Aznar Torrents Nelson P&N
Aznar Torrents Nelson P&N
Aznar Torrents Nelson P&N
DOI: 10.3922/j.psns.2008.2.009
NEUROSCIENCE
Abstract
A great deal of studies using different visual tasks (e.g., Vernier acuity tasks, tilt illusion, crowding, etc) have revealed that
our perception is strongly influenced by the orientation of the stimulus. Most studies have investigated visual acuity in two-
dimensional visual spaces (2D) but little is known about the effect of line orientation in depth perception (3D). In one experiment,
Vernier Acuity (VA) in frontoparallel (2D) and medial (3D) planes was investigated. We used a virtual reality setup inducing
inter-ocular disparities to simulate a 3D visual space, and a common computer screen to present stimuli in the frontal plane. In
the experiment, by using the method of constant stimuli, the observer compared VA in the 2D and 3D visual spaces as a function
of the stimulus orientation. Results showed that only judgments in the 3D condition were affected by the well-known ‘oblique
effect’, and some impairment in stereoacuity (lines in depth plane) in comparison to 2D acuity (lines in frontal plane) was
observed. We attributed the cause for such deterioration in stereoacuity to changes in vertical disparities. Keywords: binocular
vision, depth perception, stereoacuity, vernier acuity.
Received 13 December 2007; received in revised form 30 September 2008; accepted 16 October 2008. Available online xx December 2008
Figure 1. Standard stimulus used in the case of 2D Vernier acuity for 0º orientation.
Note (*) - We verified whether using goggles could affect both CE (Constant Error) and Weber Fraction for stimuli presented in
the orientations of 0º, 45º and 90º in the uncrossed disparity condition with regard to the case in which they were displayed on
the screen. Results for % (CE/POE), in case of the uncrossed disparity (with goggles) were 0º= 1.12; 45º= 3.38; 90º= 4.65 and,
as for Weber fractions, results were: 0º= 0.08; 45º= 0.15; 90º= 0.13. Therefore, by comparing these data in Tables 1 and 2 we
verified that these two visual conditions, namely, with and without goggles, were equivalent.
170 Aznar-Casanova, Torrents and Alves
then presented for 1500 ms. The observer had no time orientation of the lines. We fit a linear function to these
constraints to respond to the stimuli and 1 s after his points and Figure 2 shows the regression equation and
response a new trial was displayed. Each orientation block the determination coefficient for both 2D Vernier acuity
took around 10 min to be concluded. In the experimental and 3D stereoacuity. In the latter, the percentage of CE
sessions, each orientation block was presented three can be superior to 10%, whilst in the case of 2D the
times. The participant performed all the experiment in percentage of constant errors (%CE) was lower than 5%.
seven sessions of six blocks (14 blocks x 3 repetitions = In Figure 2, we can see an opposite trend between these
42 blocks). Three minutes of interval were given between two visual conditions, increasing from 0º to 90º in the
blocks to the participant, therefore each session lasted two-dimensional case, but decreasing from 0º to 90º in
90 min. In brief, the observer performed 3024 trials, the three-dimensional one. Therefore, these slopes reveal
involving 10 hours and 30 minutes of experiment. the influence of the orientation of the Vernier lines on the
Difference threshold was calculated as the between- accuracy for visual acuity. Thus, we have obtained a good
quartile half-difference in min arc by the formula: DT= linear fit in both cases, two-dimensional visual condition
(Q3 – Q1)/2 ; where Quartile 1 is the value for the (2D: R2= 0.80; S.E. = 0.66) and stereo or three-
proportion point equal to 0.25 and Quartile 3 is the value dimensional case (3D: R2= 0.95; S.E. = 0.87).
for the proportion point equal to 0.75. We want to highlight two details of these results.
