Decision Making Skills of High Performance Youth Soccer Players
Decision Making Skills of High Performance Youth Soccer Players
Decision Making Skills of High Performance Youth Soccer Players
Ger J Exerc Sport Res 2021 · 51:102–111 Dennis Murr1 · Paul Larkin2 · Oliver Höner1
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12662-020-00687-2 1
Institute of Sports Science, Eberhard Karls University, Tübingen, Germany
Received: 14 August 2019 2
Institute for Health and Sport, Victoria University, Melbourne, Australia
Accepted: 15 October 2020
Published online: 26 November 2020
© The Author(s) 2020
Decision-making skills of high-
performance youth soccer
players
Validating a video-based diagnostic
instrument with a soccer-specific motor
response
Introduction knowledge about a (current) situation players (e.g., Ruiz Pérez et al., 2014) re-
to select an appropriate decision, based search is scarce within talent promotion
In team sports like soccer, a multidimen- on one’s perceived ability to execute programs (e.g., regional association or
sional spectrum of performance factors is a context-specific motor skill. From youth national teams; youth academies)
required to perform at the elite level. This a sporting perspective, the ability to with high-performance level (e.g., elite)
has been acknowledged by Williams and make the correct decision during com- players. This statement is emphasized in
Reilly (2000) who developed a heuristic plex game situations, under high game a recent meta-analysis which explored
model for the categorization of soccer pressure and time constraints is a key cognitive functions measurements with
talent predictors. The model identifies component of in-game performance performance level as the moderator vari-
potential talent predictors across four (Höner, Larkin, Leber, & Feichtinger, able (Scharfen & Memmert, 2019). The
core areas of sport science, including 2020). Thus, decision-making has been results indicated, high-performance level
physical, physiological, psychological shown to be an important skill, with athletes had superior performance, with
and sociological characteristics. While several cross-sectional studies assess- a small to medium effect size, compared
there seems to be an emphasis on phys- ing decision-making performance and to low-performance level athletes. While
iological and physical characteristics in demonstrating that decision-making there is a plethora of studies examining
research and practice (Johnston, Wattie, skills discriminate between skilled and known group differences, a potential
Schorer, & Baker, 2018; Wilson et al., less-skilled players in team sports (e.g., reason for the lack of research examin-
2016), recently, there has been increased Diaz, Gonzalez, Garcia, & Mitchell, ing homogenous samples could lie in the
interest in the psychological attributes, 2011; Lorains, Ball, & MacMahon, 2013; fact that it is more difficult to find large
such as perceptual-cognitive factors Woods, Raynor, Bruce, & McDonald, effect sizes for discriminating athletes
(Mann, Dehghansai, & Baker, 2017). 2016). With respect to soccer, Ruiz of a similar ability (Bergkamp, Niessen,
Researchers have highlighted the im- Pérez et al. (2014) found Spanish club den Hartigh, Frencken, & Meijer, 2019).
portance of perceptual-cognitive factors players with international experience Despite the fact researchers have used
for skilled performance, with findings demonstrated better decision-making cross-sectional approaches to discrimi-
showing highly skilled players possess performance in comparison to local nate between high- and low-performance
superior decision-making, anticipation level players. Further, Höner (2005) decision-makers, Murr, Feichtinger,
and situational probability proficiency found youth national players had su- Larkin, O’Connor, and Höner (2018)
compared to their less-skilled coun- perior decision-making skills compared highlighted in their systematic review
terparts (e.g., Lex, Essig, Knoblauch, to local youth players, and additionally that there is a significant gap in the
& Schack, 2015; Ward, Ericsson, & older players (i.e., U17 age group) had literature concerning empirical evidence
Williams, 2013). a significant decision-making perfor- related to the predictive value of decision-
The focus of the current study is mance advantage over younger players making assessments. According to this
decision-making as the cognitive perfor- (i.e., U15 age group). While researchers review, only one investigation has used
mance factor. Causer and Ford (2014) have used an expertise approach to a video-based assessment to examine
define decision-making in sports as highlight superior performance of ex- the predictive ability of decision-making
a cognitive process in which one uses pert/elite players over novice/nonelite skills in soccer. O’Connor, Larkin, and
4 H1a: U17 soccer players demonstrate Ger J Exerc Sport Res 2021 · 51:102–111 https://doi.org/10.1007/s12662-020-00687-2
better decision-making skills than © The Author(s) 2020
U16 players
4 H1b: U19 soccer players demonstrate D. Murr · P. Larkin · O. Höner
better decision-making skills than Decision-making skills of high-performance youth soccer players.
