733-Article Text-728-1-10-20100323 PDF
733-Article Text-728-1-10-20100323 PDF
733-Article Text-728-1-10-20100323 PDF
89
REVIEW OF RESEARCH
WHAT IS RELIGIOSITY?
BARBARA HOLDCROFT
The University of Toledo
Lourdes College
RELIGIOSITY
DIMENSIONS OF RELIGIOSITY
Glock and Stark (1965) have been influential in defining religious orienta-
tions, origins, and dimensions. In doing so, Glock and Stark identified five
dimensions of religiosity: experiential, ritualistic, ideological, intellectual,
and consequential. The experiential dimension focuses on the personal faith
experience, perhaps a transcendent encounter, while the ritualistic domain
involves the worship experience that is involved in community. The ideolog-
ical dimension is “constituted by expectations that the religious will hold to
Catholic Education: A Journal of Inquiry and Practice, Vol. 10, No. 1, September 2006,
89-103 © 2006 Catholic Education: A Journal of Inquiry and Practice
90 Catholic Education/September 2006
The cognitive dimension is concerned with what individuals know about religion,
i.e., religious knowledge. The cultic dimension makes reference to the individ-
ual’s religious practices, i.e., ritualistic behavior. The creedal dimension is con-
cerned with a personal religious belief, and the devotional dimension refers to a
person’s religious feelings and experiences, i.e., the experiential dimension. (p. 6)
religious knowledge, that is, the knowledge of a religion’s scripture and tradi-
tion, is not considered a good measure of religiosity, simply because the major-
ity of believers surveyed, in Western countries at least, seem to be quite ignorant
of what are considered basic elements of (their own) religious tradition. (p. 109)
Conversely, Stark and Glock (1968) wrote, “it is obvious that some min-
92 Catholic Education/September 2006
imum knowledge is necessary for religious commitment; the tenets and ritu-
als of a religion must be known if they are to be believed and practiced” (p.
141). Many authors (Allport & Ross, 1967; Cardwell, 1980; Glock & Stark,
1965) agreed that beliefs and religious participation can be practiced in vir-
tual ignorance, yet they also acknowledged that all religious institutions
expect their members to know doctrine, to participate in ritual, and to have
comprehension of both.
Intellectual Dimension/Orthodoxy
The intellectual dimension of religiosity involves the expectation that the reli-
gious person will be informed about the chosen faith; the measurement of this
dimension would be with a test of faith knowledge. For example, Glock and
Stark (1965) suggested that religious literacy tests be constructed that would
include a wide range of questions on the origin and history of the religion.
The intent of Glock and Stark was to assess a view of Christianity over many
different religious traditions using a four-question measure of religious
knowledge designed and administered to adults that covered the basic knowl-
edge of the Ten Commandments, Scripture, and Old Testament prophets.
Extrinsic/Intrinsic Dimension
Approaching religiosity from the lived perspective, Allport and Ross (1967)
originally designed the Religious Orientation Scale (ROS), which was com-
posed of an 11-item extrinsic scale to measure the extent to which individ-
uals use their religion for their own ends and a nine-item intrinsic scale to
measure the extent to which individuals live their religion. Batson and
Ventis (1982) revised the ROS measure by adding a third dimension, one of
quest, which measures the degree to which an individual’s religion involves
an open-ended responsive dialogue with six items. A revised 12-item meas-
ure has been produced by Batson and Schoenrade (1991) and the authors
have now developed four new questionnaire scales that combine versions of
the above three with an orthodoxy scale (Batson, Schoenrade, & Ventis,
1993) to measure what they now term means, ends, and quest dimensions
of religiosity.
Holdcroft/WHAT IS RELIGIOSITY? 93
CONSEQUENCES OF RELIGIOSITY
The positive effects of religiosity, in both the social and educational realms,
are attracting the attention of many researchers. The application of a variety
of the dimensions of religiosity to specific situations in everyday life is the
subject of much research. Bergan and McConatha’s (2000) study of adoles-
cents, young adults, and adults in later life, demonstrated a small positive
relation between religiosity and happiness across all three age groups:
“Overall, the results of studies examining religiosity and life satisfaction
generally indicate that people who express stronger religious faith and
involvement also report fewer stressful life events and greater life satisfac-
tion” (p. 25). Religious affiliation was found to be a significant predictor of
general life satisfaction and a sense of belonging and purpose in life, as is
indicated in a number of studies, including recent studies regarding the ben-
efits of religiosity by Dezutter, Soenens, and Hutsebaut (2006), Walker
(2003), and Fontaine, Duriez, Luyten, Corveleyn, and Hutsebaut (2005).