First, in both visual conditions an oblique effect has
Results been found; that is to say, the percentage of the constant
error (%CE) increased and decreased, respectively, for
We measured the participant’s capability for the 2D and 3D conditions as a function of the orientation
discriminating line separation in 14 tests, which resulted of the lines. Second, this change surprisingly occurred
from the combination of two visual conditions (2D and 3D in an opposite sense with respect to the orientation.
separations) by seven orientations. Two psychophysical That is to say, we found a systematic trend toward some
parameters were used, one to indicate accuracy of the overestimation in the case of the 2D visual condition
comparison judgments (constant error) and another to and this trend was inverted (underestimation) in the case
indicate precision or sensitivity (difference threshold). of the 3D visual condition. More precisely, the slope
In Table 1, values of the Point of Subjective Equality shows an opposite trend, namely, in the 2D case, as the
(PSE) and constant error (CE) for all tests are presented. orientation increased, %CE also increased, whilst in the
Notice that we use the percentage of constant error 3D case, as the orientation increased, %CE decreased.
(%CE) with respect to the Point of Objective Equality Certainly, a significant difference between the slopes of
(POE), in order to enable the comparison between the the regression lines with respect to the null hypothesis
2D and 3D visual conditions. (0º inclination of the slope, which would indicate that
Figure 2 shows the percentage of CE by plotting the orientations do not affect the judgments) was found
two visual conditions (2D and 3D) as a function of the in both conditions: 2D VA [t(10)= 4.408; P<.007;
Table 1. Values of the Point of Subjective Equality (PSE) and Constant Error (CE) in millimetres (mm), minutes of arc (min arc), and
percentage of the CE with respect to the Point of Objective Equality (POE).
Orientation PSE (mm) PSE (min arc) CE (mm) CE (min arc) %(CE/POE)
Table 2. Slope values of the psychometric function, uncertainty interval (UI), differential threshold (DT), and Weber fraction (K).
Figure 3. Psychometric functions for all orientations in the case of 2D separation judgements (left panel) and in the case of 3D one (right panel).
the worst discrimination in the 3D condition coincides DT is greater when vertical disparity is greater, but only if
with the lowest vertical disparity, i.e., 0º oriented lines. segments form an angle lower than 30º.
The difference threshold (DT) was analyzed with Figure 6 shows vertical disparities and Weber fraction
an ANOVA taking visual condition (2D and 3D) and (∆I/I) for the 3D condition and reveals a common trend
orientation (0º, 15º, 30º, 45º, 60º, 75º, 90º) as between between the lines, but only if the orientation is lower
subjects factors and each repetition block (3 blocks by 4 than 45º (Rxy = 0.83). As a result of the comparison,
participants, N= 12) as a new measure. Mean percentage we can conclude that the data fit well only if vertical
of DT for the two visual conditions as a function of the disparities are greater than horizontal ones.
orientation are plotted in Figure 4. Overall sensitivity
(DT) varied with visual condition [F(1,154)= 6.201; Discussion
P<.014]. However, neither the orientation [F(1,154)=
0.382; P<.89] nor the interaction between these two With the aim of studying the oblique effect, we
factors [F(1,154)= 0.823; P<.554] were significant. conducted one experiment in which the participant was asked
In brief, the most important result is that sensitivity to compare the separation of two lines in the frontoparallel
(DTs) varied according to visual condition. No differences (2D) and median planes (3D). We designed 14 tests by
between orientations were found, except when the combining 2 visual conditions and 7 orientations. The results
orientation was close to horizontality (lower than 30º). showed that, in both cases (2D and 3D separations), if the
In order to verify if the oblique effect influences the two lines were presented obliquely, visual acuity (2D VA)
perception of vertical disparities in a 3D visual condition, and stereoacuity (3D stereo VA) varied inversely, namely,
we calculated vertical disparities between lines for all overestimation in the 2D case and underestimation in the
orientations in the 3D condition (Figure 5). The segment 3D. In this last case, such variation was proportional to the
∇v represents the size of vertical disparities in cardinal (0º) cos (θ). Additionally, as the inclination of the lines increases,
[left panel] and oblique orientations (approximately 60º) the relative separation between them decreases, which
[right panel]. From this vertical disparity, we computed the promotes a diminishing of vertical disparities. Therefore, for
distance in depth (vertical separation) for all orientations orientations lower than 45º, the effect of vertical disparity
by ∇v= cos (θ) POE, where ∇v is the vertical disparity, θ progressively diminishes.
