Visual Crowding
7 Followers
Recent papers in Visual Crowding
Tourist density and over-crowding is seen as social problems in recreational areas. Visual methods such as photographic assessment technique provide a more realistic representation of crowding levels in an area. Hence, photographic... more
Tourist density and over-crowding is seen as social problems in recreational areas. Visual methods such as photographic assessment technique provide a more realistic representation of crowding levels in an area. Hence, photographic assessment is used to determine tourists crowding perception in Tunku Abdul Rahman Park (TARP). A total of 406 respondents were asked to rate their perception on crowding based on five determinants mainly; crowding observation, preferred crowding, maximum acceptability, expected crowding and negative crowding effect on five different photographs. Descriptive analysis is used to analyse the tourist perception of crowding. Results indicate that tourists to TARP have observed a crowded situation during their visit (mean score= 4.05). This is slightly higher than the expected level of crowding (mean score= 3.91) and majority of the tourists prefer a lower crowding level (mean score= 3.72). The actual level of crowding encountered is higher than their maximum level of acceptability for crowding at the area (mean score= 3.86). The tourists also perceive a negative crowding effect (mean score= 3.91), which resulted to no intention to revisit the islands in the future. From the findings, it can be said that perception of crowding is measurable using the photographic method and it is useful tools in determining a certain use-level in tourist areas.
Keywords— Over-crowding, Crowding assessment, Crowding perception, Photographic assessment
Keywords— Over-crowding, Crowding assessment, Crowding perception, Photographic assessment
Crowding refers to the inability to recognize objects in clutter, setting a fundamental limit on various perceptual tasks such as reading and facial recognition. While prevailing models suggest that crowding is a unitary phenomenon... more
Crowding refers to the inability to recognize objects in clutter, setting a fundamental limit on various perceptual tasks such as reading and facial recognition. While prevailing models suggest that crowding is a unitary phenomenon occurring at an early level of processing, recent studies have shown that crowding might also occur at higher levels of representation. Here we investigated whether local and global crowding interference co-occurs within the same display. To do so, we tested the distinctive contribution of local flanker features and global configurations of the flankers on the pattern of crowding errors. Observers (n = 27) estimated the orientation of a target when presented alone or surrounded by flankers. Flankers were grouped into a global configuration, forming
an illusory rectangle when aligned or a rectangular configuration when misaligned. We analyzed the error distributions by fitting probabilistic mixture models. Results showed that participants often misreported the orientation of a flanker instead of that of the target. Interestingly, in some
trials the orientation of the global configuration was misreported. These results suggest that crowding occurs simultaneously across multiple levels of visual processing and crucially depends on the spatial
configuration of the stimulus. Our results pose a challenge to models of crowding with an early single pooling stage and might be better explained by models which incorporate the possibility of multilevel crowding and account for complex target-flanker interactions.
an illusory rectangle when aligned or a rectangular configuration when misaligned. We analyzed the error distributions by fitting probabilistic mixture models. Results showed that participants often misreported the orientation of a flanker instead of that of the target. Interestingly, in some
trials the orientation of the global configuration was misreported. These results suggest that crowding occurs simultaneously across multiple levels of visual processing and crucially depends on the spatial
configuration of the stimulus. Our results pose a challenge to models of crowding with an early single pooling stage and might be better explained by models which incorporate the possibility of multilevel crowding and account for complex target-flanker interactions.
Abstract—Tourist satisfaction is a main issue in the context of tourism because it involves the key measure in determining whether a tourist is satisfied with their visit and the effects it have on their surroundings. The problem of... more
Abstract—Tourist satisfaction is a main issue in the context of tourism because it involves the key measure in determining whether a tourist is satisfied with their visit and the effects it have on their surroundings. The problem of crowding have been seen as a major influence in few researches and are found to be affecting the overall satisfaction of visitors throughout their travel experiences. Few previously used theories in measuring tourist satisfaction will be used and adapted to create a new model of visitor satisfaction and perception of crowding. This model identifies tourist‟s satisfaction level based on destination image and attribute satisfaction. Secondly, this model can be used to compare between tourist‟s satisfaction level towards expectation and experience of tourists‟ recreational activities in Marine Parks. Third, this model is to be used to identify tourist acceptability of crowding in Marine Parks according to tourist groups. By these findings this model will be used to determine the tourist satisfaction based on the crowding perception in future studies.
