Benet MartinezHPSP2012
Benet MartinezHPSP2012
Benet MartinezHPSP2012
Multiculturalism:
Verónica Benet-Martínez
FINAL VERSION
March 2011
To appear in K. Deaux & M. Snyder (Eds.), Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology.
Oxford University Press.
Multiculturalism 2
Abstract
This chapter discusses the psychological and societal processes involved in the phenomenon of
theories stemming from cultural, personality, and social psychology. The chapter includes
sections devoted to defining multiculturalism at the individual, group, and societal level,
discussing the links between acculturation and multiculturalism, how to best operationalize and
measure multicultural identity, the issue of individual differences in multicultural identity, and
the possible psychological and societal benefits of multiculturalism. The chapter concludes with
multiculturalism.
Multiculturalism is a fact of life for many people. The global increase in intercultural
contact due to factors such as immigration, speed of travel and communication, and international
influence how people see themselves and others, and how they organize the world around them.
Take for instance US president Barack Hussein Obama. Obama straddles countries and cultures.
The son of a Kenyan and an American, he studied the Quran in his youth and as an adult he was
baptized. His multicultural background enables him to speak the language of a globalized world,
in which people of diverse origins encounter each other and negotiate common meaning across
shrinking cultural divides (Saleh, 2009). Obama exemplifies the word ―multiculturalism‖ as a
biracial individual from a multicultural family who has lived in various countries; also, several of
his key advisors have also lived outside the US (Bartholet & Stone, 2009), and almost half of his
cabinet are racial or ethnic minorities (Wolf, 2009). In fact, in his inaugural speech, Obama
stated that multiculturalism is a national strength (Obama, 2009), and since then, he has
deliberately set out to select a diverse cabinet, based on the premise that multicultural individuals
have insights, skills, and unique psychological experiences that contribute to society (Nguyen &
Benet-Martínez, 2010).
psychology (e.g., Hermans & Kempen, 1998; LaFromboise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993), yet the
phenomenon has been investigated empirically only in the last decade or so. However, the study
of multiculturalism has exciting and transformative implications for social and personality
psychology, as the issue of how individuals develop a sense of national, cultural, ethnic, and
mixing, and integration (Baumeister, 1986; Deaux, 2006; Phinney, 1999). Furthermore, the
individual and contextual factors that influence how an individual makes sense of his/her
multicultural experiences provide personality psychologists with another window through which
to study individual differences in identity and self-concept. In fact, as eloquently said by Phinney
(1999): ―… increasing numbers of people find that the conflicts are not between different groups
but between different cultural values, attitudes, and expectations within themselves‖ (p. 27;
italics added).
The study of multiculturalism also affords unique methodological tools to social and
personality psychologists. By virtue of having two or more cultures that can be independently
manipulated, multicultural individuals give researchers a quasi-experimental design ideal for the
study of how culture affects behavior (Hong, Morris, Chiu, & Benet-Martínez, 2000). In
multicultural individuals without the counfounding effects (i.e., differences in SES, translation
Martínez, Potter, & Pennebaker, 2006; Sanchez-Burks, Lee, Choi, Nisbett, Zhao, & Koo, 2003).
With the increase of cultural diversity in academic, political, and media spheres,
empirical research on multiculturalism has finally begun to appear in social and personality
psychology journals. The main goal of this chapter is to review and integrate this research and
propose an agenda for future studies. However, because multiculturalism issues are very new to
empirical social and personality psychology, this chapter also includes sections devoted to
defining the constructs of multiculturalism and multicultural identity, summarizing the relevant
work from the field of acculturation studies, and discussing how to best operationalize and
measure multiculturalism (see also Hong, Wan, No, & Chiu, 2007).
Multiculturalism 5
conceptualizations (e.g., cultural identifications or orientations). Broadly speaking, those who are
mixed-race and mixed-ethnic, those who have lived in more than one country (such as
expatriates, international students, immigrants, refugees, and sojourners), those reared with at
least one other culture in addition to the dominant mainstream culture (such as children of
immigrants or colonized people), and those in inter-cultural relationships may all be considered
multicultural (Berry, 2003; Padilla, 2006).1 In the US alone, multicultural individuals may
include the 13% who are foreign-born, the 34% who are non-White, and the 20% who speak a
language other than English at home (U.S. Census Bureau, 2006). High numbers of multicultural
individuals (10% of the population by some estimates) can also be found in other nations where
migration is strong (e.g., Canada, Australia, Western Europe, Singapore) or where there is a
speaking, multiculturalism can be defined as the experience of having been exposed to and
having internalized two or more cultures (Hong et al., 2000; Nguyen & Benet-Martínez, 2007).2
More specifically, multicultural individuals are those who display multicultural competence –i.e.,
display cultural behaviors such as language use, choice of friends, media preferences, value
systems, etc. that are representative of two or more cultures (LaFromboise et al., 1993).
Multicultural individuals are also those whose self-label (e.g., ―I am multicultural‖) or group
reflects their cultural pluralism. Relatedly, multicultural identity is the condition of having
Multiculturalism 6
attachments with and loyalties toward these different cultures (Benet-Martínez & Haritatos,
2005).
Note then that multicultural identity is only one component (although perhaps the most
important one) of the more complex and multidimensional notion of multiculturalism. That is, an
individual who has been exposed to and has learned more than one culture is a multicultural
person, but only when this individual expresses an attachment with these cultures we can say that
the individual has a multicultural identity. This is because acquisition of knowledge from a new
culture does not always produce identification with that culture (Hong et al., 2007). Thus
multicultural identity involves a significant degree of identification with more than one culture;
however, it does not presuppose similar degrees of identification with all the internalized
cultures. Lastly, having a multicultural identity involves following the norms of more than one
culture, or at least being cognizant of them (see later section on variations in multicultural
identity); this premise is supported by social identity research showing that individuals who
identify strongly (vs. weakly) with a culture are more likely to follow that culture‘s norms
(Jetten, Postmes, & Mcauliffe, 2002), and that for these individuals cultural norms have greater
impact on behavioral intentions than personal attitudes (Terry, Hogg, & White, 1999).
―bicultural‖ are typically used to describe individuals, but they can also be used to describe
nations and states (e.g., bicultural and bilingual Quebec, where Anglo and Francophone cultures
co-exist), institutions and policies (e.g., multicultural education), and groups (e.g., multicultural
teams). Although the term is recent, the concept of biculturalism dates back to the origins of
modern Canada (1774, when British authorities allowed French Canadians full use of their
Multiculturalism 7
language, system of civil law, and freedom to practice their Roman Catholic practices).3
Biculturalism should not be confused with bilingualism (having fluency in two languages),
although these terms are conceptually related since often (but not always) bicultural individuals
Multicultural ideology and policies advocate that society and organizations should
include and equally value distinct cultural groups (Fowers & Richardson, 1996). Although the
term multiculturalism is typically used to acknowledge the presence of the distinct cultures of
immigrant groups, sometimes it can also be applied to acknowledge the presence of indigenous
peoples in colonized nations. One assumption behind the multicultural ideology is that public
acceptance and recognition of one‘s culture and opportunities for multicultural interactions are
crucial for self-worth and well-being (Burnet, 1995). Support for this argument is found in
counseling (Sue & Sue, 2003), education (Banks & Banks, 1995), corporate (Plaut, Thomas, &
Goren, in press), and developmental contexts (Berry, Phinney, Sam, & Vedder, 2006; Yip,
Canada, Australia, and the Netherlands, for reasons that vary from country to country.
Multicultural policies influence the structures and decisions of governments to ensure that
political and economic resources are allocated equitably to all represented cultural groups.
support for media outlets (e.g., newspapers, television, radio) in minority languages, support for
multilingual policies, and acceptance of traditional and religious codes of dress and behavior in
Not all minority groups are perceived to deserve multicultural policies equally. Typically,
multicultural recognition and rights are more easily given to 'involuntary' groups (colonized
would have waived their demands and rights by voluntary leaving their country of origin. In
other words, multicultural policies tend to be less supported in relation to immigrant groups than
multicultural attitudes and their effects from both the minority and majority perspectives reveals
some interesting moderating factors (see Verkuyten, 2007; and Berry, 2006; for excellent
reviews). For instance, minorities (e.g., Turkish, Moroccan in The Netherlands) are more likely
to endorse multiculturalism than members of an ethnic majority group (e.g., Dutch). Cross-
national data on multiculturalism validates this finding (Deaux, Reid, Martin, & Bikmen, 2006;
Schalk-Soekar, 2007; Verkuyten & Martinovic, 2006; Wolsko et al., 2006). Further, in-group
while this link is negative among majority individuals (Verkuyten & Martinovic, 2006). The fact
that multiculturalism appeals more to ethnic minority groups than to majority group members is
not surprising given that the gains of this policy are more obvious to the former group (Berry,
2006; Berry & Kalin, 1995; Verkuyten & Thijs, 1999). Studies have also found that minorities‘
and majority individuals who identify strongly with their ethnic group (Verkuyten, 2009). This
suggests that multicultural recognition provides a normative context in which both majorities and
minorities with high levels of ethnic identification can feel good about themselves.
Multiculturalism 9
A promising line of research conducted by Van der Zee and colleagues (e.g., Van der
Zee, Atsma, & Brodbeck, 2004; Van der Zee & Van der Gang, 2007) has been examining the
interactive role between individual factors such as personality (i.e., traits related to multicultural
effectiveness, Van der Zee & Van Oudenhoven, 2000) and social identity, and contextual
pressures in how individual respond to situations involving cultural diversity. This work has
shown, for instance, that individuals high in extraversion and initiative respond more favorably
to intercultural situations, but these differences disappear under threat (Van der Zee & Van der
Gang, 2007). This finding suggests that the link between social traits and success in culturally
diverse contexts is not driven by a special ability to deal with the potential threat of cultural
differences but rather by the social stimulation afforded by culturally diverse situations. The
study also showed that individual differences in neuroticism are linked to reactions towards
cultural diversity only under conditions of threat. Given the increasingly global nature of today‘s
work force, this work promises to be very informative with regard to which competencies
interactions.
political segments within the US and some European nations view multiculturalism as a policy
that promotes group stereotyping and negative out-group feelings and undermines national unity,
unity or homogeneity), ‗assimilation‘ (the belief that cultural minorities should abandon their
original culture and adopt the majority culture), or ‗nativism‘ (return to the original settlers‘
cultural traits –e.g., English, Protestantism, and American liberalism in the case of the US).