is the orientation of the lines, and POE = 20 mm (standard One evidence for vertical disparities as the main
value) for the 3D case. The values of the vertical disparities feature to be processed in the case of inclined lines, i.e.,
for each orientation are presented in Table 3. Notice that in favour of the oblique effect, are the similarities with the
so-called induced-effect (Ogle, 1950), in which two halves
of a stereogram are identical, except that one is magnified
vertically with respect to the other. According to Ogle, with
such stereogram the observer stereoscopically perceives
the image as a slanted surface rotated on its vertical axis. It
is also possible to promote the same impression of a slanted
surface by manipulating horizontal disparities, namely, by
magnifying one half of the stereogram horizontally. This
is the so-called geometrical effect (Ogle, 1950). However,
an important difference between those two effects is that
the depth sign of a given vertical disparity depends on the
Figure 4. Differential threshold for the 2D and 3D visual quadrant around the fixation point, while the depth sign of
conditions as a function of orientation of the lines. a given horizontal disparity is independent of the quadrants
Vertical disparities & Oblique effect 173
Vertical disparity (mm) 20.00 19.32 17.32 14.14 10.00 5.18 0.00
Horizontal disparity (mm) 0.00 5.18 10.00 14.14 17.32 19.32 20.00
suggests that the global effect of the vertical disparity is line segment. Vision Research, 8, 493–507.
more powerful than the local effect. We expect to study, Bowker, D.O., & Mandler, M.B. (1981). Binocular flicker appears
faster than monocular flicker. Journal of the Optical Society of
in the future, the conflict between the sign of the depth America, 71, 496–498.
for the origin and ending points of the line, that is to say, Campbell, F.W., & Kulikowski, J.J. (1966). Orientational selectivity of
how these points cancel or enhance depth perception in the human visual system. Journal of Physiology, 187, 437–445.
Coppola, D.M., White, L.E., Fitzpatrick, D., & Purves, D. (1998).
stereograms with vertical disparities. Unequal representation of cardinal and oblique contours in ferret
At last, we would like to highlight two ideas. One visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
U.S.A., 95, 2621–2623.
refers to the site for processing vertical disparities. The Davidoff, J.B. (1974). An observation concerning the preferred perception
model proposed by Matthews et al. (2003) is based on of the visual horizontal and vertical. Perception, 3, 47–48.
the properties of the receptive fields associated to cells DeAngelis, G.C., Ohzawa, I., & Freeman, R.D. (1991). Depth is
encoded in the visual cortex by a specialized receptive field
in V1. However, as stated by the authors, it implies that structure. Nature, 352, 156–159.
binocular depth perception necessarily takes place in DeValois, R. L., & DeValois, K. K. (1988). Spatial vision. Oxford
V1. Subsequent processing beyond V1, probably in V2 University Press, New York.
DeValois, R.L., Yund, E.W., & Hepler, N. (1982). The orientation and
and V3 feed-forward circuits, could contribute to extract direction selectivity of cells in macaque visual cortex. Vision
vertical disparities and, after such processing, feedback Research, 22, 531–544.
neural pathways would return information to V1 to Emsley, H.H. (1925). Irregular astigmatism of the eye: Effects of
correcting lenses. Transactions of the Optical Society, 27, 28–41.
generate depth perception. The second idea is related to Essock, E.A. (1990). The influence of stimulus length on the oblique
the assumption of the radial orientation bias of the model effect of contrast sensitivity. Vision Research, 30, 1243–1246.