Crowding is the inability to identify an object among flankers in the periphery. It is due to inappropriate incorporation of features from flanking objects in perception of the target. Crowding is characterized by measuring critical... more
Crowding is the inability to identify an object among
flankers in the periphery. It is due to inappropriate
incorporation of features from flanking objects in
perception of the target. Crowding is characterized by
measuring critical spacing, the minimum distance needed
between a target and flankers to allow recognition. The
existing Bouma law states that, at a given point and
direction in the visual field, critical spacing, measured
from the center of a target object to the center of a
similar flanking object, is the same for all objects (Pelli &
Tillman, 2008). Because flipping an object about its center
preserves its center-to-center spacing to other objects,
according to the Bouma law, crowding should be
unaffected. However, because crowding is a result of
feature combination, the location of features within an
object might matter. In a series of experiments, we find
that critical spacing is affected by the location of features
within the flanker. For some flankers, a flip greatly
reduces crowding even though it maintains target–flanker
spacing and similarity. Our results suggest that the
existing Bouma law applies to simple one-part objects,
such as a single roman letter or a Gabor patch. Many
objects consist of multiple parts; for example, a word is
composed of multiple letters that crowd each other. To
cope with such complex objects, we revise the Bouma law
to say that critical spacing is equal across parts, rather
than objects. This accounts for old and new findings.
flankers in the periphery. It is due to inappropriate
incorporation of features from flanking objects in
perception of the target. Crowding is characterized by
measuring critical spacing, the minimum distance needed
between a target and flankers to allow recognition. The
existing Bouma law states that, at a given point and
direction in the visual field, critical spacing, measured
from the center of a target object to the center of a
similar flanking object, is the same for all objects (Pelli &
Tillman, 2008). Because flipping an object about its center
preserves its center-to-center spacing to other objects,
according to the Bouma law, crowding should be
unaffected. However, because crowding is a result of
feature combination, the location of features within an
object might matter. In a series of experiments, we find
that critical spacing is affected by the location of features
within the flanker. For some flankers, a flip greatly
reduces crowding even though it maintains target–flanker
spacing and similarity. Our results suggest that the
existing Bouma law applies to simple one-part objects,
such as a single roman letter or a Gabor patch. Many
objects consist of multiple parts; for example, a word is
composed of multiple letters that crowd each other. To
cope with such complex objects, we revise the Bouma law
to say that critical spacing is equal across parts, rather
than objects. This accounts for old and new findings.
The visual system is constantly bombarded with dynamic input. In this context, the creation of enduring object representations presents a particular challenge. We used object-substitution masking (OSM) as a tool to probe these processes.... more
The visual system is constantly bombarded with dynamic input. In this context, the creation of enduring object representations presents a particular challenge. We used object-substitution masking (OSM) as a tool to probe these processes. In particular, we examined the effect of target-like stimulus repetitions on OSM. In visual crowding, the presentation of a physically identical stimulus to the target reduces crowding and improves target perception, whereas in spatial repetition blindness, the presentation of a stimulus that belongs to the same category (type) as the target impairs perception. Across two experiments, we found an interaction between spatial repetition blindness and OSM, such that repeating a same-type stimulus as the target increased masking magnitude relative to presentation of a different-type stimulus. These results are discussed in the context of the formation of object files. Moreover, the fact that the inducer only had to belong to the same Btype^ as the target in order to exacerbate masking, without necessarily being physically identical to the target, has important implications for our understanding of OSM per se. That is, our results show the target is processed to a categorical level in OSM despite effective masking and, strikingly, demonstrate that this category-level content directly influences whether or not the target is perceived, not just performance on another task (as in priming).
Effects of stimulus duration and inter-letter spacing were studied in a letter-in-string identification paradigm. Participants were shown strings of 5 random consonants (e.g., PGKDM) centered on fixation and were asked to identify the... more
Effects of stimulus duration and inter-letter spacing were studied in a letter-in-string identification paradigm. Participants were shown strings of 5 random consonants (e.g., PGKDM) centered on fixation and were asked to identify the letter that had appeared at a post-cued location. Stimulus duration was manipulated in Experiment 1 (13 ms – 91 ms), and inter-letter spacing manipulated in Experiment 2 (for a fixed stimulus duration of 26 ms). We contrasted performance to outer-letters (positions1 and 5) with non-central inner letters (positions 2 and 4), the first-letter (position 1) with the final letter (position 5), and the central-letter (position 3) with the other inner letters (positions 2 and 4). The outer-letter advantage and the first-letter advantage were present throughout the entire range of exposure durations, whereas the central-letter advantage increased with longer exposures. On the other hand, increased spacing reduced both the outer-letter advantage and the first-letter advantage, whereas it led to a greater central-letter advantage. Changes in acuity and crowding as a function of stimulus exposure and inter-letter spacing, can account for this pattern of results.
