Olfaction
Olfaction
Olfaction
chemicals.[3] Olfaction, along with taste, is a form of chemoreception. The chemicals themselves that
activate the olfactory system, in general at very low concentrations, are called odorants. Although taste and
smell are separate sensory systems in land animals, water-dwelling organisms often have one chemical
sense.[4]
Volatile small molecule odorants, non-volatile proteins, and non-volatile hydrocarbons may all produce
olfactory sensations. Some animal species are able to smell carbon dioxide in minute concentrations.[5]
Contents
1 Study of olfaction
2 Main olfactory system
2.1 Receptor neuron
2.2 Olfactory bulb projections
3 Accessory olfactory system
4 Human olfactory system
5 Olfactory coding and perception
6 Genetics of olfaction
7 Interactions of olfaction with other senses
7.1 Olfaction and taste
7.2 Olfaction and audition
8 Disorders of olfaction
9 Quantifying olfaction in industry
10 Olfaction in plants and animals
10.1 Insect olfactory system
11 See also
12 References
13 Further reading
14 External links
Study of olfaction
Early scientific study of olfaction includes the extensive doctoral
dissertation of Eleanor Gamble, published in 1898, which compared
olfactory to other stimulus modalities, and implied that smell had a
lower intensity discrimination.[6] As the Epicurean and atomistic
Roman philosopher Lucretius (1st Century BCE) speculated,
different odors are attributed to different shapes and sizes of "atoms"
(odor molecules in the modern understanding) that stimulate the
olfactory organ [1]
(http://www.academia.edu/321004/Lucretius_the_Biochemistry_of_Olfaction_and_Scientific_Discovery). A
modern demonstration of that theory was the cloning of olfactory receptor proteins by Linda B. Buck and
Richard Axel (who were awarded the Nobel Prize in 2004), and subsequent pairing of odor molecules to
specific receptor proteins. Each odor receptor molecule recognizes only a particular molecular feature or
class of odor molecules. Mammals have about a thousand genes that code for odor reception.[7] Of the genes
that code for odor receptors, only a portion are functional. Humans have far fewer active odor receptor genes
than other primates and other mammals.[8] In mammals, each olfactory receptor neuron expresses only one
functional odor receptor.[9] Odor receptor nerve cells function like a key-lock system: If the airborne
molecules of a certain chemical can fit into the lock, the nerve cell will respond. There are, at present, a
number of competing theories regarding the mechanism of odor coding and perception. According to the
shape theory, each receptor detects a feature of the odor molecule. Weak-shape theory, known as odotope
theory, suggests that different receptors detect only small pieces of molecules, and these minimal inputs are
combined to form a larger olfactory perception (similar to the way visual perception is built up of smaller,
information-poor sensations, combined and refined to create a detailed overall perception). An alternative
theory, the vibration theory proposed by Luca Turin,[10][11] posits that odor receptors detect the frequencies
of vibrations of odor molecules in the infrared range by quantum tunnelling. However, the behavioral
predictions of this theory have been called into question.[12] There is no theory yet that explains olfactory
perception completely.
Molecules of odorants passing through the superior nasal concha of the nasal passages dissolve in the mucus
lining the superior portion of the cavity and are detected by olfactory receptors on the dendrites of the
olfactory sensory neurons. This may occur by diffusion or by the binding of the odorant to odorant binding
proteins. The mucus overlying the epithelium contains mucopolysaccharides, salts, enzymes, and antibodies
(these are highly important, as the olfactory neurons provide a direct passage for infection to pass to the
brain). This mucus acts as a solvent for odor molecules, flows constantly and is replaced approximately
every 10 minutes.
In insects smells are sensed by olfactory sensory neurons in the chemosensory sensilla, which are present in
insect antenna, palps and tarsa, but also on other parts of the insect body. Odorants penetrate into the cuticle
pores of chemosensory sensilla and get in contact with insect odorant binding proteins (OBPs) or
Chemosensory proteins (CSPs), before activating the sensory neurons.