Garding, J., Porrill, J., Mayhew, J.E.W., & Frisby, J.P. (1995).
of Matthews et al. (2003). Physiological differences in Stereopsis, vertical disparity and relief transformations. Vision
the number of cells processing cardinal orientations with Research, 35, 703–722.
respect to the oblique ones have been found in the cortex Heeley, D.W., & Timney, B. (1988). Meridional anisotropies of
orientation discrimination for sine wave gratings. Vision
of cats (Vidyasagar & Henry, 1990) and monkeys (Bauer Research, 28, 337–344.
& Dow, 1989). However, only psychophysical evidence Higgins, G.C., & Stultz, K. (1948). Visual acuity as measured with
is available in the case of humans (the oblique effect) various orientations of a parallel-line test object. Journal of the
Optical Society of America, 38, 756–758.
and our study provides an additional support to this Horn, G., & Hill, R.M. (1969). Modifications of receptive fields of
approach. We think that the oblique effect in stereoacuity cells in the visual cortex occurring spontaneously and associated
can be best explained by the model of Matthews et al. with bodily tilt. Nature, 221, 186–188.
Howard, I.P. (1982). Human visual orientation. Chichester, Sussex:
(2003), and that the absence of oblique effect in the case John Wiley.
of the acuity for 2D stimuli is due to the lack of vertical Howard, I.P., & Kaneko, H. (1994). Relative shear disparity and the
perception of surface inclination. Vision Research, 34, 2505–2517.
disparities in such a condition. Howard, I.P., & Rogers, B.J. (1995). Binocular vision and stereopsis.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Acknowledgements Howard, I.P., & Rogers, B.J. (2002). Seeing in depth: Depth perception
(vol. 2). Toronto: I. Porteus.
Hubel, D.H., & Wiesel, T.N. (1959). Receptive fields of single neurons in
This research was supported by grant SEJ2006-15095/ the cat’s striate cortex. Journal of General Physiology, 39, 651–673.
PSIC from the Spanish Ministry of Science and Technology. Hubel, D.H., & Wiesel, T.N. (1977). Functional architecture of
macaque monkey visual cortex. Proceedings of the Royal Society
of London B, 198, 1–59.
References Jastrow, J. (1893). On the judgment of angles and positions of lines.
American Journal of Psychology, 5, 214-248.
Andrews, D.P. (1967). Perception of contour orientation in the central Kaneko, H., & Howard, I.P. (1997). Spatial limitation of vertical-size
fovea. I. Short lines. Vision Research, 7, 975–997. disparity processing. Vision Research, 37, 2871–2878.
Anzai, A., Ohzawa, I., & Freeman, R.D. (1999a). Neural mechanisms Keil, M.K., & Cristobal, G. (2000). Separating the chaff from the
for processing binocular information: I. Simple cells. Journal of wheat: Possible origins of the oblique effect. Journal of the
Neurophysiology, 82, 891–908. Optical Society of America A, 17(4), 697–710.
Anzai, A., Ohzawa, I., & Freeman, R.D. (1999b). Neural mechanisms Kennedy, H., Martin, K., Orban, G.A., & Whitteridge, D. (1985).
for processing binocular information: II. Complex cells. Journal Receptive field properties of neurons in visual area 1 and visual
of Neurophysiology, 82, 909–924. area 2 in the baboon. Neuroscience, 14, 405–415.
Appelle, S. (1972). Perception and discrimination as a function Koenderink, J.J., & van Doorn, A.J. (1976). Geometry of binocular vision
of orientation: The “oblique effect” in man and animals. and a model for stereopsis. Biological Cybernetics, 21, 29–35.
Psychological Bulletin, 78, 266–278. Li, B., Peterson, M.R., & Freeman, R.D. (2003). The Oblique Effect:
Arditi, A., Kaufman, L., & Movshon, J.A. (1981). A simple explanation A neural basis in the visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology,
of the induced size effect. Vision Research, 21, 755–764. 90, 204–217.
Attneave, F., & Olson, R.K. (1967). Discriminability of stimuli varying Luria, S. M. (1963). The effect of body-position on meridional
in physical and retinal orientation. Journal of Experimental variations in scotopic acuity. American Journal of Psychology,
Psychology, 74, 149–157. 76, 598–606.
