Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

Toppa

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Research in Developmental Disabilities 78 (2018) 114–124

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Research in Developmental Disabilities


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/redevdis

Reading outcomes of children with delayed early vocabulary: A


T
follow-up from age 2–16

Maria Psyridoua, , Kenneth Eklundb, Anna-Maija Poikkeusa, Minna Torppaa
a
Department of Teacher Education, Faculty of Education and Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, P.O. Box 35, Ruusupuisto, Alvar Aallon katu 9,
40014-FIN, Finland
b
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Education and Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, P.O. Box 35, Kärki, Mattilanniemi 6, 40014-FIN, Finland

A R T IC LE I N F O ABS TRA CT

Number of reviews completed is 2 Background: Delays in expressive vocabulary have been associated with lower outcomes in
Keywords: reading.
Expressive vocabulary Aim: The aim is to conduct a long-term follow-up study to investigate if early expressive voca-
Receptive vocabulary bulary delay (late talking) predicts reading development in participants age 16 and under. We
Reading fluency examine further if the prediction is different in the presence of family risk for dyslexia (FR) and
Reading comprehension early receptive vocabulary delay.
Reading difficulties Methods: Expressive and receptive vocabulary skills were assessed at the age of 2–2.5 years, and
reading skills in Grades 2, 3, 8 and 9 (age 8–16). The longitudinal sample consisted of 200
Finnish-speaking children, of which 108 had FR for dyslexia and 92 came from families without
reading difficulties. We compared the reading development of five subgroups: 1) FR and no
vocabulary delay; 2) FR and late talkers, 3) FR, late talkers and co-existing receptive vocabulary
delay; 4) no FR and late talkers; and 5) no FR and no vocabulary delay.
Results: The group with FR and expressive and receptive vocabulary delay had difficulties in
reading comprehension, but not in reading fluency. The late talkers without receptive vocabulary
difficulties tended to become typical readers.
Conclusions and implications: Delays in early vocabulary can lead to a reading comprehension
deficit, with the specification that expressive vocabulary deficit alone can alleviate in time,
whereas the combined deficit is a stronger risk marker.

What this paper adds?

The present study extends the literature by investigating the reading development of children with early expressive vocabulary
delay followed up to age 16 (Grade 9) in a Finnish language context in relation to both reading fluency and reading comprehension.
In addition, we examine whether this relationship is different in the presence and absence of other co-occurring risk factors, in
particular family risk for dyslexia and early receptive vocabulary delay. The existing literature on reading development of children
with delays in early vocabulary is almost exclusively limited to findings concerning the early grades of primary school. Furthermore,
all the previous studies spanning beyond Grade 2 have been conducted using English-speaking children; thus, further research in
other orthographies is needed.
We compared the reading development of five subgroups: 1) FR and no vocabulary delay; 2) FR and late talkers; 3) FR, late talkers


Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: maria.m.psyridou@jyu.fi (M. Psyridou), kenneth.m.eklund@jyu.fi (K. Eklund), anna-maija.poikkeus@jyu.fi (A.-M. Poikkeus),
minna.p.torppa@jyu.fi (M. Torppa).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2018.05.004
Received 14 June 2017; Received in revised form 24 February 2018; Accepted 10 May 2018
Available online 24 May 2018
0891-4222/ © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
M. Psyridou et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 78 (2018) 114–124

and co-existing receptive vocabulary delay; 4) no FR and late talkers; and 5) no FR and no vocabulary delay. Our findings add to the
literature by suggesting that a trajectory of later reading comprehension difficulties is much more likely when early expressive
vocabulary delay is accompanied by receptive vocabulary delay. Our findings suggest that a delay in early vocabulary can lead to a
persistent deficit with the specification that expressive vocabulary deficit alone can be alleviated in time, whereas the combined
deficit is a stronger risk marker. Early expressive vocabulary delay represented high risk only for reading comprehension develop-
ment and only when it co-occurred with other risk factors (family risk for dyslexia and receptive vocabulary difficulties).