Receptor neuron
The binding of the ligand (odor molecule or odorant) to the receptor leads to an action potential in the
receptor neuron, via a second messenger pathway, depending on the organism. In mammals, the odorants
stimulate adenylate cyclase to synthesize cAMP via a G protein called Golf. cAMP, which is the second
messenger here, opens a cyclic nucleotide-gated ion channel (CNG), producing an influx of cations (largely
Ca2+ with some Na+) into the cell, slightly depolarising it. The Ca2+ in turn opens a Ca2+-activated chloride
channel, leading to efflux of Cl, further depolarizing the cell and triggering an action potential. Ca2+ is then
extruded through a sodium-calcium exchanger. A calcium-calmodulin complex also acts to inhibit the
binding of cAMP to the cAMP-dependent channel, thus contributing to olfactory adaptation. This
mechanism of transduction is somewhat unique, in that cAMP works by directly binding to the ion channel
rather than through activation of protein kinase A. It is similar to the transduction mechanism for
photoreceptors, in which the second messenger cGMP works by directly binding to ion channels, suggesting
that maybe one of these receptors was evolutionarily adapted into the other. There are also considerable
similarities in the immediate processing of stimuli by lateral inhibition. Averaged activity of the receptor
neurons can be measured in several ways. In vertebrates, responses to an odor can be measured by an
electro-olfactogram or through calcium imaging of receptor neuron terminals in the olfactory bulb. In
insects, one can perform electroantenogram or also calcium imaging within the olfactory bulb.
function as pheromones, although pheromones can also be detected by the main olfactory system. In the
accessory olfactory system, stimuli are detected by the vomeronasal organ, located in the vomer, between
the nose and the mouth. Snakes use it to smell prey, sticking their tongue out and touching it to the organ.
Some mammals make a facial expression called flehmen to direct stimuli to this organ.
The sensory receptors of the accessory olfactory system are located in the vomeronasal organ. As in the
main olfactory system, the axons of these sensory neurons project from the vomeronasal organ to the
accessory olfactory bulb, which in the mouse is located on the dorsal-posterior portion of the main olfactory
bulb. Unlike in the main olfactory system, the axons that leave the accessory olfactory bulb do not project to
the brain's cortex but rather to targets in the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, and from there
to the hypothalamus, where they may influence aggressive and mating behavior.
It is a general idea that the layout of brain structures corresponds to physical features of stimuli (called
topographic coding), and similar analogies have been made in olfaction with concepts such as a layout
corresponding to chemical features (called chemotopy) or perceptual features.[26] While chemotopy is a
highly controversial concept,[27] there is more evidence for perceptual information implemented in the
spatial dimensions of olfactory networks.[26]
Although conventional wisdom and lay literature, based on impressionistic findings in the 1920s, have long
presented human oflaction as capable of distinguishing between roughly 10,000 unique odors, recent
research has suggested that the average individual is capable of distinguishing between over one trillion
unique odors.[28] Researchers in the most recent study noted that this estimate is "conservative" and that
some subjects of their researchtests involved testing the psychophysical responses to combinations of over
128 unique odor molecules with combinations composed of up to 30 different component moleculesmight
be capable of deciphering between a thousand trillion odorants while they estimated their worst performer
could still distinguish between 80 million scents.[29] Authors of the study concluded, "This is far more than
previous estimates of distinguishable olfactory stimuli. It demonstrates that the human olfactory system,
with its hundreds of different olfactory receptors, far out performs the other senses in the number of
physically different stimuli it can discriminate."[30] However, it has also been noted by the authors that the
ability to distinguish between smells is not analogous to being able to consistently identify them and that
subjects were not typically capable of identifying individual odor stimulants from within the odors the
researchers had composed from multiple odor molecules. In November 2014, the study was strongly
criticized by Caltech scientist Markus Meister who wrote that the study's "extravagant claims are based on
errors of mathematical logic".[31]
Genetics of olfaction
Different people smell different odors and most of these differences are caused by genetic differences.[32]
Although odorant receptor genes make up one of the largest genes families in the human genome, only a
handful of genes have been linked conclusively to particular smells. For instance, the odorant receptor
OR5A1 and its genetic variants (alleles) are responsible for our ability (or failure) to smell -ionone, a key
aroma in foods and beverages.[33] Similarly, the odorant receptor OR2J3 is associated with the ability to
detect the "grassy" smelling odor, cis-3-hexen-1-ol.[34] The preference (or dislike) of cilantro (coriander) has
been linked to the olfactory receptor OR6A2.[35]
Olfaction and sound information has been shown to converge in the olfactory tubercles of rodents.[38] This
neural convergence is proposed to give rise to a percept termed smound.[39] Whereas a flavor results from
interactions between smell and taste, a smound may result from interactions between smell and sound.
