
models to imitate the reason they are especially willing to copy the behavior of older peers
and adults.
C. Consistency between assertions and behavior. When models say one thing and do
another-for example, announce that "it's important to help others" but rarely engage in helpful
acts-children generally choose the most lenient standard of behavior that adults demonstrate
Models are most influential during the preschool years. At the end of early childhood,
children with a history of consistent exposure to caring adults tend to behave prosocially regardless
of whether a model is present. By that time, they have internalized prosocial rules
from repeated observations of and encouragement by others.
13.4.2b Effects of Punishment. Many parents are aware that yelling at, slapping, or spanking
children for misbehavior are ineffective disciplinary tactics. Sharp reprimands or physical
force to restrain or move a child is justified when immediate obedience is necessary-for
example, when a 3-year-old is about to run into the street. In fact, parents are most likely to
use forceful methods under these conditions. When they wish to foster long-term goals, such
as acting kindly toward others, they tend to rely on warmth and reasoning. And parents often
combine power assertion with reasoning in response to very serious transgressions, such as
lying and stealing.
When used frequently, however, punishment promotes only momentary compliance, not
lasting changes in children's behavior. The more physical punishment children experience,
the more likely they are to display depression, antisocial behavior, and poor academic performance
in the future. Harsh punishment has undesirable side effects. First, when parents
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
161
spank, they often do so in response to children's aggression. Yet the punishment itself models
aggression! Second, children who are frequently punished soon learn to avoid the punishing
adult. As a result, those adults have little opportunity to teach desirable behaviors.
13.4.2c Alternatives to Harsh Punishment. Alternatives to criticism, slaps, and spankings
can reduce the undesirable side effects of punishment. A technique called time out involves
removing children from the immediate setting-for example, by sending them to their roomsuntil
they are ready to act appropriately. Time out is useful when a child is out of control. It
usually requires only a few minutes to change behavior, and it also offers a "cooling off"
period for angry parents. Another approach is withdrawal of privileges, such as playing
outside or watching a favorite TV program.
When parents decide to use punishment, they can increase its effectiveness in three ways.
The first is consistency. Permitting children to act inappropriately on some occasions but
scolding them on others confuses children, and the unacceptable act persists. Second, a warm
parent-child relationship is vital. Children of involved, caring parents find punishment
especially unpleasant and want to regain parental warmth and approval as quickly as possible.
Finally, explanations help children recall the misdeed and relate it to expectations for future
behavior.
13.4.2d Positive Discipline. The most effective forms of discipline encourage good conductby
building a positive relationship with the child, offering models of appropriate behavior,
letting children know ahead of time how to act, and praising them when they behave well.
When preschoolers have positive and cooperative relationships with parents, they show
firmer conscience development-in the form of responsible behavior, fair play in games, and
consideration for others' welfare. These outcomes continue into the school years. Parent-child
closeness leads children to want to heed parental demands because children feel a sense of
commitment to the relationship.
Parents who use positive discipline also reduce opportunities for misbehavior. For
example, on a long car trip, they bring along back-seat activities that relieve restlessness and
boredom. At the supermarket, they engage preschoolers in conversation and encourage them
to assist with shopping. Adults who help children acquire acceptable behaviors that they can
use to replace forbidden acts greatly reduce the need for punishment.
13.4.3 The Cognitive-Developmental Perspective
The psychoanalytic and behaviorist approaches to morality focus on how children acquire
ready-made standards of good conduct from adults. In contrast, the cognitive-developmental
perspective regards children as active thinkers about social rules. As early as the preschool
years, children make moral judgments, deciding what is right or wrong on the basis of
concepts they construct about justice and fairness.
Three-year-olds know that a child who intentionally knocks a playmate off a swing is
worse than one who does so accidentally. By age 4, children can tell the difference between
truthfulness and lying. By the end of early childhood, children consider a person's intentions
in evaluating lying.
Furthermore, preschoolers distinguish moral imperatives, which protect people's rights
and welfare, from two other types of action: social conventions, or customs such as table
manners and dress styles; and matters of personal choice, which do not violate rights and are
up to the individual. Three-year-olds judge moral violations (stealing an apple) as more
wrong than social-conventional violations (eating ice cream with fingers).
How do young children arrive at these distinctions? According to cognitivedevelopmental
theorists, they do so by actively make sense of their experiences. They
observe that after a moral offense, peers react emotionally, describe their own injury or loss,
tells another child to stop, or retaliate. And an adult who intervenes is likely to call attention
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
162
to the rights and feelings of the victim. In contrast, peers seldom react to violations of social
convention. And in these situations, adults tend to demand obedience without explanation or
point to the importance of obeying rules or keeping order.
13.4.4 The Other Side of Morality: Development of Aggression
Beginning in late infancy, all children display aggression from time to time as they
become better at identifying sources of anger and frustration. By the early preschool years,
two forms of aggression emerge. The most common instrumental aggression in which
children want an object, privilege, or space, and in trying to get it, they push, shout at, or
otherwise attack a person who is in the way. The other type, hostile aggression, is meant to
hurt another person.
Hostile aggression comes in two varieties. The first is overt aggression, which harms
others through physical injury or the threat of such injury-for example, hitting, kicking, or
threatening to beat up a peer. The second is relational aggression, which damages another’s
person relationships, as in social exclusion or rumor spreading. "Go away, I'm not your
friend!" and "Don't play with Mary, she's bad" are examples.
Both the form of aggression and the way it is expressed change during early childhood.
Physical aggression is gradually replaced by verbal aggression. And instrumental aggression
declines as preschoolers learn to compromise over possessions. In contrast, hostile outbursts
rise over early and middle childhood. Older children are better able to recognize malicious
intentions and, as a result, more often retaliate in hostile ways.
On average, boys are more overtly aggressive than girls, a trend that appears in many
cultures. The sex difference is due in part to biology-in particular, to male sex hormones, or
androgens. Androgens contribute to boys' greater physical activity, which may increase their
opportunities for physically aggressive encounters.
13.4.4a The Family as Training Ground for Aggressive Behavior. "I can't control him,
he's impossible;' complained Nalini, Rajesh's mother, to Leela one day. When Leela asked if
Rajesh might be troubled by something going on at home, she discovered that Rajesh's
parents fought constantly and resorted to harsh, inconsistent discipline. The same childrearing
practices that undermine moral internalization-love withdrawal, power assertion, physical
punishment, and inconsistency-are linked to aggression from early childhood through
adolescence, in children of both sexes. Compared with siblings in typical families, preschool
siblings who have critical, punitive parents are more verbally and physically aggressive to
one another. Destructive sibling conflict, in turn, contributes to poor impulse control and
antisocial behavior by the early school years. Highly aggressive children tend to be rejected
by peers, to fail in school, and (by adolescence) to seek out deviant peer groups, which lead
them toward violent delinquency and adult criminality.
13.4.4b Television and Aggression. The television programs between 6 A.M. and 11 P.M.
contain violent scenes, often in the form of repeated aggressive acts that go unpunished. In
fact, most TV violence does not show victims experiencing any serious harm, and few
programs condemn violence or depict other ways of solving problems. Of all TV programs,
children's cartoons are the most violent.
Young children are especially likely to be influenced by television. One reason is that
below age 8, children do not understand a great deal of what they see on TV. Because they
have difficulty connecting separate scenes into a meaningful story line, they do not relate the
actions of a TV character to motives or consequences. Young children also find it hard to
separate true-to-life from fantasized television content. Not until age 7 do they fully realize
that fictional characters do not retain the same roles in real life. These misunderstandings
increase young children's willingness to uncritically accept and imitate what they see on TV.
Furthermore, television violence "hardens" children to aggression, making them more
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
163
willing to tolerate it in others. Heavy TV viewers believe that there is much more violence
and danger in society, an effect that is especially strong for children who perceive televised
aggression as relevant to their own lives. As these responses indicate, violent television
modifies children's attitudes toward social reality so they increasingly match what children
see on TV.
13.4.4c Helping Children and Parents Control Aggression. Treatment for aggressive
children must begin early, before their antisocial behavior becomes so well practiced that it is
difficult to change. Breaking the cycle of hostilities between family members and replacing it
with effective interaction styles is crucial. Parents can participate in social problem-solving
training. Common conflicts, discuss effective and ineffective ways of resolving them, and
practice successful strategies. Children who receive such training show gains in social
competence.
Check Your Progress 4
State the theories of moral development.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
13.5 GENDER TYPING
The process of developing gender roles or gender linked preferences and behaviors
valued by the larger society, is called gender typing. Early in the preschool years, gender
typing is well under way. Children tended to play and form friendships with peers of their
own sex. Girls spent more time in the housekeeping, art, and reading corners, whereas boys
gathered more often in spaces devoted to blocks, woodworking, and active play.
The same theories that provide accounts of morality have been used to explain
gender-role development. Social learning theory, with its emphasis on modeling and
reinforcement, a n d cognitive-developmental theory, with its focus on children as active
thinkers about their social world, is major current approaches. Consequently, a third
perspective that combines elements of both, called gender schema theory, has gained favor.
13.5.1 Gender-Stereotyped Beliefs and Behavior
Even before children can label their own sex consistently, they stereotype their play
world. When shown pairs of gender stereotyped toys (vehicles and dolls), 18-month-olds look
longer at one stereotyped for their own gender. As soon as gender categories are established,
children sort out what they mean in terms of activities and behavior. Preschoolers associate
many toys, articles of clothing, tools, household items, games, occupations, and even colors
(pink and blue) with one sex as opposed to the other. And their actions fall in line with their
beliefs-not only in play preferences but in personality traits as well. We have already seen
that boys tend to be more active, assertive, and overtly aggressive, whereas girls tend to be
more fearful, dependent, compliant, emotionally sensitive, and relationally aggressive.
13.5.2 Genetic Influences on Gender Typing
The sex differences just described appear in many cultures around the world. Certain of
them-the preference for same-sex playmates as well as male activity level and overt
aggression and female warmth and sensitivity-are also widespread among mammalian
species. According to an evolutionary perspective, the adult life of our male ancestors was
oriented toward competing for mates, that of our female ancestors toward rearing children.
Therefore, males became genetically primed for dominance and females for intimacy and
responsiveness. Evolutionary theorists claim that family and cultural forces can affect the
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
164
intensity of biologically based sex differences, leading some individuals to be more gendertyped
than others. But experience cannot eradicate those aspects of gender typing that served
adaptive functions in human history.
13.5.3 Environmental Influences on Gender Typing
A wealth of evidence reveals that family influences, encouragement by teachers and
peers, and examples in the broader social environment combine to promote the vigorous
gender typing of early childhood.
13.5.3a The Family Beginning at birth, parents hold different perceptions and expectations
of their sons and daughters. Many parents state that they want their children to play with
"gender-appropriate" toys, and they also believe that boys and girls should be reared
differently. Parents are likely to describe achievement, competition, and control of emotion as
important for sons and warmth, "ladylike" behavior, and closely supervised activities as
important for daughters.
These beliefs carry over into actual parenting practices. Parents give toys that stress
action and competition (such as guns, cars, tools, and footballs) to boys. They give toys that
emphasize nurturance, cooperation, and physical attractiveness (dolls, tea sets, jewelry, and
jump ropes) to girls. Parents also actively reinforce independence in boys and closeness and
dependency in girls. For example, they react more positively when a son plays with cars and
trucks, demands attention, or tries to take toys from others. In contrast, they more often direct
play activities, provide help, encourage participation in household tasks, and refer to
emotions when interacting with a daughter. Furthermore, mothers more often label emotions
when talking to girls, thereby teaching them to "tune in" to others' feelings. In contrast, they
more often explain emotions, noting causes and consequences, to boys-an approach that
emphasizes why it is important to control the expression of emotion.
13.5.3b Teachers. Besides parents, teachers encourage children's gender typing. Several
times, Leslie caught herself responding in ways that furthered gender segregation and
stereotyping in her classroom. One day, she called out, "Will the girls line up on one side and
the boys on the other?" Then, as the class became noisy, she pleaded, "Boys, I wish you'd
quiet down like the girls!"
As at home, girls get more encouragement to participate in adult-structured activities at
preschool. They can frequently be seen clustered around the teacher, following directions in
an activity. In contrast, boys more often choose areas of the classroom where teachers are
minimally involved. As a result, boys and girls engage in very different social behaviors.
Compliance and bids for help occur more often in adult-structured contexts, whereas
assertiveness, leadership, and creative use of materials appear more often in unstructured
pursuits.
13.5.3c Peers. Children's same-sex peer groups strengthen gender-stereotyped beliefs and
behavior. By age 3, same-sex peers positively reinforce one another for gender-typed play by
praising, imitating, or joining in. In contrast, when preschoolers engage in "cross-gender"
activities-for example, when boys play with dolls or girls with cars and trucks-peers criticize
them. Boys are especially intolerant of "cross-gender" play in their male companions. A boy
who frequently crosses gender lines is likely to be ignored by other boys even when he does
engage in "masculine" activities!
13.5.3d The Broader Social Environment. Although children's everyday environments have
changed to some degree, they continue to present many examples of gender-typed behaviorin
occupations, leisure activities, entertainment TV, and achievements of men and women.
Children do more than imitate the many gender-linked responses they observe. They also
start to view themselves and their environment in gender-biased ways, a perspective that can
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
165
seriously restrict their interests, experiences, and skills.
13.5.4 Gender Identity
As adults, each of us have a gender identity - an image of oneself as relatively masculine
or feminine in characteristics. By middle childhood, we can measure gender identity by
asking children to rate themselves on personality traits. A child or adult with a "masculine"
identity scores high on traditionally masculine items (such as ambitious, competitive, and
self-sufficient) and low on traditionally feminine items (such as affectionate, cheerful, and
soft-spoken). Someone with a "feminine" identity does just the reverse. Although most
people view themselves in gender-typed terms, a substantial minority (especially females)
have a gender identity called androgyny, s coring high on both masculine and feminine
characteristics.
Gender identity is a good predictor of psychological adjustment. Masculine and
androgynous children and adults have higher self-esteem, whereas feminine individuals often
think poorly of themselves, perhaps because many of their traits are not highly valued by
society. Also, androgynous individuals are more adaptable in behavior-for example, able to
show masculine independence or feminine sensitivity, depending on the situation. Research
on androgyny shows that it is possible for children to acquire a mixture of positive qualities
traditionally associated with each gender-an orientation that may best help them realize their
potential.
13.5.4.a Emergence of Gender Identity. How do children develop a gender identity?
According to social learning theory, behavior comes before self-perceptions. Preschoolers
first acquire gender-typed responses through modeling and reinforcement. Only then do they
organize these behaviors into gender-linked ideas about themselves. In contrast, cognitive
developmental theory maintains that self-perceptions come before behavior. Over the
preschool years, children first acquire a cognitive appreciation of the permanence of their sex.
They develop gender constancy, the understanding that sex is biologically based and
remains the same even if clothing, hairstyle, and play activities change. Then children use
this idea to guide their behavior.
Because many young children do not see members of the opposite sex naked, they
distinguish males and females using information they do have-the way each gender dresses
and behaves. Although preschoolers who know about genital differences usually say a doll
dressed in other-sex clothing is still the same sex, they do not justify their answer by referring
to sex as an innate, unchanging quality of people. This suggests that cognitive immaturity,
not social experience, is responsible for preschoolers' difficulty grasping the permanence of
sex.
13.5.4b Gender Schema Theory. It is an information-processing approach to gender typing
that combines social learning and cognitive-developmental features. It emphasizes that both
environmental pressures and children's cognitions work together to shape gender-role
development. At an early age, children respond to instruction from others, picking up gendertyped
preferences and behaviors. At the same time, they start to organize their experiences
into gender schemas, or masculine and feminine categories, that they use to interpret their
world. A young child who says, "Only boys can be doctors;' or, "Cooking is a girl's job;'
already has some well-formed gender schemas. As soon as preschoolers can label their own
sex, they select gender schemas consistent with it, applying those categories to themselves.
As a result, their self-perceptions become gender-typed and serve as additional schemas that
children use to process information and guide their own behavior.
13.5.5 Reducing Gender Stereotyping in Young Children
Adults can begin by eliminating gender stereotyping from their own behavior and from
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
166
the alternatives they provide for children. For example, mothers and fathers can take turns
making dinner, bathing children, and driving the family car. They can provide sons and
daughters with both trucks and dolls and pink and blue clothing. Teachers can make sure that
all children spend time in both adult-structured and unstructured activities. Also, efforts can
be made to shield children from television and other media presentations that portray rigid
gender differences.
Once children notice the vast array of gender stereotypes in their society, parents and
teachers can point out exceptions. For example, they can arrange for children to see men and
women pursuing nontraditional careers. And they can reason with children, explaining that
interests and skills, not sex, should determine a person's occupation and activities. Research
shows that such reasoning is very effective in reducing children's tendency to view the world
in a gender-biased fashion. A rational approach to child rearing promotes healthy, adaptable
functioning in many other areas as well.
Check Your Progress 3
Discuss the environmental factors that influence gender typing
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
13.6 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Self-concept development
ii) Advances in Peer Sociability
iii) Foundations of Morality development
iv) environmental factors that enable gender typing
v) Emergence of gender identity
13.7 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
5. The peer interaction changes over the preschool years involves:
vi) parallel play
vii) associative play
viii) cooperative play
ix) Cultural Variations
x) First Friendships
6. The theories of moral development are:
i) The Psychoanalytic Perspective
ii) Social Learning Theory
iii) The Cognitive-Developmental Perspective
7. The environmental factors that influence gender typing could be:
i) The Family
ii) Teachers
iii) Peers
iv) The Broader Social Environment
13.8. Lesson – End Activities
1. Mention about the concept of mortality from the psychoanalytic perspective.
2. Mention the effect of punishment.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
167
13.9 References
1. Piaget, J., The Origins of Intelligence in Children, New York : International Unviersities
Press, 1992.
2. Hurlock, E.B., Child Psychology, Fokyo : McGraw Hill, 1988.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
168
LESSON – 14
LATE CHILDHOOD - PIAGET'S THEORY: THE CONCRETE OPERATIONAL
STAGE - LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT - LEARNING IN SCHOOL. ERIKSON'S
'THEORY: INDUSTRY VERSUS INFERIORITY
Contents
14.0 Aims and Objectives
14.1 Introduction
14.2 Characteristics of Late Childhood
14.2.1 Names Used by Parents
14.2.2 Name Used by Educators
14.2.3 Names Used by Psychologists
14.3 Piaget's Theory: The Concrete Operational Stage
14.3.1 Achievements of the Concrete Operational Stage
14.3.2 Limitations of Concrete Operational Thought
14.3.3 Evaluation of the Concrete Operational Stage
14.4 Language Development
14.4.1 Vocabulary
14.4.2 Grammar
14.4.3 Pragmatics
14.4.4 Learning Two Languages at a Time
14.5 Learning in School
14.5.1 Class Size
14.5.2 Educational Philosophies
14.5.3 Teacher-Student Interaction
14.5.4 Teaching Children with Special Needs
14.6 Erikson's theory: Industry versus Inferiority
14.6 Let Us Sum Up
14.7 Check your Progress
14.8 Lesson – End Activities
14.9 References
14.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help understand the characteristics and language development during
late childhood.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
xiv) understand the various names used by parents, teachers and psychologists
xv) mention the Piaget's concrete operational stage
xvi) list development of language developments that takes place
xvii) discuss the significance of school in learning
xviii) compare industry versus inferiority as stated by Erikson
14.1 INTRODUCTION
Late childhood extends from the age of six years to the time the individual becomes
sexually mature. At both its beginning and end, late childhood is marked by conditions that
profoundly affect a child's personal and social adjustments.
During the last year or two of childhood, marked physical changes take place and these,
also, are responsible for changes in attitudes, values, and behavior as this period draws to a
close and children prepare, physically and psychologically, for adolescence. The physical
changes that take place at the close of childhood bring about a state of disequilibrium in
which the accustomed pattern of life is disturbed and there is a temporary upset until adjust-
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
169
ments to the changes can be made.
Although it is possible to mark off the beginning of late childhood fairly accurately, one
cannot be so precise about the time this period comes to an end because sexual maturity-the
criterion used to divide childhood from adolescence-comes at varying ages.
This is because there are marked variations in the ages at which boys and girls become
sexually mature. As a result, some children have a longer-than average late childhood, while
for others it is shorter than average. For the average girl, late childhood extends from six to
thirteen, a span of seven years; for boys, it extends from six to fourteen, a span of eight years.
14.2 CHARACTERISTICS OF LATE CHILDHOOD
Parents, educators, and psychologists apply various names to late childhood and these
names reflect the important characteristics of the period.
14.2.1 Names Used by Parents
To many parents, late childhood is the troublesome age-the time when children are no longer
willing to do what they are told to do and when they are more influenced by their peers than
by their parents and other family members. Because most older children, especially boys, are
careless and irresponsible about their clothes and other material possessions, parents regard
late- childhood as the sloppy age-the time when children tend to be careless and slovenly
about their appearance and when their rooms are so cluttered that it is almost impossible to
get into them. Even when there are strict family rules about grooming and care of
possessions, few older children adhere to these rules unless parents demand that they do so
and threaten them with punishment.
In families where there are brothers and sisters, it is common for the boys of the family
to pick on the girls and to ridicule them-a pattern of behavior that comes from their
association with peers outside the home. When girls retaliate, quarrels ensue in which there is
much name-calling or actual physical attacks. Because of this common pattern of behavior in
families where there are siblings of both sexes, late childhood is regarded by many parents as
the quarrelsome age-the time when family fights are common and when the emotional
climate of the home is far from pleasant for all family members.
14.2.2 Name Used by Educators They call late childhood the elementary school age. It is
the time when the child is expected to acquire the rudiments of knowledge that are considered
essential for successful adjustment to adult life. It is also the time when the child is expected
to learn certain essential skills, both curricular and extracurricular.
Educators also regard late childhood as a critical period in the achievement drive-a time
when Children form the habit of being achievers, underachievers or overachievers. Once
formed, habits of working below, above, or up to one's capacity tend to persist into
adulthood. It has been reported that the level of achievement behavior in childhood is highly
correlated with achievement behavior in adulthood.
14.2.3 Names Used by Psychologists To the psychologist, late childhood is the gang age-the
time when children's major concern is acceptance by their agemates and membership in a
gang, especially a gang with prestige in the eyes of their age-mates. Because of this absorbing
concern, children are willing to conform to group-approved standards in terms of appearance,
speech, and behavior. This has led psychologists to label late childhood as the age of
conformity.
Recent studies of creativity have shown that older children, if unhampered by
environmental restraints, by criticism, or by ridicule from adults or peers, will turn their
energies into creative activities. As a result, psychologists label late childhood the creative
age, the time in the life span when it will be determined whether children will become
conformists or producers of new and original work. While the foundations for creative
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
170
expressions are laid in early childhood, the ability to use these foundations for original
activities is generally not well developed before children reach the late childhood years.
Late childhood is frequently called the play age by psychologists, not because more time
is devoted to play than at any other age-which would be impossible after the child enters
school-but rather because there is an overlapping of play activities characteristic of the
younger years and those characteristic of adolescence. It is thus the breadth of play interest
and activities rather than the time spent in play that is responsible for giving the name play
age to late childhood.
14.3 PIAGET'S THEORY: THE CONCRETE OPERATIONAL STAGE (285)
When Leela insisted that the amount of water had changed after it had been poured
from a tall, narrow container into a short, wide one at an earlier age, now at age 8, when
Leela returned, these tasks were easy. "Of course it's the same," she exclaimed. "The water's
shorter but it's also wider. Pour it back," she instructed "You'll see, it's the same amount!"
14.3.1 Achievements of the Concrete Operational Stage
Leela has entered Piaget's concrete operational stage, which spans the years from 7 to
11. During this period, thought is far more logical, flexible, and organized than it was during
early childhood.
Conservation. The ability to pass conservation tasks provides clear evidence of
operations--mental actions that obey logical rules. Notice how Leela is capable of
decentration, focusing on several aspects of a problem and relating them, rather than
centering on just one. Leela also demonstrates reversibility, the capacity to think through
a series of steps and then mentally reverse direction, returning to the starting point. It is
solidly achieved in late childhood.
Classification. Between ages 7 and 10, children pass Piaget's class inclusion problem.
This indicates that they are more aware of classification hierarchies and can focus on
relations between a general category and two specific categories at the same time-that is,
three relations at once. You can see this in children's play activities. Collections -stamps,
coins, baseball cards, rocks, bottle caps, and more-become common in late childhood.
Seriation. The ability to order items along a quantitative dimension, such as length or
weight, is called seriation. To test for it, Piaget asked children to arrange sticks of
different lengths from shortest to longest. Older preschoolers can create the series, but
they do so haphazardly. They put the sticks in a row but make many errors. In contrast, 6-
to 7-year-olds are guided by an orderly plan. They create the series efficiently by
beginning with the smallest stick, then moving to the next largest, and so on, until the
ordering is complete.
The concrete operational child can also seriate mentally, ability called transitive
inference. In a well-known transitive inference problem, Piaget showed children pairings
of differently colored sticks. From observing that Stick A is longer than Stick B and Stick
B is longer than Stick C, children must make the mental inference that A is longer than C.
Notice how this task, like Piaget's class inclusion task, requires children to integrate three
relations at once-in this instance, A-B, B-C, and A-C. About half of 6-year-olds perform
well on such problems, and performance improves considerably around age 8.
Spatial Reasoning. Piaget found that school-age youngsters have a more accurate
understanding of space. Let's take two examples: understanding of directions and maps.
Directions. When asked to name an object to the left or right of another person, 5- and 6-
year-olds answer incorrectly; they apply their own frame of reference. Between 7 and 8
years, children start to perform mental rotations, in which they align the self's frame to
match that of a person in a different orientation. As a result, they can identify left and
right for positions they do not occupy. Around 8 to 10 years, children can give clear,
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
171
well-organized directions for how to get from one place to another by using a "mental
walk" strategy in which they imagine another person's movements along a route. Without
special prompting, 6-year-olds focus on the end point without describing exactly how to
get there.
Maps. Children's drawings of familiar large-scale spaces, such as their neighborhood or
school, also change from early to late childhood. Preschoolers and young school-age children
display landmarks on the maps they draw, but their placement is fragmented. When
asked to place stickers showing the location of desks and people on a map of their classroom,
they perform better. But if the map is rotated relative to the orientation of the
classroom, they have difficulty placing the stickers accurately.
During the school years, children's maps become more organized. They draw landmarks
along an organized route of travel-an attainment that resembles their improved direction
giving. By the end of late childhood, children combine landmarks and routes into an overall
view of a large-scale space. And they readily draw and read maps when the orientation of the
map and the space it represents do not match.
14.3.2 Limitations of Concrete Operational Thought
Although school-age children are far more capable problem solvers than they were during
the preschool years, concrete operational thinking suffers from one important limitation.
Children think in an organized, logical fashion only when dealing with concrete information
they can perceive directly. Their mental operations work poorly with abstract ideas-ones not
apparent in the real world.
Piaget used the term horizontal decalage (meaning development within a stage) to
describe the g r adual mastery of logical concepts. The horizontal decalage is another
indication of the concrete operational child's difficulty with abstractions. School-age children
do not come up with the general logical principles and then apply them to all relevant
situations. Instead, they seem to work out the logic of each problem separately.
14.3.3 Evaluation of the Concrete Operational Stage
Piaget was correct that school-age youngsters approach a great many problems in
systematic and rational ways not possible in early childhood. But whether this difference
occurs because of continuous improvement in logical skills or discontinuous restructuring of
children's thinking (as Piaget's stage idea assumes) is an issue that prompts much
disagreement. Many researchers think that both types of change may be involved. From early
to late childhood, children apply logical schemes to many more tasks. Yet in the process,
their thought seems to undergo qualitative change-toward a comprehensive grasp of the
underlying principles of logical thought. Piaget himself seems to have recognized this
possibility in the very concept of the horizontal decalage. So perhaps some blend of Piagetian
and information-processing ideas holds the greatest promise for understanding cognitive
development in late childhood.
Check Your Progress 1
A. State the various names put forth during late childhood.
B. List the achievements of the concrete operational stage.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
172
14.4 LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT
Vocabulary, grammar, and pragmatics continue to develop in late childhood, although
less obviously than at earlier ages. In addition, school-age children's attitude toward language
undergoes a fundamental shift. They develop language awareness.
14.4.1 Vocabulary
As their knowledge becomes better organized, school-age children think about and use
words more precisely. Word definitions offer examples of this change. Five- and 6-year-olds
give concrete descriptions that refer to functions or appearance-for example, knife: "when
you're cutting carrots"; bicycle: "it's got wheels, a chain, and handlebars:' By the end of elementary
school, synonyms and explanations of categorical relationships appear-for example,
knife: "something you could cut with. A saw is like a knife. It could also be a weapon". This
advance reflects the older child's ability to deal with word meanings on an entirely verbal
plane. Older children can add new words to their vocabulary simply by being given a
definition.
School-age children's more reflective and analytical approach to language permits them
to appreciate the multiple meanings of words. For example, they appreciate that many words,
such as "cool" or "neat;' have psychological as well as physical meanings: "What a cool
shirt!" or "That movie was really neat!" These grasp of double meanings permits 8- to 10-
year-olds to comprehend subtle metaphors, such as "sharp as a tack" and "spilling the beans".
It also leads to a change in children's humor. Riddles and puns that go back and forth between
different meanings of a key word are common.
14.4.2 Grammar
During the school years, mastery of complex grammatical constructions improves. For
example, English-speaking children use the passive voice more frequently, and it expands
from an abbreviated structure ("It broke") into full statements ("The glass was broken by
Mary"). Although the passive form is challenging, language input makes a difference.
Another grammatical achievement of late childhood is advanced understanding of
infinitive phrases, such as the difference between "John is eager to please" and "John is easy
to please". Like gains in vocabulary, appreciation of these subtle grammatical distinctions is
supported by an improved ability to analyze and reflect on language.
14.4.3 Pragmatics
Improvements in pragmatics, the communicative side of language, also take place.
Children adapt to the needs of listeners in challenging communicative situations, such as
describing one object among a group of very similar objects. Whereas preschoolers tend to
give ambiguous descriptions, such as "the red one;' school-age children are much more
precise. They might say, "The round red one with stripes on it"
Conversational strategies also become more refined. For example, older children are
better at phrasing things to get their way. When faced with an adult who refuses to hand over
a desired object, 9-year-olds, but not 5-year-olds, state their second requests more politely.
School age children are also more sensitive than preschoolers to distinctions between what
people say and what they mean.
14.4.4 Learning Two Languages at a Time
Throughout the world, many children grow up bilingual, learning two languages, and
sometimes more than two, during childhood.
Bilingual Development. Children can become bilingual in two ways: (1) by acquiring
both languages at the same time in early childhood, or (2) by learning a second language
after mastering the first. Children of bilingual parents who teach them both languages in
early childhood show no special problems with language development. They acquire
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
173
normal native ability in the language of their surrounding community and good to native
ability in the second language, depending on their exposure to it. When children acquire a
second language after they already speak a first language, they generally take 3 to 5 years
to become as fluent as native-speaking age mates. Children who are fluent in two languages
do better than others on tests of selective attention, analytical reasoning, concept
formation, and cognitive flexibility. Also, bilingual children are advanced in ability to
reflect on language. They are more aware that words are arbitrary symbols, more
conscious of language structure and sounds, and better at noticing errors of grammar and
meaning-capacities that enhance reading achievement.
Bilingual Education. The advantages of bilingualism provide strong justification for
bilingual education programs in schools. In Tamil Nadu, where both Tamil and English
are official languages. Educators committed to truly bilingual education-developing
children's native language while fostering mastery of English. Providing instruction in the
native tongue lets children know that their heritage is respected. In addition, it prevents
semilingualism, or inadequate proficiency in both languages. When children gradually
lose the first language as a result of being taught the second, they end up limited in both
languages for a time, a circumstance that leads to serious academic difficulties.
Semilingualism is believed to contribute to high rates of school failure and dropout
among low-SES youngsters.
Check Your Progress 2
Mention the language development during late childhood.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
14.5 LEARNING IN SCHOOL
Schools are vital forces in children's cognitive development. Schools exert a powerful
influence on children. Looking at schools as complex social systems-at their class size,
educational philosophies, teacher student interaction patterns, and larger cultural context -
provides important insights.
14.5.1 Class Size
Class size influences children's learning. Small class students- scored higher in reading
and math achievement and continued to do so after they returned to regular-size classes.
Placing teacher's aides in regular-size classes had no impact. Instead, consistently being in
small classes from kindergarten through third standard predicted substantially higher
achievement from fourth through ninth standards.
Why is small class size beneficial? Teachers of fewer children spend less time
disciplining and more time giving individual attention, and children's interactions with one
another are more positive and cooperative. Also, children who learn in smaller groups show
better concentration and higher-quality class participation and express more favorable
attitudes toward school.
14.5.2 Educational Philosophies
Each teacher brings to the classroom an educational philosophy that plays a major role in
children's learning. Two philosophical approaches have received the most research attention.
They differ in what children are taught, the way they are believed to learn, and how their
progress is evaluated.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
174
Traditional versus Open Classrooms. In a traditional classroom, the teacher is the sole
authority for knowledge, rules, and decision making and does most of the talking.
Students are relatively passive, listening, responding when called on, and completing
teacher-assigned tasks. Their progress is evaluated by how well they keep pace with a
uniform set of standards for their grade.
In an open classroom, students are viewed as active agents in their own development.
The teacher assumes a flexible authority role, sharing decision making with students, who
learn at their own pace. Students are evaluated in relation to their own prior development;
comparisons with same-age students are less important. A glance inside an open
classroom reveals richly equipped learning centers, small groups of students working on
tasks they choose themselves, and a teacher responding to individual needs.
New Philosophical Directions. The philosophies of some teachers fall in between
traditional and open. They want to foster high achievement as well as critical thinking,
positive social relationships, and excitement about learning. Approaches to elementary
education, grounded in Vygotsky's sociocultural theory, represent this point of view.
Vygotsky's emphasis on the social origins of higher cognitive processes has inspired the
following educational themes:
Teachers and children as partners in learning. A classroom rich in both teacher-child
and child-child collaboration transfers culturally valued ways of thinking to children.
Experiences with many types of symbolic communication in meaningful activities. As
children master reading, writing, and quantitative reasoning, they become aware of
their culture's communication systems, reflect on their own thinking, and bring it
under voluntary control.
Teaching adapted to each child's zone of proximal development. Assistance that is
responsive to current understandings but that encourages children to take the next step
helps ensure that each child makes the best progress possible.
14.5.3 Teacher-Student Interaction
Elementary school students describe good teachers as caring, helpful, and stimulatingcharacteristics
positively associated with learning. Yet with respect to stimulation, a disappointing
finding is that teachers emphasize rote, repetitive drill more than higher-level
thinking, such as grappling with ideas and applying knowledge to new situations.
Of course, teachers do not interact in the same way with all children. Well-behaved, highachieving
students typically get more encouragement and praise, whereas unruly students are
often criticized and rarely called on in class discussions. When they seek special help or
permission, their requests are usually denied.
Unfortunately, once teachers' attitudes toward students are established, they are in danger
of becoming more extreme than is warranted by children's behavior. Of special concern are
educational self-fulfilling prophecies: Children may adopt teachers' positive or negative
views and start to live up to them. This effect is especially strong when teachers emphasize
competition and publicly compare children.
When teachers hold inaccurate views, poorly achieving students are more affected. Highachieving
students have less room to improve when teachers think well of them, and they can
fall back on their history of success when a teacher is critical. Low-achieving pupils'
sensitivity to self-fulfilling prophecies enhances their school performance when teachers
believe in them. But unfortunately, biased teacher judgments are usually slanted in a negative
direction, so only rarely do poor achievers have a chance to reap these benefits.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
175
14.5.4 Teaching Children with Special Needs
We have seen that effective teachers flexibly adjust their teaching strategies to
accommodate pupils with a wide range of characteristics. But such adjustments are
increasingly difficult at the very low and high ends of the ability distribution. The
requirement changes to serve children with special learning needs.
Children with Learning Difficulties. I n mainstreaming, s t u dents with learning
difficulties are placed in regular class rooms for part of the school day, a practice
designed to prepare them better for participation in society. Largely due to parental
pressures, mainstreaming has been extended to full inclusion –placement in regular
classrooms full time.
Some main streamed students are mildly mentally retarded – children who’s IQs fall
between 55 and 70 and who also show problems in adaptive behavior, or skills of
everyday living. But the largest number-5 to 10 percent of school-age children have
learning disabilities, great difficulty with one or more aspects of learning, usually reading.
As a result, their achievement is considerably behind what would be expected on the basis
of their IQ. The problems of these students cannot be traced to any obvious physical or
emotional difficulty or to environmental disadvantage. Instead, subtle deficits in brain
functioning seem to be involved. In most instances, the cause is unknown.
Placement of these children in regular classes at providing appropriate academic
experiences as well as integrated participation in classroom life is not positive. Although
some main streamed and fully included students benefit academically, many do not.
Achievement gains depend on both the severity of the disability and the support services
available in the regular classroom. Furthermore, children with disabilities are often
rejected by regular-classroom peers. Students with mental retardation are overwhelmed
by the social skills of their classmates; they cannot interact quickly or adeptly in a
conversation or game. And the processing deficits of some children with learning disabilities
lead to problems in social awareness and responsiveness.
These children do best when they receive instruction in a resource room for part of the
day and in the regular classroom for the remainder-an arrangement that the majority of
school-age children with learning disabilities can have. In the resource room, a special
education teacher works with students on an individual and small group, basis. Then,
depending on their progress, children join regular classmates for different subjects and
amounts of time.
Once children enter the regular classroom, special steps must to be taken to promote peer
acceptance. Cooperative learning experiences in which children with learning difficulties
and their classmates work together lead to friendly interaction and improved social
acceptance. Teachers can also prepare children for the arrival of a student with special
needs-a process best begun early, before children have become less accepting of peers
with disabilities
Gifted Children. Some children are gifted, displaying exceptional intellectual strengths.
Their characteristics are diverse. In every grade, there could be one or two students with
IQ scores above 130, the standard definition of giftedness based on intelligence test
performance. High-IQ children are particularly quick at academic work. They have keen
memories and an exceptional capacity to solve challenging academic problems. Yet
recognition that intelligence tests do not sample the entire range of human mental skills
has led to an expanded conception of giftedness in schools.
Creativity and Talent. Creativity is the ability to produce work that is original yet
appropriate-something others have not thought of that is useful in some way. High
potential for creativity can result in a child being designated as gifted. Tests of creative
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
176
capacity tap divergent thinking-the generation of multiple and unusual possibilities
when faced with a task or problem. Divergent thinking contrasts sharply with convergent
thinking, which involves arriving at a single correct answer and is emphasized on
intelligence tests.
Extreme giftedness often results in social isolation. The highly driven, nonconforming,
and independent styles of many gifted children and adolescents lead them to spend more
time alone, partly because of their rich inner lives and partly because solitude is necessary
for them to develop their talents. Still, gifted children desire gratifying peer relationships
and some-girls more often than boys-try to hide their abilities to become better liked.
Compared with their ordinary agemates, gifted youths, especially girls, report more emotional
and social difficulties, including low self-esteem and depression. Many talented
youths become experts in their fields, few become highly creative. The skill involved in
rapidly mastering an existing field is not the same as innovating in that field. The world,
however, needs both experts and creators.
Educating the Gifted. Although programs for the gifted exist in many schools, debate
about their effectiveness usually focuses on factors irrelevant to giftedness-whether to provide
enrichment in regular classrooms, to pull children out for special instruction (the
most common practice), or to advance brighter students to a higher grade. Children of all
ages fare well academically and socially within each of these models. Yet the extent to
which they foster creativity and talent depends on opportunities to acquire relevant skills.
Gardner's theory of multiple intelligences has inspired several model programs that
provide enrichment to all students. Meaningful activities, each tapping a specific intelligence
or set of intelligences, serve as contexts for assessing strengths and weaknesses and,
on that basis, teaching new knowledge and original thinking.
14.6 Erikson's theory: Industry versus Inferiority
According to Erikson, the personality changes of the school years build on Freud's
latency stage. Although Freud's theory is no longer widely accepted, children whose
experiences have been positive enter late childhood with the calm confidence Freud intended
by the term latency. Their energies are redirected from the make-believe of early childhood
into realistic accomplishment.
Erikson believed that the combination of adult expectations and children's drive toward
mastery sets the stage for the psychological conflict of late childhood: industry versus
inferiority, which is resolved positively when experiences lead children to develop a sense of
competence at useful skills and tasks. In cultures everywhere, improved physical and
cognitive capacities mean that adults impose new demands. Children, in turn, are ready to
meet these challenges and benefit from them.
In industrialized nations, the transition to late childhood is marked by the beginning of
formal schooling. With it comes literacy training, which prepares children for the vast array
of specialized careers in complex societies. In school, children become aware of their own
and others' unique capacities, learn the value of division of labor, and develop a sense of
moral commitment and responsibility. The danger at this stage is inferiority, reflected in the
sad pessimism of children who have little confidence in their ability to do things well. This
sense of inadequacy can develop when family life has not prepared children for school life or
when experiences with teachers and peers are so negative that they destroy children's feelings
of competence and mastery.
Erikson's sense of industry combines several developments of late childhood: a positive
but realistic self-concept, pride in accomplishment, moral responsibility, and cooperative
participation with agemates.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
177
Check Your Progress 3
State the factors that cater to learning in school.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
14.7 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Motor development during early child hood
ii) Piaget's theory of Preoperational Stage
vi) Vygotsky's Sociocultural Theory
vii) Erikson’s Theory of initiative versus guilt
viii) Child Rearing and emotional and social development.