The effect of Crowding has long been recognised by cognitive psychologists engaged in examining the reading process. Yet it is not generally taken into account by most field linguists involved in the development of tone orthographies for... more
The effect of Crowding has long been recognised by cognitive psychologists engaged in examining the reading process. Yet it is not generally taken into account by most field linguists involved in the development of tone orthographies for emerging African languages. True, there is a general recognition that diacritic overload is unhelpful, but this has never been articulated with the help of the more precise terminology already on offer from the field of cognitive psychology. Using an experimental tone orthography developed for Kabiye (Gur, Togo) as an example, I postulate that an near-exhaustive representation of tone by means of accents will trigger Crowding. This is a hypothesis that has yet to be tested under clinical conditions. But the aim of this article is to call the phenomenon by its name for the first time and thereby stimulate further research. I also hope to demonstrate by means of this single example the gulf that exists between the cognitive psychology and linguistics. Once we recognise that the gulf exists, we can begin to build bridges.
Crowding is a phenomenon that characterizes normal periphery limiting letter identification when other letters surround the signal. We investigated the nature of the reading limitation of crowding by analyzing eye- movement patterns. The... more
Crowding is a phenomenon that characterizes normal periphery limiting letter identification when other letters surround the signal. We investigated the nature of the reading limitation of crowding by analyzing eye- movement patterns. The stimuli consisted of two items varying across trials for letter spacing (spaced, unspaced and increased size), lexicality (words or pseudowords), number of letters (4, 6, 8), and reading modality (oral and silent). In Experiments 1 and 2 (oral and silent reading, respectively) the results show that an increase in letter spacing induced an increase in the number of fixations and in gaze duration, but a reduction in the first fixation duration. More importantly, increasing letter size (Experiment 3) produced the same first fixation duration advantage as empty spacing, indicating that, as predicted by crowding, only center-to-center letter distance, and not spacing per se, matters. Moreover, when the letter size was enlarged the number of fixations did not increase as much as in the previous experiments, suggesting that this measure depends on visual acuity rather than on crowding. Finally, gaze duration, a measure of word recognition, did not change with the letter size enlargement. No qualitative differences were found between oral and silent reading experiments (1 and 2), indicating that the articulatory process did not influence the outcome. Finally, a facilitatory effect of lexicality was found in all conditions, indicating an interaction between perceptual and lexical processing. Overall, our results indicate that crowding influences normal word reading by means of an increase in first fixation duration, a measure of word encoding, which we interpret as a modulatory effect of attention on critical spacing.
We examined how crowding (the breakdown of object recognition in the periphery caused by interference from ‘‘clutter’’) depends on the global arrangement of target and distracting flanker elements. Specifically we probed orientation... more
We examined how crowding (the breakdown of object recognition in the periphery caused by interference from ‘‘clutter’’) depends on the global arrangement of target and distracting flanker elements. Specifically we probed orientation discrimination using a near-vertical target Gabor flanked by two vertical distractor Gabors (one above and one below the target). By applying variable (opposite-sign) horizontal offsets to the positions of the two flankers we arranged the elements so that on some trials they formed contours with the target and on others they did not. While the presence of flankers generally elevated orientation discrimination thresholds for the target we observe maximal crowding not when flanker and targets were co-aligned but when a small spatial offset was applied to flanker location, so that contours formed between flanker and targets only when the target orientation was cued. We also report that observers’ orientation judgements are biased, with target orientation appearing either attracted or repulsed by the
global/contour orientation. A second experiment reveals that the sign of this effect is dependent both on observer and on eccentricity. In general, the magnitude of repulsion is reduced with eccentricity but whether this becomes attraction (of element orientation to contour orientation) is dependent on observer.We note however that across observers and eccentricities, the magnitude of repulsion correlates positively with the amount of release from crowding observed with co-aligned targets and flankers, supporting the notion of fluctuating bias as the basis for elevated crowding within contours.
global/contour orientation. A second experiment reveals that the sign of this effect is dependent both on observer and on eccentricity. In general, the magnitude of repulsion is reduced with eccentricity but whether this becomes attraction (of element orientation to contour orientation) is dependent on observer.We note however that across observers and eccentricities, the magnitude of repulsion correlates positively with the amount of release from crowding observed with co-aligned targets and flankers, supporting the notion of fluctuating bias as the basis for elevated crowding within contours.