Disorders of olfaction
The following are disorders of olfaction:[40]
Anosmia inability to smell
Dysosmia things smell different from memory or expectation
Hyperosmia an abnormally acute sense of smell.
Hyposmia decreased ability to smell
Olfactory Reference Syndrome psychological disorder which causes the patient to imagine he or she
has strong body odor
Parosmia things smell worse than they should[41]
Phantosmia "hallucinated smell," often unpleasant in nature
Many air management districts in the US have numerical standards of acceptability for the intensity of odor
that is allowed to cross into a residential property. For example, the Bay Area Air Quality Management
District has applied its standard in regulating numerous industries, landfills, and sewage treatment plants.
Example applications this district has engaged are the San Mateo, California wastewater treatment plant; the
Shoreline Amphitheatre in Mountain View, California; and the IT Corporation waste ponds, Martinez,
California.
The importance and sensitivity of smell varies among different organisms; most mammals have a good sense
of smell, whereas most birds do not, except the tubenoses (e.g., petrels and albatrosses), certain species of
vultures and the kiwis. Among mammals, it is well-developed in the carnivores and ungulates, which must
always be aware of each other, and in those that smell for their food, like moles. Having a strong sense of
smell is referred to as macrosmatic.
Figures suggesting greater or lesser sensitivity in various species reflect experimental findings from the
reactions of animals exposed to aromas in known extreme dilutions. These are, therefore, based on
perceptions by these animals, rather than mere nasal function. That is, the brain's smell-recognizing centers
must react to the stimulus detected, for the animal to show a response to the smell in question. It is estimated
that dogs in general have an olfactory sense approximately a hundred thousand to a million times more acute
than a human's. This does not mean they are overwhelmed by smells our noses can detect; rather, it means
they can discern a molecular presence when it is in much greater dilution in the carrier, air.
Scenthounds as a group can smell one- to ten-million times more acutely than a human, and Bloodhounds,
which have the keenest sense of smell of any dogs, have noses ten- to one-hundred-million times more
sensitive than a human's. They were bred for the specific purpose of tracking humans, and can detect a scent
trail a few days old. The second-most-sensitive nose is possessed by the Basset Hound, which was bred to
track and hunt rabbits and other small animals.
Bears, such as the Silvertip Grizzly found in parts of North America, have a sense of smell seven times
stronger than that of the bloodhound, essential for locating food underground. Using their elongated claws,
bears dig deep trenches in search of burrowing animals and nests as well as roots, bulbs, and insects. Bears
can detect the scent of food from up to 18 miles away; because of their immense size, they often scavenge
new kills, driving away the predators (including packs of wolves and human hunters) in the process.
The sense of smell is less-developed in the catarrhine primates, and nonexistent in cetaceans, which
compensate with a well-developed sense of taste. In some strepsirrhines, such as the red-bellied lemur, scent
glands occur atop the head. In many species, olfaction is highly tuned to pheromones; a male silkworm
moth, for example, can sense a single molecule of bombykol.
Fish too have a well-developed sense of smell, even though they inhabit an aquatic environment. Salmon
utilize their sense of smell to identify and return to their home stream waters. Catfish use their sense of smell
to identify other individual catfish and to maintain a social hierarchy. Many fishes use the sense of smell to
identify mating partners or to alert to the presence of food.
See also
Chemesthesis
Electronic nose
Machine olfaction
Nasal administration olfactory transfer
Odor
Olfactometer
Olfactory ensheathing glia
Olfactory fatigue
Phantosmia
Scent transfer unit
Vibration Theory of Olfaction
Evolution of olfaction
References
1. Wiktionary: http://en.wiktionary.org/wiki/olfactics
2. Daniel Schacter, Daniel Gilbert, Daniel Wegner (2011). "Sensation and Perception". Psychology. Worth Publishers.
pp. 166171. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2.