Multiculturalism 10
Underlying these views is the belief that the majority-based macro-culture is substantive (i.e.
essential), foundational (i.e., original and primary), and that it provides the moral center for
society; the legitimacy of this macro-culture thus is always prior to the social phenomenon that
universalism and particularism, between unity and fragmentation, between right and left
(Hartman & Gerteis, 2005). Recent multiculturalism theory departs from this aforementioned
unidimensional space and makes a distinction between the social and the cultural dimensions,
pluralism, and interactive pluralism (Hartman and Gerteis, 2005). A review of each these three
multiculturalism approaches reveals issues and constructs that are highly relevant to social
psychology, and the study social identity and intergroup dynamics in particular. For instance, the
cosmopolitan approach recognizes the social value of diversity, but it is skeptical about the
obligations and constraints that group membership and societal cohesion can place on individuals
(Hartman and Gerteis, 2005). In a way, this approach defends cultural diversity to the extent it
supports and facilitates individual rights and freedoms (Bilbeny, 2007). Thus, the cosmopolitan
approach supports a strong macro-social boundary and weak internal groups and emphasizes the
permeability of cultural group membership and boundaries (Hollinger, 1995). Here cultural
group qualities are neutralized rather than negated (as in the assimilationist approach), and
policies are to ensure that every individual is free to choose her or his place in the ethnic mosaic.
An example of this type of ‗weak‘ group identification is the white ethnic identity of many
Americans who self-identify as ―Irish American‖ or ―Italian-American.‖ Note that these group
Multiculturalism 11
affiliations do not imply adopting a separatist identity or even strong identity, because there is no
societal pressure to choose between this and other forms of cultural/ethnic identifications, and
also because there is nothing about being ―Irish‖ that is particularly in tension with being
The fragmented pluralism approach, on the other hand, endorses weaker macro-social
boundaries but very strong internal groups and boundaries given that cultural group membership
is seen as essential rather than partial and voluntaristic (Young, 2000). Structurally, this approach
is the most opposite to assimilation. In fragmented pluralism the focus is on the recognition and
maintenance of group rights and distinctive group cultures (e.g., separate institutions or
practices), and the state is seen mainly as tool for cohesion given its role as a force mediating
between different group claims and value systems, which at times may be divergent or in some
Portes and Rumbaut (2001), can be seen as evidence for the existence of fragmented pluralism in
the US: assimilation into mainstream society by immigrants and their descendents is uneven due
to the fact that different groups are available to which the immigrants may assimilate, and due to
the fact that these different groups afford different opportunities to the immigrant groups. Lastly,
the interactive pluralism approach, like the fragmented pluralism view, also prioritizes the role
of groups, but it mainly stresses groups-in-interaction. This approach sees group interactions as
essential, not only because group interactions facilitate societal cohesion and harmony but also
because from these interactions a new and constantly redefined macro-culture emerges
(Alexander, 2001; Taylor, 2001). That is, social boundaries and moral order are produced
democratic manner through the interaction of groups, and as cultural groups and their
interactions change, the nature of the macro-culture itself changes. Because this dynamic and
Multiculturalism 12
more complex macro-culture represents the complexity and reality of all groups, it is thus is
more easily recognized and valued by all. This view contrasts with cosmopolitanism or
fragmented pluralism, where the macro-culture tends to be thinner and essentially procedural in
nature.
The above constructs (macro- and group-culture) and processes (group interaction,
permeability of cultural group membership and boundaries, procedural vs. substantive views of
macro-culture) are highly relevant to some well known social psychological work. For instance,
work on the common group identity model (Gaertner, Dovidio, Nier, Ward, & Baker, 1999),
social identity complexity (Roccas & Brewer, 2002), group identity dimensionality (Roccas,
Sagiv, Schwartz, Halevy, & Eidelson, 2008), procedural justice (Huo, 2003), and system
justification theory (Jost & Banaji, 1994) speak to some of the issues and processes underlying
the above multiculturalism modes. However, the psychological validity, viability, and
this is an important gap that social psychology is in an ideal position to fill, given its theoretical
one of four outcomes of the acculturation process. Traditional views of acculturation (the process
of learning or adapting to a new culture) asserted that to acculturate means to assimilate –i.e.,
adopting the new or dominant culture requires rejecting one‘s ethnic or original culture (Gordon,
directional, and irreversible process of moving towards the new mainstream culture and away
from the original ethnic culture (Trimble, 2003). However, a wealth of acculturation studies
Multiculturalism 13
conducted in the last 25 years (see Sam & Berry, 2006; for a review), supports acculturation as a
mainstream culture is not the only way to acculturate. In other words, equating acculturation with
individuals have to deal with two central issues, which comprise the two cultural orientations of
acculturation (Berry, 2003): (1) the extent to which they are motivated or allowed to retain
identification and involvement with the culture of origin, now the non-majority, ethnic culture;
and (2) the extent to which they are motivated or allowed to identify with and participate in the
mainstream, dominant culture. The negotiation of these two central issues results in four distinct
acculturation positions (see left side of Figure 1): assimilation (involvement and identification
with the dominant culture only), integration/biculturalism (involvement and identification with
both cultures), separation (involvement and identification with the ethnic culture only), or
marginalization (lack of involvement and identification with either culture; see Rudmin, 2003 for
a thorough discussion of this strategy). Empirical work on the these four acculturation attitudes
or strategies reveals that, at least at the individual level, the most common strategy used by
assimilation, and marginalization (Berry et al., 2006; Sam & Berry, 2006). Further, there is now
acculturation and its advantages over unidimensional models in predicting a wide array of
outcomes (Flannery, Reise, & Yu, 2001; Ryder, Allen, & Paulhus, 2000).
between national/mainstream identity and ethnic identity in settler countries such as USA (r =
Multiculturalism 14
.15), Canada (.09), or New Zeeland (.32), which have a long tradition of immigration (see Table
4.1 in Phinney, Berry, Vedder, & Liebkind, 2006). However, this association is often moderately
negative in non-settler countries such as France (-.13), Germany (-.28), and The Netherlands (-
.27) (Phinney et al., 2006). This pattern of associations speaks to the prevalence of multicultural
identities across countries, which may result from the interaction of two factors: the climate of
receiving country (e.g., settler vs. non-settler) and the predominant immigrant group (e.g.,
Turkish in Europe vs. Asian and Latin groups in the settler societies).
Cultural Frame-Switching
Additional support for the idea that individuals can simultaneously hold two or more
multicultural individuals shift between their different cultural orientations in response to cultural
cues, a process called cultural frame-switching (CFS; Hong et al., 2000; Verkuyten & Pouliasi,
2006).
behavioral domains such as attribution (Benet-Martínez, Leu, Lee, & Morris, 2002; Cheng, Lee,
& Benet-Martínez, 2006; Hong et al., 2000; Verkuyten & Pouliasi, 2002), personality self-views
(Ramirez-Esparza, Gosling, Benet-Martínez, & Pennebaker, 2006; Ross, Xun, & Willson, 2002;
Verkuyten & Pouliasi, 2006), ethnic identity (Verkuyten & Pouliasi, 2002), emotional
experience (Perunovic, Heller, & Rafaeli, 2007), self-construals (Gardner, Gabriel, & Lee, 1999;
Kemmelmeier & Cheng, 2004; Lechuga, 2008), values (Fu, Chiu, Morris, & Young, 2007;
Verkuyten & Pouliasi, 2006), cooperation (Wong & Hong, 2005), auto-biographical memory
(Bender & Ng, 2009), and decision making (Briley, Morris, & Simonson, 2005) among others.
Further, the existence of dual dynamic culture-specific meaning systems among multiculturals
Multiculturalism 15
has been demonstrated both at the explicit (Pouliasi & Verkuyten, 2007) and implicit level
(Devos, 2006).
Note that CFS is not merely a knee-jerk response to cultural cues. In other for a particular
cultural cue to influence behavior, the relevant cultural schemas have to be cognitively available
(i.e., the individual has internalized values, norms, attitudes, and emotional associations relevant
to that culture), cognitively accessible (the schemas have been recently activated by explicit or
implicit contextual cues), and applicable to the situation (Hong et al., 2000; Hong, Benet-
Although CFS is often unconscious and automatic (like a bilingual individual switching
languages depending on the audience), it does not always have to be. Individual going through
acculturation may to some extent manage the CFS process by controlling the accessibility of
cultural schemas. For instance, immigrants desiring to adapt quickly to the new culture often
surround themselves with symbols and situations that prime the meaning system of the host
culture. Conversely, expatriates desiring to keep alive their original ways of thinking and feeling
–i.e., desiring to maintain the accessibility of constructs from their home culture, surround
themselves with stimuli priming that culture (e.g., ethnic food, art, and music). These active
processes of priming oneself may help multicultural individuals in their ongoing effort to
The CFS processes described above can also be understood as a form of multicultural
―identity performance‖ (Wiley & Deaux, in press). Identity performance involves ―. . . the
associated with a salient social identity‖ (Klein, Spears, & Reicher, 2007, p. 30). According to
this framework, multicultural individuals do not passively react to cultural cues; rather they
Multiculturalism 16
actively manage their identity presentation in response to the type audience and macro-context
(e.g., presence of members from one culture or the other, or both), and the categorization (e.g.,
low vs. high status) and treatment received by this audience, thus, behaving in ways designed to
elicit recognition or confirmation of their important identities (Barreto, Spears, Ellemers, &
Shahinper, 2003; Wiley & Deaux, in press). For instance, when Asian-American individuals are
in situations where their ―Americaness‖ is being questioned (because of their appearance, race,
language, or norms), they react to American cues with behaviors that assert and reinforce
―American‖ identity practices --e.g., by listing more 1980s U.S. television shows and advertising
American lifestyle (Cheryan & Monin, 2005). Interestingly, none of these reactions seems to
bring higher identification and pride with American culture or lower identification and pride with
being Asian; this would support the identity performance view that CFS and behaviors such as
the above involve strategic identity presentations rather than fundamental changes in identity
evaluation and meaning. In short, multicultural identities are expressed differently depending on
the opportunities afforded (and denied) by a given context, including other people‘s (actual and
anticipated) evaluations, expectations and behaviors (see Figure 1 in Wiley & Deaux, in press).