14.8 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
8. A. The various names put forth during late childhood could include:
i) troublesome age
ii) sloppy age
iii) quarrelsome age
iv) elementary school age
v) gang age
vi) play age
B. Achievements of the concrete operational stage are:
i.Conservation
ii.decentration
iii.Classification
iv.Seriation
v.Spatial Reasoning
9. The language development during late childhood includes:
i) Vocabulary,
ii) Grammar
iii) pragmatics
iv) Bilingual Development
10. The factors that cater to learning in school
i) Class Size
ii) Educational Philosophies
iii) Teacher-Student Interaction
iv) Teaching Children with Special Needs
14.9 Lesson – End Activities
1. Highlight the characteristics of creative children.
2. What type of student – teacher relationship you feel as important for effective learning.
14.10 References
1. Peel, E.A. The Psychological Basis of Education, London : Oliver and Boyd, 1970.
2. Skinner, C.E. (Ed.) Educations Psychology Englewood Clifts, New Jersey : prentice – Hall
1960.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
178
LESSON – 15
SELF- UNDERSTANDING – EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT - PEER RELATIONS -
GENDER TYPING - FAMILY INFLUENCES - PROBLEMS OF DEVELOPMENT
Contents
15.0 Aims and Objectives
15.1 Introduction
15.1.1 Changes in Self-Concept
15.1.2 Development of Self-Esteem
15.2 Emotional Development
15.2.1 Self-Conscious Emotions
15.2.2 Emotional Understanding
15.2.3 Emotional Self-Regulation
15.3 Peer Relations
15.3.1 Peer Groups
15.3.2 Friendships
15.3.3 Peer Acceptance
15.4 Gender Typing
15.4.1 Gender-Stereotyped Beliefs
15.4.2 Gender Identity and Behavior
15.4.3 Cultural Influences on Gender Typing
15.5 Family Influences
15.5.1 Parent-Child Relationships
15.5.2 Siblings
15.5.3 Only Children
15.5.5 Blended Families
15.5.6 Maternal Employment and Dual-Earner Families
15.6 Problems of Development
15.6.1 Fears and Anxieties
15.6.2 Child Sexual Abuse
15.6.3 Fostering Resiliency in Late childhood
15.7 Let Us Sum Up
15.8 Check your Progress
15.9 Lesson – End Activities
15.10 References
15.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help understand the self understanding, emotional development, peer
relations, gender and family influences during late child hood.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
xix) List the emotional factors
xx) State the significance of peer relations
xxi) Mention the gender identity development
xxii) Understand the family influence on development
xxiii) Discuss the problems encountered by the child
15.1 INTRODUCTION
Several transformations in self-understanding take place in late childhood. First,
children can describe themselves in terms of psychological traits. Second, they start to
compare their own characteristics with those of their peers. Finally, they speculate about the
causes of their strengths and weaknesses. These new ways of thinking about the self have a
major impact on children's self-esteem.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
179
15.1.1 Changes in Self-Concept
During the school years, children develop a much more refined me-self, or self-concept,
organizing their observations of behaviors and internal states into general dispositions, with a
major change taking place between ages 8 and 11.
Instead of specific behaviors, this child emphasizes competencies, as in "I'm a very good
violinist". Also, they could clearly describe their personality and mentions both positive and
negative traits-"truthful" but "not pretty”. Older school-age children are far less likely than
younger children to describe themselves in all-or-none ways. A major reason for these
qualified self-descriptions is that school-age children often make Social comparisons,
judging their appearance, abilities and behaviour in relation to those of others, Although 4- to
6-year-olds can compare their own performance to that of one peer, older children can
compare multiple individuals, including themselves.
There are factors responsible for these revisions in self concept. Cognitive development
affects the changing structure of the self--children's ability to combine typical experiences
and behaviors into psychological dispositions. The content of self-concept is a product of
both cognitive capacities and feedback from others. Sociologist George Herbert Mead
proposed that a well-organized psychological self emerges when the child's I-self adopts a
view of the me-self that resembles others' attitudes toward the child. Mead's ideas indicate
that perspective-taking skills- in particular, an improved ability to infer what other people are
thinking-are crucial for the development of a self-concept based on personality traits. During
the school years, children become better at "reading" the messages they receive from others
and incorporating these into their self-definitions. As school-age children internalize others'
expectations, they form an ideal self that they use to evaluate their real self. A large discrepancy
between the two can greatly undermine self-esteem, leading to sadness, hopelessness,
and depression.
15.1.2 Development of Self-Esteem
Most preschoolers have very high self-esteem. As children move into late childhood, they
receive much more feedback about their performance in different activities compared with
that of their peers. As a result, self-esteem differentiates, and it also adjusts to a more realistic
level.
15.1.2a A Hierarchically Structured Self-Esteem. When asked children to indicate the
extent to which a variety of statements, such as, "1 am good at homework" or "I'm usually the
one chosen for games;' are true of themselves. Answers reveal that by age 6 to 7, children
have formed at least four self-esteems-academic competences, social competence,
physical/athletic competence, and physical appearance-that become more refined with age.
Furthermore, the capacity to view the self in terms of stable dispositions permits school-age
children to combine their separate self-evaluations into a general psychological image of
themselves-an overall sense of self-esteem. As a result, self-esteem takes on the hierarchical
structure.
15.1.2b Changes in Level of Self-Esteem. As children evaluate themselves in various areas,
self-esteem drops during the first few years of elementary school. Typically, this decline is
not great enough to be harmful. Most (but not all) children appraise their characteristics and
competencies realistically while maintaining an attitude of self-acceptance and self-respect.
Then, from fourth to sixth standard, self-esteem rises for the majority of youngsters, who feel
especially good about their peer relationships and athletic capabilities.
15.1.3 Influences on Self-Esteem
From late childhood on, strong relationships exist between self-esteem and everyday
behavior. Academic self esteem predicts children's school achievement. Children with high
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
180
social self-esteem are better liked by peers. Boys come to believe they have more athletic
talent than girls, and they are more advanced in a variety of physical skills. Furthermore, a
profile of low self-esteem in all areas is linked to anxiety, depression, and increasing
antisocial behavior. There are social influences that might lead self esteem to be high for
some children and low for others.
15.1.3a Culture. Cultural forces profoundly affect self-esteem. For example, an especially
strong emphasis on social comparison in school may explain why some children score lower
in self-esteem than other children, despite their higher academic achievement. In Asian
classrooms, competition is tough and achievement pressure is high. At the same time, Asian
children less often call on social comparisons to promote their own self esteem. Because their
culture values modesty and social harmony, they tend to be reserved about judging
themselves positively but generous in their praise of others.
Furthermore, a widely accepted cultural belief is that boys' overall sense of self-esteem is
higher than girls', yet the difference is small. Girls may think less well of themselves because
they internalize this negative cultural message. Children and adolescents who attend schools
or live in neighborhoods where their SES and ethnic groups are well represented feel a
stronger sense of belonging and have fewer self-esteem problems.
15.1.3b Child-Rearing Practices. Children whose parents use an authoritative child-rearing
style feel especially good about themselves. Warm, positive parenting lets children know that
they are accepted as competent and worthwhile. And firm but appropriate expectations,
backed up with explanations, seem to help children evaluate their own behavior against
reasonable standards.
When parents help or make decisions for their youngsters when they do not need
assistance, children often suffer from low self-esteem. These controlling parents
communicate a sense of inadequacy to children. And overly tolerant, indulgent parenting is
linked to unrealistically high self-esteem, which also undermines development. Children who
feel superior to others tend to lash out at challenges to their overblown self-images and to
have adjustment problems, including meanness and aggression.
15.1.3c Making Achievement-Related Attributions. Attributions are our common,
everyday explanations for the causes of behavior. Children attribute their disappointing
performance to luck and to their usual success to ability. Cognitive development permits
school-age children to separate all these variables in explaining performance. Those who are
high in academic self-esteem make mastery-oriented attributions, crediting their successes to
ability-a characteristic they can improve through trying hard and can count on when faced
with new challenges. And they attribute failure to factors that can be changed and controlled,
such as insufficient effort or a very difficult task. So whether these children succeed or fail,
they take an industrious, persistent approach to learning.
Unfortunately, children who develop learned helplessness attribute their failures, not
their successes, to ability. When they succeed, they are likely to conclude that external
factors, such as luck, are responsible. Furthermore, unlike their mastery-oriented
counterparts, they have come to believe that ability is fixed and cannot be changed by trying
hard. So when a task is difficult, these children experience an anxious loss of control-in
Erikson's terms, a pervasive sense of inferiority. They give up before they have really tried.
15.1.3d Supporting Children's Self-Esteem At times, well-intended messages from adults
undermine children's competence. Attribution retraining is an intervention that encourages
learned-helpless children to believe that they can overcome failure by exerting more effort.
Most often, children are given tasks hard enough so they will experience some failure. Then
they get repeated feedback that helps them revise their attributions, such as, "You can do it if
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
181
you try harder." Children are also taught to view their successes as due to ability and effort
rather than chance, by giving them additional feedback after they succeed, such as, "You're
really good at this." Another approach is to encourage low-effort children to focus less on
grades and more on mastering a task for its own sake. Instruction in meta cognition and selfregulation
is also helpful, to make up for learning lost in this area and to ensure that renewed
effort will payoff.
15.2 EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
Greater self-awareness and social sensitivity support emotional development in late
childhood. Gains take place in experience of self-conscious emotions, understanding of
emotional states, and emotional self-regulation.
15.2.1 Self-Conscious Emotions
In late childhood, the self-conscious emotions of pride and guilt become clearly governed
by personal responsibility. An adult need not be present for a new accomplishment to spark
pride or for a transgression to arouse guilt. Also, children do not report guilt for any mishap,
as they did at younger ages, but only for intentional wrongdoing, such as ignoring
responsibilities, cheating, or lying.
Pride motivates children to take on further challenges. And guilt prompts them to make
amends and strive for self improvement as well. But harsh, insensitive reprimands from
adults-such as, "Everyone else can do it! Why can't you?" can lead to intense shame, which is
particularly destructive. As children form an overall sense of self-esteem, they can take one
or two unworthy acts to be the whole of self-worth, setting up maladaptive responses of high
self-blame and passive retreat or intense anger at others who participated in the shameevoking
situation.
15.2.2 Emotional Understanding
School-age children's understanding of mental activity means that they are more likely to
explain emotion by referring to internal states, such as happy or sad thoughts, than to external
events-the focus of preschoolers. Older children are also more aware of the diversity of
emotional experiences. Around age 8, children recognize that they can experience more than
one emotion at a time, each of which may be positive or negative and differ in intensity. For
example, recalling the birthday present a boy received from his grandmother, it reflected, "I
was very happy that I got something but a little sad that I didn't get just what I wanted."
As with self-understanding, gains in emotional understanding are supported by cognitive
development and social experiences, especially adults' sensitivity to children's feelings and
willingness to discuss emotions. Together, these factors lead to a rise in empathy as well. As
children move closer to adolescence, advances in perspective taking permit an empathic
response not just to people's immediate distress, but also to their general life condition.
15.2.3 Emotional Self-Regulation
Rapid gains in emotional self-regulation occur in late childhood. As children engage in
social comparison and care more about peer approval, they must learn to manage negative
emotion that threatens their self-esteem.
By age 10, most children have an adaptive set of strategies for regulating emotion. In
situations where they have some control over an outcome (an anxiety-provoking test at the
end of the week), they view problem solving and seeking social support as the best strategies.
When outcomes are beyond their control (having received a bad grade), they opt for
distraction or redefining the situation ("Things could be worse. There'll be another test.").
Compared with preschoolers, school-age children more often use these internal strategies to
manage emotion, due to an improved ability to reflect on their thoughts and feelings.
When emotional self-regulation has developed well, school age children acquire a sense
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
182
of emotional self-efficacy--a feeling of being in control of their emotional experience. This
fosters a favorable self-image and an optimistic outlook, which further help children face
emotional challenges. Emotionally well-regulated children are generally upbeat in mood,
more empathic and prosocial, and better liked by their peers. In contrast, poorly regulated
children are overwhelmed by negative emotion, a response that interferes with prosocial
behavior and peer acceptance.
Check Your Progress 1
Write the transformations in self-understanding and emotions that take place in late childhood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
15.3 PEER RELATIONS
In late childhood, the society of peers becomes an increasingly important context for
development. Peer contact, as we have seen, contributes to perspective taking and
understanding of self and others. These developments, in turn, enhance peer interaction,
which becomes more prosocial over the school years. In line with this change, aggression
declines, but the drop is greatest for physical attacks.
15.3.1 Peer Groups
Watch children in the school yard or neighborhood, and notice how groups of three to a
dozen or more often gather. The organization of these collectives changes greatly with age.
By the end of late childhood, children display a strong desire for group belonging. They form
peer groups, collectives that generate unique values and standards for behavior and a social
structure of leaders and followers. Peer groups organize on the basis of proximity (being in
the same classroom) and similarity in sex, ethnicity, and popularity.
The practices of these informal groups lead to a "peer culture" that typically consists of a
specialized vocabulary, dress code, and place to "hang out" during leisure hours. Their
activities included trading baseball cards, playing video games, and-just as important-keeping
girls and adults out. Unfortunately, peer groups often direct their hostilities toward their own
members, excluding no-longer "respected" children. The cast-outs are profoundly wounded,
and many find new group ties hard to establish. Their previous behavior, including expressed
contempt for outsiders, reduces their chances of being included elsewhere. Excluded children
often turn to other low-status peers for group belonging.
15.3.2 Friendships
Whereas peer groups provide children with insight into larger social structures, one-toone
friendships contribute to the development of trust and sensitivity. During the school
years, friendship becomes more complex and psychologically based. Friendship is no longer
just a matter of engaging in the same activities. Instead, it is a mutually agreed-on
relationship in which children like each other's personal qualities and respond to one another's
needs and desires. And once a friendship forms, trust becomes its defining feature. Schoolage
children state that a good friendship is based on acts of kindness that signify that each
person can be counted on to support the other. Consequently, older children regard violations
of trust, such as not helping when others need help, breaking promises, and gossiping behind
the other's back, as serious breaches of friendship.
15.3.3 Peer Acceptance
Peer acceptance refers to likability – the extent which a child is viewed by a group of
agemates, such as classmates, as a worthy social partner. It differs from friendship in that it is
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
183
not a mutual relationship. Rather, it is a one-sided perspective, involving the group's view of
an individual. Nevertheless, some social skills that contribute to friendship also enhance peer
acceptance. Consequently, better accepted children have more friends and better relationships
with them.
Children's peer acceptance likeability reveal four different categories: popular children,
who get many positive votes; rejected children, who are actively disliked; controversial
children, who get a large number of positive and negative votes; and neglected children,
who are seldom chosen, either positively or negatively.
Peer acceptance is a powerful predictor of psychological adjustment. Rejected children,
especially, are unhappy, alienated, poorly achieving children with low self-esteem. Both
teachers and parents rate them as having a wide range of emotional and social problems. Peer
rejection in late childhood is also strongly associated with poor school performance, dropping
out, antisocial behavior, and delinquency in adolescence and with criminality in young
adulthood.
15.3.3a Determinants of Peer Acceptance. What causes one child to be liked and another to
be rejected? A wealth of research reveals that social behavior plays a powerful role.
Popular Children. Although most popular children are kind and considerate, a few are
admired for their socially adept yet belligerent behavior. The large majority are popular
prosocial children, who combine academic and social competence. They perform well in
school and communicate with peers in sensitive, friendly, and cooperative ways. In
contrast, popular-antisocial children largely consist of "tough" boys who are athletically
skilled but poor students. Although they are aggressive, their peers view them as "cool;'
perhaps because of their athletic ability and shrewd but devious social skills. Many are
low-SES minority children who have concluded that they cannot succeed academically.
Although their likability may offer some protection from future maladjustment, their poor
school performance and antisocial behavior require intervention.
Rejected Children. Rejected children display a wide range of negative social behaviors.
The largest subgroup, rejected-aggressive children, show high rates of conflict, hostility,
and hyperactive, inattentive, and impulsive behavior. They are also deficient in
perspective taking and regulation of negative emotion. For example, they tend to
misinterpret the innocent behaviours of peers as hostile, to blame others for their social
difficulties, and to act on their angry feelings. In contrast, rejected – withdrawn
children are passive and socially awkward. These timid children are overwhelmed by
social anxiety, hold negative expectations for how peers will treat them, and are very
concerned about being scorned and attacked. Because of their inept, submissive style of
interaction, rejected-withdrawn children are at risk for abuse by bullies.
Controversial and Neglected Children. Consistent with the mixed peer opinion they
engender, controversial children display a blend of positive and negative social behaviors.
Like rejected-aggressive children, they are hostile and disruptive, but they also engage in
positive, prosocial acts. Even though some peers dislike them, they have qualities that
protect them from social exclusion. As a result, they appear to be relatively happy and
comfortable with their peer relationships.
Finally, perhaps the most surprising finding is that neglected children, once thought to be
in need of treatment, are usually well adjusted. Although they engage in low rates of interaction,
the majority are just as socially skilled as average children. They do not report
feeling especially lonely or unhappy, and when they want to, they can break away from
their usual pattern of playing by themselves.
Check Your Progress 2
State the types of peer relations that are increasingly important for development
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
184
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
15.4 GENDER TYPING
Children's understanding of gender roles broadens in late childhood, and their gender-role
identities (views of themselves as relatively masculine or feminine) change as well.
15.4.1 Gender-Stereotyped Beliefs
During the school years, children extend the gender-stereotyped beliefs they acquired in
early childhood. As they think more about people as personalities, they label some traits as
more typical of one gender than the other. For example, they regard "tough;' "aggressive;'
"rational;' and "dominant" as masculine and "gentle;' "sympathetic;' and "dependent" as
feminine-stereotyping that increases steadily with age. Children derive these distinctions
from observing gender differences as well as from adult treatment. Parents, for example, use
more directive speech (telling the child what to do) with girls, less often encourage girls to
make their own decisions, and less often praise girls for accomplishment.
People can cross gender lines does not mean that children always approve of doing so.
Children and adults are fairly tolerant of girls’ violations of gender roles. But they judge boys'
violations ("playing with dolls" or "wearing a dress") harshly-as just as bad as a moral
transgression.
15.4.2 Gender Identity and Behavior
Boys' and girls' gender-role identities follow different paths in late childhood. From third
to sixth grade, boys strengthen their identification with "masculine" personality traits,
whereas girls' identification with "feminine" traits declines. Girls begin to describe
themselves as having some "other-gender" characteristics. Whereas boys usually stick to
"masculine" pursuits, girls experiment with a wider range of options. These changes are due
to a mixture of cognitive and social forces. School-age children of both sexes are aware that
society attaches greater prestige to "masculine" characteristics than "feminine" occupations.
A tomboyish girl can make her way into boys' activities without losing status with her female
peers, but a boy who hangs out with girls is likely to be ridiculed and rejected.
15.4.3 Cultural Influences on Gender Typing
Girls are less likely to experiment with "masculine" activities in cultures and subcultures in
which the gap between male and female roles is especially wide. And when social and
economic conditions make it necessary for boys to take over "feminine" tasks, their
personalities and behavior are less stereotyped. In Nyansongo, a small agricultural settlement
in Kenya, mothers work 4 to 5 hours a day in the gardens. They assign the care of young
children, the tending of the cooking fire, and the washing of dishes to older siblings. Because
children of both sexes perform these duties, girls are relieved of total responsibility for
"feminine" tasks and have more time to interact with agemates.
Check Your Progress 3
Discuss how gender roles develops during late childhood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
185
15.5 FAMILY INFLUENCES
As children move into school, peer, and community contexts the parent-child relationship
changes. At the same time, children's well-being continues to depend on the quality of family
interaction.
15.5.1 Parent-Child Relationships
In late childhood, the amount of time children spend with parents declines dramatically.
The child's growing independence means that parents must deal with new issues. Although
parents face new concerns, child rearing becomes easier for those who established an
authoritative style during the early years. Reasoning works more effectively with school-age
children because of their greater capacity for logical thinking and increased respect for
parents' expert knowledge. As children demonstrate that they can manage daily activities and
responsibilities, effective parents gradually shift control from adult to child. This does not
mean they let go entirely. Instead , they engage in coregulation, a transitional form of
supervision in which they exercise general oversight while permitting children to be in charge
of moment-by-moment decision-making.
Coregulation grows out of a cooperative relationship between parent and child-one based
on give-and-take and mutual respect. Parents must guide and monitor from a distance and
effectively communicate expectations when they are with their children. And children must
inform parents of their whereabouts, activities, and problems so parents can intervene when
necessary. Coregulation supports and protects children while preparing them for adolescence,
when they will make many important decisions themselves.
15.5.2 Siblings
In addition to parents and friends, siblings are important sources of support for school-age
children. Yet sibling rivalry tends to increase in late childhood. As children participate in a
wider range of activities, parents often compare siblings' traits and accomplishments. The
child who gets less parental affection, more disapproval, or fewer material resources is likely
to be resentful.
When siblings are close in age and the same sex, parental comparisons are more frequent,
resulting in more quarreling, antagonism, and poorer adjustment. This effect is particularly
strong when parenting is cold or harsh. It is also strengthens when fathers prefer one child.
Perhaps because fathers spend less time with children, their favoritism is more noticeable and
triggers greater anger.
Parents can reduce these effects by making an effort not to compare children. Although
conflict rises, school-age siblings continue to rely on each other for companionship and
assistance. Siblings shared daily activities; older siblings often helped younger siblings with
academic and peer challenges. And both offered one another help with family issues. Siblings
whose parents are preoccupied and less involved with them sometimes fill in and become
more supportive of one another.
15.5.3 Only Children
Contrary to popular belief, only children are not spoiled. Instead, they are as well adjusted
as other children and advantaged in some respects. Children in one-child families score
higher in self-esteem and achievement motivation. Consequently, they do better in school and
attain higher levels of education. One reason may be that only children have somewhat closer
relationships with their parents, who exert more pressure for mastery and accomplishment.
15.5.4 Divorce
Children's interactions with parents and siblings are affected by other aspects of family
life. Children of divorce spend an average of 5 years in a single parent home, or almost a
third of childhood. For many, divorce leads to new family relationships. About two-thirds of
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
186
divorced parents marry a second time. Half their children eventually experience a third major
change-the end of a parent's second marriage.
These reveal that divorce is not a single event in the lives of parents and children. Instead,
it is a transition that leads to a variety of new living arrangements, accompanied by changes
in housing, income, and family roles and responsibilities. How well children fare depends on
many factors: the custodial parent's psychological health, the child's characteristics, and social
supports within the family and surrounding community.
15.5.5 Blended Families
Life in a single-parent family is often temporary. Many parents find a new partner within
a few years. Entry into these blended, or reconstituted, families leads to a complex set of new
relationships. For some children, this expanded family network is a positive turn of events
that brings greater adult attention. But for most, it presents difficult adjustments. Stepparents
often introduce new childrearing practices, and having to switch to new rules and
expectations can be stressful. In addition, children often regard step relatives as "intruders:'
but how well they adapt is, once again, related to the overall quality of family functioning.
This often depends on which parent forms a new relationship and on the child's age and sex.
15.5.6 Maternal Employment and Dual-Earner Families
Today, single and married mothers are in the labor market in nearly equal proportions, and
more than three-fourths of those with school-age children are employed. The impact of
maternal employment on early development depends on the quality of child care and the
continuing parent-child relationship. This same conclusion applies during later years.
Maternal Employment and Child Development. Children of mothers who enjoy their
work and remain committed to parenting show very favorable adjustment-higher selfesteem,
more positive family and peer relations, less gender-stereotyped beliefs, and
better grades in school. Girls, especially, profit from the image of female competence.
Daughters of employed mothers perceive women's roles as involving more freedom of
choice and satisfaction and are more achievement - and career-oriented.
However, when employment places heavy demands on the mother's schedule, children are
at risk for ineffective parenting. Working long hours and spending little time with schoolage
children are associated with less favorable adjustment. In contrast, part-time
employment and flexible work schedules seem to have benefits for children of all ages,
probably because these arrangements prevent work family role conflict, thereby helping
parents meet children's needs.
Support for Employed Parents and Their Families. In dual-earner families, the
husband's willingness to share responsibilities helps the mother engage in effective
parenting. If the father helps very little or not at all, the mother carries a double load, at
home and at work, leading to fatigue, distress, and little time and energy for children.
Employed mothers and dual-earner parents need assistance from work settings and
communities in their child-rearing roles. Reduced work hours, flexible schedules, job
sharing, and paid leave when children are ill help parents juggle the demands of work and
child rearing. Equal pay and employment opportunities for women are also important.
Check Your Progress 4
Mention the quality of family interaction during late childhood.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
15.6 PROBLEMS OF DEVELOPMENT
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
187
There are a variety of stressful experiences that place children at risk for future problems.
In the following sections, we touch on areas of concern: school age children's fears and
anxieties and the consequences of child sexual abuse and look at the factors that help children
cope effectively with stress.
15.6.1 Fears and Anxieties
Although fears of the dark, thunder and lightning, and supernatural beings (often
stimulated by movies and television) persist into late childhood, children's anxieties are also
directed toward new concerns. As children begin to understand the realities of the wider
world, the possibility of personal harm (being robbed, stabbed, or shot) and media events
(war and disasters) often trouble them. Other common worries include academic failure,
parents' health, physical injuries, and peer rejection.
Most children handle their fears constructively, by talking about them with parents,
teachers, and friends and relying on the more sophisticated emotional self-regulation
strategies that develop in late childhood. Consequently, fears decline with age, especially for
girls, who express more fears than boys throughout childhood and adolescence.
Severe childhood anxieties may arise from harsh living conditions. In inner-city slums
and war-torn areas of the world, a great many children live in the midst of constant
deprivation, chaos, and violence.
15.6.2 Child Sexual Abuse
Until recently, child sexual abuse was viewed as a rare occurrence. When children came
forward with it, adults rarely took their claims seriously. Child sexual abuse is to b e
recognized as a serious and widespread problem.
Characteristics of Abusers and Victims. Sexual abuse is committed against children of
both sexes but more often against girls. Most cases are reported in late childhood, but
sexual abuse also occurs at younger and older ages. For some victims, the abuse begins
early in life and continues for many years.
Generally, the abuser is male -a parent or someone the parent knows well. Often it is a
father, stepfather, or live-in boyfriend, somewhat less often an uncle or older brother. In a
few instances, mothers are the offenders, more often with sons. In the overwhelming
majority of cases, the abuse is serious - vaginal or anal intercourse, oral-genital contact,
fondling, and forced stimulation of the adult. Abusers make the child comply in a variety
of distasteful ways, including deception, bribery, verbal intimidation, and physical force.
Child sexual abuse are strongly linked to poverty, marital instability, and the resulting
weakening of family ties. Children who live in homes with a history of constantly
changing characters-repeated marriages, separations, and new partners-are especially
vulnerable. But middle-SES children in stable families are also victims, although their
victimization is more likely to remain undetected.
Consequences. The adjustment problems of child sexual abuse victims are often severe.
Depression, low self-esteem, mistrust of adults, and feelings of anger and hostility can
persist for years after the abusive episodes. Younger children often react with sleep
difficulties, loss of appetite, and generalized fearfulness. Adolescents may run away and
show suicidal reactions, substance abuse, and delinquency.
15.6.3 Fostering Resiliency in Late childhood
Throughout late childhood-and other phases of development-children are confronted with
challenging and sometimes threatening situations that require them to cope with
psychological stress.
At the same time, only a modest relationship exists between stressful life experiences and
psychological disturbance in childhood. The same is true when we look at school difficulties,
family transitions, and child maltreatment. The three broad factors protect against
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
188
maladjustment: (1) the child's personal characteristics, including an easy temperament and a
mastery-oriented approach to new situations; (2) a warm, well-organized family life; and (3)
an adult outside the immediate family who offers a support system and a positive coping
model.
Anyone of these ingredients of resiliency can account for why one child fares well and
another poorly. Yet most of the time, personal and environmental factors are interconnected.
Unfavorable life experiences increase the chances that children will act in ways that expose
them to further hardship. And when negative conditions pile up, such as marital discord,
poverty, crowded living conditions, neighborhood violence, and abuse, the rate of
maladjustment multiplies.
Check Your Progress 5
Bring out the problems of development during late childhood.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
15.7 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Changes in Self-Concept
ii) Development of Self-Esteem
iii) Emotional Development
iv) Peer relations and gender typing
v) Family Influences and problems of development
15.8 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. The transformations in self-understanding and emotions are:
i) Changes in Self-Concept
ii) Development of Self-Esteem
iii) Self-Conscious Emotions
iv) Emotional Understanding
v) Emotional Self-Regulation
2. The types of peer relations that are increasingly important for development include:
i) Peer Groups
ii) Friendships
iii) Peer Acceptance
3. Gender roles development is affected by:
i) Gender-Stereotyped Beliefs
ii) Gender Identity and Behavior
iii) Cultural Influences on Gender Typing
4. Family interaction during late childhood includes:
i) Parent-Child Relationships
ii) Siblings
iii) Only Children
iv) Blended Families
v) Maternal Employment and Dual-Earner Families
5. The problems of development during late childhood
i) Fears and Anxieties
ii) Child Sexual Abuse
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
189
15.9 Lesson – End Activities
1. Explain how fear and anxiety influence the development.
2. Write short note on child abuse.
15.10 References
1. Gates, A.J. Educational Psychology, New York : McMillan, 1980.
2. Smith, M. Daniel, Educational Psychology, New York : Alton & Bacon, 1978.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
190
UNIT IV
LESSON - 16
PUBERTY - BODY CHANGES -GROWTH SPURT - CONSEQUENCES OF
ABSTRACT THOUGHT
Contents
16.0 Aims and Objectives
16.1 Introduction
16.2 Characteristics of Puberty
16.2.1 Puberty Is an Overlapping Period
16.2.2 Puberty Is a Short Period
16.2.3 Puberty Is Divided into Stages
16.2.4 Puberty Is a Time of Rapid Growth and Change
16.2.5 Puberty Is a Negative Phase
16.2.6 Puberty Occurs at a Variable Age
16.3 Criteria of Puberty
16.4 Causes of Puberty
16.5 Body Changes at Puberty
16.5.1 Changes in Body Size
16.5.2 Changes in Body Proportions
16.5.3 Primary Sex Characteristics
16.5.4 Secondary Sex Characteristics
16.6 The Puberty Growth Spurt
16.7 Consequences of Abstract Thought
16.7.1 Argumentativeness
16.7.2 Self-Consciousness and Self-Focusing
16.7.3 Idealism and Criticism
16.7.4 Planning and Decision Making
16.8 Let Us Sum Up
16.9 Check your Progress
16.10 Lesson – End Activities
16.11 References
16.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help you understand Puberty and the changes that happen during this
period.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
i) State the characteristics of puberty
ii) Mention the criteria and causes of puberty
iii) Understand the changes in the body during puberty
iv) Discuss the growth spurt
v) List the consequences of abstract thought during puberty
16.1 INTRODUCTION
Puberty is the period in the developmental span when the child changes from an asexual
to asexual being. It is the stage in development during which maturation of the sexual
apparatus occurs and reproductive capacity is attained. I t is accompanied b y changes in
somatic growth and psychological perspective.
The word puberty is derived from the Latin word pubertas, which means "age of
manhood." It refers to the physical rather than the behavioral changes which occur when the
individual becomes sexually mature and is capable of producing offspring. Most primitive
people have, for centuries, recognized puberty as a time of importance in the life span of
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
191
every individual. It is customary for them to observe various rites in recognition of the fact
that, as their bodies change, children are emerging from childhood into maturity. After
successfully passing the tests that are a significant part of all puberty rites, boys and girls are
granted the rights and privileges of adulthood and are expected to assume the responsibilities
that accompany that state.
16.2 CHARACTERISTICS OF PUBERTY
Puberty is a unique and distinctive period and is characterized by certain developmental
changes that occur at no other time in the life span. The most important of these are discussed
below.
16.2.1 Puberty is an Overlapping Period
Puberty must be regarded as an overlapping period because it encompasses the closing
years of childhood and the beginning years of adolescence. Until they are sexually mature,
children are known as "pubescents" or "pubescent children." After they become sexually
mature, they are known as "adolescents" or "young adolescents."
16.2.2 Puberty is a Short Period
Considering the many and extensive changes that take place inside the body as well as
externally, puberty is a relatively short period, lasting from two to four years. Children who
pass through puberty in two years or less are regarded as “rapid maturers," while those who
require three to four years to complete the transformation into adults are regarded as "slow
maturers." Girls, as a group, tend to mature more rapidly than boys, as a group, but there are
marked variations within each sex group.
16.2.3 Puberty Is Divided into Stages
In spite of the fact that puberty is a relatively short period in the life span, it is customary
to subdivide it into three stages-the prepubescent stage, the pubescent stage, and the
postpubescent stage. When each of these three stages normally occurs and its characteristics
are described below.
Stages of Puberty
Prepubescent Stage: This stage overlaps the closing year or two of childhood when the
child is regarded as a "prepubescent"-one who is no longer a child but not yet an
adolescent. During the prepubescent (or "maturing") stage, the secondary sex
characteristics begin to appear but the reproductive organs are not yet fully developed.
Pubescent Stage: This stage occurs at the dividing line between childhood and
adolescence; the time when the criteria of sexual maturity appear-the menarche in girls
and the first nocturnal emissions in boys. During the pubescent (or "mature") stage, the
secondary sex characteristics continue to develop and cells are produced in the sex
organs.
Postpubescent Stage: This stage overlaps the first year or two of adolescence. During
this stage, the secondary sex characteristics become well developed and the sex organs
begin to function in a mature manner.
16.2.4 Puberty is a Time of Rapid Growth and Change
Puberty is one of the two periods in the life span that are characterized by rapid growth
and marked changes in body proportions. The other is the prenatal period and the first half of
the first year of life. The latter is usually referred to as the "baby growth spurt."
The rapid growth and development that occur during puberty are generally referred to as
the "adolescent growth spurt”. More correctly, it is the “puberty growth spurt” because it
precedes slightly or occurs simultaneously with the other changes of puberty. This growth
spurt lasts for a year or two before children become sexually mature and continues for six
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
192
months to a year afterward. Thus the entire period of rapid growth lasts, for almost three
years, slightly longer than the "baby growth spurt" which lasts for less than a year and a half.
The rapid changes that take place during puberty lead to confusion, to feelings of inadequacy
and insecurity, and in many cases to unfavorable behavior.
16.2.5 Puberty is a Negative Phase
Many years ago, Charlotte Buhler labeled puberty the negative phase. The term phase
suggests a period of short duration; negative suggests that the individual takes an "anti"
attitude toward life or seems to be losing some of the good qualities previously developed.
There is evidence that negative attitudes and behavior are characteristic mainly of the
early part of puberty and that the worst of the negative phase is over when the individual
becomes sexually mature. There is also evidence that the behavior characteristic of the
"negative phase" of puberty is more pronounced in girls than in boys.
16.2.6 Puberty Occurs at a Variable Age
Puberty can occur at any time between the ages of five or six and nineteen years.
However, the average girl in the Indian culture of today becomes sexually mature at thirteen
and the average boy, a year later. There are also variations in the amount of time needed to
complete the transformation process of puberty. These range from two to four years with
girls, on the average, requiring slightly less time than boys.
Variations in the age at which puberty occurs and in the time needed to complete the
transformation process of puberty create many personal as well as social problems for both
boys and girls. It is the variations in timing of puberty rather than the changes associated with
it that make puberty one of the most difficult, even though one of the shortest, periods in the
life span.
Check Your Progress 1
Mention the Characteristics of Puberty.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
16.3 CRITERIA OF PUBERTY
The criteria most often used to determine the onset of puberty and to pinpoint a particular
stage of puberty that the child has reached are the menarche, nocturnal emissions, evidence
derived from chemical analysis of the urine and x-rays of bone development.
The menarche, or the first menstruation, is commonly used as a criterion of sexual
maturity among girls, but it is neither the first nor the last of the physical changes that occur
during puberty. When the menarche occurs, the sex organs and secondary sex characteristics
have all started to develop, but none of them have yet reached a state of maturity. The
menarche is more correctly considered a midpoint in puberty.
Among boys, a popularly used criterion of puberty is nocturnal emissions. During sleep,
the penis sometimes becomes erect, and semen, or the fluid containing sperm cells, is
released. This is a normal way for the male reproductive organ to rid itself of excessive
amounts of semen. However, not all boys experience this phenomenon, and not all realize
what it is. Furthermore, nocturnal emissions, like the menarche, occur after some puberty
development has taken place and therefore cannot be used as an accurate criterion of the
onset of puberty.
Chemical analysis of the first urine passed by boys in the morning has proved to be an
effective technique for determining sexual maturity, as has analysis of girl’s urine to see
whether the female gonadotropic hormone, estrogen, is present. However, the practical
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
193
difficulty of obtaining specimens of the early-morning urine of boys and girls limits the use
of this method.
X-rays of different parts of the body, but especially the hands and knees, during the
preadolescent growth spurt can reveal whether puberty has begun and the rate at which
puberty is progressing. To date, this has proved to be the most dependable method of
determining sexual maturity, though it, like the chemical analysis of early-morning urine,
involves certain practical difficulties that make its widespread use unfeasible.
16.4 CAUSES OF PUBERTY
Until the turn of the present century, the cause or causes of the physical changes that
occur at puberty remained a mystery. With the growth of research in the field of
endocrinology, medical science has been able to pinpoint the exact causes of these changes
though, to date, endocrinologists have been unable to explain the variations in the age of
puberty and in the time needed to complete the changes of puberty.
At the present time, it is known that about five years before children become sexually
mature; there is a small excretion of the sex hormones in both boys and girls. The amount of
hormones excreted increases as time passes and this eventually leads to the maturing of the
structure and function of the sex organs.
It has been established that there is a close relationship between the pituitary gland,
located at the base of the brain, and the gonads, or sex glands. The male gonads are the testes,
and the female gonads are ovaries. The roles they play in bringing about the changes of
puberty are described below.
16.4.1 Conditions Responsible for Puberty Changes
Role of the Pituitary Gland: The pituitary gland produces two hormones: the growth
hormone, which is influential in determining the individual's size, and the gonadotropic
hormone, which stimulates the gonads to increased activity. Just before puberty, there is a
gradual increase in the amount of the gonadotropic hormone and an increased sensitivity of
the gonads to this hormone; this initiates puberty changes.
Role of the Gonads: With the growth and development of the gonads, the sex organs-the
primary sex characteristics-increase in size-and become functionally mature, and the
secondary sex characteristics, such as pubic hair, develop.
Interaction of the Pituitary Gland and the Gonads: The hormones produced by the gonads,
which have been stimulated by the gonadotropic hormone produced by the pituitary gland,
act in turn on this gland and cause a gradual reduction in the amount of growth hormone produced,
thus stopping the growth process. The interaction between the gonadotropic hormone
and the gonads continues throughout the individual’s reproductive life, gradually decreasing
as women approach the menopause and men approach the climacteric.
16.5 BODY CHANGES AT PUBERTY
During the puberty growth spurt, four important physical changes occur which transform
the child's body into that of an adult: changes in body size, changes in body proportions, the
development of the primary sex characteristics, and the development of the-secondary sex
characteristics.
16.5.1 Changes in Body Size
The first major physical change at puberty is change in body size in terms of height and
weight. Among girls, the average annual increase in the year in the preceding the menarche is
3 inches, though a 5 to 6 –inch increase is not unusual. Two years preceding the menarche,
the average increase is 2.5 inches, making a total increase of 5.5 inches in the two years
preceding the menarche. After the menarche, the rate of growth slows down to about 1 inch a
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
194
year, coming to a standstill at around eighteen years.
For boys, the onset of the period of rapid growth in height comes, on the average, at 12.8
years and ends, on the average, at 15.3 years, with a peak occurring at fourteen years. The
greatest increase in height comes in the year following the onset of puberty. After that,
growth decelerates and continues at a slow rate until the age of twenty or twenty-one. Because
of this longer growth period, boys achieve greater height by the time they are mature
than girls do.
Weight gain during puberty comes not only from an increase in fat but also from an
increase in bone and muscle tissue. Thus, even though pubescent boys and girls gain weight
rapidly, they often look thin and scrawny. Girls experience the greatest weight gain just
before and just after the menarche. Only slight increases in weight occur after that. For boys,
the maximum gain in weight comes a year or later than for girls and reaches its peak at
sixteen years, after which the gain is small.
It is not uncommon for both boys and girls to go through a fat period during puberty.
Between the ages of ten and twelve, at or near the onset of the growth spurt, children tend to
accumulate fat on the fat on the abdomen, around the nipples, in the hips and thighs, and in
the cheeks, neck and jaw. This fat usually disappears-after pubertal maturing and rapid
growth in height are well started, though it may remain for two more years during the early
part of puberty.
16.5.2 Changes in Body Proportions
The second major physical change at puberty is change in body proportions. Certain
areas of the body which, in the early years of life were proportionally much too small now
become proportionally too big because they reach their mature size sooner than other areas.
This is particularly apparent in the nose, feet, and hands. It is not until the latter part of
adolescence that the body attains adult proportions in all areas, although the most pronounced
changes take place before puberty is over.
The thin, long trunk of the older child begins to broaden at the hips and shoulders, and
a waistline develops. This appears high at first because the legs grow proportionately more
than the trunk. As the trunk lengthens, the waistline drops, thus giving the body adult
proportions. The broadness of the hips and shoulders is influenced by the age of maturing.
Boys who mature early usually have broader hips than boys who mature late and girls who
mature late have slightly broader hips than early-maturing girls.
Just before puberty, the legs are disproportionately long in relation to the trunk and
continue to be so until the child is approximately fifteen. In late maturing children, the leg
growth continues for a longer time than in early maturers. The result is that the late maturer is
a long-legged individual at maturity, while the early maturer is short-legged. The legs of the
early maturer tend to be stocky, while those of the late maturer are generally slender.