U16 players Validating a video-based diagnostic instrument with a soccer-
4 H1c: U19 soccer players demonstrate specific motor response
better decision-making skills than
U17 players Abstract
Objectives. This study aimed to develop players (U17 > U16 in SCbu: Φ = 0.24 and SCoff:
a valid video-based diagnostic instrument Φ = 0.39, p < 0.01; U19 > U16 in SCbu: Φ = 0.41
H2 Players who played more minutes that assesses decision-making with a sport- and SCoff: Φ = 0.46, p < 0.01); however, there
in official matches of the current sea- specific motor response. was no difference between U17 and U19
son demonstrate better decision-making Methods. A total of 86 German youth aca- players. Furthermore, the predictive value of
skills than players with less minutes. demy players (16.7 ± 0.9 years) viewed game the test indicates that future youth national
situations projected on a large video screen team players make better decisions with
and were required to make a decision by respect to the build-up category (SCbu:
Prognostic validity: dribbling and passing to one of three targets Φ = 0.20; p < 0.05), whereas playing position
(representing different decision options). did not significantly influence decision-
H3 Future youth national team play- The test included 48 clips separated into making competence.
ers demonstrate better decision-making two categories: build-up (bu) and offensive Conclusion. Results indicate the video-based
decisions (off). Criterion-related validity was decision-making diagnostic instrument can
skills than nonselected players.
tested based on age (i.e., U16, U17, and discriminate decision-making competence
Finally, in an explorative analysis it U19), playing status (i.e., minutes played in within a high-performance youth group.
was examined whether decision-making official matches of the current season) and The outcomes associated with national
performance is influenced by playing po- in a prospective approach relating to future youth team participation demonstrate the
sition (Objective III). youth national team status (i.e., selected or predictive value of the diagnostic instrument.
nonselected). Finally, it was investigated This study provides initial evidence to suggest
whether decision-making competence was a new video-based diagnostic instrument
Methods influenced by playing position (i.e., defenders with a soccer-specific motor response can be
vs. midfielders vs. forwards). used within a talent identification process to
Sample and design Results. Instrumental reliability demonstrated assist with assessment of decision-making
satisfactory values for SCbu (r = 0.72), and performance.
The study sample consisted of 86 youth lower for SCoff (r = 0.56). Results showed
the diagnostic instrument is suitable for Keywords
academy players, born between 1996 and discriminating between playing status (SCbu: Football · Talent identification · Adolescence ·
2001, from a professional German soc- Φ = 0.22, p < 0.01; SCoff: Φ = 0.14, p < 0.05) Perceptual cognitive factors · Athletic
cer club. The players competed in the and between younger (U16) and older performance
highest German youth league, and thus
belong to the top 1% of German players
for their age groups (i.e., U16, U17, and
U19). At the first measurement point the dent variable age group (H1) were ex- of all 86 players were captured. Play-
players were 15–19 years of age (median amined splitting the sample into three ers who participated at least in one Ger-
[Mage] = 16.7 years, standard deviation age categories (U16: N = 41, U17: N = 55 man youth national team training course
[SDage] = 0.96) and were tested over a 3- and U19: N = 44; H1a–H1c). A further in subsequent seasons (i.e., 2015/2016;
year period (i.e., near the end of seasons analysis of the criterion-related validity 2016/2017; 2017/2018) were identified as
2014/15 to 2016/17), resulting in three was conducted on the independent vari- selected (N = 16), with all other players
measurement points (T2015, T2016, and able playing status (H2) that was deter- identified as nonselected (N = 70). Fur-
T2017). Over the 3-year period, a total mined based on minutes played in of- thermore, with respect to the explorative
of 140 data points were collected. Due to ficial matches (i.e., U19/U17 German analysis (Objective III), players were sep-
the nature of professional youth academy Youth Bundesliga and U16 Oberliga). arated by playing positions as defined by
selection processes (i.e., some players are Median split procedure was utilized to their respective coaches (i.e., defenders
deselected, and new players are recruited separate players into two categories: first [DF, N = 55]; midfielders [MF, N = 61];
to the academy) which resulted in not ev- team regular (i.e., who play more minutes and forwards [FW, N = 24]).