Lastly, it is important to point out that the acculturation perspective does not presuppose
that multicultural individuals internalize and use their different cultures globally and uniformly
(Nguyen & Benet-Martínez, 2010). Acculturation changes can take place in many different
domains of life: language use or preference, social affiliation, communication style, cultural
identity and pride, and cultural knowledge, beliefs, and values (Zane & Mak, 2003); and
acculturation changes in some of these domains may occur independently of changes in other
components. For instance, a Japanese American bicultural individual may endorse Anglo-
Multiculturalism 17
American culture behaviorally and linguistically and yet be very Japanese (ethnic culture) in
terms of her/his values and attitudes. Similarly, a Mexican American bicultural individual can
behave in ways that are predominantly Mexican (e.g., speak mostly Spanish, live in a largely
Mexican neighborhood) and yet display great pride and attitudinal attachment with American
culture. In fact, some recent acculturation work suggests that, independently of how much the
mainstream culture is internalized and practiced, some immigrants and their descendents adhere
to the ethnic cultural values even more strongly than members of their home country, probably
because they can become gradually ―encapsulated‖ within the norms and values of an earlier era
in their homeland, (Kim-Jo, Benet-Martínez, & Ozer, in press; Kosmitzki, 1996). What may
drive this cultural encapsulation phenomenon? First, when immigrant groups arrive to a new
country, they bring with them the values and norms of their home culture at that time. As time
passes, the home culture may undergo change (e.g., modernization, globalization), but
immigrants continue to transmit this original cultural values and norms they brought with them
(Matsumoto, 2000). Second, as immigrants‘ multicultural contacts with both the majority and
other minority members increase, cultural clash and the possibility of cultural assimilation
(particularly for their children) become more real; therefore, reactive (conscious or unconscious)
behaviors, motives, or cognitive associations which reflect higher salience and strengthening of
the original home culture may arise in response (ethnic cultural reaffirmation effect; Bond &
The drivers and outcomes of acculturation (and its multiculturalism mode) are not
constant but rather dynamic and vary across time and local and national contexts (Schwartz &
Unger, 2010). As seen above, these forces may operate differently depending on the immigrant
Thus far, the discussion of acculturation has been at the individual level, but acculturation
is also tied to multiculturalism at the societal level. As depicted in the right side of Figure 1, at
the societal level, there are also four strategies corresponding to the four individual acculturation
strategies (Berry, 2003). Countries with public policies that promote the assimilation of
acculturating individuals are described as melting pots. Those that encourage separation are
referred to as segregationist, and those that promote marginalization are labeled exclusionary
(see also previous section where I reviewed assimilation views and three possible
(Ward & Masgoret, 2008). For example, Canada‘s multicultural policies encourage ethnic and
cultural groups to maintain, develop, and share their cultures with others as well as to accept and
interact with other groups (Berry, 1984). Although acculturating individuals by and large prefer
the bicultural or integration strategy, in reality, most host countries are melting pots, encouraging
the assimilation of acculturating individuals into the dominant culture (Van Oudenhoven, Ward,
& Masgoret, 2006). Consequently, when national policies and dominant groups‘ acculturation
attitudes do not match with acculturating individuals‘ strategies, conflicts and problems in
intergroup relations may arise (Bourhis, Moïse, Perreault, & Senécal, 1997; Jasinskaja-Lahti,
Liebkind, Horenczyk, & Schmitz, 2003). Thus, public policies regarding acculturation and
changing global migration patterns diversify many nations around the world.
and theoretical and psychometric issues involved in measuring biculturalism (and acculturation)
is beyond the scope of this paper (see Arends-Tóth & van de Vijver, 2006; Zane & Mak, 2003;
for excellent reviews). Accordingly, I provide instead a practical and brief summary of the
scores or the starting point of the scale typically reflected separation, and high scores or the other
end of the scale reflected assimilation, with biculturalism being tapped by middle scores or the
midpoint of the scale (e.g., Cuéllar, Harris, & Jasso, 1980; Rotheram-Borus, 1990; Suinn,
Rickard-Figueroa, Lew, & Vigil, 1987). These unidimensional scales should be avoided because
they equate involvement and identification with one culture to a lack of involvement and
identification with the other culture. In addition, these scales confound biculturalism and
marginalization. For example, a scale item may be ―Who do you associate with?‖ and the
response choices may be labeled with 1 = mostly individuals from the ethnic culture, 2 =
individuals from both the ethnic and dominant cultures equally, 3 = mostly individuals from the
dominant culture. A bicultural individual would select ―2‖ because he/she has many friends from
both cultures, but a marginalized individual may also select ―2‖ but because his/her lack of
increase in the number of bidimensional scales, where involvement with ethnic and dominant
cultures is measured in two separate multi-item scales. With this method, biculturalism can be
operationalized in different ways. Typically, bicultural individuals are those who have scores
above the median (e.g., Ryder et al., 2000; Tsai, Ying, & Lee, 2000) or midpoint (e.g., Donà &
Berry, 1994) on both cultural orientations. More recently, cluster analyses (e.g., Lee, Sobal, &
Frongillo, 2003) and latent class analyses (e.g., Stevens, Pels, Vollebergh, & Crijnen, 2004) have
also been used to create categories of acculturation strategies, including the integration or
individuals from other acculturating types (assimilated, separated, or marginalized) but does not
when using bidimensional scales are to add the two cultural orientation subscale scores (e.g.,
Cuéllar, Arnold, & Maldonado, 1995) or combine them into an interaction term (Birman, 1998)
so that low and high scores represent low and high level of biculturalism respectively. One
caveat of these last two methods is the difficulty in differentiating between individuals who have
medium scores on both cultural scales and those who score very high on one scale and low on
the other. Lastly, some researchers have used a method where scores on the two cultural
orientation scales are subtracted from another, so that scores close to zero denote biculturalism
(Szapocznik, Kurtines, & Fernandez, 1980). This approach is not recommended because, like
from each other. Obviously, two key advantages of these multidimensional approaches are that
the cultures of interest (e.g., ethnic, mainstream, and religious cultures), regardless of their
Multiculturalism 21
number, can be independently assessed, and that their measurement can be tailored to particular
Some researchers prefer to measure the acculturation strategies directly (e.g., Berry, Kim,
Power, Young, & Bujaki, 1989). These instruments typically include four scales with statements
capturing favorable attitudes towards the integration (biculturalism), assimilation, separation, and
acculturation strategies, a bicultural individual would be someone whose highest score is on the
integration subscale. This widely used approach has some important advantages over traditional
acculturation scales (e.g., it allows us to measure the construct of biculturalism attitudes directly)
but it suffers from some conceptual and psychometric limitations (e.g., low score reliabilities,
lack of scale independence; see Kang, 2006; Rudmin, 2003; Schwartz & Zamboanga, 2008; Zane
When time or reading levels are compromised, researchers may choose to measure
biculturalism with one or two questions. For instance, bicultural individuals can be those who
self-identify with a hyphenated label (e.g., Persian-American) rather than an ethnic (e.g., Persian)
or a national (e.g., American) label, those who endorse the label ―bicultural‖ (vs.
―monocultural‖), or those who score above the midpoint on two single items stating ―I feel/am
U.S. American‖ and ―I feel/am Chinese‖ (e.g., Benet-Martínez & Haritatos, 2005). Lastly, I
should warn against the common practice of using demographic variables such as generational
status, legal residence, or linguistic ability and preference, as a proxy for psychological
acculturation (e.g., Buriel, Calzada, & Vasquez, 1982). As mentioned earlier, bicultural
involvement and identification can occur at different rates for different life domains, for different
individuals, and for different cultural groups, and demographic variables seem to be poor to
Multiculturalism 22
modest predictors of these changes (Phinney, 2003; Schwartz, Pantin, Sullivan, Prado, &
Szapocznik, 2006).
―I had been rowing back and forth, in a relentless manner, between two banks of a wide river.
Increasingly, what I wanted was to be a burning boat in the middle of the water, visible to both
shores yet indecipherable in my fury.‖ (lê thi diem thúy, 2003)
careful review of the early (and mostly qualitative) work on this topic in the acculturation (e.g.,
Padilla, 1994; Phinney & Devich-Navarro, 1997) and popular (e.g., Chavez, 1994; O‘Hearn,
1998) literatures reveals that multicultural individuals often talk about their multiple cultural
attachments in complicated ways, including both positive and negative terms (see quotes above).
Multiculturalism can be associated with feelings of pride, uniqueness, and a rich sense of
community and history, while also bringing to mind identity confusion, dual expectations, and
value clashes. Further, multicultural individuals deal differently with the implications of different
cultural and racial stereotypes and the pressures coming from their different communities for
loyalties and behaviors (LaFromboise et al., 1993). An important issue, then, is how particular
impact the process of multicultural identity formation and the meanings associated with this
experience.
(Berry et al., 2006), research on acculturation has almost exclusively focused on individual
differences across acculturation strategies rather than within acculturation strategies. Yet, not all
bicultural individuals are alike. Early theoretical work on this issue is worth reviewing, even if
Multiculturalism 23
described two biculturalism modes: alternation and fusion. Alternating bicultural individuals
switch their behaviors in response to situational cultural demands, whereas fused bicultural
individuals are oriented to a third emerging culture that is distinct from each of their two cultures
(e.g., Chicano culture). Birman (1994) expanded on LaFromboise et al.‘s (1993) framework to
describe four types of bicultural individuals: blended (i.e., fused), instrumental (individuals
behaviorally oriented to both cultures but identified with neither), integrated (individuals
behaviorally oriented to both cultures but identified with only their ethnic culture), and explorers
(behaviorally oriented to the dominant culture but identified with only their ethnic culture).
Phinney and Devich-Navarro‘s (1997) qualitative and quantitative study sought to empirically
integrate Berry‘s (1990), LaFromboise et al.‘s (1993), and Birman‘s (1994) conceptual models of
biculturalism. This study identified two bicultural types which were given labels similar to those
with a combination of the two cultures more than with each culture separately, and alternating
biculturals – who emphasized situational differences in how they saw themselves culturally.
These researchers are credited with calling attention to bicultural individuals and for
advancing this area of research; however, a conceptual limitation of the above typologies is their
confounding of identity and behavioral markers. Specifically, whereas the labels ―blended‖ and
―fused‖ refer to identity-related aspects of the bicultural experience (e.g., seeing oneself as Asian
American or Chicano), the label ―alternating‖ refers to the behavioral domain, that is, the ability
not have to map onto each other (Roccas & Brewer, 2002; Boski, 2008). For instance, a
Multiculturalism 24
bicultural individual may have a blended or fused identity (e.g., someone who is sees him/herself
as a product of both Jewish and American cultures and accordingly identifies as Jewish
American) AND also alternate between speaking mainstream English and Yiddish depending on
the context (i.e., frame-switch). Thus researchers should be aware that the two labels ―blended‖
and ―alternating‖ do not tap different types of bicultural individuals but rather different
components of the bicultural experience (i.e., identity in the case of ‗fused‘ and behaviors in the
case of ‗alternating‘).