3. Hussain A, Saraiva LR, Korsching SI (2009). "Positive Darwinian selection and the birth of an olfactory receptor
clade in teleosts" (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2657432). PNAS 106 (11): 43138.
doi:10.1073/pnas.0803229106 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1073%2Fpnas.0803229106). PMC 2657432
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2657432). PMID 19237578
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19237578).
4. Boroditsky, Lera (1999). "Taste, Smell, and Touch: Lecture Notes" (http://www.saylor.org/site/wpcontent/uploads/2011/08/Psych306-Reading-6.1.pdf) (PDF). p. 1.
5. "Better Smelling Through Genetics: Mammalian Odor Perception"
(http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2590501/). Ncbi.nlm.nih.gov. 2012-10-19. Retrieved 2012-12-30.
6. G. A. Kimble; K. Schlesinger (1985). Topics in the History of Psychology, Volume 1. L. Erlbaum Associates.
7. Buck L, Axel R (April 1991). "A novel multigene family may encode odorant receptors: a molecular basis for odor
recognition". Cell 65 (1): 17587. doi:10.1016/0092-8674(91)90418-X (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2F00928674%2891%2990418-X). PMID 1840504 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1840504).
8. Gilad Y, Man O, Pbo S, Lancet D (March 2003). "Human specific loss of olfactory receptor genes"
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC152291). PNAS 100 (6): 33247. doi:10.1073/pnas.0535697100
(https://dx.doi.org/10.1073%2Fpnas.0535697100). PMC 152291
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC152291). PMID 12612342
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12612342).
9. Pinel, John P.J. (2006) Biopsychology. Pearson Education Inc. ISBN 0-205-42651-4 (page 178)
10. Turin L (December 1996). "A spectroscopic mechanism for primary olfactory reception"
(http://chemse.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=8985605). Chemical senses 21 (6): 77391.
doi:10.1093/chemse/21.6.773 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1093%2Fchemse%2F21.6.773). PMID 8985605
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8985605).
11. Turin L (June 2002). "A method for the calculation of odor character from molecular structure". Journal of
Theoretical Biology 216 (3): 36785. doi:10.1006/jtbi.2001.2504 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1006%2Fjtbi.2001.2504).
PMID 12183125 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12183125).
12. Keller A, Vosshall LB (April 2004). "A psychophysical test of the vibration theory of olfaction". Nature
Neuroscience 7 (4): 3378. doi:10.1038/nn1215 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1038%2Fnn1215). PMID 15034588
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15034588). See also the editorial on p. 315.
13. Doty, R. L. (2001). Olfaction. 425. Retrieved from
http://journals2.scholarsportal.info.myaccess.library.utoronto.ca/tmp/9139085232678633444.pdf
14. Bear, Connors and Paradiso, Mark, Barry and Michael (2007). Neuroscience: Exploring the Brain. USA: Lippincott
Williams & Wilkins. pp. 265275.
15. Morris, H., & Schaeffer, J. P. (1953). The Nervous system-The Brain or Encephalon. Human anatomy; a complete
systematic treatise. (11th ed., pp.1218-1219). New York: Blakiston.
16. Margot C (July 2009). "A noseful of objects". Nat. Neurosci. 12 (7): 8134. doi:10.1038/nn0709-813
(https://dx.doi.org/10.1038%2Fnn0709-813). PMID 19554043 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19554043).
17. Zhou W, Chen D (2009). "Binaral rivalry between the nostrils and in the cortex"
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2901510). Curr Biol 19 (18): 15615.
doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.052 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.cub.2009.07.052). PMC 2901510
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2901510). PMID 19699095
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19699095).
18. Navarrete-Palacios E, Hudson R, Reyes-Guerrero G, Guevara-Guzmn R (July 2003). "Lower olfactory threshold
during the ovulatory phase of the menstrual cycle". Biological Psychology 63 (3): 26979. doi:10.1016/S03010511(03)00076-0 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2FS0301-0511%2803%2900076-0). PMID 12853171
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12853171).