1. Introduction

Early years expressive language delay, or late talking, is one of the most common concerns of parents and early health care
personnel, and it is one of the common reasons for referring young children for evaluation. This is not surprising, as early language
development forms the foundation for later educational and academic achievement and has important links with social adaptation
(Reilly et al., 2010). From a clinical and educational perspective, it is important to be able to predict as early as possible which
children may be at risk for learning difficulties in their school years. Delays in expressive vocabulary have been associated with lower
outcomes in reading (Rescorla, 2002, 2005, 2009), but apart from Rescorla’s sample from the USA, little research has focused on
literacy development after the early grades of primary school using longitudinal designs. Moreover, most of the previous studies have
been conducted among English-speaking children and have focused almost solely on early expressive, but not receptive vocabulary.
The present study will extend previous studies (Duff, Reen, Plunkett, & Nation, 2015; Lyytinen, Eklund, & Lyytinen, 2005;
Rescorla, 2002, 2005, 2009) to later years of schooling by examining if late talking (delays in early expressive vocabulary) predict
reading development (fluency and comprehension) from early primary Grades 2 and 3 to lower secondary Grades 8 and 9 in Finland.
We will also examine the effects of co-occurring receptive vocabulary delay and family risk for dyslexia on the relationship between
early vocabulary skills and reading outcomes. Expressive vocabulary may not be a marker of subsequent language and literacy
difficulties when it is not accompanied by co-occurring difficulties in receptive vocabulary (Lyytinen et al., 2005). It is also possible
that family risk for dyslexia actually explains the link between expressive vocabulary delay because the children with family risk (due
to parental language or reading difficulties) are shown to be at risk for both vocabulary delays (Duff et al., 2015; Lyytinen et al., 2005;
Snowling, Gallagher, & Frith, 2003; Snowling, Muter, & Carroll, 2007) and for later language and reading outcomes (Lyytinen et al.,
2005; Reilly et al., 2010).
Early vocabulary skills (expressive or a composite score of expressive and receptive vocabulary) have been shown to be associated
with both reading fluency (Nation & Snowling, 2004; Snowling et al., 2003) and reading comprehension (Duff et al., 2015; Nation,
Cocksey, Taylor, & Bishop, 2010; Ouellette 2006; Ouellette & Beers 2010). Theories on how early vocabulary skills and reading
outcomes are linked depend, however, on the reading skill in question. According to the lexical restructuring hypothesis, the link
between early vocabulary and reading fluency is indirect and mediated via phonological skills (e.g. Walley, Metsala, & Garlock,
2003). Phonological awareness plays a critical role in the development of decoding (Ehri et al., 2001; Hulme & Snowling, 2014;
Melby-Lervåg, Lyster, & Hulme, 2012), and tasks requiring identification or manipulation at the phoneme level, in particular, have
been found to be associated with variations in decoding skills (Georgiou, Parrila, & Papadopoulos, 2008; Lervåg, Bråten, & Hulme,
2009). Walley et al. (2003) suggested that phonological awareness may be linked to vocabulary via the process of restructuring
phonological representations (the word cat, for example, first segmented at syllable level/k/-/æt/becoming segmented at phoneme
level/k/-/æ/-/t/). Vocabulary growth increases sensitivity towards phonological similarities between words, which elicits a re-
structuring of the already-existing phonological representations in the lexicon (Walley et al., 2003).
The link between reading comprehension and vocabulary skills may be more direct than that of vocabulary and reading fluency.
According to the simple view of reading model (Gough & Tumner, 1986; Hoover & Gough, 1990), reading comprehension is based on
two core skills: decoding and language comprehension. Reading comprehension has been shown to rely on a variety of oral language
comprehension skills (Hulme & Snowling, 2014), including expressive and receptive vocabulary (e.g. Nation et al., 2010) and lis-
tening comprehension (e.g. Catts, Adlof, & Weismer, 2006; Nation et al., 2010). Weaknesses in these skills have been found to be
manifested before learning to read, thus providing a possible causal link between the influence of oral language comprehension on
difficulties in reading comprehension (e.g. Catts et al., 2006; Nation et al., 2010). This association between vocabulary and reading
comprehension has also been shown through intervention studies (Clarke, Snowling, Truelove, & Hulme, 2010; Fricke, Bowyer-
Crane, Haley, Hulme, & Snowling, 2013).
The vast majority of studies on the reading development of children with expressive vocabulary delay have focused on language
outcomes in kindergarten (age range between 2 and 5.5 years) (Feldman et al., 2005; Moyle, Weismer, Evans, & Lindstrom, 2007;
Thal, Miller, Carlson, & Vega, 2005) or reading outcomes in the early grades of primary school (Grade 2) (Lyytinen et al., 2005). The
documentation on the reading development of the children with expressive vocabulary delay beyond the early grades comes from one
sample (Rescorla, 2002, 2005, 2009). Rescorla (2002) showed that children that had been identified as having expressive vocabulary
delay in early childhood continued to manifest significantly poorer reading skills (i.e., decoding, comprehension, written language
and spelling) than their peers at the ages of 8 and 9. At the age of 13, the children with expressive vocabulary delay demonstrated
significantly lower vocabulary and reading comprehension skills than the comparison children (Rescorla, 2005), whereas no dif-
ferences were observed in reading fluency (Rescorla, 2005). At the age of 17, the children with expressive vocabulary delay had
average reading skills but below average vocabulary and verbal memory (Rescorla, 2009). Rescorla’s findings show that early ex-
pressive vocabulary delay is negatively associated with reading achievement, especially after the initial stages of learning to read
(Rescorla, 2002, 2005, 2009), and the effect is primarily seen on reading comprehension rather than reading fluency (Rescorla, 2005,
2009). Rescorla, however, did not control for other risk factors or co-occurring difficulties, and it is possible that late talking is not the

115
M. Psyridou et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 78 (2018) 114–124

cause of subsequent reading difficulties. A few studies show that expressive vocabulary delay may actually be a risk factor for reading
development only when accompanied by other risk factors, such as family risk for dyslexia or language difficulties and receptive
vocabulary delays (Duff et al., 2015; Lyytinen et al., 2005; Poll & Miller, 2013).
Family risk for dyslexia is linked to both vocabulary and reading difficulties and may be the underlying cause for late talkers’
problems in reading (Duff et al., 2015; Lyytinen et al., 2005; Snowling et al., 2003, 2007; Torppa, Lyytinen, Erskine, Eklund, &
Lyytinen, 2010). It has been shown that the combination of family risk for reading difficulties and early delays in expressive and
receptive vocabulary impedes reading development in the early grades (Duff et al., 2015; Lyytinen et al., 2005). In the same Finnish
sample that is used for this study, Lyytinen et al. (2005) showed that those children with expressive vocabulary delay at the end of the
second grade (age 8 years) who also exhibited evidence of receptive vocabulary delay and a family risk for dyslexia were often
diagnosed as manifesting reading difficulties, whereas those children with only expressive vocabulary delay were not. We add to their
findings by extending the investigation to Grades 3, 8 and 9 and by examining both reading fluency and reading comprehension
development from Grade 2–9.

2. The present study

The existing literature on reading development of children with delays in early vocabulary is almost exclusively limited to
findings concerning the early grades of primary school, and all the studies except one (Lyytinen et al., 2005) have been conducted
using English-speaking children. The present study extends the literature by investigating the reading development of children with
early expressive vocabulary delay followed up to age 16 (Grade 9) with native Finnish speakers with respect to both reading fluency
and reading comprehension. In addition, we examine whether this relationship is different in the presence and absence of other co-
occurring risk factors: family risk for dyslexia and early receptive vocabulary delay. We use reading fluency and comprehension as
outcomes rather than reading accuracy because most Finnish children can read accurately after the first year of formal education, and
accuracy measures are at a ceiling by that time. The use of fluency and comprehension as indicators of reading progress is typical for
languages that have a high level of transparency of the orthographic system (Aro & Wimmer, 2003; Seymour, Aro, & Erskine, 2003;
Share, 2008). The Finnish orthographic system is considered very transparent (Aro, 2016), and in contrast to English, for instance,
there is almost a perfect one-on-one correspondence between phonemes and graphemes, with every letter always having the same
sound and every sound always being represented by the same letter. The combination of high-quality reading teaching and the high
transparency of the orthography supports faster reading acquisition than in the less consistent orthographies, such as English (e.g.
Seymour et al., 2003). In Finland, children start attending primary school in August of the year in which they turn 7. The follow-up
period of the present study was from Grade 2 (when the children’s ages varied between 8 and 9 years) to the end of Grade 9 (when
their ages varied between 15 and 16 years).
The research questions of the present study were as follows: (1) Does early expressive vocabulary delay predict difficulties in
reading fluency or reading comprehension at Grades 2, 3, 8 and 9? (2) Is the relationship between early expressive vocabulary delay
and reading development different in the presence and absence of receptive vocabulary delay and family risk for dyslexia?
Based on previous studies (Nation & Snowling, 2004; Rose & Rouhani, 2012), we expected that vocabulary skills would be
associated with reading development. However, we predicted that early vocabulary would be a better predictor for reading com-
prehension than reading fluency (Rescorla, 2005, 2009; Ricketts, Nation & Bishop, 2007). We also expected that children with early
expressive vocabulary delay would have more reading comprehension problems in the later grades than in the earlier grades
(Rescorla, 2002, 2005, 2009) and that the most severe reading problems would be found among children with both expressive and
receptive vocabulary delays (Lyytinen et al., 2005). Finally, we expected that the children with family risk for dyslexia would be
slower readers than the children of parents with no reading difficulties (Eklund, Torppa, Aro, Leppänen, & Lyytinen, 2015; Snowling
& Melby-Lervåg, 2016).