After an extensive review and synthesis of the empirical and qualitative acculturation and
multiculturalism literature, Benet-Martínez et al. (2002) proposed the theoretical construct of BII
captures the degree to which ―biculturals perceive their mainstream and ethic cultural identities
as compatible and integrated vs. oppositional and difficult to integrate‖ (Benet-Martínez et al.,
2002, p. 9). As an individual difference variable, BII thus focuses on bicultural individuals‘
subjective perceptions of managing dual cultural identities (i.e., how they cognitively and
affectively organize this experience). The emphasis here is on subjective (i.e., the perception and
experience of) cultural overlap and compatibility because, as was found in a study of over 7,000
acculturating adolescents in 13 countries, objective differences between ethnic and host cultures
Bicultural individuals with high BII tend to see themselves as part of a hyphenated
culture (or even part of a combined, emerging ―third‖ culture), and find the two cultures largely
compatible and easy to integrate. Bicultural individuals with low BII, on the other hand, tend to
see themselves as living ―in-between cultures‖ and report seeing the two cultures as largely
Multiculturalism 25
conflictual and disparate. Interestingly, high and low BIIs have consistently emerged as similar
2006; Benet-Martínez et al., 2002) and in basic demographic variables such as years spent in the
US and age of migration; however, compared with high BIIs, low BIIs tend to be less proficient
in English and less identified with American culture. This pattern underscores competence in the
In summary, bicultural individuals high and low on BII identify with both mainstream
(e.g., American) and ethnic (e.g., Chinese) cultures but differ in their ability to create a
synergistic, integrated cultural identity. Although no construct in the existing literature captures
all the nuances of BII, a few acculturation and ethnic minority theorists have discussed particular
acculturation experiences and outcomes that seem to relate (if only partially) to the identity
integration vs. opposition continuum defined by BII. Examples of these constructs are: ―identity
synthesis‖ (Schwartz, 2006), ―blendedness‖ (Padilla, 1994; Phinney & Devich-Navarro, 1997),
―bicultural competence‖ (LaFromboise et al., 1993) vs. ―cultural homelessness‖ (Vivero &
In their first study of BII, Benet-Martínez and her colleagues (Benet-Martínez et al.,
showing that variations in BII moderate the process of cultural frame-switching. Specifically,
Chinese-American biculturals high on BII (those who perceive their cultural identities as
associated with one of their cultural backgrounds (e.g., made stronger external attributions to an
ambiguous social event after being primed with Chinese icons, and made stronger internal
Multiculturalism 26
attributions to the same event after seeing American icons). However, Chinese-American
biculturals low on BII (those who perceive their cultural identities to be in opposition), behaved
in non-culturally congruent ways when exposed to these same cues. Specifically, low BIIs
words, low BIIs exhibited a type of ‗behavioral reactance‘ that the socio-cognitive literature
The above contrastive attributional responses displayed by biculturals with low levels of
BII have since then been replicated (Cheng, Lee, & Benet-Martínez, 2006; Zou, Morris, &
Benet-Martínez, 2008), and a recent study shows these effects also in the domain of personality
self-views (Zou & Morris, 2009). As discussed in Benet-Martínez et al. (2002), the prime-
inconsistent behavior of low BIIs is supported by academic and popular depictions of cultural
clash (e.g., Ogbu, 2008; Roth, 1969), where inner cultural conflict is often described as leading
particular situations. For instance, in Roth‘s novel, the conflicted bicultural protagonist finds
himself feeling and acting particularly Jewish when traveling to the Midwest, and feeling/acting
Research on BII reports a positive association between BII and (1) psychological
wellbeing, even after controlling for trait neuroticism (Chen, Benet-Martínez, & Bond, 2008;
Downie et al., 2004); (2) creative performance (Cheng, Sanchez-Burks, & Lee, 2008); (3) having
larger and more richly interconnected social networks (Mok, Morris, Benet-Martínez, &
Karakitapoglu-Aygun, 2007); (4) higher perceived similarity between one‘s minority and
Multiculturalism 27
majority cultural ingroups (Miramontez, Benet-Martínez, & Nguyen, 2008); and (6) preference
Recent work on BII has also shown that BII is not a unitary construct, as initially
suggested in earlier work (e.g., Benet-Martínez et al., 2002). Instead, BII seems to involve two
relatively independent psychological constructs, cultural harmony vs. conflict and cultural
blendedness vs. distance, each representing unique and separate aspects of the dynamic
intersection between mainstream and ethnic cultural identities within bicultural individuals
(Benet-Martínez & Haritatos, 2005). Cultural harmony vs. conflict captures the degree of
harmony vs. tension or clash felt between the two cultural orientations (e.g., ―I find it easy to
balance both Chinese and American cultures‖ vs. ―I feel caught between the two cultures‖).
Cultural blendedness vs. distance, on the other hand, captures the degree of overlap vs.
feel part of a combined culture‖ vs. ―I am simply a Chinese who lives in the U.S.‖). [See Table 2
in Benet-Martínez and Haritatos (2005) for original items and their factor structure, and
Appendix in this chapter for the newly expanded Bicultural Identity Integration Scale --Version
2: BIIS-2].
blendedness (correlations between the two scales range between .30 and .40) suggests that these
two constructs are formative --i.e., causal-- rather than reflective (i.e., effect) indicators of BII
(Bollen & Lennox, 1991). That is, rather than a latent construct with two resulting dimensions
(cultural harmony and blendedness), BII should perhaps be understood as emerging or resulting
from (rather than leading to) variations in cultural blendedness and harmony (see Figure 2).
Thus, behaviors, attitudes, and feelings described by cultural researchers under the rubric of low
Multiculturalism 28
BII (e.g., the feelings of tension and incompatibility reported in the first quote opening this
chapter‘s section) may in fact be largely capturing the resulting phenomenology of the more
Cultural harmony and blendedness are each associated with different sets of personality,
Haritatos, 2005), lack of cultural blendedness (i.e., cultural distance) is predicted by the
personality trait of close-mindedness (i.e., low openness to experience), low levels of bicultural
competence (particularly with regard to the mainstream culture), experiencing strains in the
linguistic domain (e.g., being self-conscious about one‘s accent), and living in a community that
is not culturally diverse. Perhaps low openness makes acculturating individuals perceive ethnic
and mainstream cultures more rigidly, both in terms of their ‗essential‘ defining characteristics
and the boundaries between them; it may also make them less permeable to new cultural values
and life styles. Such attitudes may lead to the belief that one‘s two cultural identities cannot
―come together‖ and must remain separate. Also, the perception that one has a noticeable accent
and that one‘s cultural background is uncommon in the local environment function as chronic
and explicit reminders of the bicultural‘ unique status as cultural minority and also accentuate
perceptions of cultural difference. Aside from these antecedents, cultural distance may also be
related to the need for optimal distinctiveness (Brewer, 1991). Specifically, some biculturals may
choose to keep their ethnic and mainstream identities separate in an effort to affirm both their
Haritatos, 2005). That is, biculturals low on cultural blendedness may be keeping ethnic (e.g.,
Multiculturalism 29
Chinese) and American cultures separate to affirm their strong ties to their Chinese culture while
also differentiating themselves from the mainstream American cultural group. Lastly, cultural
distance may be related to seeing one‘s two cultures as being very different from each other
(Ward & Kennedy, 1993). To the extent that perceptions of difference may be accentuated in the
early stages of mainstream culture acquisition (e.g., experience of cultural shock), one could
speculate that, as biculturals‘ exposure to and competence in the mainstream culture increases,
Low cultural harmony (i.e., conflict), on the other hand, is largely predicted by having a
neurotic disposition, and experiencing discrimination and strained intercultural relations (e.g.,
being told that one‘s behavior is ―too American‖ or ―ethnic‖ --see Figure 1 in Benet-Martínez &
Haritatos, 2005). Perhaps for biculturals high on neuroticism, switching cognitive and behavioral
frames in response to different cultural cues (i.e., CFS; Hong et al., 2000) brings feelings of
likely that the acculturation strains of discrimination and strained intercultural relations create a
strong discrepancy between explicit and implicit attitudes towards each culture. In other words,
Anglo/American and ethnic cultures but also experiences prejudice and rejection from members
of one or both of these groups, feelings of anger and distress may create internal discrepancy and
related personal and contextual challenges (e.g., trait of openness, linguistic fluency, living in a
culturally-diverse enclave), while cultural harmony is linked to factors that are largely intra- and
interpersonal in nature (e.g., emotional stability, lack of social prejudice and rejection). All in all,
Multiculturalism 30
understanding the bicultural experience, and the consequentiality of personality factors in the
acculturation domain (Ozer & Benet-Martínez, 2006). These patterns of relationships also
suggest that variations in BII, far from being purely subjective identity representations, are
As mentioned earlier, much of the research on BII has found that individuals with low
levels of conflict (high BII) are better adjusted and more effective in a variety of domains.
However, some research also indicates that those with low levels of BII are more cognitively
complex (Benet-Martínez et al., 2006). This suggests that conflicting cultural identities may have
positive cognitive benefits. Perhaps, inner cultural conflict leads to more systematic and careful
processing of cues from cultural situations, which in turn leads to cultural representations that are
more complex and nuanced. Other researchers have also argued that the more severe the cultural
conflict experienced, the greater the need to engage in more effortful and complex sense making
Future work on BII should identify the behavioral domains associated with biculturals‘
feelings of conflict (e.g., clashes in work values, marriage practices, gender roles, etc.), as well
compartmentalization (e.g., home vs. work, relatives vs. friends, etc.). Second, BII research
should be integrated with theory on the benefits and costs of social identity complexity (Brock,
Garcia, & Fleming, 2009; Roccas & Brewer, 2002; Settles, 2004). Second, because bicultural
identities contain multiple elements including self-categorization, and importance and meaning
attached to each identity, a bicultural individual may perceive blendedness on some of these
Multiculturalism 31
elements (e.g., self-categorization), but not on others (e.g., importance), and harmony on some
elements (e.g. meaning), but conflict on others. A full understanding of BII will require
systematic investigation of these various careful identity elements (Wiley & Deaux, in press).
variables in predicting bicultural identity structure and bicultural experiences, but there are other
relevant variables. Hong and colleagues (Chao, Chen, Roisman, & Hong, 2007; Hong, Liao, Lee,
Wood, & Chao, 2008) have shown that Asian-American biculturals who hold essentialist beliefs
about race –i.e., believe race is an essentialist entity reflecting biologically essence, unalterable,
and indicative of abilities and traits-- have more difficulties (i.e. longer latencies) in cultural
frame-switching behavior, display stronger emotional reactivity when talking about bicultural
experiences, and identify less with the host culture. The researchers have argued that essentialist
race beliefs give rise to perception of less permeability between racial and cultural group
boundaries, thus impeding an integration of experiences with both their ethnic and host cultures.
Future research show examine how essentialist beliefs about race and culture as well as BII
(particularly the blendedness vs. distance component) relate to cognitive constructs such low
openness to experience, need for closure, and low integrative complexity among acculturating
individuals (Kosic, Kruglanski, Pierro, & Mannetti, 2004; Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006).
Given the changing and often life-long nature of acculturation experiences, future studies
neuroticism), bicultural identity (e.g., BII), and racial/cultural essentialist beliefs should be
examined in longitudinal studies that are also sensitive to dynamic political/economic factors.