Research into Brain-Computer Interfaces (BCIs), which spell words using brain signals, has revealed that a desktop version of such a speller, the edges paradigm, offers several advantages: This edges paradigm outperforms the benchmark... more
Research into Brain-Computer Interfaces (BCIs), which spell words using brain signals, has revealed that a desktop version of such a speller, the edges paradigm, offers several advantages: This edges paradigm outperforms the benchmark row-column paradigm in terms of accuracy, bitrate, and user experience. It has remained unknown whether these advantages prevailed with a new version of the edges paradigm designed for a mobile device. This paper investigated and evaluated in a rolling wheelchair a mobile BCI, which implemented the edges paradigm on small displays with which visual crowding tends to occur. How the mobile edge paradigm outperforms the mobile row-column paradigm has implications for understanding how principles of visual neurocognition affect BCI speller use in a mobile context. This investigation revealed that all the overall advantages of the edges paradigm over the row-column paradigm prevailed in this setting. However, the reduction in adjacent errors for the edges paradigm was unprecedentedly limited to horizontal adjacent errors. The interpretation offered is that dimensional constraints of visual interface design on a smartphone thus affected the neurocognitive processes of crowding.
- by Qasem Obeidat and +1
- •
- Non-Invasive BCI, P300, BCI, Visual Crowding
Can people learn complex information without conscious awareness? Implicit learning—learning without awareness of what has been learned—has been the focus of intense investigation over the last 50 years. However, it remains controversial... more
Can people learn complex information without conscious awareness? Implicit learning—learning without awareness of what has been learned—has been the focus of intense investigation over the last 50 years. However, it remains controversial whether complex knowledge can be learned implicitly. In the research reported here, we addressed this challenge by asking participants to differentiate between sequences of symbols they could not perceive consciously. Using an operant-conditioning task, we showed that participants learned to associate distinct sequences of crowded (nondiscriminable) symbols with their respective monetary outcomes (reward or punishment). Overall, our study demonstrates that sensitivity to sequential regularities can arise through the nonconscious temporal integration of perceptual information.
Crowding is a phenomenon that characterizes normal periphery limiting letter identification when other letters surround the signal. We investigated the nature of the reading limitation of crowding by analyzing eye-movement patterns. The... more
Crowding is a phenomenon that characterizes normal periphery limiting letter identification when other letters surround the signal. We investigated the nature of the reading limitation of crowding by analyzing eye-movement patterns. The stimuli consisted of two items varying across trials for letter spacing (spaced, unspaced and increased size), lexicality (words or pseudowords), number of letters (4, 6, 8), and reading modality (oral and silent). In Experiments 1 and 2 (oral and silent reading, respectively) the results show that an increase in letter spacing induced an increase in the number of fixations and in gaze duration, but a reduction in the first fixation duration. More importantly, increasing letter size (Experiment 3) produced the same first fixation duration advantage as empty spacing, indicating that, as predicted by crowding, only center-to-center letter distance, and not spacing per se, matters. Moreover, when the letter size was enlarged the number of fixations did not increase as much as in the previous experiments, suggesting that this measure depends on visual acuity rather than on crowding. Finally, gaze duration, a measure of word recognition, did not change with the letter size enlargement. No qualitative differences were found between oral and silent reading experiments (1 and 2), indicating that the articulatory process did not influence the outcome. Finally, a facilitatory effect of lexicality was found in all conditions, indicating an interaction between perceptual and lexical processing. Overall, our results indicate that crowding influences normal word reading by means of an increase in first fixation duration, a measure of word encoding, which we interpret as a modulatory effect of attention on critical spacing.
