Syntax Ii Week 2: Types and Positions of Complement Clauses
Syntax Ii Week 2: Types and Positions of Complement Clauses
Syntax Ii Week 2: Types and Positions of Complement Clauses
Week 3: That-clauses
- That-clauses in the post-predicate position are by far the most common type of that-
clause – they account for 80% of all that-clauses. They typically report the speech and
thoughts of humans. The subject of the main clause usually refers to a person, the main
verb clause presents the type of reporting (e.g. speech or thought), and the that-clause
presents the reported speech or thought.
I think Stuart’s gone a bit mad.
- Each of the other types of that-clauses has particular functions in discourse also.
- There are three grammatical patterns for verbs that take a that-clause in post-predicate
position:
a) Pattern 1: verb + that-clause (e.g. agree, ask)
I didn’t agree that he should be compelled to do singing.
b) Pattern 2: verb + NP + that-clause (e.g. tell, persuade)
I persuaded [myself] that something awful might happen.
c) Pattern 3: verb + to NP + that-clause (e.g. suggest)
I suggested [to Miss Kerrison] that she sit down on the chair and wait.
- Ten verbs are especially common controlling that post-predicate clauses: think, say,
know, see, find, believe, feel, suggest, show and guess (in American English
conversation).
- Mental verbs are very common with that-clauses, reporting various mental states and
attitudes (Example: I think we picked it.)
- Speech act and other communication verbs: the second common use of that-clauses is to
report what someone said. (Example: She said that it’s lovely to wear.)
- These verbs reflect the primary function of that-clauses for reporting thoughts and
speech.
- Many of the verb + that-clause combinations also convey stance.
- Subject-position and extraposed that-clauses are much less common than post predicate
that-clauses.
- For verbs, only be is common controlling extraposed that-clauses.
- The adjectives that control that-clauses all covey stance.
- They fall into three major categories:
a) Degrees of certainty (e.g. certain, confident, evident)
b) Affective states (e.g. annoyed, glad, sad)
c) Evaluations (e.g. appropriate, odd, good, important, advisable)
- Extraposed that-clauses are far more common than subject that-clauses.
- As a result, when subject that-clauses are used, they serve special discourse functions.
- When subject that-clauses are used, they usually conform to particular characteristics of
register (subject that-clauses are rare in all registers), grammatical complexity (in almost
every case where a subject that-clause is used, it presents information that is already
presupposed as factual or generally accepted), and information structure (the main clause
predicate has a complex constructions with many phrases or clauses).
- In addition, some topics and individual writers favour subject that-clauses.
Week 4: Wh-clauses
- There are three basic types of wh-complement clauses:
a) Interrogative clauses: are used with verbs such as ask and wonder to present an
indirect question.
Jill was asking what happened.
b) Nominal relative clauses: can be paraphrased with a general head noun + relative
clause
What baffles me is how few of them can spell/The thing that baffles me is…
c) Exclamatives: they begin with how + adjective or with what as a predeterminer.
He still remembered how wonderful it had been.
- Wh-clauses can be complements of verbs, adjectives, or prepositions.
- With verbs, wh-clauses often occur in object position
You give him what he wants.
- Wh-clauses can also follow adjectival predicatives
She wanted to be careful what she said.
- Wh-clauses can be the complement of a preposition or the object of a prepositional verb
She was amazed at how exhausted she was.
- Wh-complements can occur with or without a noun phrase between the verb and wh-
clause.
- The most common verbs controlling a wh-clause fall into four major semantic domains:
mental, speech, other communication, and perception.
- There are two important grammatical patterns used with wh-complement clauses in post-
predicate position
a) Pattern 1: verb + wh-clause (e.g. know, remember, see)
I don’t know what they are.
I can’t remember how I used to be
b) Pattern 2: verb + NP + wh-clause (e.g. ask, show, tell)
I didn’t tell [you] what Emma thought.
I want you to show [me] where the car went off.
- There are 5 major patterns for post-predicate infinitive clauses following a verb:
a) Pattern 1: verb + to-clause (e.g. try, hope) - most common type.
I’m just trying to get away early.
b) Pattern 2: verb + NP + to-clause (e.g. tell, believe, enable)
It enables [the farmer] to maintain uniform.
Pattern 2P (corresponding passive form): be + verb-ed + to-clause
Heavy fighting with the government troops was expected to break out soon.
c) Pattern 3: verb + for NP + to-clause (e.g. ask, love, arrange)
Hire a van and wait for [Mrs Jones] to arrive.
d) Pattern 4: verb + bare infinitive clause (e.g. dare, help, let)
The police didn’t dare touch them because of the United Nations.
e) Pattern 5: verb + NP + bare infinitive clause (e.g. have, feel, make, help)
I'll have [Judy] do it.
He actually felt [the sweat] break out on his forehead.
- The patterns vary in the way noun phrases come between the verb and to-clause, and in
the choice of to or bare infinitive.
- Some factors to consider in the grammatical patterns with to-clauses are:
passive alternatives, the implied subject of the to-clause, the relationship between the
object of the main clause and the to-clause, and prepositional verbs.
- The semantic category of a verb often influences the grammatical patterns that are
possible for that verb.
- The verbs that are common with to-clauses vary across registers and cover many
semantic categories.
- Conversation uses far fewer verbs commonly; most of those are verbs of desire (e.g.
want, like), plus the verbs seem and try.
- The combination want + to-clause is extremely common in conversation.
- The other registers have more variation, including more verbs of effort (e.g. try), of
aspect (e.g. begin, continue), of probability (e.g. tend) and of causation (e.g. allow).
- Subject predicative to-clauses have 4 major functions:
a) introducing points
b) introducing objectives or plans
c) introducing methodologies
d) making a balanced structure when a to-clause is also the subject of the main
clause
- Likely/unlikely is the only adjective that commonly controls a to-clause.
- Many other adjectives occur, conveying certainty, ability, stance, ease/difficulty, or
evaluation
- There are three major patterns for ing-complement clauses in post-predicate position
(with and without a NP between the verb and the ing-clause):
a) Pattern 1: verb + ing-clause (e.g. begin, remember):
He began paging through old newspapers.
I remember reading this book.
b) Pattern 2: verb + NP + ing-clause (e.g. see, find)
When you see [a geek] walking down the street, give it a good throw.
Don’t be surprised to find [me] sitting on the tee in the lotus position.
c) Pattern 2a: verb + possessive determiner + ing-clause:
Did you mind [me] saying it, Stephen?
And maybe you won’t mind [my] saying that you’re getting a little old for
studying.
d) Pattern 3: prepositional verb + ing-clause:
We were thinking of bringing our video camera.
She complained of feeling feverish and went early to bed.
- Verbs of aspect and manner are the most common verbs controlling ing-clauses,
especially in conversation and fiction.
- The verb keep shows that the action in the ing-clause is continuous or repeated.
- The verbs start and stop show the beginning or ending of an activity.
- Most adjectives that control ing-clauses express a personal feeling or attitude, or some
evaluation of the idea in the ing-clause (e.g. sorry, afraid, capable).
- In most cases, the controlling adjective is followed by a preposition (e.g. sorry about,
afraid of, good at, useful in).