ery player being tested at all three time than median; N = 70) and reserve play-
points (. Table 1). ers (i.e., who play less minutes than the Decision-making diagnostic
To examine the three hypothesizes, median; N = 68). Further, to examine the instrument
the study sample was separated into three predictive value of the decision-making Stimulus materials Decision-making
subsamples. To assess diagnostic crite- diagnostic instrument relative to future competence was assessed using a newly
rion-related validity (Objective II), po- youth national team status (H3), data of developed soccer-specific video-based
tential differences between the indepen- the respective first measurement point diagnostic instrument. To create the
Φ
SCbu 73 ± 11 65 ± 16 0.20*
SCoff 69 ± 11 66 ± 13 0.08
Playing positiona DF MF FW DF vs. MF DF vs. FW MF vs. FW
(N = 55) (N = 61) (N = 24)
Φ ω
SCbu 71 ± 16 72 ± 13 63 ± 17 0.00 0.21 0.25* 0.44#
SCoff 70 ± 16 73 ± 13 69 ± 9 0.07 0.07 0.15 0.15
FTRP First team regular player, RP Reserve player, DF Defenders, MF Midfielders, FW Forwards, SCbu Decision accuracy score for build-up, SCoff Decision
accuracy score for offensive
**p < 0.01, *p < 0.05; # p < 0.10
a
Playing position correlated significantly with the number of measurement points. However, ANCOVAs (analysis of covariance) with the main factor playing
position and the covariate number of measurement points demonstrated no changes in the statistical decisions regarding the multiple group comparison.
Thus, the results pattern for playing position is independent of the number of measurement points
model indicated that a one standard up score improved the chance of being tical decisions regarding the multiple
deviation (SD = 0.153) increase in build- selected for a future youth national team group comparisons of playing position.
up score, improved the chance of being by a factor of 1.89 (= (eβ)0.153 = 64.200.153).