19. Boehm T, Zufall F (February 2006). "MHC peptides and the sensory evaluation of genotype". Trends in
neurosciences 29 (2): 1007. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2005.11.006 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.tins.2005.11.006).
PMID 16337283 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16337283).
20. Santos PS, Schinemann JA, Gabardo J, Bicalho Mda G (April 2005). "New evidence that the MHC influences odor
perception in humans: a study with 58 Southern Brazilian students". Hormones and behavior 47 (4): 3848.
doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.11.005 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.yhbeh.2004.11.005). PMID 15777804
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15777804).
21. Porter RH, Cernoch JM, Balogh RD (1985). "Odor signatures and kin recognition". Physiol Behav 34 (3): 4458.
doi:10.1016/0031-9384(85)90210-0 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2F0031-9384%2885%2990210-0). PMID 4011726
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/4011726).
22. Weisfeld GE, Czilli T, Phillips KA, Gall JA, Lichtman CM (July 2003). "Possible olfaction-based mechanisms in
human kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance.". Journal of experimental child psychology 85 (3): 27995.
doi:10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00061-4 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2FS0022-0965%2803%2900061-4).
PMID 12810039 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12810039).
23. Lundstrm JN, Boyle JA, Zatorre RJ, Jones-Gotman M (August 2009). "The neuronal substrates of human olfactory
based kin recognition". Human Brain Mapping 30 (8): 257180. doi:10.1002/hbm.20686
(https://dx.doi.org/10.1002%2Fhbm.20686). PMID 19067327 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19067327).
24. Wilson DA (June 2001). "Receptive fields in the rat piriform cortex.". Chemical senses 26 (5): 57784.
doi:10.1093/chemse/26.5.577 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1093%2Fchemse%2F26.5.577). PMID 11418503
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11418503).
25. Leon M, Johnson BA (2003). "Olfactory coding in the mammalian olfactory bulb". Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 42
(1): 2332. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(03)00142-5 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2FS0165-0173%2803%2900142-5).
PMID 12668289 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12668289).
26. Auffarth, B. (2013). "Understanding smell -- the olfactory stimulus problem."
(http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0149763413001644). Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 37:
16671679. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.009 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.neubiorev.2013.06.009).
27. Soucy, Edward R. et al. (2009). "Precision and diversity in an odor map on the olfactory bulb.". Nature Neuroscience
12.2: 210220. doi:10.1038/nn.2262 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1038%2Fnn.2262).
28. Helen Briggs (20 March 2014). "Nose can detect one trillion odours" (http://www.bbc.com/news/health-26638085).
BBC News. pp. 23 March 2014.
29. Sarah C.P. Williams (20 March 2014). "Human Nose Can Detect a Trillion Smells"
(http://news.sciencemag.org/biology/2014/03/human-nose-can-detect-trillion-smells). Science Now/AAAS News.
Retrieved 23 March 2014.
30. Bushdid, C.; Magnasco, M.O.; Vosshall, L.B.; Keller, A. (21 March 2014). "Humans Can Discriminate More than 1
million Olfactory Stimuli" (http://www.sciencemag.org/content/343/6177/1370). Science 343 (6177): 13701372.
doi:10.1126/science.1249168 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1126%2Fscience.1249168). PMID 24653035
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24653035).
31. Meister, M. (2014). "Can Humans Really Discriminate 1 Trillion Odors?" (http://arxiv.org/abs/1411.0165). arXiv.
arXiv:1411.0165.
32. Howgego, J. (2013) Sense for scents traced down to genes (http://www.nature.com/news/sense-for-scents-traceddown-to-genes-1.13493). Nature News, 1 Aug 2013
33. Jaeger SR, McRae JF, Bava CM, Beresford MK, Hunter D, Jia Y, Chheang SL, Jin D, Peng M, Gamble JC, Atkinson
KR, Axten LG, Paisley AG, Tooman L, Pineau B, Rouse SA, Newcomb RD (2013). "A Mendelian Trait for
Olfactory Sensitivity Affects Odor Experience and Food Selection". Current Biology 23 (16): 16011605.
doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.030 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.cub.2013.07.030). PMID 23910657
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23910657).