3. Method

3.1. Participants

The children (n = 200) were participants of the “Jyväskylä Longitudinal Study of Dyslexia (JLD)”, a longitudinal family risk study
following children with and without family risk for dyslexia. The participants were selected from among families of 9368 newborns
born in the province of “central Finland” between April 1993 and July 1996. The selection of the parents with dyslexia followed a
three-step procedure. The first stage was a short questionnaire including three questions concerning difficulties in learning to read
and spell among themselves and their close relatives. The second stage was a more detailed questionnaire, which focused on de-
mographic information and the occurrence of reading and writing difficulties during childhood, adolescence and among relatives.
Those who fulfilled the criteria of inclusion in the first and second stage were invited for an interview and for an assessment of their
reading and writing skills to confirm their present status of dyslexia. The criteria used to select the parents with dyslexia were as
follows: a self-reported childhood history of reading or writing difficulties, self-reported adulthood situation of reading and writing
difficulties, and present performance in diagnostic tasks of reading and writing (for full details of recruitment, see Leinonen et al.,
2001). The criteria for a child to be included in the family risk group (FR, n = 108) included that one of the parents was identified as
having reading difficulties in the reading and spelling tests, literacy problems during early school years, and at least one first-degree
relative with corresponding difficulties. In the control group without family risk, neither parent had reported reading difficulties or a
family history of dyslexia, and neither had difficulties in reading and spelling tasks (NR, n = 92). The parents’ educational

116
M. Psyridou et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 78 (2018) 114–124

backgrounds were assessed using a 7-point scale: 1 = comprehensive school without any vocational education, 2 = comprehensive
school with short-term vocational courses, 3 = comprehensive school with a vocational school degree, 4 = comprehensive school
with a vocational college degree, 5 = comprehensive school with a lower university degree (Bachelor’s) or a degree at a polytechnic,
6 = upper secondary school with a BA degree or a degree at a polytechnic, and 7 = upper secondary school with a higher university
degree (master’s or a doctorate degree). The mean of the mothers’ education was 4.18 (SD = 1.48) in the FR group and 4.52 in the
control group (SD = 1.35). The mean of the fathers’ education was 3.68 (SD = 1.27) and 3.80 (SD = 1.40), respectively. There were
no differences in parental education or in their nonverbal IQs. The IQ of all parents, assessed with the Raven B, C and D matrices, was
≥80 (Raven, Court, & Raven, 1992).

3.2. Measures

The children were assessed at ages 24 months, 30 months, 8–9 years (Grade 2), 9–10 years (Grade 3), 14–15 years (Grade 8) and
15–16 years (Grade 9). Trained testers individually assessed the children’s language skills at ages 24 and 30 months in a laboratory
setting, and school-age reading fluency and reading comprehension were assessed via group-administered tests in the classrooms.
Reading fluency was assessed with three tasks: oral text reading, oral pseudoword text reading and oral word list reading. Reading
comprehension was assessed using a short passage reading comprehension task and PISA reading comprehension (Grade 9). The
measures are described in detail below.

3.2.1. Expressive vocabulary measures at the age of 24 and 30 months


The composite score of expressive vocabulary was calculated based on the following four measures: vocabulary production and
maximum sentence length using the Finnish adaptation of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Fenson et al.,
1994) at the age of 24 months using parental reports, the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID; Bayley, 1993) expressive score
(naming pictures and objects) at the age of 24 months and the expressive score of the Reynell Developmental Language Scale (RDLS;
Reynell & Huntley, 1987) at the age of 30 months. A z-score based on control group distribution was calculated for each measure, and
the average of the z-scored measures served as the composite score for expressive language. The Cronbach alpha reliability for the
composite score of expressive language was 0.86.

3.2.2. Receptive vocabulary


The children’s verbal comprehension level was assessed using the Reynell Receptive Language Scale at the age of 30 months.

3.2.3. Oral text reading (Grades 2, 3 and 8)


In the spring term of each grade level, oral text reading was assessed by asking participants to read an age-appropriate text aloud.
In Grade 2, the children read a text (“Exciting journeys”) consisting of 19 sentences, with a total of 124 words; in Grade 3, they read a
text (“Useless belongings”) consisting of 18 sentences, with a total of 189 words; and in Grade 8, they read a text (“Fields of Lapland”)
consisting of 16 sentences, with a total of 207 words. Children’s reading performances were recorded, and the accuracy of reading
and time spent on reading were checked. The scores of the children’s speed were measured as words per minute.

3.2.4. Oral pseudoword text reading (Grades 2, 3 and 8)


The participants read aloud a short text that was made up of 19 pseudowords (Grade 2) or 38 pseudowords (Grades 3 and 8). The
pseudowords and sentence structures resembled real Finnish in form, but had no meaning. The mean word length was 7.21 letters/
word in Grade 2 and 7.29 letters/word in Grades 3 and 8. Children’s reading performances were recorded, and the accuracy of
reading and time spent on reading were checked. The scores of the children’s speed were measured as pseudowords per minute.