a first-team regular player by a factor of Regarding playing position (Objec- Discussion
1.65 (= (e β )0.153 = 26.690.153). tive III), the results for the different
With respect to the prognostic va- subsamples (i.e. DF, MF, FW) in both The aim of this study was to develop
lidity (H3), Mann–Whitney-U-test build-up and offensive decision category a valid video-based decision-making
demonstrated that selected players ranged in mean from 69–72% with only diagnostic instrument which presented
have a higher decision-making accu- one exception (i.e., the result of 63% footage from a first-person perspective
racy than nonselected players in build- from FW deviated in their nonposition- and incorporated a soccer-specific mo-
up (Z = 1.82, Φ = 0.20, p < 0.05) and of- specific build-up category compared to tor response. A further strength of the
fensive (Z = 0.78, p = 0.217) situations; other playing positions distinctly). De- study was the video stimuli were not
however, only build-up situations pro- spite these facts, the Kruskal–Wallis tests limited to one game situation, but rather
vided a significant difference. Analyzing did not reveal significant differences in different situations (i.e., build-up and of-
the prediction of future youth national any decision-making competence (SCbu: fensive decisions). While there were no
team status with regard to the logistic H(2) = 5.26, p = 0.072; SCoff: H(2) = 1.81, differences between playing positions,
regression models led to the same re- p = 0.404). In terms of possible influ- the study does provide a foundation
sult as for the playing status variable. ences caused by correlations between for future research. In particular, the
Only build-up score demonstrated sig- the manifest variable, the number of diagnostic instrument was developed as
nificance and therefore remained in the measurement points, and the indepen- a measure to discriminate decision-mak-
model (χ2(1) = 4.15, p < 0.05). A one dent variable playing position, ANCOVA ing competence within a group of youth
standard deviation increases in build- demonstrated no changes in the statis- high-performance level players. Results
showed the diagnostic instrument, which these situations by exchanging current enough to differentiate performance of
included realistic soccer video-scenes in lower reliability clips with newly devel- high-performing late adolescent athletes
combination with a soccer-specific mo- oped higher reliability clips. Also adding requires further investigation. A possi-
tor response, is a suitable instrument more items to the diagnostic instrument ble explanation as to why there was no
for discriminating playing status and could be an approach for improving significant difference between the U17
partially for age (U19 and U17 > U16). the reliability and even reduce the need and U19 players (H1c) could be due to
Further, it provides a more representa- for large sample sizes (Schweizer et al., at least two possible reasons. First, from
tive assessment compared to previous 2020). the perspective of the U19 team, the
studies where the diagnostic instrument This study advances current sport- best players from the squad had already
was limited by a lack of a soccer-specific based decision-making literature by played for the senior professional team
response (e.g., Bennett, Novak, Pluss, demonstrating the ability to develop (i.e., in either the first or reserve/second
Coutts, & Fransen, 2019). An additional a decision-making diagnostic instru- team) and therefore were not able to
aim was to examine the predictive value ment in which the process of visual participate in the investigation. In rela-
of the new video-based decision-making perception of in-game video-scenes (i.e., tion to the U17 group, two of the three
diagnostic instrument, with the findings first person perspective) is more chal- U17 cohort squads in this study were
indicating that in parts of the study (i.e., lenging by an additional soccer-specific very successful and participated in the
in build-up categories) future youth na- motor action (i.e., dribbling) and re- finals of the German U17 championship
tional team players perform better in the sponse (i.e. passing). Traditional video- (i.e., the highest level of competition at
decision-making diagnostic instrument. based instruments which assess decision- this age). Therefore, it seems feasible
Using reliable diagnostic instruments making performance generally present that despite the age difference with re-
is fundamental to scientific work. The footage from a broadcast (i.e., television spect to U19 group, both groups were of
implications associated with using a di- broadcast) or third person perspective a very similar performance level (limit-
agnostic instrument with a lack of, or associated with nonsoccer-specific de- ing the studies’ internal reliability with
unknown, reliability is whether the par- cision response (i.e., written, verbal or regard to discriminating between age
ticipant performance differences are due button response). In this context, Mann, groups). Second, this finding supports
to random test error or actual per- Farrow, Shuttleworth, Hopwood, and other studies to investigate age-related
formance changes of the participants MacMahon (2009) indicated using a di- soccer-specific performance skill differ-
(Gadotti, Vieira, & Magee, 2006). In agnostic instrument from first person- ences. For example, Huijgen, Elferink-
comparison to the assessment of manifest perspective is more realistic. However, Gemser, Post, and Visscher (2010) high-
variables (e.g., time, height, distance), it was noted this may be more difficult lighted there were no improvements
the measurement of latent constructs from a decision-making perspective than from ages 16 to 19 in dribbling and
such as decision-making competence using footage from an aerial perspective, sprinting, which the authors attributed
poses a much larger challenge. While and therefore may provide more robust to the end of puberty. Furthermore,
researchers have highlighted the limited diagnostic instruments. Furthermore, it Beavan et al. (2019) revealed in a battery
reporting of reliability for new diagnos- has been suggested the lack of sport-spe- of cognitive function tests (e.g., Vienna
tic instruments (Hadlow et al., 2018; cific responses may limit the correlation Test System, which measures inhibition
Schweizer, Furley, Rost, & Barth, 2020), between video-based tests and actual and cognitive flexibility) that there were
a key aim of the current study was to in-game decision-making performance no differences between U19 and U17 age
measure the reliability of the latent con- (e.g., van Maarseveen et al., 2016). This groups. At this stage, further research
struct decision-making (Objective I). is further supported by Travassos et al. is needed to explore whether there are
Here, an adequate level of reliability (2013) who indicated that perceptual- peak developmental phases for cognitive
for the build-up situations was found; cognitive assessments need to consider factors such as decision-making.