Much the same pattern occurs in the arms, whose growth precedes the rapid spurt of
growth in the trunk, thus making them seem disproportionately long. As is true of leg growth,
the growth of the arms is affected by the age of maturing. Early maturers tend to have shorter
arms than late maturers, just as the early maturer is shorter-legged than the late maturer. Not
until the growth of the arms and legs is nearly complete do they seem to be in the right
proportion to the hands and feet, both of which reach their mature size early in puberty.
16.5.3 Primary Sex Characteristics
The third major physical change at puberty is the growth and development of the primary
sex characteristics, the sex organs. In the case of the male, the gonads or testes, which are
located in the scrotum, or sac, outside the body, are only approximately 10 percent of their
mature size at the age of fourteen years. Then there is a rapid growth for a year or two, after
which growth slows down; the testes are fully developed by the age of twenty or twenty-one.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
195
Shortly after the rapid growth of the testes begins, the growth of the penis accelerates
markedly. The first growth is in length followed by a gradual increase in circumference.
When the male reproductive organs have become mature in function, nocturnal emissions
generally begin to occur, usually when the boy is having a sexually exciting dream, when he
has a full bladder or is constipated, when he is wearing tight pajamas, or when is too warmly
covered. Many boys are unaware of what is taking place until they see the telltale spot on
their, bedclothes or pajamas.
All part of the female reproductive apparatus grows during puberty, though at different
rates. The uterus of the average eleven- or twelve-year-old girl, for example, weighs 5.3
grams; by the age of sixteen, the average weight is 43 grams. The Fallopian tubes, ovaries,
and vagina also grow rapidly at this time.
The first real indication that a girl's reproductive mechanism is becoming mature is the
menarche, or first menstrual flow. This is the beginning of a series of periodic discharges of
blood, mucus, and broken down cell tissue from the uterus that will occur approximately
every twenty –eight days until the girl reaches the menopause, in the late forties or early fifties.
The girl's menstrual periods generally occur at very irregular intervals and vary markedly
in length for-the first year or so. This period is known as the stage of adolescent sterility.
During this time ovulation, or the ripening and release of a ripe ovum from a follicle in the
ovary, does not occur, and the girl is therefore sterile. Even after several menstrual periods, it
is questionable whether the girl's sex mechanism is mature enough to make conception
possible.
The puberty fat period in girls, which usually levels off between sixteen and eighteen
years, coincides with the period of adolescent sterility. At this time there is rapid growth in
length in the uterus and in the weight of the ovaries.
16.5.4 Secondary Sex Characteristics
The fourth major physical change at puberty is the development of the secondary sex
characteristics. These are the physical features which distinguish males from females and
which make members of one sex appealing to members of the other sex. They are unrelated
to reproduction though indirectly they are related by making males appealing to females and
vice versa. That is why they are called "secondary" as compared with the sex organs proper
which are called "primary" sex characteristics because they are directly related to
reproduction. As long as the body remains childlike in appearance, there is no "sex appeal"
This, however, changes when the secondary sex characteristics appear.
As puberty progresses, boys and girls become increasingly dissimilar in appearance. This
change is caused by the gradual development of the secondary sex characteristics which, like
other developments at puberty, follows a predictable pattern.
16.5.5 Important Secondary Sex Characteristics
Boys
Hair: Pubic hair appears about one year after the testes and penis have started to increase in
size. Axillary’s and facial hair appear when the pubic hair almost completed its growth, as
does body hair. At first, all hair is scanty, lightly pigmented and fine in texture. Later it
becomes darker, coarser, more luxuriant, and slightly kinky.
Skin: The skin becomes coarser, less transparent and sallow in colour, and the pores
enlarge.
Glands: The sebaceous or oil-producing glands in the skin enlarge and become more active,
which may cause acne. The apocrine glands in the armpits start to function, a perspiration
increases as puberty progresses.
Muscles: The muscles increase markedly in size and strength, thus giving shape to the arms,
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
196
legs, and shoulders.
Voice: Voice changes begin after some pubic hair has appeared. The voice first becomes
husky and later drops in pitch, increases in volume, and acquires a pleasanter tone. Voice
breaks are common when maturing is rapid.
Breast Knots: Slight knobs around the male mammary glands appear between the ages of
twelve and fourteen. These last for several weeks and then decrease in number and size.
Girls
Hips: The hips become wider and rounder, as a result of the enlargement of the pelvic bone
and the development of subcutaneous fat
Breasts: Shortly after the hips start to enlarge, the breasts begin to develop. The nipples
enlarge and protrude and, as the mammary glands develop, the breasts become larger and
rounder.
Hair: Pubic hair appears after hip and breast development is well under way. Axillary hair
begins to appear after the menarche, as does facial hair. Body hair appears on the limbs late
in puberty. All hair except facial hair is straight and lightly pigmented at first and then
becomes more luxuriant, coarser, darker, and slightly kinky.
Skin: The skin becomes coarser, thicker, and slightly sallow, and the pores enlarge.
Glands: The sebaceous and apocrine glands become more active as puberty progresses.
Clogging of the sebaceous glands can cause acne, while the apocrine glands in the armpits
produce perspiration, which is especially heavy and pungent just before and during the
menstrual period.
Muscles: The muscles increase in size and strength, especially in the middle of puberty-and
toward the end, thus giving shape to the shoulders, arms, and legs.
Voice: The voice becomes fuller and more melodious. Huskiness and breaks in the voice are
rare among girls.
16.6 THE PUBERTY GROWTH SPURT
The puberty growth spurt for girls begins between 8.5 and 11.5 years, with a peak
coming, on the average, at 12.5 years. From then on, the rate of growth slows down until
growth gradually comes to a standstill between seventeen and eighteen years. Boys experience
a similar pattern of rapid growth except that their growth spurt starts later and continues
for a longer time. For boys, the growth spurt starts between 10.5 and14.5 years, reaches a
peak between 14.5 and 15.5 years, and is then followed by a gradual decline until twenty or
twenty-one years, when growth is completed. Increases in height, weight, and strength come
at approximately the same time.
The rapid growth and development that occur during puberty depend partly on hereditary
factors, as they influence the endocrine glands, and partly on environmental factors, of which
nutrition has been found to be the most important. Poor nutrition in childhood causes a
diminished production of the growth hormone. Good nutrition, on the other hand, speeds up
the production of this hormone. Emotional disturbances can affect growth b causing an over
production of the adrenal steroids, which have an adverse effect on the growth hormone.
When the growth spurt of puberty is interfered with illness, poor nutrition, or prolonged
emotional tension, there will be delayed fusion of the bones and children will not attain their
full height. However, if such disturbances are detected in time and corrected, growth can be
speeded up to three or four times its normal rate and continue at that rate until children reach
their hereditary potentials. At the present time, there is no completely reliable way of
predicting adult height from the percentage of adult height reached when the secondary sex
characteristics begin to develop, or at any other time during the puberty growth spurt.
Check Your Progress 2
Explain the body changes during puberty
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
197
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
16.7 CONSEQUENCES OF ABSTRACT THOUGHT
The development of formal operations leads to dramatic revisions in the way adolescents
see themselves, others, and the world in general. But just as adolescents are occasionally
awkward in the use of their transformed bodies, they are initially faltering and clumsy in their
abstract thinking. Parents and teachers must be careful not to mistake the many typical
reactions of the teenage years-argumentativeness, self-concern, insensitive remarks, and
indecisiveness-for anything other than inexperience with new reasoning powers.
16.7.1 Argumentativeness
As adolescents acquire formal operations, they are motivated to use them. The once
pliable school-age child becomes a feisty, argumentative teenager who can marshal facts and
ideas to build a case. Some parents remark "A simple, straightforward explanation used to be
good enough to get a child to obey". Whereas "Now a thousand reasons. And worse yet, the
finds a way to contradict them all!" as long as parent-child disagreements remain focused on
principles and do not deteriorate into meaningless battles, they can promote development.
Through discussions of family rules and practices, adolescents become more aware of their
parents' values and the reasons behind them. Gradually, they come to see the validity of
parental beliefs and adopt many as their own. Teenagers' capacity for effective argument also
opens the door to intellectually stimulating pastimes, such as debate teams and endless
discussion sessions with friends over moral, ethical, and political concerns. By proposing,
justifying, criticizing, and defending a variety of solutions, adolescents often move to a
higher level of understanding.
16.7.2 Self-Consciousness and Self-Focusing
Adolescents' ability to reflect on their own thoughts, combined with the physical and
psychological changes they are undergoing, means that they start to think more about themselves.
Piaget believed that a new form of egocentrism accompanies this stage: the inability
to distinguish the abstract perspectives of self and other. As teenagers imagine what others
must be thinking, two distorted images of the relation between self and other appear.
The first is called the imaginary audience, adolescents' belief that they are the focus of
everyone else's attention and concern. Young teenagers regard themselves as always on stage.
As a result, they become extremely self-conscious, often going to great lengths to avoid
embarrassment. Sandhya, for example, woke up one Sunday morning with a large pimple on
her chin. "I can't possibly go to temple!" she cried. "Everyone will notice how ugly I look."
The imaginary audience helps us understand the hours adolescents spend inspecting every
detail of their appearance. It also accounts for their sensitivity to public criticism. To
teenagers, who believe that everyone is monitoring their performance, a critical remark from
a parent or teacher can be mortifying.
A second cognitive distortion is the personal fable. Because teenagers are so sure that
others are observing and thinking about them, they develop an inflated opinion of their own
importance. They start to feel that they are special and unique. Many adolescents view
themselves as reaching great heights of glory as well as sinking to unusual depths of despairexperiences
that others could not possibly understand. As one teenager wrote in her diary,
"My parents' lives are so ordinary, so stuck in a rut. Mine will be different. I'll realize my
hopes and ambitions.' When combined with a sensation seeking personality, the personal
fable seems to contribute to adolescent risk taking by convincing teenagers of their invul-
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
198
nerability. In one study, young people with both high personal fable and sensation-seeking
scores took more sexual risks; more often used drugs, and committed more delinquent acts
than their agemates.
The imaginary audience and personal fable are strongest during the transition to formal
operations. They gradually decline as abstract reasoning becomes better established. Yet
these distorted visions of the self may not represent a return to egocentrism. Instead, they
seem to be an outgrowth of gains in perspective taking, which cause young teenagers to be
very concerned with what others think. Adolescents also have emotional reasons for clinging
to the idea that others are preoccupied with their appearance and behavior. Doing so helps
them maintain a hold on important relationships as they struggle to separate from parents and
establish an independent sense of self.
16.7.3 Idealism and Criticism
Because abstract thinking permits adolescents to go beyond the real to the possible, it
opens up the world of the ideal and of perfection. Teenagers can imagine alternative family,
religious, political, and moral systems, and they want to explore them. Doing so is part of
investigating new realms of experience, developing larger social commitments, and defining
their own values and preferences.
The idealism of teenagers leads them to construct grand visions of a perfect world-with
no injustice, discrimination, or tasteless behavior. They do not make room for the
shortcomings of everyday life. Adults, with their longer life experience, have a more realistic
outlook. The disparity between adults' and teenagers' world views is often called the
"generation gap," and it creates tension between parent and child. Aware of the perfect
family against which their real parents and siblings do not measure up, adolescents may
become fault-finding critics.
Teenage idealism and criticism are advantageous. Once adolescents learn to see others as
having both strengths and weaknesses, they have a much greater capacity to work
constructively for social change and to form positive and lasting relationships. Parents can
help teenagers forge a better balance between the ideal and the real by tolerating their
criticism while reminding the young person that all people are blends of virtues and
imperfections.
16.7.4 Planning and Decision Making
Adolescents, who think more analytically, handle cognitive tasks more effectively than
they did at younger ages. Given a homework assignment, they are far better at cognitive self
regulation-planning what to do first and what to do next, monitoring progress toward a goal,
and redirecting actions that prove unsuccessful. For this reason, study skills improve from
middle childhood into adolescence.
But when it comes to planning and decision making in everyday life, teenagers
(especially young ones) often feel over whelmed by the possibilities before them. Their
efforts to choose among alternatives frequently break down, and they may resort to habit, act
on impulse, or not make a decision at all. On many mornings, for example, Sandhya tried on
five or six outfits before leaving for school. Often she shouted from the bedroom, "Mom,
what shall I wear?" Then, when her mother Fatima made a suggestion, Sandhya rejected it,
opting for one of the two or three sweaters she had worn for weeks. Similarly, Lakshman
procrastinated about registering for college entrance tests. When Fatima mentioned that he
was about to miss the deadline, Lakshman agonized over the forms, unable to decide when or
where he wanted to take the test.
Everyday planning and decision making are challenging for teenagers because they have so
many opportunities. When they were younger, adults usually specified their options, reducing
the number of decisions that had to be made. As adolescents gather more experience, they
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
199
make choices with greater confidence.
Check Your Progress 3
Discuss the consequences of abstract thought
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
16.8 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Characteristics of Puberty
ii) Criteria of Puberty
iii) Causes of Puberty
iv) Body Changes at Puberty
v) The Puberty Growth Spurt
vi) Consequences of Abstract Thought
16.9 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. The Characteristics of Puberty includes:
i) It is an Overlapping Period
ii) It is a Short Period
iii) It is Divided into Stages
iv) It is a Time of Rapid Growth and Change
v) It is a Negative Phase
vi) It Occurs at a Variable Age
2. The body changes during puberty are:
i) Changes in Body Size
ii) Changes in Body Proportions
iii) Primary Sex Characteristics
iv) Secondary Sex Characteristics
3. Consequences of abstract thought are in:
i) Argumentativeness
ii) Self-Consciousness and Self-Focusing
iii) Idealism and Criticism
iv) Planning and Decision Making
16.10 Lesson – End Activities
1. Is it puberty is a negative phase.
2. Mention ways of effective decision making.
16.11 References
1. Symonds, P., The Dynamics of Human Adjustment, new York : Appleton, 1968.
2. Freud, S. The Problem of Anxiety, New York : W.W. Norton & Co., 1990.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
200
LESSON – 17
EFFECTS OF PUBERTY CHANGES - DEVIANT MATURING – HAZARDS
DURING PUBERTY
Contents
17.0 Aims and Objectives
17.1 Introduction
17.1.1 Effects on Physical Well-Being
17.1.2 Effects on Attitudes and Behavior
17.1.3 Common Effects of Puberty Changes on Attitudes and Behavior
17.2 Effects of Deviant Maturing
17.2.1 Early versus Late Maturers
17.3 Hazards of Puberty
17.3.1 Physical Hazards
17.3.2 Effects of Endocrine Imbalance at Puberty
17.3.3 Psychological Hazards
17.4 Let Us Sum Up
17.5 Check your Progress
17.6 Lesson – End Activities
17.7 References
17.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help you understand effects of puberty changes and the hazards during
this period.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
i) state the effects of puberty
ii) explain the concept of deviant maturing
iii) list the hazards that happen during puberty
17.1 INTRODUCTION
The physical changes of puberty affect every area of the body, both externally and
internally, and thus it is not surprising that they also affect the pubescents physical and
psychological well-being. Even though these effects are normally only temporary, they are
severe enough while they last to bring about a change in habitual patterns of behavior,
attitudes, and personality.
17.1.1 Effects on Physical Well-Being
Rapid growth and body changes are likely to be accompanied by fatigue listlessness,
and other unfavourable symptoms. These discomforts are frequently made worse by an
increase in duties and responsibilities, just at the time when the individual is least able to
cope with them successfully.
Digestive disturbances are frequent, and appetite is finicky. The prepubescent child is
upset by glandular changes and changes in the size and position of the internal organs. These
changes interfere with the normal functions of digestion. Anemia is common at this period,
not because of marked changes in blood chemistry; but because of erratic eating habits,
which in turn increase the already present tendency to be tired and listless.
During the early menstrual periods, girls frequently experience headaches, backaches,
cramps, and abdominal pain, accompanied by fainting, vomiting, skin irritations, and even
swelling of the legs and ankles. As a result, they feel tired, depressed, and irritable at the time
of their periods. As menstruation becomes more regular, the physical and psychological
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
201
disturbances which accompany its early appearances tend to diminish.
Headaches, backaches, and a general feeling of achiness occur at other times between
menstruations. Both boys and girls suffer intermittently from these discomforts, their
frequency and severity depending to a large extent upon how rapidly the pubescent changes
are occurring and upon how healthy the individuals were when puberty began.
While puberty may be regarded as a "sickly age,” when the individual is not up to par,
relatively few diseases are characteristic of this period. If pubescent children were actually ill,
they would be treated with more sympathy and understanding than they usually are; less
would be expected of them, and much of their unsocial behavior would be understood and
tolerated, which it rarely is.
17.1.2 Effects on Attitudes and Behavior
It is understandable that the widespread effects of puberty on children's physical wellbeing
would also affect their attitudes and behavior. However, there is evidence that the
changes in attitudes and behavior that occur at this time are more the result of social than of
glandular changes, though the glandular changes unquestionably play some role through their
influence on body homeostasis. The less sympathy and understanding the pubescent child
receives from parents, siblings, teachers, and peers and the greater the social expectations at
this time, the greater the psychological effects of the physical changes.
Girls, as a general rule, are more seriously affected by puberty than boys, partly because
they usually mature more rapidly than boys and partly because more social restrictions begin
to be placed on their behavior, just at a time when they are trying to free themselves from
such restrictions.
17.1.3 Common Effects of Puberty Changes on Attitudes and Behavior
Desire for Isolation: When puberty changes begin, children usually withdraw from peer and
family activities and often quarrel with peers and family member. They spend much time in a
daydreaming about how misunderstood and mistreat they are and in experimenting with sex
through masturbation. Part of this withdrawal syndrome Includes refusal to communicate
with others.
Boredom: Pubescent children are bored with the play they formerly enjoy, with
schoolwork, with social activities, and with life in general. As a result, they do as little work
as they can, thus developing the habit of underachieving. This habit is accentuated by not
feeling up to par physically.
In coordination: Rapid and uneven growth affects habitual patterns of coordination, and
the pubescent child is clumsy and awkward for a time. As growth slows own, coordination
gradually improves.
Social Antagonism: The pubescent child is often uncooperative, disagreeable, and
antagonistic. Open hostility between the sexes, expressed constant criticism and derogatory
comments, and is common at this age. As puberty progresses, the child becomes friendlier,
more cooperative, and more tolerant of others.
Heightened Emotionality: Moodiness, sulkiness, temper outbursts, and a tendency to cry at
the slightest provocation are characteristic of the early part of puberty. It is a time of worry,
anxiety, and irritability. Depression, irritability, and negative moods are especially common
during the premenstrual and early menstrual periods of girls. As pubescent children become
more mature physically, they become less tense and exhibit more mature emotional behavior.
Loss of Self-Confidence: The pubescent child, formerly so self-assured, becomes lacking in
self-confidence and fearful of failure. This is due partly to lowered physical resistance and
partly to the constant criticism of adults and peers. Many boys and girls emerge from puberty
with the foundations of an inferiority complex.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
202
Excessive Modesty: The bodily changes that take place during puberty cause the child to
become excessively modest for fear that others will notice these changes and comment on
them unfavorably.
Because they reach puberty earlier, girls show signs of disruptive behavior sooner than
boys do. However, girls' behavior stabilizes earlier than that of boys, and they begin to act
more as they did before the onset of puberty, just as boys will do later.
How seriously puberty changes will affect behavior will be greatly influenced by the
ability and willingness of pubescent children to communicate their concerns and anxieties to
others and, in that way, get a new and better perspective on them. The affective reaction to
change is largely determined by the capacity to communicate. Communication is a means of
coping with anxiety which inevitably accompanies stress. Pubescent children who find it
difficult or impossible to communicate with others exhibit more negative behavior than those
who can and will communicate.
The psychological effects of puberty are also complicated by the social expectations of
parents, teachers, and other adults. Boys and girls are expected to act according to certain
standards appropriate for their ages. They find this relatively easy if their behavior patterns
are at the appropriate developmental levels. However, children who are maturationally
unready to fulfill the social expectations for their ages are likely to have problems.
Check Your Progress 1
State the effects of physical changes of puberty.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
17.2 EFFECTS OF DEVIANT MATURING
Children who are most affected by the physical changes that normally occur at
puberty are the deviant maturers. A deviant maturer is one whose sexual maturation deviates
by a year or more from the norm for the individual's sex group, in the time at which sexual
maturation occurs, or by a year or more from the norm for the individual's sex group in the
time needed to complete the maturation process. Children who mature sexually earlier than
their sex group are called “early maturers" while those who mature sexually later than their
sex groups are called "late maturers.” When children require less than the normal time for
their sex group to complete the maturational process they are known as “rapid maturers"
while those who need more than the normal time are called "slow maturers."
17.2.1 Early versus Late Maturers
For boys, early maturing is advantageous, especially in the area of sports, from which
they derive much of their prestige and status in the peer group. It is from the ranks of the
early maturers that most of the leaders in boys groups come, and this gives them added
prestige in the eyes of girls also.
By contrast, boys who are late maturers tend to be restless, tense, rebellious, and
attention-seeking. Because of these unsocial patterns of behavior, they are less popular with
both peers and adults and are far less often selected for leadership roles by their peers than
early maturers are. In commenting on the disadvantages of late maturing for boys,
Early maturing is less advantageous to girls than it is to boys. Early-maturing girls are
more grown-up and sophisticated in their behaviour, but their appearance and actions may
lead to a reputation of being "sexually promiscuous." In addition, early-maturing girls are
more out of step with their peers than early maturing boys, and this adds to their social
problems. In commenting on the social problems the early maturing girl is confronted with.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
203
Girls who are late maturers are less damaged psychologically than late-maturing boys.
They are less likely to engage in status-seeking behavior than boys, though they are
concerned about their normalcy, which they reflect in shy, retiring, diffident behavior.
Because this is considered sex appropriate behavior for girls, it is not as damaging to their
reputations as similar behavior in boys would be.
A study of social attitudes among members of the peer group toward early- and latematuring
boys and girls revealed that early-maturing boys were mentioned much more often
in the school newspaper than late maturers, while the reverse was true for girls.
17.2.2 Rapid versus Slow Maturers
Rapid maturers face certain problems that slow maturers are spared. All of the common
effects of puberty changes on attitudes and behavior tend to be exaggerated in rapid maturers.
For example, in coordination as shown in clumsiness and awkwardness of behavior is
exaggerated in rapid maturers because their bodies change in size so rapidly that they do not
have time to learn to control them. By contrast, changes in body size in slow maturers come
so slowly that children have time to learn to control their bodies and, as a result, they do not
show the pronounced awkwardness and clumsiness so characteristic of rapid maturers.
Similarly, because rapid maturing tends to sap energy, rapid-maturing children become
lethargic and perform below their potentials in whatever they do. As a result, they tend to
become underachievers, a tendency which can and often does become habitual during the
puberty years. Unless steps are taken to correct it, after the worst impact of puberty changes
has passed, it is likely to become persistent and the child becomes a lifelong underachiever.
The speed of sexual maturing affects attitudes unfavorably mainly when children are
slow maturers. While rapid maturers may be temporarily emotionally disturbed by their
awkwardness and clumsiness and while periods of heightened emotionality may occur more
frequently and more intensely in rapid than in slow maturers, rapid maturers have no cause
for concern about whether they will ever turn into adults. They can almost see themselves
doing so from one day to another.
By contrast, slow maturers are plagued by the fear that they will never turn into adults
and by the constant reminders of how much more like adults their peers seem to be. They
experience the same problems that late maturers experience because they lag behind their
age-mates and, as a result, are treated by both adults and age-mates as if they were younger
than they actually are.
Check Your Progress 2
Explain the effects of deviant maturing
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
17.3 HAZARDS OF PUBERTY
The hazards of puberty are generally serious mainly in terms of their long-range
consequences. This contrasts with the earlier stages of development, when the hazards
themselves tend to be more important than their long-term effects.
As is true of late childhood, the psychological hazards of puberty are more numerous and
more far-reaching in their effects than the physical ones. Furthermore, only a small
percentage of pubescent children are affected by the physical hazards, while all are affected
by the psychological ones, though to varying degrees.
17.3.1 Physical Hazards
Even though most pubescent children do not feel up to par physically, actual illness is less
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
204
common during this period than at earlier ages. Mortality also occurs infrequently among
pubescent children. Since they rarely suffer from illnesses severe enough to lead to death and
since they are so inactive and socially withdrawn that accidents-a common cause of death in
the years proceeding and following puberty-are infrequent, there is less likelihood of
mortality at this time than ill the earlier or even the postpubescent years. Actually, many
deaths reported as due to accidents are the result of suicide which pubescent children may
attempt if they become severely depressed.
The major physical hazards of puberty are due to slight or major malfunctioning of the
endocrine glands that control the puberty growth spurt and the sexual changes that take place
at this time.
17.3.2 Effects of Endocrine imbalance at Puberty
Insufficient Growth Hormone: An insufficient amount of growth hormone in late childhood
and early puberty causes the individual to be smaller than average at maturity.
Insufficient Gonadal Hormones: If the gonadal hormones are not released in adequate
amounts soon enough to check the growth hormone, growth of the limbs continues too long,
and the individual becomes larger than average. Insufficient amounts of gonadal hormones
also affect the normal development of the sex organs and the secondary sex characteristics,
with the result that the individual remains childlike or takes on characteristics of the opposite
sex, depending on when the interruption in the developmental cycle occurs.
Excessive Supply of Gonadal Hormones: An imbalance in the functioning of the pituitary
gland and the gonads can cause production of an excessive amount of gonadal hormones at a
very young age, resulting in the onset of puberty sometimes as early as five or six years of
age. This is known as precocious puberty or Puberty precox. While such children are sexually
mature in that their sex organs have begun to function, they are still small in stature and the
secondary sex characteristics are not as well developed as in those who mature at the usual
age.
17.3.3 Psychological Hazards
There are many psychological hazards of puberty, the long-term effects of which are even
more important than the immediate effects. It is this that makes the psychological hazards so
serious. Some of the most important of the psychological hazards of puberty are discussed
below.
Unfavorable Self-Concepts: Few children pass through puberty without developing
unfavorable self concepts. This is true even of those who, earlier, had good opinions of
themselves and who, as a result, had enough self-confidence to play leadership roles in their
peer groups.
There are many reasons for the development of unfavorable self-concepts during puberty,
some of which may be personal in origin and some environmental. Almost all pubescents
have unrealistic concepts about what their appearance and abilities will be when they are
grown up, concepts that often trace their origin to childhood days when the ideal self concept
is being formed. As pubescents watch their bodies change and as they observe their awkward
behavior and their tendencies toward obesity, they become increasingly disillusioned because
what they observe is so far removed from what they had anticipated. This affects their selfconcepts
unfavorably.
Because pubescents tend to be unsocial if not actually antisocial in their behavior, the
treatment they receive from others is affected by this. As a result, pubescents do not enjoy the
social acceptance they may have had earlier, nor does it come up to their hopes and
expectations. Unfavorable treatment from others seriously affects self-concepts, causing them
to be colored by negative attitudes toward self.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
205
When children develop unfavorable self-concepts, it is soon revealed in their behavior.
They either become withdrawn from others, contributing little in actions or speech to the
group, or they become aggressive and defensive, retaliating for what they regard as unfair
treatment. Regardless-of what form of expression their unfavorable self-concepts take, pubescent
children's behavior is such that it increases ""hat unfavorable social attitudes toward
them already existed.
Like most of the psychological hazards of puberty, the long-term effects of unfavorable
self-concepts are even more serious than the immediate effects. Children who develop
unfavorable self-concepts at puberty are far more likely to reinforce these unfavorable selfconcepts
with their unsocial behavior than they are to improve them. As a result, the
foundations for an inferiority complex are laid and, unless remedial steps are taken to correct
it, it will likely become persistent and color the quality of the individual's behavior
throughout the remainder of the life span.
Underachievement: With rapid physical growth comes a sapping of energy. This leads to
disinclination to work and to attitudes of boredom toward any activities that require effort on
the individual's part. While underachievement often begins around the fourth or fifth grade in
school, when early enchantment with school gives way to disenchantment, it generally
reaches its peak during puberty.
As girls accept the cultural stereotype about themselves, they realize that it is not
regarded as "feminine" to be achievers, especially when 'their achievements surpass those of
boys. This encourages girls to work below their capacities and increases the tendency to be
underachievers caused by the sapping of physical strength which is a normal accompaniment
of the rapid physical changes of puberty.
Once the tendency to work below one's capacities develops, it is likely to become habitual
as it is reinforced, month after month, by the sapping of physical energy during the rapid
growth period of puberty and by the cultural pressures on girls not to surpass boys in their
achievements. As a result, many pubescents grow up to be underachievers, not only
academically but also vocationally. They develop attitudes toward themselves and their
abilities that reinforce their lack of motivation to try to do what they are capable of. Many
enter adult life as general underachievers, a tendency to work below their capacities and
potentials in whatever they undertake because they learned patterns of behavior and attitudes
in puberty which have become habitual. Unless remedial steps are taken to correct them, they
will lead to lifelong underachievement.
Lack of Preparation for Puberty Changes: When pubescent children are not informed about
or are psychologically unprepared for both the physical and psychological changes that take
place at puberty, undergoing these changes may be a traumatic experience, As a result, they
are likely to develop unfavorable attitudes toward these changes-attitudes that are more apt to
persist than to disappear. There are many reasons why children are often unprepared for
puberty. Parents, for example, may lack adequate knowledge or they may be held back by
modesty and embarrassment. Or, the gap that often develops between pubescent children and
their parents prevents them from asking questions about the changes that are taking place in
their bodies. In addition, to avoid embarrassment, pubescent children may pretend that they
already know all they need to know. In that way they rebuff parental attempts to give them
the information they want.
Unless the school gives courses in sex hygiene or provides information about puberty in
connection with a physical hygiene course, children will not get the information they want at
school nor will they be likely to turn to a teacher for help. This is partly because of
embarrassment and partly because their attitudes toward school and toward teachers are more
likely to be unfavorable than favorable at this time. They are also unlikely to turn to their
classmates or friends for information, even if those classmates are better informed than they
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
206
are. Pride keeps most pubescent children from admitting that they know less than their
friends know.
Regardless of the reason for inadequate preparation for puberty, it is a serious
psychological hazard, especially in the case of early or late maturers. The reason for this is
that it encourages them to think that something is wrong or that their development is so
abnormal that they will never again look like their peers.
Being different is always a concern to children and young adolescents. The more they
deviate in ways that are apparent to all, the more concerned they become and the more likely
they are to feel abnormal and, consequently, inferior.
Acceptance of Changed Bodies: One of the important developmental tasks of puberty is
acceptance of the changed body. Few pubescents are able to do this. As a result, they are
dissatisfied with their appearance. Knowing how important appearance is in social
acceptance, they often blame it for their less than-hoped-for acceptance.
There are many reasons why pubescent boys and girls are dissatisfied with their changed
bodies and find it difficult to accept them. First, almost all children build up an ideal physical
self-concept based on concepts from different sources of the ideal individual of their sex
group. Few pubescents ever even remotely approach this ideal physique in real life. Under
such conditions, they are dissatisfied with their looks and find it difficult to be self acceptant.
Second, traditional beliefs about a sex-appropriate appearance tend to color pubescent
children's attitudes in ways that interfere with their acceptance of their own changed bodies.
For example, since being flat-chested is generally considered unattractive and unfeminine in
women, pubescent girls whose breasts are developing slowly may not only become
concerned about their femininity but also are likely to become self-rejectant. In the same way,
boys who accept the traditional belief that well-developed genitalia are a sign of masculinity
become concerned and self-rejectant when their penises are long and thin before their final
development has been completed.
Acceptance of Socially Approved Sex Roles: Like acceptance of the changed body,
acceptance of the sex roles pubescents are expected to play as near-adults is one of the major
developmental tasks of this age level. Throughout childhood, strong pressure is put on boys
to play the socially approved masculine sex role which, in most social groups, is the
traditional role which emphasizes the superiority of members of the male sex.
Because of the advantages and prestige associated with the traditional male sex role,
most boys are not only willing but also eager to play it. Before late childhood is over most
boys have not only accepted the stereotype of the traditional male but they have been sex-role
typed to the point where their behavior closely conforms to this stereotype. Consequently,
during puberty, acceptance of the sex role they are expected to playas near-adults presents no
real problem for pubescent boys and, consequently, cannot be regarded as a psychological
hazard for them.
This, however, is by no means true of girls. Having been far less strictly sex-role typed
during childhood then boys, and having a somewhat blurred concept of the sex role they will
be expected to play as adults, girls are now confronted with the problem of accepting the
traditional stereotype of the female and of behaving in a manner that conforms to this
stereotype.
For some pubescent girls, this presents few problems because they, like boys, have
learned to play the traditional sex role throughout the childhood years. For other girls, by
contrast, this may be a major psychological hazard to good personal and social adjustments.
Not only do they prefer the egalitarian to the traditional sex role but they have, at home and
to some extent in the play group, learned to play this role. Being expected, at puberty, to
accept a role that is less to their liking and less prestigious than the role they have played
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
207
through childhood is a psychological hazard for them.
For many pubescent girls, the psychological hazard of acceptance of the traditional female
sex role is intensified by the periodic discomforts they suffer at the time of their menstrual
periods. While not all girls are subject to these discomforts, most are during the early months
of puberty, while the sex organs are still only partially developed and therefore unready to
function in rhythmic manner as they will later.
The realization that boys are not subject to periodic discomforts similar to those they
experience at the times of their menstrual periods intensifies the resistance of many girls to
accept the traditional female sex role. Unfavorable attitudes toward menstruation are often
intensified by the unfavorable social attitudes of older women who often refer to it as the
"curse" and who emphasize the role it plays in the traditional female sex role.
It has been reported that, unfortunately, the unfavorable attitudes toward menstruation,
developed at puberty, often continue throughout life. As a result, they cause women to
become even more depressed at the time of their menstrual periods than would be justified on
the basis of the discomforts involved. For example, attempts at suicide among women occur
more frequently during menstrual periods than at other times.
Deviations in Sexual Maturing: Unquestionably one of the most serious psychological
hazards during puberty is a deviation in the age at which sexual maturing occurs or in the
time needed for the maturing. This hazard, of course, affects only those children who are
deviant enough from their age-mates in this aspect of their development to be recognized by
them as "different."
As is true of the late childhood years, it is difficult for pubescent children to be acceptant
about anything that makes them different and thus, in their view, inferior. Deviations in
sexual maturing, regardless of what form they take, are a potential psychological hazard.
Children who deviate from their age-mates in sexual maturing feel that there is something
wrong with them. They are concerned about their normalcy and about their future normalcy.
If, for example, children deviate from their age-mates in height at puberty, they worry about
their adult height.
Early maturers, There are distinct social advantages in most cultures over late maturing
youths, but youths who mature 'too early' may manifest personality difficulties. These difficulties
come from the fact that early maturers, who look older than they actually are, are
usually expected to act in accordance with their appearance rather than in accordance with
their chronological age. If they fail to do so, they are criticized and this leads to resentments
as well as feelings of inadequacy and inferiority. Studies of school dropouts have suggested
that early maturing is a common cause of dropping out. The reason for this is that teachers,
like parents, tend to expect more of early maturers than they are capable of and, as a result,
early maturers often see school as hostile and rejecting. There is no question about the fact
that all the normal effects of puberty-heightened emotionality, awkwardness, and so forth -are
accentuated in early maturers. This intensifies their feelings of inferiority.
Late maturers, who look younger than they are, may be treated accordingly by friends
and adults. This makes them doubt their ability to do what their agemates do. Slow maturers
have more time to adjust to the physical changes of puberty than rapid maturers or those who
mature at a normal rate. But concern about whether they will ever grow up counteracts this
favorable effect and encourages their belief that they are inferior to their age-mates. In the
case of boys, this may and often does lead to the "locker room" syndrome. It means that “In a
culture where athletic prowess is often as important, and sometimes more important, than
academic and creative achievement, a student's self-image is shaped by his perception of his
physical ability in relation to his classmates". Being weaker and less well developed than they
causes those who lag behind their age-mates in sexual maturing to feel inferior.
Serious as the immediate effects of deviant sexual maturing are to children's personal
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
208
and social adjustments, the long-term effects are even more serious. In the case of slow
maturers, damage results from the fact that they have longer than average time during which
to develop the undesirable patterns of behavior associated with puberty, although this need
not necessarily be permanently handicapping.
Some may develop into habitual daydreamers; some may develop a hypercritical, frictional
attitude toward others; and some may develop into restless people who find it difficult
to concentrate on any task. But if their desire for social acceptance is strong enough and if
they are able to achieve a reasonable amount of social acceptance, they will be sufficiently
motivated to break these habits and replace them with more socially acceptable patterns of
behavior.
Not all those who deviate from the norm-the early and late maturers-are damaged
permanently by this. Some, in fact, benefit, not only during puberty, but in later years as well.
Although studies of the long-term effects on behavior have so far been limited to boys,
evidence from these studies and knowledge of the effects of reinforcement through repetition
enable us to hazard a guess concerning what the long-term effects on girls might be.
Early” maturing boys normally become socially active and popular, holding
leadership roles in the peer group. They have assets that are valued in the peer group, and as a
result of repetition, these develop into habitual patterns of behavior. The early maturers are
more successful vocationally and socially as adults, just as they were during adolescence.
Their success stems from the fact that they make better impressions on others than the normal
or late maturers.
By contrast, middle-aged men who were late maturers were found to cling to the "littleboy"
patterns of behavior which caused them to be unpopular when they were younger. Thus
late maturers tend to be less active socially, less successful in business, and less likely to be
selected for leadership roles than might be expected on the basis of their abilities.
Speculation about the long-term effects of deviant maturing on girls leads one to believe
that early maturers who were embarrassed about being larger than their contemporaries and
who often developed aggressive patterns of behavior to attract the attention of boys will
continue to show similar patterns of behavior as adult women. Late maturers, by contrast,
who were better adjusted personally and socially in adolescence, are likely to continue to be
so during adulthood, unless conditions unrelated to sexual maturing interfere with this
pattern.
Check Your Progress 3
Discuss the hazards during puberty
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
17.4 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Effects on Physical Well-Being
ii) Effects on Attitudes and Behavior
iii) Common Effects of Puberty Changes on Attitudes and Behavior
iv) Effects of Deviant Maturing
v) Early versus Late Maturers
vi) Hazards of Puberty
vii) Physical Hazards
viii) Effects of Endocrine Imbalance at Puberty
ix) Psychological Hazards
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
209
17.5 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. The effects of physical changes include:
i) The Effects on Physical Well-Being
ii) Effects on Attitudes and Behavior
iii) Common Effects of Puberty Changes on Attitudes and Behavior
2. The effects of deviant maturing are:
i) early maturers
ii) late maturers
iii) rapid maturers
3. The hazards during puberty include:
i) Physical Hazards
ii) Effects of Endocrine Imbalance at Puberty
iii) Psychological Hazards
17.6 Lesson – End Activities
1. Mention the negative effects of early puberty.
2. Mention few characteristics of early matures.
17.7 References
1. Freud, S. Three Essays on the theory of Sexuality.
2. Freud, S. Beyond the Pleasure Principle, New York : Liverignt, 1968.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
210
LESSON - 18
ADOLESCENCE - CHARACTERISTICS - DEVELOPMENTAL TASKS - PIAGET'S
THEORY: THE FORMAL OPERATIONAL STAGE - COGNITIVE
DEVELOPMENT
Contents
18.0 Aims and Objectives
18.1 Introduction
18.2 Characteristics of Adolescence
18.2.1 Adolescence is an important period
18.2.2 Adolescence is a transitional period
18.2.3 Adolescence is a period of change
18.2.4 Adolescence is a Problem Age
18.2.5 Adolescence is a Time of Search for Identity
18.2.6 Adolescence is a Dreaded Age
18.2.7 Adolescence is a Time of Unrealism
18.2.8 Adolescence is the Threshold of Adulthood
18.3 Developmental Tasks of Adolescence
18.4 Physical Changes during Adolescence
18.4.1 Variations in Physical Changes
18.4.2 Effects of Physical Changes
18.4.3 Concerns about Physical Changes
18.5 Piaget’s Theory: The Formal Operational Stage
18.5.1 Hypothetico-Deductive Reasoning
18.5.2 Propositional Thought
18.6 Information processing - Cognitive Development
18.6.1 Scientific Reasoning: Coordinating Theory with Evidence
18.6.2 How Scientific Reasoning Develops
18.7 Let Us Sum Up
18.8 Check your Progress
18.9 Lesson – End Activities
18.10 References
18.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help you understand adolescence its characteristics, developmental
tasks, the physical changes and the cognitive development during this period.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
vi) State the characteristics of adolescence
vii) Mention the developmental tasks of adolescence
viii) List the physical changes that happen during adolescence
ix) Discuss the Piaget’s formal operational stage
x) Explain the cognitive development
18.1 INTRODUCTION
The word adolescence comes from the Latin word adolescere, meaning "to grow" or "to
grow to maturity." Primitive peoples-as was true also in earlier civilizations--do not consider
puberty and adolescence to be distinct periods in the life span; the child is regarded as an
adult when capable of reproduction. As it is used today, the term adolescence has a broader
meaning. It includes mental, emotional, and social maturity as well as physical maturity.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
211
18.2 CHARACTERISTICS OF ADOLESCENCE
Like every important period during the life span, adolescence has certain characteristics
that distinguish it from the periods that preceded it and the periods that will follow it. These
characteristics are explained briefly below.
18.2.1 Adolescence is an important period
As all periods in the life span are important, some are more important than others because
of their immediate effects on attitudes and behavior, whereas others are significant because of
their long term effects. Adolescence is one of the periods when both the immediate effects
and long-term effects are important. Some periods are important for their physical and some
for their psychological effects. Adolescence is important for both. Accompanying these rapid
and important physical developments, especially during the early adolescent period, rapid
mental developments occur. These give rise to the need for mental adjustments and the
necessity for establishing new attitudes, values and interests.