Diacritics convey vowel sounds in Arabic, allowing accurate word pronunciation. Mostly, modern Arabic is printed nondiacritized. Otherwise, diacritics appear either only on homographic words when not disambiguated by surrounding text or... more
Diacritics convey vowel sounds in Arabic, allowing accurate word pronunciation. Mostly, modern Arabic is printed nondiacritized. Otherwise, diacritics appear either only on homographic words when not disambiguated by surrounding text or on all words as in religious or educational texts. In an eye-tracking experiment, we examined sentence processing in the absence of diacritics and when diacritics were presented in either modes. Heterophonic homographic target verbs that have different pronunciations in active and passive (e.g., /daraba/, hit; /doriba/, was hit) were embedded in temporarily ambiguous sentences in which in the absence of diacritics, readers cannot be certain whether the verb was active or passive. Passive sentences were disambiguated by an extra word (e.g., /bijad/, by the hand of). Our results show that readers benefitted from the disambiguating diacritics when present only on the homographic verb. When disambiguating diacritics were absent, Arabic readers followed t...
Purpose To determine whether people with central field loss (CFL) from macular degeneration have improved ability to recognize a particularly difficult spatial configuration embedded in noise, the peripherally-viewed ‘ladder contour’.... more
Purpose
To determine whether people with central field loss (CFL) from macular degeneration have improved ability to recognize a particularly difficult spatial configuration embedded in noise, the peripherally-viewed ‘ladder contour’. The visibility of these configuration has been linked to general contour integration ability and crowding limitations in peripheral vision.
Methods
We used a trial-based yes-no task. CFL patients and normally-sighted controls performed the task, looking for ladder contours embedded in a field of randomly oriented Gabor patches, at a range of stimulus presentation times (varying stimulus difficulty). Viewing eccentricity in CFL patients was set by their preferred retinal loci (PRLs) and matched artificially in the control group. The contours were presented so as to be tangent to the CFL region, given a patient’s PRL location.
Results
CFL and normally-sighted groups performed similarly on the task. The only significant determinant of performance was the viewing eccentricity.
Conclusions
CFL patients do not seem to develop any improved ability to recognize ladder contours with their parafoveal retina, which suggests that there is no underlying improvement in contour integration or reduction in crowding limitations in the region of the PRL despite extended daily use.
To determine whether people with central field loss (CFL) from macular degeneration have improved ability to recognize a particularly difficult spatial configuration embedded in noise, the peripherally-viewed ‘ladder contour’. The visibility of these configuration has been linked to general contour integration ability and crowding limitations in peripheral vision.
Methods
We used a trial-based yes-no task. CFL patients and normally-sighted controls performed the task, looking for ladder contours embedded in a field of randomly oriented Gabor patches, at a range of stimulus presentation times (varying stimulus difficulty). Viewing eccentricity in CFL patients was set by their preferred retinal loci (PRLs) and matched artificially in the control group. The contours were presented so as to be tangent to the CFL region, given a patient’s PRL location.
Results
CFL and normally-sighted groups performed similarly on the task. The only significant determinant of performance was the viewing eccentricity.
Conclusions
CFL patients do not seem to develop any improved ability to recognize ladder contours with their parafoveal retina, which suggests that there is no underlying improvement in contour integration or reduction in crowding limitations in the region of the PRL despite extended daily use.
In Object Substitution Masking (OSM) a surrounding mask (typically comprising of four dots) onsets with a target but lingers after offset; under such conditions the ability to perceive the target can be significantly reduced. OSM was... more
In Object Substitution Masking (OSM) a surrounding mask (typically comprising of four dots) onsets with a target but lingers after offset; under such conditions the ability to perceive the target can be significantly reduced. OSM was originally claimed to occur only when a target was not the focus of attention, for instance when embedded in an array of distractors (Di Lollo, et al., 2000). It was argued that the distractors influenced the time taken for focal attention to reach the target. Some recent work, however, failed to find any such distractor influence: the effect of mask duration being independent of set size when steps were taken to avoid ceiling effects in the smallest set size condition (Argyropoulos et al., 2013; Filmer et al., 2014a). In three experiments we repeatedly found that set size manipulations can interact with mask duration (where neither ceiling nor floor effects are evident), the effect of the mask on target perceptibility being amplified according to the number of distractor items. However, a further experiment (Exp. 4) showed that crowding by nearby distractors was actually responsible for this ‘set size’ effect. When decoupled from crowding, set size alone did not interact with masking, though it did influence overall accuracy. Thus the presence of distractors does influence OSM but not in the way originally assumed by Di Lollo and colleagues in their model (Di Lollo et al., 2000). The crowding × OSM interaction suggests that the two phenomena involve partly overlapping mechanisms.
Related Topics