Religiosity affects not only specific demographic groups, but individu-
als as well. Beit-Hallahmi and Argyle (1997) wrote extensively about the
individual effects of religiosity, which include: developing a sense of com-
passion, honesty, and altruism as well as happiness and quality of life, health,
and mental health. Walker (2003) analyzed extensive research which
involved many aspects of the intersecting of religion and morality, conclud-
ing that the religious experience is important in moral functioning. A study
by Roccas (2005) concluded that there is a high correlation between religios-
ity and values.
MORAL DEVELOPMENT
Recently, there has been a focus on moral development in particular within
the context of the school environment. An increase in social problems with
school-aged youth since the mid-1950s has resulted in demands that schools
address moral issues and assume the responsibility of instilling a commit-
ment to basic social values. Such values as honesty, hard work, and social
responsibility are being identified as traditional values that should be advo-
cated.
Current approaches to moral education tend to focus on different aspects
of social and moral functioning based on differing theories of moral devel-
opment and socialization. “Socialization refers to the development process
through which individuals learn” sociomoral values and acquire the knowl-
edge and skills necessary to function effectively and meet their own needs
within the social community (Battistich, Schaps, Solomon, & Watson, 1991,
p. 94).
One method for assessing the effectiveness of moral education involves
the application of moral judgment or moral reasoning. Kohlberg (1969)
revised and extended Piaget’s theory of developmental stages by providing a
detailed outline of moral development describing a sequence of distinct
stages of cognitive organization. Through the use of an interview instrument
that offered hypothetical situations, Kohlberg determined that an individual’s
moral orientation progresses from one that operates in conformity with
authority, through one of identification with the norms of society, to moral-
ity in which the individual holds to, and acts upon, self-chosen and abstract
principles of justice. These principles of personal justice may or may not be
formed by knowledge of one’s faith or the cognitive dimension of religiosi-
ty. Kohlberg posited that the goal of moral education is not only the acquisi-
Holdcroft/WHAT IS RELIGIOSITY? 95
tion of the cognitive knowledge of a faith tradition, but also the stimulation
of the natural development of the individual child’s own moral judgment
capacities allowing him or her to use moral judgment to control his or her
behavior (Kohlberg, 1981).
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR
The behavioral goal of school programs that foster sociomoral development
is behavior that is commonly termed prosocial (Solomon, Watson, Schaps,
Battistich, & Solomon, 1990), which includes helping, sharing, and other
seemingly intentional and voluntary positive behaviors for which the motive
is unspecified, unknown, or not altruistic (Eisenberg, 1982). The common
foundation of these behaviors is that an individual’s actions are oriented
toward protection, maintenance, or enhancement of well being of an external
social object.
Interest in the development and maintenance of prosocial behaviors
“increased dramatically in the 1960s and early 1970s” (Eisenberg, 1982, p.
2) and continues to expand, indicating an increasing interest in prosocial
behavior, its study, and methodologies. The literature focuses on the roots
and determinants of prosocial thought and behavior, the “emergence of
prosocial behaviors in childhood” (p. 3).
Current educators approach prosocial behavior from different theoretical
perspectives (Battistich, Watson, Solomon, Schaps, & Solomon, 1991).
Philosophers, such as Aristotle (Frankena, 1965) and Kant (1959), have
taken the position that in order for behavior to be classified as moral it must
be motivated by rational cognitions that are related to duty and responsibili-
ty. Other philosophers argued for an affective as well as a cognitive basis for
moral actions (Blum, 1980). Yet another major theory is rooted in social
learning theory and emphasizes overt and observable behavior. Current
researchers have increasingly examined the relation between the cognitive
and the behavioral domains of morality, developing prosocial behavior
(Eisenberg, 1987).
Efforts to outline the factors involved in the acquisition of prosocial
behavior were studied by Eisenberg (1987). Such factors include: the inter-
nalization of humanistic values and patterns of prosocial behavior; model-
ing; and moral reasoning. She wrote of the interrelatedness of these factors,
stating that “socialization experiences undoubtedly affect cognitive func-
tions” and that the cognitive factors “undoubtedly influence the child’s reac-
tions to socialization experiences” (p. 34). More recently, Eisenberg (2004)
examined the input of genetic inheritance as well as the family environment
in the development of prosocial behavior.
96 Catholic Education/September 2006
knowledge and skills necessary to function effectively and meet their own
needs within the social community (Battistich, Schaps, et al., 1991, p. 94).