3.2.5. Oral word list reading (Grades 2, 3 and 8)


In the nationally normed Lukilasse reading test (Häyrinen, Serenius-Sirve, & Korkman, 1999), the participants had 2 min to read
aloud as many words as possible from a 90-item (Grade 2) or 105-item (Grade 3) list of words assembled vertically in columns. The
same list which was used in Grade 3 was administered in Grade 8, but the time limit was reduced to 1 min. The length of the words
increased gradually, ranging from 3 to 18 letters/word in Grade 2 and from 3 to 22 letters/word in Grades 3 and 8. The mean length
of the words was 9.08 letters in Grade 2 and 9.57 letters in Grades 3 and 8. The children received 1 point for each correct answer.

3.2.6. Reading comprehension (Grades 2 and 3)


A group-administered subtest of the nationally normed reading test battery (Ala-asteen lukutesti, or ALLU; Lindeman, 2000) was
used. The children silently read a fiction story and then answered 12 questions (11 multiple choice questions and one that required
arranging five statements into the correct sequence). The length of the text in Grade 2 was five paragraphs and had 114 words. In
Grade 3, the length of the text was 4 paragraphs and had 139 words. The children received 1 point for each correct answer
(max = 12). The maximum time allotted was 45 min. The Kuder–Richardson reliability coefficients reported by Lindeman (2000)
were 0.80 in Grade 2 and 0.75 in Grade 3.

3.2.7. Reading comprehension (Grade 9): PISA reading


The reading tasks consisted of the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) reading link items, which are con-
tained in each cycle of the PISA assessments to provide comparability of the measurement. It is a triennial international survey which

117
M. Psyridou et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 78 (2018) 114–124

aims to evaluate education systems worldwide by testing the skills and knowledge of 15-year-old students (OECD, 2010, p. 26; OECD,
2013, p. 45). The students were given a booklet with eight texts to read, and they had to answer questions in the allotted space. The
reading materials included texts, tables, graphs and figures. The total length of the material is 30 A4 pages. However, because the
material includes many pictures and tables, the estimated length of the text is approximately 10 pages. There were 15 multiple-choice
questions and 16 questions that required written responses. Of the questions, 12 required students to access and retrieve information,
12 to integrate and interpret information, and 7 to reflect and evaluate information. Students had 60 min to complete the task. A total
score was calculated representing all the PISA reading items. The Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficient for the total score in this
sample was 0.80.

3.3. Statistical analysis

First, all the variable distributions were examined. In grade 8, two extreme outliers were observed in the pseudoword text reading
and three in the text reading and skewness values were rather high. Outliers were moved to the tails of the distributions, and log10
transformations were performed. After the transformations, the distributions approximated normal distribution. In the examination
of reading fluency, a composite variable for each grade was calculated by converting the scores for each reading fluency measure into
z-scores and by calculating mean composite scores. Cronbach alpha reliability for the fluency composite was 0.91, 0.88 and 0.90 in
Grades 2, 3 and 8, respectively.
One-way ANOVAs were conducted to compare the five groups in reading fluency and reading comprehension performance. The
groups were identified with the following criteria: Expressive vocabulary delay was identified based on three tests at 2–2.5 years: 1)
vocabulary production and maximum sentence length from the Finnish adaptation of the CDI (MacArthur Communicative
Development Inventory) 2) the Bayley expressive score (Bayley, 1993), and 3) the expressive score from the Reynell Developmental
Language Scales (Reynell & Huntley, 1987). The Reynell Receptive Language Scale scores from the RDLS were used to further divide
late talkers (i.e., children identified with expressive vocabulary delay) into groups with and without receptive vocabulary delay. Of
the five subgroups, three were with family risk and two with no family risk: 1) family risk and no vocabulary delay (FR); 2) family risk
and late talkers (FR-LT1); 3) family risk, late talkers and receptive vocabulary delay (FR-LT2); 4) no family risk and late talkers (NR-
LT1), and 5) no family risk and no vocabulary delay (NR).
There were two groups of late talkers (FR-LT1 and NR-LT1) who had only expressive vocabulary delay. They had expressive
vocabulary score at least one standard deviation below the mean of children in the control group but age-appropriate receptive skills.
There were 10 children with family risk for dyslexia (FR-LT1) and 10 children with no risk for dyslexia (NR-LT1). The second subtype
of late talkers demonstrated both an expressive and receptive vocabulary delay (at least one standard deviation below the mean on
both scores). There were 12 children from the family risk sample (FR-LT2), but only three children from the no-risk sample fulfilled
these criteria, and they were subsequently excluded from the analyses. Finally, the analyses involved two groups with no vocabulary
delay: the FR group (N = 83) and the NR group (N = 79) with age-appropriate or better early expressive and receptive vocabulary. In
addition to the group comparisons, we also examined the correlations between all measures.
The descriptive statistics of the measures used in the identification of the five groups are presented in Table 1. Three children of
the family risk sample were not included in the analysis because the Reynell expressive and receptive scores were missing for two of
them and because one of the children attended school one year later than his age-mates and we did not have his reading measure
outcomes. None of the children had reports on Specific Language Impairment (SLI) diagnoses at any age. In Finland, physicians use
the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) when diagnosing specific language impairment (SLI) (Asikainen, 2005). However,
some milder language or language-related difficulties were diagnosed in 11 children (one had naming difficulty, five demonstrated
delayed language/speech development, four had dysphasic features, and one faced learning difficulties, especially in language). Of
these 11 children, six belonged to the group with family risk and both expressive and receptive vocabulary delays (FR-LT2); three
belonged to the group with family risk, but no early vocabulary delays (FR); and two belonged to the control group with expressive
vocabulary delay (NR-LT1).

4. Results

4.1. Group differences in reading fluency

The one-way ANOVAs for reading fluency showed significant group differences in Grades 2, 3 and 8 (Tables 2 and 3, Fig. 1).
Pairwise comparisons (LSD) revealed that in Grades 2 and 3 the NR group children were faster readers than the FR group children. In
grade 3, the NR group children were also faster readers than the FR-LT2 group children. In Grade 8, the pairwise comparisons
(Dunnett T3) showed that the NR group children were faster readers than the FR group children. Further inspection of the effect sizes
showed that the comparisons between the three groups with family risk for dyslexia and the two groups without family risk for
dyslexia all had small to medium effect sizes.