Backus, B.T., Banks, M.S., van Ee, R., & Crowell, J.A. (1999). Mach, E. (1861). Über das Sehen von Lagen und Winkeln durch
Horizontal and vertical disparity, eye position, and stereoscopic die Bewegung des Auges. Sitzungsberichte der Kaiserlichen
slant perception. Vision Research, 39, 1143–1170. Akademie der Wissenschaften, 43, 215–224.
Bauer, J.A., Owens, D.A., Thomas, J., & Held, R. (1979). Monkeys Maffei, L., & Campbell, F.W. (1970). Neurophysiological localization of
show an oblique effect. Perception, 8, 247–253. the vertical and horizontal visual co-ordinates in man. Science, 167,
Bauer, R., & Dow, B.M. (1989). Complementary global maps for 386–387.
orientation coding in upper and lower layers of the monkey’s Mansfield, R.J.W., & Ronner, S.F. (1978). Orientation anisotropy in
foveal striate cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 76, 503–509. monkey visual cortex. Brain Research, 149, 229–234.
Bouma, H., & Andriessen, J.J. (1968). Perceived orientation of isolated Matthews, N., Meng, X., Xu, P., & Qian, N. (2003). A physiological
Vertical disparities & Oblique effect 175
theory of depth perception from vertical disparity. Vision on single units in cat striate cortex: simultaneous stimulation
Research 43, 85–99. by single moving slit with receptive fields in correspondence.
Mayhew, J.E.W., & Longuet-Higgins, H.C. (1982). A computational Experimental Brain Research, 6, 391–410.
model of binocular depth perception. Nature, 297, 376–379. Rogers, B.J., & Bradshaw, M.F. (1993). Vertical disparities, differential
Nundy, S., Lotto, B., Coppola, D., Shimpi, A.; & Purves, D. (2000). perspectives and binocular stereopsis. Nature, 361, 253–255.
Why are angles misperceived? PNAS, 97, 5592–5597. Vidyasagar, T.R., & Henry, G.H. (1990). Relationship between
Ogle, K.N. (1950). Researches in binocular vision. Philadelphia, PA: preferred orientation and ordinal position in neurons of cat
W. B. Saunders. striate cortex. Visual Neuroscience, 5, 565–569.
Ogle, K.N. (1955). Stereopsis and vertical disparity. AMA Archives of Westheimer, G. (1984). Sensitivity for vertical retinal image
Ophthalmology, 53, 495–504. differences. Nature, 307, 632–634.
Ohzawa, I., DeAngelis, G.C., & Freeman, R.D. (1990). Westheimer, G. (2005). Anisotropies in peripheral Vernier acuity.
Stereoscopic depth discrimination in the visual cortex: Spatial Vision, 18, 159–167.
Neurons ideally suited as disparity detectors. Science, 249, Westheimer, G., & Beard, B.L. (1998). Orientation dependency for
1037–1041. foveal line stimuli: Detection and intensity discrimination,
Ohzawa, I., DeAngelis, G.C., & Freeman, R.D. (1996). Encoding of resolution, orientation discrimination and Vernier acuity. Vision
binocular disparity by simple cells in the cat’s visual cortex. Research, 38, 1097–1103.Westheimer, G., & Pettet, M.W.
Journal of Neurophysiology, 75, 1779–1805. (1992). Detection and processing of vertical disparity by the
Ohzawa, I., DeAngelis, G.C., & Freeman, R.D. (1997). Encoding of human observer. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B,
binocular disparity by complex cells in the cat’s visual cortex. 250, 243–247.
Journal of Neurophysiology, 77, 2879–2909. Zlaskova, M. (1993). Orientation identification at different background
Pettigrew, J.D, Nikara, T., & Bishop, P.O. (1968). Binocular interaction level: Its precision and distortions. Vision Research, 33, 2073–2081.