This socialization process is facilitated by providing the students with oppor-
tunities to experience positive relationships, and to develop the skills and
knowledge to function competently in school and in the larger society.
Battistich, Schaps, Solomon, and Watson (1991) felt that socialization
“is a constructivist process of active accommodation to the social environ-
ment that is pursued by the child in order to become increasingly
autonomous and effective” (p. 98). This suggests that schools can best devel-
op sociomoral values when they provide students with opportunities to expe-
rience positive relationships with adults and peers while they develop the
knowledge and skills necessary to function in the school and societal envi-
ronment.
the goal is similar—the socially and emotionally balanced student who will
be an asset to society—the focus of each program is slightly different, each
built on a slightly different foundation. Some approach this goal from the
direction of values and morals, while others focus on the end point of a spe-
cific behavior, prosocial behavior.
REFERENCES
Allport, G., & Ross, J. (1967). Personal religious orientation and prejudice. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 5, 432-443.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Batson, C., & Schoenrade, P. (1991). Measuring religion as quest: 1) Validity concerns. Journal for
the Scientific Study of Religion, 30, 416-429.
Batson, C., Schoenrade, P., & Ventis, W. (1993). Religion and the individual. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Batson, C., & Ventis, L. (1982). The religious experience: A social-psychological perspective. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Battistich, V., Schaps, E., Solomon, D., & Watson, M. (1991). The role of the school in prosocial
development. In H. Fitzgerald (Ed.), Theory and research in behavioral pediatrics (pp. 89-
127). New York: Plenum Press.
102 Catholic Education/September 2006
Battistich, V., Watson, M., Solomon, D., Schaps, E., & Solomon, J. (1991). The Child Development
Project: A comprehensive program for the development of prosocial behavior. In Handbook
of moral behavior and development (Vol. 1, pp. 1-33). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Beit-Hallahmi, B., & Argyle, M. (1997). The psychology of religious behavior, belief and experi-
ence. New York: Routledge.
Bergan, A., & McConatha, J. T. (2000). Religiosity and life satisfaction. Activities, Adaptation and
Aging, 24(3), 23-34.
Blum, L. (1980). Friendship, altruism and morality. London: Routledge & Kegan.
Boyatzis, C. A. (2005). Religion and spiritual development in childhood. In R. F. Paloutzian & C.
L. Park (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of religion and spirituality (pp. 123-143). New
York: Guilford Press.
Cardwell, J. D. (1980). The social context of religiosity. Lanham, MD: University Press of America.
Chumbler, N. R. (1996). An empirical test of a theory of factors affecting life satisfaction:
Understanding the role of religious experience. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 24, 220-232.
Congregation for the Clergy. (1997). General directory for catechesis. Washington, DC: United
States Catholic Conference.
Demerath, N. J., III, & Hammond, P. E. (1969). Religion in social context. New York: Random House.
Dezutter, J., Soenens, B., & Hutsebaut, D. (2006). Religiosity and mental health: A further explo-
ration of the relative importance of religious behaviors vs. religious attitudes. Personality and
Individual Differences, 40(4), 807-818.
Durkheim, E. (1961). Moral education. New York: Free Press. (Original work published 1925)
Eisenberg, N. (1982). The development of prosocial behavior. New York: Academic Press.
Eisenberg, N. (1987). The relation of altruism and other moral behaviors to moral cognition:
Methodological and conceptual issues. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Contemporary topics in devel-
opmental psychology (pp. 165-189). New York: Wiley.
Eisenberg, N. (2004). Prosocial and moral development in the family. In T. A.Thorkildsen & H. J.
Walberg (Eds.), Nurturing morality (pp. 119-135). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum
Publishers.
Eisenberg, N., & Mussen, P. H. (1989). The roots of prosocial behavior in children. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Eisenberg, N., & Valiente, C. (2002). Children’s prosocial and moral development. In M. Bornstein
(Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 5 (2nd ed., pp. 111-142). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Ellison, C. (1991). Religious involvement and subjective well-being. Journal of Health and Social
Behavior, 32(1), 80-99.
Ellison, C., Gay, D., & Glass, T. (1989). Does religious commitment contribute to individual life
satisfaction? Social Forces, 68, 100-123.
Fontaine, J. R. J., Duriez, B., Luyten, P., Corveleyn, J., & Hutsebaut, D. (2005). Consequences of a
multidimensional approach to religion for the relationship between religiosity and value pri-
orities. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 15(2), 123-143.
Frankena, W. K. (1965). Three historical philosophies of education. Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman.
Freud, S. (1976). The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (J.