Studies on cultural transitions such as repatriation among sojourner and immigrants (Sussman,
2000; 2002; Ttsuda, 2003) for instance, reveal a complex pattern of identity shifts and
Multiculturalism 32
adjustment outcomes that are driven by both psychological (e.g., self-concept clarity, strength of
home and host culture identities) and sociopolitical factors (e.g., economic and political situation
temporal and dynamic nature of what Levitt and Schiller (2004) call immigrants‘ ―ways of
being,‖ (actual social relations and practices that individuals engage in) and ―ways of belonging‖
group). Future work on individual differences in multicultural identity can also benefit
frameworks, Amiot and colleagues (Amiot, de la Sabionnière, Terry, & Smith, 2007) have
recently proposed a four-stage model that explains the specific processes by which multiple
social identities develop intra-individually and become integrated within the self over time. Their
theoretically rich model also specifies the factors that facilitate and hinder these identity change
Multicultural individuals may belong to one of the following five groups based on the
voluntariness, mobility, and permanence of contact with the dominant group: immigrants,
refugees, sojourners, ethnic minorities, and indigenous people (Berry, Kim, Minde, & Mok,
1987). Immigrants arrive in the host country voluntarily and usually with the intention to stay,
whereas refugees arrive in the host country by force or due to lack of other alternatives. Like
immigrants, sojourners, such as expatriates and international students, also arrive in the host
country voluntarily, but their stay is usually temporary. Ethnic minorities and indigenous people
are those born in the host country, but indigenous people differ from ethnic minorities in that the
Multiculturalism 33
host country and culture was involuntarily imposed upon them (e.g., via colonization or military
occupation). The ethnic minority group may be divided into second-generation individuals
(whose parents are immigrants or refugees) and third- or later-generation individuals (whose
parents were born in the host country; Padilla, 2006). Many mixed-race or mixed-ethnic
individuals are also multicultural, regardless of their acculturating group status (Padilla, 2006).
One can speculate about possible group-level differences among the groups mentioned
above with regard to their levels of BII due to their group‘s history in the host country, their
relations with members of the dominant group, the current political and socioeconomic situation,
and other structural variables (Nguyen & Benet-Martínez, 2010). For instance, often immigrants
and sojourners chose to migrate to the host country for economic or educational opportunities,
and some may even have the option of returning to their native countries; thus, relative to the
other groups, this type of multicultural individual may be more focused on opportunities and less
focused on cultural issues. Consequently, cultural differences may not necessarily be internalized
or translated into the experience cultural identity conflict or distance. Conversely, refugees and
indigenous people are often forced into contact with the dominant culture, and the involuntary
nature of this contact (e.g., refugees may want to return to their native countries, but this is not
possible due to conflicts between the host and native countries or within their native countries)
magnifies cultural differences and identity conflict. Relatedly, African Americans, with their
history of involuntary slavery and expatriation, may also experience more cultural identity
conflict and distance than other groups. Lastly, there are reasons to think that feelings of cultural
individuals (at least relative to immigrants and sojourners). Mixed-race and mix-ethnic
individuals are often given (implicit or explicit) messages suggesting that they are not ―enough‖
Multiculturalism 34
of one culture or the other (Root, 1998). Likewise, second-generation ethnic minorities are
sometimes considered not ―ethnic‖ enough by both their parents and dominant culture peers with
regard to certain cultural ―markers‖ (e.g., ethnic language fluency) while also not being
generational status and cultural socialization may also play a role in BII, particularly the
experience of cultural distance. Immigrants first learn their ethnic culture in their native country
and later learn the dominant culture in the host country, thus their competencies and associations
with each culture may be more compartmentalized and situation-specific (i.e., high cultural
distance) compared to other groups. This dissociation may also occur among second-generation
ethnic minorities for whom dominant and ethnic cultures are largely relegated to the public (e.g.,
work) and private (e.g., home) spheres, respectively. However, other second- and later
generation ethnic minorities (e.g., Chicano individuals) may be reared with a blend of both
cultures, and thus the structure and experience of their identities may be more blended (i.e., low
cultural distance). How these processes work for 1.5 generation individuals (immigrant children
who moved to another country early and thus are socialized early into the host country culture)
All in all, notice that the above propositions focus on the relative level of perceived
cultural distance or conflict across groups – that is, I do not assert that some groups perceive
What impact, if any, does multiculturalism have on individuals and the larger society?
The issue of whether multiculturalism is beneficial is often theoretically and empirically debated.
Multiculturalism 35
Some researchers contend that the integration/biculturalism strategy, as compared to the other
leading to greater benefits in all areas of life (e.g., Berry, 1997; Phinney, Horenczyk, Liebkind,
& Vedder, 2001). However, others have argued that this is not always the case, because the
process of dealing with two cultures and acquiring two behavioral repertories places a burden on
the individual and can lead to stress, isolation, identity confusion, and hindered performance
(e.g., Gordon, 1964; Rudmin, 2003; Vivero & Jenkins, 1999). For instance, when examining the
links between biculturalism and adjustment, some researchers have found positive associations
(e.g., Szapocznik & Kurtines, 1980; Ward & Kennedy, 1994), but others have found no link or a
negative one (e.g., Burnam, Hough, Karno, Escobar, & Telles, 1987; Rotheram-Borus, 1990). In
other words, findings have been mixed with regard to the direction and magnitude of these
associations (Myers & Rodriguez, 2003; Rogler, Cortes, & Malgady, 1991).
A recent meta-analysis suggests that the above seemingly contradictory findings may be
attributable to the ways in which biculturalism has been measured (Nguyen & Benet-Martínez,
2011; see also the review of measurement issues in this chapter). Across the 83 studies and
23,197 participant, biculturalism was found to have a significant and positive relationship with
both psychological adjustment (e.g., life satisfaction, positive affect, self-esteem) and
sociocultural adjustment (e.g., academic achievement, career success, social skills, lack of
behavioral problems). Further, this biculturalism-adjustment link was significantly stronger than
the association between each cultural orientation (dominant or ethnic) and adjustment.
type of acculturation scales used (see Figure 3). When only studies using direct measures of
acculturation strategies were included (i.e., Berry‘s scales), the relationship was weak to
Multiculturalism 36
moderate (r = .21). However, when only studies using unidimensional scales were included, the
relationship was strong (r = .54). Finally, when only studies using bidimensional scales were
used (i.e., biculturalism measured via scores above the median or midpoint on both cultural
orientations, the addition method, the multiplication method, or cluster or latent class analysis),
the relationship between biculturalism and adjustment was even stronger (r = .70). In other
words, biculturalism is related to better adjustment, but this relationship is best detected when
biculturalism is measured bidimensionally. This is not perhaps not surprising given the point
made earlier about how unidimensional acculturation scales can potentially confound
The results from the above meta-analysis clearly invalidate early accounts of bicultural
individuals as ―marginal‖ and stumped between two worlds (Gordon, 1964), and they also
suggest important future research directions for social and personality psychologists studying
increasingly diverse samples, such as examining the role that social context may play in this
that can moderate the biculturalism-adjustment relationship (e.g., Chen et al., 2008).
The positive relationship between multiculturalism and adjustment may be due to the
competencies and flexibility (social and cognitive) that multicultural individuals acquire in the
process of learning and using two cultures (Benet-Martínez et al., 2006; Leung, Maddox,
Galinsky, & Chiu, 2008). Specifically, by virtue of their frequent experiences attending to,
and organize socio-cultural information in more cognitively complex ways than monoculturals
(Benet-Martínez et al., 2006). These competencies may make bicultural individuals more adept
at adjusting to various people or situations in either of their cultures and possibly in other
Multiculturalism 37
cultures. In addition, this flexibility may buffer them from the psychological or sociocultural
maladjustment that they might have otherwise suffered as a result of challenging acculturation
experiences. It is possible that being oriented to only one culture rather than both has some
adjustment costs, resulting from rejection from or lack of belongingness with members of the
other culture (Roccas, Horenczyk, & Schwartz, 2000; Rogler et al., 1991; Ross, Xun, Wilson,
2002). In short, involvement with two or more cultures (vs. the cultural relinquishing that
and social skills as well as wider behavioral repertoires and competencies which, in turn, buffer
It is also possible that better adjusted individuals (e.g., those with higher self-esteem) find
it easier to be bicultural or are able to use resources, which would have been used to cope with
maladjustment, to participate in both cultures and to interact with people from either culture, thus
becoming more bicultural. The biculturalism-adjustment relationship may also be due to a third
variable, such as the dominant group‘s attitudes toward acculturation. For example, a host
country with multicultural policies and a dominant group that is accepting and non-
it is important to note that multiculturalism is not necessarily an individual choice; groups and
intergroup relations also play a role. For example, and individual may favor the
individuals, then assimilation may be adaptive. Although more research is needed to determine
and the prohibition of disparate treatment for different groups, may influence individuals‘ ability
Multiculturalism may also have significant implications for greater national success and
improved national functioning (Berry, 1998; Schwartz, Montgomery, & Briones, 2006). In
(Farver, Bhadha, & Narang, 2002; Régner & Loose, 2006). These educationally successful
students may be able to contribute a great deal to society when they become adults. In the
intercultural competence (Bell & Harrison, 1996; Brannen & Thomas, 2010; Thomas & Inkson,
2004). In addition, they have skills (e.g., multilingualism, cultural frame-switching, intercultural
sensitivity) that are crucial in our increasingly globalized world; thus, multicultural individuals
are ideal cultural mediators for intercultural conflicts and miscommunications within
communities, nations, and internationally (see introductory point about President Obama).
More generally, it has been found that individuals with more extensive multicultural
Martínez et al., 2006), integrative complexity (Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006; Tadmor, Tetlock, &
Multiculturalism 39
Peng, 2009), and creativity (Leung, Maddux, Galinsky, & Chiu, 2008; Maddux & Galinsky,
2009; Simonton, 1997), which are necessary for innovation and progress. Sociologist Gouldner
(1985) argued that when a person draws on more than one line of thought, he/she can escape the
control of any one of them and yet toggle between the two (or more) of them and thus forge new
understandings. Biculturals, because of their experiences moving between cultural systems, may
have richer associations with a single concept than monocultural persons, and they may have
greater tolerance for ambiguity because they are comfortable with situations in which one basic
idea may have different nuances depending on the community they inhabit at the time (Benet-
If the experience of managing different systems of thought (e.g., different sets of cultural
norms, belief systems, contextual cues, and languages) leads to richer and more complex
associations among biculturals, it is not surprising to find that the general cognitive benefits
described above are not restricted to multiculturals. Research in psycholinguistics shows that
some of these cognitive benefits also appear in individuals who speak more than one language
(Bialystock, 1999; Costa, Hernandez, Costa-Faidella, & Sebastian-Galles, 2009; Lambert, 1978).