34. McRae JF, Mainland JD, Jaeger SR, Adipietro KA, Matsunami H, Newcomb RD (2012). "Genetic Variation in the
Odorant Receptor OR2J3 is Associated with the Ability to Detect the "Grassy" Smelling Odor, cis-3-hexen-1-ol"
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3408771). Chemical Senses 37 (7): 585593.
doi:10.1093/chemse/bjs049 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1093%2Fchemse%2Fbjs049). PMC 3408771
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3408771). PMID 22714804
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22714804).
35. Callaway, E. (2012) taste of coriander linked to genetic variants (http://www.nature.com/news/soapy-taste-ofcoriander-linked-to-genetic-variants-1.11398Soapy) Nature News, 12 Sep 2012
36. Masaoka Y, Satoh H, Akai L, Homma I (2010). "Expiration: The moment we experience retronasal olfaction in
flavor". Neurosci Lett. 473 (2): 9296. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.02.024
(https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.neulet.2010.02.024). PMID 20171264
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20171264).
37. Annual Review of Psychology (February 2001), 52 (1), pg. 423-452,Richard L. Doty
38. Wesson DW, Wilson DA (2010). "Smelling Sounds: Olfactory-auditory convergence in the olfactory tubercle"
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2846283). J Neurosci 30 (8): 30131021.
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6003-09.2010 (https://dx.doi.org/10.1523%2FJNEUROSCI.6003-09.2010).
PMC 2846283 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2846283). PMID 20181598
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20181598).
39. Peeples, Lynne. "Scientific American, ''Making scents of sounds'' Lynne Peeples, 23 February 2010, accessed 25
February 2010" (http://www.scientificamerican.com/article.cfm?id=making-scents-of-sounds-n).
Scientificamerican.com. Retrieved 2012-12-30.
40. Hirsch, Alan R. (2003) Life's a Smelling Success
41. Gilbert, Avery (2008). "Freaks, Geeks, and Prodigies". What the Nose Knows. Crown Publishers. p. 52. ISBN 978-14000-8234-6.
42. Fountain, Henry (3 October 2006). "This Plant Has the Sense of Smell (Loves Tomatoes, Hates Wheat)"
(http://www.nytimes.com/2006/10/03/science/03find.html). The New York Times.
Further reading
Gordon M.s Shepherd Neurogastronomy: How the Brain Creates Flavor and Why It Matters New
York : Columbia University Press, 2012 ISBN 978-0-231-15910-4
External links
Mammalian Odor Perception through Genetics
Wikimedia Commons has
(http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2590501/)
media related to Olfactory
Research on Interesting Questions About Smells
system.
(http://andreaskeller.squarespace.com/olfaction-smell-odor/)
Insect Olfaction of Plant Odour (http://www.olfacts.nl)
Smells and Odours - How Smell Works at thenakedscientists.com
(http://www.thenakedscientists.com/HTML/Columnists/peterbrennancolumn.htm)
Olfaction at cf.ac.uk (http://www.cf.ac.uk/biosi/staff/jacob/teaching/sensory/olfact1.html)
Structure-odor relations: a modern perspective at flexitral.com
(http://www.flexitral.com/research/review_final.pdf) (PDF)
Chirality & Odour Perception at leffingwell.com (http://www.leffingwell.com/chirality/chirality.htm)
ScienceDaily Artille 08/03/2006, Quick -- What's That Smell? Time Needed To Identify Odors
Reveals Much About Olfaction at sciencedaily.com
(http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2006/08/060803091235.htm)
Scents and Emotions Linked by Learning, Brown Study Shows at brown.edu.com
(http://www.brown.edu/Administration/News_Bureau/2004-05/04-069.html)
Sense of Smell Institute at senseofsmell.org (http://www.senseofsmell.org/feature/odor/index.php).
Research arm of international fragrance industry's The Fragrance Foundation
Olfactory Systems Laboratory at Boston University (http://people.bu.edu/dmattw/)