18.2.2 Adolescence is a transitional period
Transition does not mean a break with or a change from what has gone before but rather a
passage from one stage of development to another. This means that what has happened before
will leave its mark on what happens now and in the future. Children, when they go from
childhood to adulthood, must "put away childish things" and they must also learn new
patterns of behavior and attitudes to replace those they have abandoned.
However, it is important to realize that what happened earlier has left its mark and will
influence these new patterns of behavior and attitudes. The psychic structure of the
adolescent has its roots in childhood and many of its characteristics that are generally
considered as typical of adolescence appear and are already present during late childhood.
The physical changes that take place during the early years of adolescence affect the
individual's behavioral level and lead to reevaluations and a shifting adjustment of values.
During any transitional period, the individual's status is vague and there is confusion
about the roles the individual is expected to play. The adolescent, at this time, is neither a
child nor an adult. If adolescents behave like children, they are told to "act their age." If they
try to act like adults, they are often accused of being "too big for their behaviours" and are
reproved for their attempts to act like adults. On the other hand, the ambiguous status of
today's adolescents is advantageous in that it gives them time to try out different lifestyles
and decide what patterns of behavior, values, and attitudes meet their needs best .
18.2.3 Adolescence is a period of change
The rate of change in attitudes and behavior during adolescence parallels the rate of
physical change. During early adolescence, when physical changes are rapid, changes in
attitudes and behavior are also rapid. As physical changes slow down, so do attitudinal and
behavioral changes.
There are five almost universal concomitants of the changes that occur during
adolescence. The first is heightened emotionality, the intensity of which depends on the rate
at which the physical and psychological changes are taking place. Because these changes
normally occur more rapidly during early adolescence, heightened emotionality is generally
more pronounced in early than in late adolescence.
Second, the rapid changes that accompany sexual maturing make young adolescents
unsure of themselves, of their capacities, and of their interests. They have strong feelings of
instability which are often intensified by the ambiguous treatment they receive from parents
and teachers.
Third, changes in their bodies, their interests, and in the roles the social group expects
them to play create new problems. To young adolescents, these may seem more numerous
and less easily solved than any they have had to face before. Until they have solved their
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
212
problems to their satisfaction, they will be preoccupied with them and with themselves.
Fourth, as interests and behavior patterns change, so do values. What was important to
them as children seems less important to them now that they are near-adults. For example,
most adolescents no longer think that a large number of friends is a more important indication
of popularity than friends of the type that are admired and respected by their peers. They now
recognize quality as more important than quantity.
Fifth, most adolescents are ambivalent about changes. While they want and demand
independence, they often dread the responsibilities that go with independence and question
their ability to cope with these responsibilities.
18.2.4 Adolescence is a Problem Age
While every age has its problems, those of adolescence are often especially difficult for
boys and girls to cope with. There are two reasons for this. First, throughout childhood, their
problems were met and solved, in part at least, by parents and teachers. As a result, many
adolescents are inexperienced in coping with problems alone. Second, because adolescents
want to feel that they are independent, they demand the right of coping with their own
problems, rebuffing attempts on the part of parents and teachers to help them.
Because of their inability to cope with problems alone as well as they believe they can,
many adolescents find that the solutions do not always come up to their expectations. As
Anna Freud has explained, "Many failures, often with tragic consequences in these respects,
are due not to the individual's incapacity as such but merely to the fact that such demands are
made on him at a time in life when all his energies are engaged otherwise, namely, in trying
to solve the major problem created for him by normal sexual growth and development".
18.2.5 Adolescence is a Time of Search for Identity
Throughout the gang age of late childhood, conformity to group standards is far more
important to older children than individuality. As was pointed out earlier, in dress, speech,
and behavior older children want to be as nearly like their gang-mates as possible. Any
deviation from the group standard is likely to be a threat to group belonging.
In the early years of adolescence, conformity to the group is still important to boys and
girls. Gradually, they begin to crave identity and are no longer satisfied to be like their peers
in every respect, as they were earlier.
However, the ambiguous status of the adolescent in the Indian culture of today presents a
dilemma that greatly contributes to the adolescent "identity crisis" or the problem of egoidentity.
The ways adolescents try to establish themselves as individuals is by the use of status
symbols in the form of cars, clothes, hand held music systems, mobile phones, net chat and
other readily observable material possessions. They hope, in this way, to attract attention to
them and to be recognized as individuals while, at the same time, maintaining their identity
with the peer group.
18.2.6 Adolescence is a Dreaded Age
Many popular beliefs about adolescents have definite evaluative connotations and,
unfortunately, many of them are negative. Acceptance of the cultural stereotype of teenagers
as sloppy, unreliable individuals who are inclined toward destructiveness and antisocial
behavior has led many adults who must guide and supervise the lives of young adolescents to
dread this responsibility and to be unsympathetic in their attitudes toward, and treatment of,
normal adolescent behavior.
Popular stereotypes have also influenced the self-concepts and attitudes of adolescents
toward themselves. The cultural stereotypes have also functioned as mirrors held up to the
adolescent by society reflecting an image of himself that the adolescent gradually comes to
regard as authentic and according to which he shapes his behavior. The acceptance of this
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
213
stereotype and the belief that adults have poor opinions of them make the transition into
adulthood difficult. By so doing, it leads to much friction with their parents and places a
barrier between them and their parents which prevents them from turning to their parents for
help in solving their problems.
18.2.7 Adolescence is a Time of Unrealism
Adolescents have a tendency to look at life through rose-tinted glasses. They see
themselves and others as they would like them to be rather than as they are. This is especially
true of adolescent aspirations. These unrealistic aspirations, not only for themselves but also
for their families and friends, are, in part, responsible for the heightened emotionality
characteristic of early adolescence. The more unrealistic their aspirations are, the more angry,
hurt, and disappointed they will be when they feel that others have let them down or that they
have not lived up to the goals they set for themselves.
With increased personal and social experiences, and with increased ability to think
rationally, older adolescents see themselves, their families and friends, and life in general in a
more realistic way: As a result, they suffer less from disappointment and disillusionment than
they did when they were younger. This is one of the conditions that contribute to the greater
happiness of the older adolescent.
As adolescence draws to a close, it is not uncommon for both boys and girls to be
plagued by over idealism of the single, carefree life that they will soon give up as they
achieve the status of adults. Feeling that this period of their lives is happier than what they
will face in adulthood, with its demands and responsibilities, there is a tendency to glamorize
adolescence and to feel that a happy, carefree age has been lost forever.
18.2.8 Adolescence is the Threshold of Adulthood
As adolescents approach legal maturity, they are anxious to shed the stereotype of
teenagers and to create the impression that they are near-adults. Dressing and acting like
adults, are not always enough. So, they begin to concentrate on behavior that is associated
with the adult status-smoking, drinking, using drugs, and engaging in sex, for example. They
believe that this behavior will create the image they desire.
18.3 DEVELOPMENTAL TASKS OF ADOLESCENCE
All the developmental tasks of adolescence are focused on overcoming childish attitudes
and behavior patterns and preparing for adulthood. The developmental tasks of adolescence
require a major change in the child's habitual attitudes and patterns of behavior.
Consequently, few boys and girls can be expected to master them during the years of early
adolescence. This is especially true of late maturers. The most that can be hoped is that the
young adolescent will lay foundations on which to build adult attitudes and behavior patterns.
A brief survey of the important developmental tasks of adolescence will serve to illustrate
the extent of the changes that must be made and the problems that arise from these changes.
Fundamentally, the necessity for mastering the developmental tasks in the relatively short
time that adolescents have, as a result of lowering the age of legal maturity to eighteen, is the
reason for much of the stress that plagues many adolescents.
It may be difficult for adolescents to accept their physiques if, from earliest childhood,
they have a glamorized concept of what they wanted to look like when they are grown up. It
takes time to revise this concept and to learn ways to improve their appearance so that it will
conform more to their earlier ideals.
Acceptance of the adult-approved sex role is not too difficult for boys; they have been
encouraged in this direction since early childhood. But for girls, who as children were
permitted or even encouraged to play an egalitarian role, learning what the adult approved
feminine role is and accepting it is often a major task requiring many years of adjustment.
Because of the antagonism toward members of the opposite sex that often develops
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
214
during late childhood and puberty, learning new relationships with members of the opposite
sex actually means starting from scratch to discover what they are like and how to get along
with them. Even developing new, more mature relationships with age-mates of the same sex
may not be easy.
Achieving emotional independence from parents and other adults would seem, for the
independence-conscious adolescent, to be an easy developmental task. However, emotional
independence is not the same as independence of behavior. Many adolescents who want to be
independent want and need the security that emotional dependence on their parents or some
other adults gives. This is especially true for adolescents whose status in the peer group is
insecure or who lack a close tie with a member of the peer group.
Economic independence cannot be achieved until adolescents choose an occupation and
prepare for it. If they select an occupation that requires a long period of training, there can be
no assurance of economic independence even when they reach legal adulthood. They may
have to remain economically dependent for several years until their training for their chosen
vocations has been completed.
Schools and colleges put emphasis on developing intellectual skills and concepts
necessary for civic competence. However, few adolescents are able to use these skills and
concepts in practical situations. Those who are active in the extracurricular affairs of their
schools and colleges get such practice, but those who are not active in this way-because they
must take after-school jobs or because they are not accepted by their peers-are deprived of
this opportunity.
Schools and colleges also try to build values that are in harmony with those held by
adults; parents contribute to this development. When, however, the adult-fostered values
clash with peer values, adolescents must choose the latter if they want the peer acceptance on
which their social life depends.
Closely related to the problem of developing values in harmony with those of the adult
world the adolescent is about to enter is the task of developing socially responsible behavior.
Most adolescents want to be accepted by their peers, but they often gain this acceptance at the
expense of behavior that adults consider socially irresponsible. If, for example, it is the "thing
to do" to cheat or to help a friend during an examination, the adolescent must choose between
adult and peer standards of socially responsible behavior.
The trend toward earlier marriages has made preparation for marriage one of the most
important developmental tasks of the adolescent years. While the gradual relaxing of social
taboos on sexual behavior has gone a long way toward preparing adolescents of today for the
sexual aspects of marriage, they receive little preparation-at home, in school, or in college-for
the other aspects of marriage, and even less preparation for the duties and responsibilities of
family life. This lack of preparation is responsible for one of the major pieces of "unfinished
business" which the adolescent carries into adulthood.
Check Your Progress 1
Discuss the characteristic of the adolescent period.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
18.4 PHYSICAL CHANGES DURING ADOLESCENCE
Growth is not complete when puberty ends, nor is it entirely complete at the end of early
adolescence. However, there is a slowdown of the pace of growth, and there is more marked
internal than external development. This cannot be so readily observed or identified as
growth in height and weight or the development of the secondary sex characteristics.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
215
18.4.1 Variations in Physical Changes
Like all ages, there are individual differences in physical changes. Sex differences are
especially apparent. Even though boys start their growth spurt later than girls, their growth
continues longer, with the result that, at maturity, they are usually taller than girls. Because
boys' muscles grow larger than girls' muscles, at all ages after puberty boys surpass girls in
strength, and this superiority increases with age.
Individual differences are also influenced by age of maturing. Late maturers tend to have
slightly broader shoulders than those who mature early. The legs of early-maturing boys and
girls tend to be stocky; those of late maturers tend to be more slender. Early-maturing girls
weigh more, are taller, and have greater weight for their height than do late-maturing girls.
18.4.2 Effects of Physical Changes
As physical changes slow down, the awkwardness of puberty and early adolescence
generally disappear. This is because older adolescents have had time to gain control of their
enlarged bodies. They are also motivated to use their newly acquired strength and this further
helps them to overcome any awkwardness that appeared earlier.
Because strength follows growth in' muscle size, boys generally show their greatest
increase in strength after age fourteen, while girls show improvement up to this age and then
lag, owing more to changes in interests than to lack of capacity. Girls generally attain their
maximum strength at about seventeen, while boys do not attain their maximum strength until
they are twenty-one or twenty-two.
18.4.3 Concerns about Physical Changes
Few adolescents experience body-cathexis or satisfaction with their bodies. However,
they do experience more dissatisfaction with some parts of their bodies than with other parts.
This failure to experience body-cathexis is one of the causes of unfavorable self-concepts and
lack of self-esteem during the adolescent years.
Some of the concerns adolescents have about their bodies are carry-overs of concerns
they experienced during puberty and which, in the early years of adolescence, are based on
conditions that still prevail. Concern about normalcy, for example, will persist until the
physical changes on the surface of the body have been completed and adolescents can be sure
that their bodies conform to the norms for their sex groups. Similarly, concern about sexappropriateness,
so all-prevading in puberty, continues until the primary and secondary sex
characteristics have completed their growth and development and, thus, give adolescents an
opportunity to see if their bodies conform to the cultural standard of sex-appropriateness.
Awareness of social reactions to different body builds leads to concern in adolescents
whose changing bodies fail to conform to the culturally approved standards. Knowing that
social reactions to endomorphic builds in both boys and girls are less favorable than they are
to ectomorphic and mesomorphic -builds leads to concern on the part of adolescents whose
body builds tend toward endomorphy.
For many girls, menstruation is a serious concern. This is because they suffer physical
discomforts such as cramps, weight gain, headaches, backaches, swollen ankles, and breast
tenderness and experience emotional changes, such as' mood swings, depression, restlessness,
depression, and a tendency to cry without apparent reason.
Because menstruation is commonly referred to as "the curse," it is not surprising that this
unfavorable social reaction will color girls' attitudes. Furthermore, knowing that boys do not
experience any such form of physical discomfort also colors girls' attitudes - unfavorably and
encourages them to believe that they are martyrs.
Acne and other skin eruptions are a source of concern to both boys and girls. With the
increase in the severity of acne, there is an increase in concern. This concern is often as great
for boys as for girls because they realize that acne mars their chances for physical
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
216
attractiveness and because they cannot use cosmetics to cover it up as girls can.
The tendency toward obesity that plagues most pubescent boys and girls continues to be
a source of concern during the early adolescent years. In most cases, however, with increase
in height and with efforts to control their appetites and the eating of "junk food," older
adolescents start to slim down and look less obese than they did during the puberty fat period.
In addition, careful selection of clothing helps to create the illusion that they are more slender
than they actually are.
It is unusual for adolescents, boys or girls, not to be concerned about their physical
attractiveness. Few are satisfied with their appearance and many are concerned about what
they can do to improve it.
The reason for concern comes from realization of the role attractiveness plays in social
relationships. Adolescents realize, even more than children do, that people treat those who are
attractive more favorably than they do those who are less attractive. They are also aware of
the important role attractiveness plays in choice for leadership. Consequently, when they feel
that they are less attractive than they had hoped to be when their growth was complete or
nearly complete, they are concerned about what they can do to improve their looks. Few
adolescents escape being "looks-conscious" to the point where they spend proportionally
more time and thought on how to improve their looks than most adults consider justified.
18.5 PIAGET’S THEORY: THE FORMAL OPERATIONAL STAGE
According to Piaget, around age 11 young people enter the formal operational stage, in
which they develop the capacity for abstract, scientific thinking. Whereas concrete
operational children can "operate on reality," formal operational adolescents can "operate on
operations." In other words, they no longer require concrete things and events as objects of
thought. Instead, they can come up with new, more general logical rules through internal
reflection. Let's look at two major features of the formal operational stage.
18.5.1 Hypothetico-Deductive Reasoning
At adolescence, young people first become capable of hypothetico-deductive reasoning;
When faced with a problem, they start with a general theory of all possible factors that might
affect the outcome and deduce from it specific hypotheses (or predictions) " about what might
happen. Then they test these hypotheses in an orderly fashion to see which ones work in the
real World. Notice how this form of problem solving begins with possibility and proceeds to
reality. In contrast, concrete operational children start with reality-with the most obvious
predictions about a situation. When these are not confirmed, they cannot think of alternatives
and fail to solve the problem.
Adolescents' performance on Piaget's famous pendulum problem illustrates this new
approach. Suppose we present several school-age children and adolescents with strings of
different lengths, objects of different weights to attach to the strings, and a bar from which to
hang the strings. Then we ask each of them to figure out what influences the speed with
which a pendulum swings through its arc.
Formal operational adolescents come up with four hypotheses: (1) the length of the string,
(2) the weight of the object hung on it, (3) how high the object is raised before it is released,
and (4) how forcefully the object is pushed. Then, by varying one factor at a time while
holding all the others constant, they tryout each possibility. Eventually they discover that
only string length makes a difference.
In contrast, concrete operational children experiment unsystematically. They cannot
separate the effects of each variable. They may test for the effect of string length without
holding weight constant, comparing, for example, a short, light pendulum with a long, heavy
one. Also, school-age children fail to notice variables that are not immediately suggested by
the concrete materials of the task-the height at which and forcefulness with which the
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
217
pendulum is released.
18.5.2 Propositional Thought
A second important characteristic of the formal operational stage is propositional
thought. Adolescents can evaluate the logic of propositions without referring to real world
circumstances. In contrast, children can evaluate the logic of statements only by considering
them against concrete evidence in the real world.
In a study of propositional reasoning, a researcher showed children and adolescents a
pile of tokens ( plastic round coins) and asked whether statements about the tokens were
true, false, or uncertain. In one condition, the researcher hid a token in her hand and
presented the following propositions:
"Either the token in my hand is green or it is not green:' "The token in my hand is green and
it is not green." In another condition, the experimenter held either a red or a green token in
full view and made the same statements.
School-age children focused on the concrete properties of the tokens. When the token was
hidden from view, they replied that they were uncertain about both statements. When it was
visible, they judged both statements to be true if the token was green and false if it was red.
In contrast, adolescents analyzed the logic of the statements. They understood that the
"either-or" statement is always true and the "and" statement is always false, regardless of the
poker token's color.
Although Piaget did not view language as playing a central role in children's cognitive
development, he acknowledged it is more important in adolescence. Abstract thought requires
language-based systems of representation that do not stand for real things, such as those in
higher mathematics. Secondary school students use these systems in algebra and geometry.
Formal operational thought also involves verbal reasoning about abstract concepts.
Check Your Progress 2
A. Write about the implications physical changes during Adolescence
B. State the two major features Piaget’s formal operational stage
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
18.6 COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
Information-processing theorists agree with the broad outlines of Piaget's description of
adolescent cognition. The varieties of specific mechanisms of cognitive change are:
Attention becomes more focused on relevant information and better adapted to the
changing demands of tasks.
Strategies become more effective, improving storage, representation, and retrieval of
information.
Knowledge increases, easing strategy use.
Metacognition (awareness of thought) expands, leading to new insights into effective
strategies for acquiring information and solving problems.
Cognitive self-regulation improves, yielding better moment-by-moment monitoring,
evaluation, and redirection of thinking.
Processing capacity increases due to the influence of brain development. As a result,
space in working memory is freed so more information can be held at once and
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
218
combined into highly efficient, abstract representations.
As we look at some influential findings from an information-processing perspective,
some of these mechanisms change in action. Researchers regard Metacognition-as central to
the development of abstract thought.
18.6.1 Scientific Reasoning: Coordinating Theory with Evidence
The heart of scientific reasoning is coordinating theories with evidence. Deanna Kuhn has
conducted extensive research into the development of scientific reasoning, using problems
that resemble Piaget's tasks, in that several variables might affect an outcome. In one series of
studies, third, sixth, and ninth standard and adults were provided evidence, sometimes consistent
with and sometimes conflicting with theories. Then they were questioned about the
accuracy of each theory.
For example, participants were given a problem. They were asked to theorize about
which of several features of sports balls-size (large or small), color (light or dark), texture
(rough or smooth), or presence or absence of ridges on its surface-influences the quality of a
player's serve. Next, they were told about the theory of Mr. (or Ms.) S, who believes that the
ball's size is important, and the theory of Mr. (or Ms.) C, who thinks color makes a
difference. Finally, the interviewer presented evidence by placing balls with certain
characteristics in two baskets labeled "good serve" and "bad serve".
The youngest participants often ignored conflicting evidence or distorted it in ways
consistent with their preferred theory. Instead of viewing evidence as separate from and bearing
on a theory, children often blended the two into a single representation of "the way things
are." The ability to distinguish theory from evidence and to use logical rules to examine their
relationship in complex, multivariable situations improves from childhood into adolescence
and adulthood.
18.6.2 How Scientific Reasoning Develops
What factors support adolescents' skill at coordinating theory with evidence? Greater
processing capacity, permitting a theory and the effects of several variables to be compared at
once, is vital. Beyond this, metacognition-thinking about thought-is especially important.
Individuals must be able to represent the theory as an object of thought rather than as a mirror
image of reality. And they must also set aside their own theoretical preference and consider
what the evidence says as their sole basis for judgment.
How does skill in coordinating theory with evidence increase? Performance is strongly
influenced by years of schooling. But even at advanced levels of education, scientific
reasoning is rarely taught directly. Instead, in all subject-matter areas, students receive
practice in setting aside their own experiences and beliefs to infer conclusions- that follow
from information given. Repeated opportunities to pit theory against evidence prompt
adolescents to reflect on their current strategies, revise them, and become aware of the nature
of logic.
Although adolescents and adults are much better at scientific reasoning than children, they
continue to show a self-serving bias in their thinking. They apply logic more effectively to
ideas they doubt than to those they favor. Reasoning scientifically, however, requires the
metacognitive capacity to be objective rather than self-serving.
Adolescents develop formal operational thinking in a similar, step-by-step fashion on
different types of tasks. In each type of task, adolescents mastered component skills in
sequential order by expanding their metacognitive awareness. For example, on causalexperimental
tasks, they first became aware of the many variables that could "influence an
outcome. This enabled them to formulate and test hypotheses. Over time, adolescents
combined separate skills into a smoothly functioning system. They constructed a general
model that could be applied to many instances of a given type of problem. Young people
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
219
seem to form a "hypercognitive system," or supersystem, that understands, organizes, and
influences other aspects of cognition.
Piaget underscored the role of metacognition in formal operational thought when he
spoke of "operating on operations". However, information-processing findings reveal that
scientific reasoning does not result from an abrupt, stagewise change, as Piaget believed.
Instead, it develops gradually out of many specific experiences that require children and
adolescents to match theories against evidence and reflect on and evaluate their thinking.
Check Your Progress 3
Put forth the Information-processing view of cognition development
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
18.7 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
vii) Characteristics of Puberty
viii) Criteria of Puberty
ix) Causes of Puberty
x) Body Changes at Puberty
xi) The Puberty Growth Spurt
xii) Consequences of Abstract Thought
18.8 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
2. The Characteristics of Adolescence period includes
i) It is an important period
ii) It is a transitional period
iii) It is a period of change
iv) It is a Problem Age
v) It is a Time of Search for Identity
vi) It is a Dreaded Age
vii) It is a Time of Unrealism
viii) It is the Threshold of Adulthood
2. A. The implications of physical changes during adolescence are:
i) Variations in Physical Changes
ii) Effects of Physical Changes
iii) Concerns about Physical Changes
B. The two major features Piaget’s formal operational stages are:
i) Hypothetico-Deductive Reasoning
ii) Propositional Thought
3. The Information-processing view of cognition development:
i) Scientific reasoning - Coordinating theory with evidence
ii) As to how scientific reasoning develops
18.9 Lesson – End Activities
1. Why we describe the adolescent period as a period of change.
2. Adolescent is a Time of unrealisnt – Discuss.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
220
18.10 References
1. Adler, A., Understanding Human nature, London : Alley & Unwin 1948.
2. Symonds, P., The Dynamics of Huam Adjustment, New York : Appleton, 1980.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
221
Lesson – 19
EMOTIONALITY - SOCIAL INTERESTS AND MORALITY CHANGES DURING
ADOLESCENCE - SEX INTERESTS ERIKSON’S THEORY: IDENTITY VERSUS
IDENTITY CONFUSION
Contents
19.0 Aims and Objectives
19.1 Introduction
19.2 Emotionality during Adolescence
19. 2.1 Emotional Patterns in Adolescence
19.2.2 Emotional Maturity
19.3 Social Changes during Adolescence
19.3.1 Increased Peer-Group Influence
19.3.2 Changes in Social Behavior
19.3.3 New Social Groupings
19.3.4 New Values in Social Acceptance
19.3.5 New Values in Selection of leaders
19.4 Social Interests
19.4.1 Recreational Interests
19.4.2 Social Interests
19.4.3 Personal Interests
19.4.4 Educational Interests
19.4.5 Vocational Interests
19.4.6 Religious Interests
19.4.7 Interest in Status Symbols
19.5 Let Us Sum Up
19.6 Check your Progress
19.7 Lesson – End Activities
19.8 References
19.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help you understand the emotionality during adolescence, the social
behaviour and social interests during adolescence.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
xi) State the development of emotions during adolescence
xii) List the social changes that happen during adolescence
xiii) Understand the social interests during adolescence
19.1 INTRODUCTION
Adolescence was thought to be of as a period of "storm and stress" -a time of
heightened emotional tension resulting from the physical and glandular changes that are
taking place. While it is true that growth continues through the early years of adolescence, it
does so at a progressively slower rate. What growth is taking place is primarily a completion
of the pattern already set at puberty.
19.2 EMOTIONALITY DURING ADOLESCENCE
Adolescent emotionality can be attributed mainly to the fact that boys and girls come
under social pressures and face new conditions for which they received little if any
preparation during childhood.
Not all adolescents, by any means, go through a period of exaggerated storm and stress.
True, most of them do experience emotional instability from time to time, which is a logical
consequence of the necessity of making adjustments to new patterns of behavior and to new
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
222
social expectations. For example, problems related to romance are very real at this time.
While the romance is moving along smoothly, adolescents are happy, but they become
despondent when things begin to go wrong. Similarly, with the end of their schooling in
sight, adolescents begin to worry about their future.
While adolescent emotions are often intense, uncontrolled, and seemingly irrational,
there is generally an improvement in emotional behavior with each passing year. Fourteenyear-
olds, are often irritable, are easily excited, and "explode" emotionally instead of trying
to control their feelings. Sixteen-year-olds, by contrast, say they "don't believe in worrying."
Thus the storm and stress of this period lessens as early adolescence draws to a close.
19. 2.1 Emotional Patterns in Adolescence
The emotional patterns of adolescence, while similar to those of childhood, differ in the
stimuli that give rise to the emotions and, even more important, in the degree of control the
individuals exercise over the expression of their emotions. For example, being treated "'like a
child" or being treated "unfairly" is more likely to make the adolescent angry than anything
else.
Instead of having temper tantrums, however, adolescents express their anger by sulking,
refusing to speak, or loudly criticizing those who angered them. Adolescents also become
envious of those with more material possessions. While they may not complain and feel sorry
for themselves, as children do, they are likely to take a part-time job to earn money for the
material possessions they crave or even drop out of school to get these things.
19.2.2 Emotional Maturity
Boys and girls are said to have achieved emotional maturity if, by the end of adolescence,
they do not "blow up" emotionally when others are present, but wait for a convenient time
and place to let off emotional steam in a socially acceptable manner. Another important
indication of emotional maturity is that the individual assesses a situation critically before
responding to it emotionally instead of reacting to it unthinkingly as would a child or an
immature person. This results in adolescents ignoring many stimuli that would have caused
emotional outbursts when they were younger. Finally, emotionally mature adolescents are
stable in their emotional responses and they do not swing from one emotion or mood to another,
as they did earlier.
To achieve emotional maturity, adolescents must learn to get a perspective on situations
which otherwise would lead to emotional reactions. They can do this best by discussing their
problems with others. Their willingness to disclose their attitudes, feelings, and personal
problems is influenced partly by how secure they feel in their social relationships, partly by
how much they like the "target person" (the person to whom they are willing to make the
disclosure), and by how much the target person is willing to disclose to them.
In addition, if adolescents are to achieve emotional maturity, they must learn to use
emotional catharsis to clear their systems of pent-up emotional energy. This they can do by
strenuous physical exercise, in play or work, by laughing or by crying. While all of these
provide an outlet for pent-up emotional energy that accompanies control over emotional
expressions, social attitudes toward crying are unfavorable, as they are toward laughing,
unless the laughter is held in check and occurs only when the social group approves.
Check Your Progress 1
Discuss the Adolescent emotionality
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
223
19.3 SOCIAL CHANGES DURING ADOLESCENCE
The most difficult developmental tasks of adolescence relates to social adjustments.
These adjustments must be made to members of the opposite sex in a relationship that never
existed before and to adults outside the family and school environments.
To achieve the goal of adult patterns of socialization, the adolescent must make many
new adjustments, the most important-and, in many respects, the most difficult-of which are
those to the increased influence of the peer group, changes in social behavior, new social
groupings, new values in friendship selection, new values in social acceptance and rejection,
and new values in the selection of leaders.
19.3.1 Increased Peer-Group Influence
Because adolescents spend most of their time outside the home with members of the peer
group, it is understandable that peers would have a greater influence on adolescent attitudes,
speech, interests, appearance, and behavior than the family has. Most adolescents, for
example, discover that if they wear the same type of clothes as popular group members wear,
their chances of acceptance are enhanced. Similarly, if members of the peer group experiment
with alcohol, drugs, or tobacco, adolescents are likely to do the same, regardless of how they
feel about these matters.
As adolescence progresses, peer-group influence begins to wane. There are two reasons
for this. First, most adolescents want to become individuals in their own right and to be
recognized as such. The search for identity, discussed earlier in this chapter, weakens the
influence of the peer group on the adolescent. The second reason for waning of peer-group
influence is the result of the adolescent's choice of peers as companions. No longer are
adolescents interested in large group activities, as was true during their childhood days. In
adolescence there is a tendency to narrow down friendships to smaller numbers though most
adolescents want to belong to larger social groups for social activities. Because these social
activities are less meaningful to adolescents than close, personal friendships, the influence of
the larger social group becomes less pronounced than the influence of friends.
19.3.2 Changes in Social Behavior
Of all the changes that take place in social attitudes and behavior, the most pronounced is
in the area of heterosexual relationships. In a short period of time, adolescents make the
radical shift from disliking members of the opposite sex to preferring their companionship to
that of members of their own sex. Social activities, whether with members of the same sex or
with the opposite sex, usually reach their peak during the high-school years.
As a result of broader opportunities for social participation, social insight improves
among older adolescents. They are now able to judge members of the opposite sex as well as
members of their own sex better than they could when they were younger. As a result, they
make better adjustments in social situations and they quarrel less.
The greater the social participation of adolescents, the greater their social competency, as
seen in their ability to dance, to carryon conversations, to play sports and games that are
popular with agemates, and to behave correctly in different social situations. As a result, they
gain self-confidence which is expressed in poise and ease in social situations.
Whether prejudice and discrimination will increase or decrease during adolescence will
be greatly influenced by the environment in which adolescents find themselves and by the
attitudes and behavior of their friends and associates. Because adolescents, as a group, tend to
be more "choosey" in the selection of associates and friends than they were as children, they
find adolescents of different racial, religious, or socioeconomic backgrounds less congenial
than those with similar backgrounds. However, they are more likely to ignore those they find
uncongenial than to treat them in a way that expresses their feelings of superiority, as older
children do.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
224
19.3.3 New Social Groupings
The gangs of childhood gradually break up at puberty and during early adolescence as the
individual's interest’s shift from the strenuous play activities of childhood to the less
strenuous and more formal social activities of adolescence. In their place come new social
groupings. The social groupings of boys as a rule are larger and more loosely knit while those
of girls are smaller and more sharply defined. The most common social groupings during
adolescence are described below.
Close Friends: The adolescent usually has two or three close friends, or confidants. They are
of the same sex and have similar interests and abilities. Close friends have a marked influence
on one another, though they may quarrel occasionally.
Cliques: Cliques are usually made up of groups of close friends. At first they consist of
members of the same sex, but later include both boys and girls.
Crowds: Crowds, made up of cliques and groups of close friends, develop as interest in
parties and dating grows. Because crowds are large, there is less congeniality of interest
among the members and thus a greater social distance between them.
Organized Groups: Adult-directed youth groups are established by schools and community
organizations to meet the social needs of adolescents who belong to no cliques or crowds.
Many adolescents who join such groups feel regimented and lose interest in them by the time
they are sixteen or seventeen.
Gangs: Adolescents who belong to no cliques or crowds and who gain little satisfaction from
organized groups may join a gang. Gang members are usually of the same sex, and their main
interest is to compensate for peer rejection through antisocial behavior.
There are changes in some of these social groupings as adolescence progresses. Interest in
organized groups, whose activities are planned and to a large extent controlled by adults,
wanes rapidly as independence-conscious adolescents present being told what to do. Only if
the control of the activities of these groups is turned over to them, with minimum of adult
advice and interference, will interest continue.
Crowds tend to disintegrate in late adolescence and are replaced by loosely associated
groups of couples. This is especially true of adolescents who go to work at the completion of
high school. At work they are in contact with people of all ages, most of whom have friends
and families of their own outside their jobs. Unless non college older adolescents have
friends from their school days who live and work near enough to make contacts possible they
may find themselves limited to a few friends connected with their work and out of touch with
any group large enough to form a crowd.
By contrast, the influence of the gang tends to increase as adolescence progresses. This
influence is often expressed in violent behavior committed by gang members. Adolescents
want as friends those whose interests and values are similar to theirs, who understand them
and make them feel secure, and in whom they can confide problems and discuss matters they
feel they cannot share with parents or teachers.
Most adolescents claim they want "someone to be trusted, someone to talk to, and some
one who is dependable". Because of these changed values, childhood friends will not necessarily
be friends in adolescence. Nor are adolescents interested only in friends of their own
sex. Interest in the opposite sex becomes increasingly stronger as adolescence progresses. As
a result, by the end of adolescence there is often a preference for friends of the opposite sex,
though both boys and girls continue to have a few intimate friends of their own sex with
whom they associate constantly.
To most young adolescents, popularity means having a large number of friends. As they
grow older, the kind of friends they have, becomes more important than the number.
However, adolescent values regarding the "right" kind of friends tend to change from one
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
225
year to another, depending on the values of the group with which they are identified at that
time.
Because adolescents know what they want in their friends, they insist upon the right to
select them without adult interference. This often leads to two consequences that interfere
with the stability of adolescent friendships. First, as a result of their inexperience-especially
with members of the opposite sex-they may choose friends who turn out to be less congenial
than they had thought they would be; quarreling often occurs then and friendships are broken.
Second, as in other areas of their lives, adolescents tend to be unrealistic concerning the
standards they set up for their friends. They then become critical of them if they do not come
up to these standards and try to reform them. This again usually leads to quarreling and the
break-up of friendships. In time, most adolescents become more realistic about other people,
just as they do about themselves. As a result, they are less critical and more acceptant of their
friends.
19.3.4 New Values in Social Acceptance
Just as adolescents have new values concerning their friends, so they have new values
concerning acceptable or unacceptable members of different peer groups, such as cliques,
crowds, or gangs, for example. These values are based largely on peer-group values which
are used to judge members of the group. Adolescents soon discover that they are judged by
the same standards by which they judge others.
No one trait or characteristic pattern of behavior will guarantee social acceptance during
adolescence. Instead, acceptance depends upon a constellation of traits and behavior patternsthe
acceptance syndrome-all of which make adolescents fun to be with and add to the
prestige of the clique or crowd with which they are identified. Similarly, no one trait or
behavior pattern alienates adolescents from their peers. Instead, there is a grouping of traitsthe
alienation syndrome -that makes others dislike and reject them.
19.3.5 New Values in Selection of leaders
Because adolescents feel that the leaders of their peer groups represent them in the eyes
of society, they therefore want leaders of superior ability who will be admired and respected
by others and who, in turn, will reflect favorably on them. Because there are so many
different kinds of groups in adolescence - athletic, social, intellectual, religious, and class or
community groups-the leader of one group will not necessarily have the ability to be the
leader of another. Leadership is now a function of the situation as it is in adult life.
In general, however, adolescents expect their leaders to have certain qualities. While a
good physique, in and of itself, does not makes leaders, it gives them prestige and, at the
same time, contributes favorably to their self concepts. The adolescent leader is in excellent
health and thus is energetic and eager to do things, both of which contribute to the quality of
initiative.
The clothes-conscious adolescent expects leaders to be attractive and well groomed. The
characteristic leader will also be slightly above average in intelligence, academic
achievement, and level of maturity. As a rule, leaders in adolescent social activities come
from families of higher socioeconomic status than non leaders. This not only gives them
prestige in the eyes of their peers but also makes possible better dressing and grooming, the
possession of social know-how, opportunities for entertaining, and participation in group
activities.
Because leaders, as a rule, are more active participants in social life than non leaders,
they develop social insight and self-insight. They can judge themselves realistically and can
size up the interests and wishes of the members of the groups they lead. Leaders are not "selfbound"
in the sense that they are so concerned with their personal interests and problems that
they cannot direct their energies outward and concern themselves with the interests and
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
226
problems of the other members of their groups.
Perhaps the most important single factor that contributes to leadership is personality.
Leaders have been found to be more responsible, more extroverted, more energetic, more
resourceful, and more able to take initiative than non leaders. They are emotionally stable,
well-adjusted, happy individuals with few neurotic tendencies
Check Your Progress 2
Explain the changes in social behavior during adolescence
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
19.4 SOCIAL INTERESTS
There is no such thing as a universal adolescent interest as of today. The reason for this is
that the interests of adolescents depend upon their sex, their intelligence, the environment in
which they live, the opportunities they have had for developing interests, what their peers are
interested in, and their status in the social group, their innate abilities, and the interests of
their families, and many other factors. Since girls are expected to behave in a feminine way
and boys in a masculine way, it is not surprising that girls' interests during adolescence are
usually very different from boys' interests.
As adolescence progresses, many of the interests that were carried over from childhood
wane and are replaced by more mature interests. Also, because of the greater responsibilities
older adolescents are expected to assume and the consequent decrease in time to spend as
they wish, many older adolescents a re forced to limit the range of their interests. This is
especially true of recreational interests.
Furthermore, with experience most adolescents acquire a different and more mature sense
of values. This is reflected in a shift in emphasis on different interests. Interests that were of
major importance in early adolescence, such as clothes and appearance, become less
important, while interest in a career now becomes dominant.
In spite of variations, certain adolescent interests are fairly universal as of today, though
they may vary from one part of the country to another and with the different social classes
within each area. All young adolescents possess these interests to a greater or lesser extent,
and I they all have certain specific interests that fall within the different categories, the most
important of which are recreational interests, social interests, personal interests, educational
interests, vocational interests, religious interests, and interest in status symbols.
19.4.1 Recreational Interests
As adolescence progresses, there is a breaking away from recreations that require much
expenditure of energy and the development of a preference for recreations in which the
adolescent is a passive spectator. In early adolescence, there is a carry-over of some of the
play activities of the early years and the introduction of new and more mature forms of recreation.
Gradually, the childish forms of play disappear, and when early adolescence comes to a
close, the individual's recreational pattern is much the same as it will be during the latter part
of adolescence and the early years of adulthood.
Because of the pressures of schoolwork, home duties, extracurricular activities, and
after-school or weekend jobs, most adolescents have far less time for recreation than they did
when they were younger. As a result, they select the kinds of activities that they enjoy most
or in which they excel. This limits the number of their activities.
The number of recreations adolescents engage in is also greatly influenced by how
popular they are. Because many of the recreations of adolescents require participants from the
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
227
peer group, the adolescent who does not belong to a clique and who has few friends is forced
to concentrate on solitary forms of recreation.
19.4.2 Social Interests
Social interests depend partly on what opportunities adolescents have to develop such
interests and partly on how popular they are with members of the peer group. An adolescent
whose family's socioeconomic status is low, for example, will have fewer opportunities to
develop an interest in parties and dancing than adolescents from more favorable home
backgrounds. Similarly, an adolescent who is unpopular will have a limited repertoire of
social interests.
19.4.3 Personal Interests
Interest in themselves is the strongest interest young adolescents have, partly because
they realize that their social acceptance is markedly influenced by their general appearance,
and partly because they know the social group judges them in terms of their material
possessions, independence, and school and social affiliations, as well as the amount of
spending money they have. These are "status symbols" that will enhance young adolescents'
prestige in the eyes of peers and hence increase their chances for greater social acceptance.
Interest in Appearance: Interest in appearance covers not only clothes but personal
adornment, grooming, attractive and sex-appropriate physical features. Cross and Cross have
explained why a p pearance is so important that it becomes a dominant personal interest.
According to them, "Beauty and physical attractiveness are of great practical importance for
human beings. Social acceptance, popularity, mate selection and careers are all affected by an
individual's physical attractiveness". As children develop, the appearance they presentespecially
to their peers-is a strong indicator of their interest in socialization. The reason is
that it is a proof of their togetherness with their peers.
Interest in Clothes: Because their personal and social adjustments are greatly influenced by
their age-mates' attitudes toward their clothes, most adolescents are anxious to conform to
what the group approves of in the matter of dress. As Ryan has pointed out, "One of the
primary requirements of clothing for the young adolescent is that their clothing meets the
approval of the peer group".
While boys claim not to be interested in clothes, grooming, or appearance, their behavior
indicates that their interest is greater than they will admit like girls, they recognize that
appearance plays an important role in social acceptance. This interest is heightened when
they reach the end of their schooling and prepare to enter the world of work. They realize that
an attractive appearance facilitates their getting and holding a job.
Interest in Achievements: Achievements bring personal satisfaction as well as social
recognition. That is why achievements, whether in sports, school work, or social activities
become such a strong interest as adolescence progresses.
However, if achievements are to bring satisfaction to adolescents, they must be in areas
that are important to their peers and carry prestige in the eyes of the peer group. If their peers
are interested in academic success, for example, good grades will be a satisfactory
achievement. If, on the other hand, little prestige is associated with good grades and much
prestige with athletic success, academic achievements will bring little satisfaction to the
adolescent. Adolescents tend to aspire unrealistically high. Therefore they often do not get
the satisfaction from their achievements that they would get if their aspirations were more
realistic. When they fail to reach their goals, their achievements bring them little satisfaction.