4.2. Group differences in reading comprehension

The one-way ANOVAs for reading comprehension showed significant group differences in Grades 2 and 9, but not in Grade 3
(Tables 2 and 3, Fig. 2). In Grade 2, the pairwise comparisons (Dunnett T3) showed no significant differences between the groups but
the effect sizes were large in favour of the NR group compared to the FR-LT2 and NR-LT1. In Grade 3, the pairwise comparisons (LSD)

118
M. Psyridou et al.

Table 1
Descriptive statistics for the vocabulary measures used to determine the subgroups.
Family risk, no language Family risk, Late talkers (FR- Family risk, Late talkers and receptive No family risk, Late talkers No family risk, no language
delay (FR) (N = 83) LT1) (N = 10) language delay (FR-LT2) (N = 12) (NR-LT1) (N = 10) delay (NR) (N = 79)

M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD F df

2 years
Vocabulary production (CDI) 305.92 135.79 57.70 39.81 87.25 72.88 33.10 39.24 321.36 138.50
Maximum sentence length 5.67 2.24 2.10 1.12 2.70 1.47 1.63 1.30 6.47 2.67
(CDI)
Bayley expressive vocabulary 10.32 3.51 1.40 1.95 3.18 3.52 1.10 2.33 10.97 2.89
2.5 years

119
Reynell Receptive Language 37.072 6.07 36.502 2.27 26.751 3.86 36.402 2.99 37.912 5.83 10.40 4,184***
Scale
Reynell Developmental 36.05 5.00 28.50 2.84 24.08 5.23 22.60 8.47 35.87 4.55
Language Scale
2 1 1 1 2
Composite score for expressive 0.15 0.57 −1.31 0.17 −1.27 0.49 −1.63 0.46 0.28 0.58 56.44 4,189***
vocabulary

Notes: Groups with different subscripts (1,2) differed from each other in post-hoc comparisons, using LSD. The composite score for expressive vocabulary was calculated as average of the standardized
scores for the a) Vocabulary production, b) Maximum sentence length, c) Bayley expressive vocabulary and d) Reynell Developmental Language Scale. Standardized scores are based on the distribution of
the control group. The five groups (FR, FR-LT1, FR-LT2, NR-LT1, NR) were identified with respect to status of expressive and receptive vocabulary delay and family risk for dyslexia. The FR-LT1 (N = 10)
and the NR-LT1 (N = 10) groups had at least 1 SD below the mean of children in the control group in the composite score of expressive vocabulary but age-appropriate receptive skills. The FR-LT2
(N = 12) group demonstrated both an expressive and receptive vocabulary delay (at 1 SD below the mean on both scores). The FR (N = 83) and the NR (N = 79) groups had age-appropriate or better
early expressive and receptive vocabulary. *p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01. ***p ≤ 0.001.
Research in Developmental Disabilities 78 (2018) 114–124
M. Psyridou et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 78 (2018) 114–124

Table 2
Descriptive statistics and group comparisons for reading fluency and reading comprehension.
FR FR-LT1 FR-LT2 NR-LT1 NR

M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD F df

Grade 2
Reading Fluency z −0.261 0.95 −0.181,2 1.05 −0.191,2 0.98 0.211,2 1.01 0.362 0.95 4.40** 4,185
Reading comprehension 8.99 3.00 8.44 2.92 6.55 3.33 7.00 2.55 9.77 1.79 5.27*** 4,166
Grade 3
Reading fluency z −0.221 0.98 −0.281,2 1.25 −0.321 0.93 0.051,2 1.30 0.332 0.90 3.72** 4,189
Reading comprehension 9.761 1.76 10.201 1.32 8.642 2.01 10.001,2 2.78 10.151 1.40 2.12 4,170
Grade 8
Reading fluency z
−0.231 1.13 −0.281,2 0.96 −0.221,2 0.85 −0.031,2 1.55 0.372 0.68 3.95** 4,174
Grade 9
Reading comprehension 0.12 1
0.79 −0.17 1
0.73 −1.14 2
1.12 −0.15 1
1.09 0.25 1
0.90 5.91*** 4,153

Notes: Groups with different subscripts (1,2) differed from each other in post-hoc comparisons, using either LSD or Dunnett T3, depending on equality
of the variances. Subscript z refers to standardized score. The reading fluency measures are composite scores calculated as average of the stan-
dardized scores for the three reading fluency tasks. Standardized scores are based on the distribution of the control group.*p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01,
***p ≤ 0.001.

Table 3
Effect sizes for group comparisons in reading fluency and reading comprehension.
Effect sizea

FR vs FR- FR vs FR- FR vs NR- FR vs FR-LT1 vs FR-LT1 vs FR-LT1 vs FR-LT2 vs FR-LT2 vs NR-LT1 vs


LT1 LT2 LT1 NR FR-LT2 NR-LT1 NR NR-LT1 NR NR

Grade 2
Reading fluency 0.08 0.07 0.50 0.65 0.01 0.38 0.54 0.40 0.57 0.15
Reading comprehension 0.19 0.77 0.72 0.31 0.60 0.53 0.55 0.15 1.20 1.26
Grade 3
Reading fluency 0.05 0.10 0.23 0.58 0.04 0.26 0.56 0.33 0.71 0.25
Reading comprehension 0.29 0.60 0.10 0.25 0.92 0.09 0.04 0.56 0.88 0.07
Grade 8
Reading fluency 0.05 0.01 0.15 0.64 0.07 0.19 0.78 0.15 0.77 0.33
Grade 9
Reading comprehension 0.37 1.30 0.30 0.16 1.03 0.02 0.51 0.90 1.37 0.40

Note: Large (> 0.80) effect size with bold.


a
The value of Cohen’s d was calculated using the means and standard deviations of two groups. Cohen d is considered large when d > 0.80,
medium when d > 0.50 and small when d > 0.30.

suggested that children in the FR-LT2 group were poorer comprehenders than children in the FR, FR-LT1 and NR groups. The effect
sizes were medium for the FR-LT2 comparison with FR and large in comparison with the FR-LT1 and NR groups. In Grade 9, the
pairwise comparisons (LSD) showed lower reading comprehension scores for the FR-LT2 group than for all the other groups, and
these differences were accompanied by large effect sizes.

4.3. Correlations between early expressive and receptive vocabulary and literacy measures

Correlations (Table 4) showed that expressive and receptive vocabulary measures were strongly associated. Both expressive and
receptive vocabulary had stronger correlations with reading comprehension than reading fluency. The correlations were non-sig-
nificant between the vocabulary measures and Grade 8 reading fluency. In contrast, the correlations between expressive and re-
ceptive vocabulary and reading comprehension were statistically significant across all grades.