Recently, Crisp and Turner (2010) have outlined a theoretical model that specifies the antecedent
conditions and cognitive processes through which perceiving multiple identities, in oneself and
others, can lead to generalized cognitive flexibility. Drawing from the literatures on
self-categorization, minority influence, political ideology, and social identity complexity, Crisp
and Turner posit that (a) exposure to diversity, particularly diversity defined by meaningful
incongruent multiple identities (e.g., female engineers, male midwife) leads to (b) a systematic
process of cognitive re-structuring that can temporarily trigger, and over time develop, divergent
Multiculturalism 40
thought and a more generalized flexibility in category use and that (c) can have observable
effects across a wide range of intra- (e.g., creativity, cognitive complexity) and inter-personal
(e.g., prejudice, stereotyping) domains. In sum, social policies promoting multiculturalism and
New Directions
―One and one don‘t necessarily add up to two. Cultural and racial amalgams create a third, wholy
indistinguishable category where origin and home are indeterminate‖ (O‘Hearn, 1998, p. xiv).
The possibility of being oriented to an emergent third culture has important implications
for research on multiculturalism, and future acculturation theory and research will likely
incorporate these effects (Nguyen & Benet-Martínez, 2010). The currently accepted
bidimensional model of acculturation with ethnic and dominant cultural orientations might be
replaced by a tridimensional model, where the third cultural orientation is a culture that emerges
from the integrating of two interacting cultures –e.g., Chicano culture in the U.S. (Flannery et al.,
individuals and those who identify with a global international culture (Chen et al., 2008) than
examined a third cultural orientation or compared a tridimensional model to the other models.
Understanding how emerging global cultures and multicultural spaces that integrate
elements from local and foreign cultures influence psychological processes is of paramount
importance (Chen et al., 2008; Chiu & Cheng, 2007; Nguyen, Huynh, & Benet-Martínez, 2010).
The coexistence of symbols and ideas representing different cultural traditions in the same
physical space is increasingly common (e.g., a Starbucks cafés or McDonalds restaurants placed
in traditional, and sometimes even historic, buildings throughout Europe and Asia). A recent
study sought to examine how the copresence of images from seemingly distinctive cultures in the
Multiculturalism 41
same space affects cognition (Chiu, Mallorie, Keh, & Law, 2009). This study presented
monocultural Chinese and European-American individuals with single and joint presentation of
icons from American and Chinese cultures. Chinese participants in the joint Chinese-American
icon presentation condition attributed more characteristically Chinese attributes and behaviors to
a Chinese target person than Chinese participants in the single presentation condition. Similarly,
more characteristically Western attributes and behaviors to an American target. Contrary to the
common expectation that the salience of one‘s culture will diminish with globalization, these
results show that a globalized environment which includes symbols from multiple distinctive
cultures may draw people‘s attention to their heritage culture as a way to bring coherence and
structure to the situation (see also Chiu & Cheng, 2007). Future studies are need however to
examine these effects among multicultural individuals, for whom culturally mixed situations in
all likelihood do not represent a threat or mismatch with their sense of self.
The above results from Chiu et al.‘s (2009) study with Chinese and American
orientations that characterizes biculturals with low levels of Bicultural Identity Integration
(Benet-Martínez & Haritatos, 2006) and the contrast effects often obtained with this group of
biculturals. Recall that low levels of cultural blendednes and cultural harmony are linked to
cognitive rigidity (i.e., low openness to experience) and neuroticism respectively. These
dispositions may make biculturals more prone to experience rumination and cognitive epistemic
needs, such as need for closure, when facing quickly changing and ambiguous cultural situations,
a common feature of the acculturation experience. In other words, perhaps the mere presence of
a single clear cultural cue makes biculturals low in BII ruminate about his/her two cultures (e.g.,
Multiculturalism 42
compare and contrast them), resulting in a simultaneous activation of both cultures very similar
to the one achieved by the joint cultural images used in Chiu et al.‘s (2009) study. This joint
cultural activation, in turn, may elicit need for closure, or the desire to bring structure over the
situation by focusing on and reinforcing a single cultural affiliation. But which of the two
cultural identities, you may ask? The contrast effects repeatedly found in studies with low BIIs
show that it would be the other culture, that is, the one not being initially primed or activated.
Perhaps as suggested by Mok and Morris (2009), for these conflicted biculturals, following the
lead of a particular cultural cue feels as leaving the other part of the cultural self behind, so they
affirm that other identity to restore equilibrium in the bicultural identities and regain control over
Lastly, future work should examine how much the psychology of having multiple
national, ethnic, or racial identities applies to the intersection of other types of cultures and
identities (Nguyen & Benet-Martínez, 2010). Professional, generational, and geographic cultures
are some examples, but social class and religion are also relevant (Cohen, 2009). For example,
an individual from the southern region of the US living in the northern region of the US may be
relatively prevalent in the south but not the north; therefore, southern White males living in the
north may have to adapt to the norms in the north and negotiate those two cultures (Cohen,
Nisbett, Bowdle, & Schwarz, 1996). Sexual minorities, such as gay/ lesbian individuals, may
also be bicultural, considering that they negotiate and move between gay/lesbian culture and
mainstream heterosexual culture (Fingerhut, Peplau, & Ghavami, 2005). Furthermore, the pair of
cultures to which ―biculturalism‖ refers need not be within the same category. For example,
bicultural because they must negotiate their identities as women and as non-traditional engineers
(Cheng et al., 2008; Sacharin, Lee, & Gonzalez, 2009; Settles, 2004). In addition, multicultural
experiences and identity negotiations emerge when individuals find themselves living and
working in contexts where SES levels and favored religion are very different from the ones
attached to self --e.g., low SES students attending private colleges and universities, or Muslims
living in highly secular societies (Verkuyten & Yildiz, 2007). I believe that the identity structures
and processes discussed in this chapter (e.g., cultural frame-switching, BII) may also apply to
these other types of identities, but research on this kind of identity intersectionality is desperately
The need for both social and personality psychology to respond to the theoretical and
overestimated. In their sampling and design choices, social and personality researchers
(including those who do cultural work) have often implicitly assumed that culture is a stable,
uniform influence, and that nations and individuals are culturally homogeneous. But rapid
globalization, continued massive migration, and the resulting demographic changes have resulted
in social spaces (schools, homes, work settings) that are culturally diverse, and in the growing
number of individuals who identify with, and live in more than one culture (Hong et al., 2000).
Current and future cultural studies need to move beyond traditional between-group cultural
comparisons and develop theoretical models and methodologies that capture the multiplicity and
malleability of cultural meaning within individuals. Some recent studies have taken this
approach in examining the interplay between personality dispositions and psychosocial processes
such as acculturation (Ryder et al., 2000), multicultural attitudes (Van der Zee et al., 2004),
Multiculturalism 44
bicultural identity structure (Benet-Martínez & Haritatos, 2005), and bilingualism (Chen et al,
Future cultural research can also benefit from exciting methodological advances.
Because cultural, social, and personality processes operating at the individual level may not
replicate at the cultural level and vice versa (see Tables 3-4 in Benet-Martínez, 2007),
researchers can use multilevel modeling and latent-class techniques to deal with these
complexities (e.g., Eid & Diener, 2001). These underused techniques have the potential of
fostering a fruitful synergy between the fields of personality and social psychology –which have
traits and values, majority/minority status)—and the fields of and anthropology or sociology,
which are very informative regarding culture-level phenomena (e.g., economy, religion, and
In addition, although many studies have established that cultural forces influence social
behavior and personality (i.e., culture person effects), almost no attention has been given to the
processes by which individual factors in turn influence culture (person culture effects).
Evidence from recent studies shows, for instance, that our personalities shape the cultural
contexts in which we live by influencing both micro- (e.g., personal spaces, music preferences,
content and style of personal web pages, etc.; Gosling et al., 2002; Rentfrow & Gosling, 2003;
Vazire & Gosling, 2004) and macro- (e.g., political orientation, social activism, etc.; Jost et al.,
Lastly, to the extent that social and personality psychology can be seen as two distinct
(but relatively similar) ‗cultures‘ within psychology (Funder & Fast, in press; Tracy, Robins, &
Sherman, 2009), and that the research reviewed here attests to the adjustment benefits of having
Multiculturalism 45
two cultures and integrating them with oneself, I want to argue that social and personality
psychology would benefit from being more blended. Although there is some evidence that this
integration exists already at the institutional level (e.g., Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, Society for Personality and Social Psychology), the blending and integration of
questions, methods, and theories from the two sub-disciplines is less obvious at the individual
(i.e., researcher) level. This is unfortunate given that, as shown with the studies linking
multiculturalism and multilingualism with general cognitive benefits, the integration of social
and personality psychologies could lead to research that is more innovative, multifaceted, and
significant.
Concluding Comments
and noted its consequences for how we conceptualize culture, optimal psychological functioning,
and identity development (e.g., Arnett, 2002; 2008; Hermans & Kempen, 1998). Recently,
multiculturalism has also taken center stage in popular culture. Earlier, it was mentioned that
Obama is undoubtedly multicultural and that biculturalism may refer to cultures other than ethnic
cultures. At the 2009 Radio and Television Correspondents‘ Dinner, John Hodgman, a humorist
and actor famous for his role in Apple‘s Mac vs. PC commercials, delivered a speech on
biculturalism and hybridity, and identified Obama as being of two worlds: the world of ―nerds‖
and the world of ―jocks‖ (C-SPAN, 2009). Like a nerd, Obama values science, objectivity, and
the questioning of the status quo, and like a jock, Obama is likeable, confident, and fun to be
around. As mentioned earlier, bicultural individuals often experience the external pressure of not
having or representing ―enough‖ of one culture or another. In line with this, Hodgman
questioned Obama‘s authenticity as a nerd and tested him on his nerdiness. Although delivered
Multiculturalism 46
complex issues that involve many questions and dilemmas. There are promises and there are
equality, between group identities and individual liberties.‖ (Verkuyten, 2007, p. 294).
Undoubtedly, there are different kinds of diversity and thus different forms of multicultural
policies and theories will perhaps develop to accommodate differences in history, group
fact of life, and it is our collective duty to maximize its individual and collective benefits.
Through exposure to and internalization of different cultures, minority and majority individuals
can experience different ways of learning, viewing, and reacting to the world. This experience
makes these individuals‘ cultural identities more complex and layered and enriches their
cognitive and behavioral repertoires. Research mentioned earlier shows that these psychological
processes lead to higher cognitive complexity and more creative and tolerant thinking. These
References
Alexander, J. (2001). Theorizing the 'Modes of Incorporation.' Sociological Theory, 19, 237-249.
Amiot, C. E., de la Sablonniere, R., Terry, D. J., & Smith, J. R. (2007). Integration of social
Arnett, J. J. (2008). The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less
Banks, J. A., & Banks, C. M. (1995). Handbook of research on multicultural education. New
York: MacMillan.