Strachey, Ed.). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1923)
Fukuyama, Y. (1960). The major dimensions of church membership. Review of Religious Research,
2, 154-161.
Gibbs, J., & Schnell, S. (1985). Moral development “versus” socialization. American Psychologist,
40, 1071-1080.
Glock, C. Y., & Stark, R. (1965). Religion and society in tension. San Francisco: Rand McNally.
Graczyk, P., Weissberg, R., Payton, J., Elias, M., Greenberg, M., & Zins, J. (2000). Criteria for eval-
uating the quality of school-based social and emotional learning programs. In R. Bar-On & J.
D. Parker (Eds.), Handbook of emotional intelligence: Theory, development, assessment, and
application at home, school and in the workplace (pp. 391-410). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Groome, T. H. (1980). Christian religious education. San Francisco: Harper & Row.
Groome, T. H. (1998). Educating for life. Allen, TX: Thomas Moore Press
Holdcroft/WHAT IS RELIGIOSITY? 103
Groome, T. H., & Corso, M. J. (1999). Empowering catechetical leaders. Washington, DC: National
Catholic Educational Association.
Hardy, S. A., & Carlo, G. (2005). Religiosity and prosocial behaviors in adolescence: The mediat-
ing role of prosocial values. Journal of Moral Education, 34(2), 231-249.
Hoffman, J. (1985). Pastoral letter on Catholic schools. Toledo, OH: Diocese of Toledo.
Hyde, K. H. (1990). Religion in childhood and adolescence. Birmingham, AL: Religious Education
Press.
Ji, C. C. (2004). Religious orientations in moral development. Journal of Psychology and
Christianity, 23(1), 22-30.
Kant, I. (1959). Foundations of the metaphysics of morals (L. W. Beck, Trans.). New York: Macmillan.
Kedem, P., & Cohen, D. W. (1987). The effects of religious education on moral judgment. Journal
of Psychology and Judaism, 11, 4-14.
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence, the cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In
D. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347-480). New York:
Rand McNally.
Kohlberg, L. (1976). Moral stages and moralization: The cognitive-developmental approach. In T.
Lickona (Ed.), Moral development and behavior: Theory, research, and social issues (pp. 31-
53). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Kohlberg, L. (1981). The meaning and measurement of moral development. Worcester, MA: Clark
University Press.
Kristensen, K., Pedersen, D., & Williams, R. (2001). Profiling religious maturity: The relationship
of religious attitude components to religious orientations. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 12, 75-86.
Lee, J. (1985). The content of religious instruction. Birmingham, AL: Religious Education Press.
Lenski, G. (1963). The religious factor. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
Lewis, N. (1978). The new Roget’s thesaurus in dictionary form. New York: Putnam.
Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World. (Original
work published 1932)
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child (H. Weaver, Trans.). New York: Basic
Books.
Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development. New York: Praeger.
Roccas, S. (2005). Religion and value systems. Journal of Social Issues, 61(4), 747-759.
Schuttloffel, M. (2000). Promises and possibilities: The Catholic elementary school curriculum. In
J. Youniss, J. Convey, & J. A. McLellan (Eds.), The character of Catholic schools (pp. 103-
123). Notre Dame, IN: The University of Notre Dame Press.
Sealey, J. (1985). Religious education: Philosophical perspectives. Boston: George Allen University.
Solomon, D., Watson, M., Schaps, E., Battistich, V., & Solomon, J. (1990). Cooperative learning as
part of a comprehensive classroom program designed to promote prosocial behavior. In E.
Sharan (Ed.), Cooperative learning: Theory and research (pp. 231-260). New York: Praeger.
Stark, R., & Glock, C. Y. (1968). American piety: The nature of religious commitment. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Walker, L. J. (2003) Morality, religion, spirituality—the value of saintliness. Journal of Moral
Education, 32(4), 373-384
Walker, L. J., Pitts, R. C., Hennig, K. H., & Matsuba, M. K. (1999). Reasoning about morality and
real-life moral problems. In M. Killen & D. Hart (Eds.), Morality in everyday life:
Development perspectives (pp. 371-407). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Watson, P., Hood, R., Morris, R., & Hall, J. (1984). Empathy, religious orientation, and social desir-
ability. Journal of Psychology, 117, 211-216.
Barbara Holdcroft teaches at The University of Toledo, Lourdes College, and St. Francis de Sales
High School. Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to Dr. Barbara Holdcroft, The
University of Toledo, 2801 W. Bancroft St., Foundations of Education Department, Mail Stop 923,
Toledo, OH 43606.