5. Discussion

The reading outcomes of children with expressive vocabulary delay were investigated in this study by analyzing their reading
development up to 16 years of age using measures of both reading fluency and comprehension. The role of the additional risk factors
of family risk for dyslexia and poor early receptive vocabulary were also examined. Five subgroups were identified based on chil-
dren’s early expressive vocabulary, receptive vocabulary and family risk status, and they were compared in reading fluency and
comprehension measures in Grades 2, 3, 8 and 9. In general, the findings suggest that early expressive language delay predicts
reading comprehension, but not reading fluency development. Late talking alone was not, however, a sufficient risk index for reading
comprehension difficulties for children with family risk for dyslexia or for children without such risk. Only the children with both
expressive and receptive vocabulary delays and family risk had clear difficulties in reading comprehension, and the difficulties were

120
M. Psyridou et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 78 (2018) 114–124

Fig. 1. Development of fluency skills by group for each grade.

Fig. 2. Development of reading comprehension by group for each grade.

sustained to adolescence.
The children who had delays in both expressive and receptive vocabulary as toddlers demonstrated persistent weaknesses in
comparison to the other groups in reading comprehension measures. As indicated by moderate to large effect sizes, these children had
lower scores compared to all the other groups at the end of Grade 2 and 3, and they continued on a declining trajectory to the end of
Grade 9. Although the comparison between all the groups in Grade 3 did not reveal statistically significant differences, the differences
were clear in pairwise comparisons and in the effect sizes of the group comparisons, which become larger from Grade 3 onwards. All
the groups except FR-LT2 performed at a similar level in Grade 3.
Our findings add to the literature (Duff et al., 2015; Lyytinen et al., 2005; Rescorla, 2002, 2005, 2009) by suggesting that a
trajectory of later reading comprehension difficulties is much more likely if early expressive vocabulary delays are accompanied by
receptive vocabulary delays. In our sample, the children with both receptive and expressive vocabulary delay also had family risk for
dyslexia, which makes it difficult to separate the effects of family risk and receptive vocabulary delay. However, because the other
family risk children (the group who did not have a vocabulary delay and the group who had only an expressive delay) manifested
average reading comprehension, it can be assumed that it is the combined vocabulary difficulty (expressive and receptive), rather

121
M. Psyridou et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 78 (2018) 114–124

Table 4
Correlation table for expressive and receptive vocabulary and reading fluency and reading comprehension in grades 2, 3, 8, and 9.
1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7.

1. Expressive vocabulary
2. Receptive vocabulary 0.49***
3. Reading fluency, Grade 2 0.16* 0.18*
4. Reading fluency, Grade 3 0.20** 0.23** 0.89***
5. Reading Fluency, Grade 8 0.11 0.12 0.71*** 0.79***
6. Reading comprehension, Grade 2 0.39*** 0.34*** 0.48*** 0.44*** 0.40***
7. Reading comprehension, Grade 3 0.19* 0.26*** 0.35*** 0.36*** 0.38*** 0.45***
8. Reading comprehension, grade 9 0.32*** 0.30*** 0.29*** 0.40*** 0.33*** 0.37*** 0.45***

* p ≤ 0.05.
** p ≤ 0.01.
*** p ≤ 0.001.

than family risk per se, that underlies reading comprehension difficulties. Moreover, the fact that the combination of receptive and
expressive vocabulary delays was more common in the family risk group (12 children) than in the no-risk group (only three children)
suggests a link between family risk for reading difficulties and combined vocabulary delay.
The findings suggest that a delay in early vocabulary can lead to a reading comprehension deficit, which is in line with sug-
gestions in the previous studies (Duff et al., 2015; Lyytinen et al., 2005; Rescorla, 2005, 2009), with the specification that an
expressive vocabulary deficit alone can be alleviated in time, whereas the combined deficit is a stronger risk marker. Previous studies
have also shown correlative evidence for the predictive association between language skills and reading comprehension (Catts et al.,
2006; Duff et al., 2015; Nation et al., 2010). The present findings are in line with Rescorla’s (2005) findings indicating that children
with delays in early vocabulary demonstrated no differences in basic reading mechanisms (including fluency) at age 13, but they had
significantly lower scores in reading comprehension. In addition, they are in line with the findings of Duff et al. (2015) suggesting
that early vocabulary (in their study combination of expressive and receptive) has a stronger relationship with reading compre-
hension than reading accuracy. Because early vocabulary was measured at the age of 2–2.5 years in the present study, which was long
before reading had started to develop and which prevented any concerns about the direction of effects, a predictive association
between early vocabulary and reading comprehension outcomes could be suggested. This link between reading comprehension and
early vocabulary could be explained by the simple view of reading model (Gough & Tumner, 1986; Hoover & Gough, 1990). Reading
comprehension has been shown to rely on a variety of oral language comprehension skills (Hulme & Snowling, 2014), including
expressive and receptive vocabulary (e.g. Nation et al., 2010) and listening comprehension (e.g. Catts et al., 2006; Nation et al.,
2010). Weaknesses in these skills have been found to be manifested before learning to read, which could provide a possible causal
link between the influence of oral language comprehension on difficulties in reading comprehension (e.g. Catts et al., 2006; Nation
et al., 2010).
In contrast to reading comprehension, reading fluency was found to be linked with family risk for dyslexia and not with the
presence of early expressive vocabulary delay. The finding that family risk was predictive of reading fluency was expected and
supports previous studies indicating the strong effects of family risk in developmental dyslexia (Lyytinen et al., 2015; Snowling et al.,
2003; van Bergen, de Jong, Plakas, Maassen, & van der Leij, 2012). Based on our findings, it is thus likely that children with reading
difficulties who show early vocabulary difficulties have additional risk factors, such as family risk (Nation & Snowling, 2004;
Snowling et al., 2003; Torppa et al., 2010).
The finding of no link from late talking to reading fluency development appears to contradict the lexical restructuring hypothesis
(e.g. Walley et al., 2003), which suggests a link between early vocabulary and reading fluency mediated via phonological skills. In
addition, we have shown earlier in this same sample that early vocabulary is linked via phonological skills to kindergarten precocious
reading ability (Torppa et al., 2007), supporting the lexical restructuring hypothesis. However, in the present study, we focused on a
phase in reading development where phonological skills may not be strong predictors of reading fluency anymore, at least in the
context of a transparent orthography (see e.g. Aarnoutse, van Leeuwe, & Verhoeven, 2005; Georgiou, Torppa, Manolitsis, Lyytinen, &
Parrila, 2012). Thus, the mediated effect from vocabulary also ceases to exist. In the English language context, phonological pro-
cessing is a strong predictor of reading for a longer time (e.g. Gallagher, Frith, & Snowling, 2000; Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte,
1994). In line with this, our results for the early grades show differences only in reading comprehension, while the studies from the
English language context report differences in various reading skills (Duff et al., 2015; Rescorla, 2002). In later grades, however, our
results are similar to the findings in English language context studies (Rescorla, 2005, 2009). It seems that the transparency of the
orthography affects the speed of decoding development (e.g. Ellis et al., 2004; Seymour et al., 2003) and the importance of pho-
nological processing in the development. Consequently, the mediated impact of expressive and/or receptive vocabulary delay via
phonological processing to reading skills may thus be broader and may be sustained longer in the less transparent orthographies.
The limitations in this study include its small sample size and the inclusion of only one reading comprehension measure at each
age. Analyses with a larger sample size would have more statistical power. However, samples with extensive assessments and such a
long follow-up are rare and provide unique information. Further, it was not possible to identify a large enough group with difficulties
in both expressive and receptive vocabulary but no family risk, which would have been informative. In addition, it would have been
interesting to know how these children scored in expressive and receptive vocabulary at adolescence and whether the early language