Interest in Independence: A strong desire for independence develops in early adolescence
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
228
and reaches a peak as this period draws to a close. This leads to many clashes with parents
and other adults in authority. Because girls are expected to conform more to parental wishes
than boys are, they rebel more against home restraints. Much of the radicalism of young
adolescents can be traced to their attempts to think and act independently. If, however, adult
authority is relaxed gradually, so that adolescents can see themselves reaching their goals,
there is far less rebellion with its accompanying friction.
Interest in Money: Every adolescent sooner or later discovers that money is the key to
independence. As long as parents pay their bills and give them spending money, parents call
control adolescents' behavior. When, on the other hand, adolescents have money, they can
enjoy independence. Interest in money therefore becomes an important element in
independence. This interest centers mainly on how to earn the most money possible,
regardless of the kind of work done.
19.4.4 Educational Interests
Typically, young adolescents complain about school in general and about restrictions,
homework, required courses, food in the cafeteria, and the way the school is run. They are
critical of their teachers and the way they teach. This is the "thing to do." Young adolescents
who want to be popular with their peers must avoid creating the impression that they are
"brains." This is even more important for girls than for boys because less prestige is
associated with academic achievement among girls than among boys. However, in spite of
their stated attitudes, most young adolescents get along well both academically and socially
in school and they secretly like it.
The attitudes of older adolescents toward education are greatly influenced by their
vocational interests. If they are aspiring to occupations which require education beyond high
school, they will regard education as a stepping-stone. They will be interested in the courses
they feel will be useful to them in their chosen field of work. As is true of the younger adolescent,
the older adolescent considers success in sports and social life as important as academic
work as a stepping-stone to future success. Many factors influence the younger as well as the
older adolescent's attitude toward education.
There are three types of adolescents who have little interest in education and who usually
dislike school. They are, first, adolescents whose parents have unrealistically high aspirations
for their academic, athletic, or social achievements and who are constantly prodding them to
come up to these goals. The second type are those who find little acceptance among their
classmates and who, as a result, feel that they are missing out on the fun their age-mates are
having in extracurricular activities. Third, early maturers who feel conspicuously large
among their classmates and who, because they look older than they actually are, are often
expected to do better academic work than they are capable of.
Adolescents who have little interest in education usually show their lack of interest in the
following ways. They become underachievers, working below their capacities in all school
subjects or in the subjects they lack interest in. Others who are disinterested in education
become truants and try to gain parental permission to withdraw from school before the legal
age for leaving. Still others become dropouts as soon as they reach the legal age of school
leaving, regardless of whether they have finished the present standard. This is especially true
of early maturers who find school not only uninteresting but often a humiliating experience.
19.4.5 Vocational Interests
Boys and girls of high school age begin to think seriously about their futures. Boys are
usually more seriously concerned about an occupation than girls, many of whom regard a job
as just a stopgap until marriage.
Boys, typically, want glamorous and exciting jobs, regardless of the ability required or
the chances that such jobs will be available for them. They also want jobs with high prestige,
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
229
even if they pay less than those with lower prestige. Many boys from low status families hope
to achieve higher social status through their occupations. Girls, as a rule, show a preference
for occupations with greater security and less demand on their time. In their vocational
choices, they usually stress service to others, such as teaching or nursing.
By late adolescence, interest in a life career has often become a source of great concern.
At this time, adolescents learn to distinguish between vocational choice, vocational preference,
and vocational aspirations. Older adolescents are concerned about what they would like
to do and what they are capable of doing. The more they hear and talk about different lines of
work, the-less sure they are of what they would like to do. They are also concerned about
how they can get the kind of jobs they want.
Furthermore, older adolescents have a growing realization of how much it costs to live
and they also know what young people, just out of school, can expect to earn. As a result of
this greater realism, they approach the choice of their careers with a more practical and more
realistic attitude than they had when they were younger.
During childhood and early adolescence, many boys and girls judge different lines of
work, such as engineering and medicine, in terms of the stereotypes presented in the mass
media. As near-adults, they begin to judge them in terms of their abilities and of the time and
money required for training for these lines of work. While prestige is still an important factor
in vocational selection, the older adolescent is more concerned about the autonomy,
authority, and security the occupation will give.
Because their attitudes toward vocations have gradually become more realistic, most
adolescents change their minds often about their future occupations. They are in an
"exploratory stage" and may take after-school or summer jobs in fields they think might
interest them as a lifetime career. This experience gives them more information on which to
base their final decisions.
19.4.6 Religious Interests
Contrary to popular opinion, adolescents of today are interested in religion and feel that it
plays an important role in their lives. They talk about religion, take courses in it in school and
in college, visit churches/temples/mosques, and join various religious cults.
Many boys and girls begin to question the religious concepts and beliefs of their
childhood and this had led adolescence to be called the period of religious doubt. However,
Wagner maintains that what is often interpreted as "religious doubt" is, in reality, religious
questioning.
On the other hand, adolescents now attend church, Sunday school, and church social
events far less than adolescents of previous generations. This suggests that many of them are
disillusioned with organized religion, but not uninterested in religion per se, Jones has
explained, "There is more a decrease in enthusiasm and in positive feeling for the church than
an increase in antagonism against it." He say further, that the change in interest in religion in
adolescence reflects not a lack of belief but "a disillusionment with the church establishment
and the use of its beliefs and preachments in the solution of current social, civic, and
economic problems".
The changes in religious interest during adolescence are even more radical than the
changes in vocational interests. Like childhood vocational interests, childhood concepts of
religion are basically unrealistic, and the adolescent may become critical of earlier beliefs.
19.4.7 Interest in Status Symbols
Status symbols are prestige symbols that tell others that the person who has them is
superior or has a higher status in the group than other group members. During adolescence,
status symbols serve four important functions: they tell others that the adolescent has a high
or even a higher socioeconomic status than other members of the peer group; that the ado-
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
230
lescent is superior in some achievement that is valued by the group; that the adolescent is
affiliated with the group and is an accepted member of it because of appearance or actions
similar to those of other group members; and that the adolescent has a near-adult status in
society.
If, for example, adolescents have cars of their own as soon as they are legally able to
drive; if their families have large homes in prestigious neighborhoods; and if they have
money to spend without having to work for it, these proclaim the superior socioeconomic
status of the adolescent.
Because being grown up or near-adult means so much to almost all adolescents of today,
a new type of status symbol has become popular among them. It is known as engaging in
tabooed pleasures -forms of recreation that are thought of as symbolic of adults, and which
parents and teachers maintain adolescents are "too young" to engage in. The most common
tabooed pleasures adolescents engage in to symbolize their near-adult status and their identity
with the peer group are premarital sex, smoking, drinking, and use of different types of drugs.
Check Your Progress 3
Mention the adolescent social interest as of today
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
19.5 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Emotionality during adolescence
ii) Social Changes during adolescence
iii) Increased Peer-Group influence
iv) Changes in Social Behavior
v) Social Interests during adolescence
vi) Recreational Interests
vii) Educational Interests
viii) Vocational Interests
ix) Religious Interests
x) Interest in Status Symbols
19.6 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. The Adolescent emotionality depends on:
iv) Patterns of emotional during adolescence
v) The emotional maturity of the adolescence
2. The changes in social behavior during adolescence are:
a. Due to peer-group influence
b. The formation of new social groupings
c. Due to new values in social acceptance
d. Due to new values in selection of leaders
3. The adolescent social interest as of today are:
i) Recreational Interests
ii) Personal Interests
iii) Educational Interests
iv) Vocational Interests
v) Religious Interests
vi) Interest in Status Symbols
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
231
19.7 Lesson – End Activities
1. Briefly explain the influence of emotional skills on social development.
2. Recreational Interest leads to positive energy – Discuss.
19.8 References
1. Erickson, E., Childhood and Society, New York : Norton, 1960.
2. Piaget, J., The Origins of Intelligence in Children, New York : International Universities
Press, 1952.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
232
LESSON – 20
CHANGES IN MORALITY DURING ADOLESCENCE – SEX INTERESTS -
ERIKSON’S THEORY: IDENTITY VERSUS IDENTITY CONFUSION
Contents
20.0 Aims and Objectives
20.1 Introduction
20.2 Fundamental Changes in Morality during Adolescence
20.2.1 Changes in Moral Concepts
20.2.2 Building a Moral Code
20.2.3 Inner Control of Behavior
20.3 Sex Interests during Adolescence
20.3.1 Development of Heterosexuality
20.3.2 New Patterns of Heterosexuality
20.3.3 Common Reasons for Dating During Adolescence
20.3.4 New Attitudes toward Sexual Behavior
20.4 Erikson’s Theory: Identity versus Identity Confusion
20.5 Let Us Sum Up
20.6 Check your Progress
20.7 Lesson – End Activities
20.8 References
20.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help you understand changes in morality, sex interest during
adolescence.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
i) state the changes that happen during adolescence in morality
ii) discuss the interest in sex during adolescence
iii) understand the identity and identity confusion
20.1 INTRODUCTION
One of the important developmental tasks adolescents must master is learning what the
group expects of them and then being willing to mold their behavior to conform to these
expectations without the constant guidance, supervision, prodding’s, and threats of
punishment they experienced as children. They are expected to replace the specific moral
concepts of childhood with general moral principles and to formulate these into moral code
which will act as a guide to their behaviour. Equally important, they must now exercise
control over their behaviour, a responsibility that was formerly assumed by parents and
teachers.
20.2 FUNDAMENTAL CHANGES IN MORALITY DURING ADOLESCENCE
The individual's moral outlook becomes progressively more abstract and less concrete
Moral convictions become more concerned with what is right and less concerned with
what is wrong. Justice emerges as a dominant moral force
Moral judgment becomes increasingly cognitive. This encourages the adolescent to
analyze social and personal codes more vigorously than during childhood and to
decide on moral issues
Moral judgment becomes less egocentric
Moral judgment becomes psychologically expensive in the sense that it takes an emotional
toll and creates psychological tension.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
233
By adolescence boys and girls have reached what Piaget has called the stage of formal
operations in cognitive ability. They are now capable of considering all possible ways of
solving a particular problem and can reason on the basis of hypotheses or propositions. Thus
they can look at their problems from several points of view and can take many factors into
account when solving them.
According to Kohlberg, the third level of moral development, post conventional
morality, should be reached during adolescence. This is the level of self accepted principles,
and it consists of two stages. In the first stage, the individual believes that there should be
flexibility in moral beliefs to make it possible to modify and change moral standards if this
will be advantageous to group members as a whole. In the second stage, individuals conform
to both social standards and to internalized ideals to avoid self condemnation rather than to
avoid social censure. In this stage, morality is based on respect for others rather than on
personal desires.
Even with the best foundations, the three major tasks in achieving adult moralityreplacing
specific concepts with general moral concepts, formulating these newly developed
concepts into a moral code as a guideline for behavior, and assuming control over one's own
behavior-are difficult for many adolescents. Some fail to make the shift to adult morality
during adolescence and must finish this task in early adulthood. Others not only fail to make
the shift but they build a moral code on socially unacceptable moral concepts.
20.2.1 Changes in Moral Concepts
Two conditions make the replacement of specific moral concepts with generalized
concepts of right and wrong more difficult than it should be. The first is lack of guidance in
learning how to generalize specific concepts. Believing that adolescents have already learned
the major principles of right and wrong, parents and teachers frequently put little emphasis on
teaching them to see the relationship between the specific principles they learned earlier and
the general principles that are essential to control over behavior in adult life. Only in new
areas of behavior, such as relationships with members of the opposite sex, do adults feel that
there is any real need for further moral training.
The second condition that makes the replacement of specific moral concepts with
generalized ones difficult has to do with the kind of discipline the adolescent is subjected to
at home and in school. Because parents and teachers assume that adolescents know what is
right, their major emphasis in discipline is on punishment for what they regard as intentional
misbehavior. Little emphasis is placed on explaining to the adolescent why certain things are
right and others wrong, and even less is placed on rewarding the adolescent for doing the
right thing.
20.2.2 Building a Moral Code
When they reach adolescence, children will no longer accept in an unquestioning way a
moral code told to them by parents, teachers, or even their contemporaries. They now want to
build their own moral codes on the basis of concepts of right and wrong which they have
changed and modified to meet their more mature level of development and which they have
supplemented with laws and rules learned from parents and teachers. Some adolescents even
supplement their moral codes with knowledge derived from their religious teaching.
Building a moral code is difficult for adolescents because of inconsistencies in standards
of right and wrong they encounter in daily life. These inconsistencies confuse them and
impede their progress in building a moral code which is not only satisfactory to them but
which will also lead to socially approved behavior. Sooner or later, most adolescents
discover, for example, that peers of different socioeconomic, religious, or racial backgrounds
have different codes of right and wrong; that their parents' and teachers' codes are often
stricter than those of their contemporaries; and that in spite of the breaking down of the
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
234
traditional sex-approved roles, there is still a "double standard" which is far more lenient for
boys than for girls.
While older children may condemn lying on moral grounds, many adolescents feel that
"social lies," or lies told to avoid hurting other people's feelings, are sometimes justified.
Much the same sort of confusion is apparent in high school and college students' attitudes
toward cheating. Many feel that since it is so widespread, their contemporaries must condone
it, and they also claim that it is justified when they are pressured to get good grades in order
to be accepted by a college and thus succeed socially and economically in later life. As
interest in members of the opposite sex increases, adolescents discover that certain patterns of
behavior are not only approved but even applauded for boys while they are harshly condemned
for girls.
20.2.3 Inner Control of Behavior
Because parents and teachers cannot watch adolescents as closely as they did when they
were children, adolescents must now assume responsibility for control over their own
behavior. While it was formerly believed that fear-either of punishment or of social
disapproval-was the best deterrent to wrongdoing, today it is recognized that "outercontrolled"
sources of motivation are effective only when there is possiblity that others will
find out about the misbehavior and punish those responsible for it.
In the case of juvenile delinquents, punishment not only does not deter willful
wrongdoing but also often contributes to it. There is also evidence that fear of being shamed
loses its effectiveness as a deterrent to misbehavior when there is little likelihood that others
will know of the misbehavior, or if individuals feel they will be able to rationalize their
actions or project the blame for them on others, and thus avoid punishment or social
disapproval.
Studies of moral development have emphasized that the only effective way people of
any age can control their own behavior is through the development of a conscience-an inner
force that makes external controls unnecessary. When children or adolescents learn to
associate pleasant emotions with group-approved behavior, and unpleasant emotions with
group-disapproved behavior, they will have the necessary motivation to behave in accordance
with group standards. Under such conditions, individuals feel guilty when they realize that
their behavior is falling below social expectations, while shame is aroused only when they are
aware of unfavorable judgments of their behavior by members of the social group. Behavior
that is controlled by guilt is thus inner-controlled while that controlled by shame is outercontrolled.
In morally mature individuals, both guilt and shame are present. However, guilt plays a
more important role than shame in controlling the individual's behavior in the absence of
external control. Relatively few adolescents reach this level and, as a result, they cannot
correctly be called "morally mature" people.
Check Your Progress 1
Discuss the fundamental changes in morality that happen during Adolescence
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
20.3 SEX INTERESTS DURING ADOLESCENCE
To master the important developmental tasks of forming new and more mature
relationships with members of the opposite sex, and of playing the approved role for one's
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
235
own sex, young adolescents must acquire more mature and more complete concepts of sex
than they had as children. The motivation to do so comes partly from social pressures but
mainly from the adolescent's interest in and curiosity about sex.
Because of their growing interest in sex, adolescent boys and girls seek more and more
information about it. Few adolescents feel that they can learn all they want to know about sex
from their parents. Consequently they take advantage of whatever sources of information are
available to them-sex hygiene courses in school or college, discussions with their friends,
books on sex, or experimentation through masturbation, petting, or intercourse. By the end of
adolescence, most boys and girls have enough information about sex to satisfy their curiosity.
Interest of adolescents are primarily in knowing about sex, girls are especially curious
about birth control, the "Pill," abortion, and pregnancy. Boys, on the other hand, want to
know about venereal diseases, enjoyment of sex, sexual intercourse, and birth control. Their
major interest is in sexual intercourse, its context and its consequences.
20.3.1 Development of Heterosexuality
The first developmental task relating to sex adolescents must master is forming new and
more mature relationships with members of the opposite sex. This is far from easy for both
boys and girls, after the years during late childhood when members of the two sexes had their
own gangs and interests, and during puberty when both boys and girls developed attitudes of
resentment against members of the opposite sex.
Now that they are sexually mature, both boys and girls begin to have new attitudes
toward members of the opposite sex, and to develop an interest not only in members of the
opposite sex but also in activities in which they are involved. This new interest, which begins
to develop when sexual maturation is complete, is romantic in nature and is accompanied by
a strong desire to win the approval of members of the opposite sex. Gradually, this desire
takes the place of the desire that dominated in childhood-the desire to win the approval and
acceptance of members of the same sex.
Development of interest in members of the opposite sex – heterosexuality - follows a
predictable pattern. However, there are variations in ages at which the adolescent reaches
different stages in this development, partly because of differences in age of sexual maturing
and partly because of differences in opportunities to develop this interest. Interest in members
of the opposite sex is also markedly influenced by patterns of interest among the adolescent's
friends. If they are interested in activities involving members of both sexes, the adolescent
must also be able to retain status in the peer group.
There are two separate and distinct elements in the development of heterosexuality. The
first is the development of patterns of behavior involving members of the two sexes and the
second is the development of attitudes relating to relationships between members of the two
sexes.
In past generations, these two aspects of heterosexuality were rigidly prescribed by
tradition and little leeway was given to adolescents to deviate from these prescribed patterns
of behavior and attitudes. There was, for example, a socially approved pattern of behavior
known as "courtship," and any deviation from this pattern, either in behavior or in timing,
was frowned upon. Those involved were subjected to social disapproval or scorn.
It was not, for example, considered proper for boys to kiss girls on their first dates (in
certain cultures). When girls permitted or encouraged this, boys often regarded them as "easy
marks"-a label that did not encourage them to consider the girls seriously as future mates.
Similarly, there were socially approved attitudes both boys and girls were expected to
have toward members of the opposite sex and toward their relationships with each other.
These attitudes were colored by unrealism and were highly romanticized. Instead of seeing
boys as boys, similar in some respects and different in others from girls, boys were romanticized
to the point where they were not recognizable as boys but rather were thought of
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
236
as "conquering heroes." The tendency to romanticize girls was even more pronounced. It
used to be said that boys in love put their girls on pedestals and literally worshiped them.
These highly romanticized attitudes were also present in activities in which both sexes
were involved. In the past, a date with a girl meant getting dressed up, taking her a gift of
candy, flowers, or a book, seeing her under the parental roof, and leaving at a prescribed
time. Attitudes toward what young people did on dates were also carefully prescribed.
Kissing and petting were considered in bad taste if not actually wrong unless the couple was
engaged. Even then, petting was very limited and within the bounds of what was regarded as
"proper" and "safe."
New social attitudes toward sex, the ready availability of contraceptive devices and the
legalization of abortion in many states have brought about radical changes in sexual behavior
during adolescence and in attitudes toward sex and sexual behavior. While these changes are
by no means universal, they are widespread enough to be regarded as "typical" of adolescents
today in urban and suburban centers and, to a lesser extent, in small towns and rural
communities.
20.3.2 New Patterns of Heterosexuality
There are two features that distinguish adolescent heterosexuality today from that of past
generations. These are the telescoping of stages in heterosexual behavior, and greater
permissiveness. For the most part, adolescents now follow a pattern of development in their
heterosexual behavior similar to the traditional pattern, though they pass from one stage of
the pattern to another far more quickly than in the past. Kissing on the first date today, for
example, is common. In the past, this might readily jeopardize a beginning relationship
between a boy and a girl.
When adolescents prefer alternate lifestyles, especially in the later stages of heterosexual
behavior, they often have negative attitudes toward marriage, due to unfavorable parent-child
relationships or negative perceptions of marriage, based on their parents' marriage. "Many
youths turn to alternate life styles in an attempt to find the close relationship which they have
not found in their own families".
Dating (not all cultures or religions permit this practice) starts earlier today than in past
generations and it quickly develops into a going-steady relationship. It is not unusual, for
example, for dating to begin when girls are thirteen years old and the going steady stage to
occur before they are fourteen. Dating serves many purposes in the lives of today's
adolescents, the most common of which are explained below. Because dating serves different
purposes, it is understandable that adolescents want different types of partners for different
types of dating.
Many young adolescents prefer going steady to playing the field because it gives them a
feeling of security to know that they have an available partner for all social activities. Larson
et al., after studying boys and girls in high school, concluded that those who were going
steady not only feel personally insecure but also have lower educational-occupational aspirations
than those who were not going steady at so early an age.
Going steady does not necessarily involve plans for the future or a commitment to
marry. It does, however, sanction advanced forms of sexual behavior. Learning the accepted
dating code of the peer group is part of adolescent socialization. It is an accepted form of
behavior, for example, to kiss on the first date and to begin petting then or on a subsequent
date. Since adolescents begin to date and go steady earlier today than in the past, they engage
in different forms of sexual intimacy at earlier ages, and intercourse is common among
couples who go steady.
With the trend toward coeducational dormitories in many colleges and universities in
western countries, the relaxing of restrictions on visiting hours in segregated dormitories and
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
237
of requirements for residence in campus dormitories, and the widespread practice among adolescents
of traveling together in groups composed of members of both sexes, living together
in a premarital relationship is becoming an accepted pattern among older adolescents in
college. This is also true of older adolescents who go to work after completing high school
and live apart from their parents in order to enjoy a freedom they would not otherwise have.
There is also a growing trend toward communal living, patterned after the lifestyle of the socalled
Hippie culture.
The changed pattern of sexual behavior among today's adolescents is not regarded by
them as wrong or as promiscuous because usually they have only one sexual partner at a time
and in most cases, they expect to marry at some time in the future. Even when parents object
to these relationships, many adolescents continue to maintain them.
There are many reasons for this new pattern of sexual behavior. Among these are the
belief that it is the "thing to do" because everyone else does it; that girls and boys who are
still virgins by the time they reach senior year in high school are "different," and to
adolescents this means "inferior"; that they must comply with pressures from the peer group
if they wish to maintain their status in the group; and that such behavior is an expression of a
meaningful relationship which fills the need every adolescent has for a close association with
others, especially when this need is not filled by family relationships.
20.3.3 Common Reasons for Dating During Adolescence
Recreation: When dating is for recreation, adolescents want their partners to have the social
skills members of the peer group consider important: to be good sports and pleasant
companions. In the case of boys, having a car or access to one and money to spend are
essential.
Socialization: When peer-group members divide into dating couples, boys and girls must
date if they are to be accepted members of the peer group and enjoy its social activities.
Dating partners must want to participate in social activities and have the social skills, time,
money, and independence needed for participation in these activities.
Status: Dating for both boys and girls, especially when it takes the form of going steady,
gives them status in the peer group. The more popular the dating partner is with members of
the peer group and the more prestigious the socioeconomic status of the dating partner's
family in the community, the more this will reflect favorably on the adolescent. Dating under
such conditions is primarily a steppingstone to higher status in the peer group.
Courtship: In the courtship pattern of adolescence, dating plays a dominant role. Because
adolescents are in love and hope and plan to marry eventually, they give serious thought to
the suitability of the dating partner as a future mate.
Mate Selection: Adolescents who want to marry when they complete high school and have
no plans for higher education look upon dating as an opportunity to tryout different dating
partners to see if they have the qualities they want in their future mates. Major emphasis is
put on compatibility of interests and temperament and on ways of showing affection. This
justifies, in their minds, heavy petting and coitus. Many adolescents who are interested in
early marriage regard dating as a trial-and-error way to pick out their future mates.
20.3.4 New Attitudes toward Sexual Behavior
Marked as changes in sexual behavior are, changes in sexual attitudes are even more
pronounced. Behavior which, scarcely a generation age, would shock adolescents if it
occurred among their peers, and give them feelings of guilt and shame if it occurred in their
own lives, now are taken for granted as right and normal, or at least permissible. Even
premarital intercourse is regarded as "right" if the individuals involved are deeply in love and
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
238
committed to each other. Coitus with affection is more acceptable than petting without
affection.
In the past, girls who are engaged in heavy petting and intercourse lost the respect of boys
even though they may have been more popular as dating partners than girls who refused to
engage in these forms of sexual behavior. Today, adolescent boys maintain that marrying a
virgin is unimportant to them, though they tend to lose respect for girls who are too
promiscuous and too permissive. Thus, the "double standard" is gradually giving way to a
single standard which holds for girls as well as for boys.
Accompanying these changed attitudes are strong ideas about right and wrong in regard
to sexual behavior. Behavior which adolescents feel is "right" is accompanied by favorable
attitudes, while behavior they feel is "wrong" is accompanied by unfavorable attitudes.
Adolescents of today feel that expressions of love, regardless of the form the expression
takes, are good, provided both partners feel strongly about each other. On the other hand, if
love is missing and sexual behavior is engaged in only because others do it, or because it is
the way for a girl to ensure having a date for social events, or for excitement, they regard this
as wrong. They also regard it as wrong for a boy to force a girl to engage in intercourse if she
is unwilling or for a girl to use intercourse as a way to force a boy into marriage.
There are also new social attitudes toward pre-marital pregnancy and toward keeping the
child, even when there is no intention on the part of the parents to marry. Today, some
parents accept daughters with illegitimate children and share in the care and expenses
involved in the care. Other adolescents, when they become aware of pregnancy, marry even
though they are still students and have no independent source of support.
Older adolescents in urban and suburban communities have a more permissive attitude
toward living together without marriage-cohabitation -than those in small towns or rural
districts. Older adolescents in college tend to cohabit more frequently than those who go to
work when they finish high school. Living together without marriage is also more common
and more condoned by peers among older adolescents than among younger adolescents who
are still living under the parental roof and have not yet reached the legal age of school
leaving.
20.4 ERIKSON’S THEORY: IDENTITY VERSUS IDENTITY CONFUSION
Erikson was the first to recognize identity as the major personality achievement of
adolescence and as a crucial step toward becoming a productive, happy adult. Constructing
an identity involves defining who you are, what you value, and the directions you choose to
pursue in life. One expert described it as an explicit theory of oneself as a rational agent-one
who acts on the basis of reason, takes responsibility for those actions, and can explain them.
This search for what is true and real about the self is the driving force behind many new
commitments-to a sexual orientation; a vocation; interpersonal relationships; community
involvement; ethnic group membership; and moral, political, religious, and cultural ideals.
Erikson called the psychological conflict of adolescence identity versus identity
confusion. Successful outcomes of earlier stages pave the way to its positive resolution.
Young people who reach adolescence with a weak sense of trust have trouble finding ideals
to have faith in. Those with little autonomy or initiative do not engage in the active
exploration required to choose among alternatives. And those who lack a sense of industry
fail to select a vocation that matches their interests and skills.
Although the seeds of identity formation are planted early, not until adolescence do young
people become absorbed in this task? According to Erikson, in complex societies, teenagers
experience an identity crisis--a temporary period of confusion and distress as they experiment
with alternatives before settling on values and goals. Adolescents who go through a process
of inner soul-searching eventually arrive at a mature identity. They sift through
characteristics that defined the self in childhood and combine them with new commitments.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
239
Then they mold these into a solid inner core that provides a sense of stability as they move
through different roles in daily life. Once formed, identity continues to be refined in
adulthood as people reevaluate earlier commitments and choices.
Current theorists agree with Erikson that questioning of values, plans, and priorities is
necessary for a mature identity, but they no longer refer to this process as a "crisis". For
some young people, identity development is traumatic and disturbing, but for most it is not.
Exploration better describes the typical adolescent's gradual, uneventful approach to identity
formation. By trying out various life possibilities and moving toward making enduring
decisions, young people forge an organized self-structure.
Erikson described the negative outcome of adolescence as identity confusion. Some
young people appear shallow and directionless, either because earlier conflicts have been
resolved negatively or because society restricts their choices to ones that do not match their
abilities and desires. As a result, they are unprepared for the psychological challenges of
adulthood. For example, individuals find it difficult to risk the self-sharing involved in
Erikson's young adult stage-intimacy-if they do not have a firm sense of self (an identity) to
which they can return.
Check Your Progress 2
A. Mention the development of Sex interests during adolescence
B. Explain the psychological conflict of adolescence as stated by Erikson
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
20.5 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Fundamental Changes in Morality during Adolescence
ii) Changes in Moral Concepts
iii) Building a Moral Code
iv) Inner Control of Behavior
v) Sex Interests during Adolescence
vi) Development of Heterosexuality
vii) New Patterns of Heterosexuality
viii) Common Reasons for Dating During Adolescence
ix) New Attitudes toward Sexual Behavior
x) Erikson’s - Identity versus Identity Confusion
20.6 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. The effects of physical changes include:
i) Changes in Moral Concepts
ii) Building a Moral Code
iii) Inner Control of Behavior
2. A. The development of sex interests during adolescence are:
i) Development of Heterosexuality
ii) New Patterns of Heterosexuality
iii) Common Reasons for Dating During Adolescence
iv) New Attitudes toward Sexual Behavior
B. The psychological conflict of adolescence as stated by Erikson is:
i) autonomy or initiative
ii) identity crisis
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
240
iii) exploration
iv) identity confusion
v) stage-intimacy
20.7 Lesson – End Activities
1. Briefly describe the role of teacher in moral development.
2. Discuss the influence of peers on moral development of adolescences.
20.8 References
1. Hurlock, E.B., Child Psychology, Tokyo : McGraw – Hill, 1960.
2. Paplia, D.E. and Olds, S.N., Psychology, New York : McGraw – Hill, 1980.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
241
UNIT V
Lesson – 21
EARLY ADULTHOOD - CHARACTERISTICS - SEX - ROLE
ADJUSTMENT - ERIKSON'S THEORY: INTIMACY VERSUS
ISOLATION – FAMILY LIFE CYCLE
Contents
21.0 Aims and Objectives
21.1 Introduction
21.2 Characteristics of Early Adulthood
21.2.1 Early Adulthood is the "Settling-down Age"
21.2.2 Early Adulthood Is the "Reproductive Age"
21.2.3 Early Adulthood is a "Problem Age"
21.2.4 Early Adulthood is a Period of Emotional Tension
21.2.5 Early Adulthood is a Period of Social Isolation
21.2.6 Early Adulthood is a time of commitments
21.2.7 Early Adulthood is often a Period of Dependency
21.2.8 Early Adulthood is a Time of Value Change
21.2.9 Early Adulthood is the Time of Adjustment to New lifestyles
21.2.10 Early Adulthood is a Creative Age
21.3 Sex-Role Adjustments in Early Adulthood
21.3.1 Concepts of Adult Sex Roles
21.4 Erikson’s theory: Intimacy versus Isolation
21.5 Family life cycle
21.5.1 Leaving Home
21.5.2 Joining of Families in Marriage
21.5.3 Parenthood
21.6 Let Us Sum Up
21.7 Check your Progress
21.8 Lesson – End Activities
21.9 References
21.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help understand the characteristics of early adulthood, sex role
adjustments made during this period and the family life cycle of early adulthood.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
i) understand the characteristics of early adulthood
ii) mention the sex-role adjustment and concept of adult sex roles
iii) differentiate between the intimacy and isolation stated by Erikson
iv) state the family life cycle of the early adulthood
21.1 INTRODUCTION
The word adult comes from the same Latin verb as the term adolescence-adolescerewhich
means "to grow to maturity." However, the word adult is derived from the past
participle of that verb-adultus -which means "grown to full size and strength" or
"matured." Adults are, therefore, individuals who have completed their growth and are
ready to assume their status in society along with other adults.
Various cultures have different ages at which children reach the adult status or the age of
legal maturity, in most of the older cultures, they reached this status when their puberty
growth was complete or nearly complete and when their sex organs had developed to the
point where they were capable of procreation. Until recently, children were not considered
legally adults until they reached the age of twenty-one years. Today, adulthood is legally
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
242
reached at the age of eighteen. With a gradual increase in longevity, adulthood is now by far
the longest period in the total life span.
During the long period of adulthood, certain physical and psychological changes occur at
predictable times. Like childhood and adolescence-also long periods during which certain
physical and psychological changes occur at predictable times adulthood is customarily
subdivided on the basis of the times at which these changes take place together with the
adjustment problems and cultural pressures and expectancies stemming from them.
It is important to note that these subdivisions are not fixed and rigid. Instead, they
indicate only the ages at which the average man or woman can be expected to begin to show
some changes in appearance, bodily functions, interests, attitudes, or behavior and at which
certain environmental pressures in our culture give rise to adjustment problems which few
men or women escape. As Gould emphasized, "The precise ages at which changes occur are a
product of an individual's total personality, life style and subculture".
21.2 CHARACTERISTICS OF EARLY ADULTHOOD
Early adulthood is a period of adjustments to new patterns of life and new social
expectations. The young adult is expected to play new roles, such as that of spouse, parent,
and breadwinner, and to develop new attitudes, interests, and values in keeping with these
new roles. These adjustments make early adulthood a distinctive period in the life span and
also a difficult one. It is especially difficult because, until now, most boys and girls have had
someone parents, teachers, friends or others-to help them make the adjustments they are
faced with. Now, as adults, they are expected to make these adjustments for themselves. To
avoid being considered "immature," they hesitate to turn to others for advice and help when
they find the adjustments too difficult to cope with successfully alone.
21.2.1 Early Adulthood is the "Settling-down Age"
Childhood and adolescence are the periods of "growing up" and that adulthood is the
time for "settling down:" In past generations, it was assumed that when boys and girls
reached the age of legal maturity, their days of carefree freedom were over and the time had
come to settle down and assume the responsibilities of adult life. That meant settling into a
line of work that would be the man's career for the rest of his life, while the young Woman
was expected to assume the responsibilities of homemaker and mother-responsibilities that
would be hers for the remainder of her life.
Today, it is recognized that "settling down" too early is often laying the foundations for
discontent because of too early choices of careers or life-mates. Consequently, many young
men tryout different lines of work to see which meets their needs best and which will bring
them lifelong satisfaction. While trying out different lines of work, many young men also
tryout different women to find out if they have the qualities they want for a lifelong spouse.
This trying out of different life patterns and different individuals to share their life
patterns takes time. Consequently, young adults today usually start to settle down late than
their parents did and much later than their grandparents did. The average adult of today has
chosen a lifestyle and an individual to share that lifestyle by the early thirties, though many
do so before then.
When adults of today start to settle down depends upon two factors. First, how soon they
are able to find a lifestyle that meets their needs then and which they believe will meet their
needs throughout life. A woman, who, since the days she played with dolls always wanted to
be a wife and mother, will not need long after completing her education to choose these
occupations as her life roles. Similarly, a man who never wanted to be anything but a doctor
will not have to go through the trial-and-error process to find a career that meets his needs as
will his friends who frankly claimed, as boys, that they did not know what they wanted to do
when they reached the end of their schooling.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
243
The second condition that determines when young adults will settle down is the
responsibilities they must assume before doing so. The man who has selected medicine as a
life career certainly cannot start to settle down to the practice of medicine until the late
twenties at the earliest because he must complete college, medical school, internship, and
residency before he can hang out his shingle as a full-fledged doctor. By contrast, age-mates
who decide they want to go into overall or blue-collar jobs can start to settle down shortly
after they complete college because they will not need a period of formal training for these
jobs. The woman who wants to spend her life as wife and mother may have to delay one or
both of these roles should she decide to marry a man who must delay marriage because of
financial responsibilities related to his education or to the care of aging parents.
21.2.2 Early Adulthood Is the "Reproductive Age"
Parenthood is one of the most important roles in the lives of most young adults. Those
who were married during the latter years of adolescence concentrate on the role of
parenthood during their twenties and early thirties; some become grandparents before early
adulthood ends.
Those who do not marry until they have completed their education or have started their
life careers, do not become parents until they feel they can afford to have a family. This is
often not until the early thirties. Also, if women want to pursue careers after marriage, they
may put off having children until the thirties. For them, then, only the last decade of early
adulthood is the "reproductive age." For those who begin to have children early in adulthood
or even in the closing years of adolescence and have large families, all of early adulthood is
likely to be a reproductive age.
21.2.3 Early Adulthood is a "Problem Age"
The early adult years present many new problems, different in their major aspects, from
the problems experienced in the earlier years of life. With the lowering of the age of legal
maturity to eighteen years, young adults have been confronted with many problems they are
totally unprepared to cope with. While they are now able to vote, to own property, to marry
without parental consent, and to do many things young people could not do when the age of
legal maturity was twenty-one years, there is no question about the fact that "this new-found
freedom is creating unforeseen problems for the youthful adults, and often for their parents,
too".
There are many reasons why adjustment to the problems of adulthood is so difficult.
Three are especially common. First, very few young people have had any preparation for
meeting the types of problems they are expected to cope with as adults. Education in high
school and college provides only limited training for jobs, and few schools or colleges give
courses in the common problems of marriage and parenthood. Even those who have had
baby-sitting experience have limited preparation for parenthood because most baby-sitters are
hired only for short times when parents are out of the home and their major responsibility is
to keep the children safe and happy until the parents return.
Second, just as trying to learn two or more skills simultaneously usually results in not
learning anyone of them well, so trying to adjust to two or more new roles simultaneously
usually results in poor adjustment to all of them. It is difficult for a young adult to deal with
the choice of a career and the choice of a mate simultaneously. Similarly, adjustment to marriage
and parenthood makes it difficult for young adults to adjust to work if they marry while
they are still students.
Third, and perhaps most serious of all, young adults do not have the help in meeting and
solving their problems that they had when they were younger. This is partly their own fault
and partly that of their parents and teachers. Most young adults are too proud of their new
status to admit that they cannot cope with it. So, they do not seek the advice and help in
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
244
meeting the problems this new status gives rise to. Similarly, most parents and teachers,
having been rebuffed by adolescents who claimed they were capable of handling their own
affairs, hesitate to offer help unless they are specifically asked to do so. That is why, as was
stressed earlier, the shortening of adolescence has made the transition to adulthood especially
difficult.
21.2.4 Early Adulthood is a Period of Emotional Tension
When people are trying to get the lay of a new land in which they find themselves, they
are likely to be emotionally upset. By the early or mid-thirties, most young adults have solved
their problems well enough to become emotionally stable and calm. Should the heightened
emotionality characteristic of the early years of adulthood persist into the thirties, it suggests
that adjustments to adult life have not been satisfactorily made. When emotional tension
persists into the thirties, it is generally expressed in worries. What young adults worry about
will depend on what adjustment problems they are facing at the time and how much success
or failure they are experiencing in meeting these problems. Their worries may be mainly
concentrated on their work, because they feel they are not advancing as rapidly as they had
hoped to, or their worries may be concentrated on marital or parent hood problems. When
adults feel that they have not been able to cope with the problems in the major areas of their
lives, they are often so emotionally disturbed that they contemplate or attempt suicide.
21.2.5 Early Adulthood is a Period of Social Isolation
With the end of formal education and the entrance into the adult life pattern of work and
marriage, associations with the peer groups of adolescence wane and, with them,
opportunities for social contacts outside the home. As a result, for the first time since
babyhood even the most popular individual is likely to experience social isolation, or what
Erikson has referred to as an "isolation crisis”.
Many young adults, having become accustomed throughout childhood and adolescence to
depending on peers for companionship, experience loneliness when responsibilities at home
or at work isolate them from groups of their peers. Those who were most popular during their
school and college days, and who devoted much of their time to peer activities, find the
adjustment to social isolation in adulthood especially difficult. Whether the loneliness that
comes from this isolation will be temporary or persistent depends on how quickly and how
satisfactorily the young adult can establish new social contacts to replace those of school and
college days.
Isolation is intensified by a competitive spirit and a strong desire to rise on the vocational
ladder. To achieve success, they must compete with others thus replacing the friendliness of
adolescence with the competitiveness of the successful adult-and they must also devote most
of their energies to their work, which leaves them little time for the socialization that leads to
close relationships. As a result, they become self-centered, which contributes to loneliness.
21.2.6 Early Adulthood is a time of commitments
As young adults change their role from that of student and dependent, characteristic of
adolescence, to that of independent adult, they establish new patterns of living, assume new
responsibilities, and make new commitments. While these new patterns of living, new
responsibilities, and new commitments may change later, they form the foundations on which
later patterns of living, responsibilities, and commitments will be established.
21.2.7 Early Adulthood is often a Period of Dependency
In spite of achieving the status of legal adulthood at age eighteen, with the independence
this status carries, many young adults are partially or totally dependent on others for varying
lengths of time. This dependency may be on parents; on the educational institution they
attend on part or total scholarship, or on the government for loans to finance their education.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
245
Other young adults, while rebelling against the dependency a prolonged career training
necessitates, become so accustomed to depending on others for financial support that they
question their own ability to become economically independent. As a result, they often
become "perpetual students," going from one training center to another for more and more
training for their careers. To compensate for any feeling of guilt or shame they may have for
playing the perpetual student role, they try to convince themselves and others that the more
training they get, the greater their chances for reaching the top of the ladder in their chosen
careers. As a result, their state of dependency is often prolonged into the late twenties or early
thirties.
21.2.8 Early Adulthood is a Time of Value Change
Many of the values developed during childhood and adolescence change as experience
and social contact with people of different ages broaden and as values are considered from a
more mature standpoint. Adults who used to consider school a necessary evil may now
recognize the value of education as a stepping-stone to social and vocational success and to
personal fulfillment. As a result of such changed values, many adults who dropped out of
school or college decide to finish their education. Some find studying so stimulating that they
continue to take courses even after receiving high school or college degrees.
There are many reasons for value changes in early adulthood, three of which are very
common. First, if young adults want to be accepted by members of the adult group, they must
accept the values of the adult group just as, during childhood and adolescence, they had to
accept the values of their peer group to win acceptance. Many young adults discover that the
"sloppy Joe" appearance of their school and college days and rebellious attacks on the
establishment must give way to adult-approved appearance and behavior if they want to be
accepted in adult economic and social groups.