however, the lower reliability for offen- the task representativeness when de- A strength of this study is that the
sive situations has to be considered as veloping sport-specific diagnostic in- sample consisted of participants from
a larger limitation. A potential reason struments. By developing assessments, one of the most successful youth soccer
for the lower reliability of the offensive which consider representative task con- academies in Germany, thus, result-
situations could be due to the restric- straints, such as presenting decision- ing in a very homogeneous high-level
tion of range phenomenon within the making situations from a first-person group. This is not a common approach
high-performance level group (i.e., ho- perspective, combined with a sport- used by researchers investigating soccer-
mogeneous expert samples) the study specific motor response, enhances the specific decision-making skills as they
was conducted (Schweizer et al., 2020). validity of the assessment measure. have generally conducted cross-sectional
Despite this limitation, the current study While the current study demonstrated studies comparing and identifying skill
does provide a method for establishing the ability to differentiate U16 and older differences between groups of high-
the reliability of the offensive decision- adolescent athletes (i.e., U17 and U19, performance and low-performance level
making situations. In addition, future objective II), developing a decision- participants (i.e., sub-elite; intermediate;
studies may improve the reliability of making diagnostic instrument sensitive novice participants; Diaz et al., 2011;
age group to ensure a true representation formance youth group, in particular for Funding. Open Access funding enabled and orga-
nized by Projekt DEAL.
of the performance group. Finally, sim- playing status and to some extent age.
ilar to other studies (e.g., Höner, 2005), Although the reliability of the diagnostic
this study limited the number of options instrument is satisfied for the build-up Compliance with ethical
(i.e., three option per situation) and the category, future research should aim for guidelines
sport-specific motor response (i.e., play- higher reliability for all measured vari-
ers only had the options to pass or shoot). ables (e.g., offensive decision), by using Conflict of interest. D. Murr, P. Larkin and O. Höner
Future studies should also consider the the guidelines proposed, for example, declare that they have no competing interests.
development of diagnostic instruments by Schweizer et al. (2020). Further, the
All procedures performed in studies involving human
which include other decision-making op- results demonstrate the predictive value participants were in accordance with the ethical stan-
tions, such as dribbling. While observa- of the new video-based decision-making dards of the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later
tional studies (e.g., recording decision- diagnostic instrument, especially the amendments or comparable ethical standards. The
ethics department of the Faculty of Economics and
making skills in small-sided games) such build-up situation for national youth Social Sciences at the University of Tübingen and the
as Romeas, Guldner, and Faubert (2016) team selection. However, future investi- youth academy of the professional soccer club ap-
have attempted to assess the whole spec- gations with a longer prognostic period proved the implementation of this study. All players
and legal guardians (i.e., parents) provided informed
trum of decision-making alternatives, it (e.g., up to senior level) is of interest to consent prior to participation in the study.
