09 The Map Is Not The Territory
09 The Map Is Not The Territory
09 The Map Is Not The Territory
Context
This lesson is effective following discussions of the capacity of language to map the world
symbolically. When Alfred Korzybski declared that the map is not the territory he was referring
to language, whose representation of the world is not to be confused with reality itself.
Aims
y To consider the nature of symbolism and conventions for representing the world.
y To recognize assumptions that may arise from past structures of power.
Class Management
Two hours are required. First allow the class to take a good look at wall maps of the world. Raise
questions of projections and centring. The following should emerge.
y A flat map is a distortion of a sphere, and a wall map is inevitably a simplification of the
world’s detail.
y Particular representations carry hidden assumptions and values.
y Maps, like languages and theoretical models, are conceptual tools.
The teacher should take on primarily a questioning role, with students doing their own analysis.
However, the teacher may wish to introduce passages for reading, or give historical background.
Focus Activity
1 Display a minimum of three wall maps of the world that differ in their projections and their
placement of particular regions in their centre. Ideal maps are a Mercator projection centred
on Europe, a Peters equal-area projection, and a map which appears to be upside-down, with
south at the top. Also valuable are maps which centre on the United States, with India
repeated at both sides of the maps to allow the symmetry, any Asian or Middle Eastern maps
with their own areas at the centre, and satellite pictures of the earth from space.
2 Have ready, on handouts or overheads, a few examples of conceptual maps, in which the size
or shape of the countries is determined not by geographical size, but by other criteria such as
population, trade balance, or incidence of a disease.
3 Have ready, on handouts or overheads, maps which are supplemented with graphics— pictures
of nature or recreation (from tourist brochures), bold concentric circles for indication of ripple
impact, arrows for movement of armies (from a history text or newspaper).
Use these visual examples to raise questions about conceptual schemes which influence
representation. Each category raises slightly different questions.
Teacher Support Material—Theory of Knowledge Lessons from Around the World © IBO, August 2000 Lesson 9—page 1
Lesson 9: The Map is not the Territory
Having established that maps are not simply perceptual records, move to overt conceptual maps
to raise questions of statistics, and then to maps embellished with graphics, to consider the fine
distinction between clarification and persuasion.
Discussion Questions
Part 1: Geographical and Political World Maps
y Which wall map looks to you most natural? Why? As you look at all three of them, do they
suggest different things to you?
y Which region is in the centre? Why?
y Which region appears largest? How much is Scandinavia or Australia affected by the
projection? Which map takes as its goal the showing of regions according to their relative
size?
y Is it necessary for north to be at the top? What distinguishes north from south when there is
no up or down in space? If the poles are determined by the earth’s rotation, what then
divides east from west?
y Is the prime meridian placed by geographical necessity—or by a decision that could be
otherwise? Do we have associations other than geographical designations with west, east,
north and south?
y Are borders part of nature? Are they visible if the world is viewed from space? What do
borders and naming represent on a world map?
Teacher Support Material—Theory of Knowledge Lessons from Around the World © IBO, August 2000 Lesson 9—page 2
Lesson 9: The Map is not the Territory
Quotation
References
Harley, JB, ‘Maps, Knowledge, and Power’, The Iconography of Landscape: Essays on the Symbolic
Representation, Design and Use of Past Environments (Cambridge Studies in Historical Geography), ed.
Denis Cosgrove and Stephen Daniels, (1994) Cambridge University Press, ISBN 0521389151
Monmonier, M, How to Lie with Maps, (1996) University of Chicago Press, ISBN 0226534219
Introduction only.
Shohat, E & Stam, R, Unthinking Eurocentrism, (1994) Routledge, ISBN 0415063256
Wood, D, The Power of Maps, (1992) The Guildford Press, ISBN 0898624932
Kidron, M & Segal, R, The State of the World Atlas, fifth edition, (1995) Penguin, ISBN
0140252045. Recommended for conceptual maps.
Peters equal-area projection wall map available from the Friendship Press, P.O. Box 37844,
Cincinnati, Ohio 45222, USA
Teacher Support Material—Theory of Knowledge Lessons from Around the World © IBO, August 2000 Lesson 9—page 3
Lesson 9: The Map is not the Territory
Student Handout
Teacher Support Material—Theory of Knowledge Lessons from Around the World © IBO, August 2000 Lesson 9—page 4
Lesson 10: Thinking Logically?
Context
This lesson can be done after a consideration of the nature of reasoning, or before looking at
fallacies. It links into work on scientific methodology.
Aim
y To investigate the extent to which logical thinking is influenced by the subject matter.
Class Management
This lesson can be completed in 40 minutes, or longer if necessary.
In advance of the lesson, photocopy the two Logic Tests overleaf. You will need one copy of the
two tests for each student. Students may be given the two problems at the same time (on the
same sheet of paper), or one following the other.
Ask the students to work out and write down their answers without collaborating, and then to
report them back to the whole class. Compile a list of votes for each card on the blackboard.
Overwhelmingly, students fail to identify the 7 as one of the correct responses in Logic Test 1.
Explain to the class why 7 is correct (this card is capable of falsifying the rule) and why 2 is wrong
(this card is irrelevant to the rule).
Students normally identify the correct answers for Logic Test 2.
Discussion can then proceed as to why, given that the two problems are formally identical, one is
so much easier to solve correctly than the other. Reference can be made to the importance of
form and content in logical reasoning, and how this may affect the building of knowledge.
Teacher Support Material—Theory of Knowledge Lessons from Around the World © IBO, August 2000 Lesson 10—page 1