Second, young adults soon discover that most social groups hold conventional values
about beliefs and behavior, just as they do about appearance. While the adolescent peer
culture may have regarded premarital sex as acceptable behavior, most adults do not and
demand more conventional courtship and marriage as the price of acceptance into the social
group.
Third, young adults who become parents not only tend to change their values earlier and
more radically than those who are unmarried or childless, but they also shift to more
conservative and traditional values. In general, the values of most young adults change from
egocentric to social. Members of the "me" generation - those who think mainly of their own
happiness and desire for self-indulgence gradually become more socially conscious and
concerned as they assume the roles of spouse and parent.
21.2.9 Early Adulthood is the Time of Adjustment to New lifestyles
While lifestyles have been in a state of flux since the turn of the present century, at no
period in the life span is this truer than in early adulthood. And in no area of early adult life
are new lifestyles more prevalent than in the areas of marriage and parenthood. Instead of the
traditional courtship of the past, many young adults regard premarital sex as an accepted part
of the courtship pattern. Similarly, use of contraceptives and resort to abortion when
contraception fails are so widespread among young adults, especially those who are in
colleges, that they also are regarded as part of the courtship pattern. Nor is marriage after
pregnancy regarded as the "hush hush" matter it formerly was. Instead, many young couples
want as large and as elaborate weddings as they had hoped for before the bride became
pregnant.
Of the many adjustments young adults must make to new lifestyles the most common are
adjustments to egalitarian rather than traditional sex roles, new family-life patterns, including
divorce and one parent families, and new vocational patterns, especially large and impersonal
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
246
work units in business and industry.
21.2.10 Early Adulthood is a Creative Age
What form creativity will take in adulthood will depend upon individual interests and
abilities, opportunities to do what they want to do, and activities that give the greatest
satisfaction. Some young adults find a creative outlet in hobbies while others choose vocations
in which they can express their creativity.
Even though interest in creative activities starts in the twenties creative achievements
often do not reach their peak until middle age. This is due to the fact that creativity is more
often discouraged than encouraged in the early years of life. Thus it is that during early
adulthood men and women must not only discover where their creative interests and talents
lie but they must also develop their capacities which, in many cases, remained dormant while
the patterns of their lives were prescribed by parents and teachers. As they approach middle
age, however, men and women should have overcome these obstacles sufficiently to achieve
the maximum of which they are capable.
As adolescents, girls are given more opportunity to be creative than boys-since creativity
is regarded as more sex-appropriate for females than for males. Therefore, as young women,
they tend to be more creative in whatever they do-in their dress, their home decorations, or
their hobbies-than men. However, home duties and child-care responsibilities often hamper
expressions of creativity. As a result, when they reach middle age, women's creative
achievements often lag behind those of men who, earlier in adulthood, were less creative than
they.
Check Your Progress 1
Discuss the characteristics of early adulthood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
21.3 SEX-ROLE ADJUSTMENTS IN EARLY ADULTHOOD
Sex-role adjustments during early adulthood are extremely difficult. Long before
adolescence is over, boys and girls are well aware of the approved adult sex roles but this
does not necessarily lead to acceptance. Many adolescent girls want to play the role of wife
and mother when they reach adulthood, but they do not want to be wives and mothers in the
traditional sense-being subordinate to their husbands, devoting most of their time to their
homes and children, and having few or no outside interests.
The hope of many of today's young women for an egalitarian marriage is based not on
wishful thinking but on the realization that there have been marked changes in the adult
pattern of living. For example, wives often work until their husbands finish their education or
become established in business or they take jobs in order to acquire various status symbols
that the family would otherwise be unable to afford. Most important of all, young women are
aware of the breakdown of the "double standard," not only in sexual and moral behavior, but
also in social, business, and professional life.
In fact, the traditional concepts are gradually being modified or even replaced by new,
more egalitarian ones-concepts that stress similar behavior patterns for members of the two
sexes. These egalitarian concepts have found acceptance among all social groups, even those
which formerly held firmly to traditional concepts of the male and female roles. The
traditional and egalitarian concepts of adult sex roles are given below.
Many young women recognize the low prestige associated with the traditional role of
wife and mother, and consequently they have little motivation to learn this role. When they
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
247
become wives and mothers, they see little opportunity for escape from this role into one they
previously found more satisfying and personally rewarding. Conflict between what they
would like to do and what they know they must do further weakens their motivation to play
the traditionally prescribed sex role. The conflicts and frustrations many young women in the
American culture experience are heightened by the constant bombardment of advice from the
mass media to play a role other than that of the traditional wife and mother.
21.3.1 Concepts of Adult Sex Roles
Traditional Concepts
Traditional concepts of sex roles emphasize a prescribed pattern of behavior, regardless of
individual interests or abilities. They emphasize masculine supremacy and intolerance toward
any trait that hints of femininity or any work that is considered "woman's work."
Men: Outside the home the man holds positions of authority and prestige in the social and
business worlds; in the home he is the wage earner, decision maker, adviser and
disciplinarian of the children, and model of masculinity for his sons.
Women: Both in the home and outside, the role of the woman is other-oriented in that she
gains fulfillment by serving others. She is not expected to work outside the home except in
cases of financial necessity, and then she does only work that serves others, such as nursing,
teaching, or secretarial work.
Egalitarian Concepts
Egalitarian concepts of sex roles emphasize individuality and the egalitarian status of men
and women. Roles should lead to personal fulfillment and not be considered appropriate for
only one sex.
Men: In the home and outside, the man works with the woman in a companionship
relationship. He does not feel "henpecked" if he treats his wife as an equal, nor does he feel
ashamed if she has a more prestigious or remunerative job than he does.
Women: Both in the home and outside, the woman is able to actualize her own potentials.
She does not feel guilty about using her abilities and training to give her satisfaction, even if
this requires employing someone else to take care of the home and children.
21.4 ERIKSON’S THEORY: INTIMACY VERSUS ISOLATION
Erikson's contributions have energized the study of adult personality development. His
vision has influenced all contemporary theories. According to Erikson, adults move through
three stages, each bringing both opportunity and risk-"a turning point for better or worse".
The psychological conflict of early adulthood is intimacy versus isolation, reflected in the
young person's thoughts and feelings about making a permanent commitment to an intimate
partner.
In his definition of intimacy, Erikson stated that it should include
1. Mutuality of orgasm
2. with a loved partner
3. of the other sex
4. With whom one is able and willing to share a mutual trust
5. and with whom one is able and willing to regulate the cycles of
a. work
b. procreation
c. recreation
6. so as to secure to the offspring, too, all the stages of a satisfactory development
Erikson pointed out, that sexual intercourse should not be assumed to be the most
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
248
important aspect of intimacy between individuals. He was speaking here of far more than
sexual intimacy. He was talking about the ability to relate one's deepest hopes and fears to
another person and to accept another's need for intimacy in turn.
Those who have achieved the stage of intimacy are able to commit themselves to
concrete affiliations and partnerships with others and have developed the "ethical strength to
abide by such commitments, even though they may call for significant sacrifices and
compromises". This leads to solidarity between partners.
Erikson was found quoting Freud's response when asked what he thought a normal person
should be able to do well: "Lieben und arbeiten’ - "to love and to work." To Freud, then
sharing responsibility for mutual achievements and the loving feelings that result from them
are the essence of adulthood. Erikson fully agreed with this. Thus when Freud uses the term
genitality to describe this same period he does not merely mean sexual intercourse; he is
referring rather to the ability to share one's deeply held values, needs, and secrets with
another through the generosity that is so important in intimacy.
The counterpart of intimacy is distantiation. This is the readiness all of us have to
distance ourselves from others when we feel threatened by their behavior. Distantiation is the
cause of most prejudices and discrimination. Propaganda efforts mounted by countries at war
are examples of attempts to increase distantiation. It is what leads to isolation.
Most young adults vacillate between their desires for intimacy and their need for
distantiation. They need social distance because they are not sure of their identities. They are
always vulnerable to criticism, and since they can't be sure whether the criticisms are true or
not, they protect themselves by a "lone wolf" stance.
Although intimacy may be difficult for some males today, Erikson believed that it used to
be even more difficult for females. "All this is a little more complicated with women, because
women, at least in yesterday's cultures, had to keep their identities incomplete until they knew
their man". Now that less emphasis occurs in the female gender role on getting married and
pleasing one's husband, and more emphasis is on being true to one's own identity, Erikson
believed that both sexes have a better chance of achieving real intimacy.
Erikson believed that successful resolution of intimacy versus isolation prepares the
individual for the middle adulthood stage, which focuses on Generativity-caring for the next
generation and helping to improve society. In sum, both intimacy and Generativity emerge in
early adulthood, with shifts in emphasis that differ among young people.
Check Your Progress 2
A. Mention the concepts of Adult Sex Roles
B. State the Erikson’s intimacy vs. isolation theory
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
21.5 THE FAMILY LIFE CYCLE
For the majority of young people, the quest for intimacy leads to marriage. Their life
course takes shape within the family life cycle-a sequence of phases that characterizes the
development of most families around the world. In early adulthood, people typically live on
their own, marry and bear and rear children. As they become middle-aged and their children
leave home, their parenting responsibilities diminish. Late adulthood brings retirement,
growing old, and (mostly for women) death of one's spouse.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
249
21.5.1 Leaving Home
Departure from the parental home is a major step toward assuming adult responsibilities.
The average age of leaving has decreased in recent years as more young people live
independently before marriage.
Timing of departure varies with the reason for leaving. Departures for education tend to
occur at younger ages, those for full-time work and marriage later. Some young people who
want to escape family friction also depart early. And those from divorced, single-parent
homes tend to be early leavers, perhaps because of family stress. Compared with the previous
generation, fewer young people leave home to marry, and more do so just to be
"independent" -to express their adult status. But a difficult job market and high housing costs
mean that many must take undesirable jobs or remain financially dependent on their parents.
21.5.2 Joining of Families in Marriage
Young adults delay marriage more today than a half-century ago. The number of first and
second marriages has declined over the last few decades as more people remain single,
cohabit, or do not remarry after divorce. Marriage is often regarded as the joining of two
individuals. In actuality, it requires that two entire systems--the husband's and wife's familiesadapt
and overlap to create a new subsystem. Consequently, marriage presents couples with
complex challenges. This is especially so today because husband-wife roles have only begun
to move in the direction of a true partnership educationally, occupationally, and in emotional
connectedness.
21.5.2a Marital Roles: After wedding and honeymoon are over, Couples turn to a multitude
of issues that they had previously decided individually or that their families of origin had
prescribed. They considered everyday matters when and how to eat, sleep, talk, work, relax,
have sex, and spend money. They also discussed which family traditions and rituals to retain
and which to work out for themselves. And as they related to their social world as a couple,
they modified relationships with parents, siblings, extended family, friends, and co-workers.
Despite progress in the area of women's rights, traditional marriages, involving a clear
division of husband's and wife's roles, still exist. The man is the head of household; his
primary responsibility is the economic wellbeing of his family. The woman devotes herself to
caring for her husband and children and to creating a nurturant, comfortable home. However,
traditional marriages have changed in recent decades. Although women make motherhood the
top priority while their children are young, many return to the work force at a later date.
In egalitarian marriages, husband and wife relate as equals, and power and authority are
shared. Both partners try to balance the time and energy they devote to their occupations,
their children, and their relationship. The majority of well-educated, career-oriented women
expect this form of marriage. But women's employment has not had much impact on
household division of labor. Men in dual-earner families participate more than those in
single-earner families.
21.5.2b Marital Satisfaction: Despite its rocky beginnings, most marriages grew to be
happy. Many postponed having children until careers were under way and build a sense of
togetherness that allowed each to thrive as an individual. Patience, caring, shared values,
enjoyment of each other's company, and good conflict-resolution skills contributed to couples
compatibility.
There are factors differentiate troubled from that of gratifying marital relationships. More
men than women report being happily married. Simply being married is associated with gains
in men's mental and physical health-an outcome that stems from enhanced feelings of
attachment, belonging, and social support.
21.5.2c Marital Expectations and Myths: When fifty happily married couples were
interviewed about their marriages, each participant reported good times and bad; none was
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
250
happy all the time. Many admitted that there were moments when they wanted out, when they
felt they had made a mistake. Clearly, marital happiness did not signify a "rose garden."
Instead, it was grounded in mutual respect, pleasure and comfort in each other's company,
and joint problem solving. All couples emphasized the need to reshape their relationship in
response to new circumstances and to each partner's changing needs and desires.
21.5.3 Parenthood
In the past, the issue of whether to have children was, for many adults, "a biological given
or an unavoidable cultural demand". Now it is a matter of true individual choice. Effective
birth control techniques enable adults to avoid having children in most instances. And
changing cultural values allow people to remain childless with less fear of social criticism
and rejection than was the case a generation or two ago.
21.5.3a The Decision to Have Children: The choice of parenthood is affected by a complex
array of factors, including financial circumstances, personal and religious values, and health
conditions. Women with traditional gender identities usually decide to have children.
Whether a woman is employed has less impact on childbearing than her occupation. Women
with high-status, demanding careers less often choose parenthood and, when they do, more
often delay it than women with less time-consuming jobs. When asked about their desire to
have children, couples mention a variety of disadvantages, like loss of freedom, financial
strain, role over load, worries and risk of children, loss of privacy etc.
21.5.3b Transition to Parenthood: The early weeks after a baby enters the family are full
of profound changes: disrupted sleep schedules, new caregiving and household tasks, less
time for couples to devote to each other, and added financial responsibilities. For most new
parents, however, the arrival of a baby does not cause significant marital strain. Marriages
that are gratifying and supportive tend to remain so and resemble childless marriages in
overall happiness. In contrast, troubled marriages usually become more distressed after a
baby is born.
21.5.3c Additional Births: There are factors that influence the family size. Besides more
effective birth control, a major reason that couples in industrialized nations have fewer
children today than in the past is women's increased career orientation. Overall, a smaller
family size enhances parent-child interaction. Parents of fewer children are more patient and
less punitive. They also have more time to devote to each child's activities, schoolwork, and
other special needs. Furthermore, in smaller families, siblings are more likely to be widely
spaced (born more than 2 years apart), which adds to the attention and resources parents can
invest in each other and in each child.
21.5.3d Parent Education: In the past, family life changed little from one generation to the
next, and adults learned what they needed to know about parenting through modeling and
direct experience. Contemporary parents eagerly seek information on child rearing through
popular books. New mothers regard these sources as particularly valuable, second in
importance only to their doctors. Special parent education courses have also emerged,
designed to help parents clarify child-rearing values, improve family communication, understand
how children develop, and apply more effective parenting strategies. Yet another
benefit is social support-opportunities to discuss concerns with experts and other dedicated
parents, who share the view that no job is more important to the future of society than child
rearing.
Check Your Progress 3
Write about the sequence of phases that characterizes the development of family cycle
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
251
.....................................................................................................................................................
21.5 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
vi) Characteristics of Early Adulthood
vii) Sex-Role Adjustments in Early Adulthood
viii) The Erikson’s intimacy vs. isolation theory
ix) The sequence of phases that characterizes the development of family cycle
21.6 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
11. The characteristics of early adulthood include:
i) Early Adulthood is the "Settling-down Age"
ii) It is the "Reproductive Age"
iii) It is a "Problem Age"
iv) It is a Period of Emotional Tension
v) It is a Period of Social Isolation
vi) It is a time of commitments
vii) It is often a Period of Dependency
viii) It is a Time of Value Change
ix) It is the Time of Adjustment to New lifestyles
x) It is a Creative Age
12. A. The concept of adult sex role are:
i) Traditional Concepts
ii) Egalitarian Concepts
C. The Erikson’s intimacy vs. isolation theory suggests:
i) intimacy
ii) solidarity
iii) distantiation
iv) isolation
13. The sequence of phases that characterizes the development of family cycle are:
i) Leaving Home
ii) Joining of Families in Marriage
iii) Marital Roles
iv) Marital Satisfaction
v) Marital Expectations and Myths
vi) Parenthood
vii) The Decision to Have Children
viii) Transition to Parenthood
ix) Additional Births
x) Parent Education
21.7 Lesson – End Activities
1. Briefly describe the concept social isolation and its impact on development.
2. Describe the concept creativity in relation to emotional unbalance.
21.8 References
1. Gates, A.I. Elementary Psychology, New York : McMillan, 1960.
2. Gilmen, B. Vonhaller, Psychology (International ed.) Harper, 1970.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
252
LESSON – 22
DIVERSITY OF ADULT LIFESTYLES - CAREER DEVELOPMENT -MIDDLE
ADULTHOOD - ERIKSON’S THEORY: GENERATIVITY VERSUS STAGNATION
Contents
22.0 Aims and Objectives
22.1 Introduction
22.2 Diversity of Adult Lifestyles
22.2.1 Singlehood
22.2.2 Cohabitation
22.2.3 Childlessness
22.2.4 Divorce and Remarriage
22.2.5 Variant Styles of Parenthood
22.3 Career Development
22.3.1 Establishing a Career
22.3.2 Combining Work and Family
22.4 Characteristics of Middle Adulthood
22.4.1 Middle Adulthood Is a Dreaded Period
22.4.2 Middle Adulthood Is a Time of Transition
22.4.3 Middle Adulthood is a Time of Stress
22.4.4 Middle Adulthood Is a "Dangerous Age"
22.4.5 Middle Adulthood is an "Awkward Age"
22.4.6 Middle Adulthood is a Time of Achievement
22.4.7 Middle Adulthood is a Time of Evaluation
22.4.8 Middle Adulthood is evaluated by a Double Standard
22.4.9 Middle Adulthood is the Time of the Empty Nest
22.4.10 Middle Adulthood is a Time of Boredom
22.5 Erikson's Theory: Generativity versus Stagnation
22.6 Let Us Sum Up
22.7 Check your Progress
22.8 Lesson-End Activities
22.9 References
22.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help understand the life styles and career success of early adulthood
and the Characteristics of middle adulthood
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
xxiv) understand the different common life styles found among early adulthood
xxv) state as to how career development occurs during early adulthood
xxvi) list the characteristics of middle adulthood
xxvii) discuss the Erikson's theory on Generativity and Stagnation
22.1 INTRODUCTION
The present adult lifestyles are different from that of the past, young people question the
conventional wisdom of previous generations. Many want to find happiness. The kind of
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
253
commitments one should make to live a full and rewarding life. As the public became more
accepting of diverse lifestyles, choices seemed more available-among them, staying single,
cohabiting, remaining childless, and divorcing.
22.2 DIVERSITY OF ADULT LIFESTYLES
Today, nontraditional family options have penetrated. Many adults experience not just
one, but several options. Some adults make a deliberate decision to adopt a lifestyle, whereas
others drift into it. The lifestyle may be imposed by society, as is the case for cohabiting
homosexual couples, who cannot marry legally. Or people may decide on a certain lifestyle
because they feel pushed away from another, such as a marriage gone sour. In sum, the adoption
of a lifestyle can be within or beyond the person's control.
22.2.1 Singlehood
Singlehood-not living with an intimate partner-has increased in recent years, especially
among young adults. Besides more people marrying later or not at all, divorce has added to
the numbers of single adults. In view of these trends, it is likely that most will spend a
substantial part of their adult lives single.
Because they marry later, more young adult men than women are single. But women are
far more likely than men to remain single for many years or their entire life. With age, fewer
men are available with characteristics that most women seek in a mate-the same age or older,
equally or better educated, and professionally successful. Men find partners more easily,
since they can select from a large pool of younger unmarried women. Because of the
tendency for women to "marry up" and men to "marry down," men in blue-collar occupations
and women in prestigious careers are over represented among singles after age 30.i
Of the various advantages of singlehood, those mentioned most are freedom and
mobility. But singles also recognize drawbacks-loneliness, the dating grind, limited sexual
and social life, reduced sense of security, and feelings of exclusion from the world of married
couples. Single men have more physical and mental health problems than single women, who
usually come to terms with their lifestyle. The greater social support available to women
through intimate same-sex friendships is partly responsible. In addition, never-married men
are more likely to have conflict-ridden family backgrounds and personal characteristics that
contribute to both their singlehood and their adjustment difficulties.
22.2.2 Cohabitation
Cohabitation refers to the lifestyle of unmarried couples who have an intimate, sexual
relationship and share a residence. Until the 1960s, cohabitation in Western nations was
largely limited to low-SES adults. Since then, it has increased in all groups, with an
especially dramatic rise among well-educated, economically advantaged young people.
Young adults are much more likely than those of a generation ago to choose cohabitation as a
way of forming their first conjugal union. Among people in their twenties, cohabitation is
now the preferred mode of entry into a committed intimate partnership, with more than 50
percent of couples choosing it. Rates of cohabitation are even higher among adults with failed
marriages. Half of cohabiting relationships involve at least one partner who is separated or
divorced; one-third of these households include children.
Like singlehood, cohabitation has different meanings. For some, it serves as preparation
for marriage-a time to test the relationship and get used to living together. For others, it is an
alternative to marriage--an arrangement that offers the rewards of sexual intimacy and
companionship along with the possibility of easy departure if satisfaction declines. In view of
this variation, it is not surprising that cohabiters differ greatly in the extent to which they
share money and possessions and take responsibility for each other's children.
Certain couples who cohabit after separation or divorce often test a new relationship
carefully to prevent another failure, especially when children are involved. As a result, they
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
254
cohabit longer and are less likely to move toward marriage. Many regard their earning power
as too uncertain for marriage and continue living together, sometimes giving birth to children
and marrying when their financial status improves. Finally, homosexual couples report strong
commitment-as intense as that of married people. When their relationships become difficult,
they end more often than marriages only because there are fewer barriers to separating,
including children in common, financial dependence on a partner, and concerns about the
costs of divorce.
22.2.3 Childlessness
Some people are involuntarily childless because they did not find a partner with whom to
share parenthood or their efforts at fertility treatments did not succeed.
There are couples who choose not to have children as a matter of fact that voluntary
childlessness is not always a permanent condition. A few people decide early that they do not
want to be parents and stick to these plans. But most make their decision after they are
married and have developed a lifestyle they do not want to give up. Later, some change their
minds.
Besides marital satisfaction and freedom from child-care responsibilities, common
reasons for not having children include the woman's career and economic security. Consistent
with these motives, the voluntarily childless are usually college educated, have prestigious
occupations, and are highly committed to their work. Many were only or first-born children
whose parents encouraged achievement and independence. In cultures that negatively
stereotype childlessness, it is not surprising that voluntarily childless women are more selfreliant
and assertive.
22.2.4 Divorce and Remarriage
Divorce rates have increased as financial stability increased and marital satisfaction has
decreased. Divorces are also common during the transition to midlife, when people have
teenage children-a period of low marital satisfaction. About three-fourths of divorced people
remarry. But marital failure is even greater during the first few years of second marriages.
22.2.4a Factors Related to Divorce: The most obvious reason for failure of marriage is a
disrupted husband-wife relationship. The problem-solving style could be ineffective. Another
typical style involves little conflict, but partners increasingly lead separate lives because they
have different expectations of family life and few shared interests, activities, or friends.
Wives reported more problems than husbands, with the gender difference largely
involving the wife's emotions, such as anger, hurt feelings, and moodiness. Husbands seemed
to have difficulty sensing their wife's distress, which contributed to her view of the marriage
as unhappy. Regardless of which spouse reported the problem or was judged responsible for
it, the strongest predictors of divorce were infidelity, spending money foolishly, drinking or
using drugs, expressing jealousy, engaging in irritating habits, and moodiness.
Younger age at marriage, being previously divorced, and having parents who had
divorced increased the chances of divorce, in part because these background factors were
linked to marital difficulties. Economically disadvantaged couples who suffer multiple life
stresses are especially likely to split up. Women are twice as likely as men to initiate divorce.
22.2.4b Consequences of Divorce: Immediately after separation, both men and women are
depressed and anxious and display impulsive behavior. For most, these reactions subside
within 2 years. Women who were in traditional marriages and who organized their identities
around their husbands have an especially hard time. No custodial fathers often feel
disoriented and rootless as a result of decreased contact with their children. Others distract
themselves through a frenzy of social activity.
Finding a new partner contributes most to the life satisfaction of divorced adults. But it is
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
255
more crucial for men, who are better adjusted in the context of marriage than on their own.
Despite loneliness and reduced income, most divorced women prefer their new life to an
unhappy marriage. However, a few women--especially those who are anxious and fearful or
who remain strongly attached to their ex-spouses-show a drop in self-esteem, become
depressed, and tend to form repeated unsuccessful relationships. Job training continued education,
career advancement, and social support from family and friends play vital roles in the
economic and psychological wellbeing of divorced women.
22.2.4c Remarriage: On average, people remarry within 4 years of divorce, men somewhat
faster than women. For several reasons, remarriages are especially vulnerable to breakup.
First, although people often remarry for love, practical matters-financial security, help in
rearing children, relief from loneliness, and social acceptance-figure more heavily into a
second marriage than a first. These concerns do not provide a sound footing for a lasting
partnership. Second, some people transfer the negative patterns of interaction and problem
solving learned in their first marriage to the second. Third, people who have already had a
failed marriage are more likely to view divorce as an acceptable solution when marital
difficulties resurface. And finally, remarried couples experience more stress from step family
situations.
Divorce and remarriage, like other adult lifestyles, lead to diverse outcomes. It generally
takes 3 to 5 years for blended families to develop the connectedness and comfort of intact
biological families. Family life education, couples counseling, and group therapy can help
divorced and remarried adults adapt to the complexities of their new circumstances.
22.2.5 Variant Styles of Parenthood
Diverse family forms result in varied styles of parenthood. Among these are a growing
number of cohabiting and remarried parents, never-married parents, and gay and lesbian
parents. Each type of family presents unique challenges to parenting competence and adult
psychological well-being.
22.2.5a Stepparents: Whether stepchildren live in the household or visit only occasionally,
stepparents are in a difficult position. Since the parent-child tie predates the blended family,
the stepparent enters as an outsider. Too often, stepparents move into their new role too
quickly. Because they do not have a warm attachment bond to build on, their discipline is
usually ineffective. Stepparents frequently criticize the biological parent for being too lenient.
The parent, in turn, tends to view the stepparent as too harsh. These differences can divide the
couple. Remarried parents typically report higher levels of tension and disagreement than
first marriage parents, most centering on child-rearing issues. Because of more opportunities
for conflict, relationship quality is poorer when both adults have children from prior
marriages than when only one does.
Stepmothers, especially, are likely to experience conflict. Expected to be in charge of
family relationships, they quickly find that stepparent-stepchild ties do not develop instantly.
Often mothers are jealous, uncooperative, and possessive of their children following divorce.
Even when their husbands do not have custody, stepmothers feel stressed. As stepchildren go
in and out of the home, stepmothers compare life with and without resistant children, and
many prefer life without them. No matter how hard a stepmother tries to build a close parentchild
bond, her efforts are probably doomed to failure in the short run.
Stepfathers with children of their own have an easier time. They establish positive ties
with stepchildren relatively quickly, perhaps because they are experienced in building warm
parent-child ties and feel less pressure than stepmothers to plunge into parenting.
22.2.5b Never-Married Single Parents: Single adults occasionally decide to become parents
on their own. Births to women in high-status occupations who have not married by their
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
256
thirties have increased. However, they are still few in number, and little is known about how
these mothers and their children fare.
In the United States, the largest group of never-married parents is African-American
young women. African-American women postpone marriage more and childbirth less than
women in other American ethnic groups. Job loss, persisting unemployment, and consequent
inability of many black men to support a family have contributed to the number of African-
American never-married, single-mother families.
Never-married mothers tap the extended family, especially their own mothers, for help in
caring for children. For just over one-third, marriage occurs within 9 years after birth of the
first child, not necessarily to the child's biological father. These couples function much like
other first-marriage parents. Their children are often unaware that the father is a stepfather,
and parents do not report the child-rearing difficulties typically associated with blended
families.
Children of never-married mothers who lack the involvement of a father are poor in
school and display more antisocial behavior than children in low-SES, first-marriage
families. These adjustment problems make life more difficult for mothers. Strengthening
social support, education, and employment opportunities for low-SES parents would
encourage marriage as well as help unmarried-mother families.
22.2.5c Gay and lesbian Parents: Several million American gay men and lesbians are
parents, most through previous heterosexual marriages, a few through adoption or
reproductive technologies. In the past, laws assuming that homosexuals could not be adequate
parents led those who divorced a heterosexual partner to lose custody of their children.
Today, sexual orientation is irrelevant to custody. In others, fierce prejudice against
homosexual parents still prevails.
Gay and lesbian parents are as committed to and effective at child rearing as heterosexual
parents. Some evidence suggests that gay fathers are more consistent in setting limits and
more responsive to their children's needs than heterosexual fathers, perhaps because gay
men's less traditional gender identity fosters involvement with children. In lesbian families,
quality of mother-child interaction is as positive as in heterosexual families. And children of
lesbian mothers regard their mother's partner as very much a parent. Whether born to or
adopted by their parents or conceived through donor insemination, children in homosexual
families are as well adjusted as other children. Also, the large majority are heterosexual.
When extended-family members have difficulty accepting them, homosexual mothers and
fathers often build "families of choice" through friends, who assume the roles of relatives.
But most of the time, parents of gays and lesbians cannot endure a permanent rift. With time,
interactions between homosexual parents and their families of origin become more positive
and supportive.
Check Your Progress 1
Write about the diverse life styles found during the early adulthood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
22.3 CAREER DEVELOPMENT
Besides family life, vocational life is a vital domain of social development in early
adulthood. After choosing an occupation, young people must learn how to perform its tasks
well, get along with co-workers, respond to authority, and protect their own interests. When
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
257
work experiences go well, adults develop new competencies, feel a sense of personal
accomplishment, make new friends, and become financially independent and secure.
22.3.1 Establishing a Career
Not all people embark on the vocation of their dreams. For example, although half of
young people aspire to professional occupations, only 20 percent attain them. Even for those
who enter their chosen field, initial experiences can be discouraging. Adjusting to
unanticipated disappointments in salary, supervisors, and co-workers is difficult. As new
workers become aware of the gap between their expectations and reality, resignations are
common. On average, people in their twenties move to a new job every 2 years; five or six
changes are not unusual.
After a period of evaluation and adjustment, young adults generally settle into their work.
In careers with opportunities for promotion, high aspirations must often be revised
downward, since the structure of most work settings resembles a pyramid, with few highlevel
executive and supervisory jobs. For men who advanced very little, "work
disengagement" occurred early; family, recreation, and community service assumed greater
importance by the early thirties. Men with average levels of career success emphasized non
work roles at a later age. In contrast, men who were highly successful became more involved
in their jobs over time. Although the desire for advancement tends to decline with age, most
people still seek challenges and find satisfaction in their work roles.
Besides opportunity, personal characteristics affect career progress. A sense of self -
efficacy-belief in one's own ability to be successful-affects career choice and development.
Young people who are very anxious about the possibility of making mistakes or failing tend
to set career aspirations that are either too high or too low. When they encounter obstacles,
they quickly conclude that career tasks are too hard and give up. As a result, they achieve far
less than their abilities would permit.
Access to an effective mentor-a person with advanced experience and knowledge who is
emotionally invested in the junior person's development and who fosters a bond of trust-is
jointly affected by the availability of willing people and the individual's capacity to select an
appropriate individual. Interestingly, the best mentors usually are not top executives, who
tend to be preoccupied and therefore less helpful and sympathetic. Most of the time, young
adults fare better with a mentor lower on the corporate ladder.
22.3.2 Combining Work and Family
Women work because they want to or have to (or both). The dominant family form today
is the dual-earner marriage, in which both husband and wife are employed. Most dualearner
couples are also parents, since the majority of women with children are in the work
force. In about one-third of these families, moderate to severe conflict occurs over trying to
meet both work and family responsibilities.
The main sources of strain in dual-earner marriages are, when women returns to job after
her children were born, she could feel a sense of role overload, or conflict between work and
family responsibilities. Not only did they have a demanding career, but (like most employed
women) women shoulder most of the household and child-care tasks. Role overload is linked
to a rise in psychological stress, poorer marital relations, less e ffective parenting, and child
behavior problems.
Having two careers in one family usually means that certain career decisions become
more complex. A move to a new job can mean vocational sacrifices for one partner. Usually
this is the wife, since a decision in favor of the husband's career (typically further along and
better paid) is more likely to maximize family income. One solution to the geographical
limitations of the dual-earner marriage is to live apart. Although more couples are doing this,
the strain of separation and risk of divorce are high.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
258
When couples cooperate to surmount these, they profit greatly from involvement in both
work and family roles. Besides higher earnings and a better standard of living, a major
advantage is women's self-fulfillment and improved well-being. Multiple roles also granted
both young people expanded contexts for experiencing success and greater similarity in
everyday experiences, which fostered gratifying communication.
Check Your Progress 2
Mention the career development during early adulthood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
22.4 CHARACTERISTICS OF MIDDLE ADULTHOOD
As Middle Adulthood is a long period in the life span, it is customarily subdivided into
early Middle Adulthood, which extends from age forty to age fifty, and advanced Middle
Adulthood, which extends from age fifty to age sixty. During advanced Middle Adulthood,
physical and psychological changes that first began during the early forties become far more
apparent.
Like every period in the life span, Middle Adulthood is associated with certain
characteristics that make it distinctive. Ten of the most important of these characteristics are
listed below.
22.4.1 Middle Adulthood is a Dreaded Period
It is a dreaded period in the life span. It is recognized that, next to old age, it is the most
dreaded period in the total life span and the one adult will not admit that they have reached
until the calendar and the mirror force them to do so. As Desmond has pointed out, "Middle
Adulthood people slump into Middle Adulthood grudgingly, sadly and with a tinge of fear".
There are many unfavorable stereotypes about middle-aged people, the traditional beliefs
concerning the mental and physical deterioration that are believed to accompany the cessation
of the reproductive life, and the emphasis on the importance of youth in the as compared with
the reverence for age found in many other cultures. These influence adult attitudes
unfavorably as they approach this period in their lives. While dreading Middle Adulthood,
most adults become nostalgic about their younger years and wish that they could turn back
the hands of the clock.
22.4.2 Middle Adulthood is a Time of Transition
Just as puberty is a time of transition from childhood to adolescence and then to
adulthood, so Middle Adulthood is the time when men and women leave behind the physical
and behavioral characteristics of adulthood and enter a period of life when new physical and
behavioral characteristics will prevail. It is the time when men undergo a change in virility
and women a change in fertility.
The pattern of role changes for both men and women there must be a change to a paircentered
relationship as compared with the family-centered relationship during the early
years of adulthood when the main roles of men and women in the home are those of parents.
In addition to role changes in the home, men must adjust to the changes that impending
retirement and physical conditions necessitate in their work. For women, the adjustments
must be either to changing the role of housewife and mother for that of a worker in business,
industry, or one of the professions or of an "isolate" in a formerly busy home as the "empty
nest" period in home life sets in.
There are three common developmental crises which are as follows. First, the parenthood
crisis characterized by the "Where did we go wrong?" syndrome. This crisis occurs when
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
259
children fail to come up to parental expectations and the parents then question whether they
have used the right methods of child training, blaming themselves for their children's failures
to come up to their expectations. Second, the crisis arising from dealing with aging parents,
and "I hate to put Mother there" reaction. Many middle-aged parents, in trying to cope with
the problems of aging parents, feel guilty when they either cannot or do not want to have their
aging parents live in their homes.
Third, the crisis that comes from trying to deal with death, especially that of a spouse.
This is characterized by a "How can I go on?" attitude which colors the individual's personal
and social adjustments unfavorably until the crisis can be solved to the individual's
satisfaction.
22.4.3 Middle Adulthood is a Time of Stress
Radical adjustments to changed roles and patterns of life, especially when accompanied
by physical changes, always tend to disrupt the individual's physical and psychological
homeostasis and lead to a period of stress-a time when a number of major adjustments must
be made in the home, business, and social aspects of their lives.
Categories of Stress in Middle Adulthood are:
Somatic stress, which is due to physical evidences of aging
Cultural stress, stemming from the high value placed on youth, vigor, and success by
the cultural group
Economic stress, resulting from the financial burden of educating children and
providing status symbols for all family members
Psychological stress, which may be the result of the death of a spouse, the departure
of children from the home, boredom with marriage, or a sense of lost youth and approaching
death.
Most women, experience a disruption in homeostasis during their forties, when normally
they go through the menopause and their last children leave home, thus forcing them to make
radical readjustments in the pattern of their entire lives. For men, by contrast, the climacteric
comes later-generally in the fifties-as does the imminence of retirement with its necessary
role changes.
22.4.4 Middle Adulthood is a "Dangerous Age"
The usual way of interpreting "dangerous age" is in terms of the male who wants to
have a last fling in life, especially in his sex life, before old age catches up with him. It is a
time when individuals break down physically as a result of overwork, over worry, or careless
living. The incidence of mental illness rises rapidly in Middle Adulthood among both men
and women, and it is also a peak age for suicides, especially among men.
The threats to good adjustment that make Middle Adulthood dangerous are intensified
by sex differences in the time when upsets in physical and psychological homeostasis occur.
The so-called "middle-age revolt" of men usually coincides with the upsets in homeostasis
caused by the menopause in women. This not only strains the husband-wife relationship,
sometimes leading to separation or divorce, but it often predisposes both men and women to
physical and mental illness, alcoholism, drug addiction, and suicide.
22.4.5 Middle Adulthood is an "Awkward Age"
Just as adolescents are neither children nor adults, so middle-aged men and women are no
longer "young" nor are they yet "old." The middle-aged person "stands between the younger
'Rebel Generation' and the 'Senior Citizen Generation'-both of which is continuously in
the spotlight and suffers from the discomforts and embarrassments associated with both age
groups.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
260
Feeling that they have no recognized place in society, middle-aged people try to be as
inconspicuous as possible. The desire of middle-aged men and women to be inconspicuous is
reflected in their clothing. Most middle-aged people try to dress as conservatively as possible
and yet adhere to the prevailing styles. This conservatism rules their choice of material possessions,
such as homes and cars, and their patterns of behavior-whether it is the way they
entertain or the way they dance. The more inconspicuous they are, the less out of place they
feel in a society that worships youth.
22.4.6 Middle Adulthood is a Time of Achievement
According to Erikson, it is a crisis age in which either "Generativity" -the tendency to
produce-or "stagnation" -the tendency to stand still-will dominate. People either become
more and more successful or they stand still and accomplish nothing more. If middle-aged
people have a strong desire to succeed, they will reach their peak at this time and reap the
benefits of the years of preparation and hard work that preceded it.
Women, like men, who have worked throughout the years of early adulthood, generally
reach their peak during Middle Adulthood. However, this peak, until very recently, was far
below that of male workers. Women who spent their early adulthood in homemaking and
reentered the vocational world after their children were grown and on their own, find that
they, too, are forced to reach their peak in Middle Adulthood because employers regard them
as "too old" as they approach the sixties.
Normally, men reach their peak between forty and fifty years, after which they rest on
their laurels and enjoy the benefits of their hard-won successes until they reach the early
sixties when they are regarded as "too old" and usually must relinquish their jobs to younger
and more vigorous workers. Earnings normally reach a peak in Middle Adulthood.
Because leadership roles are generally held by middle-aged persons, they regard
themselves as the "command generation." The successful middle-aged person often
describes himself as no longer 'driven' but as now the 'driver' -in short, in command.
22.4.7 Middle Adulthood is a Time of Evaluation
As it is the peak age of achievement, it is logical that it also would be the time when they
would evaluate their accomplishments in light of their earlier aspirations and the expectations
of others, especially family members and friends. As a result of this self-evaluation, Archer
has pointed out, "The mid-years seem to require the development of a different, generally
more realistic sense of who one is. . . . In growing up, everyone nurtures fantasies or illusions
about what one is, and what one will do. A major task of the mid-life decade involves coming
to terms with those fantasies and illusions".
22.4.8 Middle Adulthood is evaluated by a Double Standard
The eighth characteristic of Middle Adulthood is that it is evaluated by a double standard,
a standard for men and a standard for women. In spite of the growing trend toward egalitarian
roles for men and women in the home, in business, industry, the professions, and in social
life, there still exists a double standard regarding aging. While this double standard affects
many aspects of the lives of middle-aged men and women, two are especially common.
The first relates to physical changes. When men's hair becomes gray, when they develop
lines and wrinkles on their faces and a middle-aged pouch in place of a once-slender
waistline, they are usually regarded as "distinguished." Similar physical changes in women
are judged as unattractive with major emphasis on the "middle-age spread."
The second area in which the double standard is apparent is in the approved way for
members of the two sexes to age. There are two different philosophies about how people
should adjust to Middle Adulthood: one, that they should stay young and active and, two, that
they should grow old gracefully, deliberately slowing down and taking life comfortably-this
is the "rocking-chair" philosophy. Women, on the whole, are more likely to adopt the
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
261
rocking-chair philosophy than men, though this holds true more for women of the lower class
than for the upper-middle and upper classes.
22.4.9 Middle Adulthood is the Time of the Empty Nest
The time when the children no longer want to live under the parental roof. Except in cases
where men and women marry later than the average age, or postpone having their children
until they are well established in their careers, or have large families spread out over a decade
or more of time, Middle Adulthood is the "empty nest" stage in marital lives.
After years of living in a family-centered home, most adults find it difficult to adjust to a
pair-centered home. This is because, during the child-rearing years, husbands and wives often
grew apart and developed individual interests. As a result, they have little in common after
mutual interests in their children wane and when they are thrown together to adjust to each
other the best they can. Unquestionably, the empty-nest period of Middle Adulthood is far
more traumatic for women than for men.
This is especially true of women who have devoted their adult years to homemaking and
who have few interests or resources to fill their time when their homemaking jobs lessen or
come to an end. Many experience a "retirement shock" similar to that experienced by men
when they retire.
22.4.10 Middle Adulthood is a Time of Boredom
Many, if not most, men and women experience boredom during the late thirties and
forties. Men become bored with the daily routine of work and with a family life that offers
little excitement. Women, who have spent most of their adulthood caring for the home and
raising children, wonder what they will do for the next twenty or thirty years. The unmarried
woman who has devoted her life to a job or career is bored for the same reason\ men are.