122
M. Psyridou et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 78 (2018) 114–124

difficulties experienced persisted and co-occurred with literacy skills in adolescence. Based on the findings by Rescorla (2005, 2009),
we expect that this is the case, but unfortunately, we could not examine the issue due to the lack of such data in our sample.
Moreover, data on family risk for reading comprehension or language skills would have been an interesting addition.
In conclusion, the findings indicate that the reading trajectories of late talkers depended on the co-occurrence of other risk factors
and on the nature of the reading outcome in question. Early expressive vocabulary delay represented a high risk only for reading
comprehension development and only when it co-occurred with other risk factors (family risk for dyslexia and receptive vocabulary
difficulties). Late talking without receptive vocabulary difficulties did not increase the risk for reading difficulties among either group
(family risk or no family risk). Our results indicate that in addition to expressive vocabulary delay, it is critical to recognize a delay in
receptive skills as an early risk factor, since a delay in only expressive vocabulary is not informative enough to predict school age
reading development. Based on the findings of the present study and those by Rescorla (2002, 2005, 2009), it is suggested that
children with expressive vocabulary delay (especially those with family risk for dyslexia and receptive vocabulary delay as additional
risk factors) should be provided extra support (e.g. exposure to language games and book reading). Finally, based on the results of the
present study and those of previous studies, we suggest that the effects of receptive vocabulary should be examined more in depth,
since all the studies so far have used a combination of expressive and receptive vocabulary delays. In addition, certain protective
factors could lead to cognitive and socio-emotional resilience and influence the reading outcomes of children with reading diffi-
culties. Based on the current findings, one potential protective cognitive skill may be receptive vocabulary. In line with the results of
the study conducted by Haft, Myers and Hoeft (2016), we propose that future studies include investigations of the factors that
contribute to the resilience of students who have reading difficulties or who are at risk of developing them.

Funding

Funding for this research was received from funding agencies in the public and not-for-profit sectors over the years. The major
funder has been the Academy of Finland (current grants #276239, #284439, and #313768).

References

Aarnoutse, C., van Leeuwe, J., & Verhoeven, L. (2005). Early literacy from a longitudinal perspective. Educational Review and Research, 11(3), 253–275. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1080/08993400500101054.
Aro, M., & Wimmer, H. (2003). Learning to read: English in comparison to six more regular orthographies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(4), 621–635. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1017/S0142716403000316.
Aro, M. (2016). In L. T. W. Verhoeven, & C. A. Perfetti (Eds.). Reading acquisition across languages and writing systems: An international handbook. Cambridge: University
Press.
Asikainen, M. (2005). Diagnosing specific language impairment. Finland: Academic dissertation, University of Tampere, Acta Electronica Universitatis Tamperensis.
Bayley, N. (1993). The bayley scales of infant development (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., & Weismer, S. E. (2006). Language deficits in poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of reading. Journal of Speech Language and
Hearing Research, 49(2), 278–293. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/023).
Clarke, P. J., Snowling, M. J., Truelove, E., & Hulme, C. (2010). Ameliorating children’s reading-comprehension difficulties: A randomized controlled trial.
Psychological Science, 21(8), 1106–1116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797610375449.
Duff, F. J., Reen, G., Plunkett, K., & Nation, K. (2015). Do infant vocabulary skills predict school-age language and literacy outcomes? Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 56(8), 848–856. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12378.
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B. V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read:
Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36(3), 250–287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.36.3.2.
Eklund, K., Torppa, M., Aro, M., Leppänen, P. H. T., & Lyytinen, H. (2015). Literacy skill development of children with familial risk for dyslexia through grades 2, 3,
and 8. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(1), 126–140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037121.
Ellis, N. C., Natsume, I., Stavropoulou, K., Hoxhallari, L., van Daal, V. H. P., Polyzoe, N., et al. (2004). The effects of the orthographic depth on learning to read
alphabetic, syllabic, and logographic scripts. Reading Research Quarterly, 39(4), 438–468. http://dx.doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.39.4.5.
Feldman, H. M., Dale, P. S., Campbell, T. F., Colborn, D. K., Kurs-Lasky, M., Rockette, H. E., & Paradise, J. L. (2005). Concurrent and predictive validity of parent
reports of child language at ages 2 and 3 years. Child Development, 76(4), 856–868. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00882.x.
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., Pethick, S. J., & Stiles, J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society
for Research in Child Development, 59 [(5, Ser. no. 242), i–185.
Fricke, S., Bowyer-Crane, C., Haley, A. J., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2013). Efficacy of language intervention in the early years. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 54(3), 280–290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12010.
Gallagher, A., Frith, U., & Snowling, M. J. (2000). Precursors of literacy delay among children at genetic risk of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
41(2), 203–213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00601.
Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency across languages varying in orthographic consistency.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(3), 566–580. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.566.
Georgiou, G. K., Torppa, M., Manolitsis, G., Lyytinen, H., & Parrila, R. (2012). Longitudinal predictors of reading and spelling across languages varying in orthographic
consistency. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25(2), 321–346. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9271-x.
Gough, P. B., & Tumner, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 6–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
074193258600700104.
Häyrinen, T., Serenius-Sirve, S., & Korkman, M. (1999). Lukilasse. Lukemisen, kirjoittamisen ja laskemisen seulontatesti ala-asteen luokille 1–6. Screening test for reading,
spelling and counting for the grades 1–6. Helsinki: Psykologien Kustannus Oy.
Haft, S. L., Myers, C. A., & Hoeft, F. (2016). Socio-emotional and cognitive resilience in children with reading disabilities. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10,
133–141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.06.005.
Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2(2), 127–160. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
BF00401799.
Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2014). The interface between spoken and written language: Developmental disorders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:
Biological Sciences, 369(1634), 20120395. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0395.
Leinonen, S., Müller, K., Leppänen, P. H. T., Aro, M., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (2001). Heterogeneity in adult dyslexic readers: Relating processing skills to the speed
and accuracy of oral text reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14(3–4), 265–296.
Lervåg, A., Bråten, I., & Hulme, C. (2009). The cognitive and linguistic foundations of early reading development: A Norwegian latent variable longitudinal study.