is a challenge developing such situations strengthen the results. With respect to
within a video stimuli. A possible alter- playing positions, no differences were Open Access. This article is licensed under a Creative
Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which
native maybe developing diagnostic in- found between positions; however, fur- permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and re-
struments that provide more graduations ther examination of playing position on production in any medium or format, as long as you
of decisions (i.e., best option; second best decision-making skills is warranted (e.g., give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and
the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons li-
option; etc.), rather than current binary are midfielder better decision-makers re- cence, and indicate if changes were made. The images
forms (e.g., Bennett et al., 2019). Further, gardless on which area of the pitch the or other third party material in this article are included
off-the-ball decision-making in defensive decision-making skill is required?). in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless in-
dicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If
game situations such as positioning close Considering these results, this study material is not included in the article’s Creative Com-
or far from the ball, which has not been provides initial evidence to suggest a soc- mons licence and your intended use is not permitted
assessed in the literature, should also be cer-specific video-based diagnostic in- by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use,
you will need to obtain permission directly from the
considered in future research. strument can be used within talent identi- copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit
fication processes to assist with the assess- http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Conclusion ment of players’ decision-making perfor-
mance. Finally, future decision-making
Sport-based decision-making is a cogni- assessment studies can use similar proce- References
tive process in which athletes use their dures to those employed in the current in-
knowledge about a (current) situation to vestigation to develop a valid and reliable Ackerman, P. L. (2014). Nonsense, common sense,
and science of expert performance: talent and
select an appropriate decision based on video-based decision-making diagnostic individual differences. Intelligence, 45, 6–17.
their perceived ability to execute a sport- instrument in other sports/domains. In https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.04.009.
specific motor skill response (Causer & addition, further exploration of the inte- Beavan, A. F., Spielmann, J., Mayer, J., Skorski, S.,
Meyer, T., & Fransen, J. (2019). Age-related
Ford, 2014). While decision-making is gration of motor specific responses with differences in executive functions within high-
defined by the ability to perceive appro- the video footage to create an even more level youth soccer players. Brazilian Journal of
priate stimuli and execute a sport-specific realistic diagnostic instrument may be Motor Behavior, 13(2), 64–75.
Bennett, K. J. M., Novak, A. R., Pluss, M. A., Coutts, A. J.,
skill response, traditional video-based possible. One approach would be to use & Fransen, J. (2019). Assessing the validity of
instruments assess decision-making us- virtual reality technology to allow body a video-based decision-making assessment for
ing nonsport-specific responses such as parts (e.g., foot) interacting with the stim- talent identification in youth soccer. Journal of
Science and Medicine in Sport, 22(6), 729–734.
verbal, written, or button responses (e.g., ulus material or to measure a player’s in https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2018.12.011.
Roca et al., 2012). This study addressed situ task constraints. Bergkamp, T. L. G., Niessen, A. S. M., den Hartigh, R. J. R.,
this limitation by developing a valid Frencken, W. G. P., & Meijer, R. R. (2019). Method-
ological issues in soccer talent identification
first-person perspective video-based Corresponding address research. Sports Medicine. https://doi.org/10.
diagnostic instrument which requires 1007/s40279-019-01113-w.
a soccer-specific motor response (i.e., Dennis Murr Bonney, N., Berry, J., Ball, K., & Larkin, P. (2019).
Institute of Sports Science, Australian football skill-based assessments: a
participants dribble with the ball while proposed model for future research. Frontiers
Eberhard Karls University
watching a video stimulus and then exe- Tübingen, Germany in Psychology, 10, 429–429. https://doi.org/10.
cute their decision by passing to a player 3389/fpsyg.2019.00429.
dennis.murr@uni-
Boone, J., Vaeyens, R., Steyaert, A., Vanden Bossche,
in the video). The results indicate the tuebingen.de L., & Bourgois, J. (2012). Physical fitness of
© Mario
video-based decision-making diagnostic elite Belgian soccer players by player position.
Heilemann Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research,
instrument can discriminate decision-
making competence within a high-per-