22.5 ERIKSON'S THEORY: GENERATIVITY VERSUS STAGNATION
Erikson's psychological conflict of midlife is called generativity versus stagnation.
Generativity involves reaching to others in ways that give to and guide the next generation.
Generativity i s under way in early adulthood, typically through child bearing and child
rearing and establishing a niche in the occupational world. It expands greatly in midlife. At
this stage, commitment extends beyond oneself (identity) and one's life partner (intimacy) to
a larger group-family, community, or society. The generative adult combines the need for
self-expression with the need for communion, integrating personal goals with the welfare of
the larger social world. The resulting strength is the capacity to care for others in a broader
way than in previous stages.
Erikson selected the term generativity to encompass everything generated that can outlive
the self and ensure society's continuity and improvement: children, ideas, products, and
works of art. Although parenting is a major means of realizing generativity, some people,
because of misfortune or special gifts, do not express it through their own children. Adults
can be generative in other family relationships as mentors in the workplace, in volunteer
endeavors, and through many forms of productivity and creativity.
Generativity brings together personal desires and cultural demands. On the personal side,
middle-aged adults feel a need to be needed; they want to attain symbolic immortality-that is,
make a contribution that will survive their death. On the cultural side, society imposes a
social clock for generativity in midlife, requiring adults to take responsibility for the next
generation through their roles as parents, teachers, mentors, leaders, and coordinators. And
according to Erikson, a culture's "belief in the species" -the conviction that life is good and
worthwhile, even in the face of human destructiveness and deprivation-is a major motivator
of generative action. Without this optimistic world view, people would not have any hope of
improving humanity.
The negative outcome of this stage is stagnation. Erikson recognized that once people
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
262
attain certain life goals, such as marriage, children, and career success, they may become selfcentered
and self-indulgent. Adults with a sense of stagnation cannot contribute to society's
welfare because they place their own comfort and security above challenge and sacrifice.
Their self-absorption is expressed in many ways through lack of involvement with and
concern for young people (including their own children), -through a focus on what they can
get from others rather than what they can give, and through taking little interest in being
productive at work, developing their talents, or bettering the world in other ways.
Just as Erikson's theory suggests, highly generative people appear especially well
adjusted-low in anxiety and depression and high in self-acceptance and life satisfaction. They
are also more open to differing viewpoints, possess leadership qualities, desire more from
work than financial rewards, and care greatly about the welfare of their children, their
partner, their aging parents, and the wider society. Furthermore, generativity is associated
with greater involvement in political activities, including voting, campaigning, and contacting
public officials, especially among adults for whom political participation was central to
identity in earlier years.
Having children seems to foster men's generative development more than women's.
Fathers are found to have high generativity than childless men. In contrast, motherhood is
unrelated to women's generativity. Perhaps parenting awakens in men a tender, caring attitude
toward the next generation that women have opportunities to develop in other ways.
Check Your Progress 3
Discuss the characteristics of middle adulthood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
22.6 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
v) Diverse life styles of early adulthood
vi) Career development during early adulthood
vii) Characteristics of Middle adulthood
viii) Erikson’s comparison of generativity and stagnation
22.7 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. The diverse life styles found during the early adulthood include:
i) Singlehood
ii) Cohabitation
iii) Childlessness
iv) Divorce and Remarriage
v) Variant Styles of Parenthood
2. The career development during early adulthood could include:
i) Establishing a Career
ii) Combining Work and Family during early adulthood
3. The Characteristics of Middle Adulthood are the following ten:
i) It is a Dreaded Period
ii) It is a Time of Transition
iii) It is a Time of Stress
iv) It is a "Dangerous Age"
v) It is an "Awkward Age"
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
263
vi) It is a Time of Achievement
vii) It is a Time of Evaluation
viii) It is evaluated by a Double Standard
ix) It is the Time of the Empty Nest
x) It is a Time of Boredom
22.8 Lesson – End Activities
1. Write short notes on the following
a) Role overload
b) Dual – earner.
2. Justify the statement “Middle Adulthood is a Time of Boredoms”.
22.9 References
1. Forrance, E.P. Guiding Creative Talent, Englewood Cliffs, N.J. : Prentice – Hall, 1980.
2. Jung, C.G., Modern Man in Search of a Soul, New York : Harcourt Porace, 1968.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
264
LESSON – 23
CHANGES IN MENTAL ABILITIES - ADAPTING TO PHYSICAL CHANGES IN
MIDLIFE - RELATIONSHIP AT MIDLIFE - VOCATIONAL LIFE
Contents
23.0 Aims and Objectives
23.1 Introduction
23.2 Changes in Mental Abilities
23.2.1 Crystallized and Fluid Intelligence
23.2.2 Individual and Group Differences
23.3 Adapting to the Physical Challenges of Midlife
23.3.1 Stress Management
23.3.2 Exercise
23.3.3 Optimistic Outlook
23.3.4 Gender and Aging: A Double Standard
23.4 Relationships at Midlife
23.4.1 Marriage and Divorce
23.4.2 Changing Parent-Child Relationships
23.4.3 Grandparenthood
23.4.4 Middle-Aged Children and Their Aging Parents
23.4.5 Siblings
23.4.6 Friendships
23.5 Vocational Life
23.5.1 Job Satisfaction
23.5.2 Career Development
23.5.3 Career Change at Midlife
23.5.4 Unemployment
23.5.5 Planning for Retirement
23.6 Let Us Sum Up
23.7 Check your Progress
23.8 Lesson – End Activities
23.9 References
23.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help you understand the changes in mental and physical abilities and
relationship patterns during midlife.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
i) State the changes that happen in the mental abilities
ii) Understand the adjustments to the physical challenges during midlife
iii) List the kind of relationships during midlife
iv) Mention the vocational life during midlife
23.1 INTRODUCTION
At age 50, when one occasionally couldn't recall a name or had to pause in the middle of
a lecture or speech to think about what to say next, this could be the first signs of an aging
mind. Majority of aging research has focused on deficits-because they are cause for concernwhile
neglecting cognitive stability and gains.
Different aspects of cognitive functioning show different patterns of change. Although
decline occurs in some areas, most people display cognitive competence, especially in
familiar contexts, and some attain outstanding accomplishment. Overall, the evidence
supports an optimistic view of adult cognitive potential.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
265
23.2 CHANGES IN MENTAL ABILITIES
The changes we are about to consider bring into bold relief core assumptions of the
lifespan perspective: development as multidimensional, or the combined result of biological,
psychological, and social forces; development as multidirectional, or the joint expression of
growth and decline, with the precise mix varying across abilities and individuals; and
development as plastic, or open to change, depending on how a person's biological and
environmental history combines with current life conditions.
23.2.1 Crystallized and Fluid Intelligence
There are consistent age-related trends in two broad mental abilities. Each includes a
diverse array of specific intellectual factors tapped by intelligence tests.
The first of these broad abilities is crystallized intelligence. It refers to skills that depend
on accumulated knowledge and experience, good judgment, and mastery of social
conventions. Together, these capacities represent abilities acquired because they are valued
by the individual's culture. On intelligence tests, vocabulary, general information, verbal
analogy, and logical reasoning items measure crystallized intelligence.
In contrast, fluid intelligence depends more heavily on basic information- processing
skills - the ability to detect relationships among stimuli, the speed with which we can analyze
information, and the capacity of working memory. Fluid intelligence often works with
crystallized intelligence to support effective reasoning, abstraction, and problem solving. But
fluid intelligence is believed to be influenced more by conditions in the brain and learning
unique to the individual, less by culture. Intelligence test items that reflect fluid abilities
include number series, spatial visualization, and picture sequencing.
Crystallized intelligence increases steadily though middle adulthood, whereas fluid
intelligence begins to decline in the twenties. These trends have been found repeatedly in
cross-sectional comparisons in which the education and health status of younger and older
participants are similar, thereby largely correcting for cohort effects (individuals born in the
same time period are influenced by a particular set of historical and cultural conditions). The
rise in crystallized abilities makes sense, since adults are constantly adding to their
knowledge and skills at work, at home, and in leisure activities. In addition, many crystallized
skills are practiced almost daily.
Some theorists believe that a general slowing of central nervous system functioning
underlies nearly all age-related declines in cognitive performance. Research reveals gains
followed by stability in crystallized abilities, despite a much earlier decline in fluid
intelligence, or basic information processing skills. First, the decrease in basic processing,
while substantial after age 45, may not be great enough to affect many well-practiced
performances until quite late in life. Second, adults often find ways to compensate for cognitive
weaknesses by drawing on their cognitive strengths. Finally as people discover that they
are no longer as good as they once were at certain tasks, they accommodate, shifting to
activities that depend less on cognitive efficiency and more on accumulated knowledge. The
basketball player becomes a coach, the quick-witted salesperson a manager.
23.2.2 Individual and Group Differences
Hidden beneath the age trends just described are large individual differences. Some
adults, because of illness or unfavorable environments, decline intellectually much earlier
than others. And a considerable number show full functioning, even in fluid abilities, at an
advanced age.
Adults who use their intellectual skills seem to maintain them longer. Declines can be
delayed for people with above-average education, highly complex occupations, and
stimulating leisure pursuits that included reading, traveling, attending cultural events, and
participating in clubs and professional organizations. People with flexible personalities,
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
266
lasting marriages (especially to a cognitively high-functioning partner), and absence of
cardiovascular and other chronic diseases were also likely to maintain mental abilities well
into late adulthood. And being economically well off was linked to favorable cognitive
development, undoubtedly because SES is associated with many of the factors just
mentioned.
Several sex differences also emerged, consistent with those obtained in childhood and
adolescence. In early and middle adulthood, women outperformed men on verbal tasks and
perceptual speed. In contrast, men excelled at spatial skills. Overall, however, changes in
mental abilities over the adult years were remarkably similar for the two sexes.
Furthermore, cohort effects were evident. For example, when the baby boom generation,
currently middle-aged, was compared with the previous generation at the same age, the baby
boom cohort performed substantially better on verbal memory, inductive reasoning, and
spatial orientation. These generational gains reflect advances in education, technology,
environmental stimulation, and health care. They are expected to continue in the twenty-first
century because today's children also attain higher mental test scores than children of past
generations. The recent generation's reliance on calculators and computers seems to have
compromised its calculation skills.
Check Your Progress 1
Explain the Changes in Mental Abilities during midlife
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
23.3 ADAPTING TO THE PHYSICAL CHALLENGES OF MIDLIFE
Middle adulthood is often a productive period in which people attain their greatest
accomplishments and life satisfactions. Nevertheless, it takes considerable stamina to cope
with the full array of changes this phase can bring.
23.3.1 Stress Management
There are negative consequences of psychological stress on the cardiovascular, immune,
and gastrointestinal systems. As adults encounter problems at home and at work, daily
hassles can add up to a serious stress load. Stress management is important at any age for a
more satisfying life. In middle adulthood, it can limit the age-related rise in illness and, when
disease strikes, reduce its severity.
Although many stressors cannot be eliminated, people can change how they handle some
and view others. People use two general strategies to cope with stress. In problem-centered
coping, they appraise the situation as changeable, identify the difficulty, and decide what to
do about it. If problem solving does not work, people engage in emotion-centered coping,
which is internal, private, and aimed at controlling distress when there is little we can do
about a situation. Adults who effectively reduce stress use a mixture of problem-centered and
emotion-centered techniques, depending on the situation. And their approach is deliberate,
thoughtful, and respectful of both themselves and others. In contrast, ineffective coping is
largely emotion-centered and either impulsive or escapist.
Constructive approaches to anger reduction are a vital health intervention. In terms of
problem-centered coping, teaching people to be assertive rather than hostile and to negotiate
rather than explode interrupts the intense physiological response that intervenes between
psychological stress and illness. If reasonable communication is not possible, it is best to
delay responding by leaving a provocative situation.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
267
People tend to cope with stress more effectively as they move from early to middle adulthood.
Many are more realistic about their ability to change situations than they were earlier.
And mid lifers may be more skilled at anticipating stressful events and taking steps to avoid
them. Furthermore, when middle-aged adults surmount a highly stressful experience, they
often report lasting personal benefits. Some describe a sense of disbelief at what they
accomplished under extremely trying conditions and a greater sense of mastery. And after a
serious illness and brush with death, changed values and perspectives-such as rediscovery of
the importance of health-promoting behaviors and of family relationships-are common. In
this way, managing intense stress can serve as a context for positive development.
23.3.2 Exercise
Regular exercise has a wide variety of physical and psychological benefits-among them,
equipping adults to handle stress more effectively. A person beginning to exercise in midlife
must overcome initial barriers and obstacles along the way, such as lack of time and energy,
inconvenience, and work conflicts. Self-efficacy-belief in one's ability to succeed-is just as
vital in adopting, maintaining, and exerting oneself in an exercise regimen as it is in career
progress. An important outcome of starting an exercise program is that it leads sedentary
adults to gain in self-efficacy, which promotes physical activity all the more. Enhanced
physical fitness, in turn, prompts middle-aged adults to feel better about their physical selves.
Over time, their physical self-esteem-sense of body conditioning and attractiveness rises.
23.3.3 Optimistic Outlook
Our ability to handle the inevitable changes of life depends in part on personality
strengths. What type of individual is likely to cope with stress adaptively, thereby reducing its
impact on illness and mortality? Searching for answers to this question, researchers have
studied a set of three personal qualities - control, commitment, and challenge - that, together,
they call hardiness.
First, many regard most experiences as controllable. "You can't stop all bad things from
happening; but you can try to do something about them." Second, a committed, involved
approach to daily activities, finding interest and meaning in almost all of them. Finally,
viewing change as a challenge – a normal part of life and a chance for personal growth.
Research shows that hardiness influences the extent to which people appraise stressful
situations as manageable, interesting and enjoyable. These positive appraisals, in turn, predict
health-promoting behaviors, tendency to seek social support and fewer physical symptoms.
Furthermore, high-hardy individuals are likely to use active, problem-centered coping
strategies in situations they can control. In contrast, low-hardy people more often use
emotion-centered and avoidant-coping Strategies - for example, saying, "I wish I could
change how I feel denying that the stressful event occurred, or eating and drinking to forget
about it. Many factors act as stress-resistant resources-heredity, diet, exercises, social support,
coping strategies, and more. Research on hardiness adds yet another ingredient: a generally
optimistic outlook and zest for life.
23.3.4 Gender and Aging: A Double Standard
Negative stereotypes of aging lead many middle-aged adults to fear physical changes.
These stereotypes are more likely to be applied to women than to men, yielding a double
standard. Despite the fact that many women in midlife say they have “hit their stride”-feel
assertive, confident, versatile, and capable of reviving life's problems-people often rate them
as less attractive and as having more negative characteristics than middle aged men. Aging
men actually gain slightly in positive judgments of appearance, maturity, and power, whereas
aging women show a decline.
The ideal of a sexually attractive woman-smooth skin, good muscle tone, and lustrous hairis
at the heart of the double standard of aging. Women prefer same-age or slightly older
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
268
sexual partners, whereas men prefer younger partners. To explain why, evolutionary theory
points to sex differences in reproductive capacity, whereas social learning theory emphasizes
gender roles. Some evidence suggests that the end of a woman's ability to bear children
contributes to negative judgments, especially by men. Media ads include middle-aged
people; they usually are male executives, fathers, and grandfathers, who are images of
competence and security. And the cosmetics industry offers many products designed to hide
signs of aging for women, but far fewer for men.
At one time in our evolutionary history, a double standard of aging may have been
adaptive. Today, as many couples limit childbearing and devote more time to career and
leisure pursuits, it has become irrelevant. Consequently, the double standard of aging is
declining-that more people are viewing middle age as a potentially upbeat, satisfying time for
both genders. Models of older women, whose lives are full of intimacy, accomplishment,
hope, and imagination, are promoting acceptance of physical aging and a new vision of
growing older-one that emphasizes gracefulness, fulfillment, and inner strength.
Check Your Progress 2
State the full array of changes that takes place in midlife
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
23.4 RELATIONSHIPS AT MIDLIFE
The emotional and social changes of midlife take place within a complex web of family
relationships and friendships. Although a few middle-aged people live alone, the vast
majority-9 out of 10 live in families, most with a spouse.
The middle adulthood phase of the family life cycle is often referred to as "launching
children and moving on”. At one time it was called the "empty nest,” but this phrase
implies a negative transition, especially for women. When adults devote themselves entirely
to their children, the end of active parenting can trigger feelings of emptiness and regret. But
for many people, middle adulthood is a liberating time, offering a sense of completion and an
opportunity to strengthen existing ties and build new ones.
A century ago, most parents reared children for almost all of their active adulthood. Due
to a declining birthrate and a longer life expectancy, contemporary parents launch children
about 20 years before retirement and then seek other rewarding activities. Because of the
lengthening of this period, it is marked by the greatest number of exits and entries of family
members. As adult children leave home and marry, middle aged people must adapt to new
roles of parent-in-law and grandparent. At the same time, they must establish a different type
of relationship with their aging parents, who may become ill or infirm and die. Let's see how
ties within and beyond the family change during this time of life.
23.4.1 Marriage and Divorce
Although not all couples are financially comfortable, middle-aged households are well off
economically compared with other age groups. Partly because of increased financial security,
and because the time between, departure of the last child and retirement is so long, the contemporary
social view of marriage in midlife is one of expansion and new horizons.
These forces strengthen the need to review and adjust the marital relationship. By middle
age, the marriage gives them satisfaction of family and individual needs, endured many
changes, and culminated in deeper feelings of love.
Marital satisfaction is a strong predictor of midlife psychological well-being. Middleaged
men who have focused only on career often realize the limited nature of their pursuits.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
269
At the same time, their wives may insist on a more gratifying relationship. In addition,
children fully engaged in adult roles remind middle-aged parents that they are in the latter
part of their lives, so many decide that the time for improving their marriages is now.
As in early adulthood, divorce is one way of resolving an unsatisfactory marriage in
midlife. Most divorces occur within 5 to 10 years of marriage. Divorce at any age takes a
heavy psychological toll, but mid lifers seem to adapt more easily than younger people.
Midlife gains in practical problem solving and effective coping strategies may reduce the
stressful impact of divorce.
Marital breakup, in midlife and earlier, is a strong contributor to the feminization of
poverty, a trend in which women who support themselves or their families have become the
majority of the adult poverty population, regardless of age and ethnic group. Because of weak
public policies safeguarding families, the gender gap in poverty is higher.
23.4.2 Changing Parent-Child Relationships
Parents' positive relationships with their grown children are the result of a gradual process
of "letting go:' starting in childhood, gaining momentum in adolescence, and culminating in
children's independent living. Most middle-aged parents adjust well to the launching phase of
the family life cycle. Investment in non parental relationships and roles, children's
characteristics, parents' marital and economic circumstances, and cultural forces affect the
extent to which this transition is expansive and rewarding or sad and distressing.
Parents who have developed gratifying alternative activities typically welcome their
children's adult status. A strong work orientation, especially, predicts gains in life satisfaction
after children depart from the home. Regardless of whether they reside with parents, adolescent
and young adult children who are "off-time" in development-not showing expected signs
of independence and accomplishment-can prompt parental strain..
Although the parental role changes, its continuation is important to middle-aged adults.
Departure of children is a relatively minor event when parent-child contact and affection are
sustained. When it results in little or no communication, parents' life - satisfaction declines.
Parents who had been warm and supportive in middle childhood and adolescence were more
likely to experience contact and closeness with their child in early adulthood.
Throughout middle adulthood, parents continue to give more assistance to children than
they receive, especially while children are unmarried or when they face difficulties, such as
marital breakup or unemployment. Providing emotional and financial support while children
get their lives under way is related to midlife psychological wellbeing.
When children marry, parents face additional challenges in enlarging the family network
to include in-laws. When warm, supportive, relationships endure, intimacy between parents
and children increases over the adult years, with great benefits for parents’, life satisfaction.
Once young adults strike out on their own, members of the middle generation, especially
mothers, usually take on the role of kin-keeper, gathering the family for celebrations and
making sure everyone stays in touch.
23.4.3 Grandparenthood
Although the stereotypical image of grandparents as very elderly persists, on average
many adults become grandparents in their mid to late forties. A longer life expectancy means
that adults will spend as much as one-third of their lifespan in the grandparent role.
23.4.3a Meanings of Grand parenthood: Grandparenthood is a highly significant milestone
to most who experience it. When asked about its meaning, people generally mention one or
more of the following gratifications:
Valued elder-being perceived as a wise, helpful person
Immortality through descendants-leaving behind not just one but two generations after
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
270
death
Reinvolvement with personal past-being able to pass family history and values to a
new generation
Indulgence-having fun with children without major child-rearing responsibilities
23.4.3b Grandparent-Grandchild Relationships: Grandparents' styles of relating to
grandchildren vary as widely as the meanings they derive from their new role. The
grandparent's and grandchild's age and sex make a difference. Typically, relationships are
closer between grandparents and grandchildren of the same sex and, especially, between
maternal grandmothers and granddaughters- a pattern found in many countries.
Grandmothers also report higher satisfaction with the grandparent role than grandfathers,
perhaps because it is an important means through which middle-aged women satisfy their
kin keeping function.
Grandparents who live far from young grandchildren usually have more distant
relationships with them, appearing mainly on holidays, birthdays, and other formal occasions
but otherwise having little contact. And even when grandparents reside far away, a strong
desire to affect the development of grandchildren can motivate them to become involved in
grandchildren's lives. As grandchildren get older, distance has less impact. Instead, the extent
to which the adolescent or young adult grandchild believes the grandparent values contact is a
good predictor of a close bond. SES and ethnicity are additional influences on grandparentgrandchild
ties. In higher-income families, the grandparent role is not central to family
maintenance and survival.
23.4.4 Middle-Aged Children and Their Aging Parents
Compared with earlier generations, today's adults spend more years not only as parents
and grandparents, but also as children of aging parents. A longer life expectancy means that
adult children and their parents are increasingly likely to grow old together. What are middleaged
children's relationships with their aging parents like? And how does life change for adult
children when an aging parent's health declines?
23.4.4a Frequency and Quality of Contact: A widespread myth is that adults of past
generations were more devoted to their aging parents than are adults of the present
generation. Although adult children spend less time in close proximity to their parents, the
reason is not neglect or isolation. Fewer aging adults live with younger generations now than
in the past because of a desire to be independent, made possible by gains in health and
financial security. Proximity increases with age. Elders who move usually do so in the direction
of kin, and younger people tend to move in the direction of their aging parents.
Middle age is a time when adults reassess relationships with their parents, just as they
rethink other close ties. Many adult children become more appreciative of their parents'
strengths and generosity.
The closer family ties were when children were growing up, the more help given and
received. Also, parents give more to unmarried children and to children with disabilities.
Similarly, children give more to widowed parents and parents in poor health. At the same
time, a shift in helping occurs over the adult years. Parent-to-child advice, household aid, gift
giving, and financial assistance decline, whereas child-to-parent help of various kinds
increases. Even when the early parent-child relationship was emotionally distant, adult children
offer more support as their parent’s age, out of a sense of altruism and family duty.
23.4.4b Caring for Aging Parents: The burden of caring for aging parents can be great.
The family structure has become more "top-heavy;' with more generations alive, but fewer
younger members as birthrates have declined. This means that more than one older family
member is likely to need assistance, with fewer younger adults available to provide it.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
271
Today's middle-aged adults with ill or frail parents often face competing demands of children
(some of whom are under age 18 and still at home) and employment. They are called the
sandwich generation because they are “sandwiched” or squeezed, between the needs of
aging parents and financially dependent children.
When an aging parent's spouse cannot provide care, adult daughters are the next most
likely relatives to do so. Even when the spouse is available, adult children-again, usually
daughters-often pitch in as needed. Men make a substantial contribution to care of aging
parents-one that should not be overlooked.
As adults move from early to later middle age, the sex difference in parental caregiving
declines. Perhaps as men reduce their vocational commitments and feel less need to conform
to a "masculine" gender role, they grow more able and willing to provide basic care. At the
same time, parental caregiving may contribute to men's greater openness to the "feminine"
side of their personalities.
23.4.5 Siblings
Siblings contact and support decline from early to middle adulthood, rebounding only
after age 70 for siblings living near one another. Decreased midlife contact is probably due to
the demands of middle-aged adults' diverse roles. However, most adult siblings report getting
together or talking on the phone at least monthly.
Despite reduced contact, many siblings feel closer in midlife, often in response to
major life events. Launching and marriage of children seem to prompt siblings to think more
about one another. Parental illness can have a profound impact on sibling ties. Brothers and
sisters who previously had little to do with one another find themselves in touch about
parental care. When parents die, adult children realize they have become the oldest
generation and must look to one another to sustain family ties. As in early adulthood, sistersister
relationships are closer than sister-brother and brother-brother ties, a trend apparent in
many industrialized nations.
In industrialized nations; sibling relationships are voluntary. In village societies, they are
generally involuntary and basic to family functioning. For example, among Indians, family
social life is organized around strong brother sister attachments. A brother-sister pair is often
treated as a unit in exchange marriages with another family. After marriage, brothers are
expected to protect sisters, and sisters serve as spiritual mentors to brothers. Families not only
include biological siblings but also grant other relatives, such as cousins, the status of brother
or sister. This leads to an unusually large network of sibling support throughout life.
In village societies, cultural norms reduce sibling conflict, thereby ensuring family
cooperation. In industrialized nations, promoting positive sibling interaction in childhood is
vital for warm, supportive sibling bonds in later years.
23.4.6 Friendships
As family responsibilities declined there will b e more time to spend with friends in
middle age. At all ages, men are less expressive with friends than are women. Men tend to
talk about sports, politics, and business, whereas women focus on feelings and life problems.
Women report a greater number of close friends and say they both receive and provide their
friends with more emotional support.
Nevertheless, for both sexes, number of friends declines with age, probably because
people become less willing to invest in non family ties unless they are very rewarding. As
selectivity of friendship increases, older adults express more complex ideas about friendship.
They also try harder to get along with friends. Having chosen a friend, middle-aged people
attach great value to the relationship and take extra steps to protect it.
By midlife, family relationships and friendships support different aspects of
psychological well-being. Family ties protect against serious threats and losses, offering
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
272
security within a long-term time frame. In contrast, friendships serve as current sources of
pleasure and satisfaction, with women benefiting somewhat more than men. As middle-aged
couples renew their sense of companionship, they may combine the best of family and
friendship. Indeed, research indicates that viewing a spouse as a best friend contributes
greatly to marital happiness.
23.5 VOCATIONAL LIFE
Midlife transition typically involves vocational adjustments. Work continues to be a
salient aspect of identity and self esteem in middle adulthood. More so than in earlier or later
years, people attempt to increase the personal meaning and self direction of their vocational
lives. At the same time, certain aspects of job performance improve. Older employees have
lower rates of absenteeism, turnover, and accidents and show no change in work productivity.
Consequently, the value of an older employee ought to be equal to, and possibly even greater
than, that of a younger employee.
A favorable transition from adult worker to older worker is hindered by negative
stereotypes of aging-incorrect beliefs about limited learning capacity, slower decision
making, and resistance to change and supervision. Furthermore, gender discrimination
continues to restrict the career attainments of many women.
23.5.1 Job Satisfaction
Job satisfaction has both psychological and economic significance. If people are
dissatisfied at work, the consequences include strikes, grievances, absenteeism, and turnover,
all of which are costly to employers.
Job satisfaction increases in midlife at all occupational levels, from executives to hourly
workers. The relationship is weaker for women than for men, probably because women's
reduced chances for advancement result in a sense of unfairness. It is also weaker for bluecollar
than for white-collar workers, perhaps because blue-collar workers have less control
over their own work schedules and activities. When different aspects of jobs are considered,
intrinsic satisfaction-happiness with the work itself-shows a strong age-related gain. Extrinsic
satisfaction contentment with supervision, pay, and promotions changes very little.
Key characteristics that predict job well-being include involvement in decision making,
reasonable workloads, and good physical working conditions. Older people may have greater
access to jobs that are attractive in these ways. Finally, having fewer alternative positions into
which they can move, older workers generally reduce their career aspirations. As the
perceived gap between actual and possible achievements declines, work involvement
increases.
23.5.2 Career Development
Career development is vital throughout work life especially during mid life.
23.5.2a Job Training
It is all too common among managers-even some who are older themselves that training
and on the-job career counselling are less available to older workers. And when career
development activities are offered, older employees may be less likely to volunteer for them.
On the person side, the degree to which an individual wants to change is important. With
age, growth needs decline somewhat in favor of security needs. Consequently, learning and
challenge may have less intrinsic value to many older workers. Perhaps for this reason, older
employees depend more on co-worker and supervisor encouragement for vocational
development. Yet we have just seen that they are less likely to have supportive supervisors.
Furthermore, negative stereotypes of aging reduce older workers' self-efficacy, or confidence
that they can renew and expand their skills-another reason they may not volunteer for training
experiences. Self-efficacy is a powerful predictor of employees' efforts to improve career
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
273
relevant skills.
Unfortunately, older workers sometimes receive more routine tasks than younger
workers. Therefore, some of their reduced motivation to engage in career-relevant learning
may be due to the type of assignments they receive. Interaction among co-workers can also
have a profound impact. Within project teams, people similar in age communicate more
often. Age-balanced work groups (with more than one person in each age range) foster onthe-
job learning because communication is a source of support as well as a means of
acquiring job-relevant information.
23.5.2b Gender and Ethnicity
The Glass Ceiling. Women and ethnic minorities rarely move into high-level management
jobs. Women and ethnic minorities face a glass ceiling, or invisible barrier to advancement
up the corporate ladder. Contrary to popular belief, their low numbers cannot be attributed to
poor management skills. Compared to men, women managers were rated as more effective
and satisfying to work for and as more likely to motivate extra effort. Characteristics that
distinguished them from their male counterparts were charisma, inspiration, and
considerateness. Modern businesses realize that the best managers must not only display
"masculine" authority and decisiveness but also build consensus through broad participation
in decision making-an approach that requires "feminine" qualities of caring and collaboration.
23.5.3 Career Change at Midlife
Although most people remain in the same vocation through middle age, career change
does occur. Midlife career changes are usually not radical; they typically involve leaving one
line of work for a related one. Most people move in the reverse direction-to careers that are
more relaxing, free of painful decisions, and less demanding in terms of responsibility for
others. The decision to change is often difficult. The individual must weigh years invested in
one set of skills, current income, and job security against present frustrations and hoped-for
gains from a new vocation.
When an extreme career shift occurs, it usually signals a personal crisis. Professionals
who abandoned their well-paid, prestigious positions for routine, poorly paid, semiskilled
work, non work problems influenced the break with an established career.
23.5.4 Unemployment
As companies downsize and jobs are eliminated, the majority of people affected are
middle-aged and older. Although unemployment is difficult at any time, middle-aged workers
show a sharper decline in physical and mental health than their younger counterparts. Older
workers affected by layoffs remain without work for a longer time, suffering substantial
income loss. In addition, people over age 40 who must reestablish occupational security find
themselves "off-time" in terms of the social clock. Consequently, job loss can disrupt major
tasks of midlife, including generativity and reappraisal of life goals and accomplishments.
Finally, having been more involved in and committed to an occupation, the older employed
worker has also lost something of greater value.
23.5.5 Planning for Retirement
Most workers report looking forward to retirement and an increasing number are leaving
full-time work in midlife. The average age of retirement declined during the past 2 decades.
Retirement is a lengthy, complex process that begins as soon as the middle-aged person first
thinks about it. Planning is important, since retirement leads to a loss of two important workrelated
rewards-income and status-and to change in many other aspects of life. Like other life
transitions, retirement is often stressful.
Retirement planning helps one to evaluate one’s options, learn about the availability of
resources, and prepare emotionally for the changes ahead. Planning results in better
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
274
retirement adjustment and satisfaction.
Unfortunately, less educated people with lower lifetime earnings are least likely to attend
retirement preparation programs, yet they stand to benefit the most. And compared with men,
women do less planning for retirement, often depending on their husband's preparations-a
finding that may change as women increasingly become equal, rather than secondary, family
earners.
Check Your Progress 3
A. Discuss the change in family relationship during midlife
B. Briefly mention the midlife transition in vocational adjustments.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
23.6 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
xv) The Changes in Mental Abilities
xvi) Adapting to the Physical Challenges of Midlife
xvii) Relationships at Midlife
xviii) Vocational Life
23.7 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. Your answer may include the following
i) Crystallized and Fluid Intelligence
ii) Individual and Group Differences
2. The physical challenges could be
i) Stress Management
ii) Exercise
iii) Optimistic Outlook
iv) A Double Standard in gender and aging
3. A. The change in family relationship during midlife is in
i) Marriage and Divorce
ii) Changing Parent-Child Relationships
iii) Grandparenthood
iv) Middle-Aged Children and Their Aging Parents
v) Siblings
vi) Friendships
B. The midlife transitions in vocational adjustments are in
i) Job Satisfaction
ii) Career Development
iii) Career Change at Midlife
iv) Unemployment
v) Planning for Retirement
23.8 Lesson – End Activities
1. What are the major factors which distract job satisfaction.
2. Career change loads to personal crisis.
23.9 References
1. Wills, G., The art of Thought, New York: Harcourt Brace, 1928.
2. Barlett, F., Thinking, New York : Basic Books, 1960.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
275
LESSON – 24
LATE ADULTHOOD - LIFE EXPECTANCY - PHYSICAL CHANGES - MEMORY
- ERIKSON'S THEORY: EGO INTEGRITY VERSUS DESPAIR
Contents
24.0 Aims and Objectives
24.1 Introduction
24.2 Characteristics of Late Adulthood
24.2.1 Late adulthood is a Period of Decline
24.2.2 Individual Differences in the Effects of Aging
24.2.3 Late adulthood is judged by Different Criteria
24.2.4 Social Attitudes toward Late adulthood
24.2.5 The Elderly Have a Minority-Group Status
24.2.6 Aging Requires Role Changes
24.2.7 Poor Adjustment is Characteristic of Late adulthood
24.2.8 The Desire for Rejuvenation is Widespread in Late adulthood
24.3 Life Expectancy
24.3.1 Variations in Life Expectancy
24.3.2 Life Expectancy in Late Adulthood
24.3.3 Maximum Lifespan
24.4 Physical Changes
24.4.1 Nervous System
24.4.2 Sensory Systems
24.4.3 Cardiovascular and Respiratory Systems
24.4.4 Immune System
24.4.5 Sleep
24.4.6 Physical Appearance and Mobility
24.5 Memory
24.5.1 Deliberate versus Automatic Memory
24.5.2 Associative Memory
24.5.3 Remote Memory
24.5.4 Prospective Memory
24.6 Erikson’s Theory: Ego Integrity versus Despair
24.7 Let Us Sum Up
24.8 Check your Progress
24.9 Lesson – End Activities
24.10 References
24.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This Lesson will help you understand characteristics of late adulthood, the life expectancy,
physical changes, and Memory during this period.
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
i) Mention the characteristics of late adulthood
ii) State the variations in life expectancy
iii) List the physical changes during late adulthood
iv) Discuss the significance of memory during late adulthood
24.1 INTRODUCTION
Late adulthood is the closing period in the life span. It is a period when people "move
away" from previous, more desirable periods-or times of "usefulness." As people move
away from the earlier periods of their lives, they often look back on them, usually regretfully,
and tend to live in the present, ignoring the future as much as possible.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
276
Age sixty is usually considered the dividing line between middle and late adulthood.
However, it is recognized that chronological age is a poor criterion to use in marking off the
beginning of late adulthood because there are such marked differences among individuals in
the age at which aging actually begins.
Because of better living conditions and better health care, most men and women today do
not show the mental and physical signs of aging until the mid sixties or even the early
seventies. For that reason, there is a gradual trend toward using sixty-five-the age of
retirement in many businesses-to mark the beginning of late adulthood.
The last stage in the life span is frequently subdivided into early old age, which extends
from age sixty to age seventy, and advanced old age, which begins at seventy and extends to
the end of life. People during the sixties are usually referred to as "elderly" -meaning
somewhat old or advanced beyond middle age-and "old'" after they reach the age of seventymeaning,
according to standard dictionaries, advanced far in years of life and having lost the
vigor of youth.
24.2 CHARACTERISTICS OF LATE ADULTHOOD
Like every other period in the life span, late adulthood is characterized by certain physical
and psychological changes. The effects of these changes determine, to a large extent, whether
elderly men and women will make good or poor personal and social adjustments. The
characteristics of late adulthood, however, are far more likely to lead to poor adjustments
than to good and to unhappiness rather than to happiness. That is why late adulthood is even
more dreaded than middle age.
24.2.1 Late adulthood is a Period of Decline
As has been stressed repeatedly, people are never static. Instead, they constantly change.
During the early part of life the changes are evolutional in that they lead to maturity of
structure and functioning. In the latter part of life, by contrast, they are mainly involution,
involving a regression to earlier stages. These changes are the natural accompaniment of what
is commonly known as "aging." They affect physical as well as mental structures and
functioning.
The period during late adulthood when physical and mental decline is slow and gradual
and when compensations can be made for these declines is known as senescence-a time of
growing old or of aging. People may become senescent in their fifties or not until their early
or late sixties, depending upon the rate of physical and mental decline.
The term "senility" is used to refer to the period during late adulthood when a more or
less complete physical breakdown takes place and when there is mental disorganization. The
individual who becomes eccentric, careless, absentminded, socially withdrawn, and poorly
adjusted is usually described as "senile." Senility may come as early as the fifties, or it may
never occur because the individual dies before deterioration sets in.
Decline comes partly from physical and partly from psychological factors. The physical
cause of decline is a change in the body cells due not to a specific disease but to the aging
process. Decline may also have psychological causes. Unfavorable attitudes toward oneself,
other people, work, and life in general can lead to senility, just as changes in the brain tissue
can.
Motivation likewise plays a very important role in decline. The individual who has little
motivation to learn new things or to keep up to date in appearance, attitudes, or patterns of
behavior will deteriorate much faster than one whose motivation to ward off aging is
stronger. The new leisure time, which comes with retirement or with the lessening of
household responsibilities, often brings boredom which lowers the individual's motivation.
24.2.2 Individual Differences in the Effects of Aging
Individual differences in the effects of aging have been recognized for many centuries.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
277
Today, even more than in the past, it is recognized that aging affects different people
differently. Thus it is impossible to classify anyone as a "typically" old person or any trait as
"typical" of late adulthood. People age differently because they have different hereditary
endowments, different socioeconomic and educational backgrounds, and different patterns of
living. These differences are apparent among members of the same sex, but they are even
more apparent when men and women are compared because aging takes place at different
rates for the two sexes.
Physical aging precedes mental aging, though sometimes the reverse is true, especially
when the individual is concerned about growing old and let’s go mentally when the first signs
of physical aging appear.
24.2.3 Late adulthood is judged by Different Criteria
As the meaning of age is vague and undefined to young children, they tend to judge age
in terms of physical appearance and activities. To them, children are smaller than adults and
must be cared for while adults are big and can take care of themselves. Old people have white
hair and no longer go to work every day.
By the time children reach adolescence, they judge late adulthood in much the same way
as adults do, namely in terms of the person's appearance and what the person can and cannot
do. Knowing that these are the two most common criteria used to judge their ages, many
elderly people do all they can to camouflage the telltale physical signs of aging by wearing
clothes like those worn by younger people, and trying to keep up a pace that often overtaxes
their strength and energy. This is their attempt to create the illusion that they are not yet
elderly or old.
24.2.4 Social Attitudes toward Late adulthood
Stereotypes about late adulthood have a pronounced influence on social attitudes toward
both late adulthood and old people. And because most stereotypes are unfavorable, social
attitudes likewise tend to be unfavorable.
The unfavorable social attitudes have been emphasized in the difference between the
social image of the elderly-the image on which social attitudes are based-and the images the
elderly have of themselves-self-images. Note that, for the most part, the elderly have a more
favorable image of themselves than the social group has of them.
The significance of unfavorable social attitudes toward the elderly is that it affects the
way elderly people are treated. Instead of the reverence and respect for the elderly,
characteristic of many cultures, social attitudes result in making the elderly feel that they are
no longer useful to the social group and, hence, are more of a nuisance than an asset.
Unfavorable social attitudes toward the elderly are fairly universal today, but they tend
to be stronger among certain racial groups and social classes than among others. People who
come from countries where respect for the elderly is customary usually treat elderly people
with more consideration and respect than do those whose families have lived in America for
several generations and who have absorbed the prevailing American social attitude toward
the elderly. Members of the upper social classes, knowing that the elderly hold the purse
strings to family fortunes, tend to treat elderly members of their social group with more
respect than do those of the middle or lower classes, who often must be financially
responsible for elderly family members and, as a result, resent them.
24.2.5 The Elderly Have a Minority-Group Status
It is a fact that the number of old people are growing, they occupy a minority-group
status-a status that excludes them to some extent from interaction with other groups in the
population and which gives them little or no power. This minority-group status is primarily
the result of the unfavorable social attitudes toward the aged that have been fostered by the,
unfavorable stereotypes of them.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
278
This "second-class citizenship" puts the elderly on the defensive and has a marked
effect on their personal and social adjustments. It makes the latter years of life far from
"golden" for most people, and it causes them to be victimized by some members of the
majority group.
The elderly are not only taken advantage of by unscrupulous business people but they
are also the victims of crimes ranging from purse snatching to rape. This is especially true of
elderly women who create the impression that they are not strong or agile enough to defend
themselves. As a result of the crimes against them, many elderly people hesitate to leave their
homes or to do so without a younger person along to protect them.
24.2.6 Aging Requires Role Changes
Just as middle-aged people must learn to play new roles, so must the elderly. Today,
where efficiency, strength, speed, and physical attractiveness are highly valued, elderly
people are often regarded as useless. Because they cannot compete with young people in the
areas where highly valued traits are needed, the social attitude toward them is unfavorable.