123
M. Psyridou et al. Research in Developmental Disabilities 78 (2018) 114–124

Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 764–781. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014132.


Lindeman, J. (2000). Ala-asteen lukutesti: Tekniset tiedot ([2. p.]). ALLU Reading test for primary school: Technical information. Turku, Finland: University of Turku, Centre
for Research on Learning.
Lyytinen, P., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2005). Language development and literacy skills in late-talking toddlers with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Annals of
Dyslexia, 55(2), 166–192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11881-005-0010-y.
Lyytinen, H., Aro, M., Richardson, U., Erskine, J., Banff, A., Li, U. H., & Shu, H. (2015). Reading skills, acquisition of: Cultural, environmental, and developmental
impediments. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences (Second Edition), 5–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.23111-5.
Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2),
322–352.
Moyle, M. J., Weismer, S. E., Evans, J. L., & Lindstrom, M. J. (2007). Longitudinal relationships between lexical and grammatical development in typical and late-
talking children. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 50(2), 508–528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/035).
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading,
27(4), 342–356. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2004.00238.x.
Nation, K., Cocksey, J., Taylor, J. S., & Bishop, D. V. (2010). A longitudinal investigation of early reading and language skills in children with poor reading com-
prehension. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(9), 1031–1039. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02254.x.
OECD (2010). PISA 2009 results: Learning trends: Changes in student performance since 2000, vol. V.
OECD (2013). PISA 2012 results: What students know and can do: Student performance in mathematics, reading and science, vol. I.
Ouellette, G., & Beers, A. (2010). A not-so-simple view of reading: How oral vocabulary and visual-word recognition complicate the story. Reading and Writing, 23(2),
189–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9159-1.
Ouellette, G. P. (2006). What's meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(3),
554–566. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.554.
Poll, G. H., & Miller, C. A. (2013). Late talking, typical talking, and weak language skills at middle childhood. Learning and Individual Differences, 26, 177–184. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.01.008.
Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1992). Standard progressive matrices. Oxford, UK: Oxford Psychologists Press.
Reilly, S., Wake, M., Ukoumunne, O. C., Bavin, E., Prior, M., Cini, E., ... Bretherton, L. (2010). Predicting language outcomes at 4 years of age: Findings from early
language in Victoria study. Pediatrics, 126(6), e1530–e1537. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-0254.
Rescorla, L. (2002). Language and reading outcomes to age 9 in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 45(2), 360–371. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/028).
Rescorla, L. (2005). Age 13 language and reading outcomes in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 48(2), 459–472. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1044/1092-4388(2005/031).
Rescorla, L. (2009). Age 17 language and reading outcomes in late-talking toddlers: Support for a dimensional perspective on language delay. Journal of Speech,
Language & Hearing Research, 52(1), 16–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0171).
Reynell, J. K., & Huntley, M. (1987). Reynell developmental language scales manual (4th ed.). Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson.
Ricketts, J., Nation, K., & Bishop, D. (2007). Vocabulary is important for some, but not all reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(3), 235–257. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1080/10888430701344306.
Rose, L. T., & Rouhani, P. (2012). Influence of verbal working memory depends on vocabulary: Oral reading fluency in adolescents with dyslexia. Mind, Brain, and
Education, 6(1), 1–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-228X.2011.01135.x.
Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94(2), 143–174. http://dx.
doi.org/10.1348/000712603321661859.
Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an outlier orthography. Psychological Bulletin,
134(4), 584–615. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584.
Snowling, M. J., & Melby-Lervåg, M. (2016). Oral language deficits in familial dyslexia: A meta-analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 142(5), 498–545. http://dx.
doi.org/10.1037/bul0000037.
Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development,
74(2), 358–373. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.7402003.
Snowling, M. J., Muter, V., & Carroll, J. (2007). Children at family risk of dyslexia: A follow-up in early adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(6),
609–618. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01725.x.
Thal, D. J., Miller, S., Carlson, J., & Vega, M. M. (2005). Nonword repetition and language development in 4-year-old children with and without a history of early
language delay. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 48(6), 1481–1495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/103).
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27(5), 276–286.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221949402700503.
Torppa, M., Poikkeus, A., Laakso, M., Tolvanen, A., Leskinen, E., Leppanen, P. H. T., & Lyytinen, H. (2007). Modeling the early paths of phonological awareness and
factors supporting its development in children with and without familial risk of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(2), 73–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
10888430709336554.
Torppa, M., Lyytinen, P., Erskine, J., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2010). Language development, literacy skills, and predictive connections to reading in Finnish
children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(4), 308–321. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219410369096.
Walley, A. C., Metsala, J. L., & Garlock, V. M. (2003). Spoken vocabulary growth: Its role in the development of phoneme awareness and early reading ability. Reading
and Writing, 16(1–2), 5–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1021789804977.
van Bergen, E., de Jong, P. F., Plakas, A., Maassen, B., & van der Leij, A. (2012). Child and parental literacy levels within families with a history of dyslexia. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(1), 28–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02418.x.

124

You might also like