Barreto, M., Spears, R., Ellemers, N., & Shahinper, K. (2003). Who wants to know? The effect
Bartholet, J., & Stone, D. (2009, January 17). A team of expatriates. Newsweek. Retrieved from
http://www.newsweek.com/id/180207
Baumeister, R. (1986). Identity: Cultural change and the struggle for self. New York: Oxford
University.
Multiculturalism 48
Bell, M. P., & Harrison, D. A. (1996). Using intra-national diversity for international
Bender, & Ng, (2009). Dynamic biculturalism: Socially connected and individuated unique
issues. In R.W. Robins, R.C. Fraley, & R. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research methods
Benet-Martínez, V., & Haritatos, J. (2005). Bicultural identity integration (BII): Components and
Benet-Martínez, V., Lee, F., & Leu, J. (2006). Biculturalism and cognitive complexity: Expertise
Benet-Martínez, V., Leu, J., Lee, F., & Morris, M. (2002). Negotiating biculturalism: Cultural
Berry, J. W. (1998). Social psychological costs and benefits of multiculturalism: A view from
Berry, J.W. (2006). Attitudes towards immigrants and ethnocultural groups in Canada.
Berry, J.W., & Kalin, R. (1995). Multicultural and ethnic attitudes in Canada: Overview of the
Berry, J. W., Kim, U., Minde, T., & Mok, D. (1987). Comparative studies of acculturative stress.
Berry, J. W., Kim, U., Power, S., Young, M., & Bujaki, M. (1989). Acculturation attitudes in
Berry, J. W., Phinney, J. S., Sam, D. L. & Vedder, P. (2006). Immigrant youth: Acculturation,
Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child
Development,70, 636-644
Bilbeny, N. (2007). La identidad cosmopolita: Los límites del patriotismo en la era global.
Bollen, K., & Lennox, R. (1991). Conventional wisdom on measurement: A structural equation
Bond, M. H., & Yang, K. (1982). Ethnic affirmation versus cross-cultural accommodation: the
Bourhis, R. Y., Moïse, L. C., Perreault, S., & Senécal, S. (1997). Towards an interactive
Brewer, M. B. (1991). The social self: On being the same and different at the same time.
Briley, D. A., Morris, M. W., & Simonson, I. (2005). Cultural chameleons: Biculturals,
conformity motives, and decision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15, 351-
362.
Brook, A. T., Garcia, J., & Fleming, M. A. (2008). The effects of multiple identities on
Buriel, R., Calzada, S., & Vasquez, R. (1982). The relationship of traditional Mexican American
Burnam, M. A., Hough, R. L., Karno, M., Escobar, J. I., & Telles, C. A. (1987). Acculturation
C-SPAN. (2009). 2009 Radio and Television Correspondents' Dinner. Retrieved June 28, 2009,
from http://www.c-
spanarchives.org/library/index.php?main_page=product_video_info&products_id=28715
3-1.
Chao, M., Chen, J., Roisman, G., & Hong, Y. (2007). Essentializing race: Implications for
341–348.
Chavez, D. (1994). Face of an angel. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux.
Chen, S. X., Benet-Martínez, V., & Bond, M. H. (2008). Bicultural identity, bilingualism, and
Cheng, C., Lee, F., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2006). Assimilation and contrast effects in cultural
Cheng, C.-Y., & Lee, F. (2009). Multiracial identity integration: Perceptions of conflict and
Cheng, C.-Y., Sanchez-Burks, J., & Lee, F. (2008). Connecting the dots within: Creative
Cheryan, S., & Monin, B. (2005). Where are you really from? Asian Americans and identity
Chiu, C. Y., & Cheng, S. Y. (2007). Toward a social psychology of culture and globalization:
Chiu, C. Y., Mallorie, L., Keh, H., & Law, W. (2009). Perceptions of culture in multicultural
space: Joint presentation of images from two cultures increases in-group attribution of
Cohen, D., Nisbett, R. E., Bowdle, B. R., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Insult, aggression, and the
170-180.
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella J., Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the Bilingual
advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don't. Cognition, 113, 135-149
Crisp, R. J. & Turner, R. N. (2010). Experiencing social and cultural diversity: A cognitive
Cross, W. E. (1995). Oppositional identity and African American youth: Issues and prospects. In
W. D. Hawley (Ed.), Toward a common destiny: Improving race and ethnic relations in
Cuéllar, I., Arnold, B., & Maldonado, R. (1995). Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican
Cuéllar, I., Harris, L. C., & Jasso, R. (1980). An acculturation scale for Mexican American
Deaux, K., Reid, A., Martin, D., & Bikmen, N. (2006). Ideologies of diversity and inequality:
Predicting collective action in groups varying in ethnicity and immigrant status. Political
Devos, T. (2006). Implicit bicultural identity among Mexican American and Asian American
college students. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 12, 381–402.
Dijksterhuis, A., Spears, R., Postmes, T., Stapel, D. A., Koomen, W., van Knippenberg, A., &
Scheepers, D. (1998). Seeing one thing and doing another: Contrast effects in automatic
Donà, G., & Berry, J. W. (1994). Acculturation attitudes and acculturative stress of Central
Downie, M., Koestner, R., ElGeledi, S., & Cree, K. (2004). The impact of cultural internalization
Eid, M., & Diener, E. (2001). Norms for experiencing emotions in different cultures: Inter- and
Farver, J. A. M., Bhadha, B. R., & Narang, S. K. (2002). Acculturation and psychological
Fingerhut, A. W., Peplau, L. A., & Ghavami, N. (2005). A dual-identity framework for
Flannery, W. P., Reise, S. P., & Yu, J. (2001). An empirical comparison of acculturation models.
Fu, H-Y., Chiu, C-Y., Morris, M. W., Young, M. (2007). Spontaneous inferences from cultural
Funder, D., & Fast, L. (in press). Personality in social psychology. In Gilbert, D. & Fiske, S.
Gaertner, S. L., Dovidio, J. F., Nier, J. A., Ward, C. W., & Banker, B. S. (1999). Across cultural
Cultural divides: Understanding and overcoming group conflict (pp. 173-212). New
Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Lee, A. Y. (1999). ―I‖ value freedom, but ―we‖ value
Gosling, S. D., Ko, S. J., Mannarelli, T., & Morris, M. E. (2002). A Room with a cue: Judgments
Greenfield, P. M. (2000). Three approaches to the psychology of culture: Where do they come
from? Where can they go? Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 3, 223-240.
Grosjean, P. (1996). Living with two languages and two cultures. In I. Parasnis (Ed.), Cultural
and language diversity and the deaf experience (pp. 20-37). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
Univ. Press
Hartmann, D., & Gerteis, J. (2005). Dealing with diversity: Mapping multiculturalism in
Hermans, H. J. M., & Kempen, H. J. G. (1998). Moving cultures: The perilous problem of
Hollinger, D. A. (1995). Postethnic America: Beyond Multiculturalism. New York: Basic Books.
Hong, Y. Y., Benet-Martínez, V., Chiu, C. Y., & Morris, M. W. (2003). Boundaries of cultural
Hong, Y., Chan, G., Chiu, C., Wong, R. Y. M., Hansen, I. G., Lee, S., et al. (2003). How are
social identities linked to self-conception and intergroup orientation? The moderating effect
Hong, Y. Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C. Y., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A
dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709-
720.
Hong, Y., Wan, C., No, S., & Chiu, C. (2007). Multicultural identities. In S. Kitayama & D.
Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology. (pp. 323-345). New York, NY: Guilford.
Huntington, S. P. (2004). Who are we? The challenges to America’s national identity. New
Huo, Y. J. (2003). Procedural justice and social regulation across group boundaries: Does
274.
Jasinskaja-Lahti, I., Liebkind, K., Horenczyk, G., & Schmitz, P. (2003). The interactive nature of
Jetten, J., Postmes, T., & Mcauliffe, B. (2002). ―We‘re all individuals‘: Group norms of
Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of stereotyping in system-justification and the
Jost, J.T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A.W., & Sulloway, F. (2003). Political conservatism as
Kang, S.-M. (2006). Measurement of acculturation, scale formats, and language competence:
Kemmelmeier, M., & Cheng, B.Y. (2004). Language and self-construal priming: a replication
and extension in a Hong Kong sample. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 35, 705–
712.
Kim-Jo, T., Benet-Martínez, V., & Ozer, D. (in press). Culture and conflict resolution styles: The
Klein, O., Spears, R., & Reicher, S. (2007). Social identity performance: Extending the strategic
Kosic, A., Kruglanski, A.W., Pierro, A., & Mannetti, L. (2004). The social cognitions of
immigrants' acculturation: Effects of the need for closure and reference group at entry.
LaFromboise, T., Coleman, H. L., & Gerton, J. (1993). Psychological impact of biculturalism:
Lambert WE. (1992). Challenging established views on social issues: The power and limitations
lê, d. t. t. (2003). The gangster we are all looking for. New York: Random House.
Lee, S.-K., Sobal, J., & Frongillo, E. A. (2003). Comparison of models of acculturation: The case
Leung, A. K.-y., Maddux, W. W., Galinsky, A. D., & Chiu, C. Y. (2008). Multicultural
experience enhances creativity: The when and how. American Psychologist, 63, 169-181.
Lin, E.-Y. (2008). Family and social influences on identity conflict in overseas Chinese.
http://www.biculturalfamily.org/myhalfidentity.html
Maddux, W. W., & Galinsky, A. D. (2009). Cultural borders and mental barriers: The
relationship between living abroad and creativity. Journal of Personality and Social
Markus, H.R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion,
Miramontez, D. R., Benet-Martínez, V., & Nguyen, A.-M. D. (2008). Bicultural identity and
Mok, A., Morris, M., Benet-Martínez, V., & Karakitapoglu-Aygun, Z. (2007). Embracing
American culture: Structures of social identity and social networks among first-
Mok, A., & Morris, M. W. (2009). Cultural chameleons and iconoclasts: Assimilation and
Myers, H. F., & Rodriguez, N. (2003). Acculturation and physical health in racial and ethnic
in theory, measurement, and applied research (pp. 163-185). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Nguyen, A.-M. D, & Benet-Martínez, V. (2010). Multicultural Identity: What it is and why it
matters. In R. Crisp (Ed.), The psychology of social and cultural diversity. Hoboken, NJ:
Wiley-Blackwell.
Compass, 1, 101-114.
Nguyen, A.-M. D., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2011). Biculturalism is linked to adjustment: A meta-
Nguyen, A.-M. D., Huynh, Q., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2010). Glocalization and cultural
reaffirmation. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Society of Personality and
O‘Hearn, C. C. (1998). Half and half: Writers on growing up biracial and bicultural. New York:
Pantheon Books.
Obama, B. H. (2009, January 21). Inaugural address. Retrieved July 8, 2009, from
http://www.whitehouse.gov/blog/inaugural-address/
Multiculturalism 60
Ogbu, J. U. (2008). Minority status, oppositional culture, and schooling . New York: Routledge.
Oyserman, D., Sorensen, N., Reber, R., Chen, S. X., & Sannum, P. (2009). Connecting and
Ozer, D. J., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2006). Personality and the prediction of consequential
Malgady & O. Rodriguez (Eds.), Theoretical and conceptual issues in Hispanic mental
28, 467-497.
Perunovic, W. Q. E., Heller, D. and Rafaeli, E. (2007) Within-person changes in the structure of
emotion: The role of cultural identification and language. Psychological Science, 18, 607-
613.
Phinney, J., Berry, J. W., Vedder, P., & Liebkind, K. (2006). The acculturation experience:
L. Sam & P. Vedder (Eds.), Immigrant Youth in Transition: Acculturation, Identity, and
Adolescence, 7, 3-32.
Phinney, J. S., Horenczyk, G., Liebkind, K., & Vedder, P. (2001). Ethnic identity, immigration,
Plaut, V. C., Thomas, K. M., & Goren, M. J. (in press). Is multiculturalism or colorblindness
Portes, A., & Rumbaut, R. G. (2001). Legacies: The Story of the immigrant second generation.
Pouliasi, K., & Verkuyten, M. (2007). Networks of meaning and the bicultural mind: A structural
Ramirez-Esparza, N., Gosling, S., Benet-Martínez, V., Potter, J. & Pennebaker, J. (2006). Do
Régner, I., & Loose, F. (2006). Relationship of sociocultural factors and academic self-esteem to
school grades and school disengagement in North African French adolescents. British
Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2003). The do re mi‘s of everyday life: The structure and
Roccas, S., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). Social identity complexity. Personality and Social
Roccas, S., Sagiv, L., Schwartz, S. H., Halevy, N., & Eidelson, R. (2008). Towards a unifying
Rogler, L. H., Cortes, D. E., & Malgady, R. G. (1991). Acculturation and mental health status
among Hispanics: Convergence and new directions for research. American Psychologist,
46, 585-597.
Preliminary results from the biracial sibling project. Cultural Diversity and Mental
Health, 4, 237-247.
Ross, M., Xun, W. Q. E., & Wilson, A. E. (2002). Language and the bicultural self. Personality
Sacharin, V., Lee, F., & Gonzalez, R. (2009). Identities in harmony? Gender-work identity
Saleh, N. (2009, January 29). The world's first global president. Miller-Mccune. Retrieved from
http://www.miller-mccune.com/politics/the-world%27s-first-global-president-960.
Sanchez-Burks, J., Lee, F., Choi, I., Nisbett, R., Zhao, S., & Koo, J. (2003). Conversing across
Schalk-Soekar, S. (2007). Multiculturalism: A stable concept with many ideological and political
Schwartz, S. J., Montgomery, M. J., & Briones, E. (2006). The role of identity in acculturation
Schwartz, S. J., Pantin, H., Sullivan, S., Prado, G., & Szapocznik, J. (2006). Nativity and years in
Schwartz, S. J., & Unger, J. (2010). Biculturalism and context: What is biculturalism, and when
confirmatory latent class approach. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology,
14, 275–295.
Settles, I. H. (2004). When multiple identities interfere: The role of identity centrality.
Simonton, D. K. (1997). Foreign influence and national development: The impact of open
milieus on Japanese civilization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 86-
94.
Sue, D. W., & Sue, F. (2003). Counseling the culturally diverse: Theory and practice (4th ed.).
Suinn, R. M., Rickard-Figueroa, K., Lew, S., & Vigil, P. (1987). The Suinn-Lew Asian Self-
47, 401-407.
Sussman, N. M. (2000). The dynamic nature of cultural identity throughout cultural transitions:
Why home is not so sweet. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 355-373.
Sussman, N. M. (2002). Testing the cultural identity model of the cultural transition cycle:
process: Theory, models, and some new findings (pp. 139-159). Boulder, CO: Westview.
Tadmor, C. T., & Tetlock, P. E. (2006). Biculturalism: A model of the effects of second-culture
Tadmor, C. T., Tetlock, P. E., Peng, K. (2009). Acculturation strategies and integrative
Swidler, & S. M. Tipton (Eds), Meaning and modernity: Religion, polity and the self (pp.
Terry, D. J., Hogg, M. A., & White, K. M. (1999). The theory of planned behaviour: Self-identity,
social identity and group norms. British Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 225-244.
Thomas, D. C., & Inkson, K. (2004). Cultural intelligence: People skills for global business. San
Francisco: Berrett-Koehler.
Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Sherman, J. W. (2009). The practice of psychological science:
Tsai, J. L., Ying, Y.-W., & Lee, P. A. (2000). The meaning of ―being Chinese‖ and ―being
Tsuda, T. (2003). Strangers in the ethnic homeland: Japanese Brazilian return migration in
U.S. Census Bureau. (2006). Retrieved March 18, 2008, from at http://www.census.gov
Van der Zee, K. I., Atsma, N., & Brodbeck, F. C. (2004). The influence of social identity and
Van der Zee, K. I., & Van der Gang, I. (2007). Personality, threat and affective responses to
Van der Zee, K. I., & Van Oudenhoven, J. P. (2000). Psychometric qualities of the Multicultural
Van Hook, E., & Higgins, E. T. (1988). Self-related problems beyond the self-concept:
Van Oudenhoven, J. P., Ward, C., & Masgoret, A.M. (2006). Patterns of relations between
immigrants and host societies. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 30, 637-
651.
Multiculturalism 67
Vazire, S., & Gosling, S. D. (2004). E-perceptions: Personality impressions based on personal
Verkuyten, M. (2005). Ethnic group identification and group evaluation among minority and
and majority groups in the Netherlands. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 419-427.
Verkuyten, M., & Martinovic, B. (2006). Understanding multicultural attitudes: The role of
Verkuyten, M. & Pouliasi, K. (2002). Biculturalism among older children: Cultural frame
Verkuyten, M., & Pouliasi, K., (2006) Biculturalism and group identification: The mediating role
312-326.
Verkuyten, M., & Thijs, J. (2002). School satisfaction of elementary school children: The role of
performance, peer relations, ethnicity, and gender. Social Indicators Research, 59, 203–
228.
Multiculturalism 68
Verkuyten, M., & Yildiz, A. A. (2007). National (dis)identification and ethnic and religious
Vivero, V. N., & Jenkins, S. R. (1999). Existential hazards of the multicultural individual:
Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. (1994). Acculturation strategies, psychological adjustment, and
Ward, C., & Masgoret, A.M. (2008). Attitudes toward immigrants, immigration, and
Wiley, S., & Deaux, K. (in press). The bicultural identity performance of immigrants. In Azzi,
Wolf, R. (2009, April 20). Most diverse Cabinet in history still incomplete. USA Today.
Wolsko, C., Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2006). Considering the tower of babel:
Wong, R. Y.-m., & Hong, Y.-y. (2005). Dynamic influences of culture on cooperation in the
Yip, T., Seaton, E. K., & Sellers, R. M. (2006). African American racial identity across the
lifespan: Identity status, identity content and depressive symptoms. Child Development,
77, 1504–1517.
Zane, N., & Mak, W. (2003). Major approaches to the measurement of acculturation among
Psychological Association.
Zou, X., Morris, M. W., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2008). Identity motives and cultural priming:
Author Note
Pompeu Fabra University, She can be reached at veronica.benet@upf.edu. This chapter benefited
greatly by the ideas and suggestions provided by Angela-MinhTu Nguyen.Some sections of this
chapter include revised and updated material from Nguyen and Benet-Martínez (2010
Multiculturalism 71
Table 1
Bicultural Identity Integration Scale –Version 2 (BIIS-2; Huynh & Benet-Martínez, 2011)
Note: * Original items from the BIIS-1 (Benet-Martínez & Haritatos, 2005). R = Reverse score these items. The
BIIS-2 can be used with any ethnic minority culture and adapted to any host culture.
Multiculturalism 72
Figure Captions
Figure 1. Acculturation and multiculturalism at the individual vs. societal levels. Adapted from
Figure 2. High vs. low levels of Bicultural Identity Integration result from variations in cultural
HIGH HIGH
Integration Separation Multiculturalism Segregation
CULTURAL
DISPOSITIONAL
FACTORS HARMONY
VS.
e.g., Openness, CONFLICT
Neuroticism
BICULTURAL
IDENTITY
INTEGRATION
CONTEXTUAL
FACTORS CULTURAL
e.g., Acculturation BLENDEDNESS
stressors VS.
COMPARTMENTALIZATION
Multicultural Identity 75
Adjustment
Biculturalism
Multicultural Identity 76
Endnotes
1
For the sake of simplicity and consistency, in this chapter I favor the broader term
―multicultural‖ or ―multiculturalism‖ over the term ―bicultural.‖ Regardless of the term used, I
always refer to individuals and societies who position themselves between two (or more) cultures
and incorporate this experience (i.e., values, knowledge, and feelings associated to each of these
identities and their intersection) into their sense of who they are.
2
Chiu and Chen (2004) define culture as ―a loosely organized network of knowledge that
is produced, distributed, and reproduced among a collection of interconnected people‖ (p. 173).
This ‗loose‘ view of culture contrasts with the ‗systemic‘ view (e.g., Greenfield, 2000; Markus &
Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, 1996) which sees culture as a coherent system of meanings with an
identifiable central theme around which all cultural meanings are organized (e.g., independence
vs. interdependence).
3
See Lambert (1992) for a review of his ambitious research program on the social
idea that having two linguistic systems within one's brain divides a person's cognitive resources
and reduces efficiency of thought and language. Instead, Lambert‘s work provided strong
Sorensen, Reber, Chen, & Sannum, 2009), cross-cultural differences in behavior are due to
cross-national difference in the likelihood that particular mind-sets will be cued at a particular
moment in time. Institutions, media, folklore, and practices within each culture drive the types of
cues and their ubiquity, and thus the mind-sets that will be more frequently cued.
Multicultural Identity 77
5
A recent meta-analysis of the aggregate reliability of three well-known bidimensional
acculturation instruments found that variability in the reliability estimates was associated with
scale length, gender, and ethnic composition of the samples, and that this pattern of association
was different for ethnic and mainstream culture orientations (Huynh, Howell, & Benet-Martínez,
2009).
6
BII is typically conceptualized as a relatively stable individual difference tapping a
bicultural‘ overall feelings and perceptions regarding the compatibility and integration of his/her
dual cultural orientations; however, like most other individual difference constructs, BII should
also be seen as an emerging from the interaction of the person and his/her audience, and thus as
experience (Cheng & Lee, 2009). This study also established the malleability of BII: a
blendedness and harmony, while recall of negative multiracial experiences resulted in decreases.