Furthermore, it is expected that old people will play a decreasingly less active role in
social and community affairs, as well as in the business and professional worlds. As a result,
there is a marked reduction in the number of roles the elderly person is able to play, and there
are changes in some of the remaining roles. While these changes are due in part to the individual's
preferences, they are due mainly to social pressures.
24.2.7 Poor Adjustment is Characteristic of Late adulthood
Because of the unfavorable social attitudes toward the elderly that are reflected in the way
the social group treats them, it is not surprising that many elderly people develop unfavorable
self-concepts. These tend to be expressed in maladjustive behavior of different degrees of
severity. Those who have a history of poor adjustments tend to become more maladjusted as
age progresses than those whose earlier personal and social adjustments were more favorable.
Elderly people tend, as a group, to be more subject to maladjustments than those who are
younger. Due to increased loss of status in a society dominated by the young, a desire to
protect their finances for their wives, and a desire to escape partial - helplessness or pain.
24.2.8 The Desire for Rejuvenation is Widespread in Late adulthood
The minority-group status accorded to most elderly persons has naturally given rise to
a desire to remain young as long as possible and to rejuvenate when the signs of aging
appear. Ancient people used elixirs or potions, witchcraft and sorcery were used to achieve
youth. Today, medicine is being taken to replace sex hormones. Sex therapy, such as
Gerovital, the youth drug is used popularly.
Check Your Progress 1
Explain the characteristics of late adulthood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
24.3 LIFE EXPECTANCY
Average life expectancy-the number of years that an individual born in a particular
year can expect to live-provide powerful support for the multiplicity of factors that slow
biological aging, including improved nutrition, medical treatment, sanitation, and safety.
Twentieth-century gains in life expectancy were so extraordinary that they equaled those of
the previous 5,000 years. Steady declines in infant mortality are a major contributor to longer
life expectancy. But death rates among adults have decreased as well. For example, heart
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
279
disease, the leading cause of overall adult death has dropped by nearly 50 percent in the past
30 years, due to declines in risk factors (such as high blood pressure and smoking) and,
mostly, advances in medical treatment.
24.3.1 Variations in Life Expectancy
Consistent group differences in life expectancy underscore the joint contribution of
heredity and environment to biological aging. On average, women can look forward to 4 to 7
more years of life than men-a difference found in almost all cultures. The female life
expectancy advantage also characterizes several animal species, including rats, mice, and
dogs. The protective value of the female's extra X chromosome is believed to be responsible.
Because men are at higher risk for disease and early death, they reap somewhat larger
generational gains from positive lifestyle changes and new medical discoveries.
Length of life-and even more important, quality of life in late adulthood-can be predicted
by a country's health care, housing, and social services, along with lifestyle factors. When researchers
estimate active lifespan, the number of years of vigorous, healthy life an individual
born in a particular year can expect Japan ranks first. Japan's low rate of heart disease, linked
to its low-fat diet, in combination with favorable health care and other policies for the aged,
account for its leading status.
24.3.2 Life Expectancy in Late Adulthood
As at earlier ages, life expectancy continues to be greater for women than for men. Today,
the 65- to 69-year age group consists of 111 women for every 100 men; for people age 85 and
older, this number climbs to 160. Discrepancies like these occur in all countries. They are not
always present in the developing world because of high death rates of women during
childbirth. Although women outnumber men by a greater margin, differences in average life
expectancy between the sexes decline as elders advance in age.
24.3.3 Maximum Lifespan
The maximum life span or the genetic limit to length of life for a person free of external
risk factors varies between 70 and 110 for most people, with 85 about average. The oldest
verified age to which an individual has lived is 122 years.
Do these figures reflect the upper bound of human longevity, or can our life spans be
extended further? At present, scientists disagree on answers to this question. Some believe
that about 85 or 90 years is as much as most humans can expect, since gains in average life
expectancy are largely the result of reducing health risks in the first 20 or 30 years. Expected
life for people age 65 and older has increased very little-only about 5 months-over the past
decade. Others think we have not yet identified the human genetic limit because the life spans
of several species have been stretched in the laboratory-through selective breeding, genetic
engineering, and dietary calorie restriction.
Check Your Progress 2
Discuss the life expectancy during late adulthood.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
24.4 PHYSICAL CHANGES
The programmed effects of specific genes and the random cellular events believed to
underlie biological aging make physical declines more apparent in late adulthood. More
organs and systems of the body are affected. Nevertheless, most body structures can last into
our eighties and beyond, if we take good care of them.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
280
24.4.1 Nervous System
Aging of the nervous system affects a wide range of complex thoughts and activities.
Although brain weight declines throughout adulthood, the loss becomes greater after age 60
and may amount to as much as 5 to 10 percent by age 80, due to death of neurons and
enlargement of ventricles (spaces) within the brain. Neuron loss occurs throughout the
cerebral cortex but at different rates in different regions. In the visual, auditory, and motor
areas, as many as 50 percent of neurons die. In contrast, parts of the cortex (such as the
frontal lobes) that are responsible for integration of information, judgment, and reflective
thought show less change. Besides the cortex, the cerebellum (which controls balance and
coordination) loses neurons-in all, about 25 percent. Glial cells, which myelinate neural
fibers, decrease as well, contributing to diminished efficiency of the central nervous system.
But the brain can overcome some of these declines. Aging neurons established new
synapses after other neurons had degenerated. Furthermore, the aging cerebral cortex can, to
some degree, generate new neurons. And brain imaging research reveals that compared with
younger adults, elders who do well on memory tasks sometimes show more widely
distributed activity across areas of the cerebral cortex.
24.4.2 Sensory Systems
Changes in sensory functioning become increasingly noticeable in late life. Older adults
see and hear less well, and taste, smell, and touch sensitivity may also decline. Hearing
impairments are far more common than visual impairments, and they extend trends described
for middle adulthood, in that many more men than women are affected.
24.4.2a Vision: Structural changes in the eye make it harder to focus on nearby objects, see
in dim light, and perceive color. In late adulthood, vision diminishes further. For example, the
cornea (clear covering of the eye) becomes more translucent and scatters light, which blurs
images and increases sensitivity to glare. The lens continues to yellow, leading to further
impairment in color discrimination. From middle to old age, cloudy areas in the lens called
cataracts increase, resulting in foggy vision and eventual blindness. Besides biological
aging, heredity, sun exposure, and certain diseases (such as diabetes) increase the risk of
cataracts. Fortunately, removal of the lens and replacement with an artificial lens implant or
corrective eyewear are highly successful in restoring vision.
24.4.2b Hearing: Reduced blood supply and natural cell death in the inner ear and auditory
cortex along with stiffening of membranes (such as the eardrum), cause hearing to decline in
late adulthood. Decrements are greatest at high frequencies, although detection of soft sounds
diminishes throughout the frequency range. In addition, responsiveness to startling noises
lessens, and discriminating complex tone patterns becomes harder.
Although hearing loss has less impact on self-care than vision loss, it affects safety and
enjoyment of life. Of all hearing difficulties, the age-related decline in speech perception has
the greatest impact on life satisfaction. Ability to detect the content and emotionally
expressive features of conversation declines after age 70, a difficulty that worsens in noisy
settings.
As with vision, most elders do not suffer from hearing loss great enough to disrupt their
daily lives. Of those who do, compensating with a hearing aid and minimizing background
noise are helpful. Beginning at birth, our perception is intermodal (combines information
from more than one sensory system). By attending to facial expressions, gestures, and lip
movements, older adults can use vision to help interpret the spoken word. When family
members and others speak in quiet environments, older people are far more likely to convey
an image of alertness and competence than of reduced sensitivity to the surrounding world.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
281
24.4.2c Taste and Smell: Older adults have greater difficulty recognizing familiar foods by
taste alone. But no change in the number or distribution of taste buds takes place late in life,
so the drop in taste sensitivity just described may be due to factors other than aging.
Smoking, dentures, medications, and environmental pollutants can affect taste perception.
When taste is harder to detect, food is less enjoyable, increasing the likelihood of deficiencies
in the elderly person's diet. Flavor additives can help make food more attractive to older
adults.
Smell contributes to enjoyment of food and also has a self protective function. An aging
person who has difficulty detecting rancid food, gas fumes, or smoke may be in a lifethreatening
situation. A decrease in the number of smell receptors after age 60 contributes to
declines in odor sensitivity. Older adults are less accurate at linking odors with such
descriptors as floral, musky, fruity, or sweet.
24.4.2d Touch: Touch sensitivity is especially crucial for certain adults, such as the severely
visually impaired who must read in Braille and people who make fine judgments about
texture in their occupations or leisure pursuits-for example, in art and handicraft activities. To
measure touch perception, researchers determine how close two stimuli on the skin must be
before they are perceived as one. Aging brings a sharp decline on the hands, especially the
fingertips, and less of a drop on the arms and lips. Decreased touch sensitivity may be due to
loss of touch receptors in certain regions of the skin and slowing of blood circulation to the
extremities. After age 70, nearly all elderly are affected.
24.4.3 Cardiovascular and Respiratory Systems
Aging of the cardiovascular and respiratory systems proceeds gradually during early and
middle adulthood, usually without notice. Signs of change are more apparent in late
adulthood, and they prompt concern among aging individuals, who know these organ systems
are vital for quality and length of life.
As the years pass, the heart muscle becomes more rigid and some of its cells die while
others enlarge, leading the walls of the left ventricle (the largest heart chamber, from which
blood is pumped to the body) to thicken. In addition, artery walls stiffen and accumulate
some plaque (cholesterol and fats) due to normal aging (much more if the person has
atherosclerosis). Finally, the heart muscle becomes less responsive to signals from pacemaker
cells within the heart, which initiate each contraction.
The combined result of these changes is that the heart pumps with less force, maximum
heart rate decreases, and blood flow throughout the circulatory system slows. This means that
sufficient oxygen may not be delivered to body tissues during high physical activity.
Changes in the respiratory system compound the reduced oxygenation just described.
Because lung tissue gradually loses its elasticity, between ages 25 and 80, vital capacity
(amount of air that can be forced in and out of the lungs) is reduced by half. As a result, the
lungs fill and empty less efficiently, causing the blood to absorb less oxygen and give off less
carbon dioxide. This explains why older people increase their breathing rate more and feel
more out of breath when exercising.
Cardiovascular and respiratory deficiencies are more extreme in people who have smoked
cigarettes throughout their lives, failed to reduce dietary fat, or had many years of exposure to
environmental pollutants.
24.4.4 Immune System
As the immune system ages, T cells, which attack antigens (foreign substances) directly,
become less effective. In addition, the immune system is more likely to malfunction by
turning against normal body tissues in an autoimmune response. A less competent immune
system can increase the elderly person's risk for a variety of illnesses, including infectious
diseases (such as the flu), cardiovascular disease, certain forms of cancer, and a variety of
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
282
autoimmune disorders, such as rheumatoid arthritis and diabetes. But an age-related decline
in immune functioning is not the cause of most illnesses among the elderly. It merely permits
disease to progress, whereas a stronger immune reaction would have stamped out the disease
agent.
24.4.5 Sleep
Older adults require about the same total sleep time as younger adults: around 7 hours per
night. Yet as people age, they have more difficulty falling asleep, staying asleep, and sleeping
deeply-a trend that begins after age 30 for men and after age 50 for women. The timing of
sleep tends to change toward earlier bedtime and earlier morning awakenings. Changes in
brain structures controlling sleep and higher levels of stress hormones in the bloodstream,
which have an alerting effect on the central nervous system, are believed to be responsible.
Until age 70 or 80, men experience more sleep disturbances than women for several
reasons. First, enlargement of the prostate gland, which occurs in almost all aging men, constricts
the urethra (the tube draining the bladder) and leads to a need to urinate more often,
including during the night. Second, men are more prone sleep apnea, a condition in which
breathing ceases for 10 seconds or longer, resulting in many brief awakenings. The incidence
of sleep apnea in elderly men is high; 30 to 50 percent have twenty or more episodes per
night. Finally, periodic rapid movement of the legs sometimes accompanies sleep apnea but
also occurs at other times of night called ‘restless legs’ these movements may be due to
muscle tension, reduced circulation, or age-related changes in motor areas of the brain.
Although common among the elderly and not dangerous, they do disrupt sleep.
24.4.6 Physical Appearance and Mobility
The inner physical declines we have considered are accompanied by many outward signs
of growing older-involving the skin, hair, facial structure, and body build. Because these
changes occur gradually, older adults may not notice their elderly appearance until its arrival
is obvious.
Creasing and sagging of the skin, extends into late adulthood. In addition, oil glands that
lubricate the skin become less active, leading to dryness and roughness. “Age spots” increase;
in some elderly individuals, the arms, backs of the hands and face may be dotted with these
pigmented marks. Moles and other small skin growths may also appear. Blood vessels can be
seen beneath the more transparent skin, which has largely lost its layer of fatty support. This
loss further limits the older adult's ability to adapt to hot and cold temperatures.
The face is especially likely to show these effects because it is frequently exposed to the
sun, which accelerates aging. Other facial changes occur: The nose and ears broaden as new
cells are deposited on the outer layer of the skeleton. Teeth may be yellowed, cracked, and
chipped, and gums may recede; with better dental care, these outcomes are likely to be less
pronounced in future generations. As hair follicles under the skin's surface die, hair on the
head thins in both sexes, and the scalp may be visible. In men with hereditary pattern
baldness, follicles do not die but, instead, begin to produce fine, downy hair.
Body builds changes as well. Height continues to decline, especially in women, as loss of
bone mineral content leads to further collapse of the spinal column. Weight generally drops
after age 60 due to additional loss in lean body mass (bone density and muscle), which is
heavier than the fat deposits accumulating on the torso.
Several factors affect mobility. The first is muscle strength, which generally declines at a
faster rate in late adulthood than in middle age. Second, bone strength deteriorates because of
reduced bone mass, and tiny cracks in response to stress weaken the bones further. Third,
strength and flexibility of the joints and the tendons and ligaments (which connect muscle to
bone) diminish.
Check Your Progress 3
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
283
State the physical changes that happen during late adulthood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
24.5 MEMORY
As older adults take in information more slowly and find it harder to apply strategies,
inhibit irrelevant information, and retrieve relevant knowledge from long-term memory, the
chances of memory failure increase. A reduced capacity to hold material in working memory
while operating on it means that memory problems are especially evident on complex tasks.
24.5.1 Deliberate versus Automatic Memory
Although all of us have had memory failures from time to time, difficulties with recall
rise in late adulthood. Because working memories could hold less at once, they attended
poorly to context. When we try to remember, context serves as an important retrieval cue.
Because older adults take in less about a stimulus and its context, they sometimes cannot
distinguish an experienced event from one they imagined.
Automatic form of memory called implicit memory, or memory without conscious
awareness. In a typical implicit memory task, you would be asked to fill in a word fragment
(such as t k) after being shown a list of words. You would probably complete the sequence
with a word you had just seen (task) rather than other words (took or teak). Notice that you
engaged in recall without trying to do so.
24.5.2 Associative Memory
One way of characterizing the memory deficits in terms of a general, age-related decline
in binding information into complex memories. Researchers call this an associative memory
deficit or difficulty creating and retrieving links between pieces of information, - for
example, two items or an item and its context, such as one trying to remember the name of
the movie with the child actor or where one had seen the movie.
24.5.3 Remote Memory
Although older people often say that their remote memory or very long term recall is
clearer than their memory for recent events, research does not support this conclusion. How
about autobiographical memory, or memory for personally meaningful events, such as what
you did on your first date or how you celebrated your college graduation? To test for this type
of memory, researchers typically give a series of words (such as book, machine, sorry,
surprised) and ask adults to report a personal memory cued by each. People between 50 and
90 recall both remote and recent events more frequently than intermediate events, with recent
events mentioned most often. Among remote events recalled, most happened between ages
10 and 30.
24.5.4 Prospective Memory
Elderly people often complain that they have become more absentminded about daily
events. Prospective memory refers to remembering to engage in planned actions in the
future. The amount of mental effort required determines whether older adults have trouble
with prospective memory.
Older adults do better on event-based than on time-based prospective memory tasks. In an
event based task, an event (such as a certain word appearing on a computer screen) serves as
a cue for remembering to do something (pressing a key) while the participant engages in an
ongoing activity (reading paragraphs). As long as the event based task is not complex, older
adults do as well as younger adults.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
284
24.6 Erikson’s Theory: Ego Integrity versus Despair
The final psychological conflict of Erikson's theory, ego integrity versus despair, involves
coming to terms with ones life. Adults who arrive at a sense of integrity feel whole, complete,
and satisfied with their achievements. They have adapted to the mix of triumphs and
disappointments that are an inevitable part of love relationships, child rearing, work,
friendships, and community participation. They realize that the paths they followed,
abandoned, and never selected were necessary for fashioning a meaningful life course.
The capacity to view one's life in the larger context of all humanity-as the chance
combination of one person and one segment in history-contributes, to the serenity and
contentment that accompany integrity. In a study of people ranging in age from 17 to 82, increased
age was associated with greater psychosocial maturity, measured in terms of striving
for generativity and ego integrity in everyday behavior. Generativity and ego integrity, in
turn, largely accounted for the link between age and psychological well-being. Just as
Erikson's theory indicates, the psychosocial maturity of these later years seems to bring
increased happiness.
The negative outcome of this stage, despair, occurs when elders feel they have made
many wrong decisions, yet time is too short to find an alternate route to integrity. Without another
chance, the despairing person finds it hard to accept that death is near and is
overwhelmed with bitterness, defeat, and hopelessness. According to Erikson, these attitudes
are often expressed as anger and contempt for others, which disguise contempt for oneself.
Check Your Progress 4
Explain the tasks related to memory during late adulthood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
24.7 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) Characteristics of Late Adulthood
ii) Life Expectancy in Late Adulthood
iii) Physical Changes in Late Adulthood
iv) The tasks related to memory during late adulthood
v) Erikson’s Theory: Ego Integrity versus Despair
24.8 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
1. Your answer may include:
i) Late adulthood as a period of decline
ii) Individual Differences in the effects of aging
iii) Late adulthood as judged by different criteria
iv) Social attitudes toward late adulthood
v) The elderly have a minority-group status
vi) Aging requires role changes
vii) Poor adjustment is characteristic of late adulthood
viii) The desire for rejuvenation is widespread in late adulthood
2. The life expectancy during late adulthood includes:
i) Variations in Life Expectancy
ii) Maximum Lifespan
3. The physical changes that happen during late adulthood are:
i) Physical Changes
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
285
ii) Nervous System
iii) Sensory Systems
iv) Cardiovascular and Respiratory Systems
v) Immune System
vi) Sleep
vii) Physical Appearance and Mobility
4. Your answer may include:
i) Deliberate versus Automatic Memory
ii) Associative Memory
iii) Remote Memory
iv) Prospective Memory
24.9 Lesson – End Activities
1. Explain different types of memory.
2. Mention any one suitable method to promote memory.
24.10 References
1. Kiatsky, R.L., Human Memory, San Francisco : Freeman, 1976.
2. Hunter, Jan, M.R., Memory, London : Penguin Books, 1980.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
286
LESSON – 25
CHANGE IN SELF CONCEPT AND PERSONALITY - PSYCHOLOGICAL WELLBEING
- RELATIONSHIPS - RETIREMENT AND LEISURE
Contents
25.0 Aims and Objectives
25.1 Introduction
25.1.1 Secure and Multifaceted Self-Concept
25.1.2 Agreeableness, Sociability, and Acceptance of Change
25.1.3 Spirituality and Religiosity
25.2 Psychological Well-Being
25.2.1 Control versus Dependency
25.2.2 Health
25.2.3 Negative Life Changes
25.2.4 Social Support and Social Interaction
25.3 Relationships in Late Adulthood
25.3.1 Marriage
25.3.2 Gay and Lesbian Partnerships
25.3.3 Divorce and Remarriage
25.3.4 Widowhood
25.3.5 Siblings
25.3.6 Friendships
25.3.7 Elder Maltreatment
25.4 Retirement and Leisure
25.4.1 The Decision to Retire
25.4.2 Adjustment to Retirement
25.4.3 Leisure Activities
25.4.4 Successful Aging
25.5 Let Us Sum Up
25.6 Check your Progress
25.7 Lesson – End Activities
25.8 References
25.0 AIMS AND OBJECTIVES
This lesson will give an overall picture of the change in self concept and personality,
psychological well-being and the relationships with others and retirement and leisure of late
adulthood
After going through this Lesson, you will be able to:
v) Discuss the changes in self concept and personality during late adulthood
vi) Mention the psychological well-being during late adulthood
vii) List the relationships established during late adulthood
viii) State the retirement and leisure activities during late adulthood
25.1 INTRODUCTION
Stability of the “big five” personality traits from mid - to late life continues. The
ingredients of ego integrity: wholeness, contentment, and image of the self as part of a larger
world order. These attributes are reflected in several significant late-life changes in both selfconcept
and personality.
25.1.1 Secure and Multifaceted Self-Concept
Older adults have accumulated a lifetime of self-knowledge, leading to more secure and
complex conceptions of themselves than at earlier ages. Many believe that are good at
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
287
counselling others, growing a flower garden, giving dinner parties, budgeting money, and
figuring out who could be trusted and who couldn't. At the same time, one may believe that
she/he couldn't get around the city as easily as before.
Elders when asked to respond to the question ‘Who am I?’ participants mentioned a broad
spectrum of life domains, including hobbies, interests, social participation, family, health, and
personality traits. Adults of both ages expressed more positive than negative self-evaluations,
although a slight increase in negative comments occurred in the older group. Positive,
multifaceted self-definitions predicted psychological well-being.
As the future shortens, most elders, into their eighties and nineties, continue to mention
hoped-for selves in the areas of good health, relationships, and social responsibility and are
very active in pursuing them. These grant older adults goals in life and a sense of further
development. Even in advanced old age, when some capacities decline, the majority of older
adults retain a coherent sense of self. They regard themselves as very much the same person
they have always been.
25.1.2 Agreeableness, Sociability, and Acceptance of Change
During late adulthood, shifts in three personality characteristics take place-changes that,
once again, defy stereotypes of the elderly. Old age is not a time in which the personality
inevitably becomes rigid and morale declines. Instead, a flexible, optimistic approach to life
is common.
Rating open-ended interviews with elders in their sixties, and again when they reached
their eighties and nineties, researchers found that scores on adjectives that make up agreeableness--
generous, acquiescent, and good-natured--were higher on the second occasion
than the first for over one-third of the sample. These qualities seem to characterize people
who have come to terms with life despite its imperfections. However, participants showed a
slight dip in sociability as they aged. Perhaps this reflects a narrowing of social contacts as
people become more selective about relationships and as family members and friends dietrends.
A third, related development is greater acceptance of change-an attribute the elderly
frequently mention as important to psychological well-being. That many older adults adjust
well to change is evident in what they say when asked about dissatisfactions in their lives.
They often respond that they are not unhappy about anything! A capacity to accept life's
twists and turns, many of which are beyond one's control, is vital for positive functioning in
late adulthood. Most elders are resilient; they bounce back in the face of adversity, especially
if they tended to do so earlier in their lives.
25.1.3 Spirituality and Religiosity
How do older adults manage to accept declines and losses yet feel whole and complete
and anticipate death with calm composure? One possibility is that they develop a more
mature sense of spirituality. That is, they actively seek a higher meaning for life, knowing
that it will end in the foreseeable future. Spirituality is not the same as religion. A
transcendent sense of truth and beauty can be found in art, nature, and relationships with
others. But religion provides many people with beliefs, symbols, and rituals that guide this
quest for meaning.
Older adults attach great value to religious beliefs and behaviors. Although declining
health and transportation difficulties reduce organized religious participation in advanced old
age, informal religious activities remain prominent in the lives of today’s elders.
Furthermore, spirituality and faith may advance to a higher level in late life-away from
prescribed beliefs to a more reflective approach that emphasizes links to others and is at ease
with mystery and uncertainty. In his theory of the development of faith, James Fowler (Table
1) posits five stages, which have been confirmed in several studies. Notice how adults who
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
288
reach Stage 4 become aware of their own belief system as one of many possible world views,
contemplate the deeper significance of religious symbols and rituals, and open themselves to
other religious perspectives as sources of inspiration.
Table 1 - Fowler's Stages of Faith Development
Stage of Faith Period of
Development
Description
1. Intuitiveprojective
3-7 years Children's fantasy and imitation lead them to be
powerfully influenced by stories, moods, and behaviors
demonstrating the faith of adults. They become aware of
right and wrong actions
2. Mythicliteral
7-11 years Children begin to internalize the stories, beliefs, and
observances of their religious community, which they take
literally. For example, they often hold concrete images of
God living on top of the world and watching over
everybody
3. Syntheticconventional
Adolescence Adolescents have a coherent set of deeply felt beliefs and
values, which provides a basis for identity. They have not
yet examined this ideology systematically
4.
Individuativereflective
Adulthood Adults who reach this stage critically reflect on their
beliefs and values, recognizing that their world view is
only one of many possible world views. They actively
shape a personal ideology, forming and re-forming it over
time. About religious rituals and symbols, they ask, "What
does this really mean?"
5. Conjunctive Late
Adulthood
The few people who attain this stage form an enlarged
vision of an all-inclusive human community. They act to
bring it about by standing up against persecution and
injustice and by promoting a common good that serves the
needs of diverse groups. Great religious leaders, such as
Mahatma Gandhi and Martin Luther King, Jr., illustrate
conjunctive faith
Sex differences in religious involvement and spirituality are evident throughout
adulthood. Women are more likely than men to be church or synagogue members, to engage
in religious activities, and to report a personal quest for connectedness with a higher power.
Women's greater poverty, widowhood, and participation in caregiving, including caring for
chronically ill family members, expose them to higher levels of stress and anxiety.
Check Your Progress 1
Explain the changes in self concept and personality during late adulthood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
25.2 PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING
Most adults adapt well to old age. Yet a few feel dependent, incompetent, and worthless.
Identifying personal and environmental influences on late-life psychological wellbeing is
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
289
vital for designing interventions that foster positive adjustment.
25.2.1 Control versus Dependency
Observations of people interacting with older adults in both private homes and
institutions reveal two highly predictable, complementary behavior patterns. In the first,
called the dependency-support script, dependent behaviors are attended to immediately. In
the second, called the independence ignore script, independent behaviors are mostly
ignored. These sequences reinforce dependent behavior at the expense of independent
behavior, regardless of the older person's competencies. Even a self-reliant elder did not
always resist on others unnecessary help because it brought about social contact.
Among elders who experience no difficulty with daily activities, opportunities to interact
with others are related to high satisfaction with everyday life. In contrast, among elders who
have trouble performing daily activities, social contact is linked to a less positive everyday
existence. This suggests that social interaction while assisting elders with physical care,
household chores, and errands is often not meaningful and rewarding, but rather demeaning
and unpleasant. Consider these typical reactions of care recipients to a spouse's help with
daily activities: "felt dependent;' "felt indebted;' "felt like a weak, incapable person".
25.2.2 Health
Health is a powerful predictor of psychological well-being in late adulthood. Physical
declines and chronic disease can be highly stressful, leading to a sense of loss of personal
control-a major factor in adult mental health. Furthermore, physical illness resulting in
disability is among the strongest risk factors for late-life depression. Although fewer older
than young and middle-aged adults are depressed, profound feelings of hopelessness rise with
age as physical disability and consequent social isolation increase.
The relationship between physical and mental health problems can become a vicious
cycle, each intensifying the other. At times, the rapid decline of a sick elder is the result of
despondency and "giving up". This downward spiral can be hastened by a move to a nursing
home, requiring the older person to adjust to distance from family and friends and to a new
self-definition as "a person who can survive only in an institution” In the month after
admission, many residents deteriorate rapidly and become severely depressed. The stress of
illness together with institutionalization is associated with heightened health problems and
mortality. Depression in old age is often lethal. People age 65 and older have the highest
suicide rate of all age groups. Personal characteristics like effective coping and a sense of self
efficacy-are vitally important. But for frail elders to display these attributes, families and
caregivers must grant them autonomy by avoiding the dependency-support script. When older
adults remain in charge of personally important areas of their lives, they retain essential
aspects of their identity in the face of change and report a more favorable outlook on their
past and future.
25.2.3 Negative Life Changes
Elders are at risk for a variety of negative life changes-death of spouse, siblings, and
friends; illness and physical disabilities; declining income; and greater dependency. Negative
life changes are difficult for all people. But these events may actually evoke less stress and
depression in older than in younger adults. Many elders have learned to cope with hard times
and to accept loss as part of human existence.
Still, when negative changes pile up, they test the coping skills of older adults. In very old
age, such changes are greater for women than for men. Women over age 75 are far less likely
to be married, more often have lower incomes, and suffer from more illnesses-especially ones
that restrict mobility. Furthermore, elderly women more often say that others depend on them
for emotional support. This means that their social relations, even in very old age, are more
often a source of stress. And because of failing health, older women may not be able to meet
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
290
others' needs for caregiving, with negative consequences for their self-esteem. Not surprisingly,
women of very advanced age report a lower sense of psychological well-being than
men.
25.2.4 Social Support and Social Interaction
In late adulthood, social support continues to play a powerful role in reducing stress,
thereby promoting physical health and psychological well-being. Social support increases the
odds of living longer. And it may help explain the relationship of religious participation to
survival, noted earlier. Most of the time, elders receive informal assistance from family
members-first from their spouse or, if none exists, from children, and then from siblings. If
these individuals are not available, other relatives and friends may step in.
Nevertheless, many older adults place such high value on independence that they do not
want a great deal of support from people close to them unless they can reciprocate. When
assistance is excessive or cannot be returned, it often results in psychological distress.
Perhaps for this reason, adult children express a deeper sense of obligation toward their aging
parents than their parents expect from them
Check Your Progress 2
Briefly mention the late-life psychological wellbeing.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
25.3 RELATIONSHIPS IN LATE ADULTHOOD
The social convoy is an influential model of changes in our social networks as we move
through life. Assume that you are in the midst of a cluster of ships traveling together, granting
one another safety and support. Ships in the inner circle represent people closest to you, such
as a spouse, best friend, parent, or child. Those less close but still important travels on the
outside, with age, ships exchange places in the convoy, and some drift off while others join
the procession. But as long as the convoy continues to exist, you adapt positively.
The ways elders with diverse lifestyles sustain social networks of family members and
friends-an effort that fosters personal continuity and security in the face of major life
changes. As ties are lost, older adults draw others closer and even add replacements, although
not at the rate they did at younger ages. Tragically, for some older adults the social convoy
breaks down. There are circumstances in which elders experience abuse and neglect at the
hands of those close to them.
25.3.1 Marriage
Marital satisfaction rises from middle to late adulthood, when it is at its peak. Several
changes in life circumstance and couples, communication underlie this trend. First,
perceptions of fairness in the relationship increase as men participate more in household tasks
after retirement. For elders who experienced little social pressure for gender equality in their
youth, division of labor in the home still reflects traditional roles. Men take on more home
maintenance projects, whereas women's duties cooking, cleaning, laundry, and shoppingcontinue
as before. Among adults retiring today, “feminine” tasks are more equally shared
than they were during work life. In either case, men's increased involvement in caring for the
home results in a greater sense of equity in marriage than before.
Second, with extra time together, the majority of couples engage in more joint leisure
activities. Finally, greater emotional understanding and emphasis on regulating emotion in
relationships lead to more positive interactions. Couples married for at least 35 years resolve
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
291
conflicts in ways that are less negative and more affectionate than do middle-aged couples.
When marital dissatisfaction is present, it continues to take a greater toll on women than
on men. Women tend to confront marital problems and try to solve them. In old age, the
energy expended is especially taxing on their physical and mental health. Husbands, in contrast,
often protect themselves by withdrawing, as they did in their twenties and thirties.
25.3.2 Gay and Lesbian Partnerships
Elderly gays and lesbians in long-term partnerships have sustained their relationships
through a historical period of hostility and discrimination toward homosexuals. Nevertheless,
most report happy, highly fulfilling relationships, pointing to their partner as their most
important source of social support. And compared with homosexual elders who live alone,
homosexual partners rate their physical and mental health more favorably.
A lifetime of effective coping with an oppressive social environment may have
strengthened homosexuals, skill at dealing with late-life physical and social changes, thereby
contributing to a satisfying partnership. And greater gender-role flexibility enables gay and
lesbian couples to adapt easily to sharing household tasks following retirement. Furthermore,
because of imagined or real strain in family relationships when they told others about their
homosexuality, gays and lesbians less often assume that family members will provide support
in old age.
Nevertheless, because of continuing prejudice and lack of social recognition of their
partnerships, aging gays and lesbians face unique challenges. Health care systems are often
unresponsive to their unique needs. And if their loved one becomes frail or ill, partners are
welcome in hospitals or nursing homes or are allowed to participate in health care decisionsan
issue. These circumstances can make late-life declines and losses especially painful.
25.3.3 Divorce and Remarriage
Couples who divorce in late adulthood constitute a very small proportion of all divorces
in any given year-less than 1 percent. But the divorce rate among people age 65 and older is
increasing as new generations of elders become more accepting of marital breakup and as the
divorce risk rises for second and subsequent marriages. When asked about the reasons for
divorce, elderly men typically mention lack of shared interests and activities, whereas women
frequently cite their partner's refusal to communicate and emotional distance. Following divorce,
they find it harder to separate their identity from that of their former spouse, and they
suffer more from a sense of personal failure. Relationships with family and friends shift at a
time when close bonds are crucial for psychological wellbeing. Women suffer most from
late-life divorce because they are more likely than men to spend their remaining years living
alone. The financial consequences are severe-greater than for widowhood because many
accumulated assets are lost in property settlements.
Remarriage rates are low in late adulthood and decline with age, although they are
considerably higher among divorced than widowed elders. Older men's opportunities for remarriage
are far greater than women's. Nevertheless, the gender gap in elder remarriage is
much smaller after divorce than after widowhood. Perhaps because their previous relationship
was disappointing, divorcees find it easier than widows to enter a new relationship. Also,
divorced older women may be more motivated to remarry because of their more extreme
economic circumstances. Finally, some divorced elders leave their marriages only after a new
bond is forming.
25.3.4 Widowhood
Widows make up one-third of the elderly population. Most widows and widowers live
alone rather than in extended families. Although they are less well off financially than
married elders, most want to retain control over their time and living space and to avoid
disagreements with their adult children. When widowed elders relocate because they cannot
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
292
make mortgage payments or keep up their homes, they usually move closer to family rather
than into the same residence.
The greatest problem for recently widowed elders is profound loneliness. At the same
time, wide variation in adaptation exists, with age, social support, and personality making a
difference. Elders have fewer lasting problems than younger individuals who are widowed,
probably because death in later life is expected and viewed as less unfair. And most widowed
elders - especially those with outgoing personalities and high self-esteem, are resilient in the
face of loneliness. They try to maintain social relationships that were important before the
spouse's death and report that relatives and friends respond in kind, contacting them at least
as often as before. Also, the stronger elders' sense of self efficacy in handling tasks of daily
living, the more favorably they adjust.
25.3.5 Siblings
Elderly siblings in industrialized nations are more likely to socialize than to provide each
other with direct assistance because older adults turn to their spouse and children before they
turn to their siblings. Nevertheless, siblings seem to be an important "insurance policy" in late
adulthood. Widowed and never-married elders have more contacts with siblings, perhaps
because they have fewer competing family relationships. They are also more likely to receive
sibling support during illness.
25.3.6 Friendships
As family responsibilities and vocational pressures lessen, friendships take on increasing
importance. Having friends is an especially strong predictor of mental health among the
elderly. Elders reported more favorable experiences with friends than with family members, a
difference partly due to the many pleasurable leisure activities shared with friends. But
unique qualities of friendship interaction open ness, spontaneity, mutual caring, and common
interests-seemed especially influential. In elder friendships, affection and emotional support
are both given and received to maintain balance in the relationship. Although friends call on
each other for help with tasks of daily living, they generally do so only in emergencies or for
occasional, limited assistance. As elders avoid excessive dependency on friends, they register
their own autonomy.
25.3.7 Elder Maltreatment
Although the majority of older adults enjoy positive relationships with family members,
friends, and professional caregivers, some suffer maltreatment at the hands of these individuals.
Recent media attention has led elder maltreatment to become a serious public
concern.
Elder maltreatment takes the following forms:
Physical abuse-intentional infliction of pain, discomfort, or injury, through hitting,
cutting, burning, physical force, restraint, sexual assault, and other acts.
Physical neglect-intentional or unintentional failure to fulfill caregiving obligations,
which results in lack of food, medication, or health services or in the elderly person being
left alone or isolated.
Psychological abuse-verbal assaults (such as name calling), humiliation (being treated as
a child), and intimidation (threats of isolation or placement in a nursing home).
Financial abuse-illegal or improper exploitation of the elder's property or financial
resources, through theft or use without the elder's consent.
Check Your Progress 3
Write about the relationships established during late adulthood
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
293
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
25.4 RETIREMENT AND LEISURE
The period of retirement has lengthened due to increased life expectancy and a steady
decline in average age of retirement-trends occurring in all Western industrialized nations.
These changes have also led to a blurring of the distinction between work and retirement.
Because mandatory retirement no longer exists for most workers in Western countries, older
adults have more choices about when to retire and how they spend their time. The retirement
process may include a planning period that lasts for years, the decision itself, diverse acts of
retiring, and continuous adjustment and readjustment of activities for the rest of the life
course.
Some older adults retire gradually by cutting down their hours and responsibilities, taking
part-time bridge jobs that serve as transitions between full-time career and retirement. Others
give up their jobs but later return to work both to support themselves because of limited
financial resources and to introduce interest and challenge into the retirement years.
25.4.1 The Decision to Retire
Personal and workplace factors in addition to income that influence the decision to retire.
People in good health, for whom vocational life is central to self-esteem, and whose work
environments are pleasant and stimulating are likely to keep on working. For these reasons,
individuals in professional occupations usually retire later than those in blue-collar or clerical
jobs. Self-employed elders also stay with their jobs longer, probably because they can
flexibly adapt their working hours to changing needs. In contrast, people in declining health;
who are engaged in routine, boring work; and who have compelling leisure interests often opt
for retirement.
Societal factors also affect retirement decisions. When many younger, less costly workers
are available to replace older workers, industries are likely to offer added incentives for
people to retire, such as increments to pension plans and earlier benefits-a trend that has
contributed to a rising number of retirements.
25.4.2 Adjustment to Retirement
Because retirement involves giving up roles that are a vital part of identity and selfesteem,
it usually is assumed to be a stressful process that contributes to declines in physical
and mental health. We must be careful not to assume a cause-and-effect relationship each
time retirement and unfavorable reactions are paired. For example, a wealth of evidence
confirms that physical health problems lead elders to retire, rather than the reverse. And for
most people, mental health is fairly stable from the pre- to postretirement years, with little
change prompted by retirement itself. The widely held belief that retirement inevitably leads
to adjustment problems is contradicted by countless research findings indicating that most
people adapt well. They describe themselves as active and socially involved-major determinants
of retirement satisfaction.
25.4.3 Leisure Activities
With retirement, older adults have more time for leisure pursuits than ever before. After a
"honeymoon period" of trying out new activities, many find that leisure interests and skills
do not develop suddenly. Instead, meaningful leisure pursuits are usually formed earlier and
sustained or expanded during retirement.
Involvement in leisure activities is related to better physical and mental health and
reduced mortality. But simply participating does not explain this relationship. Instead, elders
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
294
select leisure pursuits because they permit self-expression, new achievements, the rewards of
helping others, or pleasurable social interaction. These factors account for gains in wellbeing.
25.4.4 Successful Aging
Contemporary experts' view of successful aging, in which gains are maximized and
losses minimized. Successful agers are people for whom growth, vitality, and striving limit
and, at times, overcome physical, cognitive, and social declines. Researchers want to know
more about their characteristics and development so they can help more seniors’ age well.
Yet theorists disagree on the precise ingredients of a satisfying old age. Some focus on easily
measurable outcomes, such as excellent cardiovascular functioning, absence of disability,
superior cognitive performance, and creative achievements. Yet this view has been heavily
criticized. Not everyone can become an outstanding athlete, an innovative scientist, or a
talented artist.
Recent views of successful aging have turned away from specific achievements toward
processes people use to reach personally valued goals. This perspective avoids identifying
one set of standards as "successful." Instead, it focuses on how people minimize losses while
maximizing gains.
Check Your Progress 4
Write about the retirement and leisure activities during late adulthood.
Notes: a) Write your answer in the space given below.
b) Check your answer with the one given at the end of this lesson.
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
.....................................................................................................................................................
25.5 LET US SUM UP
In this Lesson, we have touched upon the following points:
i) the changes in self concept and personality during late adulthood
ii) the psychological well-being during late adulthood
iii) the relationships established during late adulthood
iv) the retirement and leisure activities during late adulthood
25.6 Check Your Progress: Model Answers
2. The answer may include the following:
a. Secure and Multifaceted Self-Concept
b. Agreeableness, Sociability, and Acceptance of Change
c. Spirituality and Religiosity
3. The late-life psychological wellbeing are:
a. Control versus Dependency
b. Health
c. Negative Life Changes
d. Social Support and Social Interaction
3. The relationships established during late adulthood
i) Marriage
ii) Gay and Lesbian Partnerships
iii) Divorce and Remarriage
iv) Widowhood
v) Siblings
vi) Friendships
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com
295
4.The retirement and leisure activities during late adulthood are:
i) The Decision to Retire
ii) Adjustment to Retirement
iii) Leisure Activities
iv) Successful Aging
25.7 Lesson – End Activities
1. Prepare a programme for retirement life.
2. Briefly describe the psychological factors promotes the well being during old age.
25.8 References
1. Adams, H.E., Psychology of Adjustments, New York, Ronald, 1978.
2. Rivgin, L. Education for Adjustment, New York : Appleton Century, 1986.
This watermark does not appear in the registered version - http://www.